You are on page 1of 864

How to go to your page

This eBook contains three volumes. Each volume has its own page numbering scheme,
consisting of a volume number and a page number, separated by a colon.
For example, to go to page 5 of Volume 1, type vol1:5 in the page # box at the top of the
screen and click Go. To go to page 5 of Volume 2, type vol2:5 in the "page #" box and
so forth.

Feminism and Womens


Rights Worldwide

Recent Titles in
Womens Psychology
Intimate Violence against Women: When Spouses, Partners, or Lovers Attack
Paula K. Lundberg-Love and Shelly L. Marmion, editors
Daughters of Madness: Growing Up and Older with a Mentally Ill Mother
Susan Nathiel
Psychology of Women: Handbook of Issues and Theories, Second Edition
Florence L. Denmark and Michele Paludi, editors
WomanSoul: The Inner Life of Womens Spirituality
Carole A. Rayburn and Lillian Comas-Diaz, editors
The Psychology of Women at Work: Challenges and Solutions for Our Female
Workforce
Michele A. Paludi, editor

Feminism and Womens


Rights Worldwide
Volume 1
Heritage, Roles, and Issues

MICHELE A. PALUDI, EDITOR


Praeger Perspectives
Womens Psychology
Michele A. Paludi, Series Editor

PRAEGER
An Imprint of ABC-CLIO, LLC

Copyright 2010 by Michele A. Paludi


All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced,
stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any
means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise,
except for the inclusion of brief quotations in a review, without prior
permission in writing from the publisher.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Feminism and womens rights worldwide / Michele A. Paludi, editor.
v. ; cm. (Womens psychology)
Includes bibliographical references and index.
Contents: The myth of the man-hating feminist / Melinda Kanner and
Kristin J. Anderson Gender differences : the arguments regarding abilities /
Jennifer L. Martin Women in education : students and professors worldwide
/ Susan Basow In womens voices / Samantha Smith Working life as a
house : a tale of oors, walls, and ceilings / Leanne Faraday-Brash Women
as religious leaders : advances and stalemates / J. Harold Ellens The
feminine political persona : Queen Victoria, Ellen Johnson Sirleaf, and Michelle
Bachelet / Emily A. Haddad and William Schweinle Women in the military : is
it time to un-gender combat roles? / Breena E. Coates Sexual minority
women : sources and outcomes of stigmatization / Rhonda M. Schultz, and
Kristin P. Beals Special issues for women with disabilities / Martha E.
Banks Body dissatisfaction and disordered eating : the globalization of
western appearance ideals / Jaehee Jung and Gordon B. Forbes Sexual
violence to girls and women in schools around the world / Susan Strauss.
ISBN 978-0-313-37596-5 (set : hard copy : alk. paper) ISBN 978-0-31337597-2 (set : ebook) ISBN 978-0-313-37598-9 (v.1 : hard copy : alk. paper)
ISBN 978-0-313-37599-6 (v.1 : ebook) ISBN 978-0-313-37600-9 (v.2 : hard
copy : alk. paper) ISBN 978-0-313-37601-6 (v.2 : ebook) ISBN 978-0-31337602-3 (v.3 : hard copy : alk. paper) ISBN 978-0-313-37603-0 (v.3 : ebook)
1. Feminism. 2. Womens rights. 3. Sexual harassment of women. 4. Abused
womenPsychology. 5. WomenPsychology. I. Paludi, Michele Antoinette
HQ1180.F424 2010
305.42dc22
2009035343
ISBN: 978-0-313-37596-5
EISBN: 978-0-313-37597-2
14 13 12

11 10

2 3 4 5

This book is also available on the World Wide Web as an eBook.


Visit www.abc-clio.com for details.
Praeger
An Imprint of ABC-CLIO, LLC
ABC-CLIO, LLC
130 Cremona Drive, P.O. Box 1911
Santa Barbara, California 93116-1911
This book is printed on acid-free paper
Manufactured in the United States of America

For Rosa and Lucia, my maternal and paternal grandmothers


and for Antoinette, my mother:
Remember, our heritage is our power; we can know ourselves and
our capacities by seeing that other women have been strong.
Judy Chicago

This page intentionally left blank

Contents

Series Introduction

ix

Acknowledgments

xi

Introduction
Michele A. Paludi
Chapter 1: The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist
Melinda Kanner and Kristin J. Anderson

xiii
1

Chapter 2: Gender Differences: The Arguments Regarding Abilities


Jennifer L. Martin

27

Chapter 3: Women in Education: Students and Professors


Worldwide
Susan Basow

43

Chapter 4: In Womens Voices


Samantha Smith

63

Chapter 5: Working Life as a House: A Tale of Floors, Walls, and


Ceilings
Leanne Faraday-Brash

65

Chapter 6: Women as Religious Leaders: Advances and Stalemates


J. Harold Ellens

85

Chapter 7: The Feminine Political Persona: Queen Victoria, Ellen


Johnson Sirleaf, and Michelle Bachelet
Emily A. Haddad and William Schweinle

97

Chapter 8: Women in the Military: Is It Time to Un-Gender Combat


Roles?
Breena E. Coates
111

viii

Contents

Chapter 9: Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes


of Stigmatization
Rhonda M. Schultz and Kristin P. Beals

125

Chapter 10: Special Issues for Women with Disabilities


Martha E. Banks

149

Chapter 11: Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating: The


Globalization of Western Appearance Ideals
Jaehee Jung and Gordon B. Forbes

161

Chapter 12: Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools


around the World
Susan Strauss

187

Womens Studies Programs in the United States


Michele A. Paludi

233

Appendix:

About the Editor and Contributors

249

Index

255

Series Introduction

Because womens work is never done and is underpaid or unpaid or boring


or repetitious and were the rst to get red and what we look like is more
important than what we do and if we get raped its our fault and if we get
beaten we must have provoked it and if we raise our voices were nagging
bitches and if we enjoy sex were nymphos and if we dont were frigid and
if we love women its because we cant get a real man and if we ask our
doctor too many questions were neurotic and/or pushy and if we expect
childcare were selsh and if we stand up for our rights were aggressive
and unfeminine and if we dont were typical weak females and if we
want to get married were out to trap a man and if we dont were unnatural and because we still cant get an adequate safe contraceptive but men
can walk on the moon and if we cant cope or dont want a pregnancy were
made to feel guilty about abortion and . . . for lots of other reasons we are
part of the womens liberation movement.
Author unknown, quoted in The Torch, September 14, 1987

These sentiments underlie the major goals of the Praeger Perspectives


book series, Womens Psychology. The goals are as follows:
Value women: The books in this series value women by valuing children and working for affordable child care; value women by respecting
all physiques, not just by placing value on slender women; value
women by acknowledging older womens wisdom, beauty, aging; value
women who have been sexually victimized and view them as survivors;
value women who work inside and outside of the home; and value
women by respecting their choices of careers, of whom they mentor, of
their reproductive rights, their spirituality, and their sexuality.
Treat women as the norm. Thus the books in this series make up for
womens issues typically being omitted, trivialized, or dismissed from
other books on psychology.

Series Introduction

Take a non-Eurocentric view of womens experiences. The books in this


series integrate the scholarship on race and ethnicity into womens psychology, thus providing a psychology of all women. Women typically
have been described collectively; but we are diverse.
Facilitate connections between readers experiences and psychological theories and empirical research. The books in this series offer readers opportunities to challenge their views about women, feminism, sexual
victimization, gender role socialization, education, and equal rights.
These texts thus encourage women readers to value themselves and
others. The accounts of womens experiences as reected through
research and personal stories in the texts in this series have been
included for readers to derive strength from the efforts of others who
have worked for social change on the interpersonal, organizational,
and societal levels. A student in one of my courses on the psychology
of women once stated:
I learned so much about women. Women face many issues: discrimination, sexism, prejudices . . . by society. Women need to work together to
change how society views us. I learned so much and talked about much
of the issues brought up in class to my friends and family. My attitudes
have changed toward a lot of things. I got to look at myself, my life, and
what I see for the future. (Paludi, 2002)

It is my hope that readers of the books in this series will also reect
on the topics and look at themselves, their own lives, and what they
see for the future. This three-volume book set on Feminism and Womens Rights Worldwide provides readers with the opportunity to accomplish this goal and offers suggestions for all of us working for
gender justice within our friendships and romantic relationships, in
guiding institutional and social policy change in workplace and educational institutions, and in lobbying state and federal legislators on
issues related to reproductive rights, pay equity, education, sexual violence, and childcare.
Michele A. Paludi
Series Editor
REFERENCE
Paludi, M. (2002). The psychology of women. 2nd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.

Acknowledgments

Teaching and writing are separate, but serve/feed one another in so many
ways. Writing travels the road inward, teaching, the road outhelping
OTHERS move inwardit is an honor to be with others in the spirit of
writing and encouragement.
Naomi Shihab Nye

Nyes sentiment is echoed throughout this three-volume set on feminism and womens rights. Most of the contributors have taught courses
in womens studies and feminism as well as conducted research and
written about feminist issues. Many contributors have been advocates
on behalf of feminist principles through working with local, state and
federal agencies, legislators, and the United Nations. And many of us
have collaborated with students in our classes in writing chapters for
this book set. These students have made us believe that all of them, in
their individual ways, will continue to do what this book set intends:
value feminism and work toward equality. It has been exhilarating for
me to see a new generation of feminists collaborating with mentors
and colleagues on the chapters for this book set.
I have been honored to have collaborated with the contributors to
these volumes. Several friendships with contributors have been
rekindled and strengthened, and I have met many new colleagues from
around the world who taught me about their disciplines through their
writing. You have all shown me the great accomplishments of feminists
as well as the work we have yet to do. Thank you.
I wish to thank my sisters, Rosalie Paludi and Lucille Paludi, for
their support during the preparation of this book set. I also thank Carmen Paludi, Jr. for his guidance and encouragement. Our discussions
about feminism brought back wonderful memories of my mother,

xii

Acknowledgments

Antoinette, and my father, Michael, about whom I continue to learn


and continue to cherish the time I had with them.
I acknowledge several friends who encouraged me during the preparation of this set of books. Thank you to Paula Lundberg Love, Jennifer Martin, Billie Wright Dziech, Darlene C. DeFour, and Florence
Denmark.
I have been fortunate to have had the opportunity to work with students throughout my career, now at Union Graduate College. I have
thoroughly enjoyed learning from them. Thank you to students in the
Human Resource Management Certicate Program and Management
and Leadership Certicate Program. I especially acknowledge Michelle
Strand, Carrie Turco, Haimanot Kelbessa, Sarah Bennett, Sarah Boggess, Kristina Hicks, James Luciano, Sarah Henderson Maneely, Abbey
Massoud-Tastor, Marie Fuda, Jessica Wilmot, Katie Kelly, and Nick Salvatoriello. I am honored you have called me your professor.
I also thank Debbie Carvalko for supporting my visions for books
and helping them become realities. I have enjoyed working with Debbie and her colleagues at Praeger. They are a wonderful team of caring
people. They appreciate my love of writing and editing books. Debbie
somehow knew that, after the publication of the three-volume set on
the Psychology of Women and Work (2008, Praeger), which I edited, and
the political climate of the 2008 presidential campaign, especially
regarding women, I had to follow up those texts with books on feminism. She knows I share Sheila Benders sentiment:
We write because something inside says we must and we can no longer ignore
that voice.

Introduction
Michele A. Paludi
And how do you look backward? By looking forward. And what do you see?
As they look forward, they see what they had to do before they could look
backward. And there we have it all.
Gertrude Stein

Alyssa Zucker and Abigail Stewart (2007) reported in their study of


333 university alumnae that feminism is internalized quite differently
depending on the developmental stage in our lives. This research led
me to consider my own feminist socialization and feminist identity development as I began writing and editing these three volumes on feminism and womens rights. I was introduced to feminism by my
parents, Antoinette and Michael, at a very young age, even though the
label feminism was not used by them. Yet, as I came to realize much
later, their behavior was very much in keeping with feminist principles. They valued my sisters and me unconditionally; wanted to give
us educational opportunities that were denied to them because of the
generation into which they were born and because they were rst generation Americans whose parents had other values to instill in them;
they worked for equality in relationships, politics, and health care. I
was 18 the year individuals became eligible to vote at age 18, and both
my parents took me to cast my votes that year.
They believed that, like them, I had a responsibility to make things
better for the next generation. They valued voting; I was told what the
Suffragists had endured in order to win this right for us and to remember this each year I vote. I took my rst course in feminism as an
undergraduate in the early 1970s: Sex Roles in American Society
with Nancy Walbek. I would share the class discussions with my
mother, telling her about the experiences of students in class that were
different from my ownfor example, being denied the use of certain

xiv

Introduction

toys considered sex inappropriate for them; being tracked into different high school and college programs because of being women or men;
women being told by family and friends to hide their achievements
from potential dates and mates. I was unable to relate to these experiences and realized for the rst time that my parents were feminists, a
term to which I was introduced formally in this class and then subsequently as a graduate student when I took courses with Dee Graham
and Edna Rawlings. I also learned that I had been exposed to nonstereotyped role models, and because there were all girls in our family, we
were not raised to conform to stereotyped behavior.
It was in graduate school that I decided to pursue research in feminist psychology, especially in womens career development. I was fortunate to have a mentor, William Dember, who encouraged me to
pursue this research, even though it was not in his area of specialization (i.e., visual perception). Bill encouraged me to take courses with
faculty in departments in addition to psychology: educational leadership and family development. He told me this would help put pieces
together in understanding the research I was conducting. I thank Kathy
Borman and Judy Frankel for their roles in my feminist identity development.
A few years later when my father died, Charlie, who attended my
fathers wake, came to my mother, my sisters, and me and told us how
my father had impacted his life. Charlie, an African American man,
told us my father was the only coworker (both were skilled workers at
General Electric) who treated him fairly, didnt talk with him in a derogatory manner, and stopped others from making racial slurs and epithets. I learned for another time what it meant to be a feminist.
I dedicated the three-volume set on the Psychology of Women at Work
to my parents: For Antoinette and Michael Paludi, who encouraged
me to dene what womens work is for myself. They wanted all their
daughters to be independent thinkers and doers and to help others.
They gave us no templates to follow but encouraged us to navigate our
own paths. And, especially in my case, encouraged me to leave home
to attend graduate school in a city that seemed, to my parents, to be
very far awaybut they never said no.
My parents thus taught me that not only did they believe in the economic, educational, social, and political equality of women and men, but
they favored the social and legal changes necessary to achieve equality
between the sexes and among races, and they were committed to implementing these principles. Perhaps they could not effect change at the
national level, but they did do so in personal relationships with their
family and friends and on the local level. This is the legacy they left my
sisters and me. This book set is a tribute to Antoinette and Michael.
I have been reminded of Antoinette and Michael throughout the
writing and editing of these volumes on feminism and womens rights.

Introduction

xv

I am especially reminded of what my mother used to tell me: You are


there before you get there. She knew I wanted equality to happen fast
and that I grew concerned when feminists didnt win political elections, when younger women didnt know the heritage of how they
came to be accepted in graduate programs and in certain jobs, how the
glass ceiling for women and people of color is still strong, and that
worldwide, women constitute 64 percent of all adults who are illiterate
(see Susan Basows chapter in Volume 1). I have learned that she was
right; that change takes time, and to measure change differently, i.e., in
increments. As Secretary of State Hillary Clinton stated as she suspended her campaign for president of the United States in 2008:
Although we werent able to shatter this highest, hardest glass ceiling
this time, thanks to you, its got about 18 million cracks in it, and the
light is shining through like never before.
The chapters in these volumes show us where the light is shining
through on feminism. All three volumes represent what Judith Lorber
(1998) and Snelling (1999) identied: several types of feminism and
feminists. Lorber (1998) categorized feminism into three major areas:
gender reform, gender resistance, and gender rebellion. Genderreform feminism emphasizes similarities between women and men
rather than focusing on differences between them. Gender-resistance
feminism holds that formal legal rights alone will not end gender
inequality; male dominance is too ingrained into social relations.
Gender-resistance feminism focuses on how men and women are
differentcognitively, emotionally, and sociallyand urges women
to form women-centered organizations and communities. Genderrebellion feminism looks at the interrelationships among inequalities
of sex, race, ethnicity, social class, and sexual orientation. A number
of years ago my text on the psychology of women displayed a quilt
on its cover (Paludi, 2002). I asked for this design to highlight
Gentrys (1989) image of quilt making for understanding feminism.
These three volumes on feminism and womens rights also represent
quilt making in understanding feminism. Each contributor has made
one piece of the quilt that has been joined with pieces by other contributors. Each of the contributors has used different stitching on their
piece of the quilt. No one chapter is more important than the other.
We need all pieces if we are to complete the quilt that is feminism.
According to Gentry (1989):
Feminist psychology and feminism in general seem to be at the point of
trying to piece together the individual parts of a quilt. The overall pattern of the quilt that we want is still emerging. No one knows what
equality in a post-patriarchal world will look like. We are beginning to
piece the separate parts togetherto explore the kinds of stitching to use
in connecting the pieces and how to place the separate pieces into the

xvi

Introduction
pattern. But we have not stopped questioning the process of quilting
itself.

In Volume 1, Heritage, Roles, and Issues, contributors have discussed


efforts to integrate feminist scholarship into several disciplines, including education, work, science, military, religion, and politics. As Catherine Stimpson (1971) noted, there have been three kinds of problems in
the disciplines and curriculum with respect to women: omission, distortions, and trivializations. Each of the contributors to Volume 1 notes
where the sexism in the disciplines has existed and where feminist correctives have restructured the disciplines. Jennifer Martin, in her chapter concerning gender differences in abilities, noted:
Women have made signicant social, academic, and occupational gains
in the past 50 years; for example, women are entering nontraditional
elds with more frequency, participating in high school and college
sports more than ever before, and carving out more egalitarian roles for
themselves within the family. However, women have still not ultimately
achieved true equity with their male counterparts. . . . The idea that
women somehow possess different or inferior aptitudes when compared
to their male counterparts can lead to diminished expectations for
womenin terms of how they view themselves and how others view
them.

In Volume 2, Mental and Physical Health, contributors deal with violence and discrimination against girls and women and the resulting
impact on womens emotional and physical well being, interpersonal
relationships, career development, and self-concept. Types of discrimination and victimization addressed are sexual harassment, sexual violence, harassment of sexual minorities, and rape and violence in the
context of womens HIV risk. Contributors have addressed these issues
globally. Bethany Waits and Paula Lundberg-Love offer new cutting
edge evidence on neurological responses in women victims of sexual
violence. Therapeutic support for women victims of violence is also
addressed in this volume, including feminist therapy and ethnocultural
psychotherapy.
All contributors note that sexual victimization is prevalent in the
United States and globally, as is sexual harassment and sexual orientation discrimination. As Waits and Lundberg-Love note:
Female survivors of sexual violence are everywhere. They are in universities, religious institutions, court rooms, hospitals, and the military. They
are daughters, mothers, spouses, sisters, friends, next-door neighbors,
and co-workers. Many differ in age, education, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. . . . However, their lives are connected by the violence that
they have experienced.

Introduction

xvii

The international focus on feminism and womens rights is continued in Volume 3, Feminism as Human Rights. In this volume, contributors address laws on sexual harassment, pay equity, and rape.
Furthermore, contributors speak to the injustices to women with disabilities. Human rights issues such as arranged and forced marriage
for women, pornography, and the globalization of western appearance
ideals are also presented in this volume. All contributors to this volume call for further advocacy on behalf of women. As Noorfarah Merali stated:
It is only if arranged marriages are understood in light of their intentions, diverse forms, actual outcomes, and local or international contexts
that laws, policies, and human rights advocacy can be appropriately
channeled to protect and preserve womens well-being.

In addition to the scholarly reviews of research on feminism and


womens rights, I have included womens personal accounts of their
own feminist identity development. They are at different stages in life,
in their career, and in relationships and yet they are bound by shared
stories.
It is my hope that these volumes encourage individuals to self identify as feminists. Research has suggested for some time that most people reject the term feminist when describing themselves but support
feminist principlesequal pay for equal work, for example (see Paludi
et al., Volume 3). Goldners (1994) study noted that when women who
hold feminist beliefs anticipate a negative reaction from their peers to
the label feminist, they will avoid using the term to describe themselves. Goldner indicated that media is a primary source of negative
images of feminists. It is common to see photos of women identied as
feminists having clenched sts. These images are not representative of
feminists. More recent research by Rudman and Fairchild (2007) found
that the stereotype that feminists are unattractive still persists.
However, these images are rejected by individuals, especially during
adolescence and young adulthood, when maintaining gender role stereotypic behavior is reinforced and is central to their self-esteem and
self-concept. Paludi, Paludi, and DeFour (2004) noted that individuals
reject the label feminist because they view themselves as in control, as
powerful rather than as victims of gender inequality. Thus, they perceive the term feminist to imply a powerless position, which they
reject (Rhode, 1977).
The contributors to each of the three volumes of Feminism and Womens Rights Worldwide encourage us to think critically about feminism,
to value cultural experiences and to integrate our knowledge of theories and research about feminism with our own life experiences. The
chapters encouraged me to do this in remembering my own feminist

xviii

Introduction

socialization. I encourage you to do the same. It is my hope these three


volumes serve as a life raft (Klonis, Endo, Crosby, and Worell, 1997)
for feminists, especially those in the millennial generation.
REFERENCES
Gentry, M. (1989). Introduction: Feminist perspectives on gender and thought:
Paradox and potential. In M. Crawford & M. Gentry (Eds.), Gender and
thought. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Goldner, M. (1994). Accounting for race and class variation in the disjuncture
between feminist identity and feminist beliefs: The place of negative
labels and social movements. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of
the American Sociological Association, Los Angeles.
Klonis, S., Endo, J., Crosby, F., & Worell, J. (1997). Feminism as life raft. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 333345.
Lorber, J. (1998). Gender inequality: Feminist theories and politics. Los Angeles:
Roxbury.
Paludi, M. (2002). The psychology of women. 2nd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Paludi, M., ed. (2008). The psychology of women at work: Challenges and solutions
for our female workforce. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Paludi, M., Paludi, C., & DeFour, D. (2004). Introduction: The more things
change, the more they stay the same. In M. Paludi (Ed.), Praeger guide to
the psychology of gender. xixxxi. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Rhode, D. (1997). Speaking of sex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rudman, L., & Fairchild, K. (2007). The F word: Is feminism incompatible with
beauty and romance? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 125136.
Snelling, S. (1999). Womens perspectives on feminism. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 23, 247266.
Stimpson, C. (1971). Thy neighbors wife, thy neighbors servants: Womens liberation and black civil rights. In V. Gornick & B. Moran (Eds.), Woman in
sexist society: Studies in power and powerlessness. New York: Basic Books.
Zucker, A., & Stewart, A. (2007). Growing up and growing older: Feminism as
a context for womens lives. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 137145.

Chapter 1

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist


Melinda Kanner
Kristin J. Anderson

The feminist agenda is not about equal rights for women. It is about a
socialist, anti-family political movement that encourages women to leave
their husbands, kill their children, practice witchcraft, destroy capitalism,
and become lesbians.
Reverend Pat Robertson (Robertson letter attacks feminists, 1992)

Spanky: Lets start a club right now. The He-man Woman-haters. Ill be
president.
Alfalfa: And Ill be second president, and you can be third president.
Buckwheat: Thanks.
Spanky: Alright, get up and do exactly what I do. Put your hand on your
heart, and raise your other hand. We, the He-man Woman-haters club . . .
Alfalfa and Buckwheat: We, the he-man woman-haters club . . .
Spanky: . . . promise not to fall for this Valentines business . . .
Alfalfa and Buckwheat: . . . promise not to fall for this Valentines business . . .
Spanky: . . . because girls are the bunk.
Alfalfa and Buckwheat: . . . because girls are the bunk.
Hearts and Thumps (1937) from the Our Gang comedy lm series,
directed by Hal Roach

THE HE-MAN WOMAN-HATER AND THE


MAN-HATING FEMINIST
In 1934, the comedy trio The Three Stooges made their rst short
musical novelty lm, The Woman Haters. In this misogynist cinema

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

fantasy, the fraternal organization protects men from the trials of


romance and marriage and from the presumed inevitable disappointments and betrayals that accompany relationships with women. A second fantasy comes from the Little Rascals child comedy troop who
produced the Our Gang comedies for nearly two decades, a regular
feature of which was the He-Man Woman-Haters Club, which provided solace for the little boys in the gang and excluded girls from
play. Both examples from the annals of American mass media illustrate
some of the important and lasting dimensions of the myth of the feminist man-hater. First, we nd an intrinsic conict that emerges in relationships between females and males, even in these young, preadolescent ctional proxies for real-life women and men. This conict
is presented as inevitable, natural, and as fundamentally contrary to
the personal and social interests of males. Second, in these apparently
innocent and trivial ctional illustrations, we nd the production of
justication for division and suspicionnot caution and sensitivity, but
overt and institutionalized hostility toward females from males. Finally,
contained in the invention of the woman-hater, and the need for boys
to embrace such an identity, is an expression of a diametric opposition
in which the needs of men and the needs of women are set up as
antagonistic. Whether or not such antagonism is real, it is effectively
made real and asserted redundantly and repeatedly. There is a presence in male-produced and (largely) male-consumed popular culture of
the gure of the woman-hating man. There exists as well a seemingly
parallel gurethe man-hating feminist. However, this gure is not, in
a parallel way, the creation of some delirious feminist fantasy, but the
product of the same mass media organ of patriarchy. The man-hating
feminist is an invention, and a powerful and effective one at that. But
what are the sources of these images? What allows the image of the
man-hater to persist, to stick in the popular mind? And what interests
are served by perpetuating this stereotype?
No matter how we frame it, feminism has gotten a bad rap in the
cultural mind. From the myth of the bra-burner, to the negligent
mother, to the career-minded spinster, to the man-hater, the mere mention of the word feminist produces strong and often negative reactions. The feminist has endured some of the most grotesque kinds of
distortion and defamation among all the emblems of social progressivism and liberation. Much as black activists have typically been portrayed as wild-eyed, reckless, and dangerous to society, the feminist
has been tagged with many labels. She has been assigned the role of
repository of many cultural fears, and has, perhaps most potently, been
identied as a man-hater. The man-hater itself is a socially and frightening gure in the cultural imagination. The man-hater will not cooperate with the goals and practice of patriarchy and poses a threat to
the very cornerstone, the living embodiment of patriarchy: the

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

individual man himself. As with any political caricature, the images do


more than misrepresent: they obstruct and mislead.
These problems have been compounded somewhat by genuine critiques of patriarchy as a system. Much like the identication and
expose of the institutionalized racism of dominant society, feminist
critique of patriarchy necessarily identies the oppressive nature of the
social supremacy of man. The invented feminist man-hater is in
no way a natural, inevitable, or organic product of feminist politics
and philosophy. This straw woman is not linked to feminist values in
any sense. It rst serves nefarious and destructive ends aimed at
undermining the philosophy and struggle for social equality.
If this is not a real thing, how did it become coined? What specic
social and political circumstances are linked to this myth? Is there any
basis in reality to support the claims entailed in this gure? At stake
here is not just the good name of feminists, nor even the struggle for
equality. The data we present here demonstrate that it is not the case
that feminists hate men, but perhaps that anti-feminists hate men. The
man-hating feminist has no basis in reality but is part of a largescale, long-term tacit process by which power is maintained and the
myths that support gender inequality continue to circulate on its
behalf. It is part of a smoke-and-mirrors subterfuge to convert womanhaters into an epidemic of male bashing, feminist man-haters and lesbian conversion campaigns. In this process of misdirection, accusation,
and cultural myth-making, not only feminists are harmed. Any
woman, any man who resists the narrow and constricting connes of
absolute gender conformity, are victims as well.
This chapter explores the myth of the feminist-as-man-hater and
examines some of the origins and content of the myth. We do not
undertake here to review the history of the term feminist or to sort
through the hundreds of historical and recent aspersions cast on feminism through the direct effort of conservative news, and talk programming, or the more subtle but equally effective contamination of
mainstream news and entertainment media. The major contribution
offered here is an argument built on empirical evidence that actually
examines the truth-value of the claim that feminists dislike men. In our
empirical research, we investigated the real-life ideas and experiences
of respondents and discovered that the image of the feminist as manhater is far from the reality of women and men who embrace feminist
values and identify themselves as feminists.
FEMINISM: THE ANTI-FEMINIST CREATES
THE MAN-HATER FEMINISM
The very word evokes strong feelings in most people; Strong feelings and, too often, a world of misconceptions. The real meaning of

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

feminism, and what it actually means to call oneself a feminist, has


become obscured by an array of prejudices, preconceptions, and mechanisms that serve to maintain inequality. Structured inequality based
on gendermuch like inequalities based on race or ethnicity or sexual
identityhas been an established way of doing things for centuries.
For some, the question begins and ends with womens right to vote.
For others, a belief exists that gender inequality is a thing of the past,
and that women now enjoy social and economic equality in terms of
access to resources and prestige. Still others believe that feminism is an
antique effort, a fad whose time has passed. In any event, many
women and many more men continue to resist identifying themselves
as feminists.
What are some of the sources of the claims asserted by the manhating feminist myth? Some of the myths are simply defamatory and
dismissive of feminists and feminism. Through the simple accusations
of lesbian, and the homophobia easily mobilized, many are frightened away from the label that accurately describes their philosophy.
By invoking terrifying images of social outcasts, of spinsters, and
bra-burners, still others are driven into a disjuncture between values
and the adoption of an identity that accurately describes their social
principles. The deployment of the twinned terms man-hating and
feminist creates a myth with doubly harmful results. First, feminism
is denied the understanding it merits. The obfuscation and distortions
of the realities of feminismthe struggle for suffrage, the collective
striving for economic and social parity, the centuries-long drive to
achieve meaningful political participationare overwhelmed by the
fun-house mirror of misogyny held up by the accusations of manhating. Second, in the service of dismissing the central values of
feminism, such accusations additionally activate hostility toward lesbians
and intensify the sexism and misogyny that underlies both issues.
At its core, feminism is the belief in certain fundamental principles
of social, economic, political, and judicial equality. In a society in which
women and men have traditionally received unequal treatment, feminism seems a reasonable and long-overdue corrective to the historical
lack of access women continue to experience. Merriam-Webster denes
feminism as: (1) The theory of the political, economic, and social equality of the sexes; and (2) Organized activity on behalf of womens rights
and interests. The Encyclopedia Britannica denes it as: The belief in the
social, economic, and political equality of the sexes. And bell hooks
(2000) denes feminism as a movement to end sexism, sexist exploitation, and oppression. On the face of it, it would seem that all women,
and most men, would identify with the goals of feminism. In spite of
the widely agreed-upon philosophical desire for guarantees of equality,
today, few women call themselves feminists. Survey research shows
that the percentage of respondents who actually call themselves

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

feminists is strikingly small. In surveys of university women, the percentage who identify as feminists range from 8 (Myaskovsky & Wittig,
1997) to 44 percent (Bullock & Fernald, 2003) depending on the demographic makeup of the students. What accounts for these low numbers? When you consider the misconceptions we carry around with us
because of the way in which feminism is portrayed in popular culture
and politics, it is not surprising that relatively few women call themselves feminists. Anti-feminists blame feminists for a variety of social
problems: for young men entering college at a lower rate than that of
young women (Sommers, 2000); for the decline in manliness in
American culture (Manseld, 2006); and even for the terrorist attacks
of September 11, 2001 (Falwell, 2001).
MOST WOMEN ENDORSE FEMINIST PRINCIPLES
Surveys nd that women hold feminist beliefs but are hesitant to
describe themselves as feminists because they know that feminism is
viewed by some as anti-male (Alexander & Ryan, 1997; Aronson, 2003).
If it is the case that most peoplemen and womenendorse the fundamental principles of feminism, it should stand to reason that most
people would actually support feminism. Even among individuals who
endorse or embrace feminist principles, the adoption of the identity
feminist is resisted. One study of mostly white American women
who were college students found that of the women who did not consider themselves feminists, 81 percent agreed with some or all of the
goals of the feminist movement (Liss, Hoffner, & Crawford, 2000).
DO FEMINISTS HATE MEN? WHAT IS THE EVIDENCE?
Now let us address the common belief that feminists are man-haters.
While there are abundant of examples popular culture purporting to
reveal feminists attitudes toward men, there are very few empirical
studies on the subject. In addition to our own empirical study that we
describe shortly, Iazzos 1983 study is the only one we found that
examines feminists attitudes toward men. Iazzo (1983) developed the
Attitudes toward Men scale. He measured the degree to which women
agreed with statements about Marriage/Parenthood (e.g., Men consider
marriage a trap.), Sexuality (e.g., A man cannot get enough sex.),
Work (e.g., A mans job is the most important thing in his life.), and
Physical/Personality Attributes (e.g., An athletic man is to be admired.)
of men and gender roles. Women expressed their agreement on a 1 to
4 scale, and a score of 80 would indicate a neutral attitude toward
men. The control group sample was 104 mostly white women
recruited from a university, department stores, and other places of
business. They were compared with battered wives, rape victims,

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

lesbians, and feminists from a local chapter of the National Organization for Women. The control group mean score was 89.93, above the
neutral midpoint of 80.00, suggesting slightly positive attitudes toward
men. The average score of feminists was 79.54, not statistically distinguishable from the 80.00 midpoint, suggesting neutral attitudes toward
men. So feminists did not have negative attitudes toward men. What
about lesbians, a category that is often conated with feminists? Lesbians scored, on average, 70.97, so somewhat lower than neutral but
hardly indicative of man-hating. Further inspection of the statements
that make up the Attitudes toward Men scale may shed light on why
lesbians scored lower than both feminists and the control group of
women. Some of the statements may not be relevant to lesbians. For
instance, some of the items are as follows: Male sex organs are attractive, The male body is visually unappealing, and The sight of a
penis is repulsive. These are questions from the Sexuality subscale. It
would have been interesting to have analyzed how feminists and lesbians scored on each separate subscale. For instance, perhaps lesbians
had relatively anti-male attitudes on the 7 items that made up the
Sexuality scale because they do not nd mens body parts attractive.
Conversely, their scores on the other subscales could have been neutral
or positive. The limitation of Iazzos study is that many of the statements might be irrelevant to lesbians because the statements assume
that women have intimate relationships with men.
Maltby and Day (2001) studied British college students and examined various psychological characteristics as they correlate with attitudes toward women and men. For women, a feminine-stereotyped
gender role self-conceptthe degree to which people see themselves in
terms of feminine stereotypeswas found to be correlated with negative attitudes toward men. In other words, the more women saw themselves as feminine, the less they liked men. While Maltby and Days
study did not measure feminists attitudes toward men, their results
imply that perhaps it is non-feminists who do not like men because
feminists tend to have relatively more masculine and androgynous
gender role self-concepts than do non-feminists. Another way to put it
is that, in this study, women with traditional gender role orientation
who are likely to be non-feministshad more negative attitudes toward men than did women with nontraditional gender self-concepts
who are more likely to be feminists. Another study with an ethnically
diverse sample of university students found that those women who
perceived large value and belief differences between women and men
tended to like men less than did those women who did not perceive
large value and belief differences (Stephan, Stephan, Demitrakis,
Yamada, & Clason, 2000). Again, this study did not examine feminists
attitudes per se; however, we can extrapolate from the data. Other
studies have found that feminists tend to think women and men are

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

not very different (Liss et al., 2000; Liss, OConnor, Morosky, & Crawford, 2001), whereas non-feminists are more likely to think that women
and men are fundamentally different (Yoder, Fischer, Kahn, & Groden,
2007). Therefore, it appears non-feminists see women and men as fundamentally different and have more negative attitudes toward men
than do feminists.
AMBIVALENCE TOWARD MEN: TWO ASPECTS OF
ATTITUDES TOWARD MEN
The most recent method of measuring attitudes toward men has
been the Ambivalence toward Men Inventory (AMI), developed by
Glick and Fiske (1999). They found two aspects of womens (and to a
somewhat lesser extent mens) attitudes toward men. Hostility toward
men represents overtly negative attitudes toward men. It characterizes
men as inferior in ways that are safe to criticize, such as that men are
babies when they are sick. Hostility toward men also taps into resentment about mens power relative to women, mens aggressiveness, cultural attitudes that portray men as superior, and the way men exert
control within heterosexual intimate relationships. Individuals with
high hostility toward men scores tend to agree with statements such
as, When men act to help women, they are often trying to prove
they are better than women, and Most men pay lip service to equality for women, but cant handle having a woman as an equal. The
second aspect of attitudes toward men is benevolence toward men. Benevolence toward men does not represent overtly negative attitudes toward
men, but rather overtly positive or affectionate attitudes toward men. It
is a set of beliefs that includes the idea that just as women are dependent on men, so too are men dependent on women. Benevolence toward
men suggests that a womans role is to take care of a man, but only in
the domestic context. Experiencing subjectively positive feelings of affectionate protectiveness, admiration, and connection with men in intimate relationships represents benevolence toward men. Those who
score high on benevolence toward men agree with statements such as,
Women are incomplete without men, and Even if both members of
a couple work, the woman ought to be more attentive to taking care of
her man at home.
Hostility and benevolence toward men are distinct concepts, although
they tend to occur together. That is, women who have high hostility toward men scores tend to also have high benevolence toward men scores.
Thus, women may resent mens power even as they subscribe to beliefs
that support it. Women tend to score higher than men on hostility toward men and lower than men on benevolence toward men.
Attitudes of hostility and benevolence toward men are correlated
with other kinds of beliefs. For instance, benevolence toward men is

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

correlated with sexist attitudes toward women: those who believe that
men should protect women, and that women should take care of men
at home, also tend to believe that women need protection because they
are inferior to men. Interestingly, womens hostility toward men and benevolence toward men scores correlate, indicating that some women
simultaneously hold beliefs that actively support and justify male dominance (benevolence toward men) at the same time they resent the consequences of this dominance (hostility toward men). Glick and Fiske (1999)
speculate that the greater the dependence a woman has on men, the
more she is likely to experience both benevolence and hostility toward
men; the former because of her recognition of her investment in men
and the latter because of resentment over her dependence.
Although Glick and Fiske do not directly answer the question of the
relative position of feminists in terms of their benevolent or hostile attitudes toward men, they do explore the relationship between gender inequality and hostility toward men and benevolence toward men, which has
implications for feminism and attitudes toward men. In a massive
study across sixteen nations, Glick et al. (2004), along with several colleagues around the world, used many translated versions of the AMI
to investigate attitudes toward men.
Glick et al. (2004) found that in most nations, hostility toward men
was higher among women than among men. Hostility toward men
scores correlated with the national measures of gender inequality. Specically, hostility toward men was higher in traditional than in egalitarian nations. At the same time, benevolence toward men was higher in
traditional than in egalitarian nations. The authors speculated that
women in traditional nations may be more resentful toward men for
what they view as abuses of power, but that this resentment is not necessarily a challenge to gender hierarchy because it coexists with benevolent beliefs about mens roles as protectors and providers. The more
hostile men are toward women, the more women resent and show hostility toward men. Heightened resentment of mens hostility may
explain why womens hostility toward men scores increasingly outstrip
mens in more traditional cultures.
It is worth noting that there were many more gender similarities
than differences across nationswomen and men in the sixteen nations
tended to have similar attitudes toward women and men. In terms of
addressing the myth of feminists and man-haters, the Glick et al. (2004)
study on attitudes toward men suggests that man-hating is linked
more to anti-feminism and gender in equality, than it is to feminism
and gender equality.
Although the AMI is widely used, it had not been used with feminists until Anderson, Kanner, and Elsayegh (2009) conducted a study
that examined feminists and non-feminists attitudes toward men that
surveyed an ethnically diverse sample of 488 American college

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

students and asked them to respond to statements about gender roles


including the items from the AMI. Students were also asked whether or
not they are feminists. Only 14 percent of the sample of women and men
identied as feminists, which is consistent with an ethnically diverse sample. Contrary to popular stereotypes, self-identied feminists had lower
levels of hostility toward men than non-feminists. Interestingly, women
overall did tend to have higher levels of hostility toward men than did
men, but again, the hostility was not among the feminists. Feminists also
tended to have lower levels of benevolence toward men. Low levels of benevolence toward men does not mean one feels malevolence toward men, it just
means that the respondent does not agree with traditional gender roles
for instance, that women should take care of men in the home, while men
should be the main wage earners. Thus, based on our study results, it
appears that feminists, compared to non-feminists, do not have negative
attitudes toward men. Feminists do tend to reject traditional gender roles
that put women in less powerful positions than men.
Taken together, systematic empirical studies do not nd evidence
that feminists dislike men. In contrast, there is some suggestion than
non-feminists, those women who adhere to traditional gender stereotypes, dislike, or at least, resent, men. We must ask then, why does the
myth of feminist man-haters persist?
WHY DOES THE MYTH OF THE FEMINIST
MAN-HATER PERSIST?
The myth of the feminist man-hater exists in part because feminists
do not behave themselves in conventional ways. Feminists tend to violate gender role expectations, and that makes people uncomfortable.
Women who desire and have professions, women who resist the limitations of housewifery, women who do not feel themselves to be in the
irresistible grip of maternal inevitabilitythese and other women, and
men, who depart from gender conformity nd themselves in the sweep
of the accusation of man-hater. There are stiff sanctions for women,
and men, who violate gender roles. Now that we have established the
lack of empirical support for the notion that feminists are man-haters,
we are left with explaining why the myth persists and what we can do
about it. The next section begins by examining womens reactions to
conventional and nonconventional women by rst examining ambivalent sexism. Next, we will look at peoples perceptions of another type
of gender violator, women leaders. Then the supposed link between
feminism and lesbianism is examined, and the function of lesbian-baiting as a strategy to keep women in their place is discussed. Finally, the
empirical research presented in this chapter is put in its larger cultural
context by tying it to the battle of the sexes and the boy crisis
rhetoric that are currently popular.

10

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

WOMEN ARE WONDERFUL BUT FEMINISTS ARE NOT:


REWARDING TRADITIONAL AND PENALIZING
NONTRADITIONAL WOMEN
Understanding peoples negative attitudes toward feminists requires
understanding the context more generally of attitudes toward women.
Just as peoples attitudes toward men are ambivalentwith a mix of
respect and admiration along with resentment of mens power and
privilegeattitudes toward women are ambivalent as well. At rst
glance, however, attitudes toward women seem positive relative to attitudes toward men. Attitudes toward women are more positive in terms
of affect. Eagly and Mladinic (1994) have coined the phrase womenare-wonderful to illustrate this. The global category woman is viewed
more positively than the global category man. The women-are-wonderful effect occurs on explicit attitude surveys as well as with implicit
attitude measures such as when positive words such as good and
happy are associated more with women than with men (Rudman &
Goodwin, 2004). There are two important points about the positive
feelings people have about women compared to men connected to the
negative reaction some have for feminists. Just because a group is liked
does not mean that it is treated fairly and taken seriously. Also, just
because the global category women is liked more than men, this
does not mean that particular subcategories of women are liked. These
important caveats to the women-are-wonderful effect are elaborated
below.

DISCRIMINATION AND DISRESPECT


Gender-based discrimination is widespread and well documented
(see Valian, 1998, for a review). Take, for example, the disparities in
pay between women and men (see Volume 3) Women college graduates in the United States who work full time make only 75 percent of
what comparable men make (U.S. Department of Labor, 2005). In the
United States, girls and women are more likely to live in poverty than
are boys and men (Bishaw & Stern, 2006). And while women in the
United States held half of all management and professional positions
in 2004, only 14 percent of architects and engineers and 29 percent of
physicians and surgeons are women, whereas 86 percent of paralegals
and legal assistants are women (U.S. Department of Labor, 2005). In
terms of representations of women in popular culture in the United
States, men are overrepresented on prime time television shows (Screen
Actors Guild, 2005), in television commercials (Ganahl, Prinsen, &
Netzley, 2003), feature lms (Lauzen & Dozier, 2005), music television
(Seidman, 1999), and in newspaper comics (Glascock & PrestonSchreck, 2004). In terms of political representation, women make up

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

11

only 16.1 percent of the members of the U.S. House of Representatives


and Senate (Center for American Women and Politics, 2008).
Eagly and Mladinics (1994) work on the women-are-wonderful effect
revealed, women may be liked, but they are not necessarily respected. Fiske,
Cuddy, Glick, and Xu (2002) have found that groups that are traditionally
targets of discrimination are judged along two dimensions: warmth and
competence. For instance, people tend to think Jews are highly competent,
but do not feel warmly toward themthey are respected but not liked.
People tend to feel warmly toward old people, but do not respect them
they are high on warmth, low on respect. As you might guess, women
are viewed as warm and therefore likeable, but they are less likely to be
seen as competent and are therefore less respected. Men, relative to
women, are less liked but are viewed as more competent.
If women are liked more than respected, where do feminists t in?
Haddock and Zanna (1994) found that people have different views of
two categories of women that are seen as opposites: housewives and
feminists. Like Eagly and Mladinics (1994) work on the women-are-wonderful effect and Fiske et al.s (2002) work on warmth and competence,
Haddock and Zanna found that when Canadian college students were
asked to form a mental image of the typical woman and typical man,
women were evaluated more favorably than were men. However,
when subcategories of women were considered, different attitudes
emerged. Feminists tend to be evaluated more negatively than housewives, even though feminists and housewives are both part of the
larger category of women. Haddock and Zanna further found that
those who dislike feminists believe that feminists violate traditional
values and customs. In other words, feminists are seen as a threat to
the status quo in a way that housewives are not.
AMBIVALENT SEXISM: THE CARROT AND THE
STICK OF PATRIARCHY
Because there are differing views of different types of women, sexism,
the institutionalized prejudice and discrimination against women is not
a single, unitary concept. Glick and Fiske (1997) developed ambivalent
sexism as a measure that captures subjectively positive and negative
feelings toward women. Racial/ethnic groups may, and often do, avoid
kinship ties (or almost any kind of contact) with other racial/ethnic
groups, however, heterosexual women and men have to be intimate.
For instance, one might be against marrying someone of another race,
but it is unlikely that a heterosexual man will decide not to be involved
with a woman. And although men may wish to exclude women from
certain activities and roles, few (even among the most rabidly sexist)
wish to banish women completely from their lives. You can avoid
another ethnic group, but it is hard to avoid another gender.

12

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Similar to their measure of ambivalence toward men discussed earlier,


Glick and Fiske nd that mens (and peoples, generally) ambivalence
toward women can be broken down into two kinds of sexism, hostile
sexism and benevolent sexism. Hostile sexism is what most people think
when they think of sexism. It consists of overtly hostile feelings toward
women, with negative feelings toward, and stereotyping of, nontraditional women in particular. Hostile sexism seeks to justify male power,
traditional gender roles, and mens exploitation of women as sexual
objects through derogatory characterizations of women. Hostile sexists
agree with statements such as, When women lose to men in a fair competition, they typically complain about being discriminated against, and
Most women fail to appreciate fully all that men do for them. Benevolent
sexism is something of a slippery concept because it involves subjectively positive attitudes toward women. Women are characterized as
pure creatures who need protection from men. It is the view that
women are adored by men and are necessary to make a man complete.
Benevolent sexism relies on kinder and gentler justications of male
dominance and prescribed gender roles; it recognizes mens dependence on women and a romanticized view of heterosexual relationships.
Ideologies of what Glick and Fiske (1997) refer to as benevolent paternalism allow members of dominant groups to characterize their privileges as well deserved, even as a responsibility they must bear (similar
to the white mans burden). Men are willing to sacrice their own
needs to care for the women in their lives. Benevolent sexists agree
with statements such as, No matter how accomplished he is, a man is not
truly complete as a person unless he has the love of a woman, and Women
should be cherished and protected by men. For women, benevolent sexism
undermines womens resistance to male dominance: benevolent sexism
is disarming because it is subjectively favorable and also promises that
mens power will be used to womens advantage, as long as they can
secure a high-status male protector. People do not immediately recognize benevolent sexism as sexist, and some women are even attered by
the attitudes of benevolent sexism (Barreto & Ellemers, 2005).
Although hostile sexism and benevolent sexism are separate and
contradictory concepts, people can, and often do, experience hostile
and benevolent sexism simultaneously. People can have loving and
hating attitudes toward women. People tend to feel hostile sexism toward women who violate traditional gender roles (e.g., feminists, sexually active women) and benevolent sexism toward conventional
women (e.g., homemakers). Benevolent sexism can result in the
women-are-wonderful effect because traditional women are considered
to be wonderful because of their purity and nurturance. The way Glick
and Fiske describe the workings of ambivalent sexism, benevolent sexism is the carrotthe reward of positive feelings toward and protectiveness given to women who embrace traditional roles; and hostile

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

13

sexism is the stickthe hostility that women who reject traditional


roles in favor of taking on traditionally masculine roles face from men
who wish to keep them in their places. Punishment (through hostile
sexism) alone is not the most effective means of shaping behavior
because that might result in only resentment and resistance. However,
punishment for some and reinforcement for others maintains patriarchy and the gender status quo (Glick & Fiske, 2001).
Benevolent sexism, then, is insidious for three reasons. First, it
doesnt seem like prejudice to male perpetrators because men do not
view it as something negative. Second, women may nd its sweet
allure difcult to resist (Glick & Fiske, 2001, pp. 114115). Praising
womens nurturing traits is part of expressing the belief that women
are especially suited to domestic roles. Furthermore, stereotypes of
women as nurturing and communal justify their subordinated status
(Jost & Kay, 2005). Third, benevolent sexism can drive a wedge
between women. Women (e.g., feminists) who reject the overtly negative aspects of hostile sexism as well as the more hidden negative
aspects of benevolent sexism are at odds with traditional women who
are rewarded by benevolent sexism and reject feminism because they
want to hold on to the little power they get as a result of benevolent
sexism. So, while feminists and traditional women should be working
in solidarity to ght gender discrimination, they are split by being on
two opposite sides of benevolent sexism.
Like their work on ambivalence toward men, Glick and Fiske have
analyzed patterns of hostile and benevolent sexism in a variety of cultures (Glick et al., 2004). In general, mens hostile sexism is higher than
womens, and women are more receptive to benevolent sexist beliefs
than hostile sexist beliefs. In nations where hostile sexism was
endorsed, women were especially likely to embrace benevolent sexism,
in some cases, even more so than the men. This points to the irony of
women who are forced to seek protection from members of the very
group that threatens them: The greater the threat, the stronger the incentive to accept benevolent sexisms protective ideology. This explains
the tendency for women in the most sexist societies to endorse benevolent sexism more strongly than do men. Furthermore, the countries in
which women rejected both benevolent and hostile sexism were the
ones in which men had low hostile sexism scores. As sexist hostility
declines, women may feel able to reject benevolent sexism without fear
of a hostile backlash.
Ambivalent sexism addresses the question of whether or not chivalry is good for women. In excluding women from the outside world
of work and from positions traditionally held by men, benevolent sexists exclude women from roles that offer more status in society. Thus,
some women (specically traditional women) are protected to some
extent by chivalry, but at great cost. Ambivalent sexism is a concept

14

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

that can also be related to other objectionable attitudes. Feminists, who


may reject chivalry for good reason, get an angry, defensive response
from men who feel that feminists are ingrates.
In a study of Spanish womens reactions (Moya, Glick, Exp
osito, de
Lemus, & Hart, 2007) to discriminatory scenarios (e.g., losing a promotion), the same acts of discrimination were perceived as less serious
when the perpetrators expressed a benevolent, protective justication
than when they expressed a hostile one. Furthermore, women who
scored higher in benevolent sexism were more likely to excuse both
hostile treatment from a husband and benevolently-justied discrimination by non-intimate men (e.g., a boss). But this pattern of response
only occurred among women participants who were without paid
employment. This nding suggests that women who are highly dependent on men are prone to forgive even hostile acts, perhaps reinterpreting them as signs of the husbands passionate attachment. One
study with Turkish and Brazilian respondents, found that individuals
(both women and men) with high levels of hostile sexism found wife
abuse more acceptable than those with low levels of hostile sexism
(Glick, Sakalli-Ugurlu, Ferreira, & de Souza, 2002). Benevolent sexism
has been linked to attributions of blame against women for acquaintance rape. Individuals high in benevolent sexism attributed less blame
to perpetrators and recommended shorter sentences for an acquaintance rape perpetrator than did low benevolent sexist individuals (Viki,
Abrams, & Masser, 2004). A study of Zimbabwean male college students found that those men with higher levels of hostile sexism
reported that they were more likely to commit acquaintance rape than
men with lower levels of hostile sexism (Viki, Chiroro, & Abrams,
2006). Thus, hostile sexism rationalizes mistreatment of women who
violate traditional roles, while benevolent sexism provides a framework
for what is acceptable (i.e., traditional) behavior for women.
The work on ambivalent sexism demonstrates that while traditional
women tend to elicit positive feelings from people, nontraditional
women such as feminists have hostile reactions directed toward them.
Even though the supposed protective qualities of benevolent sexism
are alluring to some women, that protection comes with the price of restricted options and strong sanctions to women who appear to violate
traditional roles.
PENALTIES FOR NONTRADITIONAL WOMEN
A central feature of negative attitudes toward women is the dislike
of women who do not t into the traditional feminine role (e.g., feminists, lesbians, women athletes). From the discussion of Glick and
Fiskes (1997) work on ambivalent sexism and Fiske et al.s (2002) work
on warmth and competence as a relevant dimension of judging social

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

15

categories, it should be clear that what makes feminists threatening is


that they violate (or appear to violate) and reject traditional gender
norms for women.
Women leaders also violate peoples expectations about women and
therefore threaten the gender hierarchy. While women who are leaders
are not necessarily feminists, and feminists are not necessarily women
in leadership positions, both engender similar reactions. Think of the
strong reactions toward Hillary Clinton, Martha Stewart, and Condoleezza Rice. While women in leadership positions in the work domain
have gradually increased, expectations about what women are
like have not kept pace with womens changing roles. Research ndings have indicated that women who behave in ways typically reserved
for men are found to be less socially appealing than men who behave
similarly or women who behave in ways that are more in line with
normative prescriptions. When a woman is acknowledged to have been
successful at performing male gender-typed work, she is, by denition,
thought to have the attributes necessary to effectively execute the tasks
and responsibilities required. But it is these same attributes that are in
violation of gender-prescriptive norms (Valian, 1998). So, although
there is a good t between what the woman is perceived to be like and
what the job entails, there is a bad t between what the woman is
perceived to be like and the conception of what she should be like.
One study illustrates the subtlety with which judgments about
women who violate gender expectations get played out (Heilman, Wallen, Fuchs, & Tamkins, 2004). American college students were given
packets that contained a prole of a clearly successful or ambiguously
successful woman or man in a male-dominated job (assistant vice president in mechanics and aeronautics). Students were asked to rate the
candidate on competence, likeability, and interpersonal hostility. When
students rated the obviously successful candidate, women and men
were rated equallythey were both given credit for their successes.
However, gender did play a role when the candidates qualications
were ambiguous. When information about the candidates performance
was ambiguous, the woman was rated as less competent than the man.
There were results associated with liking ratings as well. When there
was ambiguity about the target persons performance, there was no
signicant difference between the liking ratings of women and men
targets. But when there was clear evidence of success, the woman was
liked less than the man. In other words, the clearly successful woman
was liked signicantly less than the clearly successful man, the unsuccessful woman, and unsuccessful man. A similar nding emerged in
terms of judgments of hostility. The woman candidate was rated as less
hostile than the man in the ambiguous performance outcome condition
but was rated as more hostile than the man in the clearly successful
condition. These results suggest the double standard used when

16

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

judging women in male-dominated occupations: Women were viewed


as less competent than men only when there was ambiguity about how
successful they had been; when the womens success was made
explicit, there were no differences in these characterizations. However,
when success was explicit, women were viewed as less likeable than
men. Women, although rated less competent than men when information about them was ambiguous, were at least rated as less hostile
interpersonally. But the switch when success was clear is dramatic:
women who are acknowledged as successful were viewed not merely
as indifferent to others but as downright uncivil. And these patterns
held for both women and men participants, so these gender stereotypic
norms and the tendency to penalize those who violate them are meaningful for both women and men. Heilman et al. (2004) also found that
dislike was associated with not being recommended for promotions
and salary increases. The authors conclude that while there are many
things that lead an individual to be disliked in the job setting, it is only
women who are disliked when they are successful.
LESBIAN-BAITING
Understanding the link between feminism and lesbianism reveals
some of the fundamental sources of the discomfort and antagonism toward feminism we have explored so far. Indeed, in casual contexts and
in mass media, lesbian is, erroneously, often portrayed as interchangeable with feminist where the presumption is made that lesbians are, by
denition feminists, and feminists are presumed to be lesbians. Both
lesbians and feminists are understood as women who disrupt and
threaten gender, and both terms describe nontraditional women. Both
feminists and lesbians seem inherently unladylike, assertive, and outspoken, and women like this threaten the gender status quo (Alexander
& Ryan, 1997).
Homophobia, in addition to sexism, creates an additional set of tactical opportunities to discredit and marginalize feminisms efforts to
achieve comprehensive equality for women. Like the accusation of
male-bashing, the framing of lesbianism as the inevitable result of feminism or as a necessary dimension of feminism, are scare tactics
designed to frighten people away from associating with feminism and
feminist activism. The very positioning of lesbianism as a source of discrediting reveals the underlying layer of homophobia that often joins
with sexism to maintain systems of oppression and retain privilege.
Women who have worked actively against sexual assault and rape are
often the target of lesbian-baiting. Framed as insults and debasement,
accusations of lesbianism, along with descriptions of feminists as angry, unladylike, and unfeminine, are employed to make feminists, and
by extension, the goals of feminism, unattractive and repellent. Grant

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

17

(2000), who has studied community responses to anti-violence activists,


argues that these slurs are the result of people feeling as though
women are acting out of their place by complaining too much about
mens violence against women. It is as though it is okay to believe that
rape is wrong, but that women should not complain about it, or at
least if they complain, they should not complain loudly. Battered womens shelters and rape crisis centers have been vandalized with grafti
such as No means dyke, or No means tie her up. Rape crisis centers have been charged with turning women into lesbians or being
man-hating. Womens activism is seen as a threat. Lesbian, as much
as it is an expression of sexual identity, also functions as a regulatory
term (Grant, 2000). It refers to women who are independent from men.
That is why it can be used when a woman refuses sexual advances
from a man. Since lesbian is often conated with feminist, and because
of homophobia, feminists are often required to prove they are not
lesbians.
Lesbian-baiting can also be a form of sexual extortion, especially in
the military. Corbett (1997) has written about lesbian-baiting since the
emergence of the U.S. military policy of Dont Ask, Dont Tell, Dont
Pursue. According to Corbett, accusations of lesbianism are a threat to
all military women, regardless of their sexual orientation. The antigay
policy gives harassers and rapists tools of sexual extortion. Allegations
of lesbianism can ruin a womans career. It doesnt matter whether or
not the allegations are true. Women soldiers who refuse sexual advances from men may be accused of being lesbians and subjected to investigation for homosexual conduct. Thus, the Dont Ask policy is being
used as a weapon of retaliation against women who report sexual harassment or rape, against those who rebuff sexual advances, or against
those who succeed in their careers. Obviously, if lesbians and gay men
could serve openly in the military, this would be a less effective
weapon against service members.
Although lesbians, like feminists, are seen as man-haters, there is no
empirical evidence suggesting they are. Markey begins her Redbook article, Male Bashing, with, I used to be a rather accomplished malebasher. After all, I was married to a man. . . (Markey, 1993, p. 104).
Magazines from the popular press actually imply that male-bashers are
heterosexual women with traditional gender roles: women complain
about mens indelity (Lego, 1999), inept husbands (Heckard, 1998),
and men who are not domesticable (Heard, 1989). Lesbians likely
have different relationships with men and therefore do not have the
complaints, disappointments, and frustrations that some heterosexual
women have. Grant (2000) interviewed lesbian feminist activists who
reported that, rather than disliking men, they felt that men were either
neutral players (e.g., male relatives) or just not relevant to their lives.
One lesbian interviewee reported that men are not a major part of her

18

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

life and that heterosexual women complain about men all the time.
More systematic research needs to be done in the area of lesbians-asman-haters. We suspect another stereotype will be debunked, just as
the feminists-as-man-haters stereotype has been.
CONFUSING THE UNIT OF ANALYSIS: WAR AGAINST
THE SEXES VERSUS PATRIARCHY AND PRIVILEGE
Feminists are accused of man-hating when they object to gender discrimination because some interpret the objection as being anti-man (as
complaints about particular, individual men, or even all men) rather than
as a protest against the patriarchal system that gives power and privilege to men relative to women. Feminists see sexism as part of a system
of inequality (Kane, 2000). Those who do not understand the systemic
nature of gender inequality translate feminists activism as complaints
directed at particular men or at men as a category, as if feminists blame
each man or all men. For instance, in his book, Manliness, Harvey
Manseld describes feminism as women being none too pleased with
men and not shy about letting them know it (Manseld, 2006, p. 4).
The incorrect notion that stems from and engenders hatred of men,
rather than the accurate framing of feminism being a critique of a patriarchal system, does more than make women afraid to call themselves
feminists, thereby contributing to gender inequality. In the studies we
have reviewed in this chapter that revealed gender discrimination,
nearly all found that men and women participants discriminate against
women. Sexism and gender discrimination is not just something men
do to women. Everyone participates in a sexist system, although it is
certainly true that men benet through the male privilege inherent in a
sexist system. Ignoring the systemic nature of gender inequality also
leads men to feel stuck in a defensive response rather than being able
to see that men too are conned by gender expectations. Trivializing
feminists resistance to inequality as anger at men insults the womens
liberation movement that ghts for the right to vote, for equal pay, for
educational equity, and for reproductive freedomefforts focused on
changing the system, not on bashing men.
One manifestation of the focus on individual men versus the focus
on systemic gender discrimination and male privilege is the battle-ofthe-sexes (e.g., Heard, 1989) rhetoric that is prevalent in popular
culture. Battle-of-the-sexes rhetoric produces false neutrality and false
parallelism of the advantages/disadvantages of women and men and
suggests that both women and men are equally advantaged and disadvantagedjust in different ways (e.g., OBeirne, 2006). For instance, in
Time magazine article, Men, Are They Really That Bad? Morrow
(1994) takes on what he describes as the overt man bashing of recent
years (p. 54). He says, both men and women have been oppressed

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

19

by the other sex, in different ways (p. 56), and American men and
women should face the fact that they are hopelessly at odds (p. 59).
Judy Markey (1993) says, How can we gripe that they put us down as
a group, if we do the same thing to them? (p. 105) and, Well wind
up sounding like squabbling children crying, He started it! No, she
did! (p. 105). This popular discourse that women-and-men-areat-odds suggests that womens and mens complaints are parallel and
equal. The sex wars rhetoric trivializes genuine critiques about patriarchy and male supremacy and reduces discrimination to a he-saidshe-said dynamic in which there are no real winners and no real losers,
but only miscommunication between the sexes.
This view of individual-based gender debates can reduce things
such as rape and sexual harassment to miscommunication that can
leave men victims. For instance, in his book, The Myth of Male Power,
Warren Farrell (1993) writes Feminism has taught women to sue men
for creating a hostile environment or for date rape when men initiate
with the wrong person or with the wrong timing (p. 18). Similarly,
Morrow (1994) claims that a successful approach to a woman is called
romance and courtship. Sexual harassment, according to Morrow, is
simply an unsuccessful approach, and, in his view, is unfairly treated
as a crime. This rhetoric suggests that the real victims of sexual harassment and rape are not women, but men who are victimized by womens irtations and mixed messages.
STEALING THE CENTER STAGE OF OPPRESSION:
THE BOY CRISIS
In recent years, another manifestation of the accusation of manhating comes in the form of the popular discourse on the boy crisis.
Beginning in the 1980s, there was an increase in awareness regarding
the male bias in clinical and popular psychological theories that treated
girls like deviants and boys as the norm, with books such as Carol
Gilligans (1982), In a Different Voice and Mary Piphers (1994) Reviving
Ophelia. Part of this focus was a critique of the educational system that
seemed more geared toward the benet of boys. Myra and David
Sadkers (1994) book, Failing at Fairness: How Americas Schools Cheat
Girls, as well as a report from the American Association of University
Women (1992), generated headlines in the popular press. As these
works grew in popularity, a backlash in the form of a recovery effort
for boys supposedly wounded by the alleged disproportionate attention given to girls and women during the 1980s and early 1990s began
to grow as well. Several anti-feminist pop psychology books on boys
development became best sellers. Christina Hoff Sommers (2000) book,
The War Against Boys, and now more recently, Kate OBeirnes (2006)
book, Women Who Make the World Worse and How Their Radical Feminist

20

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Assault is Ruining Our Schools, Families, Military, and Sports, accompanied hundreds of books and newspaper and magazine articles published in the United States, Europe, and Australia about the boy
crisis. Writers cite the disproportionate numbers of women entering
and graduating from college compared to men who do so as their evidence of women getting one up on men. Typical newspaper and magazine articles of this type are entitled At Colleges, Women Are Leaving
Men in the Dust (Lewin, 2006), Silence of the Lads (Stark & Ebenkamp, 1999), and How Boys Lost Out to Girl Power (Lewin, 1998).
According to Meads (2006) analysis of National Assessment of Educational Progress data, in primary school, boys overall academic
achievement is increasing, but girls academic achievement is increasing at a faster rate. Girls still outperform boys in reading and writing
and boys outperform girls in math and science. Thus, mens higher
education attainment is not declining; its increasing, albeit at a slower
rate than that of women. Women still earn fewer than half of U.S. doctorates and professional degrees, such as those in law and medicine.
Women earn more masters degrees than men, but these are heavily
concentrated in female-stereotyped elds, such as education and psychology. Womens college degrees are more likely to be for low-paying,
low-status occupations such as teaching, and recent women college
graduates earn less than men even after controlling for choice of eld.
Anderson and Accomando (2002) analyzed the literature on the
boy crisis that claims that girls have myriad advantages over boys.
They nd that this literature reveals a panic reaction that amounts to
center-stealing (Grillo & Wildman, 1997). Center-stealing occurs when
members of a privileged group imagine a threat when attention, even
temporarily and briey, is directed away from them and toward members of a marginalized group. Center-stealing occurs when the dominant and privileged group steals back attention from the subordinate
group, putting the focus back on the dominant group. While books
and articles that focus on how the educational system has been biased
against girls assume that it is necessary to redress past wrongs including sexism, discrimination, and exclusion, boy crisis authors see the
focus on girls as a takeover by girls and women. The boy crisis
authors assume that the playing eld for girls and boys (and men and
women) was level before this relatively brief focus on girls, rather than
seeing the decades of disadvantage of girls. The brief moment of academic, educational, and popular focus on the inhospitable nature of
classrooms for girls and of the workplace for women has been perceived as a conquest by girls and women.
This backlash against feminism may account for some of the apparent internal contradiction among those who, while claiming to support
egalitarianism, think that feminists have gone too far. Much in the
ways that cries of reverse racism attract attention, engender fear,

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

21

and draw upon the accumulated confusion and misinformation that


surrounds gender and race politics (Anderson, in press), those who are
genuinely disenfranchised become the accused, and the oppressors
either actual or symbolicare both exculpated and their imaginary
injuries are nursed and tended publicly.
CONCLUSION
How do we understand the myth of the man-hating feminist? How
do we explain the combined pedestalizing and devaluing of women
in mass media imagery? Finally, what do we make of the invention of
terms such as man-hating or male bashing? Cataldi (1995) discusses
the irony in the use of the term male bashing. To bash means to violently strike with a heavy crushing blow. Bash connotes an indiscriminate, random, confused and unmotivated lashing out. Bash suggests that
the striking of the blow is unfair, undeserved, or prejudicedsimilar to
how the word gay-bashing is usedviolently beating someone
because of their presumed homosexuality and never used in cases of
male violence against women. There is no standardized woman bashing. Verbal bashing appears to involve unjustly denouncing the members of a group, people who are innocent victims. As Cataldi reminds
us, women (in general) are not bashers, they are bashees. In the United
States, one study of more than 5,000 American women college students,
found that 28.5 percent had experienced an attempted or completed sexual assault either before or since entering college. One fth of the college
women reported experiencing an attempted or completed sexual assault
since entering college (Krebs, Lindquist, Warner, Fisher, & Martin, 2007).
One out of every 12 American women will be stalked at some point in
their lives, and 87% of the stalkers were men. Four out of ve stalking
victims were women (U.S. Department of Justice, 2006). Why arent
physical assaults on women characterized as female bashing? Cataldi
(1995) argues that conjuring up images of abused men bashed by
women and casting women in the role of bashers reverses what actually
happens. This table turning can then operate, perniciously, as a form of
victim-blaming and as a means of exaggerating the severity of any harm
done to men who are, supposedly verbally bashed by women. Another
function of co-opting the expression male-bashing and its brutality, is
to lead us into thinking that the male bashing women supposedly
engage in is equivalent to what men do to women. Those who use the
expression may also be attempting to siphon attention and support
away from women and from those who are physically harmed by men.
In designating feminists as male-bashers focus is shifted entirely from
the system, from the institutions, from the mechanisms that create, reify,
and perpetuate oppressive structures, including sexism, heterosexism,
misogyny, and homophobia.

22

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

On the very social bruises where attention should focus on the epidemic problem of mens violence against women, we nd instead
media attention proclaiming that there is a war on boys and that there
is an epidemic of male-bashing. Instead of social and educational programs, we have unsupportable claims that feminism brings with it
man-hating. The feminist critique of gender-based social inequality
may be disconcerting to men and some women; it might hurt feelings,
it might seem unfair, and it might seem to disregard mens good intentions. It certainly does problematize and complicate the privileges that
accrue to men in patriarchy. Although these challenges and their
results make menand doubtless many womenfeel resistant and
uneasy, these challenges do not constitute male bashing. Feminists are
not critical of men simply for being men.
A feminist social critique targets systems of gender-based inequality
and their connections to other forms of oppression based on sexuality,
class, and race. The stronger women become, the more gains they
make, the more pernicious are the representations of the feminist. A
living and vivid image in many domains in mass media, there appears
to be no real-life support for such fears. Indeed, given the goals and
values central to feminism, it is anti-feminists rather than feminists
who harbor and express hostility toward men in a patriarchal society.
REFERENCES
Alexander, S., & Ryan, M. (1997). Social constructs of feminism: A study of
undergraduates at a womens college. College Student Journal, 31, 555567.
American Association of University Women. (1992). The AAUW report: How
schools shortchange girls. Washington, DC: The American Association of
University Women Educational Foundation.
Anderson, K. J. (in press). Benign bigotry: The psychology of subtle prejudice. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Anderson, K. J., & Accomando, C. (2002). Real boys? Manufacturing masculinity and erasing privilege in popular books on raising boys. Feminism &
Psychology, 12, 491516.
Anderson, K. J., Kanner, M., & Elsayegh, N. (2009). Are feminists man-haters?
Feminists and nonfeminists attitudes toward men. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 33, 216224.
Aronson, P. (2003). Feminists or postfeminists? Young womens attitudes toward feminism and gender relations. Gender & Society, 17, 903922.
Barreto, M., & Ellemers, N. (2005). The burden of benevolent sexism: How it
contributes to the maintenance of gender inequalities. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 35, 633642.
Bishaw, A., & Stern, S. (2006, June 15). Evaluation of poverty estimates: A comparison of the American Community Survey and the Current Population Survey.
Washinton, DC: Poverty and Health Statistics Branch, Housing and Household Economic Statistics Division, U.S. Census Bureau.

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

23

Bullock, H. E., & Fernald, J. L. (2003). Feminism lite? Feminist identication,


speaker appearance, and perceptions of feminist and antifeminist messengers. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27, 291299.
Cataldi, S. L. (1995). Reections on male bashing. NWSA Journal, 7, 7685.
Center for American Women and Politics. (2008). Women in the U.S. Congress
2008. New Brunswick, NJ: Eagleton Institute of Politics.
Corbett, K. M. (1997, November). Lesbian-baiting: A threat to all military
women. Lesbian News, 23, 1617.
Eagly, A. H., & Mladinic, A. (1994). Are people prejudiced against women?
Some answers from research on attitudes, gender stereotypes and judgments of competence. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review
of social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 135). New York: Wiley.
Farrell, W. (1993). The myth of male power: Why men are the disposable sex. New
York: Berkley Books.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed)
stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
82, 878902.
Ganahl, D. J., Prinsen, T. J., & Netzley, S. B. (2003). A content analysis of prime
time commercials: A contextual framework of gender representation. Sex
Roles, 49, 545551.
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and womens development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Glascock, J., & Preston-Schreck, C. (2004, October). Gender and racial stereotypes in daily newspaper comics: A time-honored tradition? Sex Roles, 51,
423431.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1997). Hostile and benevolent sexism: Measuring
ambivalent sexist attitudes toward women. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
21, 119135.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1999). The Ambivalence toward Men Inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent beliefs about men. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 23, 519536.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent
sexism as complementary justications for gender inequality. American
Psychologist, 56, 109118.
Glick, P., Lameiras, M., Fiske, S. T., Eckes, T., Masser, B., Volpato, C., et al.
(2004). Bad but bold: Ambivalent attitudes toward men predict gender
inequality in 16 nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86,
713728.
Glick, P., Sakalli-Ugurlu, N., Ferreira, M. C., & de Souza, M. A. (2002). Ambivalent sexism and attitudes toward wife abuse in Turkey and Brazil. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 292297.
Grant, A. (2000). And still, the lesbian threat: Or, how to keep a good woman a
woman. Journal of Lesbian Studies, 4, 6180.
Grillo, T., & Wildman, S. M. (1997). Obscuring the importance of race: The
implication of making comparisons between racism and sexism (or other
isms). In A. K. Wing (Ed.), Critical race feminism: A reader (pp. 4451).
New York: NYU Press.

24

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1994). Preferring housewives to feminists:


Categorization and the favorability of attitudes toward women. Psychology
of Women Quarterly, 18, 2552.
Heard, A. (1989, August). Stop blaming men for everything! Mademoiselle, 183,
232, 234.
Heckard, I. R. (1998, January/February). Male bashing: Is it trash talk or harmless humor? Todays Christian Woman, 20, 4648.
Heilman, M. E., Wallen, A. S., Fuchs, D., & Tamkins, M. M. (2004). Penalties
for success: Reactions to women who succeed at male gender-typed tasks.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 416427.
hooks, b. (2000). Feminism is for everybody. Cambridge, MA: South End Press.
Iazzo, A. N. (1983). The construction and validation of Attitudes toward Men
Scale. The Psychological Record, 33, 371378.
Jost, J. T., & Kay, A. C. (2005). Exposure to benevolent sexism and complementary gender stereotypes: Consequences for specic and diffuse forms of
system justication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 498509.
Kane, E. W. (2000). Racial and ethnic variations in gender-related attitudes. Annual Review of Sociology, 26, 419439.
Krebs, C. P., Lindquist, C. H., Warner, T. D., Fisher, B. S., & Martin, S. L. (2007,
December). The campus sexual assault (CSA) study: Final report. Prepared
for National Institute of Justice. (NIJ Grant No. 2004-wg-bx-0010).
Lauzen, M. M., & Dozier, D. M. (2005). Maintaining the double standard: Portrayals of age and gender in popular lms. Sex Roles, 52, 437446.
Lego, S. (1999). Monicagate and male bashing. Perspectives in Psychiatric Care,
35, 34.
Lewin, T. (1998, December 13). How boys lost out to girl power. New York
Times, 148, Section 4, p. 3.
Lewin, T. (2006, July 9). At colleges, women are leaving men in the dust. The
New York Times. Retrieved July 11, 2006, from http://www.nytimes.com/
2006/07/09/education/09college.html.
Liss, M., Hoffner, C., & Crawford, M. (2000). What do feminists believe? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 279284.
Liss, M., OConnor, C., Morosky, E., & Crawford, M. (2001). What makes a
feminist? Predictors and correlates of feminist social identity in college
women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25, 124133.
Maltby, J., & Day, L. (2001). Psychological correlates of attitudes toward men.
The Journal of Psychology, 135, 335351.
Manseld, H. C. (2006). Manliness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Markey, J. (1993, May). Male-bashing. Redbook, 181, 104108.
Mead, S. (2006, June). The truth about boys and girls. Retrieved February 12,
2008, from http://www.educationsector.org/analysis/analysis_show.htm?
doc_id=378705.
Morrow, L. (1994, February 14). Men: Are they really that bad? Time, 143,
5359.
Moya, M., Glick, P., Exp
osito, F., de Lemus, S., & Hart, J. (2007). Its for your
own good: Benevolent sexism and womens reactions to protectively justied restrictions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 14211434.
Myaskovsky, L., & Wittig, M. A. (1997). Predictors of feminist social identity
among college women. Sex Roles, 37, 861883.

The Myth of the Man-Hating Feminist

25

OBeirne, K. (2006). Women who make the world worse. New York: Sentinel.
Pipher, M. B. (1994). Reviving Ophelia: Saving the selves of adolescent girls. New
York: Ballantine Books.
Robertson letter attacks feminists. (1992, August 26). The New York Times.
Retrieved February 19, 2008, from http://query.nytimes.com/gst/fullpage.
html?res=9E0CE7DC1430F935A1575BC0A964958260&scp=1&sq=robertson
letterattacks&st=nyt.
Rudman, L. A., & Goodwin, S. A. (2004). Gender differences in automatic ingroup bias: Why do women like women more than men like men? Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 494509.
Sadker, M., & Sadker, D. (1994). Failing at fairness: How Americas schools cheat
girls. New York: C. Scribners Sons.
Screen Actors Guild. (2005, October 5). TV performers again take hit from reality programming. Retrieved May 13, 2009, from http://www.sag.org/
content/tv-performers-again-take-hit-from-reality-programming.
Seidman, S. A. (1999). Revisiting sex-role stereotyping in MTV videos. International Journal of Instructional Media, 26, 11.
Sommers, C. H. (2000). The war against boys. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Stark, M., & Ebenkamp, B. (1999). The silence of the lads. Brandweek, 40, 2223.
Stephan, C. W., Stephan, W. G., Demitrakis, K. M., Yamada, A. M., & Clason,
D. L. (2000). Womens attitudes toward men: An integrated threat theory
approach. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 6373.
U.S. Department of Justice. (2006). Report to congress on stalking and domestic violence, 2005 through 2006. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice Ofce
on Violence Against Women.
U.S. Department of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2005, May 13). Women in
the labor force: A databook. Retrieved May 15, 2007, from www.bls.gov/cps/
wlf-table17-2005.pdf.
Valian, V. (1998). Why so slow? The advancement of women. Cambridge, MA: The
MIT Press.
Viki, G. T., Abrams, D., & Masser, B. (2004). Evaluating stranger and acquaintance rape: The role of benevolent sexism in perpetrator blame and recommended sentence length. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 295303.
Viki, G. T., Chiroro, P., & Abrams, D. (2006). Hostile sexism, type of rape, and
self-reported rape proclivity within a sample of Zimbabwean males. Violence Against Women, 12, 789800.
Yoder, J. D., Fischer, A. R., Kahn, A. S., & Groden, J. (2007). Changes in students
explanations for gender differences after taking a psychology of women
class: More constructionist and less essentialist. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 415425.

This page intentionally left blank

Chapter 2

Gender Differences: The Arguments


Regarding Abilities
Jennifer L. Martin

Women have made signicant social, academic, and occupational gains


in the past 50 years; for example, women are entering nontraditional
elds with more frequency, participating in high school and college
sports more than ever before, and carving out more egalitarian roles
for themselves within the family. However, women have still not ultimately achieved true equity with their male counterparts for a variety
of reasons, one of which stems from the self-fullling prophecy (or the
power of expectation). The idea that women somehow possess different
or inferior aptitudes when compared to their male counterparts can
lead to diminished expectations for womenin terms of how they
view themselves and how others view them (Martin, 2008). The conception that womens lacuna in achievement is somehow natural and
related to ability is perpetuated in the popular media and in academic
scholarship. For example, in January 2005, Harvard president, Lawrence Summers, suggested that innate ability might explain why
there are so few women in the highest levels of science, technology,
and engineering positions in academia (Bombardieri, 2005). Researchers
examined standardized test scores and compared the numbers of
women in mathematics and science courses and occupations to those
of men to justify gender-based differences in intellectual ability. However, there are many scholars and activists who insist that gender differences in achievement are based on cultural factors such as gender
socializationand that numbers alone do not tell the whole story about
womens abilities. It appears that the nature versus nurture debate is
still alive and wellboth in the popular press as well as in academe.

28

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Gender stereotyping has much to do with this debate, for it can lead
to gender discrimination; it can also create low expectations for the stereotyped. When people have low expectations for women attempting to
enter a nontraditional eld, for example, they may be less likely to hire
them based upon said stereotypes. Negative stereotypes based upon
gender can also lead to diminished expectations, which can affect people
on an individual level: where people expect little for themselves and live
up to that expectation. When expectations are low, so too is achievement; thus negative self-fullling prophecies are perpetuated. No matter
what the cause, gender discrimination is still a major problem for girls in
schools and for women in the workplace and in academe (Carr, Szalacha, Barnett, Caswell, & Inui, 2003). In order to be successful, girls and
women have to overcome more obstacles in a variety of different areas
simply by virtue of their gender. This fact is often compounded by
racial, socioeconomic, and heterosexist biases as well. Despite the fact
that, according to Lorber, . . . gender, like culture, is a human production that depends on everyone constantly doing gender (1994, p. 13,
as cited in Lueptow, Garovich-Szabo, & Lueptow, 2001, p. 3), people do
not generally view gender as a construction. Many view gender as a
naturally occurring phenomenon where behavioral roles are determined on the basis of biology and are thus unchangeable.
Gender socialization theory posits that gender differences in academic and career choices stem from stereotypeswhich are handed
down via socialization (Konrad, Yang, Goldberg, & Sullivan, 2005).
Women and men unknowingly accept traditional norms, values,
expectations, roles, as normal, natural, and their own (Konrad et
al., 2005; Martin, 2008). Adherence to such norms is reinforced by the
culture; those who deviate from proscribed behaviors are punished by
isolation, social and workplace exclusion, ridicule, etc. Because masculine and feminine stereotypes are still enforced for males and females,
often boys and men gravitate toward academic areas and careers that
relate to or represent the masculine role. The same is true for girls and
women (Konrad et al., 2005). Such stereotypes are bound to have an
effect on girls and womens perceptions of themselves and on what
they are capable of achieving. In sum, sociocultural inuences play a
large part in supposed gender differences (Whiston & Bouwkamp,
2003). The extent to which people challenge such norms has much to
do with individual expectation and future individual success. Although
feminist educators are working to alleviate the effect of gender stereotypes through education and to lobby for more egalitarian treatment of
girls and boys within the family, the schools, and in the workplace,
these cultural traditions and their effects still persist.
Perhaps the most telling facet of womens success in the workplace,
or lack thereof, is this psychological precept: observers perceive that the
abilities, attributes, and personality factors that enable a person to

Gender Differences: The Arguments Regarding Abilities

29

succeed in a particular activity are somehow natural to that person (or


group of people) (Eagly & Steffen, 1984). Thus, when women are absent
from certain elds or from positions of power, society deems it to be a
natural problem with women, that something within them is missing
that would enable them to hold said positions. The issue here is not necessarily inherent ability, but the perception of ability (or lack thereof).
The socialization of girls inuences gender differentiation in personality characteristics and values. For example, girls are often socialized to
believe that when they become women, they should place other things
ahead of their own career advancement, such as home and family. Even
if women do not hold these values as primary, they are often perceived
as holding these values by the corporation; corporate managers do not
promote people to positions of power who do not hold career as their
main priority. To complicate this situation, women who do not hold
these traditionally feminine realms sacred, as society deems they
should, may be harshly judged by this failure of femininity. Women
are still, to a certain extent, expected to value children and taking care of
the home and family as a priority; women who deviate from this
convention are often viewed with judgment and derision.
Kirchmeyer (2002) argues that gender roles are malleable. Although
gender socialization contributes to ones personality and dictates how
many people perform gender for life, peoples perceptions of their
gender or the expectations for their gender can be changed or altered
throughout the life cycle. Kirchmeyer (2002) argues that gender has
more to do with career choices than biological sex. Thus, gender role
socialization is a greater predictor of career choice than is biological sex.
According to Goodman, Fields, and Blum (2003), women and men possess similar career aspirations and values, which suggests that women
experience barriers to workplace advancement to explain their lack of
prominence in positions of power. The research suggests that women
have not achieved workplace equity, in terms of pay and in terms of
position, in part because of the expectations for their gender. In short,
narrow and limiting gender role expectations still exist and still negatively affect women. In fact, the essentializing of the sexes, or holding
differential expectations for women and men that are based solely on biological sex, can be dangerous and limiting for both women and men.
Kerka (1993) also cautions against the essentialization of women and
men. Some researchers (Gilligan, 1982) have called for a separate
approach to viewing women, that women have different ways of
knowing. Kerka warns that focusing on the differences, as opposed to
the similarities, between women and men can have serious negative
effects. Instead, validating a variety of types of thinking may assist in
different perspectives being valued as opposed to one. Women are
socialized to focus on relationships and intimacy; because of this, womens careers often do not follow a traditional or linear pattern (as is
common in men) (Kerka, 1993).

30

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Workplace stereotypes often hinder womens advancement (Crampton & Mishra, 1999): Women are thought to be weaker than men;
women are thought to lack commitment to their employment, for they
may leave to have or rear children. It is assumed that women cannot
work long hours because of household responsibilities; women are
thought to lack judgment in making difcult decisions or in high-stress
situations. The prevailing feminine stereotypes promote the prototype
of woman as nurturing, soft, caring, emotional, and communal. Midgley and Abrams (1974) indicate that societal expectations and stereotypes have had negative effects on womens motivation. Women must
ght these stereotypes in order to compete and succeed.
Gender schema theory argues that perceptions develop regarding
gender based upon the information we receive from a variety of areas.
As Bem (1983) states, Adults in the childs world rarely notice or
remark upon how strong a little girl is or how nurturant a little boy is
becoming, despite their readiness to note precisely these attributes in
the appropriate sex (p. 604). For todays girls and boys, such schema
can have negative effects on social and academic development and on
eventual career choices and advancement.
In the early 1980s, a series of articles was published by Benbow and
Stanley (1980, 1982) that attributed the gender differences between boys
and girls on the mathematical portion of standardized tests to biological
causes (Jacklin, 1989). The popular media then publicized these ndings. The effect of these studies and the subsequent communication of
them resulted in the support of the already popular myth that males
possess superior abilities in mathematics. As Jacklin indicates, although
these authors reported a biological explanation for gender differences,
they possessed no biological data. During this time, another larger-scale
study conducted by Eccles and Jacobs (1986) found that math anxiety,
stereotypical beliefs of parents with regard to gender, and the perception
of the value of math account for gender differences in mathematical
achievement. According to Jacklin, . . . Eccles and her colleagues have
found that most strongly related to their mothers beliefs concerning the
difculty of mathematics for their children. Mothers beliefs were also
important in that they directly and strongly inuenced their childrens
math anxiety (p. 127). Despite the results of this study, the overwhelming impact of the initial communication of innate differences between
the sexes in the press had long lasting and far-reaching effects.
This media campaign had a direct impact on parental perceptions,
which further caused more gender-based stereotypes regarding math
abilities of children. As Jacklin (1989) states, As mothers came to
believe that mathematics was much more difcult for girls than boys,
their daughters become less likely to take additional math courses
(p. 128). Linn and Petersen (1985, 1986) found that there is no consistent pattern of gender difference regarding mathematics, science, and

Gender Differences: The Arguments Regarding Abilities

31

spatial reasoning. In essence, cognitive differences based upon gender


have been reduced in the past few decades (Jacklin, 1989). In 1988,
Feingold analyzed ndings of the Differential Aptitude Tests (DAT) in
terms of gender and found that the performance advantage of males
on the spatial measures and the performance advantage of females on
the language measures decreased between the years of 1947 to 1980.
Rosenthal and Ruben (1982) also studied this phenomenon, but could
not nd a reason for it: . . . we can say that, whatever the reason, in
these studies females appear to be gaining in cognitive skill relative to
males rather faster than the gene can travel! (p. 711). Thus, it would
seem, nature, or biology alone, cannot answer the question of whether
or not gender differences exist with regard to academic ability.
One possible reason for the narrowing of the gender gap in math and
science is the attempt to make standardized tests less gender biased. It is
imperative that test developers create culturally responsible assessments
so that they are fair and equitable to all who take them. According to
Kaminski, Shafer, Neumann, and Ramos (2005), Examination of a
measures underlying factor structure and its stability across different
ethnic groups is one important step in legitimizing the widespread use
of any given measure. Failure to attend to such ethnic/cultural considerations could not only have a negative impact on assessment and therapy
but also violate ethical standards (p. 322). In short, certain groups have
historically had an advantage to perform well on standardized tests,
which has served to perpetuate societys status quo.
When examining standardized test results, oftentimes gender differences appear when data are disaggregated. For example, in general, girls
tend to outperform boys in terms of grades in the early elementary
years, but boys outperform girls on novel tasks (Robinson, Abbott, Berninger, & Bussee, 1996). However, by high school, boys outperform girls.
Most studies on gender indicate that males outperform females on mathematical, logical, and spatial abilities (Rammstedt & Rammsayer, 2002).
Males are more likely than females to score higher on measures of science and mathematics; females tend to score higher on measures of
reading comprehension (OReilly & McNamara, 2007). The largest gender differences exist in the area of visual-spatial abilities. According to
Linn and Petersen (1986), such differences can be measured around the
ages of eight or nine; these increase at the age of 18 and continue
throughout the life cycle. Halpern (1989) argues that gender differences
in cognitive abilities still exist and that studies suggesting the gap is
decreasing are faulty. However, numbers alone cannot adequately
describe the range of human experiences that contribute to such seemingly gender-based content area differences on standardized tests.
To give a strictly biological explanation for these differences is simplistic at best. There are a variety of factors at play here. For example,
boys are overrepresented in high school advanced math courses

32

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

(Robinson et al., 1996), which may affect their performance on standardized tests; girls underrepresentation in the same courses also contributes to the explanation of the achievement gap of girls. Despite the fact
that girls achievement in mathematics has improved in recent years,
girls perceptions of their math achievement remain dysfunctional
(Preckel, Goetz, Pekrun, & Kleine, 2008). This is also true of girls who
are gifted in science. As Preckel et al. (2008) indicate, In the general
population, fewer women than men chose academic courses or careers
in mathematics, the inorganic sciences, and engineering. This also
holds true for women of high scientic ability. Research has further
documented that the underrepresentation of women in these elds
grows with increasing scientic ability (p. 147).
Gender differences in math and science have much to do with the
expectations of future success and perceptions of competence (again
the self-fullling prophecy). Parents expectations do much to inuence
their childrens perceptions along these lines (Preckel et al., 2008). Girls
feel they have to work harder to achieve success in math, and they
show less condence in their abilities (Preckel et al., 2008). In sum, stereotypical gender role expectations on the part of the individual, in
conjunction with the expectations of parents and teachers, work to
shape achievement-oriented motivations, which inuence development
(Preckel et al., 2008). This can have negative consequences for girls in
mathematics and science courses. In addition, female students tend to
rate their competence levels in mathematics lower than their male
counterparts (Preckel et al., 2008). The corresponding stereotype that
still persists is that girls are better at reading and writing; this expectation can obviously do much to hinder boys in these areas.
Beginning at an early age, boys possess a higher level of general
self-esteem than do girls (Hergovich, Sirch, & Felinger, 2004). Moreover, boys have been found to possess higher self-concepts in the area
of mathematics, whereas girls possess higher self-concepts in language
(Hergovich et al., 2004). Furthermore, males tend to be more likely than
females to suffer from reading disabilities (OReilly & McNamara,
2007). Females tend to use overt study strategies more often than males
and have higher grade point averages (GPAs) (OReilly & McNamara,
2007). Reasons for gender difference include gender-stereotyped socialization (Hergovich et al., 2004). According to OReilly & McNamara
(2007), . . . we also recommend addressing gender differences by
working together with teachers and parents to reduce stereotypical attitudes and behaviors that support gender differences. This is important
because research has shown that gender differences are supported by
socialization. For example, recent work has shown that mothers engage
in more science talk with boys than they do with girls . . . and teachers
are often more responsive with boys than girls (pp. 187188). In short,
self-perceptions are often not only echoed by others, but also

Gender Differences: The Arguments Regarding Abilities

33

inuenced by them, e.g., by parents and teachers. For example, according to Hergovich et al. (2004), Parents especially tend to judge their
children in a gender-stereotypical way, so that boys are rated better in
the areas of mathematics and sports and girls are rated better in verbal
areas (p. 208). Even if performance rates do not accord with these perceptions, sons are still rated higher than daughters.
Research indicates that expectation has much to do with academic
achievement. To illustrate this point, males estimate their general intelligence higher than do females (Rammstedt & Rammsayer, 2002). Beloff
(1992) found self-estimates to be related to gender stereotypes. According to Rammstedt and Rammsayer (2002), Such gender stereotypes
may be based on minor though true gender differences that become
overpronounced by ignoring intragender differences and, concomitantly, by focusing on the intergender bias (p. 276). Thus, the story
becomes: there are biological differences based upon gender which
explain academic differences. Historically, this has been demonstrated
on standardized assessments and it was seen as natural. Now we see
that there can be cultural and sociological factors that contribute to
such explanations. Geary (1989) argues that the explanation for gender
differences in academics is not biological, but cultural: specically, that
culture can inuence the development of cognitive skills. However, this
cultural explanation (that the experiences of males and females are
becoming increasingly similar over time) does not account for the complexity of this phenomenon. In other words, Geary argues that there is
more to this than simply the effect of culture on humans. Hormones
and anatomy still play a part in cognitive development, which may
inuence choice, which then may further inuence male advantage in
certain areas. According to Geary:
Culture, however, may attenuate or exaggerate these early biologyrelated behavioral gender differences by directing the focus of the experiences of male and female individuals to be more or less similar, which
would thereby inuence the pattern of gender-related abilities. Within
this model, the male performance advantage on measures of spatial ability would be related to prenatal hormonal factors, but the magnitude of
this advantage would not be immutable and in fact could vary across
generations and across cultures. (p. 1155)

If this is in fact the case, then, as Geary argues, as females and males
continue to engage in increasingly similar activities, gender differences
in terms of cognitive abilities may cease to exist. Also, as Geary argues,
these experiential differences may interact with hormonal differences,
which may further lessen gender differences. In short, biology and
human development are not immutable. For example, as Geary states,
. . . the distribution of these early sex-dimorphic behaviors might be

34

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

continuous with later patterns of adolescent special-related activities


which, in turn, could contribute to gender, and individual, performance differences on measures of spatial ability (p. 1156).
Jacklin (1989) argues that the negative effects of the media coverage
on the continued superiority of males in math should be proactively
combated in schools. Interventions should be created that address negative stereotypes on behalf of students, parents, and teachers. Much work
has been done in this regard (see Sadker & Sadker, 1994) such that there
is currently a backlash against interventions for girls. The media now
suggests that the extra time and resources used to increase achievement
for girls in math and science has had a negative effect on boys.
In 2005, Laura Bush announced that her new focus would be the
problem of boys (2006, Newsweek). The popular media (via the New
Republic, Esquire, Newsweek, the Today show, etc.) presented the case
that at every level of education, Americas boys are falling behind in
academic achievement, graduating from high school at lower rates than
girls, attending college less frequently than girls, and exhibiting poorer
verbal skills (Mead, 2006). Single-sex classrooms and schools are opening nationwide to address this crisis by redirecting educational efforts
(and funding) to the nations boys. Feminists and Title IX are often
blamed for this new boy crisis (see, for example, Christina Hoff
Summers The War against Boys: How Misguided Feminism is Harming our
Young Men); when, in fact, opponents of Title IX have created and perpetuated the myth that expanded educational opportunities for girls
have somehow come at the expense of boys. However, this is a gross
oversimplication. This is the very nature of backlash: to minimize or
erase the real gains that girls have made by perpetuating a myth that
Americas boys are in crisis, in large part, because of the attention paid
to Americas girls. However, when one truly examines the data, the
test scores, in elementary school girls do as well or better in math than
boys. There is a gap in middle school that widens greatly between girls
and boys in high school (Mead, 2006). In fact, in terms of overall academic achievement, attainment for boys is higher than ever (Mead,
2006). According to the American Association of University Women
(AAUW) (2008) Educational achievement is not a zero-sum game, in
which a gain for one group results in a corresponding loss for the
other. If girls success comes at the expense of boys, one would expect
to see boys scores decline as girls scores rise, but this has not been
the case (p. 2).
This gap also translates to higher education. For example, in Ivy
League schools, men still outnumber women (Rivers & Barnett, 2006).
The truth is that the real crisis is a crisis of poverty: 76 percent of students in middle- to higher-income areas graduate from high school,
compared to 56 percent of students from lower-income areas (Swanson,
2004). The truth is that there are more differences within the sexes than

Gender Differences: The Arguments Regarding Abilities

35

between: boys are more different from each another than they are from
their female counterparts (Mead, 2006). The true test of equity in America is the wage gap, which still disproportionately affects women negatively (women earn .77 to a dollar earned by a man) and is a telling
example of just what kind of crisis we are still experiencing (Institute
for Womens Policy Research, 2008).
The data suggest that boys have made progress in most academic
areas, but girls have made improvements faster in certain areas, such
as math, science, and geography. Consequently, girls have narrowed
some academic achievement gaps, creating the fear that boys are in crisis (Mead, 2006). The idea that girls are surpassing their male counterparts in certain academic areas (although they are still behind in
others) seems hard for many to accept. Instead, many blame feminism
for shortchanging boys. According to some critics, feminists have
advocated for allotting monetary resources on girls (at the expense of
boys) and demonized typical male behavior. Perhaps the conversation
about gender and achievement should shift to one that is not based
upon one group at the expense of another, but on interventions that
are necessary at all levels; scholars and advocates should be able to discuss boys and achievement without unfairly undermining the gains
girls have made. When disaggregating data for race and class, white
suburban boys are not signicantly affected by this crisis. Again, the
privileged perform well on standardized assessment, and thus the status quo is maintained. The boys who truly need academic interventions
are many rural and inner-city boys.
Despite all of the attention in the popular press about boys in crisis,
the fact remains that academic faculties in U.S. institutions, especially
in the science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) areas,
are still predominantly male (Spelke, 2005). Popular belief suggests that
this is the case because rst, women are less talented in these elds;
and, second, this gender difference has a genetic explanation (dealing
with intrinsic aptitude). According to Spelke (2005), males possess
greater variability in their inherent talent in mathematics, and thus,
they predominate in professions such as mathematician and scientist:
that a genetic predisposition to learn about particular, gender-specic,
things exists. Females have made gains in these areas, largely because
of Title IX. For example, the gender gap has closed with regard to
enrollment in calculus courses (Spelke, 2005), and in 2000, women
received 47 percent of the bachelors degrees awarded in mathematics
(Spelke, 2005).
According to Spelke (2005), men do not possess greater intrinsic aptitude in mathematics and science, Although older boys and girls
show somewhat different cognitive proles, the differences are complex and subtle (it is not the case, e.g., that women are verbal and men
are spatial) (p. 956). Spelke argues that such differences are caused by

36

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

strategy choices. In terms of both average and gifted students, high


school boys and girls do equally well in math classes.
Title IX has done much to increase girls visibility in traditionally
male dominated areas: such as math, science, and sports. Such male
dominated domains have also, it has been argued by many, been due
largely to differential, innate, differences between women and men.
Prior to the 1970s, the career and technical education system in the
United States intentionally segregated students by sex. Soon after Title
IX passed, the National Collegiate Athletic Association (NCAA) and
high school administrators argued that boys sports would suffer if
girls sports had to be funded equally. Since the enforcement of Title
IX, girls participation and achievement in math and science have
increased substantially, discrimination against pregnant girls and teachers has decreased, vocational schools and classes have been opened to
both sexes, sexual harassment in schools is now illegal, and girls participation in sports has exponentially increased (National Coalition for
Women and Girls in Education, 2008). Since the passage of Title IX,
womens high school participation in sports has increased by 904 percent (from 294,015 in 19711972 to 2,953,355 in 20052006). Mens high
school sports participation has increased by 15 percent (from 3,666,917
in 19711972 to 4,206,549 in 20052006) (National Coalition for Women
and Girls in Education, 2008). At the college level, womens sports participation has increased by 456 percent since the passage of Title IX
(from 29,977 in 19711972 to 166,728 in 20042005). Mens sports participation since the passage of Title IX has increased by 31 percent (from
170,384 in 19711972 to 222,838 in 20042005) (National Coalition for
Women and Girls in Education, 2008). Thus, Title IX has not caused a
decrease by male sports participation at either the high school or the
college level.
According to the National Womens Law Center (July 2008) there are
many positive effects of sports participation for girls: it increases condence, self-esteem, and promotes pride in girls physical and social
selves. Sports participation can also reduce the likelihood of drug use
and teenage pregnancy, increase graduation rates, and decrease depression. Female student athletes are more likely to have higher grades and
are less likely to drop out of school; they also have higher graduation
rates than their nonathletic peers. Because of Title IX, the gender gap at
all grade levels has decreased signicantly since 1970 on standardized
assessments in science and math. The percentage of women obtaining
bachelors degrees in natural sciences and engineering has more than
doubled. The percentage of women obtaining doctoral degrees in these
same elds has more than quadrupled. However, women still only earn
20 to 25 percent of the degrees awarded in physics, computer sciences,
and engineering. In addition, the culture of STEM elds remains male
dominated and still isolates and excludes girls and women.

Gender Differences: The Arguments Regarding Abilities

37

Again, despite the attention in the popular press that girls and
women are outperforming boys and men in a variety of areas, there is
still much work to be done. For example, women comprise 79 percent
of public school teachers, but only 44 percent of the principals. Women
represent less than one in ve faculty members in STEM elds. In engineering in particular, women account for just over one in ten faculty
members. Women are 49 percent of all part-time academic employees
at the college level, but hold only 39 percent of full-time academic jobs.
The salaries of women K12 teachers in 1973 were 84 percent of male
teachers salaries. The discrepancy in female and male teacher earnings
is smaller than the national average for all working women in 57 percent in 1973 and 77 percent in 2006. In institutions of higher education,
overall wages for women faculty have remained at approximately 81
percent of mens earnings since the late 1970s (National Coalition for
Women and Girls in Education, 2008).
Many inequities still exist because of a lack of understanding and
implementation of the law. Title IX is often poorly understood and
poorly implemented by educators, parents, and students. For example,
relatively few education agencies comply with the Title IX regulation
to appoint, train, and make available their Title IX coordinators.
Parents and community members can help this problem locally, by
calling their local school districts and asking about Title IX coordinators, state-wide by contacting their congress members in order to put
pressure on school districts to comply with these regulations. Specically, every recipient of federal funding (under Title IX) must designate
and adequately train at least one Title IX coordinator.
Prior to Title IX, many educators and counselors accepted the stereotype that girls could not achieve in STEM subjects. Unfortunately, these
stereotypes still exist, as illustrated by the comments of Lawrence
Summers, which were discussed at the beginning of this chapter, for
example. Despite the perpetuation of these stereotypes by some, signicant progress has been made by women in many nontraditional areas,
dispelling the myth that academic ability has inherently something to
do with ones gender. However, there is still much more work to be
done. Unfortunately, girls still comprise approximately 90 percent of
students enrolled in classes, which will lead to traditionally female
occupations and only 15 percent of classes in traditionally male elds
(National Coalition for Women and Girls in Education, 2008). There are
many factors that contribute to this occurrence: the perpetuation of
gender stereotypes being one. Other factors, which may stem from the
adoption of or adherence to said stereotypes is a distinct possibility;
for example, biased career counseling, discriminatory treatment by
teachers, sexual harassment, and other sexist practices, can result in
limited educational opportunities for girls and women. This translates
to signicant negative consequences for womens current and eventual

38

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

economic security. Despite gains in educational equity, there is still a


backlash against Title IX. Some opponents to Title IX insist that the law
has led to diminished opportunities for men, contrary to the evidence.
Opportunities for men in sports have expanded since the passage of
Title IX, and continue to do sowith regard to both numbers of participating athletes and numbers of teams (National Coalition for Women
and Girls in Education, 2008).
Finally, Title IX has also been blamed for inspiring feminized curricula and learning environments that disadvantage boys. Proposals for
improving classroom settings for boys learning have included recommendations for sex-stereotyped curricula, and for single-sex schools.
Title IX compliance has, by and large, been driven by lawsuits and
threats of lawsuits. Although the law states that schools in violation of
Title IX will lose their federal funding, since the passage of Title IX in
1972 no school has ever lost federal funding for failing to comply with
the law.
POTENTIAL SOLUTIONS
The debate between socialization and essentialism is still alive in the
minds of Americans and in the pages of academic journals and popular
magazines. Yet it seems that, all in all, more differences in abilities
exist within the sexes than between them. The gap that women face in
earnings and the lower numbers of women in positions of power
within organizations and in certain male dominated elds cannot be
explained away by innate differences based upon sex. These differentials have more to do with expectation, discrimination, family structure (child care issues and the division of labor at home), and lack of
mentoring.
Women in general face more obstacles to career success and
advancement in terms of pay and position than do men. However, this
does not mean that women are helpless victims. Women have more
agency than this; they can demonstrate this by creating positive selffullling prophecies for themselves, and participating in the creation of
better realities. Women must often ght against how they were raised,
and how society views them.
Condence, or the belief in ones ability to succeed, is often associated with privilege. Men working in traditionally male elds possess
privilege and have access to the organizational norms and practices
that are often not made explicit to outsiders. Women face unique
experiences such as this that can interfere with career motivation, performance, and success. In short, a high condence level is a necessary
attribute when individuals are the minority in certain elds (such as
women in the STEM areas). Women working in nontraditional elds
are, oftentimes, already at a disadvantage because they may not be

Gender Differences: The Arguments Regarding Abilities

39

privy to implicit norms and informal networks within the organization.


Condence is a good way to inspire competence and can lead to the
breaking down of stereotypes that still may be held within the organization.
There are many things that women can do to heighten their career
success and thus contribute to the breaking down of stereotypes that
perpetuate the myth that women are somehow naturally less capable
in certain areas; men can also assist in the successful implementation
of these suggestions. It should not be assumed that, in a heterosexual
nuclear family, the mans career should dictate the behavior of others
in the household. The division of labor and care of children should be
based on the needs of all parties involved within the family. If women
have supportive partners who are true partners in the home, then they
will have a greater chance of achieving workplace success (Martin,
2008).
Women must work to value themselves, have high expectations for
themselves (Noonan, Gallor, & Hensler-McGinnis, 2004), and be proactive in managing their own careers. Women must develop an internal
locus of control (Midgley & Abrams, 1974) by attributing ones successes to ones own abilities, and not to mere luck. This is a key to
achieving and maintaining career condence.
Other integral areas that must be rectied in order for gender equity
to occur are the establishment of progressive gender role identities.
Progressive gender roles will assist in the reduction of stereotypes and
negative expectations with regard to gender. There are many ways to
work toward this end: intervene when gender harassment and sexual
harassment are witnessed, report these instances on the organizational
level, and support victims. Concerned individuals must intervene for
the health and well-being of all and for the overall health of the organization (organizational culture) (Miner-Rubino & Cortina, 2004; Welsh,
1999). Finally, we must vote for legislators who value women and who
will ght for laws that promote equity for all people.
Much work still must be done with parents, teachers, and counselors
in their understanding of the danger in adhering to gender based stereotypes and negative self-fullling prophecies. Much work also must
be done with students so that they may realize their individual potential. Interventions are important for girls, parents, and teachers that
will dispel the myths that math and science are more difcult for girls.
Current research ndings in psychology should be communicated to
teachers and parents. Girls and women should be provided with nonsexist academic career counseling. A practical solution, devised by the
Feminist Majority Foundation, is the creation of a public electronic web
listing of all Title IX coordinators. This will provide students, teachers,
and parents with contact information of their local Title IX coordinators, or where they may seek advice and lodge complaints of Title IX

40

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

violations. Such a listing will also assist Title IX coordinators nationwide in the sharing and dissemination of information.
In sum, there are many arguments for and against gender-based differences in ability leading to gaps in achievement. Similar arguments
have been made with regard to race. They are both devised to the
same end: to perpetuate privilege and maintain the status quo. Since
we know that culture inuences biology, it seems prudent to focus
energy on creating systemic change with regard to how we view gender and ability, and in creating interventions to foster gender equity
for both girls and boys.
REFERENCES
AAUW. (2008). Where the girls are: The facts about gender equity in education.
Washington DC: AAUW Educational Foundation.
Bem, S. L. (1983). Gender schema theory and its implications for child development: Raising gender-aschematic children in a gender-schematic society.
Signs, 8, 598616.
Benbow, C. P., & Stanley, J. C. (1980). Sex differences in mathematics ability:
Fact or artifact? Science, 210, 12621264.
Benbow, C. P., & Stanley, J. C. (1982). Consequences in high school and college
of sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability: A longitudinal perspective. American Educational Research Journal, 19, 598622.
Bombardieri, M. (2005, January 19). Harvard womens group rips Summers.
The Boston Globe [Online Version]. Retrieved January 15, 2009, from http://
www.Boston.com/news/education/higher/articles/2005/01/19/Harvard_
womens_group.
Carr, P. L., Szalacha, L., Barnett, R., Caswell, C., & Inui, T. (2003). A ton of
feathers: Gender discrimination in academic medical careers and how to
manage it. Journal of Womens Health, 12, 10091018.
Crampton, S. M., & Mishra, J. M. (1999). Women in management. Public Personnel Management, 28, 87106.
Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 46, 735754.
Eccles, J. S., & Jacobs, J. E. (1986). Social forces shape math attitudes and performance. Signs, 11, 367389.
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and womens development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Goodman, J. S., Fields, D. L., & Blum, T. C. (2003). Cracks in the glass ceiling:
In what kinds of organizations do women make it to the top? Group and
Organization Management, 28, 475501.
Halpern, D. F. (1989, August). The disappearance of cognitive gender differences: What you see depends on where you look. American Psychologist, 44:
11561158.
Hergovich, A., Sirsch, U., & Felinger, M. (2004). Gender differences in the
self-concept of preadolescent children. School Psychology International, 25,
207222.

Gender Differences: The Arguments Regarding Abilities

41

Institute for Womens Policy Research. (2008, August). The gender wage gap:
2007. Retrieved January 22, 2009, from http://www.iwpr.org/pdf/
C350.pdf.
Jacklin, C. N. (1989, February). Female and male: Issues of gender. American
Psychologist, 44, 127133.
Kaminski, P. L., Shafer, M. E., Neumann, C. S., & Ramos, V. (2005). Self-concept in Mexican American girls and boys: Validating the self-description
questionnaireI. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 11, 321
338.
Kerka, S. (1993). Women, human development, and learning. Columbus, OH: ERIC
Clearinghouse on Adult, Career, and Vocational Education. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED358379).
Kirchmeyer, C. (2002). Change and stability in managers gender roles. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 87, 929939.
Konrad, A. M., Yang, Y., Goldberg, C., & Sullivan, S. E. (2005). Preferences for
job attributes associated with work and family: A longitudinal study of career outcomes. Sex Roles, 53, 303315.
Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. (1986). A meta-analysis of gender differences in
spatial ability: Implications for mathematics and science achievement. In J.
S. Hyde & M.C. Linn (Eds.) The psychology of gender: Advances through metaanalysis (pp. 67101). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Lueptow, L. B., Garovich-Szabo, L., & Lueptow, M. B. (2001). Social change
and the persistence of sex typing: 19741997. Social Forces, 80, 136.
Lyness, K. S., & Thompson, D. E. (2000). Climbing the corporate ladder: Do
female and male executives follow the same route? Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 86101.
Martin, J. (2008). Shifting the load: Personality factors and women in the workplace. In M. Paludi (Ed.), The psychology of women at work: Challenges and
solutions for our female workforce. Career liberation, history, and the new
millennium (Vol 1., pp. 153200). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Midgley, N., & Abrams, M. S. (1974). Fear of success and locus of control in
young women. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 42, 737.
Miner-Rubino, K., & Cortina, L. M. (2004). Working in a context of hostility toward women: Implications for employees well-being. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 9, 107122.
National Coalition for Women and Girls in Education. (2008). Title IX at 35:
Beyond the headlines. Retrieved October 30, 2008, from http://
www.ncwge.org/PDF/ TitleIXat35.pdf.
National Womens Law Center. (2008, June). The battle for gender equity in athletics in elementary and secondary schools. Retrieved October 30, 2008, from
http://www.nwlc.org/pdf/Battle%202007.pdf.
Noonan, B. M., Gallor, S. M., & Hensler-McGinnis, N. F. (2004). Challenge and
success: A qualitative study of the career development of highly achieving
women with physical and sensory disabilities. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 6880.
OReilly, T., & McNamara, D. S. (2007). The impact of science knowledge, reading skill, and reading strategy knowledge on more traditional high
stakes measures of high school students science achievement. American
Educational Research Journal, 44, 161196.

42

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Preckel, F., Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., & Kleine, M. (2008). Gender differences in
gifted and average-ability students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 52, 146159.
Rammstedt, B., & Rammsayer, T. H. (2002). Self-estimated intelligence: Gender
differences, relationship to psychometric intelligence and moderating
effects of level of education. European Psychologist, 7, 275284.
Rivers, C., & Barnett, R. B. (2006, April). The myth of the boy crisis. Retrieved
January 10, 2009, from http://feminist.org/education/pdfs/BoyCrisis.pdf.
Robinson, N. M., Abbott, R. D., Berninger, V. W., & Busse, J. (1996). The structure of abilities in math-precocious young children: Gender similarities
and differences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 341352.
Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1982). Further meta-analytic procedures for
assessing cognitive gender differences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74,
708712.
Sadker, M., & Sadker, D. (1994). Failing at fairness: How Americas schools cheat
girls. New York: Charles Scribners Sons.
Shih, J. (2006). Circumventing discrimination: Gender and ethnic strategies in
silicon valley. Gender and Society, 20, 177206.
Spelke, E. S. (2005). Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathematics and
science? American Psychologist, 60, 950958.
Swanson, C. B. (2004, February). Statistical portrait of public high school graduation, class on 2001. Washington, DC: The Urban Institute. Retrieved October
30, 2008, from http://www.urban.org/publications/410934.html.
Welsh, S. (1999). Gender and sexual harassment. Annual Review of Sociology, 25,
169190.
Whiston, S. C., & Bouwkamp, J. C. (2003). Ethical implications of career assessment with women. Journal of Career Assessment, 11, 5975.

Chapter 3

Women in Education: Students and


Professors Worldwide
Susan Basow

Worldwide, women constitute 64 percent of all adults who are illiterate


(United Nations Population Fund [UNFPA], 2005), but there are wide
variations across cultures. In most developed countries, girls and
women actually outnumber their male counterparts on the university
level, whereas in many developing countries, boys outnumber girls
even at the primary school level (UNFPA, 2005). Despite this variation,
gender issues affect the education of girls and women in all countries.
In this chapter, we will examine the status of women in education in
terms of students educational attainment, as well as in terms of the
barriers to gender equity that they encounter in the educational system.
We also will look at the status of women as teachers and professors, as
well as the barriers to gender equity that they confront.
WOMEN AS STUDENTS
The importance of gender equity in education, especially literacy,
cannot be overstated. As the UNFPA (2005) reported, womens educational attainment has benets not only for women themselves (in terms
of more economic opportunities and protection against human immunodeciency virus [HIV]), but also for their society as a whole (in
terms of breaking the cycle of poverty, labor force participation,
engagement in public life, and fewer but healthier children). Yet there
are many barriers to gender equity in education faced by girls and
women, from lack of encouragement or actual restrictions on school
attendance to more subtle forms of gender discrimination within the

44

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

school environment. In this section, we will examine gender differences


in educational attainment, the effects of gender stereotypes and roles,
and the school environment itself.
EDUCATIONAL ATTAINMENT
According to the 2005 report of the UNFPA, the goal of achieving
gender equity in primary education has been achieved in most countries of the world except Southern Asia, sub-Saharan Africa, and Western Asia. The countries where fewer than 76 girls are enrolled for
every 100 boys are Chad, Yemen, Central African Republic, Niger,
Benin, Ethiopia, Burkina Faso, Cote dIvoire, Guinea, and Mali. Among
the factors that have been found to encourage girls to attend and stay
in school are safe transportation, separate toilet facilities, and avoiding
gender stereotyping in the classroom.
Whereas about half of all developing countries have achieved gender
parity at the primary school level, only about 20 percent have done so at
the secondary school level (UNFPA, 2005). Again, the countries with the
poorest record are in Southern Asia, sub-Saharan Africa, and Western
Asia. The countries where fewer than 76 girls are enrolled for every 100
boys are Benin, Equatorial Guinea, Cambodia, Djibouti, Ethiopia,
Burkina Faso, Niger, Eritrea, Mozambique, Senegal, Gambia, Congo,
Burundi, Lao Peoples Democratic Republic, and India (UNFPA, 2005).
Interestingly, in some developed countries (especially in Latin America
and the Caribbean), girls slightly outnumber boys in secondary school
due to the higher dropout rate for boys (UNFPA, 2005).
At the university level, an even greater disparity between developed
and developing countries emerges. Whereas men greatly outnumber
women in higher education by 92 percent in developing countries,
women actually outnumber men in many developed countries (Charles &
Bradley, 2002; UNFPA, 2005). For example, in the United States in 2006,
women received 58 percent of all bachelors and masters degrees and
almost reached parity with men for doctoral and professional degrees
(National Center for Educational Statistics [NCES], 2005). By 2014, women
are expected to receive the majority of all educational degrees beyond
high school in the United States.
It is important to note that reversal in the gender ratio of educational achievement in the United States and other developed countries
that has occurred over the last 30 years is due to the dramatic increase
in the percentage of women participating in higher education, rather
than to fewer men doing so than in the past. For example, 33 percent
of all U.S. men aged 18 to 24 years old attended college in 1967 compared to 34 percent in 2006. In contrast, only 19 percent of all women
aged 18 to 24 years old attended college in 1967 compared to 41 percent in 2006 (NCES, 2008). A similar pattern has occurred with respect

Women in Education: Students and Professors Worldwide

45

to the attainment of advanced educational degrees: Men have shown a


modest increase, whereas women have shown a more dramatic
increase (NCES, 2005). For example, the number of doctoral degrees
awarded between 2002 and 2003 is projected to increase 19 percent
overall between 2013 and 2014, but 12 percent for men and 28 percent
for women. Thus, the new pattern is less a change in mens educationrelated behavior than a tremendous increase in womens educational
attainment, especially among older women. This reversal of gender
ratios is most likely due to the reduction of barriers to girls and womens educational attainment in many developed countries.
BARRIERS TO GENDER EQUITY IN EDUCATION
Prime among the barriers confronting girls and women worldwide
are gendered roles and expectations. Because of womens reproductive
abilities, womens societal roles must be compatible with childbearing
and nursing (Basow, 1992). In developed countries, where reproductive
choice is emphasized and some societal supports exist for nonmaternal
child-care (even if costly), women constitute nearly half of the paid
labor force (UNFPA, 2005). In developing countries, often with a subsistence or agrarian basis, women are less likely to be in the paid labor
force than are men (UNFPA, 2005). Worldwide, women constitute only
39 percent of all wage and salary workers (UNFPA, 2005). While
underrepresented in the paid labor force, women are overrepresented
in the informal economy, such as agricultural production and market
work, which has little nancial security or social benets (UNFPA,
2005). Women also perform most of the household chores, which, in
developing countries, tend to be very time-consuming, such as collecting fuel and carrying water.
Because of the domestic roles women typically play, womens education has been seen historically as less valuable and important than
mens education (Basow, 1992; Lips, 2008). Even in developed countries, where women constitute nearly half of the paid labor force, womens domestic activities are presumed to be womens main priorities.
Consequently, womens employment options historically have been
limited to jobs compatible with childbearing and child rearing. Such
jobs, such as service and ofce work, typically allow for intermittent
employment and do not require higher education. Most also are low
status and poorly paid. Other traditional female jobs that do require
education beyond high school, such as teaching and nursing, also are
relatively compatible with child-care, as they allow for intermittent
employment and schedules that coincide with those of their school-age
children. These jobs are often viewed as an extension of womens nurturing activities and historically have not been highly compensated.
Even though womens employment options in developed countries

46

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

today are theoretically unrestricted, occupational segregation still


exists, and the challenge of balancing work and family life still disproportionately falls on women (Lips, 2008). Thus, one barrier to gender
equity in education worldwide is the gendered division of labor.
A second barrier to gender equity in education, closely related to
womens societal roles, is gender stereotyping. Cultures vary somewhat
regarding the full range of traits and abilities associated with each gender, but in nearly all cultures, women are more associated with communal traits and men are more associated with agentic traits (Williams &
Best, 1990). Indeed, gender stereotyping is often used to justify the nurturing roles assigned to women, although research suggests that differential social roles actually create gender stereotypes (Eagly & Steffen,
1984). That is, whoever typically cares for children or the elderly is
viewed as nurturant, regardless of gender; and whoever typically works
in high-powered jobs is viewed as active and dominant, again regardless
of gender.
Given their link with adult roles, gender stereotypes also affect educational opportunities. Because boys are assumed to be more active
and aggressive, they typically are educated to assume the leadership
roles in society, whether those roles are in business or politics. The
education of boys, therefore, is given higher priority than the education
of girls, as can be seen in the statistics on school attendance in developing countries. Boys often receive encouragement and training to pursue
the higher-paying and higher-status jobs in a culture because boys are
seen as both more naturally inclined to such work as well as the
more likely breadwinner in a family.
There also are specic academic skills that are gender-stereotyped,
although these vary somewhat cross-culturally. In the United States and
many other developed and developing countries, the elds of math, science, and technology are stereotyped as masculine, while the humanities
and the creative arts (art, music, writing) are stereotyped as feminine
(Charles & Bradley, 2002; Eccles, Jacobs, & Harold, 1990; Nosek, Banaji, &
Greenwald, 2002; Skelton, Francis, & Smulyan, 2006). These trends both
reect and reinforce academic gender typing. Evidence suggests that gender stereotypes of academic subjects affect students academic performance, as well as their career choices. Indeed, in most developed countries,
women are underrepresented in engineering, mathematics, computer science and, to a lesser degree, natural science; they are overrepresented in
education, humanities, and health elds; and there is approximate gender
parity in the social sciences (Charles & Bradley, 2002).
Many people view academic gender stereotypes as based on innate
gender differences in intellectual abilities: that is, boys are considered
innately to have better math and visual-spatial skills, while girls are
considered innately to have better verbal skills and ne-motor coordination. Some neuroscience research supports the idea that gendered

Women in Education: Students and Professors Worldwide

47

patterns in performance on such cognitive tasks may be due to gendered


patterns of brain organization (Berenbaum & Resnick, 2007; Gur & Gur,
2007; Kimura, 1999). However, most researchers (e.g., Haier, 2007; Hines,
2004) note that individual differences typically overshadow gender differences in brain structure and organization. That is, there are larger differences among boys as a group and girls as a group on all these
intellectual tasks than there are between boys and girls. Furthermore,
environmental inuences, both prenatal (e.g., maternal nutrition) and
postnatal (e.g., diet, experience), also affect brain development. In fact,
in cultures where men and women are more equal (e.g., Iceland, Norway, Sweden), the male advantage in math performance disappears,
although the female advantage in reading does not (Guiso, Monte, Sapienza, & Zingales, 2008). Most importantly, gender similarities greatly
outweigh gender differences in cognitive abilities, and what differences
exist typically are small and variable (Hyde, 2005).
Nonetheless, because of gender stereotyping and gender roles, gender often serves as a cue for differential treatment and differential experience both in school and out. One type of differential experience is
based on the gendered curriculum. In many developing countries, girls
and boys are educated differently with different coursework and materials (Skelton et al., 2006; Sohoni, 1995). Indeed, their educational experiences may be completely gender segregated, such as occurs in many
Muslim countries. In contrast, unequal gender-based educational practices have been outlawed in most developed countries since the 1970s.
For example, in the United States a series of laws (e.g., Title IX of the
Education Amendments of 1972, the Womens Educational Equity Act
of 1974, the Vocational Educational Act of 1976, and the Career Incentive Act of 1977) ended the common practices of providing technical
training only to boys, and training in home economics only to girls.
Nonetheless, certain less obvious discriminatory practices remain.
Among the more subtle discriminatory practices is the use of curricular materials that focus on boys and men more than girls and women,
conveying the message that the former are more interesting, dominant,
and important than the latter. Such is the case with primary-schoollevel storybooks, where male characters outnumber female characters
by a ratio of 2:1 or 3:1. Boys and men also are depicted in a wide variety of occupations and activities, while girls and women more often
are portrayed as mothers, bystanders, or in need of help (Diekman &
Murnen, 2004; Gooden & Gooden, 2001). In upper grades, men more
often are the focus of study than are women, whether in regard to history (mainly a chronicle of mens lives in politics and war), literature
(novels, poetry, and essays written by men), or science (great discoveries by men) (Koch, 2003).
Because of gender stereotypes and roles, as well as differential
encouragement by parents and teachers, gender differences in

48

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

academic interests start appearing by high school. In developed countries, more high school boys than girls take computer design and
Advanced Placement (AP) science courses, and more girls than boys
take AP courses in English, biology, and foreign languages (Koch,
2003). (AP courses allow high school students who achieve high grades
on standardized tests to attain college credit for their work.) Vocational
education is highly gender-segregated in virtually all cultures, with
construction, skilled trades, information technology, and engineering
viewed as the province of men; and child-care and hairdressing viewed
as the province of women (Leathwood, 2006). College majors reect
this gendering of academic interests, as well: Women are overrepresented in the humanities, some social sciences (especially psychology),
and biology; men are overrepresented in the physical and computer
sciences, business, and engineering.
Gender messages also permeate the classroom experience of girls
and boys through the interactions they have with their peers and teachers. Peer inuence is a particularly powerful barrier to gender equity in
education. Girls and boys typically grow up in different cultures,
with different language and play styles, different clothes and customs,
and different expectations from adults (Basow, 2008; Leaper & Friedman, 2007; Maccoby, 1998). For example, girls activities, such as playing house, tend to be cooperative and verbal. Boys activities and
games tend to be more competitive and physical. Because of these gendered play styles, girls and boys tend to segregate themselves into
same-gender groups, which further reinforce gendered patterns of play
and behavior.
By ignoring or discouraging play with a child of the other sex, peers
are major enforcers of gendered norms (Bigler & Liben, 2007; Koch,
2003). Peers often police gender-boundary violations by using derogatory language and harassing behaviors toward those who do not completely conform to gender expectations (Basow, 2008). For example,
boys who might be interested in the arts, or girls who might be interested in mechanics, typically get a clear message that their interests are
not gender appropriate. Because boys have a stricter and narrower gender role than girls, boys are more often punished by their male peers
via verbal denigration, physical assault, and social ostracism (Renold,
2006; Wolke, Woods, Stanford, & Schulz, 2001). In a 2001 survey on
sexual harassment conducted by the American Association of University Women (AAUW, 2001), 83 percent of boys reported having experienced unwanted sexual behaviors (including accusations of
homosexuality) during their school years, usually from male peers.
Girls too are pressured to conform to the feminine gender role by
their peers, but because their role is more exible than is boys in the
early grades in developed countries (e.g., girls can wear dresses or
pants, can be athletic or bookish, can be assertive or dependent), the

Women in Education: Students and Professors Worldwide

49

conformity pressures on girls are somewhat less intense than for boys,
at least until puberty (Basow, 2008). It is around this time that the
major imperative for girls in developed countries, to be sexually attractive to boys, intensies. Girls uninterested in boys do risk being called
queer or dyke, but most of the epithets used for girls refer to heterosexual promiscuity (slut, ho). In the 2001 AAUW sexual harassment survey, 88 percent of the girls reported having experienced
such behaviors during their school years, mainly from male peers
(AAUW, 2001). Other research supports these ndings and suggests
that sexual harassment is virtually a universal experience for adolescent girls in coeducational institutions, in the United States and elsewhere (Leach, Fiscian, & Kadzamira, 2003; Leaper & Brown, 2008). A
majority of middle- and high school girls report having been the brunt
of unwanted or inappropriate romantic attention from a boy, having
been the target of unwanted physical contact from a boy, having been
called demeaning names or told an embarrassing or mean joke about
being a girl, as well as being teased about their appearance. Furthermore, at least one in four girls in the United States report having been
teased, bullied, or threatened by a boy (Leaper & Brown, 2008). European American and African American girls report more unwanted sexual attention than do their Asian American and Latina counterparts in
the United States; still, nearly all girls experience at least some of these
harassing behaviors (Leaper & Brown, 2008). In addition to sexual harassment, about 30 percent of girls report having received some type of
academic discouragement about their math, science, or computing abilities from male peers (Leaper & Brown, 2008).
The results of peer sexual harassment are negative for both sexes,
although the negative effects are more intense and pervasive for girls
(AAUW, 2001; Ormerod, Collinsworth, & Perry, 2008). Girls who experience peer sexual harassment, compared to their male counterparts,
report feeling more self-conscious, less condent, and more negative
about their bodies and about school. Girls also are more likely to try to
avoid the harasser and to keep silent in class. Thus, peer harassment is
a major barrier to gender equity in education.
Unfortunately, teacher behaviors often create additional inequities for
girls and women in schools. In some cases, the inequities are quite
overt, such as teachers who either harass female students themselves
or who tolerate peer harassment. In a study at several high schools in
the United States, about half of all school personnel (53 percent) were
found to have sexually harassed female students themselves (Ormerod
et al., 2008). Students in general tended to perceive school personnel as
tolerating such behaviors. Consequently, most female students do not
report incidences of sexual harassment; instead, they are likely to experience lowered self-esteem and withdraw psychologically or physically
from school. In some cultures, there may not even be recognition that

50

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

sexual harassment by teachers or professors is inappropriate and/or


should be punished. For example, in countries with a collectivist orientation, group harmony may be more highly valued than individual
rights. In a cross-cultural study of university students, Sigal and colleagues (2005) found that a professor who had been described in a
written scenario as harassing his female graduate student, was viewed
as less responsible and less guilty of sexual harassment in countries
with a more collectivist orientation (Ecuador, Pakistan, the Philippines,
Taiwan, and Turkey) compared to those with a more individualist orientation (United States, Canada, Germany, and the Netherlands). In all
countries, however, female students were more likely than their male
counterparts to nd the professor guilty.
Teachers also treat their male and female students differently, mainly
by paying more attention to boys than to girls (Duffy, Warren, & Walsh,
2001; Harris, 1997; Meece & Scantlebury, 2006; Sadker & Sadker, 1994).
Teachers greater attention to male students occurs worldwide (Liu,
2006). Although some of this attention is negative (i.e., teachers both
praise and reprimand boys more than they do girls), the result is that
boys tend to dominate classroom interactions. Girls tend to receive
teacher approval mainly for being quiet and compliant. In an observational study of more than 100 fourth-, sixth-, and eighth-grade classrooms
in several communities in the United States, Myra and David Sadker
(1994) found that teachers called on and encouraged girls less often than
they did boys, although they rarely were aware of doing so. This is particularly true in math and science, subject areas gender-typed as masculine (Altermatt, Jovanovic, & Perry, 1998; Jovanovic & King, 1998). Girls
not only do not receive the encouragement given their male peers, but
they may actually receive discouraging comments from teachers related
to their math, science, or computer abilities, perhaps because teachers
tend to believe that boys are more gifted in math and science than are
girls (Ceci & Williams, 2007; Leaper & Brown, 2008; Liu, 2006). Such discouragement is particularly unfortunate since the strongest predictor of
attitudes toward science among high school students is social encouragements from teachers, parents, and peers (Stake, 2006). The lack of social
encouragement may contribute to womens underrepresentation in
math- and science-related careers.
It should be noted that student race and ethnicity also affect teachers gender-related behaviors. The nding that teachers give more
encouragement to male than female students appears predominantly
with White students. Black boys, in particular, appear to be viewed by
teachers in the United States and United Kingdom as having the least
academic potential when compared with White boys and to girls of
both races (Ross & Jackson, 1991; Ward & Robinson-Wood, 2006;
Wood, Kaplan, & McLoyd, 2007). Perhaps this explains the academic
disengagement that occurs among Black boys, who drop out of high

Women in Education: Students and Professors Worldwide

51

school at a higher rate (9.7 percent in the United States) than do White
boys (6.4 percent). Hispanic boys may be even more neglected by
teachers; their dropout rate is 25.7 percent (NCES, 2008). Black girls
also may become disengaged due to lack of teacher attention and discouragement of their typically more verbal and active behaviors (Wilkinson & Marrett, 1985). In 2006, the high school dropout rate was 5.3
percent for White girls, 11.7 percent for Black girls, and 18.1 percent
for Hispanic girls (NCES, 2008). Research conrms that teachers
expectations strongly affect students, especially students from groups
that are academically stigmatized (Blacks and Hispanics, in general,
and girls in math) (McKown & Weinstein, 2002).
On the college level, similar patterns are found. Women college students appear to receive less encouragement than men do for speaking
in class, and they are more likely to be ignored or interrupted by both
peers and professors (Sadker & Sadker, 1994; Whitt, Edison, Pascarella,
Nora, & Terenzini, 1999). The qualities that seem to facilitate more gender equity in the college classroom include a smaller class size, more
feminine-oriented or androgynous subject matter, more classroom
interactions, a more cooperative than competitive atmosphere, and a
more gender-balanced or female-dominant student gender ratio (Brady &
Eisler, 1999).
EFFECTS OF DIFFERENTIAL SCHOOL EXPERIENCES
The barriers to gender equality in education are numerous: different
social roles, gender (and racial) stereotyping of academic abilities, a
male-centered curriculum, peer harassment, and differential teacher
treatment. As a result, girls and boys typically develop different levels
of academic self-condence (Dweck, 1999). Boys in the United States, at
least European American boys, tend to believe in their ability to solve
problems, so they typically attempt and persist at challenging tasks.
Girls are less likely to believe in their ability to gure things out;
instead, girls tend to believe that they either have academic ability or
they do not have it. Thus girls, especially high-achieving ones, tend to
develop more feelings of helplessness when they encounter academic
difculties, and they give up more quickly than do their male counterparts. Girls often feel more valued for being good and for their
appearance than for their intellectual skill. Academic self-condence is
important when attempting and persisting at new tasks. It is in this
area that girls increasingly fall behind boys, even as girls generally continue to attain higher grades. This occurs on the college level as well.
Annual surveys of entering students show that nearly two out of three
men rate themselves as at least above average in academic self-condence, whereas less than one-half of the women make similar ratings
(Sax, 2007).

52

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

The gender stereotyping of academic elds by society in general and


teachers in particular may also contribute to differential student condence and achievement. As previously noted, math and science are
perceived as masculine, whereas literature, languages, and the arts
are perceived as feminine. Because people expect to succeed in tasks
that they view as gender appropriate, girls tend to have lower feelings
of self-efcacy in math and science elds than do boys (Dweck, 2007;
Eccles et al., 1990; Sax, 2007). Low expectations of success can lead girls
to become uninterested in those elds and the occupations related to
them. The same is true regarding boys and the humanities. Furthermore, when such gender stereotypes are made salient (e.g., by a
teacher remarking that boys typically are better at math than are girls),
students actual performance can be affected. As numerous studies
have conrmed, when a students gender identity is primed, such as
when she has to write down (or check a box) regarding her gender
before taking a test, her academic performance on a masculine gendertyped task can suffer (Steele, 1997; Steele & Ambady, 2006). For example, female students who are identied as such or who are a numeric
minority in a testing situation perform more poorly on a math test than
male students and than a matched female control group whose gender
identity has not been primed (Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev, 2000). Just taking a
math test in a classroom with boys may elicit girls performance decrement, as found in a study of French middle-school students (Huguet &
Regner, 2007). This stereotype threat phenomenon is robust and occurs
for any group performing a stereotype-related task when a stigmatized
identity is made salient. For example, when Asian American womens
gender identity was made salient, their math performance decreased
relative to a control condition, but when their Asian identity was made
salient, their math scores increased because of the stereotype that
Asians are good at math (Shih, Pittinsky, & Ambady, 1999).
Thus, academic performance and interest can be boosted or
impaired depending upon the stereotyped nature of the task and the
identity that is made salient, and this seems especially likely to occur
for those individuals who identify most strongly with the group in
question (Chavous, Harris, Rivas, Helaire, & Green, 2004; Oswald,
2008; Steele, 1997). In this respect, teachers expectancies and transmittal of academic achievement stereotypes can become prophecies that
students fulll. For example, the stereotype that African Americans are
not as academically competent as European Americans can negatively
affect the academic performance of the former group and can even lead
to disidentication and disengagement with academic achievement in
general (Steele, 1997). The phenomenon of stereotype threat may contribute to the higher high school dropout and lower college attendance
rates of African Americans and Hispanics compared to European
Americans.

Women in Education: Students and Professors Worldwide

53

As noted in the beginning of this chapter, womens education is


critical for the welfare of the woman and her family, as well as her
country. Because of its importance, the elimination of sex discrimination in education and training was a key component of the Beijing Declaration and Platform for Action adopted at the Fourth World
Conference on Women that took place in 1995 (United Nations Division for the Advancement of Women, 2007). It remains a central
focus of the United Nations Division for the Advancement of Women
(WomenWatch, 2008).
WOMEN AS TEACHERS AND PROFESSORS
Women in front of the classroom also are affected by gender factors
in terms of their representation, their teaching styles, and the reactions
of their students, colleagues, and supervisors.
Representation
In most countries, female teachers are overrepresented in the lower
grades and underrepresented in the higher grades (Gaskell & Mullen,
2006). As with most job categories, the percentage of women decreases as
the status and pay of a eld increases. The teaching jobs with the least status and lowest pay are those in early childhood education, and nine out
of ten of these jobs are held by women (NCES, n.d.). The teaching jobs
with the most status and pay are those in the upper ranks of university
teaching; men disproportionately hold these jobs (75 percent of U.S. full
professors were men in 2007) (NCES, 2008). Jobs in educational administration (principals, superintendents, provosts, university presidents)
reect the same gender hierarchy. In 20032004, women were 56 percent
of U.S. elementary school principals (despite the fact that women constitute 91 percent of the teachers) and 26 percent of secondary school principals (despite the fact that women constitute 55 percent of teachers at this
level) (NCES, 2007). At U.S. colleges and universities, women were only
23 percent of college presidents in 2006, despite the fact that they constitute 41 percent of the professorate (King & Gomez, 2007). Women were
most likely to head 2-year colleges and least likely to head doctorategranting universities (King & Gomez, 2007).
Gender segregation also occurs in terms of eld of study, at least in
most developed countries (Charles & Bradley, 2002). Not only are
women more likely than men to be associated with early childhood
education, but women also are more likely than men to be associated
with the humanities and health elds. In contrast, men are overrepresented in engineering, mathematics, computer science and, to a lesser
degree, natural science. There is approximate gender parity in the
social sciences.

54

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

There has been change over time in the United States; increased
numbers of women have joined educational administration, the professorate, and science elds. For example, in the decade from 1993 to
2003, the percentage of women principals in elementary and secondary
schools increased 10 percent, and the percentage of women teaching in
colleges and universities increased 6 percent (NCES, 2008). These numbers still fall short of gender parity, however. Furthermore, gains by
women in traditional male-dominated careers have not been matched
by increased numbers of men in traditional female-dominated careers,
such as early childhood education and the humanities. Thus, the overall pattern is one of many changes for women in terms of nontraditional career choices but few changes for men. Gender segregation in
eld of study and careers may be particularly difcult to change
because it appears to be based less on beliefs about gender equality
than on beliefs about innate gender differences in abilities and interests
(Charles & Bradley, 2002).
Another barrier to equal representation by women and men in education is the same one affecting women in nearly all occupations: balancing work/family responsibilities (Gambles, Lewis, & Rapoport, 2006;
Ward & Wolf-Wendel, 2004). Because women still are primarily responsible for child-care, employed women with children often are more limited than their male counterparts in terms of time, energy, and overall
freedom to pursue their careers. For example, a recent study of female
Harvard University alumnae who had gone on to attain a doctorate
found that 91.5 percent of those without children were employed fulltime 15 years after college graduation, compared to 65 percent of
those with one child and 57.5 percent of those with two or more children (Jaschik, 2009). Having children did not affect the employment
rates of their male counterparts. Indeed, one of the few careers open to
educated women for many years was grade-school teaching, since such
jobs were fairly compatible with caring for ones own children in terms
of hours. (That is, teachers work pretty much the same hours and number of days that their children are in school.) In higher education, however, time demands are much greater (if somewhat exible), especially
when research and publications are required. Not surprisingly, then,
women are over-represented in more teaching-oriented higher education
positions, while men are overrepresented at major research universities
(American Association of University Professors, 2008). In fact, both
women and men considering academic careers view research universities as not family friendly (June, 2009).
Gendered Teaching Styles
Not only are women and men frequently teaching in different grades,
types of schools, and disciplines, but gender also may affect teaching

Women in Education: Students and Professors Worldwide

55

styles, perhaps in interaction with discipline. Research has found that


men are more likely than women to use a lecture-based teaching style,
whereas women are more likely than men to use a more discussionbased teaching style (Basow & Montgomery, 2005; Brady & Eisler, 1999;
Canada & Pringle, 1995). Some of this apparent gender difference in
teaching style may be due to the subject matter being taught. A lecturebased style may suit the disciplines in which men are overrepresented,
such as the sciences, better than a discussion-based style. Similarly, a
discussion-based style may be more appropriate than a lecture-based
style in disciplines where women have greater representation, such as
the humanities. Still, even when faculty members are matched in terms
of rank and discipline, female faculty still are found to be more studentoriented and to engage students more in discussions than their male
counterparts (Statham, Richardson, & Cook, 1991). In contrast, male faculty appear more likely than female faculty to assert their authority in
the classroom through public reprimands and corrections.
Different teaching styles may appeal to different students. There is
some evidence that female students may particularly appreciate female
professors, as they are more likely than male students to nominate a
female instructor as their best teacher (Basow, Phelan, & Capotosto,
2006), and they frequently rate female professors higher than male
students do on evaluation forms (Basow, 1998). Traditional pedagogical
styles appear to benet male students more than female students
(Gabriel & Smithson, 1990), whereas female friendly styles (such as
those involving more cooperation than competition) benet both sexes
(Johnson, Kahle, & Fargo, 2007; Rosser, 1997). Thus, the increasing
number of women on college faculties may be helping to increase gender equity in college classrooms, and may partially account for the
increased retention of female students in higher education.
Although female faculty seem to be particularly appreciated by
female college students, such is not the case with male college students. The latter are less likely to cite a female faculty member as their
best instructor than would be expected based on the number of female
professors they have had, and they frequently rate female faculty lower
than female students do and lower than they rate male faculty (Basow,
1998; Basow et al., 2006). Given the nature of gender roles (men have
more status than women and masculinity is dened, at least in part, as
not-feminine [Basow, 1992, 2008; Brannon, 1976]), perhaps it is not
surprising that male college students value male faculty more than
female faculty. A similar process is at work with respect to other cultural gures: Boys are less likely than girls to read books or watch television shows or movies with predominantly female main characters,
and they are less likely to cite a female role model as inuential. Girls,
in contrast, watch, read about, and choose role models of both sexes
(Basow, 1992, 2008; Basow & Howe, 1980).

56

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

GENDER BIAS IN EVALUATING WOMEN PROFESSORS


As indicated earlier, female faculty often are perceived and reacted
to differently by their male and female students, whereas male faculty
tend to be perceived and reacted to similarly. Women are marked for
gender in ways men are not, especially when women are in nontraditional roles (such as when teaching on the college level, especially
in such male-associated elds as science and technology). The normative professor is still a man. When a woman is in this role, students
(and others) often note this unusual event by speaking of their female
professor; when their professor is a man, they rarely note his gender.
This marking of gender for women often creates a double bind for
women in higher education: she must meet the expectations both of
being a good professor (e.g., knowledgeable, competent) as well as a
good woman (e.g., warm and nurturant). The overlap between the
stereotypic gender traits for women and the stereotypic professor role
is slight. Thus, female professors walk a narrow line: needing to demonstrate both masculine traits of agency and competence as well as
feminine traits of warmth and compassion. Male professors do not
experience the same double bind since their gender and professional
roles overlap considerably.
These dual (and generally incompatible) sets of expectations for
female professors exemplify role incongruity and can result in biased
ratings by others (Eagly & Karau, 2002). Such gender bias can be discerned in the ways students perceive and evaluate female professors.
While students may appreciate warmth and nurturance in male professors (e.g., Basow, 2000; Basow et al., 2006; Freeman, 1994), men can still
receive high student ratings even if they do not demonstrate such
traits. But for female professors, such traits are required. For example,
research in the United States has found that to receive comparable ratings, it is more important for female professors to be friendly (to smile
and be available) than male professors (Bennett, 1982; Kierstead,
DAgostino, & Dill, 1988). If female professors are not perceived as
warmer/friendlier than their male counterparts, women receive lower
ratings, especially from their male students (Sinclair & Kunda, 2000).
In general, female professors generally do receive higher ratings from
both male and female students on questions relating to facultystudent
interactions and quality, but their overall ratings tend to be similar to
male professors (Bachen, McLoughlin, & Garcia, 1999; Basow et al.,
2006; Basow & Montgomery, 2005; Bennett, 1982).
With respect to grade school and high school, some concern has
been expressed recently that boys poorer educational performance relative to their female peers may be due to the fact that most of their
teachers are women (e.g., Gurian & Henley, 2001; Gurian & Stevens,
2005; Sax, 2005). However, research with nearly 1,000 Australian

Women in Education: Students and Professors Worldwide

57

students aged 12 to 16 years old did not show that boys beneted more
from male than from female teachers (Marsh, Martin, & Cheng, 2008).
In fact, both boys and girls typically had higher self-efcacy in classes
taught by women. Other research also has failed to show a signicant
effect of male teachers on boys behavior or achievement (e.g., Bricheno &
Thornton, 2007, in the United Kingdom), although more research is
needed on this topic, especially in elementary school, where female
teachers typically outnumber male teachers 9:1 (NCES, n.d.).
In summary, women in education are typically overrepresented in
the lower grades and lower-status teaching positions and institutions,
and underrepresented in the highest status positions and institutions.
Within the classroom they often teach in different elds and appear to
be more student-oriented than their male peers. While male professors
are generally perceived and evaluated similarly by their male and
female students, female professors typically receive different ratings as
a function of student gender (and discipline). In general, male students
tend to rate their female professors lower than their male professors,
while female students sometimes do the reverse: rate their female professors higher than their male professors (and higher than their male
peers).

CONCLUSION
In this chapter, we have examined the status of women in education.
As students, girls and women are disadvantaged in several ways. For
example, in developing countries, girls often are not encouraged to
attend school. In nearly all countries, girls are viewed in stereotypic
ways that may restrict and/or shape their educational and occupational potential. Girls are likely to experience both a gendered curriculum as well as sexist treatment from their male peers as well as from
their teachers. These educational experiences may cause girls and
women to doubt their academic abilities and impair their academic
performance, especially in nontraditional elds. Although women in
developed countries currently are matching or surpassing their male
counterparts in terms of educational achievement, their predominant
elds of study still are gendered.
As teachers, women are overrepresented in lower grades and lower
status educational positions, and underrepresented in the higher status
ones. In colleges and universities, women are more likely to use student-focused methods (e.g., discussions, group work) than their male
peers, while their teaching prociency may be more heavily scrutinized. Overall, women professors are marked for gender in ways their
male counterparts are not, a fact that contributes to the heavier burden
they bear to prove their worth.

58

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

REFERENCES
Altermatt, E. R., Jovanovic, J., & Perry, M. (1998). Bias or responsivity? Sex and
achievement-level effects on teachers classroom questioning practices.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 516527.
American Association of University Professors [AAUP]. (2008). 20062007
Report on the economic status of the profession, Table 12. Retrieved January 31,
2009, from http://www.aaup.org/AAUP/comm/rep/Z/ecstatreport200607/TOC.htm.
American Association of University Women [AAUW]. (2001). Hostile hallways:
Bullying, teasing, and sexual harassment in schools. Washington, DC: Author.
Bachen, C. M., McLoughlin, M. M., & Garcia, S. S. (1999). Assessing the role of
gender in college students evaluations of faculty. Communication Education,
48, 193210.
Basow, S. A. (1992). Gender: Stereotypes and roles (3rd ed.). Pacic Grove, CA:
Thomson Learning.
Basow, S. A. (1998). Student evaluations: The role of gender bias and teaching
styles. In L. H. Collins, J. C. Chrisler, & K. Quina (Eds.), Arming Athena:
Career strategies for women in academe (pp. 135156). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Basow, S. A. (2008). Gender socialization: Or, how long a way has baby come?
In J. C. Chrisler, C. Golden, & P. D. Rozee (Eds.), Lectures on the psychology
of women (4th ed., pp. 8095). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Basow, S. A., & Howe, K. G. (1980). Role model inuence: Effects of sex and
sex-role attitudes in college students. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 4,
558572.
Basow, S. A., & Montgomery, S. (2005). Student evaluations of professors and
professor self-ratings: Gender and divisional patterns. Journal of Personnel
Evaluation in Education, 18, 91106.
Basow, S. A., Phelan, J., & Capotosto, L. (2006). Gender patterns in college students choices of their best and worst professors. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 30, 2535.
Bennett, S. K (1982). Student perceptions of and expectations for male and
female instructors: Evidence relating to the question of gender bias in
teaching evaluation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 170179.
Berenbaum, S. A., & Resnick, S. (2007). The seeds of career choices: Prenatal sex
hormone effects on psychological sex differences. In S. J. Ceci & W. M. Williams (Eds.), Why arent more women in science: Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 147157). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental inter-group theory: Explaining and reducing childrens social stereotyping and prejudice. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 162166.
Brady, K. L., & Eisler, R. M. (1999). Sex and gender in the college classroom: A
quantitative analysis of faculty-student interactions and perceptions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 127145.
Brannon, R. (1976). The male sex role: Our cultures blueprint for manhood,
and what its done for us lately. In D. David & R. Brannon (Eds.), The
forty-nine percent majority (pp. 148). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Bricheno, P., & Thornton, M. (2007). Role model, hero, or champion? Childrens
views concerning role models. Educational Research, 49, 383396.

Women in Education: Students and Professors Worldwide

59

Canada, K., & Pringle, R. (1995). The role of gender in college classroom interactions: A social context approach. Sociology of Education, 68, 161186.
Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (Eds.). (2007). Why arent more women in science?
Top researchers debate the evidence. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Charles, M., & Bradley, K. (2002). Equal but separate: A cross-national study of
sex segregation in higher education. American Sociological Review, 67, 573599
Chavous, T. M., Harris, A., Rivas, D., Helaire, L., & Green, L. (2004). Racial
stereotypes and gender in context: African Americans at predominantly
Black and predominantly White colleges. Sex Roles, 51, 116.
Diekman, A. B., & Murnen, S. K. (2004). Learning to be little women and little
men: The inequitable gender equality of nonsexist childrens literature. Sex
Roles, 50, 373385.
Duffy, J., Warren, K., & Walsh, M. (2001). Classroom interactions: Gender of
teacher, gender of student, and classroom subject. Sex Roles, 45, 579593.
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Dweck, C. S. (2007). Is math a gift? Beliefs that put females at risk. In S. J. Ceci &
W. M. Williams (Eds.), Why arent more women in science? Top researchers
debate the evidence (pp. 4755). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward
female leaders. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 233256.
Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 46, 735754.
Eccles, J., Jacobs, J., & Harold, R. (1990). Gender role stereotypes, expectancy
effects, and parents socialization of gender differences. Journal of Social
Issues, 46, 183201.
Freeman, H. R. (1994). Student evaluations of college instructors: Effects of type
of course taught, instructor gender and gender role, and student gender.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 627630.
Gabriel, S. L., & Smithson, I. (Eds.) (1990). Gender in the classroom: Power and
pedagogy. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Gambles, R., Lewis, S., & Rapoport, R. (2006). The myth of work-life balance: The
challenge of our time for men, women and societies. New York: Wiley.
Gaskell, J., & Mullen, A. L. (2006). Women in teaching: Participation, power
and possibility. In C. Skelton, B. Francis, & L. Smulyan (Eds.), The Sage
handbook of gender and education (pp. 453468). London: Sage.
Gooden, A. M., & Gooden, M. A. (2001). Gender representation in notable
childrens picture books: 19951999. Sex Roles, 45, 89101.
Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture, gender, and
math. Science, 320, 11641165.
Gur, R. C., & Gur, R. E. (2007). Neural substrates for sex differences in cognition. In S. J. Ceci & W. M. Williams (Eds.), Why arent more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 189198). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Gurian, M., & Henley, P. [with Trueman, T.]. (2001). Boys and girls learn differently! A guide for teachers and parents. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

60

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Gurian, M., & Stevens, K. (2005). The minds of boys: Saving our sons from falling
behind in school and life. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Haier, R. J. (2007). Brains, bias, and biology: Follow the data. In S. J. Ceci & W. M.
Williams (Eds.), Why arent more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 113119). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Harris, L. (1997). The Metropolitan Life survey of the American teacher 1997: Examining gender issues in public schools. New York: Louis Harris and Associates.
Hines, M. (2004). Brain gender. New York: Oxford University Press.
Huguet, P., & Regner, I. (2007). Stereotype threat among schoolgirls in quasi-ordinary classroom circumstances. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 545560.
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60,
581592.
Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. A. (2000). A threatening intellectual environment:
Why females are susceptible to experiencing problem-solving decits in
the presence of males. Psychological Science, 11, 365371.
Jaschik, S. (2009). New questions on women, academe and careers. Inside Higher
Ed. Retrieved January 31, 2009, from http://www.insidehighered.com/
news/2008/09/22/women.
Johnson, C. C., Kahle, J. B., & Fargo, J. D. (2007). Effective teaching results in
increased science achievement for all students. Science Education, 91, 371383.
Jovanovic, J., & King, S. S. (1998). Boys and girls in the performance-based science classroom: Whos doing the performing? American Educational Research
Journal, 35, 477496.
June, A. W. (2009, January 23). Students think twice about jobs in academe. The
Chronicle of Higher Education. Retrieved January 31, 2009, from http://
chronicle.com/weekly/v55/i20/20a00102.htm.
Kierstead, D., DAgostino, P. D., & Dill, H. (1988). Sex role stereotyping of college professors: Bias in students ratings of instructors. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 342344.
Kimura, D. (1999). Sex and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute
of Technology Press.
King, J., & Gomez, G. (2007). The American college president: 2007 edition.
Washington, DC: American Council on Education.
Koch, J. (2003). Gender issues in the classroom. In W. M. Reynolds & G. E.
Miller (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Educational psychology (Vol. 7, pp.
259281). New York: Wiley.
Leach, F., Fiscian, V., & Kadzamira, E. (2003). An investigative study of the abuse of
girls in African schools. London: Department for International Development.
Leaper, C., & Brown, C. S. (2008). Perceived experiences with sexism among
adolescent girls. Child Development, 79, 685704.
Leaper, C., & Friedman, C. K. (2007). The socialization of gender. In J. E.
Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research
(pp. 561587). New York: Guilford.
Leathwood, C. (2006). Gender equity in post-secondary education. In C. Skelton, B. Francis, & L. Smulyan (Eds.), The Sage handbook of gender and education (pp. 166178). London: Sage.
Lips, H. (2008). Sex & gender: An introduction (6th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Liu, F. (2006). School culture and gender. In C. Skelton, B. Francis, & L.
Smulyan (Eds.), The Sage handbook of gender and education (pp. 425438).
London: Sage.

Women in Education: Students and Professors Worldwide

61

Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together.


Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Marsh, H. W., Martin, A. J., & Cheng, J. H. S. (2008). A multilevel perspective
on gender in classroom motivation and climate: Potential benets of male
teachers for boys? Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 7895.
McKown, C., & Weinstein, R. S. (2002). Modeling the role of child ethnicity
and gender in childrens differential response to teacher expectations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 159184.
Meece, J. L., & Scantlebury, K. (2006). Gender and schooling: Progress and persistent barriers. New York: Oxford University Press.
National Center for Education Statistics [NCES]. (n.d.). Special analysis 2005:
Mobility in the teacher workforce. Retrieved February 1, 2009, from http://
nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/2005/analysis/index.asp.
National Center for Education Statistics [NCES]. (2005). Projection of education
statistics to 2014. Retrieved February 1, 2009, from http://nces.ed.gov/
programs/projections/projections2014/sec_4b.asp.
National Center for Education Statistics [NCES]. (2007). Contexts of elementary
and secondary education: School characteristics and climate. Retrieved February
1, 2009, from http://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/2007/section4/indicator34.asp.
National Center for Education Statistics [NCES]. (2008). Digest of education
statistics: 2007. Retrieved February 1, 2009, from http://nces.ed.gov/
programs/digest/d07/.
Nosek, B., Banaji, M., & Greenwald, A. (2002). Math = male, me = female,
therefore math not-equal-to me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
83, 4459.
Ormerod, A. J., Collinsworth, L. L., & Perry, L. A. (2008). Critical climate: Relations among sexual harassment, climate, and outcomes for high school
girls and boys. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 113125.
Oswald, D. L. (2008). Gender stereotypes and womens reports of liking and
ability in traditionally masculine and feminine occupations. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 32, 196203.
Renold, E. (2006). Gendered classroom experiences. In C. Skelton, B. Francis, &
L. Smulyan (Eds.), The Sage handbook of gender and education (pp. 439452).
London: Sage.
Ross, S. I., & Jackson, J. M. (1991). Teachers expectations for Black males and
Black females academic achievement. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 17, 7882.
Rosser, S. V. (1997). Re-engineering female friendly science. New York: Teachers
College Press.
Sadker, M., & Sadker, D. (1994). Failing at fairness: How our schools cheat girls.
New York: Scribner.
Sax, L. (2005). Why gender matters: What parents and teachers need to know about
the emerging science of sex differences. New York: Doubleday.
Sax, L. J. (2007, September 28). College women still face many obstacles in
reaching their full potential. Chronicle of Higher Education, p. B46.
Shih, M., Pittinsky, T. L., & Ambady, N. (1999). Stereotype susceptibility: Identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychological Science,
10, 8083.
Sigal, J., Gibbs, M. S., Goodrich, C., Rashid, T., Anjum, A., Hsu, D., Perrino, C.
S., et al. (2005). Cross-cultural reactions to academic sexual harassment:

62

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Effects of individualist vs. collectivist culture and gender of participants.


Sex Roles, 52, 201215.
Sinclair, L., & Kunda, Z. (2000). Motivated stereotyping of women: Shes ne if
she praised me but incompetent if she criticized me. Personality & Social
Psychology Bulletin, 26, 13291342.
Skelton, C., Francis, B., & Smulyan, L. (Eds.) (2006). The Sage handbook of gender
and education. London: Sage.
Sohoni, N. K. (1995). The burden of girlhood: A global inquiry into the status of
girls. Oakland, CA: Third Party Publishing.
Stake, J. E. (2006). The critical mediating role of social encouragement for science motivation and condence among high school girls and boys. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 36, 10171045.
Statham, A., Richardson, L., & Cook, J. (1991). Gender and university teaching: A
negotiated difference. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual
identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613629.
Steele, J. R., & Ambady, N. (2006). Math is hard! The effect of gender priming on womens attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42,
428436.
United Nations Population Fund [UNFPA]. (2005). State of world population
2005: Gender equality and education. Retrieved January 2, 2009, from http://
www.unfpa.org/swp/2005/presskit/factsheets/facts_gender.htm.
United Nations Division for the Advancement of Women [UNDAW]. (2007).
United Nations Fourth World Conference on Women. Retrieved January 10,
2009, from http://www.un.org/womenwatch/daw/beijing/index.html.
Ward, J. V. & Robinson-Wood, T. L. (2006). Room at the table: Racial and gendered realities in the schooling of black children. In C. Skelton, B. Francis, &
L. Smulyan (Eds.), The Sage handbook of gender and education (pp.
325338). London: Sage.
Ward, K., & Wolf-Wendel, L. (2004). Academic motherhood: Managing complex roles in research universities. Review of Higher Education, 27, 233257.
Whitt, E. J., Edison, M. I., Pascarella, E. T., Nora, A., & Terenzini, P. T. (1999).
Womens perceptions of a chilly climate and cognitive outcomes in college: Additional evidence. Journal of College Student Development, 40,
163177.
Wilkinson, L. C., & Marrett, C. B. (Eds.) (1985). Gender inuences in classroom
interaction. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1990). Sex and psyche: Gender and self viewed crossculturally. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Wolke, D., Woods, S., Stanford, K., & Schulz, H. (2001). Bullying and victimization of primary school children in England and Germany: Prevalence and
school factors. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 673696.
WomenWatch. (2008). Directory of UN resources on the education and training
of women. Retrieved January 10, 2009, from http://www.un.org/women
watch/directory/education_and_training_of_women_3002.htm.
Wood, D., Kaplan, R., & McLoyd, V. C. (2007). Gender differences in the educational expectations of urban, low-income African American youth: The
role of parents and the school. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 417427.

Chapter 4

In Womens Voices
Samantha Smith

The feminist movement has provided the opportunity for women to


have more choices in their lives. Throughout history, women as well as
men have fought to gain equal rights for women in regard to education, employment, enfranchisement, and relationships. I feel fortunate
that much of the hard work of this movement was completed before
my generation came of age. The benets that I enjoy include applying
to and attending the college of my choice, continuing my education
through graduate studies, and pursuing any career that I wish. So
many important strides have been made for women over the past centuries. Due to some of my own experiences as a young adult, I wonder
about the future of feminism and what aspects of life feminism will
impact.
One of my goals always been to be a wife and mother. I never
thought that this was a lowly goal, nor did I think that other women
might consider it to be so. An early experience in college made me
rethink feminism, as well as the role of a woman in the twenty-rst
century. During a lecture, one of my female professors commented
that no modern woman aspires to being only a homemaker. From this
I inferred that, if you did aspire to homemaking, you would not be
expected to be in her class or to ever attend college. Her comment
made me feel nave, but it also surprised me. I grew up in a household
in which my mother was a homemaker with a masters degree,
and my father had a doctorate. I was raised with the belief that one
should pursue education for its own sake, and that one had the choice
to use education as one desired. Whether that meant as a homemaker/
mother or as a career woman was ones own choice. After years of
discussions with other young women, I realize that many feel that opting for motherhood over a career is seen as a step backward. It is as

64

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

though all that feminism achieved is being undone by this choice, and
that women who only aspire to motherhood are not modern, progressive women. There are other women, of course, on the opposite side of
this argument who feel, as I do, that women should have the choice to
pursue whatever avenue they want.
As I came to the end of my undergraduate studies, my goals
included attending graduate school and pursuing a career in addition
to being a mother. After I complete my studies, I will be confronted
with the dilemma that so many women face. I will be married, starting
a career, and deciding when to start a family. This leads to the question of whether I am going to continue working while raising my children or whether I am going to put my career on hold and be a stay-athome mother while my children are young. Despite the strides that
feminism has made, many women feel the challenge of simultaneously
pursuing a career and raising a family. Feminism has given women the
right to choose, but not all aspects of society have changed to allow
women to follow through with their choices. It is helpful that more
men today are open-minded and are being brought up with the idea
that women are equals and that various domestic duties should be
shared, but I do not feel that this is yet the norm. The feminist movement provided women with more choices, but as a result, many
women feel pressure to perform at the highest level both inside and
outside the home.
In my opinion, feminism still has many areas in which progress is
necessary. Feminism is about helping and supporting each other and
not about discouraging women or making them feel that any of their
life choices are less valuable than others. Some of the issues that need
to be addressed include helping women to balance career and family
and helping them to achieve equal pay and promotion in the workplace. These issues may be advanced through government programs,
such as mandatory daycare in ones place of employment and family
leave. Although society and the government have given women more
rights and choices, a support system must be established so that
women can utilize all of their choices and be free to pursue their goals.

Chapter 5

Working Life as a House: A Tale of


Floors, Walls, and Ceilings
Leanne Faraday-Brash

It may be embarrassing to admit, but I felt validated when I read a broadsheet book review about a quaint little paperback titled Why Women
Shop (Minahan & Beverland, 2005). The short answer to the question of
why women (or men) shop would surely be: to buy something. Undoubtedly, some people would draw the analogous (and simplistic) conclusion
to the question of why women work outside the home. Their response to
that may be: to earn something. And yet just as I learned (and how normalizing it was!) that women shop for a whole raft of reasons that include
the feel-good benet of retail therapy, so do we generate a plethora of
explanations as to why women (choose to) engage in paid work and
begin to contemplate an onslaught of potential barriers, threats, and challenges they meet along the way. Of course with various motives to work
outside the home, it is humbling and important to acknowledge that not
all women believe they have the luxury of a choice to work, or a particular role to accept or decline. Some women may not ever believe they are
afforded the opportunity to set their sights on something as lofty and selfactualizing as a career. However, cant they still boast the right to enjoy
that which may consume their time and energy for over half their waking
hours? Therefore, in the approach I have taken to this chapter, I humbly
acknowledge the need of many women to work through sheer nancial
necessity, the schema and societal inuences that may have shaped some
womens self-limiting beliefs (which should not be seen as any criticism
of those women), and the fact that women who choose to be full time
stay-at-home careers make enormous familial and communal contributions. They are simply not the subject of our conversation.

66

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

I pay tribute to all women who dedicate their lives to full-time


homemaking and caregiving (whether to children or others). For our
purposes here, any reference to women who work does not imply
that women in unpaid work do not do meaningful work, but rather,
the clause women who work is shorthand for women in paid work
outside the home.
At the time of writing this chapter, there was an outcry in France
over the return to work of French Justice Minister, Rachida Dati, ve
days after giving birth (Courcol, 2009). Dati, who is single, rose to
prominence in 2007, when she became the rst politician of North African origin to be appointed to a senior French government post. French
feminists have accused her of being a poor role model to other women
by succumbing to the pressure of the job, her own ambition, and her
fear of being kept out of the loop as reasons offered for why she
must have returned to work so early. Datis decision was scandalous
according to Maya Sturduts, the Head of the National Collective of the
Rights of Women. Womens rights activist, Florence Montraynaud,
compared Dati to working women in the 1920s who gave birth in the
factories and lamented Datis decision, which she claimed would exacerbate the divide between supermoms and wimps in the workforce.
Opposition went further in decrying her actions by saying her decision will put pressure on countless other women who might otherwise
exercise their right to take longer leave of absence, which will then be
used by unscrupulous employers in difcult economic times to disadvantage those women.
One might also imagine, notwithstanding her single status, that Dati
would have had the nancial means, if not direct governmental assistance, to pay for quality child care for her newborn. Should or
shouldnt her decision be one of personal choice? If she has the ways
and means to be able to return to work after childbirth (which after all
is not a disease or illness), should she? Does she automatically raise
the bar for other women and create an unrealistic and unreasonable expectation in employers of other women after childbirth? Is that Datis
responsibility or her problem, and is that fair?
Does the fact that she also happens to be slim and glamorous create
feelings of inadequacy in every woman who doesnt believe they have
earned the moniker of yummy mummy within days of childbirth?
Furthermore, is it reasonable or possibly patronizing to assume that
Dati returned to work so soon, as asserted by the secretary general of
the Planned Parenthood Association, because of the pressure to
defend her standing in Frances male-dominated politics rather than
intrinsic dedication to her work?
In this example of one womans experience and an aftermath of
almost hysterical proportions, we collide with a myriad of issues that
reect the complexity of the demanding context and the psychosocial

Working Life as a House: A Tale of Floors, Walls, and Ceilings

67

aura that surrounds women who work outside the home. If we consider all the research surrounding the psychological devaluation of
women who devote themselves to full-time care giving (Adams, 2008),
we could be forgiven for thinking we are damned if we do and
damned if we dont.
Even in 2009, a woman rising to the top of her profession (particularly a male-dominated profession like the law) appears to acquire an
automatic prole a la Dati. The hopes and aspirations of other women
accompany her wherever she goes and the acerbic judgments and perceptions of men and women seem to follow her every decision and
action.
In this chapter, I would like to reect on the relevant contextual factors
for women working today, explore some of the difculties and barriers
that still challenge us (including some of our own making), and provide
some commentary on the hot contemporary issues for women who work
outside the home. In relation to specic aspects of legislation, change to
Equal Employment Opportunity (EEO) law and government agency
direction, I will be referencing the Australian context.
MAPPING THE TERRAIN
There are many interesting discourses on the psychology of women
and the issues they face as a longstanding minority group (in sphere of
inuence if not always numbers), and I dont intend to duplicate their
efforts here. One comprehensive and well-written account of womens
lives and issues of particular relevance to women is discussed in
Denmark and Paludi (1993). Some political, social, and cultural markers
would seem to indicate that things are certainly happening, albeit at a
pace best described as painstakingly slow.
WORKFORCE PARTICIPATION AND EARNING PARITY
It is an all too common indictment of the gender gap that women in
Australia are paid 85 cents in every male wages dollar, and the Australian disparity is typical of other Western countries. Australian Bureau
of Statistics (ABS) (Ofce for Women, 2009; ABS, 2005) data suggested
that on average, hourly earnings of full-time males and females (allowing for the exclusion of overtime) are $30.41 and $25.87, respectively.
This is an earning ratio of 85 percent, which has narrowed by around 4
percentage points over the last decade. The original compelling metaphor of the glass ceiling was coined to describe this gap in earnings
between the genders (Wirth, 2002). The gender-wages gap has long
been considered a powerful and telling metric by which to analyze
gender inequality. The existence of a gap reects two discriminatory
phenomena; women being paid less money to do the equivalent job of

68

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

maleseven in the same pay scale women can start at the bottom of
the scale while men are paid at the middle or the topand, women
tend to dominate the so-called pink jobs (i.e., secondary labor market
job roles that reect gender stereotypes in male-dominated societies
because they are seen to echo domestic responsibilities, (e.g., librarianship, nursing, teaching, secretarial).
These roles are remunerated at a lower level than the so-called
blue-collar jobs (manual labor requiring technical ability and
strength) or white collar (e.g., salaried professionals like engineers,
accountants, lawyers, some managers) with the latter roles regarded as
being higher in complexity and accountability (Fontenot, 2007). Nevertheless, not all disparity can be accounted for by the differential complexity of these jobs or the higher percentages of women working
part-time.
Despite the remarkable changes in recent labor market structure,
women in 2001 held just 1.3 percent of the top management positions
in the largest Australian companies (Kee, 2006) According to the Annual Report of the Government Equal Opportunity for Women in the
Workplace Agency (EOWA), only two of the top 200 companies were
chaired by women and just four had women chief executives (EOWA,
2004).
Over the past few years, more women in Australia pursued tertiary
study than men. It is reported that 51.7 percent of professionals with
bachelors degrees in 2005 were women (Kee, 2006), and that is on the
rise. According to a professional survey in 2008 by Beaton Consulting,
women now comprise over half the graduates in accounting yet
account for less than 15 percent of accounting rm partners. While one
would not expect the numbers entering the profession at the bottom
end to reect in leadership composition at the top end overnight, the
persisting differential is hard to ignore. Similarly, the percentages of
women at lower job classications in banking and nance (e.g., tellers)
is high yet numbers of senior women in banking and nance is still
very low. These ndings underscore the slow tortuous progress in equity experienced by women on two distinct fronts. We see systemic
discrimination, which perpetuates the cycle of disadvantage for minority groups including women and/or women can vote with their feet
and elect not to pursue senior positions in sectors or companies that
appear to them to be so inhospitable toward women.
Thus we can see a duality of disadvantage where women are involuntarily blocked and/or potentially self-select out of what they believe
will be a working life lled with unacceptable challenges once they do
the sums on the payoffs and penalties. In our discussion on ceilings,
oors, and walls later in the chapter, the glass ceiling and the maternal
wall are representations of the rst phenomenon, and the sticky oor,
the second.

Working Life as a House: A Tale of Floors, Walls, and Ceilings

69

WOMEN WITH POWER: THE VISIBILITY OF WORKING


WOMEN
Several watershed appointments in the past ve years would appear
to indicate that female representation at the top echelons of government and business in Australia is clearly on the rise. The Australian
federal election in 2008 precipitated a change in government and the
appointment of Julia Gillard to deputy prime minister. Indeed at the
time of writing this chapter, our new prime minister (PM) was so busy
forging ties with key governments and opinion leaders overseas that
Gillard was at the helm for over two months in the PMs rst year in
ofce (69 days according to The Australian Newspaper, November 2008).
While the former conservative government had been in power for over
10 years, no woman had risen to such prominence, and people were
generally critical of that governments failure to bring about tangible
equality in providing paid maternity leave, assigning women ministerial positions in large and complex government portfolios, and addressing pay inequity and government sponsored child-care; the latter two
regarded as critical success factors in the ght for gender equality (Broderick, Burrow, & Ridout, 2008).
Notwithstanding public acceptance of her candidacy for her parliamentary seat and subsequent appointment to a super Ministry of
three portfolios, Gillard (2007), who faced unprecedented media attention for being childless by choice, said in a television interview:
I think women are still making their way into politicsobviously a lots
changed for women but theres still a few things to change still. . . . that
theres been more attention on private life questions for me than perhaps
male politicians. . . . I think that one of the problems for women is that
historically theres been no right answerif you dont have kids, then
people say you cant understand everyone elses life experience, and if
you do have kids, then people say whos looking after the kids while
youre doing all of this. (Interview with Channel 7 Network)

Other notable appointments were directorships for women in large


public companies including the national airline, Qantas, CEO appointments in government agencies and private organizations, a woman
governor general (who had previously headed up the Federal Sex Discrimination Commission), and the biggest in my mind, the appointment of Christine Nixon, a woman from the state of New South Wales
as chief commissioner of police for the state of Victoria in 2001. However, it is fair to say that these stand out because they were the exception rather than the rule. Board representation in Australian companies
stands at a disgraceful 10.9 percent notwithstanding broader representation of women at middle and upper management and down from 12
percent in 2006 (Medd, 2008). As already mentioned, women still earn

70

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

85 cents in every male dollar across all sectors, and workforce participation rates among women between 25 and 44 years is one of the lowest in OECD countries (Abhayaratna & Lattimore, 2006) even after
adjusting for variances in statistical practices. The Human Rights and
Equal Opportunity Commission (HREOC) blames that in large part on
problems with child care and the absence of taxpayer-funded maternity
leave and other measures to support primary careers.
WOMEN AS CONSUMERS
Interestingly, women in the almost identical age bracket (25- to 40year-olds) are the fastest-growing Australian wealth demographic and
are being targeted for potential sales growth (Young, 2008). Furthermore, the proliferation of new businesses of small and medium size is
attributable predominantly to women. In a study commissioned by a
major non-bank lender (Wizard Home Loans, 2006), women declared
they were the home managers in 93 percent of cases, that one-third
made decisions on nance loans and other nancial products entirely
on their own, and that in 61 percent of cases they were joint decision
makers on big home nancial decisions.
Women are becoming a consumer force to be reckoned with. However, interest in nancial planner seminars on how to attract the female
dollar are spawned by the recognition of business opportunity; not necessarily underpinned by sincere moral and feministic attempts to right
the injustices of the past. There are still a lot of obstacles to women
achieving their potential, including structural barriers to workforce participation and therefore to nancial independence. Women now ofcially
number just under half the Australian workforce (45 percent) (ABS,
2007), but sheer numbers dont tell a story of equity and access unless
we look at the proportion of women in an organization vertically. In
other words, if the numbers of men and women in an organization are
exactly balanced but the vast majority of positions at the top end of
organizations are occupied by men, then we have achieved equity of
access (to work) but not equity of outcomes (to workplace opportunities). Most human rights commentators agree it is happening but far too
slowly despite almost three decades of an Equal Employment Opportunity (EEO) regime that outlaws discrimination on gender grounds.
WOMEN AND EEO LAW
Australia has a 25-year history of gender (sex) discrimination legislation at the federal level, and for the most part the legislation has served
them well. The federal Sex Discrimination Act (SDA) was passed in
1984 and outlaws direct and indirect discrimination on the basis of
gender, marital status, pregnancy, potential pregnancy (being female

Working Life as a House: A Tale of Floors, Walls, and Ceilings

71

and perceived to be of childbearing age), and career status/family


responsibilities. The overarching principle has been one of meritocracy:
the best person for the job on objective job-related criteria. Australia
resisted mandating Afrmative Action in the form of positive discrimination, quotas, or even targets but organizations (particularly mid to
large size) are expected to demonstrate by way of formal and regular
reports to the Equal Opportunity for Women in the Workplace Agency
(EOWA) what they are doing to eliminate the barriers to women at
work, such that, they may enjoy access to workplace opportunities
(assuming they are the most suitable candidate for that opportunity).
That is not to say individual organizations do not elect to go beyond
the minimum requirement and actively promote those of minority
group status. The pervasive long-term doctrine has been merit-based
selection. In an era of rampant gender discrimination, the merit principle was undoubtedly a clear, fair, and unequivocal stance, but it didnt
stop organizations giving a potential job applicant the standard line
There was another applicant more suitableand seemingly getting
away with it. The law (and organizational policies emanating from it)
did, however, empower any would-be complainant to demand an explanation for decisions made, and it has undoubtedly resulted in more
rigorous, transparent, and legally defensible processes over time. What
an examination of this system some twenty-plus years on would suggest is that many working women may enjoy better access, but as per
the selection example just given, not necessarily equality of outcomes.
At the time of writing, state EEO legislation is exploring the notion
of positive obligations on the part of employers as a way to achieve equity. There is a growing perception that EEO law, and specically the
merit principle, have not done enough and that what has been
achieved has come too slowly. Comprehensive, ethically motivated,
and articulate policies mounted behind perspex on the walls of lavishly
appointed board rooms wont count for anything if cowboy gatekeepers, committed to preserving the status quo, are allowed to perpetuate
systemic disadvantage of minority groups with no fear or experience
of actual consequences.
It is the culture of organizations that have found informal, often systemic, ways to hold women back that has been our less obvious problem. The SDA also covers sexual harassment, which goes beyond
unfair disadvantage of women (and men) at work in respect of workplace opportunities like promotion, pay, and benets but to how they
are treated in the context of their work. Interestingly, there have been
two iterations to this legislation. Australia witnessed a watershed when
changes were made on the 10th anniversary of the Sex Discrimination
Act that encompassed a shift in the denition of sexual harassment.
With the advent of the legislation in 1984, complainants had to
argue that they suffered adverse impact in their jobs by rejecting the

72

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

unwelcome attention of alleged sexual harassers, or believed they


would, in order to have a sexual harassment complaint substantiated.
With the legislative changes in 1994, the denition of sexual harassment changed to unwelcome attention of a sexual nature that offends,
intimidates or humiliates someone or makes the workplace unpleasant. Thus, women and men did not have to experience unlawful
workplace discrimination in rejecting the unwelcome attention. They
simply had to feel emotionally or psychologically put upon. This not
only marked a shift in employee relations legislation but effectively in
human rights.
The re-interpretation of the legislation heralded an important shift
from viewing inequity (i.e., differential treatment) in the workplace as
a function of unlawful direct or indirect disadvantage to issues of culture and the psychosocial elements of work relationships. Because the
vast majority of people who hold senior positions in organizations are
men, that the subordinate roles are commonly occupied by women (see
earlier discussion on pink jobs vs. blue and white jobs), and considering sexual harassment is often an abuse of power, it is not surprising
that consistently over 90 percent of complaints made in a jurisdiction
on sexual harassment have been made by women against men. Structural barriers to opportunities clearly limit opportunities, but the issue
of hospitable or alienating culture (as demonstrated by unwelcome
behavior, including sexual behavior) will also create perceptions that
will in turn drive behavior. If the belief held by senior men in a consulting engineering rm is that female applicants wont t in to their
culture and they then disregard the applications from women, few if
any women will have the chance to prove them wrong in their workplace. Thus, gender balance will remain skewed. However, women
who perceive they will get a raw deal in such workplaces, hear anecdotally of other women being given a hard time, observe poor gender
balance, or hear hostile sexist comments will likely vote with their feet
when opportunities arise. Other women down the track, contemplating
whether or not to work there, may conclude organizational culture is
indeed a cowboy culture as evidenced by the poor gender ratio of
professional staff or a persisting gender wages gap, therefore declining
offers and maintaining the status quo. The same distaste for inhospitable or backward culture may also inuence womens decisions to
remain employees or go out on their own as small business owners. It
may well impact their decision to return to work after maternity leave
with all the associated advantages afforded the organization that
doesnt have to start all over again. It may and does also impact womens decisions to have children and/or delay starting a family.
This has compounded implications for some as they delay until such
time as they believe they have established themselves professionally
only to discover they encounter fertility problems. In this argument, we

Working Life as a House: A Tale of Floors, Walls, and Ceilings

73

see the difference between the glass ceiling, where disadvantage and
unfair treatment morph into each other but are perpetrated by others, to
what we might refer to as the sticky oor, where the fears and apprehension of women, combined with the goals they want to achieve, are
best addressed by other means (e.g., becoming small independent homebusiness owners). In this paradigm, even if opportunities present themselves, women may elect not to pursue them.
GLASS CEILINGS
As noted earlier, the so-called glass ceiling is a powerful metaphor for invisible yet actual barriers to the ascendance of women in
workplaces, often reected and measured by a gender wages gap that
can not be accounted for by number of hours worked or job complexity. Thus, the glass ceiling attests to the inequity of outcomes, not just
access, for women at work. Moreover, the notion of a glass ceiling
implies that the barriers are extrinsic to the hopes and aspirations of
those women and are enacted and enforced by others in positions of
power (presumably men). The barriers may come in the form of spoken and unspoken barriers to entrywomen need not applyand/
or unspoken barriers to advancement. The barriers come in the form of
gender-based roles with differential income earning potential and the
complementary devaluing of pink jobs nancially and societally.
(For a fascinating discourse on the devaluing of motherhood, see Crittenden, 2001.) The barriers can be measured quantitatively in the form
of differential pay more than fty years after the 1951 International
Labor Organization (ILO) convention mandating equal pay for work of
equal value was ratied and the barriers can represent indirect blockages to advancement in the form of restricted/differential access to
training, coaching, and mentoring.
More subtle factors are also at work here, which may involve more
than the conscious rejection of everything female or feminine by male
powerbrokers. The reality is that business organizations are typically
still male-led and are dominated by male culture and assumptions.
Corporate language is often competitive, even ruthless: idioms are
based on sport and war, deals are made on the golf course, corporate
boxes at the tennis are legitimate business expenses, and men receive
informal mentoring and support and they know it.
In the early 1990s, working as a senior consultant with a major Australian Bank, I was struck by how many times other women there told
me how my story inspired them. There was nothing terribly remarkable about my story as far as I was concerned except that I had a middle-management position in a bank and several children. I think it did
occur to me when I was pregnant with my third child that perhaps I
was somewhat of an anomaly for the bank when they told me they

74

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

didnt have a policy provision on what would happen with my company car when I went on maternity leave. I was told they would have
to go away and consider it. When I asked for clarication they told me
they had never had a woman at my level that was still of childbearing
age, had actually decided to have a baby, intended to take maternity
leave, and actually return. As I tried to determine whether women at
my age didnt tend to hold such positions, or didnt have babies or
didnt come back after babies, I was told the answer was all of the
above.
It was certainly the case that some women rose to the position of
chief manager in the bank at that time, but every one of them resigned
within a year citing lack of acceptance at best or downright sabotage at
worst. The culture was relentless in its expectation of these women
(almost all of whom were single and driven) or ruthless in the exclusion or harassment these women experienced, yet the bank continued
to pat itself on the back for their appointment. It appeared to me even
then that there were few overt suggestions these women werent competent, far more so that they simply werent welcome.1
Women face other difculties that may be partially explained by
gender attribution theory (Wagner, Ford, & Ford, 1986). Behavior of
the respective genders is perceived and labeled differently. Assertive
men are nagging women; strong, decisive results-oriented women
can be referred to as ball breakers. A tough negotiation stance can
be perceived as aggressive. In addition, women who are comfortable
with their femininity are often seen by other women to be exploiting it
to get ahead. With that phenomenon we observe alarmingly that
women can be just as tough on other women, thus buying in to the
male conspiracy. The archetypal feminine values reect a socialization of women taught to t in, collaborate, be nice, to be not pushy
nor to sound their own trumpet. Even if they do, some women sanction them for breaking the unspoken rule of equality (Peltier, 2001).
Karina Butera, an Australian sociologist, has done extensive research
into intergender conict among women and concludes that women surveyed typically said they would rather work with men and that interpersonal conict at work was their major source of stress (Butera,
2008). One biological explanation is that to ensure the survival of the
species, women must gravitate toward men, not women. However,
most of Buteras research points to psychosocial explanations for
female intragender conict. Butera adds that women do tend to be
other-oriented and can go out of their way to avoid upsetting others.
However, given they legitimately need reection and emotional release,
they may speak to others about negative feelings rather than directly
address the party with whom they have an issue. Men expressing anger or frustration as they feel it are often described as straight
shooters or using the Australian idiom, they call a spade a spade

Working Life as a House: A Tale of Floors, Walls, and Ceilings

75

(or a spade a shovel if theyre very direct!). Thus, women in their gender-based behavior can reinforce unsavory stereotypes among men and
other women as bitchy, dishonest (i.e., indirect), or irrational.
If we postulate that masculine traits are still heavily valued in
organizations but only if exhibited by men, women are caught in a
double bind; wanting to be taken seriously, expected to get results but
judged harshly for adopting male mores.
This pressure or even deep-seeded value to be nice does not
explain why some women can be so hard on other women. It is also
not easy to determine whether women displaying masculinized
behaviors ascend in organizations because of it or feel pressured to
adopt such behaviors in order to stay there. However, in an era where
women have been the pervasive minority for so long at senior levels in
organizations, one could intuitively hypothesize that consciously or
unconsciously women might see other women as their competition for
choice roles and business opportunities.
It has often been said that the wise executive will nurture, mentor
others, and succession plan to free himself or herself for even bigger
and better things. But this takes a degree of security and emotional
intelligence that may be lacking in those consumed with what I will
call the scarcity mentality. It is as if those women looking around
the deck on the Titanic and seeing that iceberg approaching believe it
is the other women in the executive boardroom who are really competing for the few working life rafts available on the ship. This could be
further exacerbated with a gender neutral but powerful cultural norm
that rewards competitive behavior as people scramble over each other
to get to the top.
Whatever the reasons for any women treating women badly, some
feminists will say weve had men to do that for centuries. Is it fair to
expect more of women by virtue of their attribution as naturally collaborative and harmonizing? One sobering reality is that gender attribution theory as portrayed here judges womens language, style, and
behavior by a double standard. It is not fair and just to judge them by
a second; a higher moral plane than that by which men might be
judged. In the past few months as the global economic crisis has seen
businesses crumble and the spotlight has turned sharply on executive
remuneration, I do not recall reading or hearing anything that blights
the entire population of men in business.
The minority of women who treat women badly are responsible to
themselves and accountable to the organizations that hire them. People
treating other people badly at work is not a gender-based issue but rather
a cultural one. As Butera says, gone are the days when we had to ght
for the attention of cavemen (to procreate). Women can push hard
against the glass ceiling, use the best of their feminine and masculine traits
and build strong, constructive relationships and strategic networks.

76

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

At the end of the day, the most thriving organizations I have seen
are those in which emotional intelligence, meritocracy, empathy, visionary leadership, and social conscience are practiced and afrmed.
Furthermore, they are cultures in which bad behavior, whether it be
indirect destructive communication, bullying or unethical treatment of
other staff is actively disincentivized and if it is committed, is both
called and consequenced. Women and men should be able to
thrive in organizations where all are genuinely afrmed for their contribution to their organization and where collective pride in what they
do and why they do it is found in shared purpose and discretionary
effort akin to being a corporate religion (Kunde, 2002).
Putting aside issues (and double standards) around acculturated
behavior, Peltier (2001) claims that women face an additional challenge,
which is that women usually do not get second chances. He argues
that women are given less scope to experiment and err in the learning
process and that after the rst big mistake, instead of being given the
benet of the doubt, they are out the door. Couple this with the selfresponsibility many women feel to pave the way and pioneer for other
women (that other-orientation), and you could forgive women for deciding the challenges are insurmountable and the risks just not worth taking.
THE STICKY FLOOR
Let us acknowledge there are self-imposed limitations on women
who could achieve more in work and career but decline opportunities or
elect not to pursue them at all. I would prefer to distinguish this phenomenon as the sticky oor as opposed to the glass ceiling (Kee,
2006). Many times, well-intentioned and egalitarian senior male clients
rue the decisions of female staff when those women decline to be interviewed for more senior positions. The same males are often frustrated
and shocked when they hear these competent women expressing a lack
of condence in their own ability to meet the demands of the job. Some
of these sincere and supportive males comment on the fact that they
have never heard a male applicant express apprehension about the possibility of letting (his) team down. Indeed these feministic men have
reected on how often they nd themselves seeking to lower the expectations of male applicants who feel the next step up is their birthright. In
interviewing so many working women over many years, I have found
their reluctance to set their sights higher usually stems from one or more
realistic concerns. First, they voice some doubt they will be seriously
considered for the job and dont want the humiliation of a tokenistic
recruitment exercise (thus concerns about a glass ceiling). Second, their
perceptions may be that the culture that exists in that upper echelon will
be inhospitable, and their attempts at success may be/will be thwarted
or sabotaged (referencing the notion of the sticky oor keeping them

Working Life as a House: A Tale of Floors, Walls, and Ceilings

77

where they are). Third, they feel they have seen and heard enough about
the pressures on those performing in those roles to believe the demands
are unrealistic and incompatible with their family obligations (thus the
existence of a maternal wall) with real or imagined consequences
keeping them where they are as soon as they commit the seemingly ultimate crimemotherhood (Sachs & Painton, 1993).
THE MATERNAL WALL
Yes, weve come a long way from the days where a woman working
for the government or as a teacher got married and received two gifts
in the forms of a wedding present from all the staff and termination of
her employment. This strategy was, no doubt, deemed to be the ultimate in practical workforce planning, as organizations would not have
to worry nervously about whether or not the married woman in question might disrupt the workplace with a pregnancy and subsequent absence. The organization was proactive in terminating her employment
before she and her new partner could get a twinkle in their eyes.
While it is clearly unlawful to discriminate against any woman
today on pregnancy or potential pregnancy (assuming she could perform the inherent requirements of the job), the law can scarcely monitor the cultural or psychosocial backlash against the woman who
seemingly commits the ultimate career slap in the face and decides to
have a baby. In some cases that is clearly observed to be the ultimate
in ungracious behavior, and the men (and women) whove taken that
risk on them, only to have it blow up in their faces may demonstrate
their displeasure. A landmark case in Australia in 1998 went the way
of the complainant, Marea Hickie, who had attained partnership with a
law rm Hunt and Hunt and found as she returned from maternity
leave that the majority of her big les had been handed on to someone
else (their concern was that she wouldnt be able to cope) and that she
had ostensibly been demoted. She was awarded damages in the
Human Rights Commission and subsequently went on to attain partnership in another rm (Hickie v. Hunt & Hunt, 1998).
In one sense, the maternal wall is a synthesis of the glass ceiling and
the sticky oor where the demands of the job and/or the culture are
fundamentally inhospitable to women with children. It could mean
their status as mothers is obstructing opportunities and/or they are
reluctant to seize them. A woman may be discriminated against and be
directly or indirectly undermined in the role because she has a child or
may be harassed by those who pay out on her lack of commitment
because she refuses to work ridiculous hours. As in the case of Hunt
and Hunt an organization can assign sexy high-prole projects/les
elsewhere citing work/life balance as their excuse to give the plum
opportunities to others. Management may simply assert the working

78

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

mother is not a team player because she wont stay back on Friday
night for drinks with the others or show enthusiasm for residential
retreats and overseas conferences. However, the woman seeing this
played out in front of her may not take this lying down. She can and
may make changes. In the research discussed in a Time Magazine article
by Andrea Sachs, 53 percent of almost one thousand Harvard graduates said theyd changed their jobs or specialties because of family
responsibilities, and 25 percent of female Harvard MBAs had bugged
out of the workforce completely. This is alarming if one considers that
Harvard MBAs may have been more empowered and had more attractive choices and better earning capacity than some other women. On
the other hand, it is also possible the Harvard grads believed their
qualications would open all doors (excessive Harvard hubris?), and
they became more frustrated and disappointed sooner than most.
Such can be the fear of the maternal wall that women may delay or
abandon starting a family until they have got to a place (job and/or career) that they believe will make them seemingly impervious to the
threat of the wall. Wheeler (2005) refers to them in her studies as
postponers as opposed to early deciders; the latter who are distinguished as making the decision not to have children independently of
whether or not they are or arent in a relationship and how their partner feels about their decision.
Wheelers study is interesting because the widely held view is that
women will remain childless by choice almost exclusively because of
the maternal wall. Wheeler found that while this was certainly true for
many, and work was a strong driver, a distinct and substantial percentage of women expressed negative sentiment about the motherhood role
and the impact of potential motherhood on a range of life aspects. In
2002, the percentage of Australian women who would not have children was 24 percent, with an estimated 7 percent having fertility problems and the remaining 17 percent making the choice. Researchers are
agreed this gure will rise (ABS, 2002).
According to the fourteenth annual report on pregnancy and childbirth in Australia released by the Australian Institute of Health and
Welfare (AIHW) National Perinatal Statistics Unit, over 40 percent of
births in 2004 were to rst-time mothers, who, at an average age of 28
years were about two years older than their 1991 counterparts. Mothers
aged 35 years or older made up 12.5 percent of new mothers in 2004,
compared with 6.9 percent in 1995 when the average age of rst-time
mothers was 26.5 years.
Fertility specialists are in high demand. Assisted pregnancies and in
vitro fertilization (IVF) are booming industries in this country. Having
babies while single has been destigmatized to a degree, and same-sex
couples are accessing assisted reproductive technologies via recent
changes to legislation.

Working Life as a House: A Tale of Floors, Walls, and Ceilings

79

The maternal wall can be no more than a perception based on culture


and observed practice, but the maternal wall may be built of structural
inhibitors to active participation including roles at senior levels. In Australian Equal Opportunity Law there are but two defenses to unlawful
discrimination. The lesser one is the defense of unjustiable hardship;
that is, the accommodations one might have to make for a person of minority group status are claimed to be unaffordable. Clearly, if this were
relevant, it would only apply to small-ish businesses and would be hard
to substantiate on purely gender grounds. The second and more substantive justication for discrimination is the operational requirements
of the business or the inherent requirements of the job. Lack of
workplace exibility, narrow minded and rigid obstinacies about a
requirement for workers in certain positions to work full-time can make
it impracticable, even untenable, for women to pursue certain opportunities. In the context of otherwise agrant gender discrimination the male
or female manager may use the requirement of full time in a given role
as a pretext to exclude women with family responsibilities or those
inferred to be potentially pregnant.
The Australian Human Rights and Equal Opportunity Commission
(HREOC) continually cites paid maternity leave and exible work practices as the two most enabling of all policies/instrument to create better access and opportunities for women. The focus on paid maternity
leave has raged for some years, with different organizations putting
idiosyncratic policies in place on an enterprise basis. Indeed some
organizations are adamant that not only do their exible work practices
improve retention and morale, they serve as a heavy-duty competitive
advantage in a tight and shrinking labor market where the attraction
and retention of talent rules as the number one organizational imperative. They have moved along a continuum that began with a commitment to equity (of access and opportunity), to diversity, to social
responsibility to strategic opportunity (David Morgan, n.d.).
While there will be a number of sociological and other variables to
account for the demographic shifts and not all roads lead to the fear of
the maternal wall, the desire to exercise different choices has most certainly been catalyzed in large part by (a) the removal of barriers to
womens participation in the workforce and (b) growing consciousness
of the right to enjoy not just nancial income or independence but
enduring careers; something which should be the province of both
men and women.
A FINAL WORD ON SOCIETAL EXPECTATIONS
AND WORK/FAMILY CONFLICT
Some women would argue that while doors to workplaces have
opened, there has not been a corresponding decrease in pressure to

80

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

maintain standards in parenting, maintain the home, entertain and


remain sexually active. So are we now free to do it all and have it all
as long as we dont drop the ball on anything else? The highly accessible world of womens magazines, YouTube, and talk shows places
enormous emphasis on body image, healthy eating, working out, gourmet food preparation, and agelessness. It can seem as if were expected
to have the gure and athleticism of Lara Croft Tomb Raider while
holding down the job as World Vision CEO (or nd extra time to do
unpaid humanitarian work), be able to entertain like Nigella Lawson,
and raise perfect well-socialized children with Mensa IQs. To top it off,
we have to achieve that within three weeks of giving birth to them
(Google the yummy mummy phenomenon). I think I would rather
just concede that Angelina wins.
The old adage suggests that a picture creates a thousand words, but
it may be more helpful here to reect on the way in which a few
words may tell a big (and controversial) story. While often the stuff of
humorous sisterhood weekends away, nights out with the girls, and
comic emails doing the rounds, the choice of language used by men
and women says a lot about their mental models (Senge, 1990/2006).
While these stories are anecdotal, it is not uncommon to hear working
women talk about male partners who do or dont help around the
house (as the case may be!). Related to this are the comments from
other women who, on establishing that their friends have partners who
share the unpaid work in the home, tell their friends they are lucky.
The language used would appear to indicate that this is womans work
and they are either fortunate (or not) that partners assist them in this
unpaid work. Again anecdotally, one can hear women and men say
that their menfolk are babysitting (not parenting?) the children that
night while women go out.
A recent study of interviews with 21,000 people by the U.S. Department of Labor (2009) found that working women on average spent one
hour more per day in housework and caring for family members than
men while men spent approximately one hour more per day at work.
In percentage terms the areas in which women and men differ the
most in hours at work can be accounted for by the part-time status of
some women surveyed but the biggest discrepancy in the study was in
relation to unpaid work.
Increasingly, research is being devoted to the struggle with juggle
(Faraday-Brash, 2006) or work-to-family conict being the extent to
which work imposes itself on life at home (eg the helicopter parent/
the latchkey child), but more recently, has included the pressure of
family-to-work conict also (Duxbury, 2003). Statistics show that adult
women are more vulnerable to depression than adult men. Thus, while
the prevalence of depression is only one measure of health, increased
access for women to work, better pay, and career advancement doesnt

Working Life as a House: A Tale of Floors, Walls, and Ceilings

81

automatically mean they will travel well. The happiness movement has
discovered a lot about what contributes to health and well-being, and
if women believe they can have it all rather than have it most, they are
potentially doomed to abject disappointment (as well as accusations of
delusional thinking).
Regretfully, it seems clear we are still a long way from World Peace,
but we can work on achieving a better form of inner peace. Dening
our goals and being purposeful, negotiating for what we want, looking
after other women and helping them be successful, afrming men who
support our successes, furthering our own cause by refusing to succumb to age-old stereotypical versions of ourselves, and realistically
appraising what is possible based on real needs and values will be the
things that help get us there.
Even if we move beyond equity of access at work to tangible equality
of outcomes, it is humbling to remember that a persons identity is not
dened solely by their work, nor could we say that factors outside the
workplace will have no bearing on a persons status at work. What is
possible for women at work is merely an extension of, or adjunct to,
the societal context in which women live. Indeed, a holistic approach is
required to ensure womens standing in society is elevated.
I was lucky enough to grow up in an era where women started to
appreciate they could have it most. Exercising self-responsibility and
owning the choices we make demonstrates the emotional intelligence
we want everyone to demonstrate. My daughters truly believe they can
have it all. Life may not quite live up to that version of utopia, and
there may always be a struggle with juggle, but the will to strive is a
big part of what makes us human and ironically, being human is not
gender-specic.
NOTE
1. Interestingly and heartwarmingly, the bank was headed up the past ten
years by a most visionary and compassionate egalitarian leader, and engagement in the same organization under vastly different leadership would be the
envy of all other major Australian banks.

REFERENCES
Abhayaratna, J., & Lattimore, R. (2006, December). Workforce participation rates:
How does Australia compare? Staff Working Paper (Report for the Australian
Government Productivity Commission). Retrieved August 2009 from http://
www.pc.gov.au/research/staffworkingpaper/workforceparticipation.
Adams, J. (2008). Womens place is in the home: The ideological devaluation of
womens work. Report for the American Anthropological Association, 12, 211.
Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS). (2002). Australian social trends 2002. Cat.
No. 4102.0. Canberra: ABS.

82

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS). (2005). Trends in the gender pay gap. Cat.
No. 6306. Canberra: ABS.
Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS). (2007). Australian social trends 2007.
Cat. No. 4102.0. Canberra: ABS.
Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. (2008, December 9). Media release:
More births to older mother trend continuing. Australian Institute of Health
and Welfare. Retrieved August 2009 from http://www.aihw.gov.au/
mediacentre/2008/mr20081209.cfm.
Beaton Consulting. (2008). Annual business and professions study. Melbourne:
Beaton Consulting.
Broderick, E., Burrow, S., & Ridout, H. (2008). Maternity scheme is overdue in
Productivity Commission research on federally funded maternity leave (Joint project with Australian Industry Group, Australian Council of Trade Unions
and Human Right and Equal Opportunity Commission. Retrieved April 8,
2009 from http://www.theage.com.au/news/opinion/maternity-scheme-isoverdue/2008/04/07/1207420296235.
Butera, K. J. (2008). Breaking down inter-gender workplace conict. Corporate
Wellness, Summer 08/09, 3841.
Courcol, C. (2009). Supermum MP angers French feminists. Retrieved
January 9, 2009, from http://news.ninemsn.com.au/world.
Crittenden, A. (2001). The price of motherhood: Why the most important job in the
world is still the least valued. New York: Metropolitan Books (Holt).
Denmark, F. L., & Paludi, M. A. (Eds.) (1993). The psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Duxbury, L., & Higgins, C. (2003, October). Work-life conict in Canada in
the new millennium: A status report. Ottawa, ON: Public Health Agency of
Canada.
Equal Opportunity for Women in the Workplace Agency. (2004). Annual report.
Canberra: EOWA. Retrieved August 2009 from http://www.eowa.gov.au/
EOWA_Events_LandingPage.asp.
Faraday-Brash, L. (2006). Struggling with juggling. Retrieved March 30, 2009
from www.brashconsulting.com.au.
Fontenot, R. (2007). Blue, pink, and white collars. Retrieved March 3, 2009 from
http://hercules.gcsu.edu.
Hickie v. Hunt & Hunt. (1998). Human Rights and Equal Opportunity Commission (HREOCA) 8.38. 2008.
Kee, H. J. (2006). Glass ceiling or sticky oor: Exploring the gender pay gap.
Economic Record, December, pp. 408-427.
Kunde, J. (2002). Corporate religion. London: United Kingdom: Financial Times/
Prentice Hall.
Medd, R. (2008). Still no cracks in the glass ceiling. Retrieved October 30, 2008,
from http://www.abc.net.au/unleashed/stories/s2405232.htm.
Minahan, S., & Beverland, M. (2005). Why women shop: Secrets revealed. Qld:
John Wiley & Sons AustraliaWrightbooks.
Morgan, D. (2009, March 3). Getting off the treadmill and on the program. Ofce
for Women. (2009). Women in Australia 2009, Chap. 3. Canberra.
Peltier, B. (2001). The psychology of executive coaching: Theory and application. Sussex: Brunner-Routledge.
Sachs, A., & Painton, P. (1993, May 10). The maternal wall, Time Magazine.

Working Life as a House: A Tale of Floors, Walls, and Ceilings

83

Senge, P. (1990, 2006). The fth discipline. New York: Doubleday/Currency.


The Australian. (2008, November 24). Business as usual for Gillard the Caretaker. U.S. Department of Labor. (2009). American time use survey. http://
salary.nytimes.com.
Wagner, D., Ford, R., & Ford, T. (1986). Can gender inequalities be reduced?
American Sociological Review, 51, 4761.
Wheeler, J. (2005). Decision making styles of women who choose not to have children.
Institute of Family Studies Conference. Unpublished paper.
Williams, J., & Segal, N. (2003). Beyond the maternal wall: Relief for family
caregivers who are discriminated against on the job. Harvard Womens Law
Journal, 26, 77162.
Wirth, L. (2002). Women in management: Closer to breaking through the glass
ceiling? In M. Lout (Ed.), Women Gender and Work. Geneva: International
Labour Ofce.
Wizard Home Loans. (2006, March). Emerging trends in womens home ownership.
Young, V. (2008, March 12). Advisers target female dollar: wealthy women a
nancial catch, Investor Daily.

This page intentionally left blank

Chapter 6

Women as Religious Leaders:


Advances and Stalemates
J. Harold Ellens

The history of women in religious ministries is an odyssey of ts,


starts, and regressions; of spectacular successes and pitiful failures.
This is true of all faith groups and particularly of all of the various iterations of Judaism and Christianity. The 2001 World Christian Encyclopedia of Oxford Press informs us that in Protestant Christianity alone
there are 33,830 denominations worldwide (Barrett, Kurian, & Johnson,
2001). I remember my church history professor in seminary informing
us, while discussing the various orders of medieval and modern Catholicism, that one should never forget that there is greater variety
within the faith community of Roman Catholicism than in all of Protestantism. I am not so sure that such a statement can safely be made
today, 60 years later. What can be said, however, is that the great variety of forms and institutions of religion that have marked human history until this day have been the matrix of a painful and precarious
pilgrimage for women endeavoring to express their inherent desire and
heroic struggle to be certied and legitimized in authentic religious
leadership roles.
Most Westerners can name at least ve different Jewish denominations and that, undoubtedly, does not indicate the great variety of
subdivisions within each of those. Likewise, such other faith groups as
Buddhism, while it is constituted mainly by the distinctive schools of
Mahayana, Vajrayana, Theravada, and Hinayana communities, is
almost innitely divided into more than fty specialized subgroups
within each of the four major denominations, as well independent
national Buddhist communities. Recently, the various forms of Islam

86

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

(al Qaeda, Susm, Shiite, and Suni Muslims) are very much present in
human awareness worldwide. Every one of these religious systems has
been an arena in which the issue of women in religious leadership has
been a signicant contest and the source of noteworthy ferment.
At least since the shift from matriarchal human communities to patriarchalism, about 5000 BCE, in all facets of society the role of women
has been repressed, suppressed, and oppressed. This aspect of the
human experiment has been a relatively consistent history of keeping
women under control; that is, whether consciously intentional or motivated by other drivers, the story is one of power brokering in which
women have been most of the time disenfranchised. The most unfortunate aspect of this tragic narrative is the fact that religious institutions
have, until recently, been the most powerful agents and agencies of this
injustice. The misfortune is compounded by the fact that repressing the
contribution of women has been a deprivation and self-defeat of those
very persons and institutions that have perpetrated and perpetuated
this unwisdom and injustice.
It started very early in the history of religious institutions. Though
women have always been a key part of religious communities of every
kind, in the last 3,000 years for which we have some kind of historical
record, their role has been relatively subservient most of the time. For
about 250 years, in the early centuries of the Jesus Movement and the
Christian faith groups that followed it, women were quite obviously the
prominent leaders of the developing religious communities. When Christendom was established by Constantine, creating the Imperial Church in
325 CE, the role of women was severely and permanently suppressed,
as was the role of dissenting spiritual or theological opinion.
Karen Torjeson (1993), Dorothy Irvin (1980), and Joan Morris (1973)
published notable books on this issue, naming the Constantinian revolution and its women-suppressive antecedents the scandal of their subordination. In elegant elegiac prose, Torjeson (1993) regales us with a
story that xes her thesis rmly in empirical historical data. She writes:
Under a high arch in a Roman basilica dedicated to two women saints,
Prudentiana and Praxedis, is a mosaic portraying four female gures: the
two saints, Mary, and a fourth woman whose hair is veiled and whose
head is surrounded by a square haloan artistic technique indicating
that the person was still living at the time the mosaic was made. The
four faces gaze out serenely from a glistening gold background. The faces
of Mary and the two saints are easily recognizable. But the identity of
the fourth is less apparent. A carefully lettered inscription identies the
face on the far left as Theodora Episcopa, which means Bishop Theodora.
The masculine form for bishop in Latin is episcopus; the feminine form is
episcopa. The mosaics visual evidence and the inscriptions grammatical
evidence point out unmistakably that Bishop Theodora was a woman.
(pp. 910)

Women as Religious Leaders: Advances and Stalemates

87

Similarly, on Santorini, the Greek Island of Thera, a gravesite bears


the epitaph of a woman named Epiktas, who was a priest sometime
between 200 and 400 CE.
This should not surprise us as much as it does. It is clear from St.
Pauls contributions to the New Testament that most of his churches
were established and led by prominent, highly accomplished women.
It is difcult to discover indicators in the Pauline and Deutero-Pauline
letters that there were any of the numerous congregations he established in Asia Minor, Greece, and Italy that were led by men or even
had prominent men in their membership. The eventual appointment of
Timothy to the role of pastor seems to be a singularly notable exception to that state of affairs.
Moreover, Jesuss twelve disciples are given prominence in the
apparently male-authored narratives in the New Testament, but it does
not take much reading between the lines to note that there was a larger
entourage of women sharing in the Jesus Movement. In the Jesus story,
they were, after all, the rst to recognize his distinctiveness, the main
company of disciples at his crucixion, the rst at his tomb, and the
rst to witness and report his resurrection. Hence, they became the rst
messengers of the news of the resurrection appearances around which
the entire hopeful Christian faith community was formed. In the Gospel
of Mary, a Nag Hammadi manuscript from the second century, Mary
Magdalene is the disciple who inspires the rest of Jesuss disciples to
remember the teaching of Jesus and get on with the mission for which
he had chosen them (Fiorenza, 1975).
Roger Gryson (1976) represents the generations of male historians
who have consistently contributed to the suppression of this kind of empirical evidence from art, archaeology, early Christian literature, and corollary ancient testimonies. He erroneously and defensively declares:
From the beginnings of Christianity, women assumed an important role
and enjoyed a place of choice in the Christian Community. Paul praised
several women who assisted him in his apostolic works. Women also possessed the charism of prophecy. There is no evidence, however, that they
exercised leadership roles in the community. Even though several women
followed Jesus from the onset of his ministry in Galilee and gured among
the privileged witnesses of his resurrection, no women appeared among
the Twelve or even among the other apostles. As Epiphanius of Salamis
pointed out, there have never been women presbyters. (p. 109)

Gryson is, of course, patently incorrect in his claims, having been


unable to take into consideration the wealth of evidence to the contrary
that has been generated by the signicant scholarship that has developed
on this issue since the publication of his book three decades ago. The work
done during the late 1970s to the mid-1990s by James M. Robinson and his

88

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

teams of scholars at the Institute for Antiquity and Christianity, deciphering and publishing the library of Gnostic manuscripts discovered at Nag
Hamadi in upper Egypt, contributed greatly to our understanding of early
Christian practices. In many of them, we have indications that in the second to the fth century CE there were still lively memories and legends of
women as religious leaders, indeed women ordained as bishops, in the
early church.
As the Constantinian revolution established a church polity model
that not only suppressed women religious leadership but excluded it
completely in Christendom, so also after the fall of the Roman Empire
in the West, the church of the Middle Ages afforded virtually no
role for woman religious leadership. The only exception to this statement is the revolutionary role some women carved out for themselves
in the form of operational ministries in applied modes, down on the
ground where the great needs were, so to speak. These roles were
usually carried on outside the ofcial structure of the church. They
eventually led to the rise of sanctioned orders of women in ministry,
usually regularized by attachment to male orders, such as the female
order associated with the Franciscans (Clara), the Jesuits of St. Ignatius,
and the Dominicans (Catherine of Siena).
Julian of Norwich, St. Theresa of Avila, and the other noted Medieval female mystics represent a mere tip of a huge iceberg of incredibly
devoted women who carried out ministry within and beyond the
bounds of the Medieval Church. When Protestantism arose after the
Lutheran, Calvinist, and Anabaptist Reformations, women continued to
have distinctive roles in the diaconate and its ministries of mercy, consolation, and healing to suffering humanity. Regardless of the patriarchal pressure to the contrary, the presence of women in ministry has
always been irrepressible. The repression and overt oppression that
existed in the Christian community for 18 centuries, however, took the
form of a glass ceiling that obstructed the movement of women into
the ofces, authority, and empowerment of ordination.
Only in the mid-twentieth century did women nally manage to
achieve ordination to Christian ministry in some Protestant Churches.
Methodism in the United States began to ordain women to the ofces
of Minister of Word and Sacraments in the late 1930s. Independents
and Pentecostals preceded that date but only in the cases of exceptional, independent, charismatic, self-appointed, and self-made prophets such as Amy Semple McPherson and Mary Baker Eddy.
Presbyterianism began to ordain women to the ofces of the word and
sacraments in 1957. A similar pattern of development took place at
approximately the same time in the various Jewish denominations. Of
course, in Muslim communities no gain has been achieved in ordination of women to such roles as leaders of congregations, ofcials of
mosques, or the function of Iman. So the regularization of the roles of

Women as Religious Leaders: Advances and Stalemates

89

women in religious leadership at the ofcial level is very recent and


sparsely distributed among the religious institutions of the Western
World.
In 1999, Audrey Brosnan wrote an interesting study of women going
through transition in ministry roles in the Roman Catholic Church. She
focused on the perplexities and pain of women in religious leadership
in the Roman Catholic faith community. In a private conversation, Dr.
Brosnan highlighted key factors in the historic struggles that have prevailed over the centuries, down to this present moment. She pointed
out crisply that the plight of women in religious leadership in Christendom has always been a turbulent quest for their clear discernment
of authentic vocation and the mode and method of ministry to which
they were called. Every movement for women in religious leadership
was resisted, criticized, and obstructed until they were organized into
identiable and named mass movements that could appeal to the ecclesiastical authorities for institutionalization; for example, in the form of
religious orders of cloistered women.
Unfortunately, even these orders were usually given authoritative
and legitimated status only as adjuncts to established male orders.
Thus, the Jesuits of St. Ignatius, the Franciscans, the Dominicans, the
Augustinians, and the Benedictines all had womens orders associated
with them. A recent assessment of ofcial Roman Catholic womens
orders listed approximately 100 different such orders worldwide, each
having a surprising number of daughter organizations in various countries of the world (Wikipedia, 2009)
None of these women in divine vocation could ever be ordained to
the priesthood, of course. Only Catholic men have been ordained, and
women in religious leadership have always been required to attach
themselves in one way or another to ordained men. Highly accomplished women in heroic and profound forms of ministry stand out in
this historic tradition over the centuries, such as Catherine of Siena in
the Dominican tradition, and St. Clara in the Franciscans womens
order. The list is long. Those rare women who achieved individual notable status in Christendom before the modern era were such wellknown gures as Julian of Norwich, Hildegaard of Bingen, St. Teresa
of Avila, and Joan of Arc. These set the precedent for such modern gures as Mother Theresa, St. Theresa of Lisieux, and numerous lesser
known but equally signicant persons still among us today.
Great women today who have a vision and vocation of ministry as
women in religious leadership, but who are unable to achieve ordination because of the polity constraints of their faith community, strongly
wish and often militate for a place at the table where the decisions are
made for the present operation and the future shape of the church. In
mainline Presbyterian and Methodist churches in the United States, in
which ordination is now equally accessible to women as it is to men,

90

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

many if not most of the major committees and commissions at the


national organizational level, of those denominations, are lead by
women. This pattern increases every decade, if not every year.
In denominations in which women still cannot be ordained, there is
a sturdy undercurrent of desire for such certied roles of women religious leadership. As I write this, the Roman Catholic Curia under Pope
Benedict XVI is carrying out a systematic assessment of all women religious orders with the purpose of weeding out those progressive thinking women who militate in favor of womens ordination to the
priesthood in the Roman Catholic Communion. This is a step further
along a course instituted by Benedict XVI while he was still head of
the Curia in Rome during the papal tenure of Pope John Paul II. A decade or more ago he carried out a house-cleaning in all Roman Catholic
Seminaries in the United States. It that intervention ecumenical initiatives by the seminaries toward other denominations were curtailed,
non-Catholic faculty members were removed from Catholic seminaries,
non-Catholic students could no longer matriculate in or even attend
the Masters of Divinity courses leading to ordination to the priesthood,
and non-Roman Catholic students were systematically amortized from
the Master of Arts program in Religious Studies at the seminaries.
This has caused and will now increasingly cause a signicant distancing of the Roman Catholic communion from other communities of
Christian faith traditions, as well as from those in Catholic membership
who feel that the restrictions of the church against women in religious
leadership is neither biblical, nor wise, nor fair. The predicament of
such women in Roman Catholic communities is paradigmatic of that of
women in any denomination or walk of life in which gender is an
obstruction to vocation, promotion, and achievement. The situation places women with skills, talents, natures, and divine vocations for religious leadership in an impasse in which they are unable to be true to
their own authentic selves. They are forced to decide whether they
should simply continue alone in the pursuit of an interior personal spirituality or persist in pressing for such ordination, promotion, and
leadership roles as permit them to exercise their sense of divine vocation. Are they to be satised with personal contemplation of the mystery of a meaningfully transcendent private spirituality or militate for
the institutionalization of their rightful sense of calling in ministry?
Benedict XVI, earlier as head of the Curia and now as Pope, has
rolled back the progress toward proper certication of women that was
advanced by Vatican II. As a result, Catholic women in religious leadership are forced to give up the hope for certication in roles equivalent to those of religious male leaders. This spills over into the general
culture in many ways, mainly in the devaluing of women in their
desire for advancement in secular forms of leadership commensurate
with their education/training, abilities, and style. Such generalized

Women as Religious Leaders: Advances and Stalemates

91

erosion of hope in church and society can corrupt the victims vision of
her own destiny, and in the case of religious leadership issues, prompt
singularly able persons into tasks and careers that are not real ministry
and are inferior to their ability and vocation.
This raises the profound and life-shaping issues of values, objectives,
goals, and identity in terms of womens authentic vocation. Then a
woman must try to discern where one can still serve best, in spite of
being shut out of the arena of esteemed and standardized ministry. This
is ultimately what religious and secular women face today. Every individual woman then is faced with making that isolated personal decision.
Mother Theresa of Calcutta and her similar saintly antecedents were
forced into that position and resolved it by heroic individual service to
the most needy in our world, person by person and situation by situation. Inadvertently, her solitary ministry grew into a communal calling
for many women, through her houses of ministry, throughout the world.
Women have always sublimated their loss of institutional certication
and ordination by pouring their spiritual energies and sense of vocation
into such community or worldwide missions. They are the ones who
chose to move away from the struggle for ordination and simply
immersed themselves in ministry to specic human needs, such as contemplation and prayer, teaching, or care of the needy and suffering.
They are the ones who bore the burden and the heat, while they could
have been those who spiritually conquered and subdued the land.
Religious institutions, which resist honoring the ministry of women
and the institutionalization of womens leadership, invite thereby a
counterforce to their institution in the form of a congealing group of
those women who see it as a matter of ethical integrity to stay and
ght. It is these women that the present papal housecleaning of the
female orders is endeavoring to eliminate from places of inuence.
There is, of course, in every institution or society that represses religious leadership by women, a group of women who prefer to stay in
the community, adapt to its prescriptions, dene themselves in terms
of its rubrics, and enjoy that traditional status quo. Such women or
groups tend to go to the margins and work in peripheral ministries
without pursuit of denitive central leadership roles. Others enter the
academy and invest their energies there instead of in the church, and
still others simply abandon the cause of religious service and move
into a kind of freedom of forced secularity.
If this were only true of the Roman Catholic Church and the Muslim
congregations and Mosque-centered communities, it might be possible
for most of us to ignore them and leave them to their benighted and
misbegotten selves. However, the trouble, as hinted above, lies in the
fact that this ancient mode of repressing women in religious institutions tends to reinforce the long-standing pattern of those same regressive values and behaviors in the culture and society in general. When

92

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

the biblical community of Israel became an empire under Davids


dynasty, prophets arose to remind them that this was not the intent of
the covenant. Prophets challenged the status quo, claiming that God
does not have a predestined system of order that is unfolding in history, no matter how humans behave. The prophets claimed that the
world is not the way God intended it, and humans are responsible to
set it right.
So should women simply stay in repressive institutions and roles,
acquiesce to the exploitation, and rationalize away the perdy and
immorality of such societal structures? Or is one duty bound and under
ethical imperative to get out of the institution and establish an alternative structure that will function morally? Each woman must make her
own decision regarding this watershed issue and discern where, for her,
is the tipping point of the ethical and personal dilemma.
Numerous biblical scholars, over the last three decades, have referred
to the illustration of Jesuss life and work, in which he obviously cultivated Mary Magdalene to be his equal in ministry, not his lover (Schaberg, 2002, 2006). Elizabeth Schussler Fiorenza (1984, 2009) casts the issue
of this ethical dilemma as the universal problem of women against
empire and its patriarchal structures. A similar posture is advanced by
Walter Wink (1984) in his work on the powers of human abuse and devaluation that are afoot throughout the world of contemporary human society. Wherever repression and oppression of any kind have prevailed in
this world, it has often been up to women to initiate ministry and service
to the marginalized, victimized, outcast, and disempowered. It was usually women that opened the doors and windows of new initiatives and
experimental ministries for the needy, helpless, hopeless, and forlorn.
How often throughout history the prophetic voices of women have given
the sturdy and challenging leadership that delivered all kinds of persons
from the denigration of their humanness, and so greatly relieved the
human spirit of its travail (Wink, 1984, 1986, 1992, 1998, 2003).
Surely this is the reason that Jesuss message and style had such inordinate appeal to women, and that they have always sought ofcial
and certied ministry in his name. Jesus stood against all forms of
entrenched power and oppression throughout his ministry. That was
certainly the main point of his very existence. That is also the reason
why, from the outset of his ministry, his interventionary voice and his
unconventional claims on the sources of power in his society were suppressed and ultimately exterminated. Jesus stood against the powers of
this world and so was himself oppressed, suppressed, and cast out. He
cared greatly for those for whom others cared little, the outcast, suppressed, and oppressed.
Women understood immediately and intuitively the sounds, signs,
and signicance of such a mission as Jesuss, and its importance for the
unempowered. They had been there, had done that trip, and had

Women as Religious Leaders: Advances and Stalemates

93

gotten the T-shirt, so to speak. So it should not surprise us that


throughout history there have risen to the top of the tide of human
endeavor, women of great ability and redemptive presence who have
called for a legitimation of all forms of womens religious leadership.
They have long known the essential issues, have long sought deliverance, and they have militated appropriately for their opportunity to
bind up the broken hearted and afford the ministry of deliverance and
hope to all who are oppressed.
The net outcome of this long historic struggle for the equality of
women, for their freedom from sick dependencies and oppression, and
for their ordination to and/or certication in religious leadership roles,
turns out to be very interesting in many ways. This observation applies
equally to women in the political arena, in social services agencies, in
the academy, and in ordained religious roles, in those communities of
faith of every religion and denomination that allows it.
The fact that is most interesting about this modern day outcome is
as follows. In the historically most highly regarded professions in the
Western World, namely, religion, medicine, and university faculties,
men are no longer the most prominent candidates or appointees. I
came onto the university scene in 1970 and had a lovely time for a decade. However, in the subsequent quarter of a century, key university
posts tended to be given to women or black candidates. If you were a
qualied black woman, your chances were even better for an appointment. If you were a black Puerto Rican woman of Spanish extraction
whose mother tongue was Hebrew, it was a cinch.
The very specic reasons for this lie in two facts. First, the wholesome advantages of equal opportunity legislation, and second, the fact
that, during the last two generations, males with rst-line brains, so to
speak, have not sought out the ministry, medicine, and the academy,
as of primary interest to them. They have tended strongly, instead, to
professions having to do with research, technology, and computer engineering, rather than the humanities and social sciences.
At the same time, equal opportunity legal provisions opened the
doors for women with rst-line brains to seek and nd signicant
appointments in ordained ministry, medicine, and university faculties.
In those communions of faith and those institutions of medicine and
learning that were open to senior roles for women, this began to provide the opportunity for woman of superior ability and training to acquire the leading positions in these historically esteemed professions.
Thus, in Christian churches, which now ordain women to religious
leadership positions, we are getting a preponderance of women with
rst-line brains entering our seminaries and our signicant positions of
religious leadership as ordained clergy. At the same time, a high percentage of the males who are going into ordained ministry, medicine,
and university professorships have third- and fourth-line brains; that

94

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

is, they are of far less ability, skill, motivation, and productivity than
their women colleagues, for the most part. This is not true of all males,
but a high percentage of those becoming ordained ministers, doctors,
and university professors. Conversely, excellence does not mark all
women entering the historically prized professions, but only a high
percentage of them. This sociological and psychological shift represents
a major modication in our culture.
I was, for the rst half of my professional career, an ordained minister in a denomination that would not ordain women to the roles of either deacon or teaching (preachers and pastors) and ruling elders.
Many of us young men carried a brilliantly burning torch for the cause
of womens ordination. We were fathers of brilliant and appropriately
passionate daughters whose future in ordained ministry was quite
obvious. We gained some ground in the late 1970s and succeeded in
getting women ordained as deacons. When we overtured the denomination to ordain women as elders and ministers, an aggressive political
backwash both denied the overture for ordination of women as ministers, and rescinded the previous decision of years before regarding
ordination of women deacons.
After a decade of this struggle, constantly failing in our objective, I
left that denomination on the grounds of its failure to measure up to
the biblical and ethical imperatives for the equality of women and their
opportunities as women religious leaders. This struggle continues in
many religious communities, and most unfortunately of all, the Roman
Catholic Church under Benedict XVI is rushing that communion rapidly into the Middle Ages. All prospects of any gains in ordained
women in religious leadership in that most inuential of all Christian
communions is being regressed severely, with a vengeance.
So the picture and prospects for women as religious leaders in the
Americas, and throughout the Western World, are quite ambiguous. In
the Christian denominations and non-Christian communities of faith
that invite women as religious leaders, women are generally doing a
brilliant work and exceedingly effective service to those institutions
and their constituencies, as the effectiveness of males in comparable
roles is in decline. On the other hand, in those religious groups who
resist women as religious leaders, the anti-feminine prejudice is hardening and becoming more and more regressive, to the great detriment
of those institutions and their constituencies, especially of the women
in their communions. There seems to be more and more likelihood that
women will increase in prominence and effectiveness in religious leadership where they are certied to do so; and less and less likelihood
that women will ever have that opportunity in such regressive communions as the Roman Catholic Churchat least under the present Medieval papacy.

