You are on page 1of 30

Critical Thinking: A Literature Review

October 21, 2002

Table of Contents I. II. III. IV. V. VI. VII. VIII. IX. X. XI. XII. XIII. XIV. XV. General Definitions of Critical Thinking2 Misrepresentations about Critical Thinking3 Critical Thinking and Information Processing Critical Thinking and Education. Critical Thinking for Science Education Critical Thinking for Agricultural Education. Critical Studies in Critical Thinking Critical Thinking Skills vs. Dispositions. Critical Thinking Skills Critical Thinking Dispositions. Discipline-specific Critical Thinking Predictors and Correlates of Critical Thinking Skill. Teaching for Critical Thinking.. The Need for Critical Thinking in Biotechnology Education... References.

General Definitions of Critical Thinking


Over the last several decades, critical thinking has been discussed and contemplated in educational circles. Many definitions of critical thinking have been offered. In 1991, Pascarella and Terenzini compiled several definitions, stating that critical thinking typically involves the individuals ability to do some or all of the following: identify central issues and assumptions in an argument, recognize important relationships, make correct inferences from data, deduce conclusions from information or data provided, interpret whether conclusions are warranted on the basis of the data given, and evaluate evidence or authority (p. 118). Critical thinking involves the formation of logical inferences (Simon & Kaplan, 1989). Some scholars and educators erroneously assume critical thinking to be higher order thinking or cognitive processing (Paul, 1994). According to Elder and Paul (1994), Critical thinking is best understood as the ability of thinkers to take charge of their own thinking. This requires that they develop sound criteria and standards for analyzing and assessing their own thinking and routinely use those criteria and standards to improve its quality. Critical thinking can be set apart from problem solving (Hedges, 1991) in that problem solving is a linear process of evaluation, while critical thinking is a comprehensive set of abilities allowing the inquirer to properly facilitate each stage of the linear problem-solving process. According to Chafee (1988) critical thinking is "our active, purposeful, and organized efforts to make sens e of our world by carefully examining our thinking, and the thinking of others, in order to clarify and improve our understanding" (p.29). According to Halpern (1989) critical thinking is "thinking that is purposeful, reasoned and goal directed. It is the kind of thinking involved, in solving problems, formulating inferences, calculating likelihoods, and

making decisions" (p. 5). Simply put, critical thinking is the "reasonable and reflective thinking that is focused upon deciding what to believe or do" (Norris & Ennis, 1989, p. 18).

Misrepresentations about Critical Thinking


Critical thinking is looked at and viewed in many different ways. To gain a more complete understanding of critical thinking, it is useful to look at what critical thinking is not. Critical thinking is not about being superior to someone else. It is different from problem solving, and it is not higher order thinking or cognitive processing. Many scholars engage in, what Richard Paul refers to as pseudo critical thinking, which is a form of intellectual arrogance masked in self-delusion or deception, in which thinking is deeply flawed (1994, p. 14). Other well- meaning educators simply use the term critical thinking in place of other types of information processing that are very similar to, but at the same time different from critical thinking, such as problem solving. Dr. Lowell Hedges (1991) is one researcher who understood the difference between problem solving and critical thinking. He contended that problem solving is a linear process of evaluation, while critical thinking is an overlying set of abilities that allow the inquirer to properly facilitate each stage of the linear problem-solving process. Chart of Hedges views on critical thinking and problem solving . Critical Thinking 1. The ability to identify and formulate problems, as well as the ability to solve them. 2. The ability to recognize and use inductive reasoning, as well as the ability to solve them. 3. The ability to draw reasonable conclusions from information found in various sources, whether written, spoken, tabular, or graphic, and to defend ones Problem Solving 1. Recognizing a problem situation.

2. Defining the problem

3. The ability to comprehend, develop, and use concepts and generalizations.

conclusions rationally. 4. The ability to comprehend, develop, and use concepts and generalizations. 5. The ability to distinguish between fact and opinion.

4. Testing hypotheses and gathering data. 5. Revising hypotheses and testing revised or new hypotheses. 6. Forming a conclusion.

Critical Thinking and Inquiry-based Learning in Education


Since education is our principal means of preparing students our future citizens for an active and responsible life within our technologically-based society, school at all levels should become the focus for the fostering/development of critical thinking (Costa, 1991). According to Nelson (1994), Enabling students to think critically is one of the central objectives of liberal and professional education. Critical thinking in education calls on students to evaluate their own thought process (Kalman, 2002). Critical thinking accompanies a movement in education toward inquiry-based or problem-based learning. According to Schamel and Ayres (1992), s tudents learn best by doing, or preparing their own questions based on their observations rather than participating in a predetermined exercise with a forgone conclusion. Students are fully engaged in learning and cooperative group learning helps students interact with one another (Ahern-Rindell, 1999).

