You are on page 1of 19

jfpp_272

571..589

EFFECTS OF CONCHING TIME AND INGREDIENTS ON PREFERENCE OF MILK CHOCOLATE


M. PRAWIRA and S.A. BARRINGER1 Department of Food Science and Technology The Ohio State University Columbus, OH
Accepted for Publication May 21, 2008

ABSTRACT The objective of this study is to determine how the conching time and the quantity of sucrose, lecithin, cocoa butter and whole milk powder affect consumer preference for milk chocolate. Untrained panelists performed a sensory study consisting of acceptability, preference and attribute intensity. Longer conching time produced signicantly smoother chocolate with smaller particle size. The longest conche times had the smallest particle size and were the most mouthcoating. There was no change in avor with conching. The longer conche times were preferred. Panelists preferred higher sucrose levels, and increasing sucrose decreased bitterness and increased chocolate avor. Increasing lecithin increased smoothness, but less lecithin was preferred, possibly due to off-avors at high levels of lecithin. Increasing cocoa butter yielded softer chocolate but did not affect bitterness. Panelists preferred 10% over higher levels of cocoa butter. More milk powder produced smoother chocolate with more caramel avor and was preferred.

PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS The perceived quality of milk chocolate is affected by conching time, sucrose, lecithin, cocoa butter and whole milk powder. Texture was affected the most by conching, milk powder, lecithin and cocoa butter. Flavor was affected the most by milk powder and sugar. The only variable that did not affect acceptability and preference of milk chocolate was time for underconched samples. While this study did not determine the optimum conditions for milk chocolate, the most preferred and/or acceptable samples were
1

Corresponding author. Department of Food Science and Technology, The Ohio State University, 2015 Fyffe Road, Columbus 43210, OH. TEL: 614-688-3642; FAX: 614-292-0218; EMAIL: barringer.11@osu.edu

Journal of Food Processing and Preservation 33 (2009) 571589. DOI: 10.1111/j.1745-4549.2008.00272.x 2009 The Author(s) Journal compilation 2009 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

571

572

M. PRAWIRA and S.A. BARRINGER

conched for at least 12 h, had 35 to 50% sucrose, 00.5% lecithin, 510% cocoa butter and 1330% milk powder.

INTRODUCTION Consumer preference is crucial in determining the success of a product in the market, including chocolate. Chocolate is consumed more as an indulgence rather than need; therefore, it is important to know which attributes and parameters drive consumer liking of chocolate. Conching is a mixing step that involves volatilization of fatty acids and aldehydes and development of smooth texture. Volatilization reduces the bitterness of the chocolate and develops the typical chocolate avor. The solid particles such as sugar, nonfat cocoa and milk powder are coated with fat, dissociated by friction and become rounded. The coating of particles by fat promotes the smooth texture and snap desired in chocolate. Sucrose is added to promote sweetness in chocolate but also affects other avors. Increased level of sucrose results in a decreased level of bitterness (Guinard and Mazzucchelli 1999). More sucrose creates a more intense chocolate avor perception in chocolate puddings (Geiselman et al. 1998). Lecithin is an emulsier that is commonly added in chocolate. It is added at approximately 0.5% to reduce the viscosity during processing, increasing the efciency of conching. The addition of lecithin reduces the amount of cocoa butter needed to achieve the desired texture. Thus, addition of lecithin can reduce the production cost. Addition of 0.3% lecithin reduces chocolate viscosity and increases chocolates tolerance to moisture (Afoakwa et al. 2007). However, too much lecithin causes off-avors and increases the viscosity of chocolate. Above 0.5% lecithin, yield value and viscosity increases (Chevalley 1994; Rector 2000). Cocoa butter is added to give smooth texture in chocolate. Increasing fat content is related to a richer mouthfeel, faster melting rate and thus, smoother chocolate (Talbot 2005). The addition of cocoa butter coats the bitter compounds, and thus bitterness level decreases as the level of cocoa butter increases (Guinard and Mazzucchelli 1999). Cocoa butter is solid at room temperature and liquid at mouth temperature. This enables maximum release of chocolate avor during consumption. Milk powder is added for milky and caramel avor in chocolate. The caramel avor in dairy products is caused by caramelization of sugars and Strecker degradation of casein in milk (Patton 1955). The addition of milk fat reduces the hardness of chocolate since it dilutes the cocoa butter, and thus increases the amount of the liquid phase (Full et al. 1996). Both instrumental and sensory evaluation of the hardness of milk chocolates decreased with

PREFERENCE OF MILK CHOCOLATE

573

increasing milk fat content (Bolenz et al. 2003). This study was done to investigate the effects of conching time and adding different amounts of sucrose, lecithin, cocoa butter and milk powder on acceptability, preference and attribute intensity of milk chocolate.