Women as Religious Leaders: Advances and Stalemates

95

REFERENCES
Barrett, D., Kurian, G., & Johnson, T. (2001). World Christian encyclopedia (2nd
ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Brosnan, A. (1999). Discerning ministerial transition. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Catholic Theological Union, Chicago.
Fiorenza, E. S. (1975). Mary Magdalene: Apostle to the Apostles. United Theological Seminary Journal, April, 2224
Fiorenza, E. S. (Ed.). (1984). In memory of her (2nd ed.). New York: Crossroads.
Fiorenza, E. S. (2009, April 16). Unpublished lecture, University of Detroit Mercy.
Gryson, R. (1976). The ministry of women in the early church. Collegeville, MN:
Liturgical Press.
Morris, J. (1973). The lady was a bishop: The hidden history of women with clerical
ordination and jurisdiction of bishops. New York: Macmillan.
Schaberg, J. (2002). The resurrection of Mary Magdalene: Legends, apocrypha and the
Christian testament. New York: Continuum.
Schaberg, J. (2006). Mary Magdalene understood. New York: Continuum.
Torjeson, K. (1993). When women were priests: Womens leadership in the early
Church and the scandal of their subordination in the rise of Christianity. San
Francisco: Harper.
Wikipedia. (2009). Denominations. Retrieved May 10, 2009, from http://www.
wikipedia.com/denominations.
Wink, W. (1984). Naming the powers: The language of power in the New Testament.
Philadelphia, PA: Fortress Press.
Wink, W. (1986). Unmasking the powers: The invisible forces that determine human
existence. Philadelphia: Fortress Press.
Wink, W. (1992). Engaging the powers: Discernment and resistance in a world of
domination. Minneapolis: Fortress Augsburg.
Wink, W. (1998). The powers that be. New York: Doubleday.
Wink, W. (2003). Jesus and nonviolence. Minneapolis: Fortress Augsburg.

This page intentionally left blank

Chapter 7

The Feminine Political Persona:


Queen Victoria, Ellen Johnson Sirleaf,
and Michelle Bachelet
Emily A. Haddad
William Schweinle

Women have historically been and currently are underrepresented


among international political leaders. The proportions of men and
women in leadership roles have been slowly moving toward gender
parity, but as of 2007, only 17 percent of national parliament members
were women. In the U.S. Congress, women represent only about 16
percent of the members (Eagley & Carli, 2007). Never has a woman
been elected to the U.S. Presidency, though during the 1990s, twentyve women served as president or prime minister in other countries.
Eagley and Carli (2007) note two successful women presidents who
were elected to lead their respective countries while maintaining a feminine political persona and leadership style: Ellen Johnson Sirleaf of
Liberia and Michelle Bachelet of Chile. Moreover, Rubin (2006) argues
that the elections of Johnson Sirleaf and Bachelet suggest that people
want feminine attributes in their leaders. Perhaps as important is the
media attention that has been given to the feminine personae that these
two women fostered in their campaigns and in their presidencies.
Much of this attention seems to focus on their evident femininity, especially as displayed in their family relationships.
The reign of Great Britains Queen Victoria provides an early paradigm for feminine national leadership. Before we present Victorias
public persona as a model for her twentieth- and twenty-rst-century

98

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

heirs, we briey summarize some scientic ndings regarding women


leaders. These ndings clearly indicate that women can lead as effectively as men while maintaining a feminine leadership style. We conclude that while women are underrepresented among political leaders,
the historical evidence about Queen Victoria and the current success of
Presidents Johnson Sirleaf and Bachelet clearly indicate that women are
able to deploy conventionally feminine attributes to their advantage in
developing their public personae as leaders.
LEADERSHIP STYLES: TRANSACTIONAL VERSUS
TRANSFORMATIONAL
From the 1950s to the 1980s, behavioral scientists tried a number of
methods and measures to explain the overwhelming gender disparity in
leadership. Much of this research focused on the differences between
mens and womens leadership styles. Eagley and Johnson (1990) metaanalyzed 162 of these studies and concluded that men and women do
indeed differ in their leadership styles. Generally speaking, women tend
to lead with a more transformational style that emphasizes interpersonal
relationships and task accomplishment, whereas men tend to lead with
a more transactional style that places less emphasis on interpersonal relationships and focuses instead on the contractual exchange between leaders and followers, usually money for work (Eagley & JohannesenSchmidt, 2001; Ridgway, 2001; Walumba, Wu, & Ojode, 2004).
A more transactional or masculine leadership situation tends to be
based in a contingent rewards systema system of social and other
contracts between the leader and his followers. The leader communicates the desired outcomes to his subordinates. The subordinates then
produce the outcomes and are rewarded for having achieved them.
This system tends to create an autocratic environment in which competitive, self-condent, aggressive, dominant, and forceful people will
be at an advantage (Walumba et al., 2004). These characteristics
competitiveness, self-condence, aggression, etc.tend to describe the
male gender role throughout the world (Eagley & Wood, 1991) and are
expressed by men more frequently than by women. In particular, men
are more likely to display a desire to lead in a competitive and assertive style (Eagley & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2001).
In contrast, a transformational or feminine leadership style is more
focused on individual interactions, inspiration, motivation, intellectual
stimulation, and positive inuence on subordinates (Walumba et al.,
2004). Feminine leaders tend to be less concerned with personal power
and more concerned with the organization as a whole, as well as with
its positive group dynamics. For instance, matriarchies tend to foster
economic equality and balance to a society, as Goettner-Abendroth
(2008) argues. The advantage inherent to a more transformational

The Feminine Political Persona

99

leadership style is that the leader or organization can become or


remain a leader without having to possess or control the transactional
rewards that are necessary to maintaining a transactional leadership
role. This may be particularly important to organizations or nations
that are in a state of relative economic and/or social collapse, where
the organization and its political leaders do not possess the rewards
with which to bargain for and sustain their desired outcomes.
Despite the differences in leadership styles, Eagley and Johnson
(1990) conclude that men and women leaders do not differ in their effectiveness. The success of one leadership style over another appears to be
more contingent on context than on the gender or style of the leader.
This chapter develops Duff-McCall and Schweinles (2009) line of argument to show that for certain female heads of state, the foregrounding of
feminine characteristics forms an essential dimension not only of leadership style but also of political persona. For all three of the leaders discussed, a political persona based in part on female family roles (mother,
wife) appears to complement the leadership style itself.
FAMILIAL FEMININITY: QUEEN VICTORIA AS A MODEL FOR
THE FEMALE HEAD OF STATE
Victoria became queen of England in 1837, only a few weeks after
her eighteenth birthday, and died on the throne 63 years later, in 1901.
Her rule dened the age of British geopolitical ascendancy. Victoria
reigned as London became Europes banking center, and as England
became the most commercially active and fully industrialized nation in
Europe. The British Empire dominated the world stage at the turn of
the century; an estimated 25 percent of the globes population inhabited territory that belonged to England. Although the reputation of the
Victorian period now has more to do with prudish morals than political or economic primacy, Victorias reign coincided with Englands
self-conscious transition to modernity.
Victorias direct role in the government of England and its empire
was always limited, yet she offers a durable model for public rule of a
modern state by a woman. A dening feature of this model is its reference to a family-based, middle-class vision of the female. Perhaps
pretty as a young queen, Victoria lost what conventional feminine
attractiveness she had by the time she was middle-aged, yet her popularity was greatest in the nal decades of her reign. Her construction of
a successful and specically female public image depended not on
physical appeal or the direct display of political power but rather on
her (self-) presentation as a wife and mother.
Victorias immediate predecessors on the throneher uncles George
IV and William IVhad led lives of excess and had been faulted for
that by the public.1 Regardless of her sex, then, Victoria stood to gain

100

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

in public esteem by distinguishing herself as someone whose personal


life was irreproachable. It is possible that, as Thompson (1990) argues,
Victorias gender made her more acceptable as a gurehead and less
threatening to those who opposed the monarchy (pp. 138139). As a
female monarch, however, she faced a complicated challenge in this
regard. If she had adopted the moral conventions of feminine behavior
that were predominant at this time, she would have subordinated herself to her husband. At the same time, such subordination was impossible because she had to maintain an image of authority that was
consistent with her royal position and responsibilities.2 In meeting this
challenge, Victoria used her private family role and obligations as the
basis for her public persona as a female ruler. Ultimately, Victorias
domestic success as the mother of nine children and the devoted wife
(and later, long-grieving widow) of the Prince Consort, Albert, served
as gurative credentials for her sovereignty. Thus, whereas the only
previous English queen of similar importance, Elizabeth I, ruled, as
Homans (1998) and Duff-McCall and Schweinle (2008) have pointed
out, by deferring her femininity and operating in a somewhat masculine, but at times, politically, feminine way, Victorias relations with the
state and the public originate in her explicitly female role within the
family unit.3
The extent to which Victorias family life pervaded public perception
of her may be gauged from a brief examination of a few of the memorial volumes that appeared in both England and America just after her
death in 1901.4 Generally laudatory, these books offer a compact overview of attitudes toward Victoria as her reign ended, and writers had
the opportunity to consider it in toto. The tables of contents offer a
place to begin. In a volume by the Marquis of Lorne, John Douglas
Sutherland Campbell Argyll (1901), for instance, seven of the thirteen
chapter titles refer directly to some aspect of the Queens personal life,
and four of them to her status as a wife or widow. The authorized biography of Victoria by Windsor Castles librarian, Richard R. Holmes
(originally published in 1897, reprinted in 1901), tells the story of the
Queens life in ten chapters; ve titles refer to her personal life, and
three of those to her married life. American versions include Morriss (1901) memorial volume, called The Life of Queen Victoria and the
Story of Her Reign: A Beautiful Tribute to Englands Greatest Queen in Her
Domestic and Ofcial Life. Morriss chapter titles suggest much more
attention to the ofcial than either Holmes or Argyll grant, but even
so there are eight chapters titled with a personal focus. A volume by
John Rusk (1901), also an American, presents personal and ofcial content in proportions similar to Morriss, but a couple of his titles integrate the two. One chapter is called Family Life and National Duties.
Another, entitled The Labors of a Monarch, covers the birth of Victorias rst two children as well as her involvement in reforming the

The Feminine Political Persona

101

postal system, among other topics. Clearly, the Queen labored in the
delivery of both babies and mail.
As they introduce their subject, each of these memorial writers
presents Victoria as a woman whose domestic life informed her public
existence. Morris (1901) says that no other British sovereign lived so
noble and pure a life and presided over such a grand era of progress
as the royal lady Victoria, whose late decease plunged the nation into
such a depth of grief (p. xi). The regal state and sovereign rank
strike the imagination, Morris adds, but the homely virtues of Victoria, her maternal love, her life-long touching devotion to the memory
of the Prince Consort, the picture of domestic felicity in which she is
represented as the central and venerable gure, appeal to the common
heart (pp. xiixiii). Along the same lines, Rusk (1901) announces Victoria as one whose life and name stood not alone for the sceptered
majesty of a great kingdom and empire, but also for one of the noblest
and purest ideals of womanhood (p. 23). She was a queenly woman
and a womanly queen; had she been less worthy as a woman she
could not have been so great as a queen (p. 41). Argyll (1901), an Englishman, is more specic about both Victorias traits and her accomplishments, but he joins his American contemporaries in linking her
family role with her sovereignty. He concludes his preface as follows:
The English love cleanliness and healthiness, and so did their Queen, in
this a typical Englishwoman. In one word, she did all that woman and
sovereign could do to inuence for good all movements of her time.
Through a moderating, wise, motherly mind, she worked with effect for
her countrymen in their relations with foreign powers, in the bettering of
their own legislation, and for the social life of the whole community. She
made herself understood, beloved, and revered. (p. xii)

Originally written as Victorias authorized biography and then


republished in recognition of her death, Holmess volume is less effusive than the others. Yet in the elegiac nal chapter of his book,
Holmes too integrates Victorias domestic obligations with her regal
ones. For example, he presents the growing Empire and the Queens
growing family as the two factors increasing the pressure of work
for her, and observes, Whether matters of State had to be discussed
or arrangements of family or household to be decided, all were settled
with a soundness of judgment and an invariable kindness of heart
(p. 329). Her monument, he concludes, will be . . . in the ideal of
sovereignty which she created (p. 330).
In all of these retrospective accounts of Victorias life and rule, that
ideal of sovereignty seems dened by the construction of public
duties as somehow coinciding ontologically with the specically
female, familial duties of a wife and mother. The distinction between

102

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

the domestic and the ofcialeven when assertedis thus largely


erased. Visual indication of this outcome might be found in the funeral
portrait that serves as the frontispiece of Rusks (1901) book. It shows
Victorias body laid out, with a headdress that could be either crown
or bridal veil. The Queens hands, crossed on her chest, are almost
obscured by a large quasi-bridal bouquet, but her wedding ring is
clearly visible on her nger. Although she typically wore black after
the Prince Consorts death, this portrait shows her in a light-colored
dress. A large, square, white cross appears above her where the background color of the photograph has been removed. The caption reads:
The end of a glorious reign. In confusing the bridal with the funereal, the portrait seems to bring together Victorias entire life as a
woman, yet the caption and the awkwardly inserted cross summarize
the life not as that of a woman, but rather as that of an imperial ruler
and the head of a church. Victoria is literally lying in state.5
Although Victoria clearly played no part in the photographic representation of her body after death,6 the construction of her public persona during her lifetime depended heavily on her willingness to use
both photography and publication of her own writing to mold public
perception of her. The development of Victorias persona has been of
especial interest to historians and other scholars of British culture in
the past fteen years. Plunkett (2003) terms Victoria the rst media
monarch, reminding us that she was the rst monarch to preside
over conditions approaching a mass urban and industrial society
(p. 1). Plunkett argues that in responding to these pressures, she
became a populist sovereign, an overarching yet intimate gure
that was always aware of its own fabrication (pp. 1, 11, 10). In concluding that Victoria was, [b]y the end of her reign . . . the symbolic
hub of the British Empire (p. 240), Plunkett emphasizes the civic
publicness of Victoria and her family. The specic content of these
public images, as both Homans and Plunkett show, has the effect of
democratizing Victorias image (Homans, 1998, pp. 4557; Plunkett,
2003, p. 163), chiey by representing Victoria as a mother with her husband and children. Together, Plunketts emphasis on the royal family
as an essential aspect of Victorias image-making, and Homanss corresponding focus on the photographic representation of the marital relationship, inevitably place Victorias familial role at the center of her
public persona.7 As such, this persona also participates, as Langland
(1997) notes, in an increasingly potent discourse of domesticity that
comes to dene social relations in Victorian England. Langland
explains, The domestication of the monarch helped produce and was
itself produced by an ideology of domesticity that [may] dene the last
half of the nineteenth century in England (p. 23). Victorias own conscious and vigorous participation in this ideological production is evident in her remark late in her life, that she knew perfectly well what

The Feminine Political Persona

103

my people like and appreciate and that is home life and simplicity
(Arnstein, 2003, p. 155).8
Throughout her reign, Victoria consistently represented herself visually and verbally as the fulllment of this ideal, ultimately transform
[ing] the gendered body into a national icon (Armstrong, 2001,
p. 522). As an icon, Victoria remained dened by an explicitly familial
version of femininity that corresponded to the ideals of her people.
Female heads of state in the early twenty-rst century can respond to
the hopes of their own constituents by deploying a feminine political
persona that follows Victorias model. The remainder of this chapter
will discuss two examples, Ellen Johnson Sirleaf and Michelle Bachelet,
as contemporary leaders whose public images are familial at the same
time as they are political, and whose leadership tends to follow a transformational style associated with femininity.
ELLEN JOHNSON SIRLEAF: MA ELLEN
Ellen Johnson Sirleaf was sworn in as Liberias president in 2006 after twenty-ve years of war, dictatorship, and coups in that country.
Born in 1938, she is divorced and has four sons and six grandchildren.
Johnson Sirleaf earned an economics degree from the University of Colorado and a Masters degree in Public Administration from Harvard
(Caballero, 2006). She was also an ofcial at Citibank. During the 2005
Liberian presidential runoff election, the Liberian media focused on
Johnson Sirleafs strengths of experience and credentials over the popularity of George Weah, her male opponent (Shaw, 2007). These experiences speak to her qualications as president, but they do not dene
her leadership style or her political persona.
Johnson Sirleaf has been described by her constituents and in the
press with a number of feminine terms, including as a diminutive
grandmother gure who campaigned as someone who would bring
motherly sensitivity and emotion to the presidency (Johnson Sirleaf,
2006), enabling Liberia to heal after twenty-ve years of civil war, dictatorship, and coups. Liberians refer to her as Ma Ellen or just
Ma, a woman who sees Liberia as someone who needs to be taken
care of with the dedication and commitment that a mother takes care
of a sick child (Johnson Sirleaf as cited in Caballero, 2006). Comments
such as this show that Johnson Sirleafs public persona is not simply
feminine but specically maternal.
Johnson Sirleafs own writing speaks to a transformational leadership style in which consensus building or bridging social divisions can
heal the collapse brought on by competition (in the form of sociopolitical rivalries) in Liberia. She sees the historical conict between the
various ethnic factions in Liberia as needing a transformational leader
who will create consensus and a shared identity:

104

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Liberia stands in the midst of a crisis that is unparalleled in its history.


Its systems of government and economy have virtually collapsed. Internal military conict has caused death and destruction of enormous proportions. The social fabric of the country is torn by deep-seated rivalries.
In short, the nation is at risk of losing all semblance of its identity. However, therein lies its opportunity. There is now a chance to bridge the
social division between the descendents of the settlers and the descendents of the seventeen indigenous ethnic groups . . . a foundation for reconciliation can be found. (Johnson Sirleaf, 1991)

Ellen Johnson Sirleaf appears to have been very successful in leading


Liberia toward positive social change. Feust (2008) argues that Liberian
women have gained substantially in terms of their gender role, work
and business opportunities, political inuence, and educational opportunities. Furthermore, Kinder and Stanger (2008) argue that her administration has maintained political stability, improved Liberias nancial
management, and reduced corruption, though Johnson Sirleaf acknowledged in a 2008 press conference that Liberias troubles are far from
near an end (An African First, 2009). Her success as a leader is further evidenced by her being named by Forbes Magazine in 2007 as the
100th most powerful woman in the world and in 2008 as the 66th.
MICHELLE BACHELET: MAMA
Michelle Bachelet was inaugurated as Chiles rst female president
in 2006 at the age of fty-four. She is separated from her husband and
has three children. She is a licensed pediatrician and a socialist, who,
with her parents, was imprisoned and tortured under the Pinochet regime. Her father died of torture while in prison. She studied defense
issues in Washington DC and was the Defense Minister of Chile before
becoming its president. As Defense Minister, Bachelet helped unify
Chilean military and political leadership.
Despite having these impressive professional qualications and
being president, Michelle Bachelet is, like Ellen Johnson Sirleaf,
referred to by her constituents as Mama and as someone who radiates empathy and genuine concern for people (Polgreen & Rohter,
2006). Her presidential campaign was based on her personal appeal,
charm, and compassion for the people (Angell & Reig, 2006). In fact,
her political opponents in the campaign tried to use these traits in a
sexist way by overemphasizing her sympathy and caring, suggesting
that these personal qualities were indicative of weakness. Her opponents campaign strategy did not work; it would appear that the Chilean people were looking for a maternal gure with the hope that
she could help heal the wounds left by the abusive dictatorship of
Augusto Pinochet (Pohlgreen & Rohter, 2006). It is clear that Bachelet

The Feminine Political Persona

105

campaigned and was received by a majority of the Chilean people as a


feminine maternal leader who was closer to the people than a male
leader and whose ability to restore the health of the country depended
upon her feminine attributes.
In her inauguration address, Bachelet described her successful election as the culmination of [Chiles] long and painful journey from
repression and dictatorship to democracy (as cited in Rohter, 2006). In
a particularly poignant example, Bachelet describes her own reaction
when she and her mother encountered one of her torturers on the
street. Instead of reacting with anger or aggressing against her torturer,
Bachelet describes the mans guilty reaction, thus acknowledging the
price he is paying for having been a torturer. In focusing on his evident
guilt, Bachelet also afrms a fundamental good in her torturer, which
is the capacity for shame and remorse:
One day I was walking with my mother and we bumped into [one of
their torturers]. We identied ourselves, and what we saw next was a
human being who was crying and lacked the courage to look into our
eyes. A completely diminished character carrying a bag lled with guilt.
(Bachelet as cited in Daniels, 2006)

Interestingly, when her political party was considering whether she


was a viable candidate, Bachelet was asked what her dream was. Her
reply was, Very simple. To walk along the beach holding hands with
my lover (Daniels, 2006). This is a stereotypically romantic feminine
dream, one that a masculine candidate would not have dared to utter
for fear of seeming weak or, perhaps, feminine. Though this comment
does not speak directly to Bachelets qualications for the presidency
or about her transformational leadership style, it does, however, reveal
her as a feminine sexual being. One has to wonder whether Hillary
Clinton or Sarah Palin could have responded similarly and have maintained their respective political success. For Bachelet, however, this
public expression of a conventional feminine fantasy was consistent
with the rest of her feminine political persona, and as such did not
prove detrimental to her campaign.
Bachelets presidential campaign and presidency have been based
on a transformational political leadership model called democracia de
los acuerdos (democracy by agreement). Under this leadership
model, Bachelet and her political party, Concertaci
on, have reformed
the Chilean constitution, revised the Chilean tax code, increased the
availability of state-run childcare, and improved the scope of anticorruption and welfare legislation (Siavelis, 2007).
Bachelet is also successfully bringing greater gender equity to Chile,
a predominantly Roman Catholic country in which 15 percent of babies
are born to teenage mothers, and in which divorce was legalized only

106

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

in 2004. Half of her cabinet is women. She has worked to allow prescriptions for contraception and morning-after-pills for all women over
the age of fourteen. Although Chilean society is historically patriarchal,
she is drafting controversial legislation that will mandate gender quotas among the political nominations to the Chilean Congress. She is
working to give Chilean women more protection from abusive husbands. Bachelets achievements are evidence of her success as Chiles
transformational leader and of her ability to use a specically feminine
political persona to advance policy goals, which include gender parity
and the rights of women.
CONCLUSION
This chapter speaks to possibility, not probability or proportion.
Women are still underrepresented in political leadership. This may be
in part attributable to a more male/transformational leadership tradition that views feminine women as being weak. Given such a context,
a woman political leader may become more masculine/transformational in her leadership style, as Margaret Thatcher (Prime Minister of
Britain, 19791990) and Golda Meir (Prime Minister of Israel, 1969
1974) tended to do, or may become a more feminine transformational
leader, who risks being perceived by voters as weak. Currently, at least
two successful women leaders cultivate and maintain feminine political
personae: Ellen Johnson Sirleaf and Michelle Bachelet. Elected after
long histories of political unrest and violence in their respective countries, both women are qualied for the positions that they hold, and
both are successfully using feminine transformational leadership styles
in conjunction with a feminine political persona.
Queen Victoria had no choice but to adopt a transformational style
of leadership to some extent, simply because as the titular head of a
parliamentary monarchy, her direct role in government was constitutionally limited.9 She relied on personal relationships with her prime
ministers and others in parliament to give her preferences a political
life. As Arnstein (2003) explains, her persuasion was often effective,
with the result that she had a greater inuence in the governance of
Britain and the Empire than has sometimes been assumed. The emphasis that Ellen Johnson Sirleaf and Michelle Bachelet have placed on
social healing and the repair of damage to the body politic suggests
that they too have found that a transformational style enables them to
be effective leaders, despite the very substantial differences between
their contemporary contexts and that of their nineteenth-century predecessor.
Moreover, as our discussion has shown, these three leaders deployment of a transformational style is supported, and perhaps even made
possible, by their common emphasis on the sort of familial femininity

The Feminine Political Persona

107

pioneered by Victoria as she developed her public persona. All three of


these heads of state have presented themselves in relation to their family roles, especially those of mother and grandmother. In turn, their
people and their media have accepted their familial status not only as
an essential aspect of their individual lives, but also as a basic credential for leadership.
There are other current and past women heads of state who might
deserve similar consideration. Violetta Chamorro, former President of
Nicaragua (see Eagley & Carli, 2007, p. 95), and two current presidentsCristina Fernandez de Kirchner of Argentina and Gloria Macapagal-Arroyo of the Philippinesall appear to maintain or to have
maintained feminine political personae. Investigation of them and their
peers, present and future, may conrm that public appreciation for
conventional feminine roles, especially motherhood, can help to form a
political persona that effectively supports a transformational leadership
style.

NOTES
1. Arnsteins (2003) comment is to the point: Except for the time of Princess
Charlottes courtship and marriage, for half a century the royal family had
been identied with madness, with eccentricity, with proigacy, and with old
age (p. 33).
2. Casteras (1997) analyzes images of Victoria as a girl and young woman to
show that she was a symbolic child as well as a mother gure (p. 183).
For further discussion of the conict between Victorias marital role and her
obligation to the state, see Armstrong (2001, pp. 498500), Casteras (1997, p.
192), Houston (1997, pp. 172176), Plunkett (2003, p. 125), and Thompson
(1990, p. 143144). Munichs argument that the ideology of Victorian femininity constantly disrupts the discourse of the monarchical body (p. 47) is especially relevant in this respect, as is much of the argument offered by Munich in
Queen Victorias Secrets (1996).
3. Along the same lines, Watson (1997) notes that Elizabeth I was culpably
unwomanly because . . . she had retained absolute executive authority (p. 82).
Langland (1997) also offers a productive comparison of Victoria and Elizabeth I
(pp. 2729).
4. There were more than a dozen of these published 1901 and 1902.
5. A productive comparison might be made between this portrait and the
three-dimensional efgy of Victoria created for her tomb just after she was
widowed in 1861. In this recumbent efgy (an image of which is reproduced in
Arnsteins book), she is very clearly crowned, and her only accessory is a scepter. Although a veil descends from her crown, it is of a heavy material, not the
light fabric associated with a bridal veil, and there are no owers in evidence.
The four decades that intervened between the sculpting of this efgy and the
Queens death apparently allowed for a very different approach to the representation of the dead monarch.

108

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

6. According to Hibbert and Thompson, Victoria left precise instructions


about the preparation of her body and cofn for burial (Hibbert, 2000, p. 497;
Thompson, 1990, pp. 7677), but it is not clear from either description that Victoria issued any orders about how her lying-in-state should be photographed.
7. Arnstein makes a similar point about the coalescence of Victorias family
life and her political authority, citing an 1851 poem by poet laureate Alfred
Tennyson:
Her court was pure; her life serene;
God gave her peace; her land reposed
A thousand claims to reverence closed
In her as Mother, Wife, and Queen. (cited in Arnstein, 2003, p. 57)
As Arnstein, Langland, Munich, and others have pointed out, there is plentiful
evidence that Victorias mothering was problematic, even by the standards of her
day, but this had no impact on the public presentation of her as a domestic gure.
8. As Thompson (1990) notes, Victorias production of a middle-class selfrepresentation bears little relation to the actuality of her life (p. 124). Our
point in this chapter is not to claim that the Queen shared her subjects lifestyle, but rather to show how her public persona incorporated aspects of that
lifestyle. Munichs remark that Queen Victoria acted out an imagined or ideal
Victorian life in the privileged stage reserved for a monarch (Munich 1996,
p. 5) is very much to the point.
9. For a precise and cogent explanation of the extent of Victorias legal
powers, see Arnstein (2003, pp. 3537).

REFERENCES
An African First. (2009, April 8). The Economist, 391, 77.
Angell, A., & Reig, C. (2006). Change or continuity? The Chilean elections of
2005/2006. Bulletin of Latin American Studies, 25, 481502.
Argyll, J. (1901). V.R.I.: Queen Victoria, her life and empire. New York: Harper.
Armstrong, N. (2001). Monarchy in the age of mechanical reproduction. Nineteenth-Century Contexts, 22, 495536.
Arnstein, W. (2003). Queen Victoria. Basingstoke, England: Palgrave Macmillan.
Caballero, M. (2006, September 21). Ma Ellen, African symbol of hope,
returns to Harvard. Harvard University Gazette. Retrieved April 15, 2009,
from http://www.news.harvard.edu/gazette/2006/09.21/11-liberia.html.
Casteras, S. (1997). The wise child and her offspring: Some changing faces of
Queen Victoria. In M. Homans & A. Munich (Eds.), Remaking Queen Victoria (pp. 182199). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Daniels, A. (2006, March). From torture victim to president. The Progressive, 70,
3032.
Eagley, A., & Carli, L. (2007). Through the labyrinth. Boston: Harvard Business
School Press.
Eagley, A., & Johnson, B. (1990). Gender and leadership style: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin, 108, 233256.
Eagley, A. H., & Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C. (2001). The leadership styles of
women and men. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 781797.

The Feminine Political Persona

109

Feust, V. (2008). This is time to get in front: Changing roles and opportunities in Liberia. African Affairs, 107 (427), 201224.
Goettner-Abendroth, H. (2008). Matriarchies as societies of peace: Rethinking
matriarchy. Off Our Backs, 38, 4952.
Hibbert, C. (2000). Queen Victoria: A personal history. Cambridge, MA: Da Capo
Press.
Holmes, R. (1901). Queen Victoria, 18191901. London: Longmans, Green and
Company.
Homans, M. (1998). Royal representations: Queen Victoria and British culture,
18371876. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Houston, G. (1997). Reading and writing Victoria: The conduct book and the
legal constitution of female sovereignty. In M. Homans & A. Munich
(Eds.), Remaking Queen Victoria (pp. 159181). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Johnson Sirleaf, E. (1991, Spring). The causes and consequences of the Liberian
civil war. International Review, 3235.
Johnson Sirleaf, E. (2006, March 8). Keynote address by HE Ellen Johnson
Sirleaf, President of Liberia at the opening of the International Roundtable
on Women in Politics, Paris, France.
Kinder, M., & Stanger, E. (2008). What will the revitalization of Liberias economy mean for women at its center? Retrieved May 1, 2009, from http://
www.scribd.com/doc/4692340/KinderStanger-Policy-Brief.
Langland, E. (1997). Nation and nationality: Queen Victoria in the developing
narrative of Englishness. In M. Homans & A. Munich (Eds.), Remaking
Queen Victoria (pp. 1332). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Morris, C. (1901). The life of Queen Victoria and the story of her reign: A beautiful tribute
to Englands greatest queen in her domestic and ofcial life. s.i. Philadelphia: W. E.
Scull.
Munich, A. (1996). Queen Victorias secrets. New York: Columbia University Press.
Munich, A. (2003). Good and plenty: Queen Victoria gures the imperial body. In
T. Heller & P. Moran (Eds.), Scenes of the apple: Food and the female body in nineteenth- and twentieth-century womens writing (pp. 4564). Albany: SUNY Press.
Plunkett, J. (2003). Queen Victoria: First media monarch. Oxford: Oxford UP Print.
Pohlgreen, L., & Rohter, L. (2006, January 22). Where political clout demands a
maternal touch. The New York Times, section 4, p. 4.
Rubin, T. (2006). Will women show a new way? Current History, 105 (689), 100101.
Rusk, J. (1901). The beautiful life and illustrious reign of Queen Victoria: A memorial
volume. Chicago: J. S. Ziegler.
Shaw, I. (2007). The medias agenda-setting role in Liberias 2005 presidential
runoff election. African Journalism Studies, 28, 5680.
Siavelis, P. (2007, February). How new is Bachelets Chile? Current History, 106,
7076.
Thompson, D. (1990). Queen Victoria: The woman, the monarchy, and the people.
New York: Pantheon.
Walumba, F., Wu, C., & Ojode, L. (2004). Gender and instructional outcomes:
The mediating role of leadership style. Journal of Management Development,
23, 124140.
Watson, N. (1997). Gloriana Victoriana: Victoria and the cultural memory of
Elizabeth I. In M. Homans & A. Munich (Eds.), Remaking Queen Victoria
(pp. 79104). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

This page intentionally left blank

Chapter 8

Women in the Military: Is It Time to


Un-Gender Combat Roles?
Breena E. Coates

The global war on terrorism (GWOT) has brought lm footage of the


conicts in Afghanistan and Iraq into American homes, to where pictures and videos of women in the military serving in diverse roles are
commonplace. The U.S. strategy in the GWOT is now heading into its
ninth year of operations. This has reopened the debate about the role
of women in combat. The debate centers on the Department of Defense
(DoD) ground combat policy of 1994, that states: Service members
are eligible to be assigned to all positions for which they are qualied,
except that women shall be excluded from assignment to units below
the brigade level whose primary mission is to engage in direct combat
on the ground.1 The statement does not correlate with practice. In
both Afghanistan and Iraq, we see women who supposedly are protected from being in combat, actually engaged in direct confrontation
with the enemy, in the uid, assymetrical nature of modern warfare.
Desert Storm was a turning point for women, showing that the boundaries between front lines and non-combat zones were being blurred,
and that more women in non-combat2 positions were actually engaging with the enemy.
Current policy that permits women to serve in so-called defensive
positions is contradictory and confusing. Females cannot serve in offensive positions like multiple rocket launcher systems; they are prohibited from ying special operations by helicopter; yet, they are
permitted to kill the enemy while ying Apaches (which puts them in
combat risk), and have other restrictions on service, due to combat
risk. Females are permitted by DoD policy to serve as police

112

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

personnel on patrol on the streets, gunnery personnel on convoys, or


can perform searches at checkpoints. These are all positions where often become part of the conict, The DoD has used a direct combat
rule policy3 that excludes women to units below the brigade level
whose primary mission is direct ground combat4 (Government
Accounting Ofce, 1999). Furthermore, positions for those units that
collocate (i.e., are located in supportive roles to the troops on the
ground) are also prohibited for women. Yet the military would be
hard-pressed to meet its mission without females serving as police,
medical personnel, truck drivers, and other such positions, which are
positions that put them in danger. These are, in fact, combat roles in
an irregular war where the battleeld is uid and front lines do not
exist per se. Thus, argues Botters (2008), the combat exclusion rule has
become irrelevant.
Military women have always faced such contradictions. In the past,
women were restricted from ying aircraft that could shoot back, but
were permitted to y into enemy territory in helicopters and cargo airplanes, which put them in perilous circumstances.
In cognizance of the uid battlespace, President G.H.W. Bush (the
43rd president) created the Presidential Commission on the Assignment
of Women in the Armed Forces (PCAWAF) to ascertain whether women
could be placed in combat positions. The study, however, reiterated the
objections to women in combat due to lack of physical stamina,5 biological functions like pregnancy, negative impacts on group cohesion, and a
number of other issues. By a vote of ten against and two abstentions, it
voted against having women serve in combat positions.6
It is well-documented that throughout the U.S. military history, when
freedom has been threatened, women have shown that they have the
hearts of warriors and have responded with invaluable service (United
States Army War College [USAWC], PAO factsheet). Today, when we
speak of equality in the workplace more than ever before, it is only
appropriate that women seek to be given credit for the female warrior
ethos in policy and practice. This begs the following question: Why is
Congress and DoD saying one thing in statute, or policy, and another in
practice? Some further questions also arise from incongruence between
policy and action: Does this policy falsely try to maintain a culture of
exclusion, that contradicts reality? In other words, is it hypocritical in
nature; and, is it time to un-gender combat roles? This chapter addresses
the issues, arguments, and background surrounding the debate.
GOAL DISPLACEMENT, DISTORTION, AND DRIFT
The work of Chris Argyris and Donald Schon (1974, 1978) and their
followers has examined how individuals create idealized maps,
espoused theories, or governing variables on how to do things in

Women in the Military: Is It Time to Un-Gender Combat Roles?

113

their heads. These strategic maps are often in conict with the actual
actions of an individual. In other words, people say one thing and do
another. While on occasion there is deliberate hypocrisy in organizations, very often people and organizations are completely unaware that
they are saying one thing and doing another. The organizational drift
(Coates, 2009) or divergence from espoused values and action can be a
mild drift that corrects itself, but at other times it moves away more
strongly leading to goal distortion or goal neglect (Gouldner, 1959).
Goal distortion can lead to the greater pathology of goal displacement
when parts of the strategic map dissociate from, and lack relationship
to, what actually happens in practice (Merton, 1957)7 (see Figure 8.1).
In such cases, the organization could appear to be hypocritical in the
eyes of many of its constituents, when in fact, the organization is
adapting to survive in an uncertain environment. The idea that the organization is being hypocritical is exacerbated over time, when
espoused values continue to be pronounced by the leadership, despite
evidence to the contrary from the eld.
Goal drift to distortion and displacement often comes about through
the unintentional workings of street-level bureaucrats who make
amendments to policy guidelines while operating in the eld (Lipsky,
1980).8 These come about primarily due to environmental imperatives.
If these policy distortions are either not reversed to be congruent
with policy, or if the policy does not recognize the need to adapt to
environmental conditions, then organizational truth-telling becomes
compromised. It is the job of leadership to dene strategy, provide

Figure 8-1. Goal Drift

114

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

sense-making of strategy, explain emerging tensions, but also to change


that strategy when it no longer applies in given cases. When this does
not happen, organizational participants perceive the contradictions and
become confused between values and practice.
Distortion of goals from drift to displacement is also true of the
idealized blueprints, or strategies for action, that are developed in complex organizations like the DoD, and this is also true of the case of the
combat exclusion policy. In the case under discussion, the drift is signicantenough to raise questions about the congruence of words to
deeds. It can and it does raise feelings of discomfort and anger because
women in the armed service have elected to put their lives on the
line.9 As argued by Col. Cheri Provancha, This war has proven that
we need to revisit the policy, because they [women] are out there
doing it [serving in combat positions] (2005).10
Why should women care about this issue? Surely it is a good thing
to have protection from harm provided to them as a group? The answer is fourfold. For one thing, there is a need to remain true to the
aphorism of the Soldiers Creed that is taken by both male and female
recruits, alike. It states: I stand ready to deploy, engage and destroy
the enemies of the United States of America in close combat.11
Another, more important issue relates to the development and
acknowledgement of a womans full potential in the armed forces.
Under current policy this is not the case. Today, combat experience is
most often the stepping stone to the highest levels of military service.
Most general ofcers come from careers in the infantry, armor, cannon
eld artillery, short-range air defense artillery, and special forces.
Because these are closed to women, few women serve in the General
Ofcer ranks. It was only in 2008 that a woman, General Ann E.
Dunwoody, was promoted to a four-star ranking. In a provocative
essay dealing with the Army and the combat exclusion rule, Michele
Putko (2008) notes that, seeing few females at the top has a dramatic
effect on the entire female ofcer population. Putko goes on to ask,
Why should female ofcers desire to serve in the Army where there
seems to be reduced opportunities for advancement and where they
cannot be part of the mainstream? Related to this a question can be
raised, is this a form of hidden job discrimination? Regrettably
answers to this question have been clouded by extreme and divisive
arguments on both sides of the issue (Field & Nagl, 2001). The writers
suggest that perhaps those holding conservative views on the role of
women in our society seek to nd a scapegoat for the problems plaguing the military today and thus place exaggerated blame onto females
in combat. Much of the current debate surrounding the presence of
women in the positions in which they now serve is extremist and destructive. We turn now to facts in policy and law, and the espoused
values that the strategic statements embody. A third argument for

Women in the Military: Is It Time to Un-Gender Combat Roles?

115

recognizing women in combat would be to provide appropriate training for women who might nd themselves in combat roles, as did the
women in the documentary lm Lioness. If one is not supposed to
be in combat, there is less training provided. Finally, women in combat
may, and often do, require posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) therapy. However, as the true case of Shannon Morgan who returned with
PTSD from Iraq after being caught in a reght showed in the lm the
therapists did not have the context to help her. Again, this is an issue
of women being invisible.
HISTORY AND LAW
While one may think that women in uniform is a new phenomenon
arising out the Womens Armed Forces Integration Act of 1948,12 and
other civil rights legislation in the twentieth century, and as Mitchell
(1998) asserts political correctness put women in the military
(p. 341),13 in reality, women have served in the military since the Revolutionary War (Lindon, 2008) in positions such as laundry services,
seamstresses, cooks and water bearers, and even spies. It is known that
hundreds of women disguised themselves as men in order to ght
(Willens, 1996) were wounded in battle alongside men. The most wellknown case is that of Deborah Sampson, who took the name of Robert
Surtlief (McSally, 2007). Sampson, it is said, herself cut out a musketball from her thigh rather than go to a doctor and thus reveal her gender. Another woman, Margaret Corbin, took up her slain husbands
position in the artillery after he died at Fort Washington. Corbin was
later wounded herself, and when her gender was discovered she was
discharged (McSally, 2007). Another famous case was that of Lyons
Wakemand, alias Sarah Wakeman, who served in the Civil War. Thus,
while women served in the military, it often was a service provided in
hiding, as the battleeld was seen as a male-centric workplace. Society
was then not able to stomach the very idea of women serving in the
military, getting wounded and killed. Today, even though it is
acknowledged that the service of women is essential to getting the job
done in the military, there are still lingering cultural barriers thrown
up institutionally. For example, Michele Putko (2008) observed that at
the U.S. Military Academy at West Point and other military institutions, there is a distinct combat arms supremacy attitude. Female
cadets at West Point, noted Putko, are often advised by mentors to
join the Military Police Corps which is the closest branch to Infantry
that is open to women. At the same time, there is overt pressure on
males to join combat arms elds, and it is mandatory at West Point
that at least 80 percent do so (Putko, 2008).
In 1973 when Secretary of Defense Melvin Laird made the announcement of the end of the conscription (the draft), and the creation of an

116

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

All-Volunteer Force, a new type of military force arose. The number of


women joining the military increased, but they were still not permitted to
serve in direct combat units or in units that collocated with such. This was
known as the Combat Exclusion Policy or the combat exclusion rule. An
attempt was made to rescind it in 1979 under the Carter Presidency, but
the hearings died quietly, due to lack of support. The Combat Exclusion
Policy was later codied in 1988 into what came to be known as the Risk
Rule, which standardized procedures for women volunteers to be
excluded or protected from service in combat units. As observed by an
ofcer the all volunteer Army cannot afford to needlessly constrain itself
by coding support units as male only because they are on the same base
camp or are habitually in support of a combat unit.14
In 1991, Congresswoman Pat Schroeder introduced a measure to allow
women to y combat missions as Navy and Air Force pilots. The measure
was presented to the House Armed Services Committee, behind closed
doors and with a voice vote, and it passed. In the Defense Authorization
Acts of 1992 and 1993, Congress allowed women to serve on Navy, Air
Force, and Marine aircraft. Secretary of Defense Aspin asked the Services
to study what other possibilities might exist to open up positions to
women. In 1994 the Risk Rule was repealed in response to the changing
nature of warfare.
In 2005 Congressman Duncan Hunter introduced a bill requiring the
Army to restrict women from front lines: . . . in Heavy Infantry Brigade
Combat Teams and equivalent elements of Stryker Brigades, a total of
21,925 spaces currently open to female soldiers would be closed [emphasis
added.] Congressman Hunter was echoing the sentiment of conservatives
that the public would not accept seeing women killed in action. However,
the old sensitivities may be a relic of the past, says Putko (2008).15 To
date more than 60 women have been killed in combat. Also in response to
the conservative viewpoint, Armys Vice Chief of Staff, General Richard
Cody observed that in new theatre of irregular warfare, where there is no
clear front line, women are already serving on the so-called front lines.
He observed that the amendment would cause confusion in the ranks
and send the wrong signal to soldiers ghting in the GWOT. Subsequently, upon receipt of assurances from Secretary of Defense Donald
Rumsfeld that DoD would review the assignment of women and provide
a report to Congress, Congressman Hunter reluctantly withdrew his bill.
At the end of the 2005 year, Congress passed compromise legislation that
required the DoD to notify it (Congress) within 30 days if women were
being assigned or collocated16 with ground combat units on the front lines.
The problem is that today the shifting battleeld makes the concept of a
front-line obsolete. As noted by a senior service ofcer: The current
debate surrounding this issue by members of Congress who have never
served a day in uniform is disingenuous to the women who currently
serve in the Iraqi and Afghani AORs (Note #29, USAWC survey).17

Women in the Military: Is It Time to Un-Gender Combat Roles?

117

Nevertheless, the debate around terminology such as collocation,


assignment to, and attachment to linked with the matter of the
shifting battleeld, tends to center around semantics and sophistry in
leadership circuits. So what are the opinions of the women themselves,
and the men about the issue of women in combat, about collocated,
assigned to, attached to, and the reality of irregular war? This
issue was studied at the USAWC via a qualitative and quantitative survey in 2006. The survey results present a useful thermometer of the
perceptions of senior-level ofcers from all branches of the military
and Department of Defense executives.

OBJECTIONS TO SERVICE WOMEN IN


COMBAT-UNDERLYING PREMISES
Biological Thesis
This argument asserts that military service frequently requires physical strength and endurance that not all women can manage. This is an
old, but enduring, argument used in other venues of employment opportunity to restrict the labor of females. Nevertheless, studies have
shown that women can increase strength and endurance with extra
training. The Army Times, July 29, 1996, contended that in the opinion
of experts in stamina studies, some women have the necessary resilience and resistance at the levels of males to greater resistance to fatigue and stress on the physical body. The Army Research Institute of
Environmental Medicine studied women with respect to lifting weights
and jogging with weights and concluded that women were capable of
matching male standards with six months of extra training (Worth,
1999). In the Persian Gulf War, twenty-three women were awarded the
Combat Action Ribbon because they were engaged by Iraqi troops. As
of 2007, women have been killed in action in Iraq and Afghanistan,
been captured as prisoners of war, and wounded in battle. The military
now trains all troops in basic combat skills. Given the blurring of lines
on the modern battleeld of irregular warfare, the biological thesis
ceases to be persuasive.
President G.W.H Bushs PCAWAF noted the issue of women and
pregnancy as a cohesion problem. However, since birth control methods
have advanced today to a single shot to make a woman infertile for six
months, this is becoming less of an issue. Also related to the biological
thesis is the motherhood argument that the role of women in society
is that of motherhood and nurturancewhich ensures the continuation
of the human race and the nurturing of its young. As such society has
always accorded special protections to women. It is in this vein that
Congressman Duncan Hunter, Chair of the House Armed Services Committee (HASC) argued the American people have never wanted to have

118

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

women in combat, and this [amendment] reafrms that policy.18 This


statement appears to be a conservative mantra without substance, as
contradicted by public opinion research. A 2003 Gallup Poll found that
8 in 10 Americans think women should either have the opportunity, or
be required to serve the same combat assignments as men do. Conservative activists like Phyllis Schlaffy echo Hunters sentiment. Schlafy has
maintained that, The whole idea of men sending women, including
mothers, out to ght the enemy is uncivilized, degrading, barbaric and
embarrassing to our culture. . . . And furthermore, no one respects a
man who would let a woman do his ghting for him.19
Liberals and pragmatists naturally present counter-claims. Their
observations range from the fact that proportionate to the rest of the
society women in the military make up only a small percentage of
women in the nation to the need for the military to provide exible
deployment strategies and child-care opportunities for pregnant
women and mothers of small children. Also related to the latter is that
both men and women bear equal responsibility for the nurturing and
raising of children (Burrelli, 1996).
The Psychological Thesis
Another argument offered is that women in combat will destroy the
essential espirit de corps that bonds and unies a military organization
in combat. Such destruction, it has been argued, could spell tragedy for
the group. It has also been argued by the PCAWAF women in combat
would add an extra burden to American male soldiers, as their Western value system promotes the protection of women. However, organizational studies have shown that gender is not a factor for group
cohesion, arguing that group bonding is created by investment in, and
commitment to, the goal. This theory contends that it is leadership at
all levels that creates such investments and commitments via the building of motivation and morale within the workforce. A RAND study
(1997) reinforced that gender plays a minimal role in group cohesion
for morale and readiness. In the military combat workplace, as in
other nonmilitary venues, it is actually the goal, or end state, that motivates personnel to perform to accomplish the mission.
Sexual Relationships, Sexual Abuse, and
Gender Harassment Theories
Former Governor Janet Napolitano once noted in the Arizona Star
(2007) that there is a distinctly troubling issue that faces the more
than 160,000 female soldiers who have deployed to Iraq and Afghanistan since 2002. It has been called the double whammy: sexual abuse
and trauma, combined with exposure to combat. Its effects are

Women in the Military: Is It Time to Un-Gender Combat Roles?

119

devastating. While many such statements abound, levels of crime in


the military including sexual harassment are lower than in the civilian
sector.20 It has been noted by sociologist Laura Miller (1997) that gender harassment, rather than sexual harassment, is a more likely manifestation of mens resentment toward women in the military and can
be observed by sabotage, constant scrutiny and making women
prove themselves by making them work harder.21 Again, this phenomenon has been observed in other male-dominated workplaces in
the civilian sector, such as construction. Forced close contact in combat
situations, it is argued, could lead to sexual abuse. However, as argued
by Holm in reference to the Persian Gulf War, men and women serving side-by-side in the Gulf demonstrated that they were capable of
working together as teams; they could be comrades without fraternizing, they could share tents without sharing beds; they could share
common dangers without feigning chivalry.22 Finally, another argument for keeping women out of combat is the fear of them falling into
enemy hands and being sexually abused. However, the counter argument is that women are sexually abused in noncombat positions as
well, and this is no reason to keep them out of combat.
Privacy Infringement Hypothesis
The privacy issue on the battleeld is one that is not compelling
either, as soldiers are trained to operate in meager accommodations
where privacy is severely limited (Wan, 2006). Using the Persian Gulf
War as an example, Holm has further argued that men and women
serving side-by-side in the Gulf, demonstrated that they were capable
of working together as teams; they could be comrades without fraternizing; they could share tents without sharing beds; they could share
common dangers without feigning chivalry (Wan, 1992).
USAWC WOMEN IN COMBAT SURVEY RESEARCH
In 2007, a study was published by the USAWC in Carlisle, Pennsylvania, concerning opinions of senior service leaders from all branches
of the military regarding the issue of women in combat, entitled:
Women in Combat Compendium, at the Strategic Studies Institute, edited
by Johnson and Putko. The compendium included, among other things,
a quantitative research design, using a structured questionnaire. Details
of the survey and its conclusions are shown below.
1. Purpose: The USAWC conducted a survey to determine the perceptions
of U.S. Army War College Students23 regarding the ground combat
exclusion policy of female soldiers in the academic year 2006, published
in 2008.

120

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

2. Format: The survey was a census survey, but it was anonymous and voluntary. The survey consisted of 17 questions, with responses emplaced
within a Likert-type scale.
3. Population: N 300
4. Response Rate/Breakdown: A total of 300 students were surveyed with
a 78 percent response rate. The breakdown by service was as follows:
Army 76 percent; Air Force 8 percent; Marine Corps 6 percent;
Navy 5 percent; Coast Guard 1 percent, and Department of State
1 percent. Males 89 percent and Females 11 percent.
5. Main Outcome Measures: The survey showed that the Army does not
follow the ground combat exclusion policy, and that female soldiers are
engaged in ground combat given the asymmetric nature of the war in
Iraq and Afghanistan.
6. Individual Outcomes, Based on Particular Questions:
.

53 percent perceived the regulation that prohibits females collocating


with direct combat units is rarely enforced or not enforced at all.

70 percent strongly agreed that the regulation prohibiting collocation


of female soldiers with direct combat units should be revised.

63 percent strongly disagreed that female soldiers should NOT be


assigned to direct combat units due to lack of physical strength.

59 percent disagreed or strongly disagreed that female soldiers


should NOT be assigned to combat units due to a lack of co-ed life
support facilities.

57 percent disagreed or strongly disagreed that female soldiers


should NOT be assigned to direct combat units due to perceived lack
of public support.

59 percent disagreed or strongly disagreed that female soldiers


should NOT be assigned to direct combat units due to potential
problems in assimilation or bonding.

78 percent disagreed or strongly disagreed that female soldiers


should NOT be assigned to direct combat units to preclude exposure
to trauma associated with combat.

74 percent strongly agreed or agreed that all soldiers regardless of


gender should be assigned to positions for which they are qualied.

7. Conclusions of the Study: The study concluded that DoD should consider a revision of the female combat exclusion policy to reect a more
realistic view of the current asymmetric nature of warfare and the combat roles female soldiers are currently engaged in (USAWC, 2006).

CONCLUSION
As of September 30, 2008, DoD Manpower Research Statistics, 197,765
women serve on active duty, 163,414 are enlisted personnel, and 34,351
are ofcers. Since the September 11, 2001, attack on America, a total of 103

Women in the Military: Is It Time to Un-Gender Combat Roles?

121

women deployed to Afghanistan, Iraq and Kuwait have lost their lives
(Center for Military Readiness, 2009).24 Most of the soldiers serving in the
Middle East and Afghanistan are in harms way, whether they serve in
artillery, infantry, or armor or not. It is time to give them credit for doing
so, instead of letting their service to the nation y below the radar. Film
Directors Meg McLagan and Daria Sommers (2008) did just this in their
award-winning documentary lm, Lioness: There for the Action, Missing
from History. The lm documents the soldierly solidarity, faith, and duty
of ve of the earliest lionesses who wound up in active ghts in erce
neighborhood conicts in Iraq. Trying to restrict women from the risks of
combat is a awed position, as women already participate in the dangers
of war. Thus, the exclusion of women from combat policy should be
rescinded as voiced by Rep. Sandlin in a March 2009 showing of Lioness.
Is national security being undermined by having women in combat
positions? The positions held against women in combat do make a persuasive arguments, and at the same time evidence from the eld show
that women have served with honor, valor and dedication on the front
lines on a voluntary basis, as consistent with the Soldiers Creed that
each of them took as recruits: I stand ready to deploy, engage and
destroy the enemies of the United States of America in close combat.
This statement represents a key espoused value of the militaryservice
in combat, regardless of gender. Because of the attention this issue has
received, not changing espoused values to match enacted values might
be seen as organizational hypocrisy on the part of the DoD. It is time
to un-gender combat roles to reect reality, and thereby and move the
sacrices and valor of women from the place of invisibility to visibility.
In conclusion, some general recommendations can be extrapolated from
this examination of the issue:
. Recognize that strategy (espoused values) and practice (enacted values)
do drift apart as a natural consequence of multivariate intervening factors,
and that such has happened with respect to women in combat. Realize
that incongruence between espoused and enacted values undermine
organizational image, diminish trust among stakeholders and created confusions and contradictions that can degenerate into divisive rhetoric and
shadow boxing.
. Re-examine and Re-align organizational espoused values with enacted experience on a constant basis.
. Rescind the collocation and ground combat exclusion policy to eliminate
double standards, and adopt a gender-neutral policy in its place.

NOTES
1. Memorandum from Secretary of Defense Les Aspin to Armed Service
Secretaries, January 13, 1994, entitled, Direct Ground Combat Denition and
Assignment Rule.

122

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

2. These noncombat operations are known by many namesStability and


Support Operations, Operations Other Than War, and Peace Operations.
3. Congress, in an attempt to retain oversight of females in combat zones,
passed The National Defense Authorization Act (NDAA) in 2006. The NDAA
requires the Secretary of Defense to notify Congress of any changes to units
and assignments to which women are assigned.
4. Government Accounting Ofce. (1999). Gender Issues: Trends on the
Occupational Distribution of Military Women. Report to the Ranking Minority
member, Subcommittee on Readiness and Management Support, Committee on
Armed Services, U.S. Senate, Washington, DC.
5. Presidential Commission on the Assignment of Women in the Armed
Forces (PCAWAF) noted that there is little doubt that some women could
meet the physical standards for group combat, but the evidence shows that
few women possess the necessary physical requirements.
6. Presidential Commission on the Assignment of Women in the Armed
Forces (PCAWAF), November 15, 1992, Government Printing Ofce.
7. Merton, R. (1957). Social theory and social structure. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
8. Lipsky, M. (1980). Street-level bureaucracy: Dilemmas of the individual in
public services. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
9. USAWC Survey. (2008). Carlisle, PA: Strategic Studies Institute, USAWC.
Question #58.
10. For Female GIs Combat is a Fact. Retrieved from www.washingtonpost.
com/wpdyn/content/article/2005/05/12AR20050S51202002.html.
11. The Soldiers Creed. Retrieved from www.army.mil/SoldiersCreed/
ash_version/index.html.
12. Public Law 80625. The Womens Armed Forces Integration Act opened
up places both in the active military as well as reserves in all four services, but
it also placed an enlistment ceiling of 2 percent, and a 10 percent limit on
femaleenlisted personnel becoming ofcers.
13. Mitchell. B. (1998). Women in the military: Flirting with disaster. New York:
Regnery Press.
14. USAWC Survey. (2008). Strategic Studies Institute, USAWC, Carlisle, PA,
Question #49.
15. Putko, C. (2008). USAWC women in combat survey interpretation. Women
in Combat Compendium. Carlisle, PA: Strategic Studies Institute, Carlisle, PA.
16. Center for Military Readiness. (2006). Collocation is a term that is
widely used in discussions about women in combat. In a simple form, it means
that forward support company personnel embed with infantry and armor
maneuver battalions 100 percent of the time.
17. USAWC Survey. (2008). Carlisle, PA: Strategic Studies Institute, USAWC.
Question #29.
18. Wilkie, D., & Kreisher, O. (2005, May 18, 2005). Hunter plan bars women
from Army forward support. Copley News.
19. Schlaffy, P. Women in military combat? What it means for American
culture and defense. Heritage Lecture, #317.
20. Wan, S. (2006). Womens role in combat: Is ground combat the next
front? Journal of Academic Writing, 4.
21. Miller, L. (1997). Not just weapons for the weak: Gender harassment as a
form of protest for Army men. Sociology Quarterly, 60, 3738.

Women in the Military: Is It Time to Un-Gender Combat Roles?

123

22. Holm, J. (1992). Women in the military: An unnished revolution. Novato,


CA: Presidio Press.
23. Study body consists of all military services of ranks at Colonels and LT
Colonels.
24. Center for Military Readiness. (2009, March 19). Grim toll of military
women killed in war.

REFERENCES
Aspin, L. (1994). Direct ground combat denition and assignment rule. Memorandum, January 13, 1994. Washington, DC: Department of Defense.
Botters, R. J. (2008). How the Army can meet the intent of policy and statute
on ground combat exclusion for women. Women in Combat Compendium.
Carlisle Barracks, PA: Strategic Studies Institute, U.S. Army War College.
Center for Military Readiness (2006). Women in Combat: Background and facts
women in or near land combat. www.cmrnotes.org/WomenInCombat.
asp?docID271. Accessed August 25, 2009.
Coates, B. E. (2009, April). Concept of goal drift. Presented at The College
of Business and Public Administration, California State University, San
Bernadino, CA.
Field, K., & Nagl, J. (2001). Combat roles for women: A modest proposal.
Parameters. Carlisle Barracks, PA: U.S. Army War College.
Gouldner, A. (1959). Organizational analysis. In R. Merton, L. Broom, & L.
Cottrell (Eds.), Sociology Today (pp. 423426). New York: Basic Books.
Government Accounting Ofce. (1999). Gender issues: Trends on the occupational
distribution of military women. Report to the Ranking Minority member,
Subcommittee on Readiness and Management Support, Committee on
Armed Services, U.S. Senate, Washington, DC.
McSally, M. (2007, May 1). Women in combat: Is the current policy obsolete?
Duke Law Journal of Gender Law and Policy, 14, 10111059.
Merton, R. (1957). Social theory and social structure. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Miller, L. (1997). Not just weapons for the weak: Gender harassment as a form
of protest for Army men. Sociology Quarterly, 60, 3738.
Mitchell, B. (1998). Women in the military: Flirting with disaster. New York:
Regnery Press.
Presidential Commission on the Assignment of Women in the Armed Forces.
(1992). Washington, DC: Government Printing Ofce.
Schlaffy, P. (1991, June 3). Women in military combat? What it means for
American culture and defense. Heritage Lecture, 317.
The Soldiers Creed. Retrieved August 2, 1999 from www.army.mil/Soldiers
Creed/ash_version/index.html.
The Womens Armed Forces Integration Act. (1948). Public Law 80-625. June
12, 1948, U.S. Congress, Washington, DC.
Tyson, A. S. (2005). For female GIs, combat is a fact: Many duties put women
at risk despite restrictive policy. www.washingtonpost.com/wpdyn/content/article/2005/05/12AR20050S51202002.html.
Wan, S. (2006). Womens role in combat: Is ground combat the next front?
Journal of Academic Writing.

124

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Wilkie, D., & Kreisher, O. (2005, May 18). Hunter plan bars women from Army
forward support. Copley News, retrieved from http://www.signonsandiego.
com/uniontrib/20050518/news_1n18hunter.
Willens, J. (1996). Women in the military: Combat roles considered. Washington,
DC: Center for Defense Information.
United States Army War College (USAWC). Public Affairs Ofce (PAO) Fact
Sheet. Carlisle Barracks, PA: USAWC.
United States Army War College (USAWC). (2008). Survey. Carlisle, PA: Strategic Studies Institute, USAWC.
U.S. Congress. (2006). National Defense Authorization Act, H.R. 5122, also
known as the John Warner Act 2006.
Worth, R. (1999). Women in combat: The battle for equality. Berkeley Heightws,
NJ: Enslow Publishers.

Chapter 9

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and


Outcomes of Stigmatization
Rhonda M. Schultz
Kristin P. Beals

In undertaking an investigation into female same-sex sexual practices


across continents, nations, and cultures, it is imperative that investigators
rst garner an understanding of the specic political, historical, and
sociological factors surrounding these behaviors and/or identities. The
terms lesbian and bisexual used in Western society for the identities
surrounding some female same-sex sexual practices are tied to Western
ideas of individualism and are not applicable in cultures where sexual
identities may not be relevant. It is also important to keep in mind
that identity and individualism are cultural constructs and not delimiters
of advanced cultural evolution. The existence of a lesbian identity in
Western society does not indicate that we have progressed beyond
other societies in which no such moniker exists. In addition, the majority
of quantitative research available on outcomes of the repression of samesex sexual behavior has been conducted on samples comprised primarily
of individuals in Western nations and lesbian-identied women. While
the authors of this specic chapter have attempted to write and operate
with these understandings in mind, we cannot fully negate our own
individual histories and implicit cultural perspectives. Our resources,
expertise, and experiences have all been derived primarily from U.S.
sources and are therefore tinctured by the values and customs of Western society. Similarly, our backgrounds in research are limited to the
domain of psychology; therefore, our research perspectives may be more
heavily weighted in this area. While we have attempted to incorporate

126

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

some scholarship from queer and cultural studies into this chapter, the
bulk of the resources cited have been derived from psychological books
and journals.
UNDERSTANDING STIGMA
This chapter aims to discuss stigma against women who engage in
same-sex sexual practices or adopt a lesbian identity. Stigma is a
powerful tool of oppression throughout the world, which can be used
in many ways, for many reasons, and by many different groups. It is
important to rst identify what stigma is, where it comes from, and
what purpose it may serve in a given society. A common linguistic
mistake to make in English is to use the terms stigma, stereotype, prejudice, and discrimination interchangeably. While these terms do bear
some relationship to one another, they actually all represent somewhat
different constructs.
Stigma
Stigma refers to the devaluation of individuals within a group based
on some discernable characteristic or mark. In order for stigma to exist,
two fundamental elements must be present: there must be some distinguishing feature that is recognizable and serves as a delimiter of difference, and some devaluation of the individual (Dovidio, Major, &
Crocker, 2000). Devaluation of stigmatized individuals refers to the
beliefs that these persons are less than human, spoiled, defective, or
inferior. Goffman (1963) hypothesized that stigmas are attached to
three different types of conditions: physical deformities, tribal identities (race, religion, nationality, or sex), and blemishes of character
or weak will (imprisonment, unnatural passions, unemployment,
mental health problems). Lesbianism or same-sex sexuality would fall
under the nal category of weak will, as it has been viewed by many
as both an unnatural passion and a mental health problem, in spite
of the fact that the American Psychological Association has not considered it such in over 36 years.
Recently, it has been argued that the two most important features of
stigma are how visible or concealable they are and their perceived controllability (Dovidio et al., 2000). It appears that if one can hide their
stigmatized characteristic or if they cannot be blamed for having it,
they will be allowed to pass. This brings to light the two-sided nature of stigma. Stigmatization is not a one-way process, and the stigmatized are not simply helpless, passive objects upon which stigma is
placed. The reaction of the stigmatized is integral to the process of stigmatization (Goffman, 1963). Stigmatized individuals may shun their
stigma, nding it ludicrous or misplaced, thereby rendering it less

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes of Stigmatization

127

effective. They may also accept the stigma, internalize it, or focus heavily on it, increasing its power to harm. Internalization of the stigma
against lesbians by lesbians is commonly referred to as internalized
homonegativity and will be discussed in a later section.
Judging from the variety of conditions outlined earlier, it appears
obvious that most people will fall into at least one stigmatized group
at some point in their lives. Yet stigma persists in almost all societies
possibly because they may serve to enhance the lives of stigmatizers in
a number of ways (Dovidio et al., 2000). Stigmatizing others can raise
ones own self-esteem, relieve anxiety, and create a perception of control. In addition, stigmatization can also serve to reinforce a cultures
norms. We will explore the various norms and institutions within different cultures that contribute to the stigmatization of lesbians and
women who engage in same-sex sexual behavior.
Prejudice
Prejudice can be dened as an irrational and rigid belief about a
group of people, which can be either positive or negative. For example,
one can have a positive prejudice toward the group she considers herself a part of and a negative prejudice toward all members of another
group. Unlike stigma, prejudice can extend to any type of behavior or
identity and is not conned to social deviance. However, prejudice and
stigma are strongly linked to one another and share a great deal of
overlap in terms of application and measurement (Phelan, Link, &
Dovidio, 2008). Prejudice tends to be viewed as an attitude toward a
specic group of people. General acceptance for all heterosexuals and a
disdain for lesbians would be an example of such a prejudicial attitude.
People with prejudicial attitudes may ignore or fail to notice individual
differences between members of a group. Someone who holds a prejudicial attitude toward lesbians, for example, may view a shy, passive
woman who identies as lesbian, as not being a real lesbian. This
would be an example of a stereotype that all lesbians are aggressive
and pushy.
Stereotyping
Stereotyping is a cognitive process by which one attributes certain
characteristics to certain groups of people. According to Allport (1954),
the vast diversity and complexity of the world around us makes it
impossible to hold accurate concepts of everything in existence. While
human beings need some sense of understanding to function in the
world, complete understanding of everything is impossible. Therefore,
we tend to create some well-formed categories consisting of more complex, comprehensive understandings and other malformed categories

128

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

consisting of shaky and simplistic components (Allport, 1954). For


example, one may hold the stereotype that all bees are black and yellow. While having a simplistic understanding of bees is probably not
going to cause any trouble to the individual that holds this understanding or to bees in general, having a simplistic understanding about certain groups of people will. When a stereotype makes one unable to see
individual differences within groups of people, a stereotype becomes
maladaptive and likely harmful. The stereotype in the U.S. that lesbians are highly masculine women fails to address the vast differences
in gender expression within the lesbian community, errantly excluding
many individuals who identify as lesbian and including many individuals who do not.
The employment of prejudicial attitudes and stereotyping can lead
to discriminatory behaviors. Discrimination is dened as treating people differently because of their membership to a certain group. Similar
to prejudice, discrimination does not necessarily mean that one treats a
certain group negatively; it can also mean that a group is treated more
positively. Unfortunately, when referring to the treatment of lesbians,
discrimination generally refers to a negative behavior such as exclusion
or mistreatment. Discrimination can manifest interpersonally, institutionally, and/or culturally (Whitley & Kite, 2006). Individuals can be
discriminated against by other individuals and/or groups, policies and
laws, or cultural norms and values. Discriminatory behaviors against
lesbians can have severe consequences on the lives of the women they
are targeted toward and the societies in which they take place.

SOURCES OF STIGMA
Religion
In many countries, some religious groups provide a great deal of
fuel for the stigma against lesbians and women who engage in samesex sexual behavior (Miracle, Miracle, & Baumeister, 2003). While
same-sex sexual behavior is generally viewed as unnatural and intolerable by most religions, there are a few exceptions in the United States
where it is accepted, even blessed, according to the San Francisco
Chronicle (as cited in Miracle, et al., 2003). However, the report appears
bleak for most other religions, with homosexual acts being overwhelmingly condemned by virtually all other major religious institutions,
such as Catholicism, Mainstream Christianity, Mormonism, Muslimism,
and Orthodox and Conservative Judaism. In spite of reported ambiguity in the Quaran regarding the morality of homosexuality, 26
Muslim countries condemn homosexual acts, and 7 of them do so with
the threat of the death penalty (Helie, 2004). This widespread disdain
for homosexual identities and behaviors has been linked to Christian

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes of Stigmatization

129

and Islamic proselytizing throughout the world over the course of time
and the subsequent dissemination of repressive beliefs about sexuality
in general (Miracle et al., 2003). The far-ung disapproval and even
contempt expressed by many religious organizations has become a
painful source of stigma against lesbians and has often isolated them
from a powerful source of support and happiness: religion (Ferriss,
2002; Yakushko, 2005). In Yakushkos (2005) study of 82 lesbian, gay,
and bisexual (LGB) participants, those who attended churches they
perceived as conservative reported greater stress surrounding their
LGB identities and lower self-esteem than those attending churches
that fully accepted their LGB identity. The often painful experience
of being shunned by these hegemonic institutions can be heard in the
voice of a 26-year-old Kenyan woman:
I am Christian by religion and this has been a great challenge for me. All
my family have entered religion very deeply despite my father being initially Hindu, so I choose to ignore religion and follow what my heart
desires. I have always believed that if one has love in ones heart that is
what matters. I stopped going to church because I used to leave feeling
like a sinner. (Baraka & Morgan, 2005, p. 39)

Westernization
In some non-Western parts of the world, same-sex sexuality is considered a perversion sent from Western countries. Because Western
countries are viewed as being accepting of homosexuality (however
inaccurate this perception may be), same-sex sexual practices are often
viewed as being another grand overindulgence of the decadent West.
This means that oftentimes same-sex sexuality will be stigmatized as a
betrayal of ones nation or heritage (Blackwood & Wieringa, 1999). Little is known about the effects of this specic stigma on lesbians in
these areas, but some information has been garnered from interviews
and other research. One example is that of reactions to a hate-lled
anti-gay speech made by Zimbabwean president Mugabe at the Zimbabwean International Book Fair in 1995. In addition to a number of
cruel, scathing comments made in this speech about the LGB community, Mugabe articulated his sentiment that same-sex sexual behavior is
the sole purveyance of Americans and that such ways are stupid and
foolish (Aarmo, 1999). While many members of the lesbian community reported being scared and forced into hiding, some lesbians in
attendance at the book fair stated that they had felt free and nally
able to express themselves (Aarmo, 1999).
A recent example of same-sex sexuality being viewed as the inuence of other nations can be intuited from statements made by the
Ethics Minister of Uganda. The United Nations (UN) has recently been

130

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

organizing nations to help decriminalize homosexuality around the


world, an effort that has been met with anger by some nations where
same-sex sexual practices are a punishable crime. One article reported
that in response to the UNs efforts, Ugandas Ethics Minister, James
Nsaba Buturo announced that the UN was endorsing what he called
attempts by some nations to impose homosexuality on the rest of us
(UN spreading homosexuality, 2009). Male homosexuality is explicitly
illegal in Uganda and is punishable by life imprisonment. While the
law does not criminalize lesbianism specically, it can be intuited
as such (Nagadya & Morgan, 2005). Homosexuality is considered unAfrican and a threat to the family in Uganda. This is also true for
many other parts of Africa, except South Africa where gay marriage
and adoption by same-sex partners are both legal practices.
TRIPLE MINORITY STATUS
Recently, research investigating the experiences of ethnic minority
LGB individuals in Western countries has begun to surface, which may
lend some insight into certain cultural beliefs. However, the literature
is scant, as most researchers interested in LGB populations have generally failed to address this aspect of identity in their research. Lesbians
of color experience triple minority status or triple jeopardy in Western
society because of their status as sexual, ethnic, and gender minorities.
The experience of being a lesbian woman of color in a sexist, racist,
heterocentrist society could be a difcult experience for many lesbians
of color. This subject is a tricky research topic, as it is most likely that
these identities are not islands, but are interrelated and share some
overlap. For example, it would seem difcult to isolate ones lesbian
identity from ones female identity, and because of this it may also be
difcult to separately test the factors surrounding each (Bowleg, 2008).
Many ethnic and racial minority groups in America may suffer from
what is called fear of extinction (Greene, 1995). This fear leads members of these groups to believe that reproduction is a necessary part of
a womans existence because without it, the survival of the group is
threatened. The stigma against lesbianism may be aided by these specic cultural beliefs and create additional stress against racial and ethnic minority women (Greene, 1995). Another issue relevant to ethnic
and racial minority lesbians is that of the perceived whiteness of the
gay and lesbian identities in the United States (Harper & Schneider,
2003). The homosexual identity in the United States has been painted
and perceived as a predominantly Caucasian identity and as such,
adopting this identity as a racial or ethnic minority may be perceived
as a betrayal to ones community (Greene, 1995). Some ethnic minority
lesbians voice the belief that they have to make a choice between their
sexual identity and their ethnic identity, seeming to feel that the two

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes of Stigmatization

131

cannot coexist within one person (Espin, 1987). In addition to these


stressors, issues of class and religion can also complicate the lives of
many women living as triple minorities.

MINORITY STRESS THEORY


In recent years, researchers interested in the health and well-being
of LGB individuals have begun using the concept of minority stress to
guide some of their research. The framework of minority stress delineates the various sources of stress that LGB persons may be vulnerable
to and the techniques these individuals may employ to cope with this
stress (Brooks, 1981; Meyer, 2003). Meyer has conceptualized LGB minority stress as a group of three different sources of stress: the experience of stigma and discrimination, anticipated stigma/discrimination,
and internalized homonegativity. These experiences can lead individuals to utilize coping or resilience mechanisms as a means by which to
counteract the negative effects of minority stress (Brooks, 1981; Russell,
2003).

THE EXPERIENCE OF DISCRIMINATION


One of the key components to minority stress is the experience of
discrimination. Discrimination against LGB individuals occurs in any
number of situations, such as employment, housing, health care, and
ostracism from friends and family or legal rights such as adoption,
marriage, and military service. In extreme cases, discrimination can
mean that same-sex sexual relationships and behaviors have been
made illegal and punishable by imprisonment, violence, or death.
There are also instances of groups and individuals within societies
who beat, rape, or even murder lesbians or women who engage in
same-sex sexual behaviors. A 2001 Kaiser Family Foundation report
found that a majority of the general American public believes that LGB
individuals experience a lot of prejudice and discrimination. When
polled about their perceptions of the amount of discrimination they
believe they have experienced, 85 percent of lesbians and gay men
claimed to have experienced some kind of prejudice or discrimination
in their lifetimes (Kaiser Family Foundation, 2001). In this same study,
76 percent of LGB participants reported being discriminated against or
knowing someone who has been discriminated against in the areas of
employment (nding or keeping a job), applying to school (university
or other), housing (renting or buying), health care, or military service.
In addition to these disappointing ndings, the study also reported
that 74 percent of LGB participants reported having been verbally
assaulted and 32 percent had been the targets of physical violence.

132

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Expected Discrimination
The experience of discrimination may lead those who are discriminated against to come to expect discrimination from others. The belief
that one will come to experience prejudice or discrimination has been
referred to as stigma consciousness (Pinel, 1999). In Pinels work, lesbians and gay men high in stigma consciousness were more likely to
have greater self-consciousness and to worry about how others viewed
them. Other studies have linked stigma consciousness to a variety of
negative outcomes. One study found that stigma consciousness is
related to internalized homophobia, physical complaints, lesbianrelated stress (stress perceived to be brought on specically because of
lesbian identity), negative mood, and intrusive thoughts (Lewis, Derlega, Clarke, & Kuang, 2006).

Internalized Homonegativity
The stigma attached to homosexuality is often consciously or unconsciously adopted by the stigmatized themselves. When LGB people
internalize the stigmas and negative attitudes held by society about
their behaviors and identities, it is referred to as internalized homonegativity, internalized homophobia, or internalized heterosexism. The
experience of internalized homonegativity has been associated with a
greater risk for a number of mental and physical health problems. In a
sample of 157 U.S. lesbians, researchers found that internalized homonegativity was correlated with a number of mental health problems. In
this study, which developed a separate scale for lesbian internalized
homonegativity (LIH), LIH was highly correlated with lower selfesteem, depression, lower satisfaction with social support, and a higher
number of somatic complaints (Szymanski, Chung, & Balsam, 2001).
Internalized homonegativity has also been correlated with a number of
self-destructive behaviors in lesbians, including suicide, self-mutilation,
risky sexual behaviors, and alcoholism (Williamson, 2000). These studies lead us to believe that internalized homonegativity has the potential
to be a extremely destructive force in the lives of some lesbians and
most likely occurs because of the stigma attached to same-sex practice.
The negative effects of internalized homonegativity can also carry
over into lesbian relationships. Holding negative beliefs about ones own
sexuality may cause some LGB individuals to feel less satised or positive about their romantic relationships. LIH has been found to be associated with a decrease in satisfaction with romantic relationships and
a decrease in attraction toward ones partner (Mohr & Daly, 2008). In
a qualitative study of 40 same-sex couples, half of which were female, a
little less than half of the couples reported themes of internalized homonegativity toward themselves or their relationships (Rostosky, Riggle,
Gray, and Hatton, 2007). One in four of couples in this study articulated

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes of Stigmatization

133

low expectations for the durability of their relationship. One couple cited
the lack of social support around them stating that this factor may cause
them to feel that the relationship was bound for failure (Rostosky et al.,
2007). It seems logical that in a society where there is not only little support for your relationship, but outright hostility toward it, the possibility
of a break up may seem more likely.
Internalized homonegativity has also been highly correlated with
intimate partner violence among lesbian couples (Balsam & Syzmanski,
2005). It is believed that individuals that hold negative beliefs about
homosexuals are more likely to act violently toward them. This can
unfortunately mean that LGB persons high in internalized homonegativity are more likely to act violently toward members of their own
community, including their partners (Balsam & Syzmanski, 2005). Intimate partner violence may be yet another devastating force threatening
the health and safety of lesbians and their romantic relationships.
The experience of internalized homonegativity may not be isolated
to individualistic, Western cultures. It cannot be known at this time
whether lesbians or women who engage in same-sex sexual behaviors
in collectivistic cultures experience internalized homophobia because
there is a paucity of literature or research on this subject. There is some
evidence that these processes may be present in the Damara community of Namibia, as might be intuited by some excerpts from interviews
conducted with women who have sex with women there:
She hated me, sometimes the same kind hate each other. Once she called me
and beat me up in her classroom, saying What kind of child is this that is
acting like a boy?. . . This teacher, she became the school principal there and
is now together with another sister. (Khaxas & Wieringa, 2005, p. 137)
She told me about caressing and kissing and she told me that she felt that
she was my girlfriend and that I was her man, but because of youthful
shame I did not say something . . . When I got home I thought, to do this in
the Bible schoolyard I have done a big sin. (Khaxas & Wieringa, 2005, p. 136)

Although there are a number of texts investigating the stigma


against same-sex practices in various parts of the world, few address
the issue of the internalization of this stigma. The question as to
whether this experience is tied to identity seems to remain to be
answered. This answer may be difcult to obtain given the quiet, hidden nature of same-sex practices in many cultures, which could be a
reason for the gap in research in this area.

CONSEQUENCES OF STIGMA
Stigma and discrimination impact lesbian and bisexual women in a
number of ways. In the next section of this chapter we will discuss the
legal, nancial, social, and health impacts for lesbian and bisexual women.

134

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Legal and Economic Impact for Same-sex Female Couples


Because lesbian couples are denied a number of rights that are
afforded heterosexual couples, all-female couples have increased nancial and legal burdens. For example, in states and countries that do not
give same-sex couples marriage or marriage-like rights, gay and lesbian
couples often have to pay lawyers to secure some of the most basic
legal protections (e.g. living wills, power of attorneys, health directives). Same-sex couples also face additional legal costs in completing
second-parent adoptions. These same costs are not accrued by heterosexual couples.
In the United States and many other countries, tax systems treat
same-sex couples as if they were complete strangers. In a recent report
from the Williams Institute, Goldberg and Badgett (2009) report that
same-sex couples often pay more in taxes. This occurs for a number of
reasons. First, same-sex couples in the United States are taxed on
health insurance benets provided to a partner by an employer. This
adds an estimated $1,000 to the taxes of same-sex families. Additionally, same-sex couples often must pay estate taxes or gift taxes that opposite-sex couples are exempt from paying. Same-sex couples who pay
taxes their whole lives are denied equal social security benets upon
retirement and the death of a partner. This is estimated to cost the surviving partner of a same-sex couple on average about $8,000 a year
(Furnas & Rosenthal, 2009). This brief summary highlights some of the
inequities in the nancial and legal system between same-sex and different-sex couples.

Social Costs for Sexual Minority Women


In addition to the legal and economic costs of stigma against lesbian
and bisexual women, there are innumerable social costs. Being stigmatized for being a sexual minority is unlike being stigmatized because of
ones racial, ethnic, or religious group membership, because often a
sexual minority individual does not share that identity with any immediate family members. Instead of feeling connected to ones family, lesbian and bisexual individuals may feel very isolated. Often times,
sexual minority individuals must create their own communities of supportive others. In one study of gay and lesbian participants, it was
found that only 33 percent of mothers and fathers were rated as very
accepting of their childrens sexual orientation (Beals & Peplau, 2006).
Added to the possible isolation from family members, lesbian and
bisexual women may also feel isolated from their spiritual or religious
communities. As discussed earlier, traditional religious views often
endorse anti-gay attitudes. It can be very difcult for sexual minority
women if they feel they must choose between being true to themselves

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes of Stigmatization

135

and their religious background. Bryant and Demian, (1994) found that
out of 12 possible sources of support, sexual minority individuals rated
church as the least supportive and the most hostile. In a qualitative
study of 14 same-sex couples and their lived experiences with religion,
Rostosky, Riggle, Brodnicki, and Olson (2008) concluded that heavy
reliance on social support from family and religious communities may
exacerbate minority stress in ethnic minority and religious minority
GLB individuals who want to integrate their religious and same-sex
relational values.
Lesbian women may also feel socially isolated at places of employment. In most states in the United States, lesbian and bisexual women
can still be red for their identity and as a result, may hide their identity, thus isolating themselves. This experience is epitomized in the
U.S. militarys Dont Ask, Dont Tell policy, which forces U. S. service members to silence themselves, forgo social support for their identity and the possibility of building community. The social costs to
lesbian and bisexual women are often innumerable.
Mental and Physical Health
Sexual minority women have higher rates of a number of physical
health and mental health concerns. Actual and perceived discrimination has been found to account for a large proportion of the disparity
(Mays & Cochran, 2001). We will rst describe several of the barriers
to health prevention and care services. Next we will discuss the known
mental health and physical health disparities.
History of the Study of Lesbian Health Disparities
The burgeoning gay rights movement in the United States in the
1960s and 1970s began to take notice of the existence of health disparities for sexual minority women and men. As a result, a number of
community-based health centers began to emerge to provide sensitive
care to the sexual minority population (Mayer et al., 2008). At the same
time, a number of clinicians and doctors began to recognize that their
sexual minority patients had unique needs because of the hostile and
homophobic environment in which they live. In 1973, the American
Psychiatric Association recognized that homosexuality was not a psychiatric illness and removed the diagnosis from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM). However, removal from the manual did not
eliminate the prejudice and sometimes toxic environments in which
sexual minority individuals live.
The Institute of Medicine in 1999 issued a call for more populationbased research to understand the clinical problems in the lesbian community. Community groups lobbied hard to have the government

136

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

follow through on the Institute of Medicines report. Importantly, the


Department of Health and Human Services recognized the importance
of this and included lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender (LGBT)
health in their goals for the Healthy People 2010 initiative. This momentum has led to increased information and educational materials
(e.g., The Fenway Guide to Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender
Health) for caregivers, physicians, and clinicians.
Barriers to Health Care
Research has demonstrated that lesbian and bisexual women often
encounter barriers to receiving timely and adequate health care (Diamant, Wold, Spritzer, & Gelberg, 2000; Mayer et al., 2008). For example,
in Diamant and collegues study, only 34 percent of heterosexual
women reported difculty getting care, while 47 percent of lesbians
and 53 percent of bisexual women reported this problem.
Mayer et al. (2008) outlined four broad areas of barriers to accessing
healthcare. These included (1) reluctance to disclose sexual orientation
to providers, (2) lack of LGBT knowledge and competency on the part
of providers, (3) structural barriers to health insurance and visiting and
decision making rights for LGBT people and their partners, and (4)
lack of culturally appropriate prevention services. As Mayer and colleagues point out, each of these is troublesome alone, but the combination of these can be disastrous for the health and well-being of sexual
minority individuals.
Often, sexual minority women do not disclose their sexual orientation to a health care provider. These decisions often affect health care
from a very early age. In a study of disclosure to physicians by LGB
youth, it was found that only 35 percent had disclosed their sexual orientation to their physician (Meckler, Elliot, Kanouse, Beals, & Schuster,
2006). Participants cited condentiality concerns as a reason for not disclosing. Interestingly, participants reported that they would be more
likely to disclose if the physician would just ask. In another study,
MacEwan (1994) found that to avoid the possibility of biased care, lesbian and gay participants did not disclose their sexual orientation in
alcohol treatment settings. Nondisclosure may mean missed opportunities for a provider to offer specic care such as appropriate health and
sex education, screening tests, and discuss individual risks (Meckler et
al., 2006).
Of course, disclosure will only be benecial to the receipt of appropriate and adequate health care if the physician responds in a culturally sensitive and knowledgeable way. Several studies have found that
physicians, like other people, have biases toward sexual minorities.
Research has demonstrated that even well-intentioned service providers may behave differently with lesbian patients or clients (Brown,

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes of Stigmatization

137

1996). In fact, Cochran (2001) noted that several researchers have identied attitudinal and behavioral responses on the part of providers to
disclosure by sexual minority clients. For example, a practitioner may
focus too much or too little on sexual orientation in therapy (MacEwan,
1994), avoid topics that make him/her uncomfortable (Hardman, 1997),
and be unable to recall information that the client provided (Gelso, Fassinger, Gomez, & Latts, 1995). All of these consequences of homonegativity are thought to result in a lower quality of care (Garnets,
Hancock, Cochran, Goodchilds, & Peplau, 1991). One consequence of
lesbian and bisexual women having physicians that lack knowledge
and sensitivity is lower satisfaction with source of health care (Diamant
et al., 2000). This lower satisfaction may in turn lead to sexual minority
women choosing to delay or avoid health care.
The third barrier is more structural and legal in nature than the rst
two. Cochran and colleagues (2001) found that compared to similarly
matched women, lesbians were less likely to have health insurance
than their heterosexual counterparts. Many sexual minority women do
not have access to quality health care because they are denied insurance benets through a partners employment. Even if an employer
allows a same-sex partner to be insured, the federal government then
taxes the benet as income, thus costing the couple valuable resources and possible forcing some people to forego coverage. In a
study utilizing the federal governments population survey, Ash and
Badgett (2006) found that 18 percent of individuals in same-sex romantic relationships lacked health care compared to 11 percent of married
heterosexual individuals. Furthermore, even when a partner has insurance, the other partner in a same-sex relationship is still uninsured 15
percent of the time, compared to 4 percent of married partners. In
other words, lesbian and bisexual women are less likely to be insured
than their heterosexual counterparts. As a result, lesbian and bisexual
women are much more likely to forego health services because of nancial reasons (Diamant et al., 2000). This translates into less preventative care and a more likely chance that acute issues become chronic.
In addition, because many states to not recognize same-sex relationships with the same legal standing as marriage, partners are often not
allowed visiting rights and decision-making powers, which can impede
care provided. This varies greatly state by state, but most states do not
have any legal protection for same-sex couples. In the United States,
only four states allow same-sex marriages (with another 10 having
some form of legal protection). Around the world, only seven countries
perform same-sex marriages, with another four recognizing same-sex
marriages performed elsewhere. Women who are in relationships with
other women are often denied the legal protections that are granted to
heterosexual relationships. Some of these legal protections aid in
receiving quality and culturally sensitive health care.

138

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Finally, Mayer et al. (2008) identied the lack of LGBT-specic prevention services as a barrier to quality health care. There is a dearth of
services that deal with the effects of a stigmatizing society. These
include a lack of services that target negative health behaviors and the
increased prevalence of mental and physical health issues in the LGBT
community.
In summary, because of the stigma of a sexual minority status, many
barriers exist to receiving timely and adequate healthcare. This failure
to receive timely and adequate healthcare may translate into greater
mental and physical health disparities between heterosexual and lesbian/bisexual women. In the following section, the research on existing
health disparities is described.
Mental Health Disparities
Early work by Evelyn Hooker in the 1950s with small community
samples found no differences in the psychological adjustment of sexual
minority individuals and heterosexuals (Hooker, 1993). This work was
critical to the removal of homosexuality as a psychiatric diagnosis
(Cochran, 2001). However, in the years since Hookers important work,
large representative samples have revealed that lesbians and gay men
have a higher prevalence rate of certain mental health disorders. In
particular, sexual minority individuals appear especially susceptible to
the disorders that are most sensitive to minority stress (Cochran, 2001).
These include depression, anxiety, and substance abuse disorders
(Cochran, Sullivan, & Mays, 2003).
A number of large-scale, nationally representative samples have
found that lesbian and bisexual women have higher rates of certain psychiatric syndromes than heterosexual women (e.g., Cochran & Mays,
2000; Gilman, Cochran, & Mays, 2001; Sandfort, de Graaf, Bijl, & Schnabel, 2001). Furthermore, lesbian women were more likely than heterosexual women to have used mental health services in the prior year
(Cochran, 2001; Cochran et al., 2003). In a large survey of women in Los
Angeles County, Diamant and Wold (2003) found that lesbians reported
more poor mental health days when compared to heterosexual women.
A number of studies have used population-based surveys to extrapolate
rates of disorders among lesbian and bisexual women. The evidence is
quite strong that sexual minority women experience higher rates of certain disorders as a result of prejudice and stigma in society.
Depression and Anxiety Disorders
In a nationally representative sample of middle-aged adults in the
contiguous United States, lesbian and bisexual women reported higher
prevalence of anxiety disorder and greater likelihood of having a second

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes of Stigmatization

139

co-morbid disorder compared to heterosexual women (Cochran et al.,


2003). In a more recent study of Latino and Asian American adults
(Cochran, Mays, Alegria, Ortega, & Takeuchi, 2007), 6.4 percent of
female participants identied as lesbian or bisexual (or reported recent
same-sex sexual behavior). Those participants identied as lesbian or
bisexual were more likely to have evidenced recent and lifetime history
of depressive disorders compared to their heterosexual counterparts.
Furthermore, research has found that lesbian women who report
depression are more likely to be taking medication for depression than
heterosexual women who report depression (Diamant & Wold, 2003).
This may signal that the lesbian women are experiencing more severe
depression or have fewer ways to cope with depression than heterosexual women.
This depression may at times manifest itself in a higher incidence of
suicide attempts among sexual minority women. These data can be
very difcult to interpret for a number of reasons, including identifying
sexual orientation of participants. However, there is some evidence that
lesbian and bisexual women may have slightly higher suicide attempt
rates compared to heterosexual women. In a study of Asian and Latino
women, those identifying as lesbian or bisexual were marginally more
likely to have attempted suicide than the heterosexual participants
(Cochran et al., 2007).
Drug Dependency
Often individuals use and abuse drugs as a way to cope with depression, anxiety, and stigma. A number of research studies have indicated
that women who have female sexual partners are at greater risk of illicit
drug use (e.g., Cochran, Ackerman, Mays, & Ross, 2004; Crothers, Haller, Benton, & Haag, 2008; Sandfort et al., 2001). Reports have provided
greater evidence of lifetime use of a number of drugs including stimulants, tranquilizers, cocaine, and marijuana. Similar ndings also
emerged for use in the prior month. This use seems to translate into
more drug dependency disorders. For example, Cochran and Mays
(2000) found that the lesbian women in their sample were more likely to
have drug and alcohol dependency syndromes. In a nationally representative sample of Asian and Latino adults, lesbian and bisexual women
were more likely to have a recent history of a drug use disorder (Cochran et al., 2007). In a study of 2,011 lesbian and bisexual women living
in California (Corliss, Grella, Mays, & Cochran, 2006), it was found that
27 percent of women showed lifetime patterns of drug use that were
categorized as either high or moderate risk. Approximately 40 percent of
women with more problematic drug use had sought professional services. Interestingly, for these women, living with a romantic partner acted
as a protective factor against drug use.

140

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

In a more generalized study that compared lesbian and bisexual


women from seven different studies around the United States with
standardized samples of women, it was found that lesbian and bisexual women had greater alcohol use than the general population of
women (Cochran et al., 2000). In the United States, the research suggests that women who identify as sexual minorities use and abuse alcohol and drugs at higher rates than women who identify as
heterosexual. It appears that living in a society that stigmatizes women
for forming relationships with other women may push these women to
use drugs and alcohol as one way to cope with the stigma. As we will
see in the next section, drug and alcohol use may translate into greater
physical health concerns among sexual minority women.
Physical Health Disparities
In addition to mental health disparities between heterosexual and
sexual minority women, research has also found a number of physical
health disparities. The root of these disparities is complex, but part of
the explanation is likely the barriers to health care discussed above and
specic behavioral risk factors and preventative practices associated
with a sexual minority identity.
BEHAVIORAL RISK FACTORS AND PREVENTATIVE
HEALTH PRACTICES
A number of studies indicate that lesbian and bisexual women may
be more likely to engage in behaviors known to be risk factors for a
number of physical health concerns. For example, in a study that compared self-identied lesbians with a national sample of women, it was
found that the lesbian women were more likely to be smokers than heterosexual women (Aaron et al., 2001; Diamant et al., 2000; Gruskin,
Greenwood, Matevia, Pollack, & Bye, 2007). Smoking is a known risk
factor for a number of physical health concerns including pulmonary
and cardiac diseases and cancer. In addition, lesbian and bisexual
women were also more likely to drink alcohol compared to control
samples. In Diamant and colleagues study, it was found that almost
three-fourths of lesbians and bisexuals used alcohol compared to onehalf of heterosexuals. Furthermore, lesbians and bisexual women were
much more likely to report drinking 3 or more drinks almost daily.
Studies have also found that lesbians are more likely to be overweight or obese compared to controlled samples of heterosexuals
(Cochran et al., 2000), which is a known risk factor for a number of
health conditions. For example in one study, 48 percent of lesbian
women compared to only 32 percent of women in the national sample
were overweight (body mass index [BMI]  27.3 kg/m2). Contradictory

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes of Stigmatization

141

to past reports, the lesbian women in this sample appeared to participate in physical activity at about the same rate as the national sample
of women. In fact, lesbian women were slightly more likely than other
women to report vigorous activity. Additional research is needed to
sort out the research on weight and exercise. Despite an increased need
for health screening and preventative services, research indicates that
lesbian and bisexual women may fail to obtain proper care.
As discussed earlier, whether for fear of discrimination from a doctor, a lack of health insurance, or a misunderstanding of the risks, lesbian and bisexual women often are found to have poorer rates of
preventative health screening compared to heterosexual women. Two
large studies have found that lesbian women were less likely to have
had a papanicolaou (pap) test in the past two years compared to heterosexual women (Aaron et al., 2001). Failure to see gynecologists and
have recommended examinations is of particular alarm because many
lesbian women do not have a history of oral contraceptives, pregnancy,
and breastfeeding, all of which are protective factors for ovarian and
other cancers (American Cancer Society, n.d.).
The combination of risky behaviors and a failure to obtain preventative health screenings may account for some of the physical health disparities that are discussed in the next sections.
OUTCOMES
Although the evidence is mixed, a number of studies have found
that lesbian and bisexual women have higher incidence of a number of
physical health outcomes. A few of the studies are reviewed below.
In one study of 4,023 women who participated in the Los Angeles
County Health Survey, it was found that lesbian and bisexual women
had a higher rate of being diagnosed with heart disease. Importantly,
this nding persisted even when controlling for age, race, education,
income, health insurance, tobacco use, and obesity (Diamant & World,
2003). Furthermore controlling for those same variables, bisexual
women also reported more poor physical health days compared to
the heterosexual sample.
In another large-scale study that included heterosexual, lesbian, bisexual, and heterosexual-with-homosexual-experience women, several physical health differences emerged (Cochran & Mays, 2007). For example,
bisexual women reported a greater number of health conditions than heterosexual women. These health conditions included digestive complaints,
back problems, and chronic fatigue syndrome. Heterosexual women with
homosexual experiences reported more back problems and asthma than
other heterosexual women. And nally, lesbian women reported having
arthritis more than other heterosexual women. However, most of these
health disparities became nonsignicant once the researcher factored in

142

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

psychological distress. The root of the problem may lie in the experience
of minority stress that contributes to psychological distress. As discussed
earlier, both lesbian women and bisexual women report higher levels of
psychological distress than heterosexual women.
Drawing from psychological research on women who identify as lesbian or bisexual, it is clear that the impact of stigma is far-reaching.
Stigma effects sexual minority women in a number of ways including
in legal domains, in terms of economic resources, family and friend
relationships, and in terms of health and well-being. More research is
needed to understand how the consequences of stigma and minority
stress are experienced around the world. The next section discusses
ways in which lesbian and bisexual women may cope with stigma and
minority stress.
COPING
The experience of minority stress may lead lesbians and women
who engage in same-sex sexual behavior to adapt coping strategies to
deal with the stressors they experience. Considering the weight of the
research we have presented up until this time, the outlook may seem
bleak for members of the LGB community. However, many of the individuals within these communities do not express high levels of the
negative outcomes associated with stigma and minority stress (Herek &
Garnets, 2007). Numerous means of coping with minority stress have
been seen in gay, lesbian and bisexual populations in the United States,
which may serve as a buffer against some of the aforementioned negative outcomes.
Recently, various anti-gay legislative efforts in individual states within
the United States have provided researchers with the opportunity to
examine the coping skills employed by sexual minority persons. At the
forefront of this research is the work performed by Russell (2003) investigating responses to an amendment to the Colorado State Constitution
stripping sexual minority persons of protections as minorities, including
nondiscrimination. This amendment was designed to remove sexual
minorities from protected status and make it illegal for them to le complaints based on discriminatory treatment as minority persons. A study
of LGB people in Colorado revealed that this anti-gay campaign was
very stressful for many participants. The coping skills employed by
these participants fell neatly into 5 categories: movement perspective,
confronting internalized homonegativity, expression of affect, connecting
to community, and successful witnessing (Russell, 2003).
Taking a movement perspective entails stepping back from an antigay experience and seeing it as a small part of a larger picture, progressing toward optimal changes (Russell, 2003). In other words, individuals taking a movement perspective may not focus on a specic

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes of Stigmatization

143

incident such as being the victim of a derogatory slur, but may instead
consider it to be a lingering fragment of a gradually dissolving heterosexist society. This way of thinking could be very helpful to persons
experiencing set backs such as the passage of anti-gay legislation. The
movement perspective can be seen in statements such as the following:
I am 100 percent opposed to Amendment 2 (like I really need to say that)
but I think that its doing good for the gay/lesbian/bisexual community
in the long run. People are now being forced to deal with something
theyd rather ignore. Theyre being educated and taught that there is
more to homosexuality that perverted sex. This will eventually be seen
as the catalyst for the successful civil rights movement of the 90s.
(Russell, 2000, p. 198).

As part of the broader theme of seeing an anti-gay action as a small


part of a larger picture, individuals who take the movement perspective may articulate beliefs about these actions as being helpful for the
communitys understanding of homophobia and its implications. Taking a movement perspective may help to alleviate the powerlessness
and isolation caused by anti-gay actions. By focusing more on ones
self-efcacy and actions, LGB individuals may be able to diffuse the
negative consequences associated with anti-gay efforts aimed at them
(Russell, 2003). In addition, the ability to have your voice heard and
understood by others (successful witnessing) can serve as another form
of resilience. The social support offered in these situations can help
individuals to reduce feelings of powerlessness and isolation that could
result from anti-gay actions.
While Russells research found that anti-gay actions (campaigns and
legislation) could lead to an increase in internalized homonegativity for
some individuals, she also found the opposite. For some individuals,
anti-gay actions serve as a forum in which they can investigate and
confront their own internalization of negative beliefs about the LGB
community and its members (Russell, 2003). Some participants in Russells study reported that the anti-gay campaign and amendment
allowed them to examine their own beliefs about their community and
themselves. This experience may have allowed them to dispel some internal shame surrounding their identities. The act of coming out as a
result of these anti-gay efforts may be evidence of such a shedding of
internalized negative beliefs (Russell, 2003).
Another interesting nding from Russells research was that while
certain negative emotions such as anger could be stressful to participants, they could also be evidence of resilience when expressed in a
useful way. It is possible that if lesbians can harness anger, sadness,
and negative affect in a way that promotes action, these emotions can
help them to become more resilient and relieve stress associated with

144

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

anti-gay actions. In addition to this, interaction with the LGB community may also serve as a way to battle the stress of anti-gay actions.
The community can be both a source of information about being LGB
and a forum for activism, both of which may help buffer stress (Herek &
Garnets, 2007; Russell, 2003). Participation in the community may foster a collective identity because it expands an individuals resources to
the level of the entire group (Herek & Garnets, 2007). As a part of this
community, individuals have a new set of norms with which to compare themselves against, norms which are more accepting of their sexuality or sexual identity than those of the mainstream, heterosexual
community (Meyer, 2003). Through the lesbian community, lesbians
can engage in fruitful social comparison outside of mainstream heterosexual norms, which can lead to the amelioration of the stress that feeling shunned or outcast by the mainstream community can create
(Meyer, 2003).
While there may be many obstacles for lesbians and women who
engage in same-sex sexual practices around the world, human beings
are also a very adaptive species and may sometimes be able to combat
these negative inuences with the use of coping strategies. The formation of communities of like-minded women and individual-level coping
strategies may be powerful sources of strength and resilience in the
face of great disparity for women engaging in same-sex sexual practices and identities throughout the world.

REFERENCES
Aarmo, M. (1999). How homosexuality became un-African: The case
of Zimbabwe. In E. Blackwood & S. E. Wieringa (Eds.), Female desires
(pp. 3963). New York: Columbia University Press.
Aaron, D. J., Markovic, N., Danielson, M. E., Honnold, J. A., Janosky, J. E., &
Schmidt, N. J. (2001). Behavioral risk factors for disease and preventative
health practices among lesbians. American Journal of Public Health, 91,
972975.
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. New York: Perseus.
American Cancer Society (n.d.). Lesbians and bisexual women: Access to care
and cancer disparity fact sheet. Retrieved April 6, 2009, from http://www.
glbthealth.org/documents/FactSheetRevised-LesbiansandBisexualWomen.
pdf.
Ash, M. A., & Badgetet, M. V. L. (2006). Separate and unequal: The effect of
unequal access to employment-based health insurance on same-sex and
unmarried different-sex couples. Contemporary Economic Policy, 24, 582599.
Balsam, K. F., & Syzmanski, D. M. (2005). Relationship quality and domestic
violence in womens same-sex relationships: The role of minority stress.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 258269.
Baraka, N., & Morgan, R. (2005). I want to marry the woman of my choice
without fear of being stoned: Female marriages and bisexual women in
Kenya. In R. Morgan & S. Wieringa (Eds.), Tommy boys, lesbian men and

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes of Stigmatization

145

ancestral wives: Female same-sex practices in Africa (pp. 2550). Johannesburg:


Jacana Media.
Beals, K. P., & Peplau, L. A. (2006). Disclosure patterns within social networks
of gay men and lesbians. Journal of Homosexuality, 51, 101120.
Blackwood, E., & Wieringa, S. E. (1999). Sapphic shadows: Challenging the
silence in the study of sexuality. In E. Blackwood & S. E. Wieringa (Eds.),
Female desires (pp. 3963). New York: Columbia University Press.
Bowleg, L. (2008). When black lesbian woman? black lesbian woman: The
methodological issues of qualitative and quantitative intersectionality
research. Sex Roles, 59, 312325.
Brooks, V. R. (1981). Minority stress and lesbian women. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Brown, L. S. (1996). Ethical concerns with sexual minority patients. In R. P.
Cabaj & T. S. Stein (Eds.), Textbook of homosexuality and mental health
(pp. 897916). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Bryant, A. S., & Demian (1994). Relationship characteristics of American gay
and lesbian couples: Findings from a national survey. Journal of Gay and
Lesbian Social Services, 1, 101117.
Cochran, S. D. (2001). Emerging issues in research on lesbians and gay mens
mental health: Does sexual orientation really matter? American Psychologist,
56, 931947.
Cochran, S. D., Ackerman, D., Mays, V. M. & Ross, M. W. Prevalence of nonmedical drug use and dependence among homosexually active men and
women in the U.S. population. Addiction, 99, 989.
Cochran, S. D., & Mays, V. M. (2000). Relation between psychiatric syndromes
and behaviorally dened sexual orientation in a sample of the U.S. population. American Journal of Epidemiology, 151, 516523.
Cochran, S. D., Mays, V. M., Algeria, M., Ortega, A. N., & Takeuchi, D. (2007).
Mental health and substance use disorders among Latino and Asian American lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 75, 785794.
Cochran, S. D., Sullivan, J. G., & Mays, V. M. (2003). Prevalence of mental disorders, psychological distress, and mental health services use among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults in the United States. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 71, 5361.
Corliss, H. L., Grella, C. E., Mays, V. M., & Cochran, S. D. (2006). Drug use,
drug severity, and help seeking behaviors of lesbian and bisexual women.
Journal of Womens Health, 15, 556 568.
Crothers, L., Haller, E., Benton, C., & Haag, S. (2008). A clinical comparison of
lesbian and heterosexual women in a psychiatric outpatient clinic. Journal
of Homosexuality, 54, 280292.
Diamant, A. L., & Wold, C. (2003). Sexual orientation and variation in physical
and mental health status among women. Journal of Womens Health, 12, 4149.
Diamant, A. L., Wold, C., Spritzer, K., & Gelberg, L. (2000). Health behaviors,
health status, and access to and use of health care: A population-based
study of lesbian, bisexual, and heterosexual women. Archives of Family
Medicine, 9, 10431051.
Dovidio, J. F., Major, B., & Crocker, J. (2000). Stigma: Introduction and overview. In T. F. Heatherton, R. E. Kleck, M. R. Hebl, & J. G. Hull (Eds.), The
social psychology of stereotyping (pp. 128). New York: Guilford Press.

146

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Espin, O. M. (1987). Issues of identity in the psychology of Latina lesbians. In


The Boston Lesbian Psychologies Collective (Eds.), Lesbian psychologies:
Explorations and challenges (pp. 3555). Chicago: University of Illinois Press.
Ferriss, E. L. (2002). Religion and the quality of life. Journal of Happiness Studies,
3, 199215.
Furnas, B., & Rosenthal, J. (2009). Benets denied: After paying into the system
their whole lives, married same-sex couples will be denied thousands of
dollars in social security benets. Retrieved April 13, 2009, from http://
www.americanprogress.org/issues/2009/03/pdf/benets_denied.pdf.
Garnets, L., Hancock, K. A., Cochran, S. D., Goodchilds, J., & Peplau, L. A.
(1991). Issues in psychotherapy with lesbians and gay men: A survey of
psychologists. American Psychologist, 46, 964972.
Gelso, C. J., Fassinger, R. E., Gomez, M. J., & Latts, M. G. (1995). Countertransference reactions to lesbian clients: The role of homophobia, counselor gender, and countertransference management. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
42, 356364.
Gilman, S. E., Cochran, S. D., & Mays, V. M. (2001). Risk of psychiatric disorders among individuals reporting same-sex sexual partners in a national
comorbidity survey. American Journal of Public Health, 91, 933939.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Goldberg, N. G., & Badgett, M. V. L. (2009). Tax implications for same-sex
couples. Retrieved April 16, 2009, from http://www.law.ucla.edu/williams
institute/pdf/Website_TaxPiece.pdf.
Greene, B. (1995). Lesbian women of color: Triple jeopardy. In L. Comes-Diaz &
B. Green (Eds.), Women of color (pp. 389427). New York: Guilford Publications, Inc.
Gruskin, E. P., Greenwood, G. L., Matevia, M., Pollack, L. M., & Bye, L. L.
(2007). Disparities in smoking between the lesbian, gay, and bisexual population and the general population in California. American Journal of Public
Health, 97, 14961502.
Hardman, K. L. J. (1997). Social workers attitudes to lesbian clients. British
Journal of Social Work, 27, 545563.
Harper, G. W., & Schneider, M. (2003). Oppression and discrimination among
lesbian, gay, bisexual and transgendered people and communities: A challenge for community psychology. American Journal of Community Psychology, 31, 234252.
Herek, G. M., & Garnets, L. D. (2007). Sexual orientation and mental health.
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 5375.
Hooker, E. (1993). Reections of a 40-year exploration: A scientic view on
homosexuality. American Psychologist, 48, 450453.
Kaiser Family Foundation. (2001, November). Inside-out: A report on the experiences
of lesbians, gays and bisexuals in America and the publics view on issues and policies related to sexual orientation. Mento Park, CA: Kaiser Family Foundation.
Khaxas, E., & Wieringa, S. (2005). I dont force my feelings for other women,
my feelings have to force me: Same-sexuality amongst Ovambo women
in Namibia. In R. Morgan & S. Wieringa (Eds.), Tommy boys, lesbian men
and ancestral wives: Female same-sex practices in Africa (pp. 2550). Johannesburg: Jacana Media.

Sexual Minority Women: Sources and Outcomes of Stigmatization

147

Lewis, R. J., Derlega, V. J., Clarke, E. V., & Kuang, J. C. (2006). Stigma consciousness, social constraints, and lesbian well-being. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 53, 4856.
MacEwan, I. (1994). Differences in assessment and treatment approaches for
homosexual clients. Drug and Alcohol Review, 13, 5762.
Mayer, K. H., Bradford, J. B., Makadon, H. J., Stall, R. Goldhammer, H., &
Landers, S. (2008). Sexual and gender minority health: What we know
and what needs to be done. American Journal of Public Health, 98,
989995.
Mays, V. M., & Cochran, S. D. (2001). Mental health correlates of perceived
discrimination among lesbian, gay, bisexual adults in the United States.
American Journal of Public Health, 91, 18691876.
Meckler, G. D., Elliot, M. N., Kanouse, D. E., Beals, K. P., & Schuster, M. A.
(2006). Nondisclosure of sexual orientation to a physician among a sample
of gay, lesbian, and bisexual youth. Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine,
160, 12481254.
Meyer, I. H. (2003). Prejudice, social stress and mental health in lesbian, gay
and bisexual populations: Conceptual issues and research evidence.
Psychological Bulletin, 129, 674697.
Miracle, T. S, Miracle, A. W., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Human sexuality: Meeting your basic needs. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Mohr, J. J., & Daly, C. A. (2008). Sexual minority stress and changes in relationship quality in same-sex couples. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
25, 9891007.
Nagadya, M., & Morgan, R. (2005). Some say I am a hermaphrodite just
because I put on trousers: Lesbians and tommy boys in Kampala,
Uganda. In R. Morgan & S. Wieringa (Eds.), Tommy boys, lesbian men and
ancestral wives: Female same-sex practices in Africa (pp. 2550). Johannesburg:
Jacana Media.
Phelan, J. C., Link, B. G., & Dovidio, J. F. (2008). Stigma and prejudice: One
animal or two? Social Science and Medicine, 67, 358367.
Pinel, E. C. (1999). Stigma consciousness: The psychological legacy of social
stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 114128.
Rostosky, S. S., Riggle, E. D. B., Brodnicki, C., & Olson, A. (2008). An exploration of lived religion in same-sex couples from Judeo-Christian traditions.
Family Processes, 47, 389403.
Rostosky, S. S., Riggle, E. D. B., Grey, B. E., & Hatton, R. L. (2007). Minority
stress experiences in committed same-sex couple relationships. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 33, 392400.
Rowen, C. J., & Malcom, J. P. (2002). Correlates of internalized homophobia
and homosexual identity formation in a sample of gay men. Journal of
Homosexuality, 43, 7792.
Russell, G. M. (2000). Voted out: The psychological consequences of anti-gay politics.
New York: New York University Press.
Russell, G. M. (2003). Stressor and resilience factors for lesbians, gay men, and
bisexuals confronting antigay politics. American Journal of Community
Psychology, 31, 313328.
Sandfort, T. G. M., de Graaf, R., Bijl, R. V., & Schnabel, P. (2001). Same-sex sexual behavior and psychiatric disorders: Findings from the Netherlands

148

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS). Archives of General


Psychiatry, 58, 8591.
UN spreading homosexuality around the worldUganda (2009, April 4).
Retrieved April 19, 2009, from http://www.thezimbabwemail.com/world/
2087.html.
Whitley, B. E., & Kite, M. E. (2006). The psychology of prejudice and discrimination.
Belmont, CA: Thompson Wadsworth.
Williamson, I. R. (2000). Internalized homophobia and health issues affecting lesbians and gay men. Health Education Research: Theory and Practice, 15, 97107.

Chapter 10

Special Issues for Women


with Disabilities
Martha E. Banks

OVERVIEW OF AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION


GUIDELINES REGARDING CULTURAL COMPETENCE
Development of culturally relevant rehabilitation psychology can be
aided by application of the American Psychological Associations Guidelines on Multicultural Education, Training, Research, Practice, and Organizational Change for Psychologists (American Psychological Association
[APA], 2003). Guideline #1 indicates, Psychologists are encouraged to
recognize that, as cultural beings, they may hold attitudes and beliefs
that can detrimentally inuence their perceptions of and interactions
with individuals who are ethnically and racially different from themselves (p. 382). This involves Psychologists are encouraged to learn
how cultures differ in basic premises that shape worldview (p. 382).
Psychologists are encouraged to be aware of their attitudes and work to
increase their contact with members of other racial/ethnic groups, building trust in others and increasing their tolerance for others.
The second guideline Psychologists are encouraged to recognize the
importance of multicultural sensitivity or responsiveness, knowledge,
and understanding about ethnically and racially different individuals
(p. 385). As a way to implement this guideline, [p]sychologists are also
encouraged to understand the stigmatizing aspects of being a member of
a culturally devalued other group (p. 385). This involves more than
awareness of overt experiences of prejudice and discrimination. Individual rehabilitation psychologists developing cultural competence have
increased awareness of the negative value of their own group in the

150

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

cultural hierarchy, an understanding of stereotype threat as it impacts on


a persons functioning in assessment or during treatment, and recognition
of the uncertainty of the attribution of the stigmatizing comments and
outcomes. Understanding a clients or students or research participants
worldview, including the effect of being in a stigmatized group, helps to
understand his/her perspectives and behaviors (p. 385). For clients with
disabilities, it is important for the rehabilitation psychologist to consider
cultural factors in symptom presentation, meaning of disability, motivation and willingness to seek treatment, social support networks, perseverance in treatment, differences in perspectives and experiences of people
with lifelong and recently acquired visible and invisible disabilities, and
acknowledgment of women of all ethnicities and People of Color with
disabilities as members of multiple stigmatized groups.
Guideline #4 addresses research: Culturally sensitive psychological
researchers are encouraged to recognize the importance of conducting
culture-centered and ethical psychological research among persons from
ethnic, linguistic, and racial minority backgrounds. Major demographic
shifts in the United States . . . are underway (p. 388). As a result, it will
be necessary to expand to include the issues facing aging baby boomers,
new immigrants, younger individuals of Latino heritage, and biracial
people; Women with Disabilities are members of all of those groups.
[W]hen research does not adequately incorporate culture as a central
and specic contextual variable, behavior is misidentied, pathologized,
and, in some cases, psychologists are at risk of perpetuating harm
(p. 388). Thus, psychological researchers are encouraged to be
grounded in the empirical and conceptual literature on the ways that
culture inuences the variables under investigation, as well as psychological and social science research traditions and skills (p. 389). Therefore, cultural considerations should be incorporated into research
generation and design, assessment, analysis, and interpretation.
Multicultural research considerations are similar to the Participatory
Action Model endorsed by APA and the National Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation Research: Culture-centered psychological
researchers are encouraged to report on the sample groups cultural,
ethnic, gender, and racial characteristics and to report on the cultural
limitations and generalizability of the research results as well (p. 389).
It is also recommended that researchers design the study to be of benet
to participants and to include participants in the interpretation of results.
They are encouraged to nd ways for the results to be of benet to the
community and to represent the participants perspectives accurately and
authentically. (p. 390)

Guideline #5 concerns the importance of culturally relevant approaches


to treatment: Psychologists strive to apply culturally-appropriate skills in

Special Issues for Women with Disabilities

151

clinical and other applied psychological practices. . . . (p. 390). It is not


necessary to develop an entirely new repertoire of psychological skills to
practice in a culture-centered manner. Rather, it is helpful for psychologists to realize that there will likely be situations where culture-centered
adaptations in interventions and practices will be more effective (p. 390).
Treatment best occurs when accurate assessment has been made. A
clients symptoms must be evaluated within his or her cultural context,
using culturally appropriate assessment tools (Ackerman & Banks,
2002). Rehabilitation psychologists use a broad repertoire of interventions to provide culturally relevant treatment (Ackerman & Banks,
2003; Corbett, 2003; DiCowden, 2003; Mukherjee, Reis, & Heller, 2003;
Nabors & Pettee, 2003).
WOMEN WITH DISABILITIES AS A CULTURAL GROUP
Banks and Kaschak (2003) recognized a broad range of disabilities
experienced by women. They classied them as Mens Illness Overlooked in Women and Womens Illness Misdiagnosed or Dismissed
and Dormant and Part-Time Disabilities. These included traumatic
brain injury; chronic health problems (e.g., diabetes, cardiac disease,
chronic fatigue syndrome, HIV/AIDS) (Beatty, 2003; Feist-Price &
Wright, 2003), pain (Kendall-Tackett, Marshall & Ness, 2003), psychiatric disorders, learning disabilities (Hoffschmidt & Weinstein, 2003); situational disabilities, such as migraines, motion sickness, morning
sickness in pregnancy, social anxiety, and gastric distress during menses; limited vision, deafness, and limited mobility. Womens disabilities
occur within a variety of life circumstances, some of which involve the
social construction of disability and its impact on identity. Women with
Disabilities struggle with lack of accommodation in education and
employment in ways that are often different from men (Vande Kemp,
Chen, Erickson, & Friesen, 2003). In addition, Women with Disabilities
are confronted with a variety of sexuality issues (Crawford & Ostrove,
2003; Dotson, Stinson, & Christian, 2003; Mona, 2003). All Women with
Disabilities deal with intersections of gender, disability, ethnicity, and
social class. Women with Disabilities face multiple stigma and discrimination based on age (youth, old age), ethnicity, weight status (obesity,
underweight), and disbelief when they experience invisible disabilities.
The nondominant status of Women with Disabilities impacts negatively
on education, employment, and reproduction.
Labeling is an issue for Women with Disabilities as it inuences personal identity. For some women, there is pride in being identied with
a group of people with whom they have challenges in common. For
other women, the idea of having a disability suggests an undesired
vulnerability (Feldman & Tegart, 2003). Public images of Women with
Disabilities are quite limited. Most training materials for psychologists

152

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

do not include images of Women with Disabilities. At the other end of


the spectrum, there is a subset of pornography that specically exploits
Women with Disabilities (Elman, 1997; Fiduccia, 1999; see also Farley,
2003).
A SPECIFIC REHABILITATION ISSUE: SAFETY
CONCERNS FOR WOMEN WITH DISABILITIES
Women with Disabilities are at higher risk for physical abuse and
psychological abuse than women without disabilities. The risk is further increased if the women need to rely on others for assistance. While
there has been some consideration given to violence perpetrated by
individuals, including family members and health professionals, there are
also social systemic risk factors.
Some of the high risk factors for Women with Disabilities include nancial and other dependence, inadequate support for personal assistants, and dangers endemic to treatment settings. While many people
consider treatment settings as safe havens for People with Disabilities,
in some instances they represent risk for Women with Disabilities, due
to mistreatment at hands of others, including other clients and treatment professionals (McCarthy, 1998). Such revictimization is, of course,
unethical. Disch (2006) described the long-lasting negative effects that
sexual abuse by professionals can have on women.
There are many types of abuse experienced by Women with Disabilities. While all women are at risk for violence, there are specic types of
violence that are related to disability (Banks, 2007). These include disabilityrelated emotional abuse, disability-related physical abuse, disability-related
sexual abuse, abuse related to disability-related settings, abuse related to
helping relationships, and disability-related nancial abuse.
Disability-related emotional abuse can involve actual or threatened
abandonment (Nosek, Foley, Hughes, & Howland, 2001) and isolation
(Crawford & Ostrove, 2003). Intolerance and rejection (Nosek, Foley et
al., 2001), refusal to acknowledge disability (Corbett, 2003), family prioritization of mens disabilities over womens disabilities (Nabors & Pettee,
2003), unrealistic demands on Women with Disabilities to carry out prescribed family roles (Nabors & Pettee, 2003), and family members refusal to develop skills to communicate with a Woman with a Disability
(Corbett, 2003) are further examples of disability-related emotional
abuse. Transportation limitations (Marshall, Sanderson, Johnson, Du
Bois, & Kvedar, 2006), such as lack of, inconsistent, limited availability
of, or need for advanced appointments for specialized transportation
services, represent a systemic safety issue, as Women with Disabilities
are confronted with limited ability to leave abusive situations.
Disability-related physical abuse can take several forms. In addition
to the beating often associated with violence against women, Women

Special Issues for Women with Disabilities

153

with Disabilities are subjected to withholding of or other prevention


of the use of orthotic or other assistive devices or medication (BeckMassey, 1999; Curry, Hassouneh-Phillips, & Johnston-Silverberg, 2001;
Nosek, Foley et al., 2001; Saxton, Curry, Powers, Maley, Eckels, &
Gross, 2001), adult childrens allowing access of known abusive relatives to Women with Disabilities, and family refusal to allow access to
personal assistants (Bergeron, 2005).
Perhaps the most common type of disability-related sexual abuse is
spousal rape (Nosek, Foley, et al., 2001). People providing assistance
sometimes use the threat of physical violence to coerce sexual activity
(McCarthy, 1998); this can happen in the home as well as in treatment
settings. Blurring the line between appropriate touching as an essential
part of the job of providers and inappropriate touching makes assessment of sexual abuse difcult at times; this can happen during bathing
or dressing (Saxton et al., 2001). Nosek, Foley, and colleagues documented fondling or forcing sexual activity in return for accepting help
as sexual abuse of Women with Disabilities. Mona (2003) noted that, in
personal assistance for sexual expression, there might be confusion
between helping an individual with sexual activity and participating in
sexual activity.
Of particular concern to rehabilitation psychologists is abuse related to
disability-related settings, because the chances of implementing institution-wide change are good. Many inpatient institutions have, for convenience, desegregated units that were gender-segregated in the past. The
desegregation has, in some instances, created a particular risk for Women
with Disabilities when they are not adequately protected from males in
inpatient settings; for example, when abuse is discounted or excused as
symptom of males disabilities (Banks, 2007; McCarthy, 1998). In institutions men routinely pay for, and women routinely accept payment for,
sex . . . Sex is seen as a commodity that can be exchanged and it is a oneway exchange, i.e. the men pay the women, not the other way around
(McCarthy, 1998, p. 547). Sexual abuse by staff members occurs under the
guise of provision of healthcare (Nosek, Foley, et al., 2001).
In the treatment setting, additional forms of abuse occur. In some
facilities, there is an overuse of seclusion, restraint, and rapid tranquilization (Sequeira & Halstead, 2001). Therapy can be rendered harmful
or ineffective by therapists discounting of impact of disability (Farley,
2003; Williams & Upadhyay, 2003). Therapists misattribution of psychological presenting complaints to physical disability can result in clients receiving the wrong or no treatment (Mukherjee, Reis, & Heller,
2003). In many training facilities, Women with Disabilities experience
exposure of their nude body to others (e.g., health care students) without permission (Mona, Cameron, & Crawford, 2005).
There is disability-related abuse related to helping relationships.
With personal assistance, there is a power imbalance between the

154

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

provider and the recipient. It is important to consider who provides


personal assistance and who monitors it. Most personal assistance is
provided in private, resulting in little protection in the event of violence or other abuse. If a Woman with a Disability needs personal assistance, she is unlikely to be strong enough to defend herself (Banks,
2007). Nearly two-thirds of the People with Disabilities who need personal assistance are women (Kennedy & LaPlante, 1997, as cited by
Mona, 2003). Coble (2001) observed that many People with Disabilities
who need personal assistance are inexperienced in the selection of
effective or trustworthy personal assistants; she provided detailed recommendations to assist in interviewing and evaluation of potential
candidates (see also Saxton et al., 2001).
Some types of abuse related to helping relationships include rough
handling (Mona et al., 2005); not responding or delaying response
(Mona et al., 2005); lack of understanding by police and other helping
professionals of the nature of personal assistance relationships, due to
societal assumptions that Women with Disabilities are incompetent
(Chang et al., 2003; Gilson, DePoy, & Cramer, 2001; Mona et al., 2005;
Nosek, Howland, & Hughes, 2001; Saxton et al., 2001); infantilization
(Nosek, Foley, et al., 2001; Saxton et al., 2001); and inappropriately
attempting to transform a business relationship into a personal friendship (Saxton et al., 2001).
Saxton and colleagues (2001) described disability-related nancial
abuse that can involve [T]heft of jewelry, money, and personal
belongings; forgery; purchase of personal items when shopping with
the participants money; and withdrawal of extra money during ATM
transactions performed for the woman. A unique form of nancial
abuse commonly reported was assistants showing up late or not working their full time, but still receiving full compensation (p. 405). Bergeron (2005) discussed abuse of durable power of attorney. Corbetts
(2003) examination of Women with Color with Disabilities included an
examination of family refusal to consider nancial limitations of
Women with Disabilities (Banks & Ackerman, 2006); families expected
equal nancial contribution for care of other family members and large
contributions to family reunions.

SUMMARY
Rehabilitation Psychologists Treating Abused
Women with Disabilities
Rehabilitation psychologists have a responsibility to determine
whether a Woman with Disabilities is in danger of being abused and
should explore the nature and extent of any discovered or suspected

Special Issues for Women with Disabilities

155

abuse. Abuse is an ongoing problem for Women with Disabilities as


noted by Banks (2007):
Women with Disabilities, on average, endure domestic violence for longer periods than women without disabilities (Coker, Smith, & Fadden,
2005; Curry et al., 2001; Li, Ford & Moore, 2000) and are at high risk for
being abused by multiple perpetrators (Hassouneh-Phillips & Curry,
2002).

Family members, particularly intimate partners, are more apt than


strangers to inict violence that causes disabilities. In some cases it is
not clear which came rst: the abuse or the disability. There is a cycle
of abuse and disability, in which it is possible for abuse to result in disability (Plichta, 2004) and for a disability to be exacerbated by abuse
(Banks, 2007; Campbell & Kendall-Tackett, 2005; Curry et al., 2001;
Zlotnick, Johnson & Kohn, 2006).
Abuse can lead to early death; unchecked intimate partner violence
has been shown to end with the death of the victim (Thompson, Saltzman, & Johnson, 2003). As a result, there is a critical need for rehabilitation psychologists to pay simultaneous attention to multiple issues.
While disability is clearly a focus, it is important to determine disability before and after abuse.
Treatment must address safety. Safety planning is complicated for
Women with Disabilities, in part due to lack of accessible services
(Zweig, Schlichter, & Burt, 2002). As noted earlier, some inpatient settings represent particular danger for Women with Disabilities. Independent living is an option for some women, but vulnerability must be
considered (Hendey & Pascall, 1998). Leaving abusive relationships is
often difcult in situations in which the Woman with Disability is dependent upon an abuser (Olkin, 2003; Saxton et al., 2001). The risk of
intimate partner violence escalates at the point of separation; leaving is
extremely dangerous for abused women (Heise, Ellsberg, & Gottemoeller, 1999; Kyriacou et al., 1999). Women are most likely to be murdered
when attempting to report abuse or to leave abusive relationships
(Wuest & Merritt-Gray, 1999). Campbell and Soeken (1999) observed
women with dangerous batterers may stay with them out of fear
(p. 35). Brice-Baker (1994) described cultural issues which trap immigrant women into staying in violent relationshipsfear of deportation,
cultural belief systems about why abuse occurs, economic limitations,
and language barriers. In addition, Fleury, Sullivan, Bybee, and Davidson (1998) noted that victims prevented from calling police or who had
no telephone available, suffered more physical injuries than victims
who had access to help. It is important that professionals not fall prey
to myths that victims are unlikely to leave abusive relationships, that
victims are passive and self-defeating, and that physical violence is

156

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

more devastating that psychological violence (Gortner, Berns, Jacobson,


& Gottman, 1997).
Rehabilitation psychologists have developed a broad range of interventions and are well prepared to work as members of interdisciplinary
teams to address the complexities of issues faced by Women with Disabilities. The eld of rehabilitation psychology has moved from the traditional model through the medical model and social model to the
integrative model (Seelman, 2004). Incorporating social situations of
Women with Disabilities into the integrative model is the next logical step
toward building truly culturally competent rehabilitation psychology.

REFERENCES
Ackerman, R. J., & Banks, M. E. (2002). Epilogue: Looking for the threads:
Commonalities and differences. In F. R. Ferraro (Ed.), Minority and crosscultural aspects of neuropsychological assessment (pp. 387415). Heereweg,
Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers.
Ackerman, R. J., & Banks, M. E. (2003). Assessment, treatment, and rehabilitation for interpersonal violence victims: Women sustaining head injuries. In
M. E. Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with visible and invisible disabilities:
Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 343363).
New York: Haworth.
American Psychological Association. (2003). APA guidelines on multicultural
education, training, research, practice, and organizational change for psychologists. American Psychologist, 58, 377402.
Banks, M. E. (2003). Preface. In M. E. Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with
visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple
therapies (pp. xxixxxix). New York: Haworth.
Banks, M. E. (2007). Women with disabilities, domestic violence against. In
N. A. Jackson (Ed.), Encyclopedia of domestic violence. New York: Taylor &
Francis (pp. 723728).
Banks, M. E., & Ackerman, R. J. (2002). Head and brain injuries experienced by
African American women victims of intimate partner violence. Women &
Therapy, 25, 133143.
Banks, M. E., & Ackerman, R. J. (2006). Health disparities: Focus on disability.
In K. J. Hagglund & A. W. Heinemann (Eds.), Handbook of applied disability
and rehabilitation research (pp. 4570). New York: Springer.
Banks, M. E. & Kaschak, E. (Eds.). (2003). Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies. New York:
Haworth.
Banks, M. E., & Marshall, C. (2004). Beyond the triple-whammy: Social class
as a factor in discrimination against persons with disabilities. In J. L. Chin
(Ed.), The psychology of prejudice and discrimination: Combating prejudice and
all forms of discrimination. Volume 4: Disability, religion, physique, and other
traits (pp. 95110). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Beatty, L. (2003). Substance abuse, disabilities, and Black women: An issue
worth exploring. In M. E. Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with visible

Special Issues for Women with Disabilities

157

and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies


(pp. 223236). New York: Haworth.
Beck-Massey, D. (1999). Sanctioned war: Women, violence, and disabilities.
Sexuality and Disability, 17, 269276.
Bergeron, L. R. (2005). Abuse of elderly women in family relationships:
Another form of violence against women. In K. A. Kendall-Tackett (Ed.),
Handbook of women, stress, and trauma (pp. 141157). New York: BrunnerRoutledge.
Brice-Baker, J. R. (1994). Domestic violence in African-American and AfricanCaribbean families. Journal of Social Distress and the Homeless, 3, 2338.
Campbell, J. C., & Kendall-Tackett, K. A. (2005). Intimate partner violence:
Implications for womens physical and mental health. In K. A. KendallTackett (Ed.), Handbook of women, stress, and trauma (pp. 123140). New
York: Brunner-Routledge.
Campbell, J. C., & Soeken, K. L. (1999b). Womens responses to battering over
time: An analysis of change. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 2140.
Chang, J. C., Martin, S. L., Moracco, K. E., Dulli, L., Scandlin, D., Loucks-Sorrel,
M. B., et al. (2003). Helping women with disabilities and domestic violence: Strategies, limitations, and challenges of domestic violence programs
and services. Journal of Womens Health, 12, 699708.
Coble, A. C. (2001, Winter). When the challenge of maintaining personal care
attendants becomes the focus of treatment. Rehabilitation Psychology News,
28, 78.
Coker, A. L., Smith, P. H., & Fadden, M. K. (2005). Intimate partner violence
and disabilities among women attending family practice clinics. Journal of
Womens Health, 14, 829838.
Corbett, C. A. (2003). Special issues in psychotherapy with minority deaf
women. In M. E. Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies
(pp. 311329). New York: Haworth.
Crawford, D., & Ostrove, J. M. (2003). Representations of disability and the interpersonal relationships of Women with Disabilities. In M. E. Banks & E.
Kaschak (Eds.), Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 179194). New York: Haworth.
Curry, M. A., Hassouneh-Phillips, D., & Johnston-Silverberg, A. (2001). Abuse
of women with disabilities: An ecological model and review. Violence
Against Women, 7, 6079.
DiCowden, M. (2003). The call of the wild woman: Models of healing. In M. E.
Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 297310). New York:
Haworth.
Disch, E. (2006). Sexual victimization and revictimization of women by professionals: Client experiences and implications for subsequent treatment.
Women & Therapy, 29, 4161.
Dotson, L. A., Stinson, J., & Christian, L. (2003). People tell me I cant have
sex: Women with disabilities share their personal perspectives on health
care, sexuality, and reproductive rights. In M. E. Banks & E. Kaschak
(Eds.), Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 195209). New York: Haworth.

158

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Elman, R. A. (1997). Disability pornography: The fetishization of womens


vulnerabilities. Violence Against Women, 3, 257270.
Farley, M. (2003). Prostitution and the invisibility of harm. In M. E. Banks & E.
Kaschak (Eds.), Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 247280). New York: Haworth.
Feist-Price, S., & Wright, L. B. (2003). African American women living with
HIV/AIDS: Mental health issues. In M. E. Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.),
Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple
issues, multiple therapies (pp. 2744). New York: Haworth.
Feldman, S. I., & Tegart, G. (2003). Keep moving: Conceptions of illness and
disability of middle-aged African-American women with arthritis. In M. E.
Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 127143). New York:
Haworth.
Fiduccia, B. F. W. (1999). Sexual imagery of physically disabled women: Erotic?
Perverse? Sexist? Sexuality and Disability, 17, 277282.
Fleury, R. E., Sullivan, C. M., Bybee, D. I., & Davidson II, W. S. (1998). Why
dont they just call the cops?: Reasons for differential police contact
among women with abusive partners. Violence and Victims, 13, 333346.
Gilson, S. F., DePoy, E., & Cramer, E. P. (2001). Linking the assessment of selfreported functional capacity with abuse experiences of women with disabilities. Violence Against Women, 7, 418431.
Gortner, E., Berns, S. B., Jacobson, N. S., & Gottman, J. M. (1997). When women
leave violent relationships: Dispelling clinical myths. Psychotherapy, 34,
343352.
Hassouneh-Phillips, D., & Curry, M. A. (2002). Abuse of women with disabilities: State of the science. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 45, 96104.
Heise, L., Ellsberg, M., & Gottemoeller, M. (1999, December). Ending violence
against women. Population Reports, Series L, Number 11. Baltimore: Johns
Hopkins University School of Public Health, Population Information Program. Retrieved December 9, 2006, from http://www.jhuccp.org/pr/
l11edsum.stm.
Hendey, N., & Pascall, G. (1998). Independent living: Gender, violence and the
threat of violence. Disability & Society, 13, 415427.
Hoffschmidt, S. J., & Weinstein, C. S. (2003). The inuence of silent learning
disorders on the lives of women. In M. E. Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.),
Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple
issues, multiple therapies (pp. 8194). New York: Haworth.
Kendall-Tackett, K., Marshall, R., & Ness, K. (2003). Chronic pain syndromes
and violence against women. In M. E. Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women
with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 4556). New York: Haworth.
Kyriacou, D. N., Angelin, D., Taliaferro, E., Stone, S., Tubb, T., Linden, J. A.,
et al. (1999). Risk factors for injury to women from domestic violence. New
England Journal of Medicine, 341, 18921898.
Li, L., Ford, J. A., & Moore, D. (2000). An exploratory study of violence, substance abuse, disability, and gender. Social Behavior and Personality, 28, 6171.
Marshall, C. A., Sanderson, P. R., Johnson, S. R., Du Bois, B., & Kvedar, J. C.
(2006). Considering class, culture, and access in rehabilitation intervention

Special Issues for Women with Disabilities

159

and research. In K. J. Hagglund & A. W. Heinemann (Eds.), Handbook of


applied disability and rehabilitation research (pp. 2544). New York: Springer.
McCarthy, M. (1998). Sexual violence against women with learning disabilities.
Feminism & Psychology, 8, 544551.
Mona, L. R. (2003). Sexual options for people with disabilities: Using personal
assistance services for sexual expression. In M. E. Banks & E. Kaschak
(Eds.), Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 211221). New York: Haworth.
Mona, L. R., Cameron, R. P., & Crawford, D. (2005). Stress and trauma in the
lives of women with disabilities. In K. A. Kendall-Tackett (Ed.), Handbook
of women, stress, and trauma (pp. 229244). New York: Brunner-Routledge.
Mukherjee, D., Reis, J. P., & Heller, W. (2003). Women living with traumatic
brain injury: Social isolation, emotional functioning and implications for
psychotherapy. In M. E. Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with visible and
invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies
(pp. 326). New York: Haworth.
Nabors, N. A., & Pettee, M. F. (2003). Womanist therapy with African American Women with disabilities. In M. E. Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women
with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 331341). New York: Haworth.
Nosek, M. A., Foley, C. C., Hughes, R. B., & Howland, C. A. (2001). Vulnerabilities for abuse among women with disabilities. Sexuality and Disability, 19,
177189.
Nosek, M. A., Howland, C. A., & Hughes, R. B. (2001). The investigation of
abuse and women with disabilities: Going beyond assumptions. Violence
Against Women, 7, 477499.
Olkin, R. (2003). Women with physical disabilities who want to leave their partners: A feminist and disability-afrmative perspective. In M. E. Banks & E.
Kaschak (Eds.), Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 237246). New York: Haworth.
Plichta, S. B. (2004). Intimate partner violence and physical health consequences: Policy and practice implications. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19,
12961323.
Saxton, M., Curry, M. A., Powers, L. E., Maley, S., Eckels, K., & Gross, J. (2001).
Bring my scooter so I can leave you: A study of disabled women handling abuse by personal assistance providers. Violence Against Women, 7,
393417.
Seelman, K. (2004, JanuaryMarch). Trends in rehabilitation and disability:
Transition from a medical model to an integrative model (part 1). Disability
World, Issue 22. Retrieved October 20, 2006, from http://www.disabilityworld.org/01-03_04/access/rehabtrends1.shtml.
Sequeira, H., & Halstead, S. (2001). Is it meant to hurt, is it?: Management of
violence in women with developmental disabilities. Violence Against Women,
7, 462476.
Thompson, M. P., Saltzman, L. E., & Johnson, H. (2003). A comparison of risk
factors for intimate partner violence-related injury across two national surveys on violence against women. Violence Against Women, 9, 438456.
Vande Kemp, H., Chen, J. S., Erickson, G. N., & Friesen, N. L. (2003). ADA
accommodation of therapists with disabilities in clinical training. In M. E.

160

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 155168). New York:
Haworth.
Williams, M., & Upadhyay, W. S. (2003). To be or not to be disabled. In M. E.
Banks & E. Kaschak (Eds.), Women with visible and invisible disabilities: Multiple intersections, multiple issues, multiple therapies (pp. 145154). New York:
Haworth.
Wuest, J., & Merritt-Gray, M. (1999). Not going back: Sustaining the separation
in the process of leaving abusive relationships. Violence Against Women, 5,
110133.
Zlotnick, C., Johnson, D. M., & Kohn, R. (2006). Intimate partner violence and
long-term psychosocial functioning in a national sample of American
women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21, 262275.
Zweig, J. M., Schlichter, K. A., & Burt, M. R. (2002). Assisting women victims
of violence who experience multiple barriers to services. Violence Against
Women, 8, 162180.

Chapter 11

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered


Eating: The Globalization of Western
Appearance Ideals
Jaehee Jung
Gordon B. Forbes

In every known human society, women decorate and modify their


bodies to make them more attractive. Although beauty standards show
enormous variability across time and culture, one constant feature of
human beings has been their willingness to bear great nancial
expense and endure painful and dangerous procedures in the pursuit
of these standards. One of the reasons these efforts have persisted is
the strong relationship between womens feelings about their bodies
and their physical and mental health (Grogan, 2008).
A large research literature indicates that dissatisfaction with the
appearance of ones body is associated with depression, social anxiety,
sexual dysfunctions, psychosomatic illnesses, and a host of other feelings and experiences that devastate womens self-esteem and diminish
their quality of life (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997; Grogan, 2008). The
most extreme manifestations of dissatisfaction with appearance are lifethreatening eating disorders.
This chapter will summarize what is known about cultural differences in body dissatisfaction and eating disorders. Both body dissatisfaction and eating disorders are complex phenomena with multiple and
sometimes conicting denitions and determinants. Body dissatisfaction occurs when a woman has a negative or unfavorable body image.
The term body image refers to how a woman perceives and evaluates

162

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

her body. Body image includes perceptions of the appearance of her


physical body (i.e., short, tall, fat, thin, athletic); self-evaluations and
emotional reactions to her body (i.e., attractive, ugly, sexy, disgusting);
perceptions of how others perceive, evaluate, and respond to her body
(i.e., people think I am good looking, people think I am fat); and perceptions of her bodys functioning (e.g., athletic, healthy, weak). Eating
disorders are conditions in which peoples diet and eating behaviors
impair their quality of life and/or pose signicant risks to their physical or mental health. We will focus on conditions associated with medically unnecessary and potentially harmful practices intended to alter
the body through restricted caloric intake. The most severe form of eating disorder is anorexia nervosa. The salient feature of anorexia nervosa is a potentially life-threatening, self-induced starvation. Along with
the starvation comes important physiological changes (including neurohormonal changes leading to the cessation of menstruation), the terror
of becoming fat, and the false perception that the sufferers underweight body is fat.
Another form of eating disorder is bulimia, also called bulimia nervosa. This refers to self-induced vomiting for purposes of weight control. The vomiting typically occurs following a period of actual or
perceived excessive food consumption (binging). Bulimia usually
occurs in people of average to above average weight. It may also occur
in individuals suffering from anorexia nervosa and/or be accompanied
by the abuse of laxatives or stimulant drugs.1
Estimates of the prevalence of eating disorders among Englishspeaking Western societies show substantial variation depending on
how disordered eating is dened and measured and on how the data
are collected. The best available recent information indicates that during their lifetime about 1 percent of American women will develop anorexia nervosa, and an additional 1.5 percent will develop bulimia
(Hudson, Hiripi, Pope, & Kessler, 2007). Less information is available
on the Western countries of continental Europe, but research in the last
decade suggests that body dissatisfaction and eating disorders in continental European countries are roughly comparable to the levels found
in English-speaking countries (Makino, Tsuboi, & Dennerstein, 2004;
Miller & Pumariega, 2001; Swami et al., 2009; Wardle, Haase, & Steptoe, 2006). Although there are certainly differences within and between
these countries, we will usually follow the common practice of discussing them as the single class of Western.
BODY DISSATISFACTION IS FAR MORE THAN
EATING DISORDERS
It is important to recognize that studies of women with eating disorders involve only women whose symptoms are so severe and

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

163

debilitating that they are diagnosed as suffering from a mental illness.


For every woman with a diagnosed eating disorder, there are literally
dozens of women with some, but not all, of the symptoms associated
with these disorders. For example, as high as one-third of college-aged
women (the demographic group with one of the highest rates of eating
disorders) have at least one of the symptoms associated with eating
disorders (Prah, 2006). However, even this gure fails to reect the
magnitude of body dissatisfaction or how it impacts the daily lives of
ordinary women. This is because women who do not have symptoms
associated with clinical eating disorders may still experience substantial
dissatisfaction with their bodies.
Surveys indicate that approximately 50 to 70 percent of American
women are dissatised with one or more aspects of their bodies, particularly their weight (Cash & Henry, 1995; Frederick, Peplau, & Lever,
2006; Garner, 1997). Importantly, these high levels of weight and body
dissatisfaction are found in women of all ages, show only a modest
decline over their lifespan, and are found in most Western samples
(Cash, 2002; Forbes et al., 2005; Tiggemann, 2004).
The consequences of body and weight dissatisfaction include a pattern of constantly watching ones weight (which may be present as
early as 7 years of age, e.g., Dohnt & Tiggemann, 2006), calorie counting, self-consciousness about eating in public, shame and guilt over
actual or perceived appearance aws, and a womans ever-present anxious concern with appearance and others perceptions of her body. The
cumulative effect of these experiences is lower self-condence, lower
self-esteem, and a reduced quality of life. These consequences are experienced by many, arguably most, Western women of all ages (Bordo,
1993; Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997; Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore,
1984). In fact, body dissatisfaction is so common among Western
women that it was labeled as a normative discontent a quarter of a
century ago (Rodin et al., 1984).
Body dissatisfaction among ordinary Western women has increased
sharply over the last 30 to 40 years (Cash & Henry, 1995; Garner,
1997). Similarly, body dissatisfaction and disordered eating have begun
to appear in areas of the world where they were once very rare.
Although understanding the nature and extent of body dissatisfaction
is a crucial step in reducing and treating life-threatening eating disorders, it is also important to recognize that it has the potential to
improve the quality of life for many millions of ordinary women
around the world (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). The rest of this chapter will address this goal. The rst section will examine the history of
body dissatisfaction and the contribution of feminist theory to the
understanding of cultural differences in body dissatisfaction and disordered eating. The second will review the research on cultural similarities and differences in eating disorders and body dissatisfaction. This

164

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

section will also identify major gaps in our knowledge and suggest
how feminist theory may guide future research.
HISTORY OF EATING DISORDERS AND BODY IMAGE
Life-threatening food refusal among Western women has been
described for several hundred years. Before the nineteenth century,
published descriptions of food refusal usually occurred in a religious
context (e.g., Bynum, 1987). Although it was described as a psychiatric
syndrome in the 1870s, it was considered extremely rare for much of
the following century. In his clinical training in the 1960s the second
author can recall a supervisor commenting that he had seen only two
cases of anorexia nervosa in over 30 years of clinical practice. Similar
experiences have been reported by many mental health practitioners
trained before the mid-1970s (e.g., Gordon, 2000).
Public and professional interest in anorexia nervosa increased
greatly in the late 1970s and 1980s. In 1983, the tragic death of the
gifted popular singer Karen Carpenter focused both media and professional attention on anorexia nervosa and the previously almost
unknown disorder of bulimia. In the early 1990s, Princess Dianas public acknowledgment of her long struggle with bulimia focused public
attention on a disorder that by then approached epidemic proportions
among young women in the United States and United Kingdom. By
the 1990s, it was generally recognized that dissatisfaction with the
appearance of ones body was an essential precursor to eating disorders (Polivy & Herman, 2002). (For a history of anorexia nervosa and
other eating disorders, see Brumberg, 1988.)
EATING DISORDERS AND BODY DISSATISFACTION
AS CULTURE-LIMITED PHENOMENA
For many years, body dissatisfaction, particularly weight dissatisfaction, excessive dieting, and life-threatening eating disorders, were thought
to be culture- and social class-limited phenomena that occurred primarily
or exclusively among afuent white women in English-speaking Western
countries (Nasser, 1997; Prince, 1983). Two seemingly compelling sources
supported this view.
First, over a century of reports by mental health practitioners, and
more recent reports by researchers, indicated that eating disorders are
much more common among upper class white women than women
from lower classes or ethnic minorities (Brumberg, 1988).2
Second, a large anthropological literature demonstrated that the majority of human societies associated slender womens bodies with illness,
poverty, and diminished fecundity (Brown & Konner, 1987; Cassidy,
1991). Conversely, almost 80 percent of societies associate moderate to

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

165

high levels of body fat in women with health, physical attractiveness,


social status, and fertility (Brown, 1991). The fattening huts of some
African cultures, where girls are force-fed large quantities of food to
make them gain weight and become more appealing brides, are well
known (Brown & Konner, 1987; Swami & Furnham, 2008).3
By the early 1980s, mental health practitioners and researchers from
non-Western countries were beginning to report cases of anorexia nervosa in societies where it was previously unknown or extremely rare.
By the late 1990s, it was clear that even if the culture-limited perception of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating was once true, this
was no longer the case. To the contrary, these disorders were now
found in many developed and developing societies, and their prevalence in non-Western societies was increasing (Gordon, 2000; Nasser,
1997; Nasser, Katzman, & Gordon, 2001).
THE GROWTH AND SPREAD OF BODY DISSATISFACTION
The spread of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating is often
attributed to what Lee (2004) characterized as the . . . toxin of Westernization (p. 617). However, Westernization, or perhaps more appropriately
modernization (e.g., Kagitcibasi, 2005), is a complex and multidimensional
phenomenon. It encompasses economic and political structures; fashion
and popular culture; social roles and expectations of others; and a host of
other values, expectations, and worldviews. All, some, or none of these
may be related to body dissatisfaction and eating disorders. In addition, it
is possible that different aspects of modernization are related to body dissatisfaction in different cultures (e.g., Shefeld, Tse, & Sofronoff, 2005). As
a consequence, attributing the spread of body dissatisfaction to the nonspecic inuence of modernization or Westernization does little to further
our understanding of the phenomenon.
MODERNIZATION AND WESTERN MEDIA
Although it is likely that many aspects of modernization inuence
body perceptions, the most extensively studied, and certainly the most
vilied, aspect of modernization is Western media. Most research has
focused on medias ubiquitous portrayals of extremely thin female
bodies, its emphasis on diet and weight control, and the always
implicit, and frequently explicit, message that with sufcient effort all
women can and should look this way (Holmstrom, 2004; Thompson,
Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999). For convenience, this is often
labeled the thin body ideal. Critics of Western media, particularly
Western television, can draw on a large body of feminist criticism and
empirical research showing the pernicious effect of the thin body ideal
on the body image of women in both the developed and the

166

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

developing world (e.g., Bordo, 1993; Jeffreys, 2005; Wolf, 1991). For
recent reviews of televisions role in body dissatisfaction and disordered eating, see Gilbert, Keery, and Thompson (2005); Ward and Harrison (2005); and Cafri, Yamamiya, Brannick, and Thompson (2005).
The work of Becker, Burwell, Herzog, Hamburg, and Gilman (2002)
is a particularly dramatic example of the inuence of Western television in the developing world. In their frequently cited study, Becker
and her associates found that the introduction of Western television to
the traditional culture of Fiji was quickly followed by increased body
dissatisfaction, dieting, and behaviors associated with disordered eating (e.g., self-induced vomiting) among young women.
Although most research and theoretical discussions emphasize the
role of television and, to a lesser extent motion pictures, the print media,
particularly magazines, are also an important inuence. Studies have
shown that exposure to thin models in popular womens magazines contributes to body dissatisfaction; decreases self-esteem and condence;
and produces negative feelings of guilt, anxiety, shame, and depression
(Pinhas, Toner, Ali, Garnkel, & Stuckless, 1999; Tiggemann & McGill,
2004). Similarly, exposure to thin models in magazines has been linked
to eating disorder symptomatology in both adolescent girls and adult
women (e.g., Harrison, 2000; Pinhas et al., 1999; Vaughan & Fouts, 2003).
In the last 10 years pro-anorexia Web sites and social networking sites
have appeared that encourage young women to pursue extremely thin
bodies through severe dieting, vomiting, and other extreme weight-loss
techniques (Morris, Boydell, Pinhas, & Katzman, 2006). Although scholarly research on the inuence of these sites is still very limited, a recent
study found that exposure to these sites increased body dissatisfaction
among college women (Bardone-Cone & Cass, 2007).
THEORETICAL MODELS OF BODY DISSATISFACTION
The two most common theoretical approaches to the understanding
of body dissatisfaction are sociocultural theory and feminist theory.
The inuence of Western media plays a crucial role in both theoretical
models.
Sociocultural Theory
This theory seeks to understand body dissatisfaction as a consequence of social and cultural variables (Thompson et al., 1999). Most
of the research based on sociocultural theory has focused on the role
of the thin body ideal and the unrelenting pressure from media,
peers, and parents to attain it. Although it is clear that Western media
is a major vector in transmitting the thin body ideal to non-Western
culture, it is also clear that many other factors are involved (e.g.,
Anderson-Fye, 2003, 2004; Anderson-Fye & Becker, 2004). Feminist

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

167

theory suggests that rapid social change may be one of the important
additional factors.
Feminist Theory
This theory, which may be viewed as an expanded form of sociocultural theory, also recognizes the important role of the unrealistic body
ideals portrayed in the media. However, feminist theory goes an
important step further: it emphasizes and describes the social and
political purposes served by these body ideals. According to theorists
such as Bordo (1993), Dworkin (1974), and Faludi (1991), the purpose
of unrealistic appearance standards is to perpetuate gender inequality.
This goal is pursued through two mechanisms. First, attention is
diverted from womens competencies and accomplishments; instead it
is focused on supercial aspects of their appearance. Second, womens
emotional and nancial resources are diminished and their selfcondence is undermined by expensive, exhausting, and usually futile
attempts to conform to unrealistic appearance standards. For a review
of these theories, see Jeffreys (2005). Wolf (1991), in describing beauty
standards as a backlash against American womens strivings for gender
equality, captured a central feminist argument when she stated, The
more legal and material hindrances women have broken through, the
more strictly and heavily and cruelly images of female beauty have
come to weigh upon us (p. 10).
Both sociocultural theory and feminist theory would predict that the
more a society is exposed to Western media and Western appearance
standards, the greater the level of body dissatisfaction and disordered
eating within that society. However, feminist theory goes a step further
and suggests that increases in body dissatisfaction and disordered eating
should be largest in societies in which women have made the greatest
and most rapid strides toward social, economic, and political equality.
SOCIAL CHANGE AND BODY IMAGE
Consistent with feminist theory, rapid social change, particularly
rapidly changing roles of women, has been identied as an important
factor in increasingly unrealistic appearance standards and increased
body dissatisfaction in Western societies (Nasser et al., 2001). This relationship has been reported by clinicians treating young women with
eating disorders (e.g., Bruch, 1980) and has been widely recognized by
theorists (e.g., Travis, Meginnis, & Bardari, 2000). Consistent with these
observations, Silverstein and Perlick (1995) reported that the two
decades in the twentieth century with the greatest change in Western
womens roles, the 1920s and 1970s, were also the decades in which
Western fashion models were the most slender and least curvaceous.
Indeed, as the social and gender role changes associated with rst and

168

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

second wave feminism spread through much of Western society, the


bodies of appearance icons such as Miss America and Playboy centerfolds became increasingly more slender and increasingly discrepant
from the body of the average woman that was becoming progressively
heavier (Spitzer, Henderson, & Zivian, 1999; Sypeck, Gray, & Ahrens,
2004). It seems likely that this growing discrepancy between appearance ideals and the bodies of average women is a signicant contributor in the increases in body dissatisfaction and disordered eating
(Spitzer et al., 1999). Just as social change has also been identied as an
important factor in body dissatisfaction and disordered eating in Western societies, it has also been identied as an important factor in nonWestern societies (e.g., Catina & Joja, 2001; Gordon, 2000; Lee & Lee,
2000; Nasser, 1997, Tsai, 2000).
According to feminist theory, societies with strong patriarchal traditions, rigid gender roles, and very rapid social change, including rapidly
expanding opportunities for women, would be particularly likely to
embrace unrealistic appearance standards and have relatively high levels
of body dissatisfaction. As we will see in the next section, reports from
rapidly developing countries in East Asia, such as China (Jung & Forbes,
2007; Lee & Lee, 2000; Li, Hu, Ma, Wu, & Ma, 2005) and Korea (Kim &
Kim, 2003; Kim & Yoon, 2000; Ryu, Lyle, & McCabe, 2003), as well as
the rapidly developing former socialistic countries of Eastern Europe
(Catina & Joja, 2001; Forbes, Doroszewicz, Card, & Adams-Curtis, 2004),
are consistent with this expectation.

CROSS-CULTURAL RESEARCH ON BODY IMAGE


There has been very little systematic research on cross-cultural differences in body dissatisfaction and disordered eating. Unfortunately,
even under the best of circumstances, cross-cultural comparisons are
very difcult.4 In the case of research on body dissatisfaction and disordered eating, four problems are particularly serious. First, much of
the available literature, particularly from traditional cultures and developing countries, is based on a single sample from a single culture. That
is, these studies do not include specic comparison samples from other
cultures. To place information from these studies into a cross-cultural
context, their results must be compared with the results of studies
employing samples from other cultures. Unfortunately, this practice is
fraught with almost insurmountable difculties. Among other things,
large differences in how the samples are selected and crucial concepts
are measured often makes it impossible to draw conclusions about possible cultural differences.
Second, nearly all available studies have employed convenience
samples. As the term implies, these samples consist of whatever

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

169

participants are readily available to the researcher. In the case of clinical reports, these samples almost always consist of patients with identied eating disorders. Because only a small minority of women with
eating disorders seek professional services, particularly in cultures
where these services are limited and highly stigmatized, women in
these samples are usually from the highest socioeconomic stratum and
have very severe symptoms. Results from such samples are not representative of most women in their culture. In the case of research
reports, most studies have been done with college women. Similarly,
these samples are not representative of most women in their society.
Third, studies almost always depend on the use of Western measures
that must be translated into the local language. The use of translated
measures is always problematic (Forbes, in press). Even when translations
are carefully done, it is often difcult to determine whether the concepts
measured in the translation are the same as the concepts measured in
their native culture and language (Brislin, 1986). In addition, when specic diagnostic instruments or criteria are employed researchers make the
assumption, usually implicitly, that the symptoms of a disorder are the
same in all cultures. However, the specic symptoms associated with eating disorders may vary across cultures (Lee, Chiu & Chen, 1989; Lee et
al., 1993). Similarly, because both physical features and appearance standards vary across ethnic and cultural groups, the specic features associated with body dissatisfaction may show cultural or ethnic differences as
well (e.g., Jung & Lee, 2006; Kaw, 1993; Root, 1990).
Fourth, studies have often dened and measured eating disorders and body dissatisfaction in many different ways. Often relationships among these measures are unknown. As a consequence,
differences among cultures must often be determined using different denitions and different measurement techniques. In the case of
eating disorders these problems are lessened by the existence of standard diagnostic criteria and several widely used measures. However,
the problem is much more serious in the area of body dissatisfaction
because there are no standard criteria and very little consistency in measurement. In recent years, there have been some notable efforts to study
reasonably large numbers of countries using the same measures (e.g.,
Etcoff, Orbach, Scott, & DAgostino, 2004, 2006; Ojala et al., 2007; Wardle
et al., 2006). The 26-country, 40-sample study of preferred body type and
body dissatisfaction by Swami et al. (2009) is particularly important.
AN OVERVIEW OF CULTURAL DIFFERENCES
There is a substantial and growing body of research on ethnic differences within English-speaking countries. For reviews of these differences, see Smolak and Striegel-Moore (2001) or Wildes and Emery (2001).
These studies provide helpful information on ethnic differences within

170

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

a society, and it may be tempting to interpret these studies as if they


provided information on the participants culture of origin. However,
studies of immigrant samples, particularly if they represent postimmigration generations, at best provide only incomplete and potentially misleading information about the participants culture of origin.
Studies of ethnic differences within a specic culture are important,
but they are no substitute for cross-cultural studies (Forbes, in press).
The best source of information on cross-cultural differences would
come from carefully designed investigations with three important features. First, they would employ multiple measures of body dissatisfaction
with established cross-cultural validity. Second, they would employ samples that are comparable in age, education, and socioeconomic class.
Third, the samples would be drawn from cultures that systematically
vary along dimensions that are theoretically linked to differences in body
dissatisfaction. For example, such an investigation might include samples
of college women from three or more countries that vary in their level of
exposure to Western media. Because there are formidable obstacles to
conducting such studies, our knowledge of cross-cultural differences is
based on studies that usually fall far short of these standards.
WESTERN CULTURES AS A BENCHMARK
Because the vast majority of the research on body dissatisfaction and
disordered eating has been done with white English-speaking samples,
these samples have become the de facto standard to which samples
from other cultures are compared. Although it can be argued that this
reects Eurocentric ideas, perhaps even racist or colonialistic biases
(e.g., Kim, Yang, & Hwang, 2006; Sinha, 1996), there are several reasons
why it is useful to use the results from Western studies as a convenient
benchmark. First, far more is known about body dissatisfaction and
disordered eating in these samples. Comparison of new information
with the established body of information often enriches our understanding of both. Second, until very recently body dissatisfaction and
disordered eating appeared to be much more common in Englishspeaking countries. As a consequence, these counties form a natural
endpoint on a dimension of severity. Third, the variables with the
strongest theoretical linkages to body dissatisfaction and disordered
eating (e.g., Western media, increases in gender equality) are greatest
in Western societies. This further supports their use as a natural endpoint on theoretically important dimensions.
BODY DISSATISFACTION AND DISORDERED EATING
AROUND THE WORLD
Until recent years, Western researchers have had little interest in body
dissatisfaction or eating disorders in non-Western societies. In addition,

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

171

research with these groups has been discouraged by a lack of resources


and language and geographic obstacles and limited by the number of
local mental health practitioners and researchers. Although a growing
number of cross-cultural studies are available, most have included very
limited samples of non-Western cultures, and very few have been done
in traditional cultures. In addition, most studies have included only
highly advantaged samples, most commonly college students. These
samples have been exposed to more Western inuences, particularly
media, notions of gender equality, and other variables known to be associated with body dissatisfaction and disordered eating. It is very important to recognize that in developing and undeveloped countries,
particularly those who were once part of the Spanish or British empires,
there are often extremely large cultural and political differences between
the wealthy, who are often descendents of European colonialists, and
the very poor, who are often descendents of indigenous groups. As
Swami et al. (2009) demonstrated, differences attributable to socioeconomic class and rural or urban dwelling within developing countries are
substantially larger than the differences found between countries. This
means that generalizing from highly advantaged samples, especially in
postcolonial countries, to all segments of a society is both hazardous and
unwarranted. However, studies of advantaged samples are important
because it is likely that the level of body dissatisfaction and disordered
eating found in these samples predicts what will be found in the general
population as a society modernizes.
Of necessity we will usually discuss countries in large geographic or
cultural groups. This means that we will make broad generalizations
that will unavoidably obscure both important differences between cultures and ethnic and regional differences within cultures. Because there
is clear evidence that body dissatisfaction and disordered eating are
increasing around the world (e.g., Nasser et al., 2001), we will emphasize the contributions of studies published after 1995.
EAST ASIAN COUNTRIES
The rst clinical and research reports to question the common belief
that eating disorders were limited to afuent Western women came
from East Asian countries. These countries seemed like a most unlikely
source of such reports. This is because traditional East Asian beauty
standards include round faces and mildly plump bodies (Han, 2003),
and these countries have long associated slenderness with poverty,
poor health, and low fecundity (Lee, 1999). In addition, East Asians
tend to have smaller and more slender bodies than Western women.
As a consequence, it would be substantially easier for them to achieve
the very slender Western body ideal in the unlikely event that they
wished to pursue it (Lee, 1999).

172

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Although there were earlier reports of anorexia nervosa and eating


disorders in Japanese samples (e.g., Lerner, Iwawaki, Chihara, & Sorell,
1980; Suematsu, Ishikawa, Kuboki, & Ito, 1985), the well-known work
by Sing Lee and his associates in Hong Kong (e.g., Lee, 1991, 1993,
1998, 1999; Lee, Ho, & Hsu, 1993) appears to have been the primary
impetus for Western researchers to reconsider the conventional wisdom
that these disorders were extremely rare in East Asia. Lees reports
from Hong Kong were followed by reports from other non-Western
nations indicating that both body dissatisfaction and disordered eating
were found not only in East Asia but also in many developed and
developing societies around the world. In addition, this research indicated that both the frequency and severity of these disorders were
increasing in non-Western societies (for a review, see chapter 3 in
Gordon, 2000).
Although results showing relatively high incidences of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating in East Asian samples are no longer rare
or unexpected, recently studies have appeared indicating that some
East Asian groups actually have greater body dissatisfaction than Western comparison samples. Jung and Forbes (2006, 2007) found that
Korean and Chinese college women scored higher than U.S. college
women on ten measures of body dissatisfaction and two measures of
disorder eating. Jung, Forbes, and Lee (in press) found similar results
with ve measures of body dissatisfaction and two measures of disordered eating in samples of early adolescent girls in Korea and the
United States. In their important study of over 10,000 college women
from twenty-two countries, including the United States and 13 other
Western countries, Wardle et al. (2006) found that women from Korea,
although they had the lowest body mass index (BMI)5 of any group,
were the most likely to try to lose weight. Similarly, Swami et al.
(2009) found that women from Korea preferred the most slender
bodies. There is also good evidence of high levels of body dissatisfaction in Japan. For example, Wardle et al. (2006) found that women
from Japan, although they had the third lowest BMI, were the most
likely to perceive themselves as overweight. Similarly, large crosscountry surveys by Etcoff et al. (2004, 2006) found that Japanese
samples had higher levels of body dissatisfaction than eighteen other samples from thirteen different countries, including such benchmark Western
countries as the United States, United Kingdom, France, and Italy.
These remarkable results are particularly relevant to feminist theory.
As previously noted, both sociocultural theory and feminist theory
emphasize the role that Western media plays in the spread of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating to non-Western countries. Consequently, as East Asian countries modernize and are increasingly
exposed to Western media, both theories would predict that they
would show an increase in body dissatisfaction and disordered eating.

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

173

However, only feminist theory specically links body dissatisfaction to


changing roles for women. According to feminist theory, unrealistic
appearance standards represent a backlash against womens strides toward gender equality.
This is a very important issue because the Confucian values that
have long formed the foundation of many East Asian societies include
rigid, restrictive, and repressive gender roles for women. The central
and organizing element in these roles is womens subordination and
submissiveness to men: girls are to be submissive to their fathers,
wives are to be submissive to their husbands, and widows are to be
submissive to their eldest sons. Womens roles and opportunities in
modern East Asian societies like Japan or Korea, and in rapidly modernizing societies such as China, are radically different from traditional
womens roles. As a consequence, feminist theory would predict a
patriarchal backlash that would emphasize particularly severe and
unrealistic appearance standards. In turn, these standards would result
in high levels of body dissatisfaction. Because the speed and magnitude of changing roles for women in these East Asian societies has
been much greater than in Western societies, feminist theory would
predict that women in these societies would have greater body dissatisfaction than Western women. However, conventional sociocultural
theory, or theories that appeal to the nonspecic effects of Westernization, would not make such a prediction.
LATIN AMERICA
Most discussions of body dissatisfaction in Latin American countries
have focused on Brazil because of Brazils well-known interest, sometimes characterized as an obsession, with beautiful bodies and cosmetic surgery (Finger, 2003). As a reection of this interest and social
values of equality, Brazil is the only nation where government health
care routinely provides cosmetic surgery for the poor (Edmonds, 2007;
Finger, 2003). As the cosmetic surgeon Ivo Pitanguy observed, in
Brazil, The poor have a right to be beautiful (Edmonds, 2007).
Depending on the source, Brazil has either the highest volume of cosmetic surgery in the world or is second only to the United States (e.g.,
Edmonds, 2007; Finger, 2003; Woodsworth, 2001). Consistent with these
reports, in their samples of women from ten countries, Etcott et al.
(2006) found that urban Brazilian women have the highest level of cosmetic surgery and were the most likely to consider cosmetic surgery.
(For an anthropological perspective on Brazilian beauty ideals and
cosmetic surgery see Edmonds, 2007)
Interestingly, Brazilian cosmetic surgery practices suggest that exposure to Western appearance standards has changed Brazilian body
preferences. Traditionally, appearance standards have emphasized

174

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

relatively large buttocks and relatively small breasts. Consistent with


these standards, until recently breast augmentation was uncommon,
and most breast surgeries were reductions (Karp, 2001). However,
breast augmentations now greatly outnumber reductions, and there has
been a substantial increase in the size of the augmentations (Finger,
2003; Karp, 2001). It has been suggested that the popularity of breast
augmentations has been one of the reasons why the international
demand, particularly from the United States, for Brazilian fashion models has increased (Karp, 2001).
To the extent that cosmetic surgery reects body dissatisfaction,
Brazil would be expected to have a high level of body dissatisfaction.
Consistent with this expectation, Etcoff et al. (2004) found that Brazilian
womens level of dissatisfaction with their physical attractiveness was
second only to a sample from Japan. Similarly, Brazilians have a high
rate of dieting (Ectoff, 2006) and high scores on standard measures of
disordered eating (Nunes, Barros, Olinto, Camey, & Mari, 2003).
Results from other Latin American countries generally indicate
levels of body dissatisfaction that are less than East Asian countries
but approach levels found in Western countries. For example, a crosscultural study of university women in 23 countries found that the
percentage of Columbian and Venezuelan women who perceived
themselves as overweight or who were trying to lose weight was less
than in Japan but approximately equal to Western countries (Wardle
et al., 2006). Similarly, a recent study in ve Latin American countries
found that 50 to 62 percent of urban dwelling eighth- and ninth-grade
girls wanted to be thinner (McArthur, Holbert, & Pe~
na, 2005).6 In addition, Swami et al. (2009) found that the level of body dissatisfaction
among college women in Chile was equal to the level found in U.S.
samples. (For an important discussion of how changing social conditions, particularly changing roles for women, place Argentinean
women at high risk for body dissatisfaction and disordered eating, see
Meehan and Katzman [2001].)
AFRICA
Most research from sub-Saharan Africa has been conducted with
samples from South Africa. This research generally indicates that white
South African adolescents and college students have levels of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating that are roughly comparable to those
found in Western Europe (e.g., Caradas, Lambert, & Charlton, 2001;
Szabo & le Grange, 2001; Wassenaar, le Grange, Winship, & Lachenicht, 2000). However, until the end of apartheid it appeared that body
dissatisfaction and eating disorders were very rare in black or colored
(mixed race) South Africans. As Szabo and le Grange (2001) noted, limited mental health resources in black clinics and hospitals, along with

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

175

the racist belief that disorders found among afuent white women
would not occur among Africans, probably resulted in an underidentication of these problems during apartheid. Even allowing for this
bias, it seems clear that both body dissatisfaction and disordered eating
have greatly increased since the end of apartheid (Szabo & le Grange,
2001). In fact, most recent studies have shown no meaningful differences in the incidence of these problems in black, colored, and white adolescents or college students (e.g., Caradas et al., 2001; le Grange, Louw,
Russell, Nel & Silkstone, 2006; Senekal, Steyn, Mashego, & Nel, 2001;
Wassenaar et al., 2000). Importantly, there have been some reports that
black college women actually scored higher than white college women
on measures of eating pathology (e.g., le Grange, Telch & Tibbs, 1998).
Consistent with feminist theory, Szabo and le Grange (2001) suggest
that role conicts, particularly the conict between the greatly
expanded roles and opportunities for women and traditional African
values, play an important role in body dissatisfaction and eating pathologies. To the extent that these observations are accurate, there is a
clear possibility that in the not too distant future black and colored
South African women, like East Asian women, will have greater body
dissatisfaction than their Western cohorts.
With the exception of a few single case studies of severe eating disorders (e.g., Buchan & Gregory, 1984), very little information is available on body dissatisfaction and disordered eating in other subSaharan countries. Recent research in Tanzania (e.g., Eddy, Hennessey, &
Thompson-Brenner, 2007; Hennessey, 2008) and Nigeria (e.g., Izevbigie &
Owie, 2006; Toriola, Dolan, Evans, & Adetimole, 1996) indicate that
levels of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating approach those of
Western Europe. However, studies from Ghana suggest that, although
possibly diminishing, the preference for relatively large womens bodies
is still present in both college students (Cogan, Bhalla, Sefa-Dedeh, &
Rothblum, 1996) and adult women (Frederick, Forbes, & Berezovskaya,
2008).
There has been very little research from North Africa. Popenoes
(2004) important work suggests that among traditional North African
societies, a preference for large womens bodies persists. However, evidence from Egypt, the most highly developed North African country,
indicates that college women have levels of body dissatisfaction and
symptoms of disordered eating that are approximately equal to those
of college women in Western societies (e.g., Ford, Dolan, & Evans,
1990; Nasser, 1994a, 1994b).7
Taken as a whole, research in African countries indicates an increase
in body dissatisfaction and eating disorders as the nations modernize.
In the most developed sub-Saharan country, South Africa, the incidence of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating equals, and on
some measures exceeds, the levels found in English-speaking Western

176

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

countries. Similarly, in Egypt, body dissatisfaction and disordered eating approximate levels found in Western Europe. However, there is
evidence that the traditional African preference for larger womens
bodies persists, particularly in relatively undeveloped rural areas (e.g.,
Frederick, Forbes, & Berezovskaya, 2008; Swami et al., 2009).
INDIAN PENINSULA
Recent studies of young women in India have reported levels of
body dissatisfaction and disordered eating that are similar to, although
perhaps slightly lower than, the levels found in Western countries and
Japan (Gupta, Chaturvedi, Chandarana, & Johnson, 2001; Kayano et al.,
2008; Rubin, Gluck, Knoll, Lorence, & Geliebter, 2008). However, these
were highly advantaged urban samples. Information on body dissatisfaction in less advantaged rural Indian women is not available.
Similarly, recent studies among advantaged young women in
Pakistan reported levels of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating
that were similar to, but modestly lower than, levels found in Western
countries (Mahmud & Crittenden, 2007; Mujtaba & Furnham, 2001).
However, studies of women in very conservative and traditional Kashmir indicated levels of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating that
were substantially lower than those found in samples from the United
Kingdom (Choudry & Mumford, 1992; Mumford, Whitehouse, & Choudry, 1992). Importantly, both Mumford, Whitehouse, and Choudry
(1992) and Mahmud and Crittenden (2007) reported that body dissatisfaction and disordered eating were most common in the participants
who were the most Westernized.
SOUTHEAST ASIA AND POLYNESIA
This large area includes a wide variety of societies and nations.
Some parts, such as Singapore, are highly developed, whereas others,
such as some of the remote areas of New Guinea and Borneo, have
been relatively untouched by development. In keeping with other
research using Chinese samples, ethnic Chinese women in Singapore
had greater body dissatisfaction and a higher incidence of eating disorders than Europeans and Australians (Soh, 2008). In contrast, women
living in remote areas of New Guinea exhibited the traditional preference for relatively large bodies (Wesch, 2006). Consistent with the previously cited work of Becker et al. (2002) and Lee and Lee (2000)
linking economic development and exposure to Western culture to
body dissatisfaction and disordered eating, Weschs data suggested an
increasing preference for smaller bodies and greater body dissatisfaction among New Guinea women living in a town with electricity and
access to Western media.

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

177

Studies of college students and adolescents in the Philippines indicate


relatively high levels of behaviors associated with disordered eating. For
example, Madanat, Hawks, and Novella (2006) found that Filipino college students had a much greater incidence of behaviors and attitudes
associated with disordered eating than a U.S. comparison sample. Similarly, Kayano et al. (2008) found more behaviors associated with bulimia
among Filipino women students than among European and Japanese
comparison groups. Similarly, Lorenzo, Lavori, and Lock (2002) found
that abnormal eating attitudes among Filipino high school students were
comparable to those typically reported in Western samples.
Studies in Malaysia also report high levels of body dissatisfaction
and disordered eating. For example, there is evidence of very high levels of body dissatisfaction and high levels of dietary restraint and binge
eating among secondary school girls (Soo, Shariff, Taib, & Abu Samah,
2008). Similarly, high levels of behaviors associated with disordered
eating were reported among ethnic Malay college students (Edman &
Yates, 2004).
Research generally suggests that Western appearance standards, particularly the thin body ideal is minimizing, if not eliminating, traditional preferences for heavier female bodies in much of Southeast Asia.
Interestingly, Sharps, Price-Sharps, and Hanson (2001) found that this
change is detectable even in rural Thailand. Although women in this
region typically engaged in heavy agricultural labor, they preferred a
smaller body than Thai men and a smaller body than a U.S. comparison sample. The results suggest that the Western thin body ideal is
inuencing womens desired body size even when larger bodies are
better adapted to the long hours of harsh physical labor that characterizes their daily lives.
SUMMARY
Research over the last 30 years has made it clear that the once common understanding that eating disorders and body dissatisfaction were
found primarily or exclusively among advantaged white Western
women is not true. Indeed, body dissatisfaction and eating disorders are
found in almost every society that researchers have studied. However,
isolated, traditional, and underdeveloped societies, particularly those in
which food is scarce, may continue to show a preference for larger
bodies. Even among these groups these preferences are diminishing.
One of the most remarkable ndings from cross-cultural studies is
the small size of the differences found in body dissatisfaction. As
Swami et al. (2009) have noted, differences within societies, particularly
those associated with education and urban residence, are usually substantially larger than differences between societies. The relatively small
differences found between societies are, at least in part, a result of the

178

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

convenience samples usually used in most studies. These samples are


typically highly advantaged, urban dwelling, college women. As such,
they are the most likely members of their society to experience the values and roles associated with modernization. These values include both
Western appearance standards and Western values of gender equality:
the two variables most closely linked to body dissatisfaction and disordered eating.
As the thought-provoking and important work of Anderson-Fye
(2003, 2004) indicates, the values and experiences of modernization do
not appear in a vacuum. Instead, they are seen through the lens of the
culture into which they are introduced. With its emphasis on unrealistic appearance standards as a cultural backlash against gender equality,
feminist theory makes a unique contribution to identifying those societies in which appearance standards will be the most unrealistic, the
pressure to attain them the most severe, and the consequential
increases in body dissatisfaction and disordered eating the largest.
Feminist theory would predict that the rapidly modernizing cultures
of East Asia, with the enormous contrast between centuries-old,
Confucian-based, repressive and oppressive gender roles and the radically different roles and opportunities now available to women in these
societies, would lead to sharp increases in body dissatisfaction and disordered eating. Viewed from this perspective, it is not surprising that
recent research shows that college women in Korea, Japan, and China
have greater body dissatisfaction and disordered eating than Western
college women. As modernization and market economies continue to
impact traditional and developing societies, women in other societies
may also develop levels of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating
that exceed the levels found in their Western sisters. Scattered evidence
suggests that this may be happening already in South Africa and the
rapidly developing countries of South America.
NOTES
Although this chapter will be restricted to the experiences of
women, men also experience body dissatisfaction and eating disorders.
The absence of any discussion of these disorders in men reects the
context in which this chapter appears, and it is not intended to suggest
that the experiences of men are unimportant. For a review of weight
and shape concerns in men see McCabe and Ricciardelli (2004). For a
review of eating disorders in boys and men see Muise, Stein, and
Arbess (2003) and Freeman (2005).
1. The formal diagnostic criteria for anorexia nervosa, bulimia, and other
eating disorders can be found in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders of the American Psychiatric Association, Fourth Edition (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and the ICD-10 Classication of Mental and Behavioural

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

179

Disorders (World Health Organization, 1992). There are many excellent references on the diagnosis and treatment of eating disorders. The interested reader
should consult recent edited volumes or handbooks, such as Thompson (2004).
2. The effects of both social class and ethnicity, although still present, may
have diminished in recent years (Gard & Freeman, 1996; Grabe & Hyde, 2006;
McClelland & Crisp, 2001).
3. The most extreme preference for large female bodies appears to be found
among the Azawagh Arabs of Niger. Here, the forced feeding of girls begins at
the age of 5 or 6 and continues until marriage. Even following marriage many
women continue to eat large amounts of food in an effort to become as large
as possible. For an insightful and sensitive description of this society, see Popenoe (2004).
4. A complete discussion of the complex problems associated with crosscultural research is far beyond the scope of this chapter. The interested reader
can nd more information in van de Vijver and Leung (1997) and Ember and
Ember (2001).
5. Body mass index is a widely used measure of body size. It is computed
with the formula: BMI = Weight in Kilograms/Height in Meters2.
6. In a sixth sample from Havana, Cuba, 29 percent wanted to be thinner,
and 31 percent wanted to be heavier. This may be a consequence of food scarcity and Cubas relative isolation from Western beauty ideals.
7. For insightful historical and cultural discussion of women in Egypt,
including parallels between the new veiling phenomenon and anorexia nervosa, see Nasser (2003).

REFERENCES
Anderson-Fye, E. P. (2003). Never leave yourself: Ethnopsychology as mediator
of psychological globalization among Belizean schoolgirls. Ethos, 31, 5994.
Anderson-Fye, E. P. (2004). A Coca-Cola shape: Cultural change, body
image, and eating disorders in San Andres, Belize. Culture, Medicine and
Psychiatry, 28, 561595.
Anderson-Fye, E. P., & Becker, A. E. (2004). Sociocultural aspects of eating disorders. In J. K. Thompson (Ed.), Handbook of eating disorders and obesity
(pp. 565589). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Bardone-Cone, A., & Cass, K. (2007). What does viewing a pro-anorexia website do? An experimental examination of website exposure and moderating
effects. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 40, 537548.
Becker, A. E., Burwell, R. A., Gilman, S. E., Herzog, D. B., & Hamburg,
P. (2002). Eating behaviours and attitudes following prolonged exposure to
television among ethnic Fijian adolescent girls. British Journal of Psychiatry,
180, 509514.
Bordo, S. (1993). Unbearable weight: Feminism, Western culture, and the body.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Brislin, R. W. (1986). The wording and translations of research instruments. In
W. J. Lonner & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Field methods in cross-cultural research
(pp. 137164). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Brown, P. J. (1991). Culture and the evolution of obesity. Human Nature, 2,
3157.

180

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Brown, P. J., & Konner, M. (1987). An anthropological perspective on obesity.


Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 499, 2946.
Bruch, H. (1980). Preconditions for the development of anorexia nervosa. The
American Journal of Psychoanalysis, 40, 169172.
Brumberg, J. J. (1988). Fasting girls: The emergence of anorexia nervosa as a modern
disease. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Buchan, T., & Gregory, L. D. (1984). Anorexia nervosa in a Black Zimbabwean.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 326330.
Bynum, C. W. (1987). Holy feast and holy fast: The religious signicance of food to
medieval women. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Cafri, G., Yamamiya, Y., Brannick, M., & Thompson, J. K. (2005). The inuence
of sociocultural factors on body image: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology:
Science & Practice, 12, 421433.
Caradas, A. A., Lambert, E. V., & Charlton, K. E. (2001). An ethnic comparison
of eating attitudes and associated body image concerns in adolescent South
African schoolgirls. Journal of Human Nutrition & Dietetics, 14, 111120.
Cash, T. F. (2002). A negative body image: Evaluating epidemiological evidence. In T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of theory,
research, and clinical practice (pp. 269276). New York: Guilford.
Cash, T. F., & Henry, P. E. (1995). Womens body images: The results of a
national survey in the U.S.A. Sex Roles, 33, 1928.
Cassidy, C. M. (1991). The good body: When big is better. Medical Anthropology,
13, 181213.
Catina, A., & Joja, O. (2001). Emerging markets: Submerging women. In
M. Nasser, M. A. Katzman, & R. A. Gordon (Eds.), Eating disorders and cultures in transitions (pp. 111119). New York: Taylor & Francis.
Cogan, J. C., Bhalla, S. K., Sefa-Dedeh, A., & Rothblum, E. D. (1996). A comparison study of United States and African students on perceptions of obesity
and thinness. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 98113.
Dohnt, H., & Tiggemann, M. (2006). The contribution of peer and media inuences to the development of body satisfaction and self-esteem in young
girls: A prospective study. Developmental Psychology, 42, 929936.
Dworkin, A. (1974). Woman hating. New York: Dutton.
Eddy, K. T., Hennessey, M., & Thompson-Brenner, H. (2007). Eating pathology
in East African women: The role of media exposure and globalization.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 196202.
Edman, J. L. & Yates, A. (2004). Eating attitudes among college students in
Malaysia: An ethnic and gender comparison. European Eating Disorders
Review, 12, 190196.
Edmonds, A. (2007). The poor have a right to be beautiful: Cosmetic surgery
in neoliberal Brazil. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 13,
363381.
Ember, C. R., & Ember, M. (2001). Cross-cultural research methods. Walnut Creek,
CA: AltaMira Press.
Etcoff, N., Orbach, S., Scott, J., & DAgostino, H. (2004). The real truth about
beauty: A global report. Retrieved January 23, 2009, from http://www.
campaignforrealbeauty.com/uploadedles/dove_white_paper_nal.pdf.
Etcoff, N., Orbach, S., Scott, J., & DAgostino, H. (2006). Beyond stereotypes:
Rebuilding the foundations of beauty beliefs. Retrieved March 25, 2008, from

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

181

http://msbrandent.vo.llnwd.net/d1/msnbeet_doveus/cfrb/workshops/
beyond_stereotypes.pdf.
Faludi, S. (1991). Backlash: The undeclared war against women. New York: Crown.
Finger, C. (2003). Brazilian beauty: Brazils cosmetic surgery industry is
thriving, but why is beauty so important? Lancet, 362, 1560.
Forbes, G. B. (in press). Cross-cultural research methods and the study of gender. In Handbook of gender research in psychology (Vol. 2). New York:
Springer.
Forbes, G. B., Adams-Curtis, L., Jobe, R. L., White, K. B., Revak, J., Zivcic-Becirevic,
I., et al. (2005). Body dissatisfaction in college women and their mothers: Cohort
effects, developmental effects, and the inuences of body size, sexism, and the
thin body ideal. Sex Roles, 53, 281298.
Forbes, G. B., Doroszewicz, K., Card, K., & Adams-Curtis, L. (2004). Association of the thin body ideal, ambivalent sexism, and self-esteem with body
acceptance and the preferred body size of college women in Poland and
the United States. Sex Roles, 50, 331345
Ford, D. A., Dolan, B. M., & Evans, C. (1990). Cultural factors in the eating disorders: A study of body shape preferences of Arab students. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 34, 501507.
Frederick, D. A., Forbes, G. B., & Berezovskaya, A. (2008). Body dissatisfaction
and perceptions of the attractive female body among women and men
from the Ukraine, Ghana, and the United States. Psychological Topics, 17,
203219.
Frederick, D. A., Peplau, L. A., & Lever, J. (2006). The swimsuit issue: Correlates of body image in a sample of 52,677 heterosexual adults. Body Image,
3, 413419.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. (1997). Objectication theory: Toward understanding womens lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 21, 173206.
Freeman, A. C. (2005). Eating disorders in males: A review. South African
Psychiatry Review, 8, 5864.
Gard, M. C. E., & Freeman, C. P. (1996). The dismantling of a myth: A review
of eating disorders and socioeconomic status. International Journal of Eating
Disorders, 20, 112.
Garner, D. M. (1997). The 1997 Body Image Survey results. Psychology Today,
30, 3044, 7576, 84.
Gilbert, S. C., Keery, H., & Thompson, J. K. (2005). The medias role in body
image and eating disorders. In E. Cole & J. H. Daniel (Eds.), Featuring
females: Feminist analyses of media (pp. 4156). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Gordon, R. A. (2000). Eating disorders: Anatomy of a social epidemic (2nd ed.).
Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Grabe, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2006). Ethnicity and body dissatisfaction among
women in the United States: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132,
622640.
Grogan, S. (2008). Body image: Understanding body dissatisfaction in men, women,
and children (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.
Gupta, M. A., Chaturvedi, S. K., Chandarana, P. C., & Johnson, A. M. (2001).
Weight-related body image concerns among 18-24-year-old women in

182

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Canada and India: An empirical comparative study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 50, 193198.
Han, M. (2003). Body image dissatisfaction and eating disturbance among
Korean college female students: Relationship to media exposure, upward
comparison, and perceived quality. Communications Studies, 54, 6578.
Harrison, K. (2000). Television viewing, fat stereotyping, body shape standards,
and eating disorder symptomatology in grade school children. Communication Research, 27, 617640.
Hennessey, M. (2008). Body dissatisfaction, eating disorders and a media literacy intervention among Tanzanian females. (Doctoral dissertation, Boston
University, 2008). Dissertation Abstracts Inernational, 69, 679.
Holmstrom, A. J. (2004). The effects of media on body image: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 48, 196217.
Hudson, J. I., Hiripi, E., Pope, H. G. Jr., & Kessler, R. C. (2007). The prevalence
and correlates of eating disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey
Replication. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 348358.
Izevbigie, T. I., & Owie, I. (2006). Eating disorders and personality prole of
female secondary school students in Benin Metropolis, Nigeria. Journal of
Instructional Psychology, 33, 162169.
Jeffreys, S. (2005). Beauty and misogyny: Harmful cultural practices in the West.
New York: Routledge.
Jung, J., & Forbes, G. B. (2006). Multidimensional assessment of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating in Korean and U.S. college women: A comparative study. Sex Roles, 55, 3950.
Jung, J., & Forbes, G. B. (2007). Body dissatisfaction and disordered eating
among college women in China, South Korea, and the United States: Contrasting predictions from sociocultural and feminist theories. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 31, 381393.
Jung, J., Forbes, G. B., & Lee, Y-J. (in press). Body dissatisfaction and behaviors
associated with disordered eating among early adolescent boys and girls
from South Korea and the United States. Sex Roles.
Jung, J., & Lee, S-H. (2006). Cross-cultural comparisons of appearance selfschema, body image, self-esteem, and dieting behaviors between Korean
and U.S. women. Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal, 34, 350365.
Kagitcibasi, C. (2005). Modernization does not mean Westernization: Emergence of a different pattern. In W. Friedlmeier, P. Chakkarath, & B.
Schwarz (Eds.), Culture and human development: The importance of crosscultural research for the social sciences (pp. 255272). Hove, England: Psychology Press/Erlbaum (UK).
Karp, J. (2001, January 10). Full-gured Gisele prompts Barzillians to reassess
the breastCelebrities called siliconadas sing the praises of implants:
The Tchan has moved. Wall Street Journal, 21.
Kaw, E. (1993). Medicalization of racial features: Asian American women and
cosmetic surgery. Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 7, 7489.
Kayano, M., Yoshiuchi, K., Al-Adawi, S., Viernes, N., Dorvlo, A. S. S., Kumano,
H., et al. (2008). Eating attitudes and body dissatisfaction in adolescents:
Cross-cultural study. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 62, 1725.
Kim, O., & Kim, K. (2003). Comparisons of body mass index, perception of
body weight, body shape satisfaction, and self-esteem among Korean
adolescents. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97, 13391346.

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

183

Kim, O., & Yoon, H. (2000). Factors associated with weight control behaviors
among high school females with normal body weight. Journal of the Korean
Academy of Nursing, 30, 391410.
Kim, U., Yang, K., & Hwang, K. (2006). Contributions to indigenous and
cultural psychology: Understanding people in context. In U. Kim, K. Yang, &
K. Hwang (Eds.), Indigenous and cultural psychology: Understanding people in
context (pp. 325). New York: Springer.
Lee, S. (1991). Anorexia nervosa in Hong Kong: A Chinese perspective. Psychological Medicine, 21, 703711.
Lee, S. (1993). How abnormal is the desire for slimness? A survey of eating attitudes and behaviour among Chinese undergraduates in Hong Kong. Psychological Medicine, 23, 437451.
Lee, S. (1998). Global modernity and eating disorders in Asia. European Eating
Disorders Review, 6, 151153.
Lee, S. (1999). Fat, fatigue and the feminine: The changing cultural experience
of women in Hong Kong. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 23, 5173.
Lee, S. (2004). Engaging culture: An overdue task for eating disorders research.
Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry, 28, 617621.
Lee, S., Chiu, H. F., & Chen, C. (1989). Anorexia nervosa in Hong Kong: Why
not more in Chinese? British Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 683688.
Lee, S., Ho, T. P., & Hsu, L. K. (1993). Fat phobic and non-fat phobic anorexia
nervosa: A comparative study of 70 Chinese patients in Hong Kong. Psychological Medicine, 23, 9991017.
Lee, S., & Lee, A. M. (2000). Disordered eating in three communities of China: A
comparative study of female high school students in Hong Kong, Shenzhen,
and rural Hunan. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 317327.
Lerner, R. M., Iwawaki, S., Chihara, T., & Sorell, G. T. (1980). Self-concept, selfesteem, and body attitudes among Japanese male and female adolescents.
Child Development, 51, 847855.
Li, Y., Hu, X., Ma, W., Wu, J., & Ma, G. (2005). Body image perceptions among
Chinese children and adolescents. Body Image, 2, 91103.
Lorenzo, C. R., Lavori, P. W., & Lock, J. D. (2002). Eating attitudes in high
school students in the Philippines: A preliminary study. Eating and Weight
Disorders, 7, 202209.
Madanat, H. N., Hawks, S. R., & Novella, M. (2006). A comparison of disordered eating attitudes and behaviors among Filipino and American college
students. Eating and Weight Disorders, 11, 133138.
Mahmud, N., & Crittenden, N. (2007). A comparative study of body image of
Australian and Pakistani young females. British Journal of Psychology, 98,
187197.
Makino, M., Tsuboi, K., & Dennerstein, L. (2004). Prevalence of eating disorders: A comparison of Western and Non-Western countries. Medscape General Medicine, 6, 4962. Retrieved January 3, 2009, from http://
www.medscape.com/viewarticle/487413.
McArthur, L. H., Holbert, D., & Pena, M. (2005). An exploration of the attitudinal and perceptual dimensions of body image among male and female
adolescents from six Latin American cities. Adolescence, 40, 801816.
McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2003). Body image and strategies to lose
weight and increase muscle among boys and girls. Health Psychology, 22,
3946.

184

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

McClelland, L., & Crisp, A. (2001). Anorexia nervosa and social class. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 150156.
Meehan, O. L., & Katzman, M. A. (2001). Argentina: The social body at risk. In
M. Nasser, M. Katzman, & R. A. Gordon (Eds.), Eating disorders and cultures in transition (pp. 148162). New York: Routledge.
Miller, M. N., & Pumariega, A. J. (2001). Culture and eating disorders: A historical and cross-cultural review. Psychiatry, 64, 93110.
Muise, A. M., Stein, D. G., & Arbess, G. (2003). Eating disorders in adolescent
boys: A review of the adolescent and young adult literature. Journal of
Adolescent Health, 33, 427435.
Mujtaba, T., & Furnham, A. (2001). A cross-cultural study of parental conict
and eating disorders in a non-clinical sample. International Journal of Social
Psychiatry, 47, 2435.
Mumford, D. B., Whitehouse, A. M., & Choudry, I. Y. (1992). Survey of eating
disorders in English-medium schools in Lahore, Pakistan. International
Journal of Eating Disorders, 11, 173184.
Nasser, M. (1994a). Culture and research instruments: The performance of
the Eating Attitude Test Questionnaire in the Arabic culture. Arab Journal
of Psychiatry, 5, 111.
Nasser, M. (1994b). Screening for abnormal eating attitudes in a population of
Egyptian secondary school girls. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 29, 2530.
Nasser, M. (1997). Culture and weight consciousness. New York: Routledge.
Nasser, M. (2003). Eating disorders in Egypt: Themes and variations. In G. M. Ruggiero (Ed.), Eating disorders in the Mediterranean area: An exploration in transcultural psychology (pp. 7992). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
Nasser, M., Katzman, M. A., & Gordon, R. A. (Eds.). (2001). Eating disorders and
cultures in transition. New York: Brunner-Routledge.
Nunes, M. A., Barros, F. C., Olinto, M. T. A., Camey, S., & Mari, J. D. J. (2003).
Prevalence of abnormal eating behaviours and inappropriate methods of
weight control in young women from Brazil: A population-based study.
Eating and Weight Disorders, 8, 100106.
Ojala, K., Vereecken, C., V
alimaa, R., Currie, C., Villberg, J., Tynj
al
a, J., et al.
(2007). Attempts to lose weight among overweight and non-overweight
adolescents: a cross-national survey. The International Journal of Behavioral
Nutrition and Physical Activity, 4, 5059.
Pinhas, L., Toner, B., Ali, A., Garnkel, P., & Stuckless, N. (1999). The effects of
the ideal of female beauty on mood and body satisfaction. International
Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 223226.
Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2002). Causes of eating disorders. Annual Review of
Psychology, 53, 187213.
Popenoe, R. (2004). Feeding desire: Fatness, beauty, and sexuality among a Saharan
people. New York: Routledge.
Prah, P. M. (2006). Eating disorders: Is societal pressure to be thin to blame?
The CQ Researcher, 16, 121144.
Prince, R. (1983). Is anorexia nervosa a culture-bound syndrome? Transcultural
Psychiatric Research Review, 20, 299300.
Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1984). Women and weight: A
normative discontent. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 32, 267307.

Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

185

Root, M. P. (1990). Disordered eating in women of color. Sex Roles, 22, 525536.
Rubin, B., Gluck, M. E., Knoll, C. M., Lorence, M., & Geliebter, A. (2008). Comparison of eating disorders and body image disturbances between Eastern
and Western countries. Eating and Weight Disorders, 13, 7380.
Ryu, H. R., Lyle, R. M., & Mccabe, G. P. (2003). Factors associated with weight
concerns and unhealthy eating patterns among young Korean females.
Eating Disorders, 11, 129141.
Senekal, M., Steyn, N. P., Mashego, T. B., & Nel, J. H. (2001). Evaluation of
body shape, eating disorders and weight management related parameters
in black female students of rural and urban origins. South African Journal of
Psychology, 31, 4553.
Sharps, M. J., Price-Sharps, J. L., & Hanson, J. (2001). Body image preference in
the United States and rural Thailand: An exploratory study. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 135, 518526.
Shefeld, J., Tse, K., and Sofronoff, K. (2005). A comparison of body-image dissatisfaction and eating disturbance among Australian and Hong Kong
women. European Eating Disorders Review, 13, 112124.
Silverstein, B., & Perlick, D. (1995). The cost of competence: Why inequality causes
depression, eating disorders, and illness in women. New York: Oxford University Press.
Sinha, D. (1996). Culture as the target and culture as the source: A review of
cross-cultural psychology in Asia. Psychology and Developing Societies, 8,
83105.
Smolak, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. H. (2001). Challenging the myth of the golden
girl: Ethnicity and eating disorders. In R. H. Striegel-Moore & L. Smolak
(Eds.), Eating disorders: Innovative directions in research and practice
(pp. 111132). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Soh, N. L. (2008). Body image disturbance in young North European and East
Asian women with and without eating disorders in Australia and in Singapore. European Eating Disorders Review, 16, 287296.
Soo, K. L., Shariff, Z. M., Taib, M. N. M., & Abu Samah, B. (2008). Eating
behavior, body image, and self-esteem of adolescent girls in Malaysia.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 106, 833844.
Spitzer, B. L., Henderson, K. A., & Zivian, M. T. (1999). Gender differences in
population versus media body sizes: A comparison over four decades. Sex
Roles, 40, 545565.
Suematsu, H., Ishikawa, H., Kuboki, T., & Ito, T. (1985). Statistical studies on
anorexia nervosa in Japan: Detailed clinical data on 1,011 patients. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 43, 96103.
Swami, V., Frederick, D. A., Aavik, T., Alcalay, L., J
uri Allik, J., Anderson, D., et al.
(2009). Body weight ideals and body dissatisfaction in 26 countries across 10 world
regions: Results of the International Body Project I. Unpublished manuscript.
Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2008). The psychology of physical attraction. New
York: Routledge.
Sypeck, M. F., Gray, J. J., & Ahrens, A. H. (2004). No longer just a pretty face:
Fashion magazines depictions of ideal female beauty from 1959 to 1999.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 36, 342347.
Szabo, C. P., & le Grange, D. (2001). Eating disorders and the politics of identity: The South African experience. In M. Nasser, M. A. Katzman, & R. A.

186

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Gordon (Eds.), Eating disorders and cultures in transition (pp. 2433). New
York: Brunner-Routledge.
Thompson, J. K. (Ed). (2004). Handbook of eating disorders and obesity. Hoboken,
NJ: Wiley.
Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). Exacting beauty: Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Tiggemann, M. (2004). Body image across the adult life span: Stability and
change. Body Image, 1, 2941.
Tiggemann, M., & McGill, B. (2004). The role of social comparison in the effect
of magazine advertisements on womens mood and body dissatisfaction.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 2344.
Toriola, A. L., Dolan, B. M., Evans, C., & Adetimole, O. (1996). Weight satisfaction of Nigerian women in Nigeria and Britain: Inter-generational and
cross-cultural inuences. European Eating Disorders Review, 4, 8494.
Travis, C. B., Meginnis, K. L., & Bardari, K. M. (2000). Beauty, sexuality, and
identity: The social control of women. In C. B. Travis & J. W. White (Eds.),
Sexuality, society, and feminism (pp. 237272). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Tsai, G. (2000). Eating disorders in the Far East. Eating and Weight Disorders, 5,
183197.
van de Vijver, F., & Leung, K. (1997). Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural
research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Vaughan, K., & Fouts, G. (2003). Changes in television and magazine exposure
and eating disorder symptomatology, Sex Roles, 49, 313320.
Ward, L. M., & Harrison, K. (2005). The impact of media use on girls beliefs
about gender roles, their bodies, and sexual relationships: A research synthesis. In E. Cole & J. H. Daniel (Eds.), Featuring females: Feminist analyses of
media (pp. 323). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Wardle, J., Haase, A. M., & Steptoe, A. (2006). Body image and weight control
in young adults: International comparisons in university students from 22
countries. International Journal of Obesity, 30, 644651.
Wassenaar, D., le Grange, D., Winship, J., & Lachenicht, L. (2000). The prevalence of eating disorder pathology in a cross-ethnic population of female
students in South Africa. European Eating Disorders Review, 8, 225236.
Wesch, S. L. (2006). Factors related to appearance satisfaction among women native
to the mountain Ok area of Papua New Guinea. Retrieved from ProQuest
Digital Dissertations (AAI3187220).
Wildes, J. E., & Emery, R. E. (2001). The roles of ethnicity and culture in the
development of eating disturbance and body dissatisfaction: A meta-analytic
review. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 521551.
Wolf, N. (1991). The beauty myth: How images of beauty are used against women.
New York: Morrow.
Woodsworth, N. (2001, August 25). Operating on two levels Nicholas Woodsworth visits Rio de Janeiro, cosmetic capital of the world, to meet Jekyll
and Hyde surgeon who uses his skills on both rich and poor. Financial
Times (London), 1.
World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classication of mental and behavioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva,
Switzerland: World Health Association.

Chapter 12

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women


in Schools around the World
Susan Strauss

The joy of inquiry and learning in the classroom can sometimes be


vanquished by the power of the educational system and by the teacher,
thereby infringing on the right of a girl or woman to learn, develop,
and be nourished in a safe environment free from fear. Academia is a
microcosm of society complete with gendered dynamics where the inequality between the sexes is ripe for exploitation (Zalk, 1990).
Unfortunately, schools are not a safe haven for children, particularly
girls, and women who are deluged with sexual violence (Education
For All Global Monitoring Team, 2003) to such a degree that it becomes
a normalized aspect of school life (Leach, 2006; Mirsky, 2003; Plan,
2008). Schools that condone and approve of other types of violence,
such as corporal punishment and bullying, will also support sexual violence (Jones, Moore, Villar-Marquez, & Broadbent, 2008; Leach, 2006).
This chapter focuses on sexual violence to girls and women at school;
however, the behavior does not occur in a vacuum; it is inuenced by
nonsexual violence and the culture in which it resides.
Sexual harassment, including sexual assault, by boys and male
teachers perpetrated on girls at or around school is a worldwide abomination studied by numerous nongovernmental organizations (NGOs)
and by the two-year worldwide United Nations (UN) Study on Violence
Against Children (United Nations, 2006). Sexual violence is a violation
of the rights of girls, threatening their right to achieve an education,
and injuring their psychological and physical well-being (Amnesty
International, 2008; Jones & Espey, 2008; Jones et al., 2008; Leach, 2006;

188

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Mirsky, 2003; Ward, 2005). Leach asserted that sexual violence to girls
in schools has far reaching consequences to society:
It undermines the pursuit of internationally agreed public health goals to
enable adolescents to deal in a positive way with their sexuality and to
reduce unintended pregnancies and sexually transmitted infections
including HIV infection. For girls and young women, it severely limits
their ability to achieve their educational potential. For society, therefore,
it undercuts the transformatory power of education. Female education
has been shown not only to contribute to improved family health but to
be a major driver of social and economic development. (p. 1)

Gathering information for this chapter proved to be an exciting and


sometimes frustrating challenge. Some secondary sources are used due
to language differences in, or difculty attaining, the primary sources.
Some study results were found in online newspapers and posted on
Web sites, which make it impossible to determine the methodology
used in comparison with study outcomes from other research and to
determine the actual level of sexual harassment throughout the world.
Searching for information required attempts to contact international
agencies, NGOs, and international researchers, sometimes, unfortunately, without responses in return. Some references were unavailable;
for example, in the search for one particular book, only one copy was
foundin a library in Germany. One major challenge in studying the
literature of sexual violence in schools around the world is the lack of
a standardized gendered framework and an operationalized denitional construct (Witkowska, 2005).
A study that explored school violence in 37 nations by Akiba,
LeTendre, Baker, and Goesling (2002) rarely recognized sexual violence, other than rape, to integrate within the framework of school violence, thereby degendering the construct. Likewise, Smiths (2003)
research from 17 European countries was not sensitive to the sexual violence construct. This makes dening sexual violence across cultures a
difcult task. The methodology and context shape the ndings (Mirsky,
2003). These data represent different methodologies and timeframes in
different languages and from a variety of culturesmeaning that there
can be a potential of perceived bias on the construct of sexual violence
in schools (Barak, 1997). Cultural differences may account for what
countries dene as sexual harassment (DeSouza & Solberg, 2003), sexual violence, and sexual bullying.
Many countries do not use the term sexual harassment, but rather
use other terms such as sexual bullying (Duncan, 1999), gender-based
violence (Dunne, Humphreys, & Leach, 2003), sexual abuse (Leach &
Sitaram, 2007), and sexual violence (Mirsky, 2003), among others. In
searching the databases for information, all of the above terms were

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

189

used. As Barak (1997) noted, it is not the phenomenon of sexual harassment that is different among countries, but rather the way it is
being behaviorally manifested, which is probably due to different behavioral standards related to different cultures (p. 268). With this
thought in mind, this chapter will use the terms mentioned interchangeably to demonstrate the broad scope of the phenomenon. Some
sexually harassing behaviors may be found in some of the literature
addressing school bullying (Stein, 2003, 2005). However, the literature
on bullying was not searched in researching sexual violence in schools
for this chapter because the term, bullying, is a gender-neutral reference to behaviors that are not often studied through a gendereddynamic lens, even though some behaviors that are labeled as bullying,
would fall within sexual harassment (Stein, 1995).
Whatever the term used, the behaviors are generally indistinguishable and include a progression of behaviors from sexist and misogynist
comments, verbal sexual comments, physical fondling, sexual assault,
and rape. According to the World Health Organization (WHO), For
many young women, the most common place where sexual coercion
and harassment are experienced is in school (WHO, 2002). Gendered
violence against girls and women in education is a pandemic that transcends race, culture, geography, religion, and class (Ward, 2005).
With the electronic communication age comes cyber-harassment/
bullying through e-mails, text messaging, Facebook, and mobile
phones. Dalaimo (1997) stated that a closer look reveals that the same
types of inequalities and discrimination that plague the physical world
are also present in the virtual world. Power is inherently unequal, and
electronic communication is no exception (p. 101). Though not all
online perpetrators are classmates, students are sometimes unaware
who the harasser may be and when the harassment continues during
school, it distracts them from their schoolwork and can be psychologically traumatic (Shariff, 2004). Shariff and Gouin (2006) and Barak
(2005) describe this cyber violence in many forms including, rude, offensive, sexist, homophobic, misogynist and vulgar messages or photos,
and pornography sent to groups and individuals via text messaging,
chat rooms or Web-logs.
Sexual harassment occurs in public places of the school (Leach, 2006;
Mirsky, 2003; Stein, 1995; Timmerman, 2003), with faculty and staff
observing the behavior and often colluding with the perpetrators by
not intervening to stop the abuse. The message to the boys and the
girls is that sexual violence is an accepted school norm; this, then, can
be a catalyst for increased sexual violence within the school and the
community. When teachers do intervene, even in instances of sexual
assault, the boys face few consequences by the school.
Sexual violence is a reection of gender inequality and manifests
itself specically to cultural beliefs and tenets that also condone

190

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

gender-based violence (Amnesty International, 2008; Human Rights


Watch [HRW], 2001a; Leach, 2006; United Nations, 2006). Some teen
girls may even be killed by their families to reestablish the familys
honor for being abused (Amnesty International, 2008; United States
Agency for International Development, 2008). Gender violence is relegated by culture. The following statement by Leach (2006) acknowledged the tapestry of sexual violence.
Violence in the schools cannot be divorced from violence in the home,
the community and the workplace. This violence originates in the imbalance in power between males and females, in the gendered hierarchy
and separation of tasks and responsibilities and in socially accepted
views of what constitutes masculine and feminine behavior. The school,
alongside the family, is a prime site for the construction of gender identity and gender relations built on socially sanctioned inequalities. (p. 27)

Most of the research on gendered violence of girls in and around


schools has approached the issue from a heterosexual construct and
ignored girl-to-girl violence and student-to-teacher violence (Leach &
Humphreys, 2007). Both genders can be victims of sexual violence at
school, but the research demonstrates that girls are the more likely targets of sexual assault and heterosexist sexual harassment (Leach, 2006;
Ward, 2005). This chapter focuses on the sexual harassment of female
students by male classmates and teachers; it will not address the sexual
violence toward female faculty, female-to-female aggression, female-tomale harassment, or sexual violence to males.
DEFINITION OF SEXUAL HARASSMENT IN ACADEMIA
For the purpose of standardizing a denition of sexual harassment
for this chapter, two denitions from the United States will be used.
The U.S. Department of Education, Ofce of Civil Rights (OCR) denes
sexual harassment as the following (2000):
Unwelcome sexual advances, requests for sexual favors, and other verbal,
nonverbal, or physical conduct of a sexual nature by an employee, by
another student, or by a third party, which is sufciently severe, persistent, or pervasive to limit a students ability to participate in or benet
from an education program or activity, or to create a hostile or abusive
educational environment. (p. 264)

The National Advisory Council on Womens Educational Programs


asserted that,
Academic sexual harassment is the use of authority to emphasize the sexuality or sexual identity of the student in a manner which prevents or

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

191

impairs that students full enjoyment of educational benets, climate, or


opportunities. (Paludi & Barickman, 1991, p. 4)

Till (1980) grouped the sexual harassment experiences of college


women into ve categories, with each category advancing in severity
from the previous one. Using Tills categories, and correlating with definitions from case law, Fitzgerald, Gelfand, and Drasgow (1993) recategorized Tills ve groups of behaviors into three: gender harassment,
sexual coercion, and unwanted sexual attention. Gender harassment is
the most commonly experienced harassment (Barak, Fisher, & Houston,
1992; Vaux, 1993) and includes a sweeping range of behaviors that
denote misogyny, including sexist comments, taunts, and pornography,
to name a few. Unwanted sexual verbal and nonverbal offensive
behaviors constitute unwanted sexual behavior. And nally, sexual
coercion is the exchange of grades for sex. These three categories of
sexual harassment will be used to discuss the construct throughout the
chapter.
SEXUAL VIOLENCE IN SCHOOL LAWS, INTERNATIONAL
TREATIES, AND CONVENTIONS
It is beyond the limits of this chapter to discuss whether various
countries have national laws outlawing sexual harassment, and to
review the laws of those countries which do address sexual violence in
their schools. However, whether a country has specic laws outlawing
sexual harassment in education, there are a number of international
laws, treaties, declarations, and conventions that the majority of countries have ratied that do outlaw the behavior. Treaties carry legal
weight and declarations are a political catalyst for a variety of abuses
against women and children (Education For All Global Monitoring
Report Team, 2003). International human rights law requires countries
to prevent and to intervene against human rights violations including
sexual assault and rape. In addition, monitoring of any government
treaty is required to ensure implementation of the treaty and compliance with its requirements (Mirsky, 2003).
According to law professor Fionnuala Ni Aolain at the University of
Minnesota, in 1995, the United Nations implemented the Convention
on the Elimination of All Forms of Discrimination Against Women
(CEDAW), which all countries have ratied except eight including the
United States, which requires that women and men receive equal treatment, including the right to an education (International Womens Day
Celebration: Transforming the World Through Womens Voices, March
14, 2009 in Minneapolis, MN). Gender-based violence is considered a
form of discrimination and is therefore outlawed by CEDAW. Violence
against women is dened by CEDAW as, any act of gender-based

192

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

violence that result in, or is likely to result in, physical, sexual or psychological harm or suffering to women (United Nations, 1993). The
declaration recognizes the vulnerability of girl children and specically
singles out schools as a location of gender violence.
Governments worldwide, except the United States and Somalia
(Education For All Global Monitoring Team, 2003), committed to the
1989 Convention on the Rights of the Child (CRC). CRC requires countries to protect children from sexual abuse and sexual exploitation and
to provide equal education to both genders (HRW, 2001a).
The right to education is also proclaimed in the International Covenant on Economic, Social and Cultural Rights (ICESCR) and the Universal Declaration of Human Rights, with both ensuring equal
education to both males and females (HRW, 2001a; Education For All
Global Monitoring Team, 2003). In addition, the International Covenant
on Civil and Political Rights (ICCPR) requires equal rights for men and
women and to establish effective redress for abuse.
In 1996, the WHO created two resolutions recognizing that gender
violence to women and children was a public health priority (WHO,
2002) In 2000, Dakar hosted the United Nations Millennium Summit,
and announced the eight UN Millennium Development Goals. Goal
number two is to achieve universal primary education by 2015, and
goal three requires gender inequality in all levels of education by 2015
(http://www.un.org/millenniumgoals/gender.shtml).
Kazue (2004) asserted that legalization is not the sole answer: It is, however, important to bear in mind that legislation and regulation from
above is not the only way to deal with sexual harassment, a phenomenon
that is deeply rooted in culture, gender norms and gender power structures. Without genuine respect for women and womens human rights,
sexual harassment can still happen despite laws and orders. Sometimes,
the effect of legislation and regulations can be supercial and very limited. (p. 13)

Jones et al. (2008) identied twenty-eight international and regional


treaties addressing human rights, womens rights, childrens rights,
sexual violence, and the right to an education. To see expanded lists of
international treaties and covenants concerning this issue, refer to
Galey (1999) and United Nations Secretary-Generals Study on Violence
against Children (2006).

SEXUAL HARASSMENT IN EDUCATION IN NORTH AMERICA


The United States and Canada have pioneered much of the initial
research on sexual harassment in schools. Both quantitative and qualitative studies dealing with sexual harassment in primary, secondary,

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

193

and postsecondary educational settings exist; however, researchers


have only begun to scratch the surface of this egregious behavior.
Secondary Schools
Canada
Twenty-three percent of female teenagers in a Canadian school
study experienced a minimum of one sexual assault, including touch,
threats, and indecent exposure, with 4 percent of the girls victimized
often (Bagley, Bolitho, & Bertrand, 1997, p. 363). In comparing a
group of teenage girls who had never experienced sexual harassment/
assault to a group that had experienced the behavior, 15 percent of
those who were victimized often attempted suicide or made suicidal
gestures compared to only 2 percent of the nontargeted girls
(p. 361). Female students were more upset than their male classmates.
Yet, Canadian high school girls have become so desensitized to experiencing and witnessing sexual harassment and assault, they view it as
normal behavior (Alcoba, 2008). Sexual harassment by adult school
employees was reported by 37.5 percent of both genders (Winters,
Clift, & Maloney, 2004).
Another study conducted at a Canadian high school demonstrated
that 33 percent of students stated they had been sexually harassed
within the previous 2 years; 29 percent were sexually touched or
grabbed, and 7 percent of female students were sexually assaulted at
school (Alcoba, 2008). Research of twenty-three Canadian schools
showed that 30 percent of ninth-grade girls and 28 percent of eleventhgrade girls were sexually pinched or grabbed, 46 percent of girls were
the brunt of sexual gestures, jokes, leers, or comments (Rushowy,
2008). The results of a survey of two high schools conducted by a
school safety panel discovered that 19 percent of girls had been sexually assaulted at school within the previous two years.
United States
Sexual harassment to students in the United States has been studied
since the 1980s (Strauss, 1988). The largest study was in 1993 when the
American Association of University Women (AAUW) researched the
construct of sexual harassment throughout the country. The results
showed, among other ndings, that 85 percent of Caucasian girls, 84
percent of African American girls, and 82 percent of Hispanic girls in
grades eight through eleven experienced sexual harassment (AAUW,
1993). The AAUW repeated their study in 2001 with similar ndings;
83 percent of girls were sexually harassed, with 30 percent of those
girls indicating it occurred often. The behaviors girls identied as most

194

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

upsetting were being labeled a lesbian, sexually offensive grafti,


spreading sexual rumors, having their clothes pulled at or pulled off/
down, and being forced to do something sexual other than kissing.
Their victimization occurred most often in the hallways and classrooms
(AAUW, 1993; Stein, 1995).
An expanded study of the AAUWs 1993 research found that the
most severe harassment was experienced by girls (Lee, Croninger,
Linn, & Chen, 1996). Research by Lee et al. and by Fineran and Bennet
(1999) showed there was a relationship between the victim and the harasser, in that roughly the same number of victims of sexual harassment
admitted to also sexually harassing at least one other student. Fineran
and Bennet found that 87 percent of girls were sexually harassed by
their peers, and 77 percent of girls admitted to sexually harassing their
peers.
Sexual harassment has its roots in primary grades (AAUW, 1993;
Murnen & Smolak, 2000; Strauss, 1994, 2003). The U.S. Department of
Education, Ofce of Civil Rights (OCR) (1993) posited that even though
girls in the primary grades may not understand sexual harassment in
the same way older students understand it, they are not so nave as to
understand that the sexually obnoxious behavior and comments
directed toward them exhibit malice just because they are girls. New
York schools are nding that children as young as six are molesting
other children (New York Targets Very Young for Sex Offenses, 2000).
Those that bully in sixth grade were found to sexually harass in seventh grade, according to Pellegrini (2001). The author discovered that
there was no difference between genders in their sexual harassment
encounters as seventh graders.
Often missing from the discourse on school shootings is the gendered aspect of the phenomenon (Klein, 2006). Girls were the intended
targets in 11 of 13 highly publicized shootings in the United States over
a period of six years, including the incident of a six-year-old boy killing his six-year-old female classmate. Klein argued that sexual harassment played a role in instigating the shooting episodes, yet absent
from the media was any analysis of the sexual antecedents and hegemonic masculinity as a factor in the shootings:
Violence against girls is easy to render invisible because the behavior that
precedes actual incidents is often perceived as normal; even after fatalities have occurred, the gendered components of crimes do not seem to
register . . . normal violence against girlsindeed, social acceptance of
male hostility towards girlstends to aid in concealing even the most
dramatic incidents. (p. 148)

Special needs students, those with behavioral, cognitive, or physical


challenges, are vulnerable to sexual harassment as both a victim and a

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

195

perpetrator (Young, Allen, & Ashbaker, 2004). Disabled students experienced more sexual harassment incidents than nondisabled students,
but there were no signicant mental health differences between the
two groups as a result of their victimization (Fineran, 2002). When students are disabled and sexually harass another student, teachers must
intervene. According to the U.S. Department of Education, Ofce of
Civil Rights (1993), the students special education status cannot be
used as a defense for sexual harassment.
The HRW researched sexual harassment of U.S. gay, lesbian, bisexual, and transgendered (GLBT) students and found they are unmercifully and relentlessly harassed by their peers, teachers, staff, and
administration (George & Thonden, 2001). The students were verbally,
physically, and sexually assaulted; they were spit and urinated on,
thrown against lockers, cut with knives; some feared going to the bathroom alone, others dropped out of school, some ran away from home,
participated in risky sexual behavior, were diagnosed with depression
and committed suicide. School ofcials, who were not actively involved
in the harassment of these students, gave tacit approval by ignoring
incidents that they observed, or of which they were informed.
Many research projects have been conducted by the Gay, Lesbian,
and Straight Education Network (GLSEN) and can be found at their
Web site (http://www.glsen.org/cgi-bin/iowa/all/research/index.
html). One example is a 2007 survey of over 6,000 students which
found that 60 percent did not feel safe at school (33 percent skipped
school as a result), probably because 86 percent of GLBT students were
harassed due to their sexual orientation.
Sexual harassment by teachers is considerably less frequent than sexual harassment by peers (AAUW, 2001; Lee et al., 1996). If students
younger than 18 are sexually harassed by a teacher or school ofcial,
criminal child abuse statutes, criminal sexual assault or rape laws, as
well as other criminal and civil charges, may be instituted against the
perpetrator (Strauss, 2003). One of the highest reports of teacher sexual
abuse was the Texas Civil Right Project, in which 58 percent of female
students acknowledged being victimized by school employees (Texas
Civil Rights Project, 1997). In contrast, the 1993 AAUW study found
that 25 percent of girls experienced sexual harassment by school
employees (AAUW, 1993). Research by Lee et al. (1996) found 16 percent of students had been harassed by a teacher, 2 percent by a school
administrator, and 44 percent by another school employee. Sexual
abuse by school teachers and staff are a challenge to study because students generally do not report the abuse nor do they tell their parents
(AAUW, 1993).
The most common adult school abuser is a male, well liked by students, parents, and teachers, and involved in extra-curricular activities
with students such as athletics, art, and debate (Shakeshaft & Cohen,

196

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

1995). Examples of the abusive behavior include sexually offensive


verbal comments such as calling female students boobies, complimenting a girl on her nice legs, and commenting on the students sex life.
Physical examples include touching a females breasts and genitals,
fondling, and tickling. Male victims complaints were taken more seriously by school ofcials than were female victims complaints; female
accusers honesty was sometimes questioned, whereas there were
rarely any misgivings about a male students accusations. This discounting or minimizing of female students experiences further victimizes them
because of their gender. Superintendents were mixed on where to devote
their supportto the victim or the teacher; and abusers were often just
transferred to another school district rather than terminated or have their
teaching license revoked (Hendrie, 1998).
Colleges and Universities
Canada
According to Osborne (1992), Canadian universities are bastions of
sexually hostile environments rife with sexual assault on campuses and
sexual harassment by college womens peers and professors. Roughly 5
percent of women residents at McMaster University Residency Training Programs were sexually assaulted by their male supervising medical doctor (Cook et al., 1996). Females were more likely to experience
discrimination based on their sexual orientation, targeted with offensive comments about their body and their attire, and subjected to sexist
teaching materials.
Women, who experienced behaviors that t the objective denition
of sexual harassment, often do not subjectively identify their experiences as sexual harassment (Fitzgerald et al., 1988). With this discrepancy
in mind, Barak et al. (1992) researched the phenomenon with female
students at a Midwestern Canadian university. The womens accounts
of their experiences demonstrated signicant differences with their subjective perception. Only 1.5 percent of the women who were targeted
with gender harassment by a professor identied the behavior as sexual harassment. Of those same women, 97 percent stated they had not
been sexually harassed. When inquiring about sexual coercion or sexual assault, only 40 percent of the women who had objectively experienced these behaviors, subjectively labeled their experiences as such.
The researchers (Barak et al., 1992) correlated these results with
other variables to determine whether they impacted the womens subjective perceptions compared to their objective experiences. Women
who were younger, erotophobic, tended to repress threatening situations, who had not experienced severe forms of sexual harassment, or
who had the need for social approval, were unlikely to perceive the
incidents with their professors as sexual harassment.