Critical Thinking for Agricultural Education


Agricultural educators have attempted to study critical thinking as it relates to levels of cognition, learning styles, distance education, pre-service teacher preparation, and critical thinking dispositions at secondary and post-secondary levels. Rollins (1990) used the Cornell Critical Thinking test, which is not subject-specific to determine the critical thinking of high school agriculture students (n = 668) in Iowa. He found that seniors were better critical thinkers

than sophomores, that reading score accounted for 28% of the variance in critical thinking, and that GPA and leadership positions accounted for 2%. Many of the critical thinking studies in agricultural education deal with levels of cognition and higher order thinking. Cano and Martinez (1991), who simply defined critical thinking as A set of thinking skills needed to answer a particular question (p. 24) sought to (1) determine the cognitive level of performance of Ohio agricultural education students (n = 385) as measured by the Developing Cognitive Abilities Test (DCAT), (2) the critical thinking ability of agricultural education students as determined by the Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal (W-GCTA), and the relationship between DCAT and W-GCTA scores. All of the students scored the lowest on the critical thinking portion of the DCAT. Additionally, Cano and Martinez found a linear relationship between critical thinking and grade level as measured by the DCAT. In their study, all students were found to score the lowest on the Inference sub-skill as measured by W-GCTA, and again, 12th grade students scored higher on critical thinking than all of the others. In 1993, Cano used the DCAT and the W-GCTA and found that agricultural education students scored at higher levels of cognition than the average of other disciplines. Torres and Cano (1995) also used the DCAT with a random sample of agricultural students (n = 196) from The Ohio State University. They also found that (1) the students scored the lowest on the critical thinking portion of the DCAT and that (2) there was no significant difference in critical thinking accounting for gender. Torres (1999) also introduced baseline information regarding preservice teachers. He reported that preservice teachers are more likely to emphasize basic and application thinking skills and abilities than skills and abilities requiring critical thinking.

Whittington (1995, 2000), who concentrates on higher order thinking because she believes it is a key component of critical thinking has studied faculty and critical thinking. In a study of 28 faculty at the University of Idaho she found that faculty members (1) wanted to teach at all levels of cognition, (2) actually taught at low levels of thinking 98% of the time actually, (2) aspired to teach at levels higher than where they were assessed, (4) had favorable attitudes toward teaching at higher levels of cognition, and that (5) faculty who had experienced more educational activities held more favorable attitudes towards teaching at higher cognitive levels. Whittington (1997) has also shown that the factor having the greatest effect on thinking opportunities was the instructor. Torres and Cano (1995b) and Rudd, Baker, and Hoover (2000) hypothesized critical thinking was related to learning styles using the Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT). Torres and Cano found that learning style predicted 9% of the variance in critical thinking and Rudd, Baker, et al found no significant difference between learning style and critical thinking. The definition of critical thinking Rudd, Baker, et al provided for critical thinking is noteworthy. They defined critical thinking as A reasoned, purposive, and introspective approach to solving problems or addressing questions with incomplete evidence and information for which an incontrovertible solution is unlikely (p. 5). Whether or not distance education delivery methods are effective ways to teacher critical thinking has also been a evaluated by some agricultural educators. Miller and Pilcher (1999) found that instructors taught at the same levels of instruction for teaching critical thinking in traditional and off-campus courses. Ricketts, Irani, and Joness (in press) results generally supported the contention that distance learners were not significantly different than traditional learners with respect to perceptions of course effectiveness, opportunities to think critically and

critical thinking disposition; however, traditional learners were significantly different from distance learners in terms of change in some critical thinking disposition subscale factors, including truth-seeking and inquisitiveness.

Critical Thinking and Science Education


A current theme in science teaching reform is the emphasis on active, inquiry-based teaching and learning (National Research Council, 1996). Inquiry-based learning is a method of instruction focusing on the student and their ability to design a process for use in solving a problem, requiring higher levels of cognition (Uno, 1999). While inquiry-based learning takes the focus away from memorization of specific scientific concepts, there is supporting evidence that students learn as much factual information as they would in a traditional lecture/lab setting (Gabel, 1994). However, inquiry students tend to retain the information longer (Gabel, 1994) and usually experience higher self-efficacy and process skills than students in traditional science courses (Ebert-May, Brewer, and Allred, 1997). The case has been made for teaching critical thinking skills in school. Science classrooms provide many opportunities for inquiry-based or problem-based learning. However, in order for this inquiry-based learning to happen, changes must take place in science classrooms to move away from rote and passive application of learned formulas toward the use of critical thinking as the primary tool of learning (Zoller, Ben-Chaim, and Ron, 2000).

Critical Thinking for Biotechnology Education


One of the difficulties associated with attempting to develop greater understanding of biotechnology among our students is that the science is relatively sophisticated, rapidly

changing, and often difficult for students to grasp. Further complicating the educational process is the fact that studies have suggested that most consumers get their information on biotechnology from the media (Hoban, 1998). Indeed, studies indicate that most of the public's information about biotechnology comes from the mass media, even though their level of trust in what they learn is relatively low (Hallman & Metcalf, 1995). This is true of agricultural educators as well. In a study of teachers of agriculture from three southern states, Iverson (1998) found that the major source for information about biotechnology was the mass media, primarily newspapers. On the other hand, respondents felt their most trusted source of information about biotechnology was the land grant university. Despite these challenges, some university- level agricultural educators have begun teaching courses solely focused on biotechnology and/or to include it as a course topic in agrisciences curricula. Arguably, the ultimate objective of such educational efforts among agricultural institutions of higher learning is to enhance understanding and acceptance of food biotechnology. But how can we determine whether or not these objectives are actually being achieved? As agricultural educators, we may have the educational objective of wanting our students to know more about the science associated with biotechnology, its benefits, perceived risks, and attendant potential social issues. But while we can measure knowledge acquisition based on exposure to information, that does not necessarily inform us of actionable outcomes associated with values, belief systems, attitude formation, and change. The literature has consistently shown that simple exposure to information will not necessarily influence knowledge, attitude or change behavior (Ro gers, 1995; Salwen & Sacks, 1997; Goldberg, Fishbein & Middlestadt, 1997). As nationally known biotechnology educator Thomas Zinnen (2000) has pointed out, however, there is a distinction that needs to be made