MATERIALS AND METHODS Materials and Methods A monolayer of approximately 1,000 g of cocoa beans (Conacado, Dominican Republic) was roasted using a convection oven (Doyon JA 14, Inc., Saint Come de Kennebec, Quebec, Canada) at 149C for 10 min. The beans were cracked (Crankandstein Cocoa Mill, Chocolate Alchemy, Yoncalla, OR) and winnowed. Granulated sucrose was rened using a blender for 2 min to reduce the particle size to 180 mm. The rest of the ingredients: granulated lecithin (Soy, Chocolate Alchemy), cocoa butter (Natural, Dominican Republic, Organic and Fair Trade Certied, Chocolate Alchemy), vanilla (Beanilla Trading Company, Holt, MI) and spray-dried whole milk powder (All American Dairy Products, Inc., Malvern, PA) were weighed. The nibs were ground using a grinder (The Champion Juicer, Plastaket Manufacturing Corp., Inc., Lodi, CA). Each batch was 1,000 g to accommodate sufcient volume for conching and sensory evaluation. The preheated chocolate liquor and cocoa butter were poured into the conching equipment (Alchemists Stone Chocolate Melanger, Santha, Chocolate Alchemy). The powdered ingredients lecithin, sucrose, vanilla and milk powder were put into the microwave for 3 min to preheat them, and then mixed thoroughly. The powdered ingredients were poured slowly into the conching equipment, letting the dry ingredients incorporate before adding more ingredients. After the conching process was done, the chocolate was transferred into the tempering machine (Revolution 2, Chocovision Corp., Poughkeepsie, NY). The nished chocolate product was poured into molds and shaken for approximately 23 min. The mold was 159 72 6 mm polycarbonate that holds 113 g of chocolate and can be broken into 28 squares. The molded chocolate was immediately put into a refrigerator at 5C. The chocolate was allowed to solidify and taken out to sit at room temperature overnight before sensory evaluation tests. The basic control utilized 0.5% lecithin, 16% cocoa butter, 13% milk powder, 26% cocoa liquor, 44.3% sucrose, 0.2% vanilla and 21 h conching time. The fat content is equal to the cocoa butter plus 55% of the cocoa liquor (Carver and Meylan 1970) and 26% of the whole milk powder (Fitzpatrick et al. 2004). As each variable was adjusted, the other ingredients were kept in

574

M. PRAWIRA and S.A. BARRINGER

TABLE 1. INGREDIENT FORMULATION Set no. Fat content (%) 33.7 Sugar (%) Lecithin (%) Cocoa butter (%) 16.0 Milk powder (%) 13.0 Cocoa liquor (%) 26.0 Vanilla (%) Conching time (h)

44.3

0.500

0.200

33.7

44.3

0.500

16.0

13.0

26.0

0.200

39.3 37.1 35.2 33.7 31.9 33.8 33.7 33.5 33.2 24.5 28.7 33.8 37.0 34.2 33.8 33.1 33.5 33.5

30.0 35.0 40.0 44.3 50.0 44.5 44.3 44.1 43.6 49.8 47.1 44.75 41.8 48.2 45.7 43.4 40.68 37.9

0.583 0.551 0.522 0.500 0.473 0.000 0.500 1.000 2.000 0.562 0.532 0.505 0.472 0.543 0.515 0.490 0.467 0.427

18.7 17.6 16.7 16.0 15.1 16.1 16.0 15.9 15.8 5.00 10.0 16.0 20.0 17.4 16.5 15.7 15.0 13.7

15.2 14.3 13.6 13.0 12.3 13.1 13.0 12.9 12.8 14.6 13.6 12.9 12.5 5.00 10.0 13.0 20.0 30.0

30.3 28.7 27.2 26.0 24.6 26.1 26.0 25.9 25.6 28.6 27.7 26.3 25.0 28.3 26.8 25.5 24.3 21.9