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

197

United States
Numerous studies of sexual harassment to U.S. college women indicate that 70 percent have experienced the behaviorincluding gender
harassmentfrom a minimum of one college professor (Barickman et
al., 1990; Dziech & Weiner, 1990; Fitzgerald et al., 1988; Sandler, 1997).
Thirty to 40 percent of graduate women experienced sexual harassment
by faculty (Barickman et al., 1990). Between 70 and 90 percent of college
women experience sexual harassment by their male classmates. Kelley
and Parsons (2000) found that 19 percent of female graduate students
and one in ve undergraduate and graduate female students had been
sexually harassed. Undergraduates were more likely to identify their
male peers as the harasser while graduate women named male faculty
as the perpetrator. The students experienced gender harassment and sexual coercion with equal frequency. Most of the women ignored the
behavior or talked about it with a friend or family member; only 8 percent reported it through ofcial channels within the university. More
than 60 percent of presidents at research institutions acknowledged that
sexual harassment was a problem at their university (Boyer, 1990).
Frannklin, Moglen, Zatlin-Boring, and Angress (1981) discussed two
forms of gender harassment in college and university classrooms. The
rst form reported by female students included facultys use of female
stereotypes in addressing women students or in their pedagogy. Examples included name calling such as, fat housewives, dumb blondes, and
dirty, as well as the use of Playboy centerfolds during anatomy class,
and offensive comments about womens anatomy. This behavior created
a bond between the male faculty member and the men in the course at
the expense of the women students. The second type of gender harassment was facultys sexist comments about womens physical appearance, and if women were enrolled in a traditionally male course such as
engineering, implying lower expectations relegated towards the women.
A wide variety of behaviors may constitute sexual harassment,
according to Sandler (1997). These include asking for sex, sexual grafti,
ogling or elevator eyes, staring at body parts, fondling or grabbing,
sexually offensive jokes or comments, threatening rape or other forms of
abuse, calling women sexist or misogynist names, ongoing unwanted
sexual attention or asking for dates, sexually offensive gestures, ridiculing, sending letters or e-mails or gifts, exposing genitals, and sponsoring
degrading activities such as a wet t-shirt contest, among others.
The U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics conducted The National College Women Sexual Victimization (NCWSV)
study of 4,500 two- and four-year college and university women in
1996 (Fisher, Cullen, & Turner, 2000). The study measured sexual harassment including sexual coercion (penetration), unwanted sexual contact with force or threats but no penetration, and stalking both on and

198

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

off campus. Almost 3 percent of the women experienced a raped or an


attempted rape; 13 percent were stalked; 6 percent were shown pornography; 5 percent experienced someone exposing their genitals; a little over 2 percent were seen naked without granting permission; 50
percent experienced sexist comments, whistles, and catcalls; 20 percent
received obscene phone calls; and 10 percent had sexual rumors spread
about her.
The AAUW Educational Foundation conducted a national study to
determine the extent sexual harassment occurs at American colleges and
universities (Hill & Silva, 2005). First-year college students were the most
likely victims, with more than one-third targeted. While almost two-thirds
of students experience sexual harassment, only 10 percent report it to
school ofcials. Although both male and female students are targeted,
their experiences are quite different. Male students sexual orientation is
questioned by homophobic name calling. Women are more likely to experience sexual jokes and comments, leers, and sexual gestures.
Roughly a third of students experience physical sexual harassment,
including being forced to do something sexual and being fondled (Hill &
Silva, 2005). The harassment occurs throughout the campus. GLBT students are more likely to be sexually harassed than their heterosexual
classmates. The most likely perpetrator of sexual harassment to both
female and male students is a male; however, one-third of women
admitted to harassing someone. White students were more likely to be
victimized, and there were similarities in how white, black, and Hispanic
students both discerned and responded to sexual harassment; however,
black and Hispanic students were more willing to report incidents of
sexual harassment to college ofcials.
SEXUAL HARASSMENT IN EDUCATION IN LATIN
AMERICA AND THE CARIBBEAN
Data on sexual violence in schools in Latin America and the Caribbean is scarce. The few reports that exist often categorize school violence
as a political construct tied to gangs and drug trafcking rather than as
sexual violence (Dunne et al., 2003). Girls who live in Nicaragua, Panama, Mexico, Guatemala, Honduras, and the Dominican Republic
reported sexual coercion by teachers, including the threat of lowering
their grades if they didnt comply with their sexual demands (United
Nations Secretary-Generals Study on Violence Against Children, 2005).
Secondary Schools
Brazil
DeSouza and Ribeiro (2005) discovered that within the previous
12 months, 24 percent of Brazilian high school students experienced a

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

199

minimum of one sexual harassment incident, with boys the most likely
perpetrator. Eight percent of students (no differentiation between genders) in fth to eighth grade were bystanders to sexual violence in
school (World Bank, n.d.) They also found that if both male and female
students bullied, they were more likely to sexually harass other students as well.
Ecuador, Haiti, and Jamaica
Twenty-two percent of Ecuadorian teen girls were victims of sexual
abuse within and about the school (World Bank, 2000, cited in Blaya &
Debarbieux, 2008). Amnesty International (2008) reported that sexual
abuse of girls in Haiti is common by both teachers and administrative
staff. Girls in Jamaica experience their breasts and buttocks touched
and pressure to engage in sex so often that it is considered the norm
(DevTech Systems, USAID, 2005, cited in, Amnesty International, 2008).
Colleges and Universities
Puerto Rico
Ramoss (1999) quantitative and qualitative research of sexual harassment at the University of Puerto Rico (UPR) demonstrated that 65
percent of female students experienced a minimum of one sexual harassment incident, and 35 percent indicated that they had not experienced any incidents. Of those who experienced sexual harassment, 61
percent reported gender harassment, 28 percent identied unwanted
sexual attention, and 2 percent indicated they had experienced sexual
coercion. In addition, 49 percent experienced a combination of both
unwanted sexual attention and gender harassment. Interestingly, of
those women who reported experiencing at least one of the items on
the sexual harassment survey, only 81=2 percent labeled their experience
as sexual harassment; they were more likely to label their experiences
as offensive.
These results are similar to Hernandez (1988, cited in Ramos) who
found that 63 percent of Puerto Rican women experienced sexual harassment. Torres (1989, cited in Ramos) reported that 73 percent of victims were harassed by professors, 15 percent by their classmates, and
12 percent by directors or a librarian.
Not surprisingly, those students who were more tolerant of the
behavior were not as distressed (Ramos, 1999). Other women reported
psychological ramications such as fear, anger, depression, and humiliation. Academically they experienced lowered grades and loss of
excitement and fervor about school. Those women who were harassed
by a professor reported feelings of hopelessness due to the universitys

200

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

perceived lack of caring and their complex procedures, fear of retaliation, and the recognition that a tenured professor was protected from
consequences.
Coping responses included silence, avoiding the harasser, denial of
the behavior, dropping a professors/harassers class, informing the harasser the behavior was unwanted, and seeking out someone to talk to
(Ramos, 1999; Torres, 1989, cited in Ramos). Puerto Rican women view
avoidance as an assertive response to harassment by perceiving it as taking control of the situation (Ramos, 1999). It is rare for a victim of sexual
harassment to report the behavior. The qualitative interviews demonstrated that the women blamed the sexual harassment by men to factors
other than the men themselves; for example, the way women dress, and
on nature and society, thereby negating any male responsibility for their
own behavior. In addition, the male harassers were viewed as perverts,
sick, obsessed, weak, and with psychological problems.
SEXUAL HARASSMENT IN EDUCATION IN EUROPE
Research on sexual violence in schools is limited in the European
Union. Smith (2003) compiled school violence studies from seventeen
countries, but the construct of gendered violence towards girls and
women was quite limited. Witkowska and Menckel (2005) indicated
that addressing the sexual component of school violence is lacking in
various European Union programs and projects.
Secondary Schools
England
Duncans (1999) study of four urban multi-ethnic secondary schools
in England identied numerous forms of what he labeled as sexual
bullying, including physical assault, sexual name calling, negative commentary regarding physical appearance, verbal attacks, sexual gossip,
and propositioning. Duncans observations demonstrated sexual power
struggles (p. 131), and a sexualized comment during what would otherwise be civilized exchanges, is the marking out of gendered role limits; reminders that relationships are ultimately structured by power
and that power is gendered (p. 128). His ndings supported the
framework of gendered bullying occurring between boys, from boys to
girls, between girls, and from girls to boys.
Girls are called slags, sluts, bitches, whores, and slappers for various
types of behavior such as irting, being unfaithful to their boyfriend,
or demonstrating their sexuality in the clothes they wear (Bell, 2008).
One study found that 80 percent of students thought girls and women
brought on their own victimization by their attire. Boys pull down or

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

201

pull up a girls skirt, tell a girl he wants to nger her, and taunt girls
because of the clothes they wear or because of their weight. Boys use
electronic means to sexually bully their female classmates (and staff)
when they send sexual messages or pornography via their cell phones.
As three 14-year-old boys gang raped an 11-year-old girl, they lmed it
from the camera on their phone and sent the video to students at
school.
A British Broadcasting Company (BBC) news program reported that
government statistics showed there were 3,500 xed-period expulsions
and 140 expulsions from school in England due to sexual misconduct,
including sexual assault and rape (Murphy, 2009). Of those, 280 expulsions were from the primary grades, and twenty involved children ve
years old. These statistics are considered an underreporting of the
abuse partially because it is excused as typical behavior.
Female students in Larkins study (1994) identied how sexual harassment was normalized in their schools. First, sexual harassment was
rarely, if at all, discussed at school; second, the regularity and pervasiveness of the behavior; and third, the interpretation of the behavior
by others, especially the male perpetrators, that the behavior was fun
and no big deal.

Netherlands
Twenty-two high schools in the Netherlands were studied by Timmerman (2003) for evidence of sexual harassment to teenagers. Ninety
percent of the perpetrators were male faculty and staff, and 87 percent
were male classmates with girls more likely to be targeted by the harasser than were the boys. One in four girls was sexually harassed, and
20 percent of the harassment was perpetrated by their teachers in the
form of nonverbal sexual advances. Verbal sexual harassment was
experienced most often. Girls were twice as likely to experience more
physical forms of sexual harassment as were their male peers, and
were also more likely to experience combined types of sexual harassment. One percent of the girls experienced a rape or attempted rape
within the school.
Teacher harassment of girls often occurred in public places, with 69
percent of all students asserting it occurred in the teachers classroom,
followed by hallways and the cafeteria (Timmerman, 2003). These
results acknowledge that sexual harassment by teachers was not proscribed. Peer sexual harassment was reported to occur in small groups
in classrooms, hallways, in the immediate area outside the school, and
other public places. Girls were more likely to report psychosomatic
consequences to their health when sexually harassed by teachers than
when harassed by their peers.

202

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

France
Sexual violence to students by teachers has occurred in France
(Bodin, 2005). Though the gender of the victim and the position of the
perpetrator was not revealed, 556 sexual abuse incidents were reported
in France during one academic year, and 12 percent were rapes (Blaya &
Debarbieux, 2008). According to the Collectif feminist contre le viol
(cited in Blaya & Debarbieux, 2008), a little over 3 percent of sexual
attacks occurred within the school. Bodin (2005) found 11=2 percent of
French students experienced sexual abuse.
Finland
Sexual harassment was experienced by 41 percent of 15-year-old
Finish female students, yet only 2 percent reported it to school ofcials
(Honkatukia, 2000, cited in Laheelma, 2002). Gender conicts between
male and female students were perceived by teachers as merely joking,
irting, or playing (Aaltonen, 2002). Sexual harassment was not a term
that was used in their descriptions of the behavior because gender
tends to be invisible; consequently the gender-neutral term, bullying,
was used to dene aggression even from males to females. According
to the author, the victim of bullying was blamed.
Germany and Belgium
Bodin (2005) found that a little more than 6 percent of German students and 1 percent of Belgium students experienced sexual abuse. It is
not known who the perpetrators were or the gender of the students.
Spain
Ninety percent of both girls and boys reported sexual harassment,
with verbal harassment as the most common (Vicario, 2008). There was
no difference in the amount of sexual harassment each experienced,
with 90 percent reporting their victimization. However, the type of harassment varied in their assertions of homophobic barbs and sexual
comments, but further delineations were not provided.
Sweden
The Swedish National Agency for Education researched sexual harassment to grade school children and found over 50 percent experienced sexually offensive name calling; pressure to have sex was found
in 7 percent of the students, and 4 percent were sexually assaulted,
including 1 percent by teachers (Skolverket, Dnr 012001: 2136, cited in

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

203

Witkowska, 2005). In a study done of students in grades nine through


eleven, 50 percent reported experiencing sexual harassment (Kullenberg & Ehrenlans, 1996, cited in Witkowska, 2005).
Using the criterion of inappropriate and unacceptable, Witkowska
(2005) studied sexual harassment of 17- and 18-year-old students in a
Swedish high school. Demeaning comments about their sexuality and
gender, sexual comments and conversations, and rating the level of
girls attractiveness, were experienced by 56 percent to 77 percent of
the girls. Between 23 percent and 44 percent of the girls experienced
these behaviors anywhere from daily to monthly. Less than 40 percent
of the girls were subjected to sexual name calling, pressure for sex, and
sexual rumors. Nonverbal sexual harassment was less frequent than
verbal, with ranges of 8 percent to 61 percent. Sexual assault reports
ranged from 0.2 percent to 27 percent and spread of sexual harassment
by teachers was from 2 percent to 14 percent. Forty-nine percent of the
girls perceived sexual harassment as a problem in their school.
In another study of Swedish female students by Witkowska and
Gadin (2005), 13 percent of female students were sexually harassed,
and 66 percent saw sexual harassment as a problem in their school, yet
schools were blase to the behavior. Only 16 percent saw sexualized
conversation, rating a students attractiveness or making sexual comments by teachers as sexual harassment, and they stipulated that those
behaviors were common. Additionally, they dismissed teacher or staff
behavior of pulling students clothing, grabbing inappropriately and
touching private body parts as sexual harassment, which is quite dire.
Republic of Ireland
A large nationwide study on bullying was undertaken of over
20,000 students in over 500 primary, secondary, and vocational schools
in Ireland. Despite the research studying bullying behavior, some students disclosed they had been subjected to behavior that would constitute sexual harassment. Irish lads and lasses experienced sexual abuse
by peers and teachers, were kissed without permission, had their
clothes ripped off, and sexual rumors spread about them, and 30 indicated they were sexually harassed.
Colleges and Universities
Italy
Almost 50 percent of Italian women students reported being sexually harassed in research conducted by Lombardo, Pedrabissi, and Santinello (1996). Their experiences included sexual comments about their
bodies (29.3 percent), and quid pro quo harassment by offering a

204

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

reward for a sexual relationship (3.4 percent). Twenty-six percent of


the women reported that they were sexually assaulted.
Sweden
Bernelo and Peterson (2001, cited in Witkowska, 2005) found that
50 percent of university students experienced gender harassment,
and 30 percent indicated they were targets of sexual harassment.
Roughly 12 percent of female students at four Swedish universities
experienced sexual harassment at their university (www.allakvinnorshus.org/tjejjouren/statistik.htm, cited in Witkowska, 2005). Students
interviewed by Hagg (2002) asserted that sexual harassment at the University of Umea was rare, though they had heard it does occur. The
students indicated that if a student was victimized, the individual
would not speak out for fear of being labeled a troublemaker. Hagg
posited that perhaps because the ideology in Sweden is one of equal
opportunity, that sexual harassment may not be as prevalent as in
other countries; however, equal opportunity is not realized in Sweden
because women and girls freedom is too threatening to male power,
according to Hagg.
Netherlands
Twenty percent of female medical students at Nijmegen Medical
School in the Netherlands experienced sexual harassment (Neveille et
al., 2008).
Spain
Puigvert (2008) reported that a woman who is sexually harassed,
raped, or abused in a Spanish university will be subjected to major
roadblocks if she attempts to report her experience. Spanish universities do not have any policies to deal with sexual harassment on campus, and women who attempted to glean support from campus
ofcials were met with silence. Puigvert indicated that several research
programs about sexual harassment on Spanish universitys campuses
are underway.
SEXUAL HARASSMENT IN SCHOOLS IN ASIA PACIFIC
Asia Pacic encompasses a wide range of countries with broad economic and cultural practices. Asia, particularly, uses an authoritarian
model of teaching, and, according to Jones et al. (2008), social relations that are both highly gendered and determined by differences of
caste, ethnicity, and religion (p. ix). There is antisexual violence

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

205

legislation throughout Asia, except for Pakistan, however sexual violence within the school milieu is rarely addressed.
Secondary Schools
Japan and China
Data from the Japanese government demonstrated that 115 public
school teachers were disciplined for fondling or harassing students as
well as molesting colleagues and graduates (The Bangkok Post, December 28, 2000, cited in Haspels, Kasim, Thomas, & McCann, 2001). The
Chinese Ministry of Education requires that minor students are taught
about sexual harassment (Xinhua News Agency, 2007). China passed
the Law on the Protection of Minors in 1991 legislating sexual harassment to minors as a crime. Sexual harassment of Tibetan girls by Chinese teachers is rampant, however, with the teachers pulling the girls
into rooms to touch them (Tibet Justice Center, 1998).
Pakistan
Parents of rural Pakistani elementary school children do not want
their daughters walking long distances to get to school because they do
not want them leered at by men (Warwick & Reimers, 1995). The
authors asserted that parents removed their daughters from school,
when they feel that conditions there do not respect female students or
protect their honor (p. 28). Parents of daughters would prefer that
their daughters are not in contact with boys during the school day,
therefore, single-sexed schools are the preferred option by parents of
female students.
Secondary school female students are the more likely targets of sexual harassment to and from school and in school by older boys, school
staff, and men who are passing by (Brohi & Ajaib, 2006). The sexual violence consists of singing sexually explicit songs, writing love letters,
making sexually vulgar comments about anatomy, and touching or
pressing up against female students. The behavior is pervasive and the
boys nd it fun. According to Brohi and Ajaib, The onus of responsibility is on the woman or girl. At times, her mere existence is considered a provocation and any sexual act, with or without her consent,
carries a suffocating stigma (p. 81). Pakistan has no laws against sexual harassment.
Girls do not report their sexual harassment because to do so means
they have to choose between going to school and staying home because
their parents would not approve of their daughters victimization
(Brohi & Ajaib, 2006). In addition, Pakistan places great honor on girls
sexual purity, and there is a presumption of guilt that the girl herself
did not preserve her own sexual dignity if she is sexually harassed.

206

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Where female sexual victimization equals familial and communal


dishonor, it also affects families and communities . . . As one father
declared when referring to the benets of schooling: What is more important in society? My honour or her knowing where Yunaan (Greece)
is? (p. 87).
South Asian Countries
As in Pakistan, the distance that girls have to walk to get to school
in both Malaysia and the Philippines is an impediment to girls enrollment (King & Hill, 1993). Schools in those countries have also been
reported as hostile to girls. If there are no bathrooms in a Bangladesh
school, parents will not send their daughters because they are more
vulnerable to attack. In several South Asian countries, parents do not
want to send their daughters to coeducational schools, even in the primary grades. Once the girls enter puberty, the concern is even higher,
resulting in low female enrollment if the schools do not have boundary
walls and bathrooms for girls (Khan, 1993).
Afghanistan
When school girls classes are held outside, which is usually the case
and often under a tree, they are fodder for the verbal sexual harassment
by older boys and men who walk or drive by (Fahmia Vorgetts, Afghan
Womens Fund and Women for Afghan Women, personal communication, March 14, 2009). After school, the girls gather in groups to walk
home because they are confronted with as many as 100 boys at the gates
of the school yelling obscenities, sexually offensive comments about their
breasts or vaginas, and attempting to grope or fondle the girls. Rape is
common. In addition, girls are threatened and assaulted by having acid
thrown in their faces on their way to school. Sexually offensive comments about a womans breasts and her vagina are made by schoolboys
as young as seven or eight to middle-aged adult women.
India
Secondary school girls in Karnataka in South India are sexually harassed at school by their male peers and on their way to and from
school by men and older boys (Leach & Sitaram, 2007). Ragging is the
word used to imply, that boys derive a sadistic pleasure in teasing or
tormenting girls (p. 262). Behaviors found most offensive by the girls
included, look at us with bad eyes . . . tease girls, write love letters
and notes . . . buy bangles . . . write the names of the girls whom they
fancy on their hands or on the school walls . . . compare a girl with a
lm star, sing lm songs . . . suggesting a romantic liaison . . .

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

207

spreading of rumours . . . deliberately bump into them (pp. 265266).


The girls were most concerned with the ragging and harassment that
occurred outside the school from older boys. Due to lack of space,
classes were often held outside, sometimes under a tree, which made
the girls afraid and vulnerable to the leers from the men and older
boys who walked by and who harassed them to and from school.
Girls gossip about their female classmates liaisons with boys was
another concern carrying grim repercussions if her parents found out
and removed her from school (Leach & Sitaram, 2007). The girls spoke
of gender stereotypes by teachers, which were exemplied in the comments teachers made to the girls and to the boys. For example, a girl
may hear, Why do you behave like this, do you think you are a boy?
or conversely a boy is asked, Why do you talk so much like a girl?
(p. 269). Discussions with teachers and other school ofcials indicated that
rape of girl students by teachers had occurred but were relatively rare.
Nepal
According to the United Nations Secretary-Generals Study on Violence Against Children, 9 percent of children were abused by having
their sensitive parts kissed, oral sex, and penetration, with 18 percent
of the abusers being teachers (2005). In Nepal, teachers touch school
girls buttocks, breasts, and will unhook the girls brassieres (Save the
Children Fund, n.d., cited in Dunne, Humphreys, & Leach, 2006).
Russia
Girls in Russian primary grades were bullied two to three times more
by boys than were boys bullied by boys (Dyachenko, 2002, cited in
Zdravomyslova & Gorshkova, 2006). Though very little examination of
gender violence has occurred in Russian schools, the small amount that
has been examined indicates extensive integration of the behavior into
the milieu of Russian schools. Adolescent boys humiliate their female
peers to demonstrate masculine hegemony (Zdravomyslova & Gorshkova, 2006). Sexual jokes and innuendo, discussion about sexual topics,
and sexual pressure are directed to female students by male faculty.
Australia
An Australian anti-bullying website includes resources and references about gender and sexual harassment (www.bullyingnoway.
com.au). The site identies examples of sexually offensive names, such
as slut, whore, and bitch, when talking about girls; that the behavior
most often is perpetrated by males who are abusing their power, and
provides an avenue for redress for students.

208

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

A majority of students from four schools in Australia indicated they


would not report sexual harassment even though their schools had
anti-bullying policies (Nolan, 2004; personal correspondence from Ken
Rigby, 2004, cited in Stein, 2005). Approximately 37 percent of the students indicated that sexual harassment was a weekly occurrence.

Colleges and Universities


Malaysia and Korea
A Malaysia university study demonstrated that 80 percent of women
were harassed (Badriya, 1988 in Zaitun, 2001, cited in Haspels et al.,
2001). In Korea, a group of womens lawyers and academics formed an
ad hoc committee in response to a woman assistant who was red for
refusing her professors repeated sexual demands. The committee was
successful in taking the assistants case to court resulting in legal reform.
China
When an incident of a male university student e-mailing pornography to women students was made public in Hong Kong, it compelled
universities, corporations, and the government to designate a position
of Equal Opportunity Ofcer to deal with harassment and discrimination issues within their organizations (Haspels et al., 2001).
One of the rst comprehensive sexual harassment studies completed
in a Hong Kong university demonstrated that twice as many Chinese
graduate and undergraduate college women experience sexual harassment than do their male counterparts, with peer harassment being
twice as likely to happen as harassment by faculty (Tang et al., 1996).
Approximately one of every four female students was victimized by
the behavior with 1 percent experiencing coercive sexual harassment
by either a peer or a teacher. Twelve percent of the women students
experienced misogynist comments by their teachers as well as teachers
infringing on their body space and touching their arms, shoulders and
hands. Teachers were also reported to make comments about the womens bodies, pressure them for dates, and direct sexually suggestive
gestures towards 5 percent of women students. One student reported
sexual assault and two other women indicated their teachers bribed
them into sex. Peer harassment was more prevalent with 20 percent to
26 percent of women experiencing gender harassment and physical
seductive behaviors (p. 205); several women were bribed or coerced
into sexual activity and 10 percent of women were pressured for dates
and were the targets of sexual stares and gestures.
These behaviors were viewed as the norm by some women, and
even fewer women believed that the victims were partly to blame for

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

209

the harassment (Tang et al., 1996). Chinese students were less aware of
sexual harassment on their campus than were U.S. students awareness
of the behavior on their campuses. The authors posited that this may
be due to less sexual harassment occurring on Chinese campuses than
the United States, a difference in the sizes of the universities studied, a
Chinese patriarchal culture so that Chinese students dont view some
of the sexist comments as harassing, embarrassment within the Chinese
culture to discuss such issues, and the emphasis on harmony and mutual respect within their society.
A second study by Tang (2001, cited in Equal Opportunities Commission of Hong Kong, 20062007) showed that students from eight
institutions of higher learning believed that sexual harassment on their
campus was due to women wearing sexy attire and therefore, appearing sexy. More men than women perceived sexual harassment as irtation and admiration of women. Students also believed that sexual
harassment was really about the victim overreacting.
India
Lewd songs, harassing phone calls, sexual verbal comments, and
womens breasts as the object of mens glares, were experienced by 39
percent of female college students in Mumbai, India (Bajpai, 1999). The
womens bathrooms were surrounded by men making the location a
threatening place for women to use, as were the canteen and the entrance to the university. Male faculty subjected the women students to
sexual innuendo, touching, staring, and offering grades for sex. Fortyve percent of women students at the University of Peradeniya in Sri
Lanka discussed their sexual humiliation, called ragging (Finney, 2000).
Roughly half the women who took part in a Delhi University study
indicated that they were harassed by either a teacher or a non-teaching
employee. In addition, 92 percent of women at the universitys hostel,
experienced daily sexual harassment within the campus as well as on
busses and streets (The Lawyers Collective, 2001, cited in Haspels et
al., 2001). Almost 14 percent of female students were harassed by their
male peers in libraries and the canteen, and 5 percent were harassed
by university staff (Gender Study Group, 1996, cited in Mirsky, 2003).
Verbal harassment in Indian higher educational institutions is known
as eve-teasing, and is considered a less offensive form of harassment by
women students at Delhi University (Anagol-McGinn, 1994, cited in
Mirsky, 2003). Their male peers viewed eve-teasing as light in nature,
attering, and fun (Anagol-McGinn, 1994, cited in Mirsky, 2003, p. 18).
Forty-ve percent of the female students acknowledged that they shun
the library and specic classes to avoid sexual harassment.
Numerous universities in India have undertaken the task of reducing sexual harassment and assault on their university campuses, and

210

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

creating and implementing sexual harassment polices (Bajpai, 1999).


Her study showed that 39 percent of women acknowledged the existence of sexual harassment on campus, and 58 percent of women said
there was no harassment or discrimination. Sexual harassment was
dened as nasty and obscene behavior including comments about a
womans anatomy, lewd songs, whistling, staring at a womans breasts,
and phone calls. Classrooms, hallways, the library and canteen were
common locations where harassment occurred, especially during special events and holidays. The women said that if a female student
walked by a group of men, it was a 100 percent certainty that sexual
comments would be spewed at her. Male faculty enjoyed their own
kind of sexual harassment including ridiculing females, staring, coercion for grades, inviting the woman to his home, and sexual innuendo
(Bajpai, 1999). Most of the time there was no action taken on any sexual harassment on campus.
Thailand
There were differences in perception about workplace sexual harassment between business school students in Thailand and the United
States, according to a study by Limpaphayom, Williams, and Fadil
(2006). Thai students perceived sexual jokes and sexually explicit language as very offensive and a form of sexual coercion. U.S. students
acknowledged that exchanging sexual activity for a work advantage
and sexual remarks create an offensive work environment.
Australia
Although 53 percent of female undergraduate students in Australia
experienced sexual harassment from their professors, and 88 percent
from their male classmates, only 32 percent of students perceived they
were sexually harassed (Gardner & Felicity, 1996). Female students
were more likely to identify sexist comments from their professors as
sexual harassment than the same behavior from their male peers. Gender harassment was most common from the students professors,
whereas unwelcome attempts for a sexual relationship and sexist and
sexually offensive remarks came from their male classmates.
SEXUAL HARASSMENT IN EDUCATION IN AFRICA
Sexual violence in and around African schools has reached epidemic
proportions. Female students are subjected to sexual harassment and
sexual abuse by their teachers and their male classmates in bathrooms,
classrooms, and the perimeter of the school (Amnesty International,
2008; Leach, 2006; Education For All Global Monitoring Report Team,

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

211

2003: World Bank, n.d.) The results of six studies demonstrated that
between 16 percent and 47 percent of girls in primary and secondary
schools report experiencing sexual violence (World Bank, n.d.). Conict, poverty and war were contributing factors to the sexual exploitation of girls walking to and from school and while at school
(ActionAid International, n.d.).

Primary and Secondary Schools


Zimbabwe
An extensive qualitative study of four schools in Zimbabwe demonstrated widespread sexual harassment of girls in three co-educational
and one all-girls secondary schools (Leach, Machakanja, & Mandoga,
2000). The authors explored the abuse to girls from the perspective of a
culture that is gendered and misogynist. The study explored both sexual abuse and nonsexual abuse and found a nexus between the two in
that both co-existed within the school environment, even though nonsexual abuse in the form of corporal punishment is illegal. For example, if a girl denies a teachers sexual advances, she risks his physical
assault. By interviewing 13- to 15-year-old girls, their male classmates,
teachers, head teachers, parents, and government ofcials, it was apparent that the sexual harassment the girls experienced on their way to
and from school was from older men known as sugar daddies. Fifty
percent of girls indicated they were sexually touched, and 92 percent
were propositioned for sex in exchange for gifts or money (Amnesty
International, 2008).
The sexual harassment of school girls in Zimbabwe represents a microcosm of the amount of sexual abuse and nonsexual abuse that girls
experience outside the school milieu (Amnesty International, 2008;
Leach, 2006; Leach et al., 2000). This broader context merits a brief examination when viewing the problem of sexual harassment within and
around the school environment. Girls and women are considered less
valuable within much of African culture, and are vulnerable because of
their low socioeconomic status resulting in their dependence on men.
Additionally, males are socialized to view females as their property
and someone to serve them, and that girls and women should obey
what boys and men require of them. The girls socialization results in
their lowered self-esteem and acceptance of male aggression as the
norm, making them vulnerable to abuse. Men and boys offer of money
and gifts, therefore, becomes an enticing element in the male power
dynamic of manipulating the girls into sexual relationships. The abuse
the girls experience in school mirrors the violence perpetrated against
them in public places by male strangers, and at home in the form of
sexual abuse, beatings, neglect, excessive work, and verbal abuse.

212

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Older boys and male teachers were the most likely perpetrators of
the girls (Leach, 2006; Mirsky, 2003; Education For All Monitoring
Report Team, 2008). Sexual violence included the following behaviors:
invading the girls space in an intimidating manner, badgering them in
the hallways and on school grounds, entering their classrooms uninvited, touching their breasts and buttocks often leading to sexual
assault, and verbally abusing demeaning the female gender by shouting obscenities and calling them whores and prostitutes. The boys sent
love letters as a way to propose, but if the girls refused the boys proposals, they were met with verbal insults, name calling, or physical
assault.
Male teachers abused their power to tantalize both younger and
older girls into a sexual liaison, often using money and gifts like the
sugar daddies in the public arena (Leach, 2006; Mirsky, 2003). This was
standard accepted behavior and often perpetrated on a girl during
class in front of other students. Teachers were not punished which
increased the likelihood of the tantalizing becoming sexual abuse.
Shumba (2001) discovered that 65.6 percent of sexual abuse by teachers
included sexual intercourse, and 2 percent experienced rape or
attempted rape. Because teachers behaved so openly and egregiously
in front of their male students with no negative consequences to their
behavior, they became role models for the same behavior for the young
men in the classroom.
Even the girls that attended the all-girls school did not escape sexual
harassment (Mirsky, 2003; Leach, 2006). Like the coeducational girls,
they were harassed on their way to and from school by sugar daddies.
Once in the school, some male teachers were inappropriate towards
them as well. Because the legal ban on corporal punishment was
strictly enforced, unlike the coeducational schools, verbal abuse by both
female and male teachers was rampant.
The study demonstrated that differentiating between abuse and consent was sometimes a difcult distinction (Dunne et al., 2006). There
were some girls that accepted money and gifts from the older boys or
the sugar daddies, which from the males viewpoint meant a sexual
relationship. That said, because of the power differential, whether consensual or not, it was abuse and harassment.
The lack of school or government leadership in condemning and
punishing the boys or male teachers for the sexual harassment, sexual
abuse, and nonsexual abuse in the form of corporal punishment, gave
tacit approval for the behavior to continue thereby creating an environment of gender-based violence towards girls (Dunne et al., 2006; Leach,
2006; Mirsky, 2003). As could be expected, sometimes the girls became
pregnant by a teacher. If the teacher was reported, little consequence
was observed. Sometimes the girls parents ignored the pregnancy if
the teacher agreed to marry the pregnant teen and provide a home for

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

213

her. The girls were assigned blame for their pregnancy by parents,
their peers, and they blamed themselves as well. Female teachers were
aware of the harassment by both the boys and the male teachers but
ignored it. The message sent to boys and girls that the abuse was normalized and expected.
The girls reported a myriad of responses to the boys harassment
reecting emotional, educational, and behavioral consequences (Dunne
et al., 2003; UN World Report on Violence Against Children, 2006).
Emotionally, they reported feeling anxious, embarrassed, confused,
scared, and irritated. They had difculty concentrating in class and
reported crying. Some were too frightened to leave the classroom,
while others ensured that their trip home was with other girls, and
avoided walking near crowds of boys. If it was a teacher who was the
harasser, the girls reported doing whatever they could to avoid the
teachers attention; they worried if they denied the teachers sexual
approach, they would be retaliated against by lowered grades, forced
to do extra work, or be physically assaulted.
Kenya
Rape in Kenyan schools is commonplace (Ceneda, 2001). In 1991, 71
girls were brutally gang raped by boys from a neighboring school
resulting in the death of 19 girls. The boys were not prosecuted. When
the headmaster for the boys school expelled them, the boys parents
sued forcing the headmaster to readmit them. When a probation ofcer
was interviewed about the incident, he acknowledged that rape was a
normal aspect of the school setting to the point that if the gang rapes
hadnt resulted in deaths, the tragedy would not have made the news.
During this same time, a primary school teacher had raped nine girls;
the teachers employment was not terminated and he continued as a
teacher.
South Africa
The HRW study of sexual violence to girls in South African schools
involved visiting eight predominantly urban public schools from a
broad range of economic strata and interviewing their administrators
and teachers (George & Thonden, 2001). Thirty six girls from different
ethnic backgrounds between the ages of 7 to 17, parents, social workers, teachers, administrators, and government ofcials were also interviewed. Twenty-three incidents of rape at school were also
investigated. Jackrolling dened the snatching and gang rape of girls
and was reported in 25 schools. As in Zimbabwe, South African girls
are regularly sexually harassed, including sexual assault and rape, by
both their male classmates and male teachers. The harassment mirrors

214

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

the experiences of their Zimbabwean counterpartsthey are threatened, fondled, verbally humiliated, and raped in bathrooms, classrooms (in front of teachers) and hallways.
More often than not, the girls sexual violence is committed by two
or more boys (Mirsky, 2003). The abuse goes unchallenged by schools,
educators, and the government. If girls do report the abuse, they are
often retaliated against by school administrators and their peers. Teachers who are known abusers and were found to commit one third of the
rapes to children (Jewkes, Levin, Mbananga, & Bradshaw, 2002), were
not disciplined but rather were transferred to other schools to continue
their egregious behavior.
While most of the sexual harassment to the girls is perpetrated by
their male classmates, harassment by male teachers is epidemic and
severe. Teachers instigate a quid pro quo by promising the girls better
grades in return for sexual favors or a dating relationship. They taunt
the girls into a form of prostitution by offering money for sex. Conversely, teachers will threaten the girls with physical violence if they
dont comply with the teachers sexual demands. Teachers have raped,
sexually assaulted, and verbally sexually abused the girls with degrading sexual comments. One primary teacher instructed the girls to draw
a penis. A study done in 1998 by the Medical Research Council found
that 37.7 percent of rape victims, who identied their rapist, identied
one of their teachers or their principal as the perpetrator of their violence. Male teachers will ask the girls to run an errand for them to the
male staff lounge, thereby colluding with their male peers who are
waiting in the staff room where the girl would be sexually harassed or
raped (World Bank, n.d.). Understandably, girls were fearful of going
near the male teachers staff room, so they would pair up with one of
their female classmates to run the errand.
Girls were harassed and assaulted on their way to and from school
by taxi drivers (George & Thoonden, 2001). Because poor and black
girls are more likely to have to travel further to get to school, they are
more vulnerable to violence. Little attention has been directed to the
sexualized violence in South African primary schools (Bhana, 2006).
The consequences to the girls are multifaceted. In addition to the
girls dropping out of school and failing their higher education matriculation exams, their involvement in activities outside of school, such as
sports, are diminished (George & Thonden 2001). They have difculty
concentrating causing their grades to drop. Absenteeism is a common
response to the unchallenged harassment thereby disrupting their ability to learn. Sexual assaults lead to unwanted pregnancy and health
risks associated with sexually transmitted diseases (STDs) and HIV/
AIDS. (George & Thonden, 2001; Leach, 2006).
A study comparing the sexual harassment of students in Johannesburg and Chicago, Illinois in the United States found that the behavior

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

215

is normalized in both schools. Eighty three percent of Chicago students


and 79 percent of Johannesburg students reported sexual harassment
by their peers. An almost equal number also disclosed that they sexually harassed their classmates74 percent and 78 percent, respectively.
Both genders from both countries had similar rates for both their
victimization and their perpetration of the same behavior on their
classmates.
West Africa
Very few female teachers work in refugee schools in West Africa
where Liberian refugee girls study in Sierra Leone and Guinea (Kirk &
Winthrop, 2008). Consequently, girls are likely to be sexually exploited
by their male teachers. In 2002, the International Rescue Committee
(IRC) instigated new imitativefemale classroom assistants (CAs) to
make the school environment safer and friendlier to girls. For example,
the CAs monitor a log book in which the girls grades are recorded,
which diminishes the likelihood of transactional sex (the exchange of
money, gifts, or grades for money) between the male teacher and the
girl. Sexual abuse to Liberian school girls by male teachers and school
staff was evident in refugee camps, where teacher abuse often involved
the demand for sex for grades (United Nations, 2002, cited in United
States Agency for International Development, 2008).
Botswana, Ghana
A study by Rossetti (2001, cited in Davies, n.d.), found that 67 percent of students in Botswana schools (including some boys) had been
subjected to sexual harassment by teachers. Twenty percent of these
students were propositioned by their teacher for sex, and 42 percent of
the students complied due to fear of retaliation if they did not. In
Ghana, 13.5 percent of girls in both primary and secondary schools
were victims of sexual abuse at school (Brown, 2002).
Malawi, Cameroon, and Uganda
Amnesty International (2008) reported that 50 percent of Malawi
girls experienced unwanted sexual touching by their teachers or male
classmates (Leach, Fiscian, Kadzamira, Lemani, & Machakanja, 2003).
According to the World Bank (n.d.), eight percent of school girls from
Cameroon were sexually abused by their teachers. A student in
Uganda reported that one of her male teachers required that she wash
his feet, take water to the bathroom, and when he appeared naked
asked her to help him as a man (ActionAid, n.d., p. 4).

216

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Morocco, Benin, and Tunisia


The farther the girls have to walk to get to school, the less likely
parents in Morocco and Tunisia are willing to let their daughters
attend school (King & Hill, 1993). While the authors do not specically
identify gendered violence or sexual harassment as the cause of
parents trepidation, they recognized that the walk to school would
make their daughters vulnerable to moral and physical peril (King &
Hill, p. 33). Wible (2004) found that 43 percent of primary and 80 percent of secondary students in Benin, dropped out of school because of
sexual abuse. Another survey discovered that 34 percent of children
and 15 percent of teachers attested to sexual violence occurring in their
schools (Plan, 2008). Rarely are any of the incidents reported to school
ofcials.

Colleges and Universities


Zimbabwe
Research conducted on institutions of higher learning in Zimbabwe
uncovered considerable sexual harassment of 2,756 female students by
male lecturers (Zindi, 1998). Fear of further victimization was identied
by 90 percent of the women as a reason sexual harassment is either
underreported or not reported. Their fear was supported when 45 percent of female students told of lowered grades when they refused their
lecturers sexual advances or if they terminated an ongoing dating relationship. A little over 1 percent of the women indicated that they
would date a lecturer to improve their grades, and almost 9 percent
asserted that any student over the age of 18 had a right to date a single
lecturer. In contrast, 21 percent of females believed dating lecturers
was unethical because the student may have an unfair grade advantage
because exam papers may be provided to the lecturers student paramour prior to the exam. A full 64 percent of women students perceived
their lecturers as corrupt. Every woman who completed the survey
indicated that legislation should be enacted to protect students against
lecturers, and all of them knew lecturers who had abused their power
to take advantage of women students.
Gaidzanwa (1994) posited that female students were said to be asking for it if they ventured outside the union buildings. Incidents
called kning, which include sexual harassment of women including
rape, abuse of alcohol, and other forms of violence by male students,
were chronicled in a report written by the universitys proctor. At one
point, a student wearing a mini skirt was attacked by 100 male students. Sexual harassment by male faculty, including transactional sex,
was reported.

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

217

Kenya
According to Ceneda (2001), sexual harassment and exploitation of
female university students is rampant in Kenya, and is identied as
one of the obstacles to womens educational achievement. Women are
pressured for sex, raped, groped, fondled, and subjected to cat calls,
with rare action taken against the perpetrator.
Ghana
Trainee teachers from a university and a college are sexually harassed most frequently during 1) promotional exams, 2) continuous
assessment, 3) admission practices, 4) exam practices by typists, 5) student allowances, 6) domestication of female students, and 7) study
mates (Teni-Atinga, 2006, pp. 199200). The harassment sometimes
includes persistent sexual coercion from lecturers with threats of a lowered grade or not passing the exam to move forward with a students
education. The sexual harassment begins when women are seeking college admission and does not stop until they are through the program.
They experience anger, frustration, helplessness, and a sense of powerlessness because of the inability to bring a complaint to the school for
fear of ridicule and stigma.
Nigeria
The sexual harassment of female students by professors on Nigerias
universities gained the attention of the countrys president, General
Olusegun Obasanjo (Ladebo, 2001, cited in Ladebo, 2003). The president made a mockery out of university teachers for using female students for their own sexual hedonism. Ladebos (2003), study of three
Nigerian universities found that none of the three had any sexual
harassment policies, in all likelihood because the country has no antisexual harassment laws. Sexual harassment by male teachers to female
students had escalated to the point that management went to the leaders of the union to implore the teachers to stop their abuse. One of the
campuses is known to have high incidents of rape and sexual assaults,
and women attending the other universities reported sexual coercion
and transactional sex if they are nancially indigent.
Phallic attack is the term coined by male lecturers at two universities
that refers to the coercion of students for sex in exchange for grades
(Nwadigwe, 2007). The author asserted that the increase in sexual harassment could be partially attributed to the lack of sex education in
African countries. Single women experienced higher rates of sexual
harassment, and were more likely to succumb to their lecturers taunts.
Only 2 percent of the victims reported the menacing lecturer. Students

218

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

reported that they bought gifts for the lecturer and his family in an
attempt to avoid intercourse with him.
Women students at Lagos State University in Nigeria described their
experiences on campus as a type of rape, including threats, intimidation and force from men that attempt to disguise their relationship as
friendship (Adedokun, 2005). Male teachers are the most likely perpetrators but male students and university staff also sexually harass.
SEXUAL HARASSMENT IN SCHOOLS IN THE MIDDLE EAST
Sexual violence in schools in the Middle East is not likely to be recognized or studied because of war, political unrest, poverty, and
entrenched cultural beliefs about the role of men and women in the
region.
Secondary
Egypt
Egyptian parents are reticent to allow their daughters to attend
school if they have a long distance to walk for fear of their daughters
moral and physical peril (King & Hill, 1993, p. 33). The Egyptian
Center for Womens Rights (ECWR), an NGO, conducted the rst
country study addressing the prevalence of sexual harassment to Egyptian and tourist women on the streets of several cities (Hassan, n.d.).
The organization did not research sexual harassment to girls and
women in education specically, however, their research found 29 percent of harassment occurred in educational facilities and that schoolchildren and university students were the most likely to harass girls
and women on Egypts streets, after taxi drivers. One can speculate,
therefore, that girls and women in Egypts schools are victimized by
sexual harassment. An ECWR seminar encouraged parents to discuss
the sexual harassment in school with their children. Teachers informed
ECWR that they were at a loss as to what to tell students about sexual
harassment or what to do when they are subjected to it. ECWR is
working with the Ministry of Education to design sexual harassment
curriculum for schools to use in teaching students about the construct.
Men blamed sexual harassment on the inuence of the West, the
media, inaction by the female victim, seductive clothing, and a womans presence in specic locations (Hassan, n.d.). They also asserted
that sexual harassment should be expected because of the lust between
men and women. The men reported feeling a level of satisfaction after
harassing a woman: more masculine, and a way to show off.
The ECWR (Egyptian Center for Womens Rights, n.d.) told of a
female student whose father no longer allows her to attend school

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

219

because of the daily verbal harassment from men and boys as she and
her girlfriends travel to and from school. The girls male classmates
spread sexual rumors; her brother and father heard the rumors and
beat her. Another father sought help from ECWR when his 14-year-old
daughter refused to attend school because of the sexual harassment
she experienced on the bus ride to and from school.
Turkey
The HRW reported that girls who attend government-sponsored
medical high schools, and who are suspected of having sex or being
prostitutes, will be subjected to virginity examinations (HRW, 2001a).
The exams were banned in 1999 and reinstituted in 2001. Girls have
attempted suicide rather than be forced to the invasive and painful
exam.
Israel
A nationwide Israeli study of sexual harassment of over 10,000 public school students in seventh through eleventh grades demonstrated
that Arab boys and eighth-grade students were the most likely to
report sexual harassment (Zeira, Astor, & Benbenishty, 2002). Arab
girls, followed by Jewish girls, reported the least amount of victimizationboth Arab and Jewish boys were the most likely victims of harassment from their male classmates. Eleven percent to 36 percent of
girls reported experiencing a minimum of one act of sexual harassment, with Arab girls least likely to be harassed. Unwanted sexual
remarks and attempts to kiss the girls were the most disturbing experiences. Seven percent of the girls reported being kissed when they
didnt want to, and 11 percent indicated a classmate attempted to
touch them sexually without their consent. Out of the seven survey
items, boys experienced six of the seven considerably more often than
did the girls; the girls reports of a student attempting to take off an
item of their clothing was the only survey item in which they scored
higher. The girls most frequent sexual harassment experiences, at 11
percent, was when another student made unwanted sexual remarks
and tried to touch them sexually.
IMPACT
Hill and Silva (2005) found female students were fearful, angry,
embarrassed, and worried about whether they would have a healthy
relationship. The women reported having difculty concentrating during class and problems sleeping. According to Dziech and Weiner
(1990), denial is one of the effective coping mechanisms sexual

220

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

harassment targets employ to deal with their victimization. When the


behavior continues, targets will attempt to avoid the harasser hoping
that if they avoid or ignore the harasser, he will eventually leave her
alone (Backhouse, 1981; Hill & Silva, 2005). Students also report strategies to avoid being noticed, such as dressing down, dropping courses,
not enrolling in courses taught by specic instructors, changing their
major or career plans, and relinquishing research opportunities due to
sexual harassment (Dziech & Weiner, 1990; Fineran & Bennett, 1998;
Mirsky, 2003). Discussing boyfriends or husbands, indicating they are
too busy studying, and other tactics are instigated to cope with the perpetrator (Benson & Thomson, 1982).
Paludi and Barickman (1991) listed behaviors into ve categories
which they termed sexual harassment trauma syndrome; (a) emotional
responses such as shame, guilt, and powerlessness; (b) physical reactions including substance abuse, lethargy, weight uctuations, phobias,
and genitourinary and gastrointestinal distress; (c) changes in selfperception, for example, feelings of hopelessness, lack of control, and
negative self-esteem; (d) social, interpersonal relatedness, and sexual
effects encompassing changes in dress or physical appearance, lack of
trust, negative attitudes and behavior in sexual relationships, and
potential sexual disorders; and (e) career effects such as changes in
study and work habits, withdrawal from school, drop in academic performance, and lowered grades (pp. 2939).
Psychological sequelae include confusion and self-blame, fear and
anxiety, depression and anger, and disillusionment (Salisbury, 1986).
Girls who have been sexually abused feel ashamed and guilty causing
them to stay quiet for fear of negative ramications, such as being
killed for bringing dishonor to their families (Blaya & Debarbieus,
2008). The immediate and long-term ramications to victims of gendered violence are copious including anxiety about their personal
safety, loss of self-esteem, anxious, and an increase in suicide risk
(George, 2001; Mirsky, 2003; HRW, 2001a, 2001b). Students may resort
to using alcohol or drugs, and participating in risky sexual behavior
(HRW, 2001a, 2001b).
Other responses included feeling upset, uncomfortable, and disappointed in their college experience (Hill & Silva, 2005), trouble concentrating and learning, and truancy (Blaya & Debarbieux, 2008). In
addition to the psychological ramications, there is an increased risk for
pregnancy, STDs, cervical cancer, infertility, and the spread of HIV/
AIDS, particularly in countries in Africa where the virus is rampant.
Long-term economic, psychological, social, and physical health consequences may result when females have been victimized (Amnesty
International, 2008). Problems may arise in adulthood such as criminal
behavior and difculty in maintaining relationships (APSA Rapport,
1999, cited in Blaya & Debarbieux, 2008).

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

221

When teachers sexually harass and abuse female students in front of


the boys in the class, or when they collude with the boys who harass
by not intervening, boys learn a violent form of masculinity that then
perpetuates gendered violence in the home and in society.
RECOMMENDATIONS
According to ActionAid International (n.d.) Violence against girls
has its roots in patriarchy and unequal power relations that still exist
worldwide. Therefore the problem must be seen within this broader
framework. It is a symptom and a result of the larger problem of gender inequality that has to be tackled in all spheres (p. 4). Addressing
the complex phenomenon of sexual violence to girls in education
requires a wide range of strategies because it is a worldwide pandemic
(Leach, 2006; Mirsky, 2003). International researchers DeSouza and Solberg (2003) asserted that In countries where women have been and
are viewed as unequal to men, new laws or procedures designed to
prevent harassment may conict with hundreds of years of culture . . .
(p. 25). The nexus of human rights, public health, legislation, and education are at the heart of any potential movement for reversing this multifarious plague (Mirsky, 2003).
The following recommendations are only a cursory list briey summarizing the in-depth recommendations from Amnesty International,
HRW, and the United Nations, among others. Each of these organizations recommendations are analogous and generally fall within six categories: research, national and international efforts, school initiatives,
community and parent verve, public health programs, and active student participation.
Research
Sexual violence is an understudied construct in which more research
is required to understand the scope and severity in individual countries and worldwide. With increased data, measurable goals and objectives can be strategically designed, implemented, monitored, and
evaluated for both the prevention and intervention of the abuse in primary, secondary, and postsecondary institutions. Much of the research
has been done qualitativelyadditional quantitative data are also
required to gain knowledge about the perpetrators and what variables
are present to prevent school ofcials from intervening to both prevent
and intervene on sexual violence (and other forms of violence) to girls
and women (and boys and men). With more study of the efcacy of
prevention and intervention treatments and services, an international
best practices database could be created as a resource for stakeholders
use worldwide.

222

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

National and International Efforts


Perhaps one of the simplest steps countries could take is to ratify
the many international conventions and treaties dealing with human
rights, womens rights, and the rights of the child. The ratication is
only as effective as government follow through within each country.
Governments need to become accountable to international and national
laws, where applicable. Each countrys ministry of education should
partner with law enforcement, schools, communities, parents, NGOs,
public health agencies, and students to form local and national coalitions with the goal of stopping sexual abuse. For countries that do not
have anti-sexual harassment in school laws, legislation needs to be
enacted to stop the behavior to female students of all ages.
School Initiatives
Teachers need to be trained in sexual harassment, gender equality,
and discipline procedures that are respectful of the dignity of females
(and males). Students must learn about sexual violence, female equality, good communication skills, boundaries, and respect of gender differences. Teachers should be required to take part in ongoing training
throughout their tenure, and performance appraisals conducted by
headmasters and principals, and students should be implemented in
monitoring teacher effectiveness and behavior. More female teachers
and teachers assistance should be hired for schools where most teachers are male. Curriculum development about gender equity, sexual harassment, and effective forms of discipline, among others, should be
required in all college teacher education. Schools should develop, disseminate, and publicize effective sexual violence policies and procedures, as well as monitor their effectiveness. Teachers who sexually
abuse a student of any age should be red and referred to the police.
Disciplinary measures need to be used on students who violate the
schools policy and sexually harass their classmates.
Parents and Community Verve
The school does not operate in a vacuum; it is a reection of societys mores and values. Therefore, to change those heinous values
degrading to women and children, citizens can become members of
NGOs or other grassroots organizations working towards anti-sexual
violence programs and projects. Programs that develop men as partners in understanding and teaching other men and boys about gender
inequality and its impact on girls and women should be implemented
in communities. Each community should create their own solutions to
the problem of gender violence specic to their locale. Use of mass

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

223

media programs on radio, television, town posters, and the Internet


could be capitalized in getting the anti-sexual violence, pro-equality
message out to citizens within the community. Law enforcements
involvement will serve to diminish the likelihood of girls being sexually propositioned and accosted on their way to and from school, as
well as the abuse by their male teachers. Parents would benet from
parent education classes emphasizing gender equality, discipline, and
the importance of girls attending school. Parents should be active partners with their childrens schools to ensure competent teachers are
hired.

Public Health
Public health programs and projects dealing with HIV/AIDS and
other STDs need to be taught to all citizens within the community and
schools. Pregnancy prevention is an essential educational program
along with the use of condoms for the reduction of pregnancy, STDs,
and HIV/AIDs. Public health agencies partnering with schools and the
community would help ensure an integrated approach to dealing with
the consequences of sexual violence in schools and in the community.

Student Participation
Active student participation is essential for student buy-in in designing and implementing any process and program to stop incidents of
sexual harassment at school. Students will have some of the best problem solving capabilities about the issue because they live it on a daily
basis. Educating the boys to understand sexism, gender violence, and
the impact on their female classmates is another effective tactic in
reducing the sexual abuse. Using peer education involving older boys
teaching the younger boys would be benecial for both the boy teacher
and the boy learner. Catalyzing the boys to be advocates for their
female classmates if groups of boys are colonizing against the girls
would demonstrate positive role modeling to their male peers. Providing safe opportunities for girls to teach boys how the boys sexual
abuse impacts them emotionally, educationally, and physically may be
another catalyst in stopping the abuse.
A holistic approach, integrating strategies, agencies, organizations,
governments, and individuals will be required to effectively change a
complex epidemic that has, unfortunately, become the norm in schools
around the world. Fortunately, champions of the needed transformation have begun chipping away at this overwhelming undertaking.
This is a transformation that must be successful for girls and women,
boys and men, and for society.

224

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

CONCLUSION
The pandemic of sexual violence in schools is not well recognized,
analyzed, or researched (Dunne et al., 2006). School ofcials may deny,
minimize, and hide sexual harassment leading to inaction as their preferential response to sexual violence in schools. In the worst case scenario, teachers, staff, and administrators may be the actual perpetrators
of the abuse. This often is contradictory of national law, school policies,
and international treaties and conventions.
Stopping sexual violence in schools is an overwhelming task that
will require changing long entrenched cultural belief systems about patriarchy, masculine hegemony, and girls and womens value in society.
This paradigm shift will require a long-term commitment from individuals; NGOs; governments; primary, secondary, and postsecondary
schools and teachers; parents; and boys and girls (Plan, 2008).

REFERENCES
Aaltonen, S. (2002). Peoples interpretations of conicts in gender in school. In
V. Sunnari, J. Kangasvuo, & M. Heikkinen (Eds.), Gendered and sexualised
violence in educational environments (pp. 125137). Oulu, Finland: Oulu University Press. Retrieved from http://herkules.oulu./isbn9514271505/
isbn9514271505.pdf.
ActionAid International. (n.d.). Stop violence against girls in school. Retrieved
from www.intothelimelight.co.za/images/site/bookpdf/violenceagainst
girls.pdf.
Adedokun, O. A. (2005). Sexual harassment in Nigerian educational settings:
Preliminary notes from a qualitative assessment of Lagos State University.
Sexuality in Africa Magazine, 2. Retrieved from http://www.arsrc.org/
publications/sia/dec05/researchnote.htm.
Akiba, M., LeTendre, G. K., Baker, D. P., & Goesling, B. (2002). School victimization: National and school system effects on school violence in 37
nations. American Educational Research Journal, 39, 829853.
Alcoba, N. (2008, February 22). Violence in schools. National Post. Retrieved
from http://www.cfuw.org/index.php?option=com_search&Itemid=99999
999&searchword=violenceinschools&submit=Search&searchphrase=
exact&ordering=newest.
American Association of University Women Educational Foundation. (1993).
Hostile hallways: AAUW survey on sexual harassment in American schools.
Washington, DC: Author.
American Association of University Women Educational Foundation. (2001).
Hostile hallways: Bullying, teasing and sexual harassment in school. Washington, DC: Author.
Amnesty International. (2008). Safe schools: Every girls right. Retrieved from
http://www.amnesty.org/en/library/info/ACT77/001/2008.
Backhouse, C. (1981). Fighting sexual harassment: An advocacy handbook (2nd ed.).
Boston: Alyson Publications and The Alliance Against Sexual Coercion.

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

225

Bagley, C., Bolitho, F., & Bertrand, L. (1997). Sexual assault in school: Mental
health and suicidal behaviors in adolescent women in Canada. Adolescence,
32, 361366.
Bajpai, A. (1999). Sexual harassment in university and college campuses in
Mumbai. The Indian Journal of Social Work, 60, 606623.
Barak, A. (1997). Cross-cultural perspectives on sexual harassment. In W. O.
Donohue (Ed.), Sexual harassment theory, research, and treatment (pp. 263
300). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Barak, A. (2005). Sexual harassment on the Internet. Social Science Computer
Review, 23, 7792.
Barak, A., Fisher, W. A., & Houston, S. (1992). Individual difference correlates
of the experience of sexual harassment among female university students.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 1737.
Barickman, R. Korn, S., Sandler, B., Gold, Y., Ormerod, A., & Weitzman, L. M.
(1990). Forwards. In M. A. Paludi (Ed.), Ivory power: Sexual harassment on
campus (pp. xiixix). Albany, New York: SUNY.
Bell, R. (2008, November 4). The joke isnt funny any more. The Guardian.
Retrieved from http://www.guardian.co.uk/society/2008/nov/04/domestic-violence-childprotection.
Benson, D. J., & Thomson, G. E. (1982). Sexual harassment on a university campus: The conuence of authority relations, sexual interest, and the gender
stratications. Social Problems, 29. 236251.
Blaya, C., & Debarbieux, E. (2008, June). Expel violence! A systematic review of
interventions to prevent corporal punishment, sexual violence and bullying
in schools. Woking, England. Retrieved from http://learnwithoutfear.org/
downloads/Expel_violence.pdf.
Bodin, D. (2005). Violence at school. [Background Paper for the Regional Consultation for the United Nations Study on Violence Against Children]. Retrieved
from http://www.violencestudy.org/europe-ca/Word/Background%20papers/
School%20edited%2013.05.05.doc.
Boyer, E. L. (1990). A special report. Campus life: In search of community. Princeton, NJ: The Carnegie Foundation for the Advancement of Teaching.
Brohi, N., & Ajaib, A. (2006). Violence against girls in the education system of
Pakistan. In F. Leach & C. Mitchell (Eds.), Combating gender violence in and
around schools (pp. 8189). Sterling, VA: Trentham Books.
Brown, C. K. (2002). A study on sexual abuse in schools in Ghana. Cape Coast,
Ghana: University of Cape Coast/UNICEF Ghana.
Ceneda, S. (2001, February). No upright words: The human rights of women in
Kenya. Retrieved from www.asylumaid.org.uk.
Cook, D. J., Liutkus, J. F., Risdon, C. L., Grifth, L. E., Guyatt, G. H., & Walter,
S. D. (1996). Residents experiences of abuse, discrimination and sexual harassment during residency training: McMaster University Residency Training
Programs [Abstract]. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 154, 16571665.
Retrieved from www.cmaj.ca/cgi/content/abstract/154/11/1657.
Dalaimo, D. M. (1997). Electronic sexual harassment. In B. R. Sandler & R. J.
Shoop (Eds.), Sexual harassment on campus (pp. 85103). Needham Heights,
MA: Allyn & Bacon.
DeSouza, E. R., & Ribeiro, J. (2005). Bullying and sexual harassment among Brazilian high school students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20, 10181038.

226

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

DeSouza, E. R., & Solberg, J. (2003). Incidence and dimensions of sexual harassment across cultures. In M. Paludi & C. A. Paludi Jr. (Eds.), Academic and
workplace sexual harassment: A handbook of cultural, social science, management,
and legal perspectives (pp. 330). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Duncan, N. (1999). Sexual bullying: Gender conict and pupil culture in secondary
schools. London: Routledge.
Dunne, M., Humphreys, S., & Leach, F. (2003). Gender and violence in
schools. (EFA Global Monitoring Report 2003/4) The Leap to Equality.
Retrieved from http://unesdoc.unesco.org/images/0014/001467/146763e.
pdf.
Dunne, M., Humphreys, A., & Leach, F. (2006). Gender violence in schools in
the developing world. Gender and Education, 18, 7598.
Dziech, B. W., & Weiner, L. (1990) The lecherous professor: Sexual harassment on
campus (2nd ed.). Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
Education for All Global Monitoring Report Team. (2003). Gender and education
for all: The leap to equality [Summary Report]. Retrieved from http://unesdoc.
unesco.org/images/0013/001325/132550e.pdf.
Egyptian Center for Womens Rights. (n.d.). Clouds in Egypts Sky: Sexual
harassment: From verbal harassment to rape. Retrieved from http://www.
preventgbvafrica.org/content/clouds-egypt%E2%80%99s-sky%E2%80%9
D-sexual-harassment-verbal-harassment-rape.
Egyptian Center for Womens Rights. (n.d.). A volunteer campaign to end
sexual harassment. Available from http://ecwronline.org/index.php?
option=com_content&task=view&id=194).
Equal Opportunities Commission of Hong Kong. (20062007). Sexual harassment awareness training for college students. Retrieved from http://
www.hurights.or.jp/pub/hreas/9/06sexualharassment.html.
Fineran, S. (2002). Peer sexual harassment in high school and the experience of
students with disabilities. Paper presented at the New Hampshire Family
Violence conference.
Fineran, S., & Bennet, L. (1998). Teenage peer sexual harassment: Implications
for social work practice in education. Social Work, 43, 5564.
Fineran, S., & Bennett, L. (1999). Gender and power issues of peer sexual
harassment among teenagers. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 626641.
Finney, H. R. (2000). Breaking the earthenware jar: Lessons from South Asia to and
violence against women and girls, Kathmandu: UNICEF Regional Ofce for
South Asia.
Fisher, B. S., Cullen, F. T., & Turner, M. G. (2000, December). The sexual victimization of college women. (NCJ182369). Retrieved from http://www.
ncjrs.gov/pdfles1/nij/182369.pdf.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Shullman, S. L., Bailey, N., Richards, M., Swecker, J., Gold, Y.,
et al. (1988). The incidence and dimensions of sexual harassment in
academia and the workplace. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 32, 152175.
Frannklin, P., Moglen, H., Zatlin-Boring, P., & Angress, R. (1981). Sexual and
Gender Harassment in the Academy. New York: The Modern Language Association of America. Gaidzanwa, R. (1994). Gender violence and governance
in universities: The University of Zimbabwe. In A. M. Van Schuurman
Centrum (Ed.), Southern Africa: Womens perspective (pp. 1119). The
Netherlands: Utrecht University.

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

227

Galey, M. E. (1999). Women and education. In K. D. Askin & D. M. Koenig


(Eds.), Women and international human rights law. (Vol. 1, pp. 403439).
Ardsley, NY: Transnational Publishers.
Gardner, J., & Felicity, A. (1996). Sexual and gender harassment at university:
Experiences and perceptions of Australian women. Australian Psychologist,
3, 210216.
George, E., & Thonden, Y. (2001). Scared at school: Sexual violence against girls in
South African schools. New York: Human Rights Watch.
Hagg, K. (2002). Aspects of sexual harassment from a Swedish perspective. In
V. Sunnari, J. Kangasvuo, & M. Heikkinen (Eds), Gendered and sexualised
violence in educational environments (pp. 125137). Oulu, Finland: Oulu
University Press.
Haspels, N., Kasim, Z. M., Thomas, C, & McCann, D. (2001). Action against sexual harassment at work in Asia and the Pacic. Retrieved from http://www.
ilo.org/asia/whatwedo/publications/langen/docNameWCMS_BK_PB_
159_EN/index.htm.
Hendrie, C. (1998). Passing the trash by school districts frees sexual predators to hunt again. Education Week, 18, 1617.
Hill, C., & Silva, E. (2005). Drawing the line: Sexual harassment on campus.
Retrieved March 9, 2009, from http://www.aauw.org/research/upload/
DTLFinal.pdf.
Human Rights Watch. (2001a). Hatred in the hallways: Violence and discrimination
against lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender students in U.S. schools. Retrieved
March 9, 2009, from www.hrw.org/legacy/reports/2001/uslgbt/toc.htm.
Human Rights Watch. (2001b). Easy targets: Violence against children worldwide.
Retrieved March 9, 2009, from http://www.hrw.org/legacy/reports/2001/
children/.
Jewkes, R., Levin, J., Mbananga. N., & Bradshaw, D. (2002). Rape of girls in
South Africa. The Lancet, 359, 319320.
Jones, N. (2008). Increasing visibility and promoting policy action to tackle sexual exploitation in and around schools in Africa (Brieng paper). Retrieved from
http://learnwithoutfear.org/downloads/sex-exploit-west-africa-english.pdf.
Jones, N., & Espey, J. (2008). Increasing visibility and promoting policy action to
tackle sexual exploitation in and around schools in Africa. Retrieved from www.
odi.org.uk/resources/external/2008/10/sexual-exploitation-policy-actionschools-africa.pdf.
Jones, N., Moore, K., Villar-Marquez, E., & Broadbent, E. (2008). Painful lessons:
The politics of preventing sexual violence and bullying at school. (Working paper
295). Retrieved from http://www.odi.org.uk/resources/odi-publications/
working-papers/295-painful-lessons-preventing-sexual-violence.pdf.
Kazue, M. (2004). Sexual harassment and empowerment of women in Japan.
NiASnytt: Asia Insights, 1, 1213. Retrieved from http://www.nias.ku.dk/
nytt/2004_1/2004_11.pdf.
Kelley, M. L., & Parsons, B. (2000). Sexual harassment in the 1990s: A university-wide survey of female faculty, administrators, staff, and students. The
Journal of Higher Education, 71, 548568.
Khan, S. R. (1993). South Asia. In E. M. King, & M. A. Hill (Eds.), Womens education in developing countries: Barriers, benets, and policies (pp. 211246).
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

228

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

King, E. M., & Hill, M. A. (1993). Womens education in developing countries:


An overview. In E. M. King, & M. A. Hill (Eds.), Womens education in
developing countries: Barriers, benets, and policies (pp. 150). Baltimore, MD:
Johns Hopkins University Press.
Kirk, J., & Winthrop, R. (2008). Female classroom assistants: Agents of change
in refugee classrooms in West Africa? In M. S. Maslak (Ed.), The structure
and agency of womens education (pp. 161178). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
Klein, J. (2006). An invisible problem: Everyday violence against girls in
schools, Theoretical Criminology, 10, 147177.
Ladebo, O. J. (2003). Sexual harassment in academia in Nigeria: How real?
African Sociological Review, 7: 117-130.
Larkin, J. (1994). Walking through walls: The sexual harassment of high school
girls. Gender and Education, 3, 263280.
Leach, F. (2006). Gender violence in schools in the developing world. In F.
Leach, & C. Mitchell (Eds.), Combating gender violence in and around schools
(pp. 2329). Sterling, VA: Trentham Books.
Leach, F., Fiscian, V., Kadzamira, E., Lemani, E., & Machakanji, P. (2003). An
investigative study into the abuse of girls in African Schools. Retrieved
from http://unesdoc.unesco.org/images/0014/001467/146763e.pdf.
Leach, F., & Humphreys, S. (2007). Gender violence in schools: Taking the
girl-as-victims discourse forward. Gender & Development, 15, 5165.
Leach, F., Machakanja, P., & Mandoga, J. (2000). Preliminary investigation of
the abuse of girls in Zimbabwean junior secondary schools: (Education
research paper No. 39). Retrieved from http://www.dd.gov.uk/pubs/
les/prelinvestgirlszimschoolsedpaper39.pdf.
Leach, F., & Sitaram, S. (2007). Sexual harassment and abuse of adolescent
schoolgirls in South India. Education, Citizenship and Social Justice, 2,
257277.
Lee, V. E., Croninger, R. G., Linn, E., & Chen, X. (1996). The culture of sexual
harassment in secondary schools. American Educational Research Journal, 33,
383417.
Limpaphayom, W., Williams, R. J., & Fadil, P. A. (2006). Perceived differences
in sexual harassment between business school students in the U.S. and
Thailand. Cross Cultural Management, 13, 3242.
Mirsky, J. (2003). Beyond victims and villains: Addressing sexual violence in the education sector. (Report no. 47). London: The Panos Institute. Retrieved from
the http://www.panos.org.uk/?lid=250.
Murnen, S. K., & Smolak, L. (2000). The experience of sexual harassment
among grade school students: Early socialization of female subordination?
Sex roles, 43, 117.
Murphy, V. (2009, January 5). Rising problem of sexual bullying in schools.
Retrieved from http://news.bbc.co.uk/panorama/hi/front_page/newsid_
7811000/7811468.htm. Neveille, pp 452458.
Nolan, T. (2004). Anti-bullying Policies Failing to Cut School Harassment
[Transcript of radio broadcast June 18 by Radio National and ABC Australia]. Retrieved from http://www.abc.net.au/pm/content/2004/s1135441.
htm.
Osborne, R. L. (1992). Sexual harassment in universities: A critical view of the
institutional response. Canadian Women Studies, 12, 7276.

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

229

Paludi, M. A., & Barickman, R. B. (1991). Academic and workplace sexual harassment: A resource manual. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
Paludi, M. Nydegger, R., DeSouza, E., Nydegger, L., & Allen Dicker, K. (2006).
International perspectives on sexual harassment of college students: The
sounds of silence. In F. L. Denmark, H. Krauss, E. Halpern, & J. Sechzer
(Eds.), Violence and exploitation against women and girls. Boston: Blackwell
Publishing.
Ramos, A. M. (1999). Sexual harassment at the University of Puerto Rico. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Rushowy, K. (2008, February 7). Sexual harassment common in high schools,
study nds. TheStar.com. Retrieved from www.thestar.com/printarticle/
301336.
Salisbury, J. (1986). Counseling victims of sexual harassment. Psychotherapy, 2,
316324.
Sandler, B. R. (1997). Student to student sexual harassment. In B. R. Sandler, &
R. J. Shoop (Eds.), Sexual harassment on campus: A guide (pp. 5065). Boston:
Allyn & Bacon.
Shariff, S. (2004). Keeping schools out of court: Legally defensible models of
leadership to reduce cyber-bullying. Educational Forum, Delta Kappa Pi, 68,
222233.
Shariff, S., & Gouin, R. (2006). Cyber-hierarchies: A new arsenal of weapons for
gendered violence in schools. In F. Leach, & C. Mitchell (Eds.), Combating
gender violence in and around schools (pp. 3341). Sterling, VA: Trentham
Books.
Shumba, A. (2001). Who guards the guards in schools? A study of reported
cases of child abuse by teachers in Zimbabwean secondary schools. Sex
Education, 1, 7786.
Smith, P. K. (Ed.) (2003). Violence in schools: The response in Europe. London:
Routledge Falmer.
Stein, N. (1995). Sexual harassment in school: The public performance of gendered violence. Harvard Educational Review, 65, 145162.
Stein, N. (2003). Bulling or sexual harassment? The missing discourse of right
in an era of zero tolerance. Arizona Law Review, 45, 783799.
Stein, N. (2005). A rising pandemic of sexual violence in elementary and secondary schools: Locating a secret problem. Duke Journal of Gender Law &
Policy, 12, 3352.
Strauss, S. (1988, March). Sexual harassment in the school: Legal implications
for principals? NASSP Bulletin, 9397.
Strauss, S. (1994). Sexual harassment at an early age. Principal, 74, 2629.
Strauss, S. (2003). Sexual harassment in K-12. In M. Paludi, & C. A. Paludi (Eds.),
Academic and workplace sexual harassment: A handbook of cultural, social science,
management, and legal perspectives (pp. 106145). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Tang, C. S., Yik, M. S. M., Phil, M., Cheung, F. M. C., Choi, P., & Au, K. (1996).
Sexual harassment of Chinese college students. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
25, 201215.
Teni-Atinga, G. (2006). Ghanaian trainee teachers narratives of sexual harassment: A study of institutional practices. In F. Leach, & C. Mitchell (Eds.),
Combating gender violence in and around schools (pp. 199206). Sterling, VA:
Trentham Books.

230

Heritage, Roles, and Issues

Tibet Justice Center. (1998, December). Violence and discrimination against


Tibetan woman. A report submitted to the UN Committee on the elimination of discrimination against women. Retrieved from www.tibetjustice.org/
reports/women/index.html.
Till, F. (1980). Sexual harassment: A report on the sexual harassment of students.
Washington, DC: National Advisory Council on Womens Educational
Programs.
Timmerman, G. (2003). Sexual harassment of adolescents perpetrated by teachers and by peers: An exploration of the dynamics of power, culture, and
gender in secondary schools. Sex Roles, 48, 231244.
United Nations. (1993). UN Declaration on the Elimination of Violence Against
Women. Retrieved from www.un.org/documents/ga/res/48/a48r104.htm.
United Nations. (2006). The United Nations Secretary Generals Study of Violence Against Children: Europe and Asia. Retrieved from www.violence
study.org.
United Nations Secretary-Generals Study on Violence Against Children.
(2005). Regional desk review: Latin America. Quoted in P. S. Pinheiro,
World Report on Violence Against Children, Geneva: United Nations.
United States Agency for the International Development. (2008). Are schools safe
havens for children? Examining school related gender based violence. Washington,
DC: U.S. Agency for International Development. Retrieved from http://
www.usaid.gov/our_work/cross-cutting_programs/wid/pubs/Safe_Havens_
Corrected_10-14-08.pdf.
U.S. Department of Education, Ofce for Civil Rights. (2000). Revised Sexual
Harassment Guidance: Harassment of Students by School Employees,
Other Students, or Third Parties. Federal Register 5512.
U.S. Department of Education, Ofce for Civil Rights. (1993). Letter of Finding,
Eden Prairie, Minn., Elementary School Sexual Harassment Incident. Chicago, Ill.: U.S. Department of Education, Ofce of Civil RightsRegion V.
Vaux, A. (1993). Paradigmatic assumptions in sexual harassment research:
Being guided without being misled. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 42,
116135.
Vicario, I. (2008, April). Peer sexual harassment in adolescence: Incidence and
gender differences in a Spanish sample [Abstract]. Sexologies, 1 ( Suppl 1),
148149.
Ward, J. (2005). Violence against girls in school. In Broken bodies broken dreams:
Violence against women exposed (pp. 7381). Retrieved from http://www.
irinnews.org/pdf/bb/6IRIN%20Duo-School.pdf.
Warwick, D. P., & Reimers, F. (1995). Hope or despair? Learning in Pakistans primary schools. Westport, CT: Praeger.
WHO. (2002). World report on violence and health. Retrieved from http://
www.who.int/violence_injury_prevention/violence/world_report/en/.
Wible, B. (2004). Making schools safe for girls: Combating gender-based violence
in Benin. http://www.usaid.gov/our_work/cross-cutting_programs/wid/
pubs/Safe_Havens_Corrected_10-14-08.pdf.
Winters, J., Clift, R. J. W., & Maloney, A. (2004). Adult-student sexual harassment
in British Columbia high schools. Aggression in Organizations, 4, 177 196.
Witkowska, E. (2005). Sexual harassment in schools: Prevalence, structure and perceptions. Stockholm, Sweden: National Institute for Working Life.

Sexual Violence to Girls and Women in Schools around the World

231

Witkowska, E., & Gadin, K. G. (2005). Have you been sexually harassed in
school? What female high school students regard as harassment. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 17, 391406.
Witkowska, E., & Menckel, E. (2005). Perceptions of sexual harassment in
Swedish high schools: Experiences and school-environment problems.
European Journal of Public Health, 15, 7885.
World Bank. (n.d.). Addressing violence against women within the education
sector. Retrieved from http://siteresources.worldbank.org/INTGENDER/
Resources/Education.pdf.
Xinhua News Agency. (2007, March 9). China teaches juveniles how to fend off
sexual harassment. Retrieved from www.china.org,cn/english/education/
202174.htm.
Young, E. I., Allen, M., & Ashbaker, B. Y. (2004). Responding to sexual harassment in special education settings. Teaching Exceptional Children, 36, 6267.
Zalk, S. R. (1990). Men in the Academy: A psychological prole of harassment.
In M. A. Paludi (Ed.), Ivory power: Sexual harassment on campus
(pp. 141 175). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
Zdravomyslova, O., & Ajaib, A. (2006). The usual evil: Gender violence in
Russian schools. In F. Leach, & C. Mitchel (Eds.), Combating gender violence
in and around schools (pp. 7179). Sterling, VA: Trentham Books.
Zeira, A., Astor, R. A., & Benbenishty, R. (2002). Sexual harassment in Jewish
and Arab public schools in Israel. Child Abuse & Neglect, 26, 149166.
Zindi, F. (1998). Sexual harassment in Zimbabwes institutions of higher education. Perspectives in Education, 17, 3948.

This page intentionally left blank

Appendix: Womens Studies Programs


in the United States
Michele A. Paludi

Additional information regarding womens studies/gender studies programs may be obtained directly from the college or university and from
the Feminist Majority Foundation at http://feminist.org. The Feminist
Majority Foundation identies the degrees offered for each womens studies/gender studies program within the United States. Some programs
offer certicates, majors, and minors in womens studies/gender studies.
STATE
Alabama
Auburn University
University of Alabama
University of North Alabama
Alaska
University of Alaska, Fairbanks
Arizona
Arizona State University
Northern Arizona University
University of Arizona

234

Appendix

Arkansas
University of Arkansas
California
California Institute of Integral Studies
California Lutheran University
California Polytechnic State University
California State University, Chico
California State University, Fresno
California State University, Fullerton
California State University, Long Beach
California State University, Northridge
California State University, Sacramento
California State University, San Bernadino
California State University, San Marcos
California State University, Stanislaus
Cerritos College
Claremont Colleges
Claremont Graduate University
Foothill College
Mills College
Montclair State University
Moorpark College
New College of California
Ohlone College
Pomona College
Saddleback College
San Diego State University
San Francisco State University
Santa Clara University
Santa Monica College
Scripps College
Sonoma State University
Stanford University
University of California, Berkeley
University of California, Riverside
University of California, Santa Barbara
University of California, Santa Cruz

Appendix

University
University
University
University
University

of
of
of
of
of

California,
California,
California,
California,
San Diego

Los Angeles
Davis
Irvine
San Diego

Colorado
Colorado College
Fort Lewis College
Metropolitan State College of Denver
University of Colorado, Boulder
University of Colorado, Colorado Springs
University of Colorado, Denver
University of Denver
University of Northern Colorado
Connecticut
Connecticut College
Faireld University
Hartford College for Women
Southern Connecticut State University
Trinity College
Delaware
University of Delaware
District of Columbia
American University
George Washington University
Georgetown University
Trinity College
Florida
Eckerd College
Florida Atlantic University
Florida International University

235

236

Appendix

Florida State University


Nova Southeastern
Rollins College
Stetson University
University of Central Florida
University of Florida
University of Miami, Coral Gables
University of North Florida
University of South Florida
University of Tampa
University of West Florida
Georgia
Agnes Scott College
Clark Atlanta University
Emory University
Georgia College and State University
Georgia State University
Georgia Tech
University of Georgia
Hawaii
University of Hawaii, Manoa
Idaho
Albertson College
Idaho State University
University of Idaho
Illinois
Augustana College
Bradley University
DePaul University
Eastern Illinois University
Knox College
Loyola University

Appendix

Northeastern Illinois University


Northern Illinois University
Northwestern University
Principia College
Roosevelt University
Southern Illinois University, Carbondale
Southern Illinois University, Edwardsville
University of Chicago
University of Illinois, Chicago
University of Illinois, Springeld
Indiana
DePauw University
Earlham College
Indiana State University
Indiana University of Pennsylvania
Indiana University, South Bend
Indiana University, Purdue University Fort Wayne
St. Marys College
University of Notre Dame
Valparaiso University
Iowa
Coe College
Cornell College
Drake University
Grinnell College
Iowa State University
Luther College
Simpson College
University of Iowa
University of Northern Iowa
Kansas
Emporia State University
Kansas State University
University of Kansas

237

238

Appendix

Kentucky
Berea College
Brescia University
Eastern Kentucky University
Lexington Community College
Northern Kentucky University
University of Kentucky
University of Louisville
Louisiana
Louisiana State University
McNeese State University
Newcomb College
University of New Orleans
Maine
Bates College
Bowdoin College
Colby College
University of Maine, Farmington
University of Maine, Orono
University of Southern Maine
Maryland
Frostburg State University
Montgomery College
St. Marys College of Maryland
The Johns Hopkins University
University of Maryland, Baltimore County
University of Maryland, College Park
Massachusetts
Amherst College
Boston College
Boston University
Brandeis University

Appendix

Bridgewater State College


Clark University
College of the Holy Cross
Emmanuel College
Greeneld Community College
Hampshire College
Harvard University
Massachusetts College of Liberal Arts
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Radcliffe College
Salem State College
Simmons College
Smith College
Towson University
Tufts University
University of Massachusetts, Amherst
University of Massachusetts, Dartmouth
University of Massachusetts, Lowell
University of Massachusetts, Boston

Michigan
Albion College
Alma College
Central Michigan University
Eastern Michigan University
Grand Valley State University
Greeneld Community College
Hope College
Kalamazoo College
Michigan State University
University of Detroit
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
University of Michigan, Dearborn

Minnesota
Carleton College
Century College

239

240

Appendix

College of St. Benedict


College of St. Catherine
College of St. Scholastica
Hamline University
Macalester College
Minnesota State University, Mankato
St. Cloud State University
St. Olaf College
University of Minnesota, Twin Cities
University of Minnesota, Duluth
Mississippi
Millsaps College
Mississippi State University
University of Mississippi
Missouri
Avila College
Central Missouri State University
Maryville University of St. Louis
Saint Louis Missouri
Southwest Missouri State University
University of Missouri, Kansas
University of Missouri, St. Louis
Montana
Montana State University, Bozeman
University of Montana
Nevada
University of Nevada, Las Vegas
University of Nevada, Reno
New Hampshire
Dartmouth College
Franklin Pierce College

Appendix

Keene State College


Plymouth State University
Rivier College
New Jersey
College of New Jersey
Drew University
Georgia Court College
Monmouth University
Montclair State University
New Jersey City University
Princeton University
Ramapo College
Richard Stockton College
Rider University
Rowan University
Rutgers University
Rutgers University, Camden
New Mexico
Eastern New Mexico University
New Mexico State University
University of New Mexico
New York
Bard College
Barnard College
Brooklyn College
Canisus College
City University of New York Graduate Center
Colgate University
College of New Rochelle
College of Staten Island
Columbia University
Cornell University
Hamilton College
Hartwick College

241

242

Appendix

Hobart and William Smith Colleges


Iona College
Jewish Theological Seminary
Lehman College
Marist College
Marymount College
New School for Social Research
New York University
Pace University
Queens College
Russell Sage College
Sarah Lawrence College
Simons Rock College
Skidmore College
St. Lawrence University
State University of New York, Albany
State University of New York, Buffalo
State University of New York, Cortland
State University of New York, Fredonia
State University of New York, Geneseo
State University of New York, New Paltz
State University of New York, Oneonta
State University of New York, Oswego
State University of New York, Plattsburgh
State University of New York, Potsdam
State University of New York, Purchase
State University of New York, Rochester
State University of New York, Stony Brook
Syracuse University
Union College
North Carolina
Appalachian State University
Bennett College
Davidson College
Duke University
East Carolina University
Elton College

Appendix

Guilford College
North Carolina State University
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
University of North Carolina, Charlotte
University of North Carolina, Greensboro
University of North Carolina, Wilmington

North Dakota
North Dakota State University
University of North Dakota

Ohio
Antioch College
Bowling Green State University
Case Western Reserve University
College of Mount St. Joseph
College of Wooster
Denison University
Kent State University
Kenyon College
Marietta College
Miami University
Notre Dame College of Ohio
Oberlin College
Ohio State University
Ohio University
Ohio Wesleyan University
University of Akron
University of Cincinnati
University of Dayton
University of Toledo

Oklahoma
Oklahoma State University
University of Central Oklahoma
University of Oklahoma

243

244

Appendix

Oregon
Lewis & Clark College
Oregon State University
Portland State University
Southern Oregon University
University of Oregon
Pennsylvania
Allegheny College
Bryn Mawr College
Bucknell University
California University of Pennsylvania
Chatham College
Clarion University of Pennsylvania
Dickinson College
Franklin and Marshall College
Gettysburg College
Haverford College
Lafayette College
LaSalle University
Lehigh University
Lock Haven University
Lycoming College
Manseld University
Millersville University
Pennsylvania State University
Rosemont College
Shippensburg University
Slippery Rock University
Rhode Island
Brown University
University of Rhode Island
South Carolina
Clemson University
College of Charleston

Appendix

University of South Carolina, Columbia


University of South Carolina, Spartanburg
South Dakota
Augustana College
South Dakota State University
Tennessee
Austin Peay State University
Middle Tennessee University
Tennessee Technological University
University of Tennessee, Knoxville
University of the South
Vanderbilt University
Texas
Austin College
Rice University
Southern Methodist University
Southwest Texas State University
Southwestern University
Texas A & M University
Texas Tech University
University of Houston
University of North Texas
University of Texas, Arlington
University of Texas, Austin
University of Texas, Dallas
University of Texas, El Paso
Utah
Brigham Young University
University of Utah
Vermont
Middlebury College
Saint Michaels College
University of Vermont

245

246

Appendix

Virginia
College of William and Mary
George Mason University
Hollins University
James Madison University
Old Dominion University
Radford University
Randolph Macon Womens College
University of Richmond
University of Virginia
Washington
Clark College
Eastern Washington University
Edmonds Community College
Evergreen State College
Gonzaga University
Pacic Lutheran University
University of Puget Sound
University of Redlands
University of Washington

West Virginia
Marshall University
Mary Baldwin College
West Virginia University

Wisconsin
Beloit College
Lawrence University
Marquette University
University of Wisconsin,
University of Wisconsin,
University of Wisconsin,
University of Wisconsin,
University of Wisconsin,

Eau Claire
Green Bay
La Crosse
Madison
Milwaukee

Appendix

University
University
University
University
University
University
University
University

of
of
of
of
of
of
of
of

Wisconsin,
Wisconsin,
Wisconsin,
Wisconsin,
Wisconsin,
Wisconsin,
Wisconsin,
Wisconsin,

Oshkosh
Parkside
Platteville
River Falls
Stevens Point
Stout
Superior
Whitewater

247

This page intentionally left blank

About the Editor and Contributors

EDITOR
Michele A. Paludi, PhD, is the series editor for Womens Psychology for
Praeger Publishers, an imprint of ABC-CLIO, LLC. She is the author/editor of 33 college textbooks and more than 160 scholarly articles and
conference presentations on sexual harassment, campus violence, psychology of women, gender, and sexual harassment and victimization.
Her book, Ivory Power: Sexual Harassment on Campus (1990), received the
1992 Myers Center Award for Outstanding Book on Human Rights in the
United States). Dr. Paludi served as Chair of the U.S. Department of Educations Subpanel on the Prevention of Violence, Sexual Harassment, and
Alcohol and Other Drug Problems in Higher Education. She was one of
six scholars in the United States to be selected for this Subpanel. She also
was a consultant to and a member of former New York State Governor
Mario Cuomos Task Force on Sexual Harassment. Dr. Paludi serves as an
expert witness for court proceedings and administrative hearings on sexual harassment. She has had extensive experience in conducting training
programs and investigations of sexual harassment and other EEO issues
for businesses and educational institutions. In addition, Dr. Paludi has
held faculty positions at Franklin & Marshall College, Kent State University, Hunter College, Union College, and Union Graduate College, where
she directs the human resource management certicate program. She
teaches in the School of Management.

CONTRIBUTORS
Kristin Anderson, PhD, is an Associate Professor of Psychology at the
University of HoustonDowntown where she teaches social psychology,
psychology of prejudice, psychology of women, and psychology and the

250

About the Editor and Contributors

law. Her research interests are in the areas of prejudice, stereotyping, and
discrimination.
Martha E. Banks, PhD, is a research neuropsychologist in the Research &
Development Division of ABackans DCP, Inc., in Akron, Ohio and a former professor of Black Studies at The College of Wooster. She has been
instrumental in the development and revision of the Ackerman-Banks Neuropsychological Rehabilitation Battery; a test that is distinguished by its
inclusion of ethnic content and Women with Disabilities in the normative
sample. Dr. Banks is president of the Society for the Psychology of
Women. She is a fellow of American Psychological Association Divisions
35 (Society for the Psychology of Women), 22 (Rehabilitation Psychology),
and 56 (Trauma Psychology) and has served on the APA Council of Representatives and Board for the Advancement of Psychology in the Public
Interest. In 2008, she received a Presidential Citation from the American
Psychological Association for her expertise and service. Dr. Banks has
served on national advisory boards, including the Rosalynn Carter Institute for Human Developments Expert Panel on Caregiving for People
with Disabilities, and the National Advisory Board on Medical Rehabilitation Research. Dr. Banks is widely published; her articles include and
emphasize issues, particularly seldom addressed issues, affecting women
with disabilities and members of ethnic minority groups.
Susan Basow, PhD, is Charles A. Dana Professor of Psychology at Lafayette College in Easton, Pennsylvania, where she helped found the womens studies program and chaired the psychology department for six
years. Dr. Basow has taught courses on the psychology of gender since
1974 and is the author of the textbook, Gender: Stereotypes and Roles. A licensed psychologist, she also has published the results of many of her
studies of gender issues in course evaluations and of women and their
bodies. She has been a member of the executive committee of the Society
for the Psychology of Women since 2000.
Kristin P. Beals, PhD, earned her BA from Auburn University in 1995.
She earned her doctorate degree in social psychology from UCLA in 2003.
She has been an assistant professor at California State University, Fullerton for the past 4 years. Her research has focused on understanding how
stigma impacts the well-being of lesbians, gay men, and bisexuals.
Breena E. Coates, PhD, is Chair and Associate Professor in the Department of Management, College of Business and Public Administration at
California State University, San Bernadino. She has been a professor of
management for the department of command, leadership, and management at the United States Army War College, Carlisle, Pennsylvania. She
has a BA in English from Calcutta University and an MPA and Ph.D. in

About the Editor and Contributors

251

public policy impacts on organizational behavior from the University of


Pittsburgh. Her current research focuses on impacts of public policy on
organizational behavior, strategic management, and leadership. A second
area of interest is organizational behavior, strategy and cultural change in
the United States military. She is also researching the role of Indian policewomen who were trained and sent to keep peace in Liberia.
J. Harold Ellens is a retired university professor of philosophy and psychology, a retired U.S. Army Colonel, a retired Presbyterian pastor and
theologian, Executive Director Emeritus of the Christian Association for
Psychological Studies International, Founder and Editor in Chief Emeritus
of the Journal of Psychology and Christianity, a clinical psychotherapist in
private practice, and the author, co-author, or editor of 175 volumes and
author of 166 professional journal articles. He continues in his role as
Adjunct Professor of Philosophy and Biblical Studies at University of
Detroit Mercy, in Classics at Wayne State University, and Research Scholar
in the Department of Near Eastern Studies at the University of Michigan.
Leanne Faraday-Brash is an organizational psychologist, executive coach,
facilitator and mediator with two decades of experience in organizational
capability and culture, workplace justice, and conict resolution and leadership. While Leanne consults in a range of areas, the common thread is
the emphasis on improving organizational effectiveness and workplace
climate for all staff to drive performance and maximize organizational
health and well-being. Leanne is also the founding principal of the Workplace Justice Consortium.
Gordon B. Forbes, PhD, a licensed clinical psychologist, is a Professor of
Psychology Emeritus at Millikin University in Decatur, Illinois, and the
director and cofounder of the Millikin Project on Social Perceptions. His
research interests include cross-cultural, ethnic, gender, and generational
differences in body image; cross-cultural and gender differences in interpersonal aggression; and sexism.
Emily A. Haddad, PhD, is the author of Orientalist Poetics: The Islamic
Middle East in Nineteenth-Century English and French Poetry (2002) and has
published mainly in the eld of nineteenth-century British literary studies. She earned her PhD in Comparative Literature at Harvard University
in 1997 and is now Professor and Chair of English at the University of
South Dakota.
Jaehee Jung, PhD, is an associate professor in the Department of Fashion
and Apparel Studies at University of Delaware. Her research interests
include effects of cognitive and sociocultural variables on body image,
cross-cultural research in body image, cultures in transition and changing

252

About the Editor and Contributors

roles of women, media inuences on body image, muscular body ideals


and male body image; and ethical issues in fashion advertising.
Melinda Kanner is a Visiting Associate Professor of Anthropology at the
University of HoustonDowntown in Houston, Texas, where she teaches
courses in anthropology, mass media, and sociology. Her research
explores the media expression of identity and has focused on tourism
(Savannah), gay identity (Queer Eye For the Straight Guy), and masculinity
(Mad Men).
Jennifer L. Martin is the department head of English at a public alternative high school for at-risk students in Michigan and a lecturer at Oakland
University where she teaches graduate research methods in the department of Educational Leadership, Feminist Methods, and Introduction to
Womens Gender Studies in the department of Women and Gender Studies. She is not only a feminist teacher, but a feminist activist. She has volunteered as an assault responder and engaged in political action for
feminist causes. Currently, she is the Title IX Education Task Force Chair
for the Michigan National Organization for Women in order to advocate
for Title IX compliance in Michigans schools. She has conducted research
and written articles on the topics of peer sexual harassment, teaching for
social justice, service learning, and the at-risk student.
Rhonda M. Schultz is a graduate student in the MA program in psychology at California State University at Fullerton. She received her BA in psychology from CSU Fullerton in 2006 and has plans to continue on for her
PhD in psychology in 2010. She is currently working on research investigating the effects of the passage of anti-gay marriage legislation in California on gay, lesbian, and bisexual individuals residing there.
William Schweinle received a PhD (2002) from the University of Texas at
Arlington in Experimental (Social and Quantitative) Psychology. His
research has focused on the social psychology of mens wife directed
aggression and mens sexual harassment of women, among other areas.
He is currently an Assistant Professor in the University of South Dakota
Physician Assistant Program and Chair of the South Dakota Medical
Institutional Review Board.
Samantha Smith presently resides in Southern New England. She
earned her Bachelors degree in psychology and is currently working on
her Masters in special education.
Susan Strauss, RN, EdD, is a national and international speaker, trainer,
and consultant. Her specialty areas include harassment and workplace
bullying, organization development, and management/leadership

About the Editor and Contributors

253

development. Her clients are from business, education, healthcare, law,


and government organizations from both the public and private sector.
Dr. Strauss has authored book chapters, articles in professional journals,
written curriculum and training manuals, as well as authored the book,
Sexual Harassment and Teens: A Program for Positive Change. Susan has been
featured on The Donahue Show, CBS Evening News, and other television
and radio programs as well as interviewed for newspaper and journal
articles such as the Times of London, Lawyers Weekly, and Harvard Education
Newsletter. Susan has presented at international conferences in Botswana,
Egypt, Thailand, Israel, and the United States, and conducted sex discrimination research in Poland. She has consulted with professionals from
other countries such as England, Australia, Canada, and St. Maartin.

This page intentionally left blank

Index

Academic achievements, gender


differences in, 20; culture as cause
for, 33; mathematics and, 3031,
32; media coverage and, 34;
potential solutions for, 3840;
Title IX and, 3536, 37
ActionAid International, 221
Afghanistan, sexual harassment in
schools, 206
All-Volunteer Force, 116
Ambivalence toward Men Inventory
(AMI), 7, 89; benevolence toward
men, 7; hostility toward men, 7
Ambivalent sexism, 11; benevolent
sexism, 12, 13; hostile sexism, 12,
13; studies on, 14; women chivalry
and, 1314
American Association of University
Women (AAUW), 19, 34, 48, 49,
19394, 195, 198
American Psychiatric Association, 135
American Psychological Associations
Guidelines on Multicultural
Education, Training, Research,
Practice, and Organizational
Change for Psychologists, 14951
Anorexia nervosa, 162
Anti-gay campaign, 14244
Anxiety disorders and lesbian and
bisexual women, 13839
Arizona Star, 11819

Army Research Institute of


Environmental Medicine, 117
The Army Times, 117
Arroyo, Gloria Macapagal, 107
Australia: colleges, sexual harassment
in, 210; schools, sexual harassment
in, 2078
Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS),
67
Australian Institute of Health and
Welfare (AIHW) National
Perinatal Statistics Unit, 78
The Australian Newspaper, 69
Bachelet, Michelle, 97, 1046
Beaton Consulting, 68
Beijing Declaration and Platform for
Action, 53
Belgium, sexual harassment in
schools, 202
Benedict XVI (Pope), 90, 94
Benevolence toward men, 78, 9
Benevolent sexism: insidious effects
of, 13; womens view on, 12
Benin, sexual harassment in schools,
202
Blue-collar jobs, 68
Body dissatisfaction, 178; in Africa,
17476; cross-cultural research on,
16869; as culture-limited
phenomena, 16465; in East Asian

256

Index

countries, 17273; ethnic


differences and, 16970; growth of,
165; history of, 163; in Indian
peninsula, 176; in Latin American
countries, 17374; modernization
and western media impact on,
16566; reasons for using western
culture as benchmark, 170; social
change and, 16768; in Southeast
Asia, 17677; theoretical models of,
16667, feminist theory, 167,
sociocultural theory, 16667;
among Western women, 163
Body image, 16162
Botswana, sexual harassment in
schools, 215
Boy crisis, 1920, 34, 35
Brazil: body dissatisfaction in, 17374;
sexual harassment in schools,
19899
British Broadcasting Company (BBC),
201
Brosnan, Audrey, 89
Buddhism, 85
Bulimia, 162
Bush, G. H. W., 112, 117
Bush, Laura, 34
Butera, Karina, 74
Buturo, James Nsaba, 130
Cameroon, sexual harassment in
schools, 202
Canada: colleges, sexual harassment
in, 196; schools, sexual harassment
in, 193
Career Incentive Act of 1977, 47
Center-stealing, 20
Chamorro, Violetta, 107
China: colleges, sexual harassment in,
2089; schools, sexual harassment
in, 205
Cody, General Richard, 116
Colorado State Constitution, 142
Combat Action Ribbon, 117
Combat Exclusion Policy, 116
Combat-underlying premises,
objections for women in 117;
biological thesis, 11718; privacy
infringement hypothesis, 119;

psychological thesis, 118; sexual


abuse and gender harassment
theories, 11819
Constantinian revolution, 86, 88
Contingent rewards system, 98
Convention on the Elimination of All
Forms of Discrimination Against
Women (CEDAW), 19192
Convention on the Rights of the
Child (CRC), 192
Cyber-harassment/bullying, 189
Dati, Rachida, 66
Defense Authorization Acts (1992,
1993), 116
Democracia de los acuerdos, 105
Department of Defense (DoD), 111,
112; Manpower Research Statistics,
120
Department of Health and Human
Services, 136
Depression and lesbian and bisexual
women, 139
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
(DSM), 135
Differential aptitude tests (DAT), 31
Direct combat rule policy, 112
Discrimination: denition, 128;
expected, 132; against LGB, 131
Drug dependency of lesbian and
bisexual women, 13940
Dunwoody, General Ann E., 114
Eating disorders, 162, 178; in Africa,
17476; cross-cultural research on,
16869; as culture-limited
phenomena, 16465; diagnosing,
16263; in East Asian countries,
171172; history of, 164; in Indian
peninsula, 176; in Latin America,
17374; modernization and
western media impact on, 16566;
reasons for using western culture
as benchmark, 170; in Southeast
Asia, 17677
Ecuador, sexual harassment in
schools, 199
Education, women in, 43; barriers to
gender equity in, 4551, gender

Index
curriculum, 4748, gender
stereotyping, 4647, gendered roles
and expectations, 4546, peer
inuence, 4849, teacher behaviors,
4951; educational attainment,
4445; effects of differential school
experiences, 5153; gender bias in
evaluating women professors,
5657; women as students, 4344;
women as teachers and professors,
53, gendered teaching styles,
5455, representation, 5354
Education Amendments of 1972, 47
Egypt, sexual harassment in schools,
21819
Egyptian Center for Womens Rights
(ECWR), 21819
England, sexual harassment in
schools, 200201
Equal Employment Opportunity
(EEO), 67, 7071, 79
Equal Opportunity for Women in the
Workplace Agency (EOWA), 68, 71
Eve-teasing, 209
Failing at Fairness: How Americas
Schools Cheat Girls, 19
Farrell, Warren, 19
Fear of extinction, 131
Feminine political persona, 97, 107;
Bachelet, Michelle, 97, 1046;
Johnson Sirleaf, Ellen, 97, 1034;
leadership styles, transformational
style, 9899, transactional style, 98,
99; Queen Victoria, 9798, 99103
Feminism, 2; attitude towards men, 5;
attitude towards women, 5;
denition, 4; Smith, Samantha
views on, 6364
Feminist Majority Foundation, 39
Feminist movement, 63
Feminist theory of body
dissatisfaction, 16667
Finland, sexual harassment in
schools, 202
Fourth World Conference on Women,
53
France, sexual harassment in schools,
202

257

Gallup Poll (2003), 118


Gay, Lesbian, and Straight Education
Network (GLSEN), 195
Gay, lesbian, bisexual, and
transgendered (GLBT), sexual
harassment of, 195, 198
Gender-based discrimination, 1011
Gender bias in evaluating women
professors, 5657
Gender differences, 27; in academic
achievement, culture as cause for,
33, mathematics and, 3031, 32,
media coverage and, 34, potential
solutions for, 3840, Title IX and,
3536, 37; negative stereotypes
based on, 28; socialization of girls
and, 29; sociocultural inuences in,
28; in workplace, 2930
Gendered teaching styles, 5455
Gender equity in education, barriers
to, 4551; gender curriculum,
4748; gender stereotyping, 4647;
gendered roles and expectations,
4546; peer inuence, 4849;
teacher behaviors, 4951
Gender harassment, 191; in college of
United States, 197; in military,
11819
Gender schema theory, 30
Gender socialization theory, 28
Germany, sexual harassment in
schools, 202
Ghana: colleges, sexual harassment
in, 217; schools, sexual harassment
in, 215
Gillard, Julia, 69
Gilligan, Carol, 19
Global war on terrorism (GWOT), 111
Ground combat policy (1994), 111
Gryson, Roger, 87
Haiti, sexual harassment in schools,
199
Healthy People 2010 initiative, 136
Hickie, Marea, 77
Hickie v Hunt & Hunt, 77
Homophobia, 16
Hostile sexism, 12
Hostility toward men, 7

258

Index

House Armed Services Committee


(HASC), 116, 11718
Human Rights and Equal
Opportunity Commission
(HREOC), 70, 79
Human Rights Commission, 77
Hunter, Duncan, 116, 11718
Iazzo study, 56
In a Different Voice and Mary Piphers
(1994), 19
India: colleges, sexual harassment in,
20910; schools, sexual harassment
in, 2067
Institute for Antiquity and
Christianity, 88
Institute of Medicine, 135, 136
Internalized homonegativity of
lesbians, 13233
International Covenant on Civil and
Political Rights (ICCPR), 192
International Covenant on Economic,
Social and Cultural Rights
(ICESCR), 192
International human rights law, 191
International Labor Organization
(ILO), 73
International Rescue Committee
(IRC), 215
Ireland Republic, sexual harassment
in school, 203
Islam, 8586
Israel, sexual harassment in schools, 219
Italy, sexual harassment in colleges,
2034
Ivy League schools, 34
Jackrolling, 213
Jamaica, sexual harassment in
schools, 199
Japan, sexual harassment in schools,
205
Jesus Movement, 86
Johnson Sirleaf, Ellen, 97, 1034, 106
Kaiser Family Foundation report, 131
Kenya: colleges, sexual harassment
in, 217; schools, sexual harassment
in, 213

Kirchner, Cristina Fernandez de, 107


Kning, 216
Korea, sexual harassment in colleges,
208
Labeling and women with
disabilities, 151
Laird, Melvin, 115
Lesbian, 17. See also sexual minority
women; attitudes towards men, 6;
internalized homonegativity,
13233; lesbian-baiting, 1618;
triple minority status and, 13031
Lesbian-baiting, 1618
Lesbian internalized homonegativity
(LIH), 13233
The Life of Queen Victoria and the Story
of Her Reign: A Beautiful Tribute to
Englands Greatest Queen in Her
Domestic and Ofcial Life, 100
Lioness: There for the Action, Missing
from History, 121
Little Rascals child comedy, 2
Los Angeles County Health Survey,
141
Ma Ellen. See Johnson Sirleaf, Ellen
Malawi, sexual harassment in schools,
215
Malaysia, sexual harassment in
colleges, 208
Male bashing, 21
Mama. See Bachelet, Michelle
Man-hating feminist, 2, 3; reasons for
existence of myth, 9; sources of, 4
Manliness, 18
Manseld, Harvey, 18
McLagan, Meg, 121
McMaster University Residency
Training Programs, 196
Meir, Golda, 106
Military Police Corps, 115
Military services, women in, 121;
defensive positions for, 11112;
direct combat rule policy, 112; goal
displacement, distortion and drift,
11215; history and law, 11517;
objections: for women in combat
premises, 117, biological thesis,

Index
11718, privacy infringement
hypothesis, 119, psychological
thesis, 118, sexual abuse and
gender harassment theories,
11819; USWAC women in
combat survey research, 11920
Minority stress theory, 131
Montraynaud, Florence, 66
Morocco, sexual harassment in
schools, 216
Mugabe, Robert Gabriel, 129
The Myth of Male Power, 19
Napolitano, Janet, 118
National Advisory Council on
Womens Educational Programs,
19091
National Assessment of Educational
Progress, 20
National Collective of the Rights of
Women, 66
National College Women Sexual
Victimization (NCWSV), 19798
National Collegiate Athletic
Association (NCAA), 36
National Institute on Disability and
Rehabilitation Research, 150
National Womens Law Center, 36
Nepal, sexual harassment in schools,
207
Netherland: colleges, sexual
harassment in, 204; schools,
sexual harassment in, 201
Nigeria, sexual harassment in
colleges, 21718
Nixon, Christine, 69
Non-feminists, attitudes toward
women, 6
OBeirne, Kate, 19
Pakistan, sexual harassment in
schools, 2056
Persian Gulf War, 117, 119
Phallic attack, 217
Pink jobs, 68
Prejudice, 127
Presidential Commission on the
Assignment of Women in the

259

Armed Forces (PCAWAF), 112,


117, 118
Puerto Rico, sexual harassment in
colleges, 199
Ragging, 206, 209
Rape crisis centers, 17
Rehabilitation psychologist, 15456
Religion as stigma against same-sex
sexual behavior, 12829
Religious leaders, women as, 85, 93;
in Christian missionaries, 8691,
92; constantinian revolution and,
86, 88; in Muslim congregations,
9192; in Roman Catholic church,
89, 90
Reviving Ophelia, 19
Revolutionary War, 115
Risk Rule, 116
Robinson, James M., 87
Rumsfeld, Donald, 116
Russia, sexual harassment in schools,
207
Schlaffy, Phyllis, 118
Schroeder, Pat, 116
September 11, 2001, 12021
Sex Discrimination Act (SDA), 70, 71
Sexism, 11
Sexual bullying, 18889
Sexual coercion, 191
Sexual harassment, 19, 187, 189;
denition, 190; in education, 192,
224, in Africa, 21018, in Asia
Pacic, 20410, in Europe, 2004,
impact, 21921, in Latin America
and Caribbean, 198200, in
Middle East, 21819, in North
America, 19298; experiences of
college women, 191; national
laws prohibiting, 191;
recommendations for prohibiting,
22123, national and international
efforts, 222, parents and
community verve, 22223, public
health, 223, research, 221, school
initiatives, 222, student
participation, 223,
terminology, 18889

260

Index

Sexual harassment trauma syndrome,


220
Sexual minority women: behavioral
risk factors and health practices,
14041; consequences: barriers to
health care, 13638, depression
and anxiety disorders, 13839,
drug dependency, 13940, legal
and economic impact for same-sex
female couples, 134, lesbian health
disparities, history of, 13536,
mental and physical health, 135,
mental health disparities, 138,
physical health disparities, 140,
social costs for sexual minority
women, 13435; of stigma,
133 coping strategies, 14244;
discrimination, experience of, 131,
expected discrimination, 132,
internalized homonegativity,
13233; minority stress
theory, 131; outcomes, 14142;
sources of stigma, religion,
12829, westernization, 12930;
triple minority status,
13031; understanding stigma,
126, prejudice, 127,
stereotyping, 12728, stigma,
12627
Sexual violence, 187; as
reection of gender inequality,
18990; in schools, study on, 188
Sociocultural theory of body
dissatisfaction, 16667
Sommers, Christina Hoff, 19
Sommers, Daria, 121
South Africa, sexual harassment in
schools, 21315
South Asian countries,
sexual harassment in schools, 206
Spain: college, sexual harassment in,
204; schools, sexual harassment in,
202
Stereotyping, 12728
Stigma, 12627
Stigma consciousness, 132
Sturduts, Maya, 66
Summers, Christiana Hoff, 34
Summers, Lawrence, 27, 37

Sweden: college, sexual harassment


in, 204; schools, sexual harassment
in, 2023
Swedish National Agency for
Education, 202
Texas Civil Right Project, 195
Thailand, sexual harassment in
colleges, 210
Thatcher, Margaret, 106
Theresa, Mother, 91
Title IX, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39
Tunisia, sexual harassment in schools,
216
Turkey, sexual harassment in schools,
219
Uganda, sexual harassment in
schools, 215
United Nations, 12930, 187, 191
United Nations Millennium Summit,
192; Millennium Development
Goals, 192
United Nations Secretary-Generals
Study on Violence against
Children, 192, 207
United Nations Study on Violence
Against Children, 187
United States: Army War College in
combat survey research, 11920;
colleges, sexual harassment in,
19798; sexual harassment in
secondary schools, 19396
Universal Declaration of Human
Rights, 192
Unwanted sexual attention, 191
U.S. Military Academy, 115
U.S. Department of Education, Ofce
of Civil Rights (OCR) (1993), 190,
194, 195
U.S. Department of Labor, 80
Victoria, Queen, 9798, 103, 106;
family life of, books on, 100101;
as female monarch, 100; Plunketts
views on, 102; portrait description,
102; reign in England, 99100
Vocational Educational Act of 1976,
47

Index
The War Against Boys: How Misguided
Feminism is Harming our Young
Men, 19, 34
West Africa, sexual harassment in
schools, 215
westernization, as stigma against
same-sex sexual behavior, 12930
Williams Institute, 134
The Woman Haters, 12
Women: attitude towards men, 10;
barriers to ascendance of women
in workplaces, 7376; as
consumers, 70; discrimination
against women on pregnancy, 77;
in education. See education,
women in; EEO law and, 7073;
glass ceiling phenomenon and,
7376; in leadership, 15. See also
feminine political persona;
maternal wall phenomenon and,
7779; in military. See military
services, women in; penalties for
nontraditional, 1416; with power,
6970; sexual harassment impact
on, 21921; sticky oor
phenomenon, 7677; treating
women badly, 75; views on
benevolent sexism, 12; work/
family conict and, 7981;
workforce participation and
earning parity, 6768; in
workplace, 2829

261

Women in Combat Compendium,


119
Womens Armed Forces Integration
Act (1948), 115
Womens Educational Equity Act of
1974, 47
Women with disabilities: abuse
related to disability-related
settings, 153; abuse related to
helping relationships, 15354; as
cultural group, 15152; emotional
abuse, disability-related, 152;
nancial abuse, disability-related,
154; physical abuse,
disability-related, 15253;
rehabilitation psychologist treating
abused, 15456; rehabilitation
psychology, guidelines for, 14951;
safety concerns for, 15254;
sexual abuse, disability-related,
153
Women Who Make the World Worse
and How Their Radical Feminist
Assault Is Ruining Our Schools,
Families, Military, and Sports,
1920
World Christian Encyclopedia, 85
World Health Organization (WHO),
189, 192
Zimbabwe: colleges, sexual
harassment in, 216; schools,
sexual harassment in, 21113

This page intentionally left blank

Feminism and Womens


Rights Worldwide

Recent Titles in
Womens Psychology
Intimate Violence against Women: When Spouses, Partners, or Lovers Attack
Paula K. Lundberg-Love and Shelly L. Marmion, editors
Daughters of Madness: Growing Up and Older with a Mentally Ill Mother
Susan Nathiel
Psychology of Women: Handbook of Issues and Theories, Second Edition
Florence L. Denmark and Michele Paludi, editors
WomanSoul: The Inner Life of Womens Spirituality
Carole A. Rayburn and Lillian Comas-Diaz, editors
The Psychology of Women at Work: Challenges and Solutions for Our Female
Workforce
Michele A. Paludi, editor

Feminism and Womens


Rights Worldwide
Volume 2
Mental and Physical Health

MICHELE A. PALUDI, EDITOR


Praeger Perspectives
Womens Psychology
Michele A. Paludi, Series Editor

PRAEGER
An Imprint of ABC-CLIO, LLC

Copyright 2010 by Michele A. Paludi


All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced,
stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any
means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise,
except for the inclusion of brief quotations in a review, without prior
permission in writing from the publisher.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Feminism and womens rights worldwide / Michele A. Paludi, editor.
v. ; cm. (Womens psychology)
Includes bibliographical references and index.
Contents: The myth of the man-hating feminist / Melinda Kanner and
Kristin J. Anderson Gender differences : the arguments regarding abilities /
Jennifer L. Martin Women in education : students and professors worldwide
/ Susan Basow In womens voices / Samantha Smith Working life as a
house : a tale of oors, walls, and ceilings / Leanne Faraday-Brash Women
as religious leaders : advances and stalemates / J. Harold Ellens The
feminine political persona : Queen Victoria, Ellen Johnson Sirleaf, and Michelle
Bachelet / Emily A. Haddad and William Schweinle Women in the military : is
it time to un-gender combat roles? / Breena E. Coates Sexual minority
women : sources and outcomes of stigmatization / Rhonda M. Schultz, and
Kristin P. Beals Special issues for women with disabilities / Martha E.
Banks Body dissatisfaction and disordered eating : the globalization of
western appearance ideals / Jaehee Jung and Gordon B. Forbes Sexual
violence to girls and women in schools around the world / Susan Strauss.
ISBN 978-0-313-37596-5 (set : hard copy : alk. paper) ISBN 978-0-31337597-2 (set : ebook) ISBN 978-0-313-37598-9 (v.1 : hard copy : alk. paper)
ISBN 978-0-313-37599-6 (v.1 : ebook) ISBN 978-0-313-37600-9 (v.2 : hard
copy : alk. paper) ISBN 978-0-313-37601-6 (v.2 : ebook) ISBN 978-0-31337602-3 (v.3 : hard copy : alk. paper) ISBN 978-0-313-37603-0 (v.3 : ebook)
1. Feminism. 2. Womens rights. 3. Sexual harassment of women. 4. Abused
womenPsychology. 5. WomenPsychology. I. Paludi, Michele Antoinette
HQ1180.F424 2010
305.42dc22
2009035343
ISBN: 978-0-313-37596-5
EISBN: 978-0-313-37597-2
14 13 12

11 10

2 3 4 5

This book is also available on the World Wide Web as an eBook.


Visit www.abc-clio.com for details.
Praeger
An Imprint of ABC-CLIO, LLC
ABC-CLIO, LLC
130 Cremona Drive, P.O. Box 1911
Santa Barbara, California 93116-1911
This book is printed on acid-free paper
Manufactured in the United States of America

For Rosa and Lucia, my maternal and paternal grandmothers


and for Antoinette, my mother:
Remember, our heritage is our power; we can know ourselves and
our capacities by seeing that other women have been strong.
Judy Chicago

This page intentionally left blank

Contents

Series Introduction

ix

Acknowledgments

xi

Introduction
Michele A. Paludi

xiii

Chapter 1. International Perspectives on Women and Mental


Health
Joy Rice and Nancy Felipe Russo

Chapter 2. Ethnocultural Psychotherapy: Women of Colors


Resilience and Liberation
Lillian Comas-Diaz

25

Chapter 3. Women and Sexual Violence: Emotional, Physical,


Behavioral, and Organizational Responses
Paula Lundberg-Love and Bethany Waits

41

Chapter 4. Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls:


From Dating to Intimate Partner Violence
Janet Sigal and Dorota Wnuk Novitskie

65

Chapter 5. Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern:


Impact on Womens Emotional and Physical Well-Being
and Careers
Michele A. Paludi, Jessica Wilmot and Lindsey Speach
103
Chapter 6. From Victim to Empowered Survivor: Feminist Therapy
with Survivors of Rape and Sexual Assault
Avigail Moor
139

viii

Contents

Chapter 7. Gender Microaggressions: Implications for Mental


Health
Kevin L. Nadal

155

Chapter 8. Prejudice and Discrimination against Sexual Minorities:


A Brazilian Perspective
Eros DeSouza and Elder Cerqueira-Santos
177
Chapter 9. Frequency Rates and Consequences of Peer Sexual
Harassment: Comparing U.S. and International
Students
Eros DeSouza and Joy Chien

195

Chapter 10. In Womens Voices


Janet Boyce

209

Chapter 11. Bullying and Sexual Harassment of Adolescents


James Gruber and Susan Fineran

211

Chapter 12. Great Is Our Sin: Pseudoscientic Justications for


Oppression in American Education
Jennifer L. Martin

231

Chapter 13. Discrimination, Harassment, and Womens Physical


and Mental Health
Krystle C. Woods and NiCole T. Buchanan

235

Appendix.

Feminist and Womens Rights Organizations


Worldwide
Susan Strauss, Michelle Strand and Michele A. Paludi

253

About the Editor and Contributors

263

Index

271

Series Introduction

Because womens work is never done and is underpaid or unpaid or boring


or repetitious and were the rst to get red and what we look like is more
important than what we do and if we get raped its our fault and if we get
beaten we must have provoked it and if we raise our voices were nagging
bitches and if we enjoy sex were nymphos and if we dont were frigid and
if we love women its because we cant get a real man and if we ask our
doctor too many questions were neurotic and/or pushy and if we expect
childcare were selsh and if we stand up for our rights were aggressive
and unfeminine and if we dont were typical weak females and if we
want to get married were out to trap a man and if we dont were unnatural and because we still cant get an adequate safe contraceptive but men
can walk on the moon and if we cant cope or dont want a pregnancy were
made to feel guilty about abortion and . . . for lots of other reasons we are
part of the womens liberation movement.
Author unknown, quoted in The Torch, September 14, 1987

These sentiments underlie the major goals of the Praeger Perspectives


book series, Womens Psychology. The goals are as follows:
Value women: The books in this series value women by valuing children and working for affordable child care; value women by respecting
all physiques, not just by placing value on slender women; value
women by acknowledging older womens wisdom, beauty, aging; value
women who have been sexually victimized and view them as survivors;
value women who work inside and outside of the home; and value
women by respecting their choices of careers, of whom they mentor, of
their reproductive rights, their spirituality, and their sexuality.
Treat women as the norm. Thus the books in this series make up for
womens issues typically being omitted, trivialized, or dismissed from
other books on psychology.

Series Introduction

Take a non-Eurocentric view of womens experiences. The books in this


series integrate the scholarship on race and ethnicity into womens psychology, thus providing a psychology of all women. Women typically
have been described collectively; but we are diverse.
Facilitate connections between readers experiences and psychological theories and empirical research. The books in this series offer readers opportunities to challenge their views about women, feminism, sexual
victimization, gender role socialization, education, and equal rights.
These texts thus encourage women readers to value themselves and
others. The accounts of womens experiences as reected through
research and personal stories in the texts in this series have been
included for readers to derive strength from the efforts of others who
have worked for social change on the interpersonal, organizational,
and societal levels. A student in one of my courses on the psychology
of women once stated:
I learned so much about women. Women face many issues: discrimination, sexism, prejudices . . . by society. Women need to work together to
change how society views us. I learned so much and talked about much
of the issues brought up in class to my friends and family. My attitudes
have changed toward a lot of things. I got to look at myself, my life, and
what I see for the future. (Paludi, 2002)

It is my hope that readers of the books in this series will also reect
on the topics and look at themselves, their own lives, and what they
see for the future. This three-volume book set on Feminism and Womens Rights Worldwide provides readers with the opportunity to accomplish this goal and offers suggestions for all of us working for
gender justice within our friendships and romantic relationships, in
guiding institutional and social policy change in workplace and educational institutions, and in lobbying state and federal legislators on
issues related to reproductive rights, pay equity, education, sexual violence, and childcare.
Michele A. Paludi
Series Editor
REFERENCE
Paludi, M. (2002). The psychology of women. 2nd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.

Acknowledgments

Teaching and writing are separate, but serve/feed one another in so many
ways. Writing travels the road inward, teaching, the road outhelping
OTHERS move inwardit is an honor to be with others in the spirit of
writing and encouragement.
Naomi Shihab Nye

Nyes sentiment is echoed throughout this three-volume set on feminism and womens rights. Most of the contributors have taught courses
in womens studies and feminism as well as conducted research and
written about feminist issues. Many contributors have been advocates
on behalf of feminist principles through working with local, state and
federal agencies, legislators, and the United Nations. And many of us
have collaborated with students in our classes in writing chapters for
this book set. These students have made us believe that all of them, in
their individual ways, will continue to do what this book set intends:
value feminism and work toward equality. It has been exhilarating for
me to see a new generation of feminists collaborating with mentors
and colleagues on the chapters for this book set.
I have been honored to have collaborated with the contributors to
these volumes. Several friendships with contributors have been
rekindled and strengthened, and I have met many new colleagues from
around the world who taught me about their disciplines through their
writing. You have all shown me the great accomplishments of feminists
as well as the work we have yet to do. Thank you.
I wish to thank my sisters, Rosalie Paludi and Lucille Paludi, for
their support during the preparation of this book set. I also thank Carmen Paludi, Jr. for his guidance and encouragement. Our discussions
about feminism brought back wonderful memories of my mother,

xii

Acknowledgments

Antoinette, and my father, Michael, about whom I continue to learn


and continue to cherish the time I had with them.
I acknowledge several friends who encouraged me during the preparation of this set of books. Thank you to Paula Lundberg Love, Jennifer Martin, Billie Wright Dziech, Darlene C. DeFour, and Florence
Denmark.
I have been fortunate to have had the opportunity to work with students throughout my career, now at Union Graduate College. I have
thoroughly enjoyed learning from them. Thank you to students in the
Human Resource Management Certicate Program and Management
and Leadership Certicate Program. I especially acknowledge Michelle
Strand, Carrie Turco, Haimanot Kelbessa, Sarah Bennett, Sarah Boggess, Kristina Hicks, James Luciano, Sarah Henderson Maneely, Abbey
Massoud-Tastor, Marie Fuda, Jessica Wilmot, Katie Kelly, and Nick Salvatoriello. I am honored you have called me your professor.
I also thank Debbie Carvalko for supporting my visions for books
and helping them become realities. I have enjoyed working with Debbie and her colleagues at Praeger. They are a wonderful team of caring
people. They appreciate my love of writing and editing books. Debbie
somehow knew that, after the publication of the three-volume set on
the Psychology of Women and Work (2008, Praeger), which I edited, and
the political climate of the 2008 presidential campaign, especially
regarding women, I had to follow up those texts with books on feminism. She knows I share Sheila Benders sentiment:
We write because something inside says we must and we can no longer ignore
that voice.

Introduction
Michele A. Paludi
And how do you look backward? By looking forward. And what do you see?
As they look forward, they see what they had to do before they could look
backward. And there we have it all.
Gertrude Stein

Alyssa Zucker and Abigail Stewart (2007) reported in their study of


333 university alumnae that feminism is internalized quite differently
depending on the developmental stage in our lives. This research led
me to consider my own feminist socialization and feminist identity development as I began writing and editing these three volumes on feminism and womens rights. I was introduced to feminism by my
parents, Antoinette and Michael, at a very young age, even though the
label feminism was not used by them. Yet, as I came to realize much
later, their behavior was very much in keeping with feminist principles. They valued my sisters and me unconditionally; wanted to give
us educational opportunities that were denied to them because of the
generation into which they were born and because they were rst generation Americans whose parents had other values to instill in them;
they worked for equality in relationships, politics, and health care. I
was 18 the year individuals became eligible to vote at age 18, and both
my parents took me to cast my votes that year.
They believed that, like them, I had a responsibility to make things
better for the next generation. They valued voting; I was told what the
Suffragists had endured in order to win this right for us and to remember this each year I vote. I took my rst course in feminism as an
undergraduate in the early 1970s: Sex Roles in American Society
with Nancy Walbek. I would share the class discussions with my
mother, telling her about the experiences of students in class that were
different from my ownfor example, being denied the use of certain

xiv

Introduction

toys considered sex inappropriate for them; being tracked into different high school and college programs because of being women or men;
women being told by family and friends to hide their achievements
from potential dates and mates. I was unable to relate to these experiences and realized for the rst time that my parents were feminists, a
term to which I was introduced formally in this class and then subsequently as a graduate student when I took courses with Dee Graham
and Edna Rawlings. I also learned that I had been exposed to nonstereotyped role models, and because there were all girls in our family, we
were not raised to conform to stereotyped behavior.
It was in graduate school that I decided to pursue research in feminist psychology, especially in womens career development. I was fortunate to have a mentor, William Dember, who encouraged me to
pursue this research, even though it was not in his area of specialization (i.e., visual perception). Bill encouraged me to take courses with
faculty in departments in addition to psychology: educational leadership and family development. He told me this would help put pieces
together in understanding the research I was conducting. I thank Kathy
Borman and Judy Frankel for their roles in my feminist identity development.
A few years later when my father died, Charlie, who attended my
fathers wake, came to my mother, my sisters, and me and told us how
my father had impacted his life. Charlie, an African American man,
told us my father was the only coworker (both were skilled workers at
General Electric) who treated him fairly, didnt talk with him in a derogatory manner, and stopped others from making racial slurs and epithets. I learned for another time what it meant to be a feminist.
I dedicated the three-volume set on the Psychology of Women at Work
to my parents: For Antoinette and Michael Paludi, who encouraged
me to dene what womens work is for myself. They wanted all their
daughters to be independent thinkers and doers and to help others.
They gave us no templates to follow but encouraged us to navigate our
own paths. And, especially in my case, encouraged me to leave home
to attend graduate school in a city that seemed, to my parents, to be
very far awaybut they never said no.
My parents thus taught me that not only did they believe in the economic, educational, social, and political equality of women and men, but
they favored the social and legal changes necessary to achieve equality
between the sexes and among races, and they were committed to implementing these principles. Perhaps they could not effect change at the
national level, but they did do so in personal relationships with their
family and friends and on the local level. This is the legacy they left my
sisters and me. This book set is a tribute to Antoinette and Michael.
I have been reminded of Antoinette and Michael throughout the
writing and editing of these volumes on feminism and womens rights.

Introduction

xv

I am especially reminded of what my mother used to tell me: You are


there before you get there. She knew I wanted equality to happen fast
and that I grew concerned when feminists didnt win political elections, when younger women didnt know the heritage of how they
came to be accepted in graduate programs and in certain jobs, how the
glass ceiling for women and people of color is still strong, and that
worldwide, women constitute 64 percent of all adults who are illiterate
(see Susan Basows chapter in Volume 1). I have learned that she was
right; that change takes time, and to measure change differently, i.e., in
increments. As Secretary of State Hillary Clinton stated as she suspended her campaign for president of the United States in 2008:
Although we werent able to shatter this highest, hardest glass ceiling
this time, thanks to you, its got about 18 million cracks in it, and the
light is shining through like never before.
The chapters in these volumes show us where the light is shining
through on feminism. All three volumes represent what Judith Lorber
(1998) and Snelling (1999) identied: several types of feminism and
feminists. Lorber (1998) categorized feminism into three major areas:
gender reform, gender resistance, and gender rebellion. Genderreform feminism emphasizes similarities between women and men
rather than focusing on differences between them. Gender-resistance
feminism holds that formal legal rights alone will not end gender
inequality; male dominance is too ingrained into social relations.
Gender-resistance feminism focuses on how men and women are
differentcognitively, emotionally, and sociallyand urges women
to form women-centered organizations and communities. Genderrebellion feminism looks at the interrelationships among inequalities
of sex, race, ethnicity, social class, and sexual orientation. A number
of years ago my text on the psychology of women displayed a quilt
on its cover (Paludi, 2002). I asked for this design to highlight
Gentrys (1989) image of quilt making for understanding feminism.
These three volumes on feminism and womens rights also represent
quilt making in understanding feminism. Each contributor has made
one piece of the quilt that has been joined with pieces by other contributors. Each of the contributors has used different stitching on their
piece of the quilt. No one chapter is more important than the other.
We need all pieces if we are to complete the quilt that is feminism.
According to Gentry (1989):
Feminist psychology and feminism in general seem to be at the point of
trying to piece together the individual parts of a quilt. The overall pattern of the quilt that we want is still emerging. No one knows what
equality in a post-patriarchal world will look like. We are beginning to
piece the separate parts togetherto explore the kinds of stitching to use
in connecting the pieces and how to place the separate pieces into the

xvi

Introduction
pattern. But we have not stopped questioning the process of quilting
itself.

In Volume 1, Heritage, Roles, and Issues, contributors have discussed


efforts to integrate feminist scholarship into several disciplines, including education, work, science, military, religion, and politics. As Catherine Stimpson (1971) noted, there have been three kinds of problems in
the disciplines and curriculum with respect to women: omission, distortions, and trivializations. Each of the contributors to Volume 1 notes
where the sexism in the disciplines has existed and where feminist correctives have restructured the disciplines. Jennifer Martin, in her chapter concerning gender differences in abilities, noted:
Women have made signicant social, academic, and occupational gains
in the past 50 years; for example, women are entering nontraditional
elds with more frequency, participating in high school and college
sports more than ever before, and carving out more egalitarian roles for
themselves within the family. However, women have still not ultimately
achieved true equity with their male counterparts. . . . The idea that
women somehow possess different or inferior aptitudes when compared
to their male counterparts can lead to diminished expectations for
womenin terms of how they view themselves and how others view
them.

In Volume 2, Mental and Physical Health, contributors deal with violence and discrimination against girls and women and the resulting
impact on womens emotional and physical well being, interpersonal
relationships, career development, and self-concept. Types of discrimination and victimization addressed are sexual harassment, sexual violence, harassment of sexual minorities, and rape and violence in the
context of womens HIV risk. Contributors have addressed these issues
globally. Bethany Waits and Paula Lundberg-Love offer new cutting
edge evidence on neurological responses in women victims of sexual
violence. Therapeutic support for women victims of violence is also
addressed in this volume, including feminist therapy and ethnocultural
psychotherapy.
All contributors note that sexual victimization is prevalent in the
United States and globally, as is sexual harassment and sexual orientation discrimination. As Waits and Lundberg-Love note:
Female survivors of sexual violence are everywhere. They are in universities, religious institutions, court rooms, hospitals, and the military. They
are daughters, mothers, spouses, sisters, friends, next-door neighbors,
and co-workers. Many differ in age, education, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. . . . However, their lives are connected by the violence that
they have experienced.

Introduction

xvii

The international focus on feminism and womens rights is continued in Volume 3, Feminism as Human Rights. In this volume, contributors address laws on sexual harassment, pay equity, and rape.
Furthermore, contributors speak to the injustices to women with disabilities. Human rights issues such as arranged and forced marriage
for women, pornography, and the globalization of western appearance
ideals are also presented in this volume. All contributors to this volume call for further advocacy on behalf of women. As Noorfarah Merali stated:
It is only if arranged marriages are understood in light of their intentions, diverse forms, actual outcomes, and local or international contexts
that laws, policies, and human rights advocacy can be appropriately
channeled to protect and preserve womens well-being.

In addition to the scholarly reviews of research on feminism and


womens rights, I have included womens personal accounts of their
own feminist identity development. They are at different stages in life,
in their career, and in relationships and yet they are bound by shared
stories.
It is my hope that these volumes encourage individuals to self identify as feminists. Research has suggested for some time that most people reject the term feminist when describing themselves but support
feminist principlesequal pay for equal work, for example (see Paludi
et al., Volume 3). Goldners (1994) study noted that when women who
hold feminist beliefs anticipate a negative reaction from their peers to
the label feminist, they will avoid using the term to describe themselves. Goldner indicated that media is a primary source of negative
images of feminists. It is common to see photos of women identied as
feminists having clenched sts. These images are not representative of
feminists. More recent research by Rudman and Fairchild (2007) found
that the stereotype that feminists are unattractive still persists.
However, these images are rejected by individuals, especially during
adolescence and young adulthood, when maintaining gender role stereotypic behavior is reinforced and is central to their self-esteem and
self-concept. Paludi, Paludi, and DeFour (2004) noted that individuals
reject the label feminist because they view themselves as in control, as
powerful rather than as victims of gender inequality. Thus, they perceive the term feminist to imply a powerless position, which they
reject (Rhode, 1977).
The contributors to each of the three volumes of Feminism and Womens Rights Worldwide encourage us to think critically about feminism,
to value cultural experiences and to integrate our knowledge of theories and research about feminism with our own life experiences. The
chapters encouraged me to do this in remembering my own feminist

xviii

Introduction

socialization. I encourage you to do the same. It is my hope these three


volumes serve as a life raft (Klonis, Endo, Crosby, and Worell, 1997)
for feminists, especially those in the millennial generation.
REFERENCES
Gentry, M. (1989). Introduction: Feminist perspectives on gender and thought:
Paradox and potential. In M. Crawford & M. Gentry (Eds.), Gender and
thought. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Goldner, M. (1994). Accounting for race and class variation in the disjuncture
between feminist identity and feminist beliefs: The place of negative
labels and social movements. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of
the American Sociological Association, Los Angeles.
Klonis, S., Endo, J., Crosby, F., & Worell, J. (1997). Feminism as life raft. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 333345.
Lorber, J. (1998). Gender inequality: Feminist theories and politics. Los Angeles:
Roxbury.
Paludi, M. (2002). The psychology of women. 2nd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Paludi, M., ed. (2008). The psychology of women at work: Challenges and solutions
for our female workforce. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Paludi, M., Paludi, C., & DeFour, D. (2004). Introduction: The more things
change, the more they stay the same. In M. Paludi (Ed.), Praeger guide to
the psychology of gender. xixxxi. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Rhode, D. (1997). Speaking of sex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rudman, L., & Fairchild, K. (2007). The F word: Is feminism incompatible with
beauty and romance? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 125136.
Snelling, S. (1999). Womens perspectives on feminism. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 23, 247266.
Stimpson, C. (1971). Thy neighbors wife, thy neighbors servants: Womens liberation and black civil rights. In V. Gornick & B. Moran (Eds.), Woman in
sexist society: Studies in power and powerlessness. New York: Basic Books.
Zucker, A., & Stewart, A. (2007). Growing up and growing older: Feminism as
a context for womens lives. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 137145.

Chapter 1

International Perspectives on
Women and Mental Health
Joy Rice
Nancy Felipe Russo

Women have the right to the enjoyment of the highest attainable standard of physical and mental health. The enjoyment of this right is vital to
their life and well-being and their ability to participate in all areas of
public and private life. Health is a state of complete physical, mental and
social well-being and . . . is determined by the social, political and economic context of [womens] lives, as well as by biology. (Platform for
action: Fourth World Conference on Women, Beijing China, Chapter IV.
C.89, United Nations)

These words, contained in the national platform for action of the


United Nations Fourth World Conference on Women, provide a holistic vision of womens health, one in which physical and mental health
are inextricably intertwined and rooted in womens social, political,
and economic conditions. Understanding the links between womens
social roles and circumstances and negative mental health outcomes
thus becomes a key element in any global health agenda for women
(Koblinsky, Timyan, & Gay, 1992).
Although this holistic view of womens health has been resisted by the
biomedical establishment that dominates health care in the United States,
it is congruent with how health has been perceived globally. Indeed, for
more than three decades, the World Health Organization (WHO) has
emphasized a social model of health that has stressed the role of complex
reciprocal relationships among psychological, behavioral, social, and

Mental and Physical Health

economic factors in determining health and illness based on this holistic


denition:
Mental health is the capacity of the individual, the group, and the environment to interact with one another in ways that promote subjective
well-being, the optimal development and use of mental abilities (cognitive, affective, and relational), the achievement of the individual and collective goals consistent with justice and the attainment and preservation
of fundamental equality. (Cabral & Astbury, 2000, p. 12)

In sum, womens mental health is not simply the absence of disease,


and it is inseparable from a persons health well-being. A womans
mental health enables her to nd meaning in her life, function effectively in her social context, adapt to change, respond to crises, establish
rewarding relationships in her community, and modify her environments to meet her needs.
One outcome of the worldwide feminist movement stimulated by
the Beijing Conference was an International Consensus Statement on
Womens Mental Health that was passed by the World Psychiatric
Association (WPA) and signed by 140 WPA member associations, the
World Federation of Mental Health, and other mental health organizations, including the American Psychological Association (APA) and the
American Psychiatric Association. That statement emphasized that prioritizing womens mental health was essential for the achievement of
the Millennium Development Goals of the United Nations (UN, 2000;
http://www.un.org/millenniumgoals/), which include the achievement of universal primary education, promotion of gender equality,
reduction of child mortality, improving maternal health, and combating
human immunodeciency virus/acquired immune deciency syndrome (HIV/AIDS), among others. It also emphasized the importance
of considering the contexts of womens lives as determinants of their
mental health (Stewart, 2006).
A similar holistic biopsychosocial vision of mental health has been
promulgated in feminist psychologists in the United States and around
the world (Russo, in press; Sar & Hill, 2008; Wyche & Rice, 1997).
International issues and perspectives were important dimensions in the
feminist movement in psychology from its beginnings. Indeed, the
Association for Women in Psychology, established in the United States
in 1969 as the rst explicitly feminist organization in the eld, was designated an ofcial nongovernmental organization (NGO) for the UN in
1976 (for a more detailed history of international issues in feminist psychology, see Sar & Hill, 2008).
Today, feminist psychology in the United States is both informed by
and is a contributor to the international womens movement. The eld
has become explicitly multicultural, emphasizing the importance of

International Perspectives on Women and Mental Health

viewing mental health in its social/political context and examining


power inequities and inequalities that undermine mental health in the
lives of diverse women (Enns & Byars-Winston, 2009; Goodwin &
Fiske, 2001; Russo, in press).
Consequently, feminist therapy is now conceptualized as having a
multicultural biopsychosocial approach, one that encompasses meaningmaking and spiritual concerns, considers a woman in her social context, and has her empowerment as a therapeutic goal (Brown, 2008a;
Enns & Byars-Winston, 2009; McKay, Hill, Freedman, & Enright, 2007;
Worell & Johnson, 2001; Wyche & Rice, 1997). This perspective is
reected in the development of international practice guidelines for
counseling and therapy with girls and women described later.
WOMENS MENTAL HEALTH IN A CONTEXT
OF INEQUALITY AND OPPRESSION
The international womens movement has emphasized that around
the world, stigma, devaluation, and inequalities in power associated
with womens social roles and circumstances create conditions that
undermine womens mental health (Cabral & Astbury, 2000; Russo, in
press; Stewart et al., 2001). These conditions include povertywomen
are 70 percent of the poor around the world and earn signicantly less
than men (WHO, 2002). Related conditions include hunger, malnutrition, fatigue from overwork (with womens low-paid work often under
dangerous conditions), prejudice and discrimination, inadequate educational and economic resources, gender-based violence (including sexual
abuse, partner violence, and sexual trafcking), and social disruption
leading to displaced populations (including migration due to insufcient economic opportunity, social conict and war, and natural disaster) (Cabral & Astbury, 2000; Demyttenaere et al., 2004; Desjarlais et al.,
1995; WHO, 2000, 2002).
The mental health dimensions of sexual and reproductive health
have received increasing attention, partially because of the intersections
of unintended pregnancies, sexually transmitted infections (STIs;
including HIV), and gender-based violence, which are all known to
have a negative impact on mental health (Hamilton & Russo, 2006). In
addition, the high rates of pregnancy-associated depression among
women in developing countries compared to women in industrialized
countries (2040 percent vs. 1015 percent) are of concern (WHO,
2009). Mental health problems may be associated with lack of choice in
sexual and reproductive decisions, STIs, infertility, unintended and
unwanted pregnancy, unsafe abortion, miscarriage, and childbirth
(including premature birth), among other things (WHO, 2009; see
Chrisler, this volume, for more discussion of womens reproductive
rights).

Mental and Physical Health

The critical role of lack of power and having a disadvantaged social


status is manifested by the link between rates of depressive symptoms
with indicators of gender inequality that has been around the world
(Arrindell, Steptoe, & Wardle, 2003), and by regions (urban vs. rural)
within nations, and between states within the United States (Chen,
Subramanian, Acevedo-Garcia, & Kawachi, 2005). Womens higher
rates of depressive disorders compared to men, the consistency of this
gender gap across diverse groups and cross-nationally, and research
linking depression to hopelessness (a reection of powerlessness) and
low self-esteem (a reection of devaluation) has made theorizing the
relation of gender to depression of particular interest to feminist psychologists (Hamilton & Russo, 2006; Jackson & Williams, 2006). Consequently, we will consider womens depression in more detail below.
Women have unequal access to gender-sensitive basic health and
mental health services around the world (WHO, 2002). Furthermore,
service delivery may be affected by bias and stereotyping of providers
such that when services are available, they are inadequate or inappropriate. Given that the development and expression of mental disorder
differs for women and men, the need for gender-sensitive approaches
to diagnosis, treatment, and prevention is severe. Developing such
services will require understanding and investigating gender as a sociocultural construct (Russo & Tartaro, 2008).
GENDER IN INTERNATIONAL PERSPECTIVE
A shift in focus from the individuals characteristics and behaviors
to a gendered sociocultural view of mental health requires a more complex understanding of gender as a cultural construct. This new understanding is a necessary foundation for developing a coherent theory
of how inequality becomes translated into mental health disparities.
Gender can be conceptualized as a cultural package of many interconnected factors, including gendered emotions, identities, values, expectations, norms, roles, scripts, discourse, environments, and institutions.
These factors can inuence mental health and well-being separately, as
well as in combination (Russo, in press).
Gender denes what is considered normal and appropriate behavioral, psychological, and social characteristics for males and females and
shapes their personal and social identities. What is considered normal
varies over the life cycle, over time, and across cultures. Violations of
stereotypes and gender role expectations may lead to stigmatization,
marginalization, and discrimination, with implications for mental health
(Hamilton & Russo, 2006). When roles assigned by gender have lower
power and status, structured inequalities are created that can translate
into health disparitiesin mental health status as well as service delivery (Russo, in press; Russo & Landrine, 2009; Russo & Tartaro, 2008).

International Perspectives on Women and Mental Health

Gender operates at psychological, social, and situational levels. For


example, the relations of gender and race to mental health are
affected by perceived sexism and racism (Moradi & DeBlaere, 2009;
Moradi & Subich, 2003; Thomas, Witherspoon, & Speight, 2008); the
relationship of sexual orientation to mental health is affected by
stigma and victimization associated with homosexuality (Balsam,
Rothblum, & Beauchaine, 2005; Tjaden, Thoennes, & Allison, 1999;
Herek & Garnets, 2007).
Gender intersects with other social identities that may or may not be
stigmatized or associated with disadvantage, including identities based
on age, ethnicity, sexuality, physical disability, tribe, religion, nationality, immigrant status, occupational status, class, and caste status. There
is an urgent need for work that theorizes and investigates how complex and interacting dimensions of social difference deemed important
in a particular cultural context affect womens lived experiences in
ways that have implications for womens mental health (Brown, Riepe,
& Coffey, 2005; McCall, 2005; Russo & Vaz, 2001). More complexity in
theorizing will require overcoming epistemological, methodological,
and statistical challenges, however (Landrine & Corral, in press).
In particular, measurement equivalence issues will need to be
addressed, as the equivalence of measures may vary by gender and
culture, and measures that are equivalent across cultures for men may
not be equivalent for women (Chen & West, 2007). Such issues pose
construct validity problems in measuring sexist beliefs and attitudes
beliefs that maintain or foster gender inequalitiescross-nationally (for
a review of the literatures and measurement issues related to the
endorsement, expression, and emergence of sexism cross-nationally,
see Swim, Becker, & Lee, 2009).
In summary, advancing understanding of genders relation to mental health internationally will require investigating a complex interplay
among biological, psychological, social, cultural, and contextual factors,
including multiple personal and social identities, social locations and
conditions, and coping strategies and resources (Russo, in press; Russo
& Tartaro, 2008; Szymanski & Kashubeck-West, 2008). In considering
gender-related factors that can affect diverse womens mental health
internationally, we must also keep in mind that women will vary in
their response to such factors, and that variance in womens responses
will both reect their position in the social structure, as well as how
they integrate or engage their multiple social identities, some of which
may be specic to a particular context or culture.
WOMENS MENTAL DISORDER: THE GLOBAL BURDEN
The priority of mental health issues globally has risen as the enormous impact of mental disorders on what has been conceptualized as

Mental and Physical Health

the global burden of disease. A recent WHO (2008) report provides


a comprehensive picture of the global and regional state of the physical
and mental health of the worlds peoples. Based on extensive country
governmental data from 112 member states, it provides projections of
deaths and the global burden of disease to the year 2030 and is an
update of WHO research rst conducted in 1990. The study nds that
mental disorders are among the leading causes of disability in all
regions of the word. They account for approximately one-third of years
lost due to disability among people older than fourteen. Four out of 10
diseases with the highest burden are psychiatric disorders (Kastrup,
2007).
Violence and self-inicted injury, which are also among the leading
contributors to the disease burden, have profound implications for
womens mental health as well (Cabral & Astbury, 2000). Analyses of
data gathered from 15 sites in 10 countries participating in the WHO
multi-country study on womens health and domestic violence against
women found a strong link between gender-based violence and negative health and mental health outcomes, including higher levels of
emotional distress, risk of suicide ideation, and suicide attempts found
across all sites (Ellsberg et al., 2008).
Although males and females are generally similar in overall rates of
mental disorder around the world, the patterns and symptoms of mental disorders differ for men and men over the life cycle (Kessler, 2006;
Kessler, Chiu, Demler, & Walters, 2005; Piccinelli & Homen, 1997; Silverman & Carter, 2006). For example, in the United States, based on
the National Co-morbidity Survey, Kessler et al. (2005) identied gender differences in six classes or patterns of disorder. Here presented by
level of severity, they included the following: (1) unaffected respondents (more likely to be male); (2) pure internalizing disorders (more
likely to be female); (3) pure externalizing disorders (more likely to be
male); (4) comorbid internalizing disorders (more likely to be female);
(5) comorbid internalizing and/or externalizing disorders dominated
by comorbid social phobia and attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder
(more likely to be male); and (6) highly comorbid major depressive episodes (more likely to be female). No gender difference was found in a
seventh class, highly comorbid bipolar disorder. High comorbidity was
associated with severityclasses with the highest comorbidity (4, 6, 7)
included about 7 percent of the sample, but represented 43.6 percent of
the serious cases.
Comorbidity of mental disorders signals greater severity of illness
and disability and higher utilization of services. Women have higher
rates of lifetime and 12-month comorbidity of three or more disorders
(WHO, 2002). It has long been known that anxiety is comorbid with
depression, particularly for women (Breslau et al., 1995), and that
women have higher rates of mood and anxiety disorders (Piccinelli &

International Perspectives on Women and Mental Health

Homen, 1997; Silverman & Carter, 2006). Symptoms of anxiety disorders are correlated with other disorders, complicating diagnosis. In
particular, research is needed that claries the origins and relationships
among symptoms of anxiety and depressive disorders, which constitute
the largest contributor to the gender gap in internalizing disorders.
Countrywide studies may mask within-country variations in rates of
mental disorder. Women are more likely to be poor, and surveys in
Brazil, Chile, India, and Zimbabwe have found that rates of common
mental disorders (anxiety and depression) are higher among the poor
(Patel, Araya, Ludermir, & Todd, 1999). Furthermore, in poorer areas,
treatment may be more likely to be inadequate or nonexistent. In the
United States, rates and predictors of mental disorder vary substantially within subpopulations. For example, a national survey of Latinos
and Asian Americans (Alegria et al., 2007), revealed that among the
four Latina subethnic groups studied, Mexican heritage women were
less likely than Puerto Rican women to have a depressive disorder,
and Puerto Rican women had the highest overall lifetime and past-year
prevalence rates compared to other women.
It is important to go beyond a focus on rates and learn more about
how elements of gender affect the development, course, and context of
mental disorders among women internationally. In particular, more
needs to be known about how gender affects comobidity of mental disorder over the life cycle. For example, depression and anxiety are more
likely to be found together for women, whereas depression and substance abuse are more likely to be paired for men. The extent to which
this difference reects a gender difference in pathways to depression
versus diagnostic bias requires investigation.
Research on the patterns of being depressed found in womens daily
experiences suggests that women may be more likely to experience
short-term depressive episodes than men, possibly reecting their dayto-day experience with life stressors (Kessler, 2006).
WOMEN AND DEPRESSION: A GLOBAL PERSPECTIVE
Depression is identied in the WHO report as the leading global
cause of years of health lost to disease for both men and women, with
unipolar depression as the eighth leading cause of loss of health in
low-income countries and the primary cause of loss of health in middleand high-income countries. Depression affects around 120 million
people worldwide, and the number is projected to increase. Fewer
than 25 percent of those affected have access to adequate treatment
and health care.
For purposes of this discussion, the outstanding fact is that compared with men, the worldwide rate of depression in women globally
is 50 percent higher, and gender is perceived to be the critical

Mental and Physical Health

determinant and strongest correlate of risk for different categorized


types of depression (WHO, 2001). The biomedical evidence across
nations, cultures, and ethnicities widely documents that women are
one to three times more likely than men to develop depression and
anxiety disorders (Ustun, 2000), but there is great variation in the estimated total population prevalence across studies (Kessler, 2006).
Although there is marked variation in the rates of depression for
women in different countries, much higher rates have been found in
women attending primary health care centers in developing countries.
In Indian clinics, for example, it is estimated that between 25 to 33 percent of women patients are suffering from depression (Worley, 2006).
Despite three decades of research on gender identities and a wide
range of identied risk factors, no particular cause or interrelated set of
causes can fully explain the signicant global phenomenon of gender
differences in depression (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2001). Speculation continues in the literature about biological correlates for increased incidence
of depression for women. Major depression clusters in families and
depression in a rst-degree relative is a risk factor for depression.
While some studies nd similar levels of heritability of depression for
women and men, several others have found higher genetic loadings for
females, suggesting that the impact of some genes for risk for major
depression differs in women and men (Kendler, Gardner, Neale, &
Prescott, 2001).
The unique biology of women may in part explain their greater propensity to depression beginning in adolescence and early puberty, and
sex hormones are likely to play a role (Ge, Conger, & Elder, 1996;
Zahn-Waxler, Race, & Duggal, 2004). Depression associated with postpartum and menopausal periods is being studied in relation to hormonal factors and interactions between hormones, neurotransmitters, and
other biological systems (Mazure, Kieta, & Blehar, 2002). Women are
also more likely than men to be prescribed mood-altering psychotropic
medication and electro-convulsive therapy for depression, even where
the evidence suggests that the main conditions surrounding their diagnosis have strong social origins (Buseld, 1996).
Several psychological and environmental risk factors and social
causes of depression for women have been identied (Bertram, 2003;
Paltiel, 1993; Zahn-Waxler et al., 2004). They include the inequitable
gendered division of labor and family responsibility, womens lesser
social status and gender socialization, and the effects of poverty, abuse,
and violence. Hamilton and Russo (2006) also review research that
links unwanted pregnancy, sexualized objectication, and stigma as
contributors to the gender gap in depression. Add to these etiological
factors the astounding fact that women and children represent an estimated 80 percent of 50 million people affected by violent conicts, civil
wards, disasters, and displacement (WHO, 2006).

International Perspectives on Women and Mental Health

Paltiel concludes that the key depression risk factors for women
globally are simply that everywhere women are overworked, overlooked and undervalued, and that poverty, discrimination, violence
and powerlessness are pervasive features of womens lives (p. 197). In
many developed countries, women are often poorly paid for dangerous, labor-intensive jobs, and are undernourished as well (Lopez &
Guarnaccia, 2005). The so-called feminization of poverty is also a
worldwide phenomenon as our family structures and models change,
with an increasingly preponderance of single-parent mother families
worldwide (Rice, 2001). Clearly, it is essential to recognize how genetic,
biological, social, and psychological factors all contribute to the high
incidence of depression women worldwide, and guidelines for treatment need to be based on a biopsychosocial model of assessment,
research, practice, and policy.
Screening and access to treatment for depression is also a very signicant concern. Even in a developed, wealthy country like the United
States, only 24 percent of women who suffer from depression receive
treatment, with even lower rates for African American women (16 percent) and Hispanic women (20 percent) (U.S. Department of Health
and Human Services, 2000). Health care providers in developing countries identify less than one-half of women with depression in those
countries (WHO, 2002). A number of overwhelming challenges occur
in countries with low-resource settings including the lack of facilities,
trained mental health personnel, effective population-based screening,
and the prevalence of high cultural stigma (Worley, 2006). Communication between health workers and women patients can be extremely
authoritarian in many countries where women are still primarily
viewed as inferior with low social and economic status and often stigmatized for showing negative or depressed emotion. Furthermore,
when women dare to reveal mental health concerns, health workers
may reect these stereotyped gender biases, which leads them to either
overtreat or undertreat women (WHO, 1996).

PSYCHOLOGICAL PRACTICE GUIDELINES


FOR WOMEN AND GIRLS
International Implications and Issues
As noted in the previous section, across the world women are challenged by signicant mental health risks. These risks are associated
with a multitude of pernicious outcomes and implications, including
depression and suicide risk, anxiety disorders, and reproductive health
issues, as well as physiological and psychosomatic problems. In light
of these facts, psychotherapy and other forms of psychological intervention such as early screening and risk assessment are very important

10

Mental and Physical Health

treatment strategies for prevention and treatment of womens mental


health problems. The development of guidelines for psychological
practice for women and girls that addresses their special mental health
needs and issues and is founded on a feminist perspective of gender
equity and cultural sensitivity is an area of burgeoning concern not
only in the Western world, but globally (Ballou, Hill, & West, 2008;
Ballou & West, 2000; Comas-Diaz & Greene, 1994: Enns, 2008, 2009;
Hays, 2001).
This section will address three areas related to guidelines for practice. First, some background will be presented on the APAs Resolution
on Cultural and Gender Implications in International Psychology. This
resolution passed in 2004 (Rice & Ballou, 2002) formed the theoretical
foundation for the actual gender practice guidelines adopted in 2006
(APA, 2007). Then, the key elements in both the American and Canadian Psychological Association guidelines (CPA, 2007) will be examined with a view to how they could apply to more international and
diverse cultural perspectives cross-nationally. Finally, a summary of
some key issues that seem important in the development of feminist
guidelines for psychological practice with women and girls in any
country of the world will be presented.
Cultural and Gender Awareness in the Practice of
International Psychology
The second author of this chapter became more actively involved in
international psychology leadership by founding and serving as the
rst chair of the International Committee for Women, a very active
standing committee of the APA Division of International Psychology.
One of the rst successful projects launched by the committee took several years to complete and involved the participation of women from
many countries and organizations in drafting and passing an APA
Resolution on Cultural and Gender Awareness in International Psychology. This activity embraced the collaboration and help from many
women psychologists from other countries who were interested in feminist issues from an international perspective, and it also led to advocacy work about these issues in the association.
The goal of the resolution was to encourage and facilitate awareness
and reective consideration for psychology and psychologists engaged
in international projects, research, teaching, and practice. Underpinning
the core concepts of the resolution was the understanding that feminist,
multicultural, and critical theory, among other postmodern perspectives, had raised fundamental concerns about the values and assumptions long held in the dominant paradigm of conventional psychology
(Fox & Prilleltensky, 1997; Kitzinger, 1991; Martin-Baro, 1994; Unger,
1995). The mutual and collaborative model called for in the resolution
is important to world psychology because it offers guidance in

International Perspectives on Women and Mental Health

11

postmodern perspectives in theory, research, and practice and strong


models of psychological practice grounded in social justice. It also
assumes that psychology based on Western values could benet signicantly from the expansion of its knowledge base through an international lens that includes diverse multicultural and cross-national
perspectives. The resolution encourages psychologists to commit to ve
principles that help us understand and overcome oppressive attitudes
and practices in dominant psychology transported internationally:
1. Understanding the experiences of individuals in diverse cultures and contexts.
This rst principle is grounded in the others experience. It urges us to
understanding the experience of the other, which is embedded in multiple contexts and diverse social structures. Understanding and sincerely
appreciating the experience of the other person also implies that we validate the others worldview, their ways of knowing and their authority
for valid information and meaning (Ballou; 1996; Flowers & Richardson,
1996). One example for psychological practice with women, among the
many, might be in the realm of values. In North America, productivity
and autonomy are held as strong virtues, and many of our theoretical
constructs and normative standards are based on the value of independent thinking and action. The principle of valuing others experiences is
both obvious and subtle in its many applications. At its most basic application, how we dene the psychological problem, diagnose the problem,
and select the intervention must be based squarely in the experience of
the other.
2. Respect for pluralism based on differences. The second and closely related
principle is respect for pluralism. Respect for pluralism takes one beyond
recognizing diversity to valuing diversity. As one example, feminist
research on women in other disciplines and countries often employs ethnographic and narrative methodology as a legitimate and important way
to document and validate the actual voices and direct experiences of
women and minorities (Alcoff & Potter, 1993; Lazarus and Lykes, 2005;
Naidoo, 2005). However, in psychology these efforts may still not be seen
as valid or meeting the scientic test of traditional empirical investigation
of best practices (Riger, 1992, 2000).
3. Awareness and analysis of power. This principle points out how critical it is
to understand and become aware of power differentials and to analyze
power asymmetries and hierarchies of power as they operate in relationships, institutions, and systems (Ballou & West, 2000; Enriquez, 1992). As
an example in psychotherapy, interpersonal relationships between therapist and client are guided by this principle of reducing power asymmetries between practitioner and client. While each has different levels of
power and status, both members are involved in a reciprocal learning
process in which each can make valuable contributions to the other. This
is particularly true when the each person in the therapeutic dyad comes
from a different cultural perspective (Wyche & Rice, 1997).
4. Critical analysis of Western perspectives. The normative values and denitions employed in traditional Western psychology have been critiqued

12

Mental and Physical Health


extensively. These analyses are revealing of the cultural, historical, economic, and political agendas and perspectives embedded within the
theory and practice of psychotherapy, particularly in relation to the treatment of women and minorities (Brown, 1994; Fox & Prilleltensky, 1997;
Kitzinger, 1991; Rosenblum & Travis, 1996; Radway, 1998).
5. Interdisciplinary socialcultural perspective. This principle calls for an
awareness of the signicant impact of external and structural forces on
individuals. Such factors, for example, as poverty, violence, and war, and
the effects of the law, church, family, education, and the workplace,
result in multiple and complex forms of privilege and/or oppression
(Rice, 2001, 2007). A feminist perspective calls for an analysis of social
location and an interdisciplinary view that recognizes how anthropological, historical, and religious factors inuence and interact with ones ethnicity, class, gender, and culture (Landrine, 1995). In psychological
practice, the psychologist sometimes encounters contradictions of valuing
culture and actual cultural practices, for sometimes the cultural practices
are oppressive to women as in inequitable marital, divorce, and reproductive rights, violence, abuse, and economic dominance (Rice, 2005).

APA and CPA Guidelines for Psychological


Practice with Women and Girls
The previous ve principles formed a foundation for the succeeding
work of 35 feminist psychologists who over several years of collaboration drafted guidelines for psychological practice with girls and
women. The chairs of the task force, Roberta Nutt, Joy Rice, and Carolyn
Enns, took the guidelines through the long and extensive process of
revisions and updates required by the various APA governance bodies,
council, and membership. Adopted in 2006, these 11 guidelines were
based on the need to articulate a model for psychotherapeutic practice
that was both gender- and culturally sensitive and that employed concepts of empowerment and an understanding and appreciation of the
standpoints, world views and culture specic practices of women and
men as well (APA, 2007). The 11 guidelines are organized into three
sections: (1) diversity, social context, and power; (2) professional
responsibility; and (3) practice applications.

DIVERSITY, SOCIAL CONTEXT, AND POWER


Guideline 1: Psychologists strive to be aware of the effects of socialization,
stereotyping, and unique life events on the development of girls and
women across diverse cultural groups.
Guideline 2: Psychologists are encouraged to recognize and utilize information about oppression, privilege, and identity development as they may
affect girls and women.

International Perspectives on Women and Mental Health

13

Guideline 3: Psychologists strive to understand the impact of bias and discrimination upon the physical and mental health of those with whom they
work.

Professional Responsibility
Guideline 4: Psychologists strive to use gender and culturally sensitive,
afrming practices in providing services to girls and women.
Guideline 5: Psychologists are encouraged to recognize how their socialization, attitudes, and knowledge about gender may affect their practice with
girls and women.

Practice Applications
Guideline 6: Psychologists are encouraged to employ interventions and
approaches that have been found to be effective in the treatment of issues
of concern to girls and women.
Guideline 7: Psychologists strive to foster therapeutic relationships and
practices that promote initiative, empowerment, and expanded alternatives
and choices for girls and women.
Guideline 8: Psychologists strive to provide appropriate, unbiased assessments and diagnoses in their work with women and girls
Guideline 9: Psychologists strive to consider the problems of girls and
women in their sociopolitical context.
Guideline 10: Psychologists strive to acquaint themselves with and utilize relevant mental health, education, and community resources for girls and women.
Guideline 11: Psychologists are encouraged to understand and work to
change institutional and systemic bias that may impact girls and women.

Almost concurrently, in 2007, the CPA passed Guidelines for Ethical Psychological Practice with Women that articulates four guiding
principles:
1. Respect for the dignity of persons. This principle urges that psychologist
ensure that they do not engage in or support any gender-based discrimination and/or oppression, recognizing that there may be situations
where women clients face multiple discriminations and oppressions.
2. Responsible caring. The main point of this principle is that psychologists
strive to understand how womens lives are shaped by the interaction of
gender with other modalities like culture, ethnicity, and sexual orientation, and that is important for the practitioner to understand how the
multiple social contexts of their own life might inuence or interfere with
their attempts to help and not harm women clients.
3. Integrity in relationships. Psychologists are open, honest, and accurate in
their communications and recognize, monitor, and manage potential
biases, multiples relationships, or other conicts of interest that could

14

Mental and Physical Health


lead to the exploitation of the client and the diminishment of trust. Psychologists honestly acknowledge differences in beliefs and values with
their women clients and work collaboratively to resolve those differences
in the best interest of the woman.
4. Responsibility to society. Psychologists acknowledge that they have responsibilities to the societies in which they live and work and their concern
for the welfare of all human beings includes concern for the welfare of
women in society. They accept responsibility to do what they can to
change societal laws and structures that discriminate or lead to oppressions of women.

The underlying concepts and principles in the APA and CPA guidelines are similar, but the CPA principles are more general in their articulation and do not, for example, discuss specic practices of promoting
empowerment and expanded alternatives for women; using unbiased
assessments, diagnoses, and materials; and employing specic interventions that have been found to be helpful and effective with women
clients. Their thrust is closer to the underlying ethical principles for the
APA Resolution on Gender and Cultural Awareness in International
Psychology. The CPA guidelines, unlike those of the APA, do not provide a developmental perspective applying to younger girls.

FEMINIST PSYCHOTHERAPY IN A GLOBAL CONTEXT


Upon the completion of these guidelines, it was quickly realized that
there was a need to address and consider the potential implications,
applications, and modications of such feminist practice guidelines for
use internationally and cross-nationally. As a starting point, Enns and
her colleagues began a working group at the 2008 International Counseling Psychology Conference in Chicago, and their discussion provides some potential implications and modications of the APA
guidelines for use in international contexts. The major aim of the task
force was to generate a critical analysis of Western feminist psychotherapeutic practice and to consider some of the features of such practice
globally (Enns, 2008). APA Guideline 5 and CPA Principle 2 which
state how understanding how ones own socialization shapes ones
therapeutic perspectives and practices formed a basis for the dialogue.
Certainly, developing practice guidelines that would be applicable
internationally is a daunting endeavor. There are enormous challenges
to developing a holistic and inclusive focus to womens diverse realities across the world and their complex multiple interacting social
identities and oppressions. Nonetheless, an important beginning has
been made in articulating some of the ways in which Western models
of psychotherapy, including feminist models of treatment for women,
are ethnocentric or otherwise narrow in their geo-political-social focus.

International Perspectives on Women and Mental Health

15

Three important themes can be seen to have emerged from the work
of this beginning task force. The rst concerns our concepts of empowerment and the language of empowerment. Such concepts are often
framed in individualist terms from a Western point of view. By way of
contrast, for example, many Japanese women, both feminist and nonfeminists, dene meaningful constructs of interdependence and fulllment that are consistent with the values of a more collective society. It is
suggested that terms such as resourcefulness are likely to be less ethnocentric and more useful and meaningful in the treatment of mental
health problems for women in other cross-national contexts (Enns, 2008).
Second, the goals and strategies of psychotherapy for women need
to be framed in culturally sensitive terms. For example, although we as
Western therapists and feminists tend to see and promote gender role
differentiation as negative and as a barrier to achieving equity and a
positive sense of self, many Muslim and Asian women have worked
toward preserving and honoring difference, especially in the realm of
family and personal relations (Enns, 2008; Pharaon, 2001).
Another guiding theme identied in modifying the Western guidelines for psychological practice for women and girls was the consideration that Western society is extremely goal directed. This is also
reected in the way in which we practice psychotherapy with the goal
of being assertive or achieving a certain job or status. For women from
other cultures, the goal orientation of such a therapeutic approach may
not resonate; for example, a Japanese woman whose personality values
a role-oriented approach to life and who experiences a sense of satisfaction from fullling and honoring that lifetime role, such as nurturing family and children to the subordination of self (Enns, 2003). Thus,
the alleviation of depression associated with that role may be not to
attempt to separate the women from the role or to divorce or to separate, but to help her feel and integrate the honor her culture assigns to
that role.
Several of the Western guidelines are broad enough to apply to
many various contexts and to diverse mental health problems of
women across the world, but they would need culture-specic language, applications, interventions, and examples. In terms of language
and translation of the guidelines, the particular meanings of words and
connotations of concepts embedded in individualistic perspectives may
vary from country to country and culture to culture, necessitating consideration of cultural relevance (Enns, 2008). Every therapeutic encounter is embedded in a multi-lingual context, and both therapist and
client must be aware of the many leveled effects. There are many
opportunities for misunderstandings, as well as for mutual shared
learning (Espin, 2001).
Nonetheless, the overall relevance and importance of concepts like
empowerment, awareness of difference, sensitivity to and avoidance of

16

Mental and Physical Health

discrimination, best practices, self-examination, and education can be


seen as applicable to all women clients in various geographical settings.
By way of example, applying the guidelines to women from other
countries reveals both direct relevance and the need for cultural modication (Enns & Kasai, 2007). A few examples will sufce.
APA Guidelines 1, 2, and 3 speak directly to the direct effects of
socialization, oppression, and bias on women and girls, outlining decades
of research on these issues. Japanese women, like women in many
other cultures around the world, live in worlds of highly differentiated
gender roles; however, this is seen in a more positive light where difference is preserved and honored and the counseling reects that different perspective (Kawano, 1990). As Ueno (1997) writes, Our
primary goal is not to be like men, but to value what it means to be a
woman. Some Asian women believe that gender role differentiation
does not automatically produce greater subordination and dependency
for women on men, but in contrast independence; for example, when
women do not expect men to meet their emotional needs and turn to
other women for nurturing relationships. Amae is a widespread indigenous psychological concept in Japan. It represents a healthy othercenteredness that emphasizes attunement to the needs of others and
correspondingly positive reliance on others for emotional acceptance
and self-esteem. However, Japanese women are often expected to
shoulder the responsibility for giving amae, with limited opportunities
to receive amae (Enns & Kasai, 2001; Matsuyuki, 1998).
APA Guideline 6 states that psychologists are encouraged to use
interventions and practices that have been found to be most effective
in the psychological treatment of girls and women. The effectiveness of
particular interventions may directly vary as a function of the sociopolitical-economic context of the particular woman client. Ciftci (2008)
and Winter (2001) have worked extensively with immigrant ArabMuslim women. Women who are dislocated from their native support
systems are at special risk for depression, but our Western mental
health systems and structures that emphasize individual therapy and
psychopharmacological interventions do not necessarily meet their special needs. Speedy symptom alleviation, family therapy, cultural education, and social advocacy have been found to be effective avenues for
intervention and change, as well as working with local community
resources and support systems. Cognitive enhancement group therapy
programs have also been found to be particularly efcacious in helping
Saudi Arabian women to improve self-condence, communication
skills, and self-awareness (Pharaon, 2001).
APA Guideline 9 and CPA Principle 2 state the importance of considering the psychological problems of women and girls in their sociopolitical and socioeconomic context. The effects of socioeconomic status
have an enormous impact on the clinical issues which Mexican women

International Perspectives on Women and Mental Health

17

face (Hinkelman, 2001). Mexican women often suffer stresses that are
due to inadequate food and shelter, domestic violence, unemployment,
and oppressive political policies and structures. Religion plays a large
part in their family life, and some life events are attributed to luck,
supernatural forces, or acts of God, a fatalism that has been linked to a
high prevalence of depression and other clinical issues such as anxiety
and psychosomatic symptoms. Furthermore, Mexican women, especially in rural areas, tend to have limited information and access to
medical and mental health care resources and institutions and centers
specializing in assisting victims of violence and abuse (Pick, Contreras,
& Barker-Aguilar, 2006). All these considerations of the social context
of the depressed Mexican woman need to be considered in evaluation
of the intrapsychic and external sources of her distress and the appropriate interventions.
Finally, APA Guideline 11 and CPA Principle 4 speak to the need for
psychologists everywhere to help better their societies by engaging and
advocating for positive social change that alleviates institutional and systemic injustice and discrimination. The question that is relevant here is
whether or not there can be healing without justice. The abuse of
women worldwide and their resulting trauma makes explicit the link
between treatment and advocacy and calls forth a model of global practice for women and girls that incorporates advocacy. If justice is indeed
therapeutic, then psychologists are urged to go beyond their relatively
comfortable roles and ofce work to publicly work for their women clients in nontherapeutic settings and venues. From this perspective, we
help to forge a world in which women can live, work, and be healed
from the pernicious effects of discrimination, abuse, and violence and
the multiple mental health consequences of those conditions.
BEYOND CURRENT MODELS OF RESEARCH AND PRACTICE
As Aida Hurtado (2009) has articulated, feminist theorists have challenged traditional forms of knowledge production, staked out claims in
knowledge production, and emphasized the roots of multicultural feminist theory in the every day experiences of human beings who love,
live, laugh, cry, and think. Part of that challenge has been the development of new methodologies, qualitative and quantitative, to produce
new knowledge about womens lives and circumstances. In particular,
the development of participatory and action research techniques hold
promise for the development of an action-reection dialectic or praxis
to create an activist scholarship in international psychology (Earth,
1998; Khanna, 1996; Lykes, 1994, 2001; Lykes, Coquillon, & Rabenstein,
in press).
Stigma, cultural beliefs, and cultural norms with regard to expressing
psychological distress and help-seeking, diagnostic practices, treatment

18

Mental and Physical Health

accessibility, and preference for alternative forms of treatment vary


cross-nationally. In countries such as Uganda, where mental illness may
be seen as a punishment for bad deeds or as possession by an evil spirit,
the associated stigma may lead to social isolation, exclusion, and disadvantage (Ssebunnya, Kigozi, Lund, Kizza, & Okello, 2009). Barriers to
help-seeking may also be found in countries where the cultural taboo of
consulting in a psychiatric setting may carry the risk of marriage ineligibility or divorce. These barriers may be particularly important to women
in societies where the roles of wife and mother are central to womens
status. For example, a study of outpatients in the United Arab Emirates
(Ouali et al., 2004) found that women were less likely to seek mental
health care than men, and that when they did seek care, 70 percent were
accompanied by someone else; 60 percent of the women said they could
not have sought care if they had not been accompanied, and this
dependence on being accompanied led to irregular attendance in followup appointments. They were also more likely to report feeling stigmatized than men (37.7 percent vs. 24.4 percent). In Arab Islamic societies,
the fact that women represent a familys honor may make them reluctant
to disclose personal issues to outsiders for fear of damaging the familys
status in addition to their own. Such conditions underscore the importance of understanding the relationship of social and cultural factors to
diagnosis, treatment, and delivery of mental health services to women
and of developing culturally appropriate approaches to service provision.
While U.S. studies have much to contribute to international mental
health efforts, the reverse is true as well. The lack of a psychiatric
establishment tied to biomedical models and an infrastructure that
inuences mental health practices from the top down may provide
opportunities for bottom up approaches reective of the voices of
consumers and tailored to their contexts. A model for such an
approach is found in Where There Is No Psychiatrist: A mental health care
manual (Patel, 2003), which states the promotion of gender equality,
by empowering women to make decisions that inuence their lives
and educating men about the need for equal rights, is the most important way of promoting womens mental health (p. 220). The manual
provides practical, context-based advice for community workers and
primary care doctors, nurses, social workers, and doctors, particularly
in developing countries. For example, it provides guidance on how to
ask about stress in a domestic context, ensure regular follow-up, ask
permission to speak to family members, and deal with advocacy issues
such as establishing psychoeducational or support groups for women
in the community.
In cultures where Western diagnostic constructs are unknown and
there are no words for mental disorders, the manuals bottom up
approach, which focuses on symptoms and avoids usage of stigmatizing labels, is particularly appropriate. The idea that it is important to

International Perspectives on Women and Mental Health

19

recognize that people seek help from diverse sources and there is little
to gain by challenging beliefs in evil spirits and witchcraft is likely to
meet with substantial resistance from a Western biomedical perspective. However, the point is made that counseling approaches based
on Western psychological theories may indeed be applicable across
culturesbut to be effective, a counselor must nd what will be acceptable. A similar bottoms up effort, informed by feminist principles and guidelines for therapy with women, would provide an
interesting approach for giving multicultural feminist psychology
away to community workers and service providers who seek alternatives to traditional approaches in the United States.
CONCLUSION
International perspectives on mental health offer a holistic vision of
health that is congruent with the biopsychosocial perspective advocated by feminist psychologists. This perspective views understanding
the relation of womens social roles and circumstances to mental health
in its social/political and cultural context as necessary for the development of effective treatment and prevention. That gender has a profound impact on the development of and response to mental distress
and disorder is indisputable. The goal now is to understand the factors
and mechanisms that produce that impact, including the power
inequalities, stigma, and devaluation associated with womens social
roles and circumstances. New theories and methods, informed by multicultural and international feminist perspectives, as reected in the
guidelines for psychological practice with women and girls, hold
promise as tools for achieving that goal. However, doing so will
require viewing gender as a multidimensional cultural construct with
elements that may interact with elements of the cultural context at multiple levelsbiological, psychological, social, environmental, cultural,
and contextualand developing policies and programs aimed at eliminating the power inequities, stigma, discrimination, and gender-based
violence that continue to undermine the mental health and well being
of women over their life cycle.
REFERENCES
Alcoff, L., & Potter, E. (Eds.). (1993). Feminist epistemologies. New York:
Routledge.
Alegria, A., Mulvaney-Day, N., Torres, M., Polo, A., Cao, Z., & Canino, G.
(2007). Prevalence of psychiatric disorders across Latino subgroups in the
United States. American Journal of Public Health, 97, 6885
American Psychological Association. (2007). Guidelines for psychological practice with girls and women. American Psychologist, 62, 949979.

20

Mental and Physical Health

Ballou, M. (1996). Multicultural theory and women. In D. W. Sue, A. Ivey, &


P. Pederson (Eds.), A theory of multicultural counseling and therapy. Pacic
Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Ballou, M., Hill, M., & West, C. (Eds.). (2008). Feminist therapy theory and practice: A contemporary perspective. New York: Springer.
Ballou, M., & West, C. (2000). Feminist therapy: Turning in turning out. In
M. Biaggio & M. Hersen (Eds.), Issues in the psychology of women. New York:
Plenum Press.
Bertram, M. (2003). Women and mental health: A brief global analysis. International Journal of Language, Society & Culture, 12, 18.
Brown, L. (1994). Subversive dialogues. New York: Basic Books.
Brown, L. S., Riepe, L. E., & Coffey, R. L. (2005). Beyond color and culture: Feminist contributions to paradigms of human difference. Women & Therapy,
28, 6392.
Buseld, J. (1996). Men, women and madness: Understanding gender and mental
Disorder. London: Macmillan Press.
Cabral, M., & Astbury, J. (2000). Womens mental health: An evidence-based review.
Geneva, Switzerland: Department of Mental Health and Substance
Dependence, World Health Organization.
Canadian Psychological Association. (2007). Guidelines for ethical psychological
practice with women. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Psychological Association.
Chen, F. F., & West, S. G. (2007). Measuring individualism and collectivism:
The importance of considering differential components, reference groups,
and measurement invariance. Journal of Research in Personality, 43,
259294.
Chen, Y., Subramanian, S., Acevedo-Garcio, D., & Kawachi, I. (2005). Womens
status and depressive symptoms: A multilevel analysis. Social Science and
Medicine, 60, 4960.
Ciftci, A. (2008). Clinical implications of working with Muslim American women.
Presentation, August 2008, Annual Meeting of the American Psychological
Association. Boston: American Psychological Association..
Comas-Diaz, L., & Greene, B. (Eds.). (1994). Women of color: Integrating ethnic
and gender identities in psychotherapy. New York: Guilford.
Earth, B. (1998). Participatory research: Gender and health in rural Tanzania.
Convergence, 31, 5967.
Ellsberg, M., Jansen, H., Heise, L., Watts, C., & Garcia-Moreno, C. (2008). Intimate partner violence and womens physical and mental health in the
WHO multi-country study on womens health and domestic violence: an
observational study. Lancet, 371, 11651172.
Enns, C. Z. (2003). Hakoniwa: Japanese sandplay therapy. The Counseling
Psychologist, 31, 103112.
Enns, C. Z. (2008). Feminist psychotherapy in a global context. Presentation,
August 2008, Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association.
Boston: American Psychological Association.
Enns, C. A., & Byars-Winston, A. M. (in press). Multicultural feminist therapy.
In H. Landrine & N. F. Russo (Eds.), Handbook of diversity in feminist psychology. New York: Springer.
Enns, C. Z., & Kasai, M. (2001). Feminist psychological practice in Japan. San
Francisco: American Psychological Association.

International Perspectives on Women and Mental Health

21

Enriquez, V. G. (1992). From colonial to liberation psychology: The Philippine Experience. Quezon: University of Philippines Press.
Espin, O. (2001). The language of the soul: Psychotherapy with women across cultures. San Francisco: American Psychological Association.
Flowers, B. J., & Richardson, F. C. (1996). Why is multiculturalism good?
American Psychologist, 53, 609621.
Fox, D., & Prilleltensky, I. (1997). Critical psychology. London: Sage.
Ge, X., Conger, R., & Elder, G. (1996). Coming of age too early: Pubertal inuences on girls vulnerability to psychological distress. Child Development,
67, 33864000.
Hamilton, P., & Russo, N. F. (2006). Women and depression: Research, theory
and social policy. In C. L. M. Keyes & S. H. Goodman (Eds.), Women and
depression: A handbook for social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences (pp. 479
522). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hays, P. (2001). Addressing cultural complexities in practice: A framework for clinicians and counselors. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Hinkelman, J. (2001). Converging forces: Mexican culture and clinical issues of
Mexican women. Presentation, August 2008, Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association. San Francisco: American Psychological
Association.
Kastrup, M. (2007). Global mental health. Danish Medical Bulletin, 54, 4243.
Kawano, K. I. (1990). Feminist therapy with Japanese women. Journal of Social
Work Practice, 4, 4455.
Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., Neale, M. C., & Prescott, C. A. (2001). Genetic
factors for major depression in men and women: Similar or different heritabilities and same or partly distinct genes? Psychological Medicine, 31,
605616.
Kessler, R. (2006). Epidemiology of depression in women. In C. L. M. Keyes &
S. H. Goodman (Eds.), Women and depression: A handbook for social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences (pp. 2240). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Kessler, R. C., Chiu, C., Demler, W., & Walters, E. (2005). Prevalence, severity,
and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 617627.
Khanna, R. (1996). Participatory action research (PAR) in womens health:
SARTHI, India. In K. de Koning & M. Martin (Eds.), Participatory research
in health: Issues and experiences (pp. 6271). Johannesburg: Zed Books Ltd.
Kitzinger, C. (1991). Politicizing psychology. Feminism & Psychology, 1, 4954.
Koblinsky, M., Timyan, J., & Gay, J. (Eds.). (1992). The health of women: A global
perspective. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Landrine, H. (1995). Bringing cultural diversity to feminist psychology. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Landrine, H., & Corral, H. (in press). In H. Landrine & N. F. Russo (Eds.),
Handbook of diversity in feminist psychology. New York: Springer.
Lazarus, S., & Lykes, M. B. (2005). Indigenous and Euro-American psychologies:
Resources and barriers for generating knowledge and praxis. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Lopez, S. R., & Guarnaccia, P. J. (2005). Cultural dimensions of psychopathology: The social worlds impact on mental illness. In J. E. Maddow &

22

Mental and Physical Health

B. A. Winstead (Eds.), Psychopathology: Foundations for a contemporary understanding (pp. 2142), London: Rutledge.
Lykes, M. B. (1994). Terror, silencing, and children: International multidisciplinary collaboration with Guatemalan Maya communities. Social Science and
Medicine, 38, 543552.
Lykes, M. B. (2001). Creative arts and photography in participatory action
research in Guatemala. In P. Reason & H. Bradbury (Eds.), Handbook of
action research (pp. 363371). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Lykes, M., Coquillon, E., & Rabenstein, K. L. (in press). Theoretical and methodological challenges in participatory community-based research. In
H. Landrine & N. F. Russo (Eds.), Handbook of diversity in feminist psychology. New York: Springer.
Martin-Baro, I. (1994). Writing for a liberation psychology. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Matsuyuki, M. (1998). Japanese feminist counseling as a political act. Women
and Therapy, 21, 28310.
Mazure, C. M., Keita, G., & Blehar, M. C. (2002). Summit on women and depression: Proceedings and recommendations. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
McKay, K. M., Hill, M. S., Freedman, S. R., & Enright, R. D. (2007). Towards a
feminist empowerment model of forgiveness psychotherapy. Psychotherapy:
Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 44, 1429.
Naidoo, A. V. (2005). Constructing community, reconstructing psychology: Challenging the hegemony of Eurocentric psychology in South Africa. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2001). Gender differences in depression. Directions in
Psychological Science, 10, 173176.
Paltiel, F. L. (1993). Womens mental health: A global perspective. In
M. Koblinsky, J. Timyan, & J. Gay (Eds.), The health of women: A global perspective. Boulder, CO: Westeld Press.
Patel, V. (2003). Where there is no psychiatrist: A mental health care manual.
London: Gaskell.
Patel, V., Araya, R., de Lima, Ludermir, A., & Todd, C. (1999). Women, poverty, and common mental disorders in four restructuring societies. Social
Science & Medicine, 49, 14611471.
Pharaon, N. (2001). Personal transformation: A group therapy program for Saudi
women. San Francisco: American Psychological Association.
Piccinelli, M., & Homen, F. G. (1997). Gender differences in the epidemiology of
affective disorders and schizophrenia. A report of Nations for Mental Health: An
Initiative for Mental Health in Underserved Populations. Geneva, Switzerland:
World Health Organization.
Pick, S., Contreras, C., & Barker-Aguilar, A. (2006). Violence against women in
Mexico: conceptualization and program application. Annals of the New York
Academy of Science, 1087, 261278.
Radway, J. (1998). Gender in the eld of ideological production: Feminist cultural studies, the problem of the political subject, and the aims of knowledge production. In S. V. Rosser, J. Radway, & N. Fobre (Eds.), New
perspectives in gender studies: Research in the elds of economics, culture and life
sciences (pp. 3759). Linkoping, Netherlands: Linkoping University Department of Gender Studies.

International Perspectives on Women and Mental Health

23

Rice, J. K. (2001). Global divorce and the feminization of poverty. International


Psychology Bulletin, 4, 2124.
Rice, J. K. (2005). Social and economic support for divorce internationally. In
A. D. Thomas, N. Dayan, A. B. Bernardo, & R. Roth (Eds.), Helping others
grow ( pp. 107112). Aachen, Austria: Shaker Verlag.
Rice, J. K. (2007). Global warfare, violence and the welfare of women. In
S. McCarthy, S. Newstead, K. Karandashev, C. Prandini, C. Hutz, &
W. Gomes. Teaching psychology around the world (pp. 338345). Newcastle,
United Kingdom: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Rice, J. K. & Ballou, M. (2003). Cultural and gender awareness in international
psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, Division
52, International Psychology, International Committee for Women.
Riger, S. (1992). Epistemological debates, feminist voices: Science, social values,
and the study of women. American Psychologist, 47, 730740.
Riger, S. (2000). Transforming psychology. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford
University Press.
Rosenblum, K., & Travis, T. M. (1996). A theory of multicultural counseling and
therapy. Pacic Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Russo, N. F. (in press). Diversity and mental health. In H. Landrine & N. F. Russo
(Eds.), Handbook of diversity in feminist psychology. New York: Springer.
Russo, N. F., & Tartaro, J. (2008). Women and mental health. In F. L. Denmark
& M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories
(2nd ed., pp. 440481). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Russo, N. F., & Vaz, K. (2001). Addressing diversity in the decade of
behavior: Focus on women of color. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25,
280294.
Sar, M. P., & Hill, K. (2008). International aspects of the development of the
psychology of women and gender. In F. L. Denmark & M. A. Paludi
(Eds.), Psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories (pp. 7090).
Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Silverman, W. K., & Carter, R. (2006). Anxiety disturbance in girls and women.
In J. Worell & C. Goodheart (Eds.), Handbook of girls and womens psychological health: Gender and well-being across the lifespan (pp. 6087). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Ssebunnya, J., Kigozi, F., Lund, C., Kizza, D., & Okello, E. (2009). Stakeholder
perceptions of mental health stigma and poverty in Uganda. BMC International Health and Human Rights, 9(5). Retrieved June 15, 2009, from http://
www.biomedcentral.com/1472-698X/9/5
Stewart, D. E. (2006). The international consensus statement on womens mental health and the WPA consensus statement on interpersonal violence
against women. World Psychiatry, 5, 6164.
Stewart, D. E., Rondon, M., Damiani, G., & Honikman, J. (2001). International
psychosocial and systemic issues in womens mental health. Archives of
Womens Mental Health, 4, 37.
Swim, J. K., Becker, J., & Lee, E. (in press). Sexism reloaded: Worldwide evidence for its endorsement, expression, and emergence in multiple contexts.
In H. Landrine & N. F. Russo (Eds.), Handbook of diversity in feminist
psychology. New York: Springer.
Ueno, C. (1997). Interview/Are the Japanese feminine? Some problems of
Japanese Feminism in its cultural context. In S. Buckley (Ed.), Broken

24

Mental and Physical Health

silence: Voices of Japanese feminism (pp. 272301), Berkeley, CA: University


of California Press.
Unger, R. (1995). Cultural diversity and the future of feminist psychology. In
H. Landrine (Ed.), Bringing cultural diversity to feminist psychology (pp. 413,
432). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
United Nations. (2000). The millennium declaration. New York: United Nations.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (USDHHS). (2002). Women of
color health data book. Rockville, MD: USDHHS, National Institute of Health,
Ofce of the Director, Ofce of Research on Womens Health.
Ustun, T. B. (2000). Cross-national epidemiology of depression and gender.
Journal of Gender-specic Medicine, 3, 5458.
Winter, A. (2001). Women in the Arab/Muslim world: Therapeutic strategies. San
Francisco: American Psychological Association.
World Health Organization (WHO). (2001). Violence against women information
pack. (pp. 125). Geneva: Department of Injuries and Violence Prevention,
WHO. Retrieved romhttp://www.who.int/violence_injury_prevention.
World Health Organization (WHO). (2002). Gender disparities in mental health.
Geneva: Author.
World Health Organization (WHO). (2004). Prevention of mental disorders:
Effective interventions and policy options. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health
Organization, Department of Mental Health and Substance Abuse.
World Health Organization (WHO). (2006). Gender and womens mental health.
Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
World Health Organization (WHO). (2008). The global burden of disease: 2004
update. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.
World Health Organization (WHO). (2009). Mental health aspects of womens reproductive health: A global review of the literature. Geneva, Switzerland, Author.
Worley, H. (2006). Depression: A leading contributor to global burden of disease.
Washington, DC: Population Reference Bureau. Retrieved from http://
www.coa.gov.ph/gat/ articles/depression_062006.htm
Wyche, K., & Rice, J. K. (1997). Feminist therapy: From dialogue to tenets. In
J. Worell & N. G. Johnson (Eds.), Shaping the future of feminist psychology:
Education, research, and practice (pp. 5771). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Zahn-Waxler, C., Race, E., & Duggal, S. (2004). Mood disorders, syndromes
and symptoms: The development of depression in girls. In D. J. Bell,
S. L. Forster, & E. J. Mash (Eds.), Behavioral and emotional problems in girls
(pp. 2576). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishing.

Chapter 2

Ethnocultural Psychotherapy: Women


of Colors Resilience and Liberation
Lillian Comas-Diaz

Womens lives are embedded in multiple contexts. Their intersecting


identities inhabit complex realities. Consider Ana, a 30-year-old Colombian American woman, who consulted a therapist upon her physicians
recommendation. Anas presenting complaint: Im targeted at work
because Im a black Latina immigration lawyer, highlights the intersection of her multiple identitiesa combination of ethnicity, race, gender, and profession. Enmeshed in a collective matrix, female identity
and well-being tend to be relational, that is, connected to others and to
contexts. To heal, nurture, and sustain womens well-being, clinicians
need to address female interconnecting identities.
Psychotherapy with womenin particular, women of coloroffers
opportunities and challenges. Indeed, the rewards of working with culturally diverse women enrich therapists lives. Multicultural therapeutic encounters can be replete of gratication, excitement, and deeper
connection. Lets examine Anas rst session. Her body language
(sweating palms, fearful facial expression) suggested that she was anxious and scared. Dr. Brown, a Jewish American therapist, addressed
Anas feelings and recommended breathing exercises. Ana agreed and
while engaged in the exercise, she suddenly remembered her grandmothers breathing exercises. She shared the breathing technique with
Dr. Brown, who was pleasantly surprised to learn the source of Anas
grandmothers exerciseshamanism. Interested in indigenous healing,
Dr. Brown added a new strategy to her clinical repertoire.
Conversely, working with culturally diverse clients can be challenging. Partly due to cultural misunderstanding, the multicultural

26

Mental and Physical Health

encounter can exert a strain in therapists. Consider Judy, a Chinese


American woman who presented to therapy after a romantic breakup.
She saw Dr. Smith, a European American woman who explored the
circumstances around the breakup. When the therapist commented on
the unfullled quality of the relationship, Judy described the friendship
between her parents and her ex-ancees family. Dr. Smiths reply
You need to take care of yourself,reected an individualistic
worldview where separation and individuation from family are signs
of adulthood. Judys answer, Taking care of myself means taking care
of my parents, denoted a collectivistic worldview where interdependence among family members prevails. A cultural impasse resulted out
of these conictive worldviews. This situation generated difculties
with empathy. Dr. Smith persisted in encouraging Judy to individuate
from her parents. After a few sessions, Judy informed Dr. Smith that
she did not need therapy anymore.
What seems to be the major difference between Dr. Brown and
Dr. Smith? Research has shown that culturally diverse clients identied
their therapists cultural competence as the major source of satisfaction
with their treatment (Knipscheer & Kleber, 2004). Most experts agree
that cultural competence involves a set of knowledge, attitudes, and
skills that enables clinicians to be effective in multicultural practice
(Betancourt, Green, Carrillo, & Ananch-Firempong, 2003). Moreover,
the development of cultural competence requires empathy and a lifelong commitment. Working within a culturally relevant therapeutic
framework helps to develop cultural competence. In this chapter, I
present ethnocultural psychotherapy as a gender-afrmative, culturally
relevant healing approach for women of color.
ETHNOCULTURAL PSYCHOTHERAPY:
CONCEPTS AND GOALS
Ethnocultural psychotherapy incorporates ethnic, cultural, and gender
parameters into mental health assessment and treatment (Comas-Daz &
Jacobsen, 2004). It is a contextual healing approach that recognizes,
reclaims, and recovers gender and cultural strengths. As such, ethnocultural psychotherapists examine the role of gendered ethnicity in the lives
of women of color.
Gendered ethnicity refers to the interactive effects of gender, culture,
ethnicity, and race. Unfortunately, dominant mental health practitioners seldom understand the profound inuence of gendered ethnicity in women of color. When in treatment, women of color often
nd these services irrelevant and unresponsive to their life experiences. What is more, some fear psychotherapy to be an acculturation
tool (Ramirez, 1991), as well as a form of cultural imperialism. To
illustrate, women of color fear that psychotherapys androcentric and

Ethnocultural Psychotherapy

27

ethnocentric perspective will label them as deviant and pathological.


Judys cultural impasse with Dr. Smith exemplies this concern.
Women of color experience cultural imperialism through a systematic
indoctrination that the dominant cultural values are superior to their
own (Said, 1994). Consequently, healing requires empowering women
of color to be themselves. When in therapy, women of color frequently
talk about cultural identity, emotional expression, community expectations of gender roles, motherdaughter dyad, spirituality, sexuality,
relationship with other women of color, and class differences (Slater,
Daniel, & Banks, 2003).
A signicant number of women of color need mental health treatment. Many struggle with loss, grief, and trauma, in addition to experiencing a nefarious form of racism and sexism. Indeed, women in
general, and women of color in particular, are exposed to multiple
oppressions. Beyond gendered ethnicity, women of colors social class,
sexual orientation, skin color, immigration status, health status, age,
language, and other variables predispose them to oppression. Exposure
to continuing oppression results in a psychological adaptation to domination. For instance, oppressed individuals internalize powerlessness
and develop a fractured identity (Freire, 1973). To promote empowerment and reformulate identity, ethnocultural psychotherapists help
women to take control of their lives, overcome their oppressed mentality, and achieve a critical knowledge of themselves. These objectives
are necessary in the delivery of effective and ethical psychotherapeutic
services to women of color.

EMPOWERMENT
Ethnocultural psychotherapists aim to increase women of colors
sense of agency. In other words, ethnocultural psychotherapy empowers
women to increase their access to resources, examine options, enhance
their ability to make choices, improve self- and collective esteem, learn
culturally relevant assertiveness, rescue ethnogender strengths, overcome internalized oppression, and engage in transformative actions.
Ethnocultural psychotherapy promotes critical consciousness. Coined by
Paulo Freire (1970), critical consciousness refers to the process of
engaging in a dialectical dialogue with ones world, becoming aware of
ones circumstances, and initiating transformative actions. The critical
consciousness dialogue involves asking questions such as What?
Why? How? For whom? Against whom? By whom? In favor of whom?
In favor of what? To what end? (Freire & Macedo, 2000). Exploring
these critical questions raises consciousness and helps to examine
existential issues. In particular, a critical dialogue facilitates womens
examination of what matters regarding power differentials.

28

Mental and Physical Health

Ethnocultural psychotherapy addresses power differentials within a


liberation perspective. Unfortunately, many mainstream psychotherapists ignore the effect of power in womens realities. Consider the following vignette. Karen, a mixed race (black and white) single mother,
was in therapy with a white male clinician. Dr. Cooper treated Karen
with mainstream psychotherapy. After six months in treatment, Karen
did not get relief from her anxiety. When she shared her experiences
with her friend Carol, Karen realized that she did not feel comfortable
discussing race with her therapist. I get this unspoken message that
Dr. Cooper does not want to hear about racist sexism. To resolve her
dilemma, Karen terminated therapy and entered treatment with Carols
therapistan African American woman.
IDENTITY REFORMULATION
Besides empowerment, ethnocultural psychotherapy promotes identity reformulation. In other words, therapists encourage women to reclaim their voices. This process afrms womens ethnogender values.
Such afrmation is necessary because women of color tend to reconnect with their ethnic traditions during crises. Lamentably, many
women of color forget their ethnocultural roots to accommodate to
the dominant society. Through cultural amnesia, they neglect adaptive
functions of their gendered ethnicity.
Along these lines, ethnocultural psychotherapists examine womens
psychocultural needs. For example, lesbians of color struggle with a
combined sexism, racism, and heterosexism that require attention during therapy. Likewise, working-class women of color are exposed to a
mixture of sexism, racism, and classism. They may nd themselves in
a dilemma as their personal striving threatens their ethnoclass ties
(Kuppersmith, 1987). Regardless of social class membership, class realities are pervasive in communities of color and thus affect many
women of color. Therapists who recognize ethnoclass loyalty dilemmas
promote female empowerment.
Healing the historical, personal, and collective wounds of women of
color necessitates a gender-afrmative, culturally relevant contextual
approach.
ETHNOCULTURAL PSYCHOTHERAPY APPLICATIONS
Ethnocultural psychotherapy is gender afrmative because it acknowledges the concept of self as an internal gendered-ethnocultural representation. To elaborate, female identity is embedded and constructed within
diverse contexts. For instance, Catherine Batson (1990) empirically found
that females frequently compose their lives by adapting to life milestones,
such as coupling, motherhood, and other gender-specic events. As a

Ethnocultural Psychotherapy

29

result, conducting psychotherapy with women of color entails examining


their adaptation to their total environment. An ethnocultural assessment
facilitates such examination.
ETHNOCULTURAL ASSESSMENT
Ethnocultural assessment helps to place female realities in contexts.
This assessment explores intellectual and emotional understanding of
several historical stages of cultural identity development (Jacobsen,
1988). It acknowledges the inuences of external and internal factors
during different stages in womens lives. As both a diagnostic and
treatment tool, the ethnocultural assessment helps to unfold material
relevant to womens functioning. Additionally, it aids in the development of a therapeutic alliance. Certainly, when conducting ethnocultural assessments, therapists aim to convey genuine interest in their
client. This approach fosters a safe atmosphere for treatment. The
ethnocultural assessment examines the womens heritage, saga, niche,
adjustment, and relationships.
Heritage relates to womens ancestry, history, genetics, biology, and
sociopolitical inheritance. In this stage, therapists examine maternal
and paternal cultures of origin to delineate ethnic heritage. The examination of biological factors includes illnesses, physiological, gender,
and ethnic differences in drug metabolism. Of particular interest is the
exploration of cultural trauma in the form of soul wounds. A legacy of
pain and suffering among many minority group members, soul
wounds result from sociohistorical oppression, ungrieved losses, internalized oppression, and learned helplessness (Duran & Ivey, 2006).
Examples of soul wounds are a history of slavery, colonization, Holocaust, genocide, and wars. In addition, therapists explore the legacy of
survivors syndromes.
Examining womens cultural legacy provides a foundation for the
unearthing of the family saga. Ethnocultural assessment second stage,
saga, entails the family, clan, tribe, and group story. As an illustration,
the family saga reveals the circumstances that led a woman and or her
multigenerational family to journey through cultural transitions. Transition could be any kind of translocationa geographical move, migration, immigration, sojourn, or major transitions in life such as change
of job, marital status, in addition to milestones like pregnancy, abortion, miscarriage, and others. It is important to discern the voluntary
versus the involuntary reasons for the transition or translocation. The
translocation post analysis provides a context for clients ethnocultural
transition. Relevant questions characteristic of this stage include: How
long ago was the translocation? Was it recent or generations ago?
What are the thoughts and feelings regarding the events leading to the
translocation?

30

Mental and Physical Health

The next assessment stage, niche, refers to the outcome of the post
transition analysis. Niche is based on the clients intellectual and emotional perception of her familys ethnocultural identity in the host society since the translocation. Succinctly put, niche is the place carved by
the family after the transition. During this stage, therapists assess what
happened to the family after the translocation. Moreover, therapists
examine womens cognitive and emotional perception of their family
ethnocultural saga. They review womens interactions with members of
their own ethnocultural group. In particular, therapists explore the
family status before and after the translocation. Womens internalization of their family saga provides a blueprint of their entry into the
world. Exploring this stage facilitates the development of a contextual
framework within which to place womens subjective experiences.
Therapists ask the following questions when examining womens
niche: Have family members stayed together? Is there a sense of family unity? What is the relationship of family with the original ethnocultural group? How have they fared nancially, emotionally?
The self-adjustment stage relates to womens own perceived adaptation to the host culture (or situation) as individuals distinct from their
family. Self-adjustment explores the contrasts between the womans
ethnocultural identity and that of her family, work, and social environment. This stage involves an analysis of womens coping skills. Here,
therapists help women to analyze the functionality of their behaviors
within diverse contexts. Womens strengths are examined, paying
attention to cultural resilience. For example, among many women of
color, personal survival is connected to their collective survival.
Fostering cultural resilience, ethnocultural psychotherapy helps
women to reconnect with their ethnic beliefs. Cultural resilience is a
host of strengths, values and practices that promote coping mechanisms and adaptive reactions to traumatic oppression (Elsass, 1992).
Cultural resilience promotes resourceful responses to oppression and
adversity. Therefore, it fosters creativity, reconstruction, and evolution.
The last ethnocultural assessment stage examines womens relations.
Therapists examine womens self and other relationship. Besides examining womens signicant connections, clinicians focus on the therapeutic relationship, including transference and countertransference. They
explore their own ethnocultural background to determine specic areas
of real or potential overlap with their clients. In other words, therapists complete their own ethnocultural assessment to determine areas
of similarity and difference with their clients. Above and beyond
obtaining a wealth of information crucial for therapeutic interventions,
performance of an ethnocultural assessment frequently opens new
channels for the recognition of self in the culturally different other.
A crucial component of the ethnocultural assessment is the inclusion
of gender specic issues in all stages of the evaluation. For example,

Ethnocultural Psychotherapy

31

during the heritage stage, historical female roles, female biological


markers, female victimization, and other history of gender issues are
examined. The saga stage helps to reveal the herstory of signicant
female family members. Similarly, during the niche stage clinicians
examine female gender collective scripts such as family roles of women
and men, wantedness of children (including the client), and gender
specic family trauma. Furthermore, therapists explore the symbolic
meaning of the clients name, age cohort effect on women, acculturation, personal development, and other adjustment issues as part of
womens self-adjustment. Finally, therapists explore the gender aspects
of intimate relations, (with men and women), physical and sexual
abuse, domestic violence, incest, battered spouse syndrome, forced
prostitution, sexual abduction, and other forms of gender trauma
within relationships.
In conclusion, the ethnocultural assessment is especially effective
when treatment issues are unclear, gender and ethnocultural concerns
are presenting complaints, and when women are in a multicultural
situation.

ETHNOCULTURAL THERAPEUTIC PROCESS


Therapeutic Relationship
Ethnocultural psychotherapy acknowledges the conuence of both
the therapists and the clients realities. Such convergence is accentuated within the dyadic encounter. Consequently, ethnocultural psychotherapists recognize the therapeutic relationship as an essential agent
of change. They use it as a vehicle to promote critical awareness and
transformation. In fact, successful psychotherapy with women of color
depends on the therapists skill in establishing and managing the therapeutic relationship (Jenkins, 1985).
In addition to cultural competence, ethnocultural psychotherapists
aim to develop cultural empathy. Indeed, the development of empathy
in a multicultural context facilitates an understanding of the clients experience. Empathy is an interpersonal construct referring to a clinicians intrinsic capacity to attend to the emotional experience of others.
Within mainstream psychotherapy, empathy is composed of kinesthetic, affective and cognitive elements (Jordan & Surrey, 1986). The
kinesthetic component relates to the nonverbal communication and
body language. Empathys affective component involves an emotional
connectednessthe experience of being like the other. The cognitive element refers to an intellectual understanding of the other. Therapists
engaged solely in cognitive empathy tend to maintain their identity differentiated from their clients (Kaplan, 1991), and thus, do not experience being like the other.

32

Mental and Physical Health

As a result, therapists need to go beyond being an empathic witness


when working with women of color. They need to develop cultural
empathy. Cultural empathy is a learned ability that helps therapists to
culturally understand the experience of women of color. Cultural empathy entails a process of perspective taking by using a cultural framework
as a guide for understanding women and recognizing cultural differences between self and other (Ridley & Lingle, 1996). Indeed, therapists
able to take the perspective of the other person signicantly reduce their
stereotypic and ethnocentric attitudes (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000).
Therefore, cultural empathy promotes therapists cultural responsiveness
through a combination of perceptual, cognitive, affective, and communication skills. In short, cultural empathy involves therapists attunement
to women of colors life experiences. Within this context, attunement
refers to the process whereby the therapist focuses on the internal world
of the woman of color and in turn, she feels understood and connected
(Stern, 1985). Such a state of connection facilitates the management of
identication and projection during therapy.
ETHNOCULTURAL TRANSFERENCE AND
COUNTERTRANSFERENCE
The multicultural therapeutic encounter can provide opportunities
for projections based on ethnicity, gender, and race. These projections
infuse the therapeutic relationship with complex ethnocultural and
gender inuences. Therapists working with women of color need to
understand transference and countertransference, including their ethnocultural determinants (Varghese, 1983). Racial, gender, and ethnic
factors are available targets for projection in therapy (Jones, 1984),
which may be manifested in transference. Comas-Daz and Jacobsen
(1991) identied several types of ethnocultural transference and countertransference within the inter-ethnic and the intra-ethnic psychotherapeutic dyads. In brief, the inter-ethnic transferential reactions include
the following: (1) overcompliance and friendliness (observed when
there is a societal power differential in the client/therapist dyad); (2)
denial (when the client avoids disclosing issues pertinent to gendered
ethnicity and/or culture); (3) mistrust and suspiciousness (How can this
therapist understand me?) and (4) ambivalence (a common reaction when
working with socially marginalized individuals). Women of color in an
interethnic psychotherapy may struggle with negative feelings toward
their therapists, while simultaneously developing an attachment to
them. Issues of identication and internalization within the inter-ethnic
dyad may also foster ambivalence in the client.
The intra-ethnic transference may include the following: (1) omniscient/omnipotent therapist (idealization of the therapist frequently
with the fantasy of the reunion with the perfect parent, promoted by

Ethnocultural Psychotherapy

33

the ethnic similarity); (2) traitor (client exhibits resentment and envy at
therapists successesequated with betrayal and the selling out of his/
her culture and race); (3) auto-racist (client does not want to work with
a therapist of her own ethnicity, due to projection of the strong negative feelings about herself onto the therapist); and (4) ambivalent
(women may feel comfortable with their shared ethnocultural background, but at the same time, they may fear too much psychological
closeness).
Some countertransferential reactions within the inter-ethnic dyad
include the following: (1) denial of cultural differences; (2) the clinical
anthropologists syndrome (excessive curiosity about women of colors
ethnocultural backgrounds at the expense of their emotional needs);
(3) guilt (emerges when societal and political realities dictate a lower
status for women of color); (4) pity (a derivative of guilt or an expression of political impotence within the therapeutic hour) (5) aggression;
and (6) ambivalence (it may originate from ambivalence toward therapists own ethnoculture).
Within the intra-ethnic dyad some of the countertransferential reactions are the following: (1) overidentication; (2) us and them mentality
(shared victimization due to gendered ethnocultural discrimination
may contribute to therapists ascribing the womans problems as being
solely due to being a person of color); (3) distancing; (4) survivors
guilt (therapists of color may have the personal experience of escaping
the harsh socioeconomic circumstances of low income ethnic minorities, leaving family and friends in the process, and generating conict
and guilt); (5) cultural myopia (inability to see clearly due to ethnocultural factors that obscure therapy); (6) ambivalence (working through
the therapists own ethnic ambivalence); and (7) anger (being too ethnoculturally close to a woman of color may uncover painful, unresolved emotional issues).
The examination of ethnocultural transference and countertransference advances the psychotherapeutic process.
PHASES IN THE PSYCHOTHERAPEUTIC PROCESS
The ethnocultural psychotherapeutic process unfolds the phases of
intuition, affect, cognition, and coalescence. Although these stages seem
to follow a developmental path, in reality, they are uid and permeable.
During the intuitive stage, women of color are reading the therapists
nonverbal communication. In other words, they are literally checking
the therapist out. Thus, nonverbal communication such as body language, hunches, and vibes acquires central importance. Although both
therapist and client follow their gut feelings, women of color tend to rely
more on their intuition, while therapists are cognitively engaged in
collecting data, making a diagnosis, and developing a treatment plan.

34

Mental and Physical Health

Ethnocultural therapists pay special attention to gut feelings. Partly


due to being oppressed, women of color rely on nonverbal signs to decipher meanings within power differentials. At times, they communicate indirectlya style prevalent among sociocentric individuals. When
therapists overlook such communication, then empathic difculties
occur. In particular, women of color check out their therapist for the
presence of racism and sexism. In other words, women of color follow
their intuition. Consequently, ethnocultural psychotherapists attend to
all kinds of communicationwhat is said and not saidthroughout all
phases of the therapeutic process.
The affective phase refers to feelings and emotions. It includes womens subjective experiences of being a woman, a person of color, and a
woman of color. To facilitate understanding womens feelings, therapists ask: How do you feel being a person of color? What does it
mean to you to be a woman of color? The affective phase unfolds
womens subjective experience of being a person of color. More importantly, the woman ascertains how she feels about the therapist, and
conversely, the therapist examines how he or she feels about the client.
Indeed, the affective phase sets the stage for the emergence of ethnocultural transference and countertransference.
The cognitive phase involves the intellectual understanding of the
therapeutic alliance. Pre-therapy expectations are expressed and discussed during this phase. The cognitive stage entails a reality testing of
cultural differences and/or similarities. It signals the recognition of the
presence of transference and countertransference.
Ethnogender factors inuencing the psychotherapeutic process begin
to come together during coalescence. For example, womens feelings
regarding treatment and their clinician are articulated and negotiated during this stage. Women begin to accept their therapist as a helping person.
Cultural differences are worked through, including power differentials.
These processes provide a model for the reconciliation of differences.
Women examine their fractured identities and begin to integrate disparate
aspects of the self. For these reasons, the therapeutic restoration takes
place during coalescence and the potential for growth is enhanced.

ETHNOCULTURAL PSYCHOTHERAPY TOOLS


In addition to mainstream therapeutic strategies, clinicians use ethnocultural tools. Some of these instruments include the explanatory
model of distress, cultural genograms, cultural transitional maps, and
ethnocultural occupational inventory. Other ethnocultural tools include
narratives/storytelling, testimonies, and indigenous healing techniques.
In this chapter I briey present the explanatory model of distress, cultural genogram, and the ethnocultural occupational inventory.

Ethnocultural Psychotherapy

35

The explanatory model of distress is an ethnographical clinical tool


used to elicit clients expectations and perspectives on their illness
(Kleinman, 1980). Clinicians ask the following questions (Callan &
Littlewood, 1998; Kleinman, 1980):
What do you call your problem (distress or illness)?
What do you think your problem (illness) does?
What do you think the natural course of your illness is?
What do you fear?
Why do you think this illness or problem has occurred?
How do you think the distress should be treated?
How do want me to help you?
Who do you turn to for help?
Who should be involved in decision-making?

Therapists who ask these questions convey respect and interest in


their clients. As they earn clients trust, clinicians foster the emergence
of a healing alliance.
Another ethnocultural tool is the cultural genogram. This instrument
emphasizes the role of culture in the lives of individuals and their families
(Hardy & Laszloffy, 1995). Like regular genograms (McGoldrick, Gerson,
& Shellenberger, 1999), cultural genograms diagram the genealogical, developmental, historical, political, economical, and sociological inuences
on individuals lives. Moreover, cultural genograms place women within
their collective circumstances and identify their ethnogender, spiritual, and
racial contexts. Furthermore, cultural genograms emphasize the effects of
skin color, hair texture, body type, phenotype, and appearance in women
of color. Similarly, the cultural transitional map claries the information
regarding family transitions. The cultural transitional map elucidates
womens individual and collective history of translocation. In addition, this
tool assesses personal, familial, ethnocultural, and community mappings
in families undergoing sociocultural change and transition (Ho, 1987).
Finally, the ethnocultural occupational inventory assesses womens
experiences within work settings. For example, Dr. Brown explored
Anas experiences as a lawyer. She used the ethnocultural occupational
inventory to explore Anas experiences as a woman, a woman of color,
a mixed race woman, and a professional of color working in a predominantly white law rm. Among other areas, the ethnocultural occupational inventory explored the following:
1. Meaning of work for Ana, her family and her ethnic group
2. Family history of higher education and occupational attainment
3. Previous and current work (paid and pro bono)

36

Mental and Physical Health


4. Occupational success and failure
5. Occupational socialization
6. Discrimination in work setting
7. Sexual harassment at workplace (direct and vicarious)
8. Racial and/or ethnocultural anger and rage
9. Interaction of ethnicity, gender, race, class, and sexual orientation
10. Previous and current occupational coping skills (functional and
dysfunctional)
11. Occupational fears, fantasies, family scripts, and wishes
12. Identication and challenge of irrational and dysfunctional belief systems about work
13. Projection of family, personal, and career goals
14. Racial climate at workplace
15. Assessment of gender issues at workplace

The completion of Anas occupational inventory signaled Anas


resolve to engage in a transformative action. All of the ethnocultural
psychotherapy processes, stages, and tools afrm womens empowerment and identity reformulation. The therapeutic work lays the foundation for the development of womens ethnogender consciousness.
ETHNOGENDER CONSCIOUSNESS
Ethnocultural psychotherapy aims to provide a safe forum for
womens identity reformulation. It offers acceptance of the clients
gendered ethnicity by conveying the relevance of identity afrmation
and reformulation in healing. Ethnocultural consciousness is a pivotal
factor in recovery and liberation. Due to cultural imperialism, sexist
racism, and multiple forms of oppression, many women of color
develop cultural amnesia. To awaken their cultural legacy and reconnect with their roots, women of color need to become ethnoculturally
conscious. The process of reconnecting with ethnic, cultural and spiritual roots, ethnocultural consciousness enhances womens ability to
resist oppression. In other words, ethnocultural consciousness helps
women of color to rescue their gendered cultural strengths and
acknowledge their multiple intersecting identities. Simply put, ethnocultural consciousness promotes women of colors ability to assert
and celebrate who they are.
Therapists initiate womens consciousness during the completion of
the ethnocultural assessment. However, the development of ethnocultural and feminist consciousness requires more than the delineation of
heritage, legacy, and herstory. Consciousness needs to embrace womens
cultural resilience, strengths, and gifts.

Ethnocultural Psychotherapy

37

An important aspect in women of colors resilience is spirituality.