between individual understanding of biotechnology and acceptance of biotechnology-derived products. For one to lead to the other may require higher order thinking and evaluation that operates beyond mere exposure to factual information in the classroom or elsewhere. Therefore, it may not be enough to produce educational curricula and conduct instruction that contain balanced, fact-based, objective information in an attempt to raise awareness and enhance knowledge acquisition. It may be equally important to determine how to develop such instruction so as to specifically focus on enhancing a students ability to think and reason critically about biotechnology. This would have the added benefit of ensuring that our future graduates in the food and agricultural sciences are equipped with strong reasoning and thinking skills that will help them act, communicate and educate effectively about this important topic. According to Howe and Warren (1989), science or environmental education topics, such as biotechnology, provide a good mechanism for developing critical thinking skills for several reasons. First, science education provides topics and problems that cut across the school curriculum and can improve the integration of knowledge. Second, science education provides real issues that can be studied or simulated. Last, science education topics can be adjusted to the developmental levels of students.

Critical Studies in Critical Thinking The Delphi Study


Peter Facione (1990) conducted a Delphi study, which will be described in the next section where a group of critical thinking experts drafted the following definition of critical thinking (CT). They concluded: "We understand critical thinking to be purposeful, self-regulatory judgment which results in interpretation, analysis, evaluation, and inference, as well as explanation of the evidential,

conceptual, methodological, criteriological, or contextual considerations upon which that judgment is based. CT is essential as a tool of inquiry. As such, CT is a liberating force in education and a powerful resource in one's personal and civic life. While not synonymous with good thinking, CT is habitually inquisitive, well- informed, trustful of reason, open- minded, flexible, fair- minded in evaluation, honest in facing personal biases, prudent in making judgments, willing to reconsider, clear about issues, orderly in complex matters, diligent is seeking relevant information, reasonable in the selection of criteria, focused in inquiry, and persistent in seeking results which are as precise as the subject and circumstances will permit. Thus, educating good critical thinkers means working toward this ideal. It combines developing CT skills with nurturing those dispositions which consistently yield useful insights and which are the basis of a rational and democratic society" (p. 3). The multiple definitions of critical thinking and confusion concerning its specificity led to the need for further refinement of the composition of the critical thinking construct. Facione (1990) assembled a group of forty individuals (52% from Philosophy, 22% from Education, 20% from Social Sciences, 6% from Physical Sciences) recognized by their colleagues as having special experience and expertise in CT instruction, assessment, or theory. Facione (1990) employed the powerful qualitative research methodology known as the Delphi Method was used to develop the theoretical framework used for this study. The Delphi study (Facione, 1990), which ran from February of 1988 until November of 1989, consisted of six rounds of questions and response. The findings of the Delphi Report are as follows: 1. Critical thinking includes the dimensions of skill and disposition. 2. There was consensus that critical thinking could be improved in several ways. The experts agreed that a person could critically examine and evaluate one's own

10

reasoning processes, that they could learn how to think more objectively and logically, that they could expand their repertoire of those more specialized procedures and criteria used in different areas of human thought and inquiry, and that they could increase their base of information and life experience (p. 4). 3. "While CT skills themselves transcend specific subjects or disciplines, exercising them successfully in certain contexts demands domain-specific knowledge, some of which may concern specific methods and techniques used to make reasonable judgments in those specific contexts"(p. 5). 4. "There is a critical spirit, a probing inquisitiveness, a keenness of mind, a zealous dedication to reason, and a hunger or eagerness for reliable information which good critical thinkers possess but weak critical thinkers do not seem to have...the affective dispositions are necessary for the CT skills identified to take root and to flourish in students" (p. 11). 5. "It is inappropriate use of the term to deny that someone is engaged in CT on the grounds that one disapproves ethically of what the person is doing. What 'CT' means, why it is of value, and the ethics of its use are best regarded as three distinct concerns" (p. 12). 6. "A good critical thinker...is habitually disposed to engage in, and to encourage others to engage in a wide range of contexts and for a wide variety of purposes. Although perhaps not always uppermost in mind, the rational justification for cultivating those affective dispositions which characterize the paradigm critical thinker are soundly grounded in CT's personal and civic value. CT is known to contribute to the fairminded analysis and resolution of questions. CT is a powerful tool in the search for

11

knowledge. CT can help people overcome the blind, sophistic, or irrational defense of intellectually defective or biased opinions. CT promotes rational autonomy, intellectual freedom and the objective, reasoned and evidence-based investigation of a very wide range of personal and social issues and concerns" (p. 13). Many of the findings of the Delphi study are addressed in one way or another in this investigation. The first finding as stated above indicated that critical thinking includes the dimensions of skill and disposition. This consensual agreement among the experts was a reiterated point of critical thinking scholars preceding them (Dewey 1933; Norris and Ennis 1989), but Facione (1990) and his group of experts went a step further. They identified a set of specific skills and sub-skills for the skill dimension and a specific set of attitudes for the disposition dimension (Facione 1990).

