You are on page 1of 20

Journal of Applied Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association

2004, Vol. 89, No. 5, 835– 853 0021-9010/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.89.5.835

The Big Five Personality Traits and Individual Job Performance Growth
Trajectories in Maintenance and Transitional Job Stages

Carl J. Thoresen Jill C. Bradley


Tulane University and Cornerstone Management Resource Tulane University
Systems, Inc.

Paul D. Bliese Joseph D. Thoresen


U.S. Army Medical Research Unit—Europe, Cornerstone Management Resource Systems, Inc.
Walter Reed Army Institute of Research

This study extends the literature on personality and job performance through the use of random
coefficient modeling to test the validity of the Big Five personality traits in predicting overall sales
performance and sales performance trajectories— or systematic patterns of performance growth—in 2
samples of pharmaceutical sales representatives at maintenance and transitional job stages (K. R.
Murphy, 1989). In the maintenance sample, conscientiousness and extraversion were positively associ-
ated with between-person differences in total sales, whereas only conscientiousness predicted perfor-
mance growth. In the transitional sample, agreeableness and openness to experience predicted overall
performance differences and performance trends. All effects remained significant with job tenure
statistically controlled. Possible explanations for these findings are offered, and theoretical and practical
implications of findings are discussed.

Organizational researchers have long been interested in relation- Incorrect assumptions about the stability of performance might
ships between personality traits and job performance. With the result in erroneous conclusions about personality–performance
resurgent interest in trait theories of personality and the “discov- relationships. These assumptions could be quite costly to organi-
ery” of the Big Five model of trait structure (e.g., Goldberg, 1990; zations that rely on such research findings to make selection and
Tupes & Christal, 1992), research in this area has flourished. training decisions. As noted by Hanges, Schneider, and Niles
Authors of personality–performance studies (e.g., Bing & (1990), “Every personnel decision in organizations is predicated
Lounsbury, 2000; Crant, 1995; Judge, Erez, Bono, & Thoresen, on the belief that long-term performance can be pre-
2002; Stewart, 1999) frequently make the implicit assumption that dicted. . . . Thus the degree to which performance is stable has
performance is a stable construct and thus rely on cross-sectional, direct implications for the accuracy of any personnel decision” (p.
one-time (as opposed to multiple-time) measures of performance 658). Indeed, if personnel selection decisions are based on a
to capture something that by its very nature unfolds across time. In top– down model using trait measures demonstrated to be valid
large part, studies of the relationship between personality and static predictors of performance at a particular point in time and the
performance measures have been the norm despite longstanding rank-ordering of individuals on the criterion significantly changes
evidence that performance is dynamic (Bass, 1962; Ghiselli, 1956; with time and experience, the utility of such a system likely would
Ghiselli & Haire, 1960). be compromised (Deadrick & Madigan, 1990; Henry & Hulin,
1987, 1989; Hulin, Henry, & Noon, 1990).
Given the centrality of these two areas of inquiry—the nature of
Carl J. Thoresen, Department of Psychology, Tulane University, and relationships between personality traits and job performance and
Cornerstone Management Resource Systems, Inc., Carnegie, Pennsylvania; the changing nature of performance across time—it is somewhat
Jill C. Bradley, Department of Psychology, Tulane University; Paul D. surprising that more research has not attempted to integrate these
Bliese, U.S. Army Medical Research Unit—Europe, Walter Reed Army two literatures. In fact, authors of recent reviews have pointed to
Institute of Research; Joseph D. Thoresen, Cornerstone Management Re- the need to examine motivational constructs such as personality
source Systems, Inc. traits in the context of more sophisticated models of individual
We acknowledge Dave Hofmann and several other members of Re- performance change (Steele-Johnson, Osburn, & Pieper, 2000). In
search Methods Network (RMNET) for advice concerning data analysis for the current study we attempted to achieve such an integration by
our study and are grateful for the assistance of Sally Porter and Mary
capitalizing on recent innovations in the measurement of change.
Westermann for their clerical and data management assistance. Our thanks
also go to José Cortina for his insightful contributions. Any errors are ours
In so doing, we drew upon Murphy’s (1989) distinction between
alone. job performance at maintenance versus transitional job stages.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Carl J. First, we advanced hypotheses concerning different patterns of
Thoresen, Cornerstone Management Resource Systems, Inc., 212 Mary performance change or growth for employees working in these two
Street, Carnegie, PA 15106. E-mail: carl@cornerstone-mgtdev.com divergent job conditions. Second, we used the familiar Big Five

835
836 THORESEN, BRADLEY, BLIESE, AND THORESEN

framework to generate substantive hypotheses about individual mance during maintenance or steady-state stages. That is, once the
differences in performance trajectories—which we defined as id- necessary knowledge and skills have been obtained, personality
iosyncratic patterns of systematic performance growth across a traits should become key forces in determining job-related behav-
specified period of time or a series of performance observa- ior. In support of this reasoning, Keil and Cortina (2001) used
tions—at both maintenance and transitional job stages. meta-analysis to demonstrate a temporal deterioration of the va-
lidity of general cognitive ability for predicting performance of a
Logistics of Performance Change Measurement variety of tasks, with the degree of validity deterioration being
greatest for consistent (i.e., easily routinized or automated) tasks
Once performance is conceptualized as a dynamic construct, the than for more complex ones.
matter of how to measure performance change arises. Devising Although considerable research has been conducted examining
correct methods for the study of change is a problem that long has cognitive ability and performance change, relatively little research
plagued research in all areas of scientific psychology (Cronbach & effort has been directed at understanding the role of personality
Furby, 1970), and the industrial– organizational field is no excep- and dynamic criteria within the context of Murphy’s (1989) main-
tion (Lance, Vandenberg, & Self, 2000). Fortunately, new statis- tenance and transitional job stage model. Stewart (1999) found that
tical techniques provide sophisticated strategies for answering conscientiousness was positively associated with sales perfor-
questions about intraindividual performance change. In particular, mance for both maintenance and transitional stage employees. In
use of random coefficient modeling (RCM; Bliese & Ployhart, subsequent analyses, Stewart found that the order component of
2002), also known as hierarchical linear modeling (Bryk & conscientiousness was more strongly related to performance for
Raudenbush, 1992; Davison, Kwak, Seo, & Choi, 2002; Hofmann, transitional employees, whereas the achievement component was a
1997), has led to great advancements in this area. better predictor of performance for maintenance employees. On
One of the primary advantages of RCM is its ability to simul- the basis of these results, Stewart concluded that “when the crite-
taneously estimate within-person (Level 1; L1) and between- rion is dynamic, broad traits such as conscientiousness may be
person (Level 2; L2) effects across time. L1 analyses concern appropriate [for selection] because they exhibit robust relation-
performance stability and change—specifically, the extent to ships with the various behaviors that affect success over time” (p.
which mean levels of performance for a group of individuals 966). However, because of the cross-sectional nature of his study’s
change across a specified period of time. For example, perfor- design, Stewart was unable to explicitly model individual perfor-
mance may increase or decrease at a linear rate, accelerate or mance trajectories in these two samples as a function of
decelerate (evidenced by significant higher order polynomial conscientiousness.
terms), or display some combination of these trends (Mitchell &
James, 2001; Ployhart & Hakel, 1998; Raudenbush, 2001; Willett Level 1 Predictions
& Sayer, 1994). Conversely, L2 analyses address whether individ-
ual difference variables (e.g., abilities, personality traits) predict One possible reason that personality traits have rarely been
both between-person differences in overall performance across a examined in longitudinal analyses of performance among job
specified period of time (L2 intercept parameters; which we refer incumbents at different job stages is that the categorical distinction
to from this point forward as mean performance differences) and between maintenance and transitional stages is difficult to opera-
individual differences in shapes of performance trajectories— or tionalize. Murphy (1989) argued that people experience transition
rates of change—across this specified period (L2 slopes; Bliese & when “major duties or responsibilities of a job change” (p. 190). In
Ployhart, 2002). the abstract, maintenance stages occur when employees have been
performing a job long enough to have gained familiarity with task
Performance Change and the Job Stage Context requirements, whereas transitional stages are associated with learn-
ing and adapting to new task requirements. The most obvious
Perhaps nowhere are the issues of individual differences in illustration of a transitional stage involves a shifting of one’s job,
performance change more salient than in job stage research. In an occupation, or organization. However, it certainly is possible to
oft-cited theoretical piece, Murphy (1989) distinguished between experience a transitional stage without leaving one’s job, occupa-
maintenance and transitional job stages (see also Deadrick, Ben- tional group, or employer. In practice, there is no hard and fast
nett, & Russell, 1997; Dodd, Wollowick, & McNamara, 1970). categorical distinction between the two stages, and judgments
Murphy defined the maintenance stage as a point at which “the concerning maintenance versus transitional-stage status for a given
worker has learned to perform all major job tasks and is no longer set (or sets) of workers typically require inside knowledge of the
confronted with situations that present novel or unpredictable context surrounding data collection, because different job charac-
demands” (p. 190). In contrast, the transitional period involves a teristics make it nearly impossible to define universal criteria for
stage when “methods of operation are undefined; the workers must what constitutes a maintenance versus a transitional stage.
learn new skills and tasks and make decisions about unfamiliar We believe that there are empirical criteria that can help support
topics” (p. 190). Thus, some form of change (usually an increase) a priori assumptions about whether a group of employees is
in job performance is expected during the transitional job stage as experiencing a maintenance versus transitional job stage. For in-
workers familiarize themselves with job-specific demands. stance, if one has reason to believe that a sample of employees is
Murphy (1989) originally proposed that cognitive ability would experiencing job transition (e.g., changes in required knowledge or
be most highly predictive of performance during transitional task demands), this can be confirmed by examining the perfor-
stages, which require learning of new knowledge and skills. In mance pattern for that group of employees to see if the pattern is
contrast, volitional dispositional factors would predict perfor- consistent with a transitional stage. Conversely, if one has reason
PERSONALITY AND PERFORMANCE TRAJECTORIES 837