Indeed, feminism of color is embedded in spiritual contexts. To illustrate, womanismAfrican American womens feminismafrms
female strengths, ghts oppression, and promotes collective social justice (Walker, 1983). A womanist goal is the infusion of spirituality into
womens lives (Phillips, 2006). Likewise, Latina feminismmujerismo
is anchored in spirit. Based on liberation theology, mujeristas commit to
the decolonization of all people (Isasi-Diaz, 1994). Womanism and
mujerismo aim to empower women and their communities. Both movements are revolutionary, communal, and generative. As collective
efforts, womanism and mujerismo foment global solidarity (ComasDaz, 2008). Their multidisciplinary foundation nurtures womens ethnogender and cultural consciousness. Feminism of color spiritually
based awareness enhances women of colors cultural resilience. It leads
women to resist the pressure to revise or to repress experience, to
embrace conict rather than conformity, and endure anger and pain
rather than submitting to repression and oppression (Tal, 1996).
When women of color become conscious, they alchemize oppression
into liberation (Comas-Daz, 2008). As women reconnect with their ethnogender strengths, they rescue their gifts of power. The experience of multiple intersecting oppressions facilitates the development of gifts of power.
These giftsethnogender intuition, prophetic abilities, and healing capacitiesare spiritually based. In other words, many women of color use their
spirituality to rescue their gifts of power to empower themselves and
others. Since these gifts constitute a resistance against oppression, colonization, and cultural imperialism, they instill women with transformation
and liberation (Comas-Daz, 2008). The exibility inherent in ethnocultural
consciousness helps women to reformulate their identity.
Ethnocultural consciousness travels a developmental journey. As
such, it is similar to Maria Harriss (1991) depiction of female spiritual
evolution. She identied womens spiritual development as a rhythmic
dance of awakening, discovering, creating, dwelling, nourishing, traditioning, and transforming. All of these stages are interconnected in the
lives of women of color. For instance, within their traditioning process,
many women of color resort to creativity. Indeed, a signicant part of
cultural resilience is the nurturing of creativity. Of interest, research has
shown positive relationship between peoples multiculturalism and their
creativity (Leung, Maddux, Galinsky, & Chiu, 2008). In other words,
being exposed to multicultural experiences enhances creativity. Certainly, many women of color engage in creative healing and liberation.
CONCLUSION
Ethnocultural psychotherapy acknowledges the concept of self as an
internal gendered ethnocultural representation. It addresses female

38

Mental and Physical Health

intersecting identities and contextual realities through the integration of


diversity variables into mental health assessment and treatment. Ethnocultural psychotherapists view the therapeutic relationship as the recognition of the self in the other. Consequently, they use ethnocultural tools
to empower and afrm ethnogender identity. Indeed, ethnocultural psychotherapy helps women to go back home. They are empowered to
reconstruct and inhabit their place in the world. Ana, the lawyer who
was discriminated against for being a black Latina, remained in therapy
with Dr. Brown for a year. The completion of her ethnocultural assessment facilitated Anas reconnection with herself and with her community. Therapy helped her to reconcile and integrate diverse aspects of
her identity. Based on her responses to the occupational inventory, Ana
left the law rm where she was employed. Later on, she established her
own company specializing in immigration services. Six months after
completing therapy, Ana sent Dr. Brown a local newspaper article
announcing Anas selection as woman of the year.
REFERENCES
Betancourt, J. R., Green, A. R., Carrillo, J. E., & Ananch-Firempong, O. (2003,
JulyAugust). Dening cultural competence: A practical framework for
addressing racial/ethnic disparities in health and health care. Public Health
Reports, 118, 293302.
Callan, A. & Littlewood, R. (1998). Patient satisfaction: Ethnic origin or explanatory model? International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 44, 111.
Comas-Daz, L. (2008). Spirita: Reclaiming womanist sacredness in feminism.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 1321.
Comas-Daz, L., & Jacobsen, F. M. (1991). Ethnocultural transference and countertransference in the therapeutic dyad. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
61, 392402.
Comas-Daz, L., & Jacobsen, F. M. (2004). Ethnocultural psychotherapy. In
E. Crighead & C. Nemeroff (Eds.), The concise encyclopedia of psychology and
behavioral science (pp. 338339). New York: Wiley.
Duran, E., & Ivey, A. E. (2006). Healing the soul wound: counseling with American
Indians and other Native People. New York: Teachers College Press.
Elsass, P. (1992). Strategies for survival: The psychology of cultural resilience in
ethnic minorities. New York: New York University Press.
Freire, P. (1970). Pedagogy of the oppressed. New York: Seabury Press.
Freire, P. (1973). Education for critical consciousness. New York: Seabury.
Freire, P., & Macedo, D. (2000). The Paulo Freire reader. New York: Continuum.
Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Perspective-taking: Decreasing
stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism.
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 78, 708724.
Hardy, K. V., & Laszloffy, T. (1995). The cultural genogram: Key to training
culturally competent family therapists. Journal of Marital and Family
Therapy, 21, 227237.

Ethnocultural Psychotherapy

39

Harris, M. (1991). Dance of spirit. The seven steps of womens spirituality. New
York: Bantam Books.
Ho, M. H. (1987). Family therapy with ethnic minorities. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Isasi-Diaz, A. M. (1994). Mujeristas: A name of our own: Sisters struggling in the
spirit. In N. B. Lewis (Ed.), A women of color theological anthology (pp. 126
138). Louisville, KY: Womens Ministries Program, Presbyterian Church.
Jacobsen, F. M. (1988). Ethnocultural assessment. In L. Comas-Daz & E. H.
Grifth (Eds.), Clinical guidelines in cross-cultural mental health. New York:
John Wiley & Sons.
Jenkins, A. (1985, August 25). Dialogue and dialectic: Psychotherapy in cross cultural contexts. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association
annual convention, Los Angeles.
Jones, E. (1984). Some reections of the black patient in psychotherapy. The
Clinical Psychologist, 37, 6265.
Jordan, J. V., & Surrey, J. L. (1986). The self-in-relation: Empathy and the
motherdaughter relationship. In T. Bernay & D. W. Cantor (Eds.),
The psychology of todays woman: New psychoanalytic visions. Hillsdale, NJ:
The Analytic Press.
Kaplan, A. (1991). The self in relation: implications for depression in women.
In J. V. Jordan, A. G. Kaplan, J. B. Miller, I. P. Stiver, & J. I. Surrey (Eds.),
Womens growth in connection: Writings from the Stone Center (pp. 206222).
New York: Guilford.
Kleinman, A. (1980). Patients and healers in the context of culture: An exploration of
the borderland between anthropology, medicine, and psychiatry. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Knipscheer, J. W., & Kleber, R. J. (2004). A need for ethnic similarity in the
therapist-patient interaction? Mediterranean Migrants in Dutch mental
health care. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60, 543554.
Kuppersmith, J. (1987). The double bind of personal striving: Ethnic working
class women in psychotherapy. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 17,
203216.
Leung, A. K.-Y., Maddux, W., Galinsky, A., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2008). Multicultural
experience enhances creativity: The when and how. American Psychologist,
63, 169181.
McGoldrick, M., Gerson, R., & Shellenberger, S. (1999). Genograms: Assessment
and intervention. New York: Norton W.W. Company.
Phillips, L. (Ed.). (2006). The womanist reader. New York: Routledge.
Ramirez, M. (1991). Psychotherapy and counseling with minorities: A cognitive
approach to individual and cultural differences. New York: Pergamon.
Ridley, C., & Lingle, D. W. (1996). Cultural empathy in multicultural counseling: A multidimensional process model. In P. B. Pedersen, J. G. Draguns,
W. J. Lonner, & J. E. Trimble (Eds.), Counseling across cultures (4th ed.,
pp. 21 46). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Said, E. W. (1994). Culture and imperialism. New York: Vintage Books.
Slater, L., Daniel, J. H., & Banks, A. (2003). The complete guide for mental health
for women. Boston: Beacon Press.
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.

40

Mental and Physical Health

Tal, K. (1996). Words of hurt. Reading the literatures of trauma. Cambridge, United
Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
Varghese, F. T. N. (1983). The racially-different psychiatrist: Implications for
psychotherapy. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 17, 329333.
Walker, A. (1983). In search of our mothers garden: Womanist prose. New York:
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Chapter 3

Women and Sexual Violence:


Emotional, Physical, Behavioral, and
Organizational Responses
Paula Lundberg-Love
Bethany Waits

Female survivors of sexual violence are everywhere. They are in universities, religious institutions, court rooms, hospitals, and the military.
They are daughters, mothers, spouses, sisters, friends, next-door neighbors, and coworkers. Many differ in age, education, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status (Klump, 2006; Koss, Bailey, & Yuan, 2003). However,
their lives are connected by the violence that they have experienced.
Sexual violence is a pervasive social problem, and as Koss et al. (1994)
aptly noted, there is no safe haven for women from victimization.
National epidemiological data indicates that between 17 and 25 percent
of women in the United States report some form of sexual assault in
their lifetime (Campbell, 2008). Results of a national telephone survey
conducted in 2001 to 2003 found that approximately 2.7 million women
experienced sexual violence during the last 12 months prior to the survey. The same study also reported that approximately 11.7 million
women had been victimized at some point during their lives (Basile,
Chen, Black, & Saltzman, 2007). Globally, at least one woman in three
is beaten, coerced into sex, or otherwise abused at some point in her
life (Koss et al., 2003).
Sexual violence is a comprehensive term that describes all acts of
unwanted sexual activity including rape, sexual assault, and intimate
partner violence (IPV). According to Hedtke et al. (2008), rape typically

42

Mental and Physical Health

includes forced vaginal, oral, or anal penetration with a penis or other


objects without a persons consent. For many years, the term rape
was used to describe all acts of sexual violence. Recently, it has been
abandoned in favor of the gender-neutral term sexual assault, which
is dened as nonconsensual sexual contact obtained by physical force,
by threat of physical harm, or in situations where the victim is unable
to give consent (Golding, 1999; Lee & Kleiner, 2003). This denition
can include unwanted sexual kissing to situations involving oral, anal,
or vaginal intercourse (Golding, 1999). Furthermore, IPV is dened as
threatened, attempted, or completed physical or sexual violence or
emotional abuse by a current or former intimate partner (Black &
Breiding, 2008, p. 646). IPV can be committed by a spouse, an exspouse, a boyfriend, or a dating partner. Since the majority of sexual
assaults are committed by individuals known to the victim, it is not
surprising that many victims of sexual violence also experience IPV.
Prior to the early 1970s, research concerning sexual violence focused
almost exclusively on characteristics of the perpetrator, and little attention was given to the distress of survivors. The womens movement
was instrumental in shifting the direction of sexual assault research
from the rapist to the psychological sequelae that victims experience
(Neville & Heppner, 1999). Rape crisis centers established in the late
1970s and early 1980s provided immediate intervention and long-term
therapy to individuals, as well as early research documenting womens
psychological reactions to sexual violence, which included fear, anxiety,
and depression. These studies were critical in heightening awareness
concerning the severity of the trauma that survivors encountered
(1999). For instance, an initial survey interviewed victims four to six
years after their rape experience and found that approximately 26 percent still did not feel recovered from the crime, suggesting that the
effects of sexual violence reached far beyond the initial attack (Koss
et al., 1994). Since the 1980s, literature concerning the impact of victimization has extended past psychological sequelae to other reactions that
victims encounter. These include various physical, neurological, behavioral, and organizational responses, which will be discussed in the following sections (Kaltman, Krupnick, Stockton, Hooper, & Green, 2005).
PSYCHOLOGICAL RESPONSES
The psychological impact of sexual violence has been extensively
studied. Research has consistently documented that victims experience
intense psychological distress immediately following the attack. This
distress typically peaks in severity approximately three weeks post
assault and continues to remain at an elevated level for several months.
While initial distress may dissipate over time, longitudinal studies suggest that a signicant number of victims continue to experience chronic

Women and Sexual Violence

43

mental health problems for many years subsequent to victimization


(Neville & Heppner, 1999). Indeed, Koss et al. (2003) found that several
years after their rape, approximately one-fourth of women continued
to experience negative psychological effects. These typically include
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), major depression, sexual dysfunction, and generalized anxiety (Koss & Kilpatrick, 2001). Even when
evaluated many years later, one study found that between 31 and 65
percent of victims met the diagnostic criteria for PTSD, and 43 percent
met the criteria for major depression (Campbell, 2008). These studies
provided evidence that psychological distress following victimization is
prevalent among survivors and can cause long-term devastation to the
individuals well-being and mental health (Hedtke et al., 2008).
Several researchers have documented that the most common immediate reactions following sexual violence are fear and anxiety (Bohn &
Holz, 1996; Goodman, Koss, & Russo, 1993; Koss et al., 1994; Neville &
Heppner, 1999; Resick, 1993). Within 72 hours of the assault, 86 percent
of victims report having intense fear of their assailant and anxiety
about their personal safety. By the third week, rape-induced anxiety
typically reaches maximum levels, and studies suggest that it may not
begin to lessen for up to three years (Koss et al., 1994). For instance,
when compared to survivors of other crimes such as robbery, rape victims describe greater anxiety and fear at six months and at one year
following the assault. The most frequent fears reported by victims
included talking to the police, tough-looking people, being alone, blind
dates, going out with new people, and making mistakes (Neville &
Heppner, 1999). As with anxiety, depressive symptomatology is typically observed within a few hours to a few days following victimization (Koss et al., 1994). These symptoms consist of sad feelings about
the assault, loss of interest in normal activities, suicidal thoughts, sleep
disturbances, fatigue, frequent crying spells, and an inability to concentrate (Bohn & Holz, 1996; Goodman et al., 1993; Koss et al., 1994).
Many victims meet diagnostic criteria for major depressive disorder
only a few weeks subsequent to the attack (Koss et al., 1994). In one
study, 56 percent of rape survivors reported depressive symptoms and
43 percent met the diagnostic criteria for major depression one month
after the attack (Koss et al., 2003). Furthermore, women who have been
raped are three times more likely to meet criteria for lifetime major
depression, are two times as likely to qualify for a diagnosis of dysthymia, and are 2.5 times more likely to report recent depression when
compared to nonvictims (Koss, Figueredo, & Prince, 2002). Overall, 13
percent of victims of sexual violence suffer from a major depressive
disorder sometime in their lives as compared with 5 percent who never
experience such abuse (Koss & Kilpatrick, 2001). As major depressive
disorders become more prevalent among victims, suicidal thoughts also
may increase. According to Koss and Kilpatrick (2001), victims of

44

Mental and Physical Health

sexual violence are at an elevated risk for suicidal ideation when compared with nonvictims. Within the rst month, suicidal ideation was
reported by 33 to 50 percent of survivors, and in one study 22 percent
of sexually abused women reported suicidal ideation in the previous
12-month period as compared to 7 percent of nonabused women (Koss
et al., 1994). Since the frequency of suicidal thoughts is elevated, it
should not be surprising that approximately 19 percent of victims
report at least one suicide attempt at some point in their lifetime (Koss
et al., 2003).
Victims of sexual violence are considered the largest single group
that suffers from PTSD (Koss & Kilpatrick, 2001). According to Bohn
and Holz (1996), a wide range of symptoms such as ashbacks, intrusive recollections about the abuse, repetitive dreams and nightmares,
psychological numbing, anxiety, irritability, insomnia, hypersensitivity,
and hypervigilance are frequently reported. Approximately one-third
of female survivors are diagnosed with PTSD immediately following
the attack (Ullman, Filipas, Townsend, & Starzynski, 2007). After three
weeks, 79 percent of victims met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual,
fourth edition (DSM-IV) criteria for PTSD (Gilboa-Schechtman & Foa,
2001). In addition, prospective studies have demonstrated that within
two weeks of the assault, 90 percent of victims met symptom criteria
for PTSD while 50 percent continued to meet the criteria three months
later (Koss et al., 2003). Research from community-based samples
found that between 44 and 49 percent of women who experienced sexual violence were diagnosed with PTSD (Littleton & Breitkopf, 2006).
According to Resnick, Acierno, Holmes, Dammeyer, and Kilpatrick
(2000), the lifetime prevalence of PTSD among survivors is approximately 30 percent; however, estimates as high as 50 percent also have
been reported. Finally, another study found that individuals who experienced sexual violence were 6.2 times more likely to suffer from PTSD
than women who had never been victimized (Koss et al., 2003).
Typically, survivors reported feeling dirty and unclean following sexual victimization. While many of these feelings are related to
visible dirt and contamination from the attack, they also may result
from a sense of internal, non-visible contamination. Researchers dene
this phenomenon as mental pollution, and for many, mental pollution continues despite being visibly clean (Fairbrother & Rachman,
2004). As a result, survivors may engage in excessive washing behaviors in an attempt to remove the unclean feelings sustained from the
assault. One study conducted by Fairbrother and Rachman (2004)
assessed mental pollution among a sample of sexual assault survivors
and found that 70 percent of participants reported an urge to wash or
clean themselves subsequent to the attack. Furthermore, 49 percent of
the sample reported washing more than one time and 24 percent continued washing for several weeks. More than 25 percent of women in

Women and Sexual Violence

45

the sample who washed in response to mental pollution reported that


they continued to wash excessively for several months and 11.8 percent
persisted in this behavior for at least one year post-assault (Fairbrother
& Rachman, 2004). Few studies have determined the long-term consequences of mental pollution on victims psychological well-being. However, several case studies suggest that continued washing as a result of
mental pollution might result in the onset of obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD). While this nding has been documented in several cases,
further research is needed to understand fully the relationship between
mental pollution and OCD (Fairbrother & Rachman, 2004).
As mentioned previously, victims of sexual violence are at an
increased risk to experience fear, anxiety, depression, suicidal ideation,
PTSD, and mental pollution when compared to nonvictims. Several
other psychological sequelae including sexual dysfunction and eating
disorders frequently also are reported by survivors. According to Koss
et al. (1994), victims experience less sexual satisfaction and more sexual
problems than nonvictimized women. In one study of adult survivors,
61 percent reported sexual dysfunction, especially avoidance of sex, immediately after the crime. Other frequent problems included a lack of
desire, fear of sex, and difculty becoming aroused. Even four to six
years after the incident, 30 percent of women did not feel that their
sexual functioning had returned to normal pre-rape levels (1994). In
addition, victims of sexual violence are more likely to report eating disorders such as anorexia and bulimia nervosa than nonvictims (H.
Resnick, Acierno, & Kilpatrick, 1997). In a group of bulimic patients, 23
percent had been raped, 29 percent had been sexually abused as children, and 23 percent had experienced IPV (Koss et al., 1994). Since eating disorders may have life-threatening consequences, it is important
for clinicians to assess for previous trauma history in order to provide
more appropriate treatments for these individuals.
According to Koss and colleagues (2003), the psychological impact
associated with sexual victimization has been well established for several decades. As a result, investigators are now turning their attention
from symptomatology to the moderators and mediators that translate
abuse into psychological distress. A moderator is a variable that affects
the relationship between two other variables (i.e., sexual violence and
poor mental health), changing the direction or magnitude of the effects.
A mediator, or intervening variable, acts as a link in a causal chain
which mitigates the effect from the independent (i.e., sexual assault) to
the dependent (i.e., mental health) variable. In cases of sexual victimization, moderators and mediators typically enhance the damaging
impact of the abuse experience on victims, thereby prolonging recovery. For example, potential moderators that may exacerbate psychological distress include previous trauma history, maladaptive coping
strategies, negative social reactions, and various characteristics

46

Mental and Physical Health

associated with the assault. The most powerful mediators include


social cognitions involving self-blame and perceived control.
Evidence suggests that women who have been sexually victimized
in childhood or adolescence are more likely to be revictimized in
adulthood (Koss et al., 2003). A recent meta-analysis estimated that 15
to 79 percent of female child sexual abuse (CSA) survivors experience
rape as adults (Kaltman et al., 2005). According to Campbell et al.
(2008), women with a history of previous sexual violence who are
revictimized in adulthood have poorer mental health outcomes than
victims of a single instance of sexual assault. Indeed, these individuals
have signicantly elevated levels of PTSD and depression as compared to those without a history of previous trauma (Campbell, Greeson, Bybee, & Raja, 2008). Kaltman and colleagues (2005)
demonstrated that college sophomores who experienced CSA and
were then revictimized as adults, reported more Axis I diagnoses,
more comorbid diagnoses, including PTSD, depression, and general
distress than other victims without a history of prior abuse. A history
of CSA also has been found to prolong recovery among victims
(Krause, Kaltman, Goodman, & Dutton, 2008). For instance, longitudinal studies regarding the course of depression and PTSD suggest that
CSA contributes to chronic symptomatology and persistent, unremitting distress among individuals who were revictimized at some point
in adulthood (Koss et al., 2003).
The coping strategies utilized by women following sexual violence
are highly inuential in recovery. Research suggests that there are two
primary strategies an individual can employ when faced with a stressful event, approach coping and avoidance coping. Approach coping
is chosen when the individual decides that she has sufcient resources
to cope with her emotional reaction to the stressor (Littleton &
Breitkopf, 2006). Several of these strategies involve keeping busy,
thinking positively, obtaining support, and making life changes
(Draucker, 2001). In contrast, avoidance coping occurs when an individual lacks the coping resources necessary to handle the stressful situation (Littleton & Breitkopf, 2006). In such cases, the victim is likely
to implement strategies such as denying the existence of the stressor,
avoiding thoughts about the stressor, staying home, and withdrawing
from friends (Draucker, 2001; Littleton & Breitkopf, 2006). A growing
body of literature suggests that avoidance coping is correlated with
more severe psychological distress following sexual victimization
(Krause et al., 2008). For example, trying to forget about or block out
the assault is correlated with the prolonged recovery of survivors
(Ullman et al., 2007). Avoidance coping also has been associated with
increased PTSD symptom severity when compared to victims who
implemented other strategies (Krause et al., 2008; Ullman et al., 2007).
Although many victims employ avoidance coping, other victims who

Women and Sexual Violence

47

cope by keeping busy, thinking positively, and making life changes


(i.e., approach coping) actually report less psychological distress and
more favorable recoveries. Thus, the manner in which a victim copes
(i.e., avoidance or approach) with sexual assault may substantially
determine the severity of her symptoms and duration of her recovery.
Social reactions from friends and family members can mitigate or
magnify the psychological effects of sexual violence (Koss et al., 2003).
Several studies have documented that negative social reactions such as
blaming the victim, treating the victim differently, and trying to distract the victim result in prolonged recovery and increased psychological distress. Specically, when victims are blamed or treated differently
post-assault, PTSD symptoms are signicantly worse than when other
reactions are present. Attempting to distract victims from their pain by
telling them to move on with their lives or to stop talking about
the assault is also related to PTSD severity and long-lasting distress
(Ullman & Filipas, 2001). Although many peoples initial reaction to
rape and sexual assault is negative, research suggests that positive
social responses to victimization may result in a signicantly reduced
risk of anxiety, depression, PTSD, and suicidal ideation among sexual
assault survivors (Coker et al., 2002). Even if the victim merely perceives more positive social support from those around her, she is more
likely to report fewer psychological symptoms (Klump, 2006). Therefore, interventions targeting social support and appropriate reactions
toward victims are needed in order to promote more positive outcomes
in survivors (Campbell et al., 2008).
Certain characteristics associated with the attack also have been
linked to more negative psychological outcomes among victims
(Hedtke et al., 2008). These include the use of physical force or weapons, high perceived fear of death, physical injuries sustained during
the attack, and the victims relationship to the perpetrator (Koss et al.,
2003). In one study, rape victims with PTSD were more likely to have
been attacked by strangers, subjected to force or weapons, and sustained physical injuries than were victims without PTSD. Among a
national sample of sexual violence survivors, researchers discovered
that high perceived life threat and physical injury were related to more
PTSD symptoms as compared to controls (Ullman & Filipas, 2001).
Finally, Abbey, BeShears, Clinton-Sherrod, and McAuslan (2004) found
that women whose perpetrators used physical force as their primary
modality of victimization experienced the most extreme negative psychological consequences when compared with other victims.
According to Koss and colleagues (2002), unexpected acts such as
rape stimulate causal attributions, or attempts to answer the question,
Why did this happen to me? In response to this question, victims
may blame external forces, controllable features related to their
own behavior, or uncontrollable and enduring aspects of their

48

Mental and Physical Health

personalities. Research suggests that individuals automatically develop


specic ways of processing certain events in their lives. These processes, or social cognitions, include just world assumptions (i.e., bad
things happen to bad people) and beliefs about personal control, invulnerability, trust, self-esteem, and intimacy (Koss et al., 2002). When
incongruity exists between lived experience and social cognitions, individuals become distressed and attempt to resolve the conict by altering beliefs and modifying how the incident is interpreted. Typically,
victims of sexual assault rectify this incongruence by engaging in either
behavioral self-blame or characterological self-blame (2002). In order to
assess the effects of both characterological and behavioral self-blame on
psychological well-being, Koss et al. (2002) assessed victims attributions of responsibility (e.g., self-blame) and maladaptive beliefs (e.g.,
beliefs that result in maladaptive conclusions about the self and others
following a traumatic event) after sexual assault. Their ndings demonstrated that blaming ones own character for rape led to maladaptive
beliefs, which increased PTSD severity. Therefore, women who felt responsible for their rape developed beliefs such as the world is not a
safe place, and were in turn less likely to recover and more likely to
report long-term psychological distress (Koss et al., 2002). Other studies
also have documented this nding (Fraizer, 2003; Ullman et al., 2007).
In addition to self-blame and maladaptive beliefs, perceived control
concerning the past, the present, and the future may be a potential mediator between sexual assault and psychological sequelae. Perceived
past control refers to an individuals belief that one had control over
the occurrence of a traumatic event (Frazier, 2003). While researchers
have hypothesized that perceived past control may aid in recovery,
several studies suggest that past control is either unassociated with distress or associated with more distress among victims. According to
Frazier, past control is rarely helpful in recovery, primarily because it
is generally unrelated to measures of future control. Even if victims
believe that they had control over a negative event in the past, they do
not necessarily believe that they will have control over the same event
in the future. However, research does suggest that recovery from rape
is better among victims who do believe that they can prevent or avoid
a future occurrence of trauma (Koss et al., 2002). Other studies also
have documented that perceived future control is associated with lower
psychological distress among victims. Although future control can be
benecial, present control, which involves control over the recovery
process, has been found to be the most adaptive type of perceived control. Present control allows the victim to regain a sense of control in an
otherwise uncontrollable situation. Thus, focusing on control over the
recovery process may yield the most positive outcomes among victims
of sexual assault (Frazier, 2003).

Women and Sexual Violence

49

PHYSICAL RESPONSES
The tendency to report physical health symptoms following victimization has led to an increased rate of medical service seeking among
those who experience sexual violence (Ullman & Brecklin, 2003). Koss
and Kilpatrick (2001) reported that medical utilization among victims
increased by 31 to 56 percent approximately ve years after the attack,
compared to a 2 percent increase among nonvictims during the corresponding time period. Furthermore, among all female primary care
patients, 25 to 28 percent have a history of sexual violence, suggesting
that poor physical health is common among these individuals (Koss
et al., 1994). On standardized self-report measures of health perception
and functioning, victims report signicantly poorer health habits and
increased symptoms in all body systems except for the skin and eyes
(Resnick et al., 1997). Indeed, a number of complaints are diagnosed
disproportionately among survivors including physical injuries, gynecological disorders, gastrointestinal disorders, and chronic pain (Koss
& Kilpatrick, 2001).
Approximately 40 percent of victims receive nongenital, physical
injuries following an incident of victimization, and among those who
are injured, 54 percent seek medical treatment (Koss et al., 1994). The
most common injuries consist of abrasions to the head, neck, face,
thorax, breasts, and abdomen (Campbell, 2002; Goodman et al., 1993).
Other injuries such as bruising, contusions, bone fractures, and lacerations also have been reported (Cook, Dickens, & Thapa, 2005; Resnick
et al., 1997). Data from the National Womens Study documented that
only 4 percent of victims sustained serious injuries, suggesting that the
majority of injuries may be minor (Resnick et al., 1997). At least 50 percent of all victims treated in emergency departments report vaginal
and perineal trauma (Groer, Thomas, Evans, Helton, & Weldon, 2006).
According to Resnick et al. (2000), approximately 15 percent of women
who have been sexually victimized have signicant vaginal tears, with
1 percent requiring surgery to repair the damage.
The incidence of sexually transmitted diseases (STDs) among victims
of sexual violence is between 3.6 and 30 percent (Koss & Kilpatrick,
2001). The most prevalent STDs include gonorrhea, chlamydia, trichomonal infections, and syphilis. However, research also indicates that
victims may have an increased risk for hepatitis B and human immunodeciency virus (HIV) (Resnick et al., 2000). Although the rate of
HIV transmission due to rape is unknown, it is a great concern for a
majority of victims (Koss & Kilpatrick, 2001). One study found that 89
percent of women interviewed one month after their assaults mentioned fear of contracting HIV (Resnick et al., 2000). When assessed
several months following the attack, between 26 and 40 percent of victims spontaneously mentioned AIDS and HIV as a concern, and for

50

Mental and Physical Health

more than half of these individuals, it was their primary concern (Koss
et al., 1994). Finally, studies have consistently documented that approximately 5 percent of sexual assault cases result in pregnancy (Koss &
Kilpatrick, 2001; Resnick et al., 2000).
Gynecological disorders are the most frequently reported physical
health problem among victims of sexual violence. At one-year postassault, women continue to experience severe gynecological dysfunction (Campbell, Lichty, Sturza, & Raja, 2006). For instance, approximately 26 to 82 percent of victims report chronic pelvic pain
subsequent to the attack (Golding, 1999). In a study by Koss et al.
(1994), women who had undergone laparoscopy for chronic pelvic pain
were more likely to be victims of sexual assault than women who had
the procedure for other reasons. Chronic pelvic pain results in approximately 10 to 19 percent of all hysterectomies performed in the United
States, which could suggest that survivors may be more likely to have
this procedure than nonvictimized women (Koss et al., 1994). While
chronic pelvic pain is frequently reported among victims, other gynecological disorders and symptoms also have been mentioned. These
include dysmenorrhea, menorrhagia, dyspareunia, vaginal pain, premenstrual syndrome, urinary tract infections, vaginal bleeding, excessive vaginal discharge, painful intercourse, rectal bleeding, broids,
and multiple yeast infections (Bohn & Holz, 1996; Campbell, 2002;
Campbell et al., 2006; Chrisler & Ferguson, 2006; Koss et al., 1994).
While physical injuries, STDs, and gynecological disorders are commonly reported, other physical health disturbances also have been
documented. For instance, in eight studies, including one general population survey, the incidence of gastrointestinal disorders among victims ranged from 30 to 64 percent (Golding, 1999). Another study
found that approximately 44 percent of women evaluated at a gastroenterology clinic reported some type of sexual victimization in adulthood (Koss et al., 1994). Research suggests that nausea, vomiting,
diarrhea, constipation, spastic colon, irritable bowl syndrome, abdominal pain, and indigestion are among the most common gastrointestinal
problems reported by victims (Campbell 2002; Chrisler & Ferguson,
2006; Goodman et al., 1993). In addition, chronic pain disorders including headaches, back pain, facial pain, neck pain, temporal mandibular
joint discomfort, and bruxism are associated with sexual violence
(Campbell, 2002; Koss et al., 1994). Among women referred to a multidisciplinary pain center, 53 percent had a history of sexual abuse (Koss
et al., 1994). Golding (1999) reported that approximately 45 percent of
patients with chronic headaches, 69 percent of patients with facial pain,
and 46 percent of patients with chronic back pain had been sexually
victimized at some point in their lives. It is estimated that between 50
to 67 percent of women with bromyalgia and other musculoskeletal
disorders have a history of sexual assault (Golding, 1999). Sleep

Women and Sexual Violence

51

disturbances such as nightmares and insomnia, as well as cardiovascular disorders including hypertension, rapid heart rate, and chest pain
are also prevalent among victims (Chrisler & Ferguson, 2006). Finally,
additional research suggests that victims may be at an elevated risk for
infections including inuenza and colds (Campbell, 2002).
Since many of the physical symptoms associated with sexual violence have been established, researchers are now attempting to identify
factors that may explain this relationship.
Classical conditioning, assault characteristics, and psychological distress all have been proposed as causal links between abuse and negative health outcomes (Bohn & Holz, 1996; Resnick et al., 1997). When
an extremely distressing event (i.e., victimization) occurs, it becomes an
unconditioned stimulus that elicits an unconditioned, or automatic,
response (Resnick et al., 1997). This automatic response is characterized
by cognitive (e.g., perception that ones life is threatened or that ones
body is deled), behavioral (e.g., screaming, kicking, running, ghting,
or freezing), and physiological (e.g., alterations in respiration, heart
rate, gastrointestinal functioning, digestion, and muscle tension) components. Unfortunately, a powerful form of conditioned responding
occurs when automatic responses are paired with other cues associated
with the attack, such as the time of day the victimization occurred, the
sounds and smells present during the attack, the physical appearance
of the perpetrator, and dark shadows if the assault occurred at night
(Resnick et al., 1997). When this takes place, environmental cues
become learned or conditioned stimuli that have the capacity to elicit
the cognitive, behavioral, and physiological responses that automatically occurred during the initial trauma. Thus, physical reactions to
sexual assault such as abdominal distress, pain, nausea, increased heart
rate, shortness of breath, and shaking may become learned conditioned
responses to environmental fear triggers (i.e., dark shadows) and cause
victims to experience an increase in problematic symptoms (Resnick
et al., 1997). According to Resnick and colleagues (1997), as victims
continue to experience these health problems, they may be more likely
to develop chronic disorders such as bromyalgia and irritable bowel
syndrome.
A dose-response relationship may exist between sexual violence and
chronic health problems whereby numerous assault experiences result
in exacerbated symptoms (Ullman & Brecklin 2003). In one study conducted by Ullman and Brecklin (2003), correlates of past-year chronic
medical conditions were examined among women with different sexual
victimization histories identied from the National Comorbidity Survey. Findings suggested that for adult victims, more lifetime traumatic
sexual encounters were related to increased chronic medical conditions
compared to those who experienced a single act of sexual violence
(2003). Another study conducted by Campbell et al. (2006) reported

52

Mental and Physical Health

that the number of vaginal assaults alone was signicantly and positively associated with a higher frequency of pelvic pain, vaginal bleeding, discharge, painful intercourse, and painful urination. Those who
experienced multiple oral or anal assaults were more likely to report
chronic pain and gynecological disorders than those who did not
repeatedly experience these types of trauma (2006). Furthermore,
research has documented that victims who believed that their lives
were in danger during the assault were more likely to experience
severe health problems when compared to others devoid of such a history (Ullman & Brecklin, 2003).
Recent evidence suggests that psychological distress and mental
health sequelae may mediate the physical health conditions associated
with sexual violence. Among women recruited from a primary care
clinic, sexual assault victims had higher rates of medical complaints
only if they also reported psychological distress (Ullman & Brecklin,
2003). Specically, research suggests that PTSD symptoms are positively correlated with more severe physical symptoms following victimization (Groer et al., 2006). A study conducted by Zoellner,
Goodwin, and Foa (2000) assessed survivors with chronic PTSD in
order to determine if PTSD symptoms were related to negative health
outcomes. Results indicated that negative life events, anger, depression,
and PTSD severity all were related to an increase in self-reported
health symptoms. However, PTSD symptom severity predicted physical symptoms to a greater degree than the other variables. Thus,
although research has repeatedly demonstrated impairment of physical
health after sexual assault, it may not be the assault per se, but rather
the associated psychological sequelae, such as PTSD, that are responsible for the decline in victims well-being (Zoellner et al., 2000). Other
studies also have reported a relationship between PTSD severity and
physical health symptomatology (Campbell et al., 2008). For instance,
Rebecca Campbell et al. (2008) found that PTSD symptom severity was
more likely to be associated with persistent health conditions, especially those related to chronic pain, as compared to other factors. As
studies continue to document the role of PTSD as a mediator of physical health outcomes, the need for effective treatments and interventions
for PTSD becomes more apparent (Campbell et al., 2008).
NEUROLOGICAL RESPONSES
Initially, literature concerning the neurological sequelae associated
with sexual violence was limited to psychogenic seizures, fainting, and
convulsions (Campbell, 2002; Koss et al., 1994). In the last twenty years
however, research has documented that cognitive functioning, brain
structures, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activity, cortisol
levels, and recovery from traumatic brain injury (TBI) are different

Women and Sexual Violence

53

among victims of sexual violence when compared to nonvictims


(Campbell, 2002). Evidence suggests that victims of sexual trauma,
especially those with a diagnosis of PTSD, may experience cognitive
decits in attention, learning, memory, and executive functioning (Jenkins, Langlais, Delis, & Cohen, 2000; Stein, Kennedy, & Twamley,
2002). For instance, several studies have documented that survivors exhibit impairments on neurological measures of attention including the
Trail Making Test (Part B), the Digit Span and Digit Symbol subtests of
the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III), the Continuous
Performance Test (CPT), and the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task
(PASAT) (Jenkins et al., 2000). Jenkins and colleagues (2000) found that
victims who were diagnosed with rape-related PTSD were more likely
to demonstrate decits on measures of sustained and focused attention
relative to non-PTSD and nontraumatized controls. One theory explaining these results suggests that factors associated with PTSD such as trying to avoid intrusive recollections, sleeping poorly, and being easily
startled might result in impaired performance on tests of sustained
attention. Furthermore, dissociation, which can range from daydreaming to a complete trance state, is often observed among victims with
PTSD and may interfere with an individuals ability to concentrate and
remain focused on a specic task (Jenkins et al., 2000). According to
Stein et al. (2002), survivors who report PTSD symptoms are also likely
to experience problems with learning, memory, and executive functioning. In one study, visuoconstruction, visual memory, and executive
function all were impaired signicantly in sexual assault victims (Stein
et al., 2002). In addition, ndings from three large samples using standardized memory assessments have demonstrated that rape memories
are more affectively intense and negative than other memories when
compared to non-rape traumatic events (Koss & Kilpatrick, 2001).
As with cognitive impairments, research investigating differences in
brain structures has largely focused on the role of PTSD in mediating
these effects. Studies utilizing structural magnetic resonance imaging
among individuals with rape-related PTSD have consistently found
smaller hippocampal volumes compared to those without histories of
trauma. Among victims with recent-onset PTSD, right hippocampal
volume was signicantly smaller than controls (Wignall et al., 2004).
Furthermore, Wignall and colleagues (2004) found that whole brain
volume was signicantly smaller in traumatized individuals with PTSD
compared to nonvictims. Other studies have documented signicantly
smaller intracranial volumes, slightly larger ventricular volumes, and
smaller cerebral volumes in victims who have experienced sexual violence and subsequent PTSD symptomatology (Fennema-Notestine,
Stein, Kennedy, Archibald, & Jernigan, 2002). In an effort to determine
the effects of PTSD on the whole brain, Fennema-Notestine and colleagues (2002) examined volumes of specic brain regions in adult

54

Mental and Physical Health

women with a history of IPV, including sexual assault, compared to


women without serious trauma histories. Among the IPV women, half
had a current diagnosis of PTSD, and half had never reported PTSD
symptoms. Their results suggest that regardless of PTSD classication,
women who experienced IPV had signicantly smaller supratentorial
cranial vault volumes compared with controls. Additionally, frontal
and occipital gray matter volumes were signicantly smaller in all IPV
women, which was associated to increased Trails B time performance,
especially in women diagnosed with PTSD. Thus, ndings from this
study reveal that aspects of neuropsychological impairment in victims
of sexual violence may be rooted in frontal and mesial temporal abnormalities following victimization (Fennema-Notestine et al., 2002).
The HPA axis controls the bodys natural reaction to stress and has
been extensively studied in relation to traumatic events including sexual victimization (Girdler et al., 2007). When an individual is exposed
to a stressful situation, the hypothalamus becomes activated and begins
to release corticotropin-releasing factor, or CRF. This chemical acts at
the anterior pituitary gland to induce the release of adrenocorticotropic
hormone, ACTH, which in turn activates the adrenal cortex. Once the
adrenal cortex is activated, it stimulates the release of cortisol and other
glucocorticoids into the general circulation, which enhances energy
production to help the body deal with the stressful event. As the body
detects higher levels of glucocorticoids in the blood, a dual negativefeedback loop is initiated that directly reduces the release of CRF in
the hypothalamus while simultaneously acting on the hippocampus,
which also inhibits HPA functioning (Meyer & Quenzer, 2005). Consequently, it should not be surprising that research on the neurobiology
of sexual victimization has documented that abuse can result in longterm changes in the HPA axis, which may increase a victims risk for
negative psychological and physical health sequelae (Hedtke et al.,
2008). Although the results of these studies have been mixed, the majority suggest that sexual violence results in lower cortisol concentrations, leading to an overall hyporesponsiveness of the HPA system
(Girdler et al., 2007; Seedat, Stein, Kennedy, & Hauger, 2003). Rebecca
Campbell and colleagues (2008) have proposed that stress associated
with sexual violence triggers an acute response, which increases the
sensitivity of the negative-feedback loop in the HPA axis. When this
feedback system becomes overly sensitive to the presence of glucocorticoids in the blood, it suppresses both the hypothalamus and the hippocampus, which results in lowered cortisol levels (Campbell et al., 2008).
The long-term implications for lowered cortisol levels and HPA dysfunction are alarming. For instance, researchers hypothesize that
reduced activity of the HPA axis is associated with stress-related disorders such as chronic fatigue syndrome, chronic headaches, bromyalgia, rheumatoid arthritis, and abdominal pain. Since many of these

Women and Sexual Violence

55

disorders are reported as common sequelae among victims, dysfunction of the HPA axis and lowered cortisol levels may be another mediator between violence and health-related outcomes (Campbell et al.,
2008).
Finally, research suggests that there may be an association between
sexual victimization and recovery following traumatic brain injury
(TBI). According to Reeves, Beltzman, and Killu (2000), patients with a
history of sexual violence who have subsequently sustained a TBI frequently report reemergence of sequelae related to the abuse. These typically include intense ashbacks, behavioral disturbances, nightmares,
and hypervigilance. Indeed, such individuals who have not reported
PTSD symptomatology for many years may spontaneously re-experience
vivid, intrusive, and disruptive episodes of past sexual trauma following TBI. Even more disturbing, evidence suggests that victims who
have made the most adaptive recoveries from the violence, experience
the most disruptive PTSD-symptoms after TBI occurs (Reeves et al.,
2000). One possible explanation for this effect is that traumatized individuals develop a hyperaroused limbic system that over time may be
inhibited by prefrontal neural structures, especially those in the right
prefrontal area. However, in many cases of TBI, particularly those
involving a motor vehicle accident, bony protrusions on the interior of
the skull result in neurological insult to prefrontal brain tissue. Consequently, when the prefrontal area is damaged, it is unable to govern
the effects of hyperarousal in the limbic system, resulting in the reoccurrence of PTSD symptomatology. Since recovery is typically
prolonged in these individuals, future research is needed to better
understand the relationship of victimization on subsequent TBI (Reeves
et al., 2000).
BEHAVIORAL RESPONSES
The literature reviewed thus far provides ample evidence that sexual
violence against women results in long-lasting and pervasive psychological, physical, and neurological sequelae. In addition to these negative effects, research also suggests that victims experience behavioral
disturbances following victimization (Koss & Kilpatrick, 2001). For
example, survivors are more likely to smoke cigarettes, engage in risky
sexual activities, abuse substances, and exhibit negative social behaviors than other women (Resnick et al., 1997). Victims are even less
likely to wear seat belts while driving than those without a history of
sexual assault (Koss & Kilpatrick, 2001). According to Resnick et al.
(1997), the most signicant change in behavior subsequent to victimization is cigarette smoking. One study found that nearly 40 percent of
women with a history of sexual violence were current smokers, compared to 25 percent of nonvictimized women. Even when controlling

56

Mental and Physical Health

for the effects of race, education, and psychopathology, the prevalence


of current smoking among victims was signicantly higher than
controls.
In addition, studies also have documented that individuals who experience sexual violence are more likely to engage in risky sexual
behaviors such as having sex without using contraceptives (Neville &
Heppner, 1999). In a sample of adults selected on the basis of engaging
in risk behaviors for acquiring or transmitting HIV infection, approximately 54 percent of the women surveyed reported a history of sexual
assault (Koss et al., 1994). Several other studies also have found that
rape victims are more likely to engage in HIV risk behaviors including
prostitution, intravenous drug use, and sex with other drug users compared to nonvictims (Resnick et al., 1997).
Research suggests that survivors of sexual violence are more likely
to abuse substances than nonvictimized individuals (Sturza & Campbell, 2005), although the direction of this relationship remains unclear.
Substance abuse has been identied as both a precursor and a consequence of victimization, and several alternatives have been proposed to
explain this relationship (Champion et al., 2004). One theory suggests
that substance abuse heightens an individuals risk for subsequent sexual violence. For instance, women who abuse alcohol and drugs may
have an impaired ability to detect potential assailants compared to
women who do not abuse these substances. These women may be targeted by perpetrators because they are viewed as more vulnerable to
attack than other people (Champion et al., 2004). Other data support
these assumptions, documenting a higher prevalence of sexual victimization among substance abusers. In studies among individuals presenting to hospital emergency departments, higher rates of alcohol or
drug involvement have been related to injuries associated with sexual
assault and rape. Furthermore, longitudinal research has reported that
women who had used drugs in the previous year were signicantly
more likely to experience sexual assault before the next assessment period than women who were not drug users (H. Resnick et al., 2000).
A second theory indicates that victims abuse substances as a means
of coping with the negative consequences of violence (i.e., selfmedication) (Sturza & Campbell, 2005). After an assault, women may
increase their use of alcohol and drugs in order to reduce symptoms of
fear, anxiety, and depression (Resnick et al., 1997). Prospective data
from the National Womens Study has conrmed this theory, suggesting that sexual assault leads to substance abuse in previously nonusing individuals. Several studies conducted in accident and emergency departments also found that severe sexual violence preceded
both alcohol and drug abuse in most cases (Campbell, 2002). According
to Jacquelyn Campbell (2002), women with severe PTSD also may
abuse substances to cope with specic groups of symptoms that are

Women and Sexual Violence

57

particularly distressing such as intrusive recollections, nightmares, and


hyperarousal. Indeed, high comorbidity between assault-related PTSD
and substance use disorders has been consistently documented in several studies. For example, approximately 45 percent of individuals
diagnosed with PTSD subsequent to victimization also met criteria for
substance use or abuse disorders, with 31 percent meeting criteria for
alcohol abuse or dependence (Resnick et al., 2000).
A nal theory suggests that there is a reciprocal relationship
between sexual violence and substance abuse, whereby victimization
leads to substance abuse, which in turn leads to subsequent victimization. As mentioned, sexual assault may lead women to engage in
increased consumption of alcohol and drugs in order to alleviate negative sequelae. In turn, women who abuse substances may have an
impaired ability to detect potential assailants making them more vulnerable to attack. Therefore, those who abuse substances as a coping
behavior may be more likely to experience revictimization later in life
than other individuals (Champion et al., 2004).
According to Neville and Heppner (1999), sexual violence has a profound effect on a womans ability to continue to function in her various
life roles including partner, friend, mother, and worker. The impact of
violence on work performance persists for up to eight months. In some
cases, women lose their jobs or are unable to continue working because
factors associated with the victimization make it too difcult for them
to function at pre-assault levels. Changing phone numbers, moving,
staying home, and feeling suspicious of all male strangers are other frequent behaviors exhibited by assault victims (Neville & Heppner,
1999). Moreover, surveys have demonstrated that survivors of sexual
assault are less likely than other individuals to marry or to report at
least weekly contact with friends and relatives (Sarkar & R. Sarkar,
2005). A study conducted by Abdulrehman and De Luca (2001) found
signicant differences on the Social Dysfunction Rating Scale between
women with a history of sexual abuse and nonvictimized individuals.
For instance, women who were sexually abused as children had signicantly higher levels of social dysfunction and had a tendency to report
fewer satisfying relationships, friendships, and social interactions than
nonabused controls. Also, these individuals were more likely to report
higher levels of disinterest and lower levels of participation in community activities and affairs (Abdulrehman & De Luca, 2001). Clearly, sexual violence can have a signicant effect on victims social functioning
and behavior.
ORGANIZATIONAL RESPONSES
Despite signicant organizational encouragement, formal policies
and procedures to aid victims in recovery remain slow to appear in

58

Mental and Physical Health

primary-care settings, especially in the legal, medical, and mental


health systems (Koss et al., 2003). This is reected in research documenting that among all victims of sexual violence only 26 to 40 percent
report their assault to the police, 27 to 40 percent seek medical care
and forensic examinations, and 16 to 60 percent obtain mental health
services (Campbell, 2008). Furthermore, the reactions of these organizations to a victims traumatic experience can have profound implications
for recovery and healing. If women do not receive needed services
and/or are treated insensitively, post-assault help-seeking may become
a second victimization to the initial traumatic experience (Campbell,
2008). A comprehensive article by Rebecca Campbell (2008) discusses
organizational reactions to sexual violence, including the legal, medical,
and mental health systems, and negative sequelae that victims experience as a result of these responses. This information will be reviewed
in the following section.
When a woman seeks help following sexual victimization, her rst
encounter with the legal system is with a patrol ofcer who asks her to
describe the assault. She is then assigned to a detective who conducts
further investigation in order to determine if the case should be
referred to a prosecutor. During this stage, she is repeatedly questioned
about characteristics of the crime such as penetrations, use of force, or
other control tactics in order to check for consistency. Many report that
this questioning strays into issues involving what they were wearing at
the time of the attack, their prior sexual histories, and whether or not
they responded sexually to the assault. Victims rate these questions as
particularly traumatic and emotionally unsettling, as well as simultaneously reinforcing with respect to feelings of shame and guilt. Research
suggests that the investigation is designed to assess the prosecutorial
strength of the cases. Indeed, approximately 56 to 82 percent of all
reported rape cases are dropped (i.e., not referred to prosecutors) by
law enforcement.
If a case does progress past the investigation phase, prosecutors often conduct their own interviews with the victim before deciding to le
criminal charges. Prosecutors who are disinclined to charge typically
engage in a lengthy exploration of any discrepancies in the account of
the victim, pressing for explanations and proof. Those that do press
charges attempt to coach survivors to respond appropriately when
faced with difcult questions in court proceedings. Either way, these
women are forced consistently to relive the assault and defend their
characters to law enforcement personnel. In one study, interviews were
conducted among 47 victims whose cases made it to trial or plea bargaining. Findings indicated that approximately one-third of these
women felt inadequately prepared by prosecutors. Although they were
repeatedly questioned, they were given little information about what to
expect during their hearings. For every 100 sexual assault cases

Women and Sexual Violence

59

reported to law enforcement, on average 33 are referred to prosecutors,


16 are charged and moved into the court system, 12 result in a successful conviction, and 7 end in a prison sentence.
Many victims describe their encounters with the legal system as a
dehumanizing ordeal characterized by interrogation, intimidation, and
blame. Even those who had the opportunity to go to trial described the
experience as frustrating, embarrassing, and distressing. Research suggests that secondary victimization can result in negative effects on
womens mental health. For instance, in self-report characterizations of
their psychological well-being, survivors indicated that as a result of
their contact with the legal system, 87 percent felt bad about themselves, 71 percent felt depressed, 89 percent felt violated, 53 percent felt
distrustful of others, and 80 percent were reluctant to seek further help.
In a series of studies assessing victim/police contact, ndings suggested that insufcient legal action (i.e., the case did not progress or
was dropped) was associated with increased PTSD symptomatology.
Likewise, high secondary victimization was related to increased PTSD
severity.
Victims of sexual violence have extensive post-assault medical needs
such as treatment for physical injuries, forensic examinations, screening
for STDs, pregnancy testing, and emergency contraception. Although
the majority of women are not physically injured following their
attacks, law enforcement personnel, rape crisis centers, and social service agencies advise them to undergo a medical forensic exam. The
medical forensic exam or rape kit typically involves plucking head
and pubic hairs, swabbing the vagina, rectum, and mouth, and obtaining ngernail scrapings in the event that the victim scratched the perpetrator. Blood samples may also be collected for DNA, toxicology,
and ethanol testing. Because sexual assault is rarely life-threatening,
victims often experience long waits prior to the exam and are not
allowed to eat, drink, or urinate so that the physical evidence remains
intact. When these individuals are nally admitted, they receive a brief
explanation about the procedure and are often shocked to discover that
a pelvic exam is required immediately after being sexually violated.
Although forensic exams are the primary focus of hospital emergency departments, survivors also need information on the risk of
STDs, HIV, prophylaxis (i.e., preventative medication to treat any STDs
contracted during the assault), and pregnancy. Several federal agencies
recommend that all sexual assault victims receive STD and HIV prophylaxis on a case-by-case basis. However, meta-analyses of hospital
records suggest that only 34 percent of sexually victimized individuals
receive these treatments. In one study, victims who were raped by
someone they knew were less likely to receive information on STD or
HIV prophylaxis than those who were raped by a stranger. Another
study found that Caucasian women were signicantly more likely than

60

Mental and Physical Health

ethnic minority women to obtain information on HIV. Post-assault


pregnancy services were also inconsistently provided. Only 40 percent
to 49 percent of survivors receive information about the risk of pregnancy following sexual assault and 21 percent to 43 percent of women
who need emergency contraception actually receive it.
In the process of administering the forensic exam, STD services, and
pregnancy care, doctors and nurses ask questions about the assault
including what the victim was wearing, her sexual response, and what
she did to cause the attack. As with law enforcement, medical professionals may view these questions as appropriate. However, many
victims nd them very distressing. For example, negative responses
from the medical system signicantly exacerbated PTSD symptomatology in one sample of survivors. Another study found that women who
did not receive basic medical services rated their medical experience as
more hurtful, which was associated with increased PTSD severity. Specically, nonstranger assault victims who encountered secondary victimization and minimal services had the worst outcomes, even
compared to those who did not seek medical services.
Survivors of sexual violence may obtain mental health services from
treatment outcome research, community clinics, private practice, or specialized agencies. Women who receive services by participating as
research subjects typically receive high-quality treatment and numerous
benets. Unfortunately, this option is only available for individuals who
live in communities where research is being conducted and who t eligibility criteria. These treatments are not intended to provide large-scale
services but hope to establish empirically supported treatments that can
be utilized by many mental health professionals. Indeed, the most common way that victims receive services frequently involves care provided
by psychologists, psychiatrists, counselors, and social workers in private
or public community settings. Several studies indicate that women tend
to have positive experiences with mental health professionals and characterize the therapists help as useful and supportive. Research also suggests that community based mental health services are especially helpful
for victims who have had negative encounters with the legal and medical systems. For instance, survivors with rape-related PTSD who were
unable to obtain needed services from legal and medical professionals
had signicantly decreased PTSD symptomatology following mental
health services compared to controls. Furthermore, victims may also
receive services from specialized agencies such as rape crisis centers and
domestic violence shelters. Rape crisis centers help victims negotiate
with the legal and medical systems and provide individual and group
counseling. One study compared PTSD symptoms before and after
counseling among victims receiving services at a rape crisis center and
found signicant reductions in distress levels and self-blame compared
to other individuals.

Women and Sexual Violence

61

CONCLUSIONS
Sexual violence is prevalent in the United States, with 25 percent of
women reporting assault at some point in their lives (Campbell, 2008).
Research has consistently documented that victims experience intense
psychological distress immediately following the attack (Neville &
Heppner, 1999). The most common immediate reactions include fear
and anxiety. However, depression, PTSD, mental pollution, and sexual
dysfunction are also frequently reported among survivors (Bohn & Holz,
1996; Fairbrother & Rachman, 2004; Koss & Kilpatrick, 2001). Moderators
and mediators that explain the relationship between sexual violence and
psychological sequelae have also been documented in the literature.
These include previous trauma history, coping strategies, negative social
reactions, and characteristics associated with the assault, as well as social
cognitions involving self-blame and perceived control (Koss et al., 2003).
Furthermore, a number of physical health complaints are diagnosed disproportionately among survivors of sexual violence such as physical
injuries, gynecological disorders, gastrointestinal disorders, and chronic
pain (Koss & Kilpatrick, 2001). Several researchers suggest that classical
conditioning, assault characteristics, and psychological distress might
act as causal links between abuse and negative health outcomes; however, further research is needed to fully understand this relationship
(Bohn & Holz, 1996; Resnick et al., 1997).
A growing body of research has reported that sexual violence has
harmful effects on neurological functioning. Studies indicate that cognitive functioning, brain structures, HPA axis activity, cortisol levels, and
recovery from TBI are different among victims of sexual violence when
compared to nonvictims (Campbell, 2002; Fennema-Notestine et al.,
2002; Jenkins et al., 2000; Wignall et al., 2004). In addition, survivors of
sexual assault experience behavioral disturbances following victimization (Koss & Kilpatrick, 2001). These individuals are more likely to
smoke cigarettes, engage in risky sexual activities, abuse substances,
and exhibit negative social behaviors than other women (Resnick et al.,
1997). Research had documented that victims are even less likely to
wear seat belts while driving than those without a history of sexual
assault (Koss & Kilpatrick, 2001). The organizational impact of violence
has typically focused on the legal, medical, and mental health systems
reactions to survivors. Several studies have reported that negative
experiences with legal personnel and medical professionals exacerbated
PTSD severity. Victims who obtained mental health services subsequent to the attack had signicantly decreased PTSD symptoms compared to those who did not receive these services (Campbell, 2008).
Clearly, the evidence presented demonstrates that women who are sexually victimized experience a wide range of deleterious effects after the
initial violence perpetrated against them.

62

Mental and Physical Health

REFERENCES
Abbey, A., BeShears, R., Clinton-Sherrod, A. M., & McAuslan, P. (2004). Similarities and differences in womens sexual assault experiences based on
tactics used by the perpetrator. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28, 323332.
Abdulrehman, R. Y., & De Luca, R. V. (2001). The implications of childhood
sexual abuse on adult social behavior. Journal of Family Violence, 16,
193203.
Basile, K. C., Chen, J., Black, M. C., & Saltzman, L. E. (2007). Prevalence and
characteristics of sexual violence victimization among U.S. adults, 2001
2003. Violence and Victims, 22, 437448.
Black, M. C., & Breiding, M. J. (2008). Adverse health conditions and health
risk behaviors associated with intimate partner violence United States,
2005. Journal of the American Medical Association, 300, 646649.
Bohn, D. K., & Holz, K. A. (1996). Sequelae of abuse: Health effects of childhood
sexual abuse, domestic battering, and rape. Journal of Nurse-Midwifery, 41,
442456.
Campbell, J. C. (2002). Health consequences of intimate partner violence.
Lancet, 359, 13311336.
Campbell, R. (2008). The psychological impact of rape victims experiences
with the legal, medical, and mental health systems. American Psychologist,
63, 702717.
Campbell, R., Greeson, M. R., Bybee, D., & Raja, S. (2008). The co-occurrence of
childhood sexual abuse, adult sexual assault, intimate partner violence,
and sexual harassment: A mediational model of posttraumatic stress disorder and physical health outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 194207.
Campbell, R., Lichty, L. F., Sturza, M., & Raja, S. (2006). Gynecological health
impact of sexual assault. Research in Nursing & Health, 29, 399413.
Champion, H. L., Foley, K. L., DuRant, R. H., Hensberry, R., Altman, D., &
Wolfson, M. (2004). Adolescent sexual victimization, use of alcohol and
other substances, and other health risk behaviors. Journal of Adolescent
Health, 35, 321328.
Chrisler, J. C., & Ferguson, S. (2006). Violence against women as a public health
issue. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1087, 235249.
Coker, A. L., Smith, P. H., Thompson, M. P., McKeown, R. E., Bethea, L., &
Davis, K. E. (2002). Social support protects against the negative effects of
partner violence on mental health. Journal of Womens Health & GenderBased Medicine, 11, 465476.
Cook, R. J., Dickens, B. M., & Thapa, S. (2005). Caring for victims of sexual
abuse. International Journal of Gynecology and Obstetrics, 91, 194199.
Draucker, C. B. (2001). Learning the harsh realities of life: Sexual violence, disillusionment, and meaning. Health Care for Women International, 22, 6784.
Fairbrother, N., & Rachman, S. (2004). Feelings of mental pollution subsequent
to sexual assault. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 173189.
Fennema-Notestine, C., Stein, M. B., Kennedy, C. M., Archibald, S. L., & Jernigan,
T. L. (2002). Brain morphometry in female victims of intimate partner violence
with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 51,
10891101.

Women and Sexual Violence

63

Frazier, P. A. (2003). Perceived control and distress following sexual assault: A


longitudinal test of a new model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 12571269.
Gilboa-Schechtman, E. & Foa, E. B. (2001). Patterns of recovery from trauma: The
use of intraindividual analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 392400.
Girdler, S. S., Leserman, J., Bunevicius, R., Klatzkin, R., Pedersen, C. A., &
Light, K. C. (2007). Persistent alterations in biological proles in women
with abuse histories: Inuence of premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Health
Psychology, 26, 201213.
Golding, J. M. (1999). Sexual-assault history and long-term physical health
problems: Evidence from clinical and population epidemiology. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 191194.
Goodman, L. A., Koss, M. P., & Russo, N. F. (1993). Violence against women:
Physical and mental health effects. Part I: Research ndings. Applied &
Preventive Psychology, 2, 7989.
Groer, M. W., Thomas, S. P., Evans, G. W., Helton, S. & Weldon, A. (2006).
Inammatory effects and immune system correlates of rape. Violence and
Victims, 21, 796808.
Hedtke, K. A., Ruggiero, K. J., Fitzgerald, M. M., Zinzow, H. M., Saunders, B. E.,
Resnick, H. S., et al. (2008). A longitudinal investigation of interpersonal violence in relation to mental health and substance use. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 76, 633647.
Jenkins, M. A., Langlais, P. J., Delis, D., & Cohen, R. A. (2000). Attentional
dysfunction associated with posttraumatic stress disorder among rape
survivors. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 14, 712.
Kaltman, S., Krupnick, J., Stockton, P., Hooper, L., & Green, B. L. (2005).
Psychological impact of types of sexual trauma among college women.
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18, 547555.
Klump, M. C. (2006). Posttraumatic stress disorder and sexual assault in
women. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy, 21, 6783.
Koss, M. P., Bailey, J. A., & Yuan, N. P. (2003). Depression and PTSD in survivors of male violence: Research and training initiatives to facilitate recovery. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27, 130142.
Koss, M. P., Figueredo, A. J., & Prince, R. J. (2002). Cognitive mediations of
rapes mental, physical, and social health impact: Tests of four models in
cross-sectional data. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70,
926941.
Koss, M. P., Goodman, L. A., Browne, A., Fitzgerald, L. F., Keita, G. P., &
Russo, N. F. (1994). The physical and psychological aftermath of rape. No
safe haven: Male violence against women at home, at work, and in the community
(pp. 177199). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Koss, M. P., & Kilpatrick, D. G. (2001). Rape and sexual assault. In E. Gerrity,
T. M. Keane, & F. Tuma (Eds.), The mental health consequences of torture
(pp. 177193). New York: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Krause, E. D., Kaltman, S., Goodman, L. A., & Dutton, M. A. (2008). Avoidant
coping and PTSD symptoms related to domestic violence exposure: A
longitudinal study. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21, 8390.
Lee, E. & Kleiner, B. H. (2003). How organizations should respond to rape in
the workplace. Journal of Employment Counseling, 40, 123128.

64

Mental and Physical Health

Littleton, H., & Breitkopf, C. R. (2006). Coping with the experience of rape.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, 106116.
Meyer, J. S., & Quenzer, L. F. (2005). Psychopharmacology: Drugs, the brain and
behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
Neville, H. A., & Heppner, M. J. (1999). Contextualizing rape: Reviewing
sequelae and proposing a culturally inclusive ecological model of sexual
assault recovery. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 8, 4162.
Reeves, R. H., Beltzman, D., & Killu, K. (2000). Implications of traumatic brain
injury for survivors of sexual abuse: A preliminary report of ndings.
Rehabilitation Psychology, 45, 205211.
Resnick, H., Acierno, R., Holmes, M., Dammeyer, M., & Kilpatrick, D. (2000).
Emergency evaluation and intervention with female victims of rape and
other violence. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56, 13171333.
Resnick, H. S., Acierno, R., & Kilpatrick, D. G. (1997). Health impact of interpersonal violence II: Medical and mental health outcomes. Behavioral
Medicine, 23, 6578.
Resick, P. A. (1993). The psychological impact of rape. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 8, 223255.
Sarkar, N. N., & Sarkar, R. (2005). Sexual assault on woman: Its impact on her
life and living in society. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 20, 407419.
Seedat, S., Stein, M. B., Kennedy, C. M., & Hauger, R. L. (2003). Plasma cortisol
and neuropeptide Y in female victims of intimate partner violence. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 796808.
Stein, M. B., Kennedy, C. M., & Twamley, E. W. (2002). Neuropsychological
function in female victims of intimate partner violence with and without
posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 10791088.
Sturza, M. L., & Campbell, R. (2005). An exploratory study of rape survivors
prescription drug use as a means of coping with sexual assault. Psychology
of Women Quarterly, 29, 353363.
Ullman, S. E., & Brecklin, L. R. (2003). Sexual assault history and health-related
outcomes in a national sample of women. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
27, 4657.
Ullman, S. E., & Filipas, H. H. (2001). Predictors of PTSD symptom severity
and social reactions in sexual assault victims. Journal of Traumatic Stress,
14, 369389.
Ullman, S. E., Filipas, H. H., Townsend, S. M., & Starzynski, L. L. (2007).
Psychosocial correlates of PTSD symptom severity in sexual assault
survivors. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20, 821831.
Wignall, E. L., Dickson, J. M., Vaughan, P., Farrow, T. F., Wilkinson, I. D.,
Hunter, M. D., et al. (2004). Smaller hippocampal volume in patients with
recent-onset posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 832836.
Zoellner, L. A., Goodwin, M. L., & Foa, E. B. (2000). PTSD severity and health
perceptions in female victims of sexual assault. Journal of Traumatic Stress,
13, 635649.

Chapter 4

Cross-Cultural Violence against


Women and Girls: From Dating to
Intimate Partner Violence
Janet Sigal
Dorota Wnuk Novitskie

Violence against women is a worldwide problem that has generated


considerable research, political, and international interest, but that has
failed to be reduced or eliminated to a signicant degree even in the
twenty-rst century. In a recent opening speech at a United Nations
(UN) event during the Commission on the Status of Women in March
2009, the UN Secretary General Ban Ki Moon declared that the time
for talk is over; the time for action is now!
In this chapter, we will consider cross-cultural research, explanatory
models, and empirical research on violence against women and girls
within two contexts: dating violence and intimate partner violence
(IPV) or domestic violence. We conceptualize both areas on a continuum:
young girls, adolescents, or young adult women, possibly after being
exposed to violent interactions between parents or peers, become victims of dating violence. This experience may lead young women to
visualize violence between either dating partners or intimate partners
as normal or expected. Once they become involved with a man in a
long-term relationship, either by marriage or partnership, the violent
pattern may continue. At some point, women in these relationships
may accept the violence as normal, thus exhibiting attitudes that condone and tolerate the violent interactions.

66

Mental and Physical Health

In each of our sections, rst we will examine some of the denitions


associated with dating violence and IPV. In the following sections, we
will review models that can be used to explain these types of violence,
some estimates of statistics, empirical cross-cultural studies related to
each type of violence, consequences to victims, types of perpetrators,
and some proposed recommendations or programs to reduce or eliminate these behaviors.
Before addressing each type of violent pattern separately, some
issues relevant to both dating violence and IPV should be discussed.
To begin, accurate statistics are very difcult to determine even in
developed countries but particularly in developing countries. In some
areas of the world, neither dating violence nor IPV are considered
appropriate to discuss in public or even in private. As the World
Health Organization (WHO; 2005) report on violence against women
suggested, unless accurate prevalence gures are obtained, there cannot be any accountability by nations to reduce this type of violence. In
addition, often dating violence and IPV victims experience the same
consequences and may blame themselves for their victimization. Perpetrators also may exhibit similar characteristics in dating violence and
IPV settings.
There are, however, differences in both situations. Obviously, targets
of violence in dating settings are younger in general than in IPV, and
also may be less educated and established in society. In some parts of
the world, particularly in patriarchal societies, dating is prohibited, so
that violence in relationships may not occur until a couple is engaged
prior to an arranged marriage. In addition, although many countries
have specic laws or statutes against IPV, whether or not these laws
are enforced, dating violence often is covered under general violence
and assault, or sexual assault laws. Peer inuence or pressure may be
considered to have more of an impact on adolescent or young adults
than adults in a marriage or long-term partnership relationship, but
pressure also can emanate from cultural or societal contexts.
There are some other issues that we will emphasize in this chapter.
We believe that cross-cultural attitudes toward dating and IPV are important to ascertain because cultural attitudes can inuence whether or
not policy or laws against these types of violence are developed and
enforced, and even whether victims are willing to go forward with
complaints to the police or to the authorities. On a positive note, if attitudes informed by culture can be changed, then there is the possibility
that laws against violence in dating and IPV may be adopted. In addition, particular care to maintain condentiality and protect research
subjects must be adopted in these cross-cultural studies to prevent
harm from coming to victims who volunteer to participate. Finally,
approaches to reduce or eliminate these types of violence must include
men in these efforts. As Ban Ki Moon (March 2009) so strongly and

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

67

clearly emphasized, it must be understood that real men do not hit


women and also that real men do not stand by and watch other men
abuse young girls and women without intervening.
DATING VIOLENCE
According to the Bureau of Justice Special Report on Intimate Partner Violence in the United States (2000), women ages 16 to 24 experience the highest per capita rates of intimate violence. This rate
accounts for roughly 20 women out of 1000 (Rennision & Welchans,
2000). The report further indicates that one in three high school students will be, or already have been, in an abusive relationship, with
some high schools reporting up to 50 percent of their female students
having been abused by a signicant other. Such statistics are echoed in
a study published by the Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine (Roberts & Klein, 2003), which reported that one-third of the 920
students surveyed experienced dating violence during high school.
One out of ve college-aged women experience some form of dating
violence, and more than four in every ten incidents of domestic violence involves nonmarried but romantically involved individuals (Bureau of Justice Special Report: Intimate Partner Violence, May 2000).
Furthermore, in 1995, 7 percent of all murder victims were young
women who were killed by their boyfriends. Seventeen-year-old
Heather Norris met such a fate, when she was stabbed, dismembered
and discarded in trash bags at the hands of her boyfriend of three
years after several attempts to leave the relationship (Olson, 2009).
Recent news coverage of dating abuse in the United States has become
more prevalent, as illustrated by the widely circulated report of a domestic assault by her boyfriend Chris Brown that left singer Rihanna with
bruises and a scratch on her face (Blow, 2009). While these reports fall
short of offering specic details of the assault, media attention has
allowed new discussions to emerge regarding the need for prevention of
dating violence. Again, limited research is available regarding the link
between the media and dating violence; however, Manganello (2008) suggests that the media may serve as a risk factor in a teen violence. The
study further indicates that teens may be prone to spending time acting
out in real life that what they see on TV or on the Internet.
DEFINITIONS
Dating violence is the physical, emotional and verbal abuse of one
partner by the other partner in a romantic relationship. Abusive behavior is any act carried out by one partner aimed at hurting or controlling
the other. Dating violence occurs in heterosexual and homosexual relationships. A violent relationship means more than physical aggression

68

Mental and Physical Health

by a person who claims to love the other. Violence in a relationship


often is about power and control (Eaton et al., 2007). Research regarding violence in dating has been limited as the focus of interpersonal
violence historically has been on married persons. However, the same
type of violence happens between people who are dating, and at times
may even be more violent and hurtful. Teenagers are not as experienced in relationships and may feel pressured by peers to remain with
a violent partner due to reasons of popularity or status or simply for
the sake of having a relationship. Oftentimes teens are not aware of the
potential signs of a violent relationship. Such relationships may begin
with verbal and emotional abuse and control, which then may lead to
physical violence (Roberts & Klein, 2003).
CLASSIC MODELS OF TEEN DATING VIOLENCE
Some experts hold that men and women are equally aggressive. They
further suggest that this behavior should be seen as an extension of or
part of a larger pattern of family conict and discord. Supporters of this
viewpoint generally focus only on the studies that measure the number
of times a person perpetrates or experiences certain acts of violence,
such as pushing, slapping, or hitting. What is of interest is that these
studies tend to show that women admit to perpetrating slightly more
physical violence than men (Archer, 2000). The majority of studies that
investigate teen dating violence have focused mainly on using the violence act scales. Another school of thought in regard to dating violence is that men are more likely to cause serious injury to a woman,
especially when she is his intimate partner. Experts suggest that men
tend to come from patriarchal societies. Therefore, the use of violence is
a means of exerting and maintaining power and control over women
(Dobash & Dobash, 1980). This line of research tends to avoid act
scales, as act scales are not believed to accurately portray the nature
of violence in intimate relationships. This criticism is based on the lack
of consideration of the degree of injury inicted, coercion and controlling behaviors in these measures. Act scale studies do not consider
the fear experienced by the victim, or the circumstances under which
the acts occurred (Kimmel, 2002). These arguments further state that
studies using act scales lack information on the effects of power and
control and generally focus on more common and relatively minor
forms of aggression. The more severe, albeit relatively rare, forms of violence in dating and intimate partner relationships tend to be overlooked
(Dobash & Dobash, 1980). Researchers from this perspective use data
related to severe injuries, and generally conduct in-depth interviews
with victims and perpetrators (Archer, 2000). While the above views are
classic theories of dating violence, historically they stem from adult perspectives. Application to adolescent relationships may be problematic

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

69

and also not appropriate. Although these views of adult IPV can be informative and helpful in understanding some dynamics of teen dating
violence, they cannot be relied on to answer all questions. Therefore,
there is a need to better understand how teen dating relationships differ
from adult romantic relationships.
THEORETICAL MODEL OF DATING VIOLENCE
Riggs and OLeary (1989) proposed a model of dating violence. This
model offered a comprehensive background situational perspective that
drew on social learning theory. The model stipulated that behaviors
are learned by watching and imitating others, and then are maintained
through various forms of reinforcement. The following situational
variables were examined in this model: alcohol or drug use, use of
aggression by the partner, skills in resolving conict, and length of
relationship. This model also included the following contextual factors:
presence of aggression in dating relationship, exposure to aggression
by parent, view of aggression as an appropriate response to problems,
and prior aggression. Therefore the effect of situational and contextual
factors on teenage relationships was examined.
Luthra and Gidycz (2006) evaluated the Riggs and OLeary model of
dating violence using a number of surveys and measures. Their results
indicated that specic constructs can be useful in prediction of future
dating violence. However, they also found the model to be more predictive of females (83 percent) rather than males (30 percent). It should
be noted that the most signicant predictor of female violence was
presence of aggression by a male partner. These women were 108 times
more likely to act out violently against their partner, compared to those
women who were not physically assaulted. For men, however, the
most signicant predictor of perpetration of violence found was
the length of relationship. For every six months that the man was in
the relationship, his likelihood of aggression doubled.
FACTORS AFFECTING DATING VIOLENCE
Gender Power Differential
One striking difference between teen and adult romantic relationships
is the power differential between the man and the woman. Teen relationships tend to lack the elements traditionally associated with greater
male power in adult relationships (Wekerle & Wolfe, 1999). This lack of
power is a result of several factors. One factor is that unless there are
extenuating circumstances, teenage girls are not nancially dependent
on romantic partners. Unless there are children involved, and there is a
need to provide and protect, adolescent girls tend to be more dependent

70

Mental and Physical Health

on their parents. One study of seventh, ninth and eleventh graders in


Toledo (Giordano, 2007) interviewed boys and girls to investigate the
power dynamics in these relationships. The majority of the boys and
girls interviewed said they had a relatively equal balanced relationship in regard to power. However, in cases where a power imbalance
was noted, the female students were more likely to admit to having the
power over the boyfriend. The boys surveyed agreed, indicating that
they had less power than the girls did in the relationship. Furthermore,
boys who reported a presence of physical aggression felt even less
empowered than the boys who were not in physically aggressive relationships. The presence or lack of aggression did not make a difference
in the girls perception of power; girls reported no perceived difference
in power regardless of whether or not their relationships included physical aggression (Giordano, 2007).
LACK OF EXPERIENCE
Another major difference between violent adult and violent adolescent relationships is the lack of experience teens have in maneuvering
through their romantic relationships. If the teens in the relationships
do not know how to negotiate, communicate, and relate to their romantic partner, that can lead to reinforcement of poor coping strategies,
such as verbal and physical aggression (Dutton, 1995). A teen who has
not had much experience in romantic relationships may struggle to
express affection and may act out aggressively when he or she experiences frustration or jealousy toward the romantic partner. This concept
was supported by a study in which boys and girls participated in focus
groups on dating. The results found that physical aggression can stem
from an inability to communicate feelings and a decit in skills that
ordinarily lead to constructive methods of dealing with frustration
(Laursen & Collins, 1994).
As adolescents get older, their idealist view of a romantic relationship becomes more grounded in reality. In addition, with age, their
ability to experience closeness and intimacy increases (Montgomery,
2005). This change is positive, as adolescents who hold on to idealistic
beliefs about dating relationships can at times feel disillusioned
and may not be able to cope effectively with interpersonal conict
(Kerpelman, 2007). In addition, many adolescents express themselves
through aggression as they have not yet learned how to appropriately
express and experience intimacy and communication.
INFLUENCE OF PEERS
Friends are extremely important during the adolescent years. During
this time, friends exert more inuence on each other than at any other

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

71

time in their development. (Giordano, 2007). Research shows that peer


interactions and behaviors have a great impact on adolescents attitudes and behaviors, especially in relation to dating violence (Adelman
& Kil, 2007). In addition to this inuence, friends during the teen years
are present in day to day activities and can be very involved in a teenage couples social life, as teens tend to date in groups or double date
more than twice as much as they go on one-on-one dates (Molidor &
Tolman, 1998). Therefore, relationship dynamics tend to be very public,
and often are witnessed in groups of friends or during school time
hours. Boys and girls tend to act very differently when surrounded by
peers, to t into the expected norms. One study found that boys would
respond with physical aggression when hit by their girlfriend, just
to save themselves the embarrassment of being hit in front of peers
(Fredland et al., 2005).
Teens also argue about various issues. For example, many teens
argue about jealousy, how much time is dedicated to one another, and
threats of a new signicant other (Fredland et al. 2005). Although these
conicts can be viewed as normal and almost appropriate for this age
group, dealing with such issues when they are not emotionally and
developmentally ready can lead teens to experience conict, physical
aggression, and other problematic coping strategies, such as efforts to
gain control, stalking, and psychological or verbal abuse.
INTERNATIONAL STUDIES ON DATING VIOLENCE
Cross-cultural research on dating violence is limited. Much of the
data regarding this type of IPV comes from the International Dating
Violence Study, which although comprehensive in scope, is limited to
just one study (Straus, 2007). The countries involved in this study
include two African nations: South Africa and Tanzania; seven Asian
countries: China, Hong Kong, Taiwan, India, Japan, South Korea, and
Singapore; Australia and New Zealand; thirteen European countries:
Belgium (Flemish and French sites), Germany, Great Britain, Greece,
Hungary, Lithuania, Malta, Netherlands, Portugal, Romania, Russia,
Sweden, Switzerland (French and German sites); four Latin American
countries: Brazil, Guatemala, Mexico, and Venezuela; two Middle
Eastern countries: Iran and Israel; and two North American countries:
Canada (Anglo and French sites) and the United States (Mexican American, historically black colleges, and other sites) (Straus, 2007).
Straus (2007) investigated the widely held beliefs that interpersonal
physical violence is almost entirely perpetrated by men, and that the
major risk factor for interpersonal violence is male dominance in the
relationship. Straus tested his hypothesis across the aforementioned 32
countries and surveyed 13,601 university students. The results showed
that almost one-third of the female as well as male students physically

72

Mental and Physical Health

assaulted a dating partner in the previous 12 months, and that the


most frequent pattern was bidirectional where both partners were violent, followed by female-only violence. Violence by only the male
partner was the least frequent pattern according to both male and
female participants. Straus also investigated whether dominance by
one partner is a crucial aspect of the etiology of partner violence. The
results showed that dominance by either the male or the female partner is associated with an increased probability of violence. These
results question the assumption that interpersonal violence is primarily
a male crime and that women are only violent at times of self-defense
(Straus, 2007).
Using the information from the above study, Hines and Straus
(2007) also evaluated the relationship between binge drinking and
aggression in dating relationship. The study sampled 7,921 students at
32 universities worldwide. The strongest positive association between
binge drinking and violence was found in Pune, India. Similar ndings
also emerged in Mississippi, United States, and Leicester, England. No
gender differences were found.
Another focus of the data from the International Dating Violence
Study was an evaluation of gender in the prevalence and chronic
nature of dating aggression (Straus & Ramirez, 2007), with a focus on
participants from universities in Mexico and America. Findings indicated that students, across the four universities surveyed, experienced
dating violence anywhere from 29.7 percent to 46 percent in the last 12
months, with students in Juarez, Mexico, reporting the highest percentage. Chronicity of severity of assaults was consistent across the four
universities. Another interesting nding was that when only one partner was violent, the female was almost twice as likely (19 percent) as
the male (9.8 percent) to perpetrate the violence.
Lysova and Douglas (2008) also utilized the data from the International Dating Violence Study to focus on dating violence among
Russian college students. Their ndings indicated no gender difference
regarding victimization (females: 23.1 percent; males: 28.6 percent).
However, statistical signicance was found between the genders
regarding reporting having assaulted an intimate partner, with 35.6
percent of females reporting acts of aggression and 20.6 percent of
males. Although not statistically signicant, women reported higher
rates of psychological aggression (66.7 percent) compared to men (56.5
percent). The authors suggested that men in Russian universities either
report or commit lower rates of dating violence due to fear of expulsion from universities or forced enlistment in the military.
Doroszewicz and Forbes (2008) also utilized the same data pool and
focused on physical aggression and injury among college age students
in Poland. The ndings indicated that 15 percent of men and 25 percent
of women reported having perpetrated physical violence against a

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

73

partner at least once. Few other gender differences were noted; however,
women did report engaging in more frequent psychological aggression
than men. The authors attributed the ndings to high rates of domestic
violence in Poland, suggesting that violence is a learned behavior. In
addition, recent and rapid changes in womens role in the home and in
the workplace are believed to have contributed to these rates.
Wang and Petula (2007) conducted interviews and focus groups to
explore mens and womens views of physical aggression by women in
dating relationships. The sample was limited to 20 participants (13
women and 7 men) and offered insight into womens identication
with an aggressive female character in a popular Asian movie Sassy
Girl. Female participants identied with the aggressive character, and
to some extent were reported to appreciate the aggression perpetrated
by this character, often justifying and defending her behavior. Men on
the other hand, in an attempt to maintain levels of masculinity,
reported the womans aggression to be playful and not hostile. Furthermore, the men surveyed reported that men cannot be hurt by such
physical contact and cannot be defeated by such challenges (p. 628).
In another cross-cultural study, Sherer (2009) focused on dating violence in Jewish and Arab male and female teens in Israel. The study
examined the effect of sociodemographic variables on male and female
behavior. The sample consisted of 1,357 Arab and Jewish youths who
were surveyed on measures of threatening, relational, physical, sexual
and verbal abuse. Dating violence was found to be high among teens
in Israel, with the highest rates found among Arab youths. Although
girls were involved in every type of dating violence, boys exhibited
higher scores. The author explained that such prevalence can be understood in terms of cultural norms and expectations of roles of men and
women in society.
VIOLENCE IN ENGAGED COUPLES
Halford, Sanders, and Behrens (2004) studied the relationship
between physical aggression in engaged couples and the presence of
violence in the family of origin in Australia. The researchers surveyed
71 engaged couples. Men exposed to parental violence experienced more
negative emotions and thoughts, and found it difcult to participate in
subsequent focus group discussions. Couples in which only the woman
was exposed to parental violence did not appear to have concerns
regarding negative affect or conict between the two engaged people.
Haj-Yahia (2006) also examined violence perpetrated against Arab
women in Israel who were engaged. Of 1,111 engaged Arab women in
the sample, between 1 and 11 percent of the women reported experiencing physical aggression, and between 8 and 48 percent reported experiencing psychological aggression. When conicts with their partner were

74

Mental and Physical Health

not discussed or worked through, these women were more likely to suffer from low self-esteem, depression, stress and anxiety.
Haj-Yahia and Edleson (1994), in an earlier related study, surveyed
Arab-Palestinian men living in Israel as to their means of working
through conict with their ancees. The research focused on the following three frameworks: male dominance, intergenerational learning,
and interpersonal skills decits to determine the role played by these
factors in mens relationships. Haj-Yahia and Edleson found that men
who did not come from violent homes were more likely to engage in
discussion and reasoning as a means of resolving conict, and were
more open to an egalitarian household. On the other hand, men who
grew up in violent homes were more likely to be verbally and physically abusive toward their ancees, were less likely to see what was
wrong with their actions, and were more likely to hold patriarchal
views.
OUTCOMES
Effects on Mental Health
Chan, Straus, Brownridge, Tiwari, and Leung (2008) evaluated the
prevalence of dating violence and suicidal ideation among male and
female university students. Information also was obtained from the
International Dating Violence Study, with a focus on the prevalence of
physical assault, sexual coercion, and suicidal ideation among these
students. The authors sampled 16,000 students from 22 universities in
21 countries. The results showed that although there were large differences among countries, even the lowest rates of dating violence were
quite high. Male and female students had similar rates regarding the
proportion of having physically assaulted a partner, and having been a
victim of sexual coercion. An increased rate of suicidal ideation was
found among both perpetrators and victims of physical assault. This
nding was linked to the occurrence of depression. This study highlighted a need for universal screening and targeted services for violence, depression and suicide prevention.
Effects of Abusive and Neglectful Childhood
Straus and Savage (2005) investigated the relationship between
neglectful parents and the child becoming involved in a violent dating
relationship. The study was conducted with university students from
17 countries, and the data was once again collected through the International Dating Violence Study. Students in Pusan, Korea, reported
experiencing the highest level of childhood neglect (34.4 percent) followed by 28.6 percent of students in Hong Kong. The lowest reported

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

75

prevalence of neglect was in New Hampshire (3.2 percent). Straus and


Savage also found that the more neglectful behaviors that the student
had experienced, the more likely they were to have assaulted a dating
partner. The link between neglect experienced as a child and dating
violence was strongest at universities where dating violence was more
prevalent.
TYPOLOGIES OF ADOLESCENT DATING
VIOLENCE PERPETRATION
Foshee et al. (2007) interviewed 116 boys and girls who were previously identied to have been perpetrators of dating violence. Using
narrative descriptions, the authors developed typologies of dating violence perpetration that were context specic. Most of the boys acts of
violence were attributed to escalation prevention, or attempts to stop
a girlfriend from inicting further violence. Female perpetration was
identied by motive, preceding events, and abuse by parents. From
these reports, the following four types of perpetrators were proposed:
patriarchal terrorism response, anger response, ethic enforcement, and
rst-time aggression response.
Patriarchal terrorism response was created from responses that girls
offered indicating that the boyfriends attempted to control them
through physical or psychological means. Acts committed by the girls
that were coded in this typology included the following: described
physical violence against a boyfriend who had historically been psychologically or physically abusive, or acting violently against a boyfriend immediately after he was physically or psychologically abusive.
It should be noted that 38.5 percent of females met the criteria for
patriarchal terrorism response.
Anger response differed from the type listed above in motive, precipitating events, and history of violence. For an act to be labeled as
anger response, the description given by the female had to meet the
following criteria: no indicated history of past physical abuse by the
boyfriend, no report of immediately precipitating violence by the boyfriend before the violent act, or the girl needed to report that the
reason for such violence was motivated by a desire to be violent.
Twenty-ve percent of females met the criteria for anger response.
Ethic enforcement was reported in 19.2 percent of female participants. For ethic enforcement to be coded, the female had to report the
following: violence was used to communicate to the boyfriend that he
had done something wrong and she was not going to accept it, no evidence of history of abuse by boyfriend was noted, and there was no
report of violence being used against the girl immediately before her
violent act. Some common wrongs that resulted in the girls violence
included the following: cheating, irting with another girl, indulging in

76

Mental and Physical Health

too much alcohol or drugs, talking ugly to her, removing himself


from an argument, pressuring her to have sex, and ruining prom night.
First time aggression response was reported in 17.3 percent of the
females surveyed. For the act to be labeled as rst time aggression, the
following had to be reported: there was a report of the boy using physical violence against the girl immediately before the act, and there was
no history of physical aggression by the boy prior to this incident. In
these instances, most of the girls surveyed reported their aggression to
be attributed to self-defense, while others reported retaliation against
the boy for wronging them.
For boys, most of their acts were identied as escalation prevention or attempts to stop the escalation of violence by their female
counterparts. For the acts to be labeled as such, a male had to report
the following: he used restraint while the girlfriend was being violent
against him, he attempted to stop her from harming him with a
weapon, or stop the possibility of having violence against him. Other
acts were noted by the authors; however no commonalities were noted
to offer additional specic typologies.
RECOMMENDATIONS AND PROGRAMS
Black et al. (2008) evaluated factors that resulted in teenaged victims
of dating seeking help. The authors found a large risk factor for not
seeking help was when the violence occurred in isolation, with no witnesses. However, when the violence was witnessed or observed by a
third party, the victim was more likely to begin to speak about the violence, become open to the possibility of intervention, and seek help. In
addition, boys were less likely than girls to seek help. In light of such
ndings, Safe Dates programs have gained greater popularity. After
a four-year study, participants in safe date programs have seen a
decrease in physical, serious physical, and sexual dating violence perpetration and victimization (Foshee et al., 2007).
Wekerle and Wolfe (1999) conducted a literature review of six relationship violence prevention programs designed for and practiced with
teenage students. One of the programs discussed was in a larger community, while the remaining ve programs were school-based. Prevention of dating violence was targeted on two levels: toward all high
school students, as well as toward selected adolescent populations,
such as youths with histories of maltreatment, or problems with peer
violence. These programs addressed specic skills and knowledge that
opposed the use of violent and abusive behavior toward intimate partners. In addition, one program addressed interpersonal violence more
generally, and was also included in this review because of its implications for dating violence initiatives. Positive changes were found
across studies in violence-related attitudes and knowledge. In addition,

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

77

positive gains were noted in self-reported decreases in perpetration of


dating violence, with less consistent evidence in self-reported victimization. Limited follow-up and generalizability was noted; therefore the
possibility of greater application is questionable.
Foshee et al. (2001) studied predictors of dating violence. The study
focused on eighth and ninth graders in one county in a U.S. school.
These participants were asked to complete baseline questionnaires and
were again evaluated 18 months later. Some risk factors found for
males being abused by females in a dating relationship included having friends who were victims of dating violence, using alcohol, and
being of a race other than white. However, risk factors for males perpetrating the violence included attitudes that condoned dating violence.
The researchers also suggested that any interventions that are implemented for school-aged children should be separate and specic for
males and females. In addition, those found to be at greater risk should
be targets for intervention rst, with other interventions becoming
more available only when there is funding.
Valls, Puigvert, and Duque (2008) reported that schools in Spain are
working to implement preventive socialization in the eld of education.
The authors proposed that a correlation exists between prevention of
dating violence measures and promotion of power equality in relationships. They suggested that the current presentation of violence in the
media is gloried and made attractive to young teens. Therefore, work
should be done to make nonviolent movies, videos, and games just as
attractive and normative. The authors suggested that this approach also
would undermine double standards in relationships, and healthy care
and passion could be viewed as a possible replacement for aggression.
IPV: A CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE
The WHOs (2005) Summary Report on its multi-country study on
IPV and violence against women, stated that Since the World Conference on Human Rights held in Vienna in 1993, and the Declaration on
the Elimination of Violence Against Women in the same year, civil
society and governments have acknowledged that violence against
women is a public policy and human rights concern (p. vii). The Foreword of the Report further stated that Violence against women is both
a consequence and a cause of gender inequality (p. viii). CEDAW, or
the Convention on the Elimination of Discrimination against Women,
is a UN tool designed to address this issue.
DEFINITIONS
In 1993, the UN General Assembly adopted the following denition
of violence against women: Any act of gender-based violence that

78

Mental and Physical Health

results in, or is likely to result in, physical, sexual, or psychological


harm or suffering to women, including threats of such acts, coercion,
or arbitrary deprivation of liberty, whether occurring in public or private life (UN, 1993, Article 1).
Although recently, considerable research attention has been focused on
whether IPV or domestic violence is perpetrated only by men against
women, or can be identied with either partner in a marriage or relationship (Archer, 2000; Frieze, 2005), for the purposes of this chapter, we will
restrict our discussion to IPV perpetrated solely by men against women.
MODELS OF IPV
Feminist Power Model
In this classic model, male-dominated societies are viewed as reinforcing traditional gender roles in which men are the wage earners and make
all the decisions, and women stay at home and take care of the husband
and the family. As a result, men have more power and status than women
(Sigal & Annan, 2008). As described above, the WHO Report (2005)
emphasized inequality between men and women as a cause of IPV.
Sexist attitudes also can provide a foundation for gender-based violence that is related to the inequality between men and women. For
example, the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (ASI) which was developed
by Glick and Fiske (1997) distinguished between Hostile Sexism, measuring hostility toward the equality of women and men, and Benevolent
Sexism, which is rather paternalistic and implies that women are weak
and fragile and must be protected. In each case, women are seen as
having inferior status. It was established in cross-cultural studies using
the ASI, that higher scores on this measure were associated with more
gender inequality in the countries sampled.
The Gender Equity Index (2008) has compiled a list of gender equity
in many countries around the world and will be referred to again later
in this chapter.
Hypermasculinity
This model is an extension of the feminist power model. According
to this approach, macho men often see violence as an acceptable
way to achieve and maintain power over women. In addition, these
individuals may associate violence against women with being more
masculine (Kilmartin & Allison, 2007).
Patriarchal Culture and Culture of Honor Models
Both of these models stem from an extension of the feminist power
models conception of women having inferior status.

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

79

Patriarchal Culture
As Haj Yahia (2002) describes in his article, the patriarch, or male
head of the family, has complete control over the family including
making all the decisions for the family. In particular, since patriarchal
societies often view men as sexual predators (although without the
condemnation that this behavior would produce in other types of cultures), their women must be protected at all times. Therefore, they cannot go food shopping without a male relative or even seek medical
help unless their husband allows them to do so. In fact, after the birth
of a child, many women lose their lives because of their inability to
access emergency medical help.

Culture of Honor
This type of culture is an extreme version of a patriarchal society.
As described by Vandello and Cohen (2003), in these cultures, the
honor of the male head of the family is directly related to the purity
or chastity of the women in his family. If a woman strays, either by
committing adultery, being raped, or being seen in the company of
men who are not relatives, the patriarch is required to punish her,
often violently. The patriarch must wash the honor with blood to
restore his honor and that of his family (p. 999). In many culture of
honor societies, the women in the family are under the control of their
fathers until they are married, under the control of their husbands
when they are wives, and under the control of their sons when they
are widows. In extreme forms, the punishment may take the form of
an honor killing. As a result of increased punishment for honor killings in some countries (for example, Jordan), some young women in
rural areas have been locked in a room with a gun until they kill themselves, an act which is now termed honor suicides.
There have been many egregious examples of violence against
women in honor cultures, but we will just mention a few to illustrate
our description of these models.
A few years ago, in Saudi Arabia, a girl was in a car with her boyfriend when she was raped by a number of men. She was sentenced to
160 lashings and possibly a jail term, but after the case attracted media
attention worldwide, the King pardoned her. More recently, in 2009, a
woman in her seventies in Saudi Arabia walked outside her house with
two workmen and also was sentenced to several lashings. Media attention so far has not been successful in this case.
Another example was in the fall of 2008 in Pakistan. Three girls
refused to marry older men chosen for them by their fathers (they were
teenagers and the men were in their 40s and 50s), because they wanted
to choose their own husbands. These young girls, and two older

80

Mental and Physical Health

women who supported them, were all buried alive. Finally, in the
spring of 2009, a 17-year-old girl was repeatedly lashed by several men
in a rural area of Pakistan for an unknown crime which many suggested was fraudulent. However, after cell phone photographs were
sent all over Pakistan, the incident caused a tremendous outcry, both
in Pakistan and throughout the world. The Pakistan chief judge was
not satised with the legal response to this outrageous violent behavior
and sent a government ofcial to the area to report on what actually
occurred.
INTERNATIONAL STUDIES ON IPV
In this section, we will review a selective number of cross-cultural
research investigations on the prevalence of IPV and attitudes toward
IPV. We will relate these studies to models and explanations described
in an earlier section. Clearly, all cross-cultural researchers agree that
estimates of IPV most likely represent underreporting worldwide. In
addition, comparison of prevalence gures is extremely difcult and
any conclusions must be made with caution, due to the variability of
the methods used in these estimates.
In some of the studies that we will review, the focus has been on
attitudes toward IPV rather than on the prevalence of IPV. However,
IPV prevalence estimates tend to be highly correlated with attitudes
condoning or even supporting IPV. If attitudes are positive toward
violence against women, it is likely that women in these cultures will
experience IPV to a higher degree than women in cultures that disapprove of and punish perpetrators of IPV.
One of the most signicant investigations of IPV cross-culturally
was sponsored by the WHO in 2005. This study encompassed
data from women residing in several countries, and in 15 locations
within these countries. Interviewers obtained data from countries representing a widely diverse sampling including 24,000 respondents from
Bangladesh, Brazil, Ethiopia, Japan, Namibia, Peru, Samoa, Serbia and
Montenegro, Thailand, and the United Republic of Tanzania. Lifetime
prevalence of IPV ranged from 15 percent (Japan) to just over 70 percent. Prevalence in the year before the study again revealed large variability among samples. The lowest IPV rate was in Japan, with just
under 5 percent, and the highest was in Ethiopia, which had a rate of
almost 55 percent.
There were many advantages of this worldwide study of IPV including the sponsorship of this major international health organization,
WHO, the collaboration of a wide variety of experts and womens
organizations, the very large sample size, the variety of countries
involved in the project, and the use of standardized measures and intensive training of female interviewers because of the sensitivity of the

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

81

topic. However, there were several criteria that were used to determine
which countries would be included in the study that may limit the
generalization of the results. Prior to sampling women in a particular
country, the researchers determined if there was a positive political
atmosphere in the country which would suggest that the government
would respond positively to the survey, there had to be a lack of information on the amount of IPV in the country, and womens groups had
to be active in the country in order to turn the data-collecting phase
into the stage of implementation of the recommendations stemming
from the report. Although these criteria were reasonable, the question
of whether the results may generalize to other countries which may
not exhibit these same characteristics is at issue. A further question
related to the methods of the study, is that the emphasis was placed
on physical and sexual IPV, despite the suggestion in the report that
emotional or psychological abuse often is associated with physical
abuse, and can have long-lasting negative consequences. However,
despite these considerations, the study is one of the best in terms of
careful planning, training and collection of data in so many countries
using standardized measures.

ASIAN STUDIES
Hong Kong and Mainland China
Prevalence of gures from Hong Kong and mainland China vary.
Xu et al. (2005) sampled about 600 women (with close to a 90 percent
acceptance and return rate) at a clinic in Fuzhou, China. Face-to-face
measures modied from the WHO multi-country study were administered to determine the percentage of IPV in the sample. Results indicated that 43 percent of women from a sample in mainland China
reported having experienced IPV in the lifetime of their marriage, and
26 percent had been abused in the last year. Marital conict and nancial control by husbands were two of the factors associated with IPV.
Attitudes were supportive of wife beating: 36 percent of the women
surveyed believed that wife-beating was appropriate and approved of
if the wife had committed adultery. Although the large sample supports the validity of the conclusions, limitations of the study include
the self-report nature of the methodology and the lack of participation
of men. One other interesting conclusion stemmed from the research.
Xu et al. suggested that although the Chinese society supposedly now
supports more economic equality between men and women (women
should hold up half of the sky, p. 84), their respondents did not
appear to have internalized or accepted this concept.
Another recent investigation by Chan, Brownridge, Tiwari, Fong,
and Leung (2008) concluded that IPV still is a signicant problem in

82

Mental and Physical Health

Hong Kong and Chinese societies. As part of a larger study employing


face-to-face interviews in Hong Kong (there was a very high response
rate of about 70 percent), the authors selected a sample of 1,870 women
and included an additional question on the effect of in-law conict on
IPV. The larger study utilized the Revised Conict Tactics Scale
(CTS2), an often-used standardized measure, to investigate prevalence
of IPV. Prevalence gures in this study were lower than in the Xu
et al. investigation (lifetime gures were approximately 8.5 percent,
and the previous year gures were about 4.5 percent). Since the Chan
et al. study was conducted in Hong Kong and the Xu et al. study in
mainland China, it is difcult to directly compare these gures.
Although Hong Kong has experienced more of a Western inuence
than mainland China, it still is questionable whether either self-report
gure is accurate.
In the article, Chan et al. suggested that common risk factors that
have been discovered in other countries also may be important in the
explanation of IPV victimization in China. The main risk markers in
the Chan et al. study included a younger age of the wife, and nancial
situations being unfavorable in the home, as well as the partners alcohol abuse. An interesting nding was that if the wife had a higher
income she was more at risk for abuse. The authors offered the explanation that a nancially successful wife might demand more equality
in the home, and that might cause increased conict with her husband.
Other studies have produced conicting results regarding the relationship between the wifes education and income and prevalence of IPV.
The major contribution of the Chan et al. research was to investigate
the role of in-law conict in IPV. The authors found that this type of
conict, particularly the involvement of the mother-in-law, was an important risk factor in predicting increased IPV in these families.
Although this result may seem inconsistent with the stereotyped view of
the Chinese culture as a patriarchal society, the authors suggested a
complex interpretation of this relationship. According to Chan et al.,
Chinese womens status in the family does not change when their sons
get married; they are still under the domination of their husbands to a
large extent and of their sons as well. However, since Chinese women
may be assumed to identify with the men in their family, if their sons
are engaging in IPV, the mother-in-law may feel constrained to participate or at least to condone and encourage her son to continue abusing
his wife. Another suggested explanation of the complex role that in-law
conict may play in Chinese marriages, is that if there is conict with
the husbands family, then the wife can be accused of disrespecting her
husbands family, which can be used as a justication for IPV. However,
conict between the husband and the wifes family may lead to a diminished amount of time spent with that family, which can reduce the
amount of support the wife can experience from her own family.

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

83

Although this study is quite recent, and the sampling procedure and
return rate were excellent, as the authors pointed out, the examination
of the role of in-law conict was based on one question which could be
interpreted in different ways. As in all these cross-cultural studies, selfreport measures were used, and only women were sampled.
Japan
Weingourt, Maryama, Sawada, and Yoshino (2001) conducted a selfreport survey in Japan, which was completed by approximately 180
women with a relatively low response rate (close to 25 percent). As an
explanation of this low response rate, the authors pointed out that to
avoid offending respondents by pressuring them to participate in a
study on such a sensitive issue, the researchers avoided contacting
individuals who did not send back the survey. The results of the survey indicated that in this middle-class sample in Sapporo in the north
of Japan, close to 60 percent were psychologically abused by their partners, around 30 percent were physically battered, and about 25 percent
were sexually abused. Very few of these women told anyone about
their experienced abuse. The authors explained this lack of disclosure
by theorizing that IPV is viewed as the fault of the woman who thinks
she has been a bad wife, and therefore she is fearful about disclosing
the abuse to anyone. In addition, the wife is considered a property
of the husband and thus he is permitted to do anything to punish her
if she has not been a good wife. This explanation ts within the framework of the patriarchal society discussed earlier.
In addition to the lack of follow-up mailings which contributed to
the relatively low response rate in the Weingourt et al. survey study,
the authors indicated that the survey which was developed for use in a
study in Tokyo, has not been tested for reliability and validity. Therefore, it is unclear how valid these results would be in comparison to
studies that utilize standardized measures such as the CTS.
India
Wilson-Williams, Stephenson, Juvekar, and Andes (2008) investigated 64 womens attitudes toward domestic violence through focus
groups in Gangadhar, which is a small community south of Mumbai.
The community was described as patriarchal in nature, and the women
strongly supported the traditional gender roles associated with the
patriarchal culture. Women in this sample were basically from poor
households and many were illiterate. Respondents indicated that they
viewed physical and psychological abuse as normal within the culture,
and suggested that a man has the right to beat a wife in order for
her to learn the correct behavior. At times, it was suggested that

84

Mental and Physical Health

mothers-in-law could be involved in the process, as was found in the


Chan et al. study described earlier. Women were expected to be sexually available to their husbands at all times, and were not expected to
use contraceptives as a means of preventing pregnancy. If a woman
began using contraceptives, that was seen as a precursor to domestic
violence. Once again, the authors took particular care to protect their
respondents from any adverse effects of participating in this study.
Prior to the focus groups convening, a discussion was held with the
entire village, and participants were recruited following this meeting.
Although the Wilson-Williams et al. investigation revealed several
interesting ndings that t within the framework of the effect of the
patriarchal culture on domestic violence, the authors mentioned that it
was difcult to get women to speak out on this sensitive topic. Since it
was self-selected, there clearly is a concern about the representativeness
of the sample. In addition, it is difcult to compare attitudes in a qualitative study using focus groups with more quantitative investigations.
However, this study was one of the rst in this type of small community, and the authors also attempted to connect the desire to use contraceptives with the consequences of subsequently becoming a victim
of IPV.
Another study conducted in a rural area of India (Jejeebhoy, 1998)
found that 40 percent of the sample of about 1,800 women reported having experienced IPV. Many of the respondents again believed that IPV
was justied because she engaged in the wrong behaviors. However,
Jejeebhoy reiterated the point that we have been stating, that this gure
is most likely an underestimation of the actual level of IPV in India.
Turkey
Ozcakir, Bayram, Ergin, Selimoglu, and Bilget (2008) stated that
there were no large scale studies of the prevalence of IPV in Turkey. If
IPV occurs, it is supposed to be kept within the family, and even if
some women actually call the police, only a very few ever prepare a
complaint. Turkey is an unusual combination of eastern and western
inuences, but according to the authors, Turkish law endorses a patriarchal family model in which the husband is named the head of the
family (p. 636). Therefore, the patriarchal model would suggest that
men and women in Turkey might condone and accept physical punishment of women by their partners.
In their study, the authors examined attitudes of men toward wife
beating. Almost 1,200 married men in the city of Bursa, Turkey, were
recruited at medical centers and interviewed in a face-to-face setting.
Several demographic questions were asked, along with a number of
questions about the conduct of marital partners and attitudes toward
wife beating. Respondents also were asked have you ever beaten your

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

85

wife? (p. 633) and have you ever yelled, shouted or used abusive
language to your wife? (p. 633). A relatively lower rate of physical
abuse (29 percent) than in previous regional Turkish studies was determined from respondents self-report, and close to 60 percent acknowledged that they had verbally abused their spouses. Risk factors
included the wifes lack of education or poor educational background,
the husbands alcohol problems, and if the husband had been physically abused as a child. Economic issues played a role in marital conict as well. In addition, close to 20 percent of the men surveyed
believed that men had the right to engage in IPV.
This study was a signicant contribution to the literature because it
is important to ascertain what men believe is appropriate and acceptable to be able to attempt to eliminate IPV in Turkey. Many studies only
sample womens attitudes and experiences, but including men in these
studies will be useful in determining the extent of the problem. In
addition, the nding that childhood physical abuse was related to
whether or not men engaged in IPV suggested to the authors that men
began to see IPV as a normal part of family life. However, although
the sample was large and the response rate was over 80 percent in this
large Turkish city, only self-report data was obtained, and two of the
signicant measures consisted of one question each concerning physical and verbal IPV. These factors, in addition to the issue of only sampling men, possibly could have led to a considerable underreporting of
IPV. In fact, the authors reported several regional studies based on
womens self-reports, which estimated very much higher levels of IPV.
Despite the patriarchal structure of the Turkish society, men may have
been reluctant to report engaging in IPV.

AFRICA
Nigeria
Antai and Antai (2008) examined rural womens attitudes toward
domestic violence in Nigeria. The authors indicated that rural Nigerian
communities embody the ideals of the patriarchal culture. Men hold all
the power and women accept their subordinate position in the family
and the community. Once again, in accord with our contention, Antai
and Antai suggest that it is important to determine womens attitudes
toward IPV because if the victim perceives IPV to be an integral part
of male supremacy (p. 2), and that the culture supports and condones
violence against marital partners, she is unlikely to report this behavior
and will see it as a normal part of the marriage. In support of this contention, at a UN symposium in 2007 a woman from Africa asked how
she could report her husbands violence against her when her marriage
vows ordered her to obey him?

86

Mental and Physical Health

In a manner similar to several other national studies, the authors utilized data from the 2003 Demographic and Health Survey conducted in
Nigeria. An unusually high response rate of more than 90 percent
enabled the researchers to investigate attitudes toward IPV among
more than 3900 rural women. Data were collected in face-to-face interviews. More than 40 percent of the rural sample suggested that IPV
was acceptable under certain conditions, including arguing with the
husband, and not agreeing to sex. Interestingly, there were several variations in condoning IPV related to the specic communities in various
geographical locations in the country. Similar to other studies, women
with poorer educational backgrounds, Muslim women, and women living in economic hardship, were more likely to justify IPV. However,
the authors also found other risk factors. Of particular interest to this
chapter, women with little say in the family and those without the ability to read about the news or to be exposed to other media tended to
approve of IPV. Antai and Antai suggested that empowerment consists
of three components; autonomy in domestic decisions, access to
media, and literacy level (p. 4).
This study was important for a number of reasons. First, the methodology employed a standardized national face-to-face interview process which had a very high response rate. Second, the authors focused
on rural women, an often-overlooked type of sample. Third, the questions included signicant categories including specication of empowerment, a concept very much associated with prevention and reduction
of IPV. Fourth, the authors identied variations among women
respondents that can lead to more complex and possibly more valid
interpretations of data. Finally, the interpretations were related to the
concept of a patriarchal society, which is support for one of the most
common explanations of IPV worldwide. Of course, Antai and Antais
study is subject to the limitations of self-report measures as well as the
focus only on womens attitudes toward IPV.
Kenya and Zambia
A study similar to the Antai and Antai study described above was
conducted by Lawoko (2008) in Kenya and Zambia. Lawoko suggested
that there might be varied interpretations of a mans positive attitude
toward IPV depending on conditions in the country. In general, he
indicated that Kenya has a more favorable economic and social climate
than Zambia. However, both countries embody the traditional gender
role approach suggesting that deviations from the female gender role
may have adverse consequences for the wife.
Prevalence of IPV in Kenya and Zambia appear to be comparable to
other African countries, and are somewhere between 20 and 30 percent.
Lawoko indicated that there are no comparative studies of attitudes

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

87

toward IPV among men in these two countries. In both countries, interview data were obtained through a national Demographic and Health
Survey (DHS) of men in many households. High response rates (close
to 90 percent in each country) increased the validity of the studys ndings. Scenarios were constructed to measure mens attitudes toward
IPV following hypothetical transgressions by women. For example, if
the woman neglects her household duties or refuses to have sex with
her husband, men were asked if she deserved to be punished.
In general, men from both countries demonstrated a high and similar rate of acceptance of wife beating ranging from 65 to 71 percent.
Most of the justications for the IPV revolved around the wifes deviating from normative gender-role expectations, but the correlations and
interpretations differed for the two countries. In Kenya, younger marriage partners and living outside of cities predicted higher support for
wife beating, but that was not the case in Zambia. Conversely, lower
educational levels were associated with higher tolerance of IPV in
Kenya but not in Zambia. It was suggested by Lawoko that the educational material in each country might differ. Perhaps part of the educational information in Kenya might lead to a change in patriarchal
ideals but not in Zambia. Similarly, access to the media was associated
with a lower justication of IPV in Zambia but not in Kenya. Perhaps,
as Lawoko indicated, the media in Zambia may support empowering
women. Along the same lines of reasoning but in the opposite direction, Lawoko found that mens positive attitudes toward sharing in
decision-making in the home predicted a lower acceptance of IPV in
Kenya but not in Zambia.
Once again, this comparative study of mens attitudes toward IPV in
two African countries was illuminating and important in identifying
certain possible causes of IPV. In addition, by showing that predisposing factors may work differently in various countries, Lawoko made
the case for developing culture and country-specic interventions.
However attitudes relating to IPV were examined only for men, and
both these countries were described as relatively peaceful. Therefore,
the results cannot be generalized to African countries experiencing a
great deal of conict. Lawoko described other limitations of his study
including the questionable validity of the DHS measures, the plausibility of extensive underreporting because of the face-to-face interviews,
and the lack of consideration of alternative factors affecting mens attitudes toward IPV in addition to issues related to patriarchy.
MIDDLE EAST
Haj-Yahia (2002) conducted a study in Jordan investigating attitudes
of approximately 350 married Jordanian women toward IPV. This convenience sample of women (with a response rate of more than 80

88

Mental and Physical Health

percent) was recruited from several clinics in a variety of locations


including a city, a village, and a refugee encampment. Participants completed surveys in private, since interviews would be considered to be
difcult to conduct on such a sensitive subject. Several measures were
included such as the Beliefs about Wife Beating and Sex-Role Stereotyping, a measure of attitudes toward women, and the Familial Patriarchal
Beliefs scale. All measures were translated and adjusted for cultural
appropriateness. Haj-Yahias ndings suggested that a high proportion
of the Jordanian respondents (ranging from about 35 percent to 65 percent depending on the offense) justied IPV, felt that wives would
become better wives as a result, blamed the women for their behavior,
did not think that violent partners should be punished (men will be
men) and thought that the problem should stay in the family and not
be dealt with by the government in any form. As in studies described
previously, factors associated with increased wife blaming included poor
educational levels, more traditional gender-role attitudes and patriarchal
beliefs. This study strongly supported our contention that patriarchal
belief structures will support and condone, and possibly even demand
punishment of bad wives. Haj-Yahia pointed out some difculties
with his study including questions about the generalizability of his ndings to all women even in Jordan, the use of self-report questionnaires,
and the lack of a measure of whether or not the respondents had experienced IPV. In addition, only women were surveyed in Haj-Yahias
research report. However, the study remains one of the few to examine
attitudes toward IPV in the Middle East. In fact, in a meta-analysis, by
Boy and Kulczycki (2008), the authors reported that only 21 studies contained statistics on occurrence of IPV in the Middle East and North
Africa even though the abuse is known to be extensive and widely condoned and accepted both by men and women.

LATIN AMERICA
Peru
Flakes study (2005) of IPV in Peru surveyed more than 15,000
women and revealed that about 40 percent had experienced physical
abuse. Several factors were related to the abuse, including poor educational background for the woman, forced early marriages, and alcoholic
excesses of the husband. Once again, effects of the womans status
were unpredictable. Under certain circumstances higher status protected the woman from IPV, but if her status was superior to her partner, it exposed her to more IPV. Flakes interpretation of these results
revolved around the concept of the frequency of patriarchal family
relationships in Peru. If a man feels threatened by a womans status or
achievements, he may use violence to emphasize his control over his

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

89

partner. Although this study supports our emphasis on the importance


of the effects of a patriarchal culture on IPV, and the large sample is
very positive, the measure of IPV was based on one question and selfreports of the prevalence of IPV are questionable especially in cultures
where the issue is sensitive and is not discussed very much in public.
Another study in Latin America was conducted by Ceballo, Ramirez,
Castillo, Caballero, and Lozoff (2004) using the Conict Tactics Scale
(Strauss, 1979). Thirty percent of the more than 200 poor women
sampled in Chile said they had been physically abused, and approximately 80 percent had been verbally abused by their partners. Ceballo
et al. identied Chile as a patriarchal society and indicated that IPV is
not discussed outside the family. The authors also revealed that until
1989 a husband was considered to own his wife and therefore he
could do anything he liked with his property. Although the Ceballo
et al. study surveyed an unusual sample of poor women, and used the
CTS which is a standardized measure of IPV, the authors indicated that
problems existed in their study including the use of self-report measures which most often underestimate the extent of IPV and the fact
that not all types of violence were measured. The advantage of an unusual sample of poor women also is undercut by the fact that the
results may not be representative of married women in Chile.
Both of these Latin American studies support our contention that
patriarchal societies are key elements in the perpetration of physical
and verbal abuse particularly in developing nations in Latin America,
the Middle East and Africa.
CANADA
Brownridge (2003) investigated the differences in prevalence in IPV
between Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal Canadian women. Brownridges data were based on a large national study, Statistics Canada,
conducted in 1999. He analyzed the data from interviews conducted by
telephone, sampling close to 150 Aboriginal and close to 7,000 nonAboriginal women. The survey suggested that Aboriginal women were
likely to be abused at least three times as much as non-Aboriginal
women. On all risk markers, including younger age, living in rural
areas, having a partner with a strong belief in patriarchal power, and
having a partner abusing alcohol, Aboriginal women were more likely
to exhibit these markers than non-Aboriginal Canadian women. However, again illustrating the inconsistency of ndings related to the educational level of women and the connection to IPV victimization, as
Aboriginal women became more highly educated their chances of
experiencing IPV actually increased. Brownridge suggested that
Aboriginal men may be reacting against their low status in Canadian
society, on the one hand, but also may be internalizing the negative

90

Mental and Physical Health

views of women characterizing some views of non-Aboriginal men. In


addition, if Aboriginal men have low status they may resent the higher
status and educational level of Aboriginal women which may lead to
increased levels of IPV.
Although this study utilized data from a representative Canadian
sample, it still is subject to the limitations of a self-reported interview
and a relatively small number of Aboriginal women participants. It
also is unclear as to how participants were protected from the consequences of participating in this type of study. In addition, only women
respondents data were considered by Brownridge.
OUTCOMES FOR IPV VICTIMS
Cwikel, Lev-Weisel, and Al-Krenawi (2003) questioned Bedouin Arab
women living in Israel in the Negev area, about the physical and psychological consequences of IPV victimization. The authors described the
Bedouin society as highly collectivistic and as exhibiting a patriarchal
structure, although there have been some changes as the Bedouins move
from a nomadic life to living in a more settled manner in towns. The society also may be termed a culture of honor and women are punished
for straying in order to preserve the honor of the family. Based on a
revision of a national Israeli measure that was translated and back translated from the Hebrew, prevalence of physical abuse and consequences
of the abuse were investigated. The outcome measures included the
shortened form of the CES-D, the Center for Epidemiological StudiesDepression Scale (Sherbourne, Dwight-Johnson, & Klap, 2001). About
200 Bedouin women visiting a medical center were interviewed in person by an Arab nurse. Since the women knew the nurse and trusted her,
she obtained cooperation from all the women she approached.
Results indicated that Bedouin women were very fertile until about
age 40, and that they experienced increased IPV during their fertile
years. As a consequence, women who had experienced IPV exhibited
symptoms of depression, low self-esteem, hopelessness and/or helplessness, low social support . . . (p. 250). One other nding that adds
to our discussion of the effect of educational level on IPV experiences,
showed that women with a higher educational attainment experienced
higher levels of IPV. The authors used an interpretation that we have
observed in other studies suggesting that women who have been educated apparently are more likely to challenge their partners dominance
and the restrictions placed on them by their husbands and the culture.
The authors acknowledge the limitations of self-report measures which
most probably underestimated the extent of the IPV problem. In addition, they suggested that it will be very difcult to develop culturally
appropriate interventions because IPV is closely connected to the heart
of patriarchal societies such as the Bedouin culture in Israel.

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

91

A study in Kenya utilizing the previously mentioned Demographic


and Health Survey of 2003, was conducted by Emenike, Lowoko, and
Dalal (2008). In examining the relationship between IPV and physical
consequences to women, the authors looked at data from more than
4,000 respondents. IPV was correlated with a higher rate of a pregnancy ending before term, and a greater chance of the baby not surviving. As indicated by the authors there obviously is no causal link but
just suggestions that IPV in Kenya might make women more susceptible to problems in childbirth and infants to possible death.
Another cross-cultural study by Weingourt et al. (2001), described in
an earlier section of the chapter, examined the consequences of IPV
using the General Health questionnaire which was translated into Japanese. A higher percentage of women exposed to IPV expressed symptoms of depression and anxiety, and somatic symptoms, as well as sleep
problems, than women who had not experienced domestic violence.
Finally, in the previously discussed study by Ceballo et al., the
authors reported on the psychological symptoms experienced by their
relatively small sample of poor women in suburbs of Santiago, Chile.
The CES-D (Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale) and
a measure of womens experiences of posttraumatic stress disorder
(PTSD) revealed severe levels of depression and PTSD-type symptoms.
All of these studies conrm predictions from researchers that victims
of both physical and emotional abuse will demonstrate high levels of
psychological distress as a result. Therefore, it is crucial for professionals
in countries worldwide to develop prevention and intervention programs to combat this prevalent physical and mental health problem.

TYPES OF PERPETRATORS
Recently, some attempts have been made to classify IPV perpetrators. One of the most popular categorizations was developed by Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart (1994). The different types of possible
perpetrators included:
1. Family-only batterers who have poor social and conict resolution
skills, may belong to a peer group which is supportive of IPV, and tend
to be violent only toward their spouses;
2. Dysphoric-borderline batterers have severe psychological problems,
may exhibit borderline characteristics, may abuse substances and may be
violent toward people outside their family;
3. Violent antisocial batterers are violent in every setting, may be criminals and are characterized by antisocial personality disorder diagnoses.

Although these categories have proved useful in identifying which


types of perpetrators may respond to an intervention to reduce or

92

Mental and Physical Health

eliminate IPV in America, and possibly in other Westernized countries,


it is unclear how well the categories will apply cross-culturally. For
example, in a culture of honor country such as Jordan, if a patriarch
violently punishes his wife, or even engages in an honor killing
according to westernized views, and the perpetrator categories
described above, he denitely would be classied at least as a familyonly batterer, or more likely as a spouse murderer. In his country,
however, this type of individual would be perceived as following the
dictates of the culture of the country, at least until recently. Therefore,
it is important to develop categories of abusers which would apply
cross-culturally to determine what types of interventions would work
best in different cultures.
RECOMMENDATIONS TO ELIMINATE
OR REDUCE IPV WORLDWIDE
WHO (2005) developed a series of recommendations to eliminate
IPV across the world. We will briey review some of these recommendations, suggest that there are issues and problems with implementing
these recommendations, and describe some intervention programs.
The rst major recommendation in the WHO report was to enforce
adherence to international agreements protecting human rights of
women around the world. In particular, patriarchal cultures that subordinate women and place women under mens control must be
changed. If women are to be treated as equals, with the same rights as
men, and if these patriarchal cultures stop condoning and even
demanding that women be harshly punished for straying then violence may no longer be a viable means of gaining control over women
by men. Particularly if men and boys are punished for engaging in violence against women, then the behavior will decrease.
Although this recommendation is the most important suggestion, it
is also true that it will be the most difcult to implement. Individuals
in countries where patriarchy and culture of honor principles are
deeply ingrained will resist these changes even if the government
does sign onto international agreements. For example, in 2004 the UN
General Assembly passed a resolution prohibiting honor-related violence against women (Womens UN Report Network, April 25, 2009).
However, in countries such as Saudi Arabia and Pakistan, as our previous examples illustrated, the practices still continue. Key government
ofcials as well as community and religious leaders, particularly men,
must be enlisted in the ght to change the violence-supportive patriarchal cultures. From a social psychological perspective, it is clear that
attitudes that are based on strongly held values are the most difcult
to change. Therefore, there must be a concerted effort by many individuals, at the government, local, community and religious levels to

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

93

engage the populace in a discussion of the harm that is being done, to


the women who are victims, to the children who observe and possibly
imitate the violence, and to the male perpetrators themselves.
Even though many countries where violence is endemic espouse the
patriarchal structure of societies, it also is clear that there are other viable explanations and models of IPV which may t other societies. It is
important that researchers, as well as academic and community agents
of change recognize that IPV is a complex issue that may be related to
a number of different risk factors. It also is crucial to understand that
although some risk factors are common across the world, others are
specic to individual countries.
A related recommendation is that all countries begin to collect data
on the prevalence of all types of violence against women, including
IPV. These data should be available to international organizations, and
the UN should send a special rapporteur around the world to determine whether countries are adhering to the agreements and if in fact,
violence against women has decreased.
Creating safe environments for women is an essential part of the recommendations. Shelters for battered women should be established with
safeguards to prevent men from invading the shelters. Women in the
shelters should be provided with treatment for physical and psychological injuries as well as skill-building to permit the women to reduce
their dependence on their abusive spouses. The WHO report suggests
that women may be identied as victims through clinics associated
with reproduction, since many women visit these clinics. Other venues
and professionals including primary health care workers should be
involved in identifying and protecting women.
Education for women and girls is emphasized all through the crosscultural research on IPV. However, since the ndings are inconsistent,
unless the patriarchal culture is changed, if women receive a high level
of education and achieve a high status, it is possible that this situation
may become a risk factor for IPV.
Even if laws are changed to punish perpetrators of violence against
women, law enforcement ofcials attitudes must be changed as well.
Women must be encouraged to report IPV, and police and other
authorities must provide women with sympathetic and helpful ofcers
who do not identify with and embody patriarchal values.
Both primary and secondary prevention programs should be implemented to reduce violence against women. In particular, the media
may be useful in primary prevention programs designed to reach
many individuals within the country. In Turkey for example, at one
point, all the media outlets in Istanbul were owned by women who
decided to begin a primary and secondary prevention effort to stop
violence against women. Television ads were produced including one
showing a young child dressing up in her mothers clothes and shoes,

94

Mental and Physical Health

and putting a black eye on as well since she had seen her mother in
that condition so many times. In addition, ads stated that the shame
is not yours while showing an abused women. Other interventions,
including training individuals to engage couples in improving their
conict resolution and communication skills, as well as instituting hotlines in the Istanbul area, were designed to attack the problem. Therefore, the media can be used as a positive force to combat this
worldwide problem. What also should be included in any of these programs is an evaluation component. The WHO report stated that there
should be additional cross-cultural research on the causes of violence
and programs to prevent or eliminate violence against women.
The WHO report also emphasized the importance of involving men
and boys in the effort to eliminate violence against women. Until men
who do not engage in violence against women and oppose it speak out
and intervene when they see violence occurring, perpetrators will continue to engage in this behavior. Once again the media can act as a
forum for nonviolent men to speak out, and these men may become
good role models for spousal interactions.
Flood (2001) discussed three different ways in which men can
become involved in the efforts to stop violence against women.
1. Men can join groups which emphasize that men must take responsibility for stopping mens violence, (p. 3) possibly though public action
such as protests. Flood describes the White Ribbon campaign in which
men wear white ribbons to show the world that not only will they avoid
violence but they will speak out and intervene if they observe violence.
Flood emphasized the importance of reaching out to boys and young
males to try and change fundamental attitudes which condone and support violence against women. These men also can act as proactive role
models to teach young men how to interact with women in a positive
way. However, Flood contends that mens groups must work in cooperation with womens groups in the same effort.
2. Men can become involved in educational media campaigns based on the
same concept espoused by Ban Ki Moon: Real men dont bash or rape
women. Flood described some programs in Australia which serve this
educational function. Sports gures were recruited for advertisements in
one of the Australian programs to target men who identify with athletes.
Once the athletes dene violence against women as wrong, it is hoped
that men identifying with these athletes will internalize their new views.
However, it is difcult for these programs to succeed because of the
violence-supportive culture of many types of sports. Another type of
Australian program which targeted violent males found that one of the
most effective approaches was to connect mens violence against women
to the negative effects on the children in their family.
3. Men can become directly involved in programs treating violent men, or
even men incarcerated for violence against women. However, Flood

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

95

argues that many of these programs have not been rigorously evaluated.
In addition, even if these programs were effective, unless the culture and
attitudes of members of society are changed to oppose violence against
women, single programs will not eradicate the problem.

As a conclusion to this section, we will present an overall program


that was instituted in Romania. Wimmer and Harrington (2008)
described a comprehensive program, Floare de Colt, (p. 623) developed to address IPV in Sighisoara, Romania. This grant-funded project
involved international partners who traveled to Sighisoara to hold a
three-day conference with community individuals, trained local participants, and provided social work students with the means to live in
the town for a brief period to assist in the program. Wimmer and Harrington emphasized that the rst step was to achieve community
involvement and sensitize the community to the existence of IPV and
the need for services for victims. An overall evaluation of the program
indicated that as a result, a wide range of services for victims had been
established, and utilized by various types of individuals, not just IPV
victims. However, the authors reported that several years after the creation of the project, the need for shelters and programs which focus on
changing perpetrators behavior, as well as access to public funding,
still remains.
The article by Wimmer and Harrington was an important stage in
illuminating the complex nature of any type of IPV prevention and
intervention program. The emphasis on involving community leaders
and representatives from the beginning of the project, the role that outside international experts can play as partners, and the signicance of
grants and funding to at least begin a program cannot be overestimated. Despite the success of the initial program, this article also illustrated the difculty of maintaining a program once the funding and
international partnerships are no longer involved.
One of the issues associated with this project was the viability of
the evaluation process. The authors defended the involvement of the
original international partners in the evaluation by suggesting that a
sufcient distance between these professionals and the on-site program had been established to enable the researchers to maintain
their objectivity. That involvement still may be seen as a barrier to
an objective evaluation of the effectiveness of the project. In addition, the lack of direct interviews with victims and the lack of any
input of victims in the process of evaluation other than documentation of the use of the services, also may be viewed as a serious
drawback of the evaluation process. However, since many programs
do not include essential evaluations in their descriptions, the
Wimmer and Harrington article represents a signicant contribution
to the literature.

96

Mental and Physical Health

COMPARISONS BETWEEN IPV AND


DATING VIOLENCE STUDIES
As we predicted in the opening section of this chapter, there is a
considerably larger volume of cross-cultural studies on IPV than on
dating violence. One of the issues is that IPV is more prevalent worldwide, since in some cultures dating is not permitted. However, it also
seems clear that dating violence most often is subsumed under a more
general category of violence or sexual assault, whereas IPV is conceptualized as a separate form of violence. As a result, there are many
more studies on IPV and more estimates of the prevalence than there
are for dating violence.
Similarities still exist in terms of the estimates of both types of violence. As a result of underreporting which occurs even in countries
where these behaviors are discussed publicly, accurate prevalence gures are impossible to obtain. Women victims of IPV and dating violence are reluctant to report because of feelings of self-blame, shame
and fear of retaliation. Even in cross-cultural research where considerable efforts have been made to protect participants from retaliation
from partners, these individuals still are fearful to give accurate
responses. Another issue relates to the variations in methodology
among the different studies. Methods range from randomly sampling
women from large Demographic and Health Surveys, or WHO surveys, where the response rate is quite high, to individual surveys
designed specically for one country and which are completed by as
few as 25 percent of those sampled. As a result, it is impossible to compare estimates of IPV and dating violence among countries when the
methods of obtaining these data are so diverse. The situation is even
more complex when investigating IPV and dating violence in countries
where these issues are kept within the family or under the veil (in
Arab countries). Even women in many of these countries condone IPV
and believe that women deserve the abuse to help them become
better wives (Haj-Yahia, 2002).
We also hypothesized that the prevalence of both IPV and dating
violence would be more expected and would occur to a more extreme
degree in patriarchal and culture of honor societies than in countries
with higher gender equity. In addition to the strong support that our
contention received in our selective review of cross-cultural studies,
this pattern is relevant to our ongoing multicultural investigation of
undergraduate students perceptions of IPV. In our study, we constructed a scenario describing a domestic violence incident in which a
husband abused his wife either after nding out that she had been
unfaithful or had been visiting her friend rather than taking care of his
dinner. This scenario was followed by a series of measures based on
the domestic violence incident. The study has been conducted in Spain,

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

97

the United States, the United Kingdom, Croatia, Israel, Romania,


Greece, Georgia, Ghana, Malaysia, Lebanon, and India, and has been
translated into seven languages. Based on the Gender Equity Index of
2008 which ranks countries in terms of gender equity (the rankings are
a summary of the Education Gap, Economic Activity Gap and the
Empowerment Gap), students from countries higher in gender equity (the rst six of the countries listed above) were more sympathetic
toward the wife and more negative toward the husband, whereas
undergraduate participants in the latter six countries showed the
reverse reactions to the scenario.
Since gender equity showed such a strong relationship with prevalence of violence against women worldwide, the recommendation of
the WHO that patriarchal attitudes and culture of honor norms be
changed is a reasonable approach to the issue. However, as we
described earlier, it will be extremely difcult to change such ingrained
attitudes and customs which have been characteristic of some cultures
for centuries. It is most important that the impetus for change emanates from the top down: male leaders of these societies, both in government and in the religious community, must be at the forefront of
these efforts. Men and boys denitely must be involved in the change
approach. International organizations should act as models and also
pressure member nations to act according to international agreements
and eliminate this aversive behavior. The media also can play a role in
creating the image that wife-beating and dating violence is wrong and
has severe negative consequences. In addition, law enforcement ofcials also must be involved in the process.
Although the type of culture clearly has been implicated in IPV and
dating violence, patriarchy is not the sole reason or even the most
plausible explanation of these types of violence in all countries. Since
IPV and dating violence also occur in countries where gender equity is
high, other models and theories must be established to fully comprehend this complex behavior. One other point is important for us to
emphasize. In addition to involving men in the effort to eliminate violence against women worldwide, we must focus more attention on
types of perpetrators and treatment and punishment for violent men. If
we only propose treatment and shelters for women, we may be engaging in an implicit type of blaming the victim.
Different trends in reports of violence are noted in dating relationships than what is seen in domestic disputes. These discrepancies can
be attributed to the following factors: teens generally do not cohabitate
with romantic partners, and therefore stereotypical relationship and
power dynamics that are seen in marriages have not yet had a chance
to develop; adolescent girls are more likely to report abuse and assault,
regardless of who the perpetrator was, whereas married women may
nd such disclosure shameful; adolescent boys, on the other hand, may

98

Mental and Physical Health

be prone to underreporting or not be willing to accept the blame for


assault, and therefore the numbers are lower; and some cultures are in
fact changing and shifting, as was noted in the Wang and Ho article
(2007), which results in young women feeling more empowered and
able to stand up for themselves, even if their actions are not always
appropriate, or respected by their romantic counterparts.
In conclusion, IPV and dating violence are pervasive and complex
behaviors which adversely affect women all around the world. It is important to conduct more cross-cultural comparisons, using standardized measures, in order to obtain accurate gures, as well as to
develop models to understand and eliminate the violent behaviors.

REFERENCES
Adelman, M., & Kil, S. H. (2007). Dating conicts: Rethinking dating violence
and youth conict. Violence Against Women, 13, 12961318.
American Psychological Association. (1996). APA Presidential Task Force on
Violence and the Family Report. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Antai, D. E., & Antai, J. B. (2008). Attitudes of women toward intimate partner violence: A study of rural women in Nigeria. The International Electronic Journal of
Rural Remote Health Research, Education, Practice, and Policy, 8, 112.
Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences in aggression between heterosexual partners:
A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 651680.
Black, B. M., Tolman, R. M., Callahan, M., Saunders, D. G., & Weisz, A. N.
(2008). When will adolescents tell someone about dating violence victimization? Violence Against Women, 14, 741758.
Blow, C. (2009). Editorial: Love shouldnt hurt. New York Times, February 12,
2009.
Boy, A., & Kulczycki, A. (2008). What we know about intimate partner violence
in the Middle East and North Africa. Violence Against Women, 14, 5370.
Brownridge, D. A. (2003). Male partner violence against Aboriginal women in
Canada. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 6583.
Ceballo, R., Ramirez, C., Castillo, M., Caballero, G. A., & Lozoff, B. (2004). Domestic
violence and womens mental health in Chile. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
28, 298308.
Chan, K. L., Brownridge, D. A., Leung, W. C., Tiwari, A., & Ho, H. W. Y.
(2008). Mental health prole of sexual violence perpetrators among university students in Hong Kong. In J. K. Quinn & I. G. Zambini (Eds.), Family
relations: 21st century issues and challenges. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.
Chan, K. L., Brownridge, D. A., Tiwari, A., Fong, D. Y., & Leung, W.-C. (2008).
Understanding violence against Chinese women in Hong Kong: An analysis of risk factors with a special emphasis on the role of in-law conict.
Violence Against Women, 14, 12951312.
Cwikel, J., Lev-Wiesel, R., & Al-Krenawi, A. (2003). The physical and psychosocial health of Bedouin Arab women of the Negev area of Israel. Violence
Against Women, 9, 240257.

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

99

Dobash, R. E., & Dobash, R. P. (1980). Violence against wives: A case against the
patriarchy. London: Open Books.
Doroszewicz, K., & Forbes, G. B. (2008). Experiences with dating aggression
and sexual coercion among Polish college students. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 23, 5873.
Douglas, E. M., & Straus, M. A. (2006). Assault and injury of dating partners
by university students in 19 countries and its relation to corporal punishment experienced as a child. European Journal of Criminology, 3, 293318.
Dutton, D. G. (1995). Intimate abusiveness. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Practice, 2, 207224.
Eaton, D. K., Davis, K. S., Barrios, L., Brenner, N. D., & Noonan, R. K. (2007).
Associations of dating violence victimization with lifetime participation,
co-occurrence, and early initiation of risk behaviors among U.S. high
school students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 22, 585602.
Emenike, E., Lawoko, S., & Dalal, K. (2008). Intimate partner violence and
reproductive health of women in Kenya. International Nursing Review, 55,
97102.
Flake, D. F. (2005). Individual, family, and community risk markers for domestic violence in Peru. Violence Against Women, 11, 353373.
Flood, M. (2001). Mens collective anti-violence activism and the struggle for
gender justice. Development (Special Issue: Violence Against Women and
the Culture of Masculinity), 44, 4227.
Foshee, V. A., Bauman, K. E., Ennett, S. T., Linder, G. F., Beneeld, T., &
Suchindran, C. (2004). Assessing the long-term effects of the safe dates
program and a booster in preventing and reducing adolescent dating
violence victimization and perpetration. American Journal of Public Health,
94, 619624.
Foshee, V. A., Bauman, K. E., Linder, F., Rice, J., & Wilcher, R. (2007). Typologies of adolescent dating violence: Identifying typologies of adolescent
dating violence perpetration. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 22, 498519.
Foshee, V. A., Linder, F., MacDougall, J. E., & Bangdiwala, S. (2001). Gender
differences in the longitudinal predictors of adolescent dating violence.
Preventative Medicine, 32, 128141.
Foshee, V. A., Lindner, F., Rice, J., & Wilcher, R. (2007). Typologies of adolescent dating violence: Identifying typologies of adolescent dating violence
perpetration. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 22, 498519.
Fredland, N. M., Ricardo, I. B., Campbell, J. C., Sharps, P.W., Kub, J. K., &
Yonas, M. (2005). The meaning of dating violence in the lives of middle
school adolescents: A report of a focus group study. Journal of School
Violence, 4, 95114.
Frieze, I. H. (2005). Female violence against intimate partners: An introduction.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 229237.
Gender Equity Index. (2008). Retrieved March 10, 2009, from http://www.
socialwatch/org/en/avncesyRetrocesos/IEG_2008/tablas/SWG EI.htm
Giordano, P. (2007). Recent research on gender and adolescent relationships: Implications for teen dating violence research/prevention. Presentation at U.S. Departments of Health and Human Services and Justice Workshop on Teen
Dating Violence: Developing a Research Agenda to Meet Practice Needs,
December 4, 2007, Crystal City, VA.

100

Mental and Physical Health

Glick, P., & Fiske, S.T. (1997). Hostile and benevolent sexism: Measuring
ambivalent sexist attitudes toward women. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
21, 119135.
Haj-Yahia, M. M. (2002). Beliefs of Jordanian women about wife beating. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 209219.
Haj-Yahia, M. M. (2006). Patterns of violence against engaged Arab women
from Israel and some psychological implications. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 24, 209219.
Haj-Yahia, M. M., & Edleson, J. L. (1994). Predicting the use of conict resolution tactics among engaged Arab-Palestinian men in Israel. Journal of
Family Violence, 9, 4762.
Halford, W. K., Sanders, M. R., & Behrens, B. C. (2004). Repeating the errors of
our parents? Family-of-origin spouse violence and observed conict
management in engaged couples. Family Process, 39, 219235.
Hines, D. A., & Straus, M. A. (2007). Binge drinking and violence against dating partners: The mediating effect of antisocial traits and behaviors in a
multinational perspective. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 441457.
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Stuart, G. L. (1994). Typologies of male batterers:
The subtypes and the differences among them. Psychological Bulleting, 116,
426497.
Jejeebhoy, S. J. (1998). Wife beating in rural India: A husbands right? Evidence
from survey data. Economic and Political Weekly, 33, 855862.
Kerpelman, J. L. (2007). Youth focused relationships and marriage education.
The Forum for Family and Consumer Issues, 12, 3746.
Kilmartin, C., & Allison, J. (2007). Mens violence against women: Theory, research,
and activism. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Kimmel, M. S. (2002). Gender symmetry in domestic violence: A substantive
and methodological research review. Violence Against Women, 8, 13321363.
Laursen, B., & Collins, W. A. (1994). Interpersonal conict during adolescence.
Psychological Bulletin, 115, 197209.
Lawoko, S. (2008). Predictors of attitudes toward intimate partner violence: A
comparative study of men in Zambia and Kenya. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 23, 10561074.
Luthra, R., & Gidycz, C. (2006). Dating violence among college men and
women: Evaluation of a theoretical model. Journal of Interpersonal Violence,
21, 717731.
Lysova, A. V., & Douglas, E. M. (2008). Intimate partner violence among male
and female Russian university students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 23,
15791599.
Manganello, J. A. (2008). Teens, dating violence, and media use: A review of
the literature and conceptual model for future research. Trauma, Violence,
& Abuse, 9, 318.
Molidor, C., & Tolman, R. M. (1998). Gender and contextual factors in adolescent dating violence. Violence Against Women, 4, 180194.
Montgomery, M. J. (2005). Psychosocial intimacy and identity: From early adolescence to emerging adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 346374.
Olson, E. (2009, January 4). A rise in efforts to spot abuse in youth dating. The
New York Times, p. A12.

Cross-Cultural Violence against Women and Girls

101

Ozcakir, A., Bayram, N., Ergin, N., Selimoglu, K., & Bilgel, N. (2008). Attitudes
of Turkish men toward wife beating: A study from Bursa, Turkey. Journal
of Family Violence, 23, 631638.
Rennison, C. M., & Welchans, S. (2000). Bureau of Justice Statistics Special Report:
Intimate partner violence, National Crime Victimization Survey. Washington,
DC: Rennison & Welchans.
Riggs, D., & OLeary, K. (1989). A theoretical model of courtship aggression. In
M. A. Pirog-Good & J.E. Stets (Eds.), Violence in dating relationships (pp. 53
71). New York: Praeger.
Roberts, T.A., & Klein, J. (2003). Intimate partner abuse and high-risk behavior
in adolescents. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 157, 375380.
Sherer, M. (2009). The nature and correlates of dating violence among Jewish
and Arab youths in Israel. Journal of Family Violence, 24, 1126.
Sigal, J., & Annan, V. (2008). Violence against women: International workplace
sexual harassment and domestic violence. In F. L. Denmark & M. A. Paludi
(Eds.), Psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories (pp. 590622).
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conict and violence: The conict
scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 7588.
Straus, M. A. (2007). Validity of cross-national research based on convenience
samples: The case of the international dating violence study data. In
Violence against dating partners in world perspective: The International Dating
Violence Study. Durham, NH: Family Research Laboratory, University of
New Hampshire.
Straus, M. A., & Ramirez, I. L. (2007). Gender symmetry in prevalence, severity, and chronicity of physical aggression against dating partners by university students in Mexico and USA. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 28190.
Straus, M. A., & Savage, S. A. (2005). Neglectful behavior by parents in the life
history of university students in 17 countries and its relation to violence
against dating partners. Child Maltreatment, 10, 124135.
United Nations. (1993). Declaration on the elimination of violence against
women. General Assembly Resolution 48/104, December 20, 1993.
Retrieved September 29, 2006, from http://www.ohchr.org/english/law/
elimination.vaw.
United Nations General Assembly Resolution A/C.3/59/L.25:15 (2004,
October). Crimes of honor. Retrieved April 25, 2009, from http://www.wurn.
com.
Valls, R., Puigvert, L., & Duque, E. (2008). Gender violence among teenagers:
Socialization and prevention. Violence Against Women, 14, 759785.
Vandello, J. A., & Cohen, D. (2003). Male honor and female delity: Implicit
cultural scripts that perpetuate domestic violence. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 84, 9971010.
Wang, X., & Ho, P. S. K. (2007). My sassy girl: A qualitative study of womens
aggression in dating relationships in Beijing. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 22, 623638.
Weingourt, R., Maruyama, T., Sawada, I., & Yoshino, J. (2001). Domestic violence and womens mental health in Japan. International Nursing Review,
48, 102108.

102

Mental and Physical Health

Wekerle, C., & Wolfe, D.A. (1999). Dating violence in mid-adolescence: Theory,
signicance, and emerging prevention initiatives. Clinical Psychology
Review, 19, 435456.
Wilson-Williams, L., Stephenson, R., Juvekar, S., & Andes, K. (2008). Domestic
violence and contraceptive use in a rural Indian village. Violence Against
Women, 14, 11811198.
Wimmer, J. S., & Harrington, P. A. (2008). Domestic violence services in
Romania: A longitudinal case study. International Social Work, 51, 623633.
Xu, X., Zhu, F., OCampo, P., Koenig, M. A., Mock, V., & Campbell, J. (2005).
Prevalence of and risk factors for intimate partner violence in China. American Journal of Public Health, 95, 7885.

Chapter 5

Intimate Partner Violence as a


Workplace Concern: Impact
on Womens Emotional and Physical
Well-Being and Careers
Michele A. Paludi
Jessica Wilmot
Lindsey Speach

Barbara Cavalier had been married to her husband, Chris Cavalier, for
seven years. During the course of their marriage, Chris had been abusive
toward Barbara. When he put a gun to her head, she decided to leave
him. For six months her living arrangements were kept secret. One day
Chris walked into the Elmwood siding supply business and saw Barbara
where she was working as a data-entry clerk. Subsequently, Chris
walked into the store, armed with two guns, a .45 caliber automatic pistol
and a .357 caliber Magnum revolver. Chris killed Barbara and her coworker, Stephanie Revolta, who had tried to defuse the situation. Stephanie
had placed a 911 call, but by the time assistance arrived, Barbara and
Stephanie were dead. Chris also took his own life. Barbaras coworkers
reported that Chris had been harassing Barbara all day, calling her at
work, and stealing her truck. Authorities had found a note in Chriss
house in which he assigned power of attorney and listed valuables that
he wanted to give away. This behavior led police to believe that Chris
had planned the murders that day (cited in Paludi, Nydegger, & Paludi,
2006).
Ellen works for a small shipping company in the western Canada city
of Vancouver. She has been unhappily married to Paul for more than

104

Mental and Physical Health

20 years, and she and her two daughters bear the brunt of Pauls verbal
taunts and controlling behavior. Though he has never physically abused
the children, he often beats Ellen so severely that vicious bruises cover
her arms and legs, and she regularly lies to her coworkers about their
origin, claiming clumsiness, embarrassed by their true cause. At least
once a month Ellen is so badly hurt that she must call out of work. In
the past two years alone, she has lost 22 days of work, and thousands of
dollars in wages.
One night, Paul angrily smacks Ellens younger daughter, and she
falls down the stairs, cutting open her knee. After years of abuse, Ellen
has nally had enough, and leaves. She moves in with a friend, and
changes her phone number and personal email address. For a while
things are ne. But soon Ellen begins receiving threatening prank phone
calls at the ofce, and nasty emails to her work email account. She thinks
it might be Paul, and her suspicions are conrmed when one evening
she discovers him waiting for her in the ofce parking lot. In a dark and
menacing tone Paul threatens to kill herand their childrenunless she
returns to him.
She doesnt know who to turn to or where to go. For years she has
been hiding her abuse from her friends, family, coworkers and employers, and the threats continue to escalate . . . (cited in Soroptimist International of the Americas, 2007).

The experiences of Barbara and Ellen are not unique; approximately


2,600,000 women are victims of intimate partner violence each year
(Swanberg & Logan, 2005). Homicide is the leading cause of occupational death for women in the United States (National Institute for
Occupational Safety and Health, 2009; Reeves & OLeary-Kelly, 2007;
Swanberg & Logan, 2007; Swanberg, Macke, & Logan, 2006). Furthermore, women who are victims perpetrated by their mates/spouses
account for one-fourth of all women who are murdered in a given year
(McHugh & Frieze, 2006; Rathus & Feindler, 2004). Swanberg et al.
(2005) reported that more women in the United States are victimized
by their spouses/mates than are harmed because of reported automobile accidents, muggings, and rapes combined.
Straus, Gelles, and Steinmetz (1980) noted that one out of every six
couples engage in at least one violent act each year. Over the course of a
marriage, just over one-fourth of couples will experience intimate partner violence. Intimate partner violence is not conned to marriage.
Research suggests that violence in dating relationships and among
unmarried couples who live together is even greater than among married couples. Ryan, Frieze, and Sinclair (1999) noted that approximately
one-third of U.S. college students reported using or being victims of intimate partner violence. Abuse in relationships includes intense criticisms
and put-downs, verbal harassment, sexual coercion and assault, pushing,
grabbing, shoving, physical attacks and intimidation, stalking, choking,

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

105

striking a partner with an object, restraint of normal activities and freedoms, and denial of access to resources (Butts Stahly, 1999; McHugh,
Livingston, & Frieze, 2008; Ryan et al., 1999). Straus et al. (1980) concluded: The American family and American home are perhaps as or
more violent than any other American institution or setting (with the
exception of the military, and only then in time of war (p. 4).
The Fourth United Nations (UN) International Conference on
Women concluded that in all societies . . . women and girls are subjected to physical, sexual and psychological abuse that cuts across lines
of income, class and culture (Walker, 1999, p. 21). Tran and Des
Jardins (2000) reported that the incidence of intimate partner violence
experienced by Vietnamese and Korean communities is similar to U.S.
incidence rates. Horne (1999) noted that intimate partner violence rates
in Russia exceed United States rates by four to ve times.
Intimate partner violence is prevalent in all races and ethnic groups
and among women in urban, rural, and suburban areas and in lesbian,
gay, and heterosexual relationships (Coleman, 1991; McHugh & Frieze,
2006; Paludi, Nydegger & Paludi, 2006; Potocziak, Murot, CrosbieBurnett, & Potoczni, 2003). In addition, while women are more likely
to be victims of intimate partner violence, men may be battered (Heise,
1998; McHugh & Frieze, 2006). Men batter because they want control
in the relationship. Women, however, batter in self-defense because of
fear of being murdered. Heise (1998) found that more women are seriously injured and killed by male partners each year than men are by
female partners. Tjaden and Thoennes (2000) reported that over a lifetime, the prevalence of intimate partner violence for women is triple
the prevalence for men. In addition, research suggested that the
incidence of same-sex intimate partner violence is similar to that of heterosexual intimate partner violence (McHugh & Frieze, 2006; Potocziak
et al., 2003).

INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE SPILLS OVER INTO


WORKPLACES
Swanberg and Logan (2005) asked U.S. women about the ways in
which intimate partner violence spilled over into their workplace.
Womens responses indicated that batterers interfered with womens
employment before, during, and after work. Pre-work interference prevented 56 percent of the women in this study from going to work,
including physical restraint, having clothes cut up, being beaten,
and being denied access to the car. Swanberg and Logan (2005) and
Wetterstein et al. (2004) noted that women reported experiencing prework incidents of violence at least once a week. Interference with womens employment at the workplace included the following behaviors:

106

Mental and Physical Health

phone harassment, harassment of the womens supervisors, threatening


comments, stalking, and being physically forced to leave work.
Thus, the workplace is not a safe haven for women victims of intimate partner violence; the violence spills over into the workplace. The
workplace provides a site where batterers, like Chris Cavalier, can nd
their victims. And, as Swanberg et al. (2006) noted:
When partner violence traverses the boundaries of womens jobs, the victimized partner is no longer the only victim. Other people on the workplace premises, including supervisors, other workers, and customers, are
at risk for injury or some other form of trauma. (p. 573)

Intimate partner violence costs employers more than $727.9 million


annually on costs related to lost productivity, including 7.9 million
paid workdays each year. In addition, there are additional costs related
to health care costs associated with intimate partner violence. Intimate
partner violence is thus a workplace issue.
In this chapter we review the empirical research on the impact of
intimate partner violence on womens emotional and physical health,
self-concept, interpersonal relationships with coworkers and supervisors, and career goals. We offer recommendations for employers in
exercising reasonable care in responding to women employees who
are victims of intimate partner violence, including developing and
enforcing an effective policy and investigatory procedures and training
programs on intimate partner violence awareness and the organizations policy.
IMPACT OF INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE ON EMPLOYEES
The biggest challenge is to convince a woman that its not her fault. My
daughter is 25; my stepdaughter is 22. As a mother, I want them to know
that if a boyfriend is abusive, you cannot ignore itOh, hes been drinking, or He had a bad day. There is no excuse for a man hitting a
woman. Ever.
Sen. Debbie Stabenow, Michigan

Research on intimate partner violence has documented impact on


several areas of functioning, including emotional/psychological, physiological or health related, career, interpersonal, and self-perception
(Lundberg-Love & Marmion, 2006; McHugh et al., 2008; OLeary &
Maiuro, 2001; Reeves & OLeary, 2007; Swanberg et al., 2006). Examples of emotional/psychological effects of intimate partner violence
include, but are not limited to, guilt, denial, withdrawal from social
settings, shame, depression, fear, anger, anxiety, phobias, isolation, fear
of crime, helplessness, frustration, shock, and decreased self-esteem
(Cunradi, Ames, & Moore, 2008; Swanberg et al., 2006).

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

107

The following are reported physical/health-related effects of intimate


partner violence: headaches, tiredness, respiratory problems, substance
abuse, sleep disturbances, eating disorders, lethargy, gastrointestinal disorders, post traumatic stress disorder, the hostage syndrome, and inability to concentrate (Lundberg-Love & Wilkerson, 2006; McHugh & Frieze,
2006; Walker, 2006; Zorza, 2002). In addition, battered women experience
injuries including bruises, cuts, concussions, black eyes, broken bones,
scars from burns, knife wounds, loss of hearing and/or vision, and joint
damage (Felblinger, 2008; Lundberg-Love & Wilkerson, 2006). Pregnant
women who experience intimate partner violence face the risk of severe
outcomes for their fetus as well as themselves (Sagrestano, Carroll,
Rodriguez, & Nuwayhid, 2004).
The impact intimate partner violence has on social and interpersonal
relationships includes the following: withdrawal, fear of new people,
lack of trust, and changes in social network patterns at work (Lundberg-Love & Wilkerson, 2006; Swanberg et al., 2006).
Research by Swanberg (e.g., Swanberg et al., 2006; Swanberg &
Logan, 2007) suggested that the impact of intimate partner violence on
women employees contributed to women victims inability to (a) concentrate and solve problems at work, (b) perform their job, (c) go to
work, (d) stay at work, and (e) keep their jobs. In addition, women victims of intimate partner violence may receive threatening emails, calls,
and/or faxes at work. This has implications for women employees
mental state and contributes to their inability to concentrate and failure
to follow their job responsibilities and thus be red.
Similar results were obtained by Lloyd and Taluc (1999), who found
that out of the 824 women in their sample, 18 percent indicated that
they had experienced intimate partner violence, 11.9 percent had
incurred more severe violence at the hand of their mate/spouse and
40.3 percent said they had been coerced and threatened by their mate/
spouse. In addition, 28.4 percent had experienced abuse at the criminal
assault level. These women reported experiencing unemployment,
emotional and physical health problems, and higher welfare rates than
women who did not experience intimate partner violence.
Ridleys (2004) research with the Maine Department of Labor and
Family Crisis Services indicated that 74 percent of batterers reported
having easy access to their partners workplace. Twenty-one percent of
these men had contacted their partner at the company in violation of a
restraining order, and 48 percent indicated they could not concentrate
at their own job because of their preoccupation with their partner. Of
these men, 19 percent had a workplace accident; 42 percent were late
for work because of engaging in battering.
Intimate partner violence also has direct implications for a mothers
effectiveness to parent. The dysfunction and disorganization of the
home offer little or no support, structure, nurturance, or supervision

108

Mental and Physical Health

for children (Walker, 1999). Children are often neglected by mothers


who are too emotionally and physically abused to care for them (Black
& Newman, 2000).
IMPACT OF INTIMATE PARTNER ABUSE ON CHILDREN
Furthermore, the impact of intimate partner violence on children is signicant. Children are in the middle of domestic violence in a number of
ways, including arguments about child rearing practices and childrens
behavior. Children blame themselves for the violence as a consequence of
their stage of cognitive development even though they are not the cause
(Paludi, 2002). Walker (1999) and Graham and Rawlings (1999) estimated
that each year approximately 3.3 million children in the United States
between the ages of 3 and 17 years are at risk of exposure to their mothers
being battered by a male spouse/mate. Children learn to become part of
a conspiracy of silence. They lie to prevent inappropriate behavior; they
learn to suspend fulllment of their needs rather than risk a confrontation
with the batterer. Thus, as Paludi (2002) stated, they live in a world of
make-believe (p. 351). They themselves are at greater risk for physical
abuse than children whose mothers are not battered (Black & Newman,
2000). Homicide is currently one of the ve leading causes of child mortality in the United States (Paludi & Paludi, 2000).
WOMENS FEAR OF LEAVING THE VIOLENT RELATIONSHIP
According to Lundberg-Love and Wilkerson (2006):
Being a victim of domestic violence can result in serious and long-lasting
psychological consequences. The initial emotion reported after a battering
incident tends to be helplessness. Over time, most women become fearful, anxious, angry, and depressed . . . many battered women develop
PTSD. The duration of the abuse and the victims history of prior abuse
impact the severity of the symptoms she may experience. (p. 40)

Most battered women remain in the violent relationship because they


believe their situation is inescapable. They feel helpless about changing
their lives and fear that any action they take will contribute to additional
violence, which is justied (Butts Stahly, 1999; Zorza, 2002). Furthermore, cultural factors contribute to remaining silent to protect the familys honor (Marmion & Faulkner, 2006). Battered women remain in
violent relationships for several well-founded reasons (McHugh &
Frieze, 2006; Paludi, 2002; Butts Stahly, 1996) including:
a. Threats to her life and the lives of her children if she leaves the home
b. Fear of not getting custody of her children

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

109

c. Financial dependence
d. Feeling of responsibility for keeping the relationship together
e. Love for the batterer
f. The batterer is not always violent

Grothues and Marmion (2006) noted that women victims of intimate


partner violence frequently attempt to leave the relationship; their
attempts are thwarted by their partners who exercise more control and
coercion with threats. Furthermore, it has been reported that economic
issues must be taken into consideration when understanding women
remaining in a violent relationship:
Women who leave their spouse often have no good alternatives for housing or support for themselves or their children. Because of the nature of
the abuse, which often involves increasing isolation from others, victims
tend to have a very small support system. Shelters are not readily available in all communities, and even this option has limitations and has an
impact on the children. It is not simply a case of not wanting to leave;
most women do wish to do so. However, the costs of leaving are signicant. (Grothues & Marmion, 2006, p. 11)

We note that Butts Stahlys (1999) review of the National Crime


Survey of the Department of Justice indicated that 70 percent of intimate partner violence occurs after the relationship has ended. Similar
ndings were reported by Walker (1995) and Birns (1999), who
reported that women are at an increased risk for homicide following
the break up of a relationship, more so than women who remain in
the relationship.
EMPLOYEE ASSISTANCE PROGRAM (EAP) ASSISTING
WOMEN VICTIMS OF INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE
An EAP assists employees with dealing with non-work-related
issues that interfere with their ability to perform their job (Smith &
Mazin, 2004). EAPs provide short-term counseling on the telephone or
in-person, as well as refer employees for help in the community. An
EAP can be utilized to help women employees deal with the symptomatology of intimate partner violence reviewed above (Paludi & Paludi,
2000; Rothman, Hathaway, Stidsen, & deVries, 2007). Recommendations for EAPs include: (a) helping women to develop a sense of trust
and safety in the current environment, (b) helping women foster relationships with appropriate nonviolent male models, (c) understanding
womens insecurity about their future, (d) countering any sense of guilt
about having caused the violence and/or not being able to prevent the
battering, and (e) increasing womens self-esteem.

110

Mental and Physical Health

An EAP can also assist children of battered women through play


assessment and play therapy to encourage preschool children to
express feelings about the trauma, individual counseling for children,
and women-children support groups (Paludi & Paludi, 2000).
Most importantly, women who have left a battering relationship
often report that they had learned about intimate partner violence and
the impact of this violence on themselves and their children from listening to an EAP counselor discuss the issue at a noon-time seminar.
Such programs facilitated by an EAP should include information about
local domestic violence shelters, the importance of considering psychological abuse as a form of intimate partner violence, emotional
responses to intimate partner violence, consequences of intimate partner violence, religious beliefs and remaining in a battering relationship,
and cultural issues involved in reporting the abuse (e.g., Bostock, Plupton,
& Pratt, 2009; Mitchell et al., 2006; Watlington & Murphy, 2006).
We discuss subsequently in this chapter the role the EAP can play
in training supervisors to recognize changes in employees behavior as
a consequence of intimate partner violence and to refer them to the
EAP in a condential manner (DeCenzo & Robbins, 2007; Paludi &
Paludi, 2000).

IMPACT OF INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE


ON THE WORKPLACE
As we have previously discussed, intimate partner violence impacts
the primary employed individual, coworkers, clients, vendors, and customers. Intimate partner violence thus contributes to stress for all in
the workplace. This stress consequently results in lost productivity,
decreased morale, increased absenteeism, and increased employee
turnover. In addition, Gurchiek (2005) reported that intimate partner
violence has a sweeping effecton employee safety, work performance and even the employers bottom line (p. 1). As we noted earlier,
intimate partner violence costs employers more than $727.9 million
annually on costs related to lost productivity.
The health-related costs of intimate partner violence, including stalking, rape, and murder, exceed $5.8 billion per year. In addition,
approximately $4.1 billion of this cost is for employees who require
physical and mental health care, both paid for by the employer. Direct
costs also include ambulance transport, paramedic assistance, physical
therapists assistance, and emergency department visits (Swanberg et
al., 2005). It is important to note that mental health care may continue
for several years after the victimization. Thus, the estimated costs of
intimate partner violence are an underestimate in any year (Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention, 2009).

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

111

The American Institute on Domestic Violence (2009) reports that victims of intimate partner violence lose approximately 8 million days of
paid workdays each year. The average absenteeism rate of victims of
intimate partner violence is approximately 30 percent higher than the
average employee absenteeism rate (Urban, 2000). In addition, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2009) measured expected
value of lost earnings that victims of intimate partner violence who
were murdered would have contributed to society if they had lived out
their full life expectancies. The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention estimated this gure to be $892.7 million, approximately $713,000
per woman.
Willmans (2007) review of Fortune 1000 companies indicated that
49 percent of corporate leaders indicated intimate partner violence had
harmful effects on the organizations productivity. In addition, 47 percent indicated the violence had a harmful impact on attendance. Fortyfour percent said the violence was harmful to their health care costs
and therefore, intimate partner violence impacts their bottom lines.
ORGANIZATIONS USE OF REASONABLE CARE
IN DEALING WITH INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE AS A
WORKPLACE ISSUE
Reasonable care, adapted from rulings in Burlington Industries, Inc.
v. Ellerth (1998) and Faragher v. City of Boca Raton (1998) includes the
following at a minimum:
1. Establish and enforce an effective policy.
2. Establish and enforce effective investigative procedures.
3. Facilitate training in intimate partner violence as a workplace issue in
general and in the organizations policy and procedures specically.

These cases focused on sexual harassment; however, the Equal


Employment Opportunity Commission (1999) has maintained that
these basic standards apply to all types of prohibited harassment.
We pursue an institutional level of analysis to explain the prevalence
of intimate partner violence as a workplace issue. We thus focus on
educational, psychotherapeutic, legal, management, and sociocultural
factors in understanding intimate partner violence that spills over to
the workplace. We also discuss the importance of social science
research in helping organizations understand why intimate partner violence exists, including issues of power and control (also see McHugh &
Frieze, 2006). We thus integrate management theory, case law, and
social science research to effectively enforce policies and procedures in
an atmosphere of trust that encourages individuals to come forth with
their experiences of intimate partner violence.

112

Mental and Physical Health

EMPLOYERS BEING PUT ON NOTICE ABOUT


INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE
In Swanberg and Logans (2005) research, 46 percent of women in
their sample informed their supervisors or managers about the intimate
partner violence they were experiencing. Thus, 54 percent of the women
decided not to disclose the abuse. Furthermore, 43 percent of the women
informed one of their coworkers about the battering; 57 percent did not
do so. Swanberg and Logan also reported that those women who did
inform their coworkers or managers received support with having their
phone calls screened and physical protection from their batterer.
We note that women who did not disclose their victimization at
work did so because they feared losing their job, were ashamed of
being battered, wanted to deal with the battering themselves, were
ashamed of their appearance, and were frightened (Paludi & Paludi,
2000). In addition, women who received support from their employer
did eventually resign because they were forced by their mate/spouse
to stop working and/or because of the stress of fearing their mate/
spouse would come to their workplace (Swanberg & Logan, 2005).
We discuss the following prevention strategies to assist organizations in assisting employees who are victims of intimate partner violence: primary; secondary; and tertiary. Primary prevention strategies
include the development and enforcement of policies, investigatory
procedures, and training programs on intimate partner violence awareness and the organizations policy. Secondary prevention strategies
include facilitating individualized training with employees who are at
high risk for being victims. Finally, tertiary prevention includes working with victims of violence, providing counseling services for them.
Each of these strategies is addressed in the following sections.
PRIMARY PREVENTION STRATEGIES
Policies and Procedures
Policies that establish a zero-tolerance for workplace violence and intimate partner violence in the workplace require more than a general
statement against the behavior (Barling, Rogers, & Kelloway, 2001;
Paludi, Nydegger, & Paludi, 2006; Swanberg, Macke, & Logan, 2007). They
require the efforts and support of management at all levels and continual training of all employees (to be discussed later in this chapter), as
well as procedures that encourage employees to disclose their victimization to the organization (Kelly & Mullen, 2006). All employees would
benet from a workplace climate of respect and cooperation.
COMPONENTS OF POLICIES AND PROCEDURES
The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2009) and Society
for Human Resource Management (2009) recommend an explicit policy

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

113

statement and investigatory procedures for assisting victims of intimate


partner violence. Components of effective policy statements are the following (Paludi, Nydegger & Paludi, 2006):
a. Statement of Purpose
b. Legal Denition
c. Behavioral Examples
d. Statement Concerning Impact of Intimate Partner Violence on Individuals
and Workplace
e. Statement of Individuals Responsibility in Notifying Employer
f. Statement of Workplaces Responsibility in Assisting Employee
g. Statement Concerning Condentiality of Complaint Procedures
h. Statement Concerning Sanctions Available
i. Statement Regarding Retaliation
j. Statement of Sanctions for Retaliation
k. Statement Concerning False Complaints
l. Identication and Background of Individual(s)

DEALING WITH INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE


IN THE WORKPLACE
An important consideration for the policy is whether it sets a tone of
appropriate seriousness and concern for employees rights. Thus, the
policy needs to specify the types of behaviors that are prohibited in the
workplace and the potential consequences of these behaviors should
they occur. Paludi, Nydegger and Paludi (2006) and Occupational Safety
and Health Association (OSHA; 2009) recommend the policy including a
Personalized Safety Plan for victims of intimate partner violence that
outlines the employers responsibility in assisting victims. Components
to include in this Personalized Safety Plan include the following:
a. Providing receptionists and the buildings security ofcer with a photograph
and description of the batterer.
b. Screening the employees visitors and phone calls.
c. Accompanying the employee to and from their cars.
d. Permitting the employee to park close to the ofce building.
e. Having a formal notication letter be sent to the batterer by the president
of the organization that indicates their presence on the company premises will result in an arrest since the employee has a restraining order on
the batterer.
f. Providing referrals for individual counseling.

A sample policy statement and personalized safety plan are presented in Appendix 1 and Appendix 2. Additional suggestions for
safety plans may be found in Lundberg-Love and Marmion (2006).

114

Mental and Physical Health

We caution employers however, to be careful in implementing these


procedures since they may put employees safety at risk. As Wilman
(2007) discussed:
Abusers, who have anger and control problems, often perceive such
efforts as a conspiracy between the employer and the victim. They
become frustrated, angry, and feel out of control when employers make
it difcult for them to access their victims. (p. 7)

Paludi, Nydegger, and Paludi (2006) have identied components


for effective complaint procedures for organizations. Empirical
research has indicated that employees will feel more encouraged to
discuss their experiences with intimate partner violence when they
understand what the process entails (Swanberg et al., 2006). Thus,
employees must be given accurate and adequate information about
disclosing their victimization, written in understandable language and
terms. Failure to provide such information makes the policy statement
inhibitive.
Components of effective procedures include all of the following at a
minimum:
a. Informing employees that the workplace will not ignore any disclosure
of intimate partner violence.
b. Informing employees that the employer will not make determinations
about the employee victim based on the reputations or organizational
status of the employee involved.
c. Informing employees that they will respond promptly to any incident of
intimate partner violence.
d. Informing employees that witnesses to incidents and/or to changes in
the employees behavior will be interviewed.

Paludi and Paludi also recommended that:


a. The policy statement should be made available in languages in addition
to English for individuals and their support systems for whom English
is not their rst language.
b. The policy statement must be made available in Braille and in large type
as well as be made available on audio tape.

TRAINING PROGRAMS
For Managers
Paludi and Paludi recommended that human resource specialists
facilitate training programs for managers in two sections: (a) to

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

115

provide information on intimate partner violence awareness, e.g. denitions, incidence, impact on victims and the workplace and (b) the
organizations policy and procedures so that managers know their
rights and responsibilities with respect to assisting employees who
are victims of intimate partner violence. Lee (cited in Gurchiek, 2005)
noted that employers should provide an atmosphere for disclosure
since victims feel ashamed, embarrassed, and are fearful of losing
their job.
Results from research by Swanberg et al. (2006) indicated that
women who chose not to disclose the intimate partner violence to their
employer did so because of stigma associated with the violence (e.g.,
embarrassed, ashamed, fear of being judged), safety related (e.g.,
threatened by the abuser not to tell anyone about the violence, didnt
want coworkers to become involved) and work-environment-related
(e.g., didnt know anyone to tell about the violence, couldnt trust coworkers and supervisors with the disclosure, and the abuser also
worked for the same organization). We recommend these ndings be
made part of the training for managers so they can understand reasons
for silence and reasons why the employer must be supportive to victims of intimate partner violence.
We also believe it is important to train managers about why victims
of intimate partner violence do disclose the abuse at work. Women in
Swanberg et al. (2006)s research who did report the intimate partner
violence at work indicated doing so because they needed to talk about
the abuse and because their coworker in whom they conded was also
a friend. Women who disclosed at work also did so as a way to explain
absences and tardiness, to request time off from work to attend court
proceedings or to answer a supervisor or coworker who inquired about
any potential abuse. Furthermore, some women disclosed at work
because they feared for their lives and wanted a coworker or supervisor to know about the abuse in case of their death. These ndings
can assist managers in setting up a welcoming and safe environment
for all employees so they feel encouraged to report abuse.
Swanberg et al. (2006) noted that women victims of intimate partner
violence who disclosed the abuse at work fared better in terms of having longer job tenure, low job-quitting rates, and higher wages. Thus, it
is important to inform managers (and employee victims) that disclosing
the intimate partner violence at work does not translate into employees
losing their jobs.
Paludi, Nydegger, and Paludi (2006) identied the following components of effective training program for managers:
a. Ways to encourage employees to report problems.
b. Skills in behaving compassionately and supportively to employees who
disclose intimate partner violence.

116

Mental and Physical Health

c. Skills in handling crises.


d. Basic emergency procedures.

In addition, Paludi and Paludi (2000) recommended including a unit


in the training programs for managers that deal with stereotypes, what
Grothues and Marmion (2006) refer to as dismantling the myths
about intimate partner violence, including blaming women for their
victimization, believing intimate partner violence is a rare occurrence,
that women are masochistic and ask for the victimization, that intimate
partner violence is about sex, not power, and that intimate partner violence only occurs among poor families.
Paludi and Paludi also recommended that training programs on intimate partner violence be separate from training on workplace violence
in general. Examples of policies, procedures, and training programs
may be found in Paludi, Nydegger, and Paludi (2006) and on OSHAs
Web page, www.osha.gov.

For Threat Assessment Team


OSHA (2009) has recommended the use of threat assessment teams
or crisis intervention teams that receive, evaluate, and respond to
threats in the workplace. Members of these teams include management
and labor as well as security, human resources, legal, and operations
personnel. The teams are trained to implement policies of the company. They must not intervene inappropriately or endanger employees
by their responses. As Paludi, Nydegger, and Paludi (2006) noted,
these teams may be feared by employees as secret police spying on
employees to try to get them red:
The following steps can be implemented to insure that these teams are
effective and appropriate: (1) an inclusive group of employees to represent the organization should be selected and (2) training that offers legal,
management, and psychological perspectives should be provided to the
team on a periodic basis. (p. 92)

OSHA (2009) also recommends training for the companys threat


assessment team to deal with situations as they emerge. These teams
must receive continuing training on issues related to intimate partner
violence. Their training is identical to the components identied for
managerial training. In addition, OSHA recommends that:
a. These teams need to be professionally competent and know what to do
and when to call for help.
b. All members need to know the other team members and their individual
jobs very well.

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

117

c. They should role-play and practice a wide variety of potential problem


situations.
d. They should have frequent meetings and ongoing training.

For Employees
Paludi and Paludi recommend the same components of the training
program for managers be provided to employees as well. In addition,
employee training should include a discussion about the personalized
safety plans and the role of the EAP in assisting victims of intimate
partner violence. Emphasis should be placed on ensuring the employer
will take each victim seriously and will provide assistance to the victimized employee.
We also recommend including in the training program for employees a discussion on ways they can assist coworkers who are victims of
intimate partner violence, including being alerted to changes in the
individuals behavior. In addition, training in how to respond to a coworker who condes in employees should be provided, including
listening without judging, recognizing the difculty it took for an individual to discuss the topic of intimate partner violence, and being an
advocate for the employee by referring them to human resources, a
manager, or the EAP. We also advise that intimate partner violence be
part of all new employee orientations. As Swanberg et al. (2006) noted:
Educating employees about partner violence could help to demystify the
disgrace associated with this social problem and consequently help reduce
or eliminate the risk of partner violence entering into the workplace. (p. 574)

PEDAGOGICAL TECHNIQUES
The pedagogical techniques that have been recommended in the literature (e.g., Paludi & Paludi, 2000) for training programs are ones
that:
1. Empower employees.
2. Encourage employees to think strategically.
3. Assist employees in communicating effectively with employees.
4. Manage conict in the workplace.

The interactive pedagogy encompasses adult learning principles.


Research in educational psychology (Slavin, 2008) has identied that
adults prefer learning situations that:
a. Are practical and problem-centered.
b. Promote their positive self esteem.

118

Mental and Physical Health

c. Integrate new ideas with existing knowledge.


d. Show respect for the individual learner.
e. Capitalize on their experience.
f. Allow choice and self-direction.

We recommend facilitating the training in the following ways:


a. Provide overviews, summaries, case studies, and behavioral rehearsals
to link research to practice.
b. Use collaborative, authentic problem-solving activities.
c. Assist individuals in becoming more effective and condent through
guided practice and establishing routines.
d. Ask individuals what they would like to know about the training topic.
e. Provide a quality, well-organized, differentiated experience that uses
time effectively and efciently.

The major objective of the training modules and pedagogical techniques is to facilitate transference to the workplace. We recommend
accomplishing this goal by:
a. Association: having participants associate the new information with
something with which they are already knowledgeable.
b. Similarity: presenting information that is similar to material that
participants already know; i.e., it revisits a logical framework or
pattern.
c. Degree of original learning: the degree of original learning for the participants was high.
d. Critical attribute element: the information learned by the participants
contains elements that are extremely benecial and/or critical on the
job.

We do not recommend Web-based training programs for teaching


employees about intimate partner violence. This pedagogy generates
high levels of employees acquisition and retention of the material presented (Frisbie, 2002; Goldstein & Ford, 2002) as well as offers fastpaced learning. However, Web-based training can create frustration in
employees who are not computer literate, who would prefer learning
from an individual, not a computer, and because the issue is emotionally laden and therefore sensitive (Dessler, 2009). We do recommend
the use of behavioral rehearsal with case studies with managers, asking
their input on how to handle an incident presented to them. Discussion
of their responses must include providing critical evaluation of their
behavior, including whether or not they are following the organizations policy.

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

119

NEEDS ASSESSMENTS
In keeping with the literature in human resource management (e.g.,
Barbazette, 2006; DeCenzo & Robbins, 2007) we recommend conducting a
needs assessment with employees to identify additional issues they expect
to be covered in a training session. Brown (2002) identied four reasons
why needs assessments must be conducted prior to facilitating training
programs: (1) identify problem areas in the company, (2) obtain management support, (3) develop data for measuring the effectiveness of the
training program, and (4) determine the costs and benets of the training
program. Needs assessments may be conducted through anonymous surveys and/or focus groups (Lucier, 2008; Tyler, 2002). We recommend the
following process (also see Levy & Paludi, 2002):
1. Ask individuals to provide answers to questions regarding discrimination in the workplace via an anonymous mail survey.
2. Facilitate 2-hour focus groups with self-identied employees (no more
than 15-20 per session) to elicit in-depth responses. Structured interview
questions for individuals who participate in the focus groups center
around employees goals for training, including their needs with regard
to better understanding intimate partner violence and individuals with
viewpoints different from their own.
3. Analyze responses from the previous steps using qualitative and quantitative analyses.
4. Prepare a written report that summarizes the needs assessment, including suggestions for the following:
.

How to increase awareness

Ways to examine attitudes

Alternatives to stereotyping

Methods of supportive action

5. Make recommendations for post-training evaluations (to be discussed


subsequently in this chapter).

The main goal of the needs assessment therefore is to obtain information concerning the manner in which intimate partner violence is
addressed in the organizational climate of the company, including topics
such as empowerment, the establishment of mutual trust and respect,
methods of inclusion or exclusion, and verbal and nonverbal communication. The process of the assessment will be consistent with the goal of the
training programs in which the employees will subsequently participate.
POSTTRAINING EVALUATIONS
In further keeping with the human resource management literature,
we highlight the necessity of conducting posttraining evaluations.

120

Mental and Physical Health

Measuring the effectiveness of training programs in intimate partner


violence as a workplace issue is an important aspect of the training
program so that the organization may determine if the training delivered or failed to deliver the expected organizational benets. The measures of success for the training programs in intimate partner violence
are ones identied in the needs assessment phase. Issues in the measurement phase can be discussed in two phases: types of information
about which to measure and ways to measure whether or not the training effort achieved its goals. It is not enough to merely assume that
any training an organization offers, even if it is legally mandated, is
effective. The transfer of knowledge from the training room to the
workplace is the most important measure of success.
The most wellknown model for determining the effectiveness of
training programs is the Kirkpatrick Model (1959, 1998) that is comprised of four levels: reaction, learning, behavior, and results. Results
concerns the benets resulted from training. Behavior taps into what
extent trainees change their behavior, back in the workplace as a result
of this training. The learning level asks to what extent trainees
improved their knowledge and skills and changed their attitudes as a
result of the training programs. Finally, the reaction component determines trainees opinions about the structure of the training program,
location of the program, trainer effectiveness, and so on.
The most commonly used level of the Kirkpatrick Model is the reactions level. However, this is the least valid evaluation technique (Tan,
Hall, & Boyce, 2003). Individuals opinions are heavily inuenced by
factors that may have little to do with the training effectiveness. By
measuring reactions, organizations do not obtain information regarding
employees learning, how well they are integrating the new knowledge
and skills on their job, or whether there has been increased disclosure
of intimate partner violence posttraining.
Common performance-based evaluations that incorporate any of the
Kirkpatrick Model levels are posttraining, pre-post training, pre-post
training performance with control group, and the Solomon Four Group
Design. We encourage the reader to review Graziano and Raulin, 1996,
for additional information.
We recommend using an ecological approach for conducting
training programs on intimate partner violence (Paludi & Paludi, 2003).
This approach stresses that training be facilitated in a sequence that
ensures optimum assistance for all parties. The sequence is as follows:
Individuals hearing reports of intimate partner violence
Counselors in EAP
President and administrators
Managers and supervisors
Employees

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

121

We suggest report persons and counselors be trained initially so that


employees will have an outlet for disclosing intimate partner violence
and receiving emotional support. Typically ashbacks can occur during
training programs; we want to ensure employees have trained individuals who can assist them with these experiences.

ADDITIONAL HUMAN RESOURCE RESPONSIBILITIES


We note the importance of ensuring that victims of intimate partner
violence not be retaliated against on other human resource functions
(Lemon, 2001; Randel & Wells, 2003). For example, employee victims
should not be identied as tardy or absent if they have to go to
counseling, court, physicians appointments, and so on. In addition,
performance appraisals should not penalize employee victims on time
management, absenteeism, and so on. While managers may not fully
be aware of the employees situation, the human resource department
must ensure protection for employee victims.
Furthermore, employers must abide by the General Duty Clause
of OSHA, which states that the employer has to provide a safe and
healthy workplace free from violence for all its employees. Employers should also provide family and medical leave to employee victims. Organizations that recognize the need and adapt work to
employees lives will assist employee victims as well as win all
employees loyalty and thus have a competitive edge (Eastman,
1998, Paludi, et al., volume 3 of this book set). Organizations with
family-friendly policies report less stress for employees, lower absenteeism, higher morale, positive publicity, improved work satisfaction, lower turnover rate, stafng over a wide range of hours, child
care hours that conform to work hours, and access to quality infant,
child, and elder care (Frone & Yardley, 1996; Paludi & Neidermeyer,
2006).
Examples of family friendly policies for victims of intimate partner
violence include exible work hours, on-site health services, time off/
career break, and workplace relocation that provide employees with
the opportunity to continue working for their employer at a safer and
anonymous job site (Paludi, Vaccariello, Graham, Smith, Allen-Dicker,
& Kasprzak, 2006; Swanberg et al., 2006). Examples of such policies are
found in Paludi and Paludi (2006).
In addition, educational materials may be developed for distribution
and posting throughout the workplace, including posters with tear-off
tabs containing hotline phone numbers.
We also note that an intimate batterer may be a coworker of the employee victim. We recommend employers consider adopting a consensual relationship policy (also see Paludi, Nydegger, & Paludi, 2006).

122

Mental and Physical Health

We recognize that consensual relationships are not illegal, but they do


cause difculties for organizations because:
a. The situation involves one person exerting power over another.
b. The seduction of a much younger individual is usually involved.
c. Conict of interest issues arise, e.g. how can a supervisor fairly
evaluate an employee with whom she/he is having a sexual relationship?
d. The potential for exploitation and abuse is high.
e. The potential for retaliatory harassment is high when the sexual relationship ceases.
f. Other individuals may be affected and make accusations of favoritism
(Paludi, Nydegger & Paludi, 2006, pp. 79-80).

Sample consensual relationship policies may be found in Paludi and


Barickman (1998).
We recommend the implementation of engineering solutions to
assist with intimate partner violence as well as workplace violence in
general (Middelkoop, Gilhooley, Ruepp, Polikoski, & Paludi, 2008).
Examples of engineering solutions include:
Controlling or limiting access to work areas after dark
Creating clear escape routes
Installing locks on doors that lead to staff only areas like break or lunch
rooms
Employee identication badges
Increased lighting
Video surveillance
Alarm systems
Buddy system
On-site guard services
Weapons policy
Coded card keys for access
Equip eld staff with cellular phones and daily work plans
Providing a portable panic button for each employee
Providing handheld alarms.

Security staff are also important in prevention strategies, including:


a. Working with administrators to improve the security level of the buildings, grounds, and parking lots.
b. Serving as the liaison with local law enforcement.
c. Serving as the companys security experts, providing administrators with
a risk management analysis.

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

123

d. Serving as the companys technologies experts with respect to engineering solutions.


e. Assisting with conducting effective background checks as part of the
employment process.
f. Coordinate electronic surveillance, building security with global positioning, communications, etc. with local law enforcement to provide
quicker response times.
g. Install voice activated and Braille security call boxes.
h. Ensure a company-wide emergency notication system in the event of
violence on site, including the parking lot.

THE EAP
EAPs can offer the following services in primary prevention of intimate partner violence as well as workplace violence in general (see
Paludi, Nydegger, & Paludi, 2006):
a. Providing short term counseling to managers and employees.
b. Providing referrals for counseling outside of the workplace.
c. Consulting with and training Threat Assessment Team.
d. Training managers to deal with employee victims without diagnosing
the employee.
e. Providing referrals for legal counseling.
f. Providing referrals for nancial counseling.
g. Determining whether the employee has sought and/or obtained a protective order against the abusive partner.
h. Working closely with human resources to monitor the employee victim
in order to protect their safety.

Paludi, Nydegger, and Paludi (2006) outlined steps companies


should take for emergency action plans, including:
a. Procedures for getting help.
b. Emergency escape procedures and routes for every person and for every
place in the company.
c. Identication of personnel with training needed to perform medical
duties.
d. Training for employees on how to deal with violence and how to use
the emergency action plan.
e. Procedures for calling for medical assistance.
f. Procedures for notifying law enforcement.
g. Procedures for securing the area after a violent event.
h. Procedures for accounting for all employees after a violent event.

124

Mental and Physical Health

We thus take the approach that intimate partner violence that


spills over into the workplace is the responsibility of all members of an
organization.

SECONDARY AND TERTIARY PREVENTION


Relying on the symptomatology of victims of intimate partner violence reviewed in this chapter we can assist employees we believe are
at risk for violence as well as those who have disclosed the abuse.
The American Bar Association Commission on Domestic Violence
(1999) also noted the following observable behavior that may suggest
possible intimate partner abuse that may be helpful for secondary prevention measures (p. 16). We recommend providing this listing to
managers as well during training on intimate partner violence as a
workplace issue:
a. Unexplained tardiness and absences.
b. Unplanned use of leave time.
c. Lack of concentration.
d. A tendency to remain isolated from coworkers or reluctance to participate in social events.
e. Discomfort when communicating with others.
f. Disruptive phone calls or e-mail.
g. Frequent nancial problems indicating lack of access to money.
h. Unexplained bruises or injuries.
i. Noticeable change in use of makeup (to cover up injuries).
j. Inappropriate clothes (e.g., sunglasses worn inside the building, turtleneck worn in the summer).
k. Sudden changes of address or reluctance to divulge where she is
staying.
l. Court appearances.

The EAP counselor can assist employee victims with conict


resolution, providing guidance about options and procedures, e.g.,
domestic violence shelter, providing emotional support for concerns
about disclosing victim status to the employer, short term counseling,
referral services, and countering myths about causes of violence with
realities.
We also recommend EAP counselors discussing the cycle of violence
with employee victims (Walker, 1979). The three phases of the cycle of
violence are as follows: In the tension-building phase, there are battering incidents. The victim attempts to avoid escalation of the battering
by calming her mate and by staying away from him. However, the

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

125

tension builds too high to be controlled by these efforts and the batterer responds with an acute battering incident in the second phase of
the cycle. In the nal phase, the tension from the rst two phases has
ceased and the batterer becomes apologetic and charming toward the
victim. The level of violence increases both in frequency and severity
as the relationship continues.
CONCLUSION
Sexual, racial, gender violence and other forms of discrimination and violence in a culture cannot be eliminated without changing culture.
Charlotte Bunch

The Partnership for Prevention (2002) reported that most employers


do not have a dened policy and procedures for dealing with intimate
partner violence that spills over into the workplace. Smaller companies
are less likely to include a policy on intimate partner violence than
larger ones. Services that are most commonly offered by companies
have included victim referral services, security precautions, and educational materials, e.g., posters, brochures. Ropers (2002) study of the Liz
Claiborne survey of Fortune 1000 senior executives and managers indicated that victim resources were the only focus of these employers
attention to intimate partner violence, including emergency counseling
services, employee benets that covered the costs of medical assistance,
and referrals to organizations that deal with intimate partner violence.
We have offered recommendations in this chapter to assist workplaces
in implementing effective interventions in intimate partner violence. We
borrow from the Higher Education Centers (Langford, 2006, p. 5) suggestions for campus violence in that workplace interventions must be:
Prevention-focused
Comprehensive
Planned and evaluated
Strategic and targeted, using results from a risk assessment of the vulnerability of the workplace
Research-based
Multicomponent
Coordinated and synergistic, ensuring all prevention and response efforts
complement and reinforce each other
Multisectoral and collaborative, involving campus stakeholders, including
counselors and advisors
Supported by infrastructure and institutional commitment

In order for these recommendations for primary, secondary, and


tertiary prevention strategies to be successful, there must be support
and initiative from the president of the organization. Without this

126

Mental and Physical Health

commitment, the prevention strategies will not be effectively implemented, contributing to employees believing the organization is not
seriously committed to the issue of intimate partner violence and thus
being silenced about their experiences with this abuse. Furthermore, as
we have suggested in this chapter, dealing with intimate partner violence as a workplace issue must be based on a multidisciplinary team
approach, including human resources, EAP, security, managers, law
enforcement, attorneys, and employees themselves. Unions may also
assist by supporting the companys intimate partner violence policy,
facilitating training on intimate partner violence for new stewards/delegates, and ensuring all employees have received the companys policy
and have been trained on intimate partner violence.
We also recommend organizations conducting a safety audit to
determine whether the prevention strategies are working effectively
(Smith & Mazin, 2004), including conducting anonymous organization
climate surveys to inquire about employees perceptions of the companys commitment to dealing with intimate partner violence. A safety
audit would include:
a. Building security (e.g., automatic locked doors? Security guards on duty
for all shifts?)
b. Visitors (e.g., sign in guests with guard?)
c. Health services (e.g., EAP, wellness center?)

The audit will uncover disconnects between the workplaces policies,


procedures, and practices and what the workplace wants to achieve in
terms of meeting the needs of victims of intimate partner violence.
Results from the audit and climate survey must be used to understand
the reason for employees perceptions and to correct the policies, procedures, and training, and ensure employees feel safe disclosing the abuse.
Recommendations can be prioritized based upon the risk level assigned
to each item: high (required immediate attention), medium (required to
be dealt with in a short time frame), and low (suggestions to make the
practices more efcient). From this risk matrix, a workplace security
plan can be developed to address the most pressing issues rst (Crouhy,
Galai, & Mark, 2005). We hope workplaces will nd the recommendations in this chapter useful in meeting their goal of helping victims of
intimate partner violence so Jerry Morans sentiment becomes a reality:
Through education, improved funding and support, we can continue to
work together to provide safe environments for victims and end the cycle
of domestic violence.

Additional resources to assist organizations meeting this goal are


presented in Appendix 4.

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

127

APPENDIX 1: SAMPLE POLICY ON INTIMATE PARTNER


VIOLENCE AS A WORKPLACE CONCERN
Employees of _____ must be able to work in an atmosphere of mutual respect and trust. As a place of work, _____ should be free of violence and all forms of intimidation and exploitation. _____ is
concerned and committed to our employees safety and health. The
Firm refuses to tolerate violence in our workplace.
_____ has issued a policy prohibiting violence in the workplace. We
have a zero tolerance for workplace violence.
_____ also will make every effort to prevent violent acts in this
workplace perpetrated by spouses, mates, or lovers. The Firm is committed to dealing with intimate partner violence as a workplace issue.
_____ has a zero tolerance for intimate partner violence.
Intimate Partner Violence: Denition
Intimate partner violencealso referred to as domestic violence, battering, spouse abuse, spousal assault, and intimate partner abuseis a
global health problem. This victimization is dened as violence
between adults who are intimates, regardless of their marital status,
living arrangements, or sexual orientations. Such violence includes
throwing, shoving, and slapping as well as beatings, forced sex, threats
with a deadly weapon, and homicide.
Intimate Partner Violence: Myths and Realities
Myth: Intimate partner violence affects a small percentage of employees.
Reality: Approximately 5 million employees are battered each year
in the United States. Intimate partner violence is the leading cause of
injury and workplace death to women in the United States.
Myth: People must enjoy the battering since they rarely leave the
abusive relationship.
Reality: Very often victims of battering do leave the relationship.
Women and men remain in a battering relationship not because they
are masochistic, but for several well-founded reasons, e.g.,
. Threats to their lives and the lives of their children, especially after they
have tried to leave the batterer
. Fear of not getting custody of their children
. Financial dependence
. Feeling of responsibility for keeping the relationship together
. Lack of support from family and friends
. The batterer is not always violent
. They still love the batterer

128

Mental and Physical Health

Myth: Individuals who batter abuse their partners because they are
under a great deal of stress, including being unemployed.
Reality: Stress does not cause individuals to batter their partners. Society condones partner abuse. In addition, individuals who batter learn
they can achieve their goals through the use of force without facing
consequences.
Myth: Children are not affected by watching their parents in a battering relationship.
Reality: Children are often in the middle of domestic violence. They
may be abused by the violent parent. Children may also grow up to
repeat the same behavior patterns they witnessed in their parents.
Myth: There are no long-term consequences of battering.
Reality: There are signicant long-term consequences of battering,
including depression, anger, fear, anxiety, irritability, loss of self-esteem,
feelings of humiliation and alienation, and a sense of vulnerability.
Myth: Intimate partner violence only occurs in poor and minority
families.
Reality: Intimate partner violence occurs among all socioeconomic
classes and all racial and ethnic groups.

Services Offered by _____ for Employees who are Victims of


Intimate Partner Violence
_____ will offer the following services for our employees who are
victims of intimate partner violence:
. Provide receptionists and building security ofcer with a photograph of
the batterer and a description of the batterer
. Screen employees calls
. Screen employees visitors
. Accompany the employee to her/his car, subway, bus stop
. Permit the employee to park close to the ofce building if required
. When there is a restraining order, _____ will send a formal notication to
the batterer that indicates that his/her presence on the Firms premises
will result in arrest
. Referrals for individual counseling

Training programs dealing with intimate partner violence will be


facilitated annually for managers and employees.
Members of the Threat Assessment Team you may contact to discuss
intimate partner violence are:
Name ____________________ Phone Number ____________________

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

129

APPENDIX 2: SAMPLE PERSONALIZED SAFETY PLAN


Name:
Date Completed:
1. I can inform my immediate supervisor, security, human resources and
_____ at work that I am a victim of intimate partner violence.
2. I can ask _____to help me screen my telephone calls at work.
3. When leaving work, I can walk with _____to my car or the bus stop. I
can park my car where I will feel safest getting in and out of the car.
4. If I have problems while driving home, I can__________.
5. If I use public transit, I can __________________.
6. I can go to different grocery stores and shopping malls to conduct my
business and shop at hours that are different from those I kept when
residing with my battering partner.
7. I can use a different bank and go at hours that are different from those I
kept when residing with my battering partner.
8. I can use _____ as my code word to alert my coworkers when I am in
danger so they will call for help.

Important Telephone Numbers


Police: 911 and _____
Domestic Violence Shelter:
District Attorneys Ofce:
My Supervisors Home Phone Number:
My Clergy Contacts Phone Number:
Human Resources:
Security:
Other:
____________________
____________________
APPENDIX 3: SAMPLE TRAINING PROGRAM FOR MANAGERS
Goals of Training
To:
a. Provide information about the incidence and psychological dimensions
of intimate partner violence.
b. Discuss the impact of intimate partner violence on individuals and the
workplace.
c. Distinguish between myths and realities regarding intimate partner violence.
d. Discuss reasons employees will disclose and not disclose intimate partner violence at work.

130

Mental and Physical Health

e. Provide information on ways the workplace can encourage disclosure


regarding intimate partner violence.
f. Discuss workplaces policy statement on intimate partner violence.
g. Discuss the role of the EAP, security, unions in assisting victims of
intimate partner violence.
h. Discuss characteristics of perpetrators of intimate partner violence.

At the conclusion of the training program, managers will be able to:


a. Assess their own stereotypes and hidden biases regarding intimate partner violence.
b. Adequately label emotional, physical, interpersonal, and career impacts
of intimate partner violence on individuals and the workplace.
c. Identify employees rights and responsibilities to disclose intimate partner violence at the workplace.
d. Adequately assist employees who wish to disclose intimate partner
violence.

Topics for Presentation and Discussion


Introduction to training session and goals of training
Case presented to managers, managers offer opinions related to case
Discussion of case
Myths related to intimate partner violence:
Intimate partner violence is a rare event.
Intimate partner violence is about sex.
Women are masochistic and ask to be abused.
Intimate partner violence only includes physical assault.
Intimate partner violence only occurs in poor families.
Intimate partner violence only occurs in heterosexual couples.

Myths vs. realities


Denition of intimate partner violence.
Incidence of intimate partner violence.
Sex vs. power in intimate partner violence.
Reasons why women remain in battering relationships.
Verbal and physical examples of intimate partner violence.
Sex, ethnic, racial, and socioeconomic status related to intimate partner
violence.
Sexual orientation and intimate partner violence.

Psychological issues in disclosing or not disclosing intimate partner


violence in the workplace.

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

131

Pre-work, during work and after work threats by perpetrators.


Impact of intimate partner violence on victims.
Emotional symptoms
Physical symptoms
Behavioral symptoms
Organizational symptoms

Impact of intimate partner violence on coworkers.


Impact of intimate partner violence on workplace.
Factors contributing to intimate partner violence.
Behavioral risk factors
Social and cultural factors
Personal and psychological factors

Characteristics of perpetrators of intimate partner violence.


Managing intimate partner violence in the workplace.
Workplaces policy and procedures
Workplaces role in responding to disclosure of intimate partner violence
Identifying security procedures
Personalized safety plans
Referring employees to the EAP
Ensuring victims are not discriminated against on performance appraisals, etc.
Encouraging disclosure: Keeping stereotypes and biases in check

Discussion of case
General discussion
APPENDIX 4: RESOURCES ON INTIMATE PARTNER
VIOLENCE AS A WORKPLACE ISSUE
Abusive Men Exploring New Directions
www.amendinc.org
American Bar Association Commission on Domestic Violence
www.abanet.org/domviol/home.html
American Domestic Violence Crisis Line
www.awoscentral.com
American Psychological Association
www.apa.org
Asian and Pacic Islander Institute on Domestic Violence
www.apiahf.org/apidvinstitute
Battered Womens Justice Project
www.bwjp.org

132

Mental and Physical Health

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention


www.cdc.gov
Department of Justice Violence Against Women Ofce
www.ojp.usdoj.gov/vawo
Domestic Violence Clearinghouse and Legal Hotline
www.stoptheviolence.org
Equal Employment Opportunity Commission
www.eeoc.org
Family Violence Prevention Fund
Endabuse.org
Institute on Domestic Violence in the African American Community
www.dvinstitute.org
National Center for Victims of Crime
www.ncvc.org
National Center on Domestic and Sexual Violence
www.ncdsv.org
National Coalition Against Domestic Violence
www.ncadv.org
National Domestic Violence Hotline
www.ndvh.org
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health
www.niosh.gov
National Latino Alliance for the Elimination of Domestic Violence
www.dvalianza.org
National Organization for Women
www.now.org
National Resource Center on Domestic Violence
www.nrcdv.org
Occupational Safety and Health Association
www.osha.gov
Safe at Work Coalition
www.safeatworkcoalition.org
Society for Human Resource Management
www.shrm.org
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services
www.4woman.gov/violence/index.cfm
Violence Against Women Ofce
www.ovw.usdoj.gov
Violence Prevention
www.ama-assn.org/ama/pub/category/3242.html

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

133

REFERENCES
American Bar Association Commission on Domestic Violence. (1999). A guide
for employees: Domestic violence in the workplace. Washington, DC: Author.
American Institute on Domestic Violence. (2009). Domestic violence in the
workplace statistics. Retrieved July 8, 2009 from http:/www.aidv-usa.com/
statistics.htm
Barbazette, J. (2006). Training needs assessment: Methods, tools and techniques.
New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Barling, J., Rogers, A., & Kelloway, E. (2001). Behind closed doors: In-home
workers experience of sexual harassment and workplace violence. Journal
of Occupational Health Psychology, 6, 225269.
Birns, B. (1999). Battered wives: Causes, effects and social change. In C. Forden,
A. Hunter, & B. Birns (Eds.), Readings in the psychology of women: Dimensions of the female experience. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Black, D., & Newman, M. (2000). Children: Secondary victims of domestic
violence. In A. Shalev, R. Yehuda, & A. McFarlane. (Eds.), International
handbook of human responses to trauma. New York: Plenum.
Bostock, J., Plumpton, M., & Pratt, R. (2009). Domestic violence against women:
Understanding social processes and womens experiences. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 19, 95110.
Brown, J. (2002). Training needs assessment: A must for developing an effective
training program. Public Personnel Management, 31, 569578.
Burlington Industries v. Ellerth, 524 U.S. 742 (1998).
Butts Stahly, G. (1999). Violence against women by male partners: Prevalence,
outcomes and policy implications. American Psychologist, 48, 10771087.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2009). Retrieved July 8, 2009 from
http://www.cdc.gov/ncipc/ub-res/ipv
Coleman, V. (1991). Violence in lesbian couples: A between groups comparison.
Doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology, Los
Angeles, 1990. Dissertation Abstracts International, 51, 5634B.
Crouhy, M., Galai, D., & Mark, R. (2005). The essentials of risk management. New
York: McGraw Hill.
Cunradi, C., Ames, G., & Moore, R. (2008). Prevalence and correlates of intimate partner violence among a sample of construction industry workers.
Journal of Family Violence, 23, 101112.
DeCenzo, D., & Robbins, S. (2007). Fundamentals of human resource management.
New York: Wiley.
Dessler, G. (2009). Fundamentals of human resource management. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Eastman, W. (1998). Working for position: Women, men and managerial work
hours. Industrial Relations, 37, 5166.
Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (1999). Enforcement guidance:
Vicarious employer liability for unlawful harassment by supervisors.
Washington, DC: Equal Employment Opportunity Commission.
Faragher v. City of Boca Raton, 524 U.S. 725 (1998).
Frisbie, S. (2002). Sexual harassment: A comparison of on-line versus traditional
training methods. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas Tech University,
Lubbock, TX.

134

Mental and Physical Health

Frone, M., & Yardley, J. (1996). Workplace family-supportive programmes: Predictors of employed parents importance ratings. Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology, 69, 351366.
Goldstein, I., & Ford, J. (2002). Training in organizations: Needs assessment, development and evaluation (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Graham, D., & Rawlings, E. (1999). Observers blaming of battered wives: Who,
what, when, and why (pp. 5594). In M. Paludi (Ed.), The psychology of
sexual victimization: A handbook. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Graziano, A., & Raulin, M. (1996). Research methods: A process of inquiry. White
Plains, NY: Longman.
Grothues, C., & Marmion, S. (2006). Dismantling the myths about intimate violence against women. In P. Lundberg-Love & S. Marmion (Eds.), Intimate
violence against women (pp. 914). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Gurchiek, K. (2005, March). Study: Domestic violence spills over into workplace. HR Magazine. Retrieved July 9, 2009 from http://ndarticles.com/
p/articles/mi_m34950/ai_n13247058.
Horne, S. (1999). Domestic violence in Russia. American Psychologist, 54, 5561.
Kelly, E., & Mullen, J. (2006). Organizational response to workplace violence.
In K. Kelloway, J. Barling, & J. Hurrell (Eds.), Handbook of workplace
violence. New York: Sage.
Kirkpatrick, D. (1959). Techniques for evaluating training programs. Journal of
the American Society for Training Development, 13, 39.
Kirkpatrick, D. (1998). Evaluating training programs: The four levels. San
Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler.
Langford, L. (2006). Preventing violence and promoting safety in higher education settings: Overview of a comprehensive approach. Newton, MA: Higher Education
Center for Alcohol and Other Drug Abuse and Violence Prevention.
Lemon, N. (2001). Domestic violence law. St. Paul, MN: West Group.
Levy, A., & Paludi, M. (2001). Workplace sexual harassment (2nd ed.). Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Lloyd, S., & Taluc, N. (1999). The effects of male violence on female employment. Violence Against Women, 5, 370392.
Lucier, K. (2008). A consultative training program: Collateral effect of a needs
assessment. Communication Education, 57, 482489.
Lundberg-Love, P., & Marmion, S. (2006). Intimate violence against women.
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Lundberg-Love, P., & Wilkerson, D. (2006). Battered women. In P. LundbergLove & S. Marmion (Eds.), Intimate violence against women (pp. 3145).
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Marmion, S., & Faulkner, D. (2006). Effects of class and culture on intimate
partner violence. In P. Lundberg-Love & S. Marmion (Eds.), Intimate violence against women (pp. 131143). Westport, CT: Praeger.
McHugh, M., & Frieze, I. (2006). Intimate partner violence. In F. Denmark,
H. Krauss, E. Halpern, & J. Sechzer (Eds.), Violence and exploitation against
women and girls (pp. 121141). Boston: Blackwell Publishing.
McHugh, M., Livingston, N., & Frieze, I. (2008). Intimate partner violence: Perspectives on research and intervention. In F. Denmark & M. Paludi (Eds.),
Psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories, Westport, CT: Praeger
(pp. 555589).

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

135

Middelkoop, J., Gilhooley, J., Ruepp, N., Polikoski, R., & Paludi, M. (2008).
Campus security recommendations for administrators to help prevent and
deal with violence. In M. Paludi (Ed.), Understanding and preventing campus
violence (pp. 234235). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Mitchell, M., Hargrove, G., Collins, M., Thompson, M., Reddick, T., &
Kaslow, N. (2006). Coping variables that mediate the relation between intimate partner violence and mental health outcomes among low-income
African American women. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 15031520.
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health. (2009). www.niosh.gov
Occupational Health and Safety Administration. (2009). www.osha.org
OLeary, K., & Maiuro, R. (Eds.). (2001). Psychological abuse in violent domestic
relations. New York: Springer.
Paludi, M. (2002). The psychology of women (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Paludi, M., & Barickman, R. (1998). Sexual harassment, work, and education:
A resource manual for prevention. Albany: State University of New York Press.
Paludi, M., & Neidermeyer, P. (Eds.). (2006). Work, life and family imbalance:
How to level the playing eld. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Paludi, M., Nydegger, R., & Paludi, C. (2006). Understanding workplace violence:
A guide for managers and employees. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Paludi, C., & Paludi, M. (2000, October). Developing and enforcing an effective
workplace policy statement, procedures, and training programs on domestic violence. Paper presented at the conference on domestic violence as a workplace concern: Legal, psychological, management, and law enforcement
perspectives, Nashua, NH.
Paludi, M., & Paludi, C. (Eds.). (2003). Academic and workplace sexual harassment.
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Paludi, M., & Paludi, C. (2006). Integrating work/life: Resources for employees,
employers, and human resource specialists. In M. Paludi & P. Neidermeyer
(Eds.), (2006). Work, life and family imbalance: How to level the playing eld
(pp. 122154). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Paludi, M., Vaccariello, R., Graham, T., Smith, M., Allen-Dicker, K., Kasprzak,
H., et al. (2006). Work/life integration: Impact on womens career, employment and family. In M. Paludi & P. Neidermeyer (Eds.), Work, life and family
imbalance: How to level the playing eld (pp. 2136). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Partnership for Prevention. (2002). Domestic violence and the workplace. Washington,
DC: Author.
Potoczniak, M., Murot, J., Crosbie-Burnett, M., & Potoczni, A. (2003). Legal and
psychological perspectives on same-sex domestic violence. Journal of Family
Psychology, 17, 252259.
Randel, J., & Wells, K. (2003). Corporate approaches to reducing intimate partner violence through workplace initiatives. Clinical Occupation and Environmental Medicine, 3, 821841.
Rathus, J., & Feindler, E. (2004). Assessment of partner violence: A handbook for
researchers and practitioners. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Reeves, C., & OLeary-Kelly, A. (2007). The effects and costs of intimate partner
violence for work organizations. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 22,
327344.

136

Mental and Physical Health

Ridley, E. (2004). Impact of domestic violence offenders on occupational safety and


health: A pilot study. Augusta, ME: Family Crisis Services, Maine Department of Labor.
Roper, A. (2002). Corporate leaders on domestic violence awareness of the problem,
how its affecting their business, and what theyre doing to address it. New York:
Liz Claiborne, Inc.
Rothman, E., Hathaway, J., Stidsen, A., & deVries, H. (2007). How employment
helps female victims of intimate partner violence: A qualitative study.
Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12, 136143.
Ryan, K., Frieze, I., & Sinclair, H. C. (1999). Physical violence in dating relationships. In M. Paludi (Ed.), The psychology of sexual victimization: A handbook
(pp. 3354). Westport, CT: Greenwood.
Sagrestano, L., Carroll, D., Rodriguez, A., & Nuwayhid, B. (2004). Demographic, psychological and relationship factors in domestic violence during
pregnancy in a sample of low-income women of color. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 28, 309322.
Slavin, R. (2008). Educational psychology: Theory and practice (9th ed.). Boston:
Allyn & Bacon.
Smith, S., & Mazin, R. (2004). The HR answer book. New York: AMACOM.
Society for Human Resource Management. (2009). www.shrm.org
Soroptimist International of the Americas. (2007). White paper: Domestic violence as
a workplace concern. Retrieved June 22, 2009, from http://staging.soroptimist.
org/whitepapers/wp_dv.html
Straus, M., Gelles, R., & Steinmetz, S. (1980). Behind closed doors: Violence in the
American family. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books.
Swanberg, J., & Logan, T. (2005). Domestic violence and employment: A qualitative study of the effects of domestic violence on womens employment.
Journal of Occupational and Health Psychology, 10, 317.
Swanberg, J., & Logan, T. (2007). Intimate partner violence, employment and
the workplaces: An interdisciplinary perspective. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 22, 263267.
Swanberg, J., Logan, T., & Macke, C. (2005). Intimate partner violence, employment and the workplace: Consequences and future directions. Trauma,
Violence and Abuse, 6, 286312.
Swanberg, J., Macke, C., & Logan, T. (2006). Intimate partner violence, women,
and work: Coping on the job. Violence and Victims, 21, 561578.
Swanberg, J., Macke, C., & Logan, T. (2007). Working women making it work:
Intimate partner violence, employment, and workplace support. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 22, 292391.
Tan, J., Hall, R., & Boyce, C. (2003). The role of employee reactions in predicting training effectiveness. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 14,
397411.
Tjaden, P., & Thoennes, N. (2000). Full report of the prevalence, incidence and consequences of violence against women: Findings from the National Violence
Against Women Survey, NIJ/CDC. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
Justice. (NCJ 183781).
Tran, C., & DesJardins, K. (2000). Domestic violence in Vietnamese refugee and
Korean immigrant communities. In J. Chin (Ed.), Relationships among Asian
American women. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Intimate Partner Violence as a Workplace Concern

137

Tyler, K. (2002). Evaluating evaluations. HR Magazine, 47.


Urban, B. (2000). Anonymous foundation domestic abuse prevention program evaluation: Final client survey report. Chicago, IL: The University of Illinois at
Chicago.
Walker, L. (1979). The battered woman. New York: Harper & Row.
Walker, L. (1995). Foreword. In L. Adler & F. Denmark (Eds.), Violence and the
prevention of violence (pp. ixxii). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Walker, L. (1999). Psychology and domestic violence around the world.
American Psychologist, 54, 2129.
Walker, L. (2006). Battered woman syndrome: Empirical ndings. In F. Denmark,
H. Krauss, E. Halpern, & J. Sechzer (Eds.), Violence and exploitation against
women and girls (pp. 142157). Boston: Blackwell Publishing.
Watlington, C., & Murphy, C. (2006). The roles of religion and spirituality
among African American survivors of domestic violence. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 62, 837857.
Wetterstein, K., Rudolph, S., Paul, K., Gallagher, K., Trang, B., Adams, K., et al.
(2004). Freedom through self-sufciency: A qualitative examination of the
impact of violence on the working lives of women in shelter. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 51, 447462.
Willman, S. (2007). Too much, too long? Domestic violence in the workplace. Testimony before the U.S. Senate Subcommittee on Employment and Workplace Safety.
Zorza, J. (2002). Battering. In J. Zorza (Ed.), Violence against women. Kingston,
NJ: Civic Research Institute.

This page intentionally left blank

Chapter 6

From Victim to Empowered Survivor:


Feminist Therapy with Survivors of
Rape and Sexual Assault
Avigail Moor

Sexual violence against women is a widespread phenomenon affecting


a large percentage of women. According to available data, approximately 20 to 40 percent of all women will experience some form of sexual assault during their lifetime (Casey & Nurius, 2006; Koss, 1983;
Koss & Harvey, 1991; Moor, 2009). Rape and other forms of sexual violence are traumatic experiences that commonly result in severe psychological sequelae. These include posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD),
depression, anxiety, loss of meaning, self-devaluation, shame, and
impaired sexual functioning (Angell, 1994; Burnam, Stein, Golding, &
Siegel, 1988; Foa & Rothbaum, 1998; Gilbert, 1994; Herman, 1992;
Jaycox, Zoellner, & Foa, 2002; Lebowitz & Roth, 1994).
While the posttraumatic aspects of the clinical presentation of rape
survivors are similar to those noted in the aftermath of other types of
trauma (Foa & Rothbaum, 1998; Jaycox et al, 2002; Meadows & Foa,
1998), many of the additional symptoms observed are rape-specic and
socially determined (Koss & Harvey, 1991; Lebowitz & Roth, 1994; Moor,
2007). Sexual violence against women occurs in a very particular social
context, characterized by prejudiced, victim-blaming attitudes toward
survivors that fault, shame, and isolate the victim of the assault. Thus,
rather than receiving social support and comfort, as is typical in the case
of most other forms of trauma, victims of sexual violence are often
scorned and abandoned. This can lead to extreme levels of self-blame,
guilt, and shame, which incessantly torment victims of sexual violence.

140

Mental and Physical Health

Although some form of self-blame is common to the experience of


trauma in general, the particularly insidious rape-specic self-disdain is
incremental to the more customary self-blame, and a serious complication to recovery. Yet this reality is frequently overlooked in therapies
uninformed by the social analysis of the aftermath of rape to the detriment of survivors; hence the necessity for a therapeutic model that incorporates this understanding on a most fundamental level.
Feminist therapy is such an approach. Rooted in a contextual conceptualization of female psychological distress, it is the optimal perspective from which to treat survivors of rape. This chapter is intended
to delineate the principles of feminist therapy that are expected to augment all forms of treatment for rape survivors, regardless of theoretical
orientation. It is a philosophy of treatment and shared principles that
are meant to accompany all theoretical orientations and approaches to
treatment. It should be stated that while the focus of this chapter is on
the treatment of rape and sexual assault survivors, much of its content
applies equally to victims of other forms of sexual violence such as sexual harassment and child sexual abuse, although each of these has its
unique elements that must be addressed as well.
Having worked extensively with rape and sexual assault survivors
for more than two decades at two regional rape crisis centers and in
private practice, I have come to believe that it is the feminist aspects of
the treatment, particularly the social analysis and the egalitarian stance
of the therapist that benet these clients most. This holds true in my
experience for the majority of victims. The population that I have
worked with over the years has been highly variable in age, ethnic
background, socioeconomic status, and preassault psychological functioning, as well as in previous exposure to trauma. Their clinical presentations generally t the typical rape survivors prole, with the
majority exhibiting PTSD symptoms along with rape-specic symptomatology. Yet all have responded favorably above all else to the feminist
tenets of the treatment.
This is not to negate the importance, and even the necessity, of specic trauma resolution interventions such as eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR; Power et al., 2002; Rogers & Silver,
2002; Shapiro & Maxeld, 2002), cognitivebehavioral therapy (CBT;
Foa & Rothbaum, 1998; Jaycox et al, 2002; Meadows & Foa, 1998), prolonged exposure (Foa, Zoellner, Feeny, Hembree, & Alvarez-Conrad,
2002; Lee, Gavriel, Drummond, & Greenwald, 2002; Pitman, Altman,
Greenwald, Longpre, Machlin, Poire, & Steketee, 1991), and other
modalities. All of these techniques are positively effective in treating
the PTSD aspects of the presenting problem. However, there is also
extensive evidence that these interventions alone may not sufce in
relieving survivors sense of disempowerment and guilt, which often
persist after PTSD symptoms have subsided if not targeted directly.

From Victim to Empowered Survivor

141

For example, Pitman et al. reported that while survivors treated with
ooding showed considerable decrease in anxiety, their feelings of
shame and blame did not respond to further exposure to the narrative.
A similar account is provided in Meadows and Foas case study of a
survivor whose anxiety symptoms responded well to prolonged exposure, while her tendency to berate herself for having been at the bar
where she was assaulted actually increased. They report that these feelings of guilt and shame were not alleviated by further exposure to the
assault narrative. The integration of feminist principles, with their contextual and egalitarian emphases, into the therapeutic process offers us
the chance to provide our clients with the ultimate empowerment
needed for recovery. These principles are outlined next.
FEMINIST PSYCHOTHERAPY
Feminist therapy is a philosophy of psychotherapy rather than a distinct orientation (Hill & Ballou, 1998; Moradi, Fischer, Hill, Jome, &
Blum, 2000; Rader & Gilbert, 2005). Being woman-centered, it underscores the social context of womens distress, maintaining that
womens pain cannot be fully understood outside of social context.
Social norms, values, and attitudes are seen as greatly contributing
to the creation and maintenance of many of the problems and issues
that are brought into therapy (Feminist Therapy Institute, 1990; Hill &
Ballou, 1998; Kaschak, 1992; Worell & Remer, 1992; Worell & Johnson,
2001). Each individuals personal experiences and situations are viewed
as reective of and inuenced by societys attitudes and values. Therefore, one of the goals of therapy is to afford the clients an awareness of
these inuences so as to bring about change in perspective and an
understanding of the interactions between the various social factors
and the clients internal experiences (Brown & Brodsky, 1992; Hill &
Ballou, 1998; Rader & Gilbert, 2005). Although feminist therapists are
trained in a variety of disciplines and theoretical orientations, they are
united amid this diversity by their feminist analyses and perspectives
regarding the interactive effects of the clients internal and external
worlds.
Another central tenet of feminist therapy stemming from feminist
philosophy is a commitment to the empowerment of women and the
validation and valuing of their experience and viewpoint. Caring, compassion and respect are, accordingly, the foundation of this approach
to treatment (Brown & Brodsky; 1992; Moradi et al., 2000; Rader &
Gilbert, 2005; Worell & Remer, 1992). It is a relationship in which clients are empowered to nd their strengths and strivings, a practice in
which growth-promoting reframing of social realities allows for new
ways of thinking and being. It is a process of connectedness in which
each client is made to feel worthy of respect, affection, tenderness, and

142

Mental and Physical Health

judgment-free acceptance, an endeavor of caring that identies each


clients unique and positive qualities so as to enhance her self esteem.
In addition, the principles of feminist therapy call for an egalitarian
therapeutic relationship, which is considered a core tenet of this
approach to treatment. Recognizing the negative impact of the genderbased power imbalance in the lives of most women, feminist therapy is
intent on maintaining an egalitarian structure within its bounds (Feminist Therapy Institute, 1990; Hill & Ballou, 1998; Jordan, Kaplan, Miller,
Stiver, & Surrey, 1991; Rader & Gilbert, 2005). Accordingly, feminist
therapists are committed to maximizing the equality between therapist
and client by exercising power-sharing behaviors of various kinds.
These include refraining from placing themselves in the one-up position, working collaboratively with the client, viewing the client as her
own expert, treating her with complete respect and belief in her
strengths and knowledge, informing the client of the therapy process,
allowing the client to guide the sessions, and so on (Hill, 1990; Gilbert,
1980; Hill & Ballou, 1998; Rader & Gilbert, 2005; Worell & Remer,
1992). Equalizing the relationship with the client is seen as a primary
responsibility of the therapist, who routinely evaluates her ongoing
interactions with her clients for any evidence of hierarchy, biases or
discriminatory attitudes and practices. Moreover, she accepts responsibility for taking action to confront and change any interfering, oppressing, or devaluing biases she may have.
Feminist therapy is also devoted to the creation of social change and
the valuing of the female perspective. Therapy from this perspective is
intended to bring about individual as well as social change through
raising awareness to the oppressive social forces impacting the clients
wellbeing and offering new ways of being and perceiving. This process
reverberates beyond the bounds of therapy into ever-growing circles of
awareness and action. In order to increase the social impact of their
practice, feminist therapists attempt to reach out to large segments of
the population. They also may engage in broadly dened social activism in addition to their professional work.
It is suggested that the principles of feminist therapy offer the best
conceptual framework for understanding the myriad of symptoms
exhibited by survivors of sexual aggression. Treatment from this
perspective is likely to deliver optimally suited interventions to this
population, while providing the kind of empowering interpersonal
experiences that can counteract the utter disregard sustained in rape.
We therefore turn next to a discussion of the application of feminist
therapy to the treatment of survivors of rape and sexual assault. The
discussion begins with a feminist analysis of the social context of the
sexual victimization of women, so as to provide a conceptual foundation for the therapy. Application to treatment of each tenet of feminist
therapy is considered thereafter.