Critical Thinking Skills


The critical thinking skills that were identified by the panel of experts were used in this study because they most closely matched the definition of critical thinking that we have adopted for this study, which was "Critical thinking is a reasoned, purposive, and introspective approach to solving problems or addressing questions with incomplete evidence and information and for which an incontrovertible solution is unlikely" (Rudd, Baker et al. 2000), p. 5) and because subsequent studies have been conducted to validate their usage (Facione 1990; Jones, Hoffman et al. 1994; Jones, Hoffman et al. 1995; Giancarlo 1996; Giancarlo 1996). Faciones study (1990) concluded that at the very core of critical thinking are interpretation, analysis, evaluation, inference, explanation, and self- regulation. Inference is comprehending and expressing meaning about a wide variety of experiences, beliefs, procedures,

12

rules, etc. Analysis was found to be about identifying the relationship between statements, questions, concepts or descriptions to express beliefs, judgments or reasons. The experts thought that evaluation was about assessing credibility of statements and representations of others as well as assessing the logical strength of statements, descriptions or questions. Inference was found to be the ability to draw reasonable conclusions and/or hypotheses based on facts, judgments, beliefs, principles, concepts or other forms of representation. The experts in the Delphi study found explanation to be about stating and justifying the results of one's reasoning using each of the aforementioned abilities. Self-regulation, the last skill was found to be the ability of an individual to monitor their own personal cognitive activities to make sure that they are engaged in critical thinking. Several studies have been conducted to confirm the Delphi consensus statement. The 1990 Delphi report describing the ideal critical thinker was put to the test by Giancarlo (1996) using the California-Q sort method, which was a technique derived from the work of Block (1961). A national expert panel concerning critical thinking sorted 100 Q-sort items to achieve a result that would characterize the ideal critical thinker. The results would validate the critical thinking skills identified in the theoretical framework in this study. The following study would secure national recognition of the viability of critical thinking skills. Jones, Hoffman, Moore, Ratcliff, Tibetts, and Click (1995; 1994) further validated the use of critical thinking skills through a 1993/1994 national survey and replication study conducted by the National Center for Higher Education Teaching, Learning and Assessment at The Pennsylvania State University. As a result of this study skills, along with dispositions became recommended outcomes of post-secondary education.

13

Critical Thinking Dispositions


Critical thinking is dependent upon a persons disposition to use it (Paul , 1992). Disposition to think critically can be defined as consistent willingness, motivation, inclination and an intention to be engaged in critical thinking while reflecting on significant issues, making decisions and solving problems (Facione et al. 1995, Facione et al. 1997). According to Zoller, Ben-Chaim and Ron (2000), a students disposition to think critically is a necessary precondition for critical thinking and greatly affects critical thinking capability. In developing the widely used Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal, Glaser (1941) defined critical thinking as the "(1) attitude of being disposed to consider in a thoughtful way the problems and subjects that come within the range of one's experiences, (2) knowledge of the methods of logical inquiry and reasoning and (3) some skill in applying those methods" (p. 5-6). Later, in 1997, Taube reported statistical and empirical evidence of skills and dispositions, two distinct factors of critical thinking. Experts continue to agree that critical thinking includes the dimensions of skill and disposition (Dewey 1933; Norris and Ennis 1989). In 1990, Facione and his group of experts identified a set of specific skills and sub-skills for the skill dimensio n and a specific set of attitudes for the disposition dimension (Facione 1990). Facione (2001) developed the CCTDI (California Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory), in order to measure these skills, sub-skills and attitudes. The constructs used are Truth-Seeking, Open- mindedness, Analyticity, Systematicity, Self-confidence, Inquisitiveness, and Maturity (Facione, Facione et al. 2001). The following construct descriptions are from the CCTDI test manual (Facione, Facione, & Giancarlo, 1996).

14

1. Analyticity targets the disposition of being alert to potentially problematic situations and anticipating possible results or consequences. 2. Self-confidence refers to the level of trust one places in ones own reasoning process. 3. Inquisitiveness is innate curiousness about acquiring information and which motivates the message recipient to learn more. 4. Maturity addresses cognitive Maturity and mature thinkers are disposed to approach problems, inquiry and decision making realizing that some situations have more than one plausible option and that decisions must sometimes be made without the benefit of having all the relevant information about the situation. 5. Open-mindedness addresses the state of respecting the right of others with differing opinions. 6. Systemacity targets the disposition to being organized, orderly, focused and diligent in seeking information. 7. Truth-seeking describes thinkers who are eager to seek the truth even if the results do not support ones own interests or preconceived opinions. Although Faciones work has seemingly been the only attempt at measuring critical thinking dispositions, its validity has been brought into questions by a study conducted by Moore, Rudd, and Penfield (submitted for publication). This study examined the reliability of the subscales of the CCTDI as well as the factor strength of the whole instrument. During factor analysis, the data obtained in the Moore, Rudd, et al study did not fit the seven scale structure outlined by Facione and associates (Facione, Facione, & Giancarlo). Ana lysis of the seven factor structure resulted in only 51 of the 75 items on the instrument with factor loadings greater than .30 and

15

four to 11 items loading on seven factors. The seven factor structure had a sum of Eigenvalues of 25.33 and explained 27.2% of the variance.