0.233 0.221 0.209 0.2000 0.1892 0.201 0.200 0.1990 0.1970 0.225 0.213 0.202 0.1923 0.217 0.206 0.196 0.187 0.1709

8 12 21 76 3 4 5 6 7 21

21

21

21

The numbers in bold represent the control value and the gray cells represent the variables tested. The ingredients not being tested remain in the same proportions to each other in all tests.

the same proportion to each other. Table 1 shows the ingredient formulation for each set. The sets tested were short conching time (3, 4, 5, 6 and 7 h), longer conching time (8, 12, 21 and 76 h), sucrose level (30, 35, 40, 44.3 and 50%), lecithin content (0, 0.5, 1 and 2%), cocoa butter content (5, 10, 16 and 20%) and milk powder content (5, 10, 13, 20 and 30%). The particle size of the powders was measured using a Malvern Particle Size analyzer X (Malvern, Westborough, MA). Three replicates were performed and the average mean diameter over the volume distribution, D[4,3], was reported. The particle size of the chocolates was measured using a micrometer (Dignimatic Outside Micrometer, Mitutoyo, Japan). Micrometer readings correspond to the 90th percentile of the volume diameter of

PREFERENCE OF MILK CHOCOLATE

575

chocolate, as measured by laser light scattering (Ziegler et al. 2001). The nished chocolate samples were melted using a microwave for approximately 2 min and stirred. A small amount of the liquid chocolate was placed in the jaw of the micrometer and the diameter was recorded. Two replicates were performed and the average diameter was recorded. Sensory Experimental Design The sensory tests were conducted at The Ohio State University Department of Food Science and Technology sensory booths. Each booth was welllighted, climate controlled and odor-free. The 50 panelists were given the samples, sensory ballot and a glass of spring water (Naturally Preferred Pure Mountain Spring Water, Cincinnati, OH) for palate cleansing in between samples. Panelists were recruited through email invitations, personal communications and board advertisement. One variable was tested on each day. Samples were blind-labeled with three-digit codes to avoid bias, and sample order was randomized so that each sample appeared in each position the same number of times to avoid artifacts because of the order of presentation (Lawless 1998). The randomization of sample order was divided into ve groups, i.e., the rst 10 people were assigned to the rst group and so on. Panelists were presented with 45 samples per day to avoid fatigue and saturation. Each panelist was given four small pieces of chocolate (1.9 2.5 cm each) per sample number. The samples were placed in 59-mL plastic cups with lids (GFS Plastic Portion Cup Lids, Gordon Food Service, Grand Rapids, MI). Panelists were asked two screening questions before each test started: gender and preference for type of chocolate. Panels averaged 46% males. There were three different parts of the sensory questionnaire: 9-point hedonic scale, ranking and attribute-by-attribute evaluation. For the 9-point hedonic scale, panelists were asked to rate each sample from dislike extremely to like extremely. The second part was ranking the samples from 1 being the best and 4 or 5 (depending on the number of samples), being the worst. The last part of the questionnaire was the attribute-by-attribute evaluation. Panelists had to rate each sample according to the attribute given from none (equal to 1), to moderate to very (equal to 9). Statistical Analysis Analysis of variance was utilized to evaluate whether the samples were signicantly different from each other. Tukeys honestly signicant difference test was carried out if the variables were signicantly different from each other (P value 0.05) to determine which of the variables were different. Tukeys honestly signicant different test was chosen because of its conservative characteristics (OMahony 1986).

576

M. PRAWIRA and S.A. BARRINGER

For statistical analysis of data, each variable tested was evaluated utilizing all data, female-only data, male-only data and panelists that preferred milk chocolate. In most cases, all of the data sets produced the same results. Only for lecithin, which had 50% female panelists, did female-only data produce different results. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Conching Time: Underconched Versus Conched for the Recommended Time The underconched chocolate samples were not conched as long as is recommended, which is 1014 h (Nanci 2007). Longer conching time results in smoother chocolate with smaller particle size. A few commercial chocolates are considered to be underconched. The underconched chocolate samples were conched for 3, 4, 5, 6 and 7 h. The chocolate conched for 7 h was rated as signicantly smoother than the chocolate conched for 3 or 4 h (Fig. 1). During
8

Attribute intensity

6
ab ab

b a

a a a a a
a

3
Smoothness

2 2 3 4 5 Conching time (h) 6

Chocolate flavor

FIG. 1. SMOOTHNESS AND CHOCOLATE FLAVOR FOR UNDERCONCHED SAMPLES AFTER 3, 4, 5, 6 AND 7 H OF CONCHING Samples with different letters are signicantly different.