From Victim to Empowered Survivor

143

APPLICATION OF FEMINIST THERAPY TO THE TREATMENT


OF SURVIVORS OF RAPE AND SEXUAL ASSAULT
Feminist Analysis of Sexual Violence against Women
In accordance with the focus of feminist therapy on the social context of individual psychological distress, the treatment of victims of
sexual violence is rooted in an analysis of the interplay between social
factors and internal responses to the events. In this analysis, sexual violence against women is considered to be an integral part of the social
oppression of women. Similarly, societal attitudes toward the victims
of this aggression are viewed as constituting one of the central public
forces to negatively impact womens lives and wellbeing subsequent to
rape. Following is an analysis of the social context of rape followed by
an application of feminist therapy guidelines to the treatment of its
aftermath.
Social Context of Rape
Rape and other forms of sexual violence epitomize the social oppression of women. In fact, from the feminist standpoint, sexual violence
against women is one of the primary means by which patriarchy aims
to keep women in their place (McKinnon, 2005). The astronomical
prevalence rates of all forms of sexual violence directed at women,
coupled with the utter failure of the system to combat this menace,
suggest that society is bent, at best, on regulating rather than eliminating it. Katherine McKinnon refers to this social order as the Rape Culture. According to her, sexual violence against women is rooted in a
discriminatory social hierarchy, which both mobilizes and perpetuates
the abuse of women. The ideology of this system amounts to an eroticization of womens oppression and domination, thereby turning this
type of violence into conceivable and even appealing for many individual males, while abandoning the victims in the process. Feminist therapy, anchored in such a conceptualization of sexual violence against
women, can empower victims greatly by helping them to realize that
their victimization was not due to any contribution of their own, but
was rather part of the systematic victimization of women in patriarchy.
Consistent with the social function of sexual violence as a means of
keeping women in line, social attitudes toward victims of these crimes
are oppressive and damaging as well. In fact, the present social environment is extremely detrimental for rape survivors, with societal victimblaming and revictimization being rampant (Campbell, 1998; Campbell
& Raja, 1999; Campbell, Wasco, Ahrens, Se, & Barnes, 2001). As a consequence, rape survivors are commonly burdened with an external, societal assault of prejudiced collective attitudes that blame and shame them
for being raped (Ardovini-Brooker & Caringella-MacDonald, 2002; Burt,

144

Mental and Physical Health

1980). These stereotyped attitudes, generally subsumed in the term rape


myths (Lonsway & Fitzgerald, 1994), amount to a second victimization,
and are frequently internalized by survivors, often generating an array
of severe self-denigrating cognitions and emotional suffering.
VICTIM-BLAMING AND SECONDARY VICTIMIZATION
Present society is laden with a wide array of detrimental rape
myths. Typical myths revolve around the notion that victims somehow
contributed to their own victimization and are responsible for its occurrence (Koss & Harvey, 1991). Victim precipitation is the most common
and well-known rape myth, which directly holds the victim responsible
for the rape (Cowan, 2000). It is founded on a belief that the victim
in some way provoked the rape. It claims that she asked to be raped
by engaging in unsafe behavior, such as drinking or hitchhiking; by
placing herself in unsafe situations, such as bars or dark streets; by
wearing revealing clothes, or by behaving promiscuously (nice
girls dont get raped). Rape myths further proclaim that women can
prevent rape if they truly want to, and that no woman can be forced to
have sex against her will. Hence, if a woman is raped, she obviously
did not mind it or worse yet, secretly desired it. She may also be
viewed as having been dishonored by the rape (e.g., rape is shameful,
it only happens to certain types of women), particularly, but not solely,
if she belongs to an ethnic minority or to a low SES (Alvidrez, 1999;
Wyatt, 1992).
Finally, there are two related rape myths regarding acquaintance
rape (the most prevalent form of rape); that victims of such rapes precipitate the event by leading the perpetrators on, and that women say
no to sexual advances when they actually mean yes, and hence
there was no rape at all.
There are many indications that endorsement of rape myths is fairly
pervasive within the legal, law enforcement, and medical establishment
(Campbell, 1998; Ullman & Filipas, 2001). This has been referred to as
secondary victimization or second rape. There is also evidence that
responses of formal support providers may vary in relation to the victims ethnicity, such that minority women may encounter even harsher
reactions than do their majority counterparts (Ullman & Filipas, 2001;
Ullman & Brecklin, 2002; Wyatt, 1992).
Some evidence points to similar revictimization of rape victims
within the mental health profession (Campbell, 1998; Campbell, Wasco,
Ahrens, Se, & Barnes, 2001). In a survey of licensed mental health
professionals who work extensively with rape survivors, Campbell and
Raja (1999) found that 58 percent of the therapists surveyed expressed
the view that mental health professionals may carry out some harmful
counseling practices when treating this population. Entirely unaware at

From Victim to Empowered Survivor

145

times of their own prejudices regarding rape and rape survivors, some
clinicians may be inclined to view survivors as contributing in some
way to the occurrence of the rape. Consequently, they may engage
fairly regularly in the interpretation of survivors motives and behaviors in matters related to the assault. For example, analyzing a survivors motives for accompanying a romantic partner to his place, where
she was then raped, is a case in point. This multitude of adverse social
factors can inict serious psychological harm on survivors.
THE PSYCHOLOGICAL SEQUELAE OF THE SOCIAL
CONTEXT OF RAPE
The socially contextual elements of rape and other forms of sexual
violence seriously complicate the post-assault clinical presentation of
many survivors. This appears to hold true across varied cultural and
ethnic backgrounds. Not only must survivors deal with the aftereffects
of the humiliating violation that is rape, but also with the devastation
of the collective victim-blaming and its internalization. Together, these
factors inict severe agony and pain upon survivors, causing them
unbearable levels of self-blame and shame (Koss & Harvey, 1991;
Moor, 2007; Ullman, 1996).
The all too common accusation that the rape victim precipitated the
assault can exacerbate her self-accusation and contempt, heightening her
already strong tendency to berate herself for her carelessness (e.g.,
How could I have been so stupid . . . putting myself in such a situation
and allowing this to happen). The belief that she is to blame for being
attacked can make the survivor doubt her own perceptions, invalidating
her lived experience. While hurting to the core, she may nonetheless
consequently feel that she got exactly what she deserved (e.g., I
decided to hitchhike home so I deserve what I got. . . . there can be no
sympathy towards me). The pain and despair that result are massive.
The charge that the rape occurred because of the type of woman she is
coupled with the widespread notion that rape is dishonoring, routinely
leads to extreme shame and self-disdain (e.g., I am so ashamed; no one
can ever know of this). The dehumanizing nature of rape, intended to
denigrate and demean, typically creates a sense of humiliation, desecration, and helplessness, all of which tend to lead to intolerable shame,
disgrace, and self-disgust (e.g., I am utterly disgusting).
While the brunt of the post-assault self-blame is socially driven, it is important to keep in mind that blaming the self also has a function in dealing
with trauma (Koss & Harvey, 1991). It provides survivors with some
(albeit illusionary) sense of control over the traumatic events designed to
counteract the experience of helplessness. If a rape survivor believes that
her behavior contributed to the assault in some way, she can believe that
changing this particular behavior in the future will prevent a reoccurrence

146

Mental and Physical Health

of the event. It is important to be aware of this protective function of selfblame and guilt when treating this population.
Encountering victim-blaming often discourages survivors from disclosing the incident, thereby reducing their likelihood of obtaining social
support that could facilitate their recovery (Botta & Pingree, 1997). For
example, some stereotypes may cause survivors to doubt that their experience qualies as real rape, which may keep them, in turn, from seeking help. This problem appears to be especially, although not uniquely,
common among survivors of date rape (Alvidrez, 1999; Ullman &
Brecklin, 2002).
Similarly, accusatory and demeaning social attitudes may drive a
sizeable proportion of survivors to isolate themselves from loved ones,
friends, and family for on many occasions rape myths are actually
accepted and believed by survivors close social and familial environment (Sheldon & Parent, 2002; Ullman, 1999). It is not uncommon to
encounter victim-blaming on the part of those closest to the victim, in
the form of dismay that she behaved in a certain way or failed to do
one thing or another. Shame among family members is also common.
It may take the form of urging the survivor to keep quiet about the
assault and pretend it never happened or of outright disgracing her for
having been violated. This can be devastating.
Survivors may be further harmed if upon turning to the system for
help they meet with second victimization and rape myth endorsement
by members of the helping professions. This can increase their feelings
of culpability and shame, as well as cause them to feel completely
invalidated, misunderstood, and judged. Moreover, survivors are likely
to become distrustful of others, making them reluctant to seek further
help (Campbell & Raja, 1999; Ullman & Filipas, 2001). In fact, a majority of the therapists surveyed in Campbell & Rajas (1999) study voiced
a concern that interaction with community professionals can actually,
at times, worsen the state of survivors mental health. In comparison,
less than half (48 percent) of the clinicians surveyed believed that contacting community professionals had positive outcomes for rape victims. For this reason, 85 percent of therapists expressed a belief that
clinicians must be made more aware of the risks of secondary victimization and its repercussions for the effectiveness of treatment, to avoid
inicting further harm on survivors and to fully maximize the efcacy
of therapy. Feminist therapy incorporates such awareness, and in view
of that can offer specic treatment principles for working with rape
survivors as delineated next.
TREATMENT PRINCIPLES
In light of the socially contextual aspects of much of the pain experienced by survivors of rape and other sexual assaults, there can be little

From Victim to Empowered Survivor

147

doubt that their treatment must be rooted in a feminist conceptualization and analysis if the correction of the effects of the social factors is
to take place. In the absence of a thorough comprehension of the combined impact of rape and related victim-blaming rape myths on survivors wellbeing, therapy stands the risk of lacking in effectiveness.
Even the direct reprocessing of the traumatic events will be augmented
by approaching it from a socially contextual viewpoint. In this process,
each tenet of feminist therapy has a particularly healing function as
delineated next.

EGALITARIAN THERAPEUTIC RELATIONSHIP


Being predicated on nonjudgmental respect and genuine belief in
every womans innate value and strength, feminist therapy offers survivors the opportunity to restore their sense of dignity and self-worth
so brutally shattered by the sexual assault. At its core, this treatment
aims to counter the humiliation and dehumanization sustained in the
attack, while restoring a sense of dignity and self-worth. The type of
non-hierarchical, genuinely caring and compassionate empathy that
this approach to treatment is built upon plays a crucial, curative role in
the recovery from rape, as it imparts a sense of complete validation of
every aspect of each survivors experience. The kind of therapeutic
relationship in which each woman is made to feel worthy in all
respects, aims to foster a sense of trust, as it communicates a true
acceptance of all of her feelings and thoughts, her choices and needs.
Through these interventions, the therapist aims to convey an accurate
appraisal of the suffering endured by survivors, provide support, and
relief; and at the same time, afrm survivors emotional reality (Paivio
& Laurent, 2001; Ullman, 1999).
Out of respect for our clients who have been sexually victimized, we
aspire to gain comprehensive familiarity with all aspects of the reaction
to rape and sexual assault in order to be able to unequivocally validate
their experience and help them to put it into words. Imparting our
knowledge of what rape survivors have been known to experience provides each individual survivor with a sense of safety and trust, as well
as being understood. Her experience is normalized as she is assured
that it is a shared one, typical of other women who have been through
similar horror. This is usually highly comforting and reassuring, as it
gives survivors reassurance that their reactions are not unusual or
pathologic, as they often fear, but rather normal reactions to an abnormal situation (Herman, 1992; Koss & Harvey, 1992). In essence we
strive to use our expertise and experience to benet the client, without
taking control or power that rightfully belong to her (Feminist Therapy
Institute, 1990; Hill & Ballou, 1998; Rader & Gilbert, 2005). In the

148

Mental and Physical Health

process, the survivor begins to regain a sense of control that she was
robbed of during the assault.
By viewing the survivor as an equal, we approach therapy with the
idea that we are on this journey together, jointly aiming for her full
recovery. Our true and genuine presence gives her a sense that she matters, that she is not alone. It counteracts social isolation and loss of trust,
as it restores her self-dignity and worth. We give her strength to withstand the devastation, as we believe in her innate coping skills. We also
commit to standing by her every step of the way. A survivor expressed
the feelings that arise, Even though the pain is unbearable almost all of
the time, knowing that you are truly with me, I dont feel so alone and
actually am starting to believe that I might actually make it.
We believe in her strengths and her ability to help herself. As she
feels powerless, helpless, and despondent, we offer her hope by reframing her position as survivor rather than victim. She may nd this view
hard to accept at rst, and so we stand by her with full respect, hoping
that as we gently point to those aspects of her experience that make her
a survivor in our mind, she joins us in this view before long.
The egalitarian position translates into genuine solidarity with the
survivors that we treat. While assisting them to process the traumatic
events, we share in their pain and permit ourselves to be touched by
it, without being overwhelmed or derailed from our therapeutic stance.
Recognizing how devastating the experience of sexual violence can be,
we validate all aspects of the emotional reality that ensues. We believe
in each survivors inner wisdom, and we let her know it. We guide her
to that place of authentic knowledge, so that she may come to believe
in herself and in her own perceptions and thoughts. Where she doubts
her own experience, we stand by her conveying our belief in her ability
to nd all the answers within herself. We never doubt her account of
the assault, thus helping her to not question it herself. Nor do we
impose our conceptualization or formulation on her, but rather attune
ourselves most empathically to hers.
From our respectful view of survivors as blame-free in every way,
we never pass judgment on their conduct and do our utmost to help
ease their sense of blameworthiness and shame, so often contextually
heightened. In communicating nonjudgmental acceptance and respect,
we aim to counteract the widespread social judgment and reproach
(e.g., It is so heartbreaking to hear you berating and devaluing yourself so). This mirrors a different perspective on the selfthat of the
one who has been wronged, not the one to be blamedwhile conveying supportive caring at the same time. Self-empathy and compassion
are expected to follow, and to give way, in turn, to afrming views of
self in place of the existing self-loathing and guilt.
As survivors deal with their sense of self-disgust and delement,
desecration and shame, our genuine belief that they have not been

From Victim to Empowered Survivor

149

damaged in any way, can help survivors attend to and verbalize their
internal experience regarding the sense of self as deled and shameful.
Empathetic mirroring of their feelings of violation and humiliation can
help contain these emotions. Insisting that they are shame-free (e.g., It
sounds like your self-disgust is so impossible to bear . . . we will not
rest until we nd a way to completely free you of it) can help alleviate her shame and foster the emergence of a valued and worthy sense
of self.
Treating the survivor as an equal and endowing her with the right
to determine the pace and direction of the therapy, can also play a
major role in enabling the reprocessing of the traumatic memories.
While attuning to the survivors levels of arousal, voicing our genuine
condence in her ability to sustain the process can afford her a sense
of safety, emotional modulation, reassurance, and support, all needed
to withstand the painful process of remembering and reliving the traumatic events (Moor, 2007; Paivio & Laurent, 2001). Moreover, experiencing our true caring for her through our soothing and containing
responses can help nurture the development of self-soothing capacities
and true hope.
As the process progresses, the therapists belief in the clients
strengths is steadily internalized. We enthusiastically share her cautious
optimism, while validating and strengthening her new perceptions of
self (e.g., Sounds like youre not blaming yourself anymore . . . how
exciting). Feedback from many survivors allows us to view the egalitarian stance as instrumental in consolidating a new outlook of the self and
the future.
EMPOWERMENT
The experience of rape and other forms of sexual violence can be
utterly disempowering to most victims, shattering their sense of selfworth, control, and dignity (Herman, 1992; Koss & Harvey, 1991; Moor,
2007). It is the role of therapy to counter this devastating experience by
helping survivors to regain a sense of personal power and control. A
central goal of feminist therapy, empowerment of the client and advocacy for women (Brown & Brodsky; 1992; Feminist Therapy Institute,
1990; Hill & Ballou, 1998; Moradi et al, 2000; Rader & Gilbert, 2005;
Worell & Remer, 1992), thus has a major role to play.
Beyond the empowerment inherent in the egalitarian nature of feminist therapy, contextual reframing of various aspects of the ordeal and
reconstruction of self-devaluing cognitions based on the analysis of
their social roots are employed to bring about a corrective perceptual
shift. Feminist therapy aims to provide clients with novel ways of seeing, along with helpful information that can facilitate this shift (Rader
& Gilbert, 2005). Realizing the degree to which survivors self-loathing

150

Mental and Physical Health

cognitions are rooted in internalized societal victim-blaming myths, the


feminist therapist thus suggests new ways of perceiving intended to
bring about meaningful cognitive shifts. Any internalized prejudiced
attitudes that may underlie the demeaning perception of the self are
recognized and subject to reconstruction. Clearly, all unfounded cognitions are fully processed, including those related to safety and ability
to tolerate stress (Foa & Rothbaum, 1998; Foa & Jaycox, 1999). However, rebuilding a positive and empowered sense of self is at the very
heart of the healing process.
Our main objective is restructuring the unfounded views of self as
culpable and shameful. The entire array of distorted cognitions regarding precipitation of the assault is targeted. In this process, we encourage the survivor to assess the rationality of these beliefs, so that she
can recognize their false nature and their roots in societal victimblaming rape myths. To increase her sense of mastery, self-conviction,
and empowerment, we aim to help the survivor absolve herself of culpability on her own, rather than through our insistence. Reaching this
conviction from within makes it more meaningful and lasting. A shift
to a more realistic blame-free viewpoint should ensue, allowing her to
appropriately place fault where it belongs, namely, with the assailant.
For example, a survivor who had held on to the notion that she was to
blame for being raped because she did not ght back was helped to
see that she stood no chance against her assailant who was considerably bigger and stronger than herself. Gentle probes such as How big
was he? How strong? Now consider your relative size and strength . . .
what could you have done? should facilitate the hoped-for shift.
To further validate survivors, it is useful point to the contextual origin of much of their self-blame and shame, while letting them know
that their response is entirely typical of most rape survivors, who generally feel responsible for their plight (e.g., Like you, almost every
rape survivor blames herself mercilessly for the ordeal she had
endured. . . . and all too often this is because of common, accusatory
social attitudes towards rape victims that absolutely cannot be
ignored). Survivors frequently experience great relief upon realizing
that their self-denigration is a characteristic response to the trauma of
rape, especially in the current social context.
At this stage it is also important to acknowledge and work through
the protective function of the self-blame. Following rape, many victims
worry that if none of what happened was due to their actions, they
may not have any control over future safety either (Koss & Harvey,
1991). Thus, they hold onto self-accusation and are reluctant to let go
of it in order to maintain the sense of control it provides. Helping them
to distinguish between liability and ill-advised behavior can promote
the complete relinquishment of this self-blame. For example, a survivor
raped while hitchhiking was unable to stop berating herself for her

From Victim to Empowered Survivor

151

stupid behavior, which caused the rape until she could feel that
she would be able to keep herself safe in the future. Interventions such
as while you now realize that hitchhiking isnt safe for you, the fact
that you hitchhiked in the past in no way makes you guilty of precipitating the rape eventually enabled her to replace the self-accusation
with a more realistic appraisal of her ability to recognize and to minimize danger in the future, while not having to hold herself responsible
for being raped.
Cognitive restructuring can also be used to target the shame and the
beliefs, common to many survivors, which they are disgraceful, disgusting, and deled for having been raped. These beliefs appear to
show up in one of two ways; a direct expression of shame in oneself
(i.e., I am so disgusting and damaged) or an indirect, masked manifestation (i.e., No one must ever know that I have been raped). Interventions aimed at this aspect of the injured self can gently encourage
survivors to examine the rationality of their shame (e.g., Did you hurt
anyone or were you the one being hurt . . . Who should be ashamed,
then?), which should help them to realize that in reality, they have
nothing to be ashamed of, and that, in fact, it is the assailant, as well
as society and its prejudiced attitudes that are shameful. A positive
sense of self is the expected outcome.
Finally, much of the empowerment afforded to survivors stems simply from raising their awareness of the social roots of their distress.
The notion of the personal being political underlies all interventions
aimed at freeing the survivor from internalized victim-blaming and
shame. It also provides answers to the questions regarding the reasons
for the assault that trouble them so. Why did it happen? Why to
me? If I am not to blame then how can I explain what happened?
Helping a survivor to realize that rape generally does not occur
because of any individual womans behavior, but rather is systematically embedded in patriarchal culture, offers her an alternative explanation for her plight. Redening male abusive behavior as
conformation to societys promotion of male violence can help her
make further sense of the events. Along the way, the changed perspective of each individual survivor is quite likely to reverberate beyond
the therapeutic process, contributing in some way to social change in
line with this particular tenet of feminist therapy.
In essence, by presenting survivors with a social analysis of male
violence against women, by shifting much of what they endured from
the personal domain into the shared collective one, by reframing their
experience in growth-promoting terms, all coupled with trauma
reprocessing and cognitive restructuring, feminist therapy stands a
rather good chance of freeing rape victims from the agonizing sequelae of rape and sexual assault and turning them into empowered
survivors.

152

Mental and Physical Health

REFERENCES
Alvidrez, J. (1999). Ethnic variations in mental health attitudes and service use
among low-income African American, Latina, and European American
young women. Community Mental Health, 35, 515530.
Alvidrez, J., & Azocar, F. (1999). Distressed womens clinic patients: Preference
for mental health treatment and perceived obstacles. General Hospital
Psychiatry, 21, 340347.
Angell, S. A. (1994). Acute and chronic symptomatology of sexual trauma:
Treatment issues. National Center for PTSD Clinical Quarterly, 4, 911.
Ardovini-Brooker, J., & Caringella-MacDonald, S. (2002). Media attributions of
blame and sympathy in ten rape cases. The Justice Professional, 15, 318.
Botta, R. A., & Pingree, S. (1997). Interpersonal communication and rape:
Women acknowledging their assaults. Journal of Health Communication, 2,
197212.
Brown, L., & Brodsky, A. (1992). The future of feminist therapy. Psychotherapy,
29, 5157.
Buddie, A. M., & Miller, A. G. (2002). Beyond rape myths: A more complex
view of perceptions of rape victims. Sex Roles, 45, 139160.
Burnam, M. A., Stein, J. A., Golding, J. M., & Siegel, J. M. (1988). Sexual assault
and mental disorders in a community population. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 56, 843850.
Burt, M. R. (1980). Cultural myths and support for rape. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 38, 217230.
Campbell, R. (1998). The community response to rape: Victims experiences
with the legal, medical, and mental health systems. American Journal of
Community Psychology, 26, 355379.
Campbell, R., & Raja, S. (1999). Secondary victimization of rape victims:
Insights from mental health professionals who treat survivors of violence.
Violence and Victims, 14, 261274.
Campbell, R., Se, T., Barnes, H.E., Ahrens, C. E., Wasco, S. M., & ZaragozaDiesfeld, Y. (1999). Community services for rape survivors: Enhancing
psychological well-being or increasing trauma? Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 67, 847858.
Campbell, R., Wasco, S. M., Ahrens, C. E., Se, T., & Barnes, H. E. (2001). Preventing the second rape: Rape survivors experiences with community
service providers. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16, 12391259.
Casey, E. A., & Nurius, P. S. (2006). Trends in the prevalence and characteristics of sexual violence: A cohort analysis. Violence and Victims, 21, 629644.
Cowan, G. (2000). Beliefs about the causes of four types of rape. Sex Roles, 42,
807823.
Feminist Therapy Institute. (1990). Feminist therapy code of ethics. In
H. Lerman & M. Porter (Eds.), Feminist ethics in psychotherapy (pp. 3740).
New York: Springer.
Foa, E. B., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1998). Treating the trauma of rape: Cognitive-behavioral
therapy for PTSD. New York: Guilford.
Foa, E. B., Zoellner, L.A., Feeny, N.C., Hembree, E.A., & Alvarez-Conrad, J.
(2002). Does imaginal exposure exacerbate PTSD symptoms? Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 10221028.

From Victim to Empowered Survivor

153

Gilbert, L. A. (1980). Feminist therapy. In A. M. Brodsky & R. T. Hare-Mustin


(Eds.), Women and psychotherapy (pp. 245266). New York: Guilford.
Gilbert, B. J. (1994). Treatment of adult victims of rape. New directions for mental
health services, 64, 6778.
Herman, J. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic Books.
Hill, M. (1990). On creating a theory of feminist therapy. In L. S. Brown &
M. P. Root (Eds.), Diversity and complexity in feminist therapy (pp. 5366).
New York: The Haworth Press, Inc.
Hill, M., & Ballou, M. (1998). Making therapy feminist: A practice survey.
Women and Therapy, 21, 216
Jaycox, L. H., Zoellner, L., & Foa, E. B. (2002). Cognitivebehavior therapy for
PTSD in rape survivors. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 891906.
Jordan, J., Kaplan, A., Miller, J. B., Stiver, I., & Surrey, J. (1991). Womens growth
in connection. New York: Guilford.
Kaschak, E. (1992). Engendered lives: A new psychology of womens experience. New
York: Basic Books.
Koss, M. P. (1983). The scope of rape: Implications for the clinical treatment of
victims. The Clinical Psychologist, 38, 8891.
Koss, M. P., & Harvey, M. R. (1991). The rape victim: Clinical and community
interventions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Lebowitz, L., & Roth, S. (1994). I felt like a slut: The cultural context and
womens response to being raped. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 7, 363390.
Lee, C., Gavriel, H., Drummond, P., Richards, J., & Greenwald, R. (2002). Treatment of PTSD: Stress inoculation training with prolonged exposure
compared to EMDR. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 10711089.
Lonsway, K. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1994). Rape myths: In review. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 18, 133164.
MacKinnon, C. A. (2005). Dening rape internationally. Columbia Journal of
Cross National Law, 44, 940.
Meadows, E. A., & Foa, E. B. (1998). Intrusion, arousal, and avoidance: Sexual
trauma survivors. In V. M. Follette, J. I. Ruzek, & F. R. Abueg (Eds.),
Cognitive-behavioral therapies for trauma (pp. 100123). New York: Guilford.
Moor, A. (2007). When recounting the traumatic memories is not enough:
Treating persistent self-devaluation associated with rape and victim-blaming
rape myths. Women & Therapy, 30, 1935.
Moor, A. (2009). A preliminary assessment of the prevalence and nature of sexual violence against women in Israel (in Hebrew). Social Issues in Israel, 7,
4665.
Moradi, B., Fischer, A. R., Hill, M. S., Jome, L. M., & Blum, S. A. (2000). Does
feminist plus therapist equal feminist therapist? Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 24, 285296.
Paivio, S. C., & Laurent, C. (2001). Empathy and emotion regulation: reprocessing memories of childhood abuse. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57, 213226.
Pitman, R. K., Altman, B., Greenwald, E., Longpre, R. E., Machlin, M. L., Poire,
R. E., & Steketee, G. S. (1991). Psychiatric complications during ooding
therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 52,
1720.
Power, K., McGoldrick, T., Brown, K., Buchanan, R., Sharp, D., Swanson, V.,
et al. (2002). A controlled comparison of eye movement desensitization

154

Mental and Physical Health

and reprocessing versus exposure plus cognitive restructuring versus waiting list in the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 9, 299318.
Rader, J., & Gilbert, L. A. (2005). The egalitarian relationship in feminist therapy. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 427435.
Rogers, S., & Silver, S. M. (2002). Is EMDR an exposure therapy? A review of
trauma protocols. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 4359.
Shapiro, F., & Maxeld, L. (2002). Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR): Information processing in the treatment of trauma. Journal
of Clinical Psychology, 58, 933946.
Sheldon, J. P., & Parent, S. L. (2002). Clergys attitudes and attributions of
blame toward female rape victims. Violence Against Women, 8, 233256.
Ullman, S. E. (1996). Social reactions, coping strategies, and self-blame attributions in adjustment to sexual assault. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20,
505526.
Ullman, S. E. (1999). Social support and recovery from sexual assault: A
review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 4, 343358.
Ullman, S. E., & Filipas, H. H. (2001). Correlates of formal and informal
support seeking in sexual assault victims. Journal of Interpersonal Violence,
16, 10281047.
Ullman, S. E., & Brecklin, L. R. (2002). Sexual assault history, PTSD, and mental
health service seeking in a national sample of women. Journal of Community Psychology, 30, 261279.
Worell, J., & Johnson, D. (2001). Therapy with women: Feminist frameworks. In
R.K. Unger (Ed.), Handbook of the psychology of women and gender (pp. 317
329). New York: Wiley.
Worell, J., & Remer, P. (1992). Feminist perspectives in therapy: An empowerment
model for women. New York: Wiley.
Wyatt, G. E. (1992). The sociocultural context of African American and White
American womens rape. Journal of Social Issues, 48, 7791.

Chapter 7

Gender Microaggressions: Implications


for Mental Health
Kevin L. Nadal

For the past 40 years, psychologists, educators, and other social scientists have advocated for an increase in multicultural knowledge and
awareness in training, practice, and research. For example, the American Psychological Association (APA) has published the Guidelines on
multicultural education, training, research, practice, and organizational
change for psychologists (2003), which focus primarily on ways of
being culturally competent toward oppressed racial and ethnic minority groups, with minimal focus on other multicultural subgroups (e.g.,
gender, sexual orientation, etc.). So while such guidelines recognize the
intersections of other identities with race/ethnicity (e.g., the impacts of
gender on race and racial identity), the emphasis is primarily on race
and ethnicity. And while there is a denite need for the advocacy of
racial and ethnic minority issues, several other culturally oppressed
groups are often viewed as afterthoughts when discussing multiculturalism. Some of these groups include women, lesbian/gay/bisexual/
transgender (LGBT) persons, disabled persons, elderly, and religious
minority groups.
Research studies on multicultural competence models in psychology
tend to follow this pattern of emphasizing race and/or ethnicity exclusively. Writings involving culturally competent counseling methods,
counselor biases, identity development, and other multicultural issues
are predisposed to concentrate primarily on race (and sometimes ethnicity). This can be exemplied by the newest line of multicultural research
involving racial microaggressions, which are dened as brief and commonplace daily verbal, behavioral, or environmental indignities, whether

156

Mental and Physical Health

intentional or unintentional, that communicate hostile, derogatory,


or negative racial slights and insults toward people of color (Sue &
Capodilupo, 2007, p. 271). These authors cite that because the United
States is more politically correct today than it was twenty or thirty
years ago, racism takes more subtle and covert forms. However, because
of the cumulative nature of these subtle forms of discrimination, persons
of color may experience an array of emotions, including belittlement, anger, rage, frustration, sadness, and alienation (Sue, Bucceri et al., 2007;
Sue, Nadal et al., 2008), which consequently may lead to depression,
anxiety, trauma, and other mental health problems.
While the introduction of microaggressions into the forefront of psychology is necessary and eliciting a range of positive and negative reactions in the eld, it is important to recognize that previous research with
microaggressions focuses primarily on race, following the trend that other
social identity groups are ignored or minimized in multicultural discourses. Accordingly, research on microaggressions fails to take into
account experiences involving gender, sexual orientation, ability, age, religion, and ethnicity. The purpose of this chapter is to advocate for the
expansion of research on microaggressions to encompass other cultural
groups. Specically, this chapter will concentrate on microaggressions
involving gender, hypothesizing the various types of gender microaggressions that may impact women in everyday life and in therapy settings. Utilizing the major tenets in the taxonomy of racial microaggressions, this
chapter will dene gender microaggressions and conjecture numerous
categories of gender microaggressions that women may experience.
DEFINITIONS OF MICROAGGRESSIONS
To understand microaggressions in general, it becomes important to
recognize the literature on racial microaggressions. Racial microaggressions consist of brief statements or behaviors that send denigrating and
hurtful messages to people of color. By this denition, microaggressions
can occur between any two parties (individuals or groups or both), in
which the member(s) from a privileged/dominant group communicates
a denigrating and hurtful message toward the member(s) from an
oppressed group. For example, a man can commit a gender microaggression toward a woman; a heterosexual person can be responsible for
a sexual orientation microaggression toward a LGBT person; and an
able-bodied person can perpetrate an ability microaggression toward a
disabled person (Nadal, 2008).
Studies have also indicated there are three forms of racial microaggressions: microassaults, microinsults, and microinvalidations. Microassaults are what are closest to overt, old-fashioned racism, with
examples ranging from calling a black American the N word or yelling at an Asian American or Latin American person to go back where

Gender Microaggressions: Implications for Mental Health

157

you came from. However, microinsults and microinvalidations are


less obvious, more subtle, and often well intentioned. Microinvalidations are dened as actions that negate or nullify a person of colors
experiences or realities, while microinsults refer to actions that convey
insensitivity and are belittling to a persons racial identity. These are
the types of microaggressions that may be more harmful because the
perpetrator may not even realize that she/he is insulting or invalidating the individual. Unlike microassaults, that are often conscious and
purposefully hurtful, microinsults and microinvalidations may be more
unconscious and may have several potential impacts on the recipient.
For example, the experience of being treated as an intellectual inferior is a common experience for African Americans, in which many
individuals are consistently told that they are smart or articulate
(Sue, Nadal et al., 2008). This would be considered a microinsult
because the perpetrator is conveying that because the individual is
African American, she or he would not be expected to be smart or
would not be expected to speak well. While the statement may have
been well intentioned or meant to be a compliment, the recipient may
have a negative reaction, with subsequent emotions such as anger,
frustration, or hurt. An example of a racial microinsult that may occur
toward Asian Americans includes the experience of being exocitized,
which is demonstrated by a male individual telling an Asian that he
has an Asian fetish or that Asian women have beautiful skin or
silky hair (Sue, Bucceri, et al., 2007). Again, while the statement
might be meant to be a compliment, the Asian American recipient may
feel objectied, hurt, and insulted by the comment. Because such a
statement might occur regularly for the recipient, the cumulative
impact of similar statements might lead to emotional distress.
Some racial microaggressions may be more intentional, in that the
perpetrator might be more conscious of her/his behavior. For example,
many African American individuals report being treated as criminals
on a regular basis; this message is conveyed in situations where storeowners follow African American individuals around while they shop.
This microinsult sends the messages that black individuals are criminals who are likely to steal or damage their stores (Sue, Nadal, et al.,
2008). However, if the perpetrator in this case would be confronted
with her/his behavior, it is possible that the individual would deny
that race was involved in their decision making, because she/he is a
good person who does not see race and treats people of all racial
groups the same (Sue, 2005). So even when perpetrators of microaggressions may be aware of their behaviors toward persons of color, it
is less likely that they would be able to admit to it.
A common form of a racial microinvalidation is one where an individual
is denied her/his racial reality, often occurring when a person of color is
told that her/his experience with racism is invalid and that she/he should

158

Mental and Physical Health

stop complaining about racial stressors (Sue, Bucceri et al., 2007). In this
case, the perpetrator may not realize the impact that such a statement may
have on the recipient; the individual may believe that she/he is simply stating ones opinion and assumes that the recipient should be able to agree
with her or his statement. However, the recipient may instead feel misunderstood because the perpetrator is not accounting for her/his experiences with race, leading to potential distress at the clash of their racial
realities and feelings of belittling, frustration, or sadness.
Given these three categories of racial microaggressions, it is likely
that similar experiences can occur across different social identities. For
example, a sexual orientation microassault may include a heterosexual
person calling a gay man a faggot or calling a lesbian a dyke. An
ability microinsult might include an able-bodied person speaking to a
disabled person slowly and condescendingly, assuming that she/he
would not be able to understand the person. A religious microinvalidation might include a Christian telling a Jewish person that You complain about the Holocaust too much (Nadal, 2008). All of these types
of statements or behaviors convey a negative and derogatory message
toward the member of the oppressed group. And again, the cumulative
nature of these statements and behaviors may have lasting impacts on
the member(s) of the oppressed group.
Given the denitions of racial microaggressions and the ability to
apply the experiences of microaggressions to interactions between different social groups, it is now possible to dene microaggressions that
may occur as a result of gender. Gender microaggressions are brief and
commonplace daily verbal, behavioral, or environmental indignities
(whether intentional or unintentional) that communicate hostile, derogatory, or negative sexist slights and insults toward women. These
microaggressions are often unconscious in that the perpetrator of the
microaggressions may not realize that he is being hurtful in his statements or behaviors. Gender microaggressions are different from other
forms of sexism in that they may manifest in various forms: microassaults, microinvalidations, and microinsults; they may be subtle and
covert, in that the recipient may often question whether the microaggression would even have a lasting impact on her psychological well
being. However, similar to racial microaggressions, the cumulative
nature of these gender microaggressions may lead to mental health problems, including depression, anxiety, trauma, or issues with self-esteem.

GENDER MICROAGGRESSIONS
Sexism in Everyday Life
Little research has been written on the term gender microaggressions in psychology, education, and social sciences, but rather describes

Gender Microaggressions: Implications for Mental Health

159

sexism in different forms. The vast range of literature on sexism reveals


that sexism, much like racism, has become more concealed than it is
open and obvious (Sue & Capodilupo, 2008). Some authors contend that
sexism takes three major forms: overt, covert, and subtle (Swim &
Cohen, 1997). Overt sexism could be comparable to old-fashioned sexism toward women, in which women are directly discriminated against
and/or treated unfairly and unequally than men. This can be exemplied by a man blatantly telling a woman that he would not hire her for a
position because of her gender, or a man directly insulting a woman by
calling her a derogatory term. Covert sexism would include discrimination that is less direct and less revealed. This can be exemplied by
some men who might claim to be liberal and gender-neutral, yet
would never vote for women president of the United States. Finally,
subtle sexism is dened as unequal and unfair treatment of women
that is not recognized by many people because it is perceived to be normative, and therefore does not appear unusual (Swim, Mallett, &
Stagnor, 2004, p. 117). This can be exemplied by someone assuming
that an authority gure or professional (e.g., employer, author, professor,
or doctor) is a he without knowing the gender of the said person.
While covert and overt sexism might be conscious processes, it is likely
that subtle sexism is unconscious and/or not maliciously intended (Sue
& Capodilupo, 2008).
One study conducted on everyday sexism or the discriminatory
or prejudicial experiences with gender (Swim, Hyers, Cohen, & Ferguson,
2001) found that female participants encounter about one to two
impactful sexual incidents per week. Participants reported three major
categories of everyday sexism: (1) traditional gender role stereotypes
and prejudice, (2) demeaning and degrading comments and behaviors,
and (3) sexual objectication. Examples of traditional gender role stereotypes and privilege include a man telling a woman to fold laundry
or do the dishes, while illustrations of demeaning and degrading comments and behaviors include men using labels (e.g., bitch, slut, or
chick) to describe women. Examples of sexual objectication include
men making comments about womens body parts, as well as behaviors like unwanted staring or touching. In these studies, women
reported psychological impacts, including a decrease in comfort and
self-esteem and feelings of anger and depression. Another study highlighted experiences with sexism with adolescent girls (Leaper & Brown,
2008). The authors found the majority of the participants reported sexism related to academics (e.g., being discouraged about their computer,
math, or science abilities) and/or athletics (e.g., being teased about
their athletic ability). The majority of these female adolescents also
reported several incidents of gender harassment, namely receiving
unwanted romantic attention, being teased with an embarrassing or
mean joke or a demeaning name, and being teased about physical

160

Mental and Physical Health

appearance. These two studies on sexism exemplify types of gender


microaggressions and the negative psychological effects these experiences may cause. While they may seem like trivial everyday occurrences,
these slights may inuence a woman or girls self-esteem, worldview,
motivation to achieve, and mental health.
Understanding various measures that assess different types of sexist
slights can be helpful in understanding examples of everyday sexist
experiences. The Schedule of Sexist Events (SSE) is a measure that
assesses perceived frequencies of sexist discrimination (Klonoff &
Landrine, 1995). Recent studies have supported three subscales of the
SSE, including sexist degradation and its consequences (e.g., been
called a sexist name), unfair sexist events at work/school (e.g., denied
a raise, promotion, or tenure at work), and unfair treatment in distant
and close relationships (e.g., being treated unfairly by people in helping jobs; Matteson & Moradi, 2005). In the initial use of the SSE, it was
found that 99 percent of a given sample of women (n = 633) experienced sexism at some point in their life, while 97 percent of the sample
experienced sexism in the past year (Klonoff & Landrine, 1995). Since
then, the SSE has been used in several empirical studies which have
provided support that daily sexist experiences are prevalent in womens lives (Matteson & Moradi, 2005). However, some research has
found that most women may not respond to these sexist events, with
majority ignoring the incident (Lott, Aquith, & Doyon2001), instead of
confronting the perpetrator. This experience of dealing with sexist incidents is comparable to previous research on racial microaggressions,
which state that people of color often nd themselves in a catch 22
when they experience such racist events (Sue, Capodilupo et al., 2007).
If the recipient of the racist or sexist incident says something to the
perpetrator, she/he runs the risk of being invalidated, ridiculed, or
even physically assaulted; if she/he doesnt say something, she/he
may become distressed and regret not standing up for oneself.
Sexual Harassment
The research on sexual harassment is also important to review when
discussing gender microaggressions, as it may explain the impacts of
sexist experiences at the workplace on individual mental health. There
has been much research in the eld of psychology that has documented the psychological impact of sexual harassment on both women
and men (Schneider, Swann, & Fitzgerald, 1997). It is imperative to recognize how sexual harassment may relate to gender microaggressions,
but how it may also differ. Sexual harassment has been dened as:
unwelcome sexual advances, requests for sexual favors, and other
verbal or physical conduct of a sexual nature constitutes sexual

Gender Microaggressions: Implications for Mental Health

161

harassment when submission to or rejection of this conduct explicitly or


implicitly affects an individuals employment, unreasonably interferes
with an individuals work performance or creates an intimidating, hostile
or offensive work environment. (U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity
Commission, 2008)

It is reported that in 2007, that the Equal Employment Opportunity


Commission (EEOC) received 12,510 charges of sexual harassment
(with 16.0 percent of those charges led by males), and that EEOC
resolved 11,592 of these sexual harassment charges (U.S. Equal
Employment Opportunity Commission, 2008). However, some experts
have reported that between 40 to 60 percent of all women in the
United States have experienced some form of harassing behavior at
work, but that many of these experiences did not meet the legal criteria
for sexual harassment and may often go unreported (Murray, 1998).
Understanding measurements for sexual harassment can be valuable
in recognizing specic examples of sexist experiences in the workplace,
leading to further understandings of gender microaggressions. The
Sexual Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ) was developed to examine
the experiences of women with sexual harassment in the workplace
(Fitzgerald, Gelfand, & Drasgow, 1995) and has been used frequently
over the past two decades. According to this measure, there are three
major components used to dene sexual harassment: gender harassment, unwanted sexual attention, and sexual coercion. Gender harassment refers to verbal and nonverbal behaviors that convey insulting,
hostile, and degrading attitudes about women. Unwanted sexual attention includes a range of verbal and nonverbal behavior that is offensive, unwanted, and unreciprocated. Sexual coercion refers to the
extortion of sexual cooperation for job-related considerations.
Given these three categories, one can observe the similarities
between different types of sexual harassment and gender microaggressions. Gender harassment seems to be most parallel to unconscious
microaggressions, in which the perpetrator of these microaggressions
may not even be aware that his statements/behaviors convey a negative message to the recipient. Concurrently, unwanted sexual attention
and sexual coercion are likely to be more conscious processes, in which
the individual may be more aware of the intentions behind his statements and/or behaviors. As a result, unwanted sexual attention and
sexual coercion can be compared to microassaults, in that these
behaviors are more obvious and noticeable by both the perpetrator and
recipient. These microassaults would likely represent the types of
behaviors that would qualify for sexual harassment charges. However,
it appears that gender harassment is less obvious and is likely to be in
line with microinsults and/or microinvalidations. These types of
unconscious statements/behaviors are likely not obvious enough to

162

Mental and Physical Health

meet the legal requirements for sexual harassment, yet send insulting
and invalidating messages to the women that receive them.
OBJECTIFICATION THEORY
The literature on objectication theory is also important to review in
order to further understand illustrations of gender microaggressions
and negative psychological effects. Objectication theory can be
dened as a framework for understanding the experiential consequences of being female in a culture that sexually objecties the female
body (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997, p. 173). It further explains that
sexual objectication can occur through an array of interpersonal interactions including romantic partners, family members, friends, acquaintances, strangers, and even media outlets that depict interpersonal and
social interactions. Accordingly, women and girls in an objectifying
society may experience various mental health risks, including depression, sexual dysfunction, eating disorders, and body image issues.
Through examining current measures of objectication, it can be
benecial to understand how specic experiences of being objectied
are interrelated with gender microaggressions. The Interpersonal Sexual Objectication Scale (ISOS) was created to understand how sexual
objectication occurred interpersonally and how it related to psychological distress (Kozee, Tylka, Augustus-Horvath, & Denchik, 2007). Exploratory and conrmatory factor analyses revealed two factors: body
evaluation and unwanted explicit sexual advances. Examples of body
evaluation include: How often have you been whistled at while walking down a street? or How often have you noticed someone staring
at your breasts when you are talking to them? Examples of unwanted
explicit sexual advances include How often have you been touched or
fondled against your will? or How often has someone grabbed or
pinched one of your private body areas against your will? Scores on
the ISOS were strongly related to sexist degradation and slightly to
moderately related to other sexist events, self-objectication (e.g., internalization of the thin-ideal) and body shame. These ndings support
that the experiences of being interpersonally sexualized and/or feeling
judged about ones body can lead to psychological distress, including a
lower self-esteem and an impaired body image. Similarly to previous
research on microaggressions, the cumulative nature of these events
increases psychological distress over time.
HOSTILE AND BENEVOLENT SEXISM
There has been some research that has conceptualized two additional forms of sexism: benevolent sexism and hostile sexism. Benevolent sexism is dened as a subjectively favorable, chivalrous ideology

Gender Microaggressions: Implications for Mental Health

163

that offers protection and affection to women who embrace conventional roles (Glick & Fiske, 2001, p. 109) while hostile sexism is
dened as antipathy toward women who are viewed as usurping
mens power (Glick & Fiske, 2001, p. 109). With benevolent sexism,
men may believe that they are nonsexist and that they believe in gender equality. However, in reality these men may endorse traditional
gender roles that actually promote female inferiority. For example, men
may believe that it is their responsibility to protect and provide for
women, which may seem favorable and innocuous, but demonstrates
the belief that women need to be taken care of. On the other hand,
men may exhibit hostile sexism and feel threatened by women who are
empowered, independent, and/or assertive. These men may be averse
to feminists or female authority gures and may express this discomfort or disgust in blatant and subtle ways. For example, a man may
belittle a female supervisor by referring to her with sexist labels when
talking to his male coworkers.
Benevolent and hostile sexism are unique to other forms of sexism
in that they are personal ideologies that promote male dominance and
female inferiority. At the same time, they are related to gender microaggressions because they may be unconscious and may manifest in an
array of hurtful behaviors or statements. For example, when a man
desires for a woman to uphold traditional gender roles, his behaviors
may include asking a woman to cook or clean for him, or treating a
woman as if she was not intelligent. He may or may not be consciously
aware of the sexist message he is portraying, and he may not recognize
the impact such behaviors/statements may have on the recipient.
Depending on the consciousness and intent of his actions, these may
be considered microassaults or microinsults. Similarly hostile sexism
may also take microassaultive, microinsulting, or microinvalidating
forms. For example, a man calling a woman in power a bitch or
aggressive may be considered a microassault. A man who unconsciously ignores a womans ideas (because he feels threatened by
her power or intelligence) may be an example of a microinsult, and a
man who tells a woman (particularly an outspoken or assertive
woman) to stop playing the gender card would be an example of a
microinvalidation.
GENDER MICROAGGRESSIONS
Sue and Capodilupo discuss microaggressions that are related to
race, gender, and sexual orientation. Several categories of gender
microaggressions are introduced, citing the behaviors/statements and
the messages that are being communicated to the recipient. In the
theme of sexual objectication, men may conduct behaviors that
send the message that a womans body is a sexual object. Examples

164

Mental and Physical Health

may include a male stranger putting his hands on a womans hips to


pass her, or a man whistling or catcalling as a woman walks down
the street. The theme of denial of individual racism/sexism/heterosexism occurs when an individual denies ones own potential bias or
prejudice. An example can include a statement where a male says I
always treat men and women equally, sending the message that he is
incapable of gender bias or sexism. The theme of traditional gender
role prejudicing and stereotyping takes place when individuals
assume that others should hold traditional gender roles or stereotypes.
An example may include a person looking at a thirty-year-old womans
ring nger, sending the message that she should be married because
being a wife should be a womans main purpose in life. The theme of
second-class citizen occurs when an oppressed group member
receives less preferential treatment than a dominant group member.
For example, if a woman is not invited to a social gathering by her
coworkers, when clearly all of the male coworkers are, she may feel left
out because of her gender. Finally, the theme of use of sexist/heterosexist language speaks to the aforementioned subtle sexism (i.e., using
the pronoun he to refer to all people), but also includes other
degrading language (i.e., labeling a woman a whore). The rst of
these two examples communicates that the experience of men is universal, while the second imparts that women are meant to be passive
and/or are allowed to be sexualized. While these themes of gender
microaggressions illustrate the types of behaviors and statements that
can convey denigrating messages toward women, they are merely a
few samples of types of gender microaggressions that may occur.
Therefore, it is important for psychologists to be aware that gender
microaggressions exist, how they may impact the psychological processes of women, how they affect womens mental health, and how
they may occur in therapeutic settings.
On the basis of the previous literature involving microaggressions,
everyday sexism, objectication theory, hostile and benevolent sexism,
and sexual harassment, this chapter will provide a hypothesis to the
major types of gender microaggressions that may occur in everyday life
and in therapeutic settings. This hypothesis is based on several empirical studies that have revealed that women experience various forms of
gender discrimination in their everyday lives (see Swim et al., 2001, for
a review), in school systems (see Leaper & Brown, 2008, for a review)
and in the workplace (see Fitzgerald, Hulin, & Drasgow, 1994, for a
review). The hypothesis of these gender microaggression categories
is also based on several writings involving gender and experiences
of women from a feminist therapy perspective, which focuses on
providing culturally-competent therapy toward women (Brabeck &
Brown, 1997; Collins, 1998; Enns, 1992; Espin, 1993; McNamara &
Rickard, 1998). These empirical studies, theoretical models, social

Gender Microaggressions: Implications for Mental Health

165

justice movements, and personal narratives all provide evidence that


gender microaggressions do exist and may have a negative impact on
mental health.
In understanding these various types of gender microaggressions, it
is expected that individuals become aware of their own experiences as
perpetrators or recipients of these microaggressions, while understanding the psychological impact that these microaggressions may have. It
is also expected that psychologists become more culturally competent
in understanding these microaggressions: gaining knowledge about the
impact of these microaggressions, increasing awareness of how they
may enact these microaggressions in psychological and therapeutic settings, and learning the skills to be able to deal with these microaggressions as they occur in their lives and in the lives of their clients.
FORMS OF GENDER MICROAGGRESSIONS
Based on the previous literature on microaggressions (Nadal, 2008;
Sue, Bucceri, et al., 2007; Sue, Capodilupo, et al., 2007; Sue, Nadal,
et al., 2008), sexism (Klonoff & Landrine, 1995), gender harassment
(Fitzgerald et al., 1995), interpersonal objectication (Kozee et al., 2007),
and benevolent and hostile sexism (Glick & Fiske, 2001), there are eight
proposed themes of microaggressions that may occur toward women.
Five of these themes are originally conceived in Sue and Capodilupo
(2008) and include the following: sexual objectication, assumptions of inferiority, assumptions of traditional gender roles, use of sexist language, and
denial of individual sexism. One of the themes is a reconceptualization of
Sue and Capodilupos (2008) second-class citizen and is described as
invisibility. Two of these themes are new microaggression categories
that are derived from previous literature on microaggressions, and they
include denial of reality of sexism and environmental gender microaggressions. Table 7.1 provides examples of situations involving these microaggressions and the messages that are conveyed to the recipient.
These themes of gender microaggressions align with the previous literature with racial microaggressions, citing that (a) many of these
microaggressions may be unconscious, in that the perpetrator may not
realize the impact on the recipient, (b) these microaggressions (whether
conscious or not) communicate various oppressive messages to the
women who receive them, and (c) the various themes these microaggressions represent a spectrum of microassaults, microinvalidations,
or microinsults. In fact, one of the main ways that gender microaggression is different than other forms of sexism (e.g., everyday sexism,
objectication theory, and hostile and benevolent sexism) is that they
are clearly categorized into these three subcategories. For example,
with the theme of sexual objectication, the example of a man catcalling a woman as she walks down the street might be considered a

Table 7.1
Examples of Gender Microaggressions in Everyday Life
Example

Message

Sexual Objectication: Occurs when a


woman is treated as a sexual object.

A man glances at a womans breasts


while he compliments her shirt.
A construction worker catcalls a woman
while she walks down the street
A male stranger places his hands on a
womans hips or the small of her back
as he passes her

Your body is not yours; I have a right to


stare at you without your permission
You are a sexual object; you are meant to
entertain men.
Your body is not yours; I have a right to
touch you without your permission.

Invisibility: Occurs when a woman is


overlooked and/or when men are given
preferential treatment.

A female employee is passed up for a job


promotion.
A woman is waiting to order a drink at a
bar; the bartender serves the male
customers before her.
The male head of a company does not
know the names of his female
employees but knows the names of the
male employees.

Your service is not as valuable as a


mans.
You deserve to wait. Men are valued
more than you.

166

Theme

Assumptions of Inferiority: Occurs


when a woman is assumed to be less
competent than men (e.g., physically
or intellectually).

A woman is carrying boxes; without


permission, a man grabs the boxes and
says Let me do it.
A woman demonstrates leadership skills
at her workplace; a man is surprised
and says I didnt know you had it in
you!
A female student is told I didnt know
that women were good at math.
A man says I dont think a woman could
ever be president because women are
too emotional.

You are not valuable.

You are not physically capable of doing


things.
You are not expected to have intellectual
or leadership capacities.

You are not expected to be smart in math


or sciences.
Women should know their place.

Denial of Reality of Sexism: Occurs


when a woman is told that sexism does
not exist.

Assumptions of Traditional Gender


Roles: Occurs when an individual
assumes that a woman should maintain
traditional gender roles.

167

Denial of Individual Sexism: Occurs


when a man denies his gender biases
or prejudice.

A man tells a woman that she is


exaggerating about how many times a
day she gets catcalled on the street.
A woman is told that she didnt get the
job because she wasnt qualied, not
because of her gender.
A woman is put in charge of the ofce
party (without volunteering), when there
are other lower-ranking males at the
company.
A forty-year-old woman with a successful
career is asked why she never had any
children.
Im not sexist. I have a wife and
daughters!
I treat men and women the same all the
time.
As a person of color, Im offended that
you would imply that Im sexist.

You complain too much.

You are to blame, not your gender.

Women should know their place.

You are not serving your primary purpose


in life.
I am incapable of sexism.
I have no gender biases.
Because I belong to another oppressed
group, I am allowed to have biases.

Use of sexist language: Occurs when


language is used to degrade a woman.

A male coworker calls his female coworker


sweetie, honey, or shorty.
A male news correspondent calls a
female politician a shrew or a bitch.

You are inferior to men/I have a right to


patronize you.
Women should know their place.

Environmental Invalidations: Macrolevel aggressions that occur on a


systemic and environmental level.

Women make less money than men.

You are not as important or valuable as


men.
You are an outsider/You do not belong.

Adapted from Sue & Capodilupo (2008)

Majority of university professors in a


department are men.
A corporation has pictures of the Board
of Directors of the company featuring
all men.

You as a woman will never break the


glass ceiling.

168

Mental and Physical Health

microassault in that the man is likely more conscious of how he is


demeaning the woman and making her feel uncomfortable. While he
may not realize the impact of his behavior on her, it is an experience
that both parties would be aware. In the same theme, a man may compliment a woman on her shirt, while also glancing at her breasts. This
may be considered a microinsult in that the man is drawing attention
to a womans body, without her permission, and is objectifying her in
the process. At the same time, this microaggression may be more
unconscious or well intended by the man. He may be conscious that he
is looking at her breasts; yet, if he was confronted, he might be defensive and say that he was simply paying her a compliment. However,
this compliment is actually insulting and objectifying to the woman
and may end up causing her distress, frustration, and feelings of
objectication.
There are also some themes of microaggressions that would be considered microinvalidations, which can also be conscious or unconscious. For example, in the theme of use of sexist language, a male
individual may call his female coworker honey or sweetie when
he would never refer to a male coworker in the same way. This may
be well intended in that he may believe that he is trying to be personable and cordial with the female coworker, but he may not realize that
the recipient may feel dehumanized or belittled. This behavior may not
even be conscious by the perpetrator, in that this male individual may
call all women some form of a pet name and may not realize the
impact he has on others. Concurrently, in the same theme, the example
of a news correspondent calling a female politician a bitch or a
shrew might be a behavior that is more conscious. However, despite
awareness of the comment, it is possible that the perpetrator would
deny the invalidating messages that are conveyed if/when he is confronted. This may be supported through many anecdotes of news correspondents who dismissed allegations of sexism when referring to
Senator Hillary Clinton with such sexist terms during the U.S. presidential election of 2008.
INTERSECTIONS OF GENDER MICROAGGRESSIONS
WITH OTHER MICROAGGRESSIONS
It is important to recognize that gender microaggressions cannot
occur independently from other social identity groups (namely race,
ethnicity, social class, age, ability, or religion). In fact, previous authors
have written about the impact of identity and experiences based upon
the intersection of race and gender (Bowman et al., 2001). Accordingly,
the manifestation of these microaggressions is likely to be heavily inuenced by a womans other reference groups, especially her race and
age. For example, with the theme of invisibility, it is likely that a

Gender Microaggressions: Implications for Mental Health

169

woman of color would be viewed as more invisible than a white


woman and would likely receive substandard service or recognition.
Similarly, with the theme of inferiority, a woman of color (namely a
black American or Latina American) woman would likely be treated as
less intelligent than her white counterparts, which may even speak to
the notion that black and Latina women make signicantly less money
than white women (National Womens Law Center, 2008). Finally, with
the theme of sexual objectication, a womans race would inuence
the type of microaggression that she receives. For example, black
women are stereotyped to be independent, assertive, and aggressive
(Bowman et al., 2001), while Asian American women are stereotyped
to be passive and exotic (Sue, Bucceri, et al., 2007). As a result, it
is important to notice two trends: (1) the ways that women of different
racial groups are objectied sexually may be different because of their
race, and (2) a womans race may always have some impact on her
experiences with gender microaggressions. So while it is important to
not discount the experiences that white women may experience with
sexual objectication, it is necessary to recognize that women of all
racial groups may experience gender microaggressions differently.
Age may also impact gender microaggressions in many ways. This
can be demonstrated through the dehumanizing of women through
language. Perhaps women who are called honey or dear by their
male coworkers might be younger, which may contribute to such
patronizing language. At the same time, perhaps younger women may
struggle more with microaggressions dealing with assumptions of inferiority, in that the combination of their age and gender may contribute
to others perceptions that they would not be capable. This is not to
discount that women of all ages deal with microaggressions involving
inferiority, but rather to illuminate how the impact of age may inuence ones general experiences with microaggressions. Accordingly, it
is important for research to be conducted to understand these experiences with gender microaggressions more empirically.
GENDER MICROAGGRESSIONS IN CLINICAL PRACTICE,
EDUCATION, AND OTHER SETTINGS
Given these proposed themes with gender microaggressions, there are
several implications for clinical practice (e.g., counseling/psychotherapy)
and education (e.g., training, classroom settings). First, it is important to
recognize that these microaggressions may be enacted in counseling/psychotherapy settings, just as microaggressions may occur in everyday life.
As a result, it is important for psychologists, educators, and other practitioners to recognize different ways that microaggressions may manifest
in their professional relationships and on different levels (e.g., counselor
client, clientcounselor, supervisorsupervisee, superviseesupervisor,

170

Mental and Physical Health

professorstudent, or studentprofessor). This type of awareness is one


that may be communicated directly (e.g., a client or supervisee points out
a microaggression to a therapist or supervisor); however, it is likely that
many microaggressions are unconscious and may go unnoticed. Therefore
by understanding different types of microaggressions that may exist, psychologists and other clinicians can pay extra attention to their own behaviors and interpersonal dynamics. Table 7.2 describes various examples of
gender microaggressions that may occur in clinical and educational settings, as well as other work environments.
For clinical settings specically, there are many ways that microaggressions can be integrated into therapeutic relationships by utilizing
the two major principles of feminist psychology and feminist therapy:
(1) the personal is political and (2) relationships must be egalitarian
(Collins, 1998). In understanding that the personal is political, a counselor/psychotherapist (either female or male) must recognize that she/
he is an agent of social change that can challenge the status quo and
advocate for the well-being of all women. Thus, this individual must
be aware that she/he has a responsibility to educate her/his clients
about gender microaggressions, while also encouraging the elimination
of gender microaggressions in everyday life. This would be an advocacy that can be promoted toward both female and male clients who
may be either perpetrators or recipients of gender microaggressions.
For example, a feminist counselor/psychotherapist might gently point
out to a male client that his sexist behaviors send denigrating messages
to women, or a feminist counselor/psychotherapist might assist a
female client in understanding how her internalized sexism causes her
to enact gender microaggressions onto other women. In both of these
cases, the counselor/psychotherapist is confronting her/his clients and
pushing for individual change, while addressing the impact of microaggressions on both individual and societal levels.
Furthermore, in understanding that all relationships are egalitarian,
the feminist therapist must be willing to accept clients feedback about
her/his experiences with gender microaggressions (or any other type
of microaggression) that may occur in the therapeutic relationship. The
counselor/psychotherapist must not become defensive if she/he is provided feedback about enacting potential microaggressions in the room.
Rather the counselor/psychotherapist must be open-minded and
respect the clients right for empowerment. This is an incident that can
give power to the client, in that she/he will have learned how to communicate her/his feelings with an authority gure, which can be symbolic of a clients ability to speak her/his voice in an oppressive world.
It is also important for psychologists and other practitioners to recognize gender microaggressions, as a way of maintaining the APA Ethical
Guidelines and increasing their multicultural competence (Nadal, 2008).
In thoroughly understanding the proposed eight microaggressions (and

Table 7.2
Examples of Gender Microaggressions in Clinical Practice and Other Settings
Theme

Example

Message

171

Sexual Objectication: Occurs when a


woman is treated as a sexual object.

A male therapist tells a female that he


likes her pants, while glancing at her
body.

Your body is not yours; I have a right to


stare at you without your permission

Invisibility: Occurs when a woman is


overlooked and/or when men are given
preferential treatment.

A male psychologist at a staff meeting is


credited for a similar comment that a
female psychologist made earlier.

Womens opinions are not as valuable as


mens opinions.

Assumptions of Inferiority: Occurs


when a woman is assumed to be less
competent than men (e.g., physically or
intellectually).

A male client challenges a female


therapists competence.
A female therapist is not hired for a position working with male sex offenders.

Women should know their place.

Denial of Reality of Sexism: Occurs


when a woman is told that sexism does
not exist.

A therapist tells a client I think you spend


too much time blaming your problem on
sexism.

You are to blame, not your gender.

Assumptions of Traditional Gender


Roles: Occurs when an individual
assumes that a woman should maintain
traditional gender roles.

A career counselor encourages a student


to pursue social sciences or humanities
instead of math or sciences.
A therapist focuses sessions on why a
middle-aged woman is not married.

Women are not smart enough.

Women should know their primary purpose in life is to be a wife and mother.

Denial of Individual Sexism: Occurs


when a man denies his gender biases
or prejudice.

A male therapist says: Your gender


doesnt affect the way that I view you.

I have no gender biases and am


incapable of sexism.

Use of sexist language: Occurs when


language is used to degrade a woman.

A male therapist calls his female client


dear.

You are inferior to men/I have a right to


patronize you.

Environmental Invalidations:
Macro-level aggressions that occur on
a systemic and environmental level.

In the waiting room of a mental health


clinic, there are all pictures of historical
male psychologists.

Men are intellectually superior to women.

Adapted from Sue & Capodilupo (2008)

Women are not capable of defending


themselves.

172

Mental and Physical Health

many other potential categories of gender microaggressions that may


exist), it is expected that therapists will enhance their multicultural
competence by increasing their (a) knowledge, (b) awareness, and
(c) skills, that are described in multicultural competence models (see
Sue & Sue, 2008). Knowledge of gender microaggressions can be
increased by understanding the presence and process of microaggressions as well as the psychological impacts of microaggressions on both
perpetrators and recipients. Awareness of gender microaggressions can
be strengthened by learning how these microaggressions may occur in
therapeutic settings and how they as therapists might serve as perpetrators or recipients of microaggressions. Finally, therapists can gain
the skills to deal with gender microaggressionslearning how to confront microaggressions when they occur in therapy, assisting clients in
coping with microaggressions in their everyday lives, and healing from
microaggressions themselves.
It is important to recognize that while it has been discussed that
gender microaggressions occur from male perpetrators to female recipients that it is possible for microaggressions to occur from female perpetrator to female recipient and even male perpetrator to male recipient.
For example, a married woman may enact a microaggression involving
gender roles onto a single woman, by asking her why she is not married or why she doesnt have any children. An example of sexual
objectication that may occur between women is when a woman
makes demeaning, unwelcomed comments about another womans
body (i.e., commenting on her breasts) or if a lesbian woman catcalls
another woman as she walks down the street.
In addition, men may enact microaggressions onto other men as
well. For example, a man may use degrading language toward another
man, by referring to him as a pussy, a queer, or a wimp
(regardless of the male recipients sexual orientation). Given this, it is
also important to recognize how sexual orientation microaggressions
may exist, how they may be very similar to gender microaggressions,
and how they may often be labeled interchangeably. For example, in
the aforementioned incident of the man calling another man a queer,
it might be considered a gender microaggression because the perpetrator is sending a hurtful, denigrating message to the recipient; however,
if the recipient identies as gay, then it could also be considered a sexual orientation microaggression because the man is receiving this hateful message primarily because of his sexual orientation. Furthermore,
gender microaggressions in this context did not focus explicitly on
transgender persons and those who identify as non-gender-conforming.
Accordingly, it is important to recognize that microaggressions may
impact transgender women and men in unique ways, in which they
may receive denigrating messages about their transgender identity,
their gender presentation, or their transgender way of life.

Gender Microaggressions: Implications for Mental Health

173

Research may be benecial in further exploring the psychological


impact of gender microaggressions, sexual orientation microaggressions,
transgender microaggressions, and other microaggressions involving religion, ability, age, or size. Future research involving the intersections of
these identities (as well as the experiences of microaggressions based on
these intersections) would have signicant implications for counseling/
psychotherapy. While previous literature demonstrates that women may
cope with sexism in various ways (Lott et al., 2001) or that experiences
of sexism objectication may lead to various mental health problems
(Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997; Kozee et al., 2007), it is unclear how
women deal with gender microaggressions and how they may impact
ones psychological health. What is clear is that microaggressions need
to be further studied and exposed in the eld of psychology, in order to
improve the lives of women and of all oppressed and disenfranchised
individuals.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I would like to acknowledge Kendra Brewster, Kate Krontris, and
Silvia Mazzula Roman for their assistance in conceptualizing and editing this paper. I also would like to acknowledge my mother, aunts,
cousins, and grandmothers, for being such amazing female role models
in my life.
REFERENCES
American Psychological Association. (2003). Guidelines on multicultural education, training, research, practice, and organizational change for psychologists. American Psychologist, 58, 377402.
Brabeck, M., & Brown, L. (1997). Feminist theory and psychological practice. In
J. Worell & N. G. Johnson (Eds.), Shaping the future of feminist psychology:
Education, research, and practice (pp. 1535). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Bowman, S. L., Rasheed, S., Ferris, J., Thompson, D. A., McRae, M., & Weitzman,
L. (2001). Interface of feminism and multiculturalism: Where are the women
of color? In J. G. Ponterotto, J. M. Casas, L. A. Suzuki, & C. M. Alexander
(Eds.), Handbook of multicultural counseling (2nd ed., pp. 779798). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Collins, L. H. (1998). Illustrating feminist theory: Power and psychopathology.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22, 97112.
Enns, C. Z. (1992). Toward integrating feminist psychotherapy and feminist
philosophy. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 23, 453466.
Espin, O. M. (1993). Feminist therapy: Not for White women only. The Counseling Psychologist, 21, 103108.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Gelfand, M. J., & Drasgow, F. (1995). Measuring sexual harassment: Theoretical and psychometric advances. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17, 425445.

174

Mental and Physical Health

Fitzgerald, L. F., Hulin, C. L., & Drasgow, F. (1994). The antecedents and consequences of sexual harassment in organizations: An integrated model. In
G. P. Keita & J. J. Hurell, Jr. (Eds.), Job stress in a changing workforce: Investigating gender, diversity, and family issues (pp. 5574). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. (1997). Objectication theory: Toward understanding womens lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 21, 173206.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001) An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent
sexism as complementary justications for gender inequality. The American
Psychologist, 56, 109118.
Klonoff, E. A., & Landrine, H. (1995). The schedule of sexist events: A measure
of lifetime and recent sexist discrimination in womens lives. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 19, 439472.
Kozee, H. B., Tylka, T. L., Augustus-Horvath, C. L., & Denchik, A. (2007).
Development of psychometric evaluation of the Interpersonal Sexual
Objectication Scale. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 176189.
Leaper, C., & Brown, C. S. (2008). Perceived experiences with sexism among
adolescent girls. Child Development, 79, 685704.
Lott, B., Aquith, K., & Doyon, T. (2001). Relationship of ethnicity and age to
womens responses to personal experiences of sexist discrimination in the
United States. The Journal of Social Psychology, 141, 309315.
Matteson, A. V., & Moradi, B. (2005). Examining the structure of the Schedule
of Sexist Events: Replication and extension. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
29, 4757.
McNamara, K., & Rickard, K. M. (1998). Feminist identity development: Implications for feminist therapy with women. In D. R. Atkinson & G. Hackett
(Eds.), Counseling diverse populations (2nd ed., pp. 271282). Boston:
McGraw-Hill.
Murray, B. (1998). Psychologys voice in sexual harassment law: A psychologist
casts light on the murky area of sexual harassment. APA Monitor, 29.
Nadal, K. L. (2008). Preventing racial, ethnic, gender, sexual minority, disability, and religious microaggressions: Recommendations for promoting positive mental health. Prevention in Counseling Psychology: Theory, Research,
Practice and Training, 2, 2227.
National Womens Law Center. (2008). The Fair Pay Campaign. Retrieved
September 15, 2008, from http://www.nwlc.org/fairpay/
Schneider, K. T., Swann, S., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1997). Job-related and psychological effects of sexual harassment in the workplace: Empirical evidence
from two organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 401415.
Sue, D. W. (2005). Racism and the conspiracy of silence. Counseling Psychologist,
33, 100114.
Sue, D. W., Arredondo, P., & McDavis, R. J. (1992). Multicultural counseling
competencies and standards: A call to the profession. Journal of Counseling
& Development, 70, 477486.
Sue, D. W., Bucceri, J. M., Lin, A. I., Nadal, K. L., & Torino, G. C. (2007). Racial
microaggressions and the Asian American experience. Cultural Diversity
and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 13, 7281.

Gender Microaggressions: Implications for Mental Health

175

Sue, D. W., & Capodilupo, C. M. (2008). Racial, gender, and sexual orientation
microaggressions: Implications for counseling and psychotherapy. In
D. W. Sue & D. Sue (Eds.), Counseling the culturally diverse (5th ed.). New
York: John Wiley & Sons.
Sue, D. W., Capodilupo, C. M., Nadal, K. L., & Torino, G. C. (2008). Racial
microaggressions and the power to dene reality. The American Psychologist, 63, 277279.
Sue, D. W., Capodilupo, C. M., Torino, G. C., Bucceri, J. M., Holder, A. M., Nadal,
K. L., et al. (2007). Racial microaggressions in everyday life: Implications for
counseling. The American Psychologist, 62, 271286.
Sue, D. W., Nadal, K. L., Capodilupo, C. M., Lin, A. I., Rivera, D. P., & Torino,
G. C. (2008). Racial microaggressions against Black Americans: Implications for counseling. Journal of Counseling and Development, 86, 330338.
Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (2008). Counseling the culturally diverse (5th ed.). New
York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Swim, J. K., & Cohen, L. L. (1997). Overt, covert, and subtle sexism: A comparison between the attitudes toward women and modern sexism scales. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 103118.
Swim, J. K., Hyers, L. L., Cohen, L. L., & Ferguson, M. J. (2001). Everyday sexism: Evidence for its incidence, nature, and psychological impact from
three daily diary studies. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 3153.
Swim, J. K., Mallett, R., & Stangor, C. (2004) Understanding subtle sexism:
Detection and use of sexist language. Sex Roles, 51, 117128.
U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (2008). Sexual harassment.
Retrieved August 28, 2008, from http://www.eeoc.gov/types/sexual_
harassment.

This page intentionally left blank

Chapter 8

Prejudice and Discrimination against


Sexual Minorities: A Brazilian
Perspective
Eros DeSouza
Elder Cerqueira-Santos

This chapter focuses on a neglected populationsexual minorities (lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, and questioning [LGBTQ] individuals)
in Brazil, a developing country, which is relevant to U.S. researchers,
clinicians, and policy makers interested in sexual minorities of color
who often face similar challenges. We begin by dening some basic
concepts concerning gender, sexual orientation, and transsexualism.
Next, we briey discuss prejudice and discrimination, including subtle
discrimination toward LGBT individuals. We also discuss pertinent
material published in English or Portuguese concerning prejudice, discrimination, and violence toward sexual minorities in Brazil. Moreover,
we give a voice to such population by inserting excerpts from interviews that we conducted during 2009 with Brazilian LGBTQ individuals from Porto Alegre (southern Brazil), S~
ao Paulo, Rio de Janeiro (both
in southeastern Brazil), and Aracaju (northeastern Brazil).
Brazil is the largest Latin American country and is ranked eighth in
the world economy. It is slightly smaller than the United States, with a
population of about 200 million (54 percent white; Central Intelligence
Agency, 2009). Brazilian society differs greatly from region to region,
from rural to urban life, with industrialization and urbanization widespread in some regions (e.g., south and southeast) and not as much in
others (e.g., north and the interior). Moreover, sexuality and gender

178

Mental and Physical Health

norms have been changing substantially in contemporary Brazil


(DeSouza, Baldwin, Koller, & Narvaz, 2004). According to Parker
(1999), Brazil has the largest and most visible gay subculture . . . anywhere outside the fully industrialized West (p. 45). Gay and lesbian
sexuality are increasingly visible in Brazil, with an emergent gay and
lesbian as well as mainstream press discussing gay and lesbian cultural
and political issues and a growing commercialization of the Brazilian
gay culture, which has been inuenced by North American and Western European gay cultures (Klein, 1999; Parker, 1999).
It is impossible to discuss homosexuality in Brazil without also discussing how Brazilian society views gender and sexuality in general.
Brazil is a machista society (DeSouza et al., 2004); thus, gender relations,
as well as relations across different sexual orientations must be viewed
in that context as well.
BASIC CONCEPTS
Sex versus Gender
These terms are often used interchangeably, but they carry different
connotations. Sex is a biological category used to differentiate human
males (individuals with XY chromosomes and male genitalia) from
human females (individuals with XX chromosomes and female genitalia). There is a small percentage of humans who are inter-sexed (hermaphrodite), having ambiguous components of their biological sex;
however, the overwhelming majority of humans can be unambiguously
classied as male or female (Faust-Sterling, 2000). Gender is a socially
constructed term that describes social interpretations of what it means
to be a man or a woman in a given society; gender stereotypes are
beliefs shared by people in a given society about how men and women
differ or should differ (Larsen & Buss, 2008).
Sexual Identity versus Sexual Orientation
Sexual identity refers to ones sexual attraction (wanting) and pattern of sexual behavior (doing), having both personal and political
meanings (Starks, Gilbert, Fischer, Weston, & DiLalla, 2009). Such
meanings are interpersonally constructed; thus, they are uid, changing
over time and throughout ones life. Researchers often categorize sexual identity based on ones self-assessment (labeling) at a given time
(e.g., lesbian, gay, bisexual, heterosexual). Ones sexual identity (how
one views oneself as a sexual being) may not always coincide with
ones sexual orientation, which includes ones physical (sexual) and
affectional (emotional) attraction toward another person. Like sexual
identity, sexual orientation is uid and is best conceptualized on a

Prejudice and Discrimination against Sexual Minorities

179

continuum, from exclusively homosexual to exclusively heterosexual;


it is also complex, including ones sexual identity, desires, behaviors,
disclosures, and experiences (Horowitz & Newcomb, 2001). Research
suggests that women, compared to men, are more likely to show variability and transitions throughout their lifetimes for both sexual orientation and identity, probably due to womens more uid and less narrow
attractions than mens (e.g., Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2000).
Transvestism versus Transsexualism
Transvestism refers to obtaining pleasure or sexual fulllment by
adopting the clothes, mannerisms, or gender roles of the opposite
sex (e.g., during Brazilian Carnival, many men dress up as women;
Cardoso, 2005). According to Cardoso, transvestism is also related to
cross-dressing; for example, when gay men dress up as drag queens
for entertainment purposes. Cardoso explains transsexualism as a longing to look, feel, and be like the opposite sex from the one with which
an individual was born. Some transsexuals opt for permanent sex reassignment through surgery. Transgender is the umbrella term that
encompasses both transvestites and transsexuals.
PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION
Although human sexuality is diverse, with same-sex attraction,
desire, and behavior being normal reections of such diversity, there is
resistance in developed and developing countries alike to extend universal principles of equality, justice, freedom, and dignity to LGBTQ
individuals (Kitzinger & Wilkinson, 2004; Klein, 1999). Such resistance
may be related to sexual prejudice, which Herek (2000) denes as all
negative attitudes toward a person because of his or her homosexuality. According to Herek, sexually prejudiced individuals are usually
male, report being exclusively heterosexual, and have limited contact
with LGBTQ individuals.
A recent Brazilian study conducted with 200 students from a public
university corroborates the above ndings, including the contact hypothesis, which states that heterosexuals who interact with LGBTQ
individuals seem to develop more positive attitudes toward sexual
minorities than those with limited contact with LGBTQ individuals
(Cerqueira-Santos, Winter, Salles, Longo, & Teodoro, 2007). However,
Cerqueira-Santos et al. found that the contact hypothesis worked better
among heterosexual women, who showed signicantly less sexual prejudice, especially toward lesbians, than among heterosexual men. These
ndings suggest that Brazilian men fear being perceived as not masculine enough if they associate with sexual minorities, which is similar to
how U.S. masculinity is construed (e.g., Kimmel, 1997). Moreover, in

180

Mental and Physical Health

order for the contact hypothesis to work, others must know about ones
sexual orientation.
Unlike women and ethnic/racial minorities who have visible phenotypic
characteristics, homosexuality is often invisible, dependent on revelation to
become known to others (Sedgwick, 1990); thus, Fassinger (1991) refers to
their status as an invisible minority in society. Sexual prejudice (i.e., antipathy toward LGBTQ individuals) may lead to discrimination, which is an
act, either physical or verbal, as well as avoidance or social exclusion (i.e.,
not associating with LGBTQ individuals; Crocker, Major, & Steele, 1998).
DeSouza and Showalter (in press) recently examined microaggressions (e.g., social exclusion and spreading malicious rumors)
directed at sexual minorities that are subtle, yet detrimental. DeSouza
and Showalter sampled 133 LGBTQ college students in the United
States. After controlling for negative affectivity, they found that LGBTQ
students who experienced at least one act of subtle sexual orientation
harassment during the past year were signicantly more likely to state
intention to leave their institution and have a lower GPA than nonharassed students. In addition, DeSouza and Showalter found that
LGBTQ students who were open about their sexual orientation
reported higher levels of self-esteem and life satisfaction and lower levels of anxiety and depression than those less committed to their sexual
identity, suggesting signicant benets for coming out to others.
HOMOSEXUALITY IN BRAZIL
In the Brazilian sexual script, there is a hegemonic distinction
between masculine atividade (activity) and feminine passividade (passivity), in which sexuality is subjected to male desire, reecting widespread machismo (Parker, 1993). According to Parker, such a
distinction is reected in the daily language Brazilians use to describe
sexual relations, in which the role of the macho is to comer (literally to
eat, but loosely translated as to fuck) and the role assigned to women,
effeminate gay men, and transvestites is to dar (to give). In the Brazilian sexual universe, comer is synonymous with vencer (to win, to conquer) and possuir (to own, to possess). Such a vocabulary of sexual
meanings suggests that women, effeminate gay men, and transvestites
are socialized to be passive, receptive sexual partners, while macho
men are socialized to pursue, to penetrate, and to dominate. Thus, in
Brazil, a macho man continues to exercise the power of his virility with
any feminine individual, including a bicha or effeminate male homosexual (Mendes-Leite, 1993). In other words, as long as the macho maintains his atividade (i.e., be the active person or penetrator, not the
penetrated), he is typically not viewed as a homosexual by society.
There is also an internalization of such sexual script by some gay men,

Prejudice and Discrimination against Sexual Minorities

181

creating a sexual hierarchy in Brazil, with macho men at the top and
feminine individuals (i.e., heterosexual women, effeminate gay men,
and transgendered individuals) on the bottom.
Here I have to be macho. It does not matter what one does within four
walls; it is our secret. As long as you act like a macho in public, everything
is ne. Even among other gay men, the idea of being macho [active or penetrator] is strong. If a gay man wants to offend another, just call him a passive [or penetrated] queer.
thirty-six-year-old gay man from Aracaju
People talk bad about transvestites . . . they say we are the scummiest subgroup of all homosexuals, but nobody questions macho homosexual men who
go out with us and pay to have sex with us. They continue to be honorable
gentlemen in the eyes of society, because they live an anonymous life and often pass as heterosexual. They think they are the most macho men in the
world. They are macho because they eat [have sex with] anyone!
twenty-four-year-old transvestite from Aracaju

According to Prado and Machado (2008), the distinction between


masculine atividade (activity) and feminine passividade (passivity) is
reected in same-sex and other-sex relations throughout Latin America.
Thus, to be active during sexual intercourse is viewed as dominant,
aggressive, and masculine, whereas to be passive is viewed as weak,
submissive, and feminine.
In the Brazilian culture, the above distinction also applies to ones
public image or persona and interfaces with ones appearance and
socioeconomic status. Thus, certain terms (e.g., being called bofe or
macho gay man) carry a more positive connotation than others (e.g.,
being called frutinha or little fruit, which means to be a delicate or
effeminate gay man).
It is different to be gay in the wealthy parts of Rio than in the poor sections. There is prejudice everywhere, but in the wealthy areas we have some
protection. It is our beach! Besides, gays are a part of the social culture and
the way of life in Ipanema. If we are gone, half of the bars will close down!
thirty-eight-year-old gay man from Rio de Janeiro
In public, people think I am a heterosexual. It is funny; women irt with
me a lot. I think it is because I take care of myself . . . I am t, well-dressed,
etc. I am sure that if I were a mal-nourished poor queer it would be another
story. They would laugh at me instead. Ones appearance makes a big difference, even among other gays who think I am a dominant top, but nobody
knows what people do in bed.
twenty-ve-year-old gay man from S~
ao Paulo

Moreover, homosexuals seem to be the most oppressed group in


Brazilmuch more than women, racial minorities, and people with

182

Mental and Physical Health

disabilities (Almeida & Crillanovick, 1999)even though open displays


of homosexuality are common and widely accepted. For example, former Brazilian President Fernando Henrique Cardoso was often photographed kissing drag queens; one of Brazils top female models was
once a man; and crowds cheer nearly naked gay men every year during
Carnival (Goering, 1997). Based on historical analyses of events in Rio de
Janeiro and S~ao Paulo, Green (1999) reported a growing male homosexual appropriation and transformation of Carnival during the twentieth
century, which opened up a more accepting view of male homosexuality, at least in large cities. Green documented a paradox of permissive
same-sex eroticism during the four days of Carnival with accounts of
intolerance toward homosexuals during the rest of the year.
Everybody thinks that being gay is fantastic in Rio, but only if you are
good-looking and rich. Poor gays only show up during Carnival or during
the LGBT [Pride] parade. Afterwards we should become invisible and quiet.
There are two worlds in this city. It is not easy to be a queer from the
slums.
twenty-eight-year-old gay man from a favela (slum)
in Rio de Janeiro
When I am dressed up as a Drag Queen I am a goddess of Carnival, but
afterwards everything changes. In their eyes, I am a queer with AIDS whom
nobody wants to see or employ! The only path to survive is prostitution.
There are plenty of clients. Who are they? The same ones who deny me legal
employment and do not want to see me around.
thirty-year-old transgendered person from Rio de Janeiro

A national poll revealed a similar ambivalence toward homosexuality among a cross-section of Brazilian men and women. Fifty percent of
those surveyed indicated daily contact with homosexuals at work or in
the neighborhood; however, 56 percent said they would change their
behavior toward a colleague if they discovered he or she was a homosexual, with 20 percent avoiding contact with such colleague and 36
percent indicating they would not hire a homosexual, even if he or she
were the best-qualied candidate for the position; 58 percent reported
being opposed to homosexual couples adopting a child even if they
had lived in a committed relationship for a long time; and 79 percent
said they would be disappointed if they had a homosexual child
(O mundo gay rasga as fantasias, 1993).
A survey study conducted in the city of S~
ao Paulo (Instituto de Pesquisa
e Cultura GLS, 2000) revealed that 67 percent of the LGBT sample
reported being discriminated against due to their sexual orientation by
family members, friends, and neighbors, as well as in work, school, public, and health services settings. In addition, victims of such discrimination were re-victimized when ling complaints in police stations.

Prejudice and Discrimination against Sexual Minorities

183

A study conducted with 220 college students in northeast Brazil


indicated that only 24 percent of those sampled were not prejudiced toward homosexuals, with 38 percent being mildly prejudiced and an
additional 38 percent being blatantly so (Lacerda, Pereira, & Camino,
2002). As in the United States, Lacerda et al. found that heterosexual
Brazilian men had more negative attitudes toward gay men and lesbians than heterosexual Brazilian women did.
In addition, a recent study with 891 U.S. and Brazilian college students revealed that in both countries, bystanders often witnessed harassment of homosexuals (DeSouza & Scheinder, 2009). Overall, 77
percent of all respondents reported having experienced at least one act
of being a bystander verbal harassment of homosexuals, whereas only
25 percent of the participants reported having experienced at least one
act of bystander to exclusion of homosexuals during the past 12
months. Note that these types of bystander experiences are not mutually exclusive. Being a bystander to verbal harassment of homosexuals
had negative consequences to bystanders, especially in Brazil. Specically, being a bystander to verbal harassment of homosexuals was a
signicant predictor of multiple negative consequences (i.e., physical
illness and drug-alcohol use) in Brazil, but this may also be a source of
concern in the United States as well (via a link with drug and alcohol
use). These ndings extend past research on the negative outcomes of
being a bystander to sexual (Glomb, Richman, Hulin, & Drasgow, 1997)
or ethnic (Low, Radhakrishnan, Schneider, & Rounds, 2007) harassment.
Violence against Sexual Minorities
As in the United States, violence against sexual minorities is not a
new problem in Brazil. In fact, transgendered individuals are the main
victims of discrimination and violence based on sexual orientation
(Carrara & Vianna, 2006). According to Carrara and Vianna, the police
are indifferent to crimes committed against poor transgendered individuals who turn to prostitution to survive. Even their murder is
viewed as a normal consequence of an illegal way of earning a living. In fact, police ofcers often look the other way when confronted
with hate crimes against homosexuals (Goering, 1997).
According to Mott (2002), Brazil ranks number one in the world for
hate crimes based on sexual orientation. A recent study by Grupo Gay
da Bahia (2009) reports that 190 homosexuals were killed during 2008,
with gay men (96 percent) being by far more likely to be killed than
lesbians (4 percent). According to Grupo Gay da Bahia, northeastern
Brazil, which makes up 30 percent of the Brazilian population, has the
deepest anti-gay attitudes and the highest percentage (48 percent) of
homosexuals murdered compared to the south/southeast (28 percent),
midwest (14 percent), and north (10 percent).

184

Mental and Physical Health

A study conducted in 2001 in the state of Rio de Janeiro revealed


that during an eighteen-month period, the police received 500 reports
of violence against LGBT individuals. Of these, 6.3 percent consisted of
murder, 10.3 percent were extortion cases, 18.7 percent were of physical assault, and 20.2 percent were of discrimination based on sexual
orientation (cited in Conselho Nacional de Combate a Discriminac~ao,
2004). Every three days a person is murdered in Brazil because of his or
her sexual orientation (Rios, 2002). Note that these statistics reect crimes
that were reported, representing an underestimation of the true prevalence of crimes against LGBT individuals. Thus, it is important to conduct
survey studies that tap victimization, potentially revealing crimes committed against LGBT persons that may otherwise go unreported.
A survey study conducted in Rio de Janeiro with 416 gay men,
lesbians, and transgendered individuals revealed that 60 percent of
the respondents reported being victims of violence/discrimination
(Carrara, Ramos, & Caetano, 2002). Specically, 16.6 percent reported
experiencing physical assault (the rate almost tripled for transgendered
individuals, with 42.3 percent), 18 percent bribery or extortion (the rate
almost doubled for transgendered individuals, with 30.8 percent), 56.3
percent experienced verbal insults or threats due to their sexual orientation, and 58.5 percent were discriminated against because of their
sexual orientation. This study also revealed that 22.4 percent of the lesbians surveyed were victimized by family members because they were
women and lesbian.
The above ndings suggest that many Brazilian sexual minorities
live in silence, secrecy, threat of rejection, and marginality. In addition,
these somber statistics require consciousness of the violence against
sexual minorities that goes on in their own family of origin.
The Brazilian family usually does not accept a homosexual child,
especially a transgendered adolescent whose inappropriate gender
expression is used as an excuse for family-inicted violence; such a
child is eventually expelled from home and often survives through
prostitution (Conselho Nacional de Combate a Discriminac~ao, 2004).
There is a gendered violence in many Brazilian families that submit
individuals, physically and/or emotionally, consciously and/or unconsciously as a function of their sex (Werba & Strey, 2001, p. 72).
According to DeFrancisco (1997), as gender roles play out in Brazilian
society and in the family, they are linked to violence, especially physical and sexual abuse; such abuse is aggravated by secrecy and denial
of family-inicted violence. Machismo is often present in the typical
family scenery of abusive systems to the extent that these systems
reect more rigid gender roles that correspond to patriarchal systems
that are institutionalized in society (DeSouza et al., 2004). Thus, it is
easy to see that transgendered people, like women, are often victims of
family and social violence.

Prejudice and Discrimination against Sexual Minorities

185

Prejudice against homosexuals is everywhere, even inside us. I dont feel


accepted by my family and cannot even mention my partner at work. What
is left is a [gay] ghetto. I only befriend other gay men and sometimes they
too show machismo. I have dated a transgendered individual and they spoke
badly of this relationship.
thirty-one-year-old gay man from Porto Alegre
I ended up moving to the big city to escape. Here nobody knows me or my
life. I had to bury my past and my life began when I moved here. Now I am
myself!
thirty-six-year-old lesbian from S~
ao Paulo
When my father found out that I am a transvestite and do shows at night,
he told me not to return home. In order to see my mother, she has to come
to my house. I do not have a family anymore, you know. I had no choice. It
was me or them.
thirty-ve-year-old transgendered person from Porto Alegre

Recently, Rocha et al. (2009) interviewed eight transsexuals to nd


out their perceptions of the Brazilian public health system, which provides universal and free health services to all Brazilian nationals. The
ndings revealed stereotypes about transsexuals and sexual prejudice
by health providers at all levels (e.g., clerical staff, nurses, and doctors),
especially toward transgender sex workers. For instance, these participants experienced humiliation from health care providers when they
voiced a desire to be called by their chosen names rather than by their
birth names, even though by law transsexuals are allowed to have their
chosen names in all their medical les. One participant said, When
they call me [in the health clinic] by my birth name . . . the name written in the identication le, I stand up and go in order not to lose the
consultation with the doctor. I walk with my head down, cause I feel
embarrassed (p. 14). Another participant recalled a similar experience:
They [health ofcials] dont call you by your chosen name, you know.
They are not sensible. On top of that, you can hear the comments and
laughs as soon as you leave (p. 14). Rocha et al. reported that staff
members simply refused to call transgendered individuals by their chosen name even when explicitly asked to do so: A receptionist treated
me really bad. She said simply, I will call you R (masculine name).
I said I would really like to be called by my chosen name G (feminine).
The receptionist said, No, I will call you R (p. 15).
The interviews also showed a lack of sensitivity and training to deal
with transgendered individuals. I reckon there is no preparation
to deal with us, from those who work in the reception desk up to the
doctors. I believe they dont have a minimum of training (p. 16).
Another said: How can you develop any rapport with your doctor if,
upon arrival, the doctor starts treating you as a he. How can I develop rapport with someone who calls me sir? How can I be open

186

Mental and Physical Health

enough to talk about my life, to expose myself or talk about intimate


stuff with the doctor? There is no rapport! I may need a doctor in the
future and not go (pp. 1617).
Another participant brings up the important topic of how public
stigma, stereotype, prejudice, and discrimination against transgendered
individuals negatively affect treatment seeking, yielding signicant
harm to this population: When they [transvestites] seek health services, they face discrimination instead, and they no longer go back . . .
the public perceptions of prejudice and hatred dont change. Many of
them get really bad [with their health] . . . only when their condition
worsens signicantly, then they seek help (p. 19). Another participant
described how physicians stereotype and prejudge transgendered individuals as automatically being HIV positive: When you report any
health symptom, and they realize youre a transvestite or transsexual,
most [health care professionals] believe that you are infected with HIV
straight away . . . (p. 17).
Rocha et al.s interviews support the notion that there is widespread
public stigma against sexual minorities, especially against transgendered individuals, even by well-educated professionals. There is a
hopeful trend though: Whitam, Daskalos, Sobolewski, and Padilla
(1998) suggest that there is increasing public support for the civil rights
of sexual minorities in Brazil.
IMPORTANT LAWS AND RESOLUTIONS TOWARD EQUALITY
Historically, homosexuality has been viewed as pathological and
deviant both in the United States and Brazil. Though sodomy was
decriminalized in 1830 throughout Brazil (Rios, 2002), it remained a
crime in some states in the United States until the Supreme Courts decision in 2003, which overturned state same-sex sodomy laws (Lawrence
v. Texas, 2003).
It was only in 1985 that homosexuality was removed as a mental
disorder from the Brazilian Federal Association of Medicine (cited in
Conselho Nacional de Combate 
a Discriminac~
ao, 2004), compared to its
removal from the second edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders in 1973 in the United States (American Psychiatric Association, 1973). On March 22, 1999, the Brazilian Federal
Association of Psychology passed a resolution stating that no professional who provides therapeutic services to sexual minority clients can act
in a way that reinforces the notion of homoerotic behaviors or practices
as pathological (Resoluc~
ao CFP # 001/99, 1999). Such resolution is similar to the U.S. resolution on appropriate therapeutic responses to sexual
orientation passed by the American Psychological Association council
of representatives on August 14, 1997 (American Psychological Association, 1998). There is an important difference, however. In Brazil, such a

Prejudice and Discrimination against Sexual Minorities

187

resolution refers to actual standards that are mandatory, whereas in


the United States it refers to guidelines or recommendations aimed at
motivating psychologists to act ethically toward sexual minority clients
without enforcement mechanisms.
On May 13, 2002, former Brazilian President Fernando Henrique
Cardoso proposed a program to amend the Brazilian Constitution of
1988which does not include sexual orientation as a protected
groupthat would explicitly make sexual orientation a human rights
issue, prohibiting discrimination based on sexual orientation (Leis,
2002). Such an amendment has not yet passed. Currently, Brazil has a
patchwork of legal protection, with three states protecting LGBT individuals from discrimination, whereas the remaining 24 states lack such
protection (Conselho Nacional de Combate 
a Discriminac~ao, 2004).
Such a patchwork of legal protection parallels the situation found in
the U.S., where only 20 states ban discrimination based on sexual orientation (National Gay and Lesbian Task Force, 2008).
Unlike in the United States, Brazilian federal laws have extended
many rights to same-sex domestic partners, including social security
and inheritance (Rios, 2002). In addition, legislation was passed on
December 3, 2003, extending immigration rights to foreign homosexuals who wish to join their Brazilian partners (Silva & Barbi, 2005).
Laws against sexual harassment are relatively recent in Brazil. The
rst such law was enacted in the state of Rio de Janeiro in 1991, where
organizations could be ned and the perpetrator dismissed (Noviski,
Davoli, & Castro, 2000). On May 15, 2001, a national law passed that
criminalizes sexual harassment and provides for up to two years in jail
for perpetrators found guilty (C
odigo Penal Brasileiro, 2001). Such law
denes sexual harassment only as sexual coercion by a superior in
order to obtain sexual favors; it does not include a hostile environment
created by peers or subordinates. It emphasizes an abuse of power by
a superior. These laws are gender neutral. Pastore and Robortella
(2003) found that 9 percent of reported cases involve same-sex sexual
harassment.
According to the Brazilian Health Ministry (Conselho Nacional de
Combate a Discriminac~
ao, 2004), the government needs to take several
steps to reduce prejudice, discrimination, and violence based on sexual
orientation. We highlight the rst four because they are of interest to
U.S. researchers, clinicians, and decision makers as well. First, there is
a need for action by disseminating among all levels of society the
notion that homosexual citizenship is a human rights issue. Second, the
constitution must be amended in order to protect sexual minorities
from discrimination in employment, education, housing, mental and
health care, and the military in all Brazilian states, territories, cities,
municipalities, and public institutions. Third, it is necessary to cooperate with international organizations to ensure that human rights are

188

Mental and Physical Health

extended to all sexual minorities worldwide. Fourth, LGBTQ individuals have the right to live in a society free of violence; when crime based
on sexual orientation occurs, punishment needs to be swift and just.
SEXISM
The Brazilian Health Ministry (Conselho Nacional de Combate a
Discriminac~ao, 2004) also suggested that prejudice based on sexual orientation must be reduced alongside other types of prejudice, such as
sexism. Research has shown that sexism is related to negative attitudes
toward sexual minorities. Glick and Fiske (1996) differentiated between
two types of sexist ideologies. The rst is a subjectively positive, but
patronizing, orientation of protection, idealization, and affection toward
women (benevolent sexism), whereas the second reects mens violence
against women and exploitation of women as sex objects (hostile sexism). These two types of sexism are complementary. They reinforce
and justify patriarchy, including heterosexuals hostility toward women
who deviate from traditional gender roles (e.g., lesbians; Glick & Fiske,
1997). Research across six U.S. samples showed that men consistently
scored signicantly higher than did women on both hostile and benevolent sexism; however, the gender gap was greater for hostile sexism
than for benevolent sexism (Glick & Fiske, 1996).
Moreover, benevolent and hostile sexism have been found to be
widespread across 19 nations, including Brazil, reecting and maintaining the oppression of women (Glick et al., 2000), including lesbians.
For instance, in a study across three British samples of high school students, college students, and full-time employees, Masser and Abrams
(1999) found that benevolent sexism, hostile sexism, and neosexism
were negatively related to support for lesbians and gay mens rights.
In addition, in a study of college students at a medium-sized Midwestern U.S. public university, Whitley (2001) found that the best predictors
of attitudes toward homosexuality were gender, benevolent sexism,
endorsement of the traditional masculine role, and attitudes toward
women.
DeSouza, Solberg, and Elder (2007) examined the inuence of ones
attitudes toward women in general and lesbians in particular on perceptions of woman-to-woman sexual harassment among 952 U.S. and
Brazilian college students. The authors found signicant relationships
between negative attitudes toward lesbians with both benevolent and
hostile sexism. Thus, sexist individuals also have anti-lesbian attitudes.
The authors also found some support for the contact hypothesis, as
there was a signicant relationship between associating with homosexuals and having positive attitudes toward lesbians. There were interesting cross-cultural differences. Overall, college students in Brazil viewed
hypothetical cases involving woman-to-woman sexual harassment as

Prejudice and Discrimination against Sexual Minorities

189

signicantly more harassing and more likely to require an investigation


than did college students in the United States. There were signicant
gender differences, but only between U.S. men and women, suggesting
that Brazilian men and women are alike in the perceptions of womanto-woman sexual harassment. Finally, hostile sexism and perceptions
of what behaviors constitute woman-to-woman sexual harassment signicantly predicted views of the hypothetical case as sexual harassment and in need of an investigation. These ndings generally
supported Fiske and Glicks (1995) argument that sexist attitudes predict judgments about unwanted social-sexual behaviors.

CONCLUSION
The picture of Brazil portrayed by the media is of Carnival, where
nudity is the norm, but most Brazilians remain conservative underneath the mask of a sensual and seductive society, where there is no
sin below the equator or within four walls everything is permitted
(Parker, 1991, 1993, 1999). In fact, machismo is alive and well. However, like its views toward women, Brazilian society has been changing
its views toward LGBTQ individuals, with a signicant shift toward
acceptance of sexual minorities.
There are many lenses to understand Brazil. On the one hand, sexual minorities in Brazil experience dehumanization in the form of public stigma, stereotyping, prejudice, discrimination, and violence. On the
other hand, Brazilians are discussing homosexuality more openly,
which is changing morality from a rigid hegemonic heterosexual viewpoint toward a more accepting view of differences and diversity as desirable. According to Prado and Machado (2008), homosexuality in
Brazil has been through a political transformation: from criminalizing
and pathologizing homosexuality toward ghting for human rights
and dignity of LGBTQ individuals.
Increasing numbers of sexual minority individuals in Brazil are coming out of the closet and slowly are being accepted by family members,
friends, neighbors, and co-workers. For instance, on June 14, 2009, the
city of S~ao Paulo held its 13th Gay Pride parade, the largest in the
world with an estimated 3.5 million people in attendance (Parada Gay,
2009). The process is not uniform though. Acceptance is more common
in large urban centers, such as S~
ao Paulo and Rio de Janeiro, than in
rural areas or in the northeast of Brazil. In addition, sexual minority
individuals who publicly conform to traditional gender role expectations are more easily accepted than their less gender-typical counterparts, especially transgendered individuals who totally break away
from traditional gender expressions and are the most marginalized and
the most victimized sexual minority subgroup in Brazil.

190

Mental and Physical Health

REFERENCES
Almeida, L. M., & Crillanovick, Q. (1999). A cidadania e os direitos humanos
de gays, lesbicas e travestis no Brasil [Citizenship and human rights of
gays, lesbians and transvestites in Brazil]. In D. D. Oliveira, R. B. Lima,
S. A. Santos, & T. L. D. Tosta (Eds.), 50 anos depois: Relac~oes raciais e grupos
socialmente segregados [50 years later: Race relations and socially segregated
groups] (pp. 167183). Goi^
ania, Brazil: MNDH.
American Psychiatric Association. (1973). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
American Psychological Association. (1998). Appropriate therapeutic responses
to sexual orientation in the proceedings of the American Psychological
Association, Incorporated, for the legislative year 1997. American Psychologist, 53, 882939.
Cardoso, F. L. (2005). Invers~
oes do papel de g^enero: Drag queens, travestismo
e transexualismo [Gender identity divergence: Drag queens, transvestism,
and transexualism]. Psicologia: Reex~ao e Crtica, 18, 421430.
Carrara, S., & Vianna, A. R. B. (2006). T
a l
a o corpo estendido no ch~
ao: a viol^encia letal contra travestis no municpio do Rio de Janeiro [Over there
the body lies on the ground: Deadly violence against transvestites in the
municipality of Rio de Janeiro]. Physis: Rev Sa
ude P
ublicaRio de Janeiro,
16, 233249.
Carrara, S., Ramos, S., & Caetano, M. (2002). Poltica, direitos, viol^encia e homossexualidade [Politics, rights, violence and homosexuality]. Rio de Janeiro:
Pallas.
Central Intelligence Agency. (2009, May 14). The world fact-bookBrazil. Retrieved
June 2, 2009, from https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-worldfactbook/geos/br.html
Cerqueira-Santos, E., Winter, F., Salles, A., Longo, J., & Teodoro, M. (2007).
Contato interpessoal e crencas sobre homossexualidade: Desenvolvimento
de uma escala [Interpersonal contact and beliefs about homosexuality: A
scale construction]. Interac~ao em Psicologia, 11, 221229.
C
odigo Penal Brasileiro [Brazilian Penal Code]. (2001). Lei 10.224, Art. 216-A.
Braslia.
Conselho Nacional de Combate 
a Discriminac~
ao [National Committee to Combat Discrimination]. (2004). Brasil sem homofobia: Programa de combate a viol^encia e a discriminac~ao contra GLTB e promoc~ao da cidadania homosexual
[Brazil without homophobia: Program to combat violence and discrimination against GLTB and promote homosexual citizenship]. Braslia: Ministerio da Sa
ude.
Crocker, J., Major, B., & Steele, C. (1998). Social stigma. In D. T. Gilbert,
S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed.,
pp. 504553). New York: McGraw Hill.
DeFrancisco, V. (1997). Gender, power and practice: Or, putting your money
(and your research) where your mouth is. In R. Wodak (Ed.), Gender and
discourse (pp. 3756). London: Sage.
DeSouza, E. R., Baldwin, J., Koller, S. H., & Narvaz, M. (2004). A Latin American
perspective in the study of gender. In M. A. Paludi (Ed.), Praeger guide to the
psychology of gender (pp. 4167). Westport, CT: Praeger.

Prejudice and Discrimination against Sexual Minorities

191

DeSouza, E. R., & Schneider, K. T. (2009). Incivility based on sexual orientation: The
impact on North American and Brazilian bystanders. Manuscript in preparation.
DeSouza, E. R., Solberg, J., & Elder, C. (2007). A cross-cultural perspective on
judgments of woman-to-woman sexual harassment: Does sexual orientation matter? Sex Roles, 56, 457471.
DeSouza, E., & Showalter, B. In M. Paludi & F. Denmark (Eds.), Victims of
sexual assault and abuse: Resources and responses for individuals and families.
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Diamond, L. M., & Savin-Williams, R. C. (2000). Explaining diversity in the
development of same-sex sexuality among young women. In L. D. Garnets
(Ed.), Psychological perspectives on lesbian, gay, and bisexual experiences
(pp. 130148). New York: Columbia University Press.
Fassinger, R. E. (1991). The hidden minority: Issues and challenges in working
with lesbian women and gay men. The Counseling Psychologist, 19, 157176.
Fausto-Sterling, A. (2000). Sexing the body: Gender politics and the construction of
sexuality. New York: Basic Books.
Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (1995). Ambivalence and stereotypes cause sexual
harassment: A theory with implications for organizational change. Journal
of Social Issues, 51, 97115.
Grupo Gay da Bahia. (2009). Relatorio sobre crimes homofobicos no Brasil [Report
about hate crimes in Brazil based on sexual orientation]. Grupo Gay da
Bahia, Salvador: Grupo Gay da Bahia.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 491512.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1997). Hostile and benevolent sexism: Measuring
ambivalent sexist attitudes toward women. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
21, 119135.
Glick, P., Fiske, S. T., Mladinic, A., Saiz, J. L., Abrams, D., Masser, B., et al.
(2000). Beyond prejudice as simple antipathy: Hostile and benevolent sexism
across cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 763775.
Glomb, T. M., Richman, W. L., Hulin, C. L., & Drasgow, R. (1997). Ambient
sexual harassment: An integrated model of antecedents and consequences.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 71, 309328.
Goering, L. (1997, April 4). Brazils gay men appear singled out, attacked for
living ordinary lives. The Chicago Tribune. Chicago, IL. Retrieved March 25,
2005, from http://www.familyresearchinst.org/FRI_EduPamphet4.html
Green, J. N. (1999). Beyond carnival: Male homosexuality in twentieth-century
Brazil. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Herek, G. H. (2000). Sexual prejudice and gender: Do heterosexuals attitudes
toward lesbians and gay men differ? Journal of Social Issues, 56, 251266.
Horowitz, J. L., & Newcomb, M. D. (2001). A multidimensional approach to
homosexual identity. Journal of Homosexuality, 42, 119.
Instituto de Pesquisa e Cultura GLS [Institute of Research and Culture GLS].
(2000). 1o. Censo GLS do Brasil [First Census of GLS in Brazil]. S~
ao Paulo:
GLS Planet. Retrieved June 8, 2009, from www.censogls.com.br
Kimmel, M. S. (1997). Masculinity as homophobia: Fear, shame, and silence in
the construction of gender identity. In M. M. Gergen & S. N. Davis (Eds.),
Toward a new psychology of gender (pp. 223242). New York: Routledge.

192

Mental and Physical Health

Kitzinger, C., & Wilkinson, S. (2004). Social advocacy for equal marriage: The
politics of rights and the psychology of mental health. Analyses of
Social Issues and Public Policy, 4, 173194.
Klein, C. (1999). The ghetto is over, darling: Emerging gay communities and
gender and sexual politics in contemporary Brazil. Culture, Health, &
Sexuality, 1, 239260.
Lacerda, M., Pereira, C., & Camino, L. (2002). Um estudo sobre as formas de
~es sociais
preconceito contra homossexuais na perspectiva das representaco
[A study of prejudice forms against homosexuals anchored on social representations]. Psicologia: Reex~ao e Crtica, 15, 165178.
Larsen, R. J., & Buss, D. M. (2008). Personality psychology: Domains of knowledge
about human nature. New York: McGraw Hill.
Lawrence v. Texas, 71 U.S.L.W. 4574 (2003).
Leis [Laws]. (2002). Pride. Retrieved May 25, 2009, from http://mixbrasil.uol.
com.br/pride/direito/dh.htm
Low, K. S. D., Radhakrishnan, P., Schneider, K. T., & Rounds, J. (2007). The
experiences of bystanders of workplace ethnic harassment. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 37, 22612297.
Masser, B., & Abrams, D. (1999). Contemporary sexism: The relationships
among hostility, benevolence, and neosexism. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 23, 503517.
Mendes-Leite, R. (1993). A game of appearances: The ambigusexuality in
Brazilian culture of sexuality. Journal of Homosexuality, 25, 271282.
Mott, L. (2002). O crime anti-homossexual no Brasil [Crimes against homosexuals
in Brazil]. Salvador: Grupo Gay da Bahia.
National Gay and Lesbian Task Force. (2008, July 31). State nondiscrimination
laws in the U.S. Retrieved June 6, 2009, from http://www.thetaskforce.org/
reports_and_research/nondiscrimination_laws
Noviski, I. F., Davoli, J. C., & Castro, R. C. (2000). Assedio sexual: Quest~ao que
ultrapassa a categoria prossional do secretario executivo [Sexual harassment: A
question beyond the category of a superior]. Monograa de conclus~
ao de curso.
Universidade do Vale do Paraba. S~
ao Jose dos Campos, S~
ao Paulo.
O mundo gay rasga as fantasias [The gay world tears up the costumes]. (1993,
May 12). Veja, 5257.
Parada Gay agita S~
ao Paulo neste domingo [Gay Parade stirs S~
ao Paulo this
Sunday]. (2009, June 14). Retrieved June 16, 2009, from http://ultimosegundo.
ig.com.br/brasil/2009/06/14/paradagaydeverecebercercade35
milhoesdepessoasemsaopaulo6718909.html
Parker, R. (1991). Bodies, pleasures, and passions: Sexual culture in contemporary
Brazil. Boston: Beacon.
Parker, R. (1999). Beneath the equator: Cultures of desire, male homosexuality, and
emerging gay communities in Brazil. London: Routledge.
Parker, R. G. (1993). Within four walls: Brazilian sexual culture and HIV/
AIDS. In H. Daniel & R. Parker (Eds.), Sexuality, politics and AIDS in Brazil:
In another world? (pp. 6584). London: Falmer.
Pastore, J., & Robortella, L. C. A. (2003) Assedio sexual no trabalho [Sexual harassment at work]. S~
ao Paulo: Makron Books.
Prado, M. A. M., & Machado, F. V. (2008). Preconceito contra homossexualidades
[Prejudice against homosexualities]. S~
ao Paulo: Cortez.

Prejudice and Discrimination against Sexual Minorities

193

~o [Resolution] CFP # 001/99. (1999, March 22). Estabelecer normas de


Resoluca
atuac~
ao para os psic
ologos em relac~
ao 
a quest~
ao da orientac~
ao sexual
[Standards of performance for psychologists concerning sexual orientation].
Rios, R. R. (2002). A homossexualidade e a discriminac~
ao por orientac~
ao sexual
no direito Brasileiro [Homosexuality and discrimination due to sexual orientation in Brazilian law]. In C. Golin & L. G. Weiler (Eds.), Homossexualidades, cultura e poltica [Homosexuality, culture and politics] (pp. 1548).
Porto Alegre, Brazil: Sulina.
Rocha, K. B., Barbosa, L. H. R., Barboza, C. Z., Calvetti, P. U., Carvalho, F. T.,
Cerqueira-Santos, E., et al. (2009). Attention to health in Brazil based on
transvestites, transsexuals and transgenders perception. Unpublished
manuscript.
Sedgwick, E. K. (1990). Epistemology of the closet. Berkeley, CA: University of
California Press.
Silva, L., & Barbi, H. (2005, February 25). Seus direitos [Your rights]. Retrieved
May 25, 2009, from http://mixbrasil.uol.com.br/pride/seusdireitos/visto/
visto.asp
Starks, T. J., Gilbert, B. O., Fischer, A. R., Weston, R., & DiLalla, D. L. (2009).
Gendered sexuality: A new model and measure of attraction and intimacy.
Journal of Homosexuality, 56, 1430.
Werba, G. C., & Strey, M. N. (2001). Longe dos olhos, longe do corac~
ao: ainda
a invisibilidade da viol^encia contra a mulher [Out of sight, out of
mind [lit. heart]: The continued invisibility of violence against women]. In
P. K. Grossi & G. C. Werba (Eds.), Viol^encias e g^enero: Coisas que a gente n~ao
gostaria de saber [Violence and gender: Things we didnt want to know]
(pp. 7182). Porto Alegre, Brazil: Edipucrs, 2.
Whitam, F. L., Daskalos, C., Sobolewski, C. G., & Padilla, P. (1998). The emergence of lesbian sexuality and identity cross-culturally: Brazil, Peru, the
Philippines, and the United States. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 27, 3156.
Whitley, B. E., Jr. (2001). Gender-role variables and attitudes toward homosexuality. Sex Roles, 45, 691721.

This page intentionally left blank

Chapter 9

Frequency Rates and Consequences of


Peer Sexual Harassment: Comparing
U.S. and International Students1
Eros DeSouza
Joy Chien

Although sexual harassment is widespread on U.S. college campuses,


we know of no study that has examined the incidence of sexual harassment among international students. Thus, the purpose of the current
study described in this chapter was to ll an important gap in the literature by comparing frequency rates and psychological consequences of
peer sexual harassment between U.S.-born and international college
students. The ndings showed that female international students were
at the greatest risk of experiencing peer sexual harassment and were
the most bothered by these experiences. There was also a trend for sexually harassed international students, regardless of gender, to have
lower self-esteem compared to sexually harassed U.S.-born students.
These ndings have important practical implications for university
ofcials.

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE


Hill and Silva (2005) reported ndings from a recent national survey
conducted by the American Association of University Women in which
62 percent of U.S. male and female college students indicated having
been sexually harassed, most often by peers (80 percent); the most
common form of sexual harassment was unwanted comments, jokes,

196

Mental and Physical Health

gestures, and looks (53 percent). In addition, many scholars suggest


that sexual harassment is about power rather than sexual desire (e.g.,
Cleveland & Kerst, 1993; Fineran & Bennett, 1999; Jones, 1996; Sandler,
1997). Power may rest on the harassers gender, size, race, or any
number of other dominant characteristics the harasser possesses or is
perceived to possess by society (Strauss, 2003, p. 114).

DEFINING SEXUAL HARASSMENT


From a psychological perspective, sexual harassment is dened as
unwanted sex-related behavior at work that is appraised by the recipient as offensive, exceeding her resources, or threatening her wellbeing (Fitzgerald, Swan, & Magley, 1997, p. 15). Sexual harassment is,
then, a type of stressor that has deleterious effects on a targets health
and psychological well-being. Although we are interested in psychological sexual harassment, it is worthwhile noting that the denition of
sexual harassment varies cross culturally (DeSouza & Solberg, 2003)
and from one individual to another due to the subjective nature of sexual harassment (i.e., sexual harassment is in the eye of the beholder).
According to Paludi (1997), sexual harassment in educational settings can be dened in legal terms and in behavioral terms. As a legal
term, the U.S. Department of Education: Ofce for Civil Rights (2001)
dened sexual harassment as:
[u]nwelcome conduct of a sexual nature. Sexual harassment can include
unwelcome sexual advances, requests for sexual favors, and other verbal,
nonverbal, or physical conduct of a sexual nature. Sexual harassment of a
student can deny or limit, on the basis of sex, the students ability to participate in or to receive benets, services, or opportunities in the schools
program. Sexual harassment of student is, therefore, a form of sex discrimination prohibited by Title IX under circumstances described in this
guidance. (p. 2)

As a behavioral term, sexual harassment can also occur where no


such formal differential exists, if the behavior is unwanted by or offensive to the individual (Fitzgerald & Ormerod, 1993, p. 556).
Sexual harassment is illegal (Equal Employment Opportunity Commission, 2009; U.S. Department of Education, Ofce for Civil Rights,
2001). It can be classied into two categories: quid pro quo and hostile
environment. In quid pro quo sexual harassment, the harasser typically
has more organizational power than the target (Sandler & Shoop,
1997); that is, when the harasser is in a position to provide benets for
the target in exchange for sexual favors or to punish the target for not
cooperatingfor example, when a teacher gives an A grade to a student who does not deserve it but is open to sexual interactions, or

Frequency Rates and Consequences of Peer Sexual Harassment

197

when a teacher fails a student because he or she is not willing to participate in sexual behaviors.
The other type of sexual harassment is hostile environment, which is
dened as an environment being so hostile that it interferes with an
individuals ability to perform her or his tasks, such as attending
classes (Sandler & Shoop, 1997). Examples of behaviors that fall under
this category include, but are not limited to, sexual innuendos, sexual
bantering, unwanted touching, grabbing and pinching, sexual obscenities, computer harassment, exposure to pornographic materials, and
being asked out on a date repeatedly after having said no.
Often in situations of hostile environment, the harasser and the target are in an equal organizational position, such as both being
students. This form of hostile environment is called peer sexual harassment. However, only a few studies focused directly on peer sexual
harassment in educational settings (e.g., DeSouza & Ribeiro, 2005;
DeSouza, Schneider, & Hubbard, 2005; Goldstein, Malanchuk, DavisKean, & Eccles, 2007).
One of the reasons for the lack of attention on peer sexual harassment in educational settings is that many sexual behaviors that match
the peer sexual harassment denition have become normalized in society due to the cultures perception of gender roles. According to
Woods (2007), one aspect for men to be masculine in the United States
is to be sexual; in other words, men should be interested in sex all the
time. For example, it is common to nd a group of men judging womens appearance and body parts when women simply pass by, and it
would be naturally argued that judging women is a way of men
being men.
Peer sexual harassment occurs due to societal status bestowed upon
men (Fitzgerald & Ormerod, 1993). Thus, it is a form of gender-based
dominance and control (Fineran, Bennett, & Sacco, 2001), in which
young men show their masculinity by sexually coming on to women
(Woods, 2007). Fineran and Bennett (1999) reported that in the United
States, high school boys sexually harassed their peers more often than
high school girls did, and adolescents beliefs that men should be dominant were correlated with engaging in sexually harassing behaviors
toward peers.
Although Hill and Silvas study as well as other studies that use
their own behavioral checklists (e.g., American Association of University Women, 1993, 2001; Fineran & Bennet, 1999; Ivy & Hamlet, 1996)
are informative, these studies are largely atheoretical. They generally
use various denitions of sexual harassment and psychometric instruments, making generalizations problematic (Raver & Gelfand, 2005).
In the late 1980s, Fitzgerald et al. created a behavioral measure of
sexual harassment called the Sexual Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ),
which has become the most widely used and rigorous assessment of

198

Mental and Physical Health

sexual harassment with content, construct, and criterion validity (Arvey


& Cavanaugh, 1995; Fitzgerald, Gelfand, & Drasgow, 1995; Stark,
Chernyshenko, Lancaster, Drasgow, & Fitzgerald, 2002). Fitzgerald et
al. dened sexual harassment as a behavioral construct, comprising
three related but distinct dimensions: gender harassment (insulting, hostile, or degrading sexist behaviors), unwanted sexual attention (verbal or
nonverbal sexual behaviors that are offensive, unwanted, and unreciprocated), and sexual coercion (when better treatment or rewards are
contingent on sexual cooperation, either subtly or explicitly). Note that
the term sexual harassment is never mentioned in the SEQ.
Moreover, gender harassment and unwanted sexual attention parallel the legal denition of a hostile environment, whereas sexual coercion parallels the legal denition of quid pro quo. Research suggests that
gender harassment is the most common type of sexual harassment, followed by unwanted sexual attention, with sexual coercion being the
least common (Pryor & Fitzgerald, 2003).

DEMOGRAPHIC FACTORS RELATED TO THE


INCIDENCE OF SEXUAL HARASSMENT
Sexual harassers usually choose individuals who do not have the
power to reject behaviors that are unwelcome, degrading, offensive,
or intimidating, and that violate standards of interpersonal respect
(DeSouza, 2008). In addition, perpetrators of these acts are motivated
to maintain or enhance their social status (Berdahl, 2007).
Survey studies have shown that female workers are much more
likely to experience sexual harassment than male workers (e.g., U.S.
Merit Systems Protection Board, 1981, 1988, 1995). In educational settings, although male students can and do become targets of peer sexual
harassment, those who seem most vulnerable to be sexually harassed
by their peers are female students (Goldstein et al., 2007; Hand &
Sanchez, 2000), students of color (Goldstein et al., 2007), and effeminate male students. That is, women, sexual minorities, and racial/ethnic minorities have been traditionally viewed as being inferior in
compassion to heterosexual men and whites.
Factors such as gender, sexual orientation, race, and social economic
status reect an individuals power in society. Moreover, these factors
interface. For instance, sexism and racism have been historically
and experientially intertwined (Murrell, 1996). In fact, some researchers
concluded that racialized sexual harassment is a central factor in
the harassment experience of women of color (e.g., Berdahl &
Moore, 2006, 2005; Buchanan & Ormerod, 2002; Cortina, Fitzgerald, &
Drasgow, 2002; Schneider, Hitlan, & Radhakrishnan, 2000; Texeira,
2002; Yoder & Aniakudo, 1995).

Frequency Rates and Consequences of Peer Sexual Harassment

199

Concerning social economic status, DeSouza and Cerqueira recently


examined frequency rates, as assessed by the SEQ, and consequences
of sexual harassment in a sample of 376 poor women, who were
employed as domestic workers in Brazil. Ninety-eight percent of these
women earned about US $200 or less a month and 89 percent had less
than a high school education. Twenty-six percent of this sample was
sexually harassed at work during the past 12 months. Live-in workers
(e.g., those residing at their employers residence) were at a signicantly greater risk of experiencing sexual harassment than those who
resided in their own homes after controlling for participants age, race,
and social class. Women who lived in their employers residence used
more alcohol/drugs than their counterparts. Harassed women had signicantly higher self-esteem impairment and anxiety-depression than
nonharassed women. Nonharassed women who resided in their own
homes had the best physical well-being. When asked about ones worst
sexually harassing experience, respondents indicated that the perpetrators were typically men (75 percent), who also engaged in more severe
types of sexual harassment than female perpetrators. The emotional
reaction to such incidents was signicantly worse when perpetrated by
men than by women. Most sexually harassed domestic workers experienced some form of retaliation (social or work-related) after asking
their harassers to stop.
DeSouza and Cerqueira concluded that the low social and economic
status of domestic workers in Brazil might allow the perpetration of
sexual harassment and other forms of mistreatment with impunity.
That is, these workers may refuse to le a formal complaint with the
police due to fear of being ridiculed by ofcials and not nding a new
job, putting themselves and their families at risk of not surviving (e.g.,
not being able to pay bills).
Another factor that researchers have paid little attention to is ones
nationality/citizenship. Welsh, Carr, MacQuarrie, and Huntley (2006)
studied Filipina domestic female workers who emigrated to Canada
without full citizenship status. The authors argued that citizenship status was one of the major components that put these women at risk of
experiencing sexual harassment and the way they dened sexual harassment. The Filipina domestic workers in their study usually kept
their sexual harassment experiences unreported, because they were
afraid that if they complained about it, they would lose their right to
stay in Canada.
Xie, Meng, and Yamagami (1995) investigated the experiences of
international students from China who studied in Japan. The student
visa of a Chinese student requires sponsorship by a Japanese organization. These Chinese students did not report their experiences of sexual
assault to the police because they feared being deported. Welsh et al.
and Xie et al. concluded that individuals who do not have full

200

Mental and Physical Health

citizenship have less power in the host society. In addition, they are
fearful to report their experiences to authorities because they might
lose their legal right to stay in the host country.
According to Sandler, female international college students are more
likely to be harassed than college students born in the United States,
because the former are objectied as exotic and/or passive and, as
suggested above, they have less power in society due to their gender
and citizenship status. Unfortunately, none of the previous studies on
peer sexual harassment or any other form of sexual harassment have
investigated the experiences of international students enrolled in U.S.
colleges and universities. This is troublesome because 623,805 international students studied in the United States during the 2007/2008
academic year, accounting for 3.5 percent of the student body (Institute of International Education, n.d.). Thus, it is important to investigate the experiences of international students with peer sexual
harassment.
Moreover, targets of peer sexual harassment may end up in selfdoubt, affecting their entire education experience and future career, as
they may become afraid of attending classes or joining social events in
order to avoid the harasser and may end up transferring to another
institution or dropping out of college altogether (Sandler, 1997). Furthermore, sexual harassment is a stressor that can have negative psychological outcomes on the targets (e.g., DeSouza & Cerqueira, in
press; Willness, Steel, & Lee, 2007). In addition, even relatively mild
experiences with sexual harassment may lead to negative consequences
in ones physical and psychological health (e.g., DeSouza & Cerqueira,
in press; DeSouza & Fansler, 2003; Goldstein et al., 2007).

THE CURRENT STUDY


The purpose of the current study was to examine frequency rates
and psychological consequences of peer sexual harassment in a sample
of international and U.S.-born students enrolled in a large midwestern
state university. We examined whether being female and an international student moderated both the frequency of peer sexual harassment
and its consequences on the victims psychological well-being. Specically, we predicted that female international students would experience
the most peer sexual harassment and would be most bothered by it
because they have the lowest power in society due to their gender and
citizenship status compared to both male and female students from the
United States and international male students. We also predicted that
female international students would fare worst psychologically (i.e.,
lower self-esteem and more anxiety-depression) compared to the other
groups.

Frequency Rates and Consequences of Peer Sexual Harassment

201

All international students (n 429) enrolled in a large midwestern


state university and 426 randomly selected U.S.-born counterparts, who
were matched by age and gender, received an e-mail in the fall of 2007
asking them to participate in a Web survey about social-sexual behaviors between college students. Of these, 165 college students (69 international students and 96 U.S.-born students) completed the Web
survey, yielding a response rate of 19 percent.
The age of the total sample (N 165) ranged from 19 to 37, with
the average age being 26; 55 percent were women and 61 percent
were graduate students. Most U.S.-born students were European
American/white (88 percent), whereas most international students
were Asian (66 percent), followed by white (25 percent), black (6 percent), and Latino (3 percent). The vast majority of international students (90 percent) reported that English was their second language.
Also, most international students (64 percent) reported to be studying
in the United States for two years or less, with 36 percent studying in
the United States for three years or more. All students completed the
following measures in the order presented below. Items about psychological well-being preceded all sexual harassment items to minimize
response bias.
DEMOGRAPHIC AND PERSONAL INFORMATION
We asked all students their age, gender, race/ethnicity, student status (undergraduate or graduate student), and whether or not they were
an international student. If they were an international student, they
were asked whether or not English was their native language and how
long they have been in the United States.
PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING
Students completed 15 items that assessed their psychological wellbeing. First, they answered ve items from the Mental Health Index
(MHI), developed by Veit and Ware (1983), that measured anxietydepression during the 12 months. These items are scored on a 6-point
scale from 1 (Never) to 6 (All the time). We reversed the scores of two
positively worded items. Then, we averaged all responses, so that
higher scores represent higher self-reported levels of anxiety and
depression. A sample item reads, I have felt downhearted and blue.
In the current study the internal reliability (Cronbachs alpha) of the
MHI was .83, which indicates that it is a reliable measure of anxietydepression.
Then, students completed the 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale
(RSES; 1986), which is scored on a 4-point scale from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 4 (Strongly agree). A sample item reads, I certainly feel useless

202

Mental and Physical Health

at times. The RSES is a widely used measure of global self-esteem,


with sound construct validity and psychometric properties (Robins,
Hendin, & Trzesniewski, 2001). The Cronbachs alphas for the RSES
across various samples ranged from .77 to .88 (Blascovich & Tomaka,
1993; Rosenberg, 1986). According to Rosenbergs recommendation,
the scores of ve negatively worded statements were reversed. Then,
all scores were averaged, so that higher scores represent higher
reported levels of self-esteem. In the current study the internal reliability (Cronbachs alpha) of the RSES was .87, which indicates that it is a
reliable measure of self-esteem.

Student-to-Student Hostile Sexual Harassment


Students completed a shortened version of the SEQ (Stark et al.,
2002), containing 12 behaviors initiated by either male or female college
students during the past 12 months that may constitute hostile sexually
harassing experiences (a sample item includes, told sexual stories or
jokes that were offensive to you). That is, since hostile sexually harassing behaviors are far more common than coercive ones (Pryor &
Fitzgerald, 2003), and since we were interested in student-to-student
sexual harassment, we eliminated four items that measure sexual coercion (e.g., implied certain rewards or better treatment if you were sexually cooperative), as these behaviors would be unlikely to be
initiated by students to other students. As stated earlier, the validity
of the SEQ has been well established (Arvey & Cavanaugh, 1995;
Fitzgerald et al., 1995). Stark et al. (2002) reported reliability estimates
(Cronbachs alphas) of .91 men and .92 for women for the 16-item SEQ.
In the current study, each of the 12 SEQ items branches out into two
response sets. The rst set measures incident frequency, whereas the
second set measures the emotional reaction to the experience (i.e., how
bothersome it was if it happened at least once). That is, for each item,
students rst indicated how often it occurred on the following 5-point
scale: 1 (Never), 2 (Once or twice), 3 (Sometimes), 4 (Often), and 5 (Many
times). We averaged these responses, so that higher scores represent
higher reported frequency of sexually harassing experiences. The Cronbachs alpha for the frequency measure was .86, which is comparable
to the 16-item SEQ described above.
Next, if students had experienced a sexually harassing behavior at
least once, they were asked how bothersome it was on the following
5-point scale: 1 (Not at all), 2 (Slightly), 3 (Somewhat), 4 (A lot), and
5 (Extremely). We averaged these responses, so that higher scores represent higher reported levels of feeling bothered by such incidents. The
Cronbachs alpha for the bothersome measure was .81, which indicates
good reliability.

Frequency Rates and Consequences of Peer Sexual Harassment

203

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION


Eighty-six percent of the total sample reported having experienced
at least one sexually harassing behavior from peers during the past
12 months. This gure suggests that peer sexual harassment is widespread, supporting Hill and Silvas ndings.
The data generally supported our predictions. First, female students
(M 1.78) experienced signicant more peer sexual harassment than
male students (M 1.50; F(1, 151) 11.74, p < .01. These ndings are
in keeping with past studies, which found that female students were
much more likely to be targets of sexual harassment than were male
students (e.g., Goldstein et al., 2007; Hand & Sanchez, 2000).
Second, female students (M 1.81) were more bothered by these
experiences than were male students (M 1.48; F(1, 151) 14.06, p <
.01. These ndings are also in keeping with past studies, which found
that men perceived sexually harassing experiences as less severe, less
upsetting, less inappropriate, less bothersome, and less threatening than
did women (Berdahl, Magley, & Waldo, 1996; Cochran, Frazier, & Olson,
1997; Hurt, Maver, & Hoffman, 1999; LaRocca & Kromrey, 1999; Marks
& Nelson, 1993). Recent studies also found that female college students
were much more likely to perceive social-sexual behaviors between students as constituting peer sexual harassment than did male college men
(DeSouza & Solberg, 2004; DeSouza, Solberg, & Elder, 2007).
Although the main effect for citizenship was not signicant for
either the frequency or bothersome measure, we found signicant
interactions between gender and citizenship for both the frequency
measure, F(1, 151) 4.12, p < .05, and the bothersome measure,
F(1, 151) 3.93, p < .05. These interactions were in the expected direction, which supported our predictions. That is, female international
students (M 1.80) experienced the most peer sexual harassment and
male international students the least (M 1.25), with female U.S. students (M 1 .75) and male U.S. students (M 1.61) scoring in
between. A similar and signicant pattern was found for the bothersome measure: Female international students (M 1.83) were most
bothered by these experiences and male international students the
least (M 1.24), with female U.S. students (M 1.78) and male U.S.
students (M 1.60) scoring in between.
We did not nd a signicant interaction for either the self-esteem or
anxiety-depression measure. However, there was a trend for harassed
international students (M 3.19) to score lower on self-esteem than
did harassed U.S.-born students (M 3.37), F(1, 134) 2.93, p .089.
In addition, harassed women (M 2.61) scored signicantly higher on
anxiety-depression than did harassed men (M 2.33), F(1, 134) 5.16,
p < .05. The latter is in keeping with past research (e.g., DeSouza &
Fansler, 2003).

204

Mental and Physical Health

CONCLUSION
The current study advances our understanding of sexual harassment
by comparing its incidence and psychological consequences between
U.S.-born students and international student. Female international students experienced the most peer sexual harassment and were the ones
most bothered by these experiences, which seem to add to their acculturation stress in a new culture (Wadsworth, Hecht, & Jung, 2008).
Harassed international college students, regardless of gender, may
even blame themselves for their victimization. Thus, their lower selfesteem, compared to harassed U.S.-born college students, evidenced in
our study is easy to understand, because being sexually harassed
implies low status, as noncitizens, and a sense of powerlessness, as
they may fear losing their student visa if they complain to university
ofcials and returning home in shame.
The above ndings have important implications for university ofcials who are in a position to prevent sexual harassment in all its
forms. According to Hill and Silva, [t]he ramications of sexual harassment can be serious. Sexual harassment can damage the emotional
and academic well-being of students, provoke and exacerbate conict
among students, and contribute to a hostile learning environment . . .
society as a whole is affected as graduating students bring their attitudes about sexual harassment into the workplace and beyond (p. 4).
University ofcials have a duty to protect international students
from sexual harassment. International students may need extra help
understanding how sexual harassment is dened in the United States
and what can be done to redress the situation. Residence hall assistants
(RAs) are often the rst resource for international students residing on
campus. Thus, RAs may need additional training about how international students may have a different conceptualization of sexual harassment. For example, Brazilian laws dene sexual harassment only as
sexual coercion by superiors, that is, quid pro quo sexual harassment
(C
odigo Penal Brasileiro, 2001).

Limitations and Future Directions


One important limitation of our study is that our sample was from
one midwestern university, limiting the generalizability of our ndings. Another limitation is that our study was cross-sectional rather
than longitudinal, which precludes an examination of the consequences
of sexually harassing experiences over time. Thus, we cannot infer causality from our correlational study. Hence, future studies need to
include longitudinal designs to test the causality of the relationships
found in our study. Future research should also include qualitative
approaches, such as in-depth interviews and focus groups, in order to

Frequency Rates and Consequences of Peer Sexual Harassment

205

contextualize the sexual harassment experiences of international students. Researchers (e.g., Lim & Cortina, 2005) suggest that sexual harassment does not happen in isolation, but rather in an environment
permeated by generalized hostility. Hence, other types of mistreatment
(e.g., bullying and incivility) should be simultaneously investigated in
future studies.
NOTE
1. Preliminary ndings were presented at 2008 meeting of the International
Coalition Against Sexual Harassment and at the Association for Psychological
Science.

REFERENCES
American Association of University Women. (1993). Hostile hallways: The
AAUW survey on sexual harassment in Americas schools. Washington, DC:
Harris/Scholastic Research.
American Association of University Women. (2001). Hostile hallways: Bullying,
teasing, and sexual harassment in school. Washington, DC: Author.
Arvey, R. D., & Cavanaugh, M. A. (1995). Using surveys to assess the prevalence of sexual harassment: Some methodological problems. Journal of
Social Issues, 51, 3952.
Berdahl, J. L. (2007). Harassment based on sex: Protecting social status in the
context of gender hierarchy. Academy of Management Review, 32, 641658.
Berdahl, J. L., Magley, V. J., & Waldo, C. R. (1996). The sexual harassment of
men? Exploring the concept with theory and data. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 20, 527547.
Berdahl, J. L., & Moore, C. (2006). Workplace harassment: Double jeopardy for
minority women. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 426436.
Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1993). Measures of self-esteem. In J. P. Robinson,
P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social
psychological attitudes (3rd ed., pp. 115160). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute of
Social Research.
Buchanan, N. T. (2005). The nexus of race and gender domination: Racialized
sexual harassment of African American women. In J. E. Gruber &
P. Morgan (Eds.), In the company of men: Male dominance and sexual harassment (pp. 294320). Boston: Northeastern University Press.
Buchanan, N. T., & Ormerod, A. J. (2002). Racialized sexual harassment in the
lives of African American women. Women & Therapy, 25, 107124.
Cleveland, J. N., & Kerst, M. E. (1993). Sexual harassment and perceptions of
power: An underarticulated relationship. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 42,
4967.
Cochran, C. C., Frazier, P. A., & Olson, A. M. (1997). Predictors of responses to
unwanted sexual attention. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 207226.
C
odigo Penal Brasileiro [Brazilian Penal Code]. (2001). Lei 10.224, Art. 216-A.
Braslia.

206

Mental and Physical Health

Cortina, L. M, Fitzgerald, L. F., & Drasgow, F. (2002). Contextualizing Latina


experiences of sexual harassment: Preliminary tests of a structural model.
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 24, 295311.
DeSouza, E., & Fansler, A. G. (2003). Contrapower sexual harassment: A survey
of students and faculty members. Sex Roles, 48, 529542.
DeSouza, E., Schneider, K. T., & Hubbard, C. R. (2005, August). Sexual harassment
among high school students of color. In A. J. Ormerod & L. L. Collinsworth
(Chairs), New issues in high school sexual harassment. Symposium presented at
the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington,
DC.
DeSouza, E., & Solberg J. (2003). Incidence and dimensions of sexual harassment across cultures. In M. Paludi & C. A. Paludi, Jr. (Ed.), Academic and
workplace sexual harassment (pp. 330). Westport, CT: Praeger.
DeSouza, E. R. (2008). Workplace incivility, sexual harassment, and racial
micro-aggression: The interface of three literatures. In M. Paludi (Ed.), The
psychology of women at work: Vol 2. Obstacles and the identity juggle (pp. 65
84). Westport, CT: Praeger.
DeSouza, E. R., & Cerqueira, E. (in press). From the kitchen to the bedroom:
Frequency rates and consequences of sexual harassment among Brazilian
domestic workers. Journal of Interpersonal Violence.
DeSouza, E. R., & Ribeiro, J. (2005). Bullying and sexual harassment among
Brazilian high school students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20, 10181038.
DeSouza, E. R., & Solberg, J. (2004). Womens and mens reactions to man-toman sexual harassment: Does the sexual orientation of the victim matter?
Sex Roles, 50, 623639.
DeSouza, E. R., Solberg, J., & Elder, C. (2007). A cross-cultural perspective on
judgments of woman-to-woman sexual harassment: Does sexual orientation matter? Sex Roles, 56, 457471.
Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (2009, March 11). Sexual harassment. Retrieved May 12, 2009, from http://www.eeoc.gov/types/sexual_
harassment.html
Fineran, S., & Bennett, L. (1999). Gender and power issues of peer sexual
harassment among teenagers. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 626641.
Fineran, S., Bennett, L., & Sacco, T. (2001). Peer sexual harassment and peer
violence: South African children at risk. Social Work, 37, 211221.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Gelfand, M. J., & Drasgow, F. (1995). Measuring sexual harassment: Theoretical and psychometric advances. Basic and Applied Social
Psychology, 17, 425445.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Shullman, S., Bailey, N., Richards, M., Swecker, J., Gold, Y.,
et al. (1988). The incidence and dimensions of sexual harassment in
academia and the workplace. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 32, 152175.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Swan, S., & Magley, V. J. (1997). But was it really sexual
harassment? Legal, behavioral, and psychological denitions of the workplace victimization of women. In W. ODonohue (Ed.), Sexual harassment:
Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 528). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Fitzgerald, L. F., & Ormerod, A. (1993). Sexual harassment in academia and the
workplace (pp. 553581) In F. L. Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: Handbook of issues and theories. Westport, CT: Greenwood
Press.

Frequency Rates and Consequences of Peer Sexual Harassment

207

Goldstein, S. E., Malanchuk, O., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2007). Risk
factors of sexual harassment by peers: A longitudinal investigation of
African American and European American adolescents. Journal of Research
on Adolescence, 17, 285300.
Hand, J. Z., & Sanchez, L. (2000). Badgering or bantering? Gender differences
in experience of, and reactions to, sexual harassment among U.S. high
school students. Gender & Society, 14, 718746.
Hill, C., & Silva, E. (2005). Drawing the line: Sexual harassment on campus.
Washington, DC: American Association of University Women.
Hurt, J. L., Maver, J. A., & Hoffman, D. (1999). Situational and individual
inuences on judgments of hostile environment sexual harassment. Journal
of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 13951415.
Institute of International Education (n.d.). International students. Retrieved May
12, 2009, from http://opendoors.iienetwork.org/?p=131531
Ivy, D. K., & Hamlet, S. (1996). College students and sexual dynamics: Two
studies of peer sexual harassment. Communication Education, 45, 149166.
Jones, C. (1996). Sexual harassment. New York: Facts On File, Inc.
LaRocca, M. A., & Kromrey, J. D. (1999). Perception of sexual harassment in higher
education: Impact of gender and attractiveness. Sex Roles, 40, 921940.
Lim, S., & Cortina, L. M. (2005). Interpersonal mistreatment in the workplace:
The interface and impact of general incivility and sexual harassment.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 483496.
Marks, M. A., & Nelson, E. S. (1993). Sexual harassment on campus: Effects of
professor gender on perception of sexually harassing behaviors. Sex Roles,
28, 207217.
Murrell, A. J. (1996). Sexual harassment and women of color: Issues, challenges,
and future directions. In M. S. Stockdale (Ed.), Sexual harassment in the
workplace: Perspectives, frontiers, and response strategies (pp. 5165). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Paludi, M. A. (1997). Sexual harassment in schools. In W. ODonohue (Ed.),
Sexual harassment: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 225240). Needham
Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Pryor, J. B., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2003). Sexual harassment research in the United
States. In S. Einarsen, H. Hoel, D. Zapf, & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Bullying and
emotional abuse in the workplace: International perspectives in research and practice (pp. 79100). New York: Taylor & Francis.
Raver, J. L., & Gelfand, M. (2005). Beyond the individual victim: Linking sexual
harassment, team processes, and team performance. Academy of Management Journal, 48, 387400.
Robins, R. W., Hendin, H. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2001). Measuring global
self-esteem: Construct validation of a single-item measure and the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 151161.
Rosenberg, M. (1986). Conceiving the self. Malabar, FL: Krieger.
Sandler, B. R. (1997). Student-to-student sexual harassment. In B. R. Sandler &
R. J. Shoop (Ed.), Sexual harassment on campus (pp. 5065). Needham
Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Sandler, B. R., & Shoop, R. J. (1997). What is sexual harassment? In B. R.
Sandler & R. J. Shoop (Ed.), Sexual harassment on campus (pp. 121). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

208

Mental and Physical Health

Schneider, K. T., Hitlan, R. T., & Radhakrishnan, P. (2000). An examination of


the nature and correlates of ethnic harassment experiences in multiple
contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 312.
Stark, S., Chernyshenko, O. S., Lancaster, A. R., Drasgow, F., & Fitzgerald, L. F.
(2002). Toward standardized measurement of sexual harassment: Shortening the SEQ-DoD using item response theory. Military Psychology, 14, 4972.
Strauss, S. (2003). Sexual harassment in K12. In M. Paludi & C. A. Paludi, Jr.
(Eds.), Academic and workplace sexual harassment: A handbook of cultural, social
science, management, and legal perspectives (pp. 105145). Westport, CT:
Praeger.
Texeira, M. T. (2002). Who protects and serves me? A case study of sexual harassment in one U.S. law enforcement agency. Gender & Society, 16, 524545.
U.S. Department of Education: Ofce for Civil Rights. (2001, January 19).
Revised sexual harassment guidance: Harassment of students by school employees,
other students, or third parties. Retrieved May 12, 2009, from http://
www.ed.gov/about/ofces/list/ocr/docs/shguide.html#Guidance
U.S. Merit Systems Protection Board. (1981). Sexual harassment in the Federal
workplace: Is it a problem? Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing
Ofce.
U.S. Merit Systems Protection Board. (1988). Sexual harassment in the Federal
Government: An update. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Ofce.
U.S. Merit Systems Protection Board. (1995). Sexual harassment in the Federal
Government: Trends, progress, continuing challenges. Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Ofce.
Veit, C. T., & Ware, J. E., Jr. (1983). The structure of psychological distress and
well-being in general populations. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 51, 730742.
Wadsworth, B. C., Hecht, M. L., & Jung, E. (2008). The role of identity gaps,
discrimination, and acculturation in international students educational
satisfaction in American classrooms. Communication Education, 57, 6487.
Welsh, S., Carr, J., MacQuarrie, B., & Huntley, A. (2006). Im not thinking of it
as sexual harassment: Understanding harassment across race and citizenship. Gender & Society, 20, 87107.
Willness, C. R., Steel, P., & Lee, K. (2007). A meta-analysis of the antecedents
and consequences of workplace sexual harassment. Personnel Psychology,
60, 127162.
Woods, J. T. (2007). Gendered lives: Communication, gender, and culture. Belmont,
CA: Thomson Wadsworth.
Xie, L., Meng X., & Yamagami A. (1995). Chinese student victims of sexual
assault in Japan. International Medical Journal, 2, 214217.
Yoder, J. D., & Aniakudo, P. (1995). The responses of African American women
reghters to gender harassment at work. Sex Roles, 32, 125137.

Chapter 10

In Womens Voices
Janet Boyce

When I began writing this, I had to take a step back and think about
what feminism really means to me. I then began to ask female friends
and family members what feminism means to them. Surprisingly, I
dont think that any two women had the same response. However,
there was one predominant underlying factor, and that was the right
to be given equal treatment. Women just want to be treated as equal to
their male counterparts. Although women have made great strides over
the last 50 years, there is still a ways to go before women are viewed
as equals to men.
Traditionally, women were expected to get married, have a family,
and stay home to take care of them. This changed, in a sense, when
women went to work outside of the home during wartime and began
to experience the freedom associated with earning a paycheck. Many
of these women were then replaced by men returning from war. It was
at this time that women noticed the inequalities in the workplace both
from an economic and social standpoint.
As the baby boomers came of age in the 1960s, many with college
educations did not accept the economic and social inequalities being
imposed on them. These are the women that paved the way for the
generations to come. These women mirrored the way activists were
able to lobby for equality with the Civil Rights Act of 1964 legally prohibiting race discrimination and gender discrimination. This was later
amended by Title IX, which prohibited discrimination in education and
athletics. Although legislation made it illegal to discriminate against a
person based on their gender, the traditional biases were still there in
the workplace and in education.

210

Mental and Physical Health

As a woman born in the early 1960s, my own experiences in school


were proof of these lingering biases. One example of this was when I
entered my senior year of high school and met with my guidance
counselor to talk about college. I remember him asking me whether
I had thought about colleges and what I wanted to do after graduation.
I told him that I wanted to go to Penn State and major in accounting. I
will never forget his response, and perhaps it is still an underlying factor in what drives me today. He told me that accounting is not a womans eld, and that I should consider teaching or attending the local
community college to learn to be a secretary. I had never felt as
dejected in my life as when I walked out of his ofce that day. I am
sorry to say that I believed him and began to pursue a teaching degree.
I hated it, so I left school and did what was expectedgot married
and raised a family. I did return to school more than 20 years later to
pursue a degree in a eld of my choice.
Because of the courageous women who became activists and fought
for all women, I am glad that my daughter never had to feel that same
dejection as I did. I am thankful to say that my daughters guidance
counselor did everything he could to help her get into the college of
her choice and pursue the degree of her choice.
Although women have a come along way through the years, they still
have barriers to overcome in the workplace. One such barrier is that of
equal paywhere any two people doing the same job should earn the
same wage regardless of their gender. According to the Department of
Labor (2003), as of 2002, women earn, on average, 78 percent of the salary that their male counterparts do for the same job. This means that
white women earn on average 78 cents for every dollar a man makes,
while women of other races earn signicantly less than that. Although
the wage gap is closing, there is still signicant ground to cover.
Because women still tend to be the primary caregivers in the family
unit, if their workplaces offered more family-friendly programs and
policies, the gap could be offset. Many women today are faced with
the challenge of taking care of aging parents and their own children.
Many women in this position would opt to take advantage of a program like this in exchange for a lower wage.
In conclusion, I am a realist and know that my daughter will probably still suffer from inequalities in the workplace. I do believe that the
millennial generation will continue to push forward and equality will
be attained. I am optimistic that by the time my granddaughter comes
of age the playing eld will be more level in the workplace.
REFERENCE
Department of Labor. (2003, October 16). Womens earnings 78 percent of
mens in 2002. Retrieved June 12, 2009, from: http://www.bls.gov/opub/
ted/2003/oct/wk2/art03.htm

Chapter 11

Bullying and Sexual Harassment


of Adolescents
James Gruber
Susan Fineran

Interpersonal violence, whether dened as sexual harassment or bullying, frequently occurs in schools and is both experienced and observed
by students and school personnel alike (Stein, 1999). Sexual harassment
is dened as discrimination and is illegal, while bullying, although
abhorrent, is not considered discriminatory. However, because sexual
harassment and bullying have emerged from two different paradigms
(employment law and criminal law), exploration of their impact on students has been very different.

SEXUAL HARASSMENT AND BULLYING: DEFINITIONS


AND DIFFERENCES
Studies of bullying and sexual harassment have two different historical and theoretical roots. The problem of bullying was brought to
international attention most prominently by Dan Olweus, a Norwegian
researcher in the 1970s. Nansel et al. (2001) dened bullying in a manner that reects the perspective developed and subsequently modied
over the years by Olweus: Aggression that is intentionally harmful,
that is repeated, and centers on an imbalance of power. This asymmetry of power may be physical or psychological, and the aggressive
behavior may be verbal (e.g. name calling, threats), physical (e.g., hitting), or psychological (e.g. rumors, shunning/exclusion) (p., 2094).
School bullies have been described as children who use physical or

212

Mental and Physical Health

relational aggression in a systematic and calculated way with a group


of weaker peers (Crick, 1996; Olweus, 1993; Pellegrini, 2001; Schwartz,
Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1997).
Formal theories and denitions of sexual harassment originated in
the United States in the 1970s with the work of Catharine MacKinnon,
who dened it as a form of sex discrimination (1979). While bullying
has often been characterized as inappropriate or aggressive interpersonal behavior (between individuals or between a group and an individual (mobbing), sexual harassment was originally dened as behavior
by males who used organizational power or cultural privilege to coerce
sexual favors from women (quid quo pro). This initial formulation
expanded both theoretically and legally over the decades to include
gender- or sexually focused behaviors by men that made it exceedingly
difcult for women to work (hostile environment), and more recently,
same-sex harassment involving the use of sexual threats, taunts, or
attacks (e.g., Oncale v. Sundowners, 1998). The denition of sexual harassment by the U.S. Department of Education parallels that of other
state and local governments and government agencies insofar as it
includes both unwelcome interpersonal behavior (e.g., date pressures,
sexual contact) that is sufciently severe, persistent, or pervasive . . .
and hostile environment (1997, p. 12,038).
While bullying focuses on aggressive behavior that occurs between
individuals or by a group against an individual, it does not acknowledge differences in gender or sexuality as signicant factors in perpetration or victimization. Bullying research tends to focus on situational
and interpersonal dynamics. Sexual harassment, in contrast, is theoretically linked to hegemonic masculinity and consequently focused on
structural and culturally sanctioned roles and meanings (masculinefeminine, heterosexual-homosexual) that are key aspects of social stratication. The term sexual bullying has surfaced recently and has
muddled the denition of both sexual harassment and bullying. Some
researchers (Craig, Pepler, Connolly, & Henderson, 2001; Pellegrini,
2001) view school sexual harassment as an adolescent form of bullying.
This is an unfortunate development for two reasons. First, bullying is
constructed as an interpersonal problem, while sexual harassment is a
legal issue. Consequently, many students and their parents who perceive sexual harassment as a form of bullying may not exercise their
rights to have schools address the issue as they are legally mandated to
do. Also, when sexually-based experiences are viewed as bullying and
not identied specically as sexual harassment, problems of victimization that stem from gender or sexuality may be interpreted as private
or interpersonal troubles experienced by unfortunate students who are
caught up in difcult situations. The fact that most bullies are male,
that girls experience more harm than boys from sexual harassment,
and that homophobic comments are used routinely (mostly by boys) to

Bullying and Sexual Harassment of Adolescents

213

humiliate and control others (primarily other boys) misses the point
about the power of culturally-based stereotypes (AAUW, 2001; McGuffey & Rich, 1999; Kimmel & Mahler, 2003). Two recent studies by Jessie
Klein (2006a, 2006b) have highlighted the role that gender and sexual
stereotyping played in school shootingsa fact that has escaped public
scrutiny because of a focus on bullying: Most of the school shooters
targeted girls primarily; and the shooters, who fell well outside the
range of acceptable masculine body types, had weathered attacks on
their masculinity, including homophobic taunts, for monthsand in
some cases, years.
PREVALENCE OF SEXUAL HARASSMENT AND
BULLYING IN U.S. SCHOOLS
Four national studies of student victimizationtwo on sexual harassment and two on bullyingprovide the broadest statistics regarding
these behaviors in U.S. schools in the last two decades. The earliest
study on bullying in the United States, conducted in 1998 by the
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (Nansel
et al., 2001) that used Olweuss theoretical construction found that onethird of early adolescent children were directly involved in bullying,
with 10 percent as bullies, 13 percent as victims, and 6 percent as
bully-victims. The U.S. Department of Education School Crime Supplement to the National Crime Victimization Survey (2003) reported that
in 2001, 8 percent of middle and high school students were bullied
(9 percent males, 7 percent females). The American Association of University Women (AAUW) conducted both national studies on sexual
harassment, the rst in 1993 and the second in 2001. The two studies
showed similar results: 81 percent of students experienced some form
of sexual harassment during their school years. Fifty-nine percent of
students were harassed occasionally and 27 percent were targeted often
(AAUW, 2001). In addition, 54 percent of students said they sexually
harassed someone during their school years. One interesting contrast
with bullying outcomes, which tend to decrease with age and grade
level was that sexual harassment frequency increases with grade level:
55 percent of eighth and ninth graders and 61 percent of tenth and
eleventh graders reported that they had been physically sexually harassed at school (Hand & Sanchez, 2000).
When these four national studies are compared, it appears that sexual harassment is a national disaster that affects nearly all school children. The statistical contrast is stark: Over 80 percent of students said
they had experienced sexual harassment, while fewer than one of six
reported being bullied. Even more interesting, more than half of students said they perpetrated sexual harassment, while only 10 percent
reported bullying others; and students who reported being both

214

Mental and Physical Health

harasser and harassed constituted 55 percent (AAUW, 1993) while


those who were bullied or who bullied others were about a quarter of
that gure (Nansel et al., 2001). At rst glance, then, it appears that
sexual harassment is much more common than bullying. But signicant
differences in measurement are largely responsible for these discrepancies. Because the issues of bullying and sexual harassment emerged
from different paradigms (bullying as a criminal offense and sexual
harassment as illegal discrimination), they have evolved with different
methodologies. Research conducted on peer sexual harassment in
schools uses a behavior scale that asks students to indicate the frequency of each of 14 different sexually harassing behaviors they experienced (AAUW, 1993, 2001). In contrast, bullying research inspired by
Olweus has for the most part used only one or two questionnaire items
which contained the word bullying along with a denition (e.g.,
Elsea, Menesini, Moore, & Morita, 2003; Nansel et al., 2001; Olweus,
1993; Williams, Connolly, Pepler, & Craig, 2005). It is likely that asking
a single question (e.g., Have you ever been bullied?) that allows the respondent to decide whether the behaviors they experienced were bullying produces a signicant underreporting of experiences that might
otherwise be construed as bullying. Sexual harassment research is instructive in this regard. The use of multiple survey items (checklists)
has been standard protocol since the mid-1980s and has allowed
researchers to tap different dimensions of sexual harassment (e.g.,
Fitzgerald and her colleagues identied three forms of sexual harassmentgender harassment, unwanted sexual attention, and sexual coercion). It was discovered early in research endeavors that using the
words sexual harassment resulted in signicantly lowered estimates
of experiences that could otherwise be deemed sexual harassment
according to theoretical or legal denitions (Fitzgerald & Shullman,
1993; Gruber, 1990). In addition, most harassment surveys since the
1980s have asked respondents if they experienced uninvited and
unwanted sexual attention with no mention of the term, sexual harassment (e.g., United States Merit System Protection Board [USMSPB],
1988, 1995). In line with that practice, the AAUW surveys ask adolescents How often has anyone done the following [14] things to you
when you did not want them to?
Compounding the measurement problem, researchers have also
used different time frames. Some researchers inquired whether
respondents experiences occurred during an entire school year, while
others asked about behaviors that occurred during a short time span
(e.g., past week or month. The AAUW (1993, 2001) surveys inquired
whether students had ever had one or more sexual harassment experiences during their school years, while the National Crime Victimization Survey asked about bullying experiences that occurred during
the last 6 months. In some victimization studies different time frames

Bullying and Sexual Harassment of Adolescents

215

were used even when both phenomena were studied together.


DeSouza and Ribeiros (2005) used last 30 days for bullying and last
12 months for sexual harassment. Similarly, Williams et al. (2005)
framed bullying as experiences that occurred during the last 2 months
and sexual harassment as during the last 6 months. Another study
comparing bullying and sexual harassment behaviors conducted by Holt
and Espelage (2007), utilized AAUW (2001) questions with a one-year
time frame (during the last twelve months) and then included a 30day time frame (last 30 days) for their own bullying scale items.
Perhaps not surprisingly, their results showed that 70 percent of the students had been harassed while a little more than half (54 percent) had
been bullied. Based on this measurement conundrum, it would seem
that sexual harassment is rampant in schools compared to bullying.
In an attempt to address these measurement issues, Gruber and
Fineran (2008a) conducted a study comparing bullying and sexual harassment using experience checklists, one that asked students to indicate
the frequency of sexually harassing behaviors they experienced
(AAUW, 2001) and the second that inquired about the frequency of
bullying behaviors (Holt and Espelage, 2007). Using an identical time
frame and the same benchmark for determining whether or not victimization has occurred, bullying was revealed as a much more common
experience than sexual harassment. More than half (52 percent) of students had experienced bullying during the current school year while
approximately one-third (34 percent) were sexually harassed.
GLOBAL PREVALENCE OF SEXUAL HARASSMENT
AND BULLYING
Internationally, bullying and sexual harassment are shown to be
prevalent in many countries and reect a wide range of incidence: 15
percent to 77 percent for bullying and 18 percent to 80 percent for sexual harassment. The ensuing overview of research highlights, although
not exhaustive, shows the global prevalence of these issues and their
damaging impact on victims.
A 1999 study by Olweus found that 15 percent of 150,000 Norwegian
and Swedish students ages 7 to 16 were involved in bully-victim problems that occurred at least once a week (Olweus & Limber, 1999). A
much larger study of students in seven countries that used the same question format and same time frame designed by Olweus in each country
found incidence rates that parallel those of the United States that use the
same methodology: Bullying perpetration did not exceed 17 percent,
bully-victim incidences were less than 20 percent and bullying victimization rates were less than 25 percent (Elsea et al., 2003). Other European
studies nd somewhat higher incidence rates. In Great Britain, Stephensen and Smith (1989) estimated that 23 percent of children were involved

216

Mental and Physical Health

in bullying either as bullies or as victims, and OMoore and Hillery


(1989) found that 34 percent of Irish students were occasionally bullied
and 8 percent were bullied frequently. A South African study (Holan,
Flisher, & Lombard, 2007) that used the Olweus model found that 8.2
percent were bullies, 19.3 percent were victims, and 8.7 percent were
bully-victims.
Research that does not use the Olweus paradigm nds much higher
rates of bullying or peer aggression. A national study on bullying in
Israel found that 53 percent of elementary and middle school students
and 30 percent of high school students reported being seized and
shoved by schoolmates at least once during the previous month (Zeira,
Astor, & Benbenishty, 2003). Other bullying behavior described by students included having their personal belongings stolen. Specically, 45
percent of elementary and middle school students and 34 percent of
those in high school reported being victimized in this manner. The
study used a checklist of nineteen behaviors and asked students to
indicate the frequency and severity of these. In other parts of the
world, Brazilian and Japanese research on bullying report similar
statistics.
DeSouza & Ribeiros (2005) used a bullying scale developed by
Bosworth and found that 60 percent of Brazilian high school students
bullied a classmate during the past month. Kobayoshi (1999) reports
that 77 percent of Japanese sixth graders and 62 percent of eighth
graders were involved in bullying incidents, either as a bully or as a
victim of bullying behavior.
Similar to the prevalence of bullying world wide, sexual harassment
also appears to be a major social problem for students, and unfortunately is as widespread. Bullying has been researched much more
extensively than sexual harassment, but nonetheless there exists a number of studies showing that sexual harassment is rampant and affects
students negatively as well. Studies in Sweden, South Africa, Brazil,
and the Netherlands conrm that students consider a variety of behaviors in their schools to be problematic. A random sample of 540 female
high school students in Sweden were surveyed in 2005 by researchers
Witkowska and Menckel. They found that 49 percent of the female students identied verbal behaviors such as sexualized conversations,
attractiveness rating, demeaning comments about gender, name-calling,
and sexual personal comments as a problem present in their schools.
Fineran, Bennett, and Sacco (2003) found that 78 percent of students in
four South African high schools in Johannesburg reported experiencing
sexual harassment. A survey of 2,808 Dutch students by Timmerman
(2005) revealed that 18 percent had experienced unwanted sexual
behavior. Boys reported more verbal incidents while girls reported signicantly more physical incidents. Most of these studies examined
only victimization, but DeSouza & Ribeiros (2005) inquired about

Bullying and Sexual Harassment of Adolescents

217

perpetration of sexual harassment as well. Results showed that 24


percent of 400 high school students admitted to sexually harassing a
classmate.
PEER SEXUAL HARASSMENT AND BULLYING PERPETRATION
The 1993 AAUW study inquired broadly about sexual harassment
perpetration and found that 54 percent of the students admitted sexually harassing another student during their school years.
However, only two studies have focused specically on the perpetration of bullying and sexual harassment (Pellegrini, 2001; Pepler
et al., 2002). Pellegrinis (2001) longitudinal study of U.S. sixth- and
seventh-grade students found that bullies (who tended to be boys)
also engaged in sexual harassment, and this relation was mediated by
self-reported dating frequency. . . . That is, bullies who also estimated
their frequency of dating to be high . . . tended to sexually harass their
peers (p. 131). Thus, as bullies became interested in opposite-sex dating, their proclivity to become perpetrators of sexual harassment
increased as well. Pellegrini also found that sexual harassment behaviors at the end of seventh grade were predicted by bullying at the start
of sixth grade. Pepler et al. (2002) studied four cohorts of students in
Canadian schools grades ve through eight and examined the relationship between aggressive behavior and substance use. They found that
students who bullied others were at signicant risk for substance abuse
before entering high school. Their results also indicated that girls or
boys who acknowledged sexually harassing others were between four
and six times more likely to use alcohol than those who did not sexually harass their peers.
PEER SEXUAL HARASSMENT AND BULLYING
VICTIMIZATION: GENDER
Nationwide, the AAUW 1993 and 2001 studies found that more than
50 percent of male and female students experienced sexual jokes, comments, gestures, or looks. More than 30 percent of students also experienced physical behaviors, such as being touched, grabbed, pinched, or
brushed up against in a sexual way from schoolmates. In addition to
physical and verbal harassment, Fineran and Bennett (1999) found that
43 percent of girls and 30 percent of boys reported unwanted sexual
attention in the form of pressure for dates and sex, while a Connecticut
study found a quarter of the girls and 5 percent of boys reported
unwanted sexual attention (Permanent Commission [CT] on the Status
of Women [PCSW], 1995). Gender differences in victimization are common with girls experiencing sexual harassment more frequently than
boys and boys perpetrating sexual harassment more frequently than

218

Mental and Physical Health

girls (AAUW, 1993, 2001; Fineran & Bennett, 1999; Hand & Sanchez,
2000; Lee, Croninger, Linn, & Chen, 1996; McMaster, Connolly,
Pepler, & Craig, 2002; PCSW, 1995; Stratton & Backes, 1997; Trigg &
Wittenstrom, 1996).
With regard to bullying, Nansel et al., (2001) found that males both
bullied others and were bullied signicantly more often than females.
Results also showed that males reported being bullied by being hit,
slapped, or pushed, while females were bullied more frequently
through the use of rumors and sexual comments. A meta-analysis of
studies examining peer victimization that occurred over the past
twenty years, showed that bullied boys are four times, and girls eight
times, more likely to be suicidal than their non-bullied peers (Hawker
& Boulton, 2000).
PEER SEXUAL HARASSMENT AND BULLYING
VICTIMIZATION: SEXUAL ORIENTATION
An annual school survey, The Massachusetts Youth Risk Behavior
Survey (Commonwealth of Massachusetts, April 1998), compared gay,
lesbian, and bisexual students to their peers. Results showed that sexual minority students were four times more likely to have attempted
suicide, and ve times more likely to have missed school because of
feeling unsafe. Fineran (2002a) found that lesbian girls experienced
signicantly more sexual harassment than heterosexual girls. A recent
follow-up to the Massachusetts Youth Risk Behavior Survey (Massachusetts Department of Education, 2007) reiterated many of its nds
from their earlier study: Sexual minority students had higher suicide
rates, were more apt to skip school because they felt unsafe, had been
threatened with or injured by a weapon at school, and experienced
more dating violence and non-consensual sex. Fineran (2002a) found in
a study of 712 high school students that sexual minority students were
physically assaulted and sexually harassed more frequently than heterosexual students. She also reported that heterosexual girls were signicantly more upset and threatened by peer sexual harassment than their
heterosexual male peers. According to Williams et al. (2005), gay and
lesbian students also experienced both bullying and sexual harassment
at higher levels than their heterosexual counterparts; and, according to
Poteat and Espelage (2007), being the target of homophobic victimization had signicant psychological and social consequences for students.
A report on Michigan schools by the Gay, Lesbian and Straight Education Network [GLSEN], 2005) revealed that two-thirds of students in
their sample were harassed because they were, or appeared to be, gay
or lesbian, and more than 80 percent of these students reported hearing
derogatory homophobic comments. In a recent study (Gruber &
Fineran, 2008a), sexual minority adolescents not only had signicantly

Bullying and Sexual Harassment of Adolescents

219

higher rates of sexual harassment and bullying victimization but they


also had poorer mental and physical health, more trauma symptoms,
and higher levels of substance abuse as a result of victimization than
did heterosexuals.
According to Thurlow (2004), however, most adolescents rate homophobic slurs as both more common and less serious than racial slurs. It
appears, then, that homophobia may be a normalized means of categorizing and victimizing peers during adolescence that has devastating
consequences for some teens, in particular, sexual minorities.
PEER SEXUAL HARASSMENT AND BULLYING
VICTIMIZATION: CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES
Most of the research on peer victimization and disability takes a bullying perspective rather than a sexual harassment perspective. That is,
victimization is assessed in terms of aggression (hitting, kicking, etc.),
social isolation (having few friends or being shunned), or verbal abuse
(ridicule or parodying a disability). Only rarely is victimization determined by sexual behavior on the part of the perpetratorsfor example,
sexual touching or grabbing, spreading sexual rumors, or sexual namecalling (fag, queer, slut).
The research on peer victimization (bullying) suggests that disabled children experience a number of problems in school. A metaanalysis of research on children with learning disabilities found that
they were rated by their peers as unpopular, socially incompetent, and
socially isolated (Kavale & Forness, 1996). Learning disabled students
reported more aggression and teasing than other students (Martlew &
Hodson, 1991). A study in Great Britain found that almost two thirds
of children with special needs were bullied compared to only one quarter of mainstreamed children (Thompson, Whitney, & Smith, 1994).
Though research presents evidence of higher levels of peer victimization or bullying among disabled students, an unanswered issue is the
impact of victimization on health and school outcomes. Research indicates that bullied children have more mental health problems (depression, psychosomatic symptoms) and school problems (problems
concentrating, poor grades) than other students (see Gruber & Fineran,
2009, for a review). There is overall, however, a dearth of research on
bullying outcomes among adolescents with disabilities.
There is even less research on sexual harassment experiences and
their impact on students with disabilities. A small pilot study consisting of 26 disabled students conducted by Rousso (1996) showed that
girls were more likely to be sexually harassed by non-disabled male
peers, and both male and female students reported sexual harassment
from the adults who care for them. Fineran (2002b) found that students
with disabilities experienced signicantly more sexual harassment in

220

Mental and Physical Health

school from classmates than non-disabled students did. Bullying


research on disabled students is also sparse. A study of eighth graders
by Fineran and Gruber (2004) found while disabled and able students
had similar levels of sexual harassment victimization, disabled students
experienced more negative mental health outcomes as a result of these
experiences. A more recent study (Gruber & Fineran, 2009) revealed
that adolescents with disabilities who were harassed or bullied
had lower self-esteem, weaker attachment to school, and lower satisfaction with school. Students with disabilities were sexually harassed
more frequently than their peers, but their bullying experiences were
comparable.
PEER SEXUAL HARASSMENT AND BULLYING
VICTIMIZATION: RACE AND ETHNICITY
Research on differences in victimization among racial groups
reveals inconsistent ndings. Several recent studies (Nansel et al., 2001;
Juvonen, Nishina, & Graham, 2006; Peskin, Tortolero, & Markham,
2006) found that black adolescents were bullied more frequently than
white or Hispanic students. However, it appears that there are few differences in the types of bullying between race groups. Peskin and her
colleagues found that verbal bullying (teasing and name-calling) did
not vary between groups but physical bullying (hitting) did. In general,
it appears that specic types of bullying (e.g., name calling, teasing) do
not vary substantially in frequency across populations, whether racial,
regional, or international (Borg, 1999; Demaray & Malecki, 2003; Rigby,
2000).
While there are studies of race differences in bullying victimization,
research on adolescent sexual harassment victimization and race is
fairly sparse. The largest study to look at racial differences was conducted by the AAUW (2001). Similar percentages of African Americans
and whites of both sexes said that their school had a lot or some
sexual harassment. Some types of harassment were more prevalent
among blacks than whites: Having their clothes pulled off or down,
being touched or grabbed in a sexual manner, or being forced to kiss
someone (p. 24). The most common places where victimization occurred
did not vary by sex or by race: In halls, in classrooms, and in the gym or
on the playing eld. In another study that looked at both bullying and
sexual harassment, there were no racial differences in frequency for the
former, but a small though statistically signicant difference for the
latterblacks were harassed more often (Gruber & Fineran, 2008b).
AAUW (2001) found that blacks reported less emotional and behavioral impact than whites. This was true for both sexes. For example, a
substantially larger number of white girls said that harassment made
them feel self-conscious (49 percent), or made them feel less self

Bullying and Sexual Harassment of Adolescents

221

condent (35 percent) compared to black girls ( 25 percent and 17 percent, respectively). The fact that peer sexual harassment victimization
may have less impact for blacks than whites may be due to the fact
that racism and racial discrimination play signicant roles in the health
and well-being of African Americans. This point is made well by African American focus group members in Welsh and her colleagues
(2006) study of workplace harassment who said that sexual harassment
was easier to deal with and less pressing than racial harassment
(p. 98). In particular, they found that sexual harassment from black
males was not dened as such while similar treatment from whites
was (p. 99). Buchanan and Fitzgerald (in press) found that adverse job
and health outcomes for African American women that resulted from
workplace sexual harassment were exacerbated by racial harassment.
PEER SEXUAL HARASSMENT AND BULLYING
VICTIMIZATION: HEALTH AND SCHOOL OUTCOMES
There have been a number of studies on bullying and sexual harassment that point to negative outcomes for mental and physical health
and school performance. Nansel et al., 2001 states that research on
bullies and those bullied has consistently found that youth who are
bullied generally show higher levels of insecurity, anxiety, depression,
loneliness, unhappiness, physical and mental symptoms, and low self
esteem (p. 2095). The National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development (Nansel, 2001) also reports that students who were bullied experienced greater difculty making friends, had poorer relationships with classmates, and reported greater loneliness. In additional
studies, Espelage and Holt (2001) investigated depression among children who were bullied and found that 20 percent of middle school victims scored within the clinical range on a standard depression and
anxiety measure, while Boulton and Hawker (2000) found that bullied
kids were ve times more likely to be depressed. Hazler, Hoover, and
Oliver (1992) reported that 90 percent of bullied students experienced a
drop in school grades, while Kochenderfer and Ladd (1996) showed
that bullying victimization was related to school absenteeism or dropping out.
When examining sexual harassment victimization, AAUW studies
(1993, 2001) revealed student reports of negative psychosocial effects
such as depression, loss of appetite, nightmares or disturbed sleep, low
self esteem, and feelings of being sad, afraid, scared, or embarrassed
(Hand & Sanchez, 2000; Lee et al., 1996). Similar to bullying victims,
students also reported loss of interest in regular activities, isolation
from friends and family, and loss of friends. School performance difculties included absenteeism, decreased quality of schoolwork, skipping or dropping classes, poor grades, tardiness, and truancy (AAUW,

222

Mental and Physical Health

1993, 2001; Corbett, Gentry, & Pearson, 1993; Hand & Sanchez, 2000;
Lee et al., 1996; Loredo, Reid, & Deaux, 1995; PCSW, 1995; Roscoe,
Strouse, & Goodwin, 1994; Shakeshaft et al., 1995; Stein, Marshall, &
Tropp, 1993; Stratton & Backes, 1997; Trigg & Wittenstrom, 1996). In
addition, students also reported feeling afraid, upset, or threatened by
the sexual harassment (AAUW, 1993, 2001; Fineran & Bennett, 1999;
PCSW, 1995; Stein, Marshall, & Tropp, 1993).

SEXUAL HARASSMENT OF ADOLESCENTS AT WORK


An emerging area of litigation and policy development is sexual harassment of adolescents who hold jobs in the formal (wage) economy
while attending school. Over the past ten years, litigation led against
employers by adolescents was uncommon. For example, during 2002,
sexual harassment complaints led by teenagers comprised approximately 2 percent of more than 14,000 suits led nationwide. By 2005
that number had quadrupled to 8 percentand these rates have continued to rise (Drobac, 2007). In 2005 alone, for example, the United
States Equal Employment Opportunity Commission (EEOC) led fteen employment discrimination lawsuits on behalf of teenagers who
were sexually harassed at their jobs. The descriptions of their experiences parallel those of many adults. The following lawsuits are recent
examples of sexual harassment in the workplace that have been led
by teens. These cases reect the extent of sexual violence that is, too
often, not associated with sexual harassment. These are not in any way
fringe cases but are representative of sexual harassment occurring to
male and female teens who work.
A class action case led in February 2005 by the EEOC involved
teenage female employees who were subjected to sexual harassment by
a male assistant manager. The girls described having their breasts
grabbed, being backed against walls while having their shoulders
rubbed, and being rubbed up against. The lawsuit states that although
the female employees complained to management, no appropriate
action was taken to correct the situation (EEOC vs. GLC, Inc., 2005).
A second class action suit involved young men who were subjected
to same-sex sexual harassment by a male supervisor. Behaviors experienced by the men included requests for sex, sexual remarks, and
unwanted touching. One employee had his work hours cut in retaliation for opposing the sexual harassment (EEOC v. Pand Enterprises, Inc.,
2005). In both of these suits the EEOC asks the courts in Arizona and
New Mexico (respectively) to order (both companies doing business as)
McDonalds to provide the harassment victims with back wages, compensatory damages, and punitive damages and stop McDonalds from
engaging in any practice that discriminates based on sex or retaliation.

Bullying and Sexual Harassment of Adolescents

223

Other cases recently settled have involved sexual assault, stalking,


and verbal abuse. Successful lawsuit settlements have included payments of $150,000 to employees, along with letters of apology and a
requirement that stores train employees regarding sexual harassment
and post signs explaining employees right to a workplace free of
sexual harassment. Examples of teen work related sexual harassment
lawsuits are listed on the EEOC website: http://youth.eeoc.gov/
cases.html.
While there is much information known about the type of work
teens perform, including the number of hours they work and their
wages (e.g., Mortimer, 2005), the pervasiveness and impact of sexual
harassment has received only minimal attention (Fineran, 2002a). It is
quite apparent that research has not kept pace with growing legal and
social concerns over this issue. Numerous studies of adult sexual harassment have been conducted over the past 20 years, but few studies
have explored the problem among adolescents who work part time
while attending school. Five studies documenting adolescent workplace
sexual harassment have been conducted since 1981 (Fineran & Gruber,
in press; Fineran & Gruber, 2009; Fineran, 2002a; Stein, 1981; Strauss
and Espeland, 1992). Stein (1981) surveyed 22 female students attending a Massachusetts vocational high school. Eight students reported
experiencing workplace sexual harassment that occurred while
employed as babysitters, store clerks, or wait staff. Harassers were
reported to be employers, customers, managers, and coworkers. All of
the students eventually quit because of the harassment or were red
from their jobs for non-compliance.
A second survey conducted by Strauss and Espeland (1992) found
that 30 percent of 250 female vocational students surveyed from four
Minnesota school districts had been sexually harassed at work. This
was similar to the ndings by Fineran (2002a). In a sample of 712 high
school students, 35 percent of the 332 students who worked part-time
reported experiencing sexual harassment (63 percent girls; 37 percent
boys). Findings showed that students experienced harassment from
supervisors (19 percent), coworkers (61 percent), and unidentied
others at work (18 percent). Girls reported being signicantly more
upset and threatened by the harassment than boys. None of these
studies examined the impact of harassment on adolescent health or
well-being.
Recently, research by Fineran and Gruber (in press) found that girls
experienced a rate of sexual harassment that was higher than those
typically found in samples of adult women. More than 52 percent of
the girls reported that they had experienced some form of sexual harassment during the past year at their jobs. Harassed teens also faced
problems similar to women in terms of work stress and alienation from
coworkers and supervisors. An analysis of their most upsetting

224

Mental and Physical Health

harassment experience revealed that more than half (56 percent) of the
perpetrators were coworkers, while supervisors and vendors/customers
accounted equally for the remainder. A large majority of the perpetrators were older than the girls, with nearly half (46 percent) described
as older than 30.
Another study by the same authors (Fineran & Gruber, 2009) found
that young adolescents (ages 13-15) were impacted signicantly by sexual harassment. In particular, girls healthself esteem, physical and
mental health, and post-traumatic symptomsand work attitudes and
behavior were adversely affected more so than boys.
CONCLUSIONS
Research conducted nationally and internationally on teen sexual
harassment and bullying describes a problem of victimization that persists throughout adolescence and has a number of unhealthy effects.
Despite the varieties of sample sizes, theoretical denitions, measurements, and time frames, several important generalizations can be
gleaned from the research.
It appears at rst glance that sexual harassment occurs at a much
higher rate than bullying does. On closer inspection, it appears that a
signicant amount of this difference may be attributed to the ways in
which these forms of victimization are measured. The only study to
date (Gruber & Fineran, 2008a) that used similar measures and time
frames found that bullying was more prevalent. A clearer understanding of victimization rates across time and spacee.g., comparing victimization in 2009 to 2001, or comparing rates between the United
States and European Union nationswould result from the use of
similarly-constructed measures (e.g., a listing of behaviors found in the
AAUW studies) and similar time frames.
Across the globe, regardless of culture and language it is clear that
victimization and perpetration are not random but are indeed highly
predictable events. Boys are perpetrators and frequently victims; girls
are victims but seldom perpetrators. Also, sexual minority children are
at risk for bullying and sexual harassment victimization. Though there
is less research on them, children with disabilities also seem to be frequent targets of victimization. The research on racial and ethnic differences in bullying and harassment is mixed. Most studies nd few
differences either in the overall incidence of victimization or in the specic types of victimization. Since most perpetration and victimization
is an in-group phenomenon (e.g., blacks bully and harass other blacks),
the dynamics of cross-race experiences remain a research issue for the
future.
Research from across the globe clearly shows that bullying and harassment victimization is not a trivial event in childrens lives. Despite

Bullying and Sexual Harassment of Adolescents

225

popular claims that victimization is just part of growing up that


most children experience and weather successfully, a plethora of studies suggest otherwise. There are clear health risks associated with victimization to childrens psychological well-being (e.g., poor self esteem,
high anxiety, depression, trauma symptoms, suicidal behavior) as well
as to adjustment to and functioning within school. Victimized children
enjoy school less, perform poorly, and harbor negative attitudes toward
teachers. But these adverse health and school outcomes are not spread
evenly across the student population. Some types of students suffer
more. In particular, girls and sexual minorities experience signicant
harm as a result of victimization. The gender difference in harm seems
to be the case whether the environment is school or a workplace.
We end our chapter on a cautionary note. Recently, bullying has
become the probleme de jour both in the United States and the European
Union. The number of research papers, articles in popular literature,
and television specials devoted to the problem is quite phenomenal.
The attention to bullying, while well-meaning and well-deserved, has
diminished the attention that sexual harassment is receiving. The
research we have conducted recently suggests that placing sexual harassment in the shadow of bullying does not serve our schools and our
children well. Specically, we found that sexual harassment poses
greater health risks for boys and girls, and especially for sexual minorities. Our research on school outcomes, which remains unnished at
this point, suggests similar ndings.

REFERENCES
American Association of University Women Educational Foundation. (2001).
Hostile hallways: Bullying, teasing and sexual harassment in school. Washington,
DC: Author.
American Association of University Women Educational Foundation. (1993).
Hostile hallways: The AAUW survey on sexual harassment in American schools
(Research Rep. No. 923012). Washington, DC: Harris/Scholastic Research.
Borg, M. (1999). The extend and nature of bullying among primary and secondary
schoolchildren. Educational Research, 41, 137153.
Boulton, M., & Hawker, D. (2000). Twenty years research on peer victimization
and psychosocial maladjustment: A meta-analytic review of cross-sectional
studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41,
441455.
Buchanan, N., & Fitzgerald, L. (in press). The effects of racial and sexual harassment on work and the psychological well-being of African American
women. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology.
Corbett, K., Gentry, C., & Pearson, W. (1993). Sexual harassment in high school.
Youth & Society, 25, 93103.
Craig, W., Pepler D., Connolly, J., & Henderson, K. (2001). Developmental context of peer harassment in early adolescence: The role of puberty and the

226

Mental and Physical Health

peer group. In J. Juvonen & S. Graham, (Eds.), Peer harassment in school:


The plight of the vulnerable and victimized (pp. 242261). New York: Guilford.
Crick, N. (1996). The role of overt, relational aggression, and prosocial behavior
in the prediction of childrens future social adjustment. Child Development,
67, 23172327.
Demaray, M., & Malecki, C. (2003). Perceptions of the frequency and importance
of social support by students classied as victims, bullies, and bully/victims
in an urban middle school. School Psychology Review, 32, 471489.
DeSouza, E., & Ribeiros, J. (2005). Bullying and sexual harassment among Brazilian high school students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20, 10181038.
Drobac, J. (2007). I cant to I Kant: The sexual harassment of working adolescents, competing theories, and ethical dilemmas. The Albany Law Review,
70, 675739.
EEOC v. GLC, inc., d/b/a/McDonalds Restaurant, CIV 05-0618 PCT PGR.
EEOC v. Pand Enterprises, Inc. d/b/a McDonalds Restaurant, Civil Action
No. 05-CIV-204.
Elsea, M., Menesini, M., Moore, A., & Morita, Y. (2004). Friendship and loneliness among bullies and victims: data from seven countries. Aggressive Behavior, 30, 7183.
Espelage, D., & Holt, M. (2001). Bullying and victimization during early adolescence: Peer inuences and psychosocial correlates. In R. Geffner &
M. Loring (Eds.), Bullying behavior: Current issues, research, and interventions.
Binghamton, New York: Haworth Press Inc.
Fineran, S. (2002a). Sexual minority students and peer sexual harassment in
high school. Journal of School Social Work, 11, 5069.
Fineran, S. (2002b, August) Sexual harassment and students with disabilities. Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the Society for the Study of Social
Problems Annual Meeting, Washington, DC.
Fineran, S., & Bennett, L. (1999). Gender and power issues of peer sexual harassment among teenagers. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 626641.
Fineran, S., Bennett, L., & Sacco, T. (2003). Peer sexual harassment and peer
violence among adolescents in Johannesburg and Chicago. International
Social Work Journal, 46, 391405.
Fineran, S., & Gruber, J. E. (2009). Youth at work: Adolescent employment and
sexual harassment. Child Abuse & Neglect.
Fineran, S., Gruber, J. E., & Rioux, J. (2009, May 20). Employed Maine teens
and workplace sexual harassment. Presentation to the Maine Department
of Labor.
Fitzgerald, L., & Shullman, S. (1993). Sexual harassment: A research analysis
and agenda for the 1990s. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 42, 527.
Gay, Lesbian, and Straight Education Network. (2005). Grading our schools: The
national report evaluating our nations schools and their progress on creating safe
and afrming learning environments for gay and lesbian students and staff. New
York: Author.
Gruber, J. (1990). Methodological problems and policy implications in sexual
harassment research. Population Research and Policy Review, 9, 235254.
Gruber, J., & Fineran, S. (2008a). Comparing the impact of bullying and sexual
harassment victimization on the mental and physical health of adolescents.
Sex Roles, 59, 113.

Bullying and Sexual Harassment of Adolescents

227

Gruber, J., & Fineran, S. (2008b, August). Race, bullying and sexual harassment:
The impact on health outcomes of adolescents. Paper presented at the annual
meeting of the Society for the Study of Social Problems, Boston, MA.
Gruber, J. & Fineran, S. (2009, August). Disability, bullying and sexual harassment:
The impact on health and school outcomes for adolescents. Paper presented at
the annual meeting of the Society for the Study of Social Problems, San
Francisco, CA.
Hand, J., & Sanchez, L. (2000). Badgering or bantering? Gender differences in
experience of, and reactions to, sexual harassment among U.S. high school
students. Gender & Society, 14, 71846.
Hazler, R. J., Hoover, J. H., & Oliver, R. (1991). Student perceptions of victimization by bullies in school. Journal of Humanistic Education and Development, 29, 143150.
Holan, L., Flisher, A. J., & Lombard, C. J. (2007). Bullying, violence, and
risk behavior in South African school students. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31,
161171.
Juvonen, S., Nishina, A., & Graham, S. (2001). Ethnic diversity and perceptions
of safety in urban middle schools. Psychological Science, 17, 393400.
Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1996). Social skill decits and learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 226237.
Kimmel, M., & Mahler, M. (2003). Adolescent masculinity, homophobia, and
violence: Random school shootings, 19822000. American Behavioral Scientist, 46, 14391458.
Klein, J. (2006a). An invisible problem: Everyday violence against girls in
schools. Theoretical Criminology, 10, 147177.
Klein, J. (2006b). Sexuality and school shootings: What role does teasing play in
school massacres? Journal of Homosexuality, 51, 3962.
Kobayoshi, F. (1999). Bullying in Japanese schools. United States Department of
Education, Educational Resources Information Center. Retrieved July 7, 2007,
http://www.eric.ed.gov/ERICDocs/data/ericdocs2sql/content_storage_01/
0000019b/80/15/e6/ba.pdf
Kochenderfer, B.J., & Ladd, G.W. (1996). Peer victimization: Cause or consequence of school maladjustment. Child Development, 67, 13051317.
Lee, V., Croninger, R., Linn, E., & Chen, X. (1996). The culture of sexual harassment in secondary schools. American Educational Research Journal, 33,
383417.
Loredo, C., Reid, A., & Deaux, K. (1995). Judgements and denitions of sexual
harassment by high school students. Sex Roles, 32, 2945.
MacKinnon, C. (1979). Sexual harassment of working women. New Haven, CT:
Yale University Press.
Martlew, M., & Hodson, J. (1991). Children with mild learning difculties in an
integrated and in a special school: Comparisons of behaviour, teasing and
teachers attitudes. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 61, 355369.
McGuffey, C., & Rich, M. (1999). Playing in the gender transgression zone:
race, class, and hegemonic masculinity in middle school. Gender & Society,
13, 608627.
McMaster, L., Connolly, J., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (2002). Peer to peer sexual
harassment in early adolescence: A developmental perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 91105.

228

Mental and Physical Health

Massachusetts Department of Education. (2007). 2005 Youth risk behavior survey.


Malden, MA: Author.
Massachusetts Department of Education. (1998). 1998 Massachusetts youth risk
behavior survey results. Boston: Author.
Mortimer, J. T. (2005). Working and growing up in America. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Nansel, T., Overpeck, R., Pilla, W., Ruan, P., Scheidt, K., & Simons-Morton, B.
(2001). Bullying behaviors among U.S. youth: Prevalence and association
with psychological adjustment. Journal of the American Medical Association,
285, 20942100.
Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in the school: Bullies and whipping boys. Chichester,
United Kingdom: Wiley.
Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school. Oxford, United Kingdom: Basil Blackwell.
Olweus, D., & Limber, S. (1999). Blueprints for violence prevention. Bullying Prevention Program. Boulder: Institute of Behavioral Sciences, University of
Colorado.
OMoore, A. M., & Hillery, B. (1989). Bullying in Dublin schools. The Irish
Journal of Psychology, 10, 426441.
Oncale v. Sundowner Offshore Services, Inc., 523 U.S. 75 (1998).
Pellegrini, A. (2001). A longitudinal study of heterosexual relationships, aggression and sexual harassment during transition from primary school through
middle school. Applied Developmental Psychology, 22, 11933.
Pepler, D. J., Craig, W. M., Connolly, J., & Henderson, K. (2002). My friends
made me do it: Peer inuence on alcohol and substance use in adolescence. In C. Wekerle & A. M. Wall (Eds.), The violence and addiction equation: Theoretical and clinical issues in substance abuse and relationship violence.
Philadelphia: Brunner/Mazel.
Permanent Commission (CT) on the Status of Women. (1995). In our own backyard:
Sexual harassment in Connecticuts public high schools. Hartford, CT: Author.
Peskin, M., Tortolero, S., & Markham, C. (2006). Bullying and victimization
among black and Hispanic adolescents. Adolescence, 41, 467484.
Poteat, V., & Espelage, D. (2007). Predicting psychosocial consequences of
homophobic victimization in middle school students. Journal of Early
Adolescence, 27, 175191.
Rigby, K. (2000). Effects of peer victimization in schools and perceived social
support on adolescent well-being. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 5768.
Roscoe, B., Strouse, J. S., & Goodwin, M. P. (1994). Correlates of attitudes toward sexual harassment among early adolescents. Sex Roles, 31, 559577.
Schwartz, D., Dodge, K., Pettit, G., & Bates, J. (1997). The early socialization of
aggressive victims of bullying. Child Development, 68, 665675.
Shakeshaft, C., Barber, E., Hergenrother, M., Johnson, Y. M., Mandel, L., &
Sawyer, J. (1995). Peer harassment in schools. Journal for a Just and Caring
Education, 1, 3044.
Stein, N. (1981). Sexual harassment of high school students: Preliminary research
results. Boston, MA: Massachusetts Department of Education. Unpublished
manuscript.
Stein, N. (1999). Incidence and implications of sexual harassment and sexual
violence in K12 schools. Hamilton Fish National Institute on School and
Community Violence. Washington, DC: George Washington University.

Bullying and Sexual Harassment of Adolescents

229

Stein, N., Marshall, N. L., & Tropp, L. R. (1993). Secrets in public: Sexual harassment in our schools. Wellesley, MA: Wellesley College Center for Research
on Women.
Stephensen, P., & Smith, D. (1989). Bullying in the junior school. In D.P. Tatum
& D.A. Lane (Eds.) Bullying in schools. London: Trentham Books.
Stratton, S., & Backes, J. (1997, February/March). Sexual harassment in North
Dakota public schools: A study of eight high schools. The High School
Journal, 80, 163172.
Strauss, S., & Espeland, P. (1992). Sexual harassment and teens. Minneapolis: Free
Spirit Publishing, Inc.
Thompson, D., Whitney, I., & Smith, P.K. (1994). Bullying of children with
special needs in mainstream schools. Support for Learning, 9, 103106.
Thurlow, C. (2004). Naming the outsider within: homophobic pejoratives
and the verbal abuse of lesbian, gay and bisexual high-school students.
Journal of Adolescence, 24, 2538.
Timmerman, G. (2005). A comparison between girls and boys experiences of
unwanted sexual behaviour in secondary schools. Educational Research, 47,
291306.
Trigg, M., & Wittenstrom, K. (1996). Thats the way the world goes: Sexual
harassment and New Jersey teenagers. Initiatives, Special Issue: Sexual
Harassment 57, 5565.
U.S. Department of Education, Ofce for Civil Rights. (1997). Sexual harassment guidance: Harassment of students by school employees, other
students, or third parties. Federal Register 62, 1203412051.
U.S. Department of Education, Ofce for Civil Rights. (2003). School crime supplement to the National Crime Victimization Survey. Washington, DC: Author.
U.S. Merit Systems Protection Board (USMSPB). (1995). Sexual harassment in the
Federal workplace: Trends, progress, and continuing challenges. Washington,
DC: U.S. Government Printing Ofce.
U.S. Merit Systems Protection Board. (1988). Sexual harassment in the Federal
workplace: An update. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Ofce.
Welsh, S., Carr, J., MacQuarrie, B., & Huntley, A. (2006). Im not thinking of it
as sexual harassment: Understanding harassment across race and citizenship. Gender & Society, 20, 87107.
Williams, T., Connolly, J., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (2005). Peer victimization,
social support, and psychosocial adjustment of sexual minority adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34, 471482.
Witkowska, E., & Menckel, E. (2005). Perceptions of sexual harassment in
Swedish high schools: Experiences and school-environment problems.
European Journal of Public Health, 15, 7885.
Zeira, A., Astor, R. A., & Benbenishty, R. (2003). School violence in Israel: Findings of a national survey. Social Work, 48, 472483.

This page intentionally left blank

Chapter 12

Great Is Our Sin: Pseudoscientic


Justications for Oppression in
American Education
Jennifer L. Martin

If the misery of our poor be caused not by laws of nature, but by our institutions, great is our sin.
Charles Darwin

The above quotation by Charles Darwin is still very relevant in contemporary American society. Not only the poor, but also those who experience disenfranchisement in a variety of forms, for example, gender,
race, ethnicity, and so on, are subject to inequity on multiple levels:
personal, interpersonal, institutional, and cultural. These levels of
oppression are interactive, and it is easy to see how the majority
viewpoint can subtly make its way into the cultural framework. For
example, ones personal values, beliefs, and feelings can lead to interpersonal interaction (behavior), which can then lead to the creation of
institutional rules and policies and then nally to cultural views
involving the nature of beauty, truth, and right. It is frightening to
examine our own history and the history of the American public education system in this light, but the same is true for both: personal
values and beliefs were held that excluded and were detrimental to minority viewpoints. These racist and sexist beliefs were used to create a
sociopolitical agenda that separated people into racial categories and
promoted the superiority of the white race. In Inheriting Shame: The
Story of Eugenics and Racism in America (1999), Steven Selden discusses

232

Mental and Physical Health

how such personal values and beliefs led to institutional policy in the
American school system, the repercussions of which are still with us.
Although most people associated with American education were not
overtly radical racists, it is frightening when one considers that they
nonetheless attached their names to philosophical ideas that were the
basis for organizations that advocated for the segregation and sterilization of undesirables. For example, G. Stanley Hall, developer of Child
Study, which was highly inuential on curriculum in the twentieth century, was a rm believer in biological determinism. He felt that nature
dened educability. He also advocated for a wide range of curricula
to correspond to the wide range of abilities in individuals, which, not
surprisingly, were more often than not based on racial and gender differences. Leta Hollingworth is still recognized today as a leading
researcher and advocate for gifted and talented education, despite her
alignment with the Eugenics Movement. As Selden states: Educational
policy, in Hollingworths (1924) view, disregarded gifted children due to
a misguided social philosophy that denied innate permanent, hereditary
superiority (p. 101). Dr. Helen Putnams eugenical ideas linked the
National Education Association (NEA) with biological determinism.
Dr. Putnams report at the 1916 NEA conference, entitled The New
Ideal in EducationBetter Parents of Better Children, advocated for the
rationale that there was in fact a superior race: If humanity is to survive, Putnam informed her audience of teachers and administrators,
individualism and nationalism must conform to the laws of racial wellbeing (Putnam, quoted in Selden, p. 57). Putnam also expanded her
platform of eugenical ideas to include programs of teacher education.
Interestingly, knowledge about the existence of the notion of eugenics and the Eugenics Movement in America is not widely known by
most modern educators, or by the general public for that matter.
Unfortunately, not having this knowledge allows similar ideas to reappear in contemporary society in more covert forms, for example, in the
form of tracking students, having differential expectations based on
gender, socioeconomic class, and so on. More shocking is when such
racist notions appear quite overtly in society. For example, the racism
justied by biological determinism emerged again recently with the
publication of The Bell Curve (1994) by Charles Murray and Richard J.
Hernstein. The Bell Curve presents pseudoscientic evidence of the
genetic inferiority of blacks. As Macedo and Bartolome state:
This book has not only activated what had appeared to be a dormancy of
racism in the United States after the enactment of the civil rights laws,
but it also has resurrected an old form of intellectual lynching that,
unfortunately, has been embraced by ever more powerful representatives
of the far right and, with some exception, by liberals through a form of
silence. (1999, p. 85)

Great Is Our Sin

233

Despite the fact that knowledge of the Eugenics Movement in America is not well known by most modern educators, it is easy to trace
eugenical ideas in public education through science textbooks used
during the period between 1914 and 1948. There was little scientic
evidence to support eugenics in its infancy, but even when research
suggested that there was absolutely no scientic basis for eugenical
conclusions textbooks continued to promote individual differences
based on hierarchical notions of race and biological determinism. In
essence, textbook authors and publishers, such as George William
Hunter, believed what they wanted to believe, despite evidence to the
contrary. They used their beliefs to promote their own sociopolitical
agendas: agendas of exclusion that promoted the continued privilege
of whites. As Selden states:
Programs of selective breeding were most frequently recommended to
the high school reader: Positive eugenics, which called for the selective
matings of those judged as societys best, was cited in 64.4% of the texts,
and negative eugenics, which demanded the restriction of child-bearing
by those judged socially inferior, appeared in 46.3% of the volumes. In
addition, 19.5% of the texts recommended immigration restriction and
14.6% suggested policies of segregation and sterilization. (p. 68)

The inuence of the Eugenics Movement on American education


shows itself most clearly today in the form of the hierarchical system
of tracking students by ability. As Selden states: Eugenics promoted a
concept of schooling as an open market in which individuals competed
by means of their inherited traits for high scoresa scarce commodity
indeed (p. 37). Compensatory education has been a common practice in American public schools to deal with inequities in education.
Proponents of this philosophy argue that educational inequity results
from the failure of underachieving linguistic and ethnic minorities.
Compensatory education programs rely on remediation in an attempt
to get underachieving students up to the levels of their peers. The curriculum in compensatory education programs is reduced in content
and in scope and is delivered at a slower pace. However, students
placed in remedial tracks, for the most part, do not catch up to their
peers. As Hugh Mehan states (1997): Research has shown, however,
that the schools practices of tracking, ability grouping and testing contribute to inequality (Rosenbaum, 1978; Cicourel & Mehan, 1983;
Oakes, 1985; Page & Valli, 1991; Mehan, 1992; Oakes et al., 1992)
(p. 116). Despite this research, the practice of tracking still continues, in
one form or another, in the majority of schools in America.
It is alarming to recognize the degree to which science has been misused in the past and even today to justify existing social prejudices or
as Tavris states: to conrm the prejudice that some groups are

234

Mental and Physical Health

assigned to their subordinate roles by the harsh dictates of nature


(p. 24). Supposed biological differences between the races were used
throughout history to justify oppression and maintain white privilege.
Perhaps more alarming, is the fact that many do not question such
studies because they are reported in academic publications and
through the media as science. Our sin is great, again as Darwin suggests, for our institutions perpetuate the misery of the disenfranchised
and, to make matters worse, often pass this misery off as being caused
by the laws of natureand thus our responsibility is (or should be)
greater. As educators we must promote awareness about our own past
and present in order to create a better future for all. We must look with
a questioning eye at policies and programs for their hidden biases and
agendas. Perhaps then Margaret Meads often quoted phrase would be
cause for celebration: Never doubt that a small group of thoughtful,
committed citizens can change the world; indeed, its the only thing
that ever has.
REFERENCES
Macedo, D., & Bartolome, L. I. (1999). Dancing with bigotry: Beyond the politics of
tolerance. New York: St. Martins Press.
Mehan, H. (1997). Tracking untracking: The consequences of placing low-track
students in high-track classes. Race, ethnicity, and multiculturalism: Policy
and practice (pp. 115150). New York: Garland Publishing, Inc.
Selden, S. (1999). Inheriting shame: The story of eugenics and racism in America.
New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.
Tavris, C. (1992). The mismeasure of woman. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Chapter 13

Discrimination, Harassment, and


Womens Physical and Mental Health
Krystle C. Woods
NiCole T. Buchanan

Womens health is inextricably linked to their status in society. It benets


from equality and suffers from discrimination.
World Health Report (World Health Organization, 1988, p. 6)

Women across the world bear the burdens of discrimination, harassment, poverty, and maltreatment (United Nations Population Fund
[UNFPA], 2005). Unequal pay, lower workforce participation, and disparate access to health care place women at a disadvantage relative to
men (Pratt, 1997; UNFPA, 2002), and contribute to women representing
more than 75 percent of the worlds poor (United Nations, 2005). Societys (mis)treatment of women contributes to their higher rates of certain forms of mental illness (e.g., depression, anxiety, and eating
disorders) and their increased incidence of physical disability relative
to men (Landrine & Klonoff, 1997; Nosek, Howland, Rintala, Young, &
Chanpong, 2004).
Womens participation in paid employment determines their ability
to combat both poverty and nancial dependence, both of which put
them at risk of being abused. Yet, for many women, the workplace is
fraught with additional hazards that can place their nancial, physical,
and psychological well-being in peril. Specically, gender discrimination and harassment are reported by approximately half of all working
women in the United States (Ilies, Hauserman, Schwochau, & Stibal,
2003), with damaging effects on their mental and physical health
(Willness, Steel, & Lee, 2007). Given the high toll women pay as a

236

Mental and Physical Health

result of work-related gender discrimination and harassment, the specic negative mental and physical health outcomes associated with this
mistreatment should be well understood.
DEFINING GENDER DISCRIMINATION AND HARASSMENT
Title VII of the Civil Rights Acts of 1964 and 1991 prohibits employment discrimination based on gender, race, color, religion, and national
origin. Gender discrimination is divided into two forms: disparate
treatment and disparate impact (Cleveland, Vescio, & Barnes-Farrell,
2005). Disparate treatment occurs when individuals are deliberately
treated differently because of their gender. Examples include offering
women lower starting salaries, posing different interview questions to
male and female applicants (e.g., querying their intent to have children), or refusing to hire female applicants. Disparate impact, also
referred to as adverse impact, occurs when ostensibly neutral workplace practices have an unnecessary negative effect on members of a
protected class (e.g., women), thereby limiting the opportunities of that
group. Gender-related examples of disparate impact frequently include
the use of height, weight, or strength requirements for physically
demanding jobs. For example, the Dial Corporation required a preemployment test of physical strength that rendered 60 percent of
female applicants, but only 3 percent of male applicants, ineligible for
employment, including women who had already been successfully
employed in the position. The ruling in EEOC v. The Dial Corporation
asserted that a fair test should not screen out employees who successfully worked in the same position, therefore, the strength test had an
unnecessary adverse effect on women.
Originally, the law protected against gender-based discrimination, but
did not address sexual harassment until 1976 when the ruling in
Williams v. Saxby legally recognized it as a form of sex discrimination
in violation of Title VII (Equal Employment Opportunity Commission
[EEOC], 1980). The legal denition of sexual harassment separates it
into two categories: quid pro quo and hostile environment. Quid pro
quo includes any attempt to coerce sexual interactions via job-related
threats or promises of benets based on ones sexual compliance (e.g.,
threatening termination or promising a promotion). A hostile work environment is created when an employee perceives the general workplace
milieu as hostile as a result of unwanted gender-based comments or
behaviors. Similarly, a hostile environment can be created when these
behaviors negatively affect an employees job performance, regardless
of whether or not there are tangible or economic job consequences
(Equal Employment Opportunity Commission [EEOC], 1980). As a
psychological construct, sexual harassment is dened as unwanted
gender-based or sexual comments and behavior that an individual

Discrimination, Harassment, and Womens Physical and Mental Health 237

appraises as offensive and a threat to her/his well-being (Fitzgerald,


Swan, & Magley, 1997). There are three primary subtypes of sexual
harassment (Fitzgerald et al., 1988; Fitzgerald, Gelfand, & Drasgow,
1995): gender harassment (negative non-sexual, gender-based comments
and behaviors, such as sex-stratifying jobs as mens work); unwanted
sexual attention (verbal and nonverbal sexual comments, gestures, or
attempts at physical contact, such as asking someone on a date repeatedly or touching someone sexually); and sexual coercion (which is
equivalent to the legal construct of quid pro quo).
HOW DISCRIMINATION AND HARASSMENT HARM
PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING AND PHYSICAL HEALTH
Gender-based discrimination and sexual harassment exact a heavy
toll on the psychological and physical health of women targeted for
harassment.1 According to the Transactional Model of Stress (Lazarus
& Folkman, 1984), an experience becomes stressful when the individual
appraises a situation as exceeding his or her resources and endangering his or her well-being (p. 21). Both sexual harassment and gender
discrimination are frequently interpreted as such by targets and are
likely to result in psychological and physical sequelae common to
trauma (Avina & ODonohue, 2002; Fitzgerald, Drasgow, Hulin,
Gelfand, & Magley, 1997). The transactional model of stress also outlines several different factors that increase the extent to which an event
will be perceived as stressful: the amount of harm done to the individual, the threat of negative outcomes in the future, the inability to predict and control the situation, the pervasiveness and frequency of the
stressor, and its occurrence in settings where mastery was previously
achieved. Gender discrimination and sexual harassment frequently
include these characteristics that increase the likelihood that they will
be stressful (Huerta, Cortina, Pang, Torges, & Magley, 2006; Langhout,
Bergman, Cortina, Fitzgerald, Drasgow, & Hunter Williams, 2005). For
example, gender discrimination and sexual harassment are often
chronic stressors dened by their insidious onsets and their extended,
unpredictable endpoint (Wheaton, 1997), which result in greater detriment to ones mental and physical health than single traumatic events
(Green et al., 2000; Krupnick, Green, Stockton, Goodman, Corcoran, &
Petty, 2004; McGonagle & Kessler, 1990). As these acts are perpetrated
against women in their place of work, school, or residence, targeted
women rarely feel that they have control over their occurrence, pervasiveness, or frequency. Further, discrimination and harassment often
harm the individuals well-being at the present and have the potential
for future negative consequences (e.g., being red for refusing to comply or creating a hostile work environment that inhibits professional
growth and learning). All of these factors increase the likelihood that

238

Mental and Physical Health

gender discrimination and sexual harassment will be traumatic events


that damage womens mental and physical health.
In addition to the nature of the experiences themselves, some theorize that experiences of gender-based discrimination and harassment
can be more stressful than generic stressors (e.g., moving or starting a
new job) because they are inherently personal and attack a social identity tied to an immutable personal characteristic (e.g., biological sex)
(Landrine & Klonoff, 1997; Zucker & Landry, 2007). Compared to privileged group members, members of disadvantaged groups are more
likely to experience discrimination, view prejudice against their group
as pervasive and systemic, and perceive discrimination as uncontrollable, unavoidable, and a devaluation of a core social identity (Major &
Crocker, 1993; Schmitt et al., 2002). Such experiences negatively impact
ones perceptions of the views others have about their social group
(i.e., public regard; Allport, 1979; Cartwright, 1950), which results in
poorer physical and mental health. Further, these relationships are
stronger when one holds a meritocracy worldviewthat people are
appropriately rewarded for their efforts and thus, can determine their
own success (Major, Kaiser, OBrian, & McCoy, 2007)perhaps
because recognition of maltreatment independent of ones personal
efforts and abilities challenges ones basic understanding of the world
and ones opportunity structure.
MENTAL HEALTH OUTCOMES RELATED TO
DISCRIMINATION AND HARASSMENT
Meta-analytic reviews have found signicant associations between
gender discrimination, sexual harassment, and indicators of psychological well-being, such as depression, anxiety, posttraumatic stress disorder, disordered eating, nicotine use, and alcohol use (Cantisano,
Dominguez, & Depolo, 2008; Chan, Lam, Chow, & Cheung, 2008;
Willness et al., 2007). Moreover, studies have found that the negative
effects of harassment can persist for more than ten years after the harassment has ended (Glomb, Munson, Hulin, Bergman, & Drasgow,
1999; Street, Gradus, Stafford, & Kelly, 2007). These relationships have
been supported across a wide variety of populations, including former
military reservists (Street et al., 2007), women in the U.S. (Bergman &
Drasgow, 2003) and the Swedish (Estrada & Berggren, 2009) Armed
Forces, female employees of the U.S. federal judicial circuit (Lim &
Cortina, 2005), physicians (Shrier et al., 2007), police ofcers (Dowler &
Arai, 2008), college women (Schmitt et al., 2002), women seeking counseling (Moradi & Funderburk, 2006), lesbian/bisexual women (Szymanski, 2005), and Latina (Cortina, 2004), Asian (Patel, 2008), and black
women (Buchanan & Fitzgerald, 2008). Thus, the negative effects of
gender discrimination and sexual harassment appear to be universal.

Discrimination, Harassment, and Womens Physical and Mental Health 239

The strength of the relationship between discrimination and psychological well-being is affected by several factors (Fineran & Gruber,
2008) reecting individual (e.g., age, self-esteem, and feminist identity)
and experiential differences (e.g., appraisal, multiple forms of harassment occurring simultaneously). For example, younger working
women appear to be more negatively impacted by harassment than
their older counterparts, perhaps because they do not have the same
job stability, seniority, and security (Chan et al., 2008). Collective selfesteem or perceptions of women in general and personal self-esteem
both appear to moderate the relationship between discrimination, sexual harassment, and psychological distress (Fischer & Holz, 2007). For
example, women with a strong and positive view of themselves may
be better equipped to dismiss discrimination experiences than women
who are unsure of their personal worth (Moradi & Subich, 2004) and
those with more positive views of women in general (higher collective
self-esteem) reported less depression and anxiety following harassment
(Fischer & Holz, 2007).
Feminist consciousness is theorized to provide a framework for
understanding gender discrimination, which can reduce self-blame
associated with these experiences (Landrine & Klonoff, 1997). Feminist
attitudes have been found to be protective against the negative effects
of gender discrimination and harassment (Moradi & Subich, 2002), suggesting that women who endorse a more feminist consciousness may
avoid internalizing blame by recognizing harassment as a manifestation
of larger social injustices. Furthermore, feminist-identied women may
feel more empowered to actively cope with discrimination, making
them less vulnerable to negative outcomes (Sabik & Tylka, 2006). Notably, a feminist identity does not appear to be equally protective for all
women. Whereas a feminist identity may help white women label their
harassment experience as a social injustice instead of internalizing
blame, Rederstorff, Buchanan, and Settles (2007) found that a higher
feminist identity exacerbated the negative effects of harassment for
black women. The authors attributed these ndings to the double consciousness found among many multiply oppressed people where an
increased awareness of oppression based on one salient identity
increases awareness of ones vulnerability based on other salient identities. As a result, black women who endorsed more feminist attitudes
were more likely to report higher levels of posttraumatic stress in the
face of sexual harassment and discrimination. Another study of predominantly white lesbian and bisexual women found that participation
in feminist activities (e.g., membership in feminist organizations, participation in feminist boycotts, marches, or rallies) buffered against psychological distress at low levels of harassment, but more severe sexual
harassment was associated with signicant psychological distress,
regardless of feminist afliation (Szymanski & Owens, 2009).

240

Mental and Physical Health

Womens subjective appraisal of the harassment (e.g., how disturbing, embarrassing, threatening, frightening, or offensive they perceived
it to be) and the coping strategies they use to deal with it can buffer or
exacerbate distress. According to cognitive theories of stress, the targets subjective appraisal of a potentially stressful situation impacts the
level of distress they experience following a traumatic event (Lazarus
& Folkman, 1984). Consistent with this theory, appraisal has been
shown to mediate the relationship between sexual harassment and
mental health outcomes (Langhout et al., 2005). Similarly, women who
perceive harassment as pervasive in society or believe it is directed at
them personally experience heightened psychological distress as compared to women who believed harassment was a relatively rare phenomenon and directed at women as a group rather than a personal
attack (Dambrun, 2007; Foster & Dion, 2003; Schmitt, Branscombe, &
Postmes, 2003).
Women are frequently told they should respond directly and assertively in the face of harassment, but such responses may run counter
to their well-being. Women who utilize direct forms of coping, such as
confronting the perpetrator or ling a complaint against the harasser
often nd that the work environment worsens and their social support
from co-workers and peers wanes (Bergman, Langhout, Cortina,
Palmieri, & Fitzgerald, 2002; Cortina & Magley, 2003). For example,
sexual harassed black women in the U.S. Armed Forces who used
contemplative strategies to cope with harassment (e.g., thinking about
the event) reported signicantly higher psychological well-being compared to women who led a formal complaint (Buchanan, Settles, &
Langhout, 2007).
Finally, the race of the target and the perpetrator can signicantly
inuence the harassment experience and the extent to which it harms a
womans well-being. For example, being sexually harassed while separately experiencing racial harassment or experiencing racialized sexual
harassment has been associated with increased psychological distress
(Thomas, Witherspoon, & Speight, 2008). Black women report experiencing sexual racism and racialized sexual harassment (Buchanan,
2005; Texeira, 2002) as attacks that target their race and gender simultaneously (Buchanan & Ormerod, 2002; Mecca & Rubin, 1999; Yoder &
Aniakudo, 1995, 1996, 1997). These behaviors call upon sexualized
stereotypes of the black woman (i.e., being called a black whore)
and physical features thought to vary by race (i.e., commenting on her
large black behind). Harassment that simultaneously attacks ones
gender and race is perceived as more severe by victims (King, 2003)
and may exacerbate harm because it targets two central and salient
identities (Settles, 2006).
The relationship between racialized sexual harassment and psychological well-being is also mediated by avoidant coping, whereby black

Discrimination, Harassment, and Womens Physical and Mental Health 241

women who try to avoid and distract themselves from the negative
harassment experience report worse psychological outcomes (Thomas
et al., 2008). The use of an avoidant coping style is very prevalent
among black women (Utsey, Ponterotto, Reynolds, & Cancelli, 2000),
perhaps because of the pressure to uphold a facade of strength proscripted by gender role norms of black women (e.g., the Strong Black
Woman archetype; Thomas et al., 2008). In addition, the race of the
perpetrator mediates the relationship between sexual harassment and
resulting distress. Among black college and working women, sexual
harassment from an out-group (e.g., non-black) male was associated
with more symptoms of posttraumatic stress than harassment from an
in-group (i.e., black) male, because it was appraised more negatively
by victims (Woods, Buchanan, & Settles, 2009).
DISCRIMINATION, HARASSMENT AND SPECIFIC
FORMS OF MENTAL ILLNESS
Both daily records of sexist experiences and retrospective reports from
harassed women indicate that depression, anxiety, eating disorders, and
substance abuse are common sequelae following gender discrimination
and sexual harassment (see Avina & ODonohue, 2002; DeSouza & Fansler, 2003; Harned, 2000; Richman, Rospenda, Flaherty, Freels, & Zlatoper,
2004; Swim, Hyers, Cohen, & Ferguson, 2001; Zucker & Landry, 2007). In
fact, some researchers suggest that the gender differences in conditions
such as depression (womens rates are twice those of men; Kessler, 2003)
are due in part to womens higher rates of discrimination and harassment
(Klonoff, Landrine, & Campbell, 2000; Swim et al., 2001).
Some have also argued that because gender discrimination and
harassment are often pervasive, chronic stressors, they can be categorized as traumatic events that result in posttraumatic stress (PTS) symptoms (Avina & ODonohue, 2002; Berg, 2006; Fitzgerald, Buchanan,
Collinsworth, Magley, & Ramos, 1999). Both lifetime accounts and recent
events of sexist discrimination perpetrated at work, school, and by
strangers on the street are associated with posttraumatic stress (Berg,
2006). Researchers have argued that sexual harassment meets the criteria
for a diagnosable trauma as dened by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric
Association, 1994; Avina & ODonohue, 2002; Murdoch, Polusny,
Hodges, & Cowper, 2006). As evidence for this argument, sexual harassment experiences are associated with symptoms of posttraumatic stress
in college women (Rederstorff et al., 2007), women in the U.S. Marines
(Shipherd, Pineles, Gradus, & Resick, 2009), military reservists (Street
et al., 2007), and female veterans of the Gulf War (Vogt, Pless, King, &
King, 2005) and posttraumatic stress symptom severity increases as
sexual harassment severity increases (Murdoch et al., 2006).

242

Mental and Physical Health

Sexual harassment is associated with eating pathology and distorted


body image as a result of multiple processes. First, sexual harassment
is associated with decreased self-esteem, particularly body-based selfesteem, which concomitantly increases the likelihood that the target
will develop pathological eating behaviors (sexual harassment syndrome; Backhouse & Cohen, 1978; Larkin, Rice, & Russell, 1996).
Another model proposes that sexual harassment increases anxiety,
heightens bodily discomfort and scrutiny, and distorts ones body
image, culminating in a heightened risk for disordered eating (Barker &
Galambos, 2003; Frederickson & Roberts, 1997; Hofschire & Greenberg,
2002; Larkin & Rice, 2005; Larkin, Rice, & Russell, 1996). Finally, selfobjectication theory proposes that when womens bodies are looked
at, evaluated, and potentially objectied through experiences like sexual harassment, women may internalize these images (self-objectication),
and engage in increased body monitoring (Frederickson & Roberts,
1997). Excessive body scrutiny can lead to shame, anxiety, and body
image distortion, all of which negatively affect psychological well-being
(Frederickson & Roberts, 1997). Further, research has demonstrated a
direct relationship between self-objectication and restrictive eating,
bulimic, and depressive symptoms (Fredrickson, Roberts, Noll, Quinn,
& Twenge, 1998; Noll & Fredrickson, 1998; Joiner, Wonderlich, Metalsky,
& Schmidt, 1995; McKinley, 1998; Stice, Hayward, Cameron, Killen, &
Taylor, 2000).
Health-damaging behaviors, such as drug and alcohol use, take a
signicant toll on ones health (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2004; National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2002). Sexual harassment
has been associated with the use of alcohol and prescription drugs
(e.g., sedatives, antidepressants; Zucker & Landry, 2005) and cigarettes
(Richman et al., 1999). This may indicate that some women may use
these substances to reduce stress, depression, anxiety, hostility, and a
perceived lack of control associated with experiences of discrimination
and harassment (Grunberg, Moore, & Greenber, 1998; Ragland &
Ames, 1996; Richman & Rospenda, 2005). Longitudinal analyses and
national samples of working adults (Rospenda, Richman, & Shannon,
2009) and female college students (Zucker & Landry, 2007) also demonstrated a relationship between harassment and alcohol misuse (Freels,
Richman, & Rospenda, 2005; Rospenda, Richman, Wislar, & Flaherty,
2000).
PHYSICAL HEALTH OUTCOMES RELATED TO
DISCRIMINATION AND HARASSMENT
In addition to psychological distress, gender discrimination and
sexual harassment are sufciently stressful to result in detriment to
ones physical health, such as stress-induced physiological changes,

Discrimination, Harassment, and Womens Physical and Mental Health 243

suppressed immune system functioning, inammation, gastrointestinal


problems, fatigue, headaches, sleep problems, and back pain (Cleary,
Schmieler, Parscenzo, & Ambrosio, 1994; Dansky & Kilpatrick, 1997;
Smith, 2006; van Roosmalen & McDaniel, 1998). Meta-analyses indicate
that a perceived lack of control, chronic stress, and threats tied to social
identity are important in increasing levels of cortisol, which is an indication of stress (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004; Michaud, Matheson, Kelly,
& Anisman, 2008; Miller, Chen, & Zhou, 2007). Bodily systems that
usually serve to adaptively cope with isolated stressors, like the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic arousal system
(Goldstein & McEwen, 2002; Sapolsky, Romero, & Munck, 2000), may
become overtaxed with chronic stressors like discrimination, resulting
in negative health outcomes (McEwen, 2000, 2003). In addition, pervasive discrimination and harassment may also lead one to engage in
behaviors that are counter to positive health outcomes, such as neglecting self-care and health sustaining behaviors (e.g., exercise, healthy eating, and regular sleeping patterns), which may result in a plethora of
health problems (Zucker & Landry, 2007).
Meta-analyses specic to harassment outcomes also support the
association between harassment and negative physical health indicators
(Cantisano et al., 2008; Chan et al., 2008). Similar to mental health, the
relationship between sexual harassment and physical health is
mediated by ones appraisal of the event and general psychological
well-being (Langhout et al., 2005). Namely, sexual harassment and
gender discrimination are associated with medical symptoms like gastrointestinal (stomach-ache, atulence, diarrhea, heartburn, stomach
pains), musculoskeletal (headaches, pain in joints, muscles, back, and
neck), irritability, sleeping problems (fatigue, restlessness, sleep disturbances), cardiovascular symptoms (chest pain, tachycardia), headache,
eye strain, skin problems, itching, and worry about health hazards
(Bergman, 2003; Bildt, 2005; Estrada & Berggren, 2009; Langhout et al.,
2005; Shipherd et al., 2009). These experiences are also associate
with persistent and chronic diseases, such as hypertension, neurological disorders, diabetes, cardiovascular diseases, gastritis, cataracts, etc.
(Keskinoglu, Ucuncu, Yildirim, Gurbuz, Ur, & Ergor, 2007).
Discrimination experiences and mood also inuence stress-reactive
physiological systems. As an example, women primed to feel angry or
sad during a lab-induced discrimination event experienced changes in
salivary cortisol levels (Matheson, Gill, Kelly, & Anisman, 2008). In
particular, when primed to feel angry, they experienced sustained
arousal levels throughout the discrimination experience and increases
in systolic and diastolic blood pressure and heart rate persisted after
the discrimination event ended. Extrapolating these ndings to real
world harassment and discrimination would suggest that negative,
repeated and unpredictable discrimination events raise cortisol levels

244

Mental and Physical Health

and arousal for extended periods of time, leading to negative health


consequences (McEwen, 2003). Particularly, those discrimination events
that evoke anger are associated with increases in heart rate and cardiovascular problems over time (Matheson et al., 2008). Thus, womens
experiences of discrimination and harassment can result in signicant
detriment to their overall physical health and well-being over time.
CONCLUSION
Across the globe, working women are subjected to gender discrimination and sexual harassment at work, and an abundance of evidence
supports that such experiences result in signicant negative consequences for a womans psychological well-being and physical health. With
more than 40 percent of women reporting at least one such experience
annually (DeSouza & Solberg, 2003), gender discrimination and sexual
harassment may be the most common occupational hazard faced by
working women today. The focus of this chapter has been on the negative effects of gender discrimination and sexual harassment on targeted
women; however, the nancial toll on companies is also extensive
(Sims, Drasgow, & Fitzgerald, 2005). Increased awareness of these negative outcomes is necessary to encourage organizations to improve prevention efforts and promote anti-discrimination legislation, resulting in
a more equitable work environment for all employees.
NOTE
1. Gender-based discrimination and sexual harassment are both perpetrated
against men and women, however behavioral assessments of harassment and
formal reports alleging both forms of discrimination and harassment are more
commonly reported by women than by men (EEOC, 2009); as such, we will
commonly refer to targets of harassment as women.

REFERENCES
Allport, G. (1979). The nature of prejudice. New York: Doubleday Anchor. (Original work published 1954)
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (4th ed). Washington, DC: Author.
Avina, C., & ODonohue, W. (2002). Sexual harassment and PTSD: Is sexual
harassment diagnosable trauma? Journal of Traumatic Stress, 15, 6975.
Backhouse, C., & Cohen, L. (1978). The secret oppression. Toronto: Macmillan.
Barker, E. T., & Galambos, N. L. (2003). Body dissatisfaction of adolescent girls
and boys: Risk and resource factors. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 23,
141165.
Berg, S. H. (2006). Everyday sexism and posttraumatic stress disorder in
women: A correlational study. Violence Against Women, 12, 970988.