Discipline-specific Critical Thinking


While critical thinking skill and disposition can be defined as separate entities, both are thought to be open to educational influence, particularly when meaningfully, contextually bound (Brown 1997). Critical thinking is a valuable skill that, once learned, can be applied in many different disciplines; however, researchers have contended that there is a need to think critically within specific disciplines. According to Glaser, critical thinking is, in part, attitude of being disposed to consider in a thoughtful way the problems and subjects that come within the range of one's experiences (1941, p. 5-6). Ennis advocates contextual, domain, or subject specific critical thinking for several reasons. First, background knowledge is necessary for making justified critical thinking judgments. Second, critical thinking varies from discipline to discipline and, third, a full understanding of a discipline requires the ability to think critically in the discipline (Ennis 1990). (Halliday 2000) argues that critical thinking is to be used in the context of specific disciplines. He quotes Dunne and Morgan from their article in Irish Educational Studies. "Critical thinking is best developed through an engagement with different areas of knowledge rather than as an autonomous skill to be taught in itself. It is through cutting its teeth on actual topics, themes, an issues and problems as these arise within diverse content domains that thinking can acquire the kind of differentiation subtlety and sense of relevance that help to make it truly critical" (Dunne and Morgan, 1995, p. 115). According to Guthrie, Alao & Rinehart (1997), there is a need to situate literacy learning, such as critical thinking skills, within content areas in order to drive learning and increase both

16

literacy ability and knowledge in the content area. Finally, Facione (1990) found that "While CT skills themselves transcend specific subjects or disciplines, excercising them successfully in certain contexts demands domain-specific knowledge, some of which may concern specific methods and techniques used to make reasonble judgements in those specific contexts"(p. 5). The success of domain-specific critical thinking has been demonstrated in research. In a study of 254 seventh grade French speaking science students, researchers found a statistically significant mean gain in pre-test post-test investigation of CCTDI scores (t=4.54, p<.001), suggestive of the fact that critical thinking, indeed can be context specific (Ferguson and Vazquez-Abad 1995).

Predictors and Correlates of Critical Thinking Skill


(Facione 1998) reported on several studies (Facione, Facione, & Giancarlo, 1996; Jones, Ratliff, Tibbetts, & Glick, 1994; Giancarlo & Facione, N., 1994; Facione & Facione, 1997) that determined a significant, but relatively low relationship between critical thinking skills and dispositions. The research indicated that there is a need for both skills and dispositions in curriculum models. Facione describes the disposition toward critical think ing as the "consistent internal motivation to engage problems and make decisions by using critical thinking" (p. 5). Facione (1998) concluded "educational and professional success required nurturing one's consistent internal motivation to think as well as developing one's thinking skills" (p. 16). In a study of 193 tenth grade students in the Southwestern United States a positive correlation (r=.41) was found between CCTDI scores and CCTST (California Critical Thinking Skills Test) total scores. It was statistically significant at the p<.05 level, which might suggest that there is a positive correlation between CT skill and CT disposition. This finding would

17

suggest that critical thinking skill accounts for 16.8% of the variance in critical thinking disposition and vice versa. In the largest known study to identify relationships between critical thinking skill and disposition, as well as other demographic factors, (Facione and Facione 1997) conducted a fiveyear longitudinal investigation of 7,926 students from 50 different college level programs. Positive correlations were found between overall disposition and strength of critical thinking. Examples of the types of analyses run with the large data set were a sample of 1557 nursing students that showed weak positive correlations (r=.201, p<.001) and a sample of 793 students who again had similar results (r=.169, p<.001). These findings would suggest a significant correlation, especially with such a large sample. The study that looked at correlations between each of the subscales and found significant correlations among all of the subscales, except for the relationships between critical thinking self-confidence and awareness and the relationship between critical thinking self-confidence and evaluation. These findings indicated that each subscale skill could not be individually correlated with the corresponding disposition. (Rapps, Riegel et al. 2001) conducted a study to test a model of cognitive development which sought to determine which of the four variables, knowledge base, critical thinking skills, critical thinking dispositions, and experience were utilized to predict cognitive development. Critical thinking dispositions contributed to all of the levels of Perry's scheme of intellectual development; dualism, relativism, and commitment, and experience only predicted the commitment stage.

Age
When the study was designed age was entered into the regression equation as a variable that may have significant influence on the critical thinking skill level of youth leaders, but after

18

an extensive review of the literature only one report placed age in the predictor category. (Torres and Cano 1995) conducted a study of 92 agriculture seniors while testing for the relationship of learning style to crit ical thinking. The control variables, age, gender, and GPA, accounted for 13% of the critical thinking variance. However, it is not possible from their study to know whether age was really significant or not as the control variables were looked at as one. The majority of the studies in the literature show age as having no significant difference or no relationship to critical thinking (Cillizza 1970; Feely 1975; Facione 1990; Facione 1991; Claytor 1997; Jenkins 1998; Rodriquez 2000; Rudd, Baker et al. 2000; Thompson 2001). One reason for this maybe homogeneity of age in most of the groups studied.

Gender
Gender as a predictor of critical thinking skills or dispositions was a variable that has been evaluated by nearly all of the critical thinking studies. One of the first to consider gender in their critical thinking research was (Wilson 1989). He studied the critical thinking ability of (n=203) entering college freshmen using the Watson-Glaser test and ACT College Reports. He found that ACT standard scores significantly accounted for 28.41% of the variance in WGCTA raw scores, but also that gender was a significant predictor or critical thinking skill. Costa, McCraes and Sanchezs study (as cited in (Facione, Giancarlo et al. 1995) examined the relationship of personality. Both studies looked at genders influence on critical thinking in addition to personality and found that females were more open- minded and mature in their thinking, while males were more analytical. (Walsh 1996) conducted a study of 499 male and female undergrads. Along with highest eventual degree and major, gender was a variable predicting variance in critical thinking disposition. In a study of College of Agricultural and Life Sciences undergraduates at the