PREFERENCE OF MILK CHOCOLATE

577

80

70

60 90th Percentile (m)

50

40

30

20

10

0 0 5 10 15 20 25 Conching time (h)

FIG. 2. PARTICLE SIZE OF DIFFERENT CONCHING TIME SAMPLES

conching, the solid particles such as sugar, nonfat cocoa and milk powder are coated with fat, therefore, too short of a conching time does not allow all of the solid particles to be covered with fat (Mentink and Serpelloni 1994). The measured particle sizes of 3, 4, 5, 6 and 7 h were 69.5, 49.5, 45, 38.5 and 32.5 mm, respectively (Fig. 2). According to the particle size, all of the underconched samples should be gritty since the particle size of the solids was not reduced to the ideal size of less than 20 mm where the tongue can no longer detect them (Afoakwa et al. 2007). During conching, solid particles are ground, disassociated by friction and become rounded, (Mentink and Serpelloni 1994) thus the particle size of the solid particles in the chocolate is steadily reduced (El-Deep et al. 2000). As particle size is reduced, the texture of the chocolate becomes smoother (Liang and Hartel 2004). In the longer conching time set (8, 12, 21 and 76 h), panelists similarly rated the samples with the longer conching time, 12 to 76 h, as signicantly smoother (Fig. 3). Based on the particle size, at 8 h conching time, panelists should still be able to detect the grittiness while at the longer times, they should no longer be able to detect grittiness, since the particle size had

578

M. PRAWIRA and S.A. BARRINGER

9
Smoothness Chocolate flavor Mouth coating

8 Attribute intensity

7
b

6
a

b a

a a b

ab

a a

4 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 Conching time (h)

FIG. 3. INTENSITY RATING FOR SMOOTHNESS, CHOCOLATE FLAVOR AND MOUTHCOATING AFTER 8, 12, 21 AND 76 H OF CONCHING Samples with different letters are signicantly different.

decreased below 20 mm. The samples conched for 12 h and longer were conched for an acceptable conching time, and thus the textures of those samples should not be perceived as gritty. Mouthcoating is the afterfeel, or lm covering the mouth surfaces (Guinard and Mazzucchelli 1999). Chocolate conched longer develops a smoother texture; however, it can also develop a greasy or oily mouthfeel. Conching times of 21 and 76 h were rated as more mouthcoating than 8 h conching time (Fig. 3). At a particle size of less than 2 mm, the chocolate develops a greasy texture (Liang and Hartel 2004). Both 21 and 76 h had particle size of 2 mm, and thus a greasy texture may have developed that panelists perceived as mouthcoating. The chocolate avor for the underconched samples (3, 4, 5, 6 and 7 h) was not affected by conche time (Fig. 1). Panelists also did not detect a signicant difference in chocolate avor in the longer (8 to 72 h) conching time set (Fig. 3). There was no signicant difference in either acceptability or preference for the underconched samples (Fig. 4). With shorter conching time (3, 4, 5, 6 and 7 h), the conching process is not sufcient for the fat to cover the solid

PREFERENCE OF MILK CHOCOLATE

579

9
Acceptability

8 7
a

Ranking

Acceptability or ranking

6 5 4

3 2 1 2

a a

5 Conching time (h)

FIG. 4. ACCEPTABILITY AND RANKING FOR UNDERCONCHED CHOCOLATE AT 3, 4, 5, 6 AND 7 H OF CONCHING Samples with different letters are signicantly different.

particles in the chocolate; and the particle size is large, causing all of them to be gritty. In addition, there was no signicant difference in chocolate avor (Fig. 1). So while the longest conche time was smoother in this set, this did not translate into a difference in preference. Maniere and Dimick (1979) found no signicant change in avor acceptability during the rst 12 h of conching for dark chocolate. After 8, 12, 21 and 76 h of conching, there was no signicant difference in acceptability; however, there was a signicant difference in preference (Fig. 5). Panelists ranked the longer conching time samples, 21 and 76 h, signicantly better than the shortest conching time. Samples conched for 21 and 76 h may have been ranked signicantly better than 8 h conching time since they were signicantly smoother and had the smallest particle size. There was no difference in ranking between 12, 21 and 76 h, and similarly Maniere and Dimick (1979) found no difference in preference between 24 and 44 h for dark chocolate. Conched chocolate samples should have stronger chocolate avor than unconched samples; however, Maniere and Dimick (1979) only found differences in acceptability between 0 and 24 or 44 h conche times. At 20 and 24 h, the samples have a moderate chocolate avor compared to 0 h that has only basic chocolate notes (Maniere and Dimick 1979).