Discrimination, Harassment, and Womens Physical and Mental Health 245


Bergman, B. (2003). The validation of the women workplace culture questionnaire: Gender-related stress and health for Swedish working women. Sex
Roles, 49, 287297.
Bergman, M. E., & Drasgow, F. (2003). Race as a moderator in a model of sexual harassment: An empirical test. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,
8, 131145.
Bergman, M. E., Langhout, R. D., Cortina, L. M., Palmieri, P. A., & Fitzgerald,
L. F. (2002). The (Un)reasonableness of reporting: Antecedents and consequences of reporting sexual harassment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87,
230242.
Bildt, C. (2005). Sexual harassment: Relation to other forms of discrimination
and to health among women and men. Work, 24, 251259.
Buchanan, N. T. (2005). The nexus of race and gender domination: The racialized sexual harassment of African American women. In P. Morgan &
J. Gruber (Eds.), In the company of men: Re-discovering the links between sexual
harassment and male domination (pp. 294320). Boston: Northeastern University Press.
Buchanan, N. T., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2008). The effects of racial and sexual harassment on work and the psychological well-being of African American
women. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 13, 137151.
Buchanan, N. T., & Ormerod, A. J. (2002). Racialized sexual harassment in the
lives of African American women. Women & Therapy, 25, 107124.
Buchanan, N. T., Settles, I. H., & Langhout, R. D. (2007). Black womens coping
styles, psychological well-being, and work-related outcomes following
sexual harassment. Black Women, Gender and Families, 1, 100120.
Cantisano, G. T., Dominguez, J. F. M., & Depolo, M. (2008). Perceived sexual
harassment at work: Meta-analysis and structural model of antecedents
and consequences. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 11, 207218.
Cartwright, D. (1950). Emotional dimensions of group life. In M. L. Raymert
(Ed.), Feelings and emotions (pp. 439447). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2004). Tobacco information and
prevention source: Overview. Retrieved August 23, 2004, from http://
www.cdc.gov/tobacco/issue.htm
Chan, D. K. S., Lam, C. B., Chow, S. Y., & Cheung, S. F. (2008). Examining the
job-related, psychological, and physical outcomes of workplace sexual
harassment: A meta-analytic review. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32,
362376.
Cleary, J. S., Schmieler, C. R., Parascenzo, L. C., & Ambrosio, N. (1994). Sexual
harassment of college students: Implications for campus health promotion.
Journal of American College Heath, 43, 310.
Cleveland, J. N., Vescio, T. K., & Barnes-Farrell, J. L. (2005). Gender discrimination in organizations. In R. L. Dipboye & A. Colella (Eds.), Discrimination
at work: The psychological and organizational bases (pp. 149176). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Cortina, L. M. (2004). Hispanic perspectives on sexual harassment and social
support. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 570584.
Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2003). Raising voice, risking retaliation: Events
following interpersonal mistreatment in the workplace. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 8, 247265.

246

Mental and Physical Health

Dambrun, M. (2007). Gender differences in mental health: The mediating role


of perceived personal discrimination. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
37, 11181129.
Dansky, B. S., & Kilpatrick, D. G. (1997). Effects of sexual harassment. In
W. ODonohue (Ed.), Sexual harassment: Theory, research, and treatment
(pp. 152174). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
DeSouza, E., & Fansler, A.G. (2003). Contrapower sexual harassment: A survey
of students and faculty members. Sex Roles, 48, 529542.
DeSouza, E., & Solberg, J. (2003). Incidence and dimensions of sexual harassment across cultures. In M. Paludi & C. Paludi (Eds.), Academic and workplace sexual harassment: A handbook of cultural, social science, management, and
legal perspectives (pp. 330). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol
responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research.
Psychological Bulletin, 130, 355391.
Dowler, K., & Arai, B. (2008). Stress, gender and policing: The impact of perceived gender discrimination on symptoms of stress. International Journal of
Police Science & Management, 10, 123135.
Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (1980). Guidelines on discrimination because of sex. Federal Regulations, 43, 7467674677.
Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (2009). Sexual harassment
charges EEOC & FEPAs combined. Retrieved June 13, 2009. http://
www.eeoc.gov/stats/harass.html
Estrada, A.X., & Berggren, A.W. (2009). Sexual harassment and its impact for
women ofcers and cadets in the Swedish Armed Forces. Military Psychology, 21, 162185.
Fineran, S., & Gruber, J. (2008). Mental health impact of sexual harassment. In
M. Paludi (Ed.), The psychology of women at work: Challenges and solutions for
our female workforce, Volume 3: Self, family, and social affects (pp. 89108).
Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.
Fischer, A. R., & Holz, K. B. (2007). Perceived discrimination and womens psychological distress: The roles of collective and personal self-esteem. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 54, 154164.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Buchanan, N. T., Collinsworth, L. L., Magley, V. J., & Ramos,
A. M. (1999). Junk logic: The abuse defense in sexual harassment litigation.
Psychology, Public Policy, and the Law, 5, 730759.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Drasgow, F., Hulin, C. L., Gelfand, M. J., & Magley, V. J.
(1997). Antecedents and consequences of sexual harassment in organizations: A test of an integrated model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 578
589.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Gelfand, M. J., & Drasgow, F. (1995). Measuring sexual harassment: Theoretical and psychometric advances. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17, 425427.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Shullman, S. L., Bailey, N., Richards, M., Swecker, J., Gold, Y.,
et al. (1988). The incidence and dimensions of sexual harassment in academia and the workplace. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 32, 152175.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Swan, S., & Magley, V. J. (1997). But was it really sexual harassment? Legal, behavioral, and psychological denitions of the workplace

Discrimination, Harassment, and Womens Physical and Mental Health 247


victimization of women. In W. ODonohue (Ed.), Sexual harassment: Theory,
research, and treatment (pp. 528). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Foster, M. D., & Dion, K. L. (2003). Dispositional hardiness and womens wellbeing relating to gender discrimination: The role of minimization. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27, 197208.
Frederickson, B., & Roberts, T. (1997). Objectication theory: Toward understanding womens lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 21, 173206.
Frederickson, B. L., Roberts, T. A., Noll, S. M., Quinn, D. M., & Twenge, J. M.
(1998). That swimsuit becomes you: Sex differences in self-objectication,
restrained eating, and math performance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 75, 269284.
Freels, S. A., Richman, J. A., & Rospenda, K.M. (2005). Gender differences in
the causal direction between workplace harassment and drinking. Addictive
Behaviors, 30, 14541458.
Glomb, T. M., Munson, L. J., Hulin, C. L., Bergman, M. E., & Drasgow, F.
(1999). Structural equation models of sexual harassment: Longitudinal
explorations and cross-sectional generalizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 1428.
Goldstein, D. S., & McEwen, B. (2002) Allostasis, homeostats, and the nature of
stress. Stress, 5, 558.
Green, B. L., Goodman, L. A., Krupnick, J. L., Corcoran, C. B., Petty, R. M.,
Stockton, P., et al. (2000). Outcomes of single versus multiple trauma exposure in a screening sample. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13, 271286.
Grunberg, L., Moore, S., & Greenberg, E. S. (1998). Work stress and problem
alcohol behavior: A test of the spillover model. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 19, 487502.
Harned, M. S. (2000). Harassed bodies. An examination of the relationships
among womens experiences of sexual harassment, body image, and eating
disturbances. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 336348.
Huerta, M., Cortina, L. M., Pang, J. S., Torges, C. M., & Magley, V. J. (2006).
Sex and power in the academy: Modeling sexual harassment in the lives of
college women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 616628.
Ilies, R., Hauserman, N., Schwochau, S., & Stibal, J. (2003). Reported incidence
rates of work-related sexual harassment in the United States: Using
meta-analysis to explain reported rate disparities. Personnel Psychology, 56,
607631.
Joiner, T. E., Wonderlich, S. A., Metalsky, G. I., & Schmidt, N. B. (1995). Body
dissatisfaction: A feature of bulimia, depression, or both? Journal of Social
and Clinical Psychology, 14, 339355.
Keskinoglu, P., Ucuncu, T., Yildirim, I., Gurbuz, T., Ur, I., & Ergor, G. (2007).
Gender discrimination in the elderly and its impact on the elderly health.
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 45, 295306.
Kessler, R. C. (2003). Epidemiology of women and depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 74, 513.
King, K. R. (2003). Racism or sexism? Attributional ambiguity and simultaneous memberships in multiple oppressed groups. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 33, 223247.

248

Mental and Physical Health

Klonoff, E. A., Landrine, H., & Campbell, R. (2000). Sexist discrimination may
account for well-known gender differences in psychiatric symptoms. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 9399.
Krupnick, J. L., Green, B. L., Stockton, P., Goodman, L. A., Corcoran, C. B., &
Petty, R. M. (2004). Mental health effects of adolescent trauma exposure in
a female college sample: Exploring differential outcomes based on experiences of unique trauma types and dimensions. Psychiatry: Interpersonal, and
Biological Processes, 67, 264279.
Landrine, H., & Klonoff, E. A. (1997). Discrimination against women: Prevalence,
consequences, remedies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Langhout, R. D., Bergman, M. E., Cortina, L. M., Fitzgerald, L. F., Drasgow, F.,
& Hunter Williams, J. (2005). Sexual harassment severity: Assessing situational and personal determinants and outcomes. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 35, 9751007.
Larkin, J., & Rice, C. (2005). Beyond healthy eating and healthy weights:
Harassment and the health curriculum in middle schools. Body Image, 2,
219232.
Larkin, J., Rice, C., & Russell, V. (1996). Slipping through the cracks: Sexual
harassment, eating problems, and the problem of embodiment. Eating Disorders, 4, 526.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York:
Springer.
Lim, S., & Cortina, L.M. (2005). Interpersonal mistreatment in the workplace:
The interface and impact of general incivility and sexual harassment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 483496.
Major, B., & Crocker, J. (1993). Social stigma: The consequences of attributional
ambiguity. In D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and
stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 345370). San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Major, B., Kaiser, C. R., OBrien, L. T., & McCoy, S. K. (2007). Perceived discrimination as worldview threat or worldview conrmation: Implications
for self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 1,0681,086.
Matheson, K., Gill, R., Kelly, O., & Anisman, H. (2008). Cortisol and cardiac
reactivity in the context of sex discrimination: The moderating effects of
mood and perceived control. The Open Psychology Journal, 1, 110.
McEwen, B. S. (2000). Allostasis and allostatic load: Implications for neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22, 108124.
McEwen, B. S. (2003). Mood disorders and allostatic load. Biological Psychiatry,
54, 200207.
McGonagle, K. A., & Kessler, R. C. (1990). Chronic stress, acute stress, and depressive symptoms. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 681706.
McKinley, N. M. (1998). Gender differences in undergraduates body esteem:
The mediating effect of objectied body consciousness and actual/ideal
weight discrepancy. Sex Roles, 19, 113123.
Mecca, S. J., & Rubin, L. J. (1999). Denitional research on African American students and sexual harassment. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 23, 813817.
Michaud, K., Matheson, K., Kelly, O., & Anisman, H. (2008). Impact of stressors
in a natural context on release of cortisol in healthy adult humans: A
meta-analysis. Stress, 11, 177197.

Discrimination, Harassment, and Womens Physical and Mental Health 249


Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Zhou, E. (2007). If it goes up, must it come down?
Chronic stress and the hypothalamicpituitaryadrenal axis in humans.
Psychological Bulletin, 133, 2545.
Moradi, B., & Funerburk, J. R. (2006). Roles of perceived sexist events and perceived social support in the mental health of women seeking counseling.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 464473.
Moradi, B., & Subich, L. M. (2002). Perceived sexist events and feminist identity
development attitudes: Links to womens psychological distress. The Counseling Psychologist, 30, 4465.
Moradi, B., & Subich, L.M. (2004). Examining the moderating role of selfesteem in the link between experiences of perceived sexist events and psychological distress. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 5056.
Murdoch, M., Polusny, M. A., Hodges, J., & Cowper, D. (2006). The association
between in-service sexual harassment and post-traumatic stress disorder
among Department of Veterans Affairs disability applicants. Military Medicine, 171, 166173.
National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2002). Use of selected substances in the past
month by persons 12 years of age and over, according to age, sex, race and Hispanic origin: United States, select years 197999. Retrieved April 12, 2002,
from http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/products/pubs/pubd/hus/tables/2001/
01hus063.pdf
Noll, S. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). A mediational model linking selfobjectication, body shame, and disordered eating. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 22, 623636.
Nosek, M. A., Howland, C., Rintala, D. H., Young, M. E., & Chanpong, G. F.
(2004). National study of women with physical disabilities: Final report.
Sexuality and Disability, 19, 540.
Patel, N. (2008). Racialized sexism in the lives of Asian American women. In
C. Raghavan, A. E. Edwards, & K. M. Vas (Eds.), Beneting by design:
Women of color in feminist psychological research (pp. 116128). Cambridge:
Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Pratt, C. C. (1997). Ageing: A multigenerational, gendered perspective. Bulletin
on Ageing, No. 2/3. New York: United Nations.
Ragland, D. R., & Ames, G. M. (1996). Current developments in the study of
stress and alcohol consumption. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental
Research, 20, 51A53A.
Rederstorff, J. C., Buchanan, N. T., & Settles, I. H. (2007). The moderating roles of
race and gender-role attitudes in the relationship between sexual harassment
and psychological well-being. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 5061.
Richman, J. A., Rospenda, K. M., Flaherty, J. A., Freels, S., & Zlatoper, K.
(2004). Perceived organizational tolerance for workplace harassment and
distress and drinking over time. Women & Health, 40, 123.
Richman, J. A., Rospenda, K. M., Nawyn, S. J., Flaherty, J. A., Fendrich, M.,
Drum, M. L., et al. (1999). Sexual harassment and generalized workplace
abuse among university employees: Prevalence and mental health correlates. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 358363.
Rospenda, K. M., Richman, J. A., & Shannon, C. A. (2009). Prevalence and mental health correlates of harassment and discrimination in the workplace:
Results from a national study. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 24, 819843.

250

Mental and Physical Health

Rospenda, K. M., Richman, J. A., Wislar, J. S., & Flaherty, J. A. (2000). Chronicity of sexual harassment and generalized work-place abuse: Effects on
drinking outcomes. Addiction, 95, 1,8051,820.
Sabik, N. J., & Tylka, T. L. (2006). Do feminist identity styles moderate the relation between perceived sexist events and disordered eating? Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 30, 7784.
Sapolsky, R. M., Romero, L. M., & Munck, A. U. (2000). How do glucocorticoids inuence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Reviews, 21, 5589.
Schmitt, M. T., Branscombe, N. R., Kobrynowicz, D., & Owen, S. (2002). Perceiving discrimination against ones gender group has different implications for well-being in women and men. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 28, 197210.
Schmitt, M. T., Branscombe, N. R., & Postmes, T. (2003). Womens emotional
responses to the pervasiveness of gender discrimination. European Journal
of Social Psychology, 33, 297312.
Settles, I. H. (2006). Use of an intersectional framework to understand Black
womens racial and gender identities. Sex Roles, 54, 589601.
Shipherd, J. C., Pineles, S. L., Gradus, J. L., & Resick, P. A. (2009). Sexual harassment in the Marines, posttraumatic stress symptoms, and perceived
health: Evidence for sex differences. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 22, 310.
Shrier, D. K., Zucker, A. N., Mercurio, A.E., Landry, L.J., Rich, M., & Shrier,
L.A. (2007). Generation to generation: Discrimination and harassment
experiences of physician mothers and their physician daughters. Journal of
Womens Health, 16, 883894.
Sims, C. S., Drasgow, F., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2005). The effects of sexual harassment on turnover in the military: Time-dependent modeling. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 90, 1,1411,152.
Smith, T. W. (2006). Personality as risk and resilience in physical health. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 227231.
Stice, E., Hayward, C., Cameron R. P., Killen, J. D., & Taylor, C. B. (2000). Bodyimage and eating disturbances predict onset of depression among female adolescents: A longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 438444.
Street, A. E., Gradus, J. L., Stafford, J., & Kelly, K. (2007). Gender differences in
experiences of sexual harassment: Data from a male-dominated environment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 464474.
Swim, J. K., Hyers, L. L., Cohen, L. L., & Ferguson, M. J. (2001). Everyday sexism: Evidence for its incidence, nature, and psychological impact from
three daily diary studies. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 3153.
Szymanski, D. M. (2005). Heterosexism and sexism as correlates of psychological distress in lesbians. Journal of Counseling & Development, 83, 355360.
Szymanski, D. M., & Owens, G. P. (2009). Group-level coping as a moderator
between heterosexism and sexism and psychological distress in sexual
minority women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 33, 197205.
Texeira, M. T. (2002). Who protects and serves me? A case study of sexual
harassment of African American women in one U.S. law enforcement
agency. Gender & Society, 16, 524545.
Thomas, A. J., Witherspoon, K. M., & Speight, S. L. (2008). Gendered racism,
psychological distress, and coping styles of African American women. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14, 307314.

Discrimination, Harassment, and Womens Physical and Mental Health 251


UNFPA (United Nations Population Fund). (2002). Population aging and development. Social, health and gender issues. Population and Development Strategies Series, Number 3. New York: UNFPA.
UNFPA (United Nations Population Fund). (2005). State of World Population
2005. The Promise of Equality. Gender equality, Reproductive Health and the
Millennium Development Goals. New York: UNFPA.
United Nations. (2005). The Millennium Development Goals Reports. New York:
Author.
Utsey, S. O., Ponterotto, J. G., Reynolds, A. L., & Cancelli, A. A. (2000). Racial
discrimination, coping, life satisfaction, and self-esteem among African
Americans. Journal of Counseling and Development, 78, 7281.
van Roosmalen, E., & McDaniel, S. A. (1998). Sexual harassment in academia:
A hazard to womens health. Women & Health, 28, 3354.
Vogt, D. S., Pless, A. P., King, L. A., & King, D. W. (2005). Deployment stressors, gender, and mental health outcomes among Gulf War I veterans.
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18, 115127.
Wheaton, B. (1997). The nature of chronic stress. In B. H. Gottlieb (Ed.), Coping
with chronic stress (pp. 4374). New York: Plenum.
Willness, C. R., Steel, P., & Lee, K. (2007). A meta-analysis of the antecedents
and consequences of workplace sexual harassment. Personnel Psychology,
60, 127162.
Woods, K. C., Buchanan, N. T., & Settles, I. H. (2009). Sexual harassment across
the color line: Experiences and outcomes of cross- vs. intra-racial sexual
harassment among Black women. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority
Psychology, 15, 6776.
Yoder, J. D., & Aniakudo, P. (1995). The responses of African American women
reghters to gender harassment. Sex Roles, 32, 125137.
Yoder, J. D., & Aniakudo, P. (1996). When pranks become harassment: The case
of African American women reghters. Sex Roles, 35, 253270.

This page intentionally left blank

Appendix: Feminist and Womens


Rights Organizations Worldwide
Susan Strauss
Michelle Strand
Michele A. Paludi

We have compiled a listing of resources dealing with feminism and


womens rights internationally. We believe this listing is a good starting point for seeking additional information about womens issues and
support groups on issues including women and mental health, physical
health, employed women, legal rights, political advocacy groups, reproductive health and rights, violence against women, and women and education. Please recognize that this listing is neither complete nor exhaustive.
GENERAL ORGANIZATIONS
African Feminist Forum
http://www.africanfeministforum.org
Association for Womens Rights in Development
http://www.awid.org
Center for Global Justice
http://www.globaljusticecenter.org
Centre for Development and Population Activities
http://www.cedpa.org
Equality Now
http://www.equalitynow.org

254

Appendix

Feminist Activist Resources on the Net


http://www.women.it
Feminist Majority Foundation: Global Feminism
http://feminist.org/global
Global Fund for Women
http://www.globalfundforwomen.org
Global List of Womens Organizations
http://www.distel.ca/womlist/womlist.html
International Alliance of Women
http://www.womenalliance.org/
International Womens Organizations
http://www.africa/upenn.edu/burundiwomen/international.htm
MADRE
http://www.madre.org
Ms. Foundation for Women
http://www.ms.foundation.org
National Council for Research on Women
http://www.ncrw.org
National Council of Womens Organizations
http://www.womensorganizations.org
National Organization for Women
http://www.now.org
PDHRE
http://www.pdhre.org/rights/women.html
Regional Network on Arab Women
http://www.aucegypt.edu/src
Sudan Womens Alliance
http://www.sarah@acs.aucegypt.edu
Third Wave Foundation
http://www.thirdwavefoundation.org
United Nations Commission on the Status of Women
http://www.un.org/womenwatch/daw/csw
United Nations Division for the Advancement of Women
http://www.un.org/womenwatch/daw/
United Nations Population Fund; Gender Equality
http://www.unfpa.org/public/publications/pubs_gender

Appendix

Women and Gender Studies Web Sites


http://libr.org/wss/wsslinks/
Women for Women International
http://www.womenforwomen.org
Women Peace and Security Network, Africa
http://www.ajws.org
Womens Human Rights Resources Programme
http://www.law-lib.utoronto.ca/diana
EMPLOYED WOMEN
All Business Network
http://www.all-biz.com
Association for Women in Science
http://www.awis.org
Business and Professional Womens Organization
http://www.bpwusa.org
Center for Leadership and Change Management
http://leadership.wharton.upenn.edu
Center for Women and Work, Rutgers University
http://www.cww.rutgers.edu
Center for Women and Work, University of Massachusetts, Lowell
http://www.uml.edu/centers/women-work
Centre for Families, Work and Well-Being
http://www.worklifecanada.ca
Center for Stress Management
http://www.managingstress.com
Employee Assistance Professional Association
http://www.eapassn.org
Equal Employment Opportunity Commission
http://www.eeoc.gov
Families and Work Institute
http://www.familiesandwork.org
Feminist Majority Foundation
http://www.feminist.org
International Network of Women Engineers and Scientists
http://www.inwes.org

255

256

Appendix

National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health


http://www.cdc.gov/niosh
National Institute of Mental Health
http://www.nimh.nih.gov
National Partnership for Women and Families
http://www.nationalpartnership.org
Occupational Safety and Health Administration
http://www.osha.gov
Ofce Politics
http://www.ofcepolitics.co.uk/frame.html
The Coaching and Mentoring Network
http://www.coachingnetwork.org.uk/
Third World Organization for Women in Science
http://www.twows.org
Work and Family Connection
http://www.workfamily.com
HEALTH
American Association of People with Disabilities
http://www.aapd-dc/org
American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities
http://www.aamr.org
American Psychological Association
http://www.apa.org
Black Womens Health
http://www.blackwomenshealth.com
Breast Cancer Information Network
http://www.cancernetwork.com
Canadian Womens Health Network
http://www.cwhn.ca
Disability Rights Education and Defense Fund
http://www.dredf.org
Disabled Peoples International
http://v1.dpi.org
EngenderHealth
http://www.engenderhealth.org

Appendix

Feminist Womens Health Center


http://www.fwhc.org
International Womens Health Coalition
http://www.iwhc.org
World Association of Persons with Disabilities
http://www.wapd.org
World Health Organization
http://www.who.int
World Institute on Disability
http://www.wid.org
LEGAL RIGHTS
The Legal Research and Resource Center for Human Rights
lrrc@frco.eun.eg
Women in International Law
http://www2lib.uchicago.edu/llou/women.html
POLITICAL ADVOCACY GROUPS
Center for American Women and Politics
http://www.cawp.rutgers.edu
Center for Asia-Pacic Women in Politics
http://www.capwip.org
Centre for Advancement of Women in Politics
http://www.qub.ac.uk/cawp/
Council of Women World Leaders
http://www.womenworldleaders.org
Emilys List
http://www.emilyslist.org
European Womens Lobby
http://www.womenlobby.org
International Womens Democracy Center
http://www.lwdc.org
League of Women Voters
http://www.lwv.org
National Foundation for Women Legislators
http://www.womenlegislators.org

257

258

Appendix

National Womens Council of Ireland


http://www.nwci.ie
National Womens Political Caucus
http://www.nwpc.org
Political Parties in Africa
http://www.idea.int/africa/pp.cfm
Women in Politics
http://www.ipu.org

REPRODUCTIVE HEALTH AND RIGHTS


American Pregnancy Association
http://www.americanpregnancy.org
Australian Reproductive Health Alliance
http://www.arha.org.au
Center for Reproductive Rights
http://www.reproductiverights.org
EC/UNFPA Initiative for Reproductive Rights in Asia
http://www.asia-initiative.org
Family Care International
http://www.familycareitl.org
Federation of Family Planning Associations Malaysia
http://www.ffpam.org.my
Fertility Awareness and Natural Family Planning
http://www.FertilityUK.org
Global Action Network
http://www.globalactionnetwork.org
Health Action Information Network
http://www.hain.org
Interact Worldwide
http://www.interactworldwide.org
International Womens Health Coalition
http://www.iwhc.org
MySistahs
http://www.mysistahs.org
National Healthy Mothers, Healthy Babies Coalition
http://www.hmhb.org

Appendix

259

National Latina Institute for Reproductive Health


http://www.latinainstitute.org
National Organization for Women
http://www.now.org
Pathnder International
http://www.pathnd.org
World Health Organization
http://www.who.int/reproductive-health/
VIOLENCE
American Bar Association Commission on Domestic Violence
http://www.abanet.org/domviol/home.html
American Domestic Violence Crisis Line
http://www.awoscentral.com
Antistalking Web Site
http://www.antistalking.com
A Safe Passage
http://www.asafepassage.info
Asian and Pacic Islander Institute on Domestic Violence
http://www.apiahf.org/apidvinstitute
Battered Womens Justice Project
http://www.bwjp.org
British Columbia Institute Against Family Violence
http://www.bcifv.org
Canadian Association of Rape Crisis Centres
http://www.casac.ca/english/avcentres/avcentres.htm
Canadian National Clearinghouse on Family Violence
http://www.hc-sc.gc.ca/hpb/family violence
Clearinghouse on Abuse and Neglect of the Elderly
http://db.rdms.udel.edu:8080/CANE/index.jsp
Coalition Against Trafcking of Women
http://www.catwinternational.org
College Violence
http://youthviolence.edschool.virginia.edu/violence-in-schools/college
campus.html
Communities Against Violence Network
http://www.cavnet2.org

260

Appendix

Domestic Violence Clearinghouse and Legal Hotline


http://www.stoptheviolence.org
Family Violence Prevention Fund
http://www.endabuse.org
HPP Earth: International Domestic Violence Information
http://www.hotpeachpages.net
International Society for Research on Aggression
http://www.israsociety.com
Management of Imminent Violence
http://www.psychiatry.ox.ac.uk/cebmh/guidelines/violence/violence_
full.html
Men Can Stop Rape
http://www.mencanstoprape.org
Nation to Nation: Promoting the Safety of Native Women
http://toolkit.ncjrs.org/default.htm
National Center for Missing and Exploited Children
http://www.missingkids.com
National Center for Victims of Crime
http://www.ncvc.org/ncvc/Main.aspx
National Center on Domestic and Sexual Violence
http://www.ncdsv.org
National Center on Elder Abuse
http://www.elderabusecenter.org
National Coalition Against Domestic Violence
http://www.ncadv.org
National Domestic Violence Hotline
http://www.ndvh.org
National Latino Alliance for the Elimination of Domestic Violence
http://www.dvalianza.org
National Network to End Domestic Violence
http://www.nnedv.org
National Organization for Men Against Sexism
http://www.nomas.org
National Organization for Women
http://www.now.org/index.html

Appendix

261

National Resource Center on Domestic Violence


http://www.nrcdv.org
National Sexual Violence Resource Center
http://www.nsvrc.org
New York Model for Batterer Programs
http://www.nymbp.org
Nursing Network on Violence Against Women International
http://www.nnvawi.org
Ofce of Violence Against Women, U.S. Department of Justice
http://www.ojp.usdoj.gov/vawo
Partnerships Against Violence Network
http://www.pavnet.org
Rape, Abuse and Incest National Network
http://www.rainn.org
Security on Campus
http://www.securityoncampus.org
Stop Family Violence
http://www.stopfamilyviolence.org
Tibet Justice Center
http://www.tibetjustice.org
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services
http://www.4women.gov/violence/index.cfm
U.S. Department of Justices Victims of Crime
http://www.usdoj.gov/crimevictims.htm
Violence Against Women
http://www.vaw.umn.edu
Violence Against Women in American Indian/Native American and
Alaska Native Communities
http://www.vawn.edu
Womenslaw
http://www.womenslaw.org
WOMEN AND EDUCATION
American Association of University Women
http://www.aauw.org

262

Appendix

Canadian Congress for Learning Opportunities for Women


http://www.nald.ca/litweb/other/cclow
Central European Centre for Women and Youth in Science
http://www.cec-wys.org
International Federation of University Women
http://www.ifuw.org/index.shtml

About the Editor and Contributors

Michele A. Paludi, PhD, is the Series Editor for Womens Psychology for
Praeger Publishers. She is the author/editor of 33 college textbooks and
more than 160 scholarly articles and conference presentations on sexual
harassment, campus violence, psychology of women, gender, and sexual
harassment and victimization. Her book, Ivory Power: Sexual Harassment
on Campus (1990) received the 1992 Myers Center Award for Outstanding
Book on Human Rights in the United States. Dr. Paludi served as Chair of
the U.S. Department of Educations Subpanel on the Prevention of Violence, Sexual Harassment, and Alcohol and Other Drug Problems in
Higher Education. She was one of six scholars in the United States to be
selected for this Subpanel. She also was a consultant to and a member of
former New York State Governor Mario Cuomos Task Force on Sexual
Harassment. Dr. Paludi serves as an expert witness for court proceedings
and administrative hearings on sexual harassment. She has had extensive
experience in conducting training programs and investigations of sexual
harassment and other EEO issues for businesses and educational institutions. In addition, Dr. Paludi has held faculty positions at Franklin & Marshall College, Kent State University, Hunter College, Union College, and
Union Graduate College, where she directs the human resource management certicate program. She teaches in the School of Management.
Janet Boyce graduated from Adirondack Community College in August
2006 with an Associate in Applied Science Degree in Business Administration. In May 2008 she graduated from SUNY Plattsburgh State University with a Bachelor of Science Degree in Management. She is currently
enrolled in the MBA and Human Resource Management Certicate programs at Union Graduate College, with an anticipated graduation date of
spring 2010.

264

About the Editor and Contributors

NiCole T. Buchanan, PhD, is an Associate Professor in the Department of


Psychology at Michigan State University (MSU) and a core faculty afliate in MSUs Center for Multicultural Psychology Research, Center for
Gender in Global Context, and the Violence against Women Research &
Outreach Initiative. Her research examines the intersection of race and
gender in harassment, racialized sexual harassment, health, coping, and
resilience among women of color. Dr. Buchanan received the 2008 International Coalition Against Sexual Harassment Researcher Award; the
2008 Carolyn Payton Early Career Award for research making a signicant contribution to the understanding of the role of gender in the lives of
black women; the Association of Women in Psychologys 2007 Women
of Color Award for empirical research contributions; Michigan State Universitys 2007 Excellence in Diversity Award in the category of Individual Emerging Progress for outstanding research and teaching
accomplishments in the areas of diversity, pluralism, and social justice;
and two Clinical Faculty Awards from the National Institutes of Health
(NIH). Representative publications include: The effects of racial and sexual harassment on work and the psychological well-being of African
American women, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology; Comparing
sexual harassment subtypes for Black and White women: Double jeopardy, the Jezebel, and the cult of true womanhood, Psychology of Women
Quarterly; Sexual harassment across the color line: Experiences and outcomes of cross- vs. intra-racial sexual harassment among Black women,
Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology; and Racialized sexual
harassment in the lives of African American Women, Women & Therapy.
Elder Cerqueira-Santos, PhD, received his PhD in psychology from Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil. He is currently adjunct professor at the Federal University of Sergipe, Brazil; consultant for the
World Childhood Foundation; and a member of the Center for at Risk
Children in Brazil. His main research interest is the relationship between
risk-taking behavior and religiosity among youth in Brazil. He is also
interested in sexual harassment and sexual development (gender and sexual orientation identity). As of August 2009, he has written six book chapters and 13 scholarly articles; he has also co-authored 72 papers presented
at conferences.
Joy Chien received her Bachelor of Science degree from Illinois State University.
Lillian Comas-Diaz, PhD, is the Executive Director of the Transcultural
Mental Health Institute, a clinical psychologist in private practice in
Washington, DC, and a Clinical Professor at George Washington University School of Medicine. The former director of the American Psychological Associations Ofce of Ethnic Minority Affairs, Dr. Comas-Diaz also

About the Editor and Contributors

265

directed the Yale University Department of Psychiatry Hispanic Clinic.


The author of more than a hundred publications, Dr. Comas-Diazs writings focus on womens issues, culture, ethnicity, social class, spirituality,
and creativity. She is the co-editor of the textbooks Clinical Guidelines in
Cross Cultural Mental Health; Women of Color: Integrating Ethnic and Gender
Identities in Psychotherapy; and WomanSoul: The Inner Life of Womens Spirituality. She currently serves as an Associate Editor of the American Psychologist.
Eros DeSouza, PhD, is currently a professor of psychology at Illinois State
University. He earned his PhD in community psychology from the University of Missouri at Kansas City. He has carried out qualitative and
quantitative research on sexuality and gender issues, including sexual orientation and sexual harassment from a cross-cultural perspective. As of
August 2009, he has written nine book chapters and more than 40 scholarly articles; he has also co-authored 115 papers presented at conferences.
Nancy Felipe Russo, PhD, is Regents Professor of Psychology and Womens Studies at Arizona State University. Founding director of the Womens Programs ofce of the American Psychological Association, she is
author or editor of more than 200 publications related to the psychology
of women and womens issues; current editor of the American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry; and a former editor of the Psychology of Women Quarterly.
Dr. Russo is a Fellow of the New York Academy of Sciences, the American
Psychological Association (including divisions: 1-General, 9-Social Issues,
26-History, 34-Population and Environment, 35-Psychology of Women,
38-Health, 45-Ethnic Minority Issues, and 52-International Psychology),
and the American Psychological Society. Dr. Russos involvement in
international issues as a leader, researcher, and policy advocate has
spanned four decades. In these roles she has worked to increase and
apply psychological knowledge related to womens lives and circumstances, and to forge links between scientists, policy makers, and the public
concerned with diverse womens mental health. In doing so she has been
a pioneer in the development of a multicultural feminist psychology of
women. She has been awarded the Distinguished International Psychologist Award by APAs Division of International Psychology, the DenmarkGunvald Award for signicant contributions to the psychology of women
and gender by the International Council of Psychologists, and the American Psychological Associations Award for Distinguished Contributions
to Psychology in the Public Interest. Other honors include a Carolyn
Wood Sherif Award and a Heritage Award for Contributions to Public
Policy from APAs Division 35. She has been identied among Trailblazing Women in Community Psychology by APA Division 27s Committee
on Women, received a Distinguished Career Award from the Association
for Women in Psychology, and was recognized by APAs Board of Ethnic
Minority Affairs for contributions to ethnic minority issues.

266

About the Editor and Contributors

Susan Fineran, PhD, is an associate professor at the University of Southern Maine School of Social Work and Women and Gender Studies. Her
professional career includes clinical experience in the areas of aging, substance abuse, child and family treatment, sex discrimination, and womens issues. Her research interests include peer sexual harassment and
bullying in schools and the mental health implications for children and
adolescents. Dr. Fineran joined the University of Southern Maine School
of Social Work in 2002 after teaching on the social work faculties of Boston
University and the University of Illinois at Chicago. Her education
includes an MSW (1981) from the Catholic University of America, and a
PhD (1996) from the University of Illinois at Chicago.
James Gruber, PhD, has published research on sexual harassment for 25
years. He was among the rst researchers to conduct studies of workplace
sexual harassment in the early 1980s and cross-national studies in the
1990s. His work on the experiences of women in male-dominated occupations resulted in a recent book, In The Company of Men: Male Dominance
and Sexual Harassment (2005), co-edited with Phoebe Morgan. Since 2002,
Drs. Gruber and Susan Fineran have presented conference papers and
published journal articles on bullying and sexual harassment among adolescents in schools. Two recent publications with Dr. Fineran studied that
the effects of bullying and sexual harassment at school: The Impact of
Bullying and Sexual Harassment on Health Outcomes of Middle and
High School Girls, published in Violence Against Women (2007), and
Comparing the Impact of Bullying and Sexual Harassment Victimization
on the Mental and Physical Health of Adolescents (Sex Roles, 2008). He
has also co-authored a publication in Child Abuse & Neglect (in press) that
examines the effects of workplace sexual harassment on adolescent girls.
Paula Lundberg-Love, PhD, is a professor of psychology at the University of Texas at Tyler (UTT) and the Ben R. Fisch Endowed Professor in
Humanitarian Affairs for 20012004. Her undergraduate degree was in
chemistry, and she worked as a chemist at a pharmaceutical company for
ve years prior to earning her doctorate in physiological psychology with
an emphasis in psychopharmacology. After a three-year postdoctoral fellowship in nutrition and behavior in the Department of Preventive Medicine at Washington University School of Medicine in St. Louis, she
assumed her academic position at UTT where she teaches classes in psychopharmacology, behavioral neuroscience, physiological psychology,
sexual victimization, and family violence. Subsequent to her academic
appointment, Dr. Lundberg-Love pursued postgraduate training and is a
licensed professional counselor. She is a member of Tyler Counseling and
Assessment Center, where she provides therapeutic services for victims
of sexual assault, child sexual abuse, and domestic violence. She has

About the Editor and Contributors

267

conducted a long-term research study on women who were victims of


childhood incestuous abuse, constructed a therapeutic program for their
recovery, and documented its effectiveness upon their recovery. She is
the author of nearly 100 publications and presentations and is co-editor of
Violence and Sexual Abuse at Home: Current Issues in Spousal Battering and
Child Maltreatment as well as Intimate Violence Against Women: When
Spouses, Partners, or Lovers Attack. As a result of her training in psychopharmacology and child maltreatment, her expertise has been sought as a
consultant on various death penalty appellate cases in the state of Texas.
Jennifer L. Martin is the department head of English at a public alternative high school for at-risk students in Michigan and a lecturer at Oakland
University where she teaches graduate research methods in the department of Educational Leadership, Feminist Methods and Introduction to
Women and Gender Studies in the Department of Women and Gender
Studies. She is not only a feminist teacher, but a feminist activist. She has
volunteered as an assault responder and engaged in political action for
feminist causes. Currently, she is the Title IX Education Task Force Chair
for the Michigan National Organization for Women to advocate for Title
IX compliance in Michigans schools. She has conducted research and
written articles on the topics of peer sexual harassment, teaching for social
justice, service learning, and the at-risk student.
Avigail Moor, PhD, is a feminist clinical psychologist who specializes in
the treatment of women in general and survivors of sexual violence in
particular. She heads the Women Studies program at Tel Hai College in
Israel and is also on the faculty of the Psychology and Social Work programs. In addition, she serves as a psychological consultant to several
rape crisis centers in Northern Israel. Her past and present research
focuses primarily on the social context of sexual violence against women
and its psychological sequelae. She has also written articles on treatment
issues with survivors of this type of violence. Other research interests concern the psychology of women and the effects of gender-based power
imbalances on womens mental health.
Kevin L. Nadal, PhD, is a professor, psychologist, performer, activist,
and author, who received his doctorate in counseling psychology from
Columbia University in 2008. As an assistant professor of mental health
counseling and psychology at John Jay College of Criminal JusticeCity
University of New York, he has published several works focusing on Filipino American, ethnic minority, and LGBTQ issues in the elds of psychology and education. He is the author of the book Filipino American
Psychology: A Handbook of Theory, Research, and Clinical Practice, and his
current research on microaggressions, or subtle forms of discrimination

268

About the Editor and Contributors

towards oppressed groups, has been published in the American Psychologist and other journals. He was named one of People Magazines hottest
bachelors in 2006, he was a guest on Fox News Channels The OReilly
Factor, and he has been featured on The Filipino Channel, the History
Channel, Philippine News, and Filipinas Magazine
Dorota Wnuk Novitskie is a third-year doctoral student in the Clinical
PhD program at Fairleigh Dickinson University. Her research interests
include cross-cultural studies, domestic violence/interpersonal violence,
and family violence.
Joy Rice, PhD, Clinical Professor of Psychiatry and Emerita professor of
Educational Policy Studies and Women Studies at the University of WisconsinMadison, received the Woman of the Year Award from the American Psychological Association (APA), Section for the Advancement of
Women in Counseling Psychology at the annual APA meeting in Boston
in August 2008, for signicant contributions and promotion of the status
of women in psychology, leadership and activism on behalf of women,
and research that has signicantly advanced knowledge of womens concerns in counseling psychology. Dr. Rices pioneering work on gender
issues in psychotherapy dates from the early 1970s when she began the
rst womens studies course on women and therapy at the University of
Wisconsin. Her 1973 paper in the American Journal of Psychiatry was one of
the rst articles to address key issues of gender discrimination in psychotherapeutic theory and practice. She was co-chair of the task force that
worked for seven years in developing the APA Guidelines for Psychological
Practice with Girls and Women. It was passed by APA and published in the
American Psychologist in 2007. Dr. Rice also co-chaired the APA Resolution
on Gender and Cultural Awareness in International Psychology passed by
APA in 2004. Active in state, federal, and international advocacy for mental health parity, she currently serves as co-chair of the Wisconsin Lieutenant Governors Task Force on Women and Depression and is the
International Council of Psychologists representative to the World Federation of Mental Health. Her most recent book, Women and Leadership:
Transforming Visions and Diverse Voices (John Wiley, 2007), explores feminist models of leadership that embrace collaboration, inclusion, and social
action. Dr. Rice is a recipient of the Educational Press Association Distinguished Achievement Award and an APA Fellow of four divisions.
Janet Sigal, PhD, is a Professor of Psychology at Fairleigh Dickinson
University. She received her PhD in Social Psychology from Northwestern University. She has more than 100 presentations, and several articles
and chapters primarily in the area of womens issues, including intimate
partner violence and sexual harassment. She is currently conducting a

About the Editor and Contributors

269

12-country study on perceptions of intimate partner violence (IPV). She


is a Fellow of the American Psychological Association and a member of
the APA UN Team.
Lindsey Speach graduated from Siena College in May 2007 with a BS in
Marketing Management and a minor in Sociology. She is enrolled in the
Certicate of Human Resources program at Union Graduate College and
plans to join the MBA program. Lindsey is currently the Recruiting Coordinator for the Northeast region of KeyBank N.A.
Michelle Strand received her Bachelor of Arts degree in Psychology and
Criminology in 2006 from Wilkes University in Wilkes-Barre, Pennsylvania. While at Wilkes, she also minored in Business Administration and
Sociology. Michelle was a student in Union Graduate Colleges Masters
in Business Administration program and graduated in June 2009. Her
specialization being Human Resources (HR), Michelle has been a co-op at
General Electric in the HR department since November 2007. It was there
that she was able to put her studies to real life situations, and became
more acquainted with a variety of aspects in the eld of business. Michelles co-op experience takes place within GEs manufacturing plant for
steam turbines and generators in Schenectady, New York. While there,
she has planned, designed, and implemented a salaried new hire orientation, assisted in interviewing and on-boarding 300 production positions,
coordinated work/life balance on-site seminars, along with many other
experiences in Human Resources. Michelle is a member of the Society for
Human Resources Management and the Capital Region Human Resources Association. She has attended several conferences around the country
to improve her business knowledge, including the National Association
for Women in MBA, and the International Coalition Against Sexual Harassment. She also took a 10-day tour of Shanghai, China, in December
2008 to learn about the countrys distinctive business strategies and developments. Michelle is also an avid volunteer in her local community. She
has helped build a house for the Habitat for Humanity, taught at an elementary school for Junior Achievement, and walked dogs at the Society
for the Prevention and Cruelty of Animals, including several other volunteering activities.
Susan Strauss, RN, EdD, is a national and international speaker, trainer,
and consultant. Her specialty areas include harassment and workplace
bullying, organization development, and management/leadership development. Her clients are from business, education, health care, law, and
government organizations from both the public and private sector. Dr.
Strauss has authored book chapters, articles in professional journals, written curriculum and training manuals, as well as authored the book, Sexual

270

About the Editor and Contributors

Harassment and Teens: A Program for Positive Change. Susan has been featured on The Donahue Show, CBS Evening News, and other television and
radio programs as well as interviewed for newspaper and journal articles
such as the Times of London, Lawyers Weekly, and Harvard Education Newsletter. Susan has presented at international conferences in Botswana,
Egypt, Thailand, Israel, and the United States, and conducted sex discrimination research in Poland. She has consulted with professionals from
other countries such as England, Australia, Canada, and St. Maarten.
Bethany Waits is a graduate student in clinical psychology at the University of Texas at Tyler. She anticipates graduation in May of 2010 and plans
to pursue the Licensed Professional Counselor designation upon completion of her degree. Ultimately, she would like to work as a therapist for a
nonprot community organization, such as a crisis center. In May 2007,
she obtained her BA in psychology from UT-Tyler, graduating Summa
Cum Laude. She was accepted as a lifetime member of Psi Chi, the
national honor society in psychology, and Alpha Chi, a national college
honor society. While an undergraduate, she participated in a student
panel addressing issues related to sexual assault on campus. In addition,
she has worked with several professors on various research projects.
Jessica Wilmot earned her BS in Business Administration from Le Moyne
College in Syracuse, New York, in 2007. Afterward she took some time
away from school and traveled to Europe to spend a few months volunteering in England at a Surrey Womens Aid, a not-for-prot womens
shelter. She then returned home to continue her education and is currently a MBA student at Union Graduate College in Schenectady, New
York. As well as her MBA, she is working on her HR Certicate with Dr.
Paludi and will be graduating June 2010. Currently Jessica is an intern at
General Electric on the Pension Team and has enjoyed learning about corporate America.
Krystle C. Woodss research examines the inuence of perpetrator race
on sexual harassment outcomes, racialized sexual harassment, and
depression in African American women. Ms. Woods is a clinical doctoral
candidate in the Department of Psychology at Michigan State University.
She was the 2005 recipient of the Michigan State University Enrichment
Fellowship recognizing academic achievement, research goals, contribution to a diverse educational community and a record of overcoming
obstacles.

Index

Abusive behavior, 67
Acquaintance rape, 144
Act scales, 68
Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (ASI),
78
American Association of University
Women (AAUW), 19596, 213, 217,
220
American Bar Association
Commission on Domestic
Violence, 124
American Institute on Domestic
Violence, 111
American Psychological Association
(APA), 2, 155, 186; guidelines for
psychological practice with
women, 1213, diversity, social
context and power, 1213, practice
applications, 13, professional
responsibility, 13
American public education system,
231; compensatory education in,
233; inuence of the Eugenics
Movement, 23233
APA Resolution on Cultural and
Gender Awareness in International
Psychology, 1012, 14; awareness
and analysis of power, 11; critical
analysis of western perspective,
1112; experience of individuals
in diverse cultures, 11;

interdisciplinary socialcultural
perspective, 12; respect for
pluralism based on differences, 11
Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent
Medicine, 67
Assessment, ethnocultural, 31;
heritage stage, 29; niche stage, 30;
saga stage, 29; self-adjustment
stage, 30; womens relations
and, 30
Association for Women in
Psychology, 2
Atividade (activity), 180, 181
Baby boomers, 209
Ban Ki Moon, 65, 6667, 94
Beijing Conference, 2
Behavioral responses of sexual
violence, 5557
The Bell Curve, 232
Benevolent sexism, 16263, 188
Bicha, 180
Bofe, 181
Boyce, Janet views on feminism,
20910
Brazil, 177; homosexuality in, 178,
18083; laws and resolution
against sexual harassment in, 18688;
sexism in, 18889; sexual
harassment in, 199; sexual script in,
180; stereotypes about transsexuals,

272

Index

18586; violence against sexual


minorities in, 18386
Brazilian Federal Association of
Medicine, 186
Brazilian Federal Association of
Psychology, 186
Brazilian Health Ministry, 18788
Bullying, 2245; denition, 211;
differences between sexual
harassment and, 212; global
prevalence of, 21517;
perpetration of, 217; in U.S.
schools, 21315
Bullying: victimization and peer
sexual harassment: children with
disabilities, 21920; gender, 21718;
health and school outcomes, 2212;
race and ethnicity, 2201; sexual
orientation, 21819
Bureau of Justice Special Report on
Intimate Partner Violence, 67
Burlington Industries, Inc. v. Ellerth, 111
Canada, IPV in, 8990
Canadian Psychological Association
(CPA), 1314
Cardoso, Fernando Henrique, 182,
187
Center for Epidemiological
Studies-Depression Scale
(CES-D), 90, 91
Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, 111, 11213
Child sexual abuse (CSA), 46
Chile, IPV in, 89
China, IPV in, 8183
Civil Rights Act of 1964, 209, 236
Cognitive empathy, 31
Cognitive theories of stress, 240
Collective self-esteem, 239
Comer, 180
Commission on the Status of
Women, 65
Comorbidity of mental disorders, 6
Conict Tactics Scale, 89
Consensual relationship policy, 1213
Continuous Performance Test (CPT),
53

Convention on the Elimination of


Discrimination against Women
(CEDAW), 77
Covert sexism, 159
Critical consciousness, 27
Cultural empathy, 32
Cultural genogram, 35
Dar, 180
Dating violence, 65, 66; anger
response, 75; classic models of
teen, 6869; comparison between
IPV and, 9698; denitions, 6768;
ethic enforcement, 7576; factors
affecting, 69, gender power
differential, 6970, inuence of
peers, 7071, lack of experience,
70; rst-time aggression response,
75, 76; international studies on,
7173; outcomes, 74, effect of
abusive and neglectful childhood,
7475, mental health effects, 74;
patriarchal terrorism, 75;
perpetrators of, 75; recommendations
and programs for, 7677; theoretical
model of, 69; in United States, 67
Declaration on the Elimination of
Violence Against Women, 77
Department of Labor, 210
Depression and women, 79
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders (1973), 44, 186, 241
Dial Corporation, 236
Discrimination and harassment, 244;
harming psychological and
physical health, 23738; mental
health outcomes related to, 23841;
physical health outcomes related
to, 24244; specic forms of mental
illness, 24142
Disparate impact, 236
Disparate treatment, 236
EEOC v. Pand Enterprises, Inc., 222
EEOC v. The Dial Corporation, 236
Empathy, 31
Employees Assistance Program
(EAP): intimate partner violence

Index
and, 1234; recommendations for,
109
Empowerment and ethnocultural
psychotherapy, 278
Engaged couples, violence in, 7374
Equal Employment Opportunity
Commission (EEOC), 111, 161, 222, 223
Ethnocultural consciousness, 3637
Ethnocultural occupational inventory,
3536
Ethnocultural psychotherapy, 25, 38;
applications, 289; assessment,
2931; case studies, 256; concepts,
267; empowerment as aim of,
278; ethnogender consciousness
and, 3637; identity reformulation
and, 28; phases in process of,
3334; therapeutic relationship and,
3132; tools, 3436; transference
and countertransference, 3233
Ethnocultural transference, 32
Eugenics Movement, 232, 233
Everyday sexism, 159
Family Crisis Services, 107
Faragher v. City of Boca Raton, 111
Feminist consciousness, 239
Feminist movement, 2
Feminist psychotherapy, 1417; APA
guidelines and, 1617; CPA
principle and, 1617; empowerment
and language of empowerment, 15;
goals of psychotherapy for women,
15; principles of feminist therapy,
14142; psychological practice for
women, 15
Feminist therapy, 3, 140; denition,
141; tenets of, 14142; for treating
survivors of rape and sexual
assault, 14344
Feminization of poverty, 9
Filipina domestic female worker,
sexual harassment and, 199
Floare de Colt, 95
Frutinha, 181
Gay, Lesbian and Straight Education
Network (GLSEN), 218

273

Gender: denition, 4, 178;


discrimination, 23637, 238;
international perspective of, 45;
versus sex, 178
Gendered ethnicity, 26
Gender Equity Index, 78, 97
Gender harassment, 161, 198, 237
Gender microaggressions, 16365; age
and, 169; awareness of, 172; in
clinical practice, 16973; denition,
158; examples of, 16667; forms of,
16568, assumptions of inferiority,
165, 166, denial of individual
sexism, 165, 167, denial of reality
of sexism, 165, 167, environmental
gender microaggressions, 165, 167,
invisibility, 165, 166, sexual
objectication, 165, 166, traditional
gender roles, assumptions of, 165,
167, use of sexist language, 165,
167; intersections of, 16869;
research on, 173; sexual
harassment and, 16062; sexism in
everyday life and, 15860;
understanding types of, 165
Grupo Gay da Bahia, 183
Guidelines for Ethical Psychological
practice with Women, 1314
Guidelines on multicultural
education, training, research,
practice, and organizational
change for psychologists, 155
Hall, Stanley G., 232
Hernstein, Richard J., 232
Hollingworth, Leta, 232
Homicide, 104
Homosexuality in Brazil, 18083
Hong Kong, IPV in, 8183
Hostile environment sexual
harassment, 197, 212, 236
Hostile sexism, 162, 163, 188
Hunter, George William, 233
Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
(HPA), 52, 54
Identity reformulation and
ethnocultural psychotherapy, 28

274

Index

India, IPV in, 8384


Inheriting Shame: The Story of Eugenics
and Racism in America, 23132
International Consensus Statement on
Womens Mental Health, 2
International Counseling Psychology
Conference, 14
International Dating Violence Study,
72, 74
international studies on IPV, 80; in
Canada, 8990; in China, 8183; in
Hong Kong, 8183; in India, 8384;
in Japan, 83; in Kenya, 8687; in
Middle East, 8788; in Nigeria,
8586; in Peru, 8889; in Turkey,
8485; in Zambia, 8687
International womens movement, 3
Interpersonal physical violence, 71
Interpersonal Sexual Objectication
Scale (ISOS), 162
Interpersonal violence, 211
Intimate partner violence (IPV), 41,
54, 65, 66; comparison between
dating violence and, 9698;
denition, 42, 7778, 127; EAP
assisting women victims of,
10910; employers notice of, 112;
examples of, 1034; impact of,
abuse on children, 108, on
employees, 1068, on the
workplace, 11011; international
studies on, 8081; models of,
7880, culture of honor, 7980,
feminist power, 78,
hypermasculinity, 78, patriarchal
culture, 79; myths and realities,
1278; outcomes for victims of,
9091; perpetrators of, 9192;
prevalence of, 105; primary
prevention strategies: additional
human resource responsibilities,
1213, EAP and, 1234, employees,
training program for, 117, intimate
partner violence in workplace,
11314, managers, training
program for, 11416, needs
assessment, 119, pedagogical
techniques, 11719, policies and
procedures, 11213, posttraining

evaluations, 1191, threat


assessment plan, training program
for, 11617; recommendations to
eliminate, 9295, efforts to stop
violence against women, 9495,
international agreements for rights
of women, 9293, safe
environment for women, creating,
93; resources on, 13132;
secondary and tertiary prevention,
1245; women fear of leaving
violent relationship, 1089; in
workplace, 1056
Intra-ethnic transference, 3233
IPV. See intimate partner violence
(IPV)
Japan, IPV in, 83
Jordan, IPV in, 8788
Kenya, IPV in, 8687, 91
Kirkpatrick Model, 120
Lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender,
and questioning (LGBTQ),
177, 179; prejudice and
determination, 17980; violence
against, 18386
Machismo, 184
MacKinnon, Catharine (Katherine
McKinnon), 143, 212
Maine Department of Labor, 107
Massachusetts Youth Risk Behavior
Survey, 218
Mental health: denition, 2; issues
and women, 57; outcomes related
to discrimination and harassment,
23841
Mental Health Index (MHI), 201
Microagressions: denitions of, 156;
racial, 15658; research on, 156
Microassaults, 15657, 161, 166, 168
Microinsults, 156, 157, 158, 161, 168
Microinvalidations, 156, 15758, 161,
168
Middle East, IPV in, 8788
Mujerismo, 37
Murray, Charles, 232

Index
National Co-morbidity Survey, 6, 51
National Crime Survey of the
Department of Justice, 109
National Crime Victimization Survey,
213, 214
National Education Association
(NEA), 232
National Institute of Child Health
and Human Development, 213, 221
Nationality/citizenship as cause for
sexual harassment, 199200
National Women Study, 49, 56
Neurological responses of sexual
violence, 5255
The New Ideal in EducationBetter
Parents of Better Children, 232
Nigeria, IPV in, 8586
Objectication theory, 162
Obsessive compulsive disorder
(OCD), 45
Occupational Safety and Health
Association (OSHA), 113, 11617
Olweus, Dan, 211
Oorganizational responses of sexual
violence, 58, 61; legal system,
5859; medical services, 5960;
mental health services, 60
Overt sexism, 159
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task
(PASAT), 53
Partnership for Prevention, 125
Passividade (passivity), 180, 181
Peer sexual harassment, 217; bullying
victimization and: children with
disabilities, 21920, gender,
21718, health and school
outcomes, 2212, race and
ethnicity, 2201, sexual orientation,
21819; denition, 197; in
international students and
U.S.-born students, study on,
200201; conclusion from the study,
204; hostile sexual harassment, 202;
limitations and future directions,
2045; psychological well-being of
students, 2012; results and
discussion, 203

275

Peer victimization and disabled


children, 21920
Perceived past control, 48
Personalized Safety Plan for victims
of IPV, 11314, 129
Peru, IPV in, 8889
Physical responses of sexual violence,
49; gastro intestinal problems,
50; gynecological disorders,
50; physical injuries, 49;
sexually transmitted diseases
(STDs), 49; sleep disorders,
5051
Possuir, 180
Post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD),
43, 44, 47, 52, 53, 139, 140
Psychological responses of sexual
violence, 42, 48; child sexual
abuse (CSA), 46; coping strategies,
4647; depressive disorder,
4344; fear and anxiety, 43;
mental pollution and, 4445;
negative social reactions
and, 47
Putnam, Helen, 232
Quid pro quo sexual harassment,
19697, 212, 236
Racial microggressions, 156;
microassaults, 15657;
microinsults, 156, 157, 158;
microinvalidations, 156,
15758
Rape, social context of, 143;
psychological sequelae of, 14546;
survivors: cognitive restructuring,
151, egalitarian therapeutic
relationship, 14749,
empowerment, 14951,
treatment principles, 14647
Rape crisis centers, 42
Rape Culture, 143
Rape myths, 14344
Revised Conict Tactics Scale, 82
Rio de Janeiro, 182, 184,
187, 189
Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (RSES),
2012

276

Index

Sample personalized safety plan, 129


Sample policy on intimate partner
violence as a workplace concern,
1278
Sample training program for
managers, 12931
S~ao Paulo, 182, 189
Schedule of Sexist Events (SSE), 160
School bullies, 211
Secondary victimization rape, 144
Selden, Steven, 23132
Self-blame, 13940
Self-objectication theory, 242
Sex, denition, 178
Sexism in brazil, 18889
Sexual coercion, 161, 198, 237
Sexual Experiences Questionnaire
(SEQ), 161, 19798, 202
Sexual harassment, 2245; of
adolescent at work, 2224;
denition, 16061, 196, 211, 212,
23637; demographics related to
incidence of, 198200; differences
between bullying and, 212;
dimensions of, 198; eating
pathology and, 242; gender
microaggressions and, 16162;
global prevalence of, 21517;
hostile environment, 197; negative
effects of, 238; peer, 197; quid pro
quo, 19697; substance abuse and,
242; types of, 19697; in U.S.
schools, 21315
Sexual identity, 178
Sexually transmitted diseases (STDs),
49
Sexual orientation, 17879
Sexual violence: denition, 41;
feminist analysis of, 143; social
context of rape, 14344; women
and, 41, behavioral responses,
5557, neurological responses,
5255, organizational responses,
5760, physical responses,
4952, psychological responses,
4248
Social Dysfunction Rating Scale, 57
Social economic status as cause for
sexual harassment, 198, 199

Society for Human Resource


Management, 11213
Sodomy, 186
Substance abuse and sexual violence,
56
Subtle sexism, 159
Suicidal ideation, 74
Teen dating violence, 6869
Title IX, 209
Trail Making Test, 53
Training programs, to prevent
intimate partner violence: for
employees, 117; human resource
responsibilities, 1213; for
managers, 11416, 129; needs
assessment in, 119; pedagogical
techniques, 11718; for threat
assessment team, 11617
Transactional Model of Stress,
237
Transsexualism, 179
Transvestism, 179
Traumatic brain injury (TBI),
5253, 55
Turkey, IPV in, 8485, 93
Uganda, mental illness in, 18
United Arab Emirates, mental health
care in, 18
United Nations Fourth World
Conference on Women, 1
United Nations International
Conference on Women, 105
United States: dating abuse in, 67;
feminist psychologists in, 23;
peer sexual harassment in, 197,
200; prevalence of bullying in
schools of, 21315; prevalence
of sexual harassment in
schools of, 21315;
sexual harassment in, 195
Unwanted sexual attention, 161, 198,
237
U.S. Armed Forces, 240
U.S. Department of Education: Ofce
for Civil Rights, 196
U.S. Department of Education School
Crime Supplement, 213

Index
Vencer, 180
Victim precipitation as rape myth, 144
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III
(WAIS-III), 53
Where there is no psychiatrist: A mental
health care manual, 18
White Ribbon Campaign, 94
Williams v. Saxby, 236
Women: depression and, 79; mental
disorder and, 57; psychological
practice guidelines for, 914;
psychological risk factors for, 8, 9;

277

sexual violence and, 41, 61,


behavioral responses, 5557,
neurological responses, 5255,
organizational responses, 5760,
physical responses, 4952,
psychological responses, 4248
World Federation of Mental
Health, 2
World Health Organization (WHO),
1, 6, 66, 78, 92, 94
World Psychiatric Association
(WPA), 2
Zambia, IPV in, 8687

This page intentionally left blank

Feminism and Womens


Rights Worldwide

Recent Titles in
Womens Psychology
Intimate Violence against Women: When Spouses, Partners, or Lovers Attack
Paula K. Lundberg-Love and Shelly L. Marmion, editors
Daughters of Madness: Growing Up and Older with a Mentally Ill Mother
Susan Nathiel
Psychology of Women: Handbook of Issues and Theories, Second Edition
Florence L. Denmark and Michele Paludi, editors
WomanSoul: The Inner Life of Womens Spirituality
Carole A. Rayburn and Lillian Comas-Diaz, editors
The Psychology of Women at Work: Challenges and Solutions for Our Female
Workforce
Michele A. Paludi, editor

Feminism and Womens


Rights Worldwide
Volume 3
Feminism as Human Rights

MICHELE A. PALUDI, EDITOR


Praeger Perspectives
Womens Psychology
Michele A. Paludi, Series Editor

PRAEGER
An Imprint of ABC-CLIO, LLC

Copyright 2010 by Michele A. Paludi


All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced,
stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any
means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise,
except for the inclusion of brief quotations in a review, without prior
permission in writing from the publisher.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Feminism and womens rights worldwide / Michele A. Paludi, editor.
v. ; cm. (Womens psychology)
Includes bibliographical references and index.
Contents: The myth of the man-hating feminist / Melinda Kanner and
Kristin J. Anderson Gender differences : the arguments regarding abilities /
Jennifer L. Martin Women in education : students and professors worldwide
/ Susan Basow In womens voices / Samantha Smith Working life as a
house : a tale of oors, walls, and ceilings / Leanne Faraday-Brash Women
as religious leaders : advances and stalemates / J. Harold Ellens The
feminine political persona : Queen Victoria, Ellen Johnson Sirleaf, and Michelle
Bachelet / Emily A. Haddad and William Schweinle Women in the military : is
it time to un-gender combat roles? / Breena E. Coates Sexual minority
women : sources and outcomes of stigmatization / Rhonda M. Schultz, and
Kristin P. Beals Special issues for women with disabilities / Martha E.
Banks Body dissatisfaction and disordered eating : the globalization of
western appearance ideals / Jaehee Jung and Gordon B. Forbes Sexual
violence to girls and women in schools around the world / Susan Strauss.
ISBN 978-0-313-37596-5 (set : hard copy : alk. paper) ISBN 978-0-31337597-2 (set : ebook) ISBN 978-0-313-37598-9 (v.1 : hard copy : alk. paper)
ISBN 978-0-313-37599-6 (v.1 : ebook) ISBN 978-0-313-37600-9 (v.2 : hard
copy : alk. paper) ISBN 978-0-313-37601-6 (v.2 : ebook) ISBN 978-0-31337602-3 (v.3 : hard copy : alk. paper) ISBN 978-0-313-37603-0 (v.3 : ebook)
1. Feminism. 2. Womens rights. 3. Sexual harassment of women. 4. Abused
womenPsychology. 5. WomenPsychology. I. Paludi, Michele Antoinette
HQ1180.F424 2010
305.42dc22
2009035343
ISBN: 978-0-313-37596-5
EISBN: 978-0-313-37597-2
14 13 12

11 10

2 3 4 5

This book is also available on the World Wide Web as an eBook.


Visit www.abc-clio.com for details.
Praeger
An Imprint of ABC-CLIO, LLC
ABC-CLIO, LLC
130 Cremona Drive, P.O. Box 1911
Santa Barbara, California 93116-1911
This book is printed on acid-free paper
Manufactured in the United States of America

For Rosa and Lucia, my maternal and paternal grandmothers


and for Antoinette, my mother:
Remember, our heritage is our power; we can know ourselves and
our capacities by seeing that other women have been strong.
Judy Chicago

This page intentionally left blank

Contents

Series Introduction

ix

Acknowledgments

xi

Introduction
Michele A. Paludi
Chapter 1:
Chapter 2:

Global Feminism
Anne Sisson Runyan
Womens Human Rights and Empowerment in a
Transnational, Globalized Context: Whats
Psychology Got To Do with It?
Shelly Grabe

xiii
1

17

Chapter 3: Missing Children and Child Abductions: An


International Human Rights Issue
Michele A. Paludi and Katie L. Kelly

47

Chapter 4: Women and Sexuality: An International Perspective


Donna Castaneda and Monica Ulibarri

81

Chapter 5: Arranged and Forced Marriage


Noorfarah Merali

101

Chapter 6: Womens Reproductive Rights: An


International Perspective
Joan C. Chrisler and Cynthia Garrett

129

Chapter 7: Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International


Perspectives
Michele A. Paludi, Jennifer L. Martin, Carmen
Paludi, Jr., Sarah Metzgar Boggess, Kristina Hicks,
and Lindsey Speach

147

viii
Chapter 8:
Chapter 9:

Contents

Rape: A Global Perspective


Michelle McKenzie and Patricia Rozee

177

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women


Joseph Solberg

209

Chapter 10: Preventing Abuse of Young Girls and Women


Ayelet Giladi

239

Chapter 11: In Womens Voices


Lindsay Baker

251

About the Editor and Contributors

253

Index

259

Series Introduction

Because womens work is never done and is underpaid or unpaid or boring


or repetitious and were the rst to get red and what we look like is more
important than what we do and if we get raped its our fault and if we get
beaten we must have provoked it and if we raise our voices were nagging
bitches and if we enjoy sex were nymphos and if we dont were frigid and
if we love women its because we cant get a real man and if we ask our
doctor too many questions were neurotic and/or pushy and if we expect
childcare were selsh and if we stand up for our rights were aggressive
and unfeminine and if we dont were typical weak females and if we
want to get married were out to trap a man and if we dont were unnatural and because we still cant get an adequate safe contraceptive but men
can walk on the moon and if we cant cope or dont want a pregnancy were
made to feel guilty about abortion and . . . for lots of other reasons we are
part of the womens liberation movement.
Author unknown, quoted in The Torch, September 14, 1987

These sentiments underlie the major goals of the Praeger Perspectives


book series, Womens Psychology. The goals are as follows:
Value women: The books in this series value women by valuing children and working for affordable child care; value women by respecting
all physiques, not just by placing value on slender women; value
women by acknowledging older womens wisdom, beauty, aging; value
women who have been sexually victimized and view them as survivors;
value women who work inside and outside of the home; and value
women by respecting their choices of careers, of whom they mentor, of
their reproductive rights, their spirituality, and their sexuality.
Treat women as the norm. Thus the books in this series make up for
womens issues typically being omitted, trivialized, or dismissed from
other books on psychology.

Series Introduction

Take a non-Eurocentric view of womens experiences. The books in this


series integrate the scholarship on race and ethnicity into womens psychology, thus providing a psychology of all women. Women typically
have been described collectively; but we are diverse.
Facilitate connections between readers experiences and psychological theories and empirical research. The books in this series offer readers opportunities to challenge their views about women, feminism, sexual
victimization, gender role socialization, education, and equal rights.
These texts thus encourage women readers to value themselves and
others. The accounts of womens experiences as reected through
research and personal stories in the texts in this series have been
included for readers to derive strength from the efforts of others who
have worked for social change on the interpersonal, organizational,
and societal levels. A student in one of my courses on the psychology
of women once stated:
I learned so much about women. Women face many issues: discrimination, sexism, prejudices . . . by society. Women need to work together to
change how society views us. I learned so much and talked about much
of the issues brought up in class to my friends and family. My attitudes
have changed toward a lot of things. I got to look at myself, my life, and
what I see for the future. (Paludi, 2002)

It is my hope that readers of the books in this series will also reect
on the topics and look at themselves, their own lives, and what they
see for the future. This three-volume book set on Feminism and Womens Rights Worldwide provides readers with the opportunity to accomplish this goal and offers suggestions for all of us working for
gender justice within our friendships and romantic relationships, in
guiding institutional and social policy change in workplace and educational institutions, and in lobbying state and federal legislators on
issues related to reproductive rights, pay equity, education, sexual violence, and childcare.
Michele A. Paludi
Series Editor
REFERENCE
Paludi, M. (2002). The psychology of women. 2nd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.

Acknowledgments

Teaching and writing are separate, but serve/feed one another in so many
ways. Writing travels the road inward, teaching, the road outhelping
OTHERS move inwardit is an honor to be with others in the spirit of
writing and encouragement.
Naomi Shihab Nye

Nyes sentiment is echoed throughout this three-volume set on feminism and womens rights. Most of the contributors have taught courses
in womens studies and feminism as well as conducted research and
written about feminist issues. Many contributors have been advocates
on behalf of feminist principles through working with local, state and
federal agencies, legislators, and the United Nations. And many of us
have collaborated with students in our classes in writing chapters for
this book set. These students have made us believe that all of them, in
their individual ways, will continue to do what this book set intends:
value feminism and work toward equality. It has been exhilarating for
me to see a new generation of feminists collaborating with mentors
and colleagues on the chapters for this book set.
I have been honored to have collaborated with the contributors to
these volumes. Several friendships with contributors have been
rekindled and strengthened, and I have met many new colleagues from
around the world who taught me about their disciplines through their
writing. You have all shown me the great accomplishments of feminists
as well as the work we have yet to do. Thank you.
I wish to thank my sisters, Rosalie Paludi and Lucille Paludi, for
their support during the preparation of this book set. I also thank Carmen Paludi, Jr. for his guidance and encouragement. Our discussions
about feminism brought back wonderful memories of my mother,

xii

Acknowledgments

Antoinette, and my father, Michael, about whom I continue to learn


and continue to cherish the time I had with them.
I acknowledge several friends who encouraged me during the preparation of this set of books. Thank you to Paula Lundberg Love, Jennifer Martin, Billie Wright Dziech, Darlene C. DeFour, and Florence
Denmark.
I have been fortunate to have had the opportunity to work with students throughout my career, now at Union Graduate College. I have
thoroughly enjoyed learning from them. Thank you to students in the
Human Resource Management Certicate Program and Management
and Leadership Certicate Program. I especially acknowledge Michelle
Strand, Carrie Turco, Haimanot Kelbessa, Sarah Bennett, Sarah Boggess, Kristina Hicks, James Luciano, Sarah Henderson Maneely, Abbey
Massoud-Tastor, Marie Fuda, Jessica Wilmot, Katie Kelly, and Nick Salvatoriello. I am honored you have called me your professor.
I also thank Debbie Carvalko for supporting my visions for books
and helping them become realities. I have enjoyed working with Debbie and her colleagues at Praeger. They are a wonderful team of caring
people. They appreciate my love of writing and editing books. Debbie
somehow knew that, after the publication of the three-volume set on
the Psychology of Women and Work (2008, Praeger), which I edited, and
the political climate of the 2008 presidential campaign, especially
regarding women, I had to follow up those texts with books on feminism. She knows I share Sheila Benders sentiment:
We write because something inside says we must and we can no longer ignore
that voice.

Introduction
Michele A. Paludi
And how do you look backward? By looking forward. And what do you see?
As they look forward, they see what they had to do before they could look
backward. And there we have it all.
Gertrude Stein

Alyssa Zucker and Abigail Stewart (2007) reported in their study of


333 university alumnae that feminism is internalized quite differently
depending on the developmental stage in our lives. This research led
me to consider my own feminist socialization and feminist identity development as I began writing and editing these three volumes on feminism and womens rights. I was introduced to feminism by my
parents, Antoinette and Michael, at a very young age, even though the
label feminism was not used by them. Yet, as I came to realize much
later, their behavior was very much in keeping with feminist principles. They valued my sisters and me unconditionally; wanted to give
us educational opportunities that were denied to them because of the
generation into which they were born and because they were rst generation Americans whose parents had other values to instill in them;
they worked for equality in relationships, politics, and health care. I
was 18 the year individuals became eligible to vote at age 18, and both
my parents took me to cast my votes that year.
They believed that, like them, I had a responsibility to make things
better for the next generation. They valued voting; I was told what the
Suffragists had endured in order to win this right for us and to remember this each year I vote. I took my rst course in feminism as an
undergraduate in the early 1970s: Sex Roles in American Society
with Nancy Walbek. I would share the class discussions with my
mother, telling her about the experiences of students in class that were
different from my ownfor example, being denied the use of certain

xiv

Introduction

toys considered sex inappropriate for them; being tracked into different high school and college programs because of being women or men;
women being told by family and friends to hide their achievements
from potential dates and mates. I was unable to relate to these experiences and realized for the rst time that my parents were feminists, a
term to which I was introduced formally in this class and then subsequently as a graduate student when I took courses with Dee Graham
and Edna Rawlings. I also learned that I had been exposed to nonstereotyped role models, and because there were all girls in our family, we
were not raised to conform to stereotyped behavior.
It was in graduate school that I decided to pursue research in feminist psychology, especially in womens career development. I was fortunate to have a mentor, William Dember, who encouraged me to
pursue this research, even though it was not in his area of specialization (i.e., visual perception). Bill encouraged me to take courses with
faculty in departments in addition to psychology: educational leadership and family development. He told me this would help put pieces
together in understanding the research I was conducting. I thank Kathy
Borman and Judy Frankel for their roles in my feminist identity development.
A few years later when my father died, Charlie, who attended my
fathers wake, came to my mother, my sisters, and me and told us how
my father had impacted his life. Charlie, an African American man,
told us my father was the only coworker (both were skilled workers at
General Electric) who treated him fairly, didnt talk with him in a derogatory manner, and stopped others from making racial slurs and epithets. I learned for another time what it meant to be a feminist.
I dedicated the three-volume set on the Psychology of Women at Work
to my parents: For Antoinette and Michael Paludi, who encouraged
me to dene what womens work is for myself. They wanted all their
daughters to be independent thinkers and doers and to help others.
They gave us no templates to follow but encouraged us to navigate our
own paths. And, especially in my case, encouraged me to leave home
to attend graduate school in a city that seemed, to my parents, to be
very far awaybut they never said no.
My parents thus taught me that not only did they believe in the economic, educational, social, and political equality of women and men, but
they favored the social and legal changes necessary to achieve equality
between the sexes and among races, and they were committed to implementing these principles. Perhaps they could not effect change at the
national level, but they did do so in personal relationships with their
family and friends and on the local level. This is the legacy they left my
sisters and me. This book set is a tribute to Antoinette and Michael.
I have been reminded of Antoinette and Michael throughout the
writing and editing of these volumes on feminism and womens rights.

Introduction

xv

I am especially reminded of what my mother used to tell me: You are


there before you get there. She knew I wanted equality to happen fast
and that I grew concerned when feminists didnt win political elections, when younger women didnt know the heritage of how they
came to be accepted in graduate programs and in certain jobs, how the
glass ceiling for women and people of color is still strong, and that
worldwide, women constitute 64 percent of all adults who are illiterate
(see Susan Basows chapter in Volume 1). I have learned that she was
right; that change takes time, and to measure change differently, i.e., in
increments. As Secretary of State Hillary Clinton stated as she suspended her campaign for president of the United States in 2008:
Although we werent able to shatter this highest, hardest glass ceiling
this time, thanks to you, its got about 18 million cracks in it, and the
light is shining through like never before.
The chapters in these volumes show us where the light is shining
through on feminism. All three volumes represent what Judith Lorber
(1998) and Snelling (1999) identied: several types of feminism and
feminists. Lorber (1998) categorized feminism into three major areas:
gender reform, gender resistance, and gender rebellion. Genderreform feminism emphasizes similarities between women and men
rather than focusing on differences between them. Gender-resistance
feminism holds that formal legal rights alone will not end gender
inequality; male dominance is too ingrained into social relations.
Gender-resistance feminism focuses on how men and women are
differentcognitively, emotionally, and sociallyand urges women
to form women-centered organizations and communities. Genderrebellion feminism looks at the interrelationships among inequalities
of sex, race, ethnicity, social class, and sexual orientation. A number
of years ago my text on the psychology of women displayed a quilt
on its cover (Paludi, 2002). I asked for this design to highlight
Gentrys (1989) image of quilt making for understanding feminism.
These three volumes on feminism and womens rights also represent
quilt making in understanding feminism. Each contributor has made
one piece of the quilt that has been joined with pieces by other contributors. Each of the contributors has used different stitching on their
piece of the quilt. No one chapter is more important than the other.
We need all pieces if we are to complete the quilt that is feminism.
According to Gentry (1989):
Feminist psychology and feminism in general seem to be at the point of
trying to piece together the individual parts of a quilt. The overall pattern of the quilt that we want is still emerging. No one knows what
equality in a post-patriarchal world will look like. We are beginning to
piece the separate parts togetherto explore the kinds of stitching to use
in connecting the pieces and how to place the separate pieces into the

xvi

Introduction
pattern. But we have not stopped questioning the process of quilting
itself.

In Volume 1, Heritage, Roles, and Issues, contributors have discussed


efforts to integrate feminist scholarship into several disciplines, including education, work, science, military, religion, and politics. As Catherine Stimpson (1971) noted, there have been three kinds of problems in
the disciplines and curriculum with respect to women: omission, distortions, and trivializations. Each of the contributors to Volume 1 notes
where the sexism in the disciplines has existed and where feminist correctives have restructured the disciplines. Jennifer Martin, in her chapter concerning gender differences in abilities, noted:
Women have made signicant social, academic, and occupational gains
in the past 50 years; for example, women are entering nontraditional
elds with more frequency, participating in high school and college
sports more than ever before, and carving out more egalitarian roles for
themselves within the family. However, women have still not ultimately
achieved true equity with their male counterparts. . . . The idea that
women somehow possess different or inferior aptitudes when compared
to their male counterparts can lead to diminished expectations for
womenin terms of how they view themselves and how others view
them.

In Volume 2, Mental and Physical Health, contributors deal with violence and discrimination against girls and women and the resulting
impact on womens emotional and physical well being, interpersonal
relationships, career development, and self-concept. Types of discrimination and victimization addressed are sexual harassment, sexual violence, harassment of sexual minorities, and rape and violence in the
context of womens HIV risk. Contributors have addressed these issues
globally. Bethany Waits and Paula Lundberg-Love offer new cutting
edge evidence on neurological responses in women victims of sexual
violence. Therapeutic support for women victims of violence is also
addressed in this volume, including feminist therapy and ethnocultural
psychotherapy.
All contributors note that sexual victimization is prevalent in the
United States and globally, as is sexual harassment and sexual orientation discrimination. As Waits and Lundberg-Love note:
Female survivors of sexual violence are everywhere. They are in universities, religious institutions, court rooms, hospitals, and the military. They
are daughters, mothers, spouses, sisters, friends, next-door neighbors,
and co-workers. Many differ in age, education, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. . . . However, their lives are connected by the violence that
they have experienced.

Introduction

xvii

The international focus on feminism and womens rights is continued in Volume 3, Feminism as Human Rights. In this volume, contributors address laws on sexual harassment, pay equity, and rape.
Furthermore, contributors speak to the injustices to women with disabilities. Human rights issues such as arranged and forced marriage
for women, pornography, and the globalization of western appearance
ideals are also presented in this volume. All contributors to this volume call for further advocacy on behalf of women. As Noorfarah Merali stated:
It is only if arranged marriages are understood in light of their intentions, diverse forms, actual outcomes, and local or international contexts
that laws, policies, and human rights advocacy can be appropriately
channeled to protect and preserve womens well-being.

In addition to the scholarly reviews of research on feminism and


womens rights, I have included womens personal accounts of their
own feminist identity development. They are at different stages in life,
in their career, and in relationships and yet they are bound by shared
stories.
It is my hope that these volumes encourage individuals to self identify as feminists. Research has suggested for some time that most people reject the term feminist when describing themselves but support
feminist principlesequal pay for equal work, for example (see Paludi
et al., Volume 3). Goldners (1994) study noted that when women who
hold feminist beliefs anticipate a negative reaction from their peers to
the label feminist, they will avoid using the term to describe themselves. Goldner indicated that media is a primary source of negative
images of feminists. It is common to see photos of women identied as
feminists having clenched sts. These images are not representative of
feminists. More recent research by Rudman and Fairchild (2007) found
that the stereotype that feminists are unattractive still persists.
However, these images are rejected by individuals, especially during
adolescence and young adulthood, when maintaining gender role stereotypic behavior is reinforced and is central to their self-esteem and
self-concept. Paludi, Paludi, and DeFour (2004) noted that individuals
reject the label feminist because they view themselves as in control, as
powerful rather than as victims of gender inequality. Thus, they perceive the term feminist to imply a powerless position, which they
reject (Rhode, 1977).
The contributors to each of the three volumes of Feminism and Womens Rights Worldwide encourage us to think critically about feminism,
to value cultural experiences and to integrate our knowledge of theories and research about feminism with our own life experiences. The
chapters encouraged me to do this in remembering my own feminist

xviii

Introduction

socialization. I encourage you to do the same. It is my hope these three


volumes serve as a life raft (Klonis, Endo, Crosby, and Worell, 1997)
for feminists, especially those in the millennial generation.
REFERENCES
Gentry, M. (1989). Introduction: Feminist perspectives on gender and thought:
Paradox and potential. In M. Crawford & M. Gentry (Eds.), Gender and
thought. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Goldner, M. (1994). Accounting for race and class variation in the disjuncture
between feminist identity and feminist beliefs: The place of negative
labels and social movements. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of
the American Sociological Association, Los Angeles.
Klonis, S., Endo, J., Crosby, F., & Worell, J. (1997). Feminism as life raft. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 333345.
Lorber, J. (1998). Gender inequality: Feminist theories and politics. Los Angeles:
Roxbury.
Paludi, M. (2002). The psychology of women. 2nd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Paludi, M., ed. (2008). The psychology of women at work: Challenges and solutions
for our female workforce. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Paludi, M., Paludi, C., & DeFour, D. (2004). Introduction: The more things
change, the more they stay the same. In M. Paludi (Ed.), Praeger guide to
the psychology of gender. xixxxi. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Rhode, D. (1997). Speaking of sex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rudman, L., & Fairchild, K. (2007). The F word: Is feminism incompatible with
beauty and romance? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 125136.
Snelling, S. (1999). Womens perspectives on feminism. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 23, 247266.
Stimpson, C. (1971). Thy neighbors wife, thy neighbors servants: Womens liberation and black civil rights. In V. Gornick & B. Moran (Eds.), Woman in
sexist society: Studies in power and powerlessness. New York: Basic Books.
Zucker, A., & Stewart, A. (2007). Growing up and growing older: Feminism as
a context for womens lives. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 137145.

Chapter 1

Global Feminism
Anne Sisson Runyan

Global feminism has increasingly been associated with contemporary


international feminist nongovernmental (NGO) campaigns to make gender policymaking inroads into intergovernmental organization (IGO)
policymaking with impacts on national policymaking. Although the
United Nations (UN) Decade for Women (19751985) laid the groundwork for these initiatives, they have accelerated since the Fourth World
Conference on Women in Beijing in 1995 that produced the Beijing Platform for Action (BPA). Also responsible for the increase in gender
policymaking at international and national levels is the rise of global
governance, particularly in the post-Cold War era at a time when economic globalization, or the spread of global capital, was surging. Global
governance has come to refer to a constellation of global actors including
states; IGOs such as the UN bodies and agencies and such international
nancial institutions (IFIs) as the World Bank, the International Monetary Fund (IMF), and the World Trade Organization (WTO); regional
governments such as the European Union (EU) and regional IGOs such
as organizations of African, American, and Asian states and free-trade
regimes like the North American Free Trade Agreement (NAFTA);
global market actors such as transnational corporations (TNCs); and
global civil society actors such as transnational NGOs (Rai, 2008). States
make up IGOs, and the wealthiest and most militarily powerful states
hold primacy in them. At the same time, states are variously subordinated to IGO rules made by state representatives with increasing input
by NGO and other non-state actors, which range from international laws
and regulations to agreed-upon norms. The greater openings for nonstate actors and the seeming diffusion of power that global governance
signies has created a political opportunity structure that seems open

Feminism as Human Rights

for feminism, particularly as it takes up the discourses of human rights


and development (Ferree, 2006, p. 10).
However, the rise of global governance that has engendered global
feminist campaigns for gender policymaking is also associated with
the rise of neoliberalism, a free market ideology that subsumes all
life activities and life forms under the dictates of market efciency.
It has also provided openings for a variety of neoconservative non-state
actors hostile to feminism, particularly in relation to reproduction and
sexuality issues, but also in relation to feminist critiques of racism, neocolonialism, militarism, and capitalism. The twin forces of neoliberalism and neoconservativism have produced signicant concerns about
global feminisms entanglement with and cooptation by these forces
among those who have developed and/or identied with the alternative conceptualization of transnational feminism, which refers to
feminisms resistant to the colonizing and imperializing moves of neoliberal and neoconservative forces, including those feminisms co-opted
by or allied with them.
Thus, this treatment of global feminism not only provides a discussion of the some of the impact it has had on global policymaking,
which, in turn, has affected national policymaking, but also raises some
transnational feminist critiques of the relationship between global feminism and the imperialist priorities of neoliberal and neoconservative
forces. It will conclude with some thoughts on how this relationship is
being resisted and how it can be minimized.
GLOBAL FEMINISM AND GENDER POLICYMAKING
Although the 20082009 report of on the worlds women issued by
the UN Development Fund for Women (UNIFEM) shows only modest
gains on most measures of womens status worldwide, as well as
serious setbacks in some regions of the world in terms of womens
well-being due to war, economic policies and instabilities, and neo- or
religiously conservative regimes, setbacks that are no doubt being exacerbated and spread more widely by the current worldwide nancial
and economic crisis, much has changed in terms of the attention
brought to the issue of gender in the international community, especially over the past two decades. As IGOs became more signicant
actors in terms of affecting state behavior with the rise of global governance, global feminist attention began to focus on womens representation in these bodies. In 1990, the UN Economic and Social Council
(ECOSOC) supported the goal lobbied for by womens movements to
have 30 percent of world decision-making positions held by women by
1995 (Dahlerup, 2006). In 1995, the UN committed itself to increasing
the number of women on its professional staff to 30 percent by 1990,
which it achieved in 1991. In 1998, the General Assembly further

Global Feminism

committed to the 50/50 goal of gender parity in all posts in the


UN system (UN, 2000), but this is far from being achieved, despite a
global 50/50 campaign launched by a U.S.-based, global NGO, Womens Environment and Development Organization (WEDO), to gain
gender parity in national and international decision-making bodies
by 2020. According to UNIFEM, as of 2006, women remain concentrated in the lowest levels of the UN professional staff (2008), and it is
primarily UN agencies with social welfare portfolios and/or that
have been headed by women that have reached the parity zone of
40 percent women on professional staffs (UNIFEM, 2008, p. 105).
Although progress toward gender decision-making parity, especially
at the highest levels of IGOs, is slow and uneven, the UN has developed a substantial gender equality architecture, which includes the
Ofce of the Special Adviser on Gender Issues (OSAGI), the UNIFEM,
the United Nations Division for the Advancement of Women
(UNDAW), and the International Institute for Research and Training of
Women (UN-INSTRAW) along with gender units within many UN
agencies. However, these tend to be comparatively weak in terms of
staff and resources. For example, in 2003, UNIFEM had only 47 core
staff compared with UNICEF with 2,794 (UNIFEM, 2008). To attempt
to remedy this, WEDO and the U.S.-based Center for Womens Global
Leadership, in conjunction with 90 other global and regional feminist
NGOs headquartered in the global North and global South, spearheaded the Gender Equality Architecture Reform or GEAR campaign
to call for a consolidated UN agency for women that would combine
the OSAGI, UNDAW, and UNIFEM ofces, be headed by a new
under-secretary general, and garner signicantly more resources in
terms of staff and budget as a result of the increased status of such an
agency. In September 2008, the UN General Assembly adopted a resolution instructing the UN secretary-general to develop a paper outlining the options and making recommendations for best enhancing the
UN gender equality architecture.
Such IGO norm-setting in terms of womens formal and structural
representation brought about by global feminist NGO campaigns has
also inuenced member states in terms of the adoption of gender quotas (to get more women into elective ofce) and gender mainstreaming
(designed to avoid building gender discrimination into any government policy). Various forms of gender quotas to get women into
national public ofce have been instituted in at least 90 countries
(Dahlerup, 2006). As of 2005, only 16 percent of the worlds parliamentarians were women; however, 17 countries had reached or exceeded
the 30 percent target, with post-genocide Rwanda topping the list at
48.5 percent (and in 2008 becoming the rst country with more female
than male legislators), followed by the Nordic countries, and a mix of
Latin American and other northern European and African states

Feminism as Human Rights

(Dahlerup, 2006). Almost all of these had legal or party gender quota
systems in combination with proportional representation systems.
Although quota systems vary in form and efcacy, they were specically promoted in the BPA, which was unanimously supported by
the worlds governments, as the fast-track way to increase womens political representation. Following that conference, in Latin America
alone, 11 out of 19 governments had adopted either constitutional or
legal gender quotas by 2000 (Ara
ujo and Garcia, 2006). According to
UNIFEMs Progress of the Worlds Women Report 2008/2009, the
proportion of women in national assemblies has accelerated over the
past decade from 11.6 percent in 1995 to 18.4 percent as of May 2008,
compared with only a one percentage point gain between 1975 and
1995 (2008, p. 21). UNIFEM accounts for this relatively dramatic rise by
pointing primarily to the increased use of legally required or voluntary
quota systems and proportional representation electoral systems, practices which in some cases were stimulated by gender equality advocates working at international and national levels who have used the
BPA for leverage and by the adoption in 2000 of the UN Millennium
Development Goals (MDGs), which include Goal 3 that promotes
gender equality and womens empowerment (UNIFEM, 2008).
There are many reasons for this recent contagion of gender quotas, but among them is a growing international consensus or norm,
advocated by womens movements worldwide and supported by
feminist scholarship, that gender equality in the form of womens
greater political representation, ideally to the point of parity with
men, is necessary for policies claiming or aspiring to be modern and
democratic.
Gender mainstreaming also gained momentum and increasing acceptance during the same period that gender quotas were advancing.
Although denitions vary somewhat, gender mainstreaming refers generally to integrating the principle of gender equality into any (inter)
governmental policy (not just those associated with so-called womens
issues, such as family and violence against women) to ensure that in
practice it does not, wittingly or unwittingly, increase or sustain
inequalities between women and men (Squires, 2007). It was rst advocated in the context of economic development policies where it was
found by feminist research from the 1970s onward that approaches
taken by bodies like the World Bank, such as the promotion of capitalintensive agriculture for export, tended to privilege men who had or
were given more access to capital, agricultural inputs and machinery,
and land ownership. Women, although heavily involved in subsistence
agriculture that is the main source of family food consumption, were
not seen as farmers or landowners and, thus, did not benet from this
kind of World Bank funding. This disparity not only increased mens
power over women in agricultural work and families, but also

Global Feminism

contributed to producing more hunger and malnutrition when womens work of subsistence farming was increasingly so devalued and
unsupported. The World Bank and a number of other supranational
institutions, ranging from the UN and the Organization for Economic
Cooperation and Development (OECD) to the European Union (EU), as
well as many development agencies within states in the North, have
been convinced by such ndings to adopt gender mainstreaming, also
called for in the BPA, to try to avoid such outcomes (Squires, 2007).
There have been numerous downsides to this approach, but its institutionalization represents a sea change in its recognition that gender is
infused in all (world) political issues and legitimizes the need for gender experts in global governance and in national governments, often
taking the form of femocrats who have headed up womens policy
agencies that sprang up during the UN Decade for Women in many
countries and who are often charged with implementing gender mainstreaming at national and international levels.
No less than the UN Security Council has also been pressured by
global feminists to give some attention to gender. For example, Security
Council Resolution 1325, passed in 2000, calls for women to be present
at peace negotiating tables, a goal long advocated by womens peace
movements that have claimed women have greater interests and different stakes in ending war. It is not that women are inherently more
peaceful, but rather their predominantly civilian status means that they
are increasingly bearing the high structural costs of modern warfare.
With the rise of total wars in the twentieth century, wherein there is
little separation between the battlefront and the home front, civilians
are an increasing proportion of those left homeless, diseased, and
hungry, turned into refugees, and made victims of sexual and domestic
assault (by enemy and friendly combatants). Although combatants
still die in greater numbers from direct violence, civilian deaths
from direct violence and more often indirect violence continue to rise
(Goldstein, 2001). The majority of civilians are women and children
(both female and male), despite some increases in women in state and
non-state militaries and in child soldiers (both male and female)
pressed into combat. Women, as can be seen in the case of post-genocide Rwanda, also are critical to the mending of post-conict societies,
as they tend to be the backbone of civil society, most centrally recreating households and communities.
Although part of a longer and wider struggle for womens security
from militarized violence, an NGO Working Group on Women and
International Peace and Security, consisting of the century-old Womens International League for Peace and Freedom (WILPF), the Womens Caucus for Gender Justice, Amnesty International, International
Alert, the Womens Commission for Refugee Women and Children,
and the Hague Appeal for Peace in collaboration with UNIFEM, is

Feminism as Human Rights

credited with the nal push to have the UN Security Council pass
Resolution 1325 in 2000 (Cohn, 2008). Although nonbinding, 1325
calls on the UN and member countries to do the following: protect
women from gender-based violence in war zones and include women
(and gender perspectives) in peace negotiations, support their peacemaking initiatives in addition to providing gender-sensitive training to
peacekeepers, and engage in gender mainstreaming through UN monitoring of and reporting on the gender dimensions of conict and conict resolution, including the impact of armed conict on women and
girls and the roles of women in peacemaking.
Although 1325 was hailed as a breakthrough document, its effects
were muted at best. Widespread rapes, such as in the Democratic
Republic of Congo conict as well as in the Darfur genocide in Sudan,
and increased reports of rape by peacekeeping forces were featured in
subsequent UN secretary-general studies and reports on women, peace,
and security that were mandated by 1325. This acknowledgment of
worsening gender violence in war led to the passage of UN Security
Council Resolution 1820 in 2008. Although also nonbinding, the text of
1820 reafrms the principles of 1325, but stresses sexual violence as
a war crime, demands its cessation and no amnesty for its perpetrators, requests a policy of zero tolerance for sexual violence by UN
peacekeepers and in UN refugee camps, and requests consultation
with women and womens organizations to nd solutions to sexual violence.
Predating these Security Council resolutions was a signicant recognition and prosecution of rape as a war crime and crime against
humanity, now codied in the Rome Statute of the International Criminal Court, following the highly visible use of systematic rape in the
early 1990s in the wars in the former Yugoslavia and in Rwanda. Systematic wartime rape not only neutralizes women as threats, but also
seeks to weaken mens resolve to ght by soiling their women while
also trying to wipe out an enemy culture or ethnicity by impregnating
women with alien seed or keeping them from reproducing altogether. The assumption that rape was merely a natural spoil of war
(for men) had kept it from being fully recognized as an international
war crime until feminist activists and events in Bosnia and Rwanda
made it clear that rape was a direct violation of womens human
rights, rising to the level of torture as an instrument of warfare.
The ideas that women have human rights and that womens human
rights expand traditional denitions of human rights were signicantly
advanced by the UN Convention on the Elimination of All Forms of
Discrimination against Women (CEDAW), initially adopted in 1979 following the UN Decade for Women and going into force in 1981. By
2000, only 25 countries (including most glaringly the United States, as
well as a smattering of Muslim and least-developed countries) had

Global Feminism

failed to ratify CEDAW, making it the second most widely ratied


human rights convention (UN, 2000). As of 2009, only eight countries,
including the United States, remain non-signatories, although the
Obama administration supports CEDAW and is seeking Congressional
approval for it. Through CEDAW and subsequent UN conferences on
human rights, particularly throughout the 1990s, womens movements
and NGOs made the case that womens rights are human rights,
achieving international recognition that reproductive and, to some
degree, sexual rights were just as important as and connected to political and economic rights. As long as women are denied choices about
if, when, and under what conditions they bear children or terminate
pregnancies; are subject to sexual and domestic abuse; and are limited
in their sexual expressions and orientations, they will not be able to
exercise their political and economic rights. Although womens and
other human rights continue to be violated on a massive scale, the
widespread ratication of CEDAW has given womens movements
throughout much of the world a major tool through which to hold
their governments accountable for continued abuses.

TRANSNATIONAL FEMINIST CRITIQUES OF GLOBAL


FEMINISM
Although these global feminist gains are signicant and even astonishing given the relative speed with which they have been taken up
over the past few decades by powerful institutions, global feminism is
not without problem. Postcolonial feminist cultural theorists, in particular, have critiqued global feminism for what, they argue, is its
implication in a Western radical feminist notion of shared, worldwide
gender oppression, mostly in the form of assaults on womens bodies,
as its basis. This is productive of a missionary model of feminism,
with its emphasis on cataloguing violence against womens bodies and
situating the most barbaric forms of it in the Third World from
which women, particularly there, must be saved. Grewal and Kaplan
(1994) posited instead the idea of transnational feminist practices
that resist scattered hegemonies, which are not reducible to an overarching conception of gender oppression and take multiple forms at
multiple levels and in multiple contexts, and result from multiple sources. Most importantly, transnational practices, they argue, should resist
imperializing forces that sustain center and periphery relations of
any kind, and recommend, for example, that a truly transnational feminist practice for Western feminists would be to protest their own governments imperial actions that sustain repressive political regimes in
the global South rather than seeking to save Third World women
from their patriarchal cultures.

Feminism as Human Rights

They further use the term, transnational, to signal that there has
been a shift from international relations between states and between
social movements when national boundaries were assumed to be less
permeable to an environment in which transnational capital and institutions and their global hegemonies are ascendant. In this sense, they
argue, transnational feminism is different from earlier forms of international feminism as relations among national feminisms in that it
seeks to counter these global hegemonies while at the same time seeking not to reproduce exclusivist national or other identities in resistance to them.
While they and others (see, for example, Hesford and Kozol, 2005)
recognize that the moniker transnational also carries baggage to the
degree that it, too, is associated with transnational corporations and
capital, they and others have also distinguished transnational feminism
from global feminism. A ashpoint that particularly set up a debate
structured as transnational feminism versus global feminism was the
Womens Rights are Human Rights campaign. As Hesford and Kozol
(2005, p. 19) frame the debate, it is between the political efcacy of
claiming, codifying, and enforcing the observance of the universality of
womens human rights as advocated by global feminists and the political costs of feeding into imperial moves and re-colonizing processes
under the banner of womens human rights as argued by transnational
feminists.
Originally promulgated, primarily by feminists in the West, as a
concept and strategy to create a more inclusive notion of rights that
would particularly take into account both public and private forms of
violence against women engaged in not just by the state, but actors
below and above the state, womens human rights was assumed to be
a mechanism that had leverage in international law and could have
universal appeal to women, cutting across their differing social locations and priorities through an ever-expanding list of rights. However, the most common critiques of womens human rights practices
and discourses (and global feminisms more generally) focus on the following: the eliteness of globe-trotting NGO actors in both the North
and South who can attend a bevy of international conferences and
forums and lobby IGOs, thereby separating them from more grassroots, local actors; the dependence on IGO, and particularly Western,
funding that makes NGOs more beholden to funders than to social
movements, thereby de-radicalizing their demands; the Western liberal-inscribed notion of rights that privileges individualism and
civil and political rights over social and economic rights; the appropriation by the West of the universal that is set up against the particular
outside the West; and the tendency to single out a reductive notion of
Third World cultural oppression of women as the target for womens human rights enforcement, re-instating discourses of rescue and

Global Feminism

deecting attention away from the ongoing violence of neoliberal capitalism and militarism purveyed by the West.
Nevertheless, critiques of womens human rights have been taken
further by arguing that human rights generally, far from being antistatist, actually are used to enhance the disciplinary power of neocolonial and postcolonial states who claim they are friendly to them by
providing the cover of progressivism and modernity to engage in
selective punitive action at home and abroad against proscribed enemies of the state in the name of protecting (womens) human rights
(Grewal, 2005). While critics and proponents of realpolitik in international relations have long observed that human rights have always
been used selectively for the deployment of state power, womens
human rights has particularly constituted an instrument of U.S. foreign
policy in the post-9/11 period, making it particularly contested territory among feminists. One reading of this is the cooptation of womens
human rights discourse by U.S. neoconservatives and their front
women ranging from Laura Bush and Karen Hughes to Condoleezza
Rice. Feminist political theorist Zillah Eisenstein (2007) referred to
women as the seemingly diverse sexual decoys of the Bush
Administration who embrace militarism and its masculinist constructions while supposedly constructing a compassionate female face for
their conservativism to camouage war (p. 98). Another even more
critical reading of this is that not just far-from-feminist right wing
women, but the womens human rights regime itself, particularly as
embodied by the U.S. Feminist Majoritys campaign to free women
from the Taliban itself goaded Gulf War II, constitute imperial feminism. As Amy Farrell and Patrice MacDermott (2005) argue, the mainstream U.S. womens movements turn to international activism
served a strategic function, namely to mobilize their constituencies,
attract new and younger members, gain resources, and, perhaps most
importantly, legitimate their existence in the United States in a postfeminist era marked by anti-feminist conservative backlashes and rise
of commercial feminism, or the claiming of feminism by corporations and ad agencies that overpowered the work of feminists
engaged in prison reform, economic justice, and anti-racist activism
(pp. 4647). In this climate, the activism and emphasis on the victim
status of Third World women were not only key to keeping the U.S.
movement alive, but also central to the very construction of American feminism in the late twentieth century (Farrell & MacDermott,
2005, p. 47).
While postcolonial feminists would argue that this has always been
central to the construction of mainstream Western, white feminism,
Farrell and MacDermott also point out that the Feminist Majority was
further motivated by a desire to be recognized as a player in foreign
policy (2005, p. 47), an arena in which feminists, and women more

10

Feminism as Human Rights

generally, have been historically denied any legitimacy, as long argued


by feminist international relations scholars. As Susan Faludi chronicles
in her recent book, The Terror Dream (2007), women were completely
disappeared as commentators in the U.S. national press post-9/11 and
feminism was declared dead in that press for its soft-headedness that
endangered national security through either its anti-militarist stance or
its gender equity in the military stance, both of which emasculated the
state in a dangerous world. Not surprisingly, the net effect of pursuing
a feminist legitimation project under a neoconservative regime and
without recognition of how central racism and colonization have been
to the construction of the U.S. state and how these are reproduced in
mainstream social movements that act as its agents even as they may
contest it in some ways, the female voice which gained most attention
after September 11 was not Robin Morgans or Eleanor Smeals but that
of First Lady Laura Bush (Faludi, 2007, p. 49). Indeed, as Faludi notes,
the Feminist Majority remained vilied by U.S. neoconservative commentators who claimed to be the true saviors of Afghan women, while
particularly postcolonial feminists of color (including some outside the
United States), who dared to question the value of going to war to
save women, were subjected to death threats from members of civil
society.

GLOBAL FEMINISM AND NEOLIBERAL GOVERNMENTALITY


Although the womens human rights movement is more varied and
cannot be reduced to this particular case of its extreme dangers, perhaps the most serious critique of it, and indeed all gender mainstreaming efforts associated with global feminism, is the charge of feminism
becoming governmentalized under neoliberalism (see, for example,
Grewal, 2005). Just as women were gaining voice and ground in
international forums and policymaking arenas, particularly in the rst
decade of the postCold War period, a series of political, economic,
cultural, and environmental shocks heightened both direct and structural violence in world politics. Undergirding globalization is the
economic philosophy or ideology of neoliberalism that was developed
by economists in the global North and imposed there and on the global
South. Critics refer to this as a form of economic or market fundamentalism (Klein, 2007), as it represents a total faith in the market, unfettered by state regulation and protections for citizens, and a total belief
in the dismantling of the welfare state through the privatization for
prot of public services, leaving no accountability or universal access
to satisfaction of basic needs. The ensuing economic insecurities have
also unleashed a variety of other fundamentalisms, including political,
religious, ethnic, and other cultural ones. Neoconservativism combines

Global Feminism

11

a neoliberal faith in the market with the view that the only role for the
state is a coercive one: rst, to control other states through strong
national defense and offense (which is part and parcel of a militarist
ideology), and second, to control populations through the imposition
of laws that limit civil liberties and human rights, insisting that people
conform to a particular set of behaviors deemed moral.
Long before the current economic crisis, evidence had been piling
up that most women were bearing the brunt of the new global economy (Marchand & Runyan, 2000). For example, as reproductive workers in the voluntaristic economy, or household members given by
gender ideology the most responsibility for the creation and care of
family and community members on an unpaid basis, womenwho are
also the largest consumers and providers of social services because of
their reproductive roleshave lost the most with the reduction or privatization of social services. The retreat of the welfare state has meant
that women have had to take on additional roles in the private or
domestic sphere that used to be public services for which women
also used to be paid or paid better. As productive workers in the
cash economy, women, who have been rendered as cheap labor by
gender ideology, became the preferred labor force in low-wage service and light-industrial assembly work created by offshore production.
Some women have beneted to a degree from this newfound employment, but their working conditionsincluding low wages, lack of
union protections, poor health and safety regulations, sexual harassment, and polluted and dangerous workplaces and living spaces
have kept them in subordinate and precarious positions. Subordinate
men, such as those in the working classes, have suffered as well, having their skills, wages, and jobs feminized (devalued or eliminated).
But this effect, too, boomerangs on women in such forms as enduring
higher incidences of domestic violence, taking full responsibility for
both wage and reproductive work, and/or being left behind to sustain the household alone when men migrate for jobs elsewhere or
having to migrate themselves to nd work, thus leaving their families
behind.
The generalized response by IGOs to these gendered downsides of
neoliberal globalization that feminist academics and NGOs have documented can be characterized as neoliberal governmentality, in which
global feminist NGOs have also been implicated. Postmodern theorist
Michel Foucault (1991) coined the term governmentality to refer to
how individuals and populations could now be controlled, administrated, empowered, or disciplined through certain governmental techniques (Woehl, 2008, p. 69). Governmentality pertains not only to
state and suprastate bureaucratic apparatuses and policies but also to
civil society institutions (including NGOs) that enable governing on the
basis of rational, scientic, and statistical calculations and produce

12

Feminism as Human Rights

subjectivities that are amenable to being managed or regulated


and even participate in self-management or self-regulation in conformance with rationalized approaches to problem-solving (Woehl, 2008,
pp. 6566), now associated with good governance. Power in this
sense is more diffuse and both repressive and enabling, but rests upon
hegemony, or the inculcation of the right order that brooks no deviation. Neoliberal governmentality refers to the ascendance of the global
capitalist economy and economic rationality as the sole criteria for
governance (Woehl, 2008, p. 69). Under neoliberal governmentality,
the state is reduced primarily to the promotion of the free market,
leading to the privatization of social welfare and the marketization of
political and social life, whereby populations are to be free, self-managing, and self-enterprising individuals in different spheres of life
health, education, bureaucracy, the professions, and so on. The neoliberal subject is therefore not a citizen with claims on the state but a
self-enterprising citizen-subject who is obligated to become an entrepreneur of himself or herself (Ong, 2006, p. 14).
In this scenario, gender equality becomes the technique to free
women to be these free market actors, taking care of their own needs
without resort to the state for their welfare. The male (and class-based)
norm of free market actors, devoid of dependents and interdependencies, and an androcentric construction of states as not responsible
for the welfare of their citizenries are strengthened by this reduction of
gender equality to the production of economic woman in the form
of economic man.
The UN Millennium Development Goals (MDGs), largely the product of IGOs, and especially IFIs, but which are often touted as central
to achieving gender equality, are particularly visible features of neoliberal governmentality. The eight MDGs include (1) eradicate extreme
poverty and hunger; (2) achieve universal primary education; (3)
promote gender equality and empower women: (4) reduce child mortality; (5) improve maternal health; (6) combat HIV/AIDS, malaria, and
other diseases; (7) ensure environmental sustainability; and (8) create a
global partnership for development (UNIFEM, 2008). Progress on these
goals is measured and monitored by the UN (based on statistics provided by states) in relation to particular quantitative targets set over
time within each of the goals, such as cutting in half by 2015 the proportion of people with incomes of less than one dollar per day and suffering from hunger, as well as the more recent target of achieving full
employment in decent work for all people by that date to reach Goal 1
(UNIFEM, 2008). Various indicators are used and periodically added to
measure these targets, such as the proportions of the population living
on one dollar per day, dietary consumption rates of the poor, and
numbers of underweight children (UNIFEM, 2008). Goal 3 is the one
goal that specically targets women in general (not just mothers as in

Global Feminism

13

the case of maternal health) and promotes gender equality even though
all the other goals relate to the multiple conditions that (dis)empower
women.
On the face of it, the MDGs do represent a seeming shift in global
priorities. The dominant approach to development since World War II
and through most of the twentieth century was one focused on economic development to the exclusion of social or human development.
Large scale, capital-intensive, and high-technology projects that privileged male actors, from decision-makers to workers, were systematically promoted and funded by the World Bank to modernize (read,
Westernize) the infrastructure and economies of the so-called developing countries. Gross Domestic Product (GDP)a measurement of the
output of domestic goods and services by a nationdoes not measure
human well-being and in fact externalizes (or does not take into
account) the costs of human misery and environmental destruction, yet
it became the central indicator of growth and economic growth was
to be pursued at any human and environmental cost. The shift to development initiatives with a more human, and even female, face, however, is accomplished through neoliberal problem-solving techniques
that frame human immiseration and gender inequality not as products
of unjust global capitalism, but rather as drags on it that must be
addressed to enhance market efciency through the production of
more market ready actors. Moreover, aside from the weaknesses of
Goal 3 in terms of its singular target to end educational disparities
between males and females at all levels of education by 2015 (which is
far from being reached in the case of higher education), it shares with
all the other goals the technique of using bureaucratic methods and
quantitative indicators as reductionist tools to manage populations in
order to depoliticize social justice issues and demobilize social movements critical of neoliberal globalization.
Thus, the traction that gender equality has gained in the context of
global governance can be read more critically and most cynically as
just another neoliberal strategy, with global feminist NGOs and femocrats being turned into agents of neoliberal governmentality. Indeed,
the very proliferation of global feminist NGOs can be seen as a feature
of neoliberal governmentality, taking on roles of states to manage
(depoliticize) populations and their problems and even providing services, albeit in far more under-resourced ways, that states used to provide. This has implications as well for women gaining public ofce in
states, for if the reigning ideology is that states are to withhold public
service in favor of privatization that makes individuals solely responsible for their own welfare, then it is difcult to mount more expansive
public commitments and garner more public resources for changing
negative conditions for women (as well as children, nonelite men, and
even the planet).

14

Feminism as Human Rights

GLOBAL FEMINIST POLITICS VERSUS NEOLIBERAL


GOVERNMENTALITY
There is little question that IGOs, and especially the more powerful
IFIs, frame gender equality as a matter of modern economic efciency.
States have been most responsive to this framing, and global feminist
NGOs have often made their own arguments in relation to it. However, it is also the case that local, grassroots, and national womens
movements have drawn on these instruments of neoliberal governmentality to make claims on states, although NGOs and femocrats, to varying degrees, continue to critique the insufciencies of governmental
responses. Thus, there are contradictions in this process that open up
opportunities for resistance to neoliberal governmentality and for more
radical demands than it typically allows. However, just as neoliberal
governmentality circumscribes how (global) gender issues are framed,
the presence of women in global governance is no guarantee that they
will be acted upon or reframed in social justice terms.
Still, as Mary Hawkesworth (2006, p. 148) argues, even though
global feminism and neoliberal globalism have pursued similar avenues to leverage change through global policymaking to impact state
behavior, global feminism, particularly as transnational feminist critiques and perspectives are brought to bear on NGO work and campaigns, retains and cultivates a still oppositional stance to neoliberal
globalism. As she puts it, global feminism envisions an expanded
state provision to create adequate health care, education, welfare,
employment, personal security, and a range of equity policies that
redress gender- and race-based injustices, whereas neoliberal globalism seeks to cut back the very aspects of the state that feminist activists seek to build up (p. 149). At the same time, global feminism
envisions transformations in consciousness that would make visible,
actionable, and intolerable gender- and race-based inequities that permeate interpersonal relations, social organizations, economic and political structures, and symbol systems, whereas neoliberal globalism
seeks a transformation of consciousness that produces an export
mentality and market mentality, while whittling away protest mentalities (p. 149).
Similarly, as Val Moghadam (2006) points out, if globalization is
understood as a complex process of economic, political, and cultural
change on a world scale that entails integration, marginalization, exploitation, and resistance, then global feminism may be seen as both an
outcome of global integration and a form of resistance to domination
constituting the specter that haunts the global economy and its imperial center (p. 71). True, Moghadam argues, the discourse of womens
human rights can be and is co-opted, but it still can retain its meaning
when used by feminists who ght to end sexual violence, poverty,

Global Feminism

15

militarism, and all inequalities and call for state provisioning, democratic participation, and an end to discriminations, exploitations, and
violence (p. 71).
To better actualize these oppositional politics, however, Aili Tripp
(2006) insists that global feminists, particularly from privileged spaces
in the North (and South), must engage far better in the following transnational feminist practices: rst, paying attention to what has already
been done by local actors and taking cues from them is a necessity
for devising any global solidarity campaign; second, global campaigns
should reect local priorities and not expropriate the issues of
women in particular parts of the world to aggrandize and sensationalize the campaigns of global or other national NGOs; third, Northern
feminist NGOs should use their greater inuence to pressure their own
most powerful governments and the IGOs those governments control
to redirect policies away from militarism and neoliberal globalization;
and fourth, Northern feminists should engage in global feminist actions
not just to help change the conditions of women elsewhere, but to
change their own oppressive conditions by both learning from activists
elsewhere and making use of the global gender instruments that they
have worked to institute. Only through such noncolonizing practices
can global feminism not be haunted itself by imperialist forces and
motivations.
REFERENCES
Ara
ujo, C., & Garcia, A. I. (2006). The experience and the impact of quotas in
Latin America. In D. Dahlerup (Ed.), Women, quotas, and politics (pp. 83111).
New York: Routledge.
Cohn, C. (2008). Mainstreaming gender in UN security policy: A path to political transformation? In S. Rai & G. Waylen (Eds.), Global governance: Feminist perspectives (pp. 185206). New York: Palgrave McMillan.
Dahlerup, D. (2006). Women, quotas, and politics. New York: Routledge.
Eisenstein, Z. (2007). Sexual decoys: Gender, race, and war in imperial democracy.
New York: Palgrave.
Faludi, S. (2007). The terror dream: Fear and fantasy in post-9/11 America. New
York: Metropolitan Books, Henry Holt and Co.
Farrell, A., & McDermott, P. (2005). Claiming Afghan women: The challenge of
human rights discourse for transnational feminism. In W.S. Hesford &
W. Kozol (Eds.), Just advocacy? Womens human rights, transnational feminisms, and the politics of representation (pp. 3355). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers
University Press.
Ferree, M. M. (2006). Globalization and feminism: Opportunities and obstacles
for activism in the global arena. In M. M. Ferree & A. M. Tripp (Eds.),
Global feminism: Transnational womens activism, organizing, and human rights
(pp. 323). New York: New York University Press.

16

Feminism as Human Rights

Foucault, M. (1991). Governmentality. In G. Burchell, C. Gordon, & P. Miller


(Eds.), The Foucault effect: Studies in governmentality (pp. 87104). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Goldstein, J. S. (2001). War and gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Grewal, I. (2005). Transnational America: Feminisms, diasporas, neoliberalisms.
Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Grewal, I., & Kaplan, C. (Eds.). (1994). Scattered hegemonies: Postmodernity and
transnational feminist practices. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Hawkesworth, M. E. (2006). Globalization and feminist activism. Lanham, MD:
Rowman and Littleeld Publishers.
Hesford, W. S., & Kozol, W. (Eds.). (2005). Just advocacy? Womens human rights,
transnational feminisms, and the politics of representation. New Brunswick, NJ:
Rutgers University Press.
Klein, N. (2007). The shock doctrine: The rise of disaster capitalism. New York:
Metropolitan Books, Henry Holt and Co.
Marchand, M. H., & Runyan, A. S. (Eds.). (2000). Gender and global restructuring:
Sightings, sites and resistances. New York: Routledge.
Moghadam, V. (2006). Feminism and the global economy. Womens Studies
Quarterly, 34, 6971.
Ong, A. (2006). Neoliberalism as exception: Mutations of citizenship and sovereignty.
Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Rai, S. (2008). Analyzing global governance. In S. Rai & G. Waylen (Eds.),
Global governance: Feminist perspectives (pp. 1942). New York: Palgrave
Macmillan.
Squires, J. (2007). The new politics of gender equality. New York: Palgrave
Macmillan.
Tripp, A. M. (2006). Challenges in transnational feminist mobilization. In
M. M. Ferree & A. M. Tripp (Eds.), Global feminism: Transnational womens
activism, organizing, and human rights (pp. 296312), New York: New York
University Press.
United Nations (UN). (2000). The worlds women 2000: Trends and statistics.
New York: Author.
United Nations Development Fund for Women (UNIFEM). (2008). Progress of
the worlds women 2008/2009: Who answers to women? New York: UNIFEM.
Retrieved on June 1, 2009, from http://www.unifem.org/progress/2008/
media/POWW08_Report_Full_Text.pdf
Woehl, S. (2008). Global governance as neoliberal govenmentality: Gender
mainstreaming in the European employment strategy. In S. Rai & G. Waylen
(Eds.), Gender and global governance: Feminist perspectives (pp. 6483). New
York: Palgrave Macmillan.

Chapter 2

Womens Human Rights and


Empowerment in a Transnational,
Globalized Context: Whats
Psychology Got To Do with It?
Shelly Grabe

As the eld of transnational feminism grows in response to globalization, the discussion of womens human rights becomes ever more
relevant. However, the very premise of transnational feminism that
distinguishes it from international feminism or global sisterhoodthat
is, favoring a diversity of womens agency rather than universalized
notions of Western feminism (Naples & Desai, 2002)makes it seemingly impossible to advocate for universal recommendations regarding
womens human rights that can cross borders in the same manner that
the resources and labor that dene the modern globalized economy
cross borders. Nevertheless, the development eld has begun a concerted effort to broadly address womens human rights and empowerment with organizations ranging from the World Bank, to grassroots
nongovernmental organizations, to the United Nations (UN), all advocating for womens human rights and empowerment in an effort to
address UN Millennium Development Goal 3to promote gender
equality and empower women (UN, 2000). This chapter will discuss
why womens human rights warrant increased focus in the context of
globalization and how psychology can provide the currently missing,
but necessary, links between transnational feminism, the discourse on
womens human rights and globalization, and the international attention given to womens empowerment, to effectively ll in gaps in our

18

Feminism as Human Rights

understanding, and to contribute to systematic change in womens


well-being worldwide. As such, this chapter integrates theories of
womens human rights and capabilities with current understandings of
empowerment from both academic and development approaches and
concludes with a case example that draws together theory, intervention, and assessment to evaluate a program that has enormous implications for womens well-being in a globalized, international context.
GLOBALIZATION AND ITS IMPACTS ON WOMENS RIGHTS
One major consequence of the restructuring of the world economy
of the 1980s and 1990sor globalizationis that it exacerbates or
maintains violations of human rights. As such, several transnational
social movements and advocacy networks have emerged in response
to globalization to highlight its impact on human rights and justice
(Moghadam, 2005). Transnational feminism, in particular, is one movement that argues that women bear a disproportionate burden of the
economic and social dislocation that result from the neoliberal economic policies that characterize the global economy (Naples & Desai,
2002). For example, it is well documented that the structural adjustment of social welfare policies, the increased international activity by
multinational corporations, and the deregulation of markets and free
trade agreements that characterized the 1980s and 90s has had unique
outcomes for women (Naples & Desai, 2002; Moghadam, 2005). Some
of the consequences of this economic and social restructuring include
the following: (1) the feminization of labor and/or poverty whereby
women have expanded their unpaid labor in the home and community
to compensate for the increase in poverty and loss of local resources,
(2) biases in economic policies favoring men, (3) an increase in the
number of women trafcked for what is often involuntary sex work,
and (4) an exacerbated sexual division of labor (e.g., Acosta-Belen, &
Bose, 1990; Moghadam, 2005).
Such trends continue unabated, despite widespread international
commitments to draw increased attention to womens rights. For example, at a UN conference in 1995, 189 governments adopted the Beijing
Platform for Action, an international agenda for womens empowerment and a statement of womens rights as human rights (UN, 1995).
Importantly, the mission statement of the Beijing Platform for Action
explicitly states:
The Platform for Action is an agenda for womens empowerment. It aims
at accelerating the implementation of the Nairobi Forward-looking Strategies for the Advancement of Women and at removing all the obstacles
to womens active participation in all spheres of public and private life
through a full and equal share in economic, social, cultural and political

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

19

decision-making. This means that the principle of shared power and


responsibility should be established between women and men at home,
in the workplace and in the wider national and international communities. Equality between women and men is a matter of human rights and
a condition for social justice and is also a necessary and fundamental
prerequisite for equality, development and peace. A transformed partnership based on equality between women and men is a condition for
people-centered sustainable development. A sustained and long-term
commitment is essential, so that women and men can work together for
themselves, for their children and for society to meet the challenges of
the twenty-rst century.

Indeed, the 1990s was a period of monumental political transformation that witnessed a growing international womens movement linked
through subregional, regional, and international networks to collaborate
on efforts calling attention to unequal rights and mechanisms through
which female subordination is sustained and reproduced (Kabeer, 1994;
Razavi, 2003). Therefore, not surprisingly, in the midst of international
attention to womens rights from advocacy groups and the United
Nations alike, there has been growing awareness and interest in the
empowerment of women among development programs and policies
(Narayan, 2005). Yet, what remains unclear in this growing global
awareness is whether or not a universal set of human rights exists and,
if they do, how empowerment is related to them. Furthermore, what does
the development communityand the economists who have staked a
claim in this area of researchmean by womens empowerment? Stronger conceptualization and assessment of empowerment, which I will
demonstrate is an inarguably psychological process, as well as how it
relates to human rights, can greatly contribute to and fuel efforts aimed
at enhancing womens well-being. This chapter aims to establish that if
we are to have a better understanding of womens psychological
empowerment, contributions from psychology are necessary to integrate feminist scholarship, debates regarding human rights, and the
increasing attention given by the development community to womens
rights.

HUMAN RIGHTS AND EMPOWERMENT IN DEVELOPMENT


Discussions of human rights often imply a universal language of
rights and equality, suggesting that norms of justice can be applied
across societies. However, such simple notions of universalism raise
concern. First, in many countries, universal discourse is taken to
mean that dened by the West. As such, even when universal categories are introduced by women who live and work within the country
in question, they are commonly accused of pandering to a Western

20

Feminism as Human Rights

political agenda (Nussbaum, 2000; Tripp, 2002). This area is therefore


fraught with intellectual and political complexity. For example, one
could argue that holding standards of human rights is another example
of colonial dominationan imposition on people who are capable of
determining their own denitions of justice (Phillips, 2002). Certainly
this is consistent with the tenants of Transnational Feminism, which
argue precisely against universal denitions of feminism in favor of a
feminism that is relative to the society in which it was formed reecting the values and practices unique to the culture.
In contrast to universalism, a culturally relativistic approach argues
that norms of justice are always relative to the society in which they
are formed, reecting values and practices that vary enormously from
one society to another, and that it is therefore inappropriate to take the
norms that emerge within one society as the standard against which to
assess the norms of another (Phillips, 2002). The value of this position
is that it captures the situated nature of any principles of justice. However, while cultural relativism addresses the contextual nature of justice, it carries its own set of concerns. For example, cultural relativism
does not take into consideration that norms of justice are not constructed under the principles of gender equity. Those who defend practices that are harmful to women, in the name of preserving culture,
are often the same individuals or leaders who make decisions and
allow change that serves to protect their own political and/or economic
interests (Phillips, 2002; Tripp, 2002). Indeed, many of the structural
adjustments and development interventions stemming from neoliberal
policies have produced substantial cultural changes; however, because
these changes are often advantageous to those already in positions of
power, concerns of cultural relativism are not often invoked. What this
suggests is that cultural practices and change are tied to broader political and economic contexts that extend beyond gender and, in this way,
the suppression of womens rights is not essential to the uniqueness of
any particular culture (Tripp, 2002).
It therefore seems that when evaluating womens rights, both
approaches (i.e., universal principles that fail to adequately address
differences, and those ignoring womens rights for fear of cultural
imperialism) should be suspect in the context of this discussion. If one
follows the principles of universalism, strict equality in rights may
perpetuate inequality if no attention to differences in background or
demographics is paid. In other words, ignoring differences in gender
tends to equate equality with sameness, thereby leaving untouched systematic differences in power (Phillips, 2002). For example, equal rights
to hold property or to have access to clean water are not realized when
traditional gender roles prevent many women from ownership or control over these resources, despite their legal rights (Deere & Leon, 2001;
Lastarria, 2001, 2008; Molyneux & Razavi, 2002). This concern is

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

21

becomingly increasingly relevant, as many countries have revised their


constitutions or laws to legalize womens rights in the past several
decades. However, although legal rights are an important rst step,
customary law or cultural practices still largely prohibit women from
taking advantage of their rights and essentially render women incapable
of realizing rights they might otherwise choose. As a result, universalism, in extending equal rights to everyone regardless of background,
may have the unintended consequence of perpetuating or exaggerating
inequalities. The political implication here is that some groups may
need different interventions to achieve the same kind of outcomes.
Therefore, it appears that some combination of universalism and relativism is needed. In other words, it must be possible to be both equal
and different.
In an approximate combination of these approaches, Martha Nussbaum proposes a universal approach to human capabilities, rather than
human rights (2000). She suggests that there are central elements of
truly human functioning that are particularly essential to human worth
and dignity, and that each person should be capable of having these basic elements in their lives. She proposes a list of basic elements and
suggests that they might serve as the moral basis of constitutional
guarantees cross-culturally. Among the eight proposed capabilities or
essential human elements, having control over ones environment is
most relevant to the current discussion. She denes this capability in
two primary ways: (a) political: being able to participate effectively in
political choices that govern ones life; having the right to political participation, protections of free speech and association, and (b) material:
being able to hold property (both land and movable goods), not just
formally but in terms of real opportunity; having property rights on an
equal basis with others; and having the right to seek employment on
an equal basis with others. In this way, being capable means that in
addition to having the legal right to, for example, political participation,
a person should be free to pursue the right, presumably without consequence or undue obstacle. Thus, capabilities may require not only material and institutional resources, but legal and social acceptance of the
legitimacy of womens claims or participation.
Importantly, and consistent with the aims of this chapter, Nussbaum
also argues that the structure of social and political institutions should
be established, in part, to promote at least a threshold level of these
human capabilities, that basic rights become institutionalized such that
everyone has the opportunityor is capableof realizing their rights.
In the past two decades, this view has become incorporated into the
development community, and it has become accepted to evaluate development in terms of human capabilities and enhanced well-being
(Nussbaum & Sen, 1993; Sen, 1999). In particular, the increasing recognition in the development discourse regarding womens rights has led

22

Feminism as Human Rights

to a proliferation of programs aimed at womens empowerment. However, the inclusion of women in development programs has largely
come through the focus of income-generating programsmost notably
microcredit loaning (Goetz & Sen Gupta, 1996). However, while income
generation that stems from microcredit loans may improve a households economic status, womens receipt of the loan, or the labor associated with the loan, does not contest gender relations in the same
manner that structural or institutional interventions might. Although
research suggests that loans may increase womens economic activity,
they do little to diversify womens labor, resulting in an adherence to
a traditional occupational structure that sustains male dominance
(Kabeer, 2001). For example, it is not uncommon that a womans labor
does not belong to the woman, but falls under the control of her male
partner (Bartky, 1990). Thus, male control of the economic or material
output that may stem from womens microcredit borrowing may maintain and support the domination and control of women and their work.
Indeed, in a review of credit programs in Bangladesh, 63 percent of
female loan holders reported having only partial, very limited, or no
control over the loans they had procured (Goetz & Sen Gupta, 1996). In
support of the argument that economic activity does not increase womens power and control, Panda and Agarwal (2005) found that levels of
employment status (unemployed, seasonally employed, and regularly
employed) did not affect womens receipt of physical violence, whereas
land ownership did, suggesting that economic activity did little to alter
the gender dynamics that predict patterns of violence against women.
Effectively targeting poverty may ensure that short-term, material
needs are met but may not alter womens status or effectively give
them voice in their relationships or their community. In sum, although
there has been a proliferation of gender-focused programs among development organizations, there is limited investigation into the role of
structural interventions in addressing womens rights, capabilities, or
empowerment.
The inuence of the capabilities approach on the development community is also witnessed through assessments such as the Human Development Reports of the United Nations Development Programme
(UNDP), which now includes quality of life in assessments for governments and international agencies in addition to the previously narrow
focus on gross domestic product. Moreover, two gender measures were
introduced in the 1990s to assess the status of women in human development (UNDP, 1995). However, neither assesses empowerment in
ways that will be conceptualized in this chapter. Specically, the
Gender Related Development Index was created to assess inequalities
between women and men on factors such as life expectancy, educational attainment, and income. The Gender Empowerment Measure
(GEM) was introduced to address the noted gap in assessment

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

23

measures and is widely acknowledged as a measure of womens


agency. However, it assesses gender equality in economic and political
participation and decision making, reporting factors such as the
percentage of women in governmental positions and the ratio of
female- to male-earned income. Thus, while the GEM is currently held
as a model for assessing levels of womens empowerment, I argue that
it does not adequately measure empowerment or agency; rather, it
measures the outcomes of womens empowerment. Thus, while there
has been considerable theoretical interest within the development community regarding questions of empowerment, agency, and well-being,
there has been little empirical investigation into these exact processes.
What has resulted is a full-edged, albeit much needed, internationally
coordinated effort to enhance womens empowerment in the absence
of a sound understanding of what disempowersor subordinates
women, a clear conceptualization of womens psychological empowerment, and virtually no adequate assessment or evaluation of progress
aimed at enhancing womens well-being. In other words, despite the
proliferation of empowerment rhetoric in championing of social interventions, the explicit connections between policy or program development and empowerment theory and research are in most cases tenuous
(Perkins, 1995). The empirical gap in the literature surrounding how
womens empowerment processes operate may largely reect a lack of
clear-cut denitions and appropriate assessment tools (Goetz & Sen
Gupta, 1996; Holvoet, 2005). Psychology can substantially contribute to
this area of investigation by (a) conceptualizing and dening womens
empowerment as it relates to womens human rights, and (b) assessing
and analyzing the processes of empowerment in a development context.
CONCEPTUALIZING EMPOWERMENT
In an attempt to address developments shortcomings, many have
turned to an empowerment approach, which has become popular,
and largely unquestioned, in part because the lack of a clear denition
lends to a common sense acceptance of it (Sharp, Briggs, Yacoub, &
Hamed, 2003). Therefore, it is not quite clear what practitioners or
scholars mean by using the term empowerment, nor how it is
achieved. Yet, empowerment has become a vital construct for understanding womens well-being and a central focus across disciplines and
elds, despite that it remains loosely dened and inadequately conceptualized (Hill, 2003; Narayan, 2005; Perkins, 1995). To date, empowerment has been discussed by economists as a process of undoing
internalized oppression, and therefore, when focused on women, it
involves changing the social and cultural norms inherent in patriarchy
that sustain womens subordination (e.g., Agarwal, 1994). It has been
similarly argued that empowerment increases womens sense of agency

24

Feminism as Human Rights

or what has been termed power within (Deere & Leon, 2001; Kabeer,
1994). Kabeer (1994) argues that while agency has been operationalized
in the social science literature as decision making or negotiation, it is a
broader construct reecting ones ability to dene goals and act upon
them. She denes empowerment as a process related to resources,
agency, and achievement (Kabeer, 2005). In this way, agency is central
to empowerment because through agency choices are made and put
into effect. Sen (1985, 1999) similarly denes agency as freedom to
achieve whatever goals or values a person regards as important.
He argues that traditional gender roles curtail womens agency by
assuming their interests lie within the home and not within their person (Sen, 1995). As related to empowerment, agency includes not only
exercising choice, but doing so in a way that challenges existing power
relations. Importantly, it also has been highlighted that issues of
empowerment are distinct from psychological well-being but that each
are important when assessing change (Sen, 1985). While collectively
there appears to emerge a working understanding of empowerment,
there are notable limitations present in this literature. First, the authors
addressing womens empowerment in the context of development are
largely economists and, while they have pioneered this eld and great
strides have been made with tremendous attention being paid to womens rights, because empowerment is an inherently psychological process, understanding in this area can only be advanced by the conceptual
and methodological tools offered from within the discipline of psychology. Second, despite increasing attention to issues of empowerment and
capabilities in the development literature, current evaluative assessments
do not adequately reect the processes surrounding womens empowerment (Hill, 2003). Finally, limitations posed by multiple nonoverlapping
disciplines have not adequately connected human rights or capabilities
to empowerment. Thus, as it stands, there is a divide between the theoretical or philosophical approaches to human rights and capabilities and
the interventions evaluated by development practitioners and scholars.
The discipline of psychology is well positioned to address these gaps
with both theoretical and methodological contributions.
Psychologists dene empowerment as a process by which people
gain control and mastery over issues of concern to them (Rappaport,
1987; Zimmerman, 1995). Within psychology, empowerment theory
links subjective (personal) well-being with larger social and political
contexts (Perkins & Zimmerman, 1995; Zimmerman, 1995). Importantly, empowerment research focuses on identifying capabilities and
exploring environmental inuences on social problems such that
empowerment-oriented interventions enhance well-being, while they
also aim to ameliorate problems (Perkins & Zimmerman, 1995). In fact,
within psychology, empowerment has been described as a process
through which people lacking an equal share of valued resources gain

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

25

greater access to and control over those resources (Cornell Empowerment Group, 1989). Psychological empowerment, in particular, refers to
empowerment at the individual level of analysis and integrates perceptions of personal control, a proactive approach to life, and a critical
understanding of the sociopolitical environment. Although the study of
empowerment among psychologists has focused on U.S. community
groups, organizations, and neighborhood associations (e.g., Perkins,
1995; Zimmerman, 1990), and there has been very little attention internationally despite that empowerment has become the premier paradigm for development programs and policies, there are some obvious
and important applications for the understanding and study of womens international rights and well-being. First, the overlap between
Nussbuams denition of capabilities (i.e., having control over ones
environment) and psychologys denition of empowerment (i.e.,
a process by which people gain mastery over issues that concern them)
is striking. Both articulate that a sense of personal control and freedom
for self-chosen activity is fundamentally linked to well-being, and that
individuals capabilities can be supported by access to and control over
resources. With these clear and overlapping denitions, we can begin
to more adequately conceptualize empowerment in the context of
human rights.
Given the complexity involved in empowerment processes, accurate
conceptualization cannot be captured by a single operationalization or
measurement, nor divorced from context (Zimmerman, 1995). For
example, because powerlessness is embedded in cultural practices and
unequal institutional relations, adequate conceptualization and assessment involves investigation of the formal and informal institutional
barriers that prevent women from taking effective action to improve
their well-being within the household and society at large. Similarly,
notions of self-efcacy, competency, and control are key to measuring
empowerment, as are processes by which people engage in democratic
participation and shared leadership within communities (Perkins &
Zimmerman, 1995). Furthermore, because conceptualizations may differ
across levels of analysis, measurement of empowerment must assess
when individuals believe they have the capability to inuence a given
context (e.g., perceived agency), have perceived control in various situations (e.g., marital power/control, individual mastery over environment), have an understanding of their sociopolitical environment (e.g.,
gender ideology), interact with others to successfully master social or
political systems (e.g., marital control or power), and engage in actions
to directly inuence outcomes (e.g., participation in decision making).
A thorough assessment would therefore require an interdisciplinary,
culturally relevant approach that incorporates individual, relational,
and societal aspects of empowerment that are sensitive to sociopolitical
contexts.

26

Feminism as Human Rights

While there is a great deal of attention given to empowerment in development policies and interventions, there is little to no empirical
investigation into these processes (Perkins, 1995). In fact, despite the
complexity involved in empowerment processes, the majority of
investigations into womens empowerment conducted among scholars
employ a rather narrow conceptualization by routinely assessing
household decision making as the primary indicator of womens
empowerment (Hill, 2003; Holvoet, 2005; Kabeer, 1999). A more thorough and accurate conceptualization and assessment of empowerment
would include not only intra-household decision making, but also indicators of status and traditional gender ideology; womens power and
control within the marital relationship; womens levels of agency,
autonomy, and mastery; psychological well-being; and outcomes that
may reect empowerment such as freedom from domestic violence or
agency in reproductive health choices. What follows is a focused discussion of structural factors (i.e., the formal and informal institutional
barriers), specic to the context of globalization, that contribute to gendered power imbalances and womens subordination.
GENDERED POWER, NATURAL RESOURCES, AND
DEVELOPMENT
The disruptive consequences of the new economic policies of the
1980s and 90s introduced or exacerbated several structural factors that
have contributed to rising levels of gender inequity and marginalization. This has been particularly visible within the area of natural
resources (Razavi, 2003; Rocheleau & Edmunds, 1997). In fact, pervasive gender inequities in access to resources, land in particular, has
been recognized by a large body of international work (e.g., Food Agriculture Organization [FAO], UN Convention of the Elimination of Discrimination Against Women [CEDAW]) and postulated as a necessary
human right (Ikdahl, 2008). Thus, not surprisingly, feminist scholars in
the area of globalization and development argue that womens empowerment and well-being can be enhanced by expanding womens management of resources (Deere & Leon, 2001; Razavi, 1999). This is
consistent with the tenants of empowerment theory, which argued that
control over resources was central to empowerment. However, that
lack of secure access to and control over natural resources contributes
to a system in which female subordination is sustained and reproduced, prohibiting women from exercising control over their environmentor, in other words, limiting human capability or empowerment.
Many scholars argue that most development interventions have been
formulated from the ideological and economic interests of the industrialized countries that are promoting projects to modernize the Third
World (e.g., Acosta-Belen & Bose, 1990). Among those interventions

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

27

has been the tendency to shift from customary tenure systems, or cooperative arrangements in which natural resources such as water, forestry, and land that were traditionally viewed as community resources
have become privatized systems of individual ownership (Agarwal,
2001; Lastarria-Cornheil, 1997; Zwarteveen, 1997). Therefore, the structural adjustments of privatization have created a context whereby natural resources have become commodities, and markets have developed
for rights or titles to these resources (Zwarteveen, 1997). Moreover, in
many countries, this is occurring at the same time that population pressures have begun to affect availability of natural resources, resulting in
scarcity, even in remote areas where resources were once considered
part of the commons (e.g., Lastarria-Cornheil, 1997; Shiva, 2002). As
a result, natural resources such as water, rewood, and land for
grazingwhich are all vital resources necessary to sustain livelihood
in rural areasare being bought up and controlled in private, for-prot
scenarios. This has obvious and dangerous implications for poor people worldwide, but it has specic implications for gendered imbalances
in power and therefore womens capabilities and empowerment.
As argued earlier, the international focus on interventions such as
microcredit lending, which t perfectly into the globalized, neoliberal
market-driven economy, do not adequately address the structural
obstacles that determine womens status and, ultimately, their wellbeing. In the context of addressing violations of womens rights, it is
imperative to distinguish between those programs that seek to reduce
poverty or enhance productivity from those that seek to empower
women. Although gender scholars have been making a case for using a
gender analysis of natural resource management in the context of
global changes for nearly a decade (e.g., Rocheleau & Edmunds, 1997),
the literature on natural resource control and gender remains scarce.
The Theory of Gender and Power (Connell, 1987) postulates that
gender-based inequalities are pervasive societal characteristics, which
result in mens disproportionate power in society and control over a
number of areas, including women and their bodies. Wingood and
DiClemente (2000) extended this theory to the areas of public health
and psychology. They argued that unequal control over resources leads
to power imbalances and gender-based norms that create risk environments that adversely inuence womens health and safety. There are
clear structural components that contribute to the construction of
dominance and thereby legitimize and perpetuate womens subordinate status. Although limited, the following review will demonstrate
that in the context of globalization, the unequal gendered distribution
of control over natural resources, in particular water, trees, and land, is
one structural component that contributes to dominance and therefore
places women in a subordinate position within both the household and
the larger society. Because violations of womens human rights often

28

Feminism as Human Rights

reect a societal problem requiring changes in sex-role ideologies and


social structures that perpetuate gender hierarchy (Ozner & Bandura,
1990), examining the structure of resource ownership and control provides a compelling area of investigation into one potential structure by
which violations against women are supported and sustained. More
specically, this review will demonstrate that entrenched inequalities
in the distribution of power and resources between women and men
create a risk environment that perpetuates womens disempowerment
and that a redirection in the development approach to a more inclusive
position on gender would greatly facilitate the empowerment agenda
that is promoted by international agencies.
WATER
The 1990s witnessed large changes in water policy generated by a
number of different development trends driven largely by the neoliberal economic agenda of large donors (Coles & Wallace, 2005; Shiva,
2002; World Bank, 1993). Among these trends was the tremendous shift
toward irrigation to support large monocropped commodities grown
for export. Water development projects in line with export agriculture
often involve privatization of water sources and massive damming to
accommodate newly irrigated agricultural landscapes (Khagram, 2004).
Importantly, rights to water use in irrigation systems are granted to
farmers or landowners, with the assumption that women will indirectly
benet from their husbands access to water. As such, water rights are
intimately linked to the existing social and cultural organization and
relations of authority and power (Shiva, 2002). Within this new economic structure, access to water has taken on a new meaningone that
involves gender (Zwarteveen, 1997).
However, the limited attention to gender and water in the literature
and development discourse may be due to the notion that womens
involvement with water mainly occurs in the domestic sphere, in
implicit opposition to mens water use, which is assumed to be mainly
market-oriented (Zwarteveen, 1997). Additionally, it has been argued
that womens involvement in water development projects has been
promoted for largely economic reasons assuming that womens participation improves efciency of water projects because of their interest in
responsibly managing reliable supplies of water (Coles & Wallace,
2005). The limited analysis of gender in the management of water effectively hides that the most important aspect concerning water resources
lies not so much in gender differences of water use but in differences
with respect to access and control of water sources.
Recently, geographers have highlighted how changes in water
use and control over water resources can alter gender relations and
further divide men and womens power balance by exacerbating the

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

29

marginalization of women. For example, in a study of water irrigation


and gender in the Anatolia region of southeastern Turkey, Harris
(2006) documented how water management became important in the
maintenance and exacerbation of gender inequalities. First, among the
consequences of irrigation projects are the construction of water management committees that create new patterns of inequality in relation
to access and power associated with the management of water as a
vital resource. Specically, in the sample studied in Anatolia, membership in water committees or organizations is often restricted to heads
of household, which, in most countries, are traditionally husbands or
fathers. Similarly, the formal procedures for identication and selection
of water beneciaries implicitly or explicitly exclude women. For example, common mechanisms for obtaining water rights in the large or
newly developed irrigation systems in southeastern Turkey include the
following: (1) through owning land in the area being serviced,
(2) through participation in construction of the dams, and (3) by being
a member of the water management committee. In most countries
throughout the world, those with the most power have the most possibility to own land and be part of committees that allow access to water
(i.e., men; Lastarria-Cornheil, 1997). Therefore, women are effectively
excluded from realizing their own right to water and instead obtain
access to water by making use of the water rights of their husbands, or
other male relatives. In this manner, unequal control of water resources
exacerbates womens subordinate status.
In addition to the exclusion of women from access to water or participation in the decision-making water committees, Harris (2006) documents how daily practices of water use and water management further
widened the gender division of labor among men and women in
southeastern Turkey. For example, prior to irrigation, women were the
predominant actors in animal husbandry and sold products from animals that had been herded on open elds to earn income and provide
sustenance for their families. Women were also able to meet many subsistence needs by growing and consuming food directly from the land.
However, the introduction of large, irrigated export crops into the area
resulted in land scarcity, and the lack of land largely extinguished the
practice of animal husbandry. As such, families in many places now
purchase the produce and animal products that once determined womens limited livelihood. Thus, in effect, womens work roles were
usurped by the need to engage in unpaid, labor-intensive tasks such as
weeding and picking in their husbands crops. In contrast, their husbands were engaged in decision making regarding the uses of water
and products of the land. These changes enhanced mens relative contributions to the household and further divided the gendered power
imbalance. In this context, irrigation and control of waterregardless
of domestic usehas meant an increased work burden for women,

30

Feminism as Human Rights

greater difculty meeting the familys subsistence needs, and an exclusion from decision making. Moreover, this new labor economy has led
to a higher desirability for wives unpaid labor, and therefore, has
altered the politics of bride prices in the Anatolia region, leaving young
women dreading the heavy work burdens that come with their inevitable marriage. Finally, the changing conditions of market consumption,
driven by the introduction of irrigated export crops, have also meant
that men now travel to the market (for purchase and sale of food commodities their wives once produced) and develop social networks,
whereas, in contrast, women remain largely prohibited from traveling
to urban areas. This further exacerbates the social differences between
women and men by increasing both womens isolation and mens
socializing. The example of water use and management and gender
offered by Harris demonstrates that new water-management institutions in line with globalization serve to solidify, cement, and rigidify
social-power differentials (2006, p. 194). A stronger security of womens water tenure could signicantly alter womens participation in the
new economy and serve to empower rather than disempower them in
the context of the increasingly limited availability of water.
FORESTRY
Similarly to the development practices in water resource management, several countries (e.g., Mexico, Nepal) have privatized forests or,
more recently, organized Community Forestry Groups (CFGs) to manage forest resources in response to diminishing availability of fallen
timber for rewood collection due, in part, to conversion of forestland
for agricultural purposes (Taylor & Zabin, 2000; Varughese & Ostrom,
2001). Interestingly, the formation of CFGs was a focused attempt to
move toward establishing greater local participation and community
control among citizens in the promotion of sustainable forests (McCarthy,
2001). However, similar to water management, while major donor
agencies give token gestures to participation, a large focus remains on
economic aspects of resource management that include rigid exclusion
of nonmembers from resource use (e.g., Magrath, Grandalski, Stuckey,
Vikanes, & Wilkinson, 2007).
Although gender is typically excluded from community forestry discussions, based on extensive eldwork among CFGs in India and Nepal, Agarwal (2001) documents how gender constrains participation in
CFGs and how participatory exclusion over control of timber products
has implications for gender inequity. First, not unlike water committees, both formal and informal rules for CFGs membership exclude
women. Specically, only one member per household, the head of
household (i.e., typically the male), is allowed membership. Moreover,
long-standing conventions in South Asia exclude women from public

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

31

decision making forums and, thereby, deny women access to CFGs, despite that women are predominately responsible for gathering their
households rewood. Among the additional obstacles for womens
participation in the CFGs is included the fact that womens responsibility for housework restricts them from attending meetings held at
inconvenient times, aggressive male behavior prohibits women who
can and do attend from speaking at meetings, and social hierarchies
position women on the oor at meetings where men are seated in
chairs. As such, womens voices were not considered in the decision
making regarding the use of forest products or the discussions surrounding the use of the community funds raised from the management
of the forest. Moreover, because many CFGs invest access rights to
single owners, womens access to rewood timber, a once communal
resource, is severely restricted. Therefore, the customary exclusion of
women from village decision making bodies regarding the management
of forestry perpetuates or exacerbates gendered imbalances in power.
In the same study conducted in South Asia, Agarwal (2001) also
demonstrated how these imbalances in power are evidenced in
increased gendered divisions in labor. First, simply restricting/privatizing forestry areas means that women, who could previously meet some
of their timber collection needs in that area, are now forced to travel to
neighboring sites adding sometimes between several hours or as much
as a days time to the chore. In some areas, womens collection time
and distances traveled for a headload of rewood increased sevenfold
(from 0.5 hours to 34 hours in Vena; from 0.5 km to 89 km in
Karapara; Agarwal, 2001). In addition, women substitute fuel sources
and burn twigs or agricultural waste with detrimental consequences.
For example, the fumes from inferior fuels have negative health affects
and the additional time it takes to keep them lit prevents women from
doing simultaneous work. In sum, limiting womens access to and control over forest resources has resulted in a substantial increase in womens workload. By excluding women from control and decision making
regarding timber, the community forestry efforts are perpetuating status differences and further marginalizing women and thereby prohibiting womens capabilities and empowerment.
LAND
Issues of agrarian change and land tenure systems also have been
impacted by the imposition of a neoliberal agenda and donor agencies
that have inuenced developing countries to move from customary (often communal) systems toward private land ownership (LastarriaCornheil, 1997; Razavi, 2003). In many countries, structural adjustment
came with land reform guided by policies that favored large-scale
export agriculture, and male control over land became the dominant

32

Feminism as Human Rights

discourse supplanting previously customary laws that had provisions


regarding womens interests in land (e.g., Tsikata, 2003). Thus, one of
the imposed difculties in the transfer to privatized systems in regard
to womens rights is that there has been an erosion of the few rights
that were previously held under customary systems. In many countries, social constructions of gender, combined with cultural practices
of restricting womens access to land, have contributed to the continued abuse of womens rights to land ownership (Lastaria-Cornheil,
2001). Therefore, although many countries have legislation that declares
that men and women have equal rights to hold property, in most places
customary law dictates that men are owners of land and that women
have access to land through a male relative (Lastarria-Cornheil, 1997).
Feminist scholars suggest that restrictions on women to land ownership share core ideologies that are embedded within constructions of
masculinity and femininity and the proper roles that men and
women should assume in public spheres (Deere & Leon, 2001). For
example, women are not only marginalized from ownership of and
access to what was previously communal land, but their ability to benet fully or equitably from their labor on the land is threatened
because the landowner determines not only how the land will be used,
but also manages the products and labor of the land (LastarriaCornheil, 1997). As a result, there has been an intensication of womens unpaid agricultural labor (Razavi, 2003). In addition, property
ownership affords the landowner the opportunity to participate in a
market-based system by renting the land, using it as collateral to borrow money, and/or selling it or giving it as an inheritance to children
(Deere & Leon, 2001; Lastarria-Cornheil, 2001). As such, privatization
of land and exclusion of women as owners in patriarchal societies has
enforced and strengthened males dominant position while exacerbating womens dependent position on their husbands.
Despite slow progress, womens property rights in some countries
have improved, with land titling efforts in several Latin American and
Asian countries recognizing womens rights as beneciaries (FAO,
2004). Moreover, since the structural adjustments of the 1980s and 90s
in the agricultural sector, there has been a great deal of attention to
womens property rights from scholars in developing regions across
Africa (Lastarria-Cornheil, 1997), Latin America (Deere & Leon, 2001),
and South Asia (Agarwal, 1994). Nevertheless, global data on womens
land ownership is lacking. For example, halfway through the timeframe for achieving the Millennium Development Goals (MDG), a progress report reviewing the security of womens property rights
concluded that, despite the international call for attention to these
rights, there was insufcient data to track progress on womens property ownership (Grown, Rao Gupta, & Kes, 2008). Data collected
regarding assets and property ownership continue to be gathered at

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

33

the household level, rather than at the individual level, prohibiting an


accurate understanding of womens position as owners.
In summary, the limited review on natural resource management, in
particular water, forestry, and land suggests that strong gender biases,
and assumptions based on gender roles (e.g., decisions should fall
largely to male community leaders), exclude women, for the most part,
from decision making processes when it comes to access to and control
over vital resources. This suggests that unequal abilities to exercise
control over resources may lay the foundation for power imbalances.
In the context of development, this is a missed opportunity to restructure gender and alter womens status. Development should build institutions that manage common resources in a way that empowers all
members of the community, as opposed to further dividing existing
power structures. Moreover, while there is growing, albeit limited,
attention to the role of resource control in further marginalizing
women, there is virtually no empirical investigation into how a renegotiation of womens roles may impact their empowerment. In the context
of rising threats to womens human rights and the continued opportunity for a gender analysis in the expansion of development interventions, this work is imperative. What follows is a detailed discussion
and case study of how and why land ownership, in particular, may
impact these processes and ultimately lead to womens empowerment,
well-being, and physical safety.
LAND OWNERSHIP AND WOMENS EMPOWERMENT: A
PSYCHOLOGICAL INVESTIGATION
Based on the literature reviewed, ownership and control over natural resources among women should substantially challenge traditional
gender roles, increase womens power and inuence within the household and, in turn, provide a stronger base for womens empowerment.
In other words, ideologically shifting how women are viewed in the
household and on a societal levelthrough the process of resource
managementcan reposition women and provide structural or institutional support for human capabilities. I argue that land ownership, in
particular, is a material basis, or structural inequality, that contributes
to the subordination of women.
Central to this argument, however, is that, as landowners, women
need to effectively administer control over their property. In other
words, it is not merely possessing the title to a plot of land, but the
control or administration of it that contributes to change (Agarwal,
1994). Moreover, while benets to several forms of land use are possible and do exist (e.g., cooperative arrangements), it is important to note
that womens power regarding land occurs when they function
independently as decision makers with control over the land. While

34

Feminism as Human Rights

economic security may be attached to property ownership, it is the


psychological process that emerges from ownership that empowers a
woman to assert control over her life and her body. In other words,
the processes involved in owning and controlling land can transform
the conditions in which women exercise agency and, in turn, become
empowered to confront aspects of their subordinationas reected, for
example, by violence against women. Indeed, violence against women
is considered the most pervasive human rights violation in the world
and entrenched inequalities in the distribution of power and resources
between men and women create environments that support high levels
of gender-based violence (UNIFEM, 2006). Because land ownership
repositions women, and is a form of political and symbolic status, it
puts women in a privileged and empowered position. By empirically
examining the processes linking land ownership and control to broader
cultural ideologies, and to gender relations, a greater understanding of
the relationships between structural inequities, power/dominance,
womens empowerment and well-being can be gained.
In 1994, a link between property rights and womens rights was
introduced in the literature with the suggestion that formalizing property in a womans name could lead to benecial transformations in
gender relations and, in particular, a decline in violence against women
(Agarwal, 1994). However, it was nearly a decade later that the rst
and only published survey in this area found that in Kerala, India, as
many as 49 percent of women who did not own property suffered
long-term physical violence, compared with 18 and 10 percent, respectively, of those who owned either their land or house, and 7 percent of
those who owned both assets (Panda & Agarwal, 2005). The authors
suggest that owning land provides women with economic security and
a tangible exit option to escape violent partners. However, a wealth of
research in psychology suggests that a number of factors unrelated to
economic status prevent women from leaving violent relationships
(e.g., fear of retaliation; Hendy et al., 2003). In fact, the Kerala study
reported that levels of violence did not differ between women who
were regularly employed, seasonally employed, or unemployed, suggesting that land ownership provided a different kind of empowerment than did employment. These ndings support the notion that it
was not solely economic freedom that resulted in reduced receipt of
domestic violence. On the contrary, the ndings suggest that eliminating the practice of violence against women requires structural and ideological changes that extend beyond economic opportunity and are
focused, rather, on structural factors such as land ownership.
In 2006, the International Center for Research on Women (ICRW)
expanded on Panda and Agarwals (2005) research through qualitative
interviews with women landowners in Kerala and West Bengal to
examine the role of land in womens empowerment. They suggested

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

35

that property ownership extended womens negotiating power within the marital relationships and their ability to confront subordination,
thereby decreasing levels of domestic violence. However, a major limitation of these investigations in South Asia is that they were in communities where bilateral property ownership was the norm. Female
ownership of land did not necessarily challenge existing gender attitudes and roles. Nevertheless, these studies put forth a framework for
investigating the links between land ownership, womens empowerment, and violence.
Despite these groundbreaking ndings, this line of inquiry remains
underexplored. Moreover, there has been no investigation into this
topic in other developing regions, such as Latin America or East Africa
where land reform has received a great deal of attention. Furthermore,
virtually no attention has been given to these processes by psychologists. However, empirical examination of land ownership among
women may contribute to the discussion of womens empowerment in
the context of development, in general, and, in particular, to discussions surrounding property ownership and control. What follows sets
the context for replicating the Panda and Agarwal ndings and
expanding this investigation and examining these processes in another
region.
NICARAGUA
Of the Latin American countries that have implemented genderprogressive agrarian reform policies, Nicaragua stands out in terms of
female participation (Deere, 1985). For example, the Agrarian Reform
Laws of the 1980s and 90s that recognized equal rights for both sexes
were acknowledged as one of the most forward-looking reforms in
Latin America because, in theory, it made it possible for women to
become direct beneciaries of land allocation. Indeed, the womenspecic measures adopted in Nicaragua have beneted a greater proportion of the female population than in countries where the titling
programs are gender neutral (Deere, 1985). Although data from the rural titling ofce indicate that between 1979 and 1989, women accounted
for 8 to 10 percent of beneciaries under the agrarian reform, these low
numbers reect that land was still being allocated primarily to male
heads of household, whereas titled women were likely widowed or
unmarried women living alone.
In 1995, a major legislative leap was taken by the Nicaraguan Womens Institute by introducing provisions in agrarian legislation that
encouraged joint titling of land to couples, thereby recognizing married
womens rights to land (Act 209/95, Article 32). Subsequently, joint
titling became compulsory for married couples and for those living
in stable relationships (Act 278/97). However, as evidence of the

36

Feminism as Human Rights

customary or cultural norms, the term joint in the Joint Titling Act
was interpreted literally as two persons within the family unit.
Hence, this act did more to promote joint titling for men (fathers and
sons) than for women. Thus, despite considerable legislation that positions Nicaragua as cutting-edge in mainstreaming gender in agricultural policy, the relatively low percentage of women landowners
reects the reality that womens access remains restricted by cultural
practices that prevent the recognition of their role in property ownership and control. Nevertheless, in contrast to many other countries,
Nicaragua has the political framework for implementing development
interventions aimed at land resource distribution. However, to date,
there has been shockingly little data collected to examine the effects of
womens land ownership among women who have beneted from
some level of reform or intervention. The following study is aimed at
providing the empirical support necessary for state and development
interventions to implement more equitable policies aimed at land
ownership.
THE STUDY1
A two-group study was designed to examine whether owning land
was related to womens status and power within the marital relationship and to their overall empowerment and psychological well-being,
each of which was hypothesized to explain how and why owning land
contributed to lower levels of domestic violence. As such, a household
survey was administered to two different groups of womenone predominantly landowners and the other predominantly non-landowners.
The two groups were chosen from the same geographical location
within Nicaragua in order to most closely match them on a number of
variables. Because customary practices still largely prohibit women
from owning land, our research team collaborated with a womens organization that had a program specically aimed at facilitating womens ownership of and titling to land (intervention group). The
second group of women was selected from neighboring communities
in the same municipality and was not actively involved in the organization with which we collaborated (control group). As such, the primary difference between the two groups of women is that the majority
of women in the rst group own land, whereas the majority in the second group do not. This design allows for direct comparison of women
involved in land resource allocation aimed at empowerment and
women who were not.
Data were collected from 314 women following the guidelines on
ethics and safety developed by the World Health Organization for the
Multi-Country Study on Womens Health and Domestic Violence
Against Women that were adapted for this study (WHO, 2005). The

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

37

household surveys included the following: (1) demographic characteristics; (2) questions assessing land acquisition and land ownership
adapted from assessments used by the ICRW; (3) gender ideology as
measured by the Attitudes toward Women Scale (Spence, Helmreich, &
Stapp, 1973); (4) power in the marital relationship from the Relationship Control Subscale of the Sexual Relationship Power Scale (Pulerwitz, Gortmaker, & DeJong, 2000); (5) partner control indexed by
asking women whether their partners generally prohibited or controlled their possibilities to carry out everyday activities, or exhibited
controlling behavior or jealousy using items from the WHO (2005);
(6) empowerment as indexed by mastery over ones environment and
individual autonomy from two of the six subscales from Ryffs Scales
of Psychological Well-Being (Ryff, 1989); (7) psychological well-being
as assessed by the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Ba~
nos & Guillen, 2000)
and the Center for Epidemiologic StudiesDepression Scale (Grzywacz,
Hovey, Seligman, Arcury, & Quandt, 2006); and (8) intrahousehold decision making from two subscales that were designed by ICRW (2004)
to measure decision making within the marital relationship. Finally, to
assess womens control over their bodiesan outcome of empowermenthistory of womens violent experiences were measured with the
Conict Tactics Scale (Straus et al., 1996).
The average age of the respondents was in the early- to mid-forties
(M 45.33 intervention group, M 42.13 control group), although the
majority of the women fell between 25 and 34 years old. Approximately three-quarters of the sample were in partnered relationships
between six and ten years in duration. Most of the women respondents
were literate, although approximately a quarter of the sample never
received formal schooling.
MAIN FINDINGS2
Before proceeding to test the potential links between land ownership, womens empowerment, and receipt of violence, a series of oneway analyses of variance tests (ANOVAs) were conducted to examine
differences in levels of empowerment and violence between the two
groups. The intervention and control groups differed on several
markers of empowerment in the expected directions, suggesting that
women in the intervention group reported higher levels of empowerment and well-being than their counterparts. Specically, ANOVAs
revealed that the two groups were signicantly or marginally different
on several markers of womens empowerment: gender ideology
F (1, 308) 69.60, p < 0.00; relationship power F (1, 308) 14.72, p <
0.00; partner control/mobility F (1, 308) 2.76, p < 0.10; household decision making F (1, 308) 5.99, p < 0.02; nancial decision making
F (1, 308) 2.79, p < 0.10; autonomy F (1, 308) 3.62, p < 0.06; and

38

Feminism as Human Rights

self-esteem F (1, 308) 5.40, p < 0.03indicating that women in the


land-owning group reported higher levels of empowerment than their
counterparts. Similarly, women from the intervention group reported
marginally less violence in the past 12 months than women in the control group: F (1, 308) 2.13, p < 0.15. Finally, and importantly, main
effects from an ANOVA with employment status (employed vs. nonemployed) predicting receipt of current violence were not signicant.
A number of sociodemographic variables related to the empowerment outcomes in one or both groups of womenage, education, and
partner alcohol usewere controlled in the subsequent analyses examining the relationships between variables. To establish that land ownership was indirectly related to decreases in receipt of violence via
womens empowerment, both the Baron and Kenny (1986) criteria for
mediating conditions and a product of coefcients test were used
(MacKinnon, 2000; Sobel, 1990). Variables in the proposed model were
conceptualized in the following order: ownership status; empowerment
and psychological well-being; and violence. Specically, we hypothesized that land ownership would directly predict levels of empowerment and psychological well-being, which in turn were hypothesized
to directly predict womens receipt of violence in the past 12 months.
To test for the rst condition, several regressions were run to examine
the effect of land ownership on womens empowerment. As expected,
land ownership was marginally or signicantly related to gender ideology, relationship power, partner control/mobility, nancial decision
making, autonomy, mastery, self-esteem, and depression.
To test for the second condition, several regressions were run to
examine the effects of womens empowerment on receipt of physical
violence in the past 12 months. Relationship power, partner control/
mobility, autonomy, mastery, self-esteem, and depression were all
related to violence. Finally, to test whether land ownership was signicantly indirectly related to violence via empowerment, we calculated a
products of coefcients test for each pathway that met the rst two criteria (i.e., that the pathway from the independent variable to the process variable and the pathway from the process variable to the
dependent variable were both signicant). Results from this test provide marginal to signicant support for the indirect relation of land
ownership via: relationship power t 2.78 (p < 0.01); partner control/
mobility t 1.87 (p < 0.10); autonomy t 1.33 (p < 0.20); mastery t
1.88 (p < 0.10); self-esteem t 1.65 (p < 0.10); and depression t 1.69
(p < 0.10)suggesting that land ownership leads to decreased domestic
violence via several indicators of womens empowerment.
Given that the current study was the rst comprehensive investigation of empowerment processes in the context of natural resource
management, I examined the relations between the empowerment indicators, womens psychological well-being, and the standard indicator

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

39

of empowerment used in the social science literaturedecision making


(see Table 2.1). In addition, I hypothesized that (a) the empowerment
process would effectively alter womens subordination such that higher
levels of empowerment would predict less traditional gender roles,
and/or (b) shifts in traditional gender ideology would enhance womens power within the relationship, which would be reected in higher
levels of empowerment and psychological well-being. Only longitudinal or experimental designs can answer in which order these processes
occur; however, data from the current study indicate a pattern of signicant ndings that support a signicant relationship between less
traditional gender roles and womens empowerment. Specically, the
results suggest that less traditional gender ideology is signicantly
related to greater relationship power, less partner control, and higher
levels of autonomy, self-esteem, and household decision making. Furthermore, as expected, psychological well-being (both self-esteem and
depression) was signicantly related to all of the indicators of empowerment (relationship power, partner control, autonomy, and mastery).
Although the data do not allow us to discern the directional nature of
these ndings, the pattern of results lend support to Sens suggestion
that well-being is often inuenced by agency or autonomy (1985).
Finally, the relative lack of signicant relations between decision making and the other indicators of empowerment lends evidence to our
argument that current assessment strategies do not adequately measure
empowerment or agency, and that researchers need to begin to more
accurately assess the psychological mechanisms involved in the process
of empowerment.
The ndings from the current study therefore suggest that increasing
womens ownership of and control over land may be an important
component in addressing gender inequities, empowering women, and
curbing high levels of violence against women. Indeed, the data suggest that land ownership can alter an risk environment that supports
high levels of gender-based violence. Thus, at a minimum, state and
development policies should alter the structural barriers that prohibit
women from owning and controlling vital resources such as land.
CONCLUSION
Drawing together a very interdisciplinary literature and proposing a
novel application of psychology in this chapter allows for several important conclusions and areas of future direction in the investigation of
womens empowerment. First, the preceding review suggests that
empowerment, as it has been approached to date, is more vaguely
dened and confusing than could be useful for policy and program
implementation. Indeed, it appears that if empowerment policies and
programs continue with ambiguity and possible ineffectiveness,

Table 2.1
Correlations between empowerment, psychological well-being, and decision making variables
Gender Relationship
Partner
Ideology
Power
Control/Mobility Autonomy
Gender Ideology
Relationship
Power
Partner Control/
Mobility
Autonomy
Mastery
Self-esteem
Depression
Household
Decisions
Financial
Decisions

0.24***

Mastery

Self-esteem Depression

Household Financial
Decisions Decisions

0.12*
0.64***

0.29***
0.34***

0.09
0.32***

0.21***
0.37***

0.10t
0.38***

0.23***
0.12*

0.00
0.18**

0.23***

0.29***

0.35***

0.41***

0.04

0.09

0.51***

0.35***
0.39***

0.41***
0.49***
0.55***

0.04
0.04
0.06
0.02

0.16**
0.22**
0.05
0.16**
0.17**

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, t p < 0.10.

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

41

international development interventions aimed at empowering women


should be suspect. Therefore, the need for empowerment researchers
who can empirically evaluate empowerment-focused programs is critical in creating political change and inuencing organizations and policymakers who address womens human rights and capabilities
especially with regard to globalization. Perhaps more than most, this
area is ripe for interdisciplinary efforts and cooperative collaboration
between interventionists, activists, and researchers working for womens
human rights and social justice in an increasingly globalized context.
The ndings have implications for gender and development theory,
but more applicably for initiatives that aim to improve womens wellbeing and lead to more equitable policies for women. In particular, it
was suggested that micro-credit loaning in the international development community may be misguided and that an evaluation of natural
resource control should move to the forefront of our analysis of womens empowerment. The limited review suggests that projects and programs aimed at development in areas involving natural resources
could better improve womens rights by guaranteeing womens rights
to use of resources as well as ensuring positions in decision making in
regards to natural resource management. Indeed, the ndings from the
Nicaragua study suggest that ideologically shifting how women are
viewed on a societal levelthrough the process of land ownership and
controlrepositioned women and led to increased levels of empowerment, thereby reducing their levels of violence. In sum, this review highlights that there is an alarming need to view gender as a variable in
policy changes and that projects should ensure that appropriate infrastructures exist to support womens capabilities to exercise their rights.
The ndings also shed light on the mechanisms surrounding womens empowerment. The development literature is replete with attention to womens empowerment, although this has been in absence of
an approach that adequately denes, conceptualizes, or assesses the
complexity involved in empowerment processes. The Nicaragua study
presented is a rst attempt to apply empowerment theory, as conceptualized by psychology, to an understanding of the process by which
structural changes in status can alter womens positions and allow
them the capabilities to exert control and mastery over their own lives
and bodies. The ndings from this study provide robust data suggesting that land ownership plays an important role in (1) altering womens power and status within the household; (2) increasing womens
empowerment and psychological well-being; and (3) decreasing womens receipt of domestic violence. As such, the ndings suggest that
land ownership substantially challenges traditional gender roles and
increases womens power and inuence within the household, in turn
providing a stronger base for womens empowerment. This study
also importantly demonstrates that the conceptualization and

42

Feminism as Human Rights

methodological approach to empowerment from within the discipline


of psychology bridged the theoretical arguments surrounding human
rights with the practical implementation of development interventions,
and provided empirical support that has yet to be demonstrated
elsewhere. These groundbreaking ndings uncover the psychological
mechanisms surrounding womens empowerment and suggest that if
we are to continue putting womens empowerment at the forefront of
international attention, there is great need for psychologists in this area
of investigation. Indeed, there is great need for social interventions that
have the capacity to alter risk environments for women, particularly in
countries or regions where rates of gender-based violence are high
(Garcia-Moreno, 2002) and women are disproportionately affected by
HIV/AIDS (Dunkle et al., 2004). Through collaborative efforts, I believe
the protection of human rights by development interventions, coupled
with legislation that more broadly grants women rights, could effectively lead to the very notions of empowerment that are conceptualized
by psychologists.
NOTES
1. This was a collaborative project bringing together distinct expertise of
both science and grass roots community advocacy. It was the researchers expertise that ensured a study design that had theoretically grounded research
questions and an evaluation that used sound methodology and appropriate
assessment. It was the collaborators and program implementers expertise that
brought gender and cultural sensitivity to all aspects of the project and maintained the community relevance. Each member of the research team served an
absolutely critical role. Unique and invaluable contributions were made by (in
no particular order) the women, effort, leaders, work, dedication, and change
that make up the Xochilt-Acalt womens center; the CIERUNIC S.A. research
team led by Ver
onica Aguilera Carri
on; the suggestions, tireless translation,
and support provided by Anne McSweeney; the dedicated driving, assistance,
and support of Juan Pastor Solis Rojas; the translation and diligent commitment to womens well-being of Helen Dixon; the professional support and
coordination from Sonia Arguto at FIDEG; and the encouragement, advice, and
training provided by the Red de Mujeres Contra la Violencia.
2. For study details, analyses, and ndings, please refer to the original
manuscript Grabe & Arenas, 2009).

REFERENCES
Acosta-Belen, E., & Bose, C. E. (1990). From structural subordination to
empowerment: Women and development in third world contexts. Gender
and Society, 4, 299320.
Agarwal, B. (1994). Field of ones own: Property rights in South Asia. Cambridge,
United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

43

Agarwal, B. (2001). Participatory exclusions, community forestry, and gender:


An analysis for South Asia and a conceptual framework. World Development, 29, 16231648.
Ba~
nos, R. M., & Guillen, V. (2000). Psychometric characteristics in normal and
social phobic samples for a Spanish version of the Rosenberg self-esteem
scale. Psychological Reports, 87, 269274.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical
consideration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.
Bartky, S. (1990). Femininity and domination: Studies in the phenomenology of
oppression. New York: Routledge.
Coles, A., & Wallace, T. (2005). Gender, water and development. Oxford: United
Kingdom: Berg Publishers.
Connell, R. W. (1987). Gender and power: Society, the person and sexual politics.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Cornell Empowerment Group. (1989). Empowerment and family support. Networking Bulletin, 1, 123.
Deere, C. D. (2001). Empowering women: Land and property rights in Latin America.
Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.
Deere, C. D., & Leon, M. (2001). Empowering women: Land and property rights in
Latin America. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.
Dunkle, K. L., Jewkes, R. K., Brown, H. C., Gray, G. E., McIntryre, J. A., &
Harlow, S. D. (2004). Gender-based violence, relationship power, and risk
of HIV infection in women attending antenatal clinics in South Africa. The
Lancet, 363, 14151421.
Food Agriculture Organization. (2004). The state of food insecurity in the world.
Rome, Italy: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations.
Garcia-Moreno, C. (2002). Violence against women: Consolidating a public
health. In G. Sen, A. George, & P. Ostlin (Eds.), Engendering international
health: The challenge of equity (pp. 111142). Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts
Institute of Technology.
Goetz, A. M., & Gupta, R. S. (1996). Who takes the credit? Gender, power, and
control over loan use in rural credit programs in Bangladesh. World Development, 24, 4563.
Grabe, S., & Arenas, C. (2009). Promoting gender equality through development:
Land ownership and domestic violence in Nicaragua. Gendered perspectives on
international development (Working paper no. 295). East Lansing, MI: Gender, Development, and Globalization Program, Michigan State University.
Grown, C., Rao Gupta, G., Kes, A. (2008). Seven priorities, seven years to go: Progress on achieving gender equality. ICRW Millennium Development Goals Series Washington, DC: International Center for Research on Women.
Grzywacz, J. G., Hovey, J. D., Seligman, L. D., Arcury, T. A., & Quandt, S. A.
(2006). Evaluating short-form versions of the CES-D for measuring depressive symptoms among immigrants from Mexico. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 28, 404424.
Harris, L. M. (2006). Irrigation, gender, and social geographies of the changing
waterscapes of southeastern Anatolia. Environment and Planning D, 24,
187.

44

Feminism as Human Rights

Hendy, H. M., Weiner, K., Bakerofskie, J., Eggen, D., Gustitus, C., & McLeod, K. C.
(2003). Comparison of six models for violent romantic relationships in college men and women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 645655.
Hill, M. T. (2003). Development as empowerment. Feminist Economics, 9,
117135.
Holvoet, N. (2005). The impact of micronance on decision-making agency:
Evidence from South India. Development and Change, 36, 75102.
Ikdahl, I. (2008). Go Home & Clear the Conict: Human rights perspectives
on gender and land in Tanzania. In B. Englert & E. Daley (Eds.), Womens
land rights and privatization in Eastern Africa (pp. 1839). Oxford:
James Currey.
International Center for Research on Women. (2005). Property ownership for
women enriches, empowers, and protects: Toward achieving the third millennium
development goal to promote gender equality and empower women. Washington,
DC: Author.
International Center for Research on Women. (2006). Property ownership and inheritance rights of women for social protection: The South Asia experience. Washington, DC: Author.
Kabeer, N. (1994). Reversed realities: Gender hierarchies in development thought.
New York: Verso.
Kabeer, N. (1999). Resources, agency, achievements: Reections on the measurement of womens empowerment. Development and Change, 30, 435464.
Kabeer, N. (2001). Conicts over credit: Re-evaluating the empowerment potential of loans to women in rural Bangladesh. World Development, 29, 6384.
Kabeer, N. (2005). Is micronance a magic bullet for womens empowerment? Analysis of ndings from South Asia. Economic and Political Weekly,
47094718.
Khagram, S. (2004). Dams and development: Transnational struggles for water and
power. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Lastarria, S. (2001). Privatization of land rights and access to factor markets: A path
to gender equity? Paper presented at the Agrarian Reform and Rural Development Conference, The American University, Cairo, Egypt.
Lastarria-Cornhiel, S. (1997). Impact of privatization on gender and property
rights in Africa. World Development, 25, 13171333.
Lastarria-Cornhiel, S. (2008). Module 4: Gender issues in land policy and
administration. Gender in agriculture sourcebook (pp. 125171). Washington,
DC: World Bank Publications.
MacKinnon, D. P. (2000). Contrasts in multiple mediator models. In J. S. Rose,
L. Chassin, C. C. Presson, & S. J. Sherman (Eds.), Multivariate applications
in substance use research: New methods for new questions (pp. 141160).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Magrath, W. B., Grandalski, R. L., Stuckey, G. L., Vikanes, G. B., & Wilkinson,
R. (2007). Timber theft prevention: Introduction to security for forest managers
(Working paper no. 41242). Washington, DC: World Bank Publications.
McCarthy, J. (2005). Devolution in the woods: Community forestry as hybrid
neoliberalism. Environment & Planning, 37, 9951014.
Moghadam, V. M. (2005). Globalizing women: Transnational feminist networks.
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Womens Human Rights and Empowerment

45

Molyneux, M., & Razavi, S. (2002). Gender justice, development, and rights.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Naples, N. A., & Desai, M. (2002). Womens activism and globalization: Linking
local struggles and transnational politics. New York: Routledge.
Narayan, D. (2005). Conceptual framework and methodological challenges. In
D. Narayan (Ed.), Measuring empowerment: Cross-disciplinary perspectives
(pp. 338). Washington, DC: World Bank Publications.
Nussbaum, M. C. (2000). Women and human development: The capabilities
approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Nussbaum, M., & Sen, A. (1993). The quality of life. Oxford, United Kingdom:
Clarendon Press.
Ozer, E. M., & Bandura, A. (1990). Mechanisms governing empowerment
effects: A self-efcacy analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
58, 472486.
Panda, P., & Agarwal, B. (2005). Marital violence, human development, and
womens property status in India. World Development, 33, 823850.
Perkins, D. D. (1995). Speaking truth to power: Empowerment ideology as
social intervention and policy. American Journal of Community Psychology,
23, 765794.
Perkins, D. D., & Zimmerman, M. A. (1995). Empowerment theory, research,
and application. American Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 569579.
Phillips, A. (2002). Multiculturalism, universalism, and the claims of democracy. In M. Molyneux & S. Razavi (Eds.), Gender, justice, development, and
rights (pp. 115140). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Pulerwitz, J., Gortmaker, S. L., & DeJong, W. (2000). Measuring sexual relationship power in HIV/STD research. Sex Roles, 42, 637660.
Rappaport, J. (1987). Terms of empowerment/exemplars of prevention: Toward
a theory for community psychology. American Journal of Community Psychology, 15, 121148.
Razavi, S. (1999). Gendered poverty and well-being: Introduction. Development
and Change, 30, 409433.
Razavi, S. (2003). Introduction: Agrarian change, gender and land rights. Journal of Agrarian Change, 3, 232.
Rocheleau, D., & Edmunds, D. (1997). Women, men and trees: Gender, power
and property in forest and agrarian landscapes. World Development, 25,
13511371.
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
57, 10691081.
Sen, A. (1985). Well-being, agency, and freedom: The Dewey lectures 1984. The
Journal of Philosophy, 82, 169221.
Sen, A. (1995). Gender inequality and theories of justice. In M. Nussbaum &
J. Glover (Eds.), Women, culture, and development: A study of human capabilities (pp. 259273). New York: Oxford University Press.
Sen, A. (1999). Development as freedom. New York: Oxford University Press.
Sharp, J., Briggs, J., Yacoub, H., & Hamed, N. (2003). Doing gender and development: Understanding empowerment and local gender relations. Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers, 28, 281295.

46

Feminism as Human Rights

Shiva, V. (2002). Water wars: Privatization, pollution and prot. Cambridge, MA:
South End Press.
Sobel, M. E. (1990). Effect analysis and causation in linear structural equation
models. Psychometrika, 55, 495515.
Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R., & Stapp, J. (1973). A short version of the attitudes
towards women scale (AWS). Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 2, 219220.
Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-Mccoy, S., & Sugarman, D.B. (1996). The revised conict tactics scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283316.
Taylor, P. L., & Zabin, C. (2000). Neoliberal reform and sustainable forest management in Quintana Roo, Mexico: Rethinking the institutional framework
of the forestry pilot plan. Agriculture and Human Values, 17, 141156.
Tripp, A. M. (2002). The politics of womens rights and cultural diversity in
Uganda. In M. Molyneux, & S. Razavi (Eds.), Gender, justice, development,
and rights (pp. 413440). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
United Nations Development Fund for Women. (2006). Violence against women:
Facts and gures. New York: United Nations Development Fund
for Women.
United Nations Development Programme. (1995). Human development report
1995. New York: Oxford University Press.
United Nations 4th World Conference on Women. (1995). Report of the fourth
world conference on women No. A/Conf.177/20). Beijing, China: United
Nations.
United Nations General Assembly, 52nd session. (2000). United Nations millennium declaration No. A/Res/55/2. New York: United Nations.
Varughese, G., & Ostrom, E. (2001). The contested role of heterogeneity in collective action: Some evidence from community forestry in Nepal. World
Development, 29, 747765.
Wingood, G. M., & DiClemente, R. J. (2000). Application of the theory of gender and power to examine HIV-related exposures, risk factors, and effective interventions for women. Health Education & Behavior, 27, 539.
World Bank. (1993). Water resource management. Washington, DC: International
Bank for Reconstruction and Development/World Bank.
World Health Organization (WHO). (2005). WHO multi-country study on
womens health and domestic violence against women: Summary report of
initial results on prevalence, health outcomes and womens responses. Geneva,
Switzerland: Author.
Zimmerman, M. A. (1990). Taking aim on empowerment research: On the distinction between individual and psychological conceptions. American Journal of Community Psychology, 1, 169177.
Zimmerman, M. A. (1995). Psychological empowerment: Issues and illustrations. American Journal of Community Psychology. Special Issue: Empowerment
Theory, Research, and Application, 23, 581599.
Zwarteveen, M. Z. (1997). Water: From basic need to commodity: A discussion
on gender and water rights in the context of irrigation. World Development,
25, 13351349.

Chapter 3

Missing Children and Child Abductions:


An International Human Rights Issue
Michele A. Paludi
Katie L. Kelly

One missing child is one too many.


John Walsh

Child abduction and missing children are problems that affect families
around the world. The National Center for Missing and Exploited Children (2009) reported that each day, an average of 2,185 children are
missing. This means during the course of writing this chapter, more
than 131,100 children were reported missing. Child Find of America
(2009) and Boudreaux, Lord, and Etter (2000) reported that approximately 203,900 children each year are abducted by a parent or family
member in violation of a court decree, custody order, or other custodial
rights. Thus, during the course of co-editing and publishing this threevolume book set, more than 407,800 children were abducted by a noncustodial parent, and 1,595,050 children were missing in general.
Furthermore, the incidence of international child abduction increases
each year as a consequence of the ease of international travel and
increase in bicultural relationships (Moskowitz, 2005). The Report on
Compliance with the Hague Convention (2008) indicated that in the
2007 scal year, the U.S. Central Authority assisted left-behind
parents in the United States in responding to 575 cases of international
parental child abductions involving 821 children. In addition, the
Department of State assisted in the return to the United States in 2007
of 341 children abducted to or wrongfully retained in other countries.
Of these 341 children, 217 returned from countries that are Hague

48

Feminism as Human Rights

Convention (to be discussed subsequently in this chapter) partners


with the United States. Two hundred sixty-two children that were
abducted to or wrongfully retained in the United States were returned
to their country of origin in 2007. Convention countries with the highest incidence of reported abductions to the United States in 2007 were
Mexico, United Kingdom, Germany, Canada, and Australia. Recovery
rates for Hague Convention countries vary by how well the courts
implement the treaty (Report on Compliance with the Hague Convention, 2008). Rates of recovery of abducted children in countries not part
of the Hague Convention are extremely low.
More than 1.5 million children experience a runaway or throwaway
episode during a given year in the United States. The National Center
for Missing and Exploited Children (2009) reported that one in seven
children aged 10 to 17 received a sexual solicitation over the Internet.
In addition, 34 percent of these children experienced an unwanted exposure to sexually related material, including pictures of naked individuals or people engaging in sex. The National Center for Missing
and Exploited Children (2009) further reported that one in ve girls
and one in ten boys will be sexually victimized before they reach adulthood. When names and faces are attached to these statistics, the enormity of the problem is sobering. As Stanley (2003) noted: The names
of the missing are as common as those of the children who live next
door. . . . (p. 16).
The abduction of a child or adolescent has a radiating impact and
can thus impact the life experiences of all individuals in the childs
community and country, transforming parents, siblings, grandparents,
friends, clergy, and teachers into victims as well (Boudreaux et al.,
2000; Fravel & Boss, 1992; Penner, Dovidio, & Albrecht, 2000; Plass,
2007; Report on Compliance with the Hague Convention, 2008;
Spilman, 2006; Tedisco & Paludi, 1996). Most children and adolescents
experience an immediate postvictimized generalized distress response
that is characterized as a state of psychological shock, including intrusive waking images or dreams, depression, and emotional numbing.
Thus, they repeatedly reexperience the victimization. The extent of the
psychological and physical damage that is experienced by missing children and adolescents is severe and may not surface for several years
(Finkelhor & Browne, 1988; Report on Compliance with the Hague
Convention, 2008; Tedisco & Paludi, 1996).
In this chapter we discuss types of abductions and missing children,
including runaways, throwaways, stranger abductions, and noncustodial parental abductions (Finkelhor, Hotaling, & Sedlak, 1992; Gullotta,
2005). We also discuss the empirical research on the impact of abductions on children and adolescents, including the effects on their
psychological and physical well-being, achievement and career development, interpersonal relationships, and self-concept. Furthermore, we

Missing Children and Child Abductions

49

review the psychological research on perpetrators of abductors, including repeat offenses; the impact of power, not sex, on their modus operandi; and their manipulation process. We review the legislation
concerning missing children and child abductions. We offer recommendations for future research and responsibilities of educators, including
curriculum integration projects for children and adolescents. Finally,
we discuss a feminist restructuring of the legislation for international
abductions.
John Walsh, whose son, Adam, was abducted and murdered, once
stated that he wished that he and his wife spent more time encouraging Adam to respect his safety rather than respecting adults authority:
If I had taught him to scream, he might be alive now (quoted in Gelman, 1984, p. 86).
Personal statements help break the silence surrounding child abductions and missing children. As Tedisco and Paludi (1996) noted,
silence enhances our illusion of the invulnerability for non victims. It
is because none of our children and adolescents are safe that the
silence must be broken (p. 20).
Tedisco and Paludi used the just world hypothesis (Lerner, 1980)
to help explain why individuals are reluctant to accept the reality of
child abductions. Individuals believe that bad things occur only to
those people who bring on or deserve the consequences of their
actions. We tend to nd a personal reason to explain the abduction:
something the child said, did, wore, and so on. This is a coping mechanism for them since the alternative realization is frightening: It could
happen to me or to someone I love. It is our goal is to have this chapter contribute to break the silence surrounding child abductions, especially those abductions occurring within the context of intimate partner
violence.

TYPES OF MISSING CHILDREN AND CHILD ABDUCTIONS


Stranger Abduction
A stranger or nonfamily abduction is dened as an incident in
which a nonfamilial acquaintance or stranger detains or takes a child
without lawful authority or permission from the childs parents or
legal guardians primarily for the purposes of ransom, sadistic, or
sexual assault or murder (Boudreaux, Lord, & Dutra, 1999; Finkelhor,
Hammer, & Sedlak, 2002; Gallagher, Bradford, & Pease, 2002).
Boudreaux et al. (1999) dened a stranger as an individual the victim has never met before the abduction and who is not part of the
childs immediate family. Stereotypical abduction refers to abduction
that occurs in conjunction with ransom, murder, or the abductors
intent to keep the child permanently (Asdigian, Finkelhor, & Hotaling,

50

Feminism as Human Rights

1995; Finkelhor et al., 2002). Findings from the National Incidence Studies of Missing, Abducted, Runaway and Thrownaway Children (NISMAET) (Sedlak, Finkelhor, Hammer, & Schultz, 2002) estimated that
stranger abductions account for approximately 2 percent of all abductions. The following categories of stranger abductors have been identied in the literature (Tedisco & Paludi, 1996): pedophiles, serial killers,
proteers, and childless psychotics. Each of these categories are summarized below.
Pedophiles
Pedophilia is dened by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders of the American Psychiatric Association (2000) as:
recurrent, intense, sexual urges and sexually arousing fantasies, of at
least six months duration, involving sexual activity with a prepubescent
child. The age of the child is generally 13 or younger. The age of the person is arbitrarily set at age 16 years or older and at least ve years older
than the child.

A pedophile is thus a signicantly older individual who prefers to


have sex with persons legally considered children and whose sexual
fantasies and erotic imagery focus on children (Stevens, 2001). We note
that not all pedophiles abduct children (Greenberg, Bradford, & Curry,
1993; Lanning, 1994; Salter, 2004). Pedophiles are more commonly sexually molested at a younger age than individuals who were not sexually
molested and are not pedophiles (Greenberg et al., 1993; Lang &
Langevin, 1991; Levant & Bass, 1991; Stevens, 2001). Individuals who
are pedophiles have experienced abuse and other forms of ineffectual
parenting and disciplining, characterized by anger, hostility, and laissez-faire parenting (Stevens, 2001).
Individuals who are pedophiles thus identify with children better
than with adults (Bahroo, 2005; Levant & Bass, 1991; Salter, 2004;
Spilman, 2006). This identication with children allows them to seduce
children; they understand their problems at home or at school (Tedisco & Paludi, 1996). The majority of pedophiles use child pornography
for sexual arousal and gratication. This pornography is a prelude to
sexual activity with children (Salter, 2004). Furthermore, pedophiles use
pornography to lower childrens inhibitions (Lanning, 1994; Stevens,
2001).
Serial Killers
Stranger abductors who are serial killers use domination, power,
and control as motivational themes (Beasley, 2004; Drukteinis, 1992).

Missing Children and Child Abductions

51

They frequently move from state to state abducting and murdering


their victims.
Proteers
Proteers are dened as individuals who are criminal exploiters
who sell children to pornographers, trafckers, or adoptive parents
(Chase & Statham, 2005; Hunt & Baird, 1990; Rafferty, 2007, 2008;
Simkhada, 2008). Trafcking is characterized by physical and emotional
trauma, humiliation, degradation, and violence associated with treatment of children as a commodity (Ireland, 2006; Rafferty, 2008).
Childless Psychotics
Children, especially infants (children under two years of age), may
be abducted by individuals who have not been able to give birth to
children of their own or have recently miscarried a child or had a child
that was stillborn (Shogan, 2006; Strohman, 2005).
CHARACTERISTICS OF STRANGER ABDUCTORS
Most child abductors are male who are motivated by power, dominance, and control, not sex (Doyle & Paludi, 1997). Abductors may
fuse aggression with sexuality. Malamuth and Check (1981), for example, reported that some men have a heightened sexual arousal as
stimulated by graphic scenes of sexual violence. Abductors target victims within their own race or ethnic group. Hanand, Keppel, and
Weis (1997) reported that abductors average age is 27 years old. Most
abductors are unmarried. Hanand et al. (1997) further found that half
of the abductors studied live alone or with their parents, and half are
unemployed and have a history of sexual, alcohol, drug, and mental
problems. Two-thirds of the abductors have been arrested for violent
crimes. When women abduct children, they do so for emotional satisfaction (e.g., childless psychotics).
Girls are more likely to be abducted by strangers as well as family
members; boys are more likely to be abducted by family members. In
addition, older children are more likely to be victimized than younger
children since they are independent of the supervision of parents and
other adults (Finkelhor, 1995).
Apparent Normalcy
Salter (2004) reported that stranger abductors appear to be normal. This apparent normalcy creates the opportunity for children to
accompany a stranger or noncustodial parent. There is no research to

52

Feminism as Human Rights

date that suggests that an abductor can be easily identied by his or


her blatant mistreatment of children or adolescents (Tedisco & Paludi,
1996; Quina & Carlson, 1989). Children, especially young children, do
not know who strangers really are. Thus, potential abductors may not
be labeled by children as strangers because they do not meet the
childs own denition of the term.
Modus Operandi
Research has identied that abductors are repeat offenders with a
modus operandi (Freeman-Longo & Wall, 1986; Quina & Carlson,
1989). The majority of abductors have committed hundreds of abuses
for 10 to 15 years or more prior to their rst arrests. According to
Courtois (1988), the child is
manipulated by the unequal power in the relationship that is by the relationship with the perpetrator on whom she is dependent. The child is
further coerced by the perpetrators strong desire to keep the activity a
secret, which has the purpose of minimizing intervention and allowing
repetition. (p. 6)

Burgess and Holmstrom (1974) identied two kinds of modus operandi that apply to child abductors: blitz attack and condence
assault. In the blitz attack, a stranger appears suddenly, for example,
jumps out of a car in front of a child walking on a street. Children are
in shock at this occurrence and this shock interferes with any defensive action they may take. In addition, the shock of this individuals
behavior precludes the child from seeing or remembering facts about
the individuals appearance, comments, and so forth at a later time.
The condence assault is an elaborate scheme set up by the abductor. Lanning (1994) refers to this condence assault as a psychological
assault rather than a physical one. There are stages to the condence
assault. It rst starts with gaining the condence or trust of the targeted victim. This trust is then used to manipulate the child into physical and psychological vulnerability. By the time the child realizes the
individual is violent, the childs options for escape are limited by the
abductor. The abductor continues the condence assault by convincing
the child that he or she is a participant in the crime or caused the
crime (Quina & Carlson, 1989).
LURES USED BY STRANGER ABDUCTORS
Research has outlined several lures used by stranger abductors
(Boudreaux et al., 2000; Burgess & Holmstrom, 1974; Tedisco & Paludi,
1996): asking children for directions, asking children to help locate a

Missing Children and Child Abductions

53

missing pet, informing the child that their parent has been injured in
an accident and is hurt, ringing the doorbell when the child is home
alone to gain entry into the house, and offering to give children a ride
home.
In addition, these lures are used with vulnerable children, such as
children who are quiet, who appear to have an intense need for adult
affection and approval, are withdrawn, have poor social skills with
children their own age, or are from divorced homes. Other examples of
vulnerability were outlined by Huttinger (1984), including children
who walk alone to or from school, wait for a school bus by themselves,
ride a bicycle alone or at night, wait for rides in parking lots after dark,
or are wearing articles of clothing that prominently display their names
(permits abductors to portray familiarity). Boudreaux et al. (2000)
reported that abductors select victims from areas where they feel safe
and where their risks in abducting children are minimized.

ABDUCTION HOMICIDE
According to the Ofce of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention (Boudreaux, Lord, & Jarvis, 2001), one in four abducted children is
murdered by their abductor. Hanand et al. (1997) reported that
abductors who murder children are more likely to conceal the body
than murderers in general. The murder of children by abductors is
caused by one of three factors (Lanning, 1994): (1) inadvertent, where
the abductor may not have intended to murder the child; (2) indiscriminate, where the abductor may or may not have chosen the victim;
and (3) most commonly, murder to avoid detection. Most victims of
stranger abductions are likely to be murdered immediately or within
24 hours (Boudreaux et al., 1999; Brown & Keppel, 2006; Hanand
et al., 1997). Sex-related murders of abducted children are common
(Heide, Beauregard, & Myers, 2009). Finkelhor et al. (2002) reported
that in 40 percent of stranger kidnappings, the child is murdered. Children younger than four years and adolescents between 13 and 17 years
are at highest risk for murder (Dalley, 2000).

Abductors Explanations for Abductions


Strangers who abduct children have predictable reactions (Lanning,
1994), including denial of engaging in the abduction, minimizing their
actions, justifying the abduction, fabrication of the reason for the abduction (e.g., conducting research on missing children), having a mental illness, and pleading nolo contendere to avoid civil liability. Research by
Quina and Carlson (1989) and Russell (1975) has addressed the following psychological proles of child abductors. They

54

Feminism as Human Rights


1. express little or no concern, trust, or empathy for others, especially their
victims;
2. cannot express anger in ways that are not violent, and express their anger toward children because the latter are less likely to confront their
power;
3. typically view sexual victimization as an element of the masculine gender role in American culture;
4. may have been sexually abused themselves;
5. believe that the abduction does not have serious consequences for their
victims;
6. are likely to be repeat offenders since the underlying psychological problems are not resolved by the abduction.

RUNAWAYS AND THROWAWAYS


According to Hammer, Finkelhor, and Sedlak (2002), runaway and
throwaway youth constitute the largest component of missing children
each year. These youth make up approximately 45 percent of all children reported missing during each year. Approximately 1.6 and 2.8
million children and adolescents run away or are thrown out of their
homes in a year (Hammer et al., 2002; National Runaway Switchboard,
2009; Sanchez, Waller, & Greene, 2006). Child Find of America (2009)
found that approximately 5,000 runaways die on the streets of this
country each year.
Runaways are dened as children of 14 years or younger who leave
home without permission from a parent or guardian for at least one
night. For older adolescents, runaways are dened as individuals who
stay away from home without permission for at least two nights. Situational runaways are comprised of youth who leave home for a day or
two following a disagreement with their parent(s). This is the largest
group of runaways (Denoff, 1991; Pennbridge, Yates, David, & Mackenzie,
1990; Sanchez et al., 2006). Chronic runaways do not return to their
home but rather live in transitory housing, shelters, abandoned buildings, or underneath highway bridges (Giffords, Alonso, & Bell, 2007;
Sanchez et al., 2006).
Throwaways are dened as youth who leave home because their
parent(s) abandoned them, told them to leave, or subjected them to
extreme abuse or neglect. Throwaways (or thrownaways) are linked
with runaways; for example, it is common for youth to run away
after parents threaten them with throwing them out of their homes
(Gullotta, 2005).
Child Find of America (2009) and Kempf-Leonard and Johansson
(2007) reported that the majority of youth who run away or are thrown

Missing Children and Child Abductions

55

away are between 15 and 17 years old, with the gender ratio equal.
Reasons for children and adolescents running away include family
problems, physical abuse, peer pressure, alcohol and other drug abuse,
and sexual abuse. (Child Find of America, 2009; Khong, 2008; Peled &
Muzicant, 2008; Thompson & Pillai, 2006). Youth who run away
because of sexually abusive homes are referred to as terried runners (Finkelhor & Dziuba-Leatherman, 1974; Greene & Esselstyn,
1972; Quina & Carlson, 1989).
Palenski and Launer (1987) identied a process of running away that
involves the following stages: family disengagement, effects of friends
as role models, recognizing the right situation, shrinking alternatives,
and managing the residuals. Runaway youth report their involvement
with their families was at a minimum (family disengagement). As their
involvement with their family becomes less frequent, peers become
more attractive. They witness friends running away from their family
to deal with the disengagement. Palenski and Launer (1987) noted in
their research that youth did not perceive running away as a feasible
solution until they saw peers with similar family problems leave home.
Palenski and Launer (1987) also found that youth decide to run
away when they are in a state of extreme turmoil or in one of extreme
passivity (recognizing the right situation). Furthermore, youth who run
away do so when other alternatives to dealing with family problems
no longer seem viable (shrinking alternatives). If the difculties the
youth was facing did not necessitate bringing in authorities, for example, law enforcement, then an alternative to running away was seen as
likely. However, once a third party becomes involved, youth believe
that running away is the best way to protect themselves (Tedisco &
Paludi, 1996).
Palenski and Launer (1987) also found that youth had misgivings
about running away (managing the residuals). Youth have to consider
the realities of street life, including food, shelter, and school. There are
some runaway programs that can assist youth with these realities
(Franks & Goswami, 2009; Thompson & Pillai, 2006). However, most
youth receive assistance from abductors and abusers they encounter on
the street. Runaway youth are the people most vulnerable to further
abuse (Gullotta, 2005; Molino, 2007; Terrell, 1997).
Huttinger (1984), for example, noted that local and national call
services and buy-a-kid rings sell runaway children for a night or permanently (p. 112). Furthermore, abductors use runaways as accomplices to their own crimes (Tedisco & Paludi, 1996). In addition, runaway
youth do not have health insurance nor access to medical care. Consequently, they are prone to diseases (Millstein, 1989). Runaway youth
are considered to be in the highest groups for human immunodeciency virus (HIV) infection (Millstein, 1989); many engage in prostitution to obtain money for their food, shelter, and drugs (Flowers, 2001).

56

Feminism as Human Rights

The life of a runaway or throwaway is not the romantic, idealized


life once considered by adolescents who, because of their stage of cognitive development, believe nothing bad could ever happen to them
because of their uniqueness (Elkind, 1967). What may have been seen
to be an improvement for runaways problems with their families is in
reality an intolerable burden. As Tedisco and Paludi (1996) stated:
Runaway teens are not prepared for the callousness and indifference of
individuals they encounter on the street, let alone the cruelty of some
individuals who are waiting for the opportunity to abuse them even
more. . . . These adolescents as well as those who appear alone and
frightened are easily spotted and are quickly identiable as the new
kids. (Huttinger, 1984)

NONCUSTODIAL PARENTAL ABDUCTION


The U.S. Department of Justice reported that approximately 203,900
children were victims of a family abduction, as indicated in the
national NISMART study (2002). Noncustodial parental abduction is
dened as taking and keeping a child in violation of the custodial
parents rights. Child Find of America (2009) noted that 35 percent of
children abducted by a noncustodial parent are between six and eleven
years of age. Children younger than three years of age are typically not
abducted by a noncustodial parent because they create unique problems for the abductor: the children need constant supervision and care.
Older children and adolescents are likely to telephone home, run away,
and alert law enforcement agencies, and are thus are not likely to be
abducted by a noncustodial parent (Hyde & Hyde, 1985). Approximately 24 percent of the abductions last between one week and one
month. Included in this category of abductions are relatives of the child
who take the child on behalf of the noncustodial parent. Child Find of
America (2009) reported that 21 percent of noncustodial abductions are
committed by relatives.
The reasons why noncustodial parents and other relatives abduct
children are varied, including dissatisfaction with the courts custody
decision, denial of visitation for not paying child support, anger with
the end of their relationship and using the children as pawns in this
argument, and anger with the custodial parents new partner or lifestyle (Abrams, 1983; Allender, 2007; Child Find of America, 2009;
Donner, 2006; Johnston, Girdner, & Sagatun-Edwards, 1999). Abrams
(1983) found that malice and revenge are the most common motives
for noncustodial parental abductors who have a get-even mentality
(p. 126). In addition, a parent may fear their child is or will be abused
by the other parent and thus abduct the child to protect her or him

Missing Children and Child Abductions

57

(Humphreys, 1995; Levendosky, Lynch, & Graham-Bermann, 2000). We


will discuss this further subsequently in the chapter when we review
the literature on abductions occurring in the context of intimate partner
violence.
Hegar and Greif (1991) surveyed 371 parents who requested assistance from missing childrens organizations in order to prole families
where abduction by a noncustodial parent or relative occurred. Hegar
and Greif noted that abductors were less educated and less likely to be
employed than searching parents. The majority of noncustodial abductions involved a single child, and 55 percent of the abducting parents
were male. In addition, Hegar and Greif found that mothers who
children were abducted by their fathers reported more intimate partner
violence in their marriage, more fault-related reasons for divorce,
and more force used in abducting the children than did fathers whose
children were kidnapped by their mothers. Similarly, Cole and Bradford (1992) found abducting parents were more likely to be male, not
born in America, and had previous psychiatric histories and previous
criminal charges.
Noncustodial parental abductors tell the children that the custodial
parent is unt, ill, dead, or will kill them if they return. Furthermore,
these abductors tell children that the custodial parent has a new relationship and a new family and is not interested in having them
returned (Tedisco & Paludi, 1996). Many of these children are also
physically and/or sexually abused by the abducting parent (Faller,
1991; Hegar & Greif, 1991; Johnston et al., 1999). Consequently, noncustodial parental abductors move frequently and change their names and
the names of their children. Children describe themselves as pawns
between their feuding parents. They thus have little opportunity to develop a sense of trust, which is an important part of developing an
identity (Donner, 2006).
The National Center for Missing and Exploited Children (2009) identied conditions under which the likelihood of an international abduction of a child is great: previous abduction, friends and/or family
living in another country, nancially independent or can work anywhere, recently sold a home, closed a bank account, liquidated assets,
no strong ties to the childs home state, instability in relationships,
and/or a prior criminal record. The Report on Compliance with the
Hague Convention (2008) noted that parents who abduct their children
to other countries are similar to those who abduct their children to
other states within the United States:
They often have young children. They usually have support from family
or other individuals for what they are doing. They generally do not value
the other parents relationship with the child. Some are convinced their
actions are justied because they believe they rescued their child from

58

Feminism as Human Rights


the hands of an abusive parent. Many feel disenfranchised from American
society, and separation and divorce have intensied their sense of alienation. Some are eeing domestic violence, whereas others are controlling
and abusive themselves. (p. 1)

As a consequence of abducting parents returning to their country of


birth, the physical distance makes locating, recovering, and returning
children who were internationally abducted difcult (Girdner, 1994). In
addition, lack of sufcient funds to help locate the abducted child
makes locating the child complex, as do difculties with foreign laws
and U.S. laws.
IMPACT OF ABDUCTIONS ON CHILDREN AND FAMILIES
Research in child abductions, including international abductions, has
documented impact on several areas of functioning, including emotional/psychological, physiological or health-related, academic, interpersonal, and self-perception (Finkelhor, Hotaling, & Sedlak, 1990;
Forehand, Long, Zogg, & Parrish, 1989). Psychological problems stemming from abductions are subtle and may not surface for several years.
In addition, young children who have been abducted and abused may
not be able to verbalize the impact of this victimization until they are
older (Tedisco & Paludi, 1996).
Examples of emotional/psychological effects of abductions include,
but are not limited to, guilt, denial, withdrawal from social settings,
shame, depression, fear, anger, anxiety, phobias, isolation, fear of
crime, helplessness, frustration, shock, and decreased self-esteem
(Hatcher, Barton, & Brooks, 1992; Terr, 1983).
The following are reported physical/health-related effects of abductions: headaches, tiredness, respiratory problems, substance abuse,
sleep disturbances, eating disorders, lethargy, gastrointestinal disorders, and inability to concentrate (Finkelhor et al., 1990).
The impact of abductions on social and interpersonal relationships
has included the following: withdrawal, fear of new people, lack of
trust, changes in social network patterns, and relationship difculties
(Forehand, Long, Zogg, & Parrish, 1989; Schetky & Haller, 1983). Children victims of international abductions have difculty making friends
and thus forming this aspect of their identity (Report on Compliance
with the Hague Convention, 2008).
Koss (1990) and Lundberg-Love and Marmion (2006) noted that
adult women who were victims of child sexual abuse had identiable
degrees of impairment when compared with nonvictims, including
clinical depression, drug abuse, panic, obsessive-compulsive symptoms,
suicidal ideation, or deliberate attempts at self-harm. Plass (2007) noted
that in addition to symptoms stemming from the abduction per se,

Missing Children and Child Abductions

59

children and adolescents experience secondary victimization; that is,


boys are likely to be physically assaulted, and girls are likely to be sexually assaulted.
Children and adolescents who try to escape the abductor frequently
experience increased violence and the threat of death, blame for the
abduction and abuse, and/or death of their family members. In addition they respond to the victimization by developing a dissociative disorder as a psychological protection. Walker (1992) noted that in
children younger than ve years, multiple personalities may develop
from the dissociation. Bryant (1992) found that children may become
preverbal and may continue in a state of chronic shock if not permitted
to express feelings related to the trauma. Greif and Hegar (1993), in
their longitudinal study of the impact of abductions on children and
their parents, reported: those children who were doing the most
poorly had been missing longer, had been reunited with their families
for a shorter period of time, had no contact with the abductor, and
reportedly had a worse abduction experience (p. 54).
In addition, the U.S. Department of Justice (cited in Report on Compliance with the Hague Convention, 2008) noted that children who are
victims of international abductions suffer greater effects associated with
adapting to new cultural norms and values. Furthermore, they may miss
months or years of education. Their names are frequently changed, as
are their birthdates and physical appearance so as to conceal their identity (Report on Compliance with the Hague Convention, 2008). According to the Report on Compliance with the Hague Convention (2008):
If and when the child is reunited with the left-behind parent, they may nd
that they no longer have a relationship with that parent or even a language
in common. They may be distrustful of the left-behind parent and question
why that parent did not try harder to get them back. They may nd that the
left-behind parent has remarried and that they have a new, unfamiliar stepparent and siblings. Children who were abducted while very young may
not even remember life with the left-behind parent. (p. 48)

Spilman (2006) and Janvier, McCormick, and Donaldson (1990)


noted that parents experience trauma vicariously with their children.
Greif and Hegar (1991) also reported parents of abducted children
experienced feelings of rage, loss, loss of appetite, impaired sleep, and
depression. Forehand, Long, Zogg, and Parrish (1989) reported that the
trauma for parents did not end when the child was recovered. Hatcher
et al. (1993) noted that the majority of the parents they studied had
concerns that their child would be reabducted. When an international
child abduction occurs, left-behind parents may have to deal with
unfamiliar cultural, legal, and linguistic barriers while searching for
their child (Report on Compliance with the Hague Convention, 2008).

60

Feminism as Human Rights

Certainly nancial pressures of dealing with child abductions, both


domestic and international, make the emotional symptoms more pronounced. Left-behind parents of children who have been internationally abducted may have insufcient funds to travel to another country
and may not have an attorney who is familiar with legal issues of
abduction in that country.
Fravel and Boss (1992) noted that siblings of abducted children
develop symptoms of developmental regression and phobias. In addition, parents of abducted children overprotect the remaining children
(Forehand, Long, Zogg, & Parrish, 1989).

EDUCATIONAL PROGRAMS ON CHILD ABDUCTIONS AND


MISSING CHILDREN
Research with children (and adults) suggests that child abductions
are not well understood (e.g., Bromberg, 1997; Finkelhor & DziubaLeatherman, 1995; Newman, 1985). Tedisco and Paludi (1996) identied
myths commonly believed by individuals that are used to explain
away child abductions or diminish the impact of abductions to victims and their families. These myths include:
. An abductor is a psychotic human being, easily identiable by children
and adults.
. There are no long-term affereffects of abductions for those who are found.
. It is only young, helpless children who are the prey of abductors.
. Runaway children and adolescents are not targeted for abductions.
. Parental abduction is not a serious matter and is not a form of child
abuse.
. Abductions do not happen here to the people I knowthey happen
somewhere else.

Tedisco and Paludi (1996) noted that these myths contribute to keeping the issue of child abductions hidden. Curriculum training programs disclose the hidden issue and create an empowering learning
environment for children and adolescents. Training programs in general include three major components: (1) needs assessment, (2) facilitating the training programs, and (3) posttraining evaluations (Badhesha,
Schmidtke, Cummings, & Moore, 2008). Poorly conceptualized and
poorly facilitated training programs on child abductions and missing
children cause more harm than good (Bromberg, 1997; Johnson et al.,
2006). Despite the fact that children and adolescents may like learning
about safety education, the impact of such training may not translate
into sustained, positive results that are transferred out of the training
program. We hope that the approach identied in this next section will

Missing Children and Child Abductions

61

assist schools in measuring student behaviors and school outcomes,


not solely changes in student attitudes and perceptions.
NEEDS ASSESSMENT
There are several topics to be discussed in any training program on
abductions (e.g., legal denitions, behavioral examples, the schools
policy, common lures). In keeping with the literature in human
resource management (e.g., DeCenzo & Robbins, 2007; Dessler, 2009),
we recommend conducting a needs assessment with children and adolescents as well as school personnel to identify additional issues they
expect to be covered in a training session. Examples of additional
topics include how to tell parents about a potential abduction and how
to discuss feeling uncomfortable with a family member touching them.
Brown (2002) identied four reasons why needs assessments must
be conducted prior to facilitating training programs: to (1) identify
problem areas in the schools training, policy, and procedures;
(2) obtain school administrators support; (3) develop data for measuring the effectiveness of the training program; and (4) determine the
costs and benets of the training program. Needs assessments may be
conducted through anonymous surveys and/or focus groups (Lucier,
2008). The main goal of the needs assessment, therefore, is the establishment of mutual trust and respect, methods of inclusion, and verbal
and nonverbal communication. The process of the assessment will be
consistent with the goal of the training programs in which the children
and adolescents will subsequently participate (Tyler, 2002).
During the needs assessment, several factors must be addressed by
schools to establish an effective training program for children and adolescents on abductions (Tedisco & Paludi, 1996):
1. Parental notication and permission to teach about child abductions and
missing children.
2. Contacts with law enforcement who can assist with childrens reports of
experiences prompted by the training program.
3. A policy and procedure for children to use to report a potential abduction.

EDUCATIONAL QUALIFICATIONS OF TRAINERS


The following qualications of trainers of children and adolescents
on the topic of abductions have been noted in the literature (e.g., Tedisco & Paludi, 1996):
1. Knowledge of child and adolescent development, including cognitive,
emotional personality, peer, and physical development.

62

Feminism as Human Rights


2. Knowledge of research on sexual victimization, including intimate partner violence, child abuse, and child sexual abuse.
3. Expertise in explaining psychological terms in nontechnical language.
4. Knowledge of case law, legislation, and pending legislation at the state
and federal levels.
5. Fluency in languages in addition to English or have a co-trainer who can
meet this need.

TRAINING PROGRAM
Training Goals
Goals for training programs that have been identied in the child
abduction literature (Bromberg, 1997; Newman, 1985; Tedisco & Paludi,
1996) include:
1. Dene missing, including nonparental abductions, runaways, throwaways and stranger abductions.
2. Educate students about the legal denitions and behavioral examples of
abductions.
3. Dene stranger for children and adolescents to include family
acquaintances, neighbors, and individuals with whom families have
brief contact, e.g., letter carriers, television repairperson.
4. Discuss unsafe places.
5. Discuss the impact of being abducted on childrens and adolescents
emotional and physical well being.
6. Discuss the effects of abductions on parents, siblings, friends, and
school mates.
7. Tell children and adolescents what their rights and responsibilities are
with respect to abductions.
8. Empower children and adolescents to take control of their bodies.
9. Discuss childrens and adolescents fears and anxieties about abductions.
10. Discuss common lures of abductors.
11. Assist in making the child and adolescent knowledgeable about safety
strategies to make the abductors job of luring them more difcult.
12. To create an environment that is free of the fear of retaliation for speaking out about child abductions.

Once the goals have been established, the schools policies and procedures regarding reporting potential child abductions must be revised,
taking into account new case law and research from the behavioral sciences (see Tedisco & Paludi, 1996). Because the policy statements and

Missing Children and Child Abductions

63

procedures are part of the training session content, they must be completed prior to the training.
Training programs on child abduction awareness involve more than
a recitation of individuals rights and responsibilities and what the
law and school policy requires. Training also requires dealing with
childrens and adolescents assumptions and misconceptions, as well
as the anxieties about the training itself. Thus, training sessions must
devote ample time to dealing with the childrens and adolescents
feelings, misconceptions, and questions. Children and adolescents may
want to discuss topics related to abductions and abuse following the
training program with the trainer without hearing any comments from
peers. Because child abduction and abuse are intimate for children
and adolescents, they are unlikely to ask questions in public.

COGNITIVE MATURITY OF CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS


Effective curriculum projects take into account the cognitive stage of
development of children and adolescents. For example, children and
some young adolescents need to be provided with concrete examples,
not hypothetical situations, for them to understand concepts (Elkind,
1967). Therefore, behavioral rehearsal or behavioral skills training
(Poche, Brouwer, & Swearingen, 1981) would be recommended for children; having them role play positive solutions to conict situations that
are presented to them. Research by Fryer, Kraizer, and Miyoshi (1987)
with children in kindergarten, rst, and second grades found that
childrens susceptibility to a child lure as measured by a paper-andpencil method indicated their knowledge about what to do increased
as a result of training. However, the children were easily lured by an
adult posing as an abductor and luring them to accompany him. Thus,
cognitive-only approaches will not by themselves be effective for children and adolescents.
Geonie (cited in Tedisco & Paludi, 1996) uses behavioral rehearsal
with puppets for teaching young children about abductions that can
take place walking home from school, answering the door when children are home alone, and so on. Newman (1985) uses photographs of
each step of a scenario depicting a potential abduction. A story line
accompanies the photographs; questions and alternative answers are
posed to children to help them learn about abductions. Research by
Poche, Yoder, and Miltenberger (1988) using videotapes and behavior
rehearsal found children could learn to resist potential abductors.
Johnson et al. (2006) reported that behavioral skills training in conjunction with in situ training proved effective for teaching abduction
prevention to young children.

64

Feminism as Human Rights

Older children and adolescents may be taught about abductions


using case studies and scenarios about children who are confronted
with a potential abduction and strategies for reporting an abduction to
school ofcials, parents, law enforcement, and so on. Examples of these
case studies may be found in Tedisco and Paludi (1996).
In addition, adolescents live in the immediate present; they rarely
consider how their behavior in the present could be benecial or harmful to them in the future. They rely on a personal fable (Elkind,
1967, 1988); that is, a subjective story they tell themselves about their
uniqueness; how they are not subject to the dangers suffered by others.
Thus, they may not believe that they could be a victim of an abduction
out of the conviction that nothing bad will happen to meIm special. Curriculum projects must take into account this egocentrism and
help adolescents to understand their own vulnerability to abductions.
We note that since most of the abductions are committed by noncustodial parents, teaching children and adolescents about stranger danger may be misguided (Finkelhoret al., 1992). In addition, programs
that encourage children to Run, Yell and Tell may not be reasonable
if the potential abduction occurs in a remote area. Furthermore, as
Bromberg (1997) noted: . . . it is likely that screaming will infuriate
the would-be captor and that the adult is likely to attempt to render
the child unconscious to prevent him or her from drawing the attention
of individuals who might eventually pass within earshot of the screaming child victim (p. 5). This strategy also may not work with international child abductions.
Tedisco and Paludi (1996) also recommend the following issues to
be taken into account in designing a training program on abductions:
assistance from school psychologists during and following the training
program to assist children and adolescents who may have ashbacks,
are visibly upset, and/or are expressing anger toward the trainer
because they are angry at a parent or perpetrator.
POSTTRAINING EVALUATIONS
While there are several training programs on abductions for children
and adolescents, there have been few attempts to determine the extent
to which children can transfer the information learned to potential real
life situations (Bromberg, 1997). Measuring the effectiveness of training
programs is an important aspect of the training program so that the
school may determine if the training delivered or failed to deliver the
expected benets. The measures of success for the training programs in
abductions are ones identied in the needs assessment phase. Issues in
the measurement phase can be discussed in two phases: types of information about which to measure and ways to measure whether or not
the training effort achieved its goals. It is not enough to merely

Missing Children and Child Abductions

65

assume that any training is effective (Roberts, Alexander, & Fanurik,


1990).
The most well-known model for determining the effectiveness of
training programs is the Kirkpatrick Model (Kirkpatrick, 1959, 1998)
that is comprised of four levels: reaction, learning, behavior, and
results. Results concerns the benets resulted from training. Behavior
taps into to what extent trainees change their behavior as a result of
this training. The learning level asks to what extent trainees improved
their knowledge and skills and changed their attitudes as a result of
the training programs. Finally, the reaction component determines
trainees opinions about the structure of the training program, location
of the program, trainer effectiveness, and so on. The most commonly
used level of the Kirkpatrick Model is the reactions level. However,
this is the least valid evaluation technique (Tan, Hall, & Boyce, 2003).
Individuals opinions are heavily inuenced by factors that may have
little to do with the training effectiveness. By measuring reactions,
schools do not obtain information regarding childrens and adolescents
learning, how well they are integrating the new knowledge and skills,
or whether there has been increased reporting of problems post training. Trainers must assist children and adolescents in practicing their
knowledge to determine whether they are able to translate the knowledge into appropriate behaviors.
Bromberg (1997) recommended the following training programs
with children and adolescents that incorporate empirically validated
principles of learning: modeling new behaviors, drill and reinforcement, maintenance of behaviors, training in neutral environments to
promote generalization across settings and teaching novel problems
to ensure transfer of training to new situations. As Bromberg (1997)
concluded:
Presenting children with an abduction prevention program is insufcient;
children must receive the right kind of program. Such programming
includes frequent opportunities for rehearsal of skills and receiving
corrective feedback to shape appropriate responses to potential abductors. (p. 8)

Common performance-based evaluations that incorporate any of the


Kirkpatrick Model levels are posttraining, pre-post training, and prepost training performance with control group. In a posttraining evaluation, students knowledge of information presented in the training is
measured after they have attended the training program to determine
whether they have increased their knowledge of this information. However, some trainees may have known this information prior to participating in the training program. Thus, to simply provide a posttraining
survey or test may overstate the benets of the training program.

66

Feminism as Human Rights

Pre-post training evaluations can assist with this concern (Sadri &
Snyder, 1995). The trainer administers a test or quiz prior to the beginning of the training program. Such a quiz may ask questions regarding
the types of abductions, how to tell school ofcials, and so on. Following the training program, the trainer readministers the quiz and then
determines whether scores on the post-test quiz are higher than those
on the pretest version. This method can provide more reliable information about whether the training program contributed to increased
scores on the posttest quizzes, which is what one would expect if the
training program was effective.
To further answer the question regarding training effectiveness, a
pre-post training with a control group can be used. In this method,
two groups of children or adolescents are established and evaluated on
their knowledge, attitudes, behavior, and so on. The control group,
however, is given no training. The other group does receive training.
Both groups are given a posttest. This method corrects for factors other
than training that inuenced students performance. The group that
received training should have higher posttest scores that those in the
control group.
Sample curricula are presented in Tedisco and Paludi (1996). Legislation must be made part of the training, including training programs
for educators, school personnel, and parents.

THE PROTECT ACT OF 2003: AMBER ALERT


The Protect Act of 2003, referred to as the AMBER Alert program
(Americas Missing: Broadcast Emergency Response), was established
in 2003 as a voluntary partnership between law enforcement agencies,
transportation agencies, and media broadcasters in order to activate an
urgent bulletin in child abduction cases. AMBER Alert has as its goal
to immediately bring together an entire community to assist in the
search for and recovery of a missing child (Grifn & Miller, 2008; Zgoba,
2004). There are ve components that must be met in order for an AMBER
Alert to be issued (Miller & Clinkinbeard, 2006; Zgoba, 2004).
1. Conrmation by law enforcement that the child has been abducted;
2. Belief by law enforcement that the child is facing imminent danger;
3. Substantial information regarding the victim, offender, and vehicle;
4. The abduction is of a child 17 years or younger;
5. The childs name and other critical identication information have been
entered into the National Crime Information Center computer.

AMBER alert information is assembled for public dissemination.


Such information includes photos of the missing child, suspected

Missing Children and Child Abductions

67

abductor, and suspected vehicle. This data is faxed to radio stations


designated as primary stations under the Federal Communications
Commissions Emergency Alert System. These stations interrupt programming to announce the AMBER alert and notify television stations
and cable stations of the alert.
The National Center for Missing and Exploited Children is also
alerted by law enforcement. This center transmits the information to
authorized secondary distributors; that is, companies, businesses, and
organizations that can deliver geographically targeted messages to customers. In some states, electronic highway billboards are used in their
AMBER alert system. In addition, wireless AMBER alerts are available
for users who wish to receive such alerts.
AMBER plan statistics (2009) indicated that there have been 443 successful recoveries of missing children. International AMBER Alert plans
exist in the following countries: Australia, Canada, France, Germany,
Greece, Netherlands, and the United Kingdom. In 1999, the International Center for Missing and Exploited Children was established to
assist with international child abductions. Information about AMBER
Alert programs in the United States and internationally may be
obtained from http://www.amberalert.gov and the National Center for
Missing and Exploited Children: http://www.missingkids.com.