19

University of Florida that evaluated learning style and critical thinking disposition, (Rudd, Baker et al. 2000) found significant gender differences (alpha = .03) for scores of the CCTDI. Males scored an average of 288.1 while females at the university scored 297.8. Another study trying to ascertain learning style influence on critical thinking combined gender with age and GPA to achieve a significant variance (13%) in critical thinking (Torres and Cano 1995). Since GPA is consistently related to critical thinking, this finding fails to indicate genders influence. There have been just as many studies indicating the null nature of gender effect on critical thinking. Using the Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal, critical thinking was found to be independent of gender, neurotic and rigid personality types, as well as introvert and extrovert measurements (Hoogstraten and Christiaans 1975). The Watson Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal was also used by (Jenkins 1998) when he also found that gender was not a predictor of critical thinking. Other studies using assessments from CCTST to CCTDI to author-developed instruments have also found that gender is not related to or a predictor of critical thinking. In developing and validating an instrument to evaluate critical thinking skills of nurses, gender and ethnicity were found to be independent of critical thinking skills (Claytor 1997). (Rodriquez 2000) studied the critical thinking of (n=60) registered nurses. None of the individual predictors, age, degree, career path, years of experience, personality type, or gender were statistically significant. (Thompson 2001) was another researcher who also found that gender had no predictive value of critical thinking or learning style. (Pienaar 2000) conducted a South African study of adolescents critical thinking in the context of political issues, and found that gender, had no significant relationship with critical thinking ability.

20

The aforementioned studies indicated that gender, as a predictor of critical thinking is still a variable that should be included in the explanation of critical thinking skill. Various potential predictors of critical thinking skills have been studied. The majority of the studies in the literature show age as having no significant difference or no relationship to critical thinking (Cillizza 1970; Feely 1975; Facione 1990; Facione 1991; Claytor 1997; Jenkins 1998; Rodriquez 2000; Rudd, Baker et al. 2000; Thompson 2001). The role of gender has not been as conclusive. Some studies have shown gender to not be related to critical thinking skills (Claytor 1997), while other studies have found a significant relationship between gender and critical thinking skills (Rudd, Baker et al. 2000; Walsh 1996; (Wilson 1989).

Academic Achievement
While the role of gender in critical thinking is unclear, academic achievement status, as exemplified, for example, by high GPA scores, has been shown to be related to critical thinking. Giancarlo and Facione (2001) found that GPA was significantly correlated with four of the CCTDI scales: Openmindedness, Analycity, Systemacity and Maturity. GPA was not found to be significantly correlated, however, with Truth seeking, Confidence or Inquisitiveness. Additionally, SAT scores have consistently been shown to be significantly correlated with scores on critical thinking instruments (Facione & Facione, 1992; Erwin, 1996; Jacobs, 1995; Frisby, 1992) as have ACT scores (Mines et al., 1990; King et al., 1990). By extension, it could be assumed that other hallmarks of high academic achievement, for example, selection as an undergraduate honors program participant, might be similarly related to CCTDI scores. In 1993, Torres did a study where he surveyed all seniors in the College of Agriculture at The Ohio State University. The only thing he found predictive of critical thinking was a student's cumulative GPA while in college.

21

Psychographics
Torres and Cano (1995) surveyed 92 agriculture seniors and found that learning style predicted 9% of the variance in CT and that it must be a significant variable in developing CT skills. A conceptual model developed by Torres (1993) indicated that there were five major factors contributing to complex mental operations or critical thinking abilities. They were teacher-related variables, student-related variables, personal characteristics, learning style, and other factors. Learning style and personal characteristics such as gender, age, and GPA were the only variables looked at in this study. The three control variables, GPA, age, and gender, which were not broken out individually accounted for 13% of the variance in critical thinking ability. Controlling for the variance of the aforementioned variables, learning style (GEFT) accoounted for 9% of the variance in senior students ability to think critically (t=3.38, p<.05).

Teaching for Critical Thinking

The Need for Critical Thinking in Biotechnology Education


The controversy over genetically engineered food in Europe and more recently in the U.S. has prompted many agricultural educators and scientists to begin engaging in more open dialogue with the public in order to provide education as to the safety and benefits of this new technology. According to the National Academy of Science (NAS), it is imperative to develop a genetically literate public that understands basic biological research, understands elements of the personal and health implications of genetics, and participates effectively in public policy issues involving genetic information (Armstrong, 2000). But, given the cross cutting, interdisciplinary nature of the issues involved, the problem as to how to educate our students to understand and be able to address biotechnology issues has so far proven to be a challenge.

22

Existing Instrumentation
Instrument CCTDI or The California Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory Source Contact Information Insight Assessment 217 La Cruz Millbrae, CA 94030 (650) 697-5628 Main (650)692-0141 Fax www.insightassessment.com info@insightassessment.com Testing Purpose Measures the attributes of truthseeking, openmindedness, analyticity, systematicity, inquisitiveness, confidence in reasoning, and cognitive maturity To assess an individual's or group's critical thinking and reasoning skills To gather data for program evaluation and research on critical thinking skills development An intellectually challenging and highly reliable test specifically designed to measure those reasoning skills that are essential to success at the professional and managerial levels Focuses primarily on the evaluative aspects of critical thinking, such as judging the reliability of reports of observations that other people make Focuses primarily on the evaluative aspects Appropriate Audience Community college students, college and university undergraduate students, graduate and professional school students, adults, and working professionals For use with adults at community college, undergraduate, graduate, and professional school levels.