580

M. PRAWIRA and S.A. BARRINGER

9
Acceptability

8 7
a

Ranking

Acceptability or ranking

6
a

5 4 3 2 1 0 10 20 30 40 Conching time (h) 50 60 70 80


b ab a a

FIG. 5. ACCEPTABILITY AND RANKING AT 8, 12, 21 AND 76 H OF CONCHING Samples with different letters are signicantly different.

Sucrose Commercial milk chocolate has a sugar content of 43 to 58%, while the samples in this study were 30 to 50% sucrose. The main purpose of adding sucrose is to add sweetness. However, increasing sucrose content is also related to a stronger avor perception in chocolate pudding (Geiselman et al. 1998) and chewing gum (Davidson et al. 1999). Thus, increasing sucrose content should be related to a stronger chocolate avor. In this study, the chocolate avor was rated signicantly different with increasing sucrose content (Fig. 6). The chocolate with 40% sucrose was signicantly higher in chocolate avor than 30% sucrose despite containing less cocoa liquor (Table 1). The bitterness attribute was also signicantly affected by sucrose levels (Fig. 6). Panelists rated 30% sucrose signicantly bitterer than 44.3 and 50% sucrose. Milk chocolates with lower sucrose were also rated as more bitter than samples with higher sucrose by a trained panel (Guinard and Mazzucchelli 1999). Increased sugar level results in a decrease in the perception of bitterness by masking the bitter compounds in chocolate (Guinard and Mazzucchelli 1999). Similarly, lower sugar chocolate puddings were more bitter compared to higher sugar chocolate puddings (Geiselman et al. 1998). However, it should be noted that in those studies, the samples with the lowest sucrose also contained the most cocoa liquor.

PREFERENCE OF MILK CHOCOLATE

581

8 7 Attribute intensity 6
ab b ab

Chocolate flavor Bitterness

ab

5 4 3 2 1 25

a b ab ab a a

30

35

40

45

50

55

Sucrose content (%)


FIG. 6. CHOCOLATE FLAVOR AND BITTERNESS AT DIFFERENT SUCROSE CONTENTS Samples with different letters are signicantly different.

There was a signicant difference in both acceptability and preference of the different sucrose levels (Fig. 7). Samples with 44.3% sucrose were signicantly more acceptable than 30% sucrose, and 35 to 44.3% was preferred over 30%. The least preferred sample, 30%, was perceived to have the least amount of chocolate avor and highest in bitterness (Fig. 6). Lecithin Lecithin is added to replace some of the cocoa butter and is legally limited to 1% of the chocolate, with 0.5% being typically added commercially. The smoothness of the samples with different amounts of lecithin was signicantly different (Fig. 8); however, Tukey HSD multiple comparisons found no difference between the samples. When the data for the females-only group were evaluated, 2% lecithin was signicantly smoother than 0% lecithin (Fig. 8). Women perform better in distinguishing differences in foods than men because of their better sensory memory (Mojet and Koster 2002). Lecithin is an emulsier that is added in chocolate to create a smooth texture instead of having to add more cocoa butter, and hence lowers the production cost (Nebesny and Zyzelewicz 2005). Lecithin forms a hydrophobic surface around the sugar and cocoa particles that help the cocoa butter efciently coat the

582

M. PRAWIRA and S.A. BARRINGER

8 7
ab ab

Acceptability Ranking b ab

Acceptability or ranking

6 5 4

3
a

ab a

2 1 30 35 40 45 50 55 Sucrose content (%)


FIG. 7. ACCEPTABILITY AND RANKING AT DIFFERENT SUCROSE CONTENTS Samples with different letters are signicantly different.