HAGUE CONVENTION OF THE CIVIL ASPECTS OF


INTERNATIONAL CHILD ABDUCTION
The Hague Convention on the Civil Aspects of International Child
Abduction is a treaty developed by the Hague Conference on Private
International Law. This treaty aims to provide a quick method to
return a child younger than 16 years of age who is taken from one
member nation to another. The Preamble to this treaty states its mission: to insure the prompt return of children who have been abducted
from their country of habitual residence or wrongfully retained in a
contracting state not their country of habitual residence.
This agreement thus seeks to return abducted children to the country where they normally live so that courts of that country may decide
on issues of custody and visitation rights (Reynolds, 2006). A Hague
Convention case is not about deciding what is in the childs best interest. Rather, it concerns the return of the child to the jurisdiction that
will hear the custody matter.
The Hague Convention denes wrongful removal or retention of
a child under the following conditions:
a. It is in breach of rights of custody attributed to a person, an institution or
any other body, either jointly or alone, under the law of the State in

68

Feminism as Human Rights


which the child was habitually resident immediately before the removal
or retention; and
b. at the time of removal or retention those rights were actually exercised,
either jointly or alone, or would have been so exercised but for the removal or retention. These rights of custody may arise by operation of
law or by reason of a judicial or administrative decision, or by reason
of an agreement having legal effect under the law of the country of
habitual residence.

The Hague Convention applies only between countries that are both
parties to the Convention (Report on Compliance with the Hague Convention, 2008). In the United States, the implementing legislation
enacted in 1988 is the International Child Abduction Remedies Act.
Convention parties are listed in Appendix 1.

THE CHILDRENS PASSPORT ISSUANCE ALERT PROGRAM


The U.S. Central Authority began the Childrens Passport Issuance
Alert Program in 2007. This program gives parents the opportunity to
request their U.S. citizen children be registered in the Departments
Passport Lookout System. Any passport application for a child who is
registered in this program will alert the Department who, in turn, will
alert the parent(s). Parents will be notied in advance of a potential
abduction should a passport be requested for a child registered in this
program. To date, foreign embassies do not have to adhere to U.S. regulations for children with dual nationality (Report on Compliance with
the Hague Convention, 2008).

STATE CLEARINGHOUSES
A clearinghouse is a central point for law enforcement personnel,
education programs, prevention programs, and legislators to coordinate
their efforts in locating abducted and missing children. Clearinghouses
exist in each of the 50 states, the District of Columbia, Puerto Rico, U.S.
Virgin Islands, Canada, and the Netherlands. The National Center for
Missing and Exploited Children (2009) recommends the following functions of clearinghouses:
1. Collecting and maintaining computerized data and investigative information on missing persons in the states.
2. Compiling statistics on the missing childrens cases handled and the
number resolved by the clearinghouse each year.
3. Assisting in the training of law enforcement and other professionals on
issues relating to missing and unidentied persons.

Missing Children and Child Abductions

69

4. Operating a clearinghouse of information regarding methods of locating


and recovering missing persons.
5. Keeping and distributing information regarding methods of locating and
recovering missing persons.
6. Assisting in the preparation and dissemination of yers of missing persons and their abductors.
7. Publishing, on a regular basis, a directory of missing persons for dissemination to state and local public and nonprot agencies and to the public.
8. Establishing and operating a statewide, toll-free telephone line for reports
of missing persons and reports of sightings of missing persons.

Information about clearinghouses may be obtained from The National


Center for Missing and Exploited Children: http://www.missing
kids.com.
CONCLUSION: FEMINIST CRITIQUE OF CHILD ABDUCTION
RESEARCH AND THE HAGUE TREATY
Nobody believes that domestic violence kills and nobody believes it is detrimental to children. This world has got to wake up. To me, if there is domestic violence, if the children see it or hear it, that to me is detrimental.
Batterers should not have rights to children.
Denise Brown

Weiner (2000) reported that the Hague Convention has a masculine


bias: it was initially drafted in response to abductions of children by
fathers or other male relatives. Weiner (2000) has offered a feminist
corrective to this bias: to include in the denition abductor mothers
who are battered and are eeing for their safety and the safety of their
children.
Estimates indicate that male partners assault a minimum of 4 million
women each year in the United States. Abuse in couples relationships
include intense criticisms, put downs, verbal harassment, sexual coercion and assault, physical attacks, intimidation, restraint of normal
activities and freedoms, and denial of access to resources (Butts Stahly,
1999). Straus, Gelles, and Steinmetz (1980) once concluded that The
American family and the American home are perhaps as or more violent than any other American institution or setting (with the exception
of the military, and only then in time of war) (p. 4).
Walker (1999) noted that the Fourth United Nations International
Conference on Women in 1996 identied no country that had an absence of domestic violence. Women around the world are more at risk
of homicide as a result of battering than from all other categories of
causes (Lundberg-Love & Marmion, 2006).
Children are often in the middle of domestic violence (Graham &
Rawlings, 1999; Lundberg-Love & Marmion, 2006). Walker (1999)

70

Feminism as Human Rights

found that each year, at least 3.3 million children in the United States
between the ages of three and seventeen years are at risk of exposure
to their mothers being battered by their fathers. Children may be
abused by the violent parent. Homicide is currently one of the ve
leading causes of child mortality in the United States. In addition, children may grow up to repeat the same behavior patterns they witnessed
in their parents (Fantuzzo & Mohr, 1999; McGuigan & Pratt, 2001).
Weiner (2003) noted that seven of nine Hague Convention cases that
reached an appeals court in late 2000 involved a mother who abducted
her child to escape a battering mate. As Shetty and Edelson (2005) noted:
Approximately one third of all published and unpublished Convention
cases we have identied using online legal databases include a reference
to some type of family violence, and 70% of these include details of adult
domestic violence. (p. 120)

Research by Johnston, Sagatun-Edwards, Bloomquist, and Girdner


(2000) concerning 634 abductions in California found that mothers
who abducted were more likely to take the children when they or the
children were victims of abuse, and fathers who abducted were more
likely to take the children when they were the abusers (pp. 23).
Shetty and Edleson (2005) reported that intimate partner violence is not
seen as a legitimate concern in international abductions but rather as
an unsubstantiated allegation (p. 119).
Weiner (2003) has noted the common victim-blaming question of
women who are being battered; that is, Why does she stay with the
batterer? (e.g., Graham & Rawlings, 1999) is reversed in international
child abduction cases to Why didnt she stay in the childs country of
habitual residence to litigate with the childs father? As Shetty and
Edleson (2005) noted, having children of battered women return to
their county of habitual residence will expose them to the batterer
while custody decisions take place:
Mothers who abduct their children and ee to nd a safe haven are
not perpetrators, as the Hague Convention implies, but are victims of
their partners violence. They are also victims of an international
treaty, written with good intentions, but, when implemented, has unintended negative consequences for their safety and that of their children. (p. 135)

Thus, habitual residence for children in all countries must be redened to include a residence in which the childs well being is
secured and not violence-prone. In addition, domestic and international
laws must not work against the psychology of the victimization process
of intimate partner violence. As Riane Eisler is quoted as saying:

Missing Children and Child Abductions

71

For most of recorded history, parental violence against children and


mens violence against wives was explicitly or implicitly condoned.
Those who had the power to prevent and/or punish this violence
through religion, law, or custom, openly or tacitly approved it. . . .The
reason violence against women and children is nally out in the open is
that activists have brought it to global attention.

And, as Secretary of State Hillary Clinton stated in 1999 at the formation of the International Center for Missing and Exploited Children:
. . . ultimately these matters are not just about individual children and
the pain of victim parents, but they really are a question of human rights
and whether or not we will enforce our laws evenly and fairly to anyone
who comes within our jurisdictions.

REFERENCES
Abrams, S. (1983). Children in the crossre: The tragedy of parental kidnapping.
New York: Atheneum.
Allender, D. (2007, July). Child abductions: Nightmares in progress. FBI Law
Enforcement Bulletin, 17.
American Psychiatric Association. (2001). DSM-IV-R. Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: Author.
Asdigian, N., Finkelhor, D., & Hotaling, G. (1995). Varieties of non-family abduction of children and adolescents. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 22, 215232.
Badhesha, R., Schmidtke, J., Cummings, A., & Moore, S. (2008). The effects of
diversity training on specic and general attitudes toward diversity. Multicultural Education and Technology Journal, 2, 87106.
Bahroo, B. (2005). Pedophilia: Psychiatric insights. Family Court Review, 41, 497507.
Beasley, J. (2004). Serial murder in America: Case studies of seven offenders.
Behavioral Science and Law, 22, 395414.
Boudreaux, M., Lord, W., & Dutra, R. (1999). Child abduction: Age-based analyses of offender, victim, and offense characteristics in 550 cases of alleged
child disappearance. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 44, 539553.
Boudreaux, M., Lord, W., & Etter, S. (2000). Child abduction: An overview of
current and historical perspectives. Child Maltreatment, 5, 6371.
Boudreaux, M., Lord, W., & Jarvis, P. (2001). Understanding child homicide:
The role of access, vulnerability and routine activities theory. Trauma,
Abuse and Violence, 2, 5678.
Bromberg, D. (1997). Behavioral versus traditional approaches to prevention of
child abduction. School Psychology Review, 26, 622633.
Brown, J. (2002). Training needs assessment: A must for developing an effective
training program. Public Personnel Management, 31, 569578.
Brown, K., & Keppel, R. (2006). Child abduction murder: An analysis of the
effect of time and distance separation between murder incident sites on
solvability. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 52, 137145.

72

Feminism as Human Rights

Bryant, C. (1992). The victimology of children: A transpersonal conceptual


treatment model. In E. Viano (Ed.), Critical issues in victimology: International perspectives. New York: Springer.
Burgess, A., & Holstrom, L. (1974). Rape: Sexual disruption and recovery.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 49, 648.
Butts Stahly, G. (1999). Violence against women by male partners: Prevalence,
outcomes and policy implications. American Psychologist, 48, 10771087.
Chase, E., & Statham, J. (2005). Commercial and sexual exploitation of children
and young people in the UKA review. Child Abuse Review, 14, 325.
Child Find of America. (2009). www.childndofamerica.org
Cole, W., & Bradford, J. (1992). Abduction during custody and access disputes.
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 37, 264266.
Courtois, C. (1988). Healing the incest wound: Adult survivors in therapy.
New York: Norton.
Dalley, M. (2000). Stranger abduction. Canadas Missing Children Annual
Report. Ottawa, ON, Canada: Missing Childrens Registry, Royal Canadian
Mounted Police.
Decenzo, D., & Robbins, S. (2007). Fundamentals of human resource management.
New York: Wiley.
Denoff, M. (1991). Irrational beliefs, situational attributions, and the coping
responses of adolescent runaways. Journal of Rational-Emotive and CognitiveBehavior Therapy, 9, 113135.
Dessler, G. (2009). Fundamentals of human resource management. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Donner, M. (2006). Tearing the child apart: The contribution of narcissism,
envy, and perverse modes of thought to child custody wars. Psychoanalytic
Psychology, 23, 542553.
Doyle, J., & Paludi, M. (1997). Sex and gender: The human experience. New York:
McGraw Hill.
Drukteinis, A. (1992). Serial murder: The heart of darkness. Psychiatric Annals,
22, 532538.
Elkind, D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development, 38, 10251034.
Elkind, D. (1988). The hurried child: Growing up too fast too soon. New York:
Addison-Wesley.
Faller, K. (1991). Possible explanations for child sexual abuse allegations in
divorce. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 61, 8691.
Fantuzzo, J., & Mohr, W. (1999). Prevalence and effects of child exposure to
domestic violence. The Future of Children, 9, 2132.
Finkelhor, D. (1995). The victimization of children: A developmental perspective. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 65, 177193.
Finkelhor, D., & Browne, A. (1988). The traumatogenic effects of child sexual
abuse. In L. Walker (Ed.), Handbook on sexual abuse of children. New York:
Springer.
Finkelhor, D., & Dziuba-Leatherman, J. (1994). Victimization of children.
American Psychologist, 49, 173183.
Finkelhor, D., Hammer, H., & Sedlak, A. (2002). Nonfamily abducted children:
National estimates and characteristics. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
Justice.

Missing Children and Child Abductions

73

Finkelhor, D., Hotaling, G., & Sedlak, A. (1990). Missing, abducted, runaway and
thrownaway children in America. First Report of National Incidence Studies.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
Finkelhor, D., Hotaling, G., & Sedlak, A. (1992). The abduction of children by
strangers and non-family members: Estimating the incidence using multiple methods. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7, 226243.
Flowers, R. (2001). Runaway kids and teenage prostitution: Americas lost, abandoned
and sexually exploited children. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Forehand, R., Long, N., Zogg, C., & Parrish, E. (1989). Child abduction: Parent
and child functioning following return. Clinical Pediatrics, 28, 311316.
Franks, M., & Goswami, H. (2009). Rural runaways: Rurality and its implications for services to children and young people who run away. Children
and Society.
Fravel, D. L., & Boss, P. (1992). An in-depth interview with the parents of missing children. In J. Gilgun, K. Daly, and G. Handel (Eds), Quantitative Methods in Family Research, 126145, Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Freeman-Longo, R. & Wall, R. (1986, March). Changing a lifetime of sexual
crime. Psychology Today, 5864.
Fryer, G., Kraizer, S., & Miyoshi, T. (1987). Measuring actual reduction of risk
to child abuse: A new approach. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 173179.
Gallagher, B., Bradford, M., & Pease, K. (2002). The sexual abuse of children by
strangers: Its extent, nature and victims characteristics. Children and Society, 16, 346359.
Gelman, D. (1984, March 19). Stolen children. Newsweek, 7886.
Giffords, E., Alonso, C., & Bell, R. (2007). A transitional living program
for homeless adolescents: A case study. Child and Youth Care Forum, 36,
131151.
Girdner, L. (1994). Introduction. In L. Girdner & P. Holt (Eds.), Obstacles to the
recovery and return of parentally abducted children (pp. 113). Final Report.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
Graham, D., & Rawlings, E. (1999). Observers blaming of battered wives: Who,
what, when, and why (pp. 5594). In M. Paludi (Ed.), The psychology of sexual victimization: A handbook. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Greenberg, D., Bradford, J., & Curry, S. (1993). A comparison of sexual victimization in the childhoods of pedophiles and hebephiles. Journal of Forensic
Sciences, 38, 432436.
Greene, N., & Esselstyn, T. (1972). The beyond-control girl. Juvenile Justice, 23,
1319.
Grifn, T., & Miller, M. (2008). Child abduction, AMBER alert and crime control theatre. Criminal Justice Review, 33, 159176.
Gullotta, T. (2005). Leaving home: The runaway and the forgotten throwaway.
In G. Adams & M. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook on adolescence.
New York: Wiley.
Hammer, H., Finkelhor, D., & Sedlak, A. (2002). Children abducted by family
members: National estimates and characteristics. NISMART Bulletin. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
Hanand, K., Keppel, R., & Weis, J. (1997). Case management for missing children
homicide investigation. Olympia, WA: Attorney General of Washington.

74

Feminism as Human Rights

Hatcher, C., Barton, C., & Brooks, L. (1992). Families of missing children. Final
Report. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
Hegar, R., & Greif, G. (1991). Abduction of children by their parents: A survey
of the problem. Social Work, 36, 421426.
Heide, K., Beauregard, E., & Myers, W. (2009). Sexually motivated child abduction murders: Synthesis of the literature and case illustration. Victims and
Offenders, 4, 5875.
Humphreys, J. (1995). Dependent-care by battered women: Protecting their
children. Health Care for Women International, 16, 920.
Hunt, P., & Baird, M. (1990). Children of sex rings. Child Welfare, 69, 195207.
Huttinger, B. (1984). My child is not missing: A parents guidebook for the prevention and recovery of missing children. Plantation, FL: Child Safe Products.
Hyde, M., & Hyde, L. (1985). Missing children. New York: Franklin Watts.
Ireland, K. (2006). Sexual exploitation of children and international travel and
tourism. Child Abuse Review, 2, 263270.
Janvier, R., McCormick, K., & Donaldson, R. (1990). Parental kidnapping: A
survey of left-behind parents. Juvenile and Family Court Journal, 41, 18.
Johnson, B., Miltenbergere, R., Knudson, P., Egemo-Helm, K., Kelso, P.,
Jostad, C., et al. (2006). A preliminary evaluation of two behavioral skills
training procedures for teaching abduction prevention skills to schoolchildren. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 2534.
Johnston, J., Girdner, L., & Sagatun-Edwards, I. (1999). Developing proles of
risk for parental abduction of children from a comparison of families victimized by abduction with families litigating custody. Behavioral Sciences and
the Law, 17, 305322.
Johnston, J., Sagatun-Edwards, I., Blomquist, M., & Girdner, L. (2000). Prevention of family abduction through early identication of risk factors. Washington,
DC: Ofce of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, U.S. Department of Justice.
Kempf-Leonard, K., & Johansson, P. (2007). Gender and runaways: Risk factors,
delinquency, and juvenile justice experiences. Youth Violence and Juvenile
Justice, 5, 308327.
Khong, L. (2008). Runaway youths in Singapore: Exploring demographics, motivations and environments. Children and Youth Services Review, 31, 125139.
Kirkpatrick, D. (1959). Techniques for evaluating training programs. Journal of
the American Society for Training Development, 13, 39.
Kirkpatrick, D. (1998). Evaluating training programs: The four levels. San
Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler.
Koss, M. (1990). The womens mental health research agenda: Violence against
women. American Psychologist, 45, 374380.
Lang, R., & Langevin, R. (1991). Parentchild relations in offenders who commit violent sexual crimes against children. Behavioral Sciences and the Law,
9, 6171.
Lanning, K. (1994). Child molesters: A behavioral analysis. School Safety, Spring,
1217.
Lerner, M. (1980). The belief in a just world. New York: Plenum.
Levendowsky, A., Lynch, S., & Graham-Bermann, S. (2000). Mothers perceptions of the impact of woman battering on their parenting. Violence Against
Women, 6, 247271.

Missing Children and Child Abductions

75

Levant, M., & Bass, B. (1991). Parental identication of rapists and pedophiles.
Psychological Reports, 69, 463466.
Lucier, K. (2008). A consultative training program: Collateral effect of a needs
assessment. Communication Education, 57, 482489.
Lundberg-Love, P., & Marmion, S. (2006). Intimate violence against women. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Malamuth, N., & Check, J. (1981). Penile tumescence and perceptual response
to rape as a function of victims perceived reactions. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 10, 528547.
McGigan, W., & Pratt, C. (2001). The predictive inuence of domestic violence
on three types of child maltreatment. Child Abuse and Neglect, 25, 869883.
Miller, M., & Clinkinbeard, S. (2006). Improving the AMBER alert system: Psychology research and policy recommendations. Law and Psychology Review,
30, 121.
Millstein, S. (April 1989). Behavioral risk factors for AIDS among adolescents. Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City, MO.
Molino, A. (March 2007). Characteristics of help-seeking street youth and non-street
youth. Paper presented at the 2007 National Symposium on Homelessness
Research, Washington, DC.
Moskowitz, G. (2005). The Hague Convention on international child abduction
and the grave risk of harm exception. Family Court Review, 41, 580596.
National Center for Missing and Exploited Children. (2009). http://www.missingkids.com
National Runaway Switchboard. (2009). National Runaway Switchboard. http://
www.1800runaway.org/
Newman, S. (1985). Never say yes to a stranger. New York: Perigee.
Palenski, J., & Launer, H. (1987). The process of running away: A redenition.
Adolescence, 22, 347362.
Peled, E., & Muzicant, A. (2006). The meaning of home for runaway girls. Journal of Community Psychology, 36, 434451.
Penner, L., Dovidio, J. & Albrecht, T. (2000). Helping victims of loss and
trauma: A social psychological perspective. In J. Harvey & E. Miller (Eds.),
Loss and trauma: General and close relationship perspectives (pp. 6285). Philadelphia: Brunner-Routledge.
Pennbridge, J., Yates, G., David, T., & Mackenzie, R. (1990). Runaway and
homeless youth in Los Angeles County, California. Journal of Adolescent
Health Care, 11, 159165.
Plass, P. (2007). Secondary victimizations in missing child events. American
Journal of Criminal Justice, 32, 3044.
Poche, C., Brouwer, R., & Swearingen, M. (1981). Teaching self-protection to
young children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 169176.
Poche, C., Yoder, P., & Miltenberger, R. (1988). Teaching self-protection to children using television techniques. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21,
253261.
Quina, K., & Carlson, N. (1989). Rape, incest, and sexual harassment: A guide for
helping survivors. New York: Praeger.
Rafferty, Y. (2007). Children for sale: Child trafcking in Southeast Asia. Child
Abuse Review, 16, 401422.

76

Feminism as Human Rights

Rafferty, Y. (2008). The impact of trafcking on children: Psychological and


Social Policy Perspectives. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 1318.
Report on Compliance with the Hague Convention. (2008). Civil aspects of international child abduction. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of State.
Reynolds, S. (2006). International parental abduction: Why we need to expand
custody rights protected under the Child Abduction Convention. Family
Court Review, 44, 464483.
Roberts, M., Alexander, K., & Fanurik, D. (1990). Evaluation of commercially
available materials to prevent child sexual abuse and abduction. American
Psychologist, 45, 782783.
Russell, D. (1975). The politics of rape. New York: Stein & Day.
Sadri, G., & Snyder, P. (1995). Methodological issues in assessing training effectiveness. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 10, 3032.
Salter, A. (2004). Predators: Pedophiles, rapists, and other sex offenders. New York:
Basic Books.
Sanchez, R., Waller, M., & Greene, J. (2006). Who runs? A demographic prole
of runaway youth in the United States. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39,
778781.
Schetky, D., & Haller, I. (1983). Child psychiatry and law: Parental kidnapping.
Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 22, 279285.
Sedlak, A., Finkelhor, D., Hammer, H., & Schultz, D. (2002). National estimates of
missing children, an overview. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
Shetty, S., & Edleson, J. (2005). Adult domestic violence in cases of international parental child abduction. Violence Against Women, 11, 115138.
Shogan, M. (2006). Emergency management plan for newborn abduction. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic and Neonatal Nursing, 31, 340346.
Simkhada, P. (2008). Life histories and survival strategies amongst sexually
trafcked girls in Nepal. Children and Society, 22, 235248.
Spilman, S. (2006). Child abduction, parents distress, and social support. Violence and Victims, 21, 149165.
Stanley, P. (2003, September). Giving kids a ghting chance. USA Today, 1617.
Stevens, D. (2001). Inside the mind of sexual offenders: Predatory rapists, pedophiles,
and criminal proles. Bel Air, CA: Authors Choice Press.
Straus, M., Gelles, R., & Steinmetz, S. (1980). Behind closed doors: Violence in the
American family. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books.
Strohman, L. (2005). Stranger infant abductions: Offense characteristics, victim
selection, and offender motivation of female offenders. Dissertation, Drexel
University.
Tan, J., Hall, R., & Boyce, C. (2003). The role of employee reactions in predicting
training effectiveness. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 14, 397411.
Tedisco, J., & Paludi, M. (1996). Missing children. Albany, NY: State University
of New York Press.
Terr, L. (1983). Child snatching: A new epidemic of an ancient malady. The
Journal of Pediatrics, 103, 151156.
Terrell, N. E. (1997). Street life: Aggravated and sexual assaults among homeless and runaway adolescents. Youth Society, 28, 267290.
Thompson, S., & Pillai, V. (2006). Determinants of runaway episodes among
adolescents using crisis shelter services. International Journal of Social Welfare, 15, 142149.

Missing Children and Child Abductions

77

Tyler, K. (2002). Evaluating evaluations. HR Magazine, 47.


Walker, L. (1992). Traumatized populations: Role and responsibilities of professionals. In E. Viano (Ed.), Critical issues in victimology: International perspectives. New York: Springer.
Walker, L. (1999). Psychology and domestic violence around the world.
American Psychologist, 54, 2129.
Weiner, M. (2000). International child abduction and the escape from domestic
violence. Fordham Law Review, 69, 593.
Weiner, M. (2003). The potential and challenges of transnational litigation for
feminists concerned about domestic violence here and abroad. American
University Journal of Gender, Social Policy, and the Law, 11, 749800.
Zgoba, K. (2004). The Amber Alert: The appropriate solution to preventing
child abduction? Journal of Psychiatry and Law, 32, 7188.

APPENDIX 1: PARTNERS OF THE HAGUE CONVENTION OF


THE CIVIL ASPECTS OF INTERNATIONAL CHILD ABDUCTION
Argentina
Australia
Austria
Bahamas
Belgium
Belize
Bosnia and Herzegovina
Brazil
Bulgaria
Burkina Faso
Canada
Chile
China (Hong Kong and Macau only)
Columbia
Costa Rica
Croatia
Cyprus
Czech Republic
Denmark
Dominican Republic
Ecuador
El Salvador
Estonia
Finland

78

Feminism as Human Rights

France
Germany
Honduras
Hungary
Iceland
Ireland
Israel
Italy
Latvia
Lithuania
Luxembourg
Macedonia
Malta
Mauritius
Mexico
Monaco
Montenegro
Netherlands
New Zealand
Norway
Panama
Paraguay
Peru
Poland
Portugal
Romania
Saint Kitts and Nevis
San Marino
Serbia
Slovakia
Slovenia
South Africa
Sweden
Switzerland
Turkey
Ukraine
United Kingdom (Bermuda, Cayman Islands, Falkland Islands,
Isle of Man, Montserrat)

Missing Children and Child Abductions

79

Uruguay
Venezuela
Zimbabwe
APPENDIX 2: RESOURCES ON CHILD ABDUCTIONS
AND MISSING CHILDREN
AMBER Alert
http://www.amberalert.gov
American Bar Association
http://www.abanet.org
Committee for Missing Children, Inc.
http://www.ndthekids.org
Child Abduction Resource Center
http://www.globalmissing.com
Child Find of America
http://www.childndofamerica.org
Child Focus (Belgium)
http://www.childfocus.org
Childnd Canada (Canada)
http://www.childnd.ca/
Find the Children
http://www.ndthechildren.com
Hague Conference Permanent BureauChild Abduction Section
Hcch.e-vision.nl/index_en.php?
Inter-American Bar Association
http://www.iaba.org
Lost Childrens Network
http://www.lostchildren.org
Missing Children Society of Canada
http://www.mcsc.ca
National Association for the Education of Homeless Children and Youth
http://www.naehcy.org
National Center for Missing Adults
http://www.missingadults.org
National Center for Missing and Exploited Children
http://www.missingkids.com
National Center for Prosecution of Child Abuse
http://www.ndaa-apri.org

80

Feminism as Human Rights

National Runaway Switchboard


http://www.1800runaway.org/
Ofce of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention
http://www.ojjdp.ncjrs.org
Polly Klaas Foundation
http://www.pollyklaas.org/
Prevent Child AbuseAmerica
http://www.preventchildabuse.org
Team HOPE-Support for Families with Missing Children
http://www.teamhope.org
U.S. Department of State, Ofce of Childrens Issues
http://www.travel.state.gov
Vanished Childrens Alliance
http://www.vca.org
WomensLaw.org
http://www.womenslaw.org

Chapter 4

Women and Sexuality: An


International Perspective
Donna Castaneda
Monica Ulibarri

Sexuality is a fundamental aspect of human experience, but the study


of sex at a global level has been minimal. Some countries have large
research databases on sexual behavior, but almost none of these are in
the developing countries, and basic sexuality information is lacking for
whole regions of the world. No worldwide organization is charged
with collecting and archiving information on sexuality, in contrast
to organizations such as the World Health Organization or the Pan
American Health Organization, which maintain health statistics worldwide or across large geographic regions (Mackay, 2001). In particular,
womens experience of sexuality, except as it relates to fertility and
childbirth, has received little attention at the international level. Even
basic information on womens sexual experience, such as average age
at rst intercourse or average number of sexual encounters with a
partner per week, is unclear except in a relatively small number of
countries, primarily those in the Western world. More nuanced and
detailed knowledge of womens sexualityhow women in diverse cultures navigate sexual identity, desire, and pleasure in differing social,
political, and cultural contexts, for example, is difcult to nd.
This situation notwithstanding, our effort here is to present and discuss the available information on various aspects of womens sexuality,
such as sexual desire, sexual behavior, sexual problems, and the concept of womens sexual rights and its relationship to human rights.
Our goal is to present, as much as possible given the incredible

82

Feminism as Human Rights

diversity of societies in the world, the differences as well as commonalities in womens sexual lives across the above themes in womens
sexuality.
DEFINITION OF WOMENS SEXUALITY
While no universal denition of sexuality exists, and how sexuality
is constructed, managed, and acted out by women varies historically
and cross-culturally, an important premise in any understanding of
womens sexuality is that it is very much embedded within and grows
out of political struggles, gender relations, cultural meanings surrounding femininity, and legal and social denitions of human rights. Sexuality is a personal psychological, emotional, and physical experience, but
how women learn about, understand, and approach their sexuality cannot be separated from the larger sociocultural context of their lives.
This contextualized approach to the study of womens sexuality has
not always existed and only relatively recently (the last 15 to 20 years)
has a fuller understanding of the social and cultural embeddedness of
sexuality been articulated (for examples see Blackwood & Wieringa,
1999a; Correa & Parker, 2004; Parker et al., 2004). Sexuality has been
primarily viewed as a natural and immutable aspect of life, and the
biomedical model, along with studies of specic sexual behaviors, has
been the prevailing framework governing the study of womens sexuality (see Amaro, Navarro, Conron, & Raj, 2002 for a discussion). On the
other hand, globalization, whether through economic, technological, or
cultural processes, has led to exposure to and engagement with a much
wider array of cultural constructions surrounding sexual and relational
intimacy, thus highlighting the limitations of the standard sexuality
frameworks (Obermeyer, 2000; Padilla, Hirsch, Mu~
noz-Laboy, Sember,
& Parker, 2007; Parker et al., 2004). Out of these larger globalization
processes, the denitions of sexuality have expanded to include not
only behavioral and biological aspects, but also recognition of the multiplicity of factors that may inuence sexuality (Promotion of Sexual
Health: Recommendations for Action, 2000). This change can be seen in
the working denition of sexuality developed by the Pan American
Health Organization, World Health Organization, and World Association of Sexology that is useful to present here:
Sexuality is a central aspect of being human throughout life and encompasses sex, gender identities and roles, sexual orientation, eroticism,
pleasure, intimacy and reproduction. Sexuality is experienced and
expressed in thoughts, fantasies, desires, beliefs, attitudes, values, behaviours, practices, roles and relationships. While sexuality can include all of
these dimensions, not all of them are always experienced or expressed.
Sexuality is inuenced by the interaction of biological, psychological,

Women and Sexuality: An International Perspective

83

social, economic, political, cultural, ethical, legal, historical, religious and


spiritual factors. (Promotion of Sexual Health: Recommendations for
Action, 2000)

This denition does not overtly incorporate a gendered perspective,


such as, compared to men, the greater subordination of women and
efforts at control of womens sexuality that are seen worldwide. It
includes the suggestion of diversity in gender identity and sexual orientation, but it does not include the notion of compulsory heterosexuality (Rich, 1983) that constrains many womens sexual options, even in
what are considered sexually liberal countries and societies. Nevertheless, the above denition represents a broadened perspective on sexuality that stems from greater global and cross cultural engagement, not
just by researchers, corporations, and government entities, but also by
ordinary women as they traverse geographic, cultural, and political
boundaries due to a myriad of reasons such as economic necessity,
family reasons, or political conict and oppression.
Along with the expansion in the concept of sexuality, another critical
and related change has been the increased acceptance of love as the basis for entering marriage. The past 100 years, particularly since the
1950s, have seen changes in ideals and purposes of marriage. Duty,
dened gender roles and responsibilities, and procreation were the primary basis for entering a marriage relationship. Sexuality was of course
part of these relationships, but the purpose of sexuality was procreation rather than pleasure. Love and emotional intimacy might occur in
a marital relationship, but it was not necessarily the impetus or goal of
marriage. As Jennifer Hirsch states (2007, p. 94), Marriage was a system for organizing social reproduction, not a project for personal satisfaction. At this juncture in time, the modern marital ideal in many
very diverse cultures has come to mean a companionate relationship
that emphasizes pleasure, intimacy, and personal satisfaction of the
relationship partners. A key component of these companionate relationships is sexual enjoyment and the fulllment of mutual sexual needs
(DEmilio & Freedman, 1988; Hirsch, 2007). Researchers point to the
role of literacy, fertility decline, the media, the intertwining of sexuality, modernity, desire, and commodication (Hirsch, 2007, p. 101) as
important forces for social change in the denition of ideal marital relationships at a global level and, in fact, it is affective processes and
adult love relationships that have increasingly become strategies for
dening oneself as modern.
Another term that has emerged within the global sexuality discourse, particularly in response to the HIV/AIDS pandemic, and that
must be distinguished from sexuality, is sexual health. It is related to
the concept of sexualityin fact, sexuality and health always intersect
in numerous direct and indirect waysbut cannot be conated with or

84

Feminism as Human Rights

substituted for it. Sexual health for women refers not just to the absence of sexual dysfunction and sexual diseases, such as HIV/AIDS or
other sexually transmitted diseases, nor does it refer narrowly to positive reproductive health such as healthy pregnancy, childbirth, and
contraceptive use. It includes these dimensions, but it also incorporates
the notion of an overarching well being experienced when a person
has the ability to freely, responsibly, and safely express their sexual
desires without the fear of coercion, discrimination, or violence (Promotion of Sexual Health: Recommendations for Action, 2000; Sexual
HealthA New Focus for WHO, 2004). The sexual health concept is
situated within the larger domain of public health and, as such, a consequence and necessary component of the notion of sexual health is
education services about sexuality, reproductive biology, how to control fertility, and protection against sexually transmitted diseases.
Because of the growing understanding among social scientists, program planners, and international health advocates that gender inequality is at the heart of womens vulnerability to sexual health problems
(Raimondo, 2005), such as their risk for HIV/AIDS and sexual victimization, these services must be provided in a manner that increases
womens autonomy and freedom of choice, and supports and empowers them to confront hostile, oppressive, or demeaning gender arrangements and ideologies in their social and personal environments
(Ruklos Hampton, McWatters, Jeffery, & Smith, 2006; Undie, Crichton,
& Zulu, 2007).
SEXUAL RIGHTS AS HUMAN RIGHTS
For women to participate in a sexual life of their own choice and to
achieve and maintain sexual health, their sexual rights, as a component
of basic human rights, must be recognized (Promotion of Sexual
Health: Recommendations for Action, 2000). As Palesa Beverley Ditsie
eloquently stated in 1995 at the UNs Fourth World Conference
on Women, . . . no woman can determine the direction of her own
life without the ability to determine her sexuality . . . (as cited in
Wieringa & Blackwood, 1999b, p. 26). Addressing sexuality within the
context of human rights means addressing complex issues for women,
such as the integrity of the body, the role and value of sexual pleasure
and nonprocreative sex, sexual choice and sexual agency, sexual
diversity that includes same-gender sexual relationships, and nally,
determining how do these relate to the rights of the individual and
her humanity (Weeks, 2007). Thus, in examining womens sexuality
internationally, the connection of sexuality to broader issues of
human rights is a theme that emerges prominently.
The awareness of the connection between sexual rights and human
rights has grown in the last 20 or so years and is due to the

Women and Sexuality: An International Perspective

85

intersection of multiple streams of separate and interwoven social


change activities and events, such as the feminist, lesbian and gay,
womens health, and civil rights movements that began in the 1960s
and 70s in Western countries; the transnational womens health and
reproductive rights movements and development work that emerged
in the 1980s and 1990s, and the concomitant advocacy to prioritize
womens reproductive rights, particularly by international womens
nongovernmental organizations (NGOs); the activism to incorporate
into human rights convention reports language supporting sexual
health, sexual rights, and gender equality; and the HIV/AIDS pandemic, the focus on sexual diversity that grew out of it, and the resulting womens and lesbian, bisexual, gay, transgender, and queer
movements that grew in developing countries in the wake of HIV/
AIDS (Mongrovejo, 1999; Parker, 2007; Parker et al., 2004; Petchesky,
2003; Tiefer, 2002a; Weeks, 2007). It is also due to the rapid pace of
globalization in the last quarter century that has led to fundamental
changes in national and international economies, political systems and
institutions, media, work, family life, and most importantly, in conceptualizations of gender relations, identity, and sexualities (Carnoy &
Castells, 2001; Hirsch, 2007; Weeks, 2007).
Taken together, these events and activities have provided new
vocabularies, concepts, and spaces for a larger, more informed, analysis
of sexual life (Parker, 2007). For instance, in development work, sexuality has often been seen as peripheral to the important issues of inequality and poverty. However, a focus on sexual rights makes apparent the
false dichotomy between sexuality and human rights by making visible
the linkages between womens sexuality on the one hand, and poverty
and poverty-related problems such as lack of educational access for
girls, sexual violence and victimization, homophobia and compulsory
heterosexuality, and the legal and economic structures countries put
into place to regulate and control womens daily life, on the other hand
(Correa & Jolly, 2008; Petchescky, 2003).
Not surprisingly, placing womens sexuality at the cornerstone of
human rights has also stimulated resistance to change and, despite important global changes and the increased understanding of the link
between sexuality, womens rights, and social justice, the promise of
a transformed approach to womens sexuality is still unevenly
achieved. Progressive language contained in international human rights
documents is nonbinding and has not necessarily been translated into
policies at the level of individual countries (Tiefer, 2002a). Political, corporate, and conservative religious interests that are served by maintaining gender inequality, sometimes in the guise of allowing individual
countries to maintain oppressive cultural traditions and norms surrounding gender and sexuality, are still powerful. This can be seen in
policies, and consequently funding, that promote abstinence only as a

86

Feminism as Human Rights

strategy to reduce HIV/AIDS infection and other sexually transmitted


diseases, provide contraception only to married women, restrict or outlaw access to safe abortion services, or that outlaw and severely punish
the expression of womens sexuality outside marriage or with other
women (Dabash & Roudi-Fahimi, 2008; Goldberg, 2009; Petchesky,
2003). Despite oppressive responses to the growing discourse on womens sexual rights, or possibly because of them, a key realization is that
sexuality can no longer be viewed as a private, personal process,
unconnected to domestic or international politics and policies, but has
taken shape on a global level . . . as a key contested domain or eld
of struggle (Correa & Parker, 2004, p. 19).
SEXUAL BEHAVIOR
With publication of the Kinsey et al. work on womens sexual
behavior in 1953 and the work of Masters and Johnson in 1966, the
thinking on human sexuality moved into its modern forms. With their
clearly empirical emphasis on documenting actual sexual practices and
behaviors, these works were a leap forward in sexuality research. Most
importantly for women, their sexual experiences and priorities were
given a prominence previously absent in the sexuality literature. However, the study of womens sexual behavior has progressed little
beyond this focus on acts or even sequences of behaviors, or sexual
scripts, and further illumination of womens sexual lives is still needed.
International studies of womens sexual behavior from across a spectrum of social class origins are difcult to nd and research that does
exist focuses heavily on potential for disease or health problems, particularly those that may affect reproduction and fertility. This has led to
important information about which sexual behaviors put women
most at risk for sexual health problems, but it has not necessarily led
to an increased understanding of womens sexual practices, needs, or
experiences.
One of the few cross-national surveys of sexual behavior, the Durex
Sexual Well-being Survey (2007), included samples from 26 countries
in Europe (e.g., United Kingdom, Poland, France), Asia (e.g., India,
China, Malaysia), Latin America (e.g., Brazil, Mexico), North America
(e.g., Canada, United States), Australia, and Africa (e.g., South Africa,
Nigeria). It was conducted over the Internet except in Nigeria where
face-to-face or self-completed surveys were used due to low Internet
and telephone use. Certainly, the sample is one that has access to computer technology and is likely more comfortable with sexuality in general, thus, ndings reect the experiences of a possibly small
demographic subset within each of these countries. However, the study
provides interesting insights into sexual behavior of women from a
large number of countries, particularly when differences by sexual

Women and Sexuality: An International Perspective

87

orientation are presented. For instance, bisexual women report an average of 13 male, and three female sexual partners, homosexual women
report an average of 11 female partners, and heterosexual women
reporting an average of seven male sexual partners. Austrian women
report the highest average number of sexual partners, 29, while Chinese women report the lowest average number of sexual partners, at
only two. The number of sexual partners was higher for men in every
country except New Zealand, where women reported an average of 20
sexual partners, while men reported an average of 17 sexual partners.
With regard to sexual behaviors, bisexual women show the highest
rate of vaginal sex (94 percent), followed by heterosexual women
(84 percent), and homosexual women (77 percent). Regarding anal sex,
again bisexual women are most likely to give (23 percent) and receive
(39 percent) anal sex, compared to 18 percent of heterosexual and
15 percent of homosexual women receiving anal sex, and 8 percent of
heterosexual and 10 percent of homosexual women giving anal sex.
Use of sexual devices by women giving anal sex was not claried. Both
bisexual and homosexual women report higher rates of giving oral
sex, 71 and 77 percent respectively, than heterosexual women, 56 percent. Bisexual and homosexual women also report higher rates of
receiving oral sex, 75 and 74 percent, respectively, than heterosexual
women, 55 percent. Finally, for more unusual sexual activities, such as
bondage/S&M and telephone sex, bisexual and homosexual women
report higher rates than heterosexual women. Twenty-three percent
of bisexual women and 28 percent of homosexual women report
bondage/S&M activities, compared to only 5 percent of heterosexual
women; 24 percent of bisexual women and 48 percent of homosexual
women report telephone sex, while only 9 percent of heterosexual women
report engaging in telephone sex. Finally, 22 percent of women report
masturbation once a week, compared to 43 percent of men.
Other results in the Durex survey show that the weekly rate of sexual activity and satisfaction with that activity varies considerably
worldwide. Respondents in Japan reported the lowest weekly rate of
sexual activity, 34 percent, and they also reported the lowest satisfaction with that activity, 15 percent. Greece had the highest percentage of
respondents who reported having sex weekly, 87 percent, but only
51 percent reported being satised with that activity; on the other
hand, in Nigeria, while 53 percent of respondents reported having sex
weekly, 67 percent of them were satised with this activity.
Not enough information about the conduct of the Durex survey is
available to determine the validity of these results for women. Nevertheless, where results are presented by gender, they provide an intriguing picture of womens sexual behavior worldwide. First, bisexual
women, and to a lesser extent homosexual women, appear to engage
in a wider range of sexual activities than heterosexual women and to

88

Feminism as Human Rights

do so with both women and men. They also report more sexual partners than heterosexual women. These results are consistent with other
research that shows that women who have sex with both women and
men tend to engage in a wider range of sexual behaviors and to
have more sexual partners than women who have sex with men only
(Casta~
neda & G
omez Bastidas, 2005; Lemp et al., 1995; Reinisch,
Sanders, & Ziemba-Davis, 1995; Richters, Bergin, Lubowitz, & Prestage,
2002). They also demonstrate that sexual identity labels (bisexual,
homosexual, heterosexual) do not necessarily indicate the gender of
ones sexual partners or the behaviors in which one is most likely to
engage. They are also similar to research that shows that women
engage in masturbation to a lesser extent than men (Oliver & Shibley
Hyde, 1993) and that persons from certain Asian countries, such as
Japan, report less satisfaction with sex (Laumann et al., 2006).
In addition to simply understanding what sexual behaviors women
engage in with their partners, investigating womens sexual behavior
cross-culturally highlights several related issues important to mention.
First, labels for sexual acts cannot be applied uncritically across cultures and sexual acts have meanings that vary depending on the society one is examining. The relatively few cross-national studies of
sexual behavior tend to have a predetermined set of sexual behaviors
to be investigated, such as heterosexual intercourse, oral sex, and so on
(e.g., Durex Sexual Well-being Survey, 2007; Nicolosi et al., 2004), but
this should not preclude the possibility of other sexual behaviors that
may be more common among women. Sexual behavior is not nite or
standardized, and how it is carried out can be variable and highly dependent on the situational and relational context. That being said, however, sexual acts, in whatever context they occur, have a materiality
and physicality that cannot be overlooked by researchers and theoreticians, just as the women involved do not overlook them. These behaviors exist in time and space, they involve the body, as well as
emotional and cognitive processes.
Second, anthropological work demonstrates that the connection
between sexual behavior and sexual identity is conceived differently in
different cultures. In the West, sexual identity is much more strongly
categorical and compartmentalized, and it is also more highly linked to
sexual acts and to the gender of ones sexual partner. But in other cultures conceptualizations of sexual identity are more uid, and the
notion of sexual behavior as a signier of a static and categorical sexual
identity may not be applicable. For instance, among Creole women in
Suriname, South America, mati work, or erotic attachments among
women, is common. But mati women do not identify themselves as lesbian and they continue to have relationships with men through marriage, concubinage, or visiting relationships. In this case, sex with both
women and men is part of womens sexuality. Mati work reects an

Women and Sexuality: An International Perspective

89

ideology of sexuality for Surinamese women that is uid and multiply


dened, rather than a component of a xed identity. Reective of this
is that when asked women themselves say I mati instead of I am
mati (Wekker, 1999, p. 126). Another example of the differing relationship between sexual behavior and sexual identity is in Guatemala. Lesbian organizing in Guatemala did not begin until the 1990s, and it
grew out of the civil war that lasted throughout the 1970s and 1980s
and the resulting national emphasis on human and citizen rights. In
this Guatemala is similar to other countries in Latin America where lesbian and gay rights movements developed out of internal liberation
struggles (Sarda, 2008). This contrasts with the identity politics that
dominates the discourse in the lesbian movement in the United States
and the West generally. For many lesbians in Guatemala, lesbianism is
dened as sexual behaviors and practices, not as an inherent identity
(Berger, 2006).
Lastly, in any discussion of sexual behavior the notion of social power
is critical to address, as only an incomplete understanding of womens
sexuality is possible without doing so. Most societies are stratied in
terms of social power, and the ability to carry out sexual behavior is very
much inuenced by where one stands in terms of the particular power
hierarchy in a society. Gender and sexual orientation are key elements
that determine social power. For example, in most societies heterosexual
women and men, and lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender individuals do not have the same freedom to choose their sexual partners, marry
whom they want or stay single, have or not have children, or even
receive sex-related education or information because they typically
do not have the same status, power, and privilege in a given society
(Reinfelder, 1996a). In fact, the ability to have safe and nonabusive
sexual experiences is differentially distributed depending on gender and
sexual orientation (along with other factors such as wealth, kinship, and
ethnicity/cultural group, etc.) (Grieg, 2008).
SEXUAL DESIRE
At its most basic, sexual desire refers to the interest one has in seeking out and engaging in sexual activity, but even this simple denition
belies a great deal of complexity, especially as it applies to women
(Bailey, 2009; Basson, 2005; Wood, Barthalow Koch, & Kernoff Manseld,
2006). While it is often assumed to be natural and universal and to operate
similarly in women and men, research has shown that sexual desire does
not operate in the same manner for women as for men and that it may
not even be understood in similar ways. For instance, instead of a linear
progression through the sexual response cycle that is initiated by sexual
desire, researchers have found that sexual desire for women may sometimes precede or follow sexual arousal (Graham, Saunders, Milhausen,

90

Feminism as Human Rights

& McBride, 2004; Laan & Both, 2008); despite low sexual desire being
one of the most commonly reported sexual problems among women,
only a small percentage of these women report being unhappy with their
sexual experiences; and unlike men, where a strong correlation exists
between genital vasocongestion and subjective report of sexual arousal,
studies of womens response to erotic stimuli, usually in the form of videos, show that while they may show signs of physiological sexual
arousal, such as vasocongestion, they do not necessarily report subjective
sexual arousal (Laan, Everaerd, van Bellen, & Hanewald, 1994; Laan &
Janssen, 2007).
As a concept sexual desire is sometimes confused with the need for
intimacy or to be loved, sexual arousal, sex drive, and overt sexual
behavior (Wood, Barthalow Koch, & Kernoff Manseld, 2006). However, when heterosexual women and men are asked to dene sexual
desire, they tend to come up with a similar denitionthat it is primarily a psychological experience characterized by a sense of longing,
urge, yearning, need, want, and so forth, but they differ in what
they consider to be the goals and objects of sexual desire (Regan &
Berscheid, 1996). Women emphasize love, emotional intimacy, and
commitment as the goals of sexual desire more than men, while men
are more likely than women to specify a physically and/or sexually
attractive other as the object of sexual desire.
Most of the above research has been done with samples from the
West and how women from around the world conceive, dene, and act
upon sexual desire has not received as much research attention. Nevertheless, discourses on womens sexual desire outside Western or
European context do exist. For example, Bennett (2005), in her work on
single Muslim women in contemporary Indonesia, shows that these
young women are very aware of their embodied sexual desireeven
young women who do not have a sexual relationship are able to speak
about their sexual desires. Despite living in a social context where
openly sexually active women are stigmatized and where young single
women must carefully navigate their sexual desires, the needs of their
male partners, and social obligations, they can readily articulate their
sexual desires and they courageously and actively seek out their own
sexual autonomy.
The practice of female genital cutting (FGC), which can range from a
slight cutting of the clitoral hood to removal of the clitoris, the labia
major and minora, and in its most extreme version includes inbulation, the sewing together of the two sides of the remaining tissue with
only a small opening left for urination and menstruation, is common in
some parts of Africa, particularly, although not exclusively, among
Muslim cultures in Africa. Much international controversy surrounds
the practice of FGC due to its perceived negative health effects on
women and that it is often performed on girls before they reach

Women and Sexuality: An International Perspective

91

menarche. Among feminists in the West, FGC has been considered one
of the most dramatic and harmful symbols of womens subordination
(see Hernlund & Shell-Duncan, 2007, for a discussion), although over
time a more complex approach to this topic has evolved that takes into
consideration voices, experiences, and agency of African women themselves. One of the reoccurring questions with regard to FGC is whether
the various forms of FGC inuence sexual functioning or even sexual
desire. Although FGC is presumed to negatively affect womens ability
to experience sexual pleasure, available evidence demonstrates that
sexual desire, sexual pleasure, erotic feelings, and orgasm are experienced even among women with the most extreme forms of FGC
(Ahmadu, 2007; Dopico, 2007). In fact, qualitative interviews with
women who have experienced FGC demonstrate that many have satisfactory and pleasurable sexual relationships with men and they afrm
that sexual pleasure is equally important for women as for men.
When they note problems, they attribute them to the quality of their
relationships with partners, presence of children, stress from work, and
difcult economic circumstancesfactors that inuence the sexual
enjoyment of women who have not experienced FGC. Furthermore, as
Ahmadu (2007) notes, while chastity and modesty are typically part of
the cultural underpinnings for FGC, these do not condition women to
repress their sexuality in Islamic or African traditional societies
(pp. 294295). She contends that sexual passivity and repression for
women in the West stem from psychoanalytic models that are then
applied to understanding African womens sexual relationships (also
see Obermeyer, 2000, for a discussion). The uncritical application of
Western constructions of womens sexuality and sexual desire to
African women, particularly those who have experienced FGC, negates
the possibility of emergence of counter narratives surrounding sexuality
and they may also make difcult the work to address the sexual and
health problems such women may actually experience. Unequivocal
application of understandings, values, and priorities surrounding
womens sexual desire from Western centres to non-Western margins
(Phillips, 2007, p. 148) can, as in the case of sexuality among women
who experience FGC, obscure sexual meanings rather than clarify them.
Clearly, despite societal restrictions and cultural control, women across
the world can and do practice a sexual agency and it is not simply a
form of resistance, but also an assertion of their own erotic desires
(Blackwood & Wieringa, 1999b).
Another example of how cultural constructions of womens sexual
desire differ and how these constructions may organize and dene
womens own behavior and understanding of their sexual selves, is
that in many cultures where FGC occurs, womens sexual desire is
seen as stronger than that of men (Obermeyer, 2000). FGC, along with
other cultural controls on womens autonomy, such as veiling and

92

Feminism as Human Rights

restrictions on movements, are thought to be necessary to reign in this


powerful drive. This view is in contrast with the perception in the
United States, where women are believed to have lower sexual desire
than men and mens desire for sex is believed to be harder to control
(Baumeister, Catanese, & Vohs, 2001). Despite the apparent differences
in how sexual desire is believed to operate among women and men,
what is similar in both cases is that women from very different parts
of the world continue to have less freedom than men to openly express
and carry out their sexual desires. Desire and the search for sexual
pleasure are certainly part of womens sexuality everywhere, but
womens sexuality has always contained elements of both pleasure
and danger and the consequences for women who transgress traditional sexual customs, norms, and ideologies, whatever those may
be in a particular social and cultural context, can be quite serious
(Vance, 1985).
SEXUAL PROBLEMS
Both qualitative and quantitative research with international samples
of women demonstrates that the experience of sexual problems by
women, such as decient or absent sexual desire, insufcient lubrication, dyspareunia, and so forth, is not uncommon (Dopico, 2007;
Laumann, Paik, & Rosen, 1999; Parish, Luo, Laumann, Kew, & Yu,
2007; Shokrollahi, Mirmohamadi, Mehrabi, & Babei, 1999). Over the last
eight years, there has been a lively debate in the academic literature
over the denition, categorization, and diagnosis of womens sexual
health problems. On the one side is the medical model which focuses
on physiological processes and physical causes of womens sexual dysfunctions, on the other is the New View of Female Sexual Problems
which takes a more bio-psycho-social perspective on womens sexual
problems (Tiefer, 2002b). As is suggested in the labeling alone, the new
view of womens sexual problems sees the medical models use of the
term dysfunction as limiting, distorting, and pathologizing womens
experiences (McHugh, 2006). Its emphasis on sexual physiology and
performance reinforces narrow denitions of sexuality, ignores possible
contextual, social, and psychological factors, limits styles of sexuality to
genital arousal and orgasm only, and leads to the likelihood of treatment of sexual problems with pharmaceutical solutions rather than
counseling (Bancroft, 2002; Bancroft, Graham, & McCord, 2001; Moynihan, 2003; Moynihan, 2005; Tiefer, 2002b; McHugh, 2006; Nicholls,
2008). In response to the medical model, in 2000 a multidisciplinary
group of scholars, therapists, and researchers convened to conceptualize and design an alternative classication system for womens sexual
concerns taking into account social, political, emotional, relational, and
physical aspects of womens sexual experiences (Tiefer, 2001, 2002b).

Women and Sexuality: An International Perspective

93

Their work culminated in the production of a manifesto/document,


classication scheme, and numerous commentaries and research
articles promoting their New View of female sexual problems
(Wood, Barthalow Koch, & Kernoff Manseld, 2006).
The New View work group prefers the term sexual problems to
sexual dysfunction. They dene sexual problems as discontent or
dissatisfaction with any emotional, physical, or relational aspect of sexual experience (Kaschak & Tiefer, 2002). Their classication system,
based upon psychological, biological, and social causes of womens
sexual problems, has four categories that women can use to identify
their own sexual problems (Kaschak & Tiefer, 2002). The four categories that encompass issues such as lack of sexual information, cultural
attitudes and messages, family and work stress, partner and relationship issues, and psychological factors like depression and anxiety
are as follows: (1) sociocultural, political, or economic factors; (2)
partner and relationship factors; (3) psychological factors; and (4)
medical factors.
The New View perspective on womens sexual problems ts well
with the more contextualized approach to womens sexuality taken by
global researchers, however, almost no research is available that tests
the utility of the New View classication system or compares it to the
medical model with actual samples of women from diverse cultures
and societies experiencing sexual problems. Nicholls (2008) has provided one of the rst studies documenting the validity of the New
View categorization system. Nichols surveyed 49 British heterosexual
women about their sexual problems to see whether they could classify
the problems using the New View classication scheme. Overall, 98 percent of the reported problems were categorized using the New View
scheme; 65 percent were attributed to relational factors, 20 percent to
contextual/external, 8 percent to individual psychological, and 7 percent to medical factors. Interestingly, medical factors accounted for the
least amount of sexual problems, whereas relational and contextual
factors accounted for the most. These results demonstrate the importance of taking into consideration womens social and environmental
contexts, as well as their relationships when assessing their sexual
problems. Likewise, medical research is beginning to acknowledge the
importance of partner- and contextual-related factors on womens sexual functioning (Chedraui, Perez-Lopez, San Miguel, & Avila, 2009;
Domoney, 2009).
The new view of womens sexual problems is predicated upon a
Western ideal of the companionate heterosexual relationship, rather
than the variety of relationship forms and customs outside the West
where marriage is primarily a system of organizing social reproduction
and where the sexual needs and problems of non-married women may
not even be acknowledged. However, anthropological research points

94

Feminism as Human Rights

to the extensive existence and valuing of both passionate and companionate love in many societies around the world, even in those societies
where womens choice of marriage partner is made for them by
the family (see Inhorn, 2007, for a discussion; Jankowiak & Fischer,
1992). As globalization processes further expand the discourse on
womens sexuality and relationship options, hopefully, we will see in
the upcoming decade the emergence of more empirical studies demonstrating the strengths of the New View perspective at an international
level which may help to expand our knowledge of and treatment for
womens sexual problems everywhere.
CONCLUSION
At the beginning of this new millennium, sexuality is undergoing a
revolution, one that is maybe less apparent, but no less real, than those
that occurred in the 1920s and 1960s. While the changes in sexual
behavior and attitudes seen in those previous periods were geographically focused on the United States and Western Europe, the current
sexual revolution is seen across Africa, Asia, the Middle East, and
Latin America (Francoeur & Noonan, 2004; Mahdavi, 2007; Reinfelder,
1996b), as well as in North America and Europe. The changes may be
small in any given community, uneven across different worldwide
societies and cultures, but they are most clearly seen in the slow but
incremental changes in sexual behavior and attitudes among women
and men. As Weeks (2007) states we are living in a time of transition,
in the midst of a long, convoluted, messy, unnished but profound
revolution that has transformed the possibilities of living our sexual diversity and creating intimate lives (p. 3).
REFERENCES
Ahmadu, F. (2007). Aint I a woman too? Challenging myths of sexual dysfunction in circumcised women. In Y. Hernlund & B. Shell-Duncan (Eds.),
Transcultural bodies: Female genital cutting in global context (pp. 278310).
New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Amaro, H., Navarro, A. M., Conron, K. J., & Raj, A. (2002). Cultural inuences
on womens sexual health. In G. Wingood & R. J. DiClemente (Eds), Handbook of womens sexual and reproductive health (pp. 7192). New York: Kluwer
Academic/Plenum.
Bailey, J. M. (2009). What is sexual orientation and do women have one? In
D. A. Hope (Ed.), Contemporary perspectives on lesbian, gay, and bisexual identities (pp. 4363).
Bancroft, J. (2002). The medicalization of female sexual dysfunction: The need
for caution. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 451455.
Bancroft, J., Graham, C. A., & McCord, C. (2001). Conceptualizing womens
sexual problems. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 27, 95103.

Women and Sexuality: An International Perspective

95

Basson, R. (2005). Womens sexual dysfunction: Revised and expanded denitions: Canadian Medical Association, 172, 13271333.
Baumeister, R. F., Catanese, K. R., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Is there a gender difference in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, conceptual distinctions,
a review of relevant evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5,
242273.
Bennett, L. R. (2005). Women, Islam, and modernity. New York: Routledge/
Cuzon.
Berger, S. A. (2006). Guatemaltecas: The Womens Movement 19862003. Austin,
TX: University of Texas Press.
Blackwood, E., & Wieringa, S. E. (Eds.). (1999a). Same-sex relationships and female
desires: Transgender practices across cultures. New York: Columbia University
Press.
Blackwood, E., & Wieringa, S. E. (1999b). Sapphic shadows: Challenging the
silence in the study of sexuality. In E. Blackwood & S. E. Wieringa (Eds.),
Same-sex relationships and female desires: Transgender practices across cultures
(pp. 3966). New York: Columbia University Press.
Carnoy, M., & Castells, M. (2001). Globalization, the knowledge society, and
the Network State: Poulantzas at the millennium. Global Networks, 1, 118.
Casta~
neda, D., & G
omez Bastidas, E. (2005). HIV/AIDS risk behavior among
Mexican women working in a maquiladora. Revista Interamericana de
Psicologia, 39, 267274.
Chedraui, P., Perez-Lopez, F. R., San Miguel, G., & Avila, C. (2009). Assessment of sexuality among middle-aged women using the Female Sexual
Function Index. Climacteric: The Journal of the International Menopause Society, 12, 213221.
Correa, S. & Jolly, S. (2008). Developments encounter with sexuality: Essentialism and beyond. In A. Cornwall, S. Correa, & S. Jolly (Eds.), Development
with a body: Sexuality, human rights, & development (pp. 2242). London:
Zed Books.
Correa, S., & Parker, R. (2004). Sexuality, human rights, and demographic
thinking: Connections and disjunctions in a changing world. Sexuality
Research & Social Policy, 1, 1538.
Dabash, R. & Roudi-Fahimi, F. (2008, September). Abortion in the Middle East
and North Africa, policy brief. Washington, DC: Population Reference
Bureau, Gynuity Health Projects.
DEmilio, J., & Freedman, J. B. (1988). Intimate matters: A history of sexuality in
America. New York: Harper & Row.
Domoney, C. (2009). Sexual function in women: what is normal? International
Urogynecology Journal and Pelvic Floor Dysfunction, 20, S917.
Dopico, M. (2007). Inbulation and the orgasm puzzle: Sexual experiences of
inbulated Eritrean women in rural Eritrea and Melbourne, Australia. In
Y. Hernlund & B. Shell-Duncan (Eds.), Transcultural bodies: Female genital
cutting in global context (pp. 224247). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Durex Sexual Well-being Survey (2007). Retrieved May 16, 2009, from http://
www.durex.com/en-US/SexualWellbeingSurvey/Fequency%20of%20Sex/
Pages/default.aspx

96

Feminism as Human Rights

Francoeur, R. T., & Noonan, R. J. (2004). Global trends: Some nal impressionsfrom The Contiuum Complete International Encyclopedia of Sexuality (CCIES).
Retrieved March 11, 2009, from http://www.kinseyinstitute.org/ccies/
globaltrends.php
Goldberg, M. (2009). The means of reproduction: Sex, power, and the future of the
world. New York: Penguin Press.
Graham, C. A., Saunders, S. A., Milhausen, R. R., & McBride, K. R. (2004).
Turning on and turning off: a focus group study of the factors that affect
womens sexual arousal. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33, S27S38.
Grieg, A. (2008). The rights of men. In A. Cornwall, S. Correa, & S. Jolly (Eds.),
Development with a body: Sexuality, human rights, & development (pp. 8692).
London: Zed Books.
Hernlund, Y. & Shell-Duncan, B. (2007). Transcultural positions: Negotiating
rights and culture. In Y. Hernlund & B. Shell-Duncan (Eds.), Transcultural
bodies: Female genital cutting in global context (pp. 145). New Brunswick,
NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Hirsch, J. S. (2007). Love makes a family: Globalization, companionate marriage, and the modernization of gender inequality. In M. B. Padilla,
J. S. Hirsch, M. Mu~
noz-Laboy, R. E. Sember, & R. G. Parker (Eds.), Love
and globalization: Transforming intimacy in the contemporary world (pp. 93
106). Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University Press.
Inhorn, M. C. (2007). Loving your infertile Muslim spouse: Notes on the globalization of IVF and its romantic commitments in Sunni Egypt and Shia
Lebanon. In M. B. Padilla, J. S. Hirsch, M. Mu~
noz-Laboy, R. E. Sember, &
R. G. Parker (Eds.), Love and globalization: Transforming intimacy in the contemporary world (pp. 139162). Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University Press.
Jankowiak, W. R., & Fischer, E. F. (1992). A cross-cultural perspective on
romantic love. Ethnology, 31, 149155.
Kaschak, E., & Tiefer, L. (Eds.). (2002). A new view of womens sexual problems.
New York: Haworth Press.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebbard, P. H. (1953). Sexual
behavior in the human female. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.
Laan, E., & Both, S. (2008). What makes women experience desire? Feminism &
Psychology, 18, 502514.
Laan, E., Everaerd, W., van Bellen, G., & Hanewald, G. (1994). Womens sexual
and emotional responses to male- and female-produced erotica. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 23, 15369.
Laan, E., & Janssen, E. (2007). How do men and women feel? Determinants of
subjective experience of sexual arousal. In E. Janssen (Ed.), The psychophysiology of sex (pp. 278290). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
Laumann, E. O., Paik, A., & Rosen, R. C. (1999). Sexual dysfunction in the
United States: Prevalence and predictors. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 281, 537544.
Laumann, E. O., Paik, A., Glasser, D. B., Kang, J., Wang, T., Levinson, B., et al.
(2006). A cross-national study of subjective sexual well being among older
women and men: Finding from the Global Study of Sexual Attitudes and
Behaviors. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 145161.
Lemp, G. F., Jones, M., Kellogg, T. A., Nieri, G. N., Anderson, L., Withum, D.,
et al. (1995). HIV seroprevalence and risk behaviors among lesbians and

Women and Sexuality: An International Perspective

97

bisexual women in San Francisco and Berkeley, California. American Journal of Public Health, 85, 15491552.
Mackay, J. (2001). Global sex: Sexuality and sexual practices around the world.
Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 16, 7182.
Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1966). Human sexual response. Boston, MA:
Little, Brown.
Mahdavi, P. (2007). Passionate uprisings: Young people, sexuality and politics
in post-revolutionary Iran. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 9, 445457.
McHugh, M. C. (2006). What do women want? A new view of womens sexual
problems. Sex Roles, 54, 361369.
Mongrovejo, N. (1999). Sexual preference, the ugly duckling of feminist
demands: The lesbian movement in Mexico. In E. Blackwood & S. E. Wieringa
(Eds.), Same-sex relations and female desires: Transgender practices across culture
(pp. 308335). New York: Columbia University Press.
Moynihan, R. (2003). The making of a disease: Female sexual dysfunction. British Medical Journal, 326, 4547.
Moynihan, R. (2005). The marketing of a disease: Female sexual dysfunction.
British Medical Journal, 330, 192194.
Nicholls, L. (2008). Putting the new view classication scheme to an empirical
test. Feminism & Psychology, 18, 515526.
Nicolosi, A., Laumann, E. O., Glasser, D. B., Moreira, E., Paik, A., & Gingell, C.
(2004). Sexual behvior and sexual dysfunctions after age 40: The Global
Study of Sexual Attitudes and Behavior. Urology, 64, 991997.
Obermeyer, C. M. (2000). Sexuality in Morocco: Changing context and contested domain. Culture, Health, & Sexuality, 2, 239254.
Oliver, M. B., & Shibley Hyde, J. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 2951.
Padilla, M. B., Hirsch, J. S., Mu~
noz-Laboy, M., Sember, R. E., & Parker, R. G.
(2007). Introduction: Cross-cultural reections on an intimate intersection.
In M. B. Padilla, J. S. Hirsch, M. Mu~
noz-Laboy, R. E. Sember, &
R. G. Parker (Eds.), Love and globalization: Transforming intimacy in the contemporary world (pp. ixxxxi). Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University Press.
Parish, W. L., Luo, Y., Laumann, E. O., Kew, M., & Yu, Z. (2007). Unwanted
sexual activity in married women in urban China. Journal of Sex Research,
44, 158171.
Parker, R. (2007). Editorial: Sexuality, health, and human rights. American Journal of Public Health, 97, 972973.
Parker, R., di Mauro, D., Filiano, B., Garcia, J., Mu~
noz-Laboy, M., & Sember,
R. (2004). Global transformations and intimate relations in the 21st century: Social science research on sexuality and emergence of sexual
health and sexual rights frameworks. Annual Review of Sex Research, 15,
362398.
Petchesky, R. P. (2003). Global prescriptions: Gendering health and human rights.
London: Zed Books Ltd.
Phillips, R. (2007). Histories of sexuality and imperialism: Whats the use? History Workshop Journal, 63, 136153.
Promotion of Sexual Health: Recommendations for Action. (2000, May 1922). Proceedings from a regional consultation convened by the Pan American
Health Organization, World Health Organization, in collaboration with the

98

Feminism as Human Rights

World Association of Sexology. Retrieved April 16, 2009, from http://


www.paho.org/English/HCP/HCA/PromotionSexualHealth.pdf
Raimondo, M. (2005). Intensications: Representing gender and sexuality at the
UN General Assembly Special Session on HIV/AIDS. In W. S. Hesford &
W. Kozol (Eds.), Just advocacy? Womens human rights, transnational feminisms, and the politics of representation (pp. 195220). New Brunswick, NJ:
Rutgers University Press.
Regan, P. C., & Berscheid, E. (1996). Beliefs about the state, goals, and objects
of sexual desire. Journal of Marital & Sexual Therapy, 22, 110120.
Reinfelder, M. (1996a). Persecution and resistance. In M. Reinfelder (Ed.), Amazon
to Zami: Towards a global lesbian feminism (pp. 1129). London: Cassell.
Reinfelder, M. (1996b). Introduction: Weaving international webs. In M. Reinfelder (Ed.), Amazon to Zami: Towards a global lesbian feminism (pp. 19).
London: Cassell.
Reinisch, J. M., Sanders, S. A., & Ziemba-Davis, M. (1995). Self-labeled sexual
orientation and sexual behavior: Considerations for STD-related biomedical
research and education. In M. Stein & A. Baum (Eds.), Chronic diseases: Perspectives in behavioral medicine (pp. 241257). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Rich, A. (1983). Compulsory heterosexuality and lesbian experience. In A. Snitow,
C. Stansell, & S. Thompson (Eds.), Powers of desire: The politics of sexuality
(pp. 177205). New York: Monthly Review Press.
Richters, J., Bergin, S., Lubowitz, S., & Prestage, G. (2002). Women in contact
with Sydneys gay and lesbian community: Sexual identity, practices and
HIV risks. AIDS Care, 14, 193202.
Ruklos Hampton, M., McWatters, B., Jeffrey, B., & Smith, P. (2006). Sexual
health of young women: Context and care make a difference. In W. Schissel (Ed.), Geographies of self, place, and space: Home/bodies (pp. 161178). Calgary, Alberta: University of Calgary Press.
Sard
a, A. (2008). Discrimination against lesbians in the workplace. In A. Cornwall, S. Correa, & S. Jolly (Eds.), Development with a body: Sexuality, human
rights, & development (pp. 107120). London: Zed Books.
Sexual HealthA New Focus for WHO. (2004). Progress in Reproductive Health
Research (Vol. 67). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.
Shokrollahi, P., Mirmohamadi, M., Mehrabi, F., & Babaei, G. (1999). Prevalence
of sexual dysfunction in women seeking services at family planning centers in Tehran. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 25, 211215.
Tiefer, L. (2001). The consensus conference on female sexual dysfunction:
Conicts of interest and hidden agendas. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy,
27, 227236.
Tiefer, L. (2002a). The emerging global discourse of sexual rights. Journal of Sex &
Marital Therapy, 28, 439444.
Tiefer, L. (2002b). Beyond the medical model of womens sexual problems: A
campaign to resist the promotion of female sexual dysfunction. Sexual
and Relationship Therapy, 17, 127135.
Undie, C., Crichton, J., & Zulu, E. (2007). Metaphors we love by: Conceptualizations of sex among young people in Malawi. African Journal of Reproductive Health, 11, 221235.

Women and Sexuality: An International Perspective

99

Vance, C. S. (1985). Pleasure and danger: Toward a politics of sexuality. In


C. S. Vance (Ed.), Pleasure and danger: Exploring female sexuality (pp. 127).
Boston, MA: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Weeks, J. (2007). The world we have won. London: Routledge.
Wekker, G. (1999). Whats identity got to do with it? Rethinking identity in
light of the Mati work in Suriname. In E. Blackwood & S. E. Wieringa
(Eds.), Same-sex relationships and female desires: Transgender practices across
cultures (pp. 119138). New York: Columbia University Press.
Wieringa, S. E., & Blackwood, E. (1999). Introduction. In E. Blackwood &
S. E. Wieringa (Eds.), Same-sex relationships and female desires: Transgender
practices across cultures (pp. 138). New York: Columbia University Press.
Wood, J. M., Barthalow Koch, P., & Kernoff Manseld, P. (2006). Womens sexual desire: A feminist critique. Journal of Sex Research, 43, 236244.

This page intentionally left blank

Chapter 5

Arranged and Forced Marriage


Noorfarah Merali

The family is the cornerstone of society, making marriage a highly valued social institution in every part of the world. Marriage is a legally
and socially recognized union that entails sexual, economic, and social
rights and obligations for the partners, with the expectation of a lifelong relationship (Bachrach, Hindin, & Thompson, 2000). The United
Nations branding of 1994 as the International Year of the Family led to
the explication of the basic human rights of individuals within marital
relationships, and to increased attention being paid to their protection
and preservation (Levesque, 2001). Sexual rights in marital relationships involve individuals having autonomous input in sexual and
reproductive decision making (Levesque, 2001). Economic rights
comprise a broad category that asserts each individuals entitlement
to material sustenance in terms of food, shelter, health care, and any
other requirements to satisfy ones basic needs (Hellsten, 2005;
Levesque, 2001; Texier, 2005). This category of rights also emphasizes
each family members access to and decision-making power over personally acquired resources (Gilbert, 2005a), such as employment income.
The right to education, employment, and access to community resources
and opportunities for ones betterment is also an economic right, as well
as a developmental right accorded to youth (United Nations, 1990;
Wilson, 2005). Social rights address both family relations and interactions within the surrounding cultural milieu. This category of rights
emphasizes freedom of association (Eide, 2005; McBride, 2005), and freedom in forming and exiting marital and family relationships (Probert,
2005). Cultural social rights include each persons right to cultural preservation, cultural participation, and some level of cultural self-determination
(Gilbert, 2005b; Levesque, 2001).

102

Feminism as Human Rights

The protection of all of these basic human rights in marital relationships is predicated on accepting the equality in status and worth of
marital partners and on upholding each persons overarching right to
personal security in family life (Levesque, 2001; MacPherson, 2004).
Levesque postulates that women are often reliant upon their families to
protect their rights and well-being. Therefore, the above rights intersect
with womens rights to gender equality and freedom from domestic violence (Cook & Cusack, 2005; Guichon, 2005).
While Western societies have emphasized personal initiation of
the mate selection and marriage process, many other societies around
the world have historically practiced arranged marriages. These
include China (Xiaohe & Whyte, 1990; Zang, 2008), Japan (Blood, 1967;
Langen, Streltzer, & Kai, 1997), Vietnam (Wisensale, 1999), Israel
(Rockman, 1994), Romania (Oprea, 2005), Nepal (Ghimire, Axinn,
Yabiku, & Thornton, 2006), Pakistan (Zaidi & Shuraydi, 2002), Kenya
(Cattell, 1992), India (Madathil & Benshoff, 2008), Bangladesh and Sri
Lanka (Kurian, 1991), and various societies in the Middle East (Bhopal,
1999). These societies vary in the ethnic, cultural, and religious composition of their resident populations, attesting to the widespread practice
of arranged marriages across diverse groups. For the purpose of this
chapter, an arranged marriage is dened as a marriage that is formed
through third-party intervention. Arranged marriages usually involve
parents or relatives identifying potential mates for their sons or daughters and planning the marriage ceremony. However, some arranged
marriages involve religious gures or matchmakers that families enlist
to facilitate the joining of two parties who have compatible spiritual,
astrological, or cultural proles. The use of religious or cultural mediators by families arranging the marriages of their sons or daughters
has occurred across various groups, such as some Orthodox Jews
(Rockman, 1994) and families of Japanese descent (Blood, 1967).
Marriages that are arranged by parents tend to have the goal of uniting two families to fulll various obligations to each other rather than
to bond two autonomous individuals. They are based on cultural values that emphasize collectivism or family and group-centered living,
rather than the rugged individualism of Western society. Familyarranged marriages tend to be perceived as sacramental unions that
are based on perceived compatibility between the couple in family
background, education, culture, and religious beliefs and values
(Bhopal, 1999). The assumption that often lies behind family-arranged
marriages is that love is not sufcient to sustain a lifelong relationship;
it may actually lead individuals to make erroneous decisions in the
mate selection process due to idealization of their desired mate (Xiaohe
& Whyte, 1990). In contrast, similarity in background and fundamental
values is viewed to facilitate a secure bond that can endure the challenges of childrearing, aging, and the passage of time. It is also

Arranged and Forced Marriage

103

assumed that love and passion will eventually develop in arranged


marriages after a true friendship and partnership is formed. Xiaohe
and Whyte (1990) state this common assumption frankly: Love
matches start out hot and grow cold, while arranged marriages start
out cold and grow hot (p. 709).
Arranged marriages may occur at any point during the lifespan,
from childhood through widowhood (Cattell, 1992; Levesque, 2001)
and involve varying levels of choice and active participation among
the parties whose lives are being joined. Some arranged marriages
occur with the assumption of a monogamous relationship, whereas
others involve entering a specic rank order in pre-existing polygamous family systems (Catell, 1992). The level of gender equity
or inequity in family relations may be affected by the nature of
the arranged marriage process, such as negotiation of a bride price
(Rastogi & Therly, 2006), or by cultural norms related to family life in
the surrounding society (Wisensale, 1999).
This chapter discusses the variability in arranged marriage practices,
considering issues of consent and coercion as they relate to basic
human rights of freedom of association, personal security, and opportunities for personal development. The inuence of state laws, immigration policies, modernization, acculturation, and sociodemographic
variables on arranged marriage practices is also examined. The chapter
subsequently reviews research on the interpersonal and personal outcomes of arranged marriages with and without coercion. These outcomes include marital satisfaction, domestic violence, mental health
status, self-harm/suicide attempts, and criminality. Where comparative
data is available for arranged marriages versus self-initiated unions,
these alternative family formation strategies are evaluated in terms of
their outcomes. The chapter concludes with a critical analysis of existing laws, policies, and arranged marriage outcomes to outline future
directions that respect cultural differences in structuring family life and
fundamental human rights of individuals within family relationships.
SUBTYPES OF ARRANGED MARRIAGE
Three subtypes of arranged marriage have been described in the literature. They involve different levels of womens participation and
choice in the process. The rst subtype is the traditional or planned
pattern. In this form of arranged marriage, parents identify, screen,
and select a mate for their daughter through contact with relatives,
family friends, or religious mediators. There may be little or no interaction between the woman and her potential husband. In cases where
the woman does not have the opportunity to see the selected man prior
to her wedding day, this is referred to as a blind marriage. In other
cases, she may only see a photograph and then meet him for the rst

104

Feminism as Human Rights

time at the marriage ceremony. Alternatively, she may have one or two
face-to-face meetings with him, and the meetings may be supervised
by family members (Qureshi, 1991; Stopes-Roe & Cochrane, 1990).
The second subtype of arranged marriage is the modied traditional
or delegation pattern. In this form of arranged marriage, the daughter
has some input in the type of mate she is seeking, and parents take
into account this input in identifying a suitable mate for her. Upon the
identication of a potential husband, the woman is then given the opportunity to say yes or no. If she declines the identied candidate, the
parents continue their search for another suitable person for her consideration.
The last subtype of arranged marriage is the cooperative or jointventure pattern. Both parents and their daughter may be involved in
the identication, screening, and marriage planning process, but parental consent is an essential prerequisite for the marriage. Some dating
and courtship may occur prior to the marriage, and this may be supervised or unsupervised (Qureshi, 1991; Stopes-Roe & Cochrane, 1990).
CONSENT AND COERCION
It is important to note that although all three subtypes of arranged
marriage emphasize parental choice and consent for the daughters
marriage, the daughters level of choice and consent may vary across
the subtypes. A forced marriage occurs when parties are coerced into
marriage against their will with both physical and emotional pressure
that results in signicant duress (Dostrovsky, Cook, & Gagnon, 2007).
Forced marriages violate both the basic human right to freedom of
association in entering marital relationships and the right to personal
security in the application of physical consequences for declining the
proposed union.
A daughter who sincerely believes that her parents are acting on her
best interests and will identify the best marriage partner for her may
voluntarily submit to a blind marriage. This willingness to participate
in such an arrangement would make it consensual, and there would be
no violation of the womans human rights. In contrast, a daughter who
has a desire to have personal input into the process may perceive the
traditional or planned pattern of arranged marriage as coercive if she
has voiced this desire and her desire has been disregarded. Similarly, it
is possible that a daughter whose family is practicing the modied traditional pattern, where the parents are presenting potential mates for
her to consider, may still feel coerced into the process out of obligation
to her parents. This is particularly likely if she wishes to independently
seek and join with her potential husband (Zaidi & Shuraydi, 2002). If a
daughter desires a mate that her parents are not likely to approve
(for example, a male from outside of her cultural group), even the

Arranged and Forced Marriage

105

cooperative or joint venture arranged marriage pattern may be experienced as a forced marriage process. Therefore, any of the three subtypes of arranged marriage may occur without any coercion or with
the application of varying levels of emotional or physical pressure.
Because arranged marriages occur at various stages in the lifespan
across different cultural groups and world societies, the level of choice
and consent that a person has in the process may vary with the individuals age and developmental stage at the time of the marriage. The
power to exercise personal decision-making may further be affected by
a womans economic and social resources. In many parts of the world,
parents have arranged the marriages of young girls to adolescent or
adult males (Cattell, 1992; Levesque, 2001; Oprea, 2005; Tariq & Tariq,
1993). This practice is referred to as child marriage. It represents a violation of childrens rights to experiences and opportunities to facilitate
their optimal cognitive, emotional, educational, and social development
as stipulated in the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the
Child (United Nations, 1990). These rights of children and youth parallel adult economic and social rights. Child marriage may also violate
childrens right to personal security, as they most often occur through
the application of force and can represent cases of sexual assault of
minors (Cattell, 1992; Levesque, 2001; Tariq & Tariq, 1993).
The most common form of arranged marriage occurs among young
adult daughters and sons. Although there may still be application of
some coercion in these cases, adults may have some options for resisting the marriage attempt. Nevertheless, they may face serious consequences for their resistance, such as a loss of family honor, being
socially ostracized by the cultural community, abuse, and in the worst
case scenario, honor killings (Werbner, 2007; Wilson, 2007).
In some cultures, such as some African subcultures, the marriages of
widows are also arranged (Potash, 1986). These second marriages may
occur in childhood among girls who have had child marriages, during
middle adulthood, or in later life. Cultural practices surrounding the
arranged marriage of widows initially stemmed from the benevolent
intent to ensure that women and their children were taken care of and
provided material sustenance after the death of their husbands. To
ensure adequate caretaking, widow marriages were most often
arranged to other immediate family members, such as the deceased
husbands brother or cousin (Cattell, 1992). However, such marriages
do not take into account widows desires or intentions and tend to be
forced by family members. Many women do not consent to unite with
their husbands kin and instead want to pursue an independent existence (Cattell, 1992). Some actively protest or resist consummating a
marriage with their husbands relatives, but limitations in their economic and social resources may make them vulnerable to coercion
(Potash, 1986). In some of the African cultures practicing arrangement

106

Feminism as Human Rights

of widow marriages, the widow is married into a polygamous family,


and may occupy the lowest status among other co-wives, leading to a
potential for collective maltreatment (Cattell, 1992). Coerced widow
marriages represent a violation of womens right to freedom in entering and exiting marital and family relationships in international human
rights statutes (Probert, 2005).
LAWS RELATED TO ARRANGED MARRIAGE
Bachrach et al.s (2000) denition of marriage emphasized both legal
and social recognition of the union between marriage partners.
Although some cultural groups may desire to practice arranged marriage, making it a socially sanctioned strategy for mate selection, applicable laws in their jurisdictions may not recognize some or all types of
arranged marriages. Lack of legal recognition of arranged marriages
would present a major barrier to the utilization of this family formation
strategy. Laws related to marriage and family life vary greatly across
countries, despite the existence of international statutes on human
rights, womens rights, and childrens rights that have direct bearing
on marriage practices (Levesque, 2001). Existing laws reect cultural
and political changes that have occurred in various societies over time.
They address multiple aspects of the marriage and family establishment process, such as choice of a marriage partner (Dostrovsky et al.,
2007; Xiaohe & Whyte, 1990; Zang, 2008), the minimum age at the time
of marriage (Dostrovsky et al., 1007; Wisensale, 1999), monetary
exchange in the marriage process (Rastogi & Therly, 2006), living
arrangements of the marriage partners (Wisensale, 1999), and gender
equality in the marital relationship (Wisensale, 1999). Examples of state
laws that cover each of these areas and their impacts are provided in
this section.
In North America, legislation on marriage and family life emphasizes personal initiative and choice in mate selection (Levesque, 2001;
MacPherson, 2004). Other societies in the world have attempted to encourage personal choice and initiative in the mate selection and marriage process as a result of Western cultural inuence, or in response
to an inux of immigrant and refugee groups practicing arranged marriage. For example, for centuries, blind marriages were the most common form of marriage in China across all social classes. Chinese young
adults would typically not even meet until their wedding day and the
entire marriage process was strictly controlled and coerced by their
parents (Xiaohe & Whyte, 1990). With increasing Western inuence in
the Republican era and the subsequent victory of the Chinese Communists, there was a major movement toward increasing personal choice
in mate selection. Under the Communist government, the Marriage
Law of the Peoples Republic of China was adopted in 1950, denouncing

Arranged and Forced Marriage

107

the arbitrary and feudal marriage system . . . which ignores the childrens
interests and proclaiming that marriage shall be based upon the complete
willingness of the two parties. Neither party shall use compulsion, and no
third party shall be allowed to interfere (Yang, 1959, p. 221, as cited in
Xiaohe & Whyte, 1990).
The new Marriage Law withdrew legal recognition for any type of
arranged marriage, in attempt to abolish forced marriage. This major
legislative change dramatically reduced the frequency of the practice of
arranged marriage in Chinese society. In a multiple cohort study of
marriages that took place in China from the 1930s to the 1980s, Xiaohe
and Whyte (1990) found a consistent decline in arranged marriages
over each decade. Close to 70 percent of marriages that took place in
the 1930s were arranged or forced, whereas only 10 percent of marriages that occurred in the 1980s were arranged or forced according to
the reports of married Chinese women.
Zang (2008) obtained similar ndings of increased personal choice
and initiative in mate selection and marriage across recent decades in
another multiple cohort study of a large-scale sample in China. Zangs
(2008) study is particularly important because it included not only
Chinese participants, but also Turkish Muslim immigrants residing in
China over the same decades. Therefore, the ndings of the study
attested to the effectiveness of laws in reducing marriage arrangement
across a multi-ethnic sample. In response to the inux of immigrants
and refugees who practice arranged marriage with varying levels of
force into many world societies, a number of countries have also
introduced laws that criminalize forced marriage. These countries
include Britain, Norway, Belgium, Australia, Denmark, and Germany
(Dostrovsky et al., 2007). However, these pluralistic societies have not
criminalized third-party intervention in family formation, as marriage
arrangement can also occur without emotional or physical pressure
and distress. Allowing for noncoercive third-party intervention in family formation would serve to protect and uphold basic human rights
related to cultural preservation and cultural self-determination.
Political changes in Vietnamese society over time also led to the
introduction of new laws related to marriage and family life
(Wisensale, 1999). In addition to banning arranged and forced marriages, these laws increased the minimum age of marriage to prevent
child and adolescent marriages. This step would serve to protect and
promote youths rights to educational and social opportunities and
experiences critical for their optimal development in accordance with
the United Nations Declaration on the Rights of the Child (United
Nations, 1990). Acknowledging womens assistance in defeating French
colonists in Vietnam during the mid-1950s, the Marriage and Family
Law, which took effect in 1960, set a number of parameters to protect
and promote womens rights, as well as the rights of men and children.

108

Feminism as Human Rights

The law abolished arranged marriages, aiming to guarantee men and


women equal freedom in the choice of a marriage partner. It also made
polygamy illegal in Vietnamese society, enforcing monogamous relationships as the state norm. Similar to the effect of Chinese legislation,
this change led to a decrease in the proportion of arranged marriages
from 60 percent to less than 20 percent over only a single decade after
the passage of the law, as well as to subsequent reductions in the following decades. Monogamy also became the only surviving form of
marriage and family life (Wisensale, 1999). The law raised the minimum age of marriage for women to 18, and the minimum age for marriage was raised to age 21 for men (Wisensale, 1999). Therefore, there
would be no legal recognition of child or adolescent marriages, making
them subject to being nullied. Following suit, a number of nations
hosting cultural groups that have traditionally promoted early marriage have raised the minimum age of marriage in matrimonial laws,
including Great Britain, France, Gabon, and Indonesia (Dostrovsky
et al., 2007).
It is important to note that Vietnams Marriage and Family Law of
1960 was particularly unique from other state laws undergoing reform
at that time in developing countries in two ways. First, the law
included provisions emphasizing womens freedom of living arrangements after the marriage takes place. It specied that women shall not
be forced to follow customary rules under which the wife usually
moved in with her husbands parents (Law on Marriage and the Family as cited in Wisensale, 1999, p. 607). This aspect of the law is revolutionary. Extended family living arrangements are common across a
number of cultures and societies, and may provide extra social support
and assistance during the childrearing process. However, newly married women living in such arrangements have been found to be vulnerable to collective maltreatment by the husband, in-laws, and husbands
siblings, due to the inherent power imbalance in the marital relationship in this context (Dasgupta, 2000; Levesque, 2001; Raj & Silverman,
2002). The new Vietnamese marriage law allowed newly married couples the option of nuclear family living if this was the desire of the
woman and/or man in the relationship.
Second, the Vietnamese law attempted to institute gender equality
in marital relations, specifying men and womens equal rights in their
interactions and relationships in all spheres, including property ownership (Wisensale, 1999). Wisensale (1999) points out, however, that it is
difcult to enforce the laws related to gender equality and to assess
their impact given the privacy of the domestic sphere. His research
found that substantial gender inequality still persists in male-female
relationships in Vietnamese society.
Another aspect of arranged marriages that has been addressed
through legislation is the exchange of money or goods in the marriage

Arranged and Forced Marriage

109

process. In some cultures and countries, such as some South Asian


nations, there has historically been a bride price or dowry that the
family of the female has to provide to the family of the groom to
accept their daughter into the new family she is joining. The amount of
the dowry tends to depend on the mans qualities, in terms of his socioeconomic status, education, family background, and appearance. The
greater the mans perceived worth, the greater the dowry that is
demanded for acceptance of a particular bride as his wife. The dowry
is usually given at the time of the marriage (Rastogi & Therly, 2006). In
other cultures, such as some African cultures, a similar exchange
may take the form of goods, such as cattle that a specic family owns
(Cattell, 1992).
Arranged marriages that are for the prot of one party or family in
the relationship have clear implications for the power balance in the
resulting marriage and for the perceived value of the man and woman
in the union. Also, as Rastogi and Therly (2006) explain, the exchange
of money or goods at the time of marriage often sets the stage for continuing demands placed on one family by the other, that may culminate in severe maltreatment or even murder of the new bride if they
are not met. In a review of studies related to dowry-related deaths in
India, Rastogi and Therly (2006) discovered that dowry-related violence
and deaths accounted for more fatalities among women than any other
causes of death. They also indicated that the range of annual deaths
due to dowries in specic parts of the country was between 421 and
5,582. These numbers are clearly alarming.
Rastogi and Therly (2006) describe the new marriage law that was
introduced in India in 1961 that criminalized the act of giving or
receiving a dowry on the part of either family in the marriage process.
Despite the development of this law, the practice of dowry and related
violence against women has continued to occur under the guise of
wedding gifts being given to the grooms family based on their
requests. These gifts may take the form of a down payment for a family home, a car, and so on. Demands for such so-called gifts have still
tended to continue after arranged marriages, jeopardizing the welfare
of the new bride if her familys nancial resources have already been
depleted. Since the demand for dowry is no longer publicly acknowledged, it makes it difcult for law enforcement ofcials to become
aware of dowry-related marital problems until after violence occurs. In
fact, many of the studies Rastogi and Therly (2006) reviewed attesting
to a disturbingly high prevalence of dowry-related deaths were conducted after the passage of the 1961 law. The involvement of dowry in
the fatalities was most often identied in investigating the womens
cause of death.
The ndings of the various studies reviewed above elucidate the
variable impact of legislation surrounding arranged marriages. Laws in

110

Feminism as Human Rights

various parts of the world have been successful in increasing personal


choice in mate selection and protecting and promoting childrens rights.
However, legislation appears to be substantially less effective in promoting gender equality and in controlling marriages for prot. Therefore,
women remain vulnerable to violations of their basic rights to equality
and personal security.

INDIVIDUAL-LEVEL VARIABLES AFFECTING ARRANGED


MARRIAGE PRACTICES
Laws exercise their inuence on marriage practices at the macro
level of societies or cultural groups within specic societies. Existing
research suggests that a number of microlevel individual variables also
affect the frequency of arranged marriages and the forms that they take.
These include Western cultural exposure through media inuence,
opportunities for interaction with peers, non-family living arrangements,
education, employment, gender, and acculturation.
The rst few variables emerged as important in a large-scale study
conducted across four different ethnic groups residing in Nepal
(Hindus, Tibetan Burmese, Hill Burmese, and Newar groups). Nepal
is another example of a country that had a widespread practice of
arranged marriage. In contrast to other developing nations that had
new legislation introduced related to marriage and family life between
the 1930s and 1960s, Nepal had a new king who began to encourage
more open interaction and exchange with non-Hindu nations starting
in the 1950s (Ghirmire, Axinn, Yaiku, & Thornton, 2006). This led to
increased exposure to Western culture and practices among the residents of Nepal, as well as advances in educational and social opportunities. Since that time, the frequency of arranged marriages among
residents of Nepal has dropped to 50 percent of all marriages
(Ghirmire et al., 2006). Ghirmire et al. (2006) found that the amount of
TV viewing, listening to Western radio, and reading of popular magazines was a signicant predictor of participation and choice in the
selection of a marriage partner among Nepal residents, regardless of
their cultural group membership. These types of media consumption
exposed young people in Nepal to Western ideals relating to romantic
love and autonomous decision making. Similarly, involvement in youth
clubs or activities that provided opportunities for peer interaction signicantly increased study participants involvement in decision making
related to their marriages. Other factors that were related to personal
initiative in the marriage process versus arranged marriage were educational attainment, moving away from the home to pursue education
in another city (non-family living arrangements), and employment outside of the home (Ghirmire et al., 2006). These variables would also

Arranged and Forced Marriage

111

provide youth with some independence from their families, which may
have been reinforced by their exposure to Western media.
Zang (2008) was interested in examining whether relationships
between increased education, employment status, and power in marital
decision making hold across both genders. The author hypothesized
that these relationships will not be evidenced among females from cultural groups that consider women as the main bearers and protectors
of family honor, emphasizing their purity and chastity. Zang (2008)
examined differences in arranged marriage rates between Han Chinese
and Turkish Uyghur Muslims residing in China after the changes to
marriage and family laws were instituted. Zang (2008) found that
arranged marriage rates declined steadily among both cultural groups
in each decade cohort. Education and employment status were clear
predictors of personal involvement in mate selection rather than family-arranged marriages; when these variables were controlled, there
was no difference between Han Chinese males and Turkish Uyghur
Muslim males in the rate of arranged marriages. However, even when
female educational level and occupational status was controlled, the
Uyghur Muslim women experienced higher levels of arranged marriages than the Han Chinese women. They also experienced higher levels of arranged marriage than the males from both cultural groups. The
ndings supported the researchers hypothesis: Cultural beliefs related
to female honor may lead women to be more frequently subject to parental intervention in mate selection and marriage than their male
counterparts, even if they are educated and employed.
A nal important individual-level variable affecting arranged marriage practices is acculturation. Berry (2006) describes acculturation as
a process of sociocultural transition resulting from intercultural contact
that produces attitudinal or behavioral change. In pluralistic societies,
immigrants from countries and cultures that have historically practiced
specic forms of arranged marriage may be exposed to different ways
of engaging in mate selection. They may also be exposed to cultural
ideals related to love and romance. This cross-cultural exposure
may change their expectations about the marriage process and their
preferences for how traditional practices should be adapted in their
new societies of residence. Zaidi and Shuraydis (2002) research serves
as a case example. These researchers investigated the marital attitudes
of second-generation young adult Pakistani Muslim females residing in
Canada and the United States. The females were part of a culture that
has a long history of practicing arranged marriages of the traditional
or planned subtype. Due to their experiences growing up in North
American society, the majority of their study participants expressed the
desire for shifting to the modied traditional pattern of arranged marriage where they have some choice in mate selection. Alternatively,
some expressed the desire to be free to self-initiate a love marriage.

112

Feminism as Human Rights

Many of the participants expressed strong statements about various


forms of resistance or rebellion they would engage in if their parents
were not willing to adapt the arranged marriage practice to allow them
greater choice and participation.

ARRANGED MARRIAGE OUTCOMES


It is critical that further legislative actions, policies, and human
rights advocacy in relation to arranged marriage be based on a solid
understanding of the interpersonal and personal outcomes of these
marriages, rather than on value judgments about how particular
cultures establish and structure family life. The following sections
review the emerging research in this area. Two interpersonal outcomes
of arranged marriages have been studied: marital satisfaction and
spousal abuse. In terms of personal outcomes, existing research has
examined personal well-being, mental health problems, self-harm/
suicide attempts, and criminality among individuals in arranged marriage situations.
Interpersonal Outcomes
Marital Satisfaction
Four studies have compared levels of marital satisfaction among
individuals in arranged versus self-initiated marriages. The ndings of
these studies have been inconsistent. However, the nature of the studies and the research samples employed shed light on how the specic
nature of arranged marriage practices and the match between personal
expectations of marriage and the reality of married life relate to marital
satisfaction.
Blood (1967) was the rst researcher to compare the outcomes of
arranged and self-initiated marriages. He wanted to empirically assess
the assumption of proponents of arranged marriage that love matches
start out hot and grow cold, while arranged marriages start out cold
and grow hot (Xiaohe & Whyte, 1990, p. 709). He administered a marital satisfaction survey to a sample of 444 couples living in various districts in Tokyo, Japan, who experienced either type of marriage.
Bloods study was a longitudinal study examining marital satisfaction
across various lengths of time during the rst decade after marriage
(i.e., less than two years, two to four years, ve to eight years, and nine
or more years). Bloods (1967) ndings challenged the assumption he
was testing. Overall, he discovered a signicant decline in self-reported
marital satisfaction over time among both arranged and self-initiated
marriages. However, there were important gender differences. Men in
arranged marriages appeared to show a signicantly smaller decline in

Arranged and Forced Marriage

113

marital satisfaction over time than men in self-initiated marriages. The


men with arranged marriages maintained higher levels of satisfaction
over longer periods of married life. This trend was reversed for
women, with women in arranged marriages showing a signicantly
greater decline in marital satisfaction with longer lengths of marriage
than their counterparts in self-initiated unions. Based on these ndings,
Blood (1967) concluded that the advantages of arranged marriages for
the maintenance of marital satisfaction and strong family bonds only
hold for husbands.
It is important to understand the ndings of Bloods (1967) study in
the context of Japanese arranged marriage customs and life in Tokyo
at the time of the research. Arranged marriage customs tended to
involve the young couple having a ritualized meeting. They also had
some opportunities to express an opinion about the family-chosen partner prior to entering the marriage (Blood, 1967). This suggests that
their arranged marriages were not necessarily coerced. Also, it is essential to take into account the fact that the time period during which
Bloods study was done was marked by increasing contact with Western culture and society. This contact may have led Japanese couples to
be exposed to ideals of romantic love that could have affected the
levels of marital satisfaction of those who perceived themselves as less
traditional, even if they were in traditional marriage arrangements.
Existing research on acculturation suggests that women from other cultures often nd greater liberation in Western culture than their male
counterparts (Dion & Dion, 2001), possibly accounting for the decline
in womens marital satisfaction over the time of Bloods (1967) study.
Xiaohe and Whyte (1990) attempted to replicate Bloods (1967) study
in China, but they focused their study exclusively on women. They
compared the self-reported levels of marital satisfaction of more than
500 Chinese women who had arranged versus self-initiated marriages
using a general questionnaire on marital quality. To allow for a simultaneous examination of changes in marital quality over time across
these two marriage methods, the researchers compared women who
got married in various decades prior to and after the change in Chinese
marriage and family laws. This included women who got married in
19331948, 19491957 and so on, up to the 19771987 decade. Therefore, their study utilized a multiple cohort, cross-sectional design.
In contrast to Bloods ndings, Xiaohe and Whyte (1990) found a
statistically signicant increase in womens levels of marital satisfaction
over time in both arranged and self-initiated marriages. Women who
had been married for 20 or more years reported the highest levels of
marital satisfaction across both marriage methods. In comparing satisfaction levels between women in arranged and self-initiated unions
across women married for different lengths of time, Chinese women in
self-initiated marriages reported higher satisfaction levels at each stage

114

Feminism as Human Rights

of married life. It is important to reiterate that the primary form of


arranged marriage practiced in China was blind marriage involving
signicant coercion (Xiaohe & Whyte, 1990). Therefore, the lower satisfaction of women in arranged marriages may simply reect longstanding discontent over forced marriage. It may not accurately reect the
effects of marriage arrangement per se. As was discussed in previous
sections, any of the three subtypes of arranged marriage may also
occur without coercion.
Madathil and Benshoff (2008) wanted to take into account expectations of marriage and their relationship to marital satisfaction in the
process of comparing arranged and self-initiated marriages. Their
study involved the best and most rigorous methodology out of all the
studies discussed so far, because (a) it conducted both a within-culture
comparison and a cross-cultural comparison to examine the possible
effects of Western exposure and acculturation on marital satisfaction;
(b) it included participants of both genders; (c) rather than using a general measure of marital satisfaction, it assessed multiple components of
satisfaction independently and in combination; and (d) the demographic data collected addressed participants level of interaction with
their spouse prior to marriage and whether their opinions about their
prospective partner were considered in the arranged marriage process.
Their study aimed to compare the perceived importance of various
marital characteristics and marital satisfaction among couples from
three groups: Asian Indians in arranged marriages residing in India,
Asian Indians in arranged marriages residing in the United States, and
Caucasian Americans in marriages of their own initiative. Their study
included approximately 100 couples from each of these groups.
It is important to highlight that 95 percent of the Asian Indian couples in the study who were residing in the United States reported that
they had met their partners prior to marriage and their opinions were
taken into account. Similarly, 82 percent of the Asian Indian couples
residing in India reported that they had seen their partners prior to the
arranged marriage and that their family considered their opinions. This
suggests that the forms of arranged marriages they experienced were
likely the modied traditional pattern or the cooperative pattern, and
there was no coercion involved. The mean age of the study participants
was fairly similar across the three groups. Their average ages ranged
from 39 to 45. The average length of time the couples had been married was also relatively similar across the three groups. They were all
in their second decade of married life, having been married between 12
and 18 years (Madathil & Benshoff, 2008).
Madathil and Benshoff administered the Characteristics of Marriage
Inventory (CHARISMA; Rosen-Grandon, 1998) to the couples across all
three groups. Respondents are asked to rate the level of importance
they attach to the marital issues addressed in each of the 18 items, as

Arranged and Forced Marriage

115

well as to rate their satisfaction with each marital issue. The 18 items
on the scale assess three core characteristics of marriage. The rst is
loving, which involves open communication, demonstration of affection, mutual respect, forgiveness, and interpersonal sensitivity. The
second is loyalty, which involves strong commitment to the marriage,
a high degree of sexual activity, and strong moral values in the
marital relationship. The nal characteristic is shared values,
which emphasizes high priorities placed on religiosity and childrearing, traditional gender roles, and management of conicts (RosenGrandon, 1998).
The Asian Indian couples in Madathil and Benshoffs study scored
signicantly higher than their American counterparts in relation to the
perceived importance of all three core marital characteristics across
both genders. More specically, the Asian Indian couples residing in
the United States most rmly endorsed all three dimensions as
extremely important to them. The most pronounced difference among
these core marital characteristics among the three groups was in
relation to shared values. American couples in marriages of their own
initiation rated shared values as substantially less important to them
than they were for Asian Indians in arranged marriages.
A comparison of total levels of marital satisfaction and satisfaction
in relation to each of the three core marital characteristics across
the three groups using factorial analysis of variance revealed some
very interesting results. First, there was no signicant difference in the
overall level of marital satisfaction between Asian Indians in arranged
marriages who were residing in India and Americans in self-initiated
marital relationships. An earlier study that also used the CHARISMA
scale, comparing marital satisfaction between Asian Indians in arranged
marriages living in India and Americans in self-initiated marriages,
obtained similar results (Myers, Madathil, & Tingle, 2005). Second, there
were no signicant gender differences in marital satisfaction. Third,
Asian Indians in arranged marriages who were living in the United
States had a signicantly higher level of overall marital satisfaction than
the other two groups. This nding occurred across almost every one of
the three core characteristics of marriage, which they had endorsed most
highly. The researchers concluded that the match between expectations
of marriage and the reality of arranged marriages can likely account for
the high satisfaction rates among this group. They specically emphasized how U.S. living may allow Asian Indian couples some independence from their families and freedom to acculturate, enabling them to
mold their arranged marriages in a way that meets their ideals
and maximizes personal satisfaction (Madathil & Benshoff, 2008).
Madathil and Benshoff emphasized that their results support the idea
that arranged marriages are a viable form of establishing a satisfying
family life.

116

Feminism as Human Rights

Spousal Abuse
Besides examining possible variance in positive outcomes of married
life in the form of marital satisfaction across arranged and self-initiated
marriages, researchers have investigated negative interpersonal outcomes, specically spousal maltreatment. In their extensive review of
existing research, Raj and Silverman (2002) report that there is no difference in domestic violence rates across arranged and self-initiated
marriages that occur in the same locality. However, international
arranged marriages have been found to increase womens risk for various forms of spousal abuse, including physical battering, sexual abuse,
and emotional abuse. Unique forms of immigration abuse have also
been reported among women who have had international arranged
marriages, such as threats of deportation and husbands imposing barriers to womens English language learning and cultural integration
(Bui & Morash, 1999; Chin, 1994; Cote, Kerisit, & Cote, 2001; Dasgupta,
2000; Raj & Silverman, 2002; Shirwadkar, 2004).
International arranged marriages most often occur when families
who have immigrated to Western countries collaborate with relatives
from abroad to identify wives for their sons from their home countries.
The desire of many immigrants to retain their cultural heritage
and ensure a traditional family life leads to the application of arranged
marriages across national borders. Women of the same cultural heritage who are already living in Western nations may be seen as acculturated rather than traditional (Raj and Silverman, 2002). In international
arranged marriages, the marriage usually takes place in the familys
country of origin. Subsequently, the male returns to his country of
current permanent residence or citizenship and les a sponsorship
application on behalf of his new bride to facilitate her immigration
(Merali, 2008).
The escalation in womens risk of being subjected to spousal abuse
in international arranged marriages is attributed to shortcomings
in family immigration policies in Western nations (Dasgupta, 2000;
Merali, 2008; Raj & Silverman, 2002). Since most female marriage
migrants are sponsored by husbands living in North America, the
spousal immigration policies of Canada and the United States will
be presented here as case examples. To be eligible to sponsor a wife,
the Canadian or American spouse must provide documentation supporting his citizenship or status as a permanent resident. He must also
provide nancial statements, such as copies of bank account balances
and paychecks, to demonstrate his ability to nancially support his
wife for a period of three years after her arrival. Financial capability to
sponsor is based on having an income and savings level that would
enable the couple to live signicantly above the national poverty line.
The husband signs a contract with the national government taking full

Arranged and Forced Marriage

117

responsibility for the sponsored brides basic needs (including food,


shelter, health, and dental care) and integration needs (training in
English/French as a second language, social programs, etc.) for this
three-year period (Citizenship and Immigration Canada, 2002; U.S.
Citizenship and Immigration Services, 2007).
Once the application is approved and the foreign wife receives a
visa number, she is able to immigrate to North America to be with her
husband. In the rst three years after her arrival, the wife has no independent access to resources and is not eligible to receive any social security benets. Unlike immigrants who come to North America
independently, she is not connected to any immigration agencies or
social supports apart from her husband (Citizenship and Immigration
Canada, 2002; U.S. Citizenship and Immigration Services, 2007). It is
expected that with the husbands support, over time, she will develop
the skills to support herself and to integrate; she will learn English or
French, seek employment, socially integrate with others, and adapt to
North American ways. In Canada, once a sponsorship application is
approved, the woman is granted the opportunity to become a permanent resident of Canada upon her own initiation of a permanent
residence application (Citizenship and Immigration Canada, 2002).
However, in the United States, the woman can only gain permanent
residence status if her husband les an application on her behalf during the rst two years after her arrival. If the husband does not le this
application, she may be subject to deportation (U.S. Citizenship and
Immigration Services, 2007).
The immigration policies reviewed enforce womens economic and
social dependence on their husbands and husbands exclusive control
of all resources that women may need to access. Existing international
and cross-cultural research has implicated economic dependence and
exclusive male control of resources in the etiology of gender-based violence. These conditions amplify any existing gender inequality in the
marital relationship (Heise, 1998; Michalski, 2004). Social isolation of
women has also been found to be a contributing factor for spousal maltreatment, especially when combined with other stressors in family
relationships, such as marital conict and nancial strain (Heise, 1998;
Michalski, 2004; Sokoloff & Dupont, 2005). A failure to connect female
marriage migrants to immigration and settlement agencies for even a
single visit prevents them from having any safety net in the event of
experiencing marital or integration challenges. As their sponsors, their
husbands would also be aware that the women do not have any connections to social resources in the host society. Furthermore, a signicant proportion of male immigrants in North America experience
unemployment and underemployment due to a lack of recognition of
their foreign educational credentials. Therefore, although they can
demonstrate nancial capability to provide for their new brides, they

118

Feminism as Human Rights

may still experience nancial hardship. This may lead them to perceive
their responsibility for the wife as an added nancial burden (Raj &
Silverman, 2002). For example, some sponsored women in Meralis
(2009) study reported that their husbands were providing them with
only a single meal a day or only buying them spring coats despite
extreme winter weather.
The fact that many brides entering North America through international arranged marriages are not procient in English exacerbates their
vulnerability to spousal maltreatment. The nature of their immigration
status is often communicated to them only through their husbands or
in-laws, leaving room for miscommunication of their status in the host
society. For example, women who do not know English are often not
aware that Canadian immigration policy grants them permanent resident status upon their own initiation of the application process. Existing studies have uncovered their tendency to helplessly resign to
threats of deportation made by their husbands (Cote et al., 2001;
Merali, 2009; Shirwadkar, 2004).
Given the aforementioned shortcomings of family immigration policies in North America, some women who immigrate for international
arranged marriages have become subject to violations of four basic
human rights categories, as discussed by Merali (2009). Womens right
to personal security may be compromised through physical battering.
Womens economic right to material subsistence may be violated by
sponsors failure to provide adequate food and clothing, in direct
breach of the sponsorship agreement with the government of Canada
or United States. Violation of womens right to cultural participation
may occur as a result of sponsor-imposed barriers to learning English
as a second language. Finally, a violation of womens right to freedom
of association may occur when sponsors prohibit their interaction with
other members of the host society (Merali, 2009). One or more of these
violations have been reported across a number of studies of international marriages (Bui & Morash, 1999; Chin, 1994; Cote et al., 2001;
Dasgupta, 2000; Raj & Silverman, 2002; Shirwadkar, 2004).

PERSONAL OUTCOMES
Well-being
Similar to research on interpersonal outcomes of arranged marriages,
studies on personal outcomes have addressed both positive and negative consequences. Myers et al. (2005) are the only researchers to examine positive outcomes to date, and are also the only researchers to have
conducted a study on personal outcomes of arranged marriages that
has included a control group. In their study of couples in arranged
marriages living in India and American couples in self-initiated

Arranged and Forced Marriage

119

marriages, Myers et al. compared perceived levels of personal wellness


among the couples. They utilized the Wellness Evaluation of Lifestyle
survey (WEL; Myers, Sweeney, Hattie, & Witmer, 1998). This 120-item
self-report inventory addresses multiple components of personal wellbeing, including sense of self-worth, sense of control, emotional awareness, physical health care (i.e., nutrition and exercise), problem-solving
ability, feeling loved, and strength of spirituality and cultural identity.
It therefore considers positive functioning as a state of emotional, physical, spiritual, and cultural well-being.
Myers et al. did not nd any difference in total wellness scores on
this measure between couples in arranged versus self-initiated marriages. Similarly, in examining subscale scores, there were no signicant differences in the perceived self-worth of couples from the
two groups, or in the areas of management of emotions, level of feeling loved, and problem-solving ability. There were some differences
between the subscale scores of the couples from each group in the
areas of spirituality, cultural identity, and nutrition, where the couples
in arranged marriages scored higher than their counterparts. In contrast, in the areas of feeling a sense of control over their lives, couples
in marriages of their own initiation had higher mean scores. These
ndings make sense given the individualism of American society versus the family orientation of Indian society.
Mental Health Problems and Self-harm
The remainder of studies on personal outcomes of arranged marriage has focused on negative outcomes, such as mental health problems, self-harm, and criminality. The preponderance of studies on
negative outcomes likely reects Western cultural biases against
arranged marriage practices. It is also important to note that the studies
that will be reviewed in the following sections are either descriptive
studies or post-de-facto investigations identifying causes of mental
health problems, suicide attempts, or criminality among samples
experiencing these outcomes. None of the studies have utilized quasiexperimental designs.
Langen et al. (1997) described the phenomenon of Honeymoon
Psychosis (p. 171) among Japanese couples in arranged marriages
vacationing in Hawaii. Their research was instigated by a disproportionately large number of Japanese individuals coming to the attention
of the hospital emergency department in Honolulu, compared to their
representation among visitors to Hawaii. The cases tended to involve
severe psychological distress and delusions and hallucinations. It
was determined through the assessment process that many of the
Japanese patients coming through the hospital with psychosis were on
their honeymoons in Hawaii. Langen et al. (1997) examined the

120

Feminism as Human Rights

hospital emergency and treatment records of 16 Japanese patients diagnosed with psychosis while on their honeymoons. They also interviewed the psychiatrists who had worked with these patients. The
study was conducted over a two-year period, and the hospital record
data was obtained after the patients treatment and discharge from the
Honolulu hospital.
The majority of the Japanese honeymooner patients had arranged
marriages and had little or no premarital relationship experience. Their
case records and psychiatrist interviews attested to them being overwhelmed with the marriage process and with their rst sexual experiences. The sample included both men and women, with the mean age
of men being 30 and the mean age of women being 27. The most important fact to note about this study is that 10 of the 16 patients had
some level of pre-existing mental health problems, such as depression
or anxiety, although many had concealed them from their spouse and/
or family. Therefore, it appears that rather than being precipitated by
arranged marriages per se, honeymoon psychosis may simply be an
extreme reaction to the stressful life event of marriage among individuals with pre-existing emotional vulnerabilities (Langen et al., 1997).
Self-harm is a common outcome of the occurrence of severe emotional distress. Two studies have identied arranged marriages as a
contributing factor in attempts to harm oneself and completed suicides.
Konradsen, van der Hock, and Peiris (2006) studied the reasons for
self-inicted pesticide poisoning among 159 male and female patients
who were hospitalized for these self-harm attempts in Sri Lanka within
a single year. Konradsen et al. noted that the most common form of
suicide attempts in Sri Lanka is through ingestion of pesticides, since
the agricultural economy of the country has led to pesticides being
readily available in local corner stores. The researchers interviewed the
patients directly after their discharge from the hospital and/or interviewed family members through home visits. Their results indicated
that 22 percent of the self-inicted poisonings could be attributed to
marital problems, with forced marriages being one of the major marital
problems identied. Their study therefore links coercion in marriage
arrangement to an adverse personal outcome, rather than arranged
marriage without coercion.
Vijayakumar and Thilothammal (1993) analyzed police records of
suicide pacts that were part of investigations related to the self-harm
attempts and completed deaths of 324 males and females in India during the ve-year period from 1982 to 1987. Marriage related issues,
including arranged marriages and dowry problems and demands, were
among the most frequently occurring reasons for suicide attempts
stated in the suicide pacts. The previously discussed statistics on homicides of new brides in arranged marriages involving dowries in India
suggested that the pairing of arranged marriages with a bride price

Arranged and Forced Marriage

121

increases the likelihood that women will be maltreated. Vijayakumar


and Thilothammals ndings suggest that the phenomenon of bride
price in arranged marriages may also jeopardize womens livelihood
by contributing to their likelihood of self-harm when they are unable
to cope with dowry demands or maltreatment by others.
Criminality
Another negative personal outcome of arranged marriages that has
been studied after the fact is criminality. Tariq and Tariq (1993) interviewed female convicts who were interned in prisons across four provinces in Pakistan. The convicts ages ranged from 15 to 51, with
the majority of them falling in the 28 to 35 age group. Sixty percent
were convicted for murder (42 of the study participants). The majority
of the participants who had committed murder killed their husbands
or in-laws. Tariq and Tariq conducted a content analysis of the characteristics of their marriages based on their interview data to identify the
contributing factors for the marital problems that led to their criminality. Thirty-four percent of the female convicts had forced marriages,
and 49 percent had marriages that occurred in childhood or adolescence. In 64 percent of their marriages, there was a sharp disparity
between their ages at the time of marriage and the ages of the husbands they were married to, with the men being signicantly older.
Eighty-eight percent of the women also reported problems with their
in-laws in the form of maltreatment. The results of this study highlight
the types of marriage characteristics that may pose signicant challenges to womens coping ability. However, outbreaks of violence by
women may represent acts of self-defense when they have no other
options for escaping their plight. In reality, women are much more
likely to be killed by others than to kill others (Rastogi & Therly, 2006).
Rastogi and Therlys work on dowry-related deaths in arranged marriages discussed in the previous section on laws illustrates this fact.
IMPLICATIONS FOR LAW, POLICY, AND HUMAN RIGHTS
ADVOCACY
Although arranged marriages have often been considered to represent an atypical and inappropriate way of establishing family life from
the Western perspective, the literature reviewed in this chapter suggests that they have been practiced across multiple countries, cultures,
and religious groups (Bhopal, 1999; Blood, 1967; Cattell, 1992; Ghimire
et al., 2006; Kurian, 1991; Langen et al., 1997; Madathil & Benshoff,
2008; Oprea, 2005; Rockman, 1994; Xiaohe & Whyte, 1990; Wisensale,
1999; Zaidi & Shuraydi, 2002; Zang, 2008). The diversity in arranged
marriage practices likely reects the plurality among the groups

122

Feminism as Human Rights

utilizing this family formation strategy. When marriages are arranged


through third-party initiative rather than personal initiative, it may be
assumed that there is no personal choice or consent in the process.
However, each of the subtypes of arranged marriage may occur without any coercion and full consent of the partners, or through the application of various forms of emotional and physical pressure. When such
pressure results in signicant duress on the part of either of the prospective marriage partners, the arranged marriage becomes a forced
marriage from a legal standpoint (Dostrovsky et al., 2007).
The research reviewed in this chapter suggests that multiple variables may affect individuals desire for and likelihood of entering an
arranged marriage, such as exposure to Western media, level of education, employment status, and non-family living arrangements (Ghirmire
et al., 2006). Gender and acculturation have also been found to relate to
the likelihood of third-party intervention in marriage and personal marriage preferences (Zang, 2008; Zaidi & Shuraydi, 2002). Even when other
sociodemographic variables are controlled, women from cultures that
view females as the bearers of family honor are more likely to have their
marriages arranged than men (Zang, 2008).
An examination of the outcomes of arranged marriages occurring
within a particular nation suggests that when there is some level of
choice in the marriage process, they may lead to the development of
relationships that are as satisfying or more satisfying than self-initiated
marital relations (Madathil & Benshoff, 2008). This may be especially
the case when the individuals joined together in marriage have similar
expectations of the marriage and shared cultural values (Madathil &
Benshoff, 2008). In contrast, forced marriages have been found to yield
lower marital satisfaction than marriages formed through personal initiative and personal choice, particularly among women (Xiaohe &
Whyte, 1990). Research on personal outcomes of arranged marriages
occurring within a specic nation identied a similar dichotomy in outcomes based on whether marriages are volitional or forced. No difference in overall levels of well-being among individuals in these
marriages and personally-initiated unions has been found when some
choice has been exercised (Myers et al., 2005). The exception occurs in
cases where individuals have pre-existing mental health problems. The
stress of adjusting to married life with an unfamiliar person may precipitate additional mental health challenges in those with pre-existing vulnerabilities, even in the absence of force (Langen et al., 1997). When arranged
marriages are forced and occur in childhood or adolescence to older
males, they have been found to be associated with negative outcomes, such
as self-harm/suicide attempts (Konradsen et al., 2006; Vijayakumar &
Thilothammal, 1993) and criminality (Tariq & Tariq, 1993).
Laws related to arranged marriage have covered a wide range of
issues, such as third-party intervention, the minimum age of marriage,

Arranged and Forced Marriage

123

family living arrangements, the negotiation of a bride price, and gender


equality. Laws that have attempted to abolish arranged marriages altogether have been successful in dramatically reducing arranged marriage rates in nations where these laws were implemented (Wisensale,
1999; Xiaohe & Whyte, 1990; Zang, 2008). An examination of the rationale for the introduction of such laws suggests a misunderstanding of
arranged marriages. The laws seem to be based on the assumption that
all arranged marriages are forced marriages and ignore diversity
among arranged marriage practices. Taking into account the research
on arranged marriage outcomes, such laws are misguided. More
importantly, in attempting to protect individual human rights related
to freedom of association and autonomous decision-making, such laws
may contravene essential cultural rights and freedoms, such as freedom
of belief, expression, religion, and cultural tradition that are embedded
in the Universal Declaration of Human Rights (United Nations, 1948).
These culturally based rights and freedoms are also addressed to varying degrees in national policies related to diversity and pluralism
(Levesque, 2001). However, this category of rights and freedoms
may have received limited attention and respect among some of the
political regimes operating when laws abolishing arranged marriages
were introduced. In international law, cultural rights and freedoms
can only be legitimately challenged when they violate other basic
human rights (Levesque, 2001). In the case of noncoerced arranged
marriages within a specic nation, the research evidence suggests
positive personal and interpersonal outcomes. It does not provide any
evidence of higher rates of human rights violations in the form of
domestic violence.
In contrast to laws aiming to abolish arranged marriage altogether,
those attempting to criminalize forced marriage and to raise the minimum age of marriage are more well informed. Laws focusing on forced
marriage sanctions can serve to protect the human right to freedom of
a marriage partner and also reduce the likelihood of negative personal
and interpersonal outcomes of arranged marriage processes. Laws
increasing age at the time of marriage can increase childrens likelihood
of having access to educational, developmental, and social opportunities that they are accorded in the United Nations Convention on the
Rights of the Child (United Nations, 1990). Despite the fact that laws
banning the negotiation or exchange of a bride price in arranged marriages also appear well informed, the introduction of such a law in
India has led the practice to occur implicitly rather than explicitly. This
change has made it harder for law enforcers to detect dowry processes
until after serious violence or dowry-related deaths have occurred.
Therefore, there has been a consistent and startling rise in dowryrelated deaths since the introduction of this law in 1961 (Rastogi &
Therly, 2006).

124

Feminism as Human Rights

Rastogi and Therly postulate the need to initiate public education


for male and female members of communities practicing the exchange
and negotiation of bride price to inform them of the risks to the welfare of daughters, sisters, and wives in the community as a whole.
They also emphasize the need for community outreach efforts to help
concerned family members, neighbors, and friends recognize signs of
maltreatment and violence and learn how to intervene. It is surmised
that community engagement can contribute to pro-active rather
than reactive law enforcement. It is important to note that although
legislative actions and community engagement can help guard against
negative outcomes, such steps cannot guarantee gender equality in
communities where substantial inequality has existed in family relationships (Wisensale, 1999).
The conclusions reached so far focus on arranged marriages performed within a specic nation or locality. A review of existing studies
suggested that when arranged marriages occur across international borders, womens risk of being subjected to multiple forms of human
rights violations is heightened (Raj & Silverman, 2002). These violations
include various forms of abuse and neglect, as well as barriers to their
social and cultural integration in the surrounding society. This elevation in risk is attributed to family immigration policies in immigrantreceiving nations, which tend to enforce new brides economic, social,
and cultural dependence on their husbands and promote their alienation (Raj & Silverman, 2002).
Raj and Silverman (2002) and Merali (2009) outline a number of
changes that are needed to family sponsorship policies to reduce womens vulnerability to maltreatment. One of these steps is translating immigration documents into their rst languages to ensure that female
marriage migrants have direct access to information that relates to their
status as sponsored persons in the host country. This would eliminate
the need for non-English-procient women to rely on their husbands
or in-laws for translation assistance, leaving no room for miscommunication. First-language education about human rights and their protection in the country they are entering would also empower the women
to recognize and respond to any violations. Rights-based education
could occur at the foreign embassies in their countries of origin prior
to migration as a proactive step. Connecting women to immigration
and settlement agencies for at least a single visit to ensure they have a
safety net in the event of encountering abuse or sponsor-imposed
barriers to cultural or social integration is also recommended (Raj &
Silverman, 2002; Merali, 2008). To prevent neglect of womens basic
human right to material subsistence, family immigration policies may
further consider the creation of a spousal allowance for sponsored
brides that is based on the same proportion of their sponsors incomes
that governments expect that sponsors will devote to their new brides

Arranged and Forced Marriage

125

(Merali, 2008). It is only if arranged marriages are understood in light


of their intentions, diverse forms, actual outcomes, and local or international contexts that laws, policies, and human rights advocacy can be
appropriately channeled to protect and preserve womens
well-being.
REFERENCES
Bachrach, C., Hindin, M., & Thomson, E. (2000). The changing shape of ties
that bind: An overview and synthesis. In L. J. Waite, C. Bachrach, M.
Hindin, E. Thomson, & A. Thornton (Eds.), The ties that bind: Perspectives
on marriage and cohabitation (pp. 316). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine De Gruyter.
Berry, J. W. (2006). Contexts of acculturation. In D. L. Sam & J. W. Berry (Eds.),
The Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology (pp. 2742). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Bhopal, K. (1999). South Asian women and arranged marriages in East London.
In R. Barot, H. Bradley, & S. Fenton (Eds.), Ethnicity, gender, and social
change (pp. 117134). New York: St. Martins Press.
Blood, R. O. Jr. (1967). Love match and arranged marriage. New York: Free Press.
Bui, H. N., & Morash, M. (1999). Domestic violence in the Vietnamese immigrant community: An exploratory study. Violence Against Women, 5,
769795.
Cattell, M. G. (1992). Praise the lord and say no to men: Older women empowering themselves in Samia, Kenya. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 7,
307330.
Chin, K. (1994). Out of town brides: International marriages and wife abuse
among Chinese immigrants. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 25, 5369.
Citizenship and Immigration Canada. (2002). Immigration and Refugee Protection Act. Canada Gazette: Part II, Vol. 136 (No. 9).
Cook, R. J., & Cusack, S. (2005). Womens rights. In R. K. M. Smith & C.
Van den Anker (Eds.), The essentials of human rights (pp. 366369). London:
Hodder Arnold.
Cote, A., Kerisit, M., & Cote, M. L. (2001). Sponsorship for better or for worse: The
impact of sponsorship on the equality rights of immigrant women. Ottawa:
Status of Women Canada.
Dasgupta, S. D. (2000). Charting the course: An overview of domestic violence
in the South Asian community in the United States. Journal of Social Distress and the Homeless, 9, 173185.
Dion, K. K., & Dion, K. L. (2001). Gender and cultural adaptation in immigrant
families. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 511522.
Dostrovsky, N., Cook, R., & Gagnon, M. (2007). Annotated bibliography on comparative and international law relating to forced marriage. Ottawa: Department
of Justice Canada.
Eide, A. (2005). Social rights. In R. K. M. Smith & C. Van den Anker (Eds.), The
essentials of human rights (pp. 324326). London: Hodder Arnold.
Ghirmire, D. J., Axinn, W. G., Yaiku, S. T., & Thornton, A. (2006). Social
change, premarital nonfamily experience, and spouse choice in an
arranged marriage society. American Journal of Sociology, 111, 11811218.

126

Feminism as Human Rights

Gilbert, J. (2005a). The right to self-determination. In R. K. M. Smith & C. Van


den Anker (Eds.), The essentials of human rights (pp. 317319). London:
Hodder Arnold.
Gilbert, J. (2005b). Cultural rights. In R. K. M. Smith & C. Van den Anker
(Eds.), The essentials of human rights (pp. 7679). London: Hodder Arnold.
Guichon, A. (2005). Violence against women: Domestic violence. In
R. K. M. Smith & C. Van den Anker (Eds.), The essentials of human rights
(pp. 358363). London: Hodder Arnold.
Heise, L. L. (1998). Violence against women: An integrated ecological framework. Violence Against Women, 4, 262290.
Hellsten, S. K. (2005). The right to health. In R. K. M. Smith & C. Van den
Anker (Eds.), The essentials of human rights (pp. 148151). London:
Hodder Arnold.
Konradsen, F., van der Hock, W., & Peiris, P. (2006). Reaching for the bottle of
pesticide: A cry for help. Self-inicted poisonings in Sri Lanka. Social Science & Medicine, 62, 17101719.
Kurian, G. (1991). South Asians in Canada. International Migration, XXIX,
421432.
Langen, D., Streltzer, J., & Kai, M. (1997). Honeymoon psychosis in Japanese
tourists to Hawaii. Cultural Diversity and Mental Health, 3, 171174.
Levesque, R. J. R. (2001). Culture and family violence: Fostering change through
human rights law. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
MacPherson, S. (2004). Human rights in the family: An educational handbook.
Edmonton: Indo-Canadian Womens Association.
Madathil, J., & Benshoff, J. M. (2008). Importance of marital characteristics and
marital satisfaction: A comparison of Asian Indians in arranged marriages
and Americans in marriages of choice. The Family Journal: Counseling and
Therapy for Couples and Families, 16, 222230.
McBride, J. (2005). Freedom of association. In R. K. M. Smith & C. Van den
Anker (Eds.), The essentials of human rights (pp. 1820). London:
Hodder Arnold.
Merali, N. (2008). Theoretical frameworks for studying female marriage
migrants. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 281289.
Merali, N. (2009). Experiences of South Asian brides entering Canada after
recent changes to family sponsorship policies. Violence Against Women, 15,
321339.
Michalski, J. H. (2004). Making sociological sense out of trends in intimate partner violence: The social structure of violence against women. Violence
Against Women, 10, 652675.
Myers, J. E., Madathil, J., & Tingle, L. R. (2005). Marriage satisfaction and wellness in India and the United States: A preliminary comparison of arranged
marriages and marriages of choice. Journal of Counseling & Development, 83,
183190.
Myers, J. E., Sweeney, T. J., Hattie, J. A., & Witmer, J. M. (1998). The Wellness
Evaluation of Lifestyle. Palo Alto, CA: MindGarden.
Oprea, A. (2005). The arranged marriage of Ana Maria Cioaba, intra-community oppression and Romani feminist ideals: Transcending the primitive
culture argument. European Journal of Womens Studies, 12, 133148.

Arranged and Forced Marriage

127

Potash, B. (1986). Widows in African societies: Choices and constraints. Stanford,


CA: Stanford University Press.
Probert, R. (2005). The right to marriage and a family. In R. K. M. Smith &
C. Van den Anker (Eds.), The essentials of human rights (pp. 244247).
London: Hodder Arnold.
Qureshi, R. B. (1991). Marriage strategies among Muslims from South Asia. The
American Journal of Islamic Social Sciences, 10, 1014.
Raj, A., & Silverman, J. (2002). Violence against immigrant women: The roles of
culture, context, and legal immigrant status on intimate partner violence.
Violence Against Women, 8, 367398.
Rastogi, M., & Therly, P. (2006). Dowry and its link to violence against women
in India: Feminist psychological perspectives. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 7,
6677.
Rockman, H. (1994). Matchmaker matchmaker make me a match: The art and
conventions of Jewish arranged marriages. Sexual and Relationship Therapy,
9, 277284.
Rosen-Grandon, J. R. (1998). Charisma: The characteristics of marriage inventory.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of North Carolina at
Greensboro.
Shirwadkar, S. (2004). Canadian domestic violence policy and Indian immigrant women. Violence Against Women, 10, 860879.
Sokoloff, N. J., & Dupont, I. (2005). Domestic violence at the intersections of
race, class, and gender. Violence Against Women, 11, 3864.
Stopes-Roe, M., & Cochrane, R. (1990). Citizens of this country: The Asian British.
Clevedon, United Kingdom: Multilingual Matters Ltd.
Tariq, P. N., & Tariq, A. (1993). Marital maladjustment and the crime of murder among Pakistani female criminals. International Journal of Psychology,
28, 809819.
Texier, P. (2005). Economic rights. In R. K. M. Smith & C. Van den Anker
(Eds.), The essentials of human rights (pp. 100103). London: Hodder Arnold.
United Nations. (1948). Universal Declaration of Human Rights. Retrieved
April 5, 2007, from: http://www.unhchr.ch/udhr/lang/eng.htm
United Nations. (1990). Convention on the Rights of the Child. Retrieved
January 10, 2009, from: http://www.unhchr.ch//html/menu3/b/k2crc.
htm
U.S. Citizenship and Immigration Services. (2007). Sponsoring a relative to
come to the United States. Retrieved June 26, 2007, from http://www.uscis.gov/portal/site/uscis
U.S. Immigration Support Organization. (2007). Getting a green card for your
wife or husband. Retrieved June 26, 2007, from http://www.usimmigrationsupport.org/greencard_marriage.html
Vijayakumar, L., & Thilothammal, N. (1993). Suicide pacts. Crisis: The Journal of
Crisis Intervention and Suicide Prevention, 14, 4346.
Werbner, P. (2007). Veiled interventions in pure space: Honour, shame, and
embodied struggles among Muslims in Britain and France. Theory, Culture, &
Society, 24, 161185.
Wilson, A. (2007). The forced marriage debate and the British state. Race &
Class, 49, 2538.

128

Feminism as Human Rights

Wilson, D. (2005). The right to education. In R. K. M. Smith & C. Van den


Anker (Eds.), The essentials of human rights (pp. 105108). London: Hodder
Arnold.
Wisensale, S. K. (1999). Marriage and family law in a changing Vietnam. Journal of Family Issues, 20, 602616.
Xiaohe, X., & Whyte, M. K. (1990). Love matches and arranged marriages: A
Chinese replication. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 709722.
Zaidi, A. U., and Shuraydi, M. (2002). Perceptions of arranged marriages by
Pakistani Muslim women living in a Western society. Journal of Comparative
Family Studies, 33, 495514.
Zang, X. (2008). Gender and ethnic variation in arranged marriages in a
Chinese city. Journal of Family Issues, 29, 615638.

Chapter 6

Womens Reproductive Rights:


An International Perspective
Joan C. Chrisler
Cynthia Garrett

One woman somewhere in the world dies every minute of every day
from pregnancy- and childbirth-related complications. The United
Nations Fund for Population Activities (UNFPA) has estimated that
3 million girls worldwide are at risk of female genital cutting annually
(Wakabi, 2007). Each year, tens of thousands of women die, more than
5 million are admitted to a hospital, and a considerable number
become infertile as a result of unsafe abortions; 200 million women still
have an unmet need for family planning and contraceptives of any
kind (Hindin, 2007). Only 16 of 68 countries identied by the United
Nations (UN) are on target to meet the Millennium Development Goals
for reducing rates of maternal and child deaths by 2015 (Countdown
Coverage Writing Group, 2008).
Acceptance of womens reproductive rights as basic human rights
has grown in recent years. In 1994, at the International Conference on
Population and Development (ICPD) in Cairo, ofcial international recognition of reproductive rights as human rights began as representatives of more than 180 nations agreed on basic protections for womens
ability to control their reproductive lives. The UN and other regional
bodies have urged governments around the world to safeguard reproductive rights for all women (Center for Reproductive Rights, n.d.).
These are promising signs, yet, as evidenced by the facts expressed
above, we have a long way to go to guarantee reproductive justice for
the worlds women, as reproductive rights are not universally maintained around the globe.

130

Feminism as Human Rights

What do we mean by the term reproductive rights? According to


the World Health Organization, reproductive rights are the basic rights
of all couples and individuals to decide freely and responsibly the number, spacing, and timing of their children and to have the information
and means to do so, and the right to attain the highest standard of sexual and reproductive health. They also include the right of all to make
decisions concerning reproduction free of discrimination, coercion, and
violence (ICPD Programme of Action, 1994, para 7.3). Reproductive
justice is a more recent term that is used by some feminist activists to
underscore the fact that, although in some countries (e.g., the United
States) reproductive rights (e.g., abortion) are legal, many women are
not able to access the services they need to exercise those rights (e.g.,
there may be no services available where they live, those services are
not covered by health insurance). If women are not able to exercise
their rights, it does them little good to know that the government guarantees their right to make their own choices. Rights resources
accessibility justice.
POLITICS AND CULTURE INFLUENCE
REPRODUCTIVE JUSTICE
The factors that inuence the degree to which reproductive rights
are upheld are broad and interwoven. The political context of a region
is one such factor. For example, during the Cold War modern contraceptive methods (manufactured in the West) were simply not available
in the Soviet Union (Barrett & Buckley, 2007). Abortion was the primary method of preventing births in the Soviet Socialist Republics until
the mid to late 1980s. Currently, in Uzbekistan, the IUD (intra-uterine
device) is the most prevalent contraceptive method; 73 percent of married Uzbeck women report having used this method (Barrett & Buckley,
2007). The results of Barrett and Buckleys study of contraceptive
patterns in Uzbekistan suggest that womens contraceptive choice is
constrained. One factor in the high utilization of IUDs may be the
pressure put on medical clinics by government agencies that link IUD
insertion quotas and clinic funding.
Historical events, such as civil wars and other armed conicts, can
severely restrict assess to reproductive health care, especially for political refugees. In one study (McGinn, Casey, Purdin, & Marsh, 2004), the
researchers found that, during intense phases of armed conict, rates
of unsafe abortion and maternal mortality rise. The crisis management
mentality of refugee camp personnel can also restrict reproductive
choices. The risk of gender-based violence is always present during
armed conicts, as is the threat of becoming widowed and being forced
to trade sex to meet basic needs (Petchesky, 2008). In recent wars
around the globe (e.g., Bosnia, Darfur, Democratic Republic of the

Womens Reproductive Rights: An International Perspective

131

Congo) rapes have been widespread as a method of terrifying people


and diluting ethnic solidarity. Thus, in times when women might be
most in need of reproductive health care, its accessibility is especially
likely to be curtailed.
Palestinian refugees, for example, have struggled for access to reproductive health services since the beginning of the second Intifada in
September 2000. According to interviews with health care providers
(Bosmans, Nasser, Khammash, Claeys, & Temmerman, 2008), women
have requested that physicians induce labor because they are afraid of
being trapped at home by curfews when their contractions begin. There
are reports of births, stillbirths, and even women dying during labor
at military checkpoints. Although they live in an environment with
unsafe access to prenatal and other reproductive health care, Palestinian women are under intense social pressure to bear more children as
their contribution to the Intifada (Bosmans et al., 2008).
On the other hand, some populations have increased access to
reproductive health care due to political circumstances. In Israel, for
example, women have state-funded access to assisted reproductive technologies including in vitro fertilization (IVF) (Birenbaum-Carmeli &
Dimfeld, 2008). These technologies typically are offered free of charge
to any Israeli woman up to the age of 45. She is eligible for such
services up to the age of 51 if she uses donor eggs. Marital status and
sexual orientation are not barriers to these services. State funding for
such technologies is limited to two children with the womans present
partner, but there have been exceptions to this rule. In contrast, contraceptives and abortion are only partially covered by the Israeli public
health system (Birenbaum-Carmeli & Dirnfeld, 2008). Israels pronatal
stance has been linked to the Israeli-Palestinian conict as a component
of nation building. Israeli leaders have expressed fear that Jews will
become a minority population, a situation commonly referred to as the
demographic time bomb. Civil servants keep a close watch on demographic projections.
Beyond the impact of governments at the national level, policies in
one country can have a ripple effect on access to reproductive options
in other parts of the world. For example, in 1984 U.S. President Ronald
Reagans administration established a policy known as the Global
Gag Rule, which prevents performing abortions, providing counseling
about reproductive options, referring patients to services, or even lobbying for abortion in developing nations that receive funds for health
clinics from the United States (Sullivan, 2009). This particular policy
has been a hotbed of controversy throughout the years. It was repealed
during the Clinton administration, reinstated by the second Bush
administration, and then repealed again by President Obama (Sullivan,
2009). The Global Gag Rule has had far-reaching implications for at
least 20 developing countries. It has led to the disruption of delivery of

132

Feminism as Human Rights

contraceptives overseas, left many women with only unsafe options to


abort, and increased instances of maternal mortality (Kort, 2008). This
cruel policy interferes with the practice of medicine, health education,
and counseling, and its cycle of repeal and reinstatement makes it very
difcult for public health authorities in developing nations to plan and
nance reproductive health services for their citizens.
On a more positive note, international human rights bodies are
attempting to persuade governments to change their laws to ease abortion restrictions (Boland & Katzive, 2008). In 2003, for example, the
African Union adopted the Protocol to the African Charter on Human
and Peoples Rights on the Rights of Women in Africa. In brief, this
protocol states that all appropriate measures to protect reproductive
rights must be taken and that medical abortion must be allowed in case
of rape, incest, or threats to a womans physical or mental health. As
of 2007, 21 countries had ratied this protocol (Boland & Katzive,
2008). The Convention on the Elimination of All Forms of Discrimination against Women (CEDAW) was adopted by the UN General Assembly in 1979. It afrms womens reproductive rights as necessary for
womens equality in political, economic, social, cultural, and civil arenas. As we write this, the only UN member nations that have not ratied CEDAW are Iran, Nauru, Palau, Qatar, Somalia, Sudan, Tonga,
and the United States. Furthermore, international courts of human
rights are now holding governments accountable for their violations of
womens reproductive rights. K. L. v. Peru, Paulina Ramirez v. Mexico,
and Tysiac v. Poland1 are examples of cases that have been won in the
interest of reproductive justice (Center for Reproductive Rights, n.d.).
Closely intertwined with historical events and politics is the inuence of culture on womens reproductive rights. The historical events
associated with the Israeli-Palestinian conict have served to create a
culture of pronatalism on both sides that can reduce a womans choice
regarding birth spacing and, consequently, might impact her health.
Until recently, therapeutic abortions were available in Nicaragua if
three physicians agreed that the procedure was necessary for the womans health, but this right, however limited, was eliminated for the sake
of politics. Daniel Ortega, the former Sandinista leader, decided to run
for the presidency of Nicaragua in 2006. His past as a guerilla leader
hurt his popularity, and he decided to court Catholic and Evangelical
Protestant leaders and their congregations to increase his political base.
He announced that he had found Jesus, began to practice his religion
with public devotion, and mentioned God frequently in his speeches
(Goldberg, 2009). After his election, he thanked the religious leaders
for their support by pushing to outlaw all abortions. The new policy
was tied to cultural values such as marianismo,2 and the idea that motherhood is womans highest calling. Maternal mortality doubled after
the new law was enacted; women died not only because they had

Womens Reproductive Rights: An International Perspective

133

sought unsafe abortions outside of hospitals and clinics, but because


the new law is so draconian that physicians are afraid to intervene,
even in medical situations where the womans life is in imminent danger, unless they can prove that the fetus is dead (Goldberg, 2009). For
example, women with ectopic pregnancies have died as doctors waited
for the fallopian tube to burst. Thus, Nicaraguan politics has killed
women and forced physicians to choose between malpractice and jail.
The opposite situation can also occur: Culture can impact politics.
Such is the case in Ghana where marital rape was removed from the
Domestic Violence Act passed on February 22, 2007, due to public
pressure. Public opposition to the inclusion of the protection against
marital rape stems from the belief in Ghanian custom that consent to
marriage is equivalent to consent to sex (Stafford, 2008). A similar case
occurred in early 2009 when the Afghan Parliament passed a bill that
gave the minority Shia the right to enforce Sharia Law, which includes
the right of men to demand sex from their wives except under a few
circumstances (e.g., advanced pregnancy, temporary illness); in effect,
it legalizes marital rape, as saying no is not recognized as a legal
reason to avoid sexual intercourse. After an international outcry, President Karzai announced that Ministry of Justice ofcials would study
the new law to be sure that it does not violate that national constitution, which established equal rights for women and men (Afghanistan
rape law, 2009).
Culture also plays a role in another form of violence against women.
Female genital cutting remains a widespread practice in Africa despite
the fact that 16 African countries have passed laws to ban it. UNFPA
has estimated that 120 to 140 million women have been subjected to
the procedure. For some African communities, the practice represents a
passage from girlhood to womanhood. Some also believe that genital
cutting will prevent girls from being unfaithful to their future husbands and preserve their chastity. UNFPA has reported that the most
successful initiatives to eliminate this practice involve providing alternate rites of passagean attempt to establish a new cultural tradition
(Wakabi, 2007).
Misunderstandings of culture can prevent outside support for womens reproductive rights from being effective. For example, reproductive rights activists typically assume that the body belongs to the
individual. In Nigeria, members of the Ngwa-Igbo and Ubang communities differ in their beliefs regarding body ownership. In Ngwa
Igbo culture, the body belongs to the entire Ngwa society (Izugbara &
Undie, 2008). This means that individuals have limited control over the
uses of their bodies. In the instance of rape, the offense is against
the larger community, not simply against the individual woman. The
Ubang peoples body notions involve a splitting of the ownership
rights of the married woman. In marriage, her underneath (which

134

Feminism as Human Rights

symbolize the sexual and reproductive organs that bring new life) are
given to her in-laws, whereas the rights to her head (which symbolizes her own life) are retained by her birth community (Izugbara &
Undie, 2008). Interventions built upon this belief will be less effective in
cultures where the body is not believed to be owned by the individual.
Chinas one child per family policy has its cultural roots in collectivism. The value of the nation at large is considered to be more important than the choices of individual citizens (Greenhalgh, 2001). This
policy, an effort to reverse Chinas population boom, has relaxed somewhat since its initial implementation. There are currently exceptions
where two children may be approved by the state. In addition, pilot
projects are underway to increase birth control options for women.
Women typically use an IUD after their rst child and are sterilized after their second (if they are approved for a second) (Hardee, Xie, &
Gu, 2004).
Some groups within society are marginalized, and their reproductive
rights limited, because of cultural beliefs. Examples of these groups
include single women, lesbians, disabled and chronically ill women,
poor women, and those who wish to exercise reproductive choice at an
age considered inappropriate by cultural norms. Members of marginalized groups who pursue reproductive options may face additional barriers as individuals.
In Australia, as of 2007, 70 percent of same-sex couples had conceived via informal self-insemination methods (Burstin, 2007).
Although the Australian government has lessened restrictions on fertility treatments to include single women and same-sex couples, public
funding for such expensive treatment is not allowed because it is
argued that such persons are not medically infertile (Nader, 2007). In
the United Kingdom, as of April 2009, support for reproductive freedom is better; lesbians and single heterosexual women can now name
almost any adult as a second parent, which effectively removed some
of the barriers to fertility treatment (Jones, 2009).
Older women may also face cultural backlash if they seek motherhood, as societies struggle with the question of individual womens
freedom versus the best interest of the child. Quotes from medical personnel capture these sentiments: Nature sends out a good message
that we reach a certain age when we are too old to have children
(Sawer, 2008, p. 6); Just because we can do something doesnt mean
we should (Schafer, 2009, p. A13). Medical ethics and cultural beliefs
tend to change slowly, and they have not caught up to the rapid pace
of the development of reproductive technologies. In Western countries,
postmenopausal women have successfully carried pregnancies to term
with the assistance of donor eggs and IVF procedures.
Persons with disabilities may face a host of limitations on their
reproductive rights due to cultural denial that these individuals have

Womens Reproductive Rights: An International Perspective

135

sexual desires and engage in sexual activities (Henderson, 2007). In


addition, women with cognitive impairments are at greater than average risk of sexual assault (Wacker, Parish, & Macy, 2008), which violates a basic form of human rights and increases their need for sensitive
reproductive health care. Women with developmental disabilities are
often thought to be incapable of raising children, yet adequate education regarding birth control may not be provided to them (Dotson,
Stinson, & Christian, 2003).
Religion is a powerful cultural factor that can inuence womens
reproductive rights. Members of each religion report a diverse range of
opinions on these matters, yet specic examples of the stances of religious leaders illustrate the ways in which religion directly impacts
women internationally. The Roman Catholic Church is a strong advocate of natural family planning and rejects any articial contraceptive
methods. As recently as March 2009, Pope Benedict XVI remarked that
condoms are not the answer to Africas AIDS problem and could make
the problem worse (Butt, 2009). His remarks clearly suggest that
women must limit their range of contraceptives and methods to protect
themselves from this life-threatening disease. In other moves, bishops
of three Roman Catholic dioceses in the United States in 2004
announced that they would deny the sacrament of communion to politicians who support reproductive rights (More Catholic bishops deny
communion, 2004), a direct move to inuence political action in a country that prides itself on the separation of church and state.
In the United States, health care expenses have led to downsizing
and combining of services; in some cases secular hospitals have
merged with Catholic hospitals. Catholic hospitals are governed by the
guidelines set forth in the Churchs Ethical and Religious Directives,
which prohibit certain medical procedures, such as abortion, tubal ligations, emergency contraception, vasectomies, and counseling discussions about condoms and fertility treatments (Gelb & Shogan, 2005). As
a result of these mergers, women have lost access to reproductive
options previously available.
In other countries, the separation of church and state is less clearly
dened than in the United States. In the Philippines, for example,
80 percent of the population is Catholic, and many laws clearly demonstrate the inuence of Catholicism. President Arroyo openly backs the
Vaticans anticontraception stance (Austria, 2004). Natural family planning is the governments espoused method of family planning according to the Department of Healths Web site (Department of Health,
Philippines, n.d.). Although rst-trimester abortion is virtually unrestricted in most countries in the European Union, it remains a crime
in Ireland. Spain, which has some of the most progressive policies in
Europe (including gay marriage), is in turmoil as we write this chapter
because Prime Minister Zapatero and his political party are attempting

136

Feminism as Human Rights

to liberalize abortion laws (Burnett, 2009). Portugal only recently (2007)


legalized abortion during the rst 10 weeks of pregnancy, and Italys
law (established in 1978) allows legal abortion only during the rst 90
days (Burnett, 2009). All of these are countries where the Roman Catholic Church has traditionally exercised a great deal of inuence. On a
more positive note, there are feminist Catholic groups (e.g., Catholics
for a Free Choice) attempting to nd a place for reproductive rights
within the traditional moral framework (Hunt, 1996). These groups often work together with the Religious Coalition for Reproductive
Choice, which includes representative groups within all the major religions practiced in the United States. Members of mainline Protestant
churches and Jewish congregations tend to be more liberal in their perspectives regarding reproductive rights (Schenker, 2005).
Islam also represents a wide range of beliefs about reproductive
rights. Positions on family planning and abortion, for example, vary
across the Muslim world. Generally speaking, family planning is
allowed in Muslim countries of the Middle East and north Africa despite a high value placed on fertility. Although abortion is typically forbidden once the fetus achieves ensoulment (the timing of which varies
based on the beliefs of specic Islamic sects), the one exception commonly allowed is in the case of saving a womans life (Hessini, 2007).
Rape is increasingly considered a reason for abortion, although Kuwati
muftis have decided that it does not justify the need. Groups such as
the Indonesian Society for Pesantren3 and Community Development
(P3M) have been working to reinterpret Islamic texts to explore womens reproductive rights. P3M advocates for womens health and safety
with regard to reproduction, economic compensation for womens
reproductive role, and the right of women to make reproductive decisions (Sciortino, Matsir, & Masudi, 1996).
Opinions on what is considered acceptable with regard to assistive
reproductive techniques also vary by religion. Judaism supports articial insemination with the husbands sperm, but most rabbis do not
support the use of donor sperm. Catholicism and some Evangelical
Protestant churches do not accept assisted reproduction. Mainline Protestantism generally accepts all forms of assisted reproduction including
IVF. Islam encourages treatment as long as husband and wife are the
only parties involved; use cannot be made of donor eggs, sperm, or
uterus (Schenker, 2005).
Factors such as culture and religion typically inuence the acceptability of services related to reproductive rights, but poverty can prevent access to them. In both sub-Saharan Africa and south Asia,
economic status signicantly inuences the likelihood that a mother
would have a skilled birth attendant present during labor and birthing
(Merrick, 2002). In the United States, 5 percent of women experience an
unintentional pregnancy each year, and the rate disproportionately

Womens Reproductive Rights: An International Perspective

137

represents low-income women among disadvantaged groups (Finer &


Henchshaw, 2006). The World Bank has estimated that ensuring skilled
care during birthing could reduce maternal deaths by 74 percent (Obaid,
2007). Contraceptive use is also impacted by economic status, although
to a lesser degree, in sub-Saharan Africa and south Asia (Merrick, 2002).
Voluntary access to family planning methods for all women would
reduce maternal deaths by an estimated 20 to 35 percent (Obaid, 2007).
Family planning initiatives have been misused at times, usually in
ways that victimize low-income women. For example, in Peru, between
1996 and 1998, state-run family planning organizations favored sterilization over other forms of contraception, and the governments campaigns targeted poor women. Sterilizations were often carried out in
low-quality conditions and frequently did not require informed consent
(Ewig, 2006). Between 2000 and 2004, use of modern family planning
methods declined among the poorest segments of the Peruvian population, although the reasons remain unclear (Gribble, Sharma, & Menotti,
2007). Distrust is likely to be among them.
When basic reproductive health care and access to family planning
methods are a struggle, access to assisted reproductive technologies
may seem like a luxury. One article (Papreen et al., 2000) documents
the psychological pain of infertility among urban low-income residents
of Bangladesh. In a culture where having children is highly valued,
unwanted childlessness can result in a number of negative consequences. Respondents interviewed by Papreen and colleagues reported emotional problems, perceived role failure, social stigmatization from
members of the community, and physical and psychological abuse
within the home. In the United States, most private health insurance
plans do not cover assisted reproductive techniques, nor does Medicaid, the governments program for the poor uninsured. In many states,
Medicaid does not cover abortion either. Thus, in counties around the
world, poor women are left on their own to grieve for children they
want, but cannot have, and to struggle to raise children they have, but
did not want.
Another important factor in the status of womens reproductive
rights is gender equality. According to one study (Pillai & Wang, 1999)
of 101 developing nations, gender equality is the single most important
factor that impacts the achievement of womens reproductive rights.
Male dominance is associated with increased fertility rates (Keytz,
1986). In South Africa, one study (Pettifor, Measham, Reev, & Padian,
2004) with a relatively large sample size of 4,066 participants showed
that women who reported low relationship control were more likely to
use condoms inconsistently than were those who reported high relationship control. When women have less control over sexual activity,
they also have less ability to protect themselves from unwanted pregnancy and sexually transmitted diseases (Roth, 2003).

138

Feminism as Human Rights

In Egypt, a study (Diop-Sidibe, Campbell, & Becker, 2005) showed


that wives who are subject to domestic violence are less likely to use
prenatal care or contraceptives. Specically, women who reported three
or more instances of abuse were signicantly less likely currently to
use contraceptives than were those women who reported one or two
incidents. In addition, women who had ever been beaten by their husbands were at signicantly greater risk of having gone without prenatal care during their most recent pregnancy.
Progress is being made in some countries. In Honduras, for example, in a recent study (Speizer, Whittle, & Carter, 2005), more than half
of the men and women surveyed supported shared reproductive decision making. Women who lived in less urban areas, had less education,
and were of low to middle socioeconomic class were more likely than
other women to believe that men should make all of the reproductive
decisions and to live in a household where the man did make those
decisions. Despite the fact that more than one-half of households supported shared reproductive decision-making, a higher proportion of
women than men reported that they had more children than they
wanted. This nding indicates that at least some women in Honduras
have less control over their reproductive choices than they would like
to have, but cultural changes in favor of gender equality and shared
responsibility could pave the way to empower women to exert control
over their reproductive lives.
REPRODUCTIVE JUSTICE IS IMPORTANT FOR
WOMEN AND THEIR CHILDREN
The decision of whether and when to bear children is a crucial one
that has long-term consequences for womens lives (Chrisler & Johnston-Robledo, 2000), and women cannot make those decisions unless
they are able to control their own bodies and exercise their reproductive rights. Womens ability to make reproductive decisions begins
with the ability to determine whether, when, and with whom to
engage in sexual activity; depends on the availability of various, accessible, safe, effective, and inexpensive methods of contraception, including emergency contraceptives (i.e., the morning after pill); requires
safe, legal abortion as a back-up to contraceptive failure, in cases of
rape or incest, and in cases of ectopic pregnancy, miscarriage, and
other threats to womens health; benets from affordable infertility
treatments; depends on prenatal and postnatal health care for women
and their children; and requires safe and sensitive birthing assistance
that is as responsive as possible to womens preferences.
Reproductive justice is important for womens physical health. Contraception gives women the ability to control the number, and the
space between, childbirths. Spacing is important for womens health, as

Womens Reproductive Rights: An International Perspective

139

it allows them to recover fully from the previous pregnancy and birth
before becoming pregnant again. Large numbers of children, and children born close together in time, are hard on womens bodies, and can
result in medical problems, chronic strain, and fatigue (Hawkins,
Matteson, & Tabeek, 1995). Millions of women die each year from
pregnancy- and birth-related complications and from unsafe abortions
(UN, 2008; Wakabi, 2007). The UN estimates that a womans risk of
dying from preventable or treatable pregnancy- and birth-related complications is 1 in 22 in underdeveloped nations (sub-Saharan African
countries have the highest maternal mortality rates); in developed
nations that risk is 1 in 7,300. Skilled medical personnel, sanitary
equipment, and the ability to transport patients to hospitals and clinics
with proper facilities should emergencies arise are critical to reducing
maternal mortality (UN, 2008). Accessible contraception, liberalization
of abortion laws (at a minimum, abortion should be safe and available
to preserve womens physical and mental health), and proper training
and certication of abortion providers would save the lives of many
women. Female genital cutting has no health benets and many health
hazards for women who undergo it. The procedures are often carried
out in unsafe circumstances, which can lead to hemorrhaging and infections; the procedures are also associated with urinary tract ailments, dysmenorrhea, and childbirth complications (World Health Organization,
2008). Rape can result in physical injuries, potential unwanted pregnancy, and sexually transmitted diseases. Women who have been raped
have been shown to increase their health care visits by as much as 56
percent in the year following the assault (Koss, 1994), and untreated
STDs can results in cervical cancer, urinary tract infections, pelvic
inammatory disease, infertility, and even death (Chrisler, 2001). Laws
against rape (including marital and acquaintance rape) and genital mutilation should be enforced to protect womens health. Any sexual activity
or body-altering procedure must require free consent.
Reproductive justice is important for womens mental health. The
ability of individuals to exercise control over their bodies and their circumstances is important to mental health (Chrisler, 2008). Mentally
healthy people not only have a greater sense of personal control than
mentally ill people do, but they often overestimate the amount of control they have over events in their lives, which leads them to maintain
a sense of optimism for the future (Taylor & Brown, 1988; Weinstein,
1984). Rape, unwanted pregnancy, and unwanted genital cutting are
extreme examples of loss of control, and they often result in shame,
depression, anxiety, and trauma (Chrisler & Ferguson, 2006). In countries where womens bodies are considered to belong to the family or
to the community, shame associated with rape is likely to be especially
acute. Women who have been raped also complain of low self-esteem,
body image concerns, self-perceived poor health, fear of intimacy, and

140

Feminism as Human Rights

an inability to trust men (Fanslow & Robinson, 2004). Higher than


average rates of drug and alcohol abuse and eating disorders are
often seen in victims (West, 2002), and suicidal thoughts are more common among victims than among the general population (Fanslow &
Robinson, 2004). Depression is also associated with unwanted pregnancy (Rubin & Russo, 2004), miscarriage and stillbirth (Cosgrove,
2004), and seeking abortion when the decision is not supported by the
womens family and friends (Rubin & Russo, 2004). Even a wellplanned childbirth can be associated with disappointment and depression when a womans wishes for how to give birth are ignored or overruled by medical personnel due to complications (Johnston-Robledo &
Barnack, 2004). Postpartum depression, or the milder baby blues, is
not uncommon, especially after a rst birth, a difcult birth, in circumstances where the woman lacks childcare assistance and social support
as she recovers from birthing, or when the woman has had previous
episodes of depression (Chrisler & Johnston-Robledo, 2002). Womens
reactions to infertility can range from disappointment in not being able
to achieve a life goal to more serious complaints of stress, anxiety,
depression, shame, low self-esteem, body image concerns, and marital
strife (Spector, 2004). In pronatal cultures where motherhood is considered to be womens purpose and highest achievement in life, infertility
is particularly likely to damage womens mental health and cause them
to question their self-worth.
Reproductive justice is important for childrens health and development. Infant mortality is lower in smaller families. Fewer children,
whose births are spaced a few years apart, generally means healthier
children. Parents can take better care of them and spend more time
with each one, interacting with the children in ways that facilitate their
development. Fewer children in poor families means more food, clothing, and room to play for each child. Parents with fewer children are
more likely to be able to pay for their schooling, medical care, and leisure activities. Finally, children whose parents wanted them are more
likely to feel loved and secure.
Reproductive justice is essential to secure equal rights for women in
both the public and the private sphere. As Michelle Goldberg (2009,
p. 11) put it: reproductive rights are intimately related to womens
economic freedom. Having smaller families allows women to work.
When they bring nancial resources into the family, their power tends
to increase and their daughters welfare improves. Without the ability
to decide whether and when to bear children women cannot make
plans, strive to meet personal goals, seek higher education, succeed in
a career, or even manage to work outside the home. Women who have
fewer children and greater nancial resources are better able to negotiate with their partners and to exert some control over their own, and
their childrens, future.

Womens Reproductive Rights: An International Perspective

141

FEMINIST PERSPECTIVES ON WOMENS


REPRODUCTIVE RIGHTS
As we wrote this chapter, many slogans from the second wave of
feminism came to mind: The personal is political; Sisterhood is global;
Keep your laws off my body; Not the Church, not the state, women
shall decide our fate; Every child a wanted child; Motherhood by
choice, not chance; No means no. The slogans we chanted at rallies
decades ago remain relevant today for women all over the world,
including in Western democracies, when reproductive justice is not
guaranteed. The radical feminists of the 1960s warned that reproductive rights are the essential underpinning of womens liberation: If we
lack the ability to control our own bodies, how can we expect to control anything else? Reproductive justice womens empowerment.
This is no less true today, as a global culture war is raging, and its
all about who controls womens fertilityand, more broadly, women
themselves (Goldberg, 2009, p. 4).
We must not give up the ght for reproductive justice. Signicant
progress has been made in many countries, and the groundwork has
been laid for future success. As we noted earlier in this chapter, many
international organizations and agencies now accept the once radical
notion that womens rights are human rights. The UN, the Organization of American States, the European Union, and the African Union
all have ratied protocols in support of reproductive rights, although
their member states do not necessarily live up to their agreements. The
international courts have forced some countries to comply or to compensate victims, and international organizations, such as Human Rights
Watch, collect data about violations of womens reproductive rights
and use the data to embarrass the leaders of those countries. The
UN is pressuring its member nations to take steps to meet the Millennium Development Goals, which include the reduction of maternal
and infant mortality. Planned Parenthood International, the Population
Council, the Guttmacher Institute, and other organizations are providing advice and nancial support to feminist groups and medical associations in countries around the globe who are working to secure
reproductive justice. Concerned individuals can donate money and
time to organizations that help women to help themselves.
International pressure on countries whose laws oppress womens
rights (e.g., Afghanistan, Nicaragua) can be helpful, but it also can
backre. Planned Parenthood and the Population Council have their
roots in the eugenics movement and Malthusian theory (Goldberg,
2009). Although the organizations repudiated that movement long ago,
people who are aware of their history might continue to view their
efforts with distrust. Political and religious leaders, who want to maintain the patriarchal social order, often accuse international

142

Feminism as Human Rights

organizations and agencies of neocolonialism when they advocate for


womens rights in underdeveloped countries (Goldberg, 2009). These
leaders maintain that their cultural traditions (e.g., genital cutting, marital rape) are more important than womens rights or that they should
not have to change their culture to please outsiders. Thus, we can often
be most effective by supporting local groups who advocate for change
from within rather than appearing to force change from without.
It is important to remember that cultures and societies are dynamic.
They inuence each other, and they are inuenced by individuals and
groups within. Politicians come and go; new religious groups arise,
and old ones decline; artists and technologies change the way people
think. We have seen an enormous amount of cultural and social
changes during our lifetimes, and such changes will continue to occur
despite some leaders attempts to maintain the status quo. We believe
that the momentum is in womens favor, as the international protocols
indicate. Reproductive justice is on the way. The global culture war
can eventually be won by the worlds women. Sisterhood is powerful!
NOTES
1. K. L. v. Peru concerned a 17-year-old girl denied an abortion even though
her fetus was determined to be anencephalic, and doctors knew the baby
would live only a few days after birth. Paulina Ramirez v. Mexico concerned a
13-year-old rape victim who was denied an abortion when even Mexicos law
allows abortion in rape cases. Tysiac v. Poland concerned a woman denied an
abortion even though doctors warned that the continued pregnancy would
worsen her medical condition and lead to blindness (Goldberg, 2009).
2. Marianismo, which is based on Catholic principles and the idealization of
the Virgin Mary, encourages women to strive to display characteristics associated with Mary and other female saints, such as virginity, virtue, self-abnegation,
and self-sacrice (Yeager, 1994).
3. Pesantren are Islamic boarding schools.

REFERENCES
Afghanistan rape law puts womens rights front and center. (2009). Accessed
April 7, 2009, at www.cnn.com/2009/WORLD/asiapcf/04/06/afghanistan.law/index.html
Austria, C. S. R. (2004). The church, the state, and womens bodies in the context of religious fundamentalism in the Philippines. Reproductive Health
Matters, 12, 96103.
Barrett, J., & Buckley, C. (2007). Constrained contraceptive choice: IUD prevalence in Uzbekistan. International Family Planning Perspectives, 33, 5057.
Birenbaum-Carmeli, D., & Dimfeld, M. (2008). In vitro fertilization policy in
Israel and womens perspectives: The more the better? Reproductive Health
Matters, 16, 182191.

Womens Reproductive Rights: An International Perspective

143

Boland, R., & Katzvie, L. (2008). Developments in laws on induced abortion:


19982007. International Family Planning Perspectives, 34, 110119.
Bosmans, M., Nasser, D., Khammash, U., Claeys, P. & Temmerman, M. (2008).
Palestinian womens sexual and reproductive health rights in a longstanding humanitarian crisis. Reproductive Health Matters, 16, 103111.
Burnett, V. (2009, April 11). Spain steps into battle with itself on abortion.
New York Times, pp. A4, A7.
Burstin, F. (2007, September 20). Same-sex couples priced out of IVF. Herald
Sun, p. 9.
Butt, R. (2009). Condom use could make HIV/AIDS situation worse in Africa,
says pope: Controversy over Catholic Churchs stance reignitedPolicy
divides some clergy working with patients. Guardian, p. 18.
Center for Reproductive Rights. (n.d.). Are reproductive rights human rights?
Retrieved January 28, 2009, from http://reproductiverights.org/en/feature/are-repro-rights-human-rights
Chrisler, J. C. (2001). Gendered bodies and physical health. In R. Unger (Ed.),
Handbook on the psychology of women and gender (pp. 289302). New York:
Wiley.
Chrisler, J. C. (2008). Fear of losing control: Power, perfectionism, and the
psychology of women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 112.
Chrisler, J. C., & Ferguson, S. (2006). Violence against women as a public health
issue. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1087, 235249.
Chrisler, J. C., & Johnston-Robledo, I. (2000). Motherhood and reproductive
issues. In M. Biaggio & M. Hersen (Eds.), Issues in the psychology of women
(pp. 199225). New York: Plenum.
Chrisler, J. C., & Johnston-Robledo, I. (2002). Raging hormones? Feminist
perspectives on premenstrual syndrome and postpartum depression. In
M. Ballou & L. S. Brown (Eds.), Rethinking mental health and disorder
(pp. 174197). New York: Guilford.
Cosgrove, L. (2004). The aftermath of pregnancy loss: A feminist critique of the
literature and implications for treatment. Women & Therapy, 27, 107122.
Countdown Coverage Writing Group. (2008). Countdown to 2015 for maternal,
newborn, and child survival: The 2008 report on tracking coverage of
interventions. Lancet, 371, 12471258.
Department of Health, Philippines. (n.d.). Family planning: A brief description
of the program. Retrieved on April 7, 2009, from http://www.doh.gov/
ph/programs/family_planning/prole.
Diop-Sidibe, N., Campbell, J. C., & Becker, S. (2005). Domestic violence against
women in Egypt: Wife beating and health outcomes. Social Science &
Medicine, 62, 12601277.
Dotson, L. A., Stinson, J., & Christian, L. (2003). People tell me I cant have
sex: Women with disabilities share their personal perspectives on health
care, sexuality, and reproductive rights. Women & Therapy, 26, 195209.
Ewig, C. (2006). Hijacking global feminism: Feminists, the Catholic Church,
and the family planning debacle in Peru. Feminist Studies, 32, 632659.
Fanslow, J. L., & Robinson, E. (2004). Violence against women in New Zealand:
Prevalence and health consequences. Journal of the New Zealand Medical
Association, 117, 11731206.

144

Feminism as Human Rights

Finer, L., & Henshaw, S. K. (2006). Disparities in rates of unintended pregnancies in the U.S. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 38, 9096.
Gelb, J., & Shogan, C. (2005). Community activism in the USA: Catholic hospital mergers and reproductive access. Social Movement Studies, 4, 209229.
Goldberg, M. (2009). The means of reproduction: Sex, power, and the future of the
world. New York: Penguin Press.
Greenhalgh, S. (2001). Fresh winds in Beijing: Chinese feminists speak out on
the one-child policy and womens lives. Signs, 26, 847887.
Gribble, J. N., Sharma, S., & Menotti, E. P. (2007). Family planning policies and
their impacts on the poor: Perus experience. International Family Planning
Perspectives, 33, 176181.
Hardee, K., Xie, Z., & Gu, B. (2003). Family planning and womens lives in rural China. International Family Planning Perspectives, 30, 6876.
Hawkins, J., Matteson, P. S., & Tabeek, E. S. (1995). Fertility control. In
C. I. Fogel & N. F. Woods (Eds.), Womens health care: A comprehensive
handbook (pp. 281322). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Henderson, H. (2007, April 14). Sexuality and disability as social policy. Toronto
Star, p. L03.
Hessini, L. (2007). Abortion and Islam: Policies and practice in the Middle East
and north Africa. Reproductive Health Matters, 15, 7584.
Hindin, M. J. (2007). Contraception, safe abortion, and maternal morbidity.
Lancet, 370, 12941297.
Hunt, M. (1996). Good sex: Womens religious wisdom on sexuality. Reproductive Health Matters, 8, 97103.
International Conference on Population and Development. (1994). Programme of
action. Retrieved April 7, 2009, from http://who.int/reproductive-health/
gender/glossary.html
Izugbara, C. O., & Undie, C. (2008). Who owns the body? Indigenous African
discourses of the body and contemporary sexual rights rhetoric. Reproductive Health Matters, 16, 159167.
Johnston-Robledo, I., & Barnack, J. (2004). Psychological issues in childbirth:
Potential roles for psychotherapists. Women & Therapy, 27, 133150.
Jones, A. (2009, March 2). Rules eased for second parent in IVF births. Guardian, p. 10.
Keytz, N. (1986). The family that does not reproduce itself. Population and Development Review, 12, 139154.
Kort, M. (2008). Are U.S. policies killing women? Retrieved on November 12,
2008, from http://www.alternet.org/story/74469/
Koss, M. (1994). The negative impact of crime victimization on womens health
and medical use. In A. J. Dan (Ed.), Reframing womens health: Multidisciplinary research and practice (pp. 189200). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
McGinn, T., Casey, S., Purdin, S., & Marsh, M. (2004). Reproductive health for
conict-affected people: Policies, research, and programmes. London: Humanitarian Practice Network, Overseas Development Institute.
Merrick, T. (2002). Population and poverty: New views on an old controversy.
International Family Planning Perspectives, 28, 4146.
More Catholic bishops deny communion to pro-choice politicians. (2004,
October). Church & State, 1920.

Womens Reproductive Rights: An International Perspective

145

Nader, C. (2007, December 19). No Medicare for lesbians IVF: Health: Canberra rejects funding, despite baby law overhaul. Age, 4.
Obaid, T. A. (2007). Breaking the cycle of poverty in achieving the MGDS:
Investing in reproductive heath and rights. UN Chronicle, 44, 1920.
Papreen, N., Sharma, A., Sabin, K., Begum, L., Ahsan, S. K., & Baqui, A. H.
(2000). Living with infertility: Experiences among urban slum populations
in Bangladesh. Reproductive Health Matters, 8, 3344.
Petchesky, R. P. (2008). Conict and crisis settings: Promoting sexual and
reproductive rights. Reproductive Health Matters, 16, 49.
Pettifor, A. E., Measham, D. M., Rees, H. V., & Padian, N. S. (2004). Sexual
power and HIV risk, South Africa. Emerging Infectious Diseases. Retrieved
April 2, 2009, from http://www.cdc.gov/ncidod/EID/vol10no11/
04-0252.htm
Pillai, V. K., & Wang, G. (1999). Social structural model of womens reproductive rights: A cross-national study of developing countries. Canadian Journal
of Sociology, 24, 255280.
Roth, R. (2003). Sexual and reproductive life of women: Gender equality in
womens reproductive health in western Europe and the U.S. Psychological
Science, 45, 123134.
Rubin, L., & Russo, N. F. (2004). Abortion and mental health: What therapists
need to know. Women & Therapy, 27, 6990.
Sawer, P. (2008, March 23). Miracle rst baby for IVF mother at 57. Sunday
Telegraph, p. 6.
Schafer, A. (2009, February 9). The case for postmenopausal mothers. Globe and
Mail, p. A13.
Schenker, J. G. (2005). Assisted reproductive practice: Religious perspectives.
Reproductive BioMedical Online, 10, 310319.
Sciortino, R., Matsir, L. M., & Masudi, M. F. (2006). Learning from Islam: Advocacy of reproductive rights in Indonesian Pesantren. Reproductive Health
Matters, 8, 8696.
Spector, A. R. (2004). Psychological issues and interventions with infertile
patients. Women & Therapy, 27, 91105.
Speizer, I. S., Whittle, L., & Carter, M. (2005). Gender relations and reproductive decision-making in Honduras. International Family Planning Perspectives, 31, 131139.
Stafford, N. K. (2008). Permission for domestic violence: Marital rape in Ghanaian marriages. Womens Rights Law Reporter, 29, 63.
Sullivan, A. (2009). Shhh: Obama repeals the abortion gag rule, very quietly.
Retrieved January 26, 2009, from http://www.time.com/time/nation/article/0,8599,183794,00.html
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Positive illusions and well-being revisited:
Separating fact from ction. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 2127.
United Nations. (2008). The millennium development goals report. Retrieved
May 1, 2009, from www.un.org/milleniumgoals/maternal.shtml
Wacker, J. L., Parish, S. L., & Macy, R. J. (2008). Sexual assault and women
with cognitive disabilities: Codifying discrimination in the states. Journal of
Disability Studies, 19, 8694.
Wakabi, W. (2007). Africa battles to make female genital mutilation history.
Lancet, 369, 10691070.

146

Feminism as Human Rights

Weinstein, N. D. (1984). Why it wont happen to me: Perceptions of risk factors


and susceptibility. Health Psychology, 3, 431457.
West, C. M. (2002). Battered black and blue: An overview of violence in the
lives of Black women. Women & Therapy, 25, 527.
World Health Organization (WHO). (2008). Fact sheet on female genital mutilation.
Retrieved May 1, 2009, from www.who.int/mediccentre/factsheets/fs241/en/
Yeager, G. M. (Ed.). (1994). Confronting change, challenging tradition: Women in
Latin American history. Wilmington, DE: Scholarly Resources.

Chapter 7

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and


International Perspectives
Michele A. Paludi
Jennifer L. Martin
Carmen Paludi, Jr.
Sarah Metzgar Boggess
Kristina Hicks
Lindsey Speach

Women should have equal pay for equal work and they should be considered
equally eligible to the ofces of principal and superintendent, professor and
president. So you must insist that qualications, not sex, shall govern
appointments and salaries.
Susan B. Anthony

This statement by Susan B. Anthony was made in 1903. However, the


fact is that in the United States, women still do not earn the same salary as men for identical work. Since the passage of the Equal Pay Act
into law in 1963, the pay gap between womens and mens wages is
still a major issue (Gibelman, 2003; Graddy & Pistaferri, 2000; Ostroff
& Atwater, 2003; Petersen & Saporta, 2004; Rudin & Byrd, 2003). When
the Equal Pay Act was passed in 1963, full-time employed women
earned an average of 59 cents for every dollar paid to men. Census statistics released on August 26, 2008, Womens Equality Day, indicated
that average, full-time employed women earn 77.8 cents to every dollar
earned by full-time employed men (National Committee on Pay Equity,
2009). This gap only changed by less than 1 percent from 2006 to 2007.

148

Feminism as Human Rights

Womens average earnings for full-time employment were $35,102; mens


were $45,113. Dey and Hill (2007) reported that wage differences begin
directly after college graduation: women who earned a bachelors degree
in 19992000 were paid 80 percent of their male colleagues wages one
year after graduation. However, 10 years after college graduation, women
lag further behind and earn approximately 69 percent of mens salaries.
Gattiker and Cohen (1997) reported that men earn higher wages
than women in general but also earn more than women in occupations
populated by women, e.g., nurse, biochemist). Reilly and Wirjanto
(1999) also reported that the proportion of women in the discipline had
an impact on wages. They found a negative wage premium associated
with the proportion of women in the discipline even after controlling
for experience, education, tenure, industry, and so forth. As Ostroff
and Atwater concluded, in their review of the literature:
These consistent signicant negative effects of female representation on
pay or rewards can be taken as evidence that the work done by women
is valued and rewarded at lower rates than that done by men. (p. 728)

Furthermore, this wage gap is more severe for women of color


(Browne & Askew, 2006; Gee, 2006; Kim, 2006). For example, the earnings for African American women were $31,009; for Asian women,
$40,374; and for Latinas, $26,612. African American women with
undergraduate degrees make only $1,545 more per year than white
men who have only completed high school (Richardson & Sandoval,
2007). This is more startling when we take into account the fact that
African American women account for approximately 30 percent of all
female-headed families in the United States. Their median income is
$18,244 annually. Single families in the United States headed by white
men have a median income of $39,240. In addition, when we consider
the annual income of Latinas working full time ($17,837) with white
mens income over a 30-year career span, Latinas earn $510,000 less
than white men (Barbezat, 2002; Gee, 2006; Kim, 2006; National Committee on Pay Equity, 2009). Women of color in the United States suffer
from both sex and race wage inequalities (Richardson & Sandoval,
2007). Men of color do not earn as much as white men earn (Browne &
Askew, 2006; Coleman, 2003).
Thus, salary discrimination reduces total career lifespan earnings,
reduces benets from Social Security and pension plans, and inhibits
the ability to save money for retirement, to purchase a home, to pay
for college education for children and/or for themselves, and to cover
medical expenses throughout their lives, especially during their elderly
years (Greene, 2006).
In this chapter, we review the legal issues involved in wage discrimination, including the Equal Pay Act and more recent legislation

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

149

aimed at guaranteeing closure of the wage gap. In addition, we offer


an overview of pay equity issues in countries throughout the world.
We also address contributing factors from research in the social sciences to the continued disparity in wages by sex and race. Finally, we
provide recommendations for workplaces in ensuring they are committed to equal pay for equal work for both sexes and all races and ethnic
groups.
LEGISLATION: EQUAL PAY FOR EQUAL WORK
If you say, Im for equal pay, thats a reform. But if you say, Im a feminist, thats . . . a transformation of society.
Gloria Steinem
A girl should not expect special privileges because of her sex but neither should
she adjust to prejudice and discrimination.
Betty Friedan

The struggle for equal pay for equal work has a long history in the
United States. A chronology of pay equity legislation in the United
States is summarized below (also see Barbezat, 2002). We address equal
pay legislation in the European Union in the subsequent section.
FEDERAL ECONOMIC ACT
The Federal Economic Act was passed in 1932 to prohibit wives of
federal employee from working in government positions. This act also
declared that women with husbands also employed shall be the rst
on the list for ring.
NATIONAL RECOVERY ACT
The National Recovery Act of 1935 demanded that women who
worked in government jobs receive 25 percent less pay than men in the
identical job. In 1942, the War Labor Board ruled women must be paid
the identical job rate as men (who were serving in the War) were paid.
However, the war ended before this rule could be enforced. In fact,
women had to leave the workplaces so returning veterans could resume work.
EQUAL PAY LEGISLATION: 1950s
Legislation requiring equal pay for women was introduced in the
1950s by Senator Wayne Morse (D-OR) and Rep. Edith Green (D-OR).
Versions of this legislation by Republican members Katherine St.

150

Feminism as Human Rights

George and Jessica Weis (both of New York) also were introduced. The
legislation was not passed.
In addition, prior to the 1960s in the United States, separate job listings were published for each sex, for example, Help Wanted Female
and Help Wanted Male. Occasionally, the identical job advertisements were listed under both categories with one difference: the pay
scales were not identical (Brunner, 2009).
EQUAL PAY ACT OF 1963
However, in 1961 when labor activist Esther Peterson directed the
Womens Bureau, the Equal Pay Bill was introduced. With President
Kennedys initiative and support, the Equal Pay Act was passed in
1963 (effective June 11, 1964), giving women equal pay for equal work.
The Equal Pay Act of 1963 requires that women and men doing essentially identical work requiring substantially equal skill, responsibility,
and effort must initially be paid the same wage. Differences in wages
later on may be due to performance, merit systems, seniority, and so
on. Thus, the focus of equal work is on the duties performed. Job
descriptions, job classications, and job titles are not all that should be
considered in assigning wages.
According to the Equal Pay Act, equal skill, responsibility, and effort
are dened as follows (EEOC, 2009):
. SkillMeasured by factors such as the experience, ability, education, and
training required to perform the job. The key issue is what skills are
required for the job, not what skills the individual employees may have.
. EffortThe amount of physical or mental exertion needed to perform the job.
. ResponsibilityThe degree of accountability required in performing the job.

The Equal Pay Act applies to all employers covered by the Fair
Labor Standards Act of 1938. Employers must prove they are using
valid, not discriminatory criteria, in paying wages.
In addition, the Equal Pay Act bans employers from reducing the
wages of men or women in order to comply with this legislation. This
legislation also covers professional employees and includes professionals and teachers in elementary and secondary schools. This legislation
was the rst federal law to ban discrimination by private employers on
the basis of sex.
TITLE VII OF THE 1964 CIVIL RIGHTS ACT
In 1964, the Civil Rights Act passed Congress. Title VII of this legislation also bans employment discrimination against women and men.
According to Title VII:

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

151

It shall be an unlawful employment practice for an employer


1. to fail or refuse to hire or to discharge any individual, or otherwise to
discriminate against any individual with respect to his [sic] compensation, terms or conditions, or privileges of employment, because of such
individuals race, color, religion, sex, or national origin; or
2. to limit, segregate, or classify his employees or applicants for employment in any way which would deprive or tend to deprive any individual
of employment opportunities or otherwise adversely affect his [sic] status
as an employee, because of such individuals race, color, religion, sex, or
national origin.

EQUAL EMPLOYMENT OPPORTUNITY COMMISSION


With the passage of the Civil Rights Act of 1964, the Equal Employment Opportunity Commission (EEOC) was established to enforce
equal employment opportunity legislation. The EEOC investigates job
discrimination charges related to disability, race, national origin, color,
religion, sex, pregnancy, age, sexual harassment, and retaliation (EEOC,
2009). In 2007, the EEOC received 818 complaints of pay inequity, 796
of which were resolved and totaled 9.3 million in monetary benets to
complainants (EEOC, 2009).

AGE DISCRIMINATION IN EMPLOYMENT ACT


The right of employees to be free of wage discrimination is also protected under the Age Discrimination in Employment Act (ADEA) of
1967, which stated:
It shall be unlawful for an employer
(1) to fail or refuse to hire or to discharge any individual or otherwise discriminate against any individual with respect to his [sic] compensation, terms,
conditions, or privileges of employment, because of such individuals age;
(2) to limit, segregate, or classify his employees in any way which would
deprive or tend to deprive any individual of employment opportunities
or otherwise adversely affect his [sic] status as an employee, because of
such individuals age; or
(3) to reduce the wage rate of any employee in order to comply with this
chapter.

NATIONAL COMMITTEE ON PAY EQUITY


In 1979, the National Committee on Pay Equity was founded. This
Committee is a coalition of womens and civil rights organizations,

152

Feminism as Human Rights

professional organizations, labor unions, educational associations, and


individuals working to eliminate sex- and race-based salary discrimination (National Committee on Pay Equity, 2009). This coalition includes
International Union of Electronic, Electrical, Salaried, Machine, and
Furniture Workers (IUE), Business and Professional Women, League of
Women Voters, Womens Legal Defense Fund, National Organization
for Women, Coalition of Labor Union Women, American Library Association, and the National Commission on Working Women. The Equal
Pay Day began in 1996 by the National Committee on Pay Equity as a
public awareness event to highlight wage disparities between women
and men. The event is held annually on a Tuesday in April. Tuesday
was selected since it is the day of the week on which womens wages
are equal to mens wages from the previous week.

AMERICANS WITH DISABILITIES ACT


Another federal law bans wage discrimination of employees: the
Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA) of 1990. According to Title I of
this legislation:
General rule: No covered entity shall discriminate against a qualied
individual with a disability because of the disability of such individual in
regard to job application procedures, the hiring, advancement, or discharge of employees, employee compensation, job training, and other
terms, conditions, and privileges of employment.

PAYCHECK FAIRNESS ACT


Senator Hillary Clinton and Representative Rosa DeLauro introduced the Paycheck Fairness Act in March 2007. The goal of this act
was to strengthen the Equal Pay Act of 1963. This act also proposes
voluntary guidelines to show employers how to evaluate jobs and eliminate wage discrimination. Furthermore, this act would prohibit
employers from retaliating against employees for sharing salary information with coworkers. This bill was passed by the House of Representatives in July 2008 on a 256 to 163 vote. The bill is awaiting
passage in the Senate at the time of this writing.
LILLY LEDBETTER FAIR PAY ACT OF 2009
I am deeply heartened by the Senate passage of the bill that bears my name.
It is an enormous victory for the women across the country who are victims
of pay discrimination.
Lily Ledbetter

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

153

The Lily Ledbetter Fair Pay Act of 2009 was designed to overrule a
2007 United States Supreme Court decision in Ledbetter v. Goodyear Tire
and Rubber Company (550 U.S. 618), which removed individuals rights
to use the protections of civil rights laws to remedy pay discrimination.
This Fair Pay Act passed the United States Senate on January 22 by a
vote of 61 to 36. The United States House of Representatives passed
the Fair Pay Act by a vote of 225 to 199. Senator Barbara Mikulski
(D-MD), Dean of the Senate Women, stated the following at the passage of this legislation:
We say to women today who earn only 77 cents for every dollar her
male counterpart makes, its time for a new day. We say to women of
color who make even less its time for a new law. And we say to all who
have suffered wage discrimination, its truly time for a change.

President Obama signed this bill on January 29, 2009. The Supreme
Court ruled in 2007 that Ledbetter should have led her claim within
180 days after the discriminatory decision was made, which in her case
was 20 years previous. The new legislation will allow employees 180
days to sue every time they are paid.
COMPARABLE WORTH
As we subsequently discuss in more detail, social science research
has suggested for some time that individuals view occupations as
being female-oriented or male-oriented (Mednick & Thomas, 1993,
2008; Steinberg, True, & Russo, 2008). For example, occupations that
are helping-oriented, for example, secretary, teacher, nurse, social
worker, and librarian are typically associated with women, whereas
occupations such as police ofcer, truck driver, and manager are associated with men (Betz, 1993, 2008). In fact, research has indicated that
an awareness of occupational stereotypes related to gender begins in
the preschool years and is well developed by rst grade (Betz, 2008;
Heyman & Legare, 2004; Liben, Bigler, & Krogh, 2001; Raag & Rackliff,
1998). Childrens ranges of occupations are difcult to change once
they are set (Betz, 2008).
Furthermore, research has found that as the percentage of women
entering a eld becomes larger, fewer men enter this eld four to seven
years later (England et al., 2004). England (1992) has interpreted this
result in the following way: as men perceive an occupation to be feminine they avoid majoring in the eld and applying for positions since
they believe the pay associated with this eld will decline. Glen and
Feldberg (1977) noted that during the nineteenth century, clerical work
was primarily a male-populated occupation that earned men good salaries and high status. However, during the twentieth century, women

154

Feminism as Human Rights

were dominant as clerical workers, and the status accruing from this
work has dropped signicantly, as have the wages. This research has
supported Bergmanns (1974) theory of overcrowding. In general,
there are more trained women employees than there are jobs available,
especially in segregated occupations. Furthermore, occupations populated by women have lower pay scales than those populated by men.
Thus, traditional female-populated occupations tend to garner significantly less compensation than occupations that are male-populated.
This nding has created concern about comparable worth, that is,
gender-based pay systems. Two occupations may be comparable in
terms of skill needed, stress associated with the job, education required
for the position, and training for the position. However, the jobs are
not paid equally because of the belief that male-populated occupations
should pay more than female-populated occupations, for example, reghter and nurse.
Comparable worth proponents want to take into account certain factors present in each occupation, that is, skills, working conditions,
effort, and responsibilities so that jobs equivalent in these factors are
paid identically (McArthur & Obrant, 2006). As Bernstein and Russo
(2008) conclude:
We have gone beyond the time when gender stereotyping and discrimination were merely lamentable. We have a body of law that makes overt
discrimination, sexual harassment, and hostile working environments
illegal. However, overt discrimination, documentable in courts of law,
has mutated into more subtle forms of discriminationwith their damage just as potent. (p. 24)

In President Carters administration, enforcement regulations related to


comparable worth were proposed. These regulations would have mandated comparable worth for federal contractors. However, these regulations
were identied as too controversial under President Reagans administration. Thus, they were removed from the agenda (Gibelman, 2003).
PAY EQUITY: INTERNATIONAL COMPARISONS
Many countries have taken legislative action to reduce discrimination and pay inequity. Despite widespread attention to the issue, there
are disparities across countrieseven though those countries may
appear to be equally committed to eradicating inequities. For example,
Maata (2008), in a study of nine countries that all had ratied the International Labor Organization Equal Renumeration Convention, found
wide disparities when reviewing their progress in equal pay between
1919 and 2000. She studied four developing countries (i.e., Ghana,
India, the Philippines, and Zambia) and ve developed countries

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

155

(Australia, Canada, Finland, New Zealand, and the United Kingdom).


She found that although all of the countries had ratied the Convention,
their progress on equal pay was quite varied. For example, during the
time period under study, the developed countries had broadened the
scope of equal pay to include the principle to work of equal value, but
most of the developing countries had not. In addition, the developing
countries were much slower to achieve a dual breadwinner mindset
and continued to embrace a male-dominated mindset even in 2000.
Maata (2008) concluded that that most of the countries, while
expressing support for equal pay in theory, did not invest enough in
the implementation of equal pay and had inadequate systems in place
for enforcement to ensure compliance. Countries that ratify the ILO
Convention are not legally bound to enact and enforce laws that are
consistent with the principles of the Convention, she points out, so that
the actual behavior of some countries was not consistent with the Convention principles.
The Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development
(2002), an organization of 30 countries committed to democracy and
healthy market economies, conducted a 19-country study in 2002 in
which it found that the United States had one of the largest gender
earnings gap, behind only Austria and Switzerland.
Equal treatment between women and men is a fundamental principle of the European Union. In fact, unlike the United States, the European Union considers comparable worth in its legislation. According to
the Directorate-General for Employment, Social Affairs and Equal
Opportunities, adopted in 1975:
The principle of equal pay for men and women . . . hereinafter called
principle of equal pay, means, for the same work or for work to
which equal value is attributed, the elimination of all discrimination on
grounds of sex with regard to all aspects and conditions of remuneration. In particular, where a job classication system is used for determining pay, it must be based on the same criteria for both men and
women and so drawn up as to exclude any discrimination on grounds
of sex.

The Treaty of Amsterdam adopted in 1997, states that:


Each Member State shall ensure that the principle of equal pay for male
and female workers for equal work or work of equal value is applied.

Equal pay without discrimination based on sex means:


(a) that pay for the same work at piece rates shall be calculated on the basis of the same unit of measurement;
(b) that pay for work at time rates shall be the same for the same job.

156

Austria
Belgium
Denmark
Finland
France
Germany
Greece
Ireland
Italy
Luxembourg
Netherlands
Portugal
Spain
Sweden
United Kingdom

Feminism as Human Rights

67 percent
79.4 percent
70.1 percent
82 percent
82 percent
75.8 percent
75.8 percent
70.4 percent
80 percent
84.5 percent
81.7 percent
85 percent
77 percent
76.5 percent
76.9 percent
82 percent
80.6 percent

for blue-collar workers


for white-collar workers

for blue-collar workers


for white-collar workers

However, wage differentials still exist between men and women in


the Member States of the European Union. For example, womens average pay as a percentage of mens is the following for some of the member states of the European Union (European Industrial Relations
Observatory On Line, 2002).
In the 2002 Employment Guidelines of the European Union, equal
pay is stressed. These guidelines state:
The signicant level of the pay gap between women and men in many
Member States has been identied as a potential disincentive for women
to take up work or to remain at work. . . . The Member States, where
appropriate with the social partners, are thus called on to adopt a multifaceted strategy to achieve gender pay equality in both the public and private sectors, and consider the setting of targets to tackle the pay gap. Such
a strategy could include inter alla a review of job classication and pay systems to eliminate gender bias, improving statistical and monitoring systems, and awareness training and transparency as regards pay gaps.

However, comparable to the United States, legislation does not mean


pay equity will occur.
As Soumeli and Nergaard (2002) stated:
. . . at least in theory, employers are bound by law to comply with the relevant legislation and not to conclude contracts that constitute direct or indirect discrimination against women. However, in practice we have seen that
the legislation has still not achieved the necessary impact. (p. 16)

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

157

To achieve pay equity, some countries in the European Union have


adopted legislation of their own. But these initiatives have been met
with opposition from employers (Soumeli & Nergaard, 2002). In
France, for example, according to Article L.432.3.1 of their Labour
Code, all employers are required to submit an annual report indicating
wages of women and men and how employers have ensured equality
at work. In May 2001, a law on gender equality at work was passed
that requires employers to negotiate at the organization and sector levels the objectives of occupational gender equality.
Sweden passed the Act of Equality between Men and Women,
which mandates employers to produce yearly plans and reviews of the
wages for women and men. This act states that the main purpose of
the annual plans is to identify and correct wage discrimination.
Pay inequities tend to be more pronounced in Asia than Europe. In
a 2008 study of eight countries in East Asia (i.e., China and the Hong
Kong Special Administrative Region [SAR], Japan, the Republic of
Korea, Malaysia, the Philippines, Singapore, Thailand, and Vietnam),
researchers found a pronounced gender pay gap. The researchers
found that women earned less than or just half of mens rates of pay in
Japan, Malaysia, the Republic of Korea, and Singapore; approximately
60 percent in Hong Kong SAR, the Philippines and Thailand; and
about two-thirds in China and Vietnam. The level of pay inequity was
found to be unrelated to the countrys level of economic development,
but was tied to agespecically, the pay gap was wider between older
women and their male peers (Haspels & Majurin, 2008).
In East Asia, gender inequality is entrenched and high numbers of
women work as unpaid family workers and in agriculture. Many
women in East Asia are discriminated against through inadequate maternity protection; laws that are in place to protect women against jobs
that are believed to be unsuitable or hazardous to them; and laws that
require women to retire at an earlier age than men (Haspels & Majurin,
2008).
Blau and Kahn (2000) noted that decentralized pay-setting practices
in America contribute to wage disparities between women and men.
Furthermore, Blau and Kahn (2000) noted that among the member
nations of the Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development, the United States stands at an extreme with an especially low
rate of collective bargaining coverage (p. 22). Countries with more
heavily unionized economies, such as many in Western Europe, have
centralized collective bargaining processes and enjoy lower overall
wage dispersion.
Indeed, in many countries, labor unions have been a driving force in
demanding pay equity. According to Public Services International
(2009), an organization that represents public sector unions globally,
public-sector unions in Norway, Germany, Finland, France, the United

158

Feminism as Human Rights

States, Canada and Ecuador, the Philippines, and New Zealand have
been instrumental in recent years in successfully campaigning for equal
pay for their members.
We discuss additional institutional factors to account for wage disparities in the next section.

EXPLANATION FOR WAGE DISCRIMINATION: THE VIEW


FROM SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH
I know we cant abolish prejudice through laws, but we can set up guidelines for our actions by legislation.
Belva Ann Lockwood

Women experience wage discrepancies for several reasons, according to research in management including (1) being viewed by employers as having less leverage, (2) the fact that men are more likely to
change jobs more frequently than women, enabling them to be promoted to higher level jobs over women with more seniority, (3) salary
increases for women in professional positions do not reect their
above-average performance while men with equal performance receive
larger raises, and (4) women in blue-collar jobs are placed in departments with lower-paying jobs (DeCenzo & Robbins, 2007; Mock, 2004;
National Committee on Pay Equity, 2009; Society for Human Resource
Management, 2009; Woo & Khoo, 2006).
However, research has indicated that wage discrepancies between
men and women are not only explained by these factors related to
educational level or job (Acker, 1989; Agars, 2004; Ostroff & Atwater,
2003; National Committee on Pay Equity, 1995; Rynes, Gerhart, &
Parks, 2005). As Fields and Wolff (1993) noted, . . . even after adjusting for productivity related characteristics of workers . . . there still
remains a substantial wage gap at the industry level between genders
(p. 118).
Rather, discrimination in wages can be explained by bias toward
female employees (Rudin & Byrd, 2003). According to Steinberg et al.,
(2008):
Gender stereotyping has long been linked to distinct employment issues
for women, who must deal with evaluation bias, greater pressure on
their performance, exclusion from certain jobs and promotional opportunities, incivility and harassment, unequal employment rewards, and gender segregation between and within occupations. (p. 657)

Soumeli and Nergaard (2002) noted that while many factors have
been offered to explain wage differentials in Member States of the European Union, . . . there is an unexplained difference in wages, which

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

159

is presumed to be due to discrimination (p. 1). Similar ndings were


reported by The European Commission (2007):
According to the 2007 Annual report on equality between women and
men, women are driving EU job growth, but still face barriers to equality. The majority of these barriers are generated by the discrimination of
women on the grounds of sex based on gender stereotypes (p. 1).

In this section we address several research areas in the social sciences that have helped us to understand the devaluation of womens
work: gender stereotyping, performance evaluation, causal attribution
for success and failure, and job segregation. While we address each of
these issues individually, we note they are interrelated in explaining
womens wage disparity.
GENDER STEREOTYPING
The emotional, sexual, and psychological stereotyping of females begins
when the doctor says, Its a girl.
Congresswoman Shirley Chisholm

In 2005, Harvard president Lawrence Summers argued that innate


differences between women and men explain womens inability to succeed in math and science. In 1984, Clarence Pendleton, Chair, U.S.
Commission on Civil Rights, referred to pay equity as the looniest
idea since Looney Tunes (National Committee on Pay Equity, 2009).
Stereotypes such as these statements refer to individuals thoughts/
cognitions that typically do not correspond with reality. Stereotypes
occur when individuals are classied by others as having something in
common because they are members of a particular group or category
of people (e.g., women employees, women CEOs). Psychological
research has identied that stereotypes have the following characteristics (Fiske & Stevens, 1993):
1. Groups that are targeted for stereotypes are easily identied and relatively powerless.
2. There is little agreement between the composite picture of the group and
the actual characteristics of that group.
3. This misperception is difcult to modify even though individuals who
hold stereotypes have interacted with individuals of the group who disconrm the stereotypes.
4. This misperception is the product of a bias in individuals informationprocessing mechanisms.

Gender stereotyping is a psychological process that describes individuals structured set of beliefs about the personal attributes of men

160

Feminism as Human Rights

and women (e.g., nurturant, aggressive, independent, lacking leadership qualities). Psychologists have identied an emotional component
to stereotypic cognitions: prejudice as well as behavioral component to
individuals cognitions, that is, discrimination. Thus, individuals statements and nonverbal gestures toward women and men provide insight
into their structured set of beliefs and emotions about each sex (Kite,
Deaux, & Haines, 2008; Paludi, 2002; Steinberg et al., 2008).
In the United States, much of the discrimination against women
earning the same as men can be traced to stereotypes about women,
women and work, and the meaning of money for women (Crampton &
Mishra, 1999; Doyle & Paludi, 1998; Konrad, Goldberg, Sullivan, &
Yang, 2005; Martin, 2008). Betz (2008) noted that stereotypes have
existed concerning the reasons why women work outside the home:
. . . it was assumed that when women worked . . . it was because their
labor was needed by the economy, as was the case with Rosie the Riveters of World War II . . . that they were working until they could land
a husband, or that they were the most pitiable of charactersthe spinster. (p. 716)

In addition, pin money is the euphemism for a small amount of


money that a woman earned to be used for extras for her family. It
has always been considered nonessential money because it is the husbands salary that provides for the family nancially. As Doyle and
Paludi (1998) noted:
The idea of pin money devalued a womans nancial contribution to the
familys economic well-being and soothed a husbands pride that rested
to a large degree on the knowledge that his salary was the familys primary income. (p. 185)

Thus, part of the inequity in salaries is due to the cultural stereotype


that men must earn more than women because men are the primary
breadwinners of the family (Lepak & Gowan, in press).
However, the reality is that more than 50 percent of all married
women whose husbands are present in the home are employed. In
addition, the largest category of employed women in the United States
are single women, who have only one income (Doyle & Paludi, 1998;
Paludi et al., 2007).
Furthermore, there has been a substantial increase in maternal
employment in the United States (Gilbert & Kearney, 2006; Hill, Waldfogel, Brooks-Gunn, & Han, 2005; Paludi et al., 2007; Strassel, Cogan, &
Goodman, 2006). In fact, women are now as likely to be employed
when they have infants as they are when they have preschool-aged
children. Strassel et al. (2006) reported that of women who put in

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

161

overtime work, 40 percent have children under six years of age. As


Hyde (1985) commented more than 20 years ago: The working
woman then is not a variation from the norm, she is the norm
(p. 169).
Stereotypes depict men, but not women, as having the requisite
skills and characteristics for managerial and leadership positions (Cejka
& Eagly, 1999; Kite et al., 2008). Business professionals indicate a strong
preference for male applicants for a stereotypically masculine job, even
when similar information on the resumes of women and men applicants had led to perceptions of similar personality traits (Betz, 1993,
2008). These stereotypes persist even though gender differences are not
found in leadership ability or job performance (Duff-McCall &
Schweinle, 2008; Frank & Belasen, 2008; Sinangil & Ones, 2003; Werhane et al., 2006). These stereotypes get expressed behaviorally in discriminatory treatment of women in terms of wages, pay increases, and
promotions (Bem, 1981; Cleveland, Stockdale, & Murphy, 2000; Fiske,
1998; Heilman, Wallen, Fuchs, & Tamkins, 2004; Paludi, Martin, Stern,
& DeFour, 2009; Powell, Buttereld, & Parent, 2002; see Martin, volume
1 of this book set).
In addition, Heilman, Wallen, Fuchs, and Tamkins (2004) reported
that women who do not maintain gender role stereotypes are socially
censured and experience personally directed negativity. As they stated:
. . . the mere recognition that a woman has achieved success on a traditionally male task produces inferences that she has engaged in counternormative behavior and therefore causes similarly negative
consequences (p. 3). Furthermore, successful achievement for women
is costly. Wiley and Eskilson (1985) found that successful women are
described as cold vis-a-vis men. Research (e.g., Heilman, Block, Martell, & Simon, 1989) also reported successful women managers to be
depicted as bitter, quarrelsome, deceitful, and devious vis-avis successful men managers.
PERFORMANCE EVALUATION
Gender stereotypes have also been found to be operating in research
on evaluations of identical performance by men and women by Goldberg (1968). In the original study, college students evaluated (in terms
of persuasiveness, writing style, intellectual depth of article, competence of author) supposedly published journal articles on linguistics,
law, art history, education, dietetics, and city planning. For each article,
half of the research participants saw a woman authors name; half saw
a mans. Results indicated that individuals rated the article more favorably when it was attributed to a man than a woman (even in elds
considered sex-appropriate for women).

162

Feminism as Human Rights

Subsequent research used this research paradigm with noncollege


students as well and indicated a promale bias for paintings, job applicants, magazine articles, and scientic journal articles (e.g., Deaux &
Taynor, 1973; Judiesch & Lyness, 1999; Martel, 1996; Nieva & Gutek,
1980; Swim, Borgida, Maruyama, & Myers, 1989; Ward, 1979). Nieva
and Guteks (1980) meta-analysis indicated a promale bias occurs when
men are rated more favorably than women given identical performance. Adolescents and young children were also found to have evaluation biases against women (Etaugh & Brown, 1975).
Research has also noted that women are as likely to be evaluated as
being as competent as men when their performance is acknowledged
by authoritative individuals (Issacs, 1981; Jacobson & Effertz, 1974; Taynor & Deaux, 1975; Ward, 1981).

CAUSAL ATTRIBUTIONS FOR SUCCESS AND FAILURE


The only way to solve the problem of womens subordination is to change
peoples mindset and to plant the new idea of gender equality into every
mind.
Qingrong Ma

Weiner, Frieze, Kukla, Reed, Rest, and Rosenbaum (1971) proposed


a cognitive attribution theory of achievement motivation to explain differences between high- and low-achievement individuals. Highachievement oriented individuals believe themselves to be the cause of
their successes because of their ability and hard work. On the other
hand, low-achievement oriented individuals attribute their successes to
external causes (e.g., ease of task, luck) and attribute their failures to
internal ones (e.g., lack of ability).
Gender comparisons have been observed in causal attributions for
success and failure. For example, Deaux and Emswiller (1974) noted
that equivalent performances by women and men are explained by different attributions. Performance by a man on a masculine task is typically attributed to his skill, whereas an equivalent performance by a
woman on the same task is attributed to her being lucky. In addition,
Feldman-Summers and Kieslers (1974) research indicated that men
attributed more ability to a male physician than to a female physician.
They also attributed a female physicians success to the ease of her
coursework and a large amount of effort. Betz (1993, 2008), for example, reported that women were more likely to attribute their success to
luck and their failures to low ability. Mens successes and womens
failures were attributed to personal dispositions; mens failures and
womens successes were attributed to environmental factors.
Betz (1993, 2008) noted that in work settings, men are rated higher
than equivalent women in performing certain tasks and in job

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

163

qualications. Women receive lower recognition and economic rewards


for their work than men, and lower prestige, knowledge, and expertise
are attributed to them as well (Martell, 1996; Seta & Seta, 1993; Swim &
Sanna, 1996).
JOB SEGREGATION
When we talk about equal pay for equal work, women in the workplace are
beginning to catch up. If we keep going at this current rate, we will achieve
full equality in about 475 years. I dont know about you, but I cant wait
that long.
Lya Sorano

Blau (1975) and Doyle and Paludi (1998) noted that job segregation
is one of the major causes for womens lower wages. Certain occupations (e.g., elementary school teacher, server, household worker) are
nearly totally female-populated and account for the majority of
employed women in the United States. These occupations represent an
occupational ghetto for women and are referred to as the pink collar
ghetto (Doyle & Paludi, 1998). Three explanations have been offered to
account for job segregation:
(1) the belief that womens biology prevents them from competing in the
work force for higher-paying jobs,
(2) the socialization of young girls to avoid higher status positions, and
(3) the various social or institutional forces that build barriers to prevent
women from entering high-paying and high-status occupations.

We will discuss two of these explanations in more detail: the socialization of women out of the marketplace and social and institutional barriers to women in higher paying occupations.
SOCIALIZATION OF WOMEN TO AVOID HIGH-STATUS
OCCUPATIONS
Kerrs research with gifted girls (e.g., Kerr, Foley-Nicpon, & Zapata,
2005) showed that although the girls career aspirations are as high as
their male peers, gifted girls are pressured to do an about face during adolescence, that is, to shift their goals from academic achievement
to romance. As Betz (2008) stated:
. . . by the sophomore year of college, gifted young women have likely
changed their majors to less challenging areas, by their senior year they
have reduced the level of their career goals, and by college graduation
they have given up their former career dreams altogether, all because of
the pervasive culture of romance. (p. 719)

164

Feminism as Human Rights

Kerrs research supports earlier studies, for example, by Golombok


and Fivush (1994). These researchers reported that by fourth or fth
grade, approximately 90 percent of positive feedback boys receive from
their teachers concerns their academic performance; less than 80
percent of positive feedback for girls is for their academic excellence.
In addition, Golombok and Fivush (1994) found that less than 33
percent of the negative feedback boys receive from their teachers is
related to their academic performance. However, more than 66 percent
of the negative feedback for girls is related to their academic performance. Golombok and Fivush interpreted these results as follows:
From this pattern of praise and criticism, boys may be learning that they
are smart, even if they are not very well behaved. Girls, on the other
hand, are learning that they may not be very smart, but that they can get
rewards by being good. (p. 173)

The American Association of University Women (2009; Haag, 1999)


found that secondary school systems direct girls and boys into different
courses by a differential tracking system whereby girls are taught to
think in terms of becoming nurses, secretaries, and mothers while boys
are taught to think about becoming physicians and engineers. Levy,
Sadovsky, and Troseth (2000) reported that a stereotypic view of the
world reinforces many of the common gender-role stereotypes and is a
major factor in prompting young boys interest in more than twice as
many occupations as that of young girls. Girls restrict their occupational aspirations. Furthermore, girls have a more limited concept than
boys do of the career possibilities available to them in STEM careers
(i.e., science, technology, engineering, and mathematics; Bernstein &
Russo, 2008). They focus on careers that are associated with less status,
less satisfaction, and less pay than careers considered by boys
(Heymann, 2000; Richardson & Sandoval, 2007).
Hall and Sandler (1982) referred to this tracking as a chilly climate
for girls and women. Chilling behaviors include:
Undermining girls and womens condence.
Discouraging girls and womens participation in class discussions.
Interrupting girls and women in class.
Dampening career aspirations of girls and women.
Preventing girls and women from seeking help outside of class.

Since the passage of Title IX, the gender gap at all grade levels has
decreased signicantly since 1970 in nationwide assessments of science
and math performance (Feminist Majority Foundation, 2009). In addition, womens share of undergraduate degrees in natural sciences and
engineering has more than doubled. Womens share of doctoral

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

165

degrees in these elds has more than quadrupled. However, women


still earn 20 to 25 percent of degrees in physics, computer sciences, and
engineering (Bernstein & Russo, 2007, 2008).
The culture of STEM elds still isolates and excludes girls and
women, which is directly related to womens lack of access to economic opportunity compared to men (Bernstein & Russo, 2007; 2008).
Women represent less than one in ve faculty members in STEM elds.
In engineering in particular, women account for just over one in ten
faculty members (Bernstein & Russo, 2007, 2008).
SOCIAL AND INSTITUTIONAL BARRIERS
Women are typically excluded from informal networks that are crucial to career advancement within the organizational culture. In addition, women tend to be viewed stereotypically within such
organizations, making it difcult for them to obtain important assignments that are required for advancement (Bagilhole & White, 2003).
Thus, male managers select men over comparably qualied women for
upper-level managerial positions (Hitt & Barr, 1989). Women are perceived as less valuable than men as a consequence of greater power,
status, and authority attributed to men (Kanter, 1977). Some (e.g.,
Keefe, 2003; Shakeshaft, Brown, Irby, Grogan, & Ballenger, 2007) argue
there is disparity about who receives mentoring; mentors tend to mentor those most like them: In a department dominated by white males,
white men get most of the mentoring. Women and minorities too often
fall through the cracks (p. 34).
Furthermore, advancement inequity exists between women and
men. Women in academics and business are underrepresented in senior positions and are promoted and tenured more slowly than men. In
the medical eld, only 12 percent of women have achieved the rank of
full professor; this gure has increased only 2 percent in the last 20
years. The gure for men is 30 percent (Mayer, Files, Ko, & Blair,
2008). In engineering, the gure is even lower; one percent. Mayer
et al. (2008) reported that, according to Harvard Business Review, women
make up only 6 percent of high ranking positions in Fortune 500 companies.
As Martin (2008) stated:
It is easier for men, with their informal informational networks, to successfully acclimate themselves. Women often have to compensate simply
for being women. . . . Women often face tremendous obstacles to gain acceptance in an organizational culture, especially in non-traditional elds.
They must deal with negative expectations, gender-role stereotypes, and
doing more than is expected to be seen as competent. They must try to
nd a mentor or a way into the informal networks that are often closed

166

Feminism as Human Rights

to them so that they can learn to navigate the hostile waters of organizational culture. (p. 172)

In addition, women may nd it difcult to secure a male mentor


because they nd them difcult to approach. Women also fear the
presumption of a sexual relationship by others in the organization.
Potential male mentors, on the other hand, may be hesitant to seek
out or accept a female protege because of the fear of a presumption
of a sexual relationship and/or the belief that women are not capable
of advancing on par with their male counterparts. If women are in
fact deemed as capable and worthy of mentoring, male mentors may
still choose men to mentor because it is simply more comfortable
and easier (Paludi, Martin, Stern, & DeFour, 2009). More difculties
may ensue when racialized gendered stereotypes interact with race
and gender roles and add additional barriers for women of color
(Buchanan, 2005).
As Ostroff and Atwater (2003) concluded:
. . . individuals prefer to associate with and identify with winners. . . .
the winners are those with status, power and higher salaries (i.e., men).
As such, those who nd themselves surrounded by women are not
among the winners and thus may be seen as less valuable. (p. 727)

ENFORCING PAY EQUITY LEGISLATION IN THE WORKPLACE:


HUMAN RESOURCE RESPONSES
I was told Indian women dont think like that about equality. But I would
like to argue that if they dont think like that they should be given a real opportunity to think like that.
Amartya Sen
The only way to solve the problem of womens subordination is to change peoples mindset and to plant the new idea of gender equality into every mind.
Qingrong Ma

As this review of research from the social sciences suggests, the existence of a wage gap between women and men exists even when controlling for demographics, performance, and other human capital
factors (Ostroff & Atwater, 2003). Empirical support is overwhelming
for gender and occupational stereotypes being expressed in managers
behavior, including beliefs that women should not earn as much as
men because men are the primary breadwinners, that women are not
as committed to their careers as are men and women are less productive than are men. In addition, women are viewed as laissez faire with
respect to negotiating salaries and wages (Holzer & Neumark, 2000).

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

167

Furthermore, Ostroff and Atwaters (2003) research found that individuals who work with women have lower wages. Thus managers who
supervise more women than men have lower wages than managers
who supervise more men than women. As Kim (2006) questioned:
Title VII has had an enormous impact in providing employment opportunities for women and allowing them to advance. Now, four decades after
Title VII, gender inequality in the U.S. labor market persists across racial
and ethnic groups, raising the question of whether true gender-based
workplace equality is possible. (p. 284)

However, the empirical ndings we discussed highlight the necessity for training programs for administrators and employees regarding
these stereotypes and the realities of women employees careers and
life responsibilities. Such training programs, identied as culture
change strategies (Brewer, 2000) would include several issues related
to exhibiting behaviors toward both sexes and to eradicating stereotypes about women and work.
In addition, administrators must take into consideration the items
discussed here.
PAY INEQUITY AND POVERTY ERADICATION
It is important to train employees and managers in understanding
that pay equity is a family issue. Research indicates that working families lose approximately 200 billion dollars in income each year due to
the wage gap between the sexes. In addition, if married women were
paid identically to men for identical work, they would have a 6 percent
increase in their familys income. Family poverty rates would fall from
2.1 percent to 0.8 percent (American Association of University Women,
2009).
Furthermore, single employed women would gain an increase in
family income; their families would have 17 percent more income
annually. The poverty rate for single employed women would
therefore be cut in half. Thus, pay equity is associated with poverty
eradication.
PAY EQUITY AND ENGAGED WORKFORCE
An engaged employee is an individual who is fully involved in and
enthusiastic about their work (Lockwood, 2007). Engaged employees
care about the growth of the organization and invest in assisting this
growth (Konrad, 2006). Employees who are engaged are those who
believe they have choices, and consequently they will act in a way that
furthers their organizations goals and values. Pay equity helps ensure

168

Feminism as Human Rights

a highly engaged and motivated workforce. The National Committee


on Pay Equity (2000) noted that organizations where womens wages
match their value creates a positive work environment. A positive
work environment is associated with increased morale, increased productivity, reduced absenteeism, decreased turnover rate, and reduced
sick leave.

WORK/LIFE INTEGRATION
Organizations that recognize the need and adapt work to employees
lives will win employees loyalty and thus have a competitive edge
(Eastman, 1998). Organizations with family-friendly policies report less
stress for employees, lower absenteeism, higher morale, positive publicity, improved work satisfaction, lower turnover rate, stafng over a
wide range of hours, child care hours that conform to work hours, and
access to quality infant, child, and elder care (Frone & Yardley, 1996;
Paludi & Neidermeyer, 2006).
Examples of family-friendly policies include extime, job sharing,
part-time work, telecommuting, intranet work, on-site child care, onsite health services, time off/career break, compressed work week, and
eldercare referral services. Examples of such policies are found in
Paludi and Paludi (2006). A relatively new program to assisting
employees with dealing with integrating work and life roles is total life
planning (Lockwood, 2003); that is, to assess their relationships, emotional health, careers, religiosity, nancial situation, and parenting
skills. Both Lockwood (2003) and Martinez (1997) have found that total
life planning is related to increased employee energy, enthusiasm for
work, and increased productivity. Miller (2005) identied that the top
work/life programs utilized by employees in the United States companies include the following: employee assistance programs, leave for
school functions, wellness programs, u shot programs, and tness
facilities. Miller (2005) also noted that the following work/life programs are rated highest by human resource specialists for reducing
unscheduled absences:
Alternative work arrangements
Flu shot programs
Leave for school functions
Telecommuting
Compressed workweek
On-site child care
Emergency child care
Employee assistance programs

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

169

Wellness programs
On-site health services
Fitness facility
Satellite workplaces
Job sharing
Eldercare services

HUMAN RESOURCE RECOMMENDATIONS


Recommendations have been offered to assist organizations in
enforcing equal pay legislation (e.g., Cornell University School of
Industrial and Labor Relations, Glass Ceiling Commission, 2000;
DeCenzo & Robbins, 2007; Mock, 2004; National Committee on Pay Equity, 2009; Society for Human Resource Management, 2009; Woo &
Khoo, 2006) including having organizations:
a. Examine their compensation system to ensure that women and people
of color are paid identically to men and white employees for identical
work. According to the Equal Pay Act, employers are required to
establish salaries based on skill, responsibility, effort, and working conditions.
b. Examine job grades to ensure that all employees have equal opportunity
for advancement.
c. Compare the compensation system with market values, taking into
account the market has undercompensated several occupations. Verify
that the data is accurate and match the data to the specic job descriptions to appropriately value each position.
d. Conduct job evaluations to determine the value of a position within an
organization with the goal of creating a more dened wage structure.
e. Develop job descriptions and position titles.
f. Post job openings with salary ranges within the workplace.
g. Encourage employees to openly discuss compensation issues with coworkers rather than fostering an organizational culture that intimidates
employees by making it taboo to discuss salaries.
h. Frequently conduct organizational cultural climate surveys in which
employees are anonymously asked to describe their perceptions and
experiences of discriminatory treatment with respect to compensation.
i. Conduct audits of all aspects of the organization, including compliance
with Equal Employment Opportunity, Americans with disabilities, and
Equal Pay Act regulations.
j. Dissemination of policies and procedures related to Equal Pay Act, ADA
and EEO regulations.
k. Facilitation of training on Equal Pay, EEO, and ADA policies and procedures.

170

Feminism as Human Rights


l. Compliance with compensation practices for effectiveness and consistency.

m. Compliance with an established grievance channel for employee concerns or complaints.

The audit will provide information for administrators on ways discrimination is operating in the organization. The next step is to understand the reason for the discriminatory treatment and to correct the
injustices (Ostroff & Atwater, 2003). In his review of gender and justice
with respect to pay equity, William Gaedert (2004) concluded that
Although continued research on gender, racial and ethnic biases is
warranted, meaningful solutions are most likely to be based on political action (p. 216). And as Mary Anderson once stated: Equal pay
for women is a matter of simple justice.
NOTE
1. The authors would like to acknowledge Eros DeSouza, Michelle Strand,
and Anthony Ferrari, Jr., who provided comments on earlier drafts of this
chapter.

REFERENCES
Acker, J. (1989). Doing comparable worth: Gender, class, and pay equity. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Agars, M. (2004). Reconsidering the impact of gender stereotypes on the
advancement of women in organizations. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
28, 103111.
American Association of University Women. (2009). AAUWs position on pay
equity. Retrieved February 16, 2008, from http://www.aauw.org/advocacy/issue_advocacy/actionpages/payequity.cfm.
Barbezat, D. (2002). History of pay equity studies. In R. Toutkoushian (Ed.),
Conducting salary-equity studies: Alternative approaches to research. New
Directions for Institutional Research (pp. 4968). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.
Bem, S. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354364.
Bergmann, B. (1974). Occupational segregation, wages and prots when
employers discriminate by race or sex. Eastern Economic Journal, 1, 103110.
Bernstein, B., & Russo, N.F. (2008). Explaining too few women in STEM
careers: A psychosocial perspective. In M. Paludi (Ed.), The psychology of
women at work (pp. 133). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Betz, N. (1993). Career development. In F. Denmark & M. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories (pp. 627684). Westport,
CT: Greenwood Press.
Betz, N. (2008). Career development. In F. Denmark & M. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories (pp. 717752). Westport,
CT: Greenwood Press.