CCTST or The California Critical Thinking Skills Test

Insight Assessment 217 La Cruz Millbrae, CA 94030 (650) 697-5628 Main (650)692-0141 Fax www.insightassessment.com info@insightassessment.com

CRA or California Reasoning Appraisal

Insight Assessment 217 La Cruz Millbrae, CA 94030 (650) 697-5628 Main (650)692-0141 Fax www.insightassessment.com info@insightassessment.com

Individuals who are expected to have advanced reasoning skills, that is, those in the top 20% of the general population.

Cornell Critical Thinking Test, Level X

Critical Thinking Press and Software (formerly Midwest Publications) PO Box 448 Pacific Grove, CA 93950 Critical Thinking Press and Software

Appropriate for students in Grade 4 through college

Cornell Critical Thinking Test, Level

Appropriate for advanced high

23

(formerly Midwest Publications) PO Box 448 Pacific Grove, CA 93950

DCAT or Developing Cognitive Abilities Test

Ennis-Weir Critical Thinking Essay Test

HCTSR or Holistic Critical Thinking Scoring Rubric

Critical Thinking Press and Software (formerly Midwest Publications) PO Box 448 Pacific Grove, CA 93950 Insight Assessment 217 La Cruz Millbrae, CA 94030 (650) 697-5628 Main (650)692-0141 Fax www.insightassessment.com info@insightassessment.com

of critical thinking, such as judging the reliability of reports of observations that other people make Measures learning characteristics and abilities that contribute to academic performance A diagnostic and research tool for analyzing the effects of a specific curriculum Supports multi- modal assessment, for it provides evaluators with descriptors of four levels -- two positive and two negative -- where in they can categorize the critical thinking evident to them in projects, portfolios, presentations, essays, etc. and the like Majority of the items dealing with deduction

school students, college students, and adults

Designed for students in grades 212

Designed for secondary and college students

People who are using reasoned judgment to problem solve and to make decisions about what to do or what to believe

New Jersey Test of Reasoning Skills

TER or Test of Everyday Reasoning

I.A.P.C. Order Department Montclair State University Upper Montclair, NJ 07043 Phone: 973-655-4277 matkowskij@mail.montclair.e du Fax (973) 655-7834 Insight Assessment 217 La Cruz Millbrae, CA 94030 (650) 697-5628 Main (650)692-0141 Fax www.insightassessment.com info@insightassessment.com

5th grade to college level

To assess an individual's or group's basic reasoning skills To secure essential information as an element in a

General population Everyone with a sixth grade or higher reading level

24

comprehensive employment application process To gather program evaluation on reasoning and critical thinking skills. Measures reasoning skills in relation to quantitatively oriented problems

Quant-Q

Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal

Insight Assessment 217 La Cruz Millbrae, CA 94030 (650) 697-5628 Main (650)692-0141 Fax www.insightassessment.com info@insightassessment.com The Psychological Corporation 19500 Bulverde Road San Antonio, Texas 78259 http://www.psychcorpcenter.c om/ pan_reqs/order.html

Technologically and scientifically oriented persons or programs

EMI: Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory

Department of Agricultural Education and Communication University of Florida PO Box 110540 Gainesville, FL 32611-0540

The WGCTA produces a single score based upon the assessment of five critical thinking skills: Inference, Recognition of Assumptions, Deduction, Interpretation, and Evaluation of Arguments The EMI was developed from the Delphi Report.

9th grade and above

High school, college, and adult audiences.

25

References
Ahern-Rindell, A. J. (1998). Applying inquiry-based and cooperative group learning strategies to promote critical thinking. Journal of College Science Teaching, 28 (3), 203-207. Armstrong, J. (2000). The value of biotechnology as an incentive for moral evolution [On- line]. Available: http://www.anth.org/ifgene/contents.htm Ary, D., L. C. Jacobs, et al. (1996). Introduction to Research in Education. Orlando, FL, Harcourt Brace & Company. Carr (1990). How can we teach critical thinking? (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 326 304) Chaffee, J. (1988). Thinking critically. Boston, MA, Houghton Mifflin. Cheak, M., J. (1999). The development and field testing of an instrument designed to measure critical thinking in environmental education. Carbondale, IL, Marie Jaegle Cheak. Cillizza, J. E. (1970). The construction and evaluation of a test of critical thinking ability, grades 7-8. Boston, Boston University School of Education. Claytor, K. L. (1997). The development and validation of an adult medical nursing critical thinking instrument (andragogy), Indiana University. Dewey, J. (1933). How we think: A restatement of the relation of reflective thinking to the education process. Boston, Heath. Dunne, J. and M. Morgan (1995). "Thinking Critically About Critical Thinking." Irish Educational Studies, Educational Studies Association of Ireland 14. Ennis, R. H. (1990). "The extent to which critical thinking is subject-specific: Further clarification." Educational Researcher 19 : 13-16. Facione, P. A., N. Facione, et al. (2001). California Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory: CCTDI Inventory Manual. Millbrae, CA, California Academic Press. Facione, P. A., C. F. Facione, et al. (1996). The California Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory Test Manual. Millbrae, CA, California Academic Press. Facione, P. A., C. F. Facione, et al. (1992). The California Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory: Test Manual. Millbrae, CA, California Academic Press. Facione, P. A. (1991). Using the California Critical Thinking Skills Test in Research, Evaluation, and Assessment . Millbrae, CA, California Academic Press.