sugar particles (Dimick and Hoskin 1981). A 0.5% amount of lecithin is the best level for forming an interface between the fat and solid particles and to produce smooth chocolate (St John et al. 1995). Panelists did not nd any signicant difference in chocolate avor for different lecithin contents (Fig. 8). Panelists found 0 and 0.5% lecithin to be signicantly more acceptable and preferred over 2% lecithin (Fig. 9). The lower preference for the 2% lecithin might be due to off-avors that can be produced when more than 1% lecithin is used (St. John et al. 1995). Cocoa Butter The cocoa butter levels evaluated were 5, 10, 16 and 20%, which corresponds to 24.5, 28.7, 33.8 and 37% total fat content. Commercial milk chocolates contain 29 to 36% total fat. In general, increasing fat content is related to a richer mouthfeel, faster melting rate and smoother chocolate (Talbot 2005). An increase of 1% at 28% fat content greatly reduces plastic viscosity; however, at a fat content above 32%, there is no signicant effect on viscosity

PREFERENCE OF MILK CHOCOLATE

583

9 8 Attribute intensity 7
ab a a ab a a

Smoothness (female only) Smoothness Chocolate flavor

6 5 4 3

b a

a a

0.0

0.5

1.0 1.5 Lecithin content (%)

2.0

2.5

FIG. 8. SMOOTHNESS AND CHOCOLATE FLAVOR AT DIFFERENT LECITHIN CONTENTS Samples with different letters are signicantly different.
9
Acceptability

8 Acceptability or ranking 7 6
ab

Ranking

5 4 3
ab bc a c

2 1 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 Lecithin content (%)

FIG. 9. ACCEPTABILITY AND RANKING AT DIFFERENT LECITHIN CONTENTS Samples with different letters are signicantly different.

584

M. PRAWIRA and S.A. BARRINGER

Total fat content (%) 20 9


Hardness

25

30

35

40

8 7 Attribute intensity 6 5
a b a b a a a a a a a a

Smoothness Bitterness

4 3 2 1 0 5

10

15

20

25

Added cocoa butter content (%)

FIG. 10. INTENSITY OF HARDNESS, SMOOTHNESS AND BITTERNESS AT DIFFERENT COCOA BUTTER CONTENTS Samples with different letters are signicantly different.

(Afoakwa et al. 2007). In this study, the hardness of the samples decreased with increasing cocoa butter content (Fig. 10). Cocoa butter levels of 5 and 10% were signicantly harder than 16 and 20%. The smoothness attribute was found to be signicantly different between the samples (Fig. 10); however, when Tukeys HSD multiple comparisons were performed, none of the samples were different from each other. Varying cocoa butter level had no signicant effect on bitterness (Fig. 10). Fat suppresses bitterness (Preininger 2006), thus high-fat chocolate puddings are less bitter than low-fat chocolate puddings (Geiselman et al. 1998). The addition of cocoa butter coats the bitter compounds, and thus a previous study using a trained panel found that the bitterness level decreases as level of cocoa butter increases in chocolate (Guinard and Mazzucchelli 1999). Panelists rated the cocoa butter samples signicantly different from each other in terms of acceptability and preference (Fig. 11). Samples with 5% cocoa butter was signicantly more acceptable than 20%, 10% was signicantly more acceptable than 16 and 20%, and 5 and 10% samples were signicantly preferred over 20%. The middle samples, 10 and 16%, fall within the range of fat in commercial milk chocolate. Theoretically, liking of

PREFERENCE OF MILK CHOCOLATE

585

Total fat content (%) 20 9


Acceptability

25

30

35

40

8 Acceptability or ranking 7
c

Ranking

6 5 4

bc ab a

3
a a

ab

2 1 0 5 10 15 Added cocoa butter content (%) 20 25

FIG. 11. ACCEPTABILITY AND RANKING FOR DIFFERENT COCOA BUTTER CONTENTS Samples with different letters are signicantly different.