to believe that two samples of employees are at different job environments (Hesketh & Neal, 1999). In addition to contrasting
stages, one can test this hypothesis by contrasting mean levels of differences in performance trends between employees in mainte-
performance across a specified time period in these two groups. nance and transitional job stages, one of our central goals in the
Gradual, uniform increases (or complete stability) of performance current study was to examine predictors of individual differences
across time would provide indirect evidence of a maintenance in performance both in terms of (a) mean level performance across
stage, whereas drastic performance increases would suggest a a specified period of time and (b) interindividual differences in
period of transition. performance growth trajectories during this period. Partly on the
Results from several studies provide a useful starting point for basis of Murphy’s (1989) research, we proposed that different
identifying performance patterns associated with specific job personality traits may be salient at these two stages. We specifi-
stages. To illustrate, long-term studies of the relationship between cally investigated the usefulness of the Big Five personality traits
job tenure and performance have found an initially positive linear as predictors of mean performance and performance trajectories in
and then plateauing relationship (e.g., Avolio, Waldman, & Mc- maintenance- and transitional-stage samples of employees.
Daniel, 1990; Jacobs, Hofmann, & Kriska, 1990; Schmidt, Hunter,
& Outerbridge, 1986; for exceptions see Hofmann, Jacobs, & Mean Performance Differences in Maintenance and
Gerras, 1992; Russell, 2001). Thus, performance growth appears Transitional Job Stages
to be more dramatic in early (transitional) job stages and then
tapers off in later (maintenance) job stages. Further supporting Two traits should positively influence mean job performance
these conclusions concerning temporal aspects of job performance, levels in both maintenance and transitional job stages. One such
Hofmann, Jacobs, and Baratta (1993) found a mean group trend of trait is conscientiousness. Conscientious persons are dependable,
linear then plateauing performance over a 3-year period for newly reliable, and achievement oriented, whereas low scorers are care-
hired insurance sales personnel. It is clear that the initial perfor- less, lackadaisical, and undependable (Digman, 1990). Meta-
mance measures reflected a transitional phase for the new hires, analytic research has consistently supported conscientiousness as
and one might infer that the employees were entering the mainte- one of the few personality-based predictors with generalizable
nance phase when their performance gains started to decelerate. In validity across occupations and job situations (Barrick & Mount,
a similar vein, Ployhart and Hakel (1998) investigated sales per- 1991; Barrick, Mount, & Judge, 2001; Hurtz & Donovan, 2000;
formance over eight consecutive business quarters. These re- Mount & Barrick, 1995; Salgado, 1997), and such research has
searchers found support for a classic learning curve indicated by confirmed the positive association between conscientiousness and
linear, quadratic, and cubic models in which performance in- sales performance as well (Vinchur, Schippmann, Switzer, &
creased sharply in initial quarters (positive linear change) and then Roth, 1998).
continued to increase, albeit at a decelerated rate (evidenced by A second trait associated with success in sales is extraversion.
significant quadratic and cubic terms, respectively). Thus, results Extraverts are characterized by gregariousness, assertiveness, pos-
clearly supported a pattern in which the greatest incremental gains itive emotionality, activity, and sociability, whereas those low in
in performance occurred in the initial stages of the study. In sum, this trait tend to be aloof, timid, and socially withdrawn (Digman,
cumulative evidence from these studies suggests that performance 1990). Although the generalizability of extraversion in predicting
increases over time; however, the nature of this increase is more performance across all jobs has not been supported in empirical
dramatic in earlier observations. On the basis of these results, we research (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Barrick et al., 2001; Hurtz &
predicted different patterns of performance for samples of employ- Donovan, 2000; Mount & Barrick, 1995; Salgado, 1997), this trait
ees working in maintenance versus transitional stages. Specifi- does seem to have particular salience for sales effectiveness. In a
cally, we hypothesized the following: meta-analysis restricted to the predictors of sales success, Vinchur
et al. (1998) found extraversion to be a valid predictor of both
Hypothesis 1: The overall performance trend for employees supervisory ratings of sales performance and actual sales volume.
identified a priori as being in a maintenance stage will be Barrick, Stewart, and Piotrowski (2002) found that extraverts were
positive and linear. motivated to obtain status and rewards at work and subsequently
had increased sales.
Hypothesis 2: The overall performance trend for employees Because of differences between the maintenance and transi-
identified a priori as being in a transitional stage will be tional stages of employment, we also propose that openness to
positive and linear but will also show evidence of decelera- experience is related to performance in the transitional stage but
tion via a significant negative quadratic trend. not necessarily in the maintenance stage. Highly open people
display intellectual curiosity, creativity, flexible thinking, and cul-
Level 2 Predictions ture (Digman, 1990). Most meta-analytic research has failed to
support a consistent and positive correlation between openness and
Regrettably, there is a paucity of research addressing exactly job performance across broad occupational categories (Barrick et
how individual characteristics influence adaptive (i.e., transitional) al., 2001), including sales success (Vinchur et al., 1998); however,
job performance, or “responsiveness to changing job demands” Tett, Jackson, and Rothstein (1991) found that openness predicted
(Hesketh & Neal, 1999, p. 47; see also Pulakos, Arad, Donovan, & performance in a meta-analysis of 10 confirmatory studies that
Plamondon, 2000). To the extent that novel (i.e., transitional) job specifically hypothesized such a relationship. We propose that
environments would seem to constitute weak situations (Mischel, openness to experience may be a critical factor for performance
1977), relationships between personality traits and job perfor- under certain job circumstances. Specifically, we suggest that
mance may be most evident for employees embedded in novel job openness should be positively related to sales performance during
838 THORESEN, BRADLEY, BLIESE, AND THORESEN

transitional employment periods because in this stage, an em- ated with both initial growth and later plateauing, providing some
ployee will benefit from adaptability and flexibility characterizing evidence for individual differences as predictors of performance
openness (Goldberg, 1990). Results from a handful of experimen- growth patterns. Finally, Russell (2001) tested whether individual
tal and field studies support this assertion. Using an undergraduate differences in managerial competencies could predict performance
sample, LePine, Colquitt, and Erez (2000) found that openness growth curves of top-level executives. Results indicated that re-
helped participants adapt to changing task demands in a comput- source problem-solving-oriented competencies predicted initial per-
erized decision-making study. Highly open subjects were better formance and that people-oriented competencies predicted later
able to adapt their decision-making and problem-solving heuristics performance growth.
to changing situational cues. In a field setting of managerial We propose that certain traits are related to individual differ-
success, Judge, Thoresen, Pucik, and Welbourne (1999) found that ences in intraindividual performance patterns, although research in
openness was positively related to managers’ ability to cope with this area admittedly is scant. Conscientiousness—the trait most
various organizational changes, such as mergers, acquisitions, strongly and consistently related to performance—should predict
downsizing, and the like. not only mean performance but also performance growth. Behav-
Taken together, these results suggest that openness to experi- ioral correlates of this trait include the use of self-regulatory tactics
ence should foster effective performance at the transitional job such as autonomous goal setting (Barrick, Mount, & Strauss, 1993)
stage. Because of their increased ability for flexible thinking, as and time management (Macan, 1994) as well as the ability to cope
well as their preference for novel stimuli, open salespeople should with stress (Watson & Hubbard, 1996). Thus, we predict that the
be more effective during times of job change. Thus, as with performance of conscientious persons will reflect continuous im-
conscientiousness and extraversion, openness to experience should provement, although this effect may take some time to manifest
be positively associated with sales for transitional stage employ- itself. In support of this view, Helmreich, Sawin, and Carsrud
ees. We made the following hypotheses: (1986) found that the trait work orientation (which is very similar
conceptually to conscientiousness) demonstrated a stronger rela-
Hypothesis 3: Conscientiousness will be positively related to tionship with performance after several months on the job (e.g.,
mean performance differences in the maintenance sample.
when incumbents likely had moved from a transitional to a main-
Hypothesis 4: Extraversion will be positively related to mean tenance period) than with an initial “honeymoon period” measure
performance differences in the maintenance sample. of performance taken shortly posthire, when motivation should be
high among all new employees.
Hypothesis 5: Conscientiousness will be positively related to With respect to the differential predictions for mean perfor-
mean performance differences in the transitional sample. mance trajectories within maintenance versus transitional employ-
ees, we expected a slightly different relationship between consci-
Hypothesis 6: Extraversion will be positively related to mean entiousness and performance growth. Conscientiousness should be
performance differences in the transitional sample. positively correlated with linear performance growth in the main-
tenance sample, reflecting the desire for continuous performance
Hypothesis 7: Openness to experience will be positively improvement for conscientious persons in the steady-state sample.
related to mean performance differences in the transitional In the transitional sample, we would expect this same tendency to
sample. be manifested by a negative relationship between conscientious-
ness and negative quadratic growth. In other words, the perfor-
Prediction of Performance Trajectories in Maintenance mance of conscientious persons should be less likely to peak with
and Transitional Job Stages movement from the transitional or honeymoon period to a time
when job-related duties have become somewhat routinized. Hence,
The prediction of performance trajectories (slopes) is somewhat we offered the following hypotheses:
more complex. A handful of investigators have studied such rela-
tionships in an exploratory manner (e.g., Ployhart & Hakel, 1998) Hypothesis 8: Conscientiousness will be positively related to
or have used characteristics such as general cognitive ability and positive linear growth in performance within the maintenance
job tenure to forecast performance change (e.g., Deadrick et al., sample.
1997). However, we are aware of no longitudinal research directly
contrasting the validities of various personality traits within the Hypothesis 9: Conscientiousness will be negatively related to
context of maintenance and transitional stages. Deadrick et al. negative quadratic growth in performance within the transi-
(1997) found that both job experience and psychomotor ability tional sample.
were positively associated with initial individual job performance
in a sample of sewing machine operators. Additionally, general In addition to conscientiousness, past personality–performance
mental ability was positively correlated with performance in- research suggests that openness to experience should relate to
creases, whereas previous job experience was negatively related to performance trajectories for people in transitional job stages. Be-
positive performance trends for individuals. In addition, Ployhart cause openness has been shown to relate positively to effectiveness
and Hakel (1998) found that individual differences in trait empathy under changing conditions requiring adaptive performance (e.g.,
were positively and negatively associated with individual differ- Judge et al., 1999; LePine et al., 2000), people high in openness
ences in initial performance increases (linear growth) and subse- should show steady performance gains initially (i.e., a positive
quent performance plateauing (quadratic growth), respectively. linear trend). This prediction is consistent with results obtained by
Furthermore, a trait measure of persuasion was positively associ- Russell (2001), who found that resource-oriented problem solv-
PERSONALITY AND PERFORMANCE TRAJECTORIES 839