26

Facione, P. A. (1990). The California Critical Thinking Skills Test-College Level. Technical Report #2. Factors predictive of CT skills. Millbrae, CA, California Academic Press. Facione, P. A. (1990). Critical thinking: A statement of expert consensus for purposes of educational assessment and instruction. Millbrae, CA, The California Academic Press: 19. Feely, T. (1975). "Predicting students' use of evidence." Theory and Research in Social Education 3(1): 63-72. Florida, U. o. (2001). University of Florida Honors Program Admissions [On- line]. Available at http://www.honors.ufl.edu/admissions/admissions.html. Frisby, C. L. (1992). "Construct validity and psychometric properties of the Cornell Critical Thinking Test (Level Z): A contrasted group analysis." Psychological Reports 71 : 291303. Giancarlo, C. A. and P. A. Facione (2001). "A Look across Four Years at the Disposition toward Critical Thinking Among Undergraduate Students." The Journal of General Education 50(1): 29-55. Glaser, E. (1941). An experiment in the development of critical thinking. New York, J. J. Little and Ives Company. Goldberg, M. E., M. Fishbein, et al. (1997). Social Marketing: Theoretical and Practical Perspectives. Mahwah, JF, Erlbaum. Guthrie, J. T., Alao, S., & Rinehart, J. M. (1997). Engagement in reading for young adolescents. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 40(6), 438446. Halliday, J. (2000). "Critical thinking and the academic vocational divide." The Curriculum Journal 11(2): 159-175. Hallman, W. K. & Metcalf, J. (1995). Public perceptions of agricultural biotechnology: A survey of New Jersey residents. USDA National Agricultural Library [On- line]. Available: http://www.nal.usda.gov.bic/Pubpercep/ Halpern, D. F. (1989). Thought and knowledge: An introduction to critical thinking. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. Hedges, L. E. (1991). Helping students develop thinking skills through the problem-solving approach to teaching. The Ohio State University, Dr. Lowell Hedges. Hichey, M. (1990). "Reading and social studies: The critical connection." Social Education 54 : 175-179.

27

Hoban, T. (1998). Trends in consumer attitudes about agricultural biotechnology. AgBioForum, 1(1), 3-7. Huitt, W. (1998). Critical Thinking. [On- line]. Available: http://chiron.valdosta.edu/whuitt/col/cogsys/critthnk.html Iverson, M. J. (1998). Assessing information sources on biotechnology used by teachers of agriculture in the public schools--a tri-state study. Paper presented at the meeting of the Southern Association of Agricultural Scientists, Agricultural Communications Section, Little Rock, AR. Available On-line: http://agnews.tamu.edu/saas/SAAS2MI.HTM Jacobs, S. S. (1995). "Technical Characteristics and some correlates of the California Critical Thinking Skills Test forms A and B." Higher Education Research 36 : 89-108. Kalman, C.S. (2002). Developing Critical Thinking in Undergraduate Courses: A Philosophical Approach. Science & Education 11 : 83-94. King, P. M., P. K. Wood, et al. (1990). "Critical Thinking among colleges and graduates students." The Review of Higher Education 13(2): 167-186. Mertes, L. (1991). "Thinking and Writing." Middle School Journal 22 : 24-25. Mines, R. A., P. M. King, et al. (1990). "Stages of intellectual development and associated critical thinking skills in college students." Journal of College Student Development 31: 538-547. National Research Council. (1996). From analysis to action: Undergraduate education in science, mathematics, engineering and technology. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Nelson, C. E. (1994). Critical Thinking and Collaborative Learning in K. Boswald, S. J. Hamilton (eds.) Collaborative Learning: Underlying Processes and Effective Techniques. New Directions for Teaching and Learning #59: Jossey Bass Higher Education and Adult Education Series: San Francisco, CA. Norris, S. P., Ennis, R. H. (1989). Evaluating critical thinking. Teaching thinking. R. J. S. D. N. Perkins. Pacific Grove, CA, Midwest Publications. Pascarella, E. and P. Terenzini (1991). How college affects students: Findings and insights from twenty years of research. San Francisco, CA, Jossey Bass. Rogers, E. M. (1995). Diffusion of Innovations. New York, NY, Free Press. Rudd, R., M. Baker, et al. (2000). "Undergraduate Agriculture Student Learning Styles and Critical Thinking Abilities: Is there a relationship?" Journal of Agricultural Education 41(3): 2-12.

28

Salwen, M. B. & Stacks., D. W. (Eds.) (1997). An Integrated Approach to Communication Theory and Research. Mahwah, JF, Erlbaum. Schamel, D. and Ayres, M. (1992). The hands-on approach: Student creativity and personal involvement in the undergraduate science laboratory. Journal of College Science Teaching 21 : 226-229. Simon, H. A. and C. A. Kaplan (1989). In MI. Posner (Ed), Foundations of cognitive sciences. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press: 1-47. Taube, K. (1997). "Critical thinking ability and disposition as factors of performance on a written critical thinking test." Journal of General Education. Walsh, C. M. (1996). Critical thinking disposition of university students in practice disciplines (nursing, education, and business) and non-practice disciplines (english, history, and psychology): An exploratory study. College Park, MD, University of Maryland. Wilson, K. D. (1989). Predictors of proficiency in critical thinking for college freshmen. Boseman, MT, Montana State University. Zinnen, T. (2000). BioIssues. Washington, D.C., GEN National Biotechnology Summit. Zoller, U., Ben-Chaim, D. and Ron, S. (2000). The disposition toward critical thinking of high school and university science students: An inter- intra Israeli-Italian study. International Journal of Science Education 22(6): 571-582.

29

You might also like