chocolate should increase with increasing fat content since it gives a richer mouthfeel and faster melting rate in the mouth (Talbot 2005). However, in this study, panelists did not detect a difference in the bitterness or smoothness attributes for samples with different fat contents, which might have caused them to prefer the higher fat samples. Instead, they preferred the samples with lower fat, which were perceived to be harder. Milk Powder Milk chocolate with higher milk fat content is signicantly softer than milk chocolate with lower milk fat content (Subramaniam and Murphy 2001). The addition of milk fat reduces the hardness of chocolate since it dilutes the cocoa butter and thus increases the amount of the liquid phase (Full et al. 1996). Samples with 5, 10 and 20% milk powder were signicantly less smooth than 30% milk powder (Fig. 12). The chocolate with the highest milk fat (30% milk powder) was perceived as the smoothest. In terms of caramel avor, the 5%-milk-powder sample was signicantly lower than 30% milk powder (Fig. 12). Increasing the milk powder content

586

M. PRAWIRA and S.A. BARRINGER

9 8 7 Attribute intensity
b

6 5 4
a a a ab ab ab ab a b

3 2 1 0 5 10 15 20 25 Whole milk powder content (%)

Smoothness Caramel flavor

30

35

FIG. 12. SMOOTHNESS AND CARAMEL FLAVOR FOR DIFFERENT MILK POWDER CONTENTS Samples with different letters are signicantly different.

should be directly proportional to increasing the caramel avor. The caramel avor in milk is caused by sugar decomposition and Strecker degradation of amino acids in casein (Patton 1955). The 13%-milk-powder samples were signicantly more acceptable than the 5%-milk-powder samples and the 13- and 30%-milk-powder samples were signicantly preferred to the 5%-milk-powder samples (Fig. 13). The preferred samples, with more milk powder, had smoother texture and a stronger caramel avor. CONCLUSIONS The attribute texture was affected the most by conching, milk powder, lecithin and cocoa butter. The avor attribute was affected most by milk powder and sugar. All of the variables affected acceptability and/or preference of milk chocolate except for time for underconched samples. Longer conching produced smoother, more mouthcoating chocolate with no change in avor. The 7 h period of conching produced smoother chocolate than 3 h of conching. Conching times of 12, 21 and 76 h were smoother than 8 h of conching. Both 21 and 76 h of conching were more mouthcoating than

PREFERENCE OF MILK CHOCOLATE

587

9
Acceptability

8 Acceptability or ranking 7
b ab

Ranking

6 5 4 3
a a

ab

ab

b ab a ab

2 1 0 5 10 15 20 25 Whole milk powder content (%) 30 35

FIG. 13. ACCEPTABILITY AND RANKING FOR DIFFERENT MILK POWDER CONTENTS Samples with different letters are signicantly different.

8 h of conching and also had a very small particle size that causes greasiness. There was no signicant difference in either acceptability or preference of the underconched samples. Samples conched for longer, 21 to 76 h, were preferred to 8 h of conching. Sucrose masked bitterness in chocolate. Increasing sucrose from 30 to 40% resulted in an intensied chocolate avor in the samples despite having less cocoa liquor. Panelists preferred the higher sucrose samples over the lower sucrose samples. With lecithin, no differences were found in smoothness and chocolate avor; however, the female-only data found that 2% lecithin was signicantly smoother than 0% lecithin. The 0 and 0.5% lecithin samples were signicantly preferred over 2% lecithin, possibly due to off-avors. Increasing the cocoa butter content led to softer chocolate. No signicant differences were found in smoothness and bitterness. Panelists preferred 5 and 10% cocoa butter to 20% cocoa butter. The 30% milk powder was smoother than 5, 10 and 20% and had the most caramel avor. The 13 and 30% milk powder samples were most preferred. REFERENCES AFOAKWA, E.O., PATERSON, A. and FOLWER, M. 2007. Factors inuencing rheological and textural qualities in chocolate a review. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 18(6), 290298.