ing—an employee competency conceptually similar to the flexible relationship between previous individual job performance and reassign-
thinking and problem-solving ability characteristic of highly open ment to the product launch project, as this decision was made at the group
individuals (Goldberg, 1990)—predicted initial performance of level (i.e., salespeople working in primary care were shifted to the new
newly promoted executives. However, as the novelty of the tran- assignment regardless of their previous levels of effectiveness). This is a
critical point, as nonrandom assignment of individuals to the transitional
sitional stage fades as people encounter relatively fewer novel job
sample (e.g., highly conscientious persons or persons with a history of
situations, we would expect openness to be less critical for per-
outstanding performance) could complicate the interpretation of our study
formance, although we certainly would not expect that openness results.
would be a hindrance at this point. Thus, we hypothesized the Changes experienced by this group constituted entry into a transitional
following: phase for at least two reasons. First, transitional sample employees were
required to focus exclusively on the marketing of one particular product
Hypothesis 10: Openness will be positively related to positive and hence were shifting to a specialty area. This change required mastering
linear performance growth in performance within the transi- a great deal of technical information regarding issues such as the physio-
tional sample. logical mechanisms by which the product had been found to effectively
treat the condition for which it is prescribed, indications for the product,
potential interactions with other drugs, and so forth. Because the product
Method
was new, this was novel information for transitional participants. The
Participants and Procedure company provided participants with extensive technical training in these
areas, and each individual salesperson was required to pass a job knowl-
Maintenance sample. The population of interest to the maintenance edge examination covering these areas. Individuals who failed to pass this
stage study consisted of 137 sales representatives employed by a large examination on the first try were subsequently retested until a previously
pharmaceutical firm headquartered in the United States. Primary job re- specified level of declarative knowledge was reached.
sponsibilities for this position include duties such as gaining access to Second, employees assigned to the product launch were required to
potential client physicians, detailing or educating physicians as to the solicit business from an entirely new client base. Specifically, individual
indications for particular products, and strategic targeting of high-potential salespeople had to shift their focus from calling on general practitioners to
client physicians in one’s sales territory. Survey materials were sent to the specialists in obstetrics and gynecology (ob/gyn). An organizational rep-
homes of all potential respondents as part of a large, company-wide study resentative confirmed that there was little if any overlap for the transitional
of relationships between personality traits, job attitudes, and sales perfor- sample in terms of their previous versus new client bases. Thus, to achieve
mance. These surveys were accompanied by a letter from the primary success, sales representatives had to strategically target potentially high-
investigator explaining the purpose of the study, along with a similar letter prescribing ob/gyn specialists (in part on the basis of information concern-
on company letterhead from an organizational representative indicating ing prescription history of similar competitor products, as provided by the
that although purely voluntary, participation in the study could provide the research group within the organization), and “get a foot in the door” in
organization with useful information regarding factors that influence sales gaining access to these individuals. The potential frustrations due to short-
success. term setbacks inherent in the sales occupation (Corr & Gray, 1995) are
A total of 99 complete responses were received in the maintenance particularly salient here, as initial efforts to influence ob/gyn specialists to
sample, for a response rate of 72.2%. The mean age of respondents was change their prescribing habits (i.e., switch from familiar products to the
41.76 years (SD ⫽ 9.51), and respondents reported a mean job tenure of new medication) were likely to meet with failure. Furthermore, given the
11.11 years (SD ⫽ 9.45). A majority (61.6%) of respondents were men, importance of return business, it was necessary for participants in the
and 38.4% were women. A company representative confirmed that these transitional sample to establish interpersonal credibility and long-term,
figures were roughly characteristic of the demographic makeup of this professional business relationships with this new set of potential clients.
particular population. Although the two issues discussed here may not constitute an exhaustive
Transitional sample. The population of interest for the transitional list of the changes experienced by the transitional group, we believe that
sample was 78 sales representatives employed by the same organization they provide a sufficient basis for inferring that these persons were entering
involved in a product launch of a new medication. There was no overlap of a transitional job stage at the time of reassignment to the product launch.
participants between the maintenance and transitional samples. The pro-
cedure used in the transitional sample was identical to that used in the Measures
maintenance sample. We received a total of 48 responses from the transi-
tional group (response rate ⫽ 61.5%). Mean age for respondents was 36.77 The Big Five traits. The Big Five traits were assessed using the
years (SD ⫽ 7.79), and mean job tenure was 7.92 years (SD ⫽ 7.51 years). 12-item scales from the NEO-FFI (Costa & McCrae, 1992) in both the
A slight majority (52.1%) of respondents from the transitional sample were maintenance and transitional samples. This measure has been shown to
women, and 47.9% were men. Again, these figures appeared to match the predict job performance in numerous organizational field studies (Costa,
profile of the population from which the transitional sample was drawn, as 1996). All responses were scored on a 9-point scale with response options
confirmed by a company representative. ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 8 (strongly agree).1 Internal consis-
The validity of our study hinges on the difference in job responsibilities tency (coefficient alpha) reliabilities for the Big Five traits obtained in the
between the maintenance and transitional samples—that in fact, the current study were as follows: emotional stability (␣ ⫽ .80 in the mainte-
changes experienced by the latter truly were transitional in nature. This nance sample, ␣ ⫽ .87 in the transitional sample), extraversion (␣ ⫽ .79
issue deserves some further comment. All members of our transitional maintenance, ␣ ⫽ .83 transitional), openness to experience (␣ ⫽ .69
sample had previously been working in the primary care area, focusing on maintenance, ␣ ⫽ .62 transitional), agreeableness (␣ ⫽ .72 maintenance,
marketing a variety of products (similar to those carried out by participants
in the maintenance sample) to general practitioner physicians. On the basis
1
of a corporate-wide decision, members of the primary care sales force were This format represents a change to the original NEO-FFI format, which
reassigned to a new product launch involving the marketing of a hormone uses a 5-point scale. As the NEO-FFI was administered with a number of
replacement therapy product. Thus, participation in the product launch was other personality and attitude measures (not related to the current study) in
not voluntary for this sample of employees. Employee compensation was our surveys, we felt it necessary to adopt a common format for all of these
directly correlated with sales of the new product. There was no systematic measures to avoid confusing respondents (Mattell & Jacoby, 1971).
840 THORESEN, BRADLEY, BLIESE, AND THORESEN

␣ ⫽ .71 transitional), and conscientiousness (␣ ⫽ .88 maintenance, ␣ ⫽ Analyses


.78 transitional). The reliability values obtained for openness to experience
were somewhat lower than those reported in the NEO-FFI manual (Costa Power analyses. Because of the different number of participants in the
& McCrae, 1992), although the reason for this difference was unclear. maintenance and transitional samples, as well as the modest sample size in
the latter, we sought to estimate the relative power for analyses in the two
Control variables. Although we offered hypotheses concerning only
groups. Power analyses in random coefficient models are complex. Al-
three of the Big Five traits (conscientiousness, extraversion, and openness
though exact methods have been developed for power analyses of discrete
to experience), we included the remaining two traits in all estimated models
predictors, Raudenbush and Xiao-Feng (2001) noted that approximations
for two reasons. First, it is well established that the Big Five traits are not
are possible in cases in which explanatory variables are continuous (e.g.,
entirely orthogonal (Digman, 1997). Thus, for us to determine the traits, job tenure). For these analyses, we used the PINT computer program
independent contribution of conscientiousness, extraversion, and open- (Snijders & Bosker, 1999) to estimate the relative power between a sample
ness to experience as predictors of growth parameters (L2 intercept and with 99 respondents and four time periods and a sample with 48 respon-
slope terms), we felt it necessary to include the remaining traits (emo- dents and four time periods.
tional stability and agreeableness) in all of the relevant models. Second, In the comparative power analysis, we assumed that personality would
as so few studies have incorporated a temporal perspective to the have a direct and cross-level effect size (in a correlation metric) of .30. We
prediction of performance from personality traits, we felt that the chose this value because a validity coefficient of .30 corresponds to a
inclusion of these dimensions could provide valuable exploratory practically significant effect for a given trait in a selection context and is
information. consistent with meta-analytic research estimating the size of associations
Job tenure was used as a control variable in all subsequent RCM between traits such as conscientiousness and extraversion and sales per-
analyses. Although we offered no formal hypotheses concerning the rela- formance (e.g., Mount & Barrick, 1995; Vinchur et al., 1998). Given a
tionships between job tenure and performance growth parameters, we sample size of 99 in the maintenance sample and a conservative two-tailed
included this variable for three reasons. First, past meta-analytic research test of statistical significance, power was estimated at .99 for both the
has revealed moderate positive relationships between measures of job direct and cross-level effects. Using the same parameters in the transitional
tenure and work experience and job performance (Hunter & Hunter, 1984; sample with a sample size of 48 resulted in a power estimate of .89.
McDaniel, Schmidt, & Hunter, 1988a, 1988b; Quiñones, Ford, & RCM analyses. We tested random coefficient models examining the
Teachout, 1995), with the magnitude of the relationship decreasing with nature of relationships between the Big Five traits, job tenure, mean sales
increasing levels of job tenure (McDaniel et al., 1988b). Second, the performance levels, and performance trajectories by using Bliese and
Ployhart’s (2002) six-step model estimation procedure. To test hypotheses
potentially complex nature of relations between work experience and job
of interest to the current study, we estimated random coefficient models
performance trajectories is worthy of further investigation in its own right.
separately for the maintenance and transitional samples. All analyses were
In a rare study of this type, Deadrick et al. (1997) found that previous work
conducted using the Nonlinear and Linear Mixed Effects program (Pin-
experience was positively related to mean levels (i.e., intercepts) of job
heiro & Bates, 2000) in the statistical package R (Bliese & Ployhart, 2002).
performance— but negatively related to subsequent performance increases
Step 1 of the procedure involves estimating the intraclass correlation
(slopes)—in a sample of sewing machine operators. Likewise, Tesluk and coefficient (ICC1) for the criterion measure. In our context, ICC1 indicates
Jacobs (1998) have posited that the benefits of job tenure may be negated how much variability in quarterly sales (i.e., among 396 and 192 individual
by transitional job environments in which the stimuli one encounters bear observations in the maintenance and transitional samples, respectively) can
little similarity to past work experiences. Third, failing to control for tenure be attributed to between-person differences across the 4 quarters studied
could result in upwardly biased estimates of personality–performance (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992). Step 2 involves estimating the nature and
relationships if levels of the Big Five traits were substantially correlated shape of the time–performance relationship through the use of orthogonal
with job tenure in our samples and if tenure were related to performance. polynomial terms (e.g., linear, quadratic, cubic). Each polynomial term
This possibility is indirectly supported by research demonstrating robust reflects a change in the direction of performance change for the sample as
relationships between traits (in particular, conscientiousness) and voluntary a whole (Hypotheses 1–2). Step 3 involves testing for significant variability
turnover (Barrick & Mount, 1996; Barrick, Mount, & Strauss, 1994; across persons in intercepts (mean level differences in performance, as
Salgado, 2002). suggested by Hypotheses 3–7). This step also tests for individual differ-
Job performance. We used results-oriented (i.e., hard sales) criteria to ences in the slopes of time–performance relationships (Hypotheses 8 –10).
operationalize job performance for both the maintenance and transitional As Bliese and Ployhart (2002) noted, tests for intercept and slope differ-
samples. In the maintenance sample, the outcome measure was a simple ences are based on a log-likelihood ratio test contrasting models with and
count of territory sales aggregated on a quarterly basis (November 1998 – without random effects (see also Pinheiro & Bates, 2000). In Step 3,
January 1999, February 1999 –April 1999, May 1999 –July 1999, and separate tests are performed for all relevant higher order polynomial terms
August 1999 –October 1999) such that performance was assessed at four (e.g., linear, quadratic, cubic) to specifically determine potential sources of
between-person slope variability.
points in time (99 persons ⫻ 4 ⫽ 396 observations). In the transitional
Step 4 involves estimating the structure of errors (autocorrelation, het-
sample, performance was operationalized as quarterly product market share
eroskedasticity) in the criterion measure. Accounting for correlated errors
(raw sales divided by all sales in the given product class for each individual
and nonconstant variance is critical in random coefficient models for
salesperson’s territory) because of large differences in market size for
deriving accurate standard errors for estimated parameters in estimating the
products carried by this sample. Although the scaling of the criterion
L1 solution (DeShon, Ployhart, & Sacco, 1998). Results from tests of error
measure was not strictly equivalent across samples, we do not see any structures at Step 4 provide parameter estimates for the final L1 solution.
reason to believe that raw versus market-adjusted sales figures differ from On the basis of the final L1 model, one can also estimate a pseudo- R2
a construct perspective, and such market size adjustments are frequent in statistic to determine the extent to which a model specifying higher order
studies of sales success (see Cravens & Woodruff, 1973; Lucas, Weinberg, growth terms explains variance in the criterion variable for the entire
& Clowes, 1975; McManus & Brown, 1995; and Ryans & Weinberg, 1979, sample by comparing residual variances for the relevant parameter esti-
for in-depth discussions of this issue). Market share data were available for mates in the final model with the same estimates from a model specifying
the same 4 consecutive business quarters in the transitional sample as for only random variation in the criterion measure (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992;
the maintenance group, coinciding with the month of the product launch in Hofmann, 1997; Snijders & Bosker, 1994). Step 5 involves accounting for
the former sample (48 persons ⫻ 4 ⫽ 192 observations). between-person differences in intercepts— or mean sales performance
PERSONALITY AND PERFORMANCE TRAJECTORIES 841

across the 4 quarters studied— using scores from predictor variables (e.g., Maintenance Sample
Big Five traits, job tenure, as indicated by Hypotheses 3–7). Finally, Step
6 adds tests for cross-level interactions by estimating the extent to which For the maintenance group (the upper diagonal of Table 1),
individual-level predictors account for between-person differences in slope rank-order performance appeared to be highly stable across the 4
parameters (i.e., growth trajectories) for time–performance relationships quarters (Quarter 1–Quarter 4). Correlations ranged from .84 ( p ⬍
(Hypotheses 8 –10). Thus, Step 6 contains estimates for the final L2 model. .01) between Quarter 1 and Quarter 4 to .96 ( p ⬍ .01) between
Again, a pseudo-R2 can be computed to determine the extent to which Quarter 1 and Quarter 2. Thus, although these correlations do not
predictor variables account for between-person variance in L2 intercept
show the degree of rank-order instability found in some previous
and slope terms (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992; Hofmann, 1997; Snijders &
Bosker, 1994).
longitudinal performance studies (e.g., Ployhart & Hakel, 1998),
there is at least some evidence of a simplex pattern of correlations.
In addition, there is evidence of changes in performance across
Results time in the sample as a whole, with pronounced increases in mean
Comparability of Maintenance and Transitional Samples sales between Quarter 1 and Quarter 2, t(98) ⫽ 10.86 p ⬍ .001,
and Quarter 3 and Quarter 4, t(98) ⫽ 9.00, p ⬍ .001. An exami-
As discussed previously, we felt it critical to establish the rough nation of bivariate associations displayed in Table 1 reveals a
equivalence of the maintenance and transitional samples in terms consistent correlation between job tenure and sales, ranging from
of the predictor variables of interest to our study (the Big Five .23 ( p ⬍ .05) for Quarter 4 to .33 ( p ⬍ .01) at Quarter 1. As
traits, job tenure). Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for all expected, conscientiousness predicted performance, with correla-
variables in both samples are shown in Table 1. As indicated by tions ranging from .21 ( p ⬍ .05) for Quarter 3 to .28 ( p ⬍ .01) at
Table 1, the two groups appeared virtually identical with respect to Quarter 1. Extraversion was positively associated with perfor-
the Big Five traits, and two-tailed t tests for differences between mance at Quarter 1 (r ⫽ .24, p ⬍ .05) and Quarter 2 (r ⫽ .23, p ⬍
means for the maintenance versus transitional samples revealed no .05), but the relationships between extraversion and performance
differences at the .05 level of significance. Results did indicate a were marginal at Quarter 3 (r ⫽ .18, p ⬍ .10) and Quarter 4 (r ⫽
mean difference in job tenure between the two samples of 3.18 .19, p ⬍ .10). No other traits were significantly correlated with
years, t(145) ⫽ 2.04, p ⬍ .05. However, the direction of this sales performance at any time period in the maintenance group.
difference was such that tenure was higher in the maintenance than RCM results for the maintenance sample are presented in Table
the transitional group. If anything, these results confirm the ten- 2. Results for Step 1 provide strong confirmation of reliable person
dency of the maintenance sample to be employed in a steady-state effects on sales performance (ICC1 ⫽ .83). At Step 2, L1 results
job situation, although the average job tenure for the transitional reveal a positive, linear trend in performance across the four time
sample (nearly 8 years) was still quite substantial. periods and thus support Hypothesis 1. However, a visual inspec-

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations for Variables From Maintenance and Transitional Stage Samples

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

1. Job tenure (years) 11.11 9.45 — .08 .06 .03 .07 .13 .33 .30 .28 .23
7.92 7.51 —
2. Emotional stability 68.71 13.16 .80 .49 .03 .27 .40 .13 .12 .10 .11
66.85 15.43 .11 .87
3. Extraversion 70.63 10.83 .79 .09 .47 .36 .24 .23 .18 .19
71.42 11.74 .00 .45 .83
4. Openness to experience 53.44 11.42 .69 ⫺.02 ⫺.14 ⫺.02 ⫺.03 ⫺.01 .00
51.86 10.53 ⫺.16 .05 .13 .62
5. Agreeableness 67.67 10.73 .72 .32 .04 .02 ⫺.05 .02
67.00 10.15 ⫺.04 .31 .49 .03 .71
6. Conscientiousness 76.73 12.21 .88 .27 .28 .21 .25
76.81 9.93 .30 .57 .34 ⫺.10 .23 .78
7. Sales (Quarter 1) 1101.66 520.97 — .96 .90 .84
2.10 1.20 .13 .02 .23 .25 .38 .14 —
8. Sales (Quarter 2) 1324.71 631.22 — .93 .90
4.91 2.27 .15 ⫺.01 .17 .30 .34 .12 .94 —
9. Sales (Quarter 3) 1280.36 539.73 — .95
7.26 2.89 .12 ⫺.07 .20 .26 .34 .08 .91 .96 —
10. Sales (Quarter 4) 1499.08 681.38 —
9.39 3.50 .12 ⫺.12 .19 .17 .33 .06 .89 .93 .97 —

Note. Correlations for the maintenance sample are shown in upper portion of the diagonal. Correlations for the transitional sample are shown in lower
portion of the diagonal. For means, standard deviations, and internal consistency (coefficients alpha) reliability estimates, the top number refers to the
maintenance sample, and the bottom number refers to the transitional sample. Means and standard deviations for NEO–FFI scales are based on summed
scores from a 9-point scale. Sales data for the maintenance sample represent raw sales volume. Sales data for the transitional sample represent percent
market share. For maintenance sample, p ⬍ .01 at 兩 r 兩 ⫽ .26; p ⬍ .05 at 兩 r 兩 ⫽ .21; p ⬍ .10 at 兩 r 兩 ⫽ .18 for two-tailed tests. For the transitional sample,
p ⬍ .01 at 兩 r 兩 ⫽ .38; p ⬍ .05 at 兩 r 兩 ⫽ .31; p ⬍ .10 at 兩 r 兩 ⫽ .25 for two-tailed tests.
842 THORESEN, BRADLEY, BLIESE, AND THORESEN

Table 2
Random Coefficient Models Predicting Quarterly Sales in Maintenance Stage Sample

Parameter 95% CI 95% CI


Model and parameter estimate SE lower bound upper bound t

Final Level 1 model


Intercept 1301.452 57.305 1189.134 1413.770 22.71***
Linear trend 2553.981 272.044 2020.775 3087.188 9.39***
Quadratic trend ⫺21.558 115.761 ⫺248.449 205.332 ⫺0.18
Cubic trend 1180.187 157.177 872.120 1488.253 7.51***
Final Level 2 model
Job tenure (intercept) 16.129 5.787 4.787 27.471 2.79**
Emotional stability (intercept) ⫺2.719 4.924 ⫺12.370 6.932 ⫺0.55
Extraversion (intercept) 12.418 6.339 ⫺0.007 24.842 1.96†
Openness to experience (intercept) ⫺0.709 4.850 ⫺10.215 8.797 ⫺0.15
Agreeableness (intercept) ⫺9.245 5.806 ⫺20.625 2.135 ⫺1.59
Conscientiousness (intercept) 10.472 5.142 0.394 20.550 2.04*

Job Tenure ⫻ Linear (slope) ⫺19.385 30.004 ⫺78.193 39.424 ⫺0.64


Emotional Stability ⫻ Linear (slope) 2.048 25.532 ⫺47.994 52.091 0.08
Extraversion ⫻ Linear (slope) ⫺9.427 32.869 ⫺73.849 54.996 ⫺0.28
Openness to Experience ⫻ Linear (slope) 11.465 25.147 ⫺37.823 60.753 0.46
Agreeableness ⫻ Linear (slope) ⫺10.238 30.105 ⫺69.242 48.767 ⫺0.34
Conscientiousness ⫻ Linear (slope) 10.342 26.660 ⫺41.912 62.596 0.39

Job tenure ⫻ Quadratic (slope) ⫺7.803 12.455 ⫺32.214 16.608 ⫺0.62


Emotional Stability ⫻ Quadratic (slope) 5.410 10.598 ⫺15.362 26.183 0.51
Extraversion ⫻ Quadratic (slope) ⫺10.820 13.644 ⫺37.562 15.922 ⫺0.79
Openness to Experience ⫻ Quadratic (slope) 5.277 10.438 ⫺15.182 25.737 0.51
Agreeableness ⫻ Quadratic (slope) 24.354 12.496 ⫺0.139 48.847 1.95†
Conscientiousness ⫻ Quadratic (slope) 3.313 11.067 ⫺18.377 25.004 0.30

Job tenure ⫻ Cubic (slope) 19.639 16.216 ⫺12.145 51.422 1.21


Emotional Stability ⫻ Cubic (slope) ⫺12.657 13.799 ⫺39.703 14.389 ⫺0.92
Extraversion ⫻ Cubic (slope) 21.956 17.764 ⫺12.862 56.774 1.24
Openness to Experience ⫻ Cubic (slope) ⫺4.252 13.591 ⫺30.891 22.386 ⫺0.31
Agreeableness ⫻ Cubic (slope) 4.660 16.270 ⫺27.230 36.550 0.29
Conscientiousness ⫻ Cubic (slope) 35.602 14.409 7.361 63.844 2.47*

Note. For all Level 1 parameter estimates, df ⫽ 294: for parameters predicting intercept variation in Level 2 analyses, df ⫽ 92: for cross-level interaction
parameters in Level 2 analyses, df ⫽ 276. For the sake of clarity, hypothesized effects are italicized. CI ⫽ confidence interval.
† p ⬍ .10. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

tion of our data (as well as a critical examination of descriptive studied) and slope parameters (growth trajectories). Not surpris-
statistics from Table 1) reveals a large increase in performance ingly, allowing intercepts to vary significantly improved the fit of
between Quarter 1 and Quarter 2, relative stability of performance our model, ␹diff
2
(1) ⫽ 402.73, p ⬍ .001. The fit of the model was
between Quarter 2 and Quarter 3, and another large increase further improved by allowing between-person variation in the
between Quarter 3 and Quarter 4. In an attempt to model these linear slope parameter, ␹diff
2
(2) ⫽ 75.15, p ⬍ .001, was unaffected
marked group-level changes, we reestimated L1 models to include by allowing between-person variation in the quadratic slope pa-
linear, quadratic, and cubic growth terms in an exploratory fashion. rameter, ␹diff
2
(5) ⫽ 5.42, ns, and was significantly improved by
The inclusion of both quadratic and cubic growth terms is consis- allowing between-person variation in the cubic slope parameter,
tent with previous studies modeling sales performance growth ␹diff
2
(9) ⫽ 61.61, p ⬍ .001. Taken together, these results suggest
(e.g., Ployhart & Hakel, 1998). Results fail to support a statistically considerable individual variability in overall performance levels
significant quadratic effect but do support positive cubic growth. and growth trajectories.
Although we did not hypothesize such a trend, we included this Next (in Step 4), we sought to determine if the fit of our
positive cubic increase in sample mean quarterly performance in maintenance sample model could be improved through modeling
subsequent models to determine if personality traits (in particular, within-person error structures. The results fail to support the pres-
extraversion and conscientiousness) might be associated with in- ence of autocorrelation, ␹diff
2
(1) ⫽ 0.02, ns, or heteroskedasticity,
dividual differences in this later performance increase. Although ␹diff(1) ⫽ 0.00, ns, in the maintenance sample data; hence, sub-
2

the quadratic growth term was nonsignificant, we retained it in sequent models excluded these error term specifications. Final
subsequent analyses to preserve the scaling of the criterion estimates for the final L1 model are shown in Table 2. Included are
variable. parameter estimates, estimated standard errors, 95% confidence
Step 3 analyses involved testing for significant between-person intervals for estimated parameters, and corresponding t statistics.
differences in intercepts (mean performance across the 4 quarters The upper diagonal of Table 3 displays correlations between
PERSONALITY AND PERFORMANCE TRAJECTORIES 843

Table 3 linear performance increases, as well as the lack of significant


Correlations Between Growth Terms in Maintenance and quadratic trend effects at L1, the model failed to account for
Transitional Samples significant variance in linear or quadratic growth.2
Given the unexpected finding concerning the relationship be-
Variable 1 2 3 4 tween conscientiousness and positive cubic growth, we sought to
1. Intercept (mean determine the exact form of this interaction. The complexity of the
performance) — .34*** .03 .76*** Conscientiousness ⫻ Time interaction is revealed in Figure 1,
2. Linear growth .94*** — .55*** .09 which uses the final L2 model parameters to estimate predicted
3. Quadratic growth .54*** ⫺.30* — .01 performance in each of the 4 quarters for persons high (one
4. Cubic growth
standard deviation above the sample mean) and low (one standard
Note. For maintenance sample (upper diagonal), n ⫽ 99; for transitional deviation below the sample mean) on conscientiousness, con-
sample (lower diagonal), n ⫽ 48. As no cubic term is included in the trasted with predicted performance at each quarter for persons
transitional sample model, no correlations are shown for the cubic growth scoring at the sample mean on conscientiousness (Bliese & Ploy-
term. hart, 2002). Values for job tenure and the remaining traits were
* p ⬍ .05. *** p ⬍ .001.
held constant at their respective sample means in these contrasts.
The effect of conscientiousness in predicting mean performance
across time was reflected in the average performance difference
higher order growth terms. Of particular interest are positive between high- and low-conscientiousness persons across the 4
relationships between mean sales and both linear (r ⫽ .34, p ⬍ quarters, which is relatively constant once sample mean differ-
.001) and cubic (r ⫽ .76, p ⬍ .001) sales growth. These results ences in predicted performance are taken into account. A closer
indicate that effective performers (in terms of mean sales across examination of Figure 1 shows a slightly steeper slope for high
the 4-quarter period) were also likely to increase their performance conscientiousness persons between Quarter 1 and Quarter 2 than
between Quarter 1 and Quarter 2 and between Quarter 3 and for their low conscientiousness counterparts, and the same pattern
Quarter 4. As shown in Table 4, the final L1 model accounted for is observed between Quarter 3 and Quarter 4, capturing the sig-
41% of the reliable within-person variance in sales performance nificant association between conscientiousness and positive cubic
across the 4-quarter period for this group. growth.
Table 2 also shows the L2 solution for the maintenance sample.
The first set of terms listed represents predictors of individual
Transitional Sample
intercept terms, or in this case, predictors of mean level sales
performance differences (Step 5; Hypotheses 3– 4). Recall that all Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for all transitional
trait effects are conditional on job tenure. Clear support was found sample variables are shown in the upper diagonal of Table 1.
for Hypothesis 3 (conscientiousness and mean level performance), Again, we observed strong evidence for rank-order stability across
and marginal support ( p ⬍ .10) was obtained for Hypothesis 4 the 4 quarters studied (Quarter 1–Quarter 4) in this sample, with
(extraversion and mean level performance). Job tenure also was correlations ranging from .89 ( p ⬍ .001; Quarter 1–Quarter 4) to
positively associated with individual performance intercepts. Tests .97 ( p ⬍ .001; Quarter 3–Quarter 4), and there was evidence of a
for individual differences in growth parameters as a function of the slight simplex pattern to these associations. As shown in Table 1,
traits and job tenure are captured in the cross-level interaction there was strong evidence of mean performance increases between
terms shown in Table 2. The full L2 model shows cross-level Quarter 1 and Quarter 2, t(47) ⫽ 16.13, p ⬍ .001; Quarter 2 and
interaction terms for the linear, quadratic, and cubic trends in the Quarter 3, t(47) ⫽ 17.63, p ⬍ .001; and Quarter 3 and Quarter 4,
data. Hypothesis 8 stated that conscientiousness should be posi- t(47) ⫽ 15.26, p ⬍ .001. This dramatic pattern of performance
tively related to continuous performance change in the mainte- growth reaffirmed our expectation that this sample truly was in a
nance sample, which is evidenced by a positive association be- transitional period in contrast with the maintenance group. In a
tween this trait and the linear growth term. Although this relative sense, sales performance increased by 134% in the tran-
hypothesis was not strictly supported with respect to the linear sitional sample between Quarter 1 and Quarter 2 (compared with
term, conscientiousness was positively associated with the cubic a 20% increase between the same two quarters for the maintenance
term, providing some support for the general tendency of consci- group), increased by another 49% between Quarter 2 and Quarter
entiousness to predict positive job performance trends in the main- 3 (in contrast to a slight performance decline of 3% in the main-
tenance sample. The results also reveal a nonsignificant ( p ⬍ .10),
positive association between agreeableness and negative quadratic
2
growth. However, given the nonsignificance of the quadratic term In computing pseudo-R2 statistics, we actually encountered negative
(which was retained to preserve scaling) at L1, this result likely estimates for the linear and quadratic terms at L2, evidenced by the
was a statistical artifact. increase in variance for the linear term when the predictor variables were
Table 4 shows the percentage of variance in between-person added (see Table 4). As suggested by Snijders and Bosker (1994, p. 343),
this is a common occurrence in growth-based random coefficient models in
differences in both intercepts accounted for by the differences in
which L1 predictors (linear and cubic growth functions in this case) are not
levels of predictor variables. Predictor variables in the final L2 expected to vary at L2. This suggestion is consistent with our result that no
model accounted for 13% of the between-person variance in per- predictor variables were significantly (at p ⬍ .05) associated with individ-
formance intercepts (mean performance differences) along with ual differences in linear or quadratic growth in the maintenance sample.
10% of the between-person variance in cubic slopes, across the 4 Given that pseudo-R2 values represent mere approximations in random
quarters studied. Because of the lack of significant predictors of coefficient models, we assumed an R2 value of .00 in these cases.
844 THORESEN, BRADLEY, BLIESE, AND THORESEN

Table 4
Variance Accounted for in Final Level 1 and Level 2 Models for Maintenance Sample

Percent variance accounted


Model description Observed variance for in model (pseudo R2)

Level 1 .41
Unconditional on time trends 63,604.29
Conditional on linear and cubic trends 37,327.71
Level 2 intercept differences .13
Unconditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 319,030.57
Conditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 278,104.28
Level 2 linear slope differences .00
Unconditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 6,430,197.19
Conditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 6,869,322.77
Level 2 quadratic slope differences .00
Unconditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 436,504.62
Conditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 483,695.22
Level 2 cubic slope differences .10
Unconditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 1,562,525.60
Conditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 1,407,917.53

tenance sample), and finally, jumped by another 29% between level of significance for any of the 4 quarters studied in the
Quarter 3 and Quarter 4 (the positive change over the same period transitional group. Openness to experience predicted performance
for the maintenance employees was 17%). at Quarter 2 (r ⫽ .30, p ⬍ .05). Correlations between openness and
Given the difference in the scaling of performance in the main- performance were positive but nonsignificant at Quarter 1 (r ⫽
tenance (raw sales counts) and transitional (percent market share) .25) and Quarter 3 (r ⫽ .26; both ps ⬍ .10). Openness did not
samples in the four periods of interest to our study, we sought to predict performance at Quarter 4 (r ⫽ .17, ns). Surprisingly,
confirm a positive association between raw and adjusted sales conscientiousness (Hypothesis 5) failed to predict performance in
figures in the latter group. Strong, positive correlations between any of the 4 quarters studied. Although not hypothesized, agree-
raw and market share adjusted sales figures at each quarter would ableness predicted performance in each of the 4 quarters, with
support our use of the latter as criteria in the transitional sample. correlations ranging from .33 ( p ⬍ .05) at Quarter 4 to .38 ( p ⬍
We obtained the necessary data from company records and found .01) at Quarter 1. The remaining Big Five traits, as well as job
strong correlations between raw and adjusted sales of .85 at Quar- tenure, failed to predict performance at any measurement period in
ter 1, .78 at Quarter 2, .72 at Quarter 3, and .72 at Quarter 4 (all this sample.
ps ⬍ .001). The mean correlation between raw and market share RCM results for the transitional sample are presented in
adjusted sales across these four periods was .77. Table 5. At Step 1, we found ICC1 ⫽ .22. Consistent with
Regarding bivariate predictor– criterion relationships (see Table Hypothesis 2, the L1 results show a positive linear trend in
1), extraversion failed to predict performance at the traditional .05 performance followed by a significant negative quadratic trend,

Figure 1. Predicted sales in the maintenance sample at each quarter as a function of conscientiousness (C).
Total sales volume ⫽ sales in terms of the number of prescriptions written by physicians in the salesperson’s
territory.
PERSONALITY AND PERFORMANCE TRAJECTORIES 845

Table 5
Random Coefficient Models Predicting Monthly Sales in Transitional Stage Sample

95% CI 95% CI
Parameter lower upper
Model and parameter estimate SE bound bound t

Final Level 1 model


Intercept 5.916 0.349 5.233 6.599 16.97***
Linear trend 37.497 1.839 33.893 41.102 20.39***
Quadratic trend ⫺2.350 0.593 ⫺3.512 ⫺1.188 ⫺3.96***
Final Level 2 model
Job tenure (intercept) 0.059 0.046 ⫺0.030 0.148 1.29
Emotional stability (intercept) ⫺0.052 0.027 ⫺0.105 0.001 ⫺1.92†
Extraversion (intercept) 0.016 0.034 ⫺0.051 0.083 0.47
Openness to experience (intercept) 0.064 0.032 0.003 0.126 2.04*
Agreeableness (intercept) 0.091 0.037 0.019 0.163 2.47*
Conscientiousness (intercept) 0.034 0.042 ⫺0.048 0.117 0.82

Job Tenure ⫻ Linear (slope) 0.242 0.249 ⫺0.245 0.730 0.97


Emotional Stability ⫻ Linear (slope) ⫺0.359 0.148 ⫺0.649 ⫺0.069 ⫺2.43*
Extraversion ⫻ Linear (slope) 0.167 0.187 ⫺0.199 0.534 0.89
Openness to Experience ⫻ Linear (slope) 0.173 0.172 ⫺0.165 0.510 1.00
Agreeableness ⫻ Linear (slope) 0.396 0.201 0.002 0.789 1.97*
Conscientiousness ⫻ Linear (slope) 0.146 0.229 ⫺0.302 0.595 0.64

Job tenure ⫻ Quadratic (slope) ⫺0.082 0.077 ⫺0.232 0.068 ⫺1.07


Emotional stability ⫻ Quadratic (slope) ⫺0.020 0.046 ⫺0.109 0.070 ⫺0.44
Extraversion ⫻ Quadratic (slope) 0.069 0.058 ⫺0.044 0.181 1.19
Openness to Experience ⫻ Quadratic (slope) ⫺0.202 0.053 ⫺0.305 ⫺0.098 ⫺3.80**
Agreeableness ⫻ Quadratic (slope) ⫺0.065 0.062 ⫺0.186 0.056 ⫺1.05
Conscientiousness ⫻ Quadratic (slope) ⫺0.039 0.071 ⫺0.177 0.099 ⫺0.55

Note. For all Level 1 parameter estimates, df ⫽ 142; for parameters predicting intercept variation in Level 2
analyses, df ⫽ 41; for cross-level interaction parameters in level 2 analyses, df ⫽ 130. For the sake of clarity,
hypothesized effects are italicized. CI ⫽ confidence interval.
† p ⬍ .10. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

indicating a plateauing or deceleration of group-level perfor- (r ⫽ –.30, p ⬍ .05). These results suggest a pattern in which
mance (which still increases, but not at the same rate) between effective performers in the transitional sample (in terms of
Quarter 2 and Quarter 3 (Step 2). These results may suggest a mean sales) were more likely to experience performance in-
movement from a transitional to a maintenance stage at this creases early in the study and also less likely to experience
period. As in the maintenance sample, we tested an L1 model plateaued performance throughout the course of the study. As
involving a cubic term in an exploratory fashion (Ployhart & shown in Table 5, the final L1 model accounted for 88% of the
Hakel, 1998). The results fail to support cubic growth in the reliable within-person variance in sales performance across the
transitional sample— hence, the cubic term was dropped from 4-quarter period for the transitional sample.
all subsequent analyses. The results of our Step 3 analyses show Also shown in Table 5 are parameter estimates for the L2
that allowing intercepts to vary increased model fit, ␹diff2
(2) ⫽ model in the transitional sample. Again, all effects for the traits
10.98, p ⬍ .001. There was significant interindividual variation are conditional on job tenure. Hypotheses 5–7 concerned the
in both the linear, ␹diff
2
(2) ⫽ 198.94, p ⬍ .001, and quadratic, prediction of performance intercept differences (mean perfor-
␹diff(5) ⫽ 33.86, p ⬍ .001, slope parameters. Again, these
2
mance) from conscientiousness, extraversion, and openness to
results support the possibility of individual difference-based experience, respectively, with job tenure held constant. Only
predictors of the L2 terms. The results of our analyses of error Hypothesis 7 (openness to experience) received support—indi-
structures fail to support autocorrelation, ␹diff
2
(1) ⫽ 1.60, ns, or viduals high in openness tended to have higher mean perfor-
heteroskedasticity, ␹diff(1) ⫽ 0.96, ns, so these specifications
2
mance than did those low in openness. Although not hypothe-
were dropped from subsequent analyses. A correlational anal- sized, agreeableness also was positively associated with mean
ysis of trend terms (see lower diagonal of Table 3) shows a level performance.
nearly perfect correlation between mean performance and linear Hypothesis 9 concerned the effects of conscientiousness on
growth (r ⫽ .94, p ⬍ .001), perhaps a tautological result in the negative quadratic performance growth (e.g., the plateauing
sense that all salespeople in the transitional sample began the effect—such that the performance of conscientious persons
study with sales of zero. More interesting is the finding that should be less likely to peak at some point during the study). As
mean performance was negatively related to negative quadratic shown in Table 5, this hypothesis failed to receive support, as
growth (r ⫽ –.54, p ⬍ .001). Positive linear and negative conscientiousness failed to predict any of the L2 parameters.
quadratic growth also were moderately and inversely related Two traits clearly were associated with the linear term. Our
846 THORESEN, BRADLEY, BLIESE, AND THORESEN

Table 6
Variance Accounted for in Final Level 1 and Level 2 Models for Transitional Sample

Observed Percent variance accounted for


Model description variance in model (Pseudo R2)

Level 1 .88
Unconditional on time trends 11.07
Conditional on linear and quadratic trends 1.28
Level 2 intercept differences .18
Unconditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 5.51
Conditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 4.53
Level 2 linear slope differences .11
Unconditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 155.56
Conditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 137.72
Level 2 quadratic slope differences .31
Unconditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 10.09
Conditional on Big Five traits and job tenure 6.94

results indicate that agreeable salespeople were more likely and Figure 2 supports a persistent effect on mean level performance
emotionally stable individuals were less likely to increase their across the 4 quarters for agreeableness, corresponding to the sig-
sales in a positive linear fashion across the period of the study. nificant association between agreeableness and the L2 intercept
Finally, openness to experience was negatively associated with term in this sample. In addition, a close examination of Figure 2
negative quadratic growth, indicating that open salespeople shows a slightly steeper slope for persons high in agreeableness
were less likely to experience plateaued performance across the across the study period, reflecting the positive association between
4-quarter period of the study. Job tenure failed to predict both this trait and linear growth at L2. Identical effects are shown in
mean level performance and performance growth. As indicated Figure 3 for emotional stability, although this effect is in the
by Table 6, predictor variables in the final L2 model accounted opposite direction as one might expect, with low emotional stabil-
for 18% of the between-person variance in performance inter- ity (i.e., neuroticism) positively associated with mean performance
cepts and 11% and 31% of the between-person variance in differences and positive performance growth. Finally, Figure 4
linear and quadratic slopes, respectively, across the 4 quarters reveals the complexity of the interaction between openness to
studied for the transitional group. experience and time. Again, positive mean-level performance dif-
Effects on performance at each quarter for agreeableness, emo- ferences as a function of openness across the 4 quarters are
tional stability, and openness to experience in the transitional immediately apparent. The increasing divergence between high-
sample are shown in Figures 2, 3, and 4, respectively. For each and low-openness salespeople across the 4 quarters represents the
trait, predicted performance of persons one standard deviation negative association between openness and negative quadratic
above and one standard deviation below the sample mean on the growth. Although there is a slight decrease in the sample as a
given trait are contrasted along with predicted performance at each whole in the rate of performance growth between Quarter 1 and
quarter for persons scoring at the sample mean for each trait. Quarter 4 (see the line corresponding to predicted sales at sample
Again, values for job tenure and the relevant other four traits are mean levels of openness), this effect is less pronounced for those
held constant at their respective sample means in these contrasts. high in openness to experience.

Figure 2. Predicted sales in the transitional sample at each quarter as a function of agreeableness (A).
PERSONALITY AND PERFORMANCE TRAJECTORIES 847

Figure 3. Predicted sales in the transitional sample at each quarter as a function of emotional stability (ES).

Discussion change (for conscientiousness) in the maintenance sample but not in


the transitional sample. The reason for this discrepancy is unclear.
Our study adds to a growing body of literature concerning the
However, it has been argued that during the maintenance stage,
temporal nature of job performance. In addition, ours is the first
employees do not necessarily face new challenges or novel stimuli on
study of which we are aware to advance explicit hypotheses
a regular basis (Murphy, 1989). Perhaps employees low on these traits
concerning associations between personality factors and change in
may become complacent during this stage. In contrast, extraverts and
individual performance trajectories while accounting for job stage
conscientious persons are more likely to excel under these circum-
in a field setting. To date, the majority of research on personality–
performance relations has correlated personality traits with perfor- stances, as such persons have high energy levels, strive for achieve-
mance measured at only one point in time. Some of our hypotheses ment, and in the case of conscientiousness, are more likely to be
concerning personality–performance relations in the context of duty-bound (Hogan & Ones, 1997). Thus, extraversion and consci-
performance change were supported, whereas others were not. entiousness should continue to predict job performance even when
Conversely, we obtained serendipitous findings with respect to the job-related tasks have become fairly routinized, as in the maintenance
validity of agreeableness in predicting performance growth param- stage (Stewart, 1999). In fact, our results do point to conscientiousness
eters in the transitional sample. as a positive force in performance change in the maintenance sample,
Most important, the same personality factors associated with suc- as evidenced by the significant cubic trend. These traits may be less
cess did not necessarily generalize across the maintenance and tran- salient during transitional stages, in which individual differences in
sitional job contexts. For example, conscientiousness and extraversion creativity, intellectual flexibility, problem solving, and adaptability
were positively associated with mean performance and performance are brought to the forefront.

Figure 4. Predicted sales in the transitional sample at each quarter as a function of openness to experi-
ence (OE).
848 THORESEN, BRADLEY, BLIESE, AND THORESEN

Consistent with this view, openness was positively associated action (e.g., increase effort) to eliminate the unpleasant emotions
with both mean performance and performance trends in the tran- associated with stress. As people low in emotional stability are
sitional sample but unrelated to sales in the maintenance sample. more likely to appraise their job environments as threatening
Thus, openness may be a critical factor for performance when (Spector, Zapf, Chen, & Frese, 2000) and a transitional job period
employees are required to adapt to change (as hypothesized) but is full of novel challenges, some degree of neuroticism may
less important for steady state performance. One might expect that actually be beneficial for performance under these conditions.
the validity of openness to experience would wane with time in our However, researchers rarely advance such arguments, and this
transitional sample, signaling the movement to more of a mainte- finding clearly needs replication before such an interpretation
nance stage for this group of employees. Bivariate analyses from would seem credible.
our study suggest slightly weaker correlations between openness Finally, because job tenure was not a central focus of this study,
and later performance (e.g., Quarter 4). However, our RCM results we did not hypothesize any directional associations between tenure
tell a different story, as highly open salespeople had increased and either mean level performance or performance trends in the
sales aggregated across the 4-quarter period as a whole and were maintenance and transitional samples. However, our results reveal
less likely to experience plateaued performance at later periods. a positive relationship between tenure and mean performance in
An unexpected finding was the strong positive relationship the maintenance sample, indicating that employees who had
between agreeableness and both mean performance and perfor- worked in their jobs longer had higher sales. There was no asso-
mance growth in the transitional sample. Agreeableness, although ciation between tenure and sales in the transitional sample. These
sometimes argued to interact with other personality traits such as findings deserve comment. On one hand, to the extent that tenure
conscientiousness in predicting performance (Witt, Burke, Barrick, would facilitate performance, one might expect that this effect
& Mount, 2002), is rarely hypothesized to have a main effect on would be stronger for incumbents in the transitional sample, as
performance. However, in the transitional sample, we believe these persons may have similar (although not identical) experi-
salesperson agreeableness may have resulted in a positive “foot in ences in their past work with the company to those encountered in
the door” effect. That is, agreeable salespeople may have achieved their new positions. This effect would be reflected in a higher
greater success in the transitional group as a function of their positive linear trend (i.e., higher initial performance) for highly
increased ability to gain access to potential customers as well as tenured salespeople. On the other hand, consistent with Deadrick
their ability to maintain positive relationships with these persons et al.’s (1997) findings, such an effect would be expected to
across time. What would lead to such an effect? One possible dissipate with time. Neither assertion is supported by our data. The
mediating mechanism is trust. A fundamental characteristic of positive relationship between tenure and mean sales in the main-
agreeable persons is their trustworthiness, whereas disagreeable tenance sample may simply reflect the within-sample variation in
people are manipulative, cynical, and self-serving (Costa & Mc- job knowledge as a result of experience in this group (Murphy,
Crae, 1995). In addition, agreeableness has been shown to be a key 1989). Although not the central focus of the current study, a
component of employee integrity (Sackett & DeVore, 2001). As critical research question in this context might involve estimation
gatekeepers of information, it is critical that pharmaceutical sales of the diminishing returns on performance across time (e.g., pla-
representatives have trusting relationships with potential custom- teaued growth) of previous job tenure and experience as a function
ers. In fact, a recent meta-analysis of the sales literature uncovered of job characteristics such as complexity or changing task envi-
a significant positive association between customer trust percep- ronments (Schmidt, Hunter, Outerbridge, & Goff, 1988; Tesluk &
tions and sales performance (Swan, Bowers, & Richardson, 1998). Jacobs, 1998).
Thus, the quality of employee– customer interactions may have Several potential weaknesses of the current study deserve men-
represented the driving force behind the validity of agreeableness tion. First, it should be noted that in both the maintenance and
in predicting transitional group performance and performance transitional data, rank-order consistency in performance across
trends in our study. Consistent with such an interpretation, meta- time was somewhat higher than that frequently reported in other
analytic research has shown that agreeableness possesses validity studies of this type (e.g., Deadrick et al., 1997; Hofmann et al.,
comparable with that of conscientiousness for jobs focused on 1992, 1993; Ployhart & Hakel, 1998). Although the reason for this
interpersonal interactions in which “getting along” is critical for tendency in our own data is unclear, it is possible that individual
performance (Hogan & Holland, 2003; Mount, Barrick, & Stewart, performance in both samples was largely determined by tempo-
1998). rally stable factors outside the person’s control (e.g., market
More difficult to explain is the negative association between demographics, territory size), resulting in upwardly biased consis-
emotional stability and linear performance growth in the transi- tency estimates. The measurement of performance— even objec-
tional sample. To the extent that emotional stability predicts per- tive performance—is always potentially subject to confounds of
formance, meta-analytic reviews have concluded that the associa- this sort (Austin & Villanova, 1992; Muckler, 1992), and we do
tion is positive (e.g., Judge & Bono, 2001; Salgado, 1997). This not necessarily see our samples as differing from typical sales
makes sense, in light of the fact that people with low emotional samples in this regard. Such influences might place limits on the
stability are described as depressed, insecure, and anxious (Costa effects of individual difference factors as predictors of individual
& McCrae, 1995), traits that would hardly seem to facilitate mean performance differences and performance trajectories across
effective job performance under any circumstances. One possible a specified time period. However, the magnitude of bivariate
explanation for this counterintuitive finding comes from control personality–performance relationships we observed in our study
and cybernetic theories of the stress process as applied to work was comparable to those frequently reported in the literature (e.g.,
(Carver & Scheier, 1990, 1998; Edwards, 1992). These theories Barrick & Mount, 1991; Hurtz & Donovan, 2000), although our
posit that when confronted with stressful episodes, people take conclusions about which traits possess validity under maintenance
PERSONALITY AND PERFORMANCE TRAJECTORIES 849

versus transitional job conditions differed somewhat from findings information, and had to seek out a new client base, they ultimately
typically generated by studies correlating personality traits with remained working for the same employer in the same industry. It
static job performance measures, as well as several major meta- is difficult to definitively say whether our designation of this group
analyses of personality trait validity (e.g., Barrick & Mount, 1991; of employees as a transitional sample diminished the internal
Barrick et al., 2001; Hurtz & Donovan, 2000). Future validity validity of our study (Cook & Campbell, 1979), and we acknowl-
generalization research may want to consider job stage as a mod- edge this possibility. However, if the transitional stage in the
erator of personality–performance relationships by comparing the current study were truly not transitional in nature, we probably
validity of traits in high- versus low-tenured employees. The often would not have observed the strong positive linear and negative
disappointing meta-analytic results obtained for the validity of quadratic trends in group mean performance (signaling a potential
traits such as agreeableness and openness to experience may transitional to maintenance stage shift) as well as the tendency of
simply be the result of the tendency of previous meta-analytic different personality factors (e.g., openness to experience, agree-
authors to collapse across job stage variables in estimating the ableness) to be associated with success in this situation vis-à-vis
validity of these traits. the maintenance context. If anything, we believe that our results
A second potential shortcoming of our study concerns our use of may be on the conservative side, given the nature of our transi-
broadband measures of the Big Five traits—particularly in light of tional sample. That is, maintenance versus transitional differences
the null effects for conscientiousness in predicting performance in performance trends—and predictors of these trends—might
and performance growth parameters in the transitional sample. In have been more pronounced had we used a start-up sample as our
recent years, attention has returned to the longstanding bandwidth– transitional group. Future studies could avert this problem through
fidelity debate within industrial– organizational psychology as it the use of transitional samples comprising persons with no prior
relates to the relative validity of broad versus narrow traits in the experience in a given industry or occupation or at least the use of
prediction of job performance. Several researchers have argued subjective perceptual measures of job stage status on which main-
that narrow traits offer the most promise (e.g., Ashton, 1998), tenance and (ostensibly) transitional samples could be contrasted
others have suggested that broad traits are preferable (Ones & to verify the status of the latter.
Viswesvaran, 1996), and still others have pointed out that the Finally, we did not measure general mental ability (g) in the
choice of broad versus narrow measures should be determined by current study to test its relationship to individual performance
the nature of the criteria that one is trying to predict (Hogan & growth. Thus, it was not possible to test the relative importance of
Holland, 2003; Hogan & Roberts, 1996; R. J. Schneider, Hough, & g versus personality dispositions and job tenure in the prediction of
Dunnette, 1996). In the context of our study, it is possible that performance across time. However, we do not feel that this omis-
some facets of conscientiousness (e.g., order, duty, achievement, sion invalidates our results. Meta-analytic research has shown that,
competence) may be relevant for transitional job performance, because g is relatively orthogonal to the Big Five dimensions, g
whereas others are less relevant (or perhaps negatively related). If and personality uniquely add to the prediction of job performance
so, the use of a measure of conscientiousness combining items in most occupations (Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). In addition—and
from these various facets—yet not allowing for comprehensive in contradiction to previous research in this area—Vinchur et al.
assessment of any of the facets individually—would cause these (1998) found that g was uncorrelated with objective sales data in
competing influences to cancel out, producing an overall null their meta-analysis of the predictors of sales success. Thus, it is
effect for conscientiousness. In fact, several recent studies have unlikely that the omission of g from our study substantively
suggested differences in the nature and direction of facet-level affected parameter estimates for the Big Five traits in the predic-
associations within Big Five constructs for the prediction of cri- tion of performance. However, future research might incorporate
teria such as escalation of commitment in decision-making situa- both g and personality traits in the prediction of temporal changes
tions (Moon, 2001; Moon, Hollenbeck, Humphrey, & Maue, in job performance for employees at maintenance and transitional
2003), absenteeism (Judge, Martocchio, & Thoresen, 1997), and job stages. In spite of these potential shortcomings, we feel that our
most critical to our study, job performance (Stewart, 1999). Like- use of real field data, as well as our use of the Big Five taxonomy
wise, we found fairly modest effects for the broad traits in pre- to generate and test theoretically salient personality–performance
dicting some of the higher order growth trend terms (e.g., linear and performance trajectory relationships, somewhat ameliorates
growth in maintenance and transitional samples) relative to our these concerns.
ability to predict mean performance levels. It is possible that the The practical implications of our results for organizations de-
higher order growth trend terms contain specific factor variance serve some comment. Numerous management theorists have ob-
uniquely associated with the facet level (but not the broad-based) served that the nature of jobs is changing, such that in the future,
components of the Big Five traits.3 Future research might attempt workers will need to continuously increase their skill base to
to replicate our results using measures allowing for more fine experience workplace success (Howard, 1995; Patterson, 2001).
grained assessments of Big Five traits at the facet level to tease out Likewise, our economy is becoming increasingly service oriented
these potentially complex influences in predicting job performance (V. Schneider, Chung, & Yusko, 1993), and the focus on service
in a temporal context while still allowing for broad-based assess- is particularly salient for people in sales jobs. Given these chang-
ment by summing facet-level scores for each Big Five trait. ing aspects of the workplace environment, organizations may want
A third issue revolves around salespeople in the transitional to consider traits that relate to adaptability (openness) and service
sample. One may question whether the changes experienced by
these individuals were qualitatively comparable to those described
by Murphy (1989). For example, although these persons experi- 3
We thank an anonymous reviewer for bringing to our attention this
enced a shift in priorities, were required to learn new technical possibility.
850 THORESEN, BRADLEY, BLIESE, AND THORESEN

(most likely, agreeableness) in addition to factors such as consci- traits. Rather than suggesting that the validity for traits deteriorates
entiousness and general mental ability when making hiring deci- with time, our results point to the conclusion that changes in
sions. Furthermore, organizations may want to take these charac- performance at relatively distal time periods (i.e., performance
teristics into account when nominating employees for transitional growth trajectories) can actually be predicted with some degree of
job situations. Other researchers have advanced similar arguments accuracy from personality traits such as conscientiousness, open-
with respect to identifying employees most capable of coping with ness to experience, and agreeableness, depending on the nature of
the demands of changes such as organizational restructuring and the sample of job incumbents in question. That is, traits may
downsizing (Judge et al., 1999), expatriate work assignments constitute substantively important determinants of changes in job
(Ones & Viswesvaran, 1997, 1999), and the implementation of performance patterns with increased job experience. Thus, one
new technologies (Hesketh & Neal, 1999). Although the changes conclusion one might draw from our study is that the initial gains
experienced by our transitional sample may not have approached to organizations for selection based on personality traits are not
the scope of those listed here, it is interesting to note that the same epiphenomenal but that they persist (and may even increase) with
traits hypothesized to affect performance for these types of increased time on the job. In addition, even small gains in indi-
changes (in particular, openness) predicted sales effectiveness in vidual performance across time as a function of these individual
our transitional sample. differences would be expected to translate into large economic
Another practical consideration stemming from our results con- gains in the aggregate within an entire sales force or organization.
cerns the practice of banding in a personnel selection context. Of course, a 1-year time frame simply may not have been long
Simply stated, banding is a procedure that treats predictor test enough for us to observe a decline in validity for traits similar to
scores within a range of potential scores (i.e., a score band) as those often observed for cognitive ability (Keil & Cortina, 2001).
equivalent, given a previously specified level of measurement Clearly, a number of questions about the relationships between
precision in the predictor (Cascio, Outtz, Zedeck, & Goldstein, personality traits and job performance trajectories remain to be
1991). Some researchers have advocated this method as a means of addressed by future researchers in other occupational settings and
reducing adverse impact against minority groups in a selection contexts. Although the field has witnessed important theoretical
context (e.g., Cascio et al., 1991), whereas others (e.g., Schmidt, and empirical advances concerning relationships between ability
1991) have questioned the logic underlying this method. Sophis- constructs and performance in a temporal context (e.g., Ackerman,
ticated models have been developed that take into account the 1988, 1992; Hulin et al., 1990; Keil & Cortina, 2001; Murphy,
effects of unreliability in predictor tests in the process of deter- 1989; Schmidt et al., 1988), similar theories focusing on volitional
mining score bands (e.g., Murphy, 1994; Murphy, Osten, & My- variables such as personality traits have been lacking. In addition
ors, 1995). However, researchers have largely overlooked the issue to replicating the current study in other field contexts with more
of change in criterion scores (i.e., dynamic criteria) in the context sophisticated research designs, experimental laboratory research
of the practice of banding (for an exception, see Aguinis, Cortina, could test more complex temporal aspects of personality–perfor-
& Goldberg, 1998). Incorporating performance change into band- mance relationships using a task-based skill-acquisition frame-
ing models may prove problematic. Schmidt and colleagues (in work. In fact, the evolution and evaluation of testable theories
Campion et al., 2001, pp. 163–164) presented a scenario in which involving personality–performance relationships in the context of
accounting for unreliability in job performance measures resulted temporal and career stage variables may represent the next impor-
in score bands including 97% of all predicted performance tant step for personality research in applied settings. We hope that
scores— hence negating the utility of banding in such a scenario. our study provides a useful starting point for additional research in
Clearly, more research is needed that incorporates a criterion- this tradition.
based perspective and acknowledges both group- and individual-
level changes in job performance. One possible solution involves References
treating temporal change in criterion scores as systematic (rather
than random measurement error) and setting bands around pre- Ackerman, P. L. (1988). Determinants of individual differences during
dicted performance scores that incorporate expected performance skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and information processing. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 288 –318.
growth. Of course, it should be noted that there is no clear-cut
Ackerman, P. L. (1992). Predicting individual differences in complex skill
evidence of sizable group differences on measures of broad (e.g., acquisition: Dynamics of ability determinants. Journal of Applied Psy-
the Big Five) personality traits (Hough, Oswald, & Ployhart, chology, 77, 598 – 614.
2001), which would suggest that adverse impact may not be a Aguinis, H., Cortina, J. M., & Goldberg, E. (1998). A new procedure for
significant problem for selection based on measures of these computing equivalence bands in personnel selection. Human Perfor-
constructs. mance, 11, 351–365.
Finally, our design provides some basis for confidence in the Ashton, M. C. (1998). Personality and job performance: The importance of
long-term validity—and consequently, the utility— of selection narrow traits. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 289 –303.
systems based on personality traits, a point that has been debated Austin, J. T., & Villanova, P. (1992). The criterion problem: 1917–1992.
in the literature (Henry & Hulin, 1987, 1989; Schmidt et al., 1988). Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 836 – 874.
Avolio, B. J., Waldman, D. A., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Age and work
The validity of personality traits for predicting sales performance
performance in nonmanagerial jobs: The effects of experience and
did not systematically decrease across the 1-year time interval for occupational type. Academy of Management Journal, 33, 407– 422.
our study in either of the two samples. Although research has Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimen-
supported the deterioration of the validity of cognitive ability sions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44,
across time (Keil & Cortina, 2001), our results indicate cause for 1–26.
cautious optimism with respect to this question for personality Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1996). Effects of impression management
PERSONALITY AND PERFORMANCE TRAJECTORIES 851

and self-deception on the predictive validity of personality constructs. linear modeling to examine dynamic performance criteria over time.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 261–272. Journal of Management, 23, 745–757.
Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Personality and Deadrick, D. L., & Madigan, R. M. (1990). Dynamic criteria revisited: A
performance at the beginning of the new millennium: What do we know longitudinal study of performance stability and predictive validity. Per-
and where do we go next? International Journal of Selection and sonnel Psychology, 43, 717–744.
Assessment, 9, 9 –30. Deshon, R. P., Ployhart, R. E., & Sacco, J. M. (1998). The estimation of
Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Strauss, J. P. (1993). Conscientiousness reliability in longitudinal models. International Journal of Behavior and
and performance of sales representatives: Test of the mediating effects Development, 22, 493–515.
of goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 715–722. Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor
Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Strauss, J. P. (1994). Antecedents of model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417– 440.
voluntary turnover due to a reduction in force. Personnel Psychology, Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of
47, 515–535. Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1246 –1256.
Barrick, M. R., Stewart, G. L., & Piotrowski, M. (2002). Personality and Dodd, W. E., Wollowick, H. B., & McNamara, W. J. (1970). Task
job performance: Test of the mediating effects of motivation among difficulty as a moderator of long-range prediction. Journal of Applied
sales representatives. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 43–51. Psychology, 54, 265–270.
Bass, B. M. (1962). Further evidence of the dynamic nature of criteria. Edwards, J. R. (1992). A cybernetic theory of stress, coping, and well-
Personnel Psychology, 15, 93–97. being in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 17, 238 –274.
Bing, M. N., & Lounsbury, J. W. (2000). Openness and job performance in Ghiselli, E. E. (1956). Dimensional problems of criteria. Journal of Applied
U. S.-based Japanese manufacturing companies. Journal of Business and Psychology, 40, 1– 4.
Psychology, 14, 515–522. Ghiselli, E. E., & Haire, M. (1960). The validation of selection tests in the
Bliese, P. D., & Ployhart, R. E. (2002). Growth modeling using random light of the dynamic nature of criteria. Personnel Psychology, 13, 225–
coefficient models: Model building, testing, and illustrations. Organiza- 231.
tional Research Methods, 5, 362–387. Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: The
Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models: Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 59, 1216 –1229.
Campion, M. A., Outtz, J. L., Zedeck, S., Schmidt, F. L., Kehoe, J. F., Hanges, P. J., Schneider, B., & Niles, K. (1990). Stability of performance:
Murphy, K. R., & Guion, R. M. (2001). The controversy over score An interactionist perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 658 –
banding in personnel selection: Answers to 10 key questions. Personnel 667.
Psychology, 54, 149 –185. Helmreich, R. L., Sawin, L. L., & Carsrud, A. L. (1986). The honeymoon
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive effect in job performance: Temporal increases in the predictive power of
and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, achievement motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 185–188.
19 –35. Henry, R. A., & Hulin, C. L. (1987). Stability of skilled performance across
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. time: Some generalizations and limitations on utilities. Journal of Ap-
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. plied Psychology, 72, 457– 462.
Cascio, W. F., Outtz, J., Zedeck, S., & Goldstein, I. L. (1991). Statistical Henry, R. A., & Hulin, C. L. (1989). Changing validities: Ability–perfor-
implications of six methods of test score use in personnel selection. mance relations and utilities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 365–
Human Performance, 4, 233–264. 367.
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design Hesketh, B., & Neal, A. (1999). Technology and performance. In D. R.
and analysis issues for field settings. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Ilgen & E. D. Pulakos (Eds.), The changing nature of performance:
Corr, P. J., & Gray, J. A. (1995). Attributional style, socialization and Implications for staffing, motivation, and development (pp. 21–55). San
cognitive ability as predictors of sales success: A predictive validity Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
study. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 241–252. Hofmann, D. A. (1997). An overview of the logic and rationale of hierar-
Costa, P. T., Jr. (1996). Work and personality: Use of the NEO-PI-R in chical linear models. Journal of Management, 23, 723–744.
industrial/organizational psychology. Applied Psychology: An Interna- Hofmann, D. A., Jacobs, R., & Baratta, J. E. (1993). Dynamic criteria and
tional Review, 45, 225–241. the measurement of change. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 194 –
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality 204.
Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) pro- Hofmann, D. A., Jacobs, R., & Gerras, S. J. (1992). Mapping individual
fessional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. performance over time. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 185–195.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1995). Domains and facets: Hierarchical Hogan, J., & Holland, B. (2003). Using theory to evaluate personality and
personality assessment using the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. job-performance relations