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

171

Blau, F. (1975). Women in the labor force. In J. Freeman (Ed.), Women. Palo
Alto, CA: Mayeld.
Blau, F., & Kahn, L. (2000). Gender differences in pay. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 14, 2962.
Brewer, A. (2000). Work design for exible work scheduling: Barriers and gender implications. Gender, work, and organization, 7, 3344.
Browne, I., & Askew, R. (2006). Latinas at work: Issues of gender, ethnicity,
and class. In M. F. Karsten (Ed.), Gender, race and ethnicity in the workplace:
Issues and challenges for todays organizations (pp. 224251). Westport, CT:
Praeger.
Brunner, B. (2009). The wage gap: A history of pay inequity and the Equal Pay Act.
Retrieved February 16, 2009, from http://www.infoplease.com/spot/
equalpayact1.html.
Buchanan, N. (2005). The nexus of race and gender domination: The racialized
sexual harassment of African American women. In P. Morgan & J. Gruber
(Eds.), In the company of men: Re-discovering the links between sexual harassment and male domination (pp. 294320). Boston: Northeastern University
Press.
Cejka, M., & Eagly, A. (1999). Gender stereotypic images of occupations correspond to the sex segregation of employment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 413423.
Cleveland, J., Stockdale, M., & Murphy, K. (2000). Women and men in organizations: Sex and gender issues at work. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Coleman, M. (2003). Job skill and black male wage discrimination. Social Science
Quarterly, 84, 892905.
Cornell University School of Industrial and Labor Relations. (2000). About the
glass ceiling (pp. 12). Ithaca, NY: Glass Ceiling Commission.
Crampton, S., & Mishra, J. (1999). Women in management. Public Personnel
Management, 28, 87106.
Deaux, K., & Emswiller, T. (1974). Explanations of successful performance on
sex-linked tasks: Whats skill for the male is luck for the female. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 8085.
Deaux, K., & Taynor, J. (1973). Evaluation of male and female ability: Bias
works two ways. Psychological Reports, 32, 261262.
Decenzo, D., & Robbins, S. (2007). Fundamentals of human resource management.
New York: Wiley.
Dey, J., & Hill, (2007). Behind the pay gap. Washington, DC: AAUW Educational
Foundation.
Doyle, J., & Paludi, M. (1998). Sex and gender: The human experience (4th ed.).
New York: McGraw Hill.
Duff-McCall, K., & Schweinle, W. (2008). Leadership and women. In M. Paludi
(Ed.), The psychology of women at work (pp. 8799). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Eastman, W. (1998). Working for position: Women, men and managerial work
hours. Industrial Relations, 37, 5166.
England, P. (1992). Comparable worth: Theories and evidence. New York: Aldine
De Gruyter.
England P., Allison, P., Li, S., Mark N., Thompson, J., Budig, M., et al. (2004).
Why Are Some Academic Fields Tipping Toward Female? The Sex Composition
of US Fields of Doctoral Degree Receipt, 19711998.

172

Feminism as Human Rights

Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (2009). Equal pay and compensation discrimination. Retrieved February 15, 2009, from http://www.
eeoc.gov/types/epa.html
Etaugh, C., & Brown, B. (1975). Perceiving the causes of success and failure of
male and female performers. Developmental Psychology, 11, 103.
European Commission. (2007). Manual for gender mainstreaming of employment
policies. Retrieved April 3, 2009 from http://ec.europa.eu/employment_social/gender_equality/docs/2007.
European Industrial Relations Observatory On Line. (2002). Gender pay equity in
Europe. Retrieved April 3, 2009, from http://www.eurofound.europa.eu/
eiro/2002/01/study/tn0201101s.html
Feldman-Summers, S., & Kiesler, S. (1974). Those who are number two try
harder: The effect of sex on attributions of causality. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 30, 846855.
Fields, J., & Wolff, E. (1993). Industry wage differentials and the gender wage gap.
Working Papers 9325. New York: C. V. Starr Center for Applied Economics, New York University.
Fiske, S. (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. In D. Gilbert,
S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 357
414). New York: McGraw Hill.
Fiske, S., & Stevens, L. (1993). Whats so special about sex? Gender stereotyping and discrimination. In S. Oskamp & M. Costanzo (Eds.), Gender issues
in contemporary society (pp. 173196). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Frank, N., & Belasen, A. (2008). Feminist competing values leadership: An
investigation of the roles played by men and women. In M. Paludi (Ed.),
The psychology of women at work (pp. 101118). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Frone, M., & Yardley, J. (1996). Workplace family-supportive programmes: Predictors of employed parents importance ratings. Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology, 69, 351366.
Gaeddert, W. (2004). Gender, justice, and social change. In M. Paludi (Ed.),
Praeger guide to the psychology of gender (pp. 207218). Westport, CT:
Praeger.
Gattiker, U., & Cohen, A. (1997). Gender-based wage differences: The effects of
occupation and job segregation. Relations Industrielles/ Industrial Relations,
52, 507530.
Gee, M. V. (2006). Double jeopardy survival: Insights and lessons learned in
organizational battleelds. In M. F. Karsten (Ed.), Gender, race and ethnicity
in the workplace: Issues and challenges for todays organizations (pp. 185197).
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Gibelman, M. (2003). So how far have we come? Pestilent and persistent gender gap in pay. Social Work, 48, 2232.
Gilbert, L. A., & Kearney, L. (2006). Sex, gender, and dual-earner families:
Implications and applications for career counseling for women. In B.
Walsh & M. Heppner (Ed.), Handbook of career counseling for women (pp.
193218). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Glen, E., & Feldberg, R. (1977). Degraded and deskilled: The proletarianization
of clerical work. Social Problems, 25, 5264.
Goldberg, P. (1968). Are women prejudiced against women? Transaction, 5,
2830.

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

173

Golombok, S., & Fivush, R. (1994). Gender development. Cambridge: Cambridge


University Press.
Graddy, K., & Pistaferri, L. (2000). Wage differences by gender: Evidence from
recently graduated MBAs. Oxford Bulletin of Economics and Statistics, 62,
837854.
Greene, J. A. (2006). Blue-collar women in traditionally male jobs: Reconsidering the glass ceiling. In M. F. Karsten (Ed.), Gender, race and ethnicity in the
workplace: Issues and challenges for todays organizations (pp. 159178). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Haag, P. (1999). Voices of a generation: Teenage girls on sex, school, and self. Washington, DC: Project on the Status and Education of Women.
Hall, R., & Sandler, B. (1982). The classroom climate: A chilly one for women.
Washington, DC: Project on the Status and Education of Women.
Heilman, M., Block, C., Martell, R., & Simon, M. (1989). Has anything changed?
Current characterizations of men, women and managers. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 74, 935942.
Heilman, M., Wallen, A., Fuchs, D., & Tamkins, M. (2004). Penalties for success: Reactions to women who succeed at male gender-typed tasks. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 74, 935942.
Heyman, G., & Legare, C.H. (2004). Childrens beliefs about gender differences
in the academic and social domains. Sex Roles, 50, 227240.
Heymann, J. (Ed.). (2000). The widening gap: Why American working families are in
jeopardy and what can be done about it. New York: Basic Books.
Hill, J., Waldogel, J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Han, E. (2005). Maternal employment
and child development: A fresh look using newer methods. Developmental
Psychology, 41, 833850.
Hitt, M. A., & Barr, S. H. (1989). Managerial Selection Decision Models: Examination of Congural Cue Processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74,
5361.
Holzer, H., & Neumark, D. (2000). Assessing afrmative action. Journal of Economic Literature, 38, 483568.
Hyde, J.S. (1985). Half the human experience. Lexington, MA: Heath.
Issacs, M. (1981). Sex role stereotyping and the evaluation of the performance
of women: Changing trends. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 6, 187195.
Jacobson, M., & Effertz, J. (1974). Sex roles and leadership: Perception of the
leaders and the led. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 12,
383396.
Judiesch, M. K., & Lyness, K. S. (1999). Left behind? The impact of leaves of
absence on managers career success. Academy of Management Journal, 42,
641651.
Kanter, R. M. (1977). Work and family in the United States: A critical review and
agenda for research and policy. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Keefe, R. (2003, December). Practical tips for effective faculty mentoring.
Women in Higher Education. Retrieved April 3, 2009 from http://www.
ndarticles.com.
Kerr, B., Foley Nicpon, M., & Zapata, A. (2005). The development of talent in
girls and young women. In S. Robinson Kurplus, B. Kerr, & A. Harkins
(Eds.), Handbook for counseling girls and women: Talent, risk and resiliency (pp.
1540). Mesa, AZ: Nueva Science.

174

Feminism as Human Rights

Kim, J. (2006). Gender inequality in the U.S. labor market: Evidence from the
2000 census. In M. F. Karsten (Ed.), Gender, race and ethnicity in the workplace: Issues and challenges for todays organizations (pp. 270290). Westport,
CT: Praeger.
Kite, M., Deaux, K., & Haines, E. (2008). Gender stereotypes. In F. Denmark &
M. Paludi (Eds.), The psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories
(pp. 205236). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Konrad, A. (2006). Engaging employees through high-involvement work
practices. Ivey Business Journal Online. Retrieved February 16, 2008,
from
http://www.iveybusinessjournal.com/view_article.asp?intArticle_
ID617.
Konrad, A., Goldberg, C., Sullivan, S.E., & Yang, Y. (2005). Preferences for job
Attributes Associated with Work and Family: A Longitudinal Study. Sex
Roles, 53, 303315.
Ledbetter v. Goodyear Tire & Rubber Co, 550 U.S. 618 (2007).
Lepak, D., & Gowan, M. (in press). Human resource management: Managing
employees for competitive advantage. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Levy, G., Sadovsky, A., & Troseth, G. (2000). Aspects of young childrens perceptions of gender-typed occupations. Sex Roles, 14, 9931006.
Liben, L., Bigler, R., & Krogh, H. (2001). Pink and blue collar jobs: Childrens
judgments of job status and job aspirations in relation to sex of worker.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 346363.
Lockwood, N. (2003, June). Work/life balance: Challenges and solutions. Society
for Human Resource Management Research Quarterly. Retrieved August 2009
from
http://www.ispi.org/pdf/suggestedReading/11_Lockwood_Work
LifeBalance.pdf.
Lockwood, N. (2007, March). Leveraging employee engagement for competitive
advantage: HRs strategic role. HR Magazine. Retrieved August 2009 from
http://goliath.ecnext.com/coms2/gi_0199-6406683/Leveraging-employeeengagement-for-competitive.html.
Martell, R. (1996). What mediates gender bias in work behavior settings? Sex
Roles, 35, 153169.
Martin, J. (2008). Shifting the load: Personality factors and women in the workplace. In M. Paludi (Ed.), The psychology of women at work: Challenges and
solutions for our female workforce (pp. 153200). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Martinez, M. (1997, June). Work-life programs reap business benets. Society for
Human Resource Management, 10.
Mayer, A. P., Files, J. A., Ko, M. G., & Blair, J. E. (2008). Academic advancement of women in medicine: Do socialized gender differences have a role
in mentoring? Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 82, 204207.
McArthur, L., & Obrant, S. (2006). Sex biases in comparable worth analyses.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16, 757770.
Mednick, M., & Thomas, V. (1993). Women and the psychology of achievement: A view from the eighties. In F. Denmark & M. Paludi (Eds.), The
psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories (pp. 586626). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Mednick, M., & Thomas, V. (2008). Women and achievement. In F. Denmark &
M. Paludi (Eds.), The psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories
(pp. 625651). Westport, CT: Praeger.

Pay Equity as Justice: United States and International Perspectives

175

Miller, S. (2005, October). Work/life programs tackle unscheduled absenteeism:


Improve the bottom line. Society for Human Resource Management.
National Committee on Pay Equity. (1995). Retrieved April 3, 2009, from
http://www.pay-equity.org/PDFs/ProgressiveModels.pdf.
National Committee on Pay Equity. (2000). Questions and answers on pay
equity. Retrieved February 15, 2009, from http://www.pay-equity.org/
info-Q.&html.
National Committee on Pay Equity. (2009). Retrieved February 1, 2009, from
http://www.pay-equity.org.
Nieva, V., & Gutek, B. (1980). Sex effects on evaluation. Academy of Management
Review, 5, 267276.
Ostroff, C., & Atwater, L. (2003). Does whom you work with matter? Effects of
referent group gender and age composition on managers compensation.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 725740.
Paludi, M. A. (2002). The psychology of women (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River,
NJ: Prentice Hall.
Paludi, M., Martin, J., Stern, T., & DeFour, D. (2009). Promises and pitfalls of
mentoring women in business and academia. In C. Rayburn, F. Denmark,
M. Reuder, & A. Austria (Eds.), The Praeger handbook for women mentors:
Transcending barriers of stereotype, race, and ethnicity. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Paludi, M., & Neidermeyer, P. (Eds.), (2006). Work, life and family imbalance:
How to level the playing eld. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Paludi, M., & Paludi, C. (2006). Integrating work/life: Resources for employees,
employers, and human resource specialists. In M. Paludi & P. Neidermeyer. (Eds.). (2006). Work, life and family imbalance: How to level the playing
eld (pp. 122154). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Paludi, M., Vaccariello, R., Graham, T., Smith, M., Allen-Dicker, K., Kasprzak,
H., et al. (2006). Work/life integration: Impact on womens career, employment and family. In M. Paludi & P. Neidermeyer (Eds.), Work, life and family
imbalance: How to level the playing eld (pp. 2136). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Petersen, T., & Saporta, I. (2004). The opportunity structure for discrimination.
American Journal of Sociology, 109, 852901.
Powell, G., Buttereld, A., & Parent, J. (2002). Gender and managerial stereotypes: Have the times changed? Journal of Management, 28, 177193.
Raag, T., & Rackliff, C. (1998). Preschoolers awareness of social expectations of
gender: Relationships to toy choices. Sex Roles, 38, 685700.
Reilly, K., & Wirjanto, T. (1999). Does more mean less? The male/female wage
gap and the proportion of females at the establishment level. Canadian
Journal of Economics, 32, 906929.
Richardson, B., & Sandoval, P. (2007). Impact of education on gender equity in
employment and its outcomes. In S. Klein (Ed.), Handbook for achieving gender equity through education. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rudin, J., & Byrd, K. (2003). U.S. pay equity legislation: Sheep in wolves clothing. Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 15, 183190.
Rynes, S., Gerhart, B., & Parks, L. (2005). Personnel psychology: Performance
evaluation and pay for performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 56,
571600.
Sen, A. (2005). Many faces of gender inequality. Retrieved April 3, 2009 from
http://www.nu.or.cr/pnud/docs/Sen-man.pdf

176

Feminism as Human Rights

Seta, J. J., & Seta, C. E. (1993). Stereotypes and the generation of compensatory
and noncompensatory expectancies of group members. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 722731.
Shakeshaft, C., Brown, G., Irby, B., Grogan, M., & Ballenger, J. (2007). Increasing gender equity in educational leadership. In S. Klein (Ed.), Handbook for
achieving gender equity through education (2nd ed., pp. 103129). Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sinangil, H. K., & Ones, D. S. (2003). Gender differences in expatriate job performance. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 52, 461475.
Society for Human Resource Management. Retrieved April 3, 2009, from
http://www.shrm.org
Soumeli, E., & Nergaard, K. (2002). Gender pay equity in Europe. European
Industrial Relations Observatory On-line. Retrieved April 3, 2009, from
http://www.eiro.eurofound.ie/2002/01/TN0201101S.html
Steinberg, J., True, M., & Russo, N.F. (2008). Work and family roles: Selected
issues. In F. Denmark & M. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: A handbook
of issues and theories (pp. 652700). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Strassel, K., Colgan, C., & Goodman, J. (2006). Leaving women behind: Modern
families, outdated laws. New York: Rowman & Littleeld Publishers.
Swim, J., Borgida, E., Maruyama, G., & Myers, D. G. (1989). Joan McKay versus
John McKay: Do gender stereotypes bias evaluations? Psychological Bulletin,
105, 409429.
Swim, J., & Sanna, L. (1996). Hes skilled, shes lucky: A meta-analysis of
observers attributions for womens and mens successes and failures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 507519.
Taynor, J., & Deaux, K. (1975). Equity and perceived sex differences: Role
behavior as dened by the task, the mode, and the actor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 381390.
U.S. Bureau of the Census (2008). Retrieved April 1, 2009, from http://
www.census.gov/compendia/statab.
Ward, C. (1979). Differential evaluation of male and female expertise: Prejudice
against women? British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 6569.
Ward, C. (1981). Prejudice against women: Who, when and why? Sex Roles, 7,
163171.
Weiner, B., Frieze, I., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R. (1971). Perceiving the causes of success and failure (pp. 173177). Morristown, NJ:
General Learning Press.
Werhane, P., Posig, M., Gundry, L., Powell, E., Carlson, J., & Ofstein, L. (2006).
Women leaders in corporate America: A study of leadership values and
methods. In M. F. Karsten (Ed.), Gender, race and ethnicity in the workplace:
Issues and challenges for todays organizations (pp. 229). Westport, CT:
Praeger.
Wiley, M. G., & Eskilson, A. (1985). Speech style, gender stereotypes, and corporate success: What if women talk more than men? Sex Roles, 12,
9931007.
Woo, D., & Khoo, G. (2006). Corporate culture and leadership: Traditional,
legal and charismatic authority. In M. F. Karsten (Ed.), Gender, race and ethnicity in the workplace: Issues and challenges for todays organizations (pp.
80111). Westport, CT: Praeger.

Chapter 8

Rape: A Global Perspective


Michelle McKenzie
Patricia Rozee

Rape is a global pandemic, existing in every culture around the world


(Rozee, 1993). Many social structures continue a pattern of rape and often excuse the practices as normative sexual behaviors. Rape is a manifestation of womens subordinate status to men. When communities
strictly dene gender roles in which masculinity is associated with dominance, toughness, and male honor, sexual assault is reinforced (Heise,
Ellsberg, & Gottemoeller, 2002). In cultures where men have ownership
of women, and men must control their environment, rape is often condoned (Heise et al., 2002; Sanday, 1981). Other factors associated with violence against women include economic inequality, physical violence to
resolve conicts, male authority over decision making in the home, and
divorce restrictions on women (Heise et al., 2002; Levinson, 1989). Cultures with lower rates of rape and acceptance of rape exhibit more equality
between the sexes, where the sexes are complementary and dependant on
each other (Sanday, 1981). When women are considered subservient to
men, rape continues its pervasive pattern of psychologically, economically, and physically injuring women and their families.
In an international effort to combat rape and other forms of violence
against women, legal reform and funding for community prevention
has been implemented within the past 15 years. In 1993, The United
Nations (UN) Declaration on the Elimination of Violence against Women
dened violence against women as:
Physical, sexual and psychological violence occurring in the family,
including battering, sexual abuse of female children in the household,

178

Feminism as Human Rights

dowry-related violence, marital rape, female genital mutilation and other


traditional practices harmful to women, non-spousal violence and violence related to exploitation; physical, sexual and psychological violence
occurring within the general community, including rape, sexual abuse,
sexual harassment and intimidation at work, in education institutions
and elsewhere, trafcking in women and forced prostitution; physical,
sexual and psychological violence perpetrated or condoned by the State,
wherever it occurs. (UN, 1993, Article 3)

More recently, in June 2008, the UN Security Council ofcially stated


that sexual violence is a threat to international peace and security. Even
though many denitions of sexual violence exist, a comprehensive denition would incorporate all forms of sexual assault:
Any sexual act, attempt to obtain a sexual act, unwanted sexual comments or advances, or acts to trafc, or otherwise directed, against a persons sexuality using coercion, by any person regardless of their
relationship to the victim, in any setting, including but not limited to
home and work. (Krug, Dahlberg, Mercy, Zwi, & Lozano, 2002)

This chapter explores the prevalence of sexual assault worldwide


and is organized according to an ecological model that forces us to
consider the dynamic interplay between the individual and the environment. Ahrens and Rozee (2007) stress that social issues such as rape
cannot be accurately examined without considering how the context of
individual, family and small group, organizational, community, and
societal levels impact on both etiology and intervention. Thus, the
chapter is divided into sections that address violence based on the
three levels that maintain a culture of rape. Individual, organizational,
and cultural factors that maintain a culture of rape will be addressed,
including topics such as rape in the military, armed conict, and sex
trafcking. The main objective of this chapter is to raise awareness of
the widespread human rights issue of rape and discuss prevention
measures involving community, social, and global solutions. This chapter will not discuss children as victims of sexual assault or the psychological, social, or physical impact on rape survivors, which is
addressed by Waits and Lundberg Love in Volume II.

PREVALENCE
Rape prevalence varies among countries and cultures. No communities are completely rape free, but some have signicantly higher rates
than others. Specic types of rape can have higher incidence depending on the cultural ideology, economic disparity, or current political situation. Certain regions may have high rates of rape by strangers due

Rape: A Global Perspective

179

to armed conict, while others have higher rates of marital rape due to
cultural structures.
Garcia-Moreno, Jansen, Ellsberg, Heise, and Watts (2005) conducted
a cross-cultural study of 10 countries to determine the prevalence of violence against women. They found a vast difference among the countries based on the percentage of women who had experienced sexual
abuse by an intimate partner: Ethiopia (59 percent), Bangladesh (50 percent), the United Republic of Tanzania (31 percent), Thailand (30 percent), Peru (23 percent), Samoa (20 percent), Namibia (16 percent),
Brazil (14 percent), Serbia and Montenegro (6 percent), and Japan
(6 percent). Sexual violence by nonpartners was reported at a much
lower rate: Peru, Samoa, and the United Republic of Tanzania ranged
from 10 to 12 percent, but levels as low as 1 percent were reported in
Bangladesh and Ethiopia (Garcia et al., 2005). Common nonintimate
perpetrators were male family members, teachers, boyfriends, strangers, and male friends of the family (Garcia et al., 2005).
Ethnographic studies of nonindustrial societies show different prevalence rates depending on how rape is dened, ranging from 42 to 90
percent of cultures (Rozee, 1993; Rozee-Koker, 1987). Rozee (1993)
denes non-normative rape as rape that is illicit, not condoned, against
the will of the woman, and in violation of social norms. Most studies
have used Western common law denitions of rape, which tend to
underestimate rapes that do not t that legal denition. Most studies
do not include normative rape, which are rapes that are not dened as
rape because they are accepted within the normative practices of the
culture.
The rst study to use a broadened denition of rape so as to rate
the behavior independently of social sanctions found a prevalence rate
of 90 percent (Minturn, Grosse, & Haider, 1969). The most comprehensive cross-cultural study to expand previous denitions by including
rapes that occur in the context of ceremonies (such as ritual deoration) and rape of enemy women found rape in 53 percent of the sample societies (Sanday, 1981). Sandays rate was lower partly because
she coded societies where rape was reported as rare, as rape-free
cultures. Rozee studied rape in 35 world cultures and found that rape
occurred in all cultures when normative rape was included in the statistics. She also found that in a majority of cultures normative and nonnormative rape occur concurrently, concluding that rape is therefore
regulated, not prohibited, in most cultures.
One form of normative rape is rape as a form of punishment for
social transgression or as used in a disciplinary manner. Punitive rape
generally occurs as a male response to females who behave in ways
considered solely the prerogative of males, for violations of male
authority, for rejecting a male who has legitimate sexual access to her
(such as a husband), or sometimes as the instrument of punishment for

180

Feminism as Human Rights

a husbands transgressions (Rozee, 1993). Rozee reports punitive rape


as present in 14 percent of cultures. Punitive rape is also used as punishment for crimes such as adultery, public drunkenness, or political
afliation (Amnesty International, 2004; Krug et al., 2002). In Peru,
women who were believed to be afliated with the opposition armed
group, Sendero Luminoso, were sexually assaulted by their opposer
and then encountered further incidents of sexual assault in jail (Amnesty International, 2004). In Zimbabwe, certain cultures allow a man
who has committed a crime against a family or raped a woman to either pay compensation or marry the woman instead of receiving punishment for the crimes (Equality Now, 1998).
Rape is often practiced as a part of various cultural ceremonies, such
as ritual deorations or virginity rituals. Virginity tests often contain
an element of force and may entail hasty and painful penetration of a
virgin to meet the requirement of public demonstration of the hymenal
blood as proof of virginity (Olson, 1981).
Rape as a form of exchange is another ritualized form of rape practiced in many world cultures. In this case sexual access to females is
granted as a bargaining tool, gesture of solidarity, or conciliation.
Among the Kaska of Canada, a man would often offer his wife if he
lost in betting and she would be required to stay with the winner until
the debt was repaid (Honigman, 1954). Collier and Rosaldo (1981)
report the common practice of according sexual access to women as a
gesture of goodwill or solidarity.
Normative rapes are rarely reported as rape. Even within the relatively narrow (non-normative) Western legal denition of rape, sexual
assault remains one of the most underreported crimes globally. Studies
in South Africa show that only 15 percent of women report their
assault to the police (Jewkes & Abrahams, 2002). This percentage tends
to be much lower in cultures that ascribe strictly to patriarchal structures. Other factors are also involved in whether women report rape.
Jewkes and Abrahams (2002) also found that women with post-school
education were eight times more likely to report rape to the police,
and younger women were more likely to report their assault to the
police. Since underreporting is the norm and many women still do not
disclose in research interviews, statistics given in this report are likely
to be underrepresented.

INDIVIDUAL FACTORS ASSOCIATED WITH RAPE


Womens Vulnerability to Rape
Rape can happen to women of all ages, racial backgrounds, and
socioeconomic statuses. It is important to note that the main predictor
of being a rape victim is genderbeing a woman (Rozee & Koss,

Rape: A Global Perspective

181

2001). However, research has identied several factors that can increase
a womans vulnerability to sexual assault, such as youth, poverty, previous sexual abuse, multiple sexual partners, or alcohol use. Understanding that rape prevalence is higher among certain groups of
women can assist in focusing prevention programs and resistance
training.
Although women can be raped at any age, young women are at a
higher risk of rape in countries around the world (Garcia-Moreno et
al., 2005). Statistics from Chile, Malaysia, Mexico, New Guinea, Peru,
and the United States show that anywhere from one-third to two-thirds
of sexual assaults are committed against women under 17 years old
(Heise, Pitanguy, & Germain, 1994). In the United States, Pazzani
(2007) found that younger women were more likely to be sexually
assaulted than older women by both strangers and acquaintances.
Although marital rape is a considerable problem, sexual assault survivors in the United States are less likely to be married, which may also
correspond with age (Avakame, 1999; Golding, Wilsnack, & Cooper,
2002). Young women are also at particular risk for certain types of sexual violence, including sex trafcking, female genital mutilation, and
school related sexual assault.
Poverty has been linked to many rape situations. Women with lower
economic status are more likely to be coerced in sex trafcking, victimized in armed conict and refugee camps, and raped by their partners (Krug et al., 2002; U.S. Department of State, 2005). Yodanis (2004)
found that rates of sexual assault were signicantly higher in countries
where women had lower educational and occupational status than in
countries where women had higher status. Byrne, Resnick, Kilpatrick,
Best, and Saunders (1999) reported that American women living at or
below the poverty level were at heightened risk for victimization.
Additionally, they observed that women living above the poverty level
were more likely to drop below the poverty level after revictimization.
Education seems to be a good economic buffer from rape. GarciaMoreno et al. (2005) found that in many countries, women with higher
education confronted less violence, and in certain countries, such as
urban Brazil, Namibia, Peru, Thailand, and the United Republic of
Tanzania, the lowered risk of sexual violence is evident for women
who continue their education beyond secondary school. In the United
States, working women and women with higher income are also less
likely to be sexually assaulted by a stranger or an acquaintance
(Avakame, 1999; Pazzani, 2007).
Survivors of sexual assault are also at a greater risk of revictimization. Studies in the United States reported that childhood physical or
sexual assault could lead to adult sexual assault (Cloitre, Tardiff,
Marzuk, & Leon, 1996; Maker, Kemmelmeier, & Peterson, 2001; Messman-Moore & Long, 2000). Pazzani (2007) found that the relationship

182

Feminism as Human Rights

between revictimization and previous childhood or adulthood sexual


assault was primarily present when women were raped by acquaintances. In an experiment on prevention education, American college
women with a history of sexual assault had substantially more reports
of rape at three months follow-up, whether they received prevention
education or not (Gidycz et al., 2001). Many factors may contribute to
this cycle of revictimization, including self-blame (Miller, Markman, &
Handley, 2007), delayed response to danger cues (Messman-Moore &
Brown, 2006), acceptance of rape myths (Messman-Moore & Long,
2000), low self-esteem, and learned helplessness (Finkelhor & Browne,
1985). Survivors also report less frequent contact with friends and
family, as well as less emotional support (Golding et al., 2002), which
may make them more vulnerable to repeated sexual coercion. Prevention efforts should be aware of this pattern in order to reach women
who may not be seeking help but nonetheless may need it.
Finally, alcohol consumption is a major situational factor that has
been identied in U.S. studies to be connected to rape. Norris and Cubbins (1992) found that women were less likely to believe stories of rape
if both parties involved were intoxicated. Yet, if the woman only was
intoxicated, participants perceptions of the incident shifted toward
rape. In a study by Cue, George, and Norris (1996), women did not
perceive alcohol to increase their risk or other womens risk for rape.
However, approximately half of sexual assaults include alcohol consumption by the survivor, perpetrator, or both (Abbey, Zawacki, Buck,
Clinton, & McAuslan, 2004). In a study by Harrington and Leitenberg
(1994), 55 percent of women reported being somewhat intoxicated
when sexually assaulted, and many of the women engaged in consensual sexual activities prior to the rape. More than 20 percent of American college women reported that they were raped while under the
inuence of alcohol or drugs (Kalof, 1993). In the same study, 47 percent of sorority women reported rape while under the inuence of
alcohol, although many factors, such as attending more drinking events
or afliating with fraternity men who hold rape-congruent attitudes,
may be related to this large difference.
The effects of alcohol can create an environment where perpetrators
can take advantage of womens alcohol-induced passivity and loss of
control. Survivors who were raped while heavily intoxicated report
lower levels of assertive and cautious responses to sexual advances
(Macy, Nurius, & Norris, 2007). In a study by Norris et al. (2006),
intoxicated female participants were asked to project their behaviors
into a rape scenario. Alcohol consumption increased their level of consent and passivity to a sexually aggressive man. Furthermore, when
women were perceived to be in a relationship, they were more likely to
be passive at the onset of rape, as opposed to those who were perceived
to be in initial stages of dating. With the inhibiting qualities of alcohol

Rape: A Global Perspective

183

consumption, women may be at a higher risk of rape because they react


more slowly and men can use this vulnerability to commit rape.
Characteristics of Male Perpetrators
Men rape for many different reasons, inuenced by their attitudes,
personality characteristics, and life experiences (Abbey et al., 2004).
Internationally, sexual assault is linked to men and communities that
strictly dene and enforce gender roles that associate masculinity with
dominance, honor, and toughness (Heise, 1998). A shocking 35 percent
of U.S. college men revealed they would rape if there was no fear of
punishment, which indicates that a lot of men have an attraction to
sexual aggression (Malamuth, 1981; Malamuth, 1989), but men also
may be able to act on these aggressive attitudes more often in communities where men are rarely accountable for raping women.
False beliefs about rape or about rape victims seem to contribute to
rape acceptance. Rape myths are attitudes or beliefs that are generally
false but are widely and persistently held, and that serve to deny and
justify male sexual aggression against women (Lonsway & Fitzgerald,
1994, p. 134). Many different measurement scales have been used to
evaluate rape myth acceptance. Some myths imply that women deserve
rape, such as, A woman who goes to the home or apartment of a man
on the rst date is implying that she wants to have sex (Payne, Lonsway, & Fitzgerald, 1999, p. 50). Other myths support the claim that
men are unable to control themselves, for instance, When a man is
very sexually aroused, he may not even realize that the woman is
resisting (p. 49). Finally, myths may blame women for rape, like in
this statement, Usually, it is only women who do things like hang out
in bars and sleep around that are raped (p. 49). Men with greater acceptance of rape myths are more likely to engage in sexually coercive
behaviors (Senn, Desmarais, Verberg, & Wood, 2000). Studies of rape
myths have delved into the attitudes related to sexual aggression, yet
personality characteristics help to further understand these attitudes.
Factors that increase the proclivity of men to rape include life experiences (childhood sexual abuse, witnessing family violence, or having
a rejecting father), preference for impersonal sex, hostility toward
women, antisocial tendencies, coercive sexual fantasies, and alcohol or
drug use (Heise et al., 2002; Krug et al., 2002; Malamuth, Linz, Heavey,
Barnes, & Acker, 1995; Malamuth, Sockloskie, Koss, & Tanaka, 1991).
In the United States, Malamuth and his colleagues (Malamuth et al.,
1991; Malamuth et al., 1995) demonstrated an additive effect, which
reveals that each factor alone may not inuence a man to rape, but several factors together can increase the likeliness of rape. The Conuence
Model of sexual aggression identies two major paths that inuence
sexual aggression (Malamuth et al., 1991; Malamuth et al., 1995). The

184

Feminism as Human Rights

rst, hostile masculinity path is dened as an insecure, defensive,


hypersensitive, and hostile-distrustful orientation, particularly toward
women, and gratication from controlling or dominating women
(Malamuth et al., 1995, pp. 353354). This path assesses personality
characteristics related to coercion of women, such as negative masculinity, hostility toward women, and adversarial sexual beliefs. The second path, impersonal sex is a noncommittal, game-playing
orientation in sexual relations (p. 354), including variables such as
early sexual experiences, number of sexual partners, extramarital
affairs, and sexual fantasies. When men were characterized by both of
these paths, they expressed the highest levels of sexual aggression in
cross-sectional (Malamuth et al., 1991) and longitudinal studies (Malamuth et al., 1995).
Similar studies in Spain show results in support of the Conuence
Model (Martin, Vergeles, de la Orden Acevedo, del Campo Sanchez, &
Visa, 2005). Their study also found that men who were low in empathy
reported more sexually coercive behaviors. Yost and Zurbriggen (2006)
found that men with many sexual partners who endorsed casual sex
were more likely to hold negative and stereotypical attitudes about
women. As these coercive and stereotypical attitudes increased, men
were more likely to perpetrate sexual aggression. In a longitudinal
study, American men who perpetrated at two time points had more
hostility toward women, past sexual experiences, adolescent delinquency, and drinking in sexual situations (Abbey & McAuslan, 2004).
Abbey and McAuslan also found that perpetrators only at the initial
time point showed more remorse about what they did, claimed that
they learned from the experience and blamed the women less than the
repeat offenders. Although the attitudes and personalities of perpetrators have been examined, changing their mindset is a daunting task.
ACTIONS FOR MEN
Although the majority of men are not perpetrators of sexual violence, their role in eliminating rape is fundamental. Men act as role
models for other men and boys through their many responsibilities
such as father, teacher, friend, coworker, or boss. They can have a
strong impact on ending gender inequality through their words and
actions, particularly social scripts that endorse sexual violence, but also
through direct dialogue with other men about the detrimental effects
of violence against women. Although these themes should resonate in
community organizations, the focus on stopping men from sexual violence should not be fear of punishment, but the desire for women and
men to have healthy relationships and mutual respect.
Prevention programs need to include mens organizations, churches,
male community representatives, and governments, institutions where

Rape: A Global Perspective

185

men tend to have dominating roles. Many different male-focused


groups have formed around the world concentrating on changing gender norms, including attitudes related to masculinity, hostility toward
women, and interpersonal violence. In Latin America, especially mens
associations in Nicaragua, men work to reduce violence against women
by confronting issues of masculinity and aggression (Ward, 2002).
In Uganda, the Be a Man Campaign has many forums to connect men
with other men to discuss violence against women (Widyono, 2006).
They facilitate discussion meetings within communities, hold public
debates, and sponsor television, radio, and other media events to develop positive male attitudes, and encourage healthy relationships, responsible fatherhood, and nonviolent means for resolving conict.
These organizations make men allies with women in the ght for gender equality and can have major impact within communities.

ORGANIZATIONAL SUPPORTS OF RAPE


Organizational supports for rape add to the complexity of identifying and eliminating rape in all societies. In this section, we discuss sex
trafcking, military rape, rape of enemy women in wartime, and rape
of refugees. Each presents organizational challenges that will require
organization-wide commitments to prevent and eliminate.
Sex Trafcking
Sex trafcking involves cooperation among various segments of society and across cultures, each with nancial incentives that make it particularly difcult to eradicate. The trafcking of women and children
for sexual exploitation is a form of exchange rape. It is the third most
lucrative criminal activity in the world, following illegal drugs and
arms, generating approximately $7 to $32 billion annually (U.S. Department of State, 2009). These widespread human rights violations are a
form of modern-day slavery. It is difcult to accurately assess the number of women being trafcked internationally or within their own
countries, but government agencies and eld workers estimate anywhere between 700,000 and 2 million women and children annually
(Richard, 2000).
The UNs human trafcking denition states:
Trafcking in persons shall mean the recruitment, transportation,
transfer, harbouring or receipt of persons, by means of the threat or use
of force or other forms of coercion, of abduction, of fraud, of deception,
of the abuse of power or of the position of vulnerability or of the giving
or receiving of payments or benets to achieve the consent of a person
having control over another person, for the purpose of exploitation.

186

Feminism as Human Rights

Exploitation shall include, at a minimum, the exploitation of the prostitution of others or other forms of sexual exploitation, forced labour or services, slavery or practices similar to slavery, servitude or the removal of
organs. (UN, 2000, Annex II, Article 3)

The extent of involvement varies between countries, but in 2006, the


UN Ofce on Drugs and Crime Database (UNODC) reported trafcking from 127 source countries to 137 destination countries. Source
countries with the highest rates of trafcking include Albania, Bulgaria,
China, Lithuania, Nigeria, Romania, Russia, Thailand, and Ukraine
(UNODC, 2006). These source countries tend to be economically or
politically unstable, where women have few employment opportunities. Through many different recruiting practices, women are transported to the wealthiest countries in the world. The highest reported
destination countries are Belgium, Germany, Greece, Israel, Italy, Japan,
Netherlands, Thailand, Turkey, and the United States of America
(UNODC, 2006). Women are also trafcked within their own countries,
from rural areas to cities. Trafcking within the country is common in
Argentina, China, Cote dIvoire, Cuba, Guinea, India, Republic of
Congo, Russian, South Africa, and Zimbabwe (Central Intelligence
Agency, 2008). The different trafcking routes are numerous and
continuously changing. The only consistency is that women from
poverty-stricken countries are most at risk of being trafcked to primarily wealthy Western countries.
Trafckers use different means for coercing their victims, usually
abusing womens economic vulnerability. Steven Galster, of Global Survival Network, reports in a hearing before the Commission on Security
and Cooperation in Europe (1999) that he witnessed three types of
women being trafcked for sex.
Some women are completely unaware that they were being trafcked for sexual exploitation; others thought they would be doing
something related to the industry, such as stripping; and others knew
that they were going to be prostitutes. None of the women had any
idea of the impeding abuse or that slavery would be imposed on them.
Inuences that make women more susceptible to trafcking are poverty and lack of education, but also the desire for a better life for themselves and their families (U.S. Department of State, 2005). Most women
believe the promises of good paying jobs, such as nannies, models, secretaries, dancers, and maids, while some families believe they are marrying their daughter to a wealthy, honest man (Farr, 2005). Farr (2005)
describes how structural closeness of the recruiter to the woman
encourages her trust in his promises (e.g., the recruiter is a relative or
family friend). Other forceful means of obtaining women are used,
such as kidnapping or drugging (Farr, 2005). A study in China showed
the main methods for trafcking recruitment were through fraud and

Rape: A Global Perspective

187

deception, 37 percent; kidnapping, 26 percent; abuse of power or a


position of vulnerability, 17 percent; and physical violence, 5 percent
(UN Inter-Agency Project on Human Trafcking, 2008).
Once women are recruited, captors utilize many means to psychologically and physically detain their victims. Since most captors have
the same cultural background as their victims, this puts them at an
advantage to exploit their vulnerabilities. In Italy, Nigerian women
believing they are being sponsored for work are trafcked into prostitution. Their exploiters are their countrymen who use their knowledge
of Nigerian culture to psychologically trap them. These Nigerian
women are forced to perform black magic rites referred to as juju rites
where different bodily uids or pieces of clothing are placed before an
altar, while the women make vows to never reveal their captors and
pay off their debts without any problems (Aghatise, 2004). The way
that their captors use these juju rites enforce major psychological control over these women, who believe that great harm will come to them
or their families if they dishonor their vows (Aghatise, 2004).
In other cultures where psychological control will not work, violence
and threats become the main tactic for keeping women in fear. Violence used against these women includes the following: severe verbal
and physical abuse, rape, burning, forced prostitution even when ill,
menstruating, or pregnant, and murder (Aghatise, 2004; Farr, 2005).
Since most trafcked women originate from underdeveloped or poor
countries, where corruption can be rampant among government ofcials, many of these women distrust law enforcement ofcials. Their
captors will often withhold passports, and with little or no knowledge
of the laws and customs of their destination, they fear being caught by
the authorities and imprisoned for prostitution. They also believe that
their families will be in danger of retaliation by their captors if they
run away.
Many solutions are underway to prevent sex trafcking. Numerous
laws have been passed by the UN in the past decade in a collaborative
effort between countries to not only become aware of this human rights
violation but to assure womens protection when rescued. The main
goal of recent legislation is to work together in order to support victims
and convict trafckers. Since the pressure of keeping up with other
types of criminal activity is burdensome for certain governmental sectors, specialized task forces for sexual trafcking would give the issue
more direct attention. Training courses for police ofcers, immigration
authorities, customs ofcers, government ofcials, and media personnel
are also necessary to educate workers who come into contact with survivors of trafcking.
Besides legislation and trafcking detection, community efforts are
being made to prevent sex trafcking. Mu Sochua, Cambodias former
Minister of Womens and Veterans Affairs, has made crucial efforts to

188

Feminism as Human Rights

ght against sex trafcking in Cambodia. Since many trafcked women


in Cambodia come from rural areas, she visited these regions to discuss with poverty-stricken families the problems of selling their teenage daughters (Mithers, 2004). Sochua put together a major media
campaign to change the way people view gender equality by changing
a Cambodian proverb from A man is gold, a woman is a white piece of
cloth to Men are gold, women are precious gems (Mithers, 2004).
When women were referred to as white pieces of cloth, a metaphor that
promotes virginity, men dened their status. Changing the proverb promotes gender equality and reduces the importance of female virgin status.
When Mu Sochua visits rural areas, she says to the villagers:
Your children are precious. If you let your daughters go, your family
heritage is gone. They are your gems. Love them. Educate them. Protect
them. Everywhere I go, women come up to me and say, Now I realize
what you mean. Find my daughter! When that happens, we try to get
word out. Maybe 10 percent of those girls we nd. (Mithers, 2004)

This type of education is necessary to inform women and their families about sex trafcking. Prevention campaigns need to target the cultural values that reinforce sexual exploitation. The media can play an
important role in graphically depicting the reality of the dream jobs
that women nd and giving access to resources to protect themselves.
MILITARY RAPE
In this section we discuss the rape of women serving in the military,
rape of civilian women in warring countries, the rape of civilian
women to enforce group cohesion, and the use of women to provide
comfort for soldiers. In each case women seem to be the pawns of wars
caused and carried out by men.
Women have served in militaries around the world for centuries.
Their historical roles, which were conned to medical or administrative
support, have extended to active duty in the past 50 years. Even
though most countries have regulations regarding tasks women can
hold, such as not engaging in active combat positions, women are still
alongside men in dangerous combat zones. Women have continually
increased their representation in military forces globally. Within the
military dynamic, rape is commonplace and survivors are pressured to
remain silent, principally to protect group morale and cohesion. Despite this compelling force, women in the military have spoken out
about sexual assault by their fellow soldiers and commanders.
In most countries, rape prevalence rates are difcult to ascertain for
women active in the military. Research is limited, with most available
data coming from the U.S. military. In 2008, the Chair of the Homeland

Rape: A Global Perspective

189

Security Intelligence Subcommittee, Jane Harman said, Women serving in the U.S. military are more likely to be raped by a fellow soldier
than killed by enemy re in Iraq (Harman, 2008). In the U.S. military,
rape prevalence is higher than in the general population, yet reporting
and prosecution of perpetrators is lower (Harman, 2008). The West Los
Angeles Veterans Association Health Center informed Harman that 41
percent of women soldiers receiving health care at their facilities were
survivors of sexual assault, and 29 percent were raped during their
military service (Harman, 2008). Study results range from 23 to 30 percent of women in the military reporting rape by other military members, including 5 percent of women experiencing repeated rapes and 5
percent experiencing gang rapes (Hankin et al., 1999; Sadler, Booth, &
Doebbeling, 2005; Sadler, Booth, Nielson, & Doebbeling, 2000; Skinner
et al., 2000). In the U.S. Air Force, military perpetrators accounted for
14 percent of rst-time rapes and 26 percent of most recent rapes
(Bostock & Daley, 2007).
These statistics are higher than the general population, yet the actual
numbers are probably much higher. Underreporting rape is a common
trend in society but is even more likely in the military. Female soldiers
have many concerns that prevent them from reporting, including lack
of trust for the criminal justice system, fear of reprisal from their offender, fear that the chain of command will not believe their complaint
or act upon it, and reports will have a negative impact on their reputation and career advancement (Embrey et al., 2004). Militaries promote
collective attitudes, comradeship, and group cohesion. Ironically,
reporting rape may be viewed as undermining the group morale or
failing the group (Jeffreys, 2007) yet the rape itself is not!
Most of these womens fears represent the reality of military
responses to rape. Reported sexual assaults increased to 2,908 in 2008,
an 8 percent rise from 2007, and almost half of reports that were
referred to commanders to take action on behalf of the survivor
resulted in no responses by the commanders (Department of Defense,
2008, 2009). Only 15 percent of reported rapes were referred to Court
Martial in which a judge or committee would hear survivors cases
and perpetrators could receive adequate punishment. However, instead
of referring rape reports to Court Martial, commanders have the
authority to administer other forms of punishment directly. Of the total
reports of rape, 11 percent resulted in nonjudicial punishment including extra duty, restrictions of freedom, or loss of a portion of their pay,
and 12 percent resulted in administrative action including a letter of
admonition. Half of reported cases were excused because of insufcient
evidence, recanted allegations, or death of the survivor or perpetrator
before investigation commenced (Department of Defense, 2009). When
survivors reports of rape are devalued to the point where rapists punishment involves a small ne or a letter of admonition, the atmosphere

190

Feminism as Human Rights

in the military continues to perpetuate sexual assault and force women


to remain silent. Skinner et al. (2000) found that survivors of military
rape reported more problems during active duty, including stronger
feelings of isolation, loneliness, and group exclusion than other female
soldiers.
As a means to investigate and prosecute perpetrators of sexual
assault in the military, Jane Harman proposed a bill for the Department
of Defense to implement several measures. All military factions would
have to uphold the following:
Commanders to be held accountable for assaults that occur in their units
and provide justication for disposing of sexual assault cases through
non-judicial punishment and other administrative actions; The Secretary
of Defense to review current training methods for all military investigations staff, including Judge Advocates, and implement any improvements
that are necessary; Notication of local civilian law enforcement ofcials
of any military protective order issued on base to provide for continuity
of protection of victims; An opportunity for a base transfer once a service
member has notied her or his command that she or he has been sexually assaulted or raped. (Harman, 2008)

These modications to the military procedures will not be enough to


eliminate rape in the military, but they would help set precedence for
authority gures. They would also allow survivors to receive support
and protection when speaking out about rape.
In addition to raping women serving in the military, soldiers also
rape civilian women in warring countries as well as civilians they are
charged with protecting. Van Woudenberg (2005) cites cases of sexual
assaults perpetrated by UN peacekeepers in the Democratic Republic
of Congo, Bosnia, Sierra Leone, Liberia, and other countries. Among
different types of sexual assault that are inicted, coercive rape as a
means for survival for the victim is common. Survival rape involves a
transfer of food, money, protection, documentation, or another commodity in exchange for sex. Even though the UN has a zero-tolerance
policy for these actions, they have done little to prevent or prosecute
perpetrators (Van Woudenberg, 2005).
Pornography and prostitution have long been associated with the
military. Aggressive masculinity is enhanced with the use of pornography, and in wartime can help male soldiers distinguish themselves
from women. The Pakistani army used pornography to enhance their
soldiers masculinity before the attack on Bangladesh in 1971, in which
soldiers subsequently committed thousands of rapes against Bangladeshi women (Jeffreys, 2007).
Another way to enhance sexual aggression is through the thousands
of brothels located near military bases around the world. The U.S.

Rape: A Global Perspective

191

government makes agreements with foreign countries, such as Australia, Singapore, Malaysia, Korea, Philippines, and Japan for R&R
services for soldiers (Farr, 2007). These bases draw in thousands of
prostitutes; some women are poverty stricken locals while the majority
of women are specically trafcked into these brothels. With the arrival
of UN peacekeeping forces in Cambodia in 1993, the number of prostitutes increased from an estimated 1,500 to 20,000 (Farr, 2007). Similar
situations have occurred in Yugoslavia, Rwanda, Liberia, Guinea, Sierra
Leone, and East Timor (Farr, 2007). Women from neighboring countries
and locals were reported to be kidnapped or coerced into sexual slavery in the brothels near peacekeeping forces (Farr, 2007). During World
War II, an estimated 200,000 Asian women, so called comfort
women, primarily from Korea, were trafcked into Japan by the Japanese Imperial Army to work as sex slaves in comfort stations set up
for soldiers (Amnesty International, 2004). Holtzman (1994) reports that
Southeast Asian women are often sexually exploited and victimized by
sexual tourism and so-called R&R for male U.S. military personnel
and the female sex slave trade is ourishing in that region. Further violence continues as soldiers who frequent military base brothels demand
that women submit to their desires. Farr (2007) explains how women
who refuse to have anal sex, oral sex, or insert objects into their vagina
are often subject to physical abuse and rape by soldiers.
The UN made rape a war crime by passing The Military Domestic
and Sexual Violence Response Act, including implementation of UN
Security Council 1325 and The Committee on the Elimination of Discrimination Against Women, as well as utilization of the International
Criminal Court. Many countries, including the United States, have not
signed these acts, nor do they participate in the International Criminal
Court. These countries claim that they can better punish the actions of
their soldiers, which as previously stated, amounts to little or no punishment when dealing with rape. Attitudes by the military that promote masculinity and the degradation of women encourage rape of
military women and trafcking of women for soldiers pleasures as
well as the rape of enemy women. Military organizations, starting
from the top authorities, need to deconstruct their demeaning views of
women in order to diminish rape by soldiers.
RAPE DURING ARMED CONFLICT
Rape is used in warfare as the ultimate weapon to destroy cultures.
In fact rape has frequently been called the spoils of war. The systematic sexual violence during armed conict can serve many functions,
such as destabilizing populations, destroying bonds within communities and families, advancing ethnic cleansing, expressing hatred for the

192

Feminism as Human Rights

enemy, or supplying combatants with sexual services (Ward, 2002).


Serbian soldiers reported that they were ordered to rape so that the
military morale would be higher and they would ght better (Stiglmayer, 1993).
Former UN Ambassador, Stephen Lewis, spoke about the growing
practice of rape during armed conict:
Sexual violence was escalated from a battle tactic to a systematic strategy
of war. Where from the outset, its understood by military commanders,
by military factions, by military marauders, the cheapest and easiest way
to destabilize and dismember a village, a community, a province, is to
rape the women and girls. Sexual torture eviscerates the women and it
terrorizes and traumatizes their children as well, and their partners and
their extended families, the communities themselves. Its the ultimate
sabotage of human health. In the darkness and depth of the trauma,
physical and emotional, the women are rendered incapable of playing
the roles that society has assigned to them. They are lost as parents who
take their children to health clinics and schools. Lost as the mainstays of
family life, as the gathers of wood, the drawers of water, the providers of
meals, as the farmers who maintain agricultural productivity, as the
bearers of children, because in so many instances, they can no longer
bear children. (Lewis, 2008)

The explanation by Lewis expresses the goal that many raping


armies seek: the ethnic cleaning of populations. Rape is used in war to
express many power dynamics, but the massive onset of rape as a
means for ethnic cleansing has become rampant in past decades.
Women become the focal point of attack in order to eliminate the next
generation of the enemy, oftentimes by enhanced violence including
vaginal mutilation, cutting out fetuses, or forced pregnancy by the
enemy (Amnesty International, 2004). In several countries, including
Sudan, mutilated genitals were aunted as trophies of war (Amnesty
International, 2004). Rape as a form of genocide has become a normal
means of warfare and rhetoric among militaries.
In Bosnia and Herzegovina, between 30,000 and 50,000 women were
raped during the conict (Stiglmayer, 1993). Serbian soldiers frequented rape camps where Muslim and Croat women were held as sex
slaves (Stiglmayer, 1993). The underlying mission of these rapes was
ethnic cleansing. Serbians would also impregnate Muslim women and
detain them until they gave birth to Serbian babies. Other atrocities
included brutal gang rapes and the rape of young women in front of
their families (Stiglmayer, 1993). In Bangladesh, the 1971 war for independence led to the rape of 250,000 to 400,000 women in nine months,
resulting in an estimated 25,000 pregnancies (Swiss & Giller, 1993).
During the 1994 Rwanda genocide, soldiers raped 250,000 to 500,000
women, one third of whom were gang raped (Amnesty International,

Rape: A Global Perspective

193

2004). In certain villages in Uganda, more than 70 percent of the


women are rape survivors (Swiss & Giller, 1993). In Sierra Leone, as
many as 90 percent of women are believed to be rape survivors, some
with additional brutalization, including amputation (Amnesty International, 2004).
The horric suffering being inicted on women during armed conicts is devastating. Major General Patrick Cammart, the former Commander of Congos peacekeeping force said, It has probably become
more dangerous to be a woman than a soldier in an armed conict
(Lewis, 2008). Women have little means of protecting themselves and
men are ill-equipped for ghting back. The shame of rape is heightened when husbands abandon their families, partially from cultural
stigmas associated with rape, but also due to their personal blame for
failing to protect their wives. Therefore, the militias accomplish their
task of tearing apart families and communities, successful at ethnic
cleansing and clearing lands of all inhabitants.
After being raped or having their family members sexually
assaulted, women have no choice but to leave their homes to seek
safety and medical assistance. As they travel to refugee camps or hide
out in remote areas, they are once again at risk of rape. Often, their
own military or bandits scavenging through the lands will assault
women, and the threat of attack from militias is still present. Once
women nd security in refugee camps, they will soon realize that protection during an armed conict does little to safeguard women from
the atrocities of war.
RAPE OF REFUGEES AND INTERNALLY DISPLACED WOMEN
Women who are internally displaced, refugees, or living in postconict settings are at high risk of sexual assault. Refugees are also at a
greater risk for rape related to domestic violence, survival sex, involuntary prostitution, sex trafcking, and forced marriage (Human Rights
Watch, 2009; Ward, 2002). The vast range of perpetrators includes military soldiers, insurgents, border guards, host communities, peacekeeping forces, humanitarian workers, husbands, and other refugees (Ward,
2002). Of the 40 million people displaced from their homes due to
armed conict or natural disaster, an estimated 80 percent are women
and children (Amnesty International, 2004). Similar to conict regions,
sexual assault is prevalent in refugee camps. Rates of reporting rape to
authorities remains low, which is more indicative of inadequate systems for reporting or documenting incidents, as well as cultural norms
that degrade survivors (UN Refugee Agency, 2006). The UN Refugee
Agency (UNHCR, 2006) reported 7,600 cases of sexual assault in 104
refugee camps during 2006, an increase of 77 percent from 2005. These
increases are believed to be the outcome of procedural implementation

194

Feminism as Human Rights

for reporting sexual violence, as well as increased training of refugee


personnel (UNHCR, 2006).
However, the reality of sexual assault incidence in refugee camps is
much higher. In 2003, the Ministry for Social Protection in Colombia
reported that 36 percent of the internally displaced Colombian women
were sexually assaulted (Amnesty International, 2004). A survey by the
International Rescue Committee found that 27 percent of Burundi refugees experienced sexual violence after they became refugees (Hynes &
Cordozo, 2000). Another study in West Timor found there was a significant correlation between sexual violence and moving to a refugee
camp compared to women who did not displace to a camp (Hynes,
Ward, Robertson, & Koss, 2003). Richard Mollica (1986) reports that 95
percent of the Cambodian refugee women he interviewed as clients
had experienced some type of sexual assault, but it took the trust
gained through three years of therapy before they would speak of it.
Cambodian women escaping the Pol Pot regime by crossing the border
to Thai refugee camps found that their lives were dened by forced
labor, torture, starvation, beatings, and rape. In the camps it was said
that rape was as common as the night (Rozee & Van Boemel, 1989;
Van Boemel & Rozee, 1992).
Accurate statistics for refugee sexual violence are difcult to establish, since many cultures not only blame survivors of rape, but the
social stigma and societal blame pressures husbands to leave their
wives, or families disowning their daughters, if it is revealed that
theyve been raped. In many traditional cultures a woman who loses
her virginity, even by rape, is considered used goods, soiled, and a
matter of shame to her family (Rozee & Van Boemel, 1989).
To encourage a culture of speaking out against sexual violence, several UN resolutions were initiated. The UNHCR Executive Committee
Conclusion on Refugee Protection and Sexual Violence No. 73 (XLIV)
of 1993 and the Executive Committee Conclusion on Women and Girls
at Risk No. 105 (LVII) of 2006 require that sexual assault survivors in
refugee environments receive adequate medical and psychosocial care,
including culturally appropriate counseling facilities (UNHCR, 2006).
In 2005, of the camps that reported data, 62 percent met UNHCR
requirements, including legal, psychosocial, health or safety, and security, increasing to 88 percent in 2006 (UNHCR, 2006).
The International Rescue Committee launched a project on sexual violence among Burundian refugee women housed in camps in Tanzania
(Heise et al., 1999). The project opened four drop-in centers that offered
counseling, 24-hour medical services, and emergency contraception
(Heise et al., 1999). These centers are vital to survivors, especially those
who are no longer allowed to live with their families and become at
heightened risk for revictimization. Along with opening support centers for survivors, an emphasis needs to be made on informing women

Rape: A Global Perspective

195

of their rights and services available. Multi-media campaigns within


refugee communities, involving television, radio, and print, are necessary
to inform survivors about the availability of health, social services, and
legal aid, as well as conducting prevention campaigns (Ward, 2002).
The psychological and physical impact of sexual assault is extensive,
and women who have not been raped also live in constant fear of the
repercussions of rape (Rozee, 1993). This fear of rape prevents women
from collecting rewood for cooking, which has aggravated malnutrition among Ethiopian refugee families living in Sudanese refugee
camps (Heise et al., 1994). In fact, this phenomenon is evident in refugee camps worldwide. Studies in Kenya and Tanzania also found that
the majority of rapes were committed after women leave their camps
to collect rewood, vegetables, or seek employment from local villages
(Human Rights Watch, 2000; UN, 2006). For men, leaving the camps
means capture or death, so the responsibility falls on the women. With
few opportunities for income generation in the camps and no fuel to
cook their food, women have no option but to put themselves in danger for the survival of their families.
The Womens Commission for Refugee Women and Children (Womans Commission, 2006) conducted a comparison study between two
refugee camps with signicantly different rates of sexual assault outside of the camp:
. . . one camp in Darfur with high rates of sexual assault and one camp
in Nepal with low rates of sexual assault. They found that in Nepal, the
women used kerosene instead of rewood, so they did not have to
endanger their lives by leaving the camp for rewood collection (Womans Commission, 2006). The Womens Commission reported several
ways to supply women in refugee camps worldwide with alternative
means of fueling their stoves, including sources that could simultaneously create jobs for both women and men in the camps. (Womans Commission, 2006)

Revictimization is a constant threat to survivors in refugee camps.


Survivors are deemed disgraced or dishonored by their communities,
oftentimes unable to get married or keep their husband. Their victim
status exposes them to revictimization by other refugee men and police
ofcers who coerce them to have sex for protection or force them to
become their unofcial wives (Human Rights Watch, 2005). Isolation
can also put women in a position of forced prostitution or sex trafcking. Some male refugees and humanitarian workers are connected with
trafcking rings, which place women in refugee settings at increased
vulnerability.
In 2000, UN Resolution 1325 specically stressed the importance of
womens participation in post-war reconstruction efforts (Amnesty

196

Feminism as Human Rights

International, 2004). Areas of involvement include decision-making


role, UN eld-based operations, military observers, civilian police, humanitarian personnel, design of refugee camps, processes of repatriation and resettlement, mine-clearance, and disarmament (Amnesty
International, 2004).
The UNHCR and nongovernmental organizations (NGOs) have
banded together to nd prevention solutions to rape in refugee-like settings. They encourage women refugee involvement in leadership structures at the camps and decision-making positions in order to better
address the needs of women (UNHCR, 2000). Women holding these
positions can help produce an environment within the refugee camp
that is safe for women, as well as increase gender equality in the community. Afrmative action measures were implemented in Tanzania
refugee camps, leading to women holding 50 percent of the decisionmaking roles (UNHCR, 2001). Other solutions for reducing rape by
men with power would promote female recruitment into peacekeeping
and police forces (Ward, 2002). Currently, most peacekeeping forces
are almost entirely men. By creating a gender balance within these
forces, womens needs will be better served.
CULTURAL SUPPORTS OF RAPE
Many cultures worldwide support rape through patriarchal structures and gender inequality. Communities are dened by gender ideologies, which endorse what is appropriate and accepted within that
culture. The attitudes and behaviors of criminal justice systems, religions, and the media hold women as subordinate citizens and perpetuate rape within societies.
Criminal Justice System
To end violence against women, governmental policy must severely
admonish rape. If laws do not take a rm stance to protect its citizens,
a pattern of inequality and rape justication will continue. International
policy frameworks were put in place to set precedence with concern to
sexual assault. The UN (Tananta, 2008) outlined a framework for developing or amending legislation that emphasized prevention of violence
against women, punishment of perpetrators, and protection and support of survivors. Unfortunately, as of 2006, only about half of UN
Member States had laws addressing violence against women and many
of those laws were still limited: dening rape by the use of force, sexual violence as a crime against family honor, impunity from punishment if the perpetrator marries the victim, and lacking laws that
address marital rape, forced marriage, and sex trafcking (Tananta,
2008). Without a framework that specically protects women from

Rape: A Global Perspective

197

sexual assault, criminal justice systems will not be able to deter and
punish these crimes.
In countries where laws exist, women still express low levels of satisfaction when dealing with criminal justice systems. Survivors of partner violence reported less satisfaction with police ofcers, prosecutors,
judges, and victim assistance workers than survivors of nonpartner violence (Byrne, Kilpatrick, Howley, & Beatty, 1999). Perpetrators of intimate partner violence were signicantly more likely to be allowed to
plead guilty to a lesser crime (Byrne, Kilpatrick, Howley, & Beatty,
1999). Of these women, only 37 percent of partner violence survivors
and 52 percent of nonpartner violence survivors were satised with the
criminal justice system.
To assure sexual assault survivors the ability to seek legal retribution, criminal justice systems must be set up to hold men accountable
for sexual assault. This includes governmental laws that cover specic
crimes related to various forms of sexual violence, implementation of
those laws by police and judicial authorities, and womens awareness
of their rights.
Some countries have begun special educational campaigns focused
on the beliefs and values of the people. Violence against women is a
serious problem in Nigeria, including many forms of violence such as
female genital mutilation, marital rape, sex trafcking, and stateendorsed rape. To pass their Violence against Women Bill, which aimed
at criminalizing the many condoned practices related to gender violence in Nigeria, a collaborated tribunal was organized in 2001
(Nwankwo, 2008). Thirty-three women and girls spoke about their
experiences with violence, including rape and trafcking, to hundreds
of attendants including government ofcials, police ofcers, religious
leaders, schools, judges, and media personnel (Nwankwo, 2008). This
mock trial broke the silence and shame often associated with sexual
assault and other forms of violence, helping create public awareness of
this major problem (Nwankwo, 2008). The use of public forums such
as those in Nigeria where women, leaders, and citizens can come together to learn about the widespread problems of sexual assault is a
dynamic way to help people form a personal connection and responsibility for the problem, as well as encourage legal and social change.
Implementation of specic laws against all forms of sexual assault is
vital, but they will not be enough to convict perpetrators or prevent
rape. Laws are important to create equality in the framework of
nations. Implementing these laws will be an even larger task. Since
men are in positions of power to apply new laws or maintain old ones,
specic training for personnel working in the criminal justice system
must focus on attitude and awareness training. It is also essential to
inform women of their rights, so they will be able to feel safe in looking to the criminal justice system for help.

198

Feminism as Human Rights

Religion
Religion has been used to justify many actions, including sexual
assault. Many world religions hold beliefs that are misogynistic, therefore holding women at a lesser status than men. They may directly
support violence against women through doctrine that condones it, or
indirectly through the ways in which religious organizations are structured, placing men in power and women in subservient roles. These
patriarchal traditions maintain oppressive control over women and
promote violence against women (Farr, 2005).
In the Koran, rape is condoned in war through the following passage, We have made lawful to you your wives whom you have given
their dowries, and those whom your right hand possesses out of those
whom Allah has given to you as prisoners of war (33:50). The Old
Testament also accepts similar behaviors. After plundering many
towns that disobeyed God, Moses is angry that his ofcers allowed
unrighteous women to live, and said, Kill every woman who has
slept with a man, but save for yourselves every girl who has never
slept with a man (Numbers 31:7-18). These statements support the
rape of women during armed conict. With the ever-present use of
rape in war today, this type of ideology appears to remain a part of
our culture.
The Old Testament also explains rules for rape by fellow neighbors.
If there is a young woman, a virgin already engaged to be married, and
a man meets her in the town and lies with her, you shall bring both of
them to the gate of that town and stone them to death, the young woman
because she did not cry for help in the town and the man because he violated his neighbors wife. (Deuteronomy 22:23)

This statement blames women for rape if they do not put up enough
resistance to rape that it could be witnessed by people living nearby.
This corresponds with many rape myths that suggest if a woman cannot prevent rape, then she is to blame. Furthermore, the man is only
punished for violating another mans property and not for the violation
of a woman. It continues,
If a man meets a virgin who is not engaged, and seizes her and lies with
her, and they are caught in the act, the man who lay with her shall give
fty shekels of silver to the young womans father, and she shall become
his wife. (Deuteronomy 22:28)

The woman once again is not the subject of victimization; it is the


father who must be paid. This similar tradition of impunity for men
who rape if they marry their victim is still present in many countries
globally.

Rape: A Global Perspective

199

Hindu text also makes astonishing statements on the way in which


a husband should initiate sex with his wife. In the Brhadrankyaki Upanishad scripture, it reads:
When she has changed her clothes at the end of her menstrual period,
therefore, one should approach that splendid woman and invite her to
have sex. Should she refuse to consent, he should bribe her. If she still
refuses, he should beat her with a stick or with his sts and overpower
her, saying: I take away the splendor from you with my virility and
splendor.

Religious texts, especially those dating back hundreds or thousands


of years, can be interpreted in different ways. Not all believers take
each doctrine as a guide or law for their lives to follow, while others
who consider themselves fundamentalists, may be more inclined to
take each doctrine as the ofcial word of God. Religious fundamentalism may encourage misogynist values and blame women for rape. In a
study of Israeli high school students, Geiger, Fischer, and Eshet (2004)
found that students with more conservative religious orientation were
more likely to believe in stereotypes justifying sexual coercion and
attributing sexual coercion to a womans behavior on a date. Sheldon
and Parent (2002) had similar ndings with Christian clergy. Clergy
with fundamentalist religious beliefs blamed rape survivors more often
and had more negative attitudes toward rape survivors than clergy
that did not characterize themselves as fundamentalists (Sheldon &
Parent, 2002). Women attending churches that are considered fundamentalist may need additional outreach support, since they are more
inclined to seek support within their church and be exposed to more
victim blaming reactions. In addition, Aosved and Long (2006) found
that American college students with higher religious intolerance had
greater rape myth acceptance.
In southern India, girls and women of poor, low-caste families are
offered to deities of Brahmanic temples (Chattoraj, 2002). These
women, known as devadasi, are married to a deity or temple, therefore
cannot enter into marriage of their choice, and are subjected to lifetime
prostitution to upper-caste men, often times being sold in sex trafcking (Chattoraj, 2002). Prevention programs and economic empowerment programs now offer women susceptible to devadasi to work as
weavers, basket makers, and vegetable vendors (Dadian, 1997). Yet,
regulation of these practices is still difcult, since the men in power
are the men continuing the devadasi practice.
The Trokosi tradition in Ghana requires families to give virgin girls
to priests to appease the gods for crimes committed by family members
(Equality Now, 2002). Girls become slaves for the priest, as well as

200

Feminism as Human Rights

having forced intercourse (Equality Now, 2002). This practice was


made illegal in 1998, but the government has not prosecuted anyone
under the law and many women are still in servitude (Equality Now,
2002).
The Trokosi tradition dates back more than 300 years, but many emerging religions promote female subordination and sexual abuse. The Children of God/The Family, a religious movement founded in the 1960s in
the United States, reached a height of more than 10,000 followers worldwide where sexual assault was an accepted practice (Boeri, 2002). Any
male member of this group was allowed to have sex with any female, despite her age, marital status, or desire (Boeri, 2002). Many other religious
groups have victimized women in the name of God or spiritual fulllment.
Fortune (2000) expresses how religious institutions can act as either
resources or roadblocks for women healing from violence. Since
religious organizations are important support systems for women and
possibly the only support for some women, clergy and other religious
personnel need to be educated on rape myths and the best practices
for supporting survivors. When religious organizations fail to support
survivors, their repercussions can be damaging. As Fortune stated,
Many women have been abandoned by their faith communities, left
to experience shame or guilt, whereas their perpetrators have had
license to continue their abusive behavior (Fortune, 2000, p. 372). By
holding a rm stance against sexual assault, religious organizations can
be in a position to prevent violence against women.
Media
The public media, including television, Internet, lm, video games,
radio, and print media, has widespread range and powerful impact of
cultural ideology. International research supports claims that the media
can encourage rape in communities through the depiction of gender inequality, male-dominated sexual relationships, and the eroticism of
rape. In an Indian study, Derne (1999) found that the male interviewees, from regions in India known for dowry deaths and sexual harassment, had similar beliefs to the gender relationships depicted in Hindu
lms. Almost all of these lms depicted scenes of rape or attempted
rape, often between married couples (Derne, 1999).
In a U.S. study by Apanovitch, Hobfoll, and Salovey (2002), participants watched a lm about gang rape and were then asked to rate the
men in the lm for their responsibility of the rape. Men and women
who had dialogue about the lm before giving their ratings put more
responsibility on the men than participants who did not discuss the
video. The video challenged a number of popular rape myths
(Apanovitch et al., 2002, p. 456), which seemed to make it difcult for
participants to evaluate. Considering that most viewers do not discuss

Rape: A Global Perspective

201

material from different media portals, depictions of violence against


women can endorse already preexisting rape myths.
Educating media personnel, including journalists, broadcasters, and
advertisers, of the root causes of violence against women can encourage acceptable representations of men and women in the media. This
is also a valuable resource for extending survivor rights and awareness directly to women. In Spain, as part of their Integral Protection
Measures against Gender Violence, the media are encouraged to promote gender equality and remove stereotyped images, messages, and
content from all media portals (Mendez, 2008). Even though media
materials will be monitored during a transition process, their goal is
for media personnel to maintain self-regulation of these issues
(Mendez, 2008).
CONCLUSION
Sexual assault is a pervasive and deep-rooted problem, established
as it is within the very cultural structures charged with preventing it.
Effective prevention needs to challenge cultural beliefs and social structures that perpetuate rape. They must ght against sexist views that
underlie male violence. Both women and men are needed to change
gender dynamics and promote rape prevention.
Even though sexual assault pervades cultures throughout the world,
social stigmas keep women from discussing the issue. Public awareness
campaigns should counter stereotypical viewpoints of sexual assault
and direct blame toward the perpetrator. If the responsibility is put on
perpetrators, women will be more inclined to speak out. In addition,
rape crisis centers need to be set up as safe establishments for women
to speak about rape and those in need of physical, psychological, and
legal support.
Medical personnel are often in useful positions to help survivors of
violence. Women may not report their assaults to police but can still
seek medical attention. In some countries, medical personnel are mandated to report sexual violence. This can endanger the lives of women
and force women in those countries to remain silent even in the care of
their doctor. With appropriate training and condentiality rights, medical personnel can be sensitive to these issues and assist women with
necessary referrals that could help them nancially, psychologically, or
emotionally.
Rape is one of the biggest human rights violations that the global
community faces today. Efforts to ght against the ideology that perpetuates rape have begun in communities worldwide. For this process
to function, painstaking efforts must be made at all organizational and
societal levels that inuence gender equality. As men stand up to end
violence against women and women become equal partners in their

202

Feminism as Human Rights

communities, work environments, and families, we can expect to see


rape diminish across the globe.
REFERENCES
Abbey, A., & McAuslan, P. (2004). A longitudinal examination of male college
students perpetration of sexual assault. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 72, 747756.
Abbey, A., Zawacki, T., Buck, P. O., Clinton, A. M., & McAuslan, P. (2004).
Sexual assault and alcohol consumption: What do we know about their
relationship and what types of research are still needed? Aggression and
Violent Behavior, 9, 271303.
Aghatise, E. (2004). Trafcking for prostitution in Italy: Possible effects of government proposals for legalization of brothels. Violence Against Women, 10,
11261155.
Ahrens, C. E., & Rozee, P. D. (2007). Using an ecological perspective to understand and address rape on campus. In G. B. Stahly (Ed.), Gender identity,
equity, and violence: Multidisciplinary perspectives through service learning (pp.
159180). Sterling, VA: Stylus.
Amnesty International. (2004, December 4). Lives blown apart: Crimes against
women in armed conict. London: Amnesty International Publications.
Retrieved January 22, 2009, from http://www.amnesty.org/en/library/
info/ACT77/075/2004
Aosved, A. C., & Long, P. J. (2006). Co-occurrence of rape myth: Acceptance,
sexism, racism, homophobia, ageism, classism, and religious intolerance.
Sex Roles, 55, 481492.
Apanovitch, A. M., Hobfoll, S. E., & Salovey, P. (2002). The effects of social
inuence on perceptual and affective reactions to scenes of sexual violence.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 443464.
Avakame, E. (1999). Females labor force participation and rape: An empirical
test of the backlash hypothesis. Violence Against Women, 5, 926949.
Boeri, M. W. (2002). Women after the utopia: The gendered lives of former cult
members. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 31, 323360.
Bostock, D. J., & Daley, J. G. (2007). Lifetime and current sexual assault and
harassment victimization rates of active-duty United States Air Force
women. Violence Against Women, 13, 927944.
Byrne, C. A., Kilpatrick, D. G., Howley, S. S., & Beatty, D. (1999). Female victims of partner versus nonpartner violence: Experiences with the criminal
justice system. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 26, 275292.
Byrne, C. A., Resnick, H. S., Kilpatrick, D. G., Best, C. L., & Saunders, B. E.
(1999). The socioeconomic impact of interpersonal violence on women.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 362366.
Central Intelligence Agency. (2008). The 2008 World Factbook. Retrieved
December 29, 2008, from https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/
the-world-factbook/index.html
Chattoraj, S. (2002). The Devadasi System. Retrieved January 26, 2009, from
http://iml.jou.u.edu/projects/Spring02/Chattaraj/index2.html

Rape: A Global Perspective

203

Cloitre, M., Tardiff, K., Marzuk, P. M., & Leon, A. C. (1996). Childhood abuse
and subsequent sexual assault among female inpatients. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 9, 473482.
Commission on Security and Cooperation in Europe. (1999). The sex trade:
Trafcking of women and children in Europe and the United States. Hearing before the Commission on Security and Cooperation in Europe, One
hundred sixth congress, rst session. Retrieved December 29, 2008, from
http://www.csce.gov/
Cue, K. L., George, W. H., & Norris, J. (1996). Womens appraisals of sexualassault risk in dating situations. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 487504.
Dadian, M. J. (1997, June). Womens Forum: Inclusive Prevention Efforts Fight
Stigma in Rural India. Building on Success: The Next Generation of HIV/
AIDS Programs, 4(1). Retrieved January 26, 2009, from http://
www.fhi.org/en/HIVAIDS/pub/Archive/articles/AIDScaptions/volume
4no1/WomensForum.htm
Department of Defense. (2008, March 15). Report of sexual assault in the military. Retrieved February 16, 2009, from http://www.sapr.mil/contents/
references/2007%20Annual%20Report.pdf
Department of Defense. (2009, March 15). Report on sexual assault in the military. Retrieved April 3, 2009, from http://www.sapr.mil/contents/ResourcesReports/AnnualReports/DoD_FY08_Annual_Report.pdf
Derne, S. (1999). Making sex violent: Love as force in recent Hindi lms. Violence Against Women, 5, 548575.
Embrey, E. P., Arnold, K. A., Bingham, C., Cockerill, J., Dunlow, S. G., Rau, T.
J., et al. (2004, April). Care for Victims of Sexual Assault Task Force
Report. Department of Defense. Retrieved February 15, 2009, from http://
www.defenselink.mil/dacowits/tablerearch_subpage.html
Equality Now. (1998, March). Submission to the United Nations Human Rights
Committee. Zimbabwe. 62nd session.
Equality Now. (2002, May). Slavery in Ghana: The Trokosi Tradition. Retrieved
January 26, 2009, from http://www.equalitynow.org/english/actions/
action_1403_en.html
Farr, K. (2005). Sex trafcking: The global market in women and children. New
York: Worth Publishers.
Finkelhor, D., & Browne, A. (1985). The traumatic impact of child sexual abuse:
A review and conceptualization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 55,
530541.
Fortune, M. M. (2000). Religious issues and violence against women. In C. M.
Renzetti, J. L. Edleson, & R. K. Bergen (Eds.), Sourcebook on violence against
women. (pp. 371386). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Garcia-Moreno, C., Jansen, H. A. F. M., Ellsberg, M., Heise, L. L., & Watts, C.
H. (2005). WHO multi-country study on womens health and domestic violence
against women: Initial results on prevalence, health outcomes and womens
responses. Switzerland: World Health Organization.
Geiger, B., Fischer, M., & Eshet, Y. (2004). Date-rape-supporting and victimblaming attitudes among high school students in a multiethnic society
Israel. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19, 406426.
Gidycz, C. A., Layman, M. J., Rich, C. L., Crothers, M., Gylys, J., Matorin, A.,
et al. (2001). An evaluation of an acquaintance rape prevention program:

204

Feminism as Human Rights

Impact on attitudes, sexual aggression, and sexual victimization. Journal of


Interpersonal Violence, 16, 11201138.
Golding, J. M., Wilsnack, S. C., & Cooper, M. L. (2002). Sexual assault history
and social support: Six general population studies. Journal of Traumatic
Stress, 15, 187197.
Hankin, C. S., Skinner, K. M., Sullivan, L. M., Miller, D. R., Frayne, S., & Tripp,
T. J. (1999). Prevalence of depressive and alcohol abuse symptoms among
women VA outpatients who report experiencing sexual assault while in
the military. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 12, 601612.
Harman, J. (2008, July 29). Harman introduces bipartisan bill to halt rape and
sexual assault in the military. Retrieved January 1, 2009, from http://
www.house.gov/list/press/ca36_harman/July29_MST.shtml
Harrington, N. T., & Leitenberg, H. (1994). Relationship between alcohol consumption and victim behaviors immediately preceding sexual aggression
by an acquaintance. Violence and Victims, 9, 315324.
Heise, L. L. (1998) Violence against women: An integrated, ecological framework. Violence Against Women, 4, 262290.
Heise, L., Ellsberg, M., & Gottemoeller, M. (1999). Ending violence against women.
Population Reports. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University, L(11).
Heise, L., Ellsberg, M., & Gottemoeller, M. (2002). A global overview of gender-based violence. International Journal of Gynecology and Obstetrics, 78
(suppl.1), S5S14.
Heise, L. L., Pitanguy, J., & Germain, A. (1994). Violence against women: The hidden health burden. World Bank Discussion Paper #255. Washington, DC:
World Bank.
Henry, N., Ward, T., & Hirshberg, M. (2004). A multifactorial model of wartime rape. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9, 535562.
Holtzman, C. G. (1994). Multicultural perspectives on counseling survivors of
rape. Journal of Social Distress and the Homeless, 3, 8797.
Honigman, J. J. (1954). The Kaska Indians: An ethnographic reconstruction. Yale
University Publications in Anthropology, 51, 1163.
Human Rights Watch. (2000, October). Seeking protection: Addressing sexual
and domestic violence in Tanzanias refugee camps. Retrieved January 22,
2009, from http://www.hrw.org/legacy/reports/2000/tanzania/
Human Rights Watch. (2005, April 12). Sexual violence and its consequences
among displaced persons in Darfur and Chad. Retrieved January 23, 2009,
from http://www.hrw.org/en/reports/2005/04/12/sexual-violence-andits-consequences-among-displaced-persons-darfur-and-chad
Human Rights Watch. (2009, January 14). World Report. Retrieved January 23,
2009, from http://www.hrw.org/en/reports/2009/01/14/world-report2009
Hynes, M., & Cordozo, B. L. (2000). Sexual violence against refugee women.
Journal of Womens Health & Gender-based Medicine, 9, 819823.
Hynes, M., Ward, J., Robertson, K., & Koss, M. (2003, July). A determination of
the prevalence of gender based violence among conict-affected populations in East Timor. Report for The Reproductive Health for Refugees Consortium and International Rescue Committee. Retrieved January 22, 2009,
from http://www.rhrc.org/resources/index.cfm?sectorgbv

Rape: A Global Perspective

205

Jeffreys, S. (2007). Double jeopardy: Women, the U.S. military and the war in
Iraq. Womens Studies International Forum, 30, 1625.
Jewkes, R., & Abrahams, N. (2002). The epidemiology of rape and sexual coercion in South Africa: An overview. Social Science & Medicine, 55, 12311244.
Kalof, L. (1993). Rape-supportive attitudes and sexual victimization experiences
of sorority and nonsorority women. Sex Roles, 29, 767780.
Krug, E. G., Dahlberg, L. L., Mercy, J. A., Zwi, A. B., & Lozano, R. (2002).
World report on violence and health. Geneva: World Health Organization.
Levinson, D. (1989). Family violence in cross-cultural perspective. Newbury Park,
CA: Sage.
Lewis, S. (2008, July 21). A crisis of gender violence. Momentum 2008.
Retrieved November 18, 2008, from http://www.tides.org/index.php?id947
Lonsway, K. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1994). Rape myths: In review. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 18, 133164.
Macy, R. J., Nurius, P. S., & Norris, J. (2007). Sexual assault survivors: Implications for defensive coping and resistance. Journal of Interpersonal Violence,
22, 543565.
Maker, A. H., Kemmelmeier, M., & Peterson, C. (2001). Child sexual abuse,
peer sexual abuse, and sexual assault in adulthood: A multi-risk model of
revictimization. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14, 351368.
Malamuth, N. M. (1981). Rape proclivity among males. Journal of Social Issues,
37, 138157.
Malamuth, N. M. (1989). The attraction to sexual aggression scale: Part one. The
Journal of Sex Research, 26, 2649.
Malamuth, N. M., Linz, D., Heavey, C. L., Barnes, G., & Acker, M. (1995).
Using the conuence model of sexual aggression to predict mens conict
with women: A 10-year follow-up study. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 69, 353369.
Malamuth, N. M., Sockloskie, R. J., Koss, M. P., & Tanaka, J. S. (1991). Characteristics of aggressors against women: Testing a model using a national sample of
college students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 670681.
Martin, A. F., Vergeles, M. R., de la Orden Acevedo, V., del Campo Sanchez,
A., & Visa, S. L. (2005). The involvement in sexually coercive behaviors of
Spanish college men. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20, 872891.
Mendez, C. F. (2008). Spanish legislation on violence against women: Challenges and facts. United Nations expert group meeting on good practices
in legislation on violence against women, EGM/GPLVAW/2008/EP.11.
Retrieved December 21, 2008, from http://www.un.org/womenwatch/
daw/egm/vaw_legislation_2008/vaw_legislation_2008.htm
Messman-Moore, T. L., & Brown, A. L. (2006). Risk perception, rape, and sexual revictimization: A prospective study of college women. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 30, 159172.
Messman-Moore, T. L., & Long, P. J. (2000). Child sexual abuse and revictimization in the form of adult sexual abuse, adult physical abuse, and adult
psychological maltreatment. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15, 489502.
Miller, A. K., Markman, K. D., & Handley, I. M. (2007). Self-blame among sexual assault victims prospectively predicts revictimization: A perceived

206

Feminism as Human Rights

sociolegal context model of risk. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29,
129136.
Minturn, L., Grosse, M., & Haider, S. (1969). Cultural patterning of sexual
beliefs and behavior. Ethnology, 8, 301318.
Mithers, C. (2004, November). The garden of evil. Retrieved December 29,
2008, from http://www.globalfundforwomen.org/cms/press-center/2004gfw-news/garden-of-evil.html
Mollica, R. (August 1986). Cambodian refugee women at risk. Paper presented at
the American Psychological Association Annual Meeting, Washington, DC.
Norris, J., & Cubbins, L. A. (1992). Dating, drinking, and rape: Effects of victims and assailants alcohol consumption on judgments of their behavior
and traits. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 16, 179191.
Norris, J., George, W. H., Stoner, S. A., Masters, N. T., Zawacki, T., & Davis, K.
C. (2006). Womens responses to sexual aggression: The effects of childhood trauma, alcohol, and prior relationship. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 14, 402411.
Nwankwo, O. (2008, May 19). Effectiveness of legislation enacted to address violence against women in Nigeria. United Nations expert group meeting on
good practices in legislation on violence against women, EGM/GPLVAW/
2008/EP.08. Retrieved December 21, 2008, from http://www.un.org/womenwatch/daw/egm/vaw_legislation_2008/vaw_legislation_2008.htm
Olson, E. A. (1981). Socioeconomic and psychocultural contexts of child abuse
and neglect in Turkey. In J. Korbin (Ed.), Child abuse and neglect: Cross cultural perspectives (pp. 96119). Berkeley: University of California Press.
Payne, D. L., Lonsway, K. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1999). Rape myth acceptance:
Exploration of its structure and its measurement using the Illinois Rape
Myth Acceptance Scale. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 2768.
Pazzani, L. M. (2007). The factors affecting sexual assaults committed by
strangers and acquaintances. Violence Against Women, 13, 717749.
Richard, A. O. (2000). International trafcking in women to the United States:
A contemporary manifestation of slavery and organized crime. Washington, DC: DCI Exceptional Intelligence Analyst Program, Center for the
Study of Intelligence.
Rozee, P. D. (1993). Forbidden or forgiven: Rape in cross-cultural perspective.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17, 499514.
Rozee, P. D., & Koss, M. P. (2001). Rape: A century of resistance. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, Millennial Issue, 25, 295311.
Rozee, P. D., & Van Boemel, G. (1989). The psychological effects of war trauma
on Cambodian refugee women. Women and Therapy: A Feminist Quarterly,
8, 2350.
Rozee-Koker, P. D. (1987). Cross-cultural codes for seven types of rape. Behavior
Science Research, 21, 101117.
Sadler, A. G., Booth, B. M., & Doebbeling, B. N. (2005). Gang and multiple
rapes during military service: Health consequences and health care. Journal
of the American Medical Womens Association, 60, 3341.
Sadler, A. G., Booth, B. M., Nielson, D., & Doebbeling, B. N. (2000). Healthrelated consequences of physical and sexual violence: Women in the military. Obstetrics & Gynecology, 96, 473480.

Rape: A Global Perspective

207

Sanday, P. R. (1981). Female power and male dominance: On the origins of sexual inequality. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
Senn, C. Y., Desmarais, S., Verberg, N., & Wood, E. (2000). Predicting coercive
sexual behavior across the lifespan in a random sample of Canadian men.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 95113.
Sheldon, J. P. & Parent, S. L. (2002). Clergys attitudes and attributions of blame
toward female rape victims. Violence Against Women, 8, 233256.
Skinner, K. M., Kressin, N., Frayne, S., Tripp, T. J., Hankin, C. S., Miller, D. R.,
et al. (2000). The prevalence of military sexual assault among female Veterans Administration outpatients. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15, 291310.
Stiglmayer, A. (1993, January 4). A pattern of rape: A torrent of wrenching
rst-person testimonies tells of a now Serb atrocity: Systematic sexual
abuse. Newsweek. Retrieved January 22, 2009, from http://www.newsweek.com/id/115892
Swiss, S., & Giller, J. (1993). Rape as a crime of war: A medical perspective.
Journal of the American Medical Association, 270, 612615.
Tananta, F. M. M. (2008, May 24). Good practices regarding legal reforms in
the area of violence against women in Latin America and the Caribbean.
United Nations expert group meeting on good practices in legislation on
violence against Women, EGM/GPLVAW/2008/EP.05. Retrieved December 21, 2008, from http://www.un.org/womenwatch/daw/egm/vaw_legislation_2008/expertpapers/
United Nations. (1993). Declaration on the Elimination of Violence against
Women. General Assembly Resolution 48/104, December 20, 1993.
United Nations. (2000). Convention against transnational organized crime.
Fifty-fth session, A/55/383. Retrieved December 28, 2008, from http://
www.unodc.org/pdf/crime/a_res_55/res5525e.pdf
United Nations. (2006, July 6). In-depth study on all forms of violence against
women. Sixty-rst session, A/61/122/Add.1. Retrieved December 26,
2009, from http://www.un.org/womenwatch/daw/documents/ga61.htm
United Nations Inter-Agency Project on Human Trafcking, China. (2008, September). SIREN human trafcking data sheet. V. 1.0. Retrieved December
29, 2008, from http://www.notrafcking.org/content/SIREN/SIREN_pdf/
china%20siren%20data%20sheet%20sept%202008.pdf
United Nations Ofce on Drugs and Crime. (2006). Trafcking in persons,
global patterns. Retrieved December 26, 2008, from http://www.unodc.org/documents/human-trafcking/HT-globalpatterns-en.pdf
United Nations Refugee Agency (UNHCR). (2006). Statistical Yearbook.
Retrieved January 22, 2009, from http://www.unhcr.org/statistics/STATISTICS/478cda572.html
United Nations Security Council (2008, June 19). Resolution 1820, 5916th Security Council meeting, S/RES/1820. Retrieved January 26, 2009, from
http://www.un.org/Docs/sc/unsc_resolutions08.htm
U.S. Department of State. (2005). Trafcking in Persons Report. Retrieved
December 29, 2008, from http://www.state.gov/g/tip/rls/tiprpt/2005/
index.htm
U.S. Department of State. (2008). Trafcking in Persons Report. Retrieved
January 22, 2009, from http://www.state.gov/g/tip/rls/tiprpt/2008/
index.htm

208

Feminism as Human Rights

Van Boemel, G., & Rozee, P. D. (1992). Treatment efcacy for psychosomatic
blindness among Cambodian refugee women. Women and Therapy: A Feminist Quarterly, 13, 239266.
Van Woudenberg, A. (2005, February 28). MONUC: A Case for Peacekeeping
Reform. Human Rights Watch. Retrieved January 22, 2009, from http://
www.hrw.org/english/docs/2005/03/01/congo10222.htm
Ward, J. (2002, April). If not now, when? Addressing gender-based violence in
refugee, internally displaced, and post-conict settings: A global overview.
The Reproductive Health for Refugees Consortium. Retrieved December
29, 2008, from http://www.rhrc.org/pdf/gbvintro.pdf
Widyono, M. (2006, November 89). Strengthening regional work on genderbased violence: A meeting of activists, practitioners and researchers from
the Horn, East and Southern Africa, Kampala, Uganda. U.S. Agency for
International Development. Retrieved December 14, 2008, from http://
www.igwg.org/pdf/SRWGBV.pdf
Womens Commission for Refugee Women and Children. (2006, March).
Beyond rewood: Fuel alternatives and protection strategies for displaced
women and girls. Retrieved January 22, 2009, from http://www.rhrc.org/
resources/index.cfm?sectorgbv
Yodanis, C. (2004). Gender inequality, violence against women, and fear: A
cross-national test of the feminist theory of violence against women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19, 655675.
Yost, M. R., & Zurbriggen, E. L. (2006). Gender differences in the enactment of
sociosexuality: An examination of implicit social motives, sexual fantasies,
coercive sexual attitudes, and aggressive sexual behavior. Journal of Sex
Research, 43, 163173.

Chapter 9

Sexual Harassment Laws


Impact on Women
Joseph Solberg

This chapter explores court decisions related to sexual harassment and


the impact those decisions have had or may have on women in the
workplace. It will rst set out the component of the Civil Rights Act of
1964 that made workplace discrimination based on gender unlawful.
Next, it will discuss federal appellate court decisions that followed passage of the act leading up to the U.S. Supreme Courts rst decision
related to sex harassment in the workplace. It will explore in some
detail that rst decision, Meritor Savings Bank v. Vinson, which was
decided by the Court in 1986, 22 years after the passage of the Civil
Rights Act. The chapter will then detail the case of Harris v. Forklift Systems, a 1993 Supreme Court decision that will provide the backdrop for
a discussion of an issue that still affects women today: whether courts
should utilize a reasonable person or a reasonable woman/victim test
in deciding sexual harassment cases. Following the Harris discussion,
the impact of two other Supreme Court decisions on working women
will be analyzed. In the companion cases of Faragher v. City of Boca
Raton (1998) and Burlington Industries v. Ellerth (1998), the Court, in
addressing a companys liability for sex harassment committed by a
supervisor, set out an afrmative defense that generally requires the
victim to report the harassment to be successful in a sex harassment
claim. The impact on women due to this requirement will be discussed.
Finally, the chapter will discuss the degree of sexual conduct required
before courts are likely to label the behavior sexually harassing under
the law.

210

Feminism as Human Rights

TITLE VII BACKGROUND


Congress passed the Civil Rights Act in 1964. One element of the act
was Title VII, which prohibits discrimination in the workplace based
on race, color, religion, sex, and national origin. In 1980, the federal
agency that oversees the enforcement of Title VII, the Equal Employment Opportunity Commission (EEOC), promulgated rules that dene
sex harassment as follows:
Unwelcome sexual advances, requests for sexual favors, and other verbal
or physical conduct of a sexual nature constitutes sexual harassment
when (1) submission to such conduct is made either explicitly or implicitly a term or condition of an individuals employment, (2) submission to
or rejection of such conduct by an individual is used as the basis for
employment decisions affecting such individuals, or (3) such conduct has
the purpose or effect of interfering with an individuals work performance or creating an intimidating, hostile, or offensive work environment.
(Code of Federal Regulations, 2000, p. 186)

Sections one and two of the denition describe a type of sexual harassment commonly known as quid pro quo. The third describes what is
most often referred to as hostile environment harassment.
The rst federal court decision to recognize the concept of sexual
harassment in the workplace was Williams v. Saxbe (1976). Williams, a
Washington, DC, Circuit case, involved a female employee who alleged
that she was terminated after refusing the sexual advances of her
supervisor. It was followed by Bundy v. Jackson, a 1981 case that held that
an employer could be liable for sexual harassment based on sexual insults
and propositions, even if the employee suffered no tangible loss of a job
benet. Building on Bundy and its acceptance of a hostile environment
theory of recovery, the DC Circuit, in Henson v. City of Dundee (1982),
expanded the concept of hostile environment sex harassment, writing:
Sexual harassment which creates a hostile or offensive environment for
members of one sex is every bit the arbitrary barrier to sexual equality in
the workplace that racial harassment is to racial equality. Surely, a
requirement that a man or a woman run a gauntlet of sexual abuse in
return for the privilege of being allowed to work and make a living can
be as demeaning and disconcerting as the harshest of racial epithets.
(p. 902)

Finally, in yet another case decided by the DC federal court, it was


ruled that workplace violence that is not sexual in nature may still be
considered to be sex-based harassment if it can be demonstrated that
the conduct would not have taken place but for the victims sex
(McKinney v. Dole, 1985).

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

211

McKinney (1985) was followed by the Supreme Courts decision in


Meritor Savings Bank v. Vinson (1986). In Meritor, the victim, Michelle
Vinson, led a sex harassment claim against her supervisor and the
bank where she worked after having been terminated from her job as
an assistant branch manager. Her claim alleged that her supervisor
fondled her, had intercourse with her (she refused at rst but testied
that she eventually agreed out of fear of losing her job), exposed himself to her, and forcibly raped her on several occasions. The Court
ruled that sex harassment was a type of sex discrimination and therefore in violation of Title VII.
The Meritor case is important for several reasons, in addition to its
determination that sex harassment is a Title VII (1964) violation. First,
the Court ruled that voluntariness is not a defense to a claim of sex
harassment. The more appropriate question is whether the conduct
was unwelcome. In addition, the Court noted that not all offensive conduct, for example, a single epithet, is sex harassment. Finally, the Court
determined that for harassment to be actionable it must be sufciently
severe or pervasive to alter the conditions of the victims employment
and create an abusive working environment (p. 67).
The Meritor decision is also of signicant relevance for what it did
not decide. It did not precisely describe what types of behaviors might
constitute actionable harassment. Nor did it provide guidelines through
which to analyze alleged conduct. Finally, it did not set out a clear
objective standard by which to judge conduct such as a reasonable person or reasonable victim. The chapter will now address this issue.
REASONABLE PERSON/VICTIM ISSUE
To be successful in a hostile environment sexual harassment claim it
is incumbent upon the victim to prove various elements. As set out in
McWilliams v. Fairfax County Board of Supervisors (1996), they include
the following:
(1) The conduct in question was unwelcome.
(2) The harassment was because of sex.
(3) The harassment was sufciently severe or pervasive enough to create an
abusive working environment.
(4) Some basis for imputing liability to the employer. (p. 1195)

The third element, which is the focus of this discussion, involves


subjective and objective components. To establish the subjective element the victim must prove that the conduct in question offended him
or herself (Harris v. Forklift Systems, 1994). Next, the plaintiff must
demonstrate that the behavior would have affected a reasonable

212

Feminism as Human Rights

person/victim (Andrews v. City of Philadelphia, 1990). This is referred to


as the objective component. The question of which objective standard
to utilize, person or victim, has received much comment (Almony,
1992; Ashraf, 1993; Bennet-Alexander & Hartman, 2009; Cahn, 1992;
Epstein, 2004; Gettle, 1993; Glidden, 1992; Newman, 2007) and U.S.
courts are split on the issue. Because of its historical and current importance, the chapter will now discuss the issue at length.
According to ofcial EEOC statistics, women led approximately 85
percent of the sexual harassment cases brought before the agency in
2008 (EEOC, 2009). In addition, it has been demonstrated that women
generally view sexual conduct through a different lens than men (Ehirenreich, 1990; Glidden, 1992). This combination has led some to conclude that workplace harassment of women by men is unlikely to be
curtailed to any great degree if women are required to prove that the
conduct in question would have offended a reasonable person
(Ellison V. Brady, 1991). Some suggest that the behavior should be analyzed from the point of view of a reasonable victim, who in the vast
majority of cases will be a woman, rather than from that of a reasonable person (Abrams, 1989; Glidden, 1992). This would serve to compel
management, which is often dominated by males, to consider harassment from the viewpoint of the victim (Glidden, 1992) or risk increased
liability. It would, therefore, help women achieve greater workplace
equality.
Currently, two of the 12 federal circuits, the Ninth and the Third,
have clearly adopted the reasonable victim standard. Four circuits, the
Fifth, Sixth, Eighth, and Eleventh, have rejected the standard. The
Supreme Court has not issued a denitive ruling (Newman, 2007).
There are a number of reasons why some courts have failed to follow
the lead of the Third and the Ninth circuit courts. Some question
whether it is appropriate to have separate standards based solely on
the sex of the plaintiff (Glidden, 1992; Johnson, 1993). Under identical
factual situations, should it be easier for a woman to prevail in a sex
harassment case than a man? There is also a concern that the use of
separate standards in sex harassment cases will lead to their use in harassment cases based on race, religion, national origin, disability, and so
forth (Radtke v. Everett, 1993). Yet another concern is related to the
makeup of jury pools. Can a gender-conscious standard realistically be
enforced by a jury made up of members of both sexes (Gettle, 1993)?
Finally, will the use of a separate, lesser standard serve to make
women seeking equality appear unable to survive in the workplace
without special help (Radtke v. Everett, 1993)? The chapter will now
move to a detailed discussion of arguments in favor and against the
reasonable victim standard.
A Note published in the Harvard Law Review in 1984, two years
before Meritor, endorsed the reasonable victim standard as follows:

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

213

Such a standard would protect women from the offensive behavior that
results from the divergence of male and female perceptions of appropriate conduct, but it would not penalize defendants whose victims were
unusually sensitive. Courts could further protect sensitive employees by
nding liability whenever a defendant persisted in sexually related conduct after the plaintiff had notied him that she found it offensive. (Note:
Harvard Law Review, 1984, p. 1459)

This standard would ideally accomplish two purposes. First, by allowing jurors to evaluate behavior from a gender-conscious perspective, it
would make it easier for women to prove sexual harassment claims. Second, it would maintain the needed protections afforded to defendants in
cases involving a supersensitive plaintiff by still requiring plaintiffs to
overcome a reasonableness threshold (Solberg, a&b, 1995).
A number of articles have been written in support of the reasonable
victim standard. Kathryn Abrams (1989) citing a study that concluded
that 53 percent of working women believed they had experienced harassing behavior, reasons, (o)ne principal reason for the pervasiveness
of sexual harassment in the workplace is that men regard conduct,
ranging from sexual demands to sexual innuendo, differently than
women do (p. 1202). One well known survey determined that 67 percent of women nd sexual propositions at work to be insulting, while
the same percentage of men receiving similar propositions would be
attered (Gutek, 1985). Elizabeth Glidden, writing in the Iowa Law
Review, also believes that men and women perceive sexual behaviors
differently (Glidden, 1992). She favors the use of the reasonable victim
standard because the gender neutral standard fails to address psychological problems faced by working women and (i)f courts continue to
use the reasonable person standard . . ., sexually harassing environments might not be changed or challenged precisely because men in
male-dominated workplaces nd such environments normal and acceptable (p. 1839). Finally, Patricia J. Almony, in a 1992 Villanova
Law Review article, argues that the reasonable victim standard reects
reality and therefore, can address the problem of sexual harassment
most accurately, writing:
(t)he standard recognizes the different perspectives of men and women
and attempts to reconcile them with legal reality. It also deals fairly with
the complainant and the alleged harasser by ensuring that valid, reasonable claims are redressed. And regardless of whether the standard can be
practically applied, it has the preventive impact of encouraging employers to implement a harassment policy and educate their employees about
what conduct is appropriate in the workplace. (p. 221)

After the Harvard Law Review Note (1984), some courts began to consider using the reasonable victim standard in sex harassment cases. In

214

Feminism as Human Rights

a 1986 case, Rabidue v. Osceola Rening Co., Judge Damon J. Keith, in


his dissenting opinion, disagreed with the reasonable person test
employed by the majority. He referenced the Harvard Law Review (1984)
article and argued that the standard used by the majority did not fairly
consider the differences between men and women regarding their perceptions of sexual conduct. He agreed with the Notes thesis that a reasonable victim standard would maintain employer protection against
overly sensitive employees yet permit courts to consider the differences
between men and women with respect to their perceptions of sexual
behavior. He concluded with a word of caution, writing, (m)oreover,
unless the outlook of the reasonable woman is adopted, the defendants
as well as the courts are permitted to sustain ingrained notions of reasonable behavior fashioned by the offenders, in this case, men (p. 626).
The Sixth Circuit adopted Judge Keiths reasoning in Yates v. AVCO
Corp. (1987). The case involved constructive discharge based on sex
harassment. In acknowledging the argument set out by Judge Keith in
his dissent one year earlier, the court wrote:
In a sexual harassment case involving a male supervisors harassment of
a female subordinate, it seems only reasonable that the person standing
in the shoes of the employee should be the reasonable woman since
the plaintiff in this type of case is required to be a member of a protected
class and is by denition female (p. 637).

In a reference to Judge Keiths Rabidue dissent, the court acknowledged that men and women are vulnerable in different ways and
offended by different behavior (p. 637).
The Third Circuit followed the Sixth in adopting the reasonable victim standard in Andrews v. City of Philadelphia, decided in 1990. The
plaintiffs in Andrews, Pricilla Kelsey Andrews and Debra Ann Conn,
worked for the Philadelphia Police Department. They claimed that they
were the victims of harassment involving abusive language, property
destruction, and anonymous phone calls. Andrews also claimed that
she was physically harmed. The court focused on the objective element
of proof and determined that to be successful a plaintiff must establish
that the discrimination would detrimentally affect a reasonable person of
the same sex (emphasis added) (pp. 14821483). The court justied its
position by noting that the purpose of Title VII of the Civil Rights Act
of 1964 was, at least in part, to permit women to become part of the
workforce more freely. To do this, they must be able to work without
fear of harassment. The court concluded that the reasonable victim
standard was the appropriate method of alleviating those fears.
The Ninth Circuit became the last to adopt the reasonable victim
standard. In Ellison v. Brady (1991), the plaintiff, Kerry Ellison, was
employed by the Internal Revenue Service as a revenue agent. She

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

215

accepted a date with a coworker, Sterling Gray, and afterward she


alleged that he began to hang around her desk and ask her for dates,
which she declined. He also gave her a note that troubled her to the
extent that she showed it to her supervisor, who also happened to be
Grays supervisor. Eventually, Ellison led a sex harassment claim
and her case reached the appellate level. The appellate court rst analyzed Grays behavior, which it determined was more severe than an
inappropriate comment but not as egregious as rape. The former
would not amount to harassment under the law (Meritor, 1986), while
the latter surely would be considered to violate Title VII. To analyze
Grays conduct the court adopted the reasonable victim test because
a sex-blind reasonable person standard tends to be male-biased
and tends to systematically ignore the experiences of women
(p. 879). Viewed through this gender-conscious perspective, the court
concluded that (a) reasonable woman could consider Grays conduct,
as alleged by Ellison, sufciently severe and pervasive to alter a
condition of employment and create an abusive work environment
(p. 880).
Not all commentators and judges favor a gender-conscious objective
standard. Several arguments have been provided in favor of the reasonable person standard and against the use of the reasonable person
model. One proponent of the reasonable person standard (Cahn, 1992)
writes that it appears to provide a neutral and abstract standard so
that the law is not interpreted according to the whims of individual
judges or juries, but instead on a societal consensus (p. 1431). Cahn
raises a second argument, noting that even among women, behaviors
may be perceived differently, writing (s)ome women accept as normal
operating behavior actions that other women would equate with harassment (p. 1416). Concerns have also been raised regarding the possibility that a gender-based standard may lead to other groups desiring
their own standard based on race (Ashraf, 1993), national origin, religion, or whatever protected category they belong to (Radtke v. Everett,
1993). These and other arguments are eshed out below. Glidden, in
her 1992 article in favor of the reasonable victim standard, acknowledged some of the issues inherent in its adoption. She noted that it
may seem to be a lesser standard than the gender neutral standard.
She understood that it might perpetuate the stereotype of an overly
sensitive female who cannot survive in the workplace without the aid
of special treatment (pp. 1848, 1849). Despite these concerns, she
defended the reasonable victim standard because she believed that hostile environments would not change without it.
Others are not as certain as Glidden and raise additional concerns.
One problem with a gender-conscious standard, alluded to earlier,
relates to the difculty that would ensue with requiring male jurors to
analyze behavior as a reasonable woman (Gettle, 1993). Johnson (1993)

216

Feminism as Human Rights

elaborates; stating that under such a standard men would need to rely
on stereotypes of women in trying to think like one. He continues:
(t) he reasonable woman standard . . . falls short because it demands the
impossible: that male triers of fact understand and apply the perspective
of a reasonable woman, even though men are, by denition, presumed
unable to do so because of the allegedly characteristic male view that
sexual harassment is harmless amusement and the supposed male bias
that systematically ignores the experiences of women. (p. 642)

Johnson also noted that reliance on expert testimony regarding the


perceptions of a reasonable woman would be inappropriate.
In 1993, the Michigan Supreme Court provided perhaps the most
detailed judicial argument against the use of the reasonable victim
standard. In Radtke v. Everett, Tamra Radtke, the plaintiff, began working in 1984 as a veterinary technician for Clark-Everett Dog and Cat
Hospital. On May 29, 1988, a Sunday, she was working alone with
Dr. Stuart Everett. It was not uncommon to have such a short staff on
weekends, when only emergency care was provided. While taking a
break in the lounge at the end of the day Ms. Radtke alleged that
Dr. Everett engaged in inappropriate behavior and made inappropriate
comments. She resigned the next day and began psychological counseling a short time later. In December of 1988, Ms. Radtke led a lawsuit
against the hospital and Dr. Everett alleging in part that she had been
sexually harassed by Dr. Everett in violation of Michigans Civil Rights
Act (Elliott-Larsen Civil Rights Act, 1976). Eventually, the case reached
the Michigan Supreme Court.
In an opinion written by Justice Dorothy Riley, the Michigan
Supreme Court ruled that the reasonable person standard was the
appropriate legal test for the objective component of a sexual harassment claim. The Justice rst noted the parallels between the Michigan
Civil Rights Act and Title VII. Next, she acknowledged the necessity of
an objective reasonableness test to protect employers from overly sensitive employees in sexual harassment cases. Once she had established
that some type of objective test was necessary she began her discussion
of what that test should be.
The opinion approached the issue from several perspectives. It rst
pointed to the acts legislative history. The act was passed in 1976 at a
time when the only objective standards of conduct existing in Michigan
were those of the reasonable person as had been the case for more than
a century. Justice Riley wrote that if the legislature in 1976 intended to
adopt a new standard of reasonableness, it would have done so explicitly. The fact that it had done indicated they never intended to adopt
the standard. The court then considered the use of the reasonable person standard in the context of sexual harassment claims and concluded

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

217

that it is sufciently exible to incorporate gender differences


(p. 166). Next, the court quoted from Prosser and Keeton on Torts
(1984), which stresses the importance of uniform rather than specic
norms of conduct:
The standard of conduct which the community demands must be an
external and objective one, rather than the individual judgment, good or
bad, of the particular actor and it must be, so far as possible, the same
for all persons, since the law can have no favorites. (p. 173)

After concluding its discussion of the legislative intent and extolling


the virtues of the reasonable person standard, Judge Riley considered
the merits of the reasonable victim, or woman, standard. She echoed
Prosser and Keeton (1984) with her concern that a gender-conscious
standard places Undue emphasis on gender . . . while it inappropriately deemphasizes societys need for uniform standards of conduct
(p. 166). She also felt that if the logic behind the reasonable victim
standard were extended that it would inexorably lead to the fragmentation of legal standards to the detriment of society (p. 166). The
Justice concluded with strong language related to the idea that a genderconscious standard, while intending to help women in the workplace,
would do just the opposite by reintrench(ing) the very sexist attitudes
it is attempting to counter (p. 167). She summed up as follows:
Courts utilizing the reasonable woman standard pour into the stereotypic
assumptions of women which infer women are sensitive, fragile, and in
need of a more protective standard. Such paternalism degrades women
and is repugnant to the very ideals of equality that the act is intended to
protect. (p. 167)

Justice Rileys opinion also explored the concern that the use of a
gender-conscious standard will lead to a legal slippery slope, with
other groups desiring their own standard (Radtke, 1993). These fears
proved to be warranted. In Harris v. International Paper (1991), a federal
court ruled in a racial harassment case that the proper objective standard was that of a reasonable black person (p. 1516). Also cited by
Justice Riley was Stingley v. State of Arizona (1992), which referred to
Ellison v. Brady (1991) and held that its rationale in favor of the reasonable victim standard could easily be applied to racial harassment cases.
Finally, she noted a 1993 case, Duplessis v. Training Development Corp.,
which involved allegations of discrimination based on national ancestry
made by a man of Franco-Canadian ancestry. In Duplessis, the district
court ruled that the appropriate standard to be applied in determining whether Mr. Duplessis was subjected to hostile environment harassment is that of a reasonable Franco-American (p. 677).

218

Feminism as Human Rights

Interesting perspectives on the reasonable person/victim issue are


provided by feminist theorists. Some feminists oppose the genderconscious reasonable victim standard because it reinforces the stereotype that women are not as capable as men in the workplace (Cahn,
1992; Newman, 2007). They believe that women are the equal of men
in all ways and that the use of the reasonable person standard best
reects that reality (Epstein, 2004). They also note that all women do
not perceive the same conduct as harassment and that a gender-specic
standard would ignore these differences (Cahn, 1992).
The U.S. Supreme Court has not denitively ruled on the reasonable
victim standard (Newman, 2007), though it initially appeared to be
leaning against its use in sex harassment cases. This is illustrated in
Harris v. Forklift Systems (1993), in which the Court dealt with the issue
of whether the plaintiff in a sexual harassment case needed to establish
psychological harm as an element of proof. The Court, in an opinion
written by Justice OConnor, decided that such a showing was not necessary, but in the context of doing so it laid the groundwork for what
is still a matter of dispute among the federal circuits: which objective
standard to use.
The plaintiff, Teresa Harris, worked for Forklift Systems. The president of the company was Charles Hardy. She led a sexual harassment
claim alleging that Mr. Hardy engaged in numerous behaviors that she
found unwelcome. The conduct included calling her a derogatory
name in front of her coworkers, requesting that she retrieve coins from
the front pocket of his pants, asking her to pick up items off of the
oor that he had thrown in front of her, and suggesting that the two of
them go to a hotel to discuss her raise. She eventually complained to
him, and he apologized. After the behavior began again, she sued. The
case eventually reached the Supreme Court on the psychological harm
issue.
The Supreme Court determined that victims do not need to establish
psychological harm in a sex harassment claim with Justice OConnor
writing, Title VII comes into play before the harassing conduct leads
to a nervous breakdown (p. 22). The Court then set out factors that
lower courts should consider in deciding whether conduct caused an
abusive or hostile environment. They include the frequency of the
conduct; its severity; whether it is physically threatening or humiliating, or a mere offensive utterance; and whether it unreasonably interferes with an employees work performance (p. 23).
Even though the question of which objective standard to use was
not before the justices, Justice OConnors opinion and the dissenting
opinions of Justices Scalia and Ginsburg used language that seemed to
indicate that the reasonable person standard would prevail over that of
the reasonable victim (Solberg, a&b, 1995). In her discussion of Title
VII (1964) Justice OConnor wrote, (c)onduct that is not severe enough

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

219

to create an objectively hostile or abusive work environmentan environment that a reasonable person would nd hostile or abusiveis
beyond Title VIIs purview (emphasis added; p. 21). She then added
that the act (c)ertainly . . . bars conduct that would seriously affect a
reasonable persons well-being (emphasis added; p. 22). Justices Scalia
and Ginsburg, in separate concurring opinions, both make reference to
the reasonable person.
At rst blush, it would appear that the references to the reasonable
person by the three justices would decide the issue of which standard
to utilize in sexual harassment cases. And, a short time later, the Seventh Circuit Court of Appeals did follow the language from Harris
(1993). In Dey v. Colt Construction & Development Company (1994), that
court, in a sex harassment case involving a female victim, quoted the
reasonable person language from Justice OConnors opinion in reaching its decision. A district court case out of New York, however, Currie
v. Kowalewski (1994), illustrates the lack of clarity in Harris (1993). Dorris Curries sexual harassment claim against the defendant alleged that
she was the victim of unwelcomed hugging and sexual talk, along with
improper touching and advances. The court ruled in her favor, but made
note of the uncertainty left in the wake of Harris with respect to which
objective test to follow. Ultimately, the court decided that it was irrelevant
to the case at hand because (a)lthough the Harris case did not explicitly
decide whether a reasonable person or reasonable woman (or victim)
standard applies, certainly any reasonable woman or person would have
found the defendants behavior to be offensive and repulsive (p. 63).
The Supreme Court further added to the confusion in Oncale v. Sundowner Offshore Services, Inc., (1998). Oncale dealt with the issue of
whether same-sex harassment was actionable under Title VII. In deciding that question afrmatively the Court used language that once again
left unclear what objective standard should be followed in sex harassment cases. The Court rst quoted the reasonable person language
from Justice OConnors Harris opinion (1993). Shortly thereafter, however, the opinion states the objective severity of harassment should be
judged from the perspective of a reasonable person in the plaintiffs position, considering all of the circumstances (emphasis added; p. 81). The
juxtaposition of the above has some scholars arguing that the Court is
implicitly encouraging the gender/victim conscious standard (Newman, 2007; Hill, 2005). The federal circuits remain divided on the issue.
In conclusion, while there are good arguments on both sides of the
reasonable person/reasonable victim debate, if the goal is to eliminate
sexual harassment in the workplace, courts should adopt the reasonable victim standard. If 85 percent of victims are women (EEOC, 2009),
it makes sense, despite legitimate concerns to the contrary, to use a
standard more likely to curtail the conduct that society wants to
reduce.

220

Feminism as Human Rights

AFFIRMATIVE DEFENSE REQUIREMENT TO REPORT ISSUE


A second issue that has had a major impact on women in sexual
harassment cases is concerned with the ability of an employer to
escape liability for harassment between a supervisor and an employee
due to the employees failure to report the behavior (Burlington Industries v. Ellerth, 1998; Faragher v. City of Boca Raton, 1998). This section
will discuss the Supreme Courts opinions in both cases and then focus
on the impact they have had on the sexual harassment landscape. It
will address the difculty inherent in victims, more particularly female
victims, with respect to reporting harassment (De Souza & Solberg,
2004; Fitzgerald, Swan, & Fischer, 1995; Gruber, 1989; Gruber & Smith,
1995; Hebert, 2007) and discuss cases in which courts have determined
whether proffered reasons provided by plaintiffs as to why they failed
to report harassment passed judicial muster.
Kimberly Ellerth worked for Burlington Industries for fteen months
as a salesperson, beginning in March 1993. She alleged that during the
course of her employment her supervisor, Ted Slowik, subjected her to
constant sexual harassment, mostly involving offensive threats and gestures. Ellerth refused all of Slowiks advances and faced no tangible
retaliation from him, even though he did have the authority to make
hiring and promotion decisions with the approval of his supervisor. In
May 1994 Ellerth quit. In her letter of resignation, which she faxed to
the company, she made no mention of Slowiks behavior. Three weeks
later, however, she sent a second letter that indicated that her resignation was related to Slowiks harassment. While employed by Burlington, Ellerth was aware that the company had a sexual harassment
policy, but she never reported Slowiks behavior to anyone in authority
while she was employed there.
Faragher v. City of Boca Raton (1998) involved Beth Ann Faragher,
who worked part-time as a lifeguard for the Marine Safety Section of
the City of Boca Raton between 1985 and 1990. In 1992, she led a sex
harassment action against her former supervisors, Bill Terry and David
Silverman, and also against the city. She alleged in part that Terry and
Silverman violated Title VII by creating a sexually hostile environment
by engaging in behaviors with her and other women that included
uninvited and offensive touching, lewd remarks, and speaking of
women in offensive terms. Faragher never reported the behavior to
higher management. The city had a sexual harassment policy in place
during Faraghers term of employment but failed to disseminate it
among employees working in the Marine Safety Section.
Burlington (1998) and Faragher (1998) were decided by the Supreme
Court on the same day in 1998. In both cases, the Court addressed the
issue of whether companies should be strictly liable in hostile environment cases involving a supervisor and an employee when no tangible

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

221

action was taken against the employee as a result (Faragher, 1998: Burlington, 1998). Companies had long been subject to strict, or automatic
liability, in quid pro quo cases, those involving some type of sexual activity from the harassee as a condition of employment or workplace
benets (Bennet-Alexander & Hartman, 2009, p. 408). In hostile environment cases, however, along with establishing that the behavior was
unwelcome, based on sex, and created a hostile or abusive work environment (McWilliams, 1996), the plaintiff had to prove that the company knew of the harassment, or at least should have known about it
and failed to take appropriate remedial action (Katz v. Dole, 1983).
Because plaintiffs were more likely to prevail in quid pro quo cases,
courts slowly began to expand the denition to include more and more
cases (Burlington). Part of the Supreme Courts rationale for hearing the
cases was to help dene the relevant standards of employer liability
(Burlington, p. 751).
The Court rst considered the differences between quid pro quo and
hostile environment sexual harassment. It did not nd the distinction
to be signicant except for the different proof required for the respective causes of action. In quid pro quo cases it must be established that a
tangible aspect of the plaintiffs job was affected due to his/her reaction to the harassment (Lindemann & Kadue, 1992). In a hostile environment case, it must only be established that the defendant company
was aware of the conduct or should have been aware and failed to act
appropriately (Katz, 1983). In Burlington, the Court wrote that the
issue of real concern is whether Burlington has vicarious liability for
Slowiks alleged misconduct, rather than liability limited to its own
negligence (p. 753). If liability attached under a negligence theory, it
would be incumbent upon a plaintiff to establish that the defendant
acted unreasonably (Katz, 1983). For vicarious, or strict liability to
attach, it would be enough that the supervisor was acting in his ofcial
capacity (Lindemann & Kadue, 1992).
The Supreme Court looked to its Meritor (1986) opinion when considering the extent to which employers should be held liable in harassment cases involving supervisors. Meritor held that agency principles
should guide courts in determining whether companies should be held
liable for harassment. This made it rather easy for courts to attach
liability in cases involving a tangible job benet loss. In such cases, the
supervisor was acting for the company and liability attached to the
employer (Burlington). The more difcult question, which the Court
addressed in Burlington and Faragher, concerned employer liability
when no tangible benets are involved. This is because supervisors
harassing employees without attempting to attach a benet to it are
most often acting beyond the scope of their employment (Burlington).
This makes it more difcult to attach their behavior to the company for
liability purposes.

222

Feminism as Human Rights

In its Burlington decision, the Court set out a detailed analysis of


agency law principle and concluded that there are qualitative differences between coworker harassment and harassment instigated by a
supervisor, even without the threat of a tangible loss of a job benet.
Therefore, the Court reasoned, the two harassment types should be
treated differently with respect to potential employer liability. In reaching its conclusion the Court rst reiterated that a company is strictly
liable when tangible employment action is taken by a supervisor
against a subordinate in a harassment case. Justice Souters majority
opinion in Faragher (1998) then fully explained why supervisor harassment is different in kind from co-worker harassment, even without tangible threats. Two of the three reasons relate to the power difference
between supervisors and subordinates. First, employees may nd it
more difcult to report supervisor misconduct than that instigated by a
coworker. Second, it is more difcult for a subordinate to walk away
from or tell off a supervisor than it typically would be an equal. Justice
Souters third argument is that employers have a greater opportunity
to screen, educate, and monitor supervisors than they do traditional
employees, thereby allowing them to better guard against harassment
(Faragher, 1998).
After determining that the use of vicarious liability in supervisor sex
harassment cases was appropriate, the Court turned its attention to the
Meritors agency requirements. It resolved that it would be inappropriate to hold companies strictly liable for harassment committed by a
supervisor with anything less than the loss of a tangible job benet.
The Court then formulated a middle ground in such cases, one with
the potential of holding companies strictly liable, yet also permitting
them to avoid liability altogether through an afrmative defense. The
Court set out the test as follows:
An employer is subject to vicarious liability to a victimized employee for
an actionable hostile environment created by a supervisor with immediate (or successively higher) authority over the employee. When no tangible employment action is taken, a defendant employer may raise an
afrmative defense to liability or damages, subject to proof by a preponderance of the evidence. The defense comprises two necessary elements:
(a) that the employer exercised reasonable care to prevent and correct
promptly any sexually harassing behavior, and
(b) that the plaintiff employee unreasonably failed to take advantage of any
preventive or corrective opportunities provided by the employee or
provide harm otherwise (Burlington, 775; Faragher, p. 807).

The goals of the Court were to provide companies with an incentive


to adopt measures likely to prevent sexual harassment from occurring

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

223

and to encourage employees to report harassment prior to it becoming


overly pervasive or severe (Burlington). In applying the law to the facts
of Burlington (1998) and Faragher (1998), the Court ruled that the company should be permitted to raise the afrmative defense in Burlington,
but that the City of Boca Raton failed to demonstrate reasonableness in
its actions, which was demonstrated in part by its failure to disseminate its sex harassment policy (Faragher, 1998). Due to this failure to
pass the rst prong of the afrmative defense, the Court ruled that the
defendant could not raise the afrmative defense.
The Courts two-pronged afrmative defense seems to make good
sense. Companies wishing to avoid liability will no doubt, if they have
not already done so, develop antiharassment policies, train their
employees, promptly and thoroughly investigate claims, and take
appropriate remedial action. Likewise, harassed employees will hopefully report harassment early on so as to protect their legal rights (De
Souza & Solberg, 2004). Any unreasonable failure to report may lead to
their losing all hope of recovering damages from their employer.
The difculty with the afrmative defense, which will likely impact
female victims of harassment, lies in the requirement that employees
must report the harassment. It is not typically the rst response of
a victim (Fitzgerald et al., 1995). Gruber (1989) reports that as few as
10 percent to 15 percent of women respond assertively or report harassment. Gruber and Smith (1995) elaborate in a 1995 study, reporting
that the responses by victims that the public nds most appropriate,
directly confronting the harasser or reporting the harassment, are
unlikely to occur in reality. A number of reasons are proffered as to
why victims of harassment are reluctant to report the behavior. Those
cited by Fitzgerald et al. (1995) include fear of retaliation, a belief that
nothing will be done, a desire to not harm the harasser, a fear of not
being believed and thus labeled a liar, embarrassment, and possible
harm to ones own career. In addition to these concerns, Fitzgerald
et al. (1995), discuss internal coping mechanisms as well, such as
denial, self-blame, reattribution, endurance, and detachment. Victims
also resort to avoidance, appeasement, requests to stop, and threats to
expose the harasser (Fitzgerald et al., 1995). In direct opposition to
the reporting requirement of Burlington (1995) and Faragher, (1995),
Fischer et al. state that by far the most infrequent response is to seek
institutional/organizational relief (i.e., notifying a supervisor, bringing a
formal complaint, and ling a lawsuit). Victims apparently turn to
such strategies only as a last resort when all other efforts have failed
(p. 121).
Hebert (2007) reviewed the literature related to the likelihood that
victims would report harassment and concluded that women are less
likely to report harassment than men. She commented on the impact
this would have on females in the workplace, writing:

224

Feminism as Human Rights

The different ways in which women and men react to sexual harassment
are particularly problematic for the legal rights of women because
women are more likely to invoke informal means of trying to resolve
problems of sexual harassment, while judges considering the reasonableness of the victims reaction to harassing conduct place a premium on
formal means of resolving sexual harassment claims. (p. 733)

Because relatively few women report harassment in the manner that


would be satisfactory to the courts, Hebert (2007) suggests that courts
deciding whether a failure to report or delay in reporting was reasonable, should apply the reasonable women/victim standard when
addressing the issue.
The studies noted indicate that a victim will typically not report harassment. Since the vast majority of victims are likely to be female
(EEOC, 2009), the impact of the reporting requirement is likely to have
a greater impact on female victims than male victims of harassment. In
essence, assuming that the purpose of Title VII is in part to protect
women in the workplace, the Supreme Courts Burlington (1998) and
Faragher (1998) rulings may have harmed the very victims Title VII is
attempting to protect (De Souza & Solberg, 2004).
Federal courts have attempted to determine what it means for an
employee to unreasonably take advantage of opportunities provided by
an employer to prevent or correct harassment as required by the Court
in Burlington (1998) and Faragher (1998). A number of cases are illustrative. First, if an employer has an appropriate policy in place, it is incumbent upon the plaintiff to follow it. In Madray v. Publix (2000), the
victim, Connie Lynn Madray, informed mid-level managers of alleged
harassment. Publixs policy required that employees should report harassment to the store manager, district manager, or divisional personal
managers. Since Madray reported the alleged harassment to the wrong
party and because once the employer was made aware of the harassment it took appropriate corrective actions, she lost her case.
The Madray (2000) decision may be considered a mere technical
defeat for the plaintiff. Other cases, however, dealt more directly with
the juxtaposition of the requirement that employees need to report harassment with the psychological difculty in doing so. The rst set of
such cases relates to the timeliness of the plaintiffs reporting of the harassment to the appropriate parties. In Watts v. Kroger (1999) the alleged
supervisor harassment began near the end of 1993 and escalated in
1994. Watts did not complain of the harassment when it rst occurred,
but did complain to a store manager within three months of the escalation in 1994. The court concluded that her reporting as she did was
timely and thus not unreasonable.
On the other hand, in Jackson v. Arkansas (2001), the plaintiff waited
nine months to make a report of harassment and offered no

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

225

explanation as to why she delayed. The Eighth Circuit Court of


Appeals ruled that the inexplicable nine-month delay in reporting was
unreasonable and her harassment claim was defeated. Similarly, in
Gawley v. Indiana University (2001), the victim waited seven months to
report a pattern of alleged harassment, which included inappropriate
comments and touching directed at her by her supervisor. The Seventh
Circuit ruled against Gawley, writing that her neglect of the universitys formal procedures during seven months of escalating harassment
. . . constitute an unreasonable failure to take advantage of the universitys corrective procedures (p. 312). The court also noted that she
failed to report even though her numerous attempts to directly confront her supervisor had not stopped the behavior.
The above cases illustrate one of the difculties women face in their
desire to not be harassed. As indicated above (Fitzgerald et al., 1995;
Gruber, 1989; Gruber & Smith, 1995; Hebert, 2007; Swan & Fischer,
1995) victims of harassment are reluctant to report and will engage in a
number of behaviors that stop well short of reporting harassment to
the proper parties at work. These behaviors take time, and time, as the
above cases indicate, is the enemy of a successful sexual harassment
claim.
Victims of harassment who fail to report the conduct have attempted
to explain their reticence in several ways, all of which make sense in
light of the psychological literature, but which often fail to convince
courts that the delay was reasonable. One telling example is Mativa v.
Bald Head Island (2001). When Mativa was allegedly harassed by her
supervisor, she adopted a number of delaying tactics and did not
report the harassment. First, she attempted to collect evidence to support her claims because she was concerned that she might not prevail
in a he said/she said type of situation. Second, she wanted to be certain that her supervisor was acting inappropriately and was not simply
a person who was interested in her. After determining that his motivation was improper, Mativa still did not report the behavior due to a
general fear of retaliation by her co-workers. Based on the psychological literature, Mativa acted as one might expect her to under the circumstances (De Souza & Solberg, 2004; Fitzgerald et al., 1995; Gruber,
1989; Gruber & Smith, 1995; Hebert, 2007). In essence, she struggled
with dealing with the behavior directed toward her, perhaps in the
hope it would cease without her having to go forward. The Fourth Circuit appellate court, however, easily concluded that her delay was
unreasonable, and the defendant successfully established the afrmative defense to her harassment claim. In discussing her fear of retaliation and the discomfort victims may have incoming forward, the court
wrote (w)hile such events might cause an employee stress, the
unpleasantness cannot override the duty to report sexual harassment
(p. 270).

226

Feminism as Human Rights

In Barrett v. Applied Radiant Technology (2001) and Leopold v. Baccarat


(2001) courts reached the same conclusion as Mativa (2001). The victim
in Leopold was afraid to tell anyone about the alleged harassment. Despite no evidence indicating that she would be mistreated for ling a
report she still feared potential negative repercussions. The court ruled
that her general fear was not a sufcient excuse for not reporting, writing a credible fear must be based on more than the employees subjective belief. Evidence must be produced that to the effect that the
employer has ignored or resisted similar complaints or has taken
adverse actions against employees in response to such complaints (p.
246). In Barrett the victim had a more understandable reason for not
reporting harassment. She feared retaliation and doubted that she
would be taken seriously because all of the managers were friends
with the alleged harasser. The employer had a harassment policy that
allowed employees to report harassment to any manager with whom
they felt comfortable. The Court, in ruling against Barrett, reasoned
that allowing a complainants subjective fears to control whether a failure to report was reasonable would undermine Title VIIs (1964) policy
of encouraging both employers and employees to act to eliminate sexual harassment.
One case decided in 1999 (Shaw v. AutoZone, Inc.) and two more current cases decided in 2003 (Reed v. MBNA Marketing Systems, Inc.) and
2009 (Monteagudo v. Asociacion de Empleados), respectively, provide good
examples of the difculty in deciding whether a victims failure to
report is reasonable. Shaw is demonstrative of just what a plaintiff
should not do under the law. Tiffany Shaw worked for AutoZone as
an assistant manager from February until June of 1995. Her manager
was Donald Noble. After being hired, Shaw was provided with the
AutoZone Handbook and Safety Booklet, which she was required to
read and learn. The booklet discussed the companys sex harassment
policy that emphasized that all harassment should be reported. In
addition to the policy, managers at AutoZone also received periodic
training sessions related to sex harassment and other company policies.
Shaw alleged that during her rst week at AutoZone, Noble began
making comments about her physical attributes. Later, his comments
became more explicit as he would ask her questions about pornographic movies and her sex life. While employed at AutoZone, Shaw
never complained about Nobles behavior, nor did she ever ask him to
stop. She claimed that she never made a formal report because she did
not feel comfortable with anyone at the company. The court ruled
against her. While it appreciated the understandable discomfort that
people in Shaws situation no doubt feel, it concluded . . . an employees subjective fears of confrontation, unpleasantness or retaliation do
not alleviate the employees duty under Ellerth to alert the employer to
the allegedly hostile environment (p. 813).

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

227

Reed (2003) involved the alleged harassment of a young woman,


Bobbi-Lyn Reed, by her supervisor, William Appel. Ms. Reed began
working for the defendant company in June of 1999 as a telemarketer.
She was 17 years old at the time. According to Reed, Appel almost immediately began making comments to her of a sexual nature, although
none were too severe. In August 1999, however, Reed alleged that
Appels behavior turned more aggressive. She claimed that after babysitting his two-year-old at Appells house, he pressured her into performing oral sex on him. He told her to not tell anyone because they
would both be red. He also told her that his family had inuence
with the head of the company. Reed did not report the incident. She
ignored him at work for a few days, but the sexual comments began
anew as did a request to babysit. Reed left the company during the fall
of 1999. At the time she left, she had yet to tell anyone about the
behavior. Reed returned to MBNA in May 2000 and was reassigned to
Appels team. In August 2000, Appel asked Reed to babysit and told
her she looked like she needed to wrestle. She refused. On August 28,
2000, Reed complained to MBNA ofcials about Appels behavior,
including the 1999 assault. Eventually, she led a Title VII (1964) claim.
The First Circuit Court of Appeals focused on the second prong of
the afrmative defense. It pointed out that there is no bright-line rule
regarding when a failure to le a complaint becomes unreasonable. It
cited Mativa v. Bald Head Island (2001) for the proposition that more
than ordinary fear and embarrassment is needed to justify a delay in
reporting harassment. The court concluded that where there is a truly
credible threat of retaliation that the complaint mechanism will not
prevent, the employees position is more hazardous and inaction more
easily explained (p. 36). The appellate court then focused on the two
incidents in which Reed failed to report harassment. It rst addressed
the minor comments made by Appel prior to the August 1999 incident.
It concluded that the comments and behavior exhibited up to that
point may not have triggered Title VII (1964) liability, and therefore it
was not unreasonable for Reed to fail to make a report. The court
found the failure to report the August 1999 incident a more difcult
issue to resolve, especially since it would arguably have put an end to
all future misconduct. It felt that Appels statement that they would
both be red in conjunction with his comment that his family had
inuence with the head of the company did not clearly indicate a
direct threat to Reeds job. Nonetheless, the court held that we cannot
say that a jury would be acting irrationally if (as the record stands) it
resolved factual doubts in her favor and concluded that Reed was
actually cowed by the threat and reasonably so (p. 37). This case contains much of what has been discussed thus far. It initially involved
moderately bad behavior, which may or may not trigger the second
prongs reporting requirement. It then escalated into egregious

228

Feminism as Human Rights

behavior, which should be reported right away under most circumstances. Finally, the victim was understandably scared to take action
because of her age and her uncertainty as to whether the comments
made by her supervisor regarding losing her job and his familys relationship to the owner of the company were truly threats. Can the
Supreme Court in Burlington (1998) and Faragher (1998) have possibly
intended this personal and judicial uncertainty? It appears that the
appellate court could have decided this case either way with respect to
the afrmative defense. Would the result have been different, for example, if the plaintiff had been twenty-ve rather than seventeen? And, if
so, is that a desirable outcome? (Henkels & Solberg, 2005).
Monteagudo v. Asociacion de Empleados del Estado Libre Asociado de
Puerto Rico (AEELA), decided by the First Circuit Court of Appeals in
2009, provides an interesting fact pattern. Michelle Monteagudo began
working as a substitute secretary in the human resources department
at AEELA in 1999. She was supervised by Juan Francisco Arce-Diaz
(Arce). During her initial term of employment as a substitute at
AEELA, Ms. Monteagudo felt that Arce was looking at her in a way
that made her feel she was being appraised. She did not, however,
view the behavior as overly serious. Monteagudo left the position she
was in for a time when the woman she was substituting for returned
from maternity leave. In October 2000, she returned to the human
resources department as a permanent employee.
Upon beginning her second term at AEELO in the human resources
department, Ms. Monteagudo alleged that Arce started to act inappropriately toward her at work. She claimed that he would stop by her
work station every day and touch her on the shoulder (which she
rebuffed). She also complained that he repeatedly asked her to go out
on double dates with coworkers. On one occasion an AEELA messenger, Jose Francisco Figueroa-Cana, witnessed Arce place his hands on
one of Ms. Monteagudos hips. He said that Arce was always trying to
touch Monteagudo. Monteagudo testied that the behavior continued
up to the time when Arce pulled her toward him and tried to kiss her
during a group outing at a local bar. She pushed him away, but testied that she was treated harshly at work following the incident. She
received an excessive amount of work and was told by the Director of
AEELO0 S Human Resources and Labor Relations Department, that she
would be red if she complained. She quit and led a sexual harassment claim against AEELO.
Monteagudo did not report the conduct while working for AEELO
even though AEELOs sexual harassment policy required her to do so.
Due to this omission, AEELO raised the afrmative defense set out in
Burlington (1998) and Faragher (1998). She testied that her reasons for
not reporting the behavior was because all the parties to whom she
was supposed to report to were friends with each other and Arce. The

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

229

court also noted that witnesses to the harassment did not report it either and that she was 22 at the time of the alleged harassment and
Arce was 45. The court acknowledged that even though she was not a
minor as was the victim in Reed (2003), the age difference had some
relevance with respect to her failure to report. The court concluded that
while her case was not strong, a reasonable jury could conclude that
her failure was based on more than ordinary fear or embarrassment
and was therefore reasonable (p. 172).
Two nal cases illustrate the extreme difculties facing women who
are the victims of possible sexual harassment at work in light of the afrmative defense requirements (Burlington, 1998 & Faragher, 1998).
First, in Conatzer v. Medical Professional Building Services, Inc. (2003) the
plaintiff failed to report a possible incident that occurred on September
28. She did, however, le a report a few days after a subsequent incident that took place on October 11 or 12. Nonetheless, the District
Court ruled in favor of the defendant company, determining that the
plaintiffs 17-day delay in reporting the initial incident was unreasonable. Also surprising was a courts ruling in favor of the defendant in
Philips v. Taco Bell Corp. (2000). In Phillips the plaintiff, Rita Phillips, testied in a deposition that she was sexually harassed by her supervisor,
Duane Sonntag, on ve separate occasions, which the court detailed as
follows:
On March 13, 1995, Sonntag squeezed plaintiffs breasts. On June 12,
Sonntag grabbed the front of plaintiffs pants. On June 13, 1995, Sonntag
grabbed plaintiffs hand and placed it on the front of his pants. On June
17, 1995, Sonntag rubbed his hands across plaintiffs buttocks. On June
18, 1995, Sonntag rubbed his hands across and down plaintiffs back.
(p. 1033)

Ms. Phillips did not report the March 13 incident, though she was
aware of the companys sexual harassment policy. She did le a report
on June 20, 1995, two days after the series of incidents that occurred in
June. The court determined that the March 13 incident was sexually
harassing and ruled that her failure to report until three months later
was unreasonable.
The Phillips case highlights a major problem facing women who are
harassed at work. For example, the behavior in Jones v. Clinton (1998),
which included alleged indecent exposure (discussed subsequently in
more detail) was determined to be insufcient to create a hostile environment. It is likely in Phillips that the March 12, 1995, incident, even
though it involved a sexual touching of Ms. Phillips, standing alone,
did not create a hostile environment either. This would place Ms. Phillips and similarly situated victims of harassment in a situation in
which they would in all likelihood lose a sexual harassment claim if

230

Feminism as Human Rights

they report too soon and also lose if they refrain from ling a report
immediately upon receiving unwelcomed sexual attention at work
(White, 2006).
The impact on women of the afrmative defense created by the
Supreme Court in Burlington (1998) and Faragher (1998) has the potential to be signicant in light of the psychological realities faced by
female victims. It is difcult to argue that the Supreme Courts decisions are totally unreasonable. The goal of decreasing incidents of harassment in the workplace is probably best served when such incidents
are reported sooner rather than later. But several difculties arise in
attempting to reach that goal. First, some behaviors may be inappropriate, yet not rise to the level of sexual harassment. Newer employees
may have special concerns about the impact making a complaint may
have on their careers (Hebert, 2007). Yet to not report them is to invite
the afrmative defense and risk losing a sex harassment claim (Hebert).
Additionally, the decision to report behavior is, as discussed above, as
much a psychological one as a legal one for many victims. The law
requires them to do precisely what they are unlikely to . . . report the
harassment. There may be no perfect way to protect victims of harassment and at the same time treat companies that truly are working to
eliminate harassment fairly, yet the afrmative defense, and court
interpretations of it, certainly works against women choosing to le a
claim.
LEVELS OF BEHAVIOR
In Jones v. Clinton (1998), Paula Jones alleged that then Arkansas
Governor Bill Clinton engaged in inappropriate behavior toward her in
a hotel room in Little Rock. Jones, a state employee, claimed in her
lawsuit against him that he rst made comments about her physical
appearance and put his hand on her leg and started moving it toward
her pelvic area. After she moved away, he approached her, exposed
his penis, and told her to kiss it. The District Court ruled against her,
writing that the plaintiff certainly has not shown under the totality of
circumstances that the alleged incident in the hotel . . . with the Governor were so severe or pervasive that it created an abusive working
environment (p. 675). The court acknowledged that there are situations that might be considered so severe that a single incident could
create a hostile environment, but concluded, while the alleged incident, if true, was certainly boorish and offensive . . . this is not one of
those exceptional cases in which a single incident of sexual harassment,
such as an assault, was deemed sufcient to state a claim of hostile
work environment sexual harassment (p. 675).
Though this result may seem surprising, Courts are often required
to distinguish between inappropriate behavior and behavior that is

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

231

legally considered to be sexually harassing and would subject the


employer to liability under Title VII (1964) or other similar state statutes. Solberg (a&b, 1995) analyzed post Harris (1993) cases in an effort
to determine if a bright line exists that would enable employers and
employees to predict what types of conduct would cross the line from
inappropriate to illegal. Though no clear line exists, it appears that
some general notions can be drawn with respect to degrees of misconduct and the likelihood that particular actions will be considered sexually harassing. This section of the chapter will review several of those
cases to determine where the behavior exhibited might fall along the
continuum set out.
In light of the factors set out in Harris (1993): frequency, severity,
whether the conduct was physically threatening or humiliating (or
merely just an offensive utterance), and whether the behavior unreasonably interfered with the plaintiffs work performance, Solberg (a&b,
1995) felt that behavior could be put into three categories on a continuum that would be indicative, if not conclusive, of whether the conduct in question was in violation of Title VII. The most severe
behaviors, clearly sexually harassing, were exhibited in the cases of
Dey v. Colt Construction and Development Company (1994) and Carr v.
Allison Gas Turbine (1994). The behavior in Dey, which occurred over a
two and one-half year period allegedly included Deys supervisor referring to her in explicitly derogatory terms, offering to perform a sexual act upon her, unzipping his slacks while they were on an elevator
together and suggesting that the reason she did not have a tan after
returning from a vacation was because she had spent the week in bed
on her back. Dey also testied that she was subjected to comments,
gestures, and sexual innuendoes on a daily basis. The Seventh Circuit
Court of Appeals had little trouble in determining that these types of
behaviors over that length of time were harassing under the law.
Carr (1994) involved coworker harassment. Mary Carr alleged that
while working as a tinsmith apprentice she was subjected to outrageous behavior by her all male coworkers. Among the behaviors she
claimed to be subject to were referring to her by explicitly derogatory
terms, painting such a term on her toolbox, hanging nude pin-ups
around the shop where she worked, stripping to their underwear in
front of her when changing clothes at work and cutting the seat of her
work overalls. She also claimed that one worker indecently exposed
himself to her. The Court of Appeals agreed that the words and deeds
of Mary Carrs coworkers crossed the line that separates the merely
vulgar and mildly offensive from the deeply offensive and sexually
harassing (p. 1010). The court also felt it relevant that she was specically targeted by her coworkers and that defacing her property and
mutilating her clothing were more ominous, more aggressive affronts
than mere words (p. 1010).

232

Feminism as Human Rights

The above behaviors were clearly sexually harassing. Less severe,


though inappropriate behaviors were exhibited in Doe v. R. R. Donnelly
and Sons Company (1994). The behavior alleged to have occurred in this
case, while frequent, was less severe than that discussed above. The
victim, Jane Doe, worked at R. R. Donnelley in a bindery facility. She
claims that her supervisor, Charles Stewart, commented on her clothing, asked her what clothing she wore at home or at the gym, told her
that she looked attractive because she lost weight, patted her on the
buttocks on two occasions, and told her during her yearly evaluation
that she should never have an affair at work. In her subsequent sexual
harassment case against the company, the trial court ruled against her
because the behavior was not egregious enough to violate Title VII.
The appellate court, though concerned that the behavior had taken
place over a lengthy period of time, ruled against her because her complaint was untimely. Noteworthy from this case is the appellate courts
stating that even though the behavior standing alone was probably not
severe enough to be sexually harassing in the legal sense, the fact that
it continued for a long time, might have put it in the sexual harassment
category.
Milder behavior in the view of the Seventh Circuit was displayed in
Saxton v. American Telephone and Telegraph Company (1993) and Baskerville v. Culligan International Company (1995). Saxton involved behavior
that, though unprofessional and inappropriate, was not pervasive
enough to convince the court that it created a hostile work environment under the Harris (1993) rubric. The conduct alleged to have taken
place between Marcia Saxton and her group leader, Jerome Richardson,
occurred one night when the two met for drinks to discuss workplace
issues. During the course of the evening, Richardson placed his hand
on her leg above her knee on more than one occasion, rubbed her
upper thigh with his hand, pulled her into a doorway and kissed her
for a few seconds and put his hand on her leg as he drove her home.
He later apologized and said he would not act like that again. Ms. Saxton eventually led a claim which reached the Court of Appeals, which
held (a)lthough Richardsons conduct was undoubtedly inappropriate,
it was not so severe as to create an objectively hostile work environment (p. 534). Noteworthy in this case, as in Doe (1994), is that the
court considered the length of time over which the behavior continued.
Baskerville (1995) is illustrative of how juries and courts differ at
times in their view of what behaviors are sexually harassing under the
law. Valerie Baskerville began working for Culligan in 1991 as a secretary. A short time later she was assigned to work for Michael Hall.
During the next seven months she alleged that he acted improperly by
engaging in a variety of unwelcome behaviors, none overly severe.
They included calling her pretty girl (p. 430), making a grunting
sound as she left his ofce when she was wearing a leather skirt,

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

233

telling her that he left a Christmas party early because he did not want
to lose control because of all the pretty girls there, and once in her
presence looking at his hand in a way that suggested masturbation.
She also claimed that he called her a tilly (p. 430) and that his wife
had told him to clean up his act. At trial, the jury awarded Ms. Baskerville $25,000 in damages. The Seventh Circuit Court of Appeals, in an
opinion written by Justice Posner, reversed, writing We do not think
that these incidents, spread over seven months, could reasonably be
thought to add up to sexual harassment (p. 430). In commenting upon
the purpose of sexual harassment law, the Justice wrote The concept
of sexual harassment is designed to protect working women from the
kind of male attention that can make the workplace hellish for women
(p. 430) and that It is not designed to purge the workplace of vulgarity (p. 430). In a statement indicative of what the court might consider
harassing, as opposed to merely inappropriate, Justice Posner wrote in
reference to Michael Halls conduct:
(h)e never touched the plaintiff. He did not invite her explicitly or by
implication to have sex with him, or to go out on a date with him. He
made no threats. He did not expose himself, or show her dirty pictures.
He never said anything to her that could not be repeated on primetime
television. (p. 431)

An analysis of the behaviors exhibited in the above and other cases


indicates that it is possible in many instances to determine what type
of conduct is likely to be considered sexually harassing under the law.
That more likely to be considered a Title VII violation includes extreme
physical conduct, intimidating behavior, vulgar derogatory terms,
graphic sexual talk, graphic photos, and pervasive behavior (Solberg,
a&b, 1995). Conduct less likely to be harassing under the law includes
sexual banter, occasional vulgar expressions, moderate physical contact, and non-repetitive behavior, unless it involves a high level of severity (Jones v. Clinton, 1998; Solberg, 1995). It must be kept in mind
that courts need to determine that a hostile environment was created,
not merely an inappropriate moment.
To conclude this section, it is instructive to consider the facts of the
Harris (1993) case, which rst set out the objective guidelines for courts
to consider in analyzing sexual harassment cases. Based on the cases
just discussed and the decisions reached in each, it would appear that
Harris presents a close case. Though she was the victim of several types
of inappropriate behavior, there was no physical contact, graphic sexual talk, invitations to have sex, or overly vulgar derogatory terms. On
the other hand, the behavior was directed at Ms. Harris and most
importantly, went on for a long period of time, even after she had
requested that it stop. Therefore, despite the probable closeness of the

234

Feminism as Human Rights

case, a good argument may be made that Ms. Harris would prevail in
a federal appellate court (Solberg, 1995).

CONCLUSION
This chapter has attempted to address the potential impact on
women in the workplace as a result of judicial interpretations of Title
VIIs (1964) prohibition against sex discrimination in the workplace. It
has addressed three areas. First, it reviewed the literature and case law
related to whether alleged sexual harassment in the workplace should
be judged from the perspective of a reasonable person or from that of
a reasonable victim/woman. There are strong arguments for both, with
perhaps the better arguments tending to favor the gender-specic
standard. This is due in part because even though more than twentyyears has passed since the Supreme Courts Meritor (1986) decision and
women have made great strides in the workplace, women are still
overwhelmingly the victims of sexual harassment as compared to men
(EEOC, 2009). Because of this fact, it makes sense to adopt a standard
designed in theory to help the majority of victims.
The next topic discussed is the requirement that victims of sexual
harassment committed by a supervisor report the harassment.
Although this has the goal of reducing harassment through early
reporting, it places perhaps an undue burden on victims as the literature discussed indicates that victims, particularly female victims, most
often do not report harassment early on. This puts women in a situation in which they risk losing a claim due to their failure to report,
while essentially having to guess if the behavior was serious enough to
support a claim should they choose to report. It has been suggested by
one researcher that the courts adopt the reasonable women standard to
determine whether the failure to report was unreasonable in light of
the psychological reality related to making the decision whether to
report (Hebert, 2007).
Finally, the chapter detailed a series of cases in an effort to determine how egregious behavior needed to be before it could comfortably
be termed sexual harassment under the law. Though there is no bright
line, certain types of conduct were observed to be more harassing
under the law than others. The difculty with respect to the impact on
women is that many inappropriate behaviors do not quite create the
abusive or hostile environment that the law requires. This is compounded by the requirement discussed above that harassment be
reported. Since this problem will be faced by many more women than
men (EEOC, 2009), once again the major impact is on them and not
male victims.

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

235

ACKNOWLEDGMENT
I would like to thank Tamara Henkels, a former student, for her
help in researching some of the cases referenced in the chapter.
REFERENCES
Abrams, K. (1989). Gender discrimination and the transformation of workplace
norms. Vanderbilt Law Review, 42, 11831248.
Almony, P. J. (1992). Ellison v. Brady: A legal compromise with reality in cases
of sexual harassment. Villanova Law Review, 37, 195211.
Andrews v. City of Philadelphia, 895 F.2d 1469 (3rd Cir. 1990).
Ashraf, S. (1993). The reasonableness of the reasonable woman standard: An
evaluation of its use in hostile environment sexual harassment claims
under Title VII of the Civil Rights Act. Hofstra Law Review, 21, 483504.
Barrett v. Applied Radiant Technology, 240 F.3d 262 (4th Cir. 2001).
Baskerville v. Culligan International Company, 50 F.3d 428 (7th Cir. 1995).
Bennet-Alexander, D. D., & Hartman, L. P. (2009). Employment law for business,
New York: McGraw-Hill Irwin.
Bundy v. Jackson, 641 F. 2d 934 (D.C. Cir. 1981).
Burlington Industries v. Ellerth, 524 U.S. 775 (1998).
Carr v. Allison Gas Turbine, 32 F.3d 1007 (7th Cir. 1994).
Cahn, N. R. (1992). The looseness of legal language: The reasonable woman
standard in theory and practice. Cornell Law Review, 77, 13981446.
Civil Rights Act. 42 U.S.C. 2000e et seq. (1964).
Code of Federal Regulations. Volume 29, Section 1604.11 (2000).
Conatzer v. Medical Professional Building Services, Inc., 255 F. Supp. 1259 (N.D.
Okla. 2003).
Currie v. Kowalewski, 824 F. Supp. 57 (N.D.N.Y. 1994).
De Souza, E. R., & Solberg, J. J. (2004). Law v. psychology: the need for the victim of sex harassment to report it and the likelihood that it will be
reported. Midwest Law Review, 19, 7585.
Dey v. Colt Construction & Development Company, 28 F.3d 1446 (7th Cir. 1994).
Doe v. R. R. Donnelly and Sons Company, 42 F. 3d 439 (7th Cir. 1994).
Duplessis v. Training Development Corp., 835 F. Supp. 671 (1993).
Elliott-Larsen Civil Rights Act, Section 37, 2101 et seq.
Ellison v. Brady, 924 F.2d 872 (9th Cir. 1991).
Epstein, E. (2004). Employment Law Chapter: federal Sexual Harassment and
the reasonable woman standard. The Georgetown Journal of Gender and the
Law, 5, 377386.
Equal Employment Opportunity Commission (2009, March 11). Sexual Harassment Charges EEOC & FEPAs Combined: FY 1997FY 2008. Washington, DC.
Retrieved March 17, 2009, from http://www.eeoc.gov/stats/harass/html
Faragher v. City of Boca Raton, 524 U.S. 725 (1998).
Fitzgerald, L. F., Swan, S., & Fischer, K. (1995). Why didnt she just report
him? The psychological and legal implications of womens responses to
sexual harassment. Journal of Social Issues, 51, 117138.
Gawley v. Indiana University, 276 F3d 3001 (7th Cir. 2001).

236

Feminism as Human Rights

Gettle, J. A. (1993). Comment: Sexual harassment and the reasonable woman


standard: Is it a viable solution? Duquesne Law Review, 31, 841858.
Glidden, E. A. (1992). The emergence of the reasonable woman in combating
hostile environment sexual harassment. Iowa Law Review, 77, 18251853.
Gruber, J. E. (1989). How women handle sexual harassment: A literature
review. Sociology and Social Research, 74, 39.
Gruber, J. E., & Smith M. D. (1995). Womens responses to sexual harassment:
A multivariate analysis. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17, 543562.
Gutek, B. A. (1985). Sex and the workplace. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Harris v. Forklift Systems. 510 U.S. 17 (1993).
Harris v. International Paper. 765 F. Supp. 1509 (1991).
Harvard Law Review. (1984). Note: Sexual harassment claims of abusive work
environment under Title VII. 97, 14491467.
Hebert, L. C. (2007). Why dont reasonable women complain about sexual
harassment? Indiana Law Journal, 82, 711743.
Henkels, T., & Solberg, J. J. (February 2005). Victim requirements in sexual harassment cases post Burlington. Paper presented at the Pacic Southwest
Academy of Legal Studies in Business Conference, Palm Springs, CA.
Henson v. City of Dundee, 682 F. 2d 897 (D.C. Cir. 1982).
Hill, L. K. (2005). The feminist misspeak of sexual harassment. Florida Law
Review, 57, 133186.
Jackson v. Arkansas. 272 F.3d 1020 (8th Cir. 2001).
Johnson, P. B. (1993). The reasonable woman in sexual harassment law: progress or illusion? Wake Forest Law Review, 28, 619669.
Jones v. Clinton, 990 F. Supp. 657 (East. Dist. Ark. 1998).
Katz v. Dole, 709 F.2d 251 (4th Cir. 1983).
Leopold v. Baccarat, 239 F.3d 243 (2nd Cir. 2001).
Lindemann, B.T., & Kadue, D. D. (1992). Primer on sexual harassment.
Washington, DC: BNA Books.
Madray v. Publix. 208 F. 3d 1290 (11th Cir. 2000).
Mativa v. Bald Head Island. 259 F.3d 261 (4th Cir. 2001).
McKinney v. Dole. 765 F. 2d 1129 (D.C. Cir. 1985).
McWilliams v. Fairfax County Board of Supervisors. 72 F. 3d 1191 (4th Cir. 1996).
Meritor Savings Bank v. Vinson. 477 U.S. 57 (1986).
Monteagudo v. Asociacion de Empleados. 554 F.3d 164 (1st Cir. 2009).
Newman, N. (2003). Book review: the reasonable woman: has she made a difference? Boston College Third World Law Journal, 27, 529556.
Oncale v. Sundowner Offshore Services, Inc. 523 U.S. 75 (1998).
Phillips v. Taco Bell Corp. 83 F. Supp. 2d 1029 (E. D. Mo. 2000).
Prosser, W. L., & Keeton, W. P. (1984). Torts (5th ed.). Berkeley, CA: West
Group.
Rabidue v. Osceola Rening Co. 805 F.2d 611 (6th Cir. 1986).
Radtke v. Everett 501 N.W. 2d 155 (1993).
Reed v. MBNA Marketing Systems, Inc. 333 F.3d 27 (1st Cir. 2003).
Saxton v. American Telephone and Telegraph Company. 10 F.3d 526 (7th Cir. 1993).
Shaw v. Autozone, Inc. 185 F.3d 806 (7th Cir. 1999).
Solberg, J.J. (1995a). Has Harris v. Forklift brightened the behavioral line in sexual harassment cases in the seventh circuit? Proceedings of the Tri-State
Academy of Legal Studies in Business, 19, 8395.

Sexual Harassment Laws Impact on Women

237

Solberg, J. J. (1995b). The potential impact of Harris v. Forklift Systems on the


reasonable woman standard in sex harassment cases. Midwest Law Review,
13, 3342.
Stingley v. State of Arizona. 796 F. Supp. 424 (1992).
Title VII, (1964). 42 U.S.C. Section 2000e. et seq.
Watts v. Kroger, 170 F.3d 505 (5th Cir. 1999).
White, E. D. H.. (2006). Note: A hostile environment: How the severe or pervasive requirement and the employers afrmative defense trap sexual
harassment plaintiffs in a Catch22. Boston College Law Review, 47, 853890.
Williams v. Saxbe. 413 F. Supp. 654 (D.C. Cir. 1976).
Yates v. AVCO Corp. 819 F.2d 630 (6th Cir. 1987).

This page intentionally left blank

Chapter 10

Preventing Abuse of Young


Girls and Women
Ayelet Giladi

This chapter offers a sociological/feminist perspective regarding programs for preventing sexual harassment (SH) among young children.
SH is one of many forms of abuse among young girls and women. It
also presents an alternative approach for teaching young children
human rights and includes program content and methodology. The
program aims to empower young girls, providing them with practical
tools for understanding their bodies, protecting themselves, setting
boundaries, and raising self-esteem. It outlines what they needs to
know and tells where to turn for help in making their environment
safe and secure.
This program is expected to contribute to the eld of early childhood (EC) education by giving guidance to the staff on how to teach
young children to avoid violence and foster gender equality, challenging the accepted gender power relations in our society.
The chapter is divided into two parts:
1. A theoretical review of the relationship between gender and EC education. The development of gender concepts in EC is followed and their
educational inuence on gender stereotypes and power relationships
among young children evaluated. The feminist viewpoint is described,
emphasizing the importance of educating women along these lines.
The roots of injustice and abuse of girls are traced back to early gender
relations.
2. The Israeli Ministry of Education training programs for teaching young
children equality between the sexes are outlined: Getting Along

240

Feminism as Human Rights


Together, Getting Along Together 10, and Getting Along Together
in Big Time. A fourth program for seventh to ninth grades is now being
developed.

Violence against women may be physical, psychological, sexual,


spiritual, emotional, and/or structural. It may leave visible marks and
injuries, but also covers situations in which there are no physical
wounds. While violence may be a one-time incident, there may be lifelong overt or concealed damage. Abusive behavior may also be carried
out over long periods by individuals and/or institutions seeking to
control, limit, and humiliate women.
All forms of violence against women, which occur in all cultures
and societies, are major violations of human rights. Words, pictures,
stories, and images presenting unrealistic and degrading portrayals of
women and their everyday lives are universal. Rigid conventions and
myths about the nature, appearance, and appropriate behavior of
women are used for sales purposes, and to control or limit the freedom
of women. They lock usmen and womeninto stereotypes that are
constricting, damaging, and, ultimately, death-dealing. Sexual domination and violence is increasingly portrayedin lms, videos, magazines, and booksas acceptable mass entertainment. Violent
humiliation and abuse of women provides erotic sources of excitement
and pleasure.
According to research conducted by the World Bank in 35 countries,
between 25 and 50 percent of all women suffer signicant physical,
sexual, and/or psychological abuse from their partners. Abuse is not
conned to the home, but occurs on the streets, at work, in the neighborhood, in hospitals, in wars and armed conicts, and in religious
institutions (Cec, 1999).
SH is among the most common forms of gender violence experienced by young girls and women. Young girls are often abused by
their peers, the adults around them, and strangers. They do not always
understand what is going on or realize that it is wrong. Child SH victims are expected to accept violation and not to speak up.
Research carried out over the last two decades has shown that SH of
women by men is widespread in business (Fitzgerald, Drasgow, Hulin,
Gelfand, & Magley, 1997), academia (Fitzgerald et al., 1988), and in
government and military settings (Bastian, Lancaster, & Reyst, 1996).
Since males are considered the power brokers in organizations (Farely,
1978), SH in workplaces and institutions of higher education often
involve a formal power differential (McKinney, 1994; Rospenda,
Richman, & Nawyn, 1998).
While SH also occurs among young children (Stein, 1999; Strauss,
1994), it is not much discussed because it is difcult to describe the
problem in sociological and/or psychological terms and conduct solid,

Preventing Abuse of Young Girls and Women

241

reliable research in this area (Barak, 1995; Welsh, 1999). This is particularly so among young children. In addition, most adults tend to interpret SH as an act of play (Strauss, 1994). Giladis pioneering research
in Israel revealed that young children are aware of their own and the
opposite sex, which helps them maintain gender borders. SH at young
ages apparently does not stem from sexual urges but rather from
desires related to social and cultural gender concepts, primarily the
desire to demonstrate power and gain social capital within their age
group. Considering the links between gender and sexuality is a
dynamic approach to thinking about SH among young children (Giladi,
2004).
Inuenced by post-modernism and -structuralism, including feminist
post-structuralism (i.e., post-structuralist critiques of modernist discourses, inducing pedagogical discourses in academia), there is an
increasing demand for reform in theory and practice in EC education
based on a developmentally appropriate approach (Silin, 1995; Yelland,
1998). Feminist post-structuralist voices are particularly emphatic in
this context. Inspired by the work of Valerie Walkerdine (1989, 1990)
and Bronwyn Davies (1989), these studies highlight the gendered nature of EC traditions and truths and how these affect children and
their developmental possibilities. They claim that childhood innocence
and naturalness is a myth, whereas EC practices in fact naturalize
gendered violence and aggression between boys and girls. Davies
maintains that feminist post-structuralism provides a powerful theoretical basis for understanding how children are gendered, re-evaluating
gender in EC education, and for building improved feminist pedagogies. Nevertheless, gender equity approaches applied to other areas of
education have barely affected, let alone reformed, EC education.
EC education theory considers whether small children should be
exposed to sexuality (Sears & Williams, 1999). As Bickmore (Sears and
Williams, 1999) notes, sexuality features in elementary school childrens
lives. Assumptions about childrens innocence regarding sexuality
are outdated. The (mis)information about gender relations and sexuality that ows freely today in the public-space media, from public gures, in television shows, comic strips, and billboard advertising
presents powerful implicit models of what it means to be a valued
member of society (Epstein & Johnson, 1998). Gender identities, including the discomfort that arises from breaching implicit gender boundaries, are learned early in life. Garvey (1984) observes that the practice
of mislabeling gender to upset another child is common by the age of
three to four years.
As Epstein (1998) observed, considering the continuities in the modern history of ethnic and SH: Children are both threatened and dangerous. They know and probably do too much, too soon, too young.
Sexually, they are not childish enough (p. 36). Epstein uses the terms

242

Feminism as Human Rights

schooling and sexuality, while realizing that this may be disturbing


because schooling is thought of as a public activity, whereas sexuality
is dened as a private one. Nevertheless, schooling is associated with
sexuality in rich and complex ways. In schools as well as kindergartens
(from age four), sexual and other identities are practiced and developed (e.g., via doctor and nurse and chasing games, kiss/chase play,
teasing, and bullying). This occurs in the context of the cultural repertoires and institutional conditions to which children are exposed in
their schooling. As feminist and other sex educators have long claimed,
the notion of childhood innocence is little more than a harmful excuse
for keeping young children ignorant (Epstein, 2001; Silin, 1995). Children are vulnerable because of their small size and lack of power, and
relative lack of knowledge. As Jackson (1982) points out, if children are
kept ignorant, the notion of childhood serves to deny them access to
power and justies their powerlessness. As they grow up, children acquire the capacity to protect themselves by learning to obtain and evaluate knowledge. In other words, kindergarten children are capable of
understanding SH only if they are given the tools required to so.
To explain to young children the difference between men and
women, start with identifying sex by nature and then, according to
gender, that is, how a woman or girl or man or boy dene herself or
himself and experience her/his worlds. Differentiation according to
gender, based on appearance, behaviors, chosen professions, and
choice of clothing colors, is transmitted to young children starting at
age four. The use of the term gender aims to reduce or blur the
implications of biological sex on social sex.
Gender plays a major role in the way people dene themselves. The
same gender group determines how to react to a baby girl or boy, and
determines what will be expected from them as they grow up. Young
children learn cultural stereotypes referring to boys and girls behaviors, as reected in activities, social style, and preferences, in EC
(Giladi, 2004). Children in kindergarten direct their activities in terms
of gender, which they use a basis for organizing information, from the
age of four.
Martin (1998) claims that kindergartens do not create the gender differences, but reinforce and create further gender-based differences. In
addition, in her view, children come to kindergarten bearing gender
concepts created by the family and the environment, and much is
learnt from the media, and commercials on television, newspapers,
magazines, and through the Internet. It is also impossible to ignore all
the classical stories and movies for children, such as Little Red Riding
Hood, and Sleeping Beauty. As Martin notes, all these sources gender
the body. Therefore, the kindergarten teachers ll a crucial role in setting expectations by the way they react to the childrens clothes and
behaviors.

Preventing Abuse of Young Girls and Women

243

From my experience as a college professor and SH prevention program trainer, the blindness of educational teachers and their assistants
toward gender and gender stereotypes is clear. The goal is to give
them gender lters to make them see their own and their childrens
realities from a different perspective. Most of them think that are practicing equality, but analyzing their actions, they clearly feel that more
than 50 percent of womens tasks correspond with the Ministry of Interior (cooking, cleaning, education, etc.), while their husbands are the
Foreign Ministry (manage the nances and activities outside the
home). They claim they are doing these tasks with love, devoting themselves to their children. In effect, they are transferring societys expectations to their sons or daughters. At this point, the power struggle comes
into play. Our program shows that these stereotypes allow boys and men
to behave unjustly and abusively toward young girls and women. New
concepts to counter these stereotypes should be introduced in EC education in order to reduce sex- and gender-based abuse and injustice.
Researchers probing the meaning of this gender blindness in educational staff claim that women should be aware of gender indoctrination in childrens daily life and routine. Maher, Ryan, Griesshaber,
MacNaughton, and others are critical of child-centered developmental
theories, based on the child getting all the attention needed. However,
the patriarchal structure of society should also be discussed. They
claim that this blindness involves educating women not to be aware of
the gender situation and intervene in extreme cases or power struggles.
In effect, the educational staff designing the next generation should
be provided with the tools for critical thinking and gender pedagogy.
This is easier said than done. Our gender training course leads to critical
thinking among educators, children, and their parents, focusing particularly on SH prevention among the children at early ages. Young children
from age six are trained in this way at school. Several educational programs in Israel are concerned with how to deal with sexual injury.
Psychological teams take care of injustices and abuse among children and adults who have gone through such experiences. In social
behavior terms, raising awareness to prevent violence among children
with a focus on violent sexual behavior, was found to be important.
Our program gives guidance to both educational staff and children.
From a sociological point of view, many SH incidents are associated
with inappropriate behavior norms, whether they involve harassment
or encouraging it, either knowingly or unknowingly. In Israel, which is
not the only country where SH is the norm, those not engaging in it
are considered geeks or outsiders. Our society is sick, because no
distinction is made between right and wrong, and inequality reigns. A
combination of power behavior and gender roles or stereotypes creates
the conditions for injustice and abuse. Our research indicates (Giladi,
2004) that children behave in this way not to gain sexual prot but to

244

Feminism as Human Rights

get power over their friends and improve their position in their peer
group. Teachers should understand the connection between power
behavior and gender. They need to learn to recognize, understand, and
put on special gender lters.
At the start of my career, Dr. Susan Strauss, who was among the
rst to report on SH in young children, offered me moral support and
I followed in her footsteps. My PhD thesis, Perceptions and Observations of Young Childrens Experiences in Kindergarten and Early
Schooling in Israel, (2004), dealt with SH theory and empirical data.
There was clearly also a need to develop SH prevention tools to combat injustice and abusive behavior suffered by girls, who would
become the women of the future.
Four prevention programs have been or are being developed for
young children:
1. Getting Along TogetherPreventing Sexual Harassment in Young Children (for
children aged between four and seven, from kindergarten up to second
grade).
2. Getting Along Together 10Preventing Sexual Harassment in Young Children
(for children aged between 8 and 10).
3. Getting Along Together in Big TimePreventing Sexual Harassment in Young
Children (for children aged between 11 and 13).
4. A program for seventh to ninth grades is now being developed.

These SH prevention programs, based on sociological perspectives,


deal with important values, such as respect, dignity, and equality. The
teachers appreciated the attempt to bring back old values, rst mentioned in the Bible (Leviticus, chapter 9), and were pleased to use them
in daily interactions with the children and their parents. For example,
teachers were asked to introduce these tools when presenting the Hanukkah and Passover stories, and when discussing verbal violence,
physical or bullying incidents, and so on. Equality between boys and
girls was the most important value presented and injustice and abuse
were dened. Everyone has the right to feel protected in their homes
and schools, on school buses, and in the parks. The children were
taught that no one, including their fathers, mothers, relatives, best
friends, and strangers, should act toward their bodies without respect
and dignity. The teachers felt that the children understood and
believed in the program. The concepts in the SH prevention program
may be connected with feminist pedagogy, which emphasizes relationships and inuences on construction of knowledge, and subjectivism
over objectivism, and working together. The children are made aware
of the role of subjective connection to the individual voice, and the
effect of meaning construction. Feminist pedagogy is based on a variety
of perspectives and many voices: Which stories should children be told

Preventing Abuse of Young Girls and Women

245

in SH prevention programs, and how should they be told? How can an


atmosphere be created to empower mixed audience of boys and girls
to learn, think, and be critical of their environment: To distinguish
between right or wrong on social issues, dene SH in particular, create
a feeling of openness among the girls enabling them to speak freely
without been afraid of what the boys think or the teacher would think?
How can they feel protected/shielded? How can the patriarchal paradigm in society be changed by the children or by their teachers?
This particular story occurred with a group of kindergarten children
taking part in the Getting Along TogetherPreventing Sexual Harassment
in Young Children. One of the mothers came to speak in private to a
kindergarten teacher named Ilana. She asked whether her daughter
was being exposed to SH in any way. Ilana answered that the children
were learning how to protect their bodies in the Getting Along
TogetherPreventing Sexual Harassment in Young Children program. The
mother then told her that she had to leave the house to take one of her
joint children with her second husband to hospital. When she returned,
her six-year-old daughter told her that her nine-year-old stepbrother
had suggested showing her his penis and then wanted them to touch
each another. Her daughter told him that she did not want to do this
and went into her room, which she only came out of when her mother
came back very late at night. When asked where she learned to say
No, she said that Ilana told her that no one can force this kind of
behavior on her and that she should report it. The mother was very
proud of her daughter and thought that it was very important that her
child had learned about these issues because she herself had been
abused by adults when she was young and still carried the trauma
with her but had never told anyone about it until now. In tears, she
thanked Ilana for saving her daughter from having similar experiences.
In this case, the program changed the patriarchal paradigm for that
particular little girl and seems to have saved her from being abused, as
well as protecting her rights as a child.
How can teachers be given the tools to use after the end of the program? To answer these questions, it is clear that this kind of pedagogy
has to change the basic patriarchal view. The SH prevention programs
were built to include childrens real-life stories, which were used as the
basis for discussions on sexual liberation and injustice. These stories
highlight how lack of respect and equality leads to violence between
groups of boys and girls who might think they are only playing but, in
fact, are engaging in SH, for example, pulling down pants or underwear, lifting up skirts, kissing and touching without consent, or touching each others sexual organs. Throughout the program, teachers refer
to such behavior as SH, and discuss and share their feelings about it,
as an alternative to traditional pedagogies, which focus on the transmission of knowledge. Teachers now learn how to work with the

246

Feminism as Human Rights

students and be empowered by the learning process (Curtis & Rasool,


1997). In this way, feminist pedagogy has managed to change the basic
construction of the patriarchal paradigm, which is unjust and leads to
abusive behaviors, most of the victims of which are girls.
Another aspect of feminist pedagogy (Maher, 1999, 2001) is the progressive education and child in the center approach, in which children develop in a supportive, secure environment. These pedagogies
ignore power relationships among children and with their teachers.
Maher claims that the teachers role is passive in progressive education.
While teachers are expected to encourage the students to express themselves, their authority is problematic since they want to avoid traditional education systems. According to Maher, post-modernism makes
an important contribution to feminism by challenging subjective identity and the authority of teachers and educators, and the dynamic and
changing power relationships. Getting Along Together 10Preventing Sexual Harassment in Young Children for Third to Fifth Grades and Getting
Along Together in Big TimePreventing Sexual Harassment in Young Children for Fifth and Sixth Grades deal with this issue by discussing negative and positive power: Violent behavior is a form of negative power,
while the leader of the class or other children may use positive power
to prevent violence, bullying, or boycotting. The teachers authority can
be explained as using positive power to lead the class to a better
placeto be good citizens who respect others and treat them as equals.
The teacher uses authority as a dynamic force she hopes the students
will take with them. In this framework, the teachers present themselves
as knowledgeable and willing to learn at the same time. In this way,
teachers gain respect, and the students learn how to be responsible.
The result should be construction of knowledge that does not come in
a hierarchical mode, but in a developmental context. To demonstrate
this point, the program teaches how to avoid sexual violence and not
only the children but also the teachers gain from this process.
In the SH prevention program, children and their teachers are
offered an approach to both human rights and gender and alternative
feminism dialogues. Discussions are held to distinguish among various
gender and power relationships both inside and outside the classroom.
According to MacNaughton (2000), the the post-structuralism assumption claims that there are only a few forms of dialogues in society,
while our programs initiate new forms of gender relationships during
play and in the childrens relationships. After discussing various forms
of disrespect, indignity, and inequality, the connection is made
between SH and gender. Teachers are also taught how to talk to children about subjects that were previously taboo. Children learn how to
refer to their sexual organs in a respectful way in Hebrew and are told
that they will eventually get used to these words and respect themselves more. After 10 weeks of work with the children in the sixth

Preventing Abuse of Young Girls and Women

247

grade, one teacher thanked our team for the . . . special way of teaching the children such a sensitive subject in such a perceptive way,
without getting complaints from the parents. Since the children are
addressed as adults and with respect, this generates a very straightforward, honest, down-to-earth dialogue.
While presenting the teachers and the children with a feminist pedagogy (Maher, 1999, 2001), dynamic and changing power relationships
are discussed. Teachers and children are taught that power is in their
hands and they should use it in a positive way to protect their rights
over their bodies, and, when necessary, to say No, to children and
adults around them.
Such alternative dialogue needs to be adopted throughout the year
and in the entire curriculum. In Bible lessons, the values in the texts
should be used to understand what happened and re-evaluate childrens behavior in violent ghts. These values should serve as a good
basis for preventing violence, particularly SH and sexual abuse, as
forms of gender-power injustice.
As MacNaughton (2000) suggested, the way teachers regard their
students should be changed by using a curriculum suited to their developmental stage, and they should be responsible for satisfying individual childrens needs while, at the same time, acting according to
gender perspectiveswith the help of new gender lters. Therefore,
educators and teachers equipped with the tools for feminist and
human-rights discussions should understand childrens actions in
terms of gender-sex behavior, while teachers without these tools do not
identify gender-power relationships in childrens play and relationships, such as the eld-work example presented below.
The Chinese Jump Rope Game
During a recess when most of the children were playing in their gender
groups, three boys were conspicuously wandering around bored, whispering to one another, and making a noise. After a few minutes, they
approached a girl who was holding a Chinese jump rope between her
legs (over which a group of girls were jumping). The girl noticed the
threatening presence of the boys and warned them that if they did not
go away, she would call the teacher in charge. One of the boys
approached her from behind, drew close to her, and forced a kiss on her
cheek. The girl blushed and stood there in pain and embarrassment. Her
friends, who witnessed the event, stopped their game and went up to
her. The girl, meanwhile, sat down on the ground and clutched her
cheek in her hands. Her friends suggested that she go to the teacher in
charge and complain about the boy, but she did not respond. Two other
girls called out to the teacher, Amir gave Inbar a kiss. The teacher in
charge, who was busy talking to another child, did not hear them. I
looked for Amir and did not nd him in the playground; probably he
had gone into the school.

248

Feminism as Human Rights

After three minutes, the girls decided to move, and took Inbars hand
and went to the new place. I approached this new place to observe
Inbars behavior, and suddenly heard someone shout, Enough, enough
already! and a crying voice reverberated through the playground. It
seemed that Amir had attacked Inbar again. Inbar stopped playing, mustered up courage, went to the teacher in charge, and told her what had
happened. The teacher responded, He denitely wants to be your boyfriend, so maybe give him your telephone number? Inbar, apparently
realizing that the teacher did not understand how she felt, returned to
her friends with a sour expression. The friends were perplexed. They
checked her cheek, and one of them said, You dont have any mark on
your cheek. They stopped playing the Chinese jump rope game. For the
rest of the playtime I did not see Amir and his two friends in the playground. They must have ed into the school.

In this example, the teacher was not able to identify gender-power


relationships in the childrens play and relationships, which is why she
answered the girl in that way. She looked at the incident from the perspective of an adult confronting the harasser and never thought about
it from the girls point of view.
As a professor in college, one of my major aims is to allow my EC
education students to nd alternative dialogues and meanings. They
learn sociological, developmental, historical, cultural, public health,
and legal perspectives, as well as post-structural feminist theories that
challenge standard EC concepts. Children and their teachers are asked
to observe gender, power, and status relationships, as a way of preventing violence, particularly SH and abuse.
Groups of children hear, read, and act out plays and stories taken
from their routine daily life. They learn to answer questions about their
gender world, as a way of understanding how they operate, make their
choices, and use their power relationships. They are directed to analyze
them through respect, dignity, and equality between the sexes, and
share their feelings while condent, secure, and safe in their peer
groups. For example, children in recess were playing and surng the
on the slide when one of the girls came to the kindergarten teacher
complaining that one of the boys did not respect her because he said
that he would take the slide when her turn came. When the teacher
called the boy over, he said that he was not proud of his action and
would not do it next time. She used values to solve a potentially violent incident. If children were given clear and consistent boundaries,
they would react differently in many situations, both in and out of
school.
When schools do not lay down clear policies and boundaries, and
teachers do not hold a gender dialogue with them, the children are not
able to understand their subjectivism, and prevent violent behavior.
Girls suffering from injustice and abuse will grow up to be adults

Preventing Abuse of Young Girls and Women

249

who are subject to similar behavior in their homes, the army, and
workplaces.
Teachers awareness of sexual violence is vital for creating an atmosphere of equality between boys and girls. Teachers should learn how
to achieve this in order to promote a new just and non-abusive atmosphere among young children. Equality fostered by this program provides teachers with a new look at gender and power relationships in
childrens games. One way is to nurture children who challenge gender
behavior, typically using values of respect, dignity, and equality.
Because these SH prevention programs try to achieve changes from an
early age, these methods give the children and their teachers the ability
to be critical, think in a sincere way, and have autonomy, without
operating from a gender status in their own peer group. If children
understand gender and its implications, this should lead to less abuse
of women by boys and men and reduce the number of such incidents
among girls and women. They would realize that SH is not something
girls or women should get used to, but should try and prevent such
behavior, and if it happens, there is no need to be silent about it.
This chapter tried to show how links between gender, power relationships, and EC puts the child in the center within the post-structural
feminist dialogue, which sees the social context of the gender power
relations between the children. The teacher directs the children to build
alternative power relationships among themselves and with adults
who threaten their safety by violating their respect, dignity, and right
to equality. An atmosphere of justice with active involvement and
action would be expected to improve young girls faith in society and
reduce SH and sexual abuse particularly at an early age.
This chapter tried to show how schools should prepare students for
real life from an early age, the great importance of providing tools for
understanding, interpreting, and challenging social situations inside
and outside kindergarten and schools. It discusses feminist methodology aimed at empowering young girls and women educators to promote this kind of social change. These changes in EC education are
expected to contribute toward reducing injustice and abuse suffered by
young girls and women.

REFERENCES
Barak, A., Pitterman, Y., & Yitzhaki, R. (1995). An empirical test of the role of
power differential in originating sexual harassment. Basic and Applied Social
Psychology, 17, 497517.
Bastian, L., Lancaster, A., & Reyst, H. (1996). Sexual harassment survey. Arlington, VA: Defense Manpower Data Center, Department of Defense.
Curtis, C., & Rasool, J. A. (1997). Motivating future educators through empowerment: A special case. The Education Forum, 61, 307313.

250

Feminism as Human Rights

Davies, B. (1989). Frogs and snails and feminist tales: Preschool children and gender.
North Sydney, Australia: Allen and Unwin.
Epstein, D., & Johnson, R. (1998). Schooling sexualities. Philadelphia: Open University Press.
Fitzgerald, L., Drasgow, F., Hulin, C., Gelfand, M., & Magley, V. (1997). Antecedents and consequences of sexual harassment in organizations. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 82, 578589.
Fitzgerald, L., Shullman, S., Bailey, N., Richards, M., Swecker, J., Gold, Y., et al.
(1988). The incidence and dimensions of sexual harassment in academia
and workplace. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 32, 152175.
Garvey, C. (1984). Childrens talk. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Giladi, A. (2004). Sexual harassment? Perceptions and observations of young
childrens experiences in kindergarten and early schooling in Israel. APU
University, 2327, 3036, 75100, 162175, 291.
Jackson, S. (1982). Childhood and sexuality. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
MacNaughton, G. (2000). Rethinking gender in early childhood education. London:
Paul Chapman Publishing.
Maher, F. A. (1999). Progressive education and feminist pedagogies: issues in
gender, power and authority, Teachers College Record 101, 1, 3559.
Maher, F. A. (2001). John Dewey, Progressive education and feminist pedagogies: issues in gender and authority. In K. Weiler (Ed.), Feminist engagement
(pp. 1332). New York: Routledge.
Martin, K. A. (1998). Becoming a gender body: Practices of preschools. American Sociological Review, 63, 494511.
McKinney, K. (1994). Sexual harassment and college faculty members. Deviant
Behavior: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 171191.
Rospenda, K. M., Richman, J. A., & Nawyn, S. J. (1998). Doing power: A conuence of gender, race and class in contra-power sexual harassment. Gender and Society, 12, 4060.
Ryan, S., & Griesshaber, S. (2005). Shifting from developmental to postmodern
practices in early childhood teacher education. Journal of Teacher Education,
56, 3435.
Sears, J., & Williams, J. (1999). Queering elementary education. Lanham, MD:
Rowman & Littleeld Publishing Group and National Book Network.
Silin, J. (1995). Sex, death and the education of children: Our passion for ignorance in
the age of Aids. New York: Teachers College Press.
Stein, N. (1999). Classrooms and courtrooms. New York: Teachers College Press.
Strauss, S. (1994). Sexual harassment at an early age. Principal, 74, 2730.
Walkerdine, V. (1989). Counting girls out. London: Virago.
Walkerdine, V. (1990). School-girl ctions. London: Verso.
Welsh, S. (1999). Gender and sexual harassment. Annual Review of Sociology, 25,
169190.
Yelland, N. (Ed.). (1998). Gender in early childhood. New York: Routledge.

Chapter 11

In Womens Voices
Lindsay Baker

Feminism to me means equality on all levels. I see the future of


feminism focusing more on gender equality. Today there are more
women in the workplace. This began to change during the economic
depression of our society. Middle-class families were unable to sustain
a certain style of living on the single salary of a husband. Therefore,
women in households were almost forced, as it may be described, to
go outside of the home, where they had spent their days taking care of
the children and making sure dinner was ready by ve oclock, to supplement the increased costs from the decline of the economy.
Many good things have come from women in todays workforce.
Women have been given the opportunity to increase self-satisfaction
and self-esteem. In addition to personal leaps and bounds, women in
todays workforce have had the opportunity to work in a changing
economy, a changing world. They contribute to their family, as well as
to their community in the workplace.
When a woman works in the home, she is very focused on the needs
of others: cooking, cleaning, doing laundry, and other necessary home
tasks. In the workplace, women have the opportunity to spend their
days meeting their own needs. For most women, the workforce may be
considered less stressful and less demanding than being home with
their spouse and children. However, I feel that feminism is much more
than just equality in the workplace.
Beginning with the rst wave of feminist movements, women have
worked hard to develop equal rights. In the second wave, womens
cultural and political inequalities were expressed. Women spoke up
and helped others understand how these inequalities could affect their
personal lives. The third wave brought about gender indifferences, it

252

Feminism as Human Rights

helped to explore sexuality, as well as analyze what many would call


gender-specic roles.
All of the explorations in the three waves of feminism are what have
brought women to this place in todays society. Domestic violence centers have been started. Activism against inequalities takes place all over
the world. College courses on the feminist movement have been
taught. Books on gender equality have been written. All of this has
been possible because of the feminist movement.
What do I see for the future of feminism? This question is hard to
answer. In todays society the current economy has put people in
tough times. I predict that the label of feminists will begin to
phase out. I see future youth, of both genders, taking a stand. I see
pro-feminist ideas continuing to be implemented through education in
school, crisis center openings, and even more options for counseling
and issue discussion to take place.
The future is going to bring more opportunity for equality. The
future is going to bring more opportunity for societal growth. I see
feminism becoming more and more popular; however, I dont see the
title of feminist beliefs continuing. I see earlier generations integrating equality of gender in their everyday life. In the future, I would
hope that schools would help to educate our students on why gender
equality is so important. Also, I would hope to see employers allowing
employees the opportunity to be trained on these issues. I would hope
that sufcient sexual harassment training will be offered, as well as the
opportunity for all companies to pay their employees equally for equal
skill and quality work.
In addition to the youth of todays society, I hope to see a collaboration of all ages and genders in the future. By collaborating, men and
women can work together to express their views. They can work together to make changes, as well as make a stand for inequality all over
the world. Feminist politics aims to end domination to free us to be
who we areto live lives where we love justice, where we can live in
peace. Feminism is for everybody (hooks, 2000, p. 118).
REFERENCE
hooks, b. (2000). Feminism is for everybody: Passionate politics. Cambridge, MA:
South End Press.

About the Editor and Contributors

EDITOR
Michele A. Paludi, PhD, is the Series Editor for Womens Psychology for
Praeger Publishers. She is the author/editor of 33 college textbooks, and
more than 160 scholarly articles and conference presentations on sexual
harassment, campus violence, psychology of women, gender, and sexual
harassment and victimization. Her book, Ivory Power: Sexual Harassment
on Campus (1990) received the 1992 Myers Center Award for Outstanding
Book on Human Rights in the United States. Dr. Paludi served as Chair of
the U.S. Department of Educations Subpanel on the Prevention of Violence, Sexual Harassment, and Alcohol and Other Drug Problems in
Higher Education. She was one of six scholars in the United States to be
selected for this Subpanel. She also was a consultant to and a member of
former New York State Governor Mario Cuomos Task Force on Sexual
Harassment. Dr. Paludi serves as an expert witness for court proceedings
and administrative hearings on sexual harassment. She has had extensive
experience in conducting training programs and investigations of sexual
harassment and other EEO issues for businesses and educational institutions. In addition, Dr. Paludi has held faculty positions at Franklin & Marshall College, Kent State University, Hunter College, Union College, and
Union Graduate College, where she directs the human resource management certicate program. She teaches in the School of Management.

CONTRIBUTORS
Lindsay Baker graduated from Southern Vermont College in December
2008 with a BS in Business Administration and Management. She is enrolled in the General MBA Program, as well as the Certicate of Human
Resources Program at Union Graduate College. She plans to graduate in

254

About the Editor and Contributors

June 2010. Lindsay is currently interning at Horizon Bradco of Schenectady, New York, in Human Resources and Accounting.
Sarah Metzgar Boggess has more than 20 years career experience, spanning the functions of communications, marketing, journalism, governmental relations, and resource development. She currently works in the
eld of higher education. She has a bachelors degree in economics and
government from Dartmouth College and a masters degree in business
administration and certicate in Human Resources Management from
Union Graduate College.
Donna Castaneda, PhD, is professor of psychology at San Diego State
UniversityImperial Valley Campus. She received her PhD in social psychology from the University of California, Davis. Her research focuses on
gender and ethnicity and their relationship to mental and physical health.
She has investigated the impact of close relationship factors in HIV/AIDS
risk behavior, particularly among Latinos; the HIV/AIDS prevention
needs of women factory workers in Mexico; the close relationship context
and how it affects intimate partner violence; and the relationship between
marital satisfaction and mental health among wives and husbands.
Another area of interest is in the role of structural factors, or aspects of
service delivery systems, in the provision of health and mental health
services to Latinos.
Joan C. Chrisler, PhD, is Class of 1943 Professor of Psychology at Connecticut College, where she teaches courses on the psychology of women
and health psychology. She has published extensively on the psychology
of women and gender and is especially known for her work on womens
health, menstruation, weight, and body image. She edited Sex Roles: A
Journal of Research from 2002 to 2006, and is editor or co-editor of eight
books, including Women over 50: Psychological Perspectives (2007), From
Menarche to Menopause: The Female Body in Feminist Therapy (2004), Lectures
on the Psychology of Women (4th ed., 2008), and the Handbook of Gender
Research in Psychology (forthcoming).
Cynthia Garrett, MA, MSW, is Lecturer in Psychology at Connecticut
College, where she teaches laboratory sections of the Psychology as a Natural Science course. She previously practiced clinical social work at the
Wheeler Clinic and the University of Connecticut Health Center. Her
areas of expertise are trauma, women and addiction, and the clinical use
of dialectical behavior therapy.
Ayelet Giladi, PhD, is a pioneer and recognized expert on the harassment of young children in Israel. Dr. Giladi earned her undergraduate

About the Editor and Contributors

255

and masters degree at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem. Dr. Giladi, a


professor at Kibbutz College, teaches her students how to recognize sexual harassment of children, conduct qualitative research, and report on
the phenomenon. In addition, Dr. Giladi conducts research on the issue,
consults with a variety of organizations, and conducts training about sexual harassment at an early age. She has authored prevention programs for
use with children ages 4 to 7 and 11 to 13. She is the chairwoman of the
Voice of the Child Association: Prevention of Sexual Harassment Among
Young Children. She has been featured on Israeli television, radio programs, and newscasts and in Israeli newspaper articles. Dr. Giladi trains
family judges, physicians, nurses, psychologists, educators, and school
administrators about sexual harassment. She earned a doctorate in sociology education from England ARU University.
Shelly Grabe, PhD, a psychologist and research scientist at the University of CaliforniaSanta Cruz, received her training from Michigan State
University and the Universities of Missouri and Washington. Her
research interests are in the area of the psychology of women and focus
on an examination of how the treatment of womens bodies as objects contributes to the process of marginalization of women via threats to their
psychological well-being. Dr. Grabe is trained as a statistician and methodologist and has extensive experience conducting empirical research
examining the connection between womens bodies and psychological
well-being. She was a recipient of the Ruth L. Kirschstein National
Research Service Award for her research on womens body objectication
and a Visiting Scholar at the University of WisconsinMadisons Womens Research Center before beginning an Associate Professor position in
Social Psychology at the University of CaliforniaSanta Cruz in the fall of
2008. Her current interests are in using interdisciplinary, feminist
research to address social justice in the context of globalization.
Katie L. Kelly graduated from Union College in 2008 with a BS in Psychology and a Minor in Classics. She completed her HR Certicate at
Union Graduate College in August 2009. Thereafter, she hopes to pursue
an MA in Industrial Organizational Psychology, using her skills in psychology and business in the HR Field.
Kristina Hicks earned her MBA from Union Graduate College, where
she specialized in Human Resource Management. She also earned a Certicate in Human Resource Management from Union Graduate College.
Her interests include women and management and human resource
management, including training and development, recruitment and
selection, motivation, communications, and international human
resource management.

256

About the Editor and Contributors

Jennifer L. Martin is the department head of English at a public alternative high school for at-risk students in Michigan and a lecturer at Oakland
University, where she teaches graduate research methods in the department of Educational Leadership, Feminist Methods, and Introduction to
WGS in the department of Women and Gender Studies. She is not only a
feminist teacher, but a feminist activist. She has volunteered as an assault
responder and engaged in political action for feminist causes. Currently,
she is the Title IX Education Task Force Chair for the Michigan National
Organization for Women in order to advocate for Title IX compliance in
Michigans schools. She has conducted research and written articles on
the topics of peer sexual harassment, teaching for social justice, service
learning, and the at-risk student.
Michelle McKenzie received her MA in psychology from Pepperdine
University. Her academic interests include violence against women and
sexual assault prevention. She is currently working on her memoir about
date rape. She is also a speaker for the Rape, Abuse and Incest National
Network and a rape crisis counselor.
Noorfarah Merali, PhD, is an Associate Professor and the Coordinator/
Director of the Counselling Psychology Graduate Program at the University of Alberta. She is a specialist in cultural diversity and cross-cultural
adjustment. Dr. Meralis research program examines family adaptation
across cultures and through the cultural transition process instigated by
immigration. Her research addresses two family subsystems: the parentchild subsystem and the husbandwife dyad. With respect to marital relations, Dr. Merali has recently been working collaboratively with cultural
community organizations in Canada to study arranged marriage practices and their application across international borders. Her research in
this area has identied theoretical frameworks for studying marriage
migration, changes to federal government policies to reduce the vulnerability of women in international arranged marriages to intimate partner
violence, and training implications for community workers responding to
marital conict/violence and mental health problems like depression
among individuals in arranged marriage situations. Dr. Merali acts as a
consultant to settlement agencies and cultural community organizations,
taking a leadership role in ethnic community needs assessments, program
design and evaluation, and staff training. She is a Registered Psychologist
with the College of Alberta Psychologists and is a member of the Canadian Psychological Association, American Psychological Association, and
the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues (International).
Carmen A. Paludi, Jr. holds advanced degrees in Electrical Engineering
from Clarkson University and Syracuse University, and has conducted

About the Editor and Contributors

257

graduate studies in Applied Physics and Electronics Engineering at the


University of Massachusetts, and Engineering Management at Kennedy
Western University. His 32-year career spans work for the Department of
Defense in Federal Civil Service as well as the private sector. He has held
positions as Senior Principle Engineer, Member of the Technical Staff, and
Senior Scientic Advisor for the United States Air Force, Sanders Associates, The MITRE Corporation, Titan Corporation, and L-3 Communications, Inc. He has more than 20 technical publications in refereed journals,
and presentations at international symposia and conferences. Mr. Paludi
was an adjunct faculty at New Hampshire Technical College, a guest lecturer at the Advanced Electronics Technology Center at the University of
Massachusetts, and is a frequently lecturer at the Union Graduate College. He is a Capability Maturity Model Integration (CMMI) trained and
certied Appraisal Team Member by the Software Engineering Institute
at Carnegie Mellon University. He has developed and presented in-house
training programs in Requirements Management, Requirements Development, and Risk Management. He has more than 30 years of program
management experience.
Patricia D. Rozee, PhD, is a professor of Psychology and Womens Studies at California State University, Long Beach, where she teaches classes
in the Psychology of Women, and Violence Against Women. Dr. Rozee is
co-editor of the award winning textbook, Lectures on the Psychology of
Women, now in its fourth edition. She has published extensively in the
areas of sexual assault and violence against women.
Anne Sisson Runyan, PhD, Professor in (and former Head of) the
Department of Womens, Gender, and Sexuality Studies at the University
of Cincinnati with a courtesy appointment in political science, is a pioneer
of and specialist in the eld of feminist world politics. Her most recent
books include Global Gender Issues in the New Millennium (2009) and the
forthcoming Beyond Gender and Global Restructuring: Sightings, Sites, and
Resistances, and her current projects include books on transnational feminist politics and North American feminisms.
Joseph Solberg graduated from the University of Notre Dame in 1979
with a degree in Accounting. In 1979, he began law school at the Loyola
University of Chicago School of Law. He received his law degree in 1982.
He worked as an assistant states attorney in Bloomington, IL, for several
years and joined the faculty at Illinois State University in 1988. He currently teaches courses in the Legal and Ethical Environment of Business,
Business Ethics and Corporate Responsibility, Real Estate Law, and Labor
and Employment Law. His research interests include employment discrimination, with an emphasis on sexual harassment, ethics, and teaching
pedagogy.

258

About the Editor and Contributors

Lindsey Speach graduated from Siena College in May 2007 with a BS in


Marketing Management and a minor in Sociology. She is enrolled in the
Certicate of Human Resources program at Union Graduate College and
plans to join the MBA program. Lindsey is currently the Recruiting Coordinator for the Northeast region of KeyBank N.A.
Monica Ulibarri, PhD, is a Postdoctoral Fellow at the University of
CaliforniaSan Diego and a lecturer at San Diego State University. She
completed her PhD in Clinical Psychology from Arizona State University.
Her research focuses on HIV prevention in Latino/a communities, with an
emphasis on how individual- and relationship-level factors such as mental
health, substance use, history of childhood abuse, intimate partner violence, and sexual relationship power intersect with HIV risk behaviors.

Index

Abduction homicide, 5354


Abortion, 13031; rst-trimester, 135;
Global Gag Rule, 131
Acculturation, 111
Act of Equality between Men and
Women, 157
Advancement inequity, 165
Afrmative defense requirement, to
report sexual harassment, 22030;
reluctance and delay of victims in
reporting, 22330
Afghan, marital rape in, 133
African Union, 132
Age Discrimination in Employment
Act (ADEA), 151
Agency and empowerment, 24
Agrarian Reform Laws, Nicaragua, 35
Albania, 186
Alcohol consumption, and rape, 182
AMBER Alert program, 6667
American Association of University
Women, 164
Americans with Disabilities Act
(ADA) (1990), 152
Amnesty International, 5
Andrews v. City of Philadelphia, 212,
214
Animal husbandry, women in, 29
Argentina, 186
Armed conict and rape, 19193;
ethnic cleansing, 19293

Arranged marriage: consent and


coercion, 1046, child marriage,
105, choice, level of, 105, widow
marriage, 1056; denition, 102;
economic rights, 101;
individual-level variables affecting,
11012, acculturation, 111,
education, employment, gender,
11011, non-family living
arrangements, 111, opportunities
for peers interaction, 110, Western
culture exposure, 110; interpersonal
outcomes: marital satisfaction,
11215, spousal abuse, 11618; law,
policy and human rights
advocacy, implications, 12125;
laws related to: age of marriage,
107, 108, choice of mate selection,
106, dowry system, 1089, gender
equality, 108, womens freedom of
living arrangements, 108; nature of,
103; personal outcomes:
criminality, 121, mental health
problems and self-harm, 11921,
social rights, well-being, 11819;
101; subtypes: cooperative or
joint-venture pattern, 104,
modied traditional or
delegation pattern, 104, traditional
or planned pattern, 1034;
Arroyo, President, 135

260

Index

Assisted reproduction, 136


Australia, 191; reproductive rights in,
134
Austria, 155
Baker, Lindsay, 25152
Bangladesh, 179, 192; family planning
in, 137
Barrett v. Applied Radiant Technology,
226
Baskerville v. Culligan International
Company, 232
Be a Man Campaign, 185
Beijing Platform for Action (BPA), 1,
5, 1819
Belgium, 186
Benedict XVI (Pope), 135
Blind marriage, 103
Blitz attack, 52
Boggess, S. M., 147
Bosnia, 6, 190, 192
Brazil, 179, 181
Bride price, 109
Brothels, near military bases, 19091
Bulgaria, 186
Bundy v. Jackson, 210
Burlington Industries v. Ellerth, 209,
220, 221, 222, 223, 224, 226, 228,
229, 230
Burundi, 194
Bush, Laura, 9, 10
Bush administration, 131
Cambodia, 188, 191
Cammart, Patrick, 193
Canada, 158, 180
Capabilities approach, 21;
empowerment and, 25; and social
and political institutions structure,
2122
Carr v. Allison Gas Turbine, 231
Casual sex, 184
Catholic hospitals, 135
CEDAW. See Convention on the
Elimination of All Forms of
Discrimination against Women
(CEDAW)
Center for Womens Global
Leadership, 3

Characteristics of Marriage Inventory


(CHARISMA), 114, 115
Childless psychotics, 51
Child marriage, 105
Children of God/The Family
movement, 200
Childrens Passport Issuance Alert
Program, 68
Chile, 181
China, 157, 186; arranged marriages
in, 106; choice of mate selection,
107, 111; marital satisfaction,
11314; one child per family
policy, 134
Choice and agency, 24
Christian clergy and rape views, 199
Civil Rights Act (1964), 15051, 209
Clearinghouse, 6869
Clinton, H., 152
Clinton administration, 131
Coercive rape, 190
Cognitive attribution theory, of
achievement motivation, 162
Cold War, 130
Colonial domination, 20
Columbia, 194
Comfort women, 191
Commission on Security and
Cooperation in Europe, 186
Committee on the Elimination of
Discrimination Against Women,
191
Community Forestry Groups (CFGs),
3031; obstacles for women in, 31
Conatzer v. Medical Professional
Building Services, Inc., 229
Condence assault, 52
Conuence Model of sexual
aggression, 18384
Contraception, 130, 138
Control and empowerment, 25, 26
Convention on the Elimination of
All Forms of Discrimination
against Women (CEDAW), 6, 7,
132
Corporate rules, on sexual
harassment, 221
Cote dIvoire, 186
Coworker harassment, 222, 231

Index
Criminal justice system, 19697;
sexual assault and, 197
Criminality, as outcome of arranged
marriages, 121
Cuba, 186
Cultural relativism: justice and
society, 20
Culture, impact on reproductive
rights, 13334
Declaration on the Elimination of
Violence Against Women. UN, 177
DeLauro, Rosa, 152
Democratic Republic of Congo, 6, 190
Demographic time bomb, 131
Department of Health, 135
Devadasi, 199
Dey v. Colt Construction & Development
Company, 219, 231
Disabled persons, reproductive rights,
13435
Doe v. R. R. Donnelly and Sons
Company, 232
Domestic Violence Act, 133
Duplessis v. Training Development
Corp., 217
Early childhood (EC) education, 239,
241
East Africa, 35
East Asia, 157
East Timor, 191
Economic and Social Council
(ECOSOC), 2
Ecuador, 158
Educational staff, gender blindness,
24243
Egypt, parental care in, 138
Ellison v. Brady, 212, 214, 217
Employment Guidelines of the
European Union (2002), 156
Empowerment: agency and, 24;
capabilities and, 25; conceptualizing, 2326; control and, 25, 26; and
human rights in development,
1923; and land ownership, 3239,
main ndings, 3739, Nicaragua,
3536, study, 3637; meaning of,
2324; psychological, 2425

261

Equal Employment Opportunity


Commission (EEOC), 151, 210, 212
Equal Pay Act (1963), 147, 150
Equal Pay Day, 152
Ethiopia, 179
Ethnic cleansing, rape as means for,
19293
European Commission, 159
European Union (EU), 5, 155;
abortion in, 135
Executive Committee Conclusion on
Women and Girls at Risk No. 105
(LVII) (2006), 194
Fair Labor Standards Act (1938), 150
Family planning, 135, 13637
Faragher v. City of Boca Raton, 209, 220,
221, 222, 223, 224, 228, 229, 230
Federal Economic Act (1932), 149
Female genital cutting (FGC), 9091,
133, 139
Feminism, future of, 251, 252
Feminist Catholic groups, 136
Feminist Majority, U.S., 910
Feminist movements, waves of,
25152
Feminist pedagogy, 24447
Femocrats, 5
Finland, 157
Forced marriage, 104
Forestry, and women rights, 3031
Fourth World Conference on Women,
1
France, 157
Gawley v. Indiana University, 225
Gender, 24243, 24647
Gender blindness, in educational
staff, 24243
Gender-conscious standard, for
sexual harassment, 21516
Gendered power: natural resources
and development and, 26, forestry,
3031, land, 3133, water, 2830
Gender Empowerment Measure
(GEM) and women empowerment,
2223
Gender equality, 251; and
reproductive rights, 137

262

Index

Gender Equality Architecture Reform


(GEAR), 3
Gender identities, in early life, 24142
Gender mainstreaming, 45
Gender-power relationship, 24748
Gender quota system, 34; contagion
of, 4
Gender Related Development Index,
22
Gender-sexuality link, 241
Gender stereotyping, 15961
General Assembly, UN, 3
Germany, 157, 186
Getting Along Together, 10Preventing
Sexual Harassment in Young Children program, 244, 246
Getting Along Together in Big
TimePreventing Sexual Harassment
in Young Children program, 244,
246
Getting Along TogetherPreventing
Sexual Harassment in Young Children program, 244, 245
Ghana, 199; marital rape in, 133
Ginsburg, Justice, 218, 219
Global feminism, 1; critiques of,
710; and gender policymaking,
27; and neoliberal
governmentality, 1013, politics
involved, 1415
Global Gag Rule, 13132
Global governance, 1
Globalization: impact on womens
rights, 1819
Global Survival Network, 186
Governmentality, 1013; meaning of,
1112
Grabe, S., 17
Greece, 186
Gross Domestic Product (GDP), 13
Guinea, 186, 191
Guttmacher Institute, 141
Habitual residence, 70
Hague Appeal for Peace, 5
Hague Convention on the Civil
Aspects of International Child
Abduction, 6768
Harman, J., 190

Harris v. Forklift Systems, 209, 211,


218, 219, 231, 232, 233
Harris v. International Paper, 217
Harvard Business Review, 165
Harvard Law Review, 212, 213, 214
Hegemonies, 7, 8
Henson v. City of Dundee, 210
Herzegovina, 192
Hicks, K., 147
Hinduism, and rape views, 199
Honduras, reproductive rights in,
138
Honeymoon Psychosis, 119
Hong Kong SAR, 157
Hostile environment sexual
harassment, 210, 211, 221
Hostile masculinity path, 184
Hughes, Karen, 9
Human rights, of women, 6, 8; causal
attributions for success and failure,
16263; critiques of, 9; discourse
on, 1415; and empowerment, in
development, 1923; gender
stereotyping, 15961; globalization
and impact on, 1819; job
segregation, 163; land ownership
rights and, 3239; and participation in post-war reconstruction
efforts, 19596; performance evaluation, 16162; property ownership
and, 3435; violation of, 2728, 34;
wage disparity and, 15863; water
rights and, 2829
Human Rights Watch, 141
Human trafcking, UN denition of,
18586
ICPD. See International Conference on
Population and Development
(ICPD)
Immigration abuse, 116
Imperial feminism, 9
Impersonal sex, 184
India, 30, 34, 186
Indonesian Society for Pesantren and
Community Development (P3M),
136
Integral Protection Measures against
Gender Violence, 201

Index
Intergovernmental organization
(IGO), 1, 2
International Alert, 5
International Center for Missing and
Exploited Children, 67, 71
International Center for Research on
Women (ICRW), 34
International Conference on
Population and Development
(ICPD), 129
International Criminal Court, Rome
Statute, 6
International nancial institutions
(IFIs), 1
International Institute for Research
and Training of Women
(UN-INSTRAW), 3
International Labor Organization
Equal Remuneration Convention,
154
International Rescue Committee, 194
In vitro fertilization (IVF), 131
Iowa Law Review, 213
Ireland, abortion in, 135
Islam, reproductive rights in, 136
Israel, 186, 199; reproductive health
care, 131; sexual harassment in,
24344
Italy, 186, 187; abortion in, 136
IUD (intra-uterine device), 130, 134
Jackson v. Arkansas, 224
Japan, 157, 179, 186, 191
Jewish congregations, 136
Job segregation, 163
Joint Titling Act, Nicaragua, 36
Jones v. Clinton, 229, 230, 233
Judaism, reproductive rights in, 136
Juju rites, 187
Just world hypothesis, 49
Justice and society, 20
Karzai, President, 133
Katz v. Dole, 221
Keith, D. J., 214
Kenya, 195
Kindergarten children, and gender
differentiation, 242
Kirkpatrick Model, 65

263

K. L. v. Peru, 132, 142


Koran and rape views, 198
Korea, 157, 191
Kuwati muftis, 136
Land rights and women, 3133;
ownership and empowerment,
psychological investigation, 3339
Latin America, 4, 35, 185
Ledbetter v. Goodyear Tire and Rubber
Company, 153
Legalization, of womens rights, 2021
Leopold v. Baccarat, 226
Lewis, Stephen, 192
Liberia, 190, 191
Lily Ledbetter Fair Pay Act (2009),
15253
Lithuania, 186
Little Red Riding Hood, 242
Madray v. Publix, 224
Mainline Protestantism, 136
Malaysia, 157, 181, 191
Marginalization, of women, 29
Marginalized groups, reproductive
rights, 134
Marianismo, 132, 142
Marital rape, 133
Marriage Law of the Peoples
Republic of China, 1067
Martin, J. L., 147
Maternal mortality, 13839
Mativa v. Bald Head Island, 225, 226,
227
McKenzie, M., 177
McKinney v. Dole, 210
McWilliams v. Fairfax County Board of
Supervisors, 211, 221
Media and rape views, 200201
Medicaid, 137
Medical personnel, and need to help
violence survivors, 201
Mentoring, and gender disparity,
16566
Meritor Savings Bank v. Vinson, 209,
211, 221, 222, 234
Mexico, 181
Microcredit: borrowing, 22; lending, 27
Mikulski, Barbara, 153

264

Index

Military Domestic and Sexual


Violence Response Act, UN, 191
Military rape, 18891; of civilian
women in warring countries, 190;
pornography and brothels
enhancing, 19091; of women in
military, 18890
Millennium Development Goals
(MDGs), 4, 1213, 129, 141
Missing children and child
abductions, 47; abduction
homicide, 5354; AMBER Alert
program, 6667; Childrens
Passport Issuance Alert Program,
68; clearinghouse, 6869; cognitive
maturity of children and
adolescents, 6364; educational
programs, 6061; feminist critique
of abduction research and Hague
Treaty, 6971; Hague Convention
on the Civil Aspects of
International Child Abduction,
6768; impact on children and
families: developmental regression
and phobias, 60, emotional/
psychological effects, 58,
health-related aspects, 5859,
physical/health-related effects, 58,
social and interpersonal
relationships, 58; needs assessment, 61; noncustodial parental
abduction, 5658; posttraining
evaluations, 6466; postvictimized
generalized distress response, 48;
rates of recovery, 48; runaways
and throwaways, 5456; stranger
abductors characteristics, 5152;
trainers educational qualications,
6162; training program, 6263;
types: childless psychotics, 51,
pedophiles, 50, proteers, 51, serial
killers, 5051, stranger abduction,
4950
Monteagudo v. Asociacion de Empleados
del Estado Libre Asociado de Puerto
Rico (AEELA), 226, 228
Multi-Country Study on Womens
Health and Domestic Violence
Against Women, 36

Namibia, 179, 181


National Center for Missing and
Exploited Children, 47, 48, 57, 67
National Committee on Pay Equity,
15152, 168
National Recovery Act (1935), 149
Natural resources and development,
26; and gendered power: forestry,
3031, land, 3133, water, 2830
Neoconservativism, 2, 1011
Neoliberal governmentality, 1013;
global feminist politics versus,
1415; MDGS and, 1213;
neoconservativism, 1011;
state in, 12
Nepal, 30, 110
Netherlands, 186
New Guinea, 181
New View of Female Sexual
Problems, 92
New Zealand, 158
Ngwa-Igbo community, 133
Nicaragua, 185; land ownership and
reforms, 3239, main ndings,
3739, study, 3637; therapeutic
abortions in, 13233
Nicaraguan Womens Institute, 35
Nigeria, 133, 186, 197
Noncustodial parental abduction,
5658
Nongovernmental organizations
(NGOs), 196
Non-normative rape, 179, 180
Normative rape, 179, 180
Norway, 157
Obama administration, 131
OConner, Justice, 218, 219
Ofce of the Special Adviser on
Gender Issues (OSAGI), 3
Older women, reproductive rights,
134
Old Testament, and rape views, 198
Oncale v. Sundowner Offshore Services,
Inc, 219
Organization for Economic
Cooperation and Development
(OECD), 5, 155
Ortega, Daniel, 132

Index
P3M. See Indonesian Society for
Pesantren and Community
Development (P3M)
Palestinian refugees, reproductive
health service, 131
Paludi, C., Jr., 147
Paludi, M. A., 147
Pan American Health Organization,
82
Partner violence, 197
Paulina Ramirez v. Mexico, 132, 142
Paycheck Fairness Act (2007), 152
Pay equity, 147; Age Discrimination
in Employment Act (ADEA), 151;
Americans with Disabilities Act
(ADA) (1990), 152; Civil Rights Act
(1964), 15051; comparable worth,
15354; and engaged workforce,
16768; Equal Employment
Opportunity Commission (EEOC),
151; Equal Pay Act (1963), 150;
Federal Economic Act (1932), 149;
human resource recommendations,
16970; international comparisons,
15458; legislation in workplace,
enforcing, 16667; Lily Ledbetter
Fair Pay Act (2009), 15253;
National Committee on Pay Equity, 15152; National Recovery
Act (1935), 149; Paycheck Fairness
Act (2007), 152; pay inequity and
poverty eradication, 167; social
and institutional barriers, 16566;
socialization of women, to
avoid high-status occupations,
16365; wage discrimination
explanation, social science
research view, 158, causal
attributions for success and failure,
16263, gender
stereotyping, 15961, job
segregation, 163, performance
evaluation, 16162; work and life
integration, 16869
Pedophilia, 50
Pendleton, C., 159
Peru, 179, 180, 181; family planning
in, 137
Peterson, Esther, 150

265

Philippines, 157, 158, 191;


reproductive rights in, 135
Philips v. Taco Bell Corp., 229
Pink collar occupations, 163
Pin money, 160
Planned Parenthood International,
141
Population Council, 141
Pornography and military actions,
190
Portugal, abortion in, 136
Posner, Justice, 233
Postcolonial feminists, 9, 10
Post-modernism, 246
Post-structuralism, and early
childhood education, 241
Power: and gender, 27, 2829;
imbalances, 2829, 31; and water
resources, 28, 29
Privatization and structural
adjustment, 27
Proteers, 51
Protect Act of, 2003, 6667
Protestant churches, 136
Protocol to the African Charter on
Human and Peoples Rights on
the Rights of Women in Africa,
132
Psychological empowerment, 2425
Public Services International, 157
Punitive rape, 17980
Quid pro quo sexual harassment, 210,
221
Rabidue v. Osceola Rening Co., 214
Radtke v. Everett, 215, 216
Rape, 139; actions for men and,
18485; alcohol consumption and,
182; cultural supports of: criminal
justice system, 19697, media,
200201, religion, 198200; during
armed conict, 19193; fear of, 195;
as form of exchange, 180, 185;
male perpetrators, characteristics
of, 18384; military, 18891; myths,
183, 198, 199, 200; non-normative,
179, 180; normative, 179, 180;
organizational supports for, 185,

266

Index

sex trafcking, 18588; poverty


and, 181; prevalence, 178; punitive,
17980; of refugees and internally
displaced women, 19396; shame
of, 193; systematic, 6; womens
vulnerability to, 18083
Reagan administration, 131
Realpolitik, 9
Reasonable victim standard, 21119;
feminist theories on, 218;
gender-conscious standard, 21517;
legal views on, 217
Reed v. MBNA Marketing Systems, Inc.,
226, 227, 229
Refugees and internally displaced
women, rape of, 19396
Religion: inuence on reproductive
rights, 135; and sexual assault,
198200
Religious Coalition for Reproductive
Choice, 136
Report on Compliance with the
Hague Convention, 47, 59
Reproductive decision-making, 138;
shared, 138
Reproductive justice, 130; for
childrens health and development, 140; importance, 13840; politics and culture inuence, 13038;
for womens equal rights, 140; for
womens mental health, 13940;
for womens physical health,
13839
Reproductive rights, 129; denition,
130; womens perspectives on,
14142
Republic of Congo, 186
Revictimization and sexual assault,
18182; in refugee camps, 195
Rice, Condoleezza, 9
Riley, D., 216, 217
Roman Catholic Church, 135, 136;
Ethical and Religious Directives,
135
Romania, 186
Rozee, P., 177
Runaways, 5456
Runyan, A. S., 1
Russia, 186
Rwanda, 3, 5, 6, 191, 192

Samoa, 179
Saxton v. American Telephone and
Telegraph Company, 232
Scalia, Justice, 218, 219
Schooling, and sexuality, 242
Security Council, UN, 5, 191;
resolutions, 6
Self-efcacy, 25
Self-harm, 120
Self-initiated marriages vs. arranged
marriages: expectations of marriage and relationships, 114; marital characteristics, 115; marital
satisfaction in, 11213; personal
well-being, 11819; spousal abuse,
11618
Self-insemination methods, 134
Serbia and Montenegro, 179
Serial killers, 5051
Sexual assault, 201. See also rape; as
accepted practice, 200; coercive,
190; and criminal justice systems,
197; military rape and, 18990; in
refugee camps, 195; religion and,
198200; survival, 190; survivors
of, 18182
Sexual harassment (SH), 209;
afrmative defense requirement: to
report issue, 22030, reluctance
and delay of victims in reporting,
22330; behavior levels, 23034;
Civil Rights Act and Title VII,
21011; corporate rules on, 22021;
denition of, 210; prevention:
among young children, 239,
distribution, 24041, programs,
24449; reasonable victim standard, 21119; supervisor harassment
and coworker harassment, difference between, 222
Sexual health, 8384
Sexuality: and gender, 241; and
schooling, 242
Sexual rights, 7, 8486
Sexual violence, 6; denition of, 178;
escalation of, 192; by nonpartners,
179; by partners, 179; refugee
camps and, 194
Sharia Law, 133
Shaw v. AutoZone, Inc., 226

Index
Sierra Leone, 190, 191
Singapore, 157, 191
Sleeping Beauty, 242
Smaller families, 140
Sochua, Mu, 187, 188
Solberg, J., 209
Souter, Justice, 222
South Africa, 180, 186; gender
equality and reproductive rights
in, 137
South Asia, reproductive rights in,
136
Spain, 184, 201; abortion in, 135
Speech, L., 147
Spousal abuse: nancial hardships,
118; immigration policies, 11617;
social isolation, 117
Spousal immigration policies, 11617
Stereotypical abduction, 49
Stingley v. State of Arizona, 217
Stranger abductors, 4950;
characteristics: apparent normalcy,
5152, modus operandi, 52; lures
used by, 5253
Structural adjustment: and land
reform, 31; privatization of natural
resources, 27
Sub-Saharan Africa, reproductive
rights in, 136
Sudan, 6, 192, 195
Summers, L., 159
Supervisor harassment, 222, 224
Survival rape, 190
Sweden, 157
Switzerland, 155
Systematic rape, 6
Tanzania, 179, 181, 194, 195, 196
The Terror Dream, 10
Thailand, 157, 179, 181, 186
Third World women, 7, 9
Throwaways, 5456
Total life planning, 168
Transnational feminism, 2; and
critiques of global feminism, 710;
globalization and, 18; practices, 15
Treaty of Amsterdam (1997), 155
Trokosi tradition, 199200
Turkey, 29, 186
Tysiac v. Poland, 132, 142

267

Ubang community, 133


Uganda, 185, 193
Ukraine, 186
UN Decade for Women, 1, 6
UN Development Fund for Women
(UNIFEM), 2, 3, 5; Progress of the
Worlds Women Report, 4
UN Division for the Advancement of
Women (UNDAW), 3
UNHCR, Executive Committee
Conclusion on Refugee Protection
and Sexual Violence No. 73
(XLIV), 194
United Kingdom, reproductive rights
in, 134
United Nations (UN), 129, 139, 141
United States, 181, 186; assisted
reproductive techniques, 137;
reproductive rights in, 135;
unintentional pregnancy in, 13637
Universalism, 1920; and legalization
of womens rights, 2021
UN Ofce on Drugs and Crime
Database (UNODC), 186
UN Resolution 1325, 195
UN Fund for Population Activities
(UNFPA), 129, 133
Uzbekistan, contraceptive patterns in,
130
Vietnam, 157; laws related to
marriage: abolition of arranged
marriage, 108, age of marriage,
107, 108, Marriage and Family
Law (1960), 108, polygamy, 108
Violence: physical, sexual and
psychological, 17778; sexual. See
sexual violence; against women,
197; workplace, 210, 212, 213
Violence against women, 240
Violence against Women Bill, 197
Violent behavior, 246
Wage gap, and gender, 14748,
16667
War Labor Board, 149
Water rights and women, 2830
Watts v. Kroger, 224
West Los Angeles Veterans
Association Health Center, 189

268

Index

West Timor, 194


Widow marriage, 1056
Williams v. Saxbe, 210
Women and sexuality, 81; denitions,
8284; sexual behavior, 8689,
notion of social power, 89, rate of
sexual activity, 87, and sexual
identity, 8889, sexual partners, 87;
sexual desire, 8992; sexual
problems, 9294, classication
system, 93, denition, 93, medical
model, 92; sexual rights as human
rights, 8486
Womens Caucus for Gender
Justice, 5
Womens Commission for Refugee
Women and Children, 5, 195
Womens Environment and
Development Organization
(WEDO), 3
Womens Equality Day, 147

Womens International League


for Peace and Freedom (WILPF), 5
Womens reproductive rights. See
reproductive rights
Womens Rights are Human Rights
campaign, 8
Women with cognitive impairments,
sexual assault risk, 135
Workplace: equality in, 251; violence,
210, 212, 213
World Association of Sexology, 82
World Bank, 5, 137
World Health Organization, 36, 37,
82, 130
World War II, and sexual aggression,
191
Yates v. AVCO Corp., 214
Yugoslavia, 6, 191
Zapatero, Prime Minister, 135
Zimbabwe, 180, 186

You might also like