588

M. PRAWIRA and S.A. BARRINGER

BOLENZ, S., THIESSENHUSEN, T. and SCHPE, R. 2003. Fast conching for milk chocolate. Eur. Food Res. Technol. 218(1), 6267. CARVER, W.S. and MEYLAN, C.D. 1970. Cacao pressing techniques determine cocoa and cocoa butter qualities. Manuf. Confect. 50(8), 4246. CHEVALLEY, J. 1994. Chocolate ow properties. In: Industrial Chocolate Manufacture and Use, 2nd edn (S.T. Beckett, ed.), pp. 139155, Blackie Academic and Professional, Glaslow, UK. DAVIDSON, J.M., LINFORTH, R.S.T., HOLLOWOOD, T.A. and TAYLOR, A.J. 1999. Effect of sucrose on the perceived avor intensity of chewing gum. J. Agric. Food Chem. 47(10), 43364340. DIMICK, P.S. and HOSKIN, J.M. 1981. Chemico-physical aspects of chocolate processing a review. Can. Inst. Food Sci. Technol. J. 14(4), 269 282. EL-DEEP, S.H., YOUSIF, E.I., EL-AZAB, M.A. and BEDEIR, S.H. 2000. Effect of roasting, conching and tempering processes on the quality characteristics of cocoa beans and chocolate. Ann. Agric. Sci. 45(2), 585602. FITZPATRICK, J.J., IQBAL, T., DELANEY, C., TWOMEY, T. and KEOGH, M.K. 2004. Effect of powder properties and storage conditions on the owability of milk powders with different fat contents. J. Food Eng. 64(4), 435444. FULL, N.A., YELLA REDDY, S., DIMICK, P.S. and ZIEGLER, G.R. 1996. Physical and sensory properties of milk chocolate formulated with anhydrous milk fat fractions. J. Food Sci. 61(5), 10681073. GEISELMAN, P.J., SMITH, C.F., WILLIAMSON, D.A., CHAMPAGNE, C.M., BRAY, G.A. and RYAN, D.H. 1998. Perception of sweetness intensity determines womens hedonic and other perceptual responsiveness to chocolate food. Appetite 1998(31), 3748. GUINARD, J.X. and MAZZUCCHELLI, R. 1999. Effects of sugar and fat on the sensory properties of milk chocolate: Descriptive analysis and instrumental measurements. J. Sci. Food Agric. 79(11), 13311339. LAWLESS, H.T. 1998. Sensory Evaluation of Food Principles and Practices, pp. 83150, Chapman & Hall, New York. LIANG, B. and HARTEL, R.W. 2004. Effects of milk powder in milk chocolate. J. Dairy Sci. 87(1), 2031. MANIERE, F.Y. and DIMICK, P.S. 1979. Effects of conching on the avor of dark semi-sweet chocolate. Candy Snack Ind. 144(2): 42, 46, 48. MENTINK, L. and SERPELLONI, M. (Roquette Freres) 1994. Low-calorie chocolate. U.S. Patent 5,360,621, November 1, 1994. MOJET, J. and KOSTER, E.P. 2002. Texture and avor memory in foods: An incidental learning experiment. Appetite 38, 110117.

PREFERENCE OF MILK CHOCOLATE

589

NANCI, J. 2007. Conching and rening chocolate at home. Chocolate Alchemy. http://www.chocolatealchemy.com/conchingrening.php. (accessed November 2, 2007). NEBESNY, E. and ZYZELEWICZ, D. 2005. Effect of lecithin concentration on properties of sucrose-free chocolate masses sweetened with isomalt. Eur. Food Res. Technol. 220(2), 131135. OMAHONY, M. 1986. Sensory Evaluation of Food Statistical Methods and Procedures, pp. 153169, Marcel Dekker Inc, New York. PATTON, S. 1955. Browning and associated changes in milk and its products: a review. J. Dairy Sci. 38(5), 457478. PREININGER, M. 2006. Interactions of avor components in foods. In: Ingredient Interactions Effects on Food Quality, 2nd edn (A.G. Gaonkar and A. McPherson eds), pp. 478528. Taylor and Francis Group, Boca Raton, New York/London. RECTOR, D. 2000. Chocolate controlling the ow. Benets of polygycerol polyricinoleic acid. Manuf. Confect. 80(5), 6370. ST. JOHN, J.F., FETTERHOFF, J.G., CARPENTER, J.R., BROWN, B.D., AZZARA, C.D., TARKA, S.M., RANK, C. and STROHMAIER, G.K. (Hershey Foods Corporation) 1995. Reduced fat confectionery products and process. U.S. Patent 5,464,649. November 7, 1995. SUBRAMANIAM, P. and MURPHY, O. 2001. Contraction of milk chocolate. Manuf. Confect. 81(6), 6166. TALBOT, G. 2005. Cocoa butter and milk fat in chocolate. Kennedys Conf, June 2005, pp. 4951. ZIEGLER, G.R., MONGIA, G. and HOLLENDER, R. 2001. The role of particle size distribution of suspended solids in dening the sensory properties of milk chocolate. Int. J. Food Prop. 4(2), 353370.

You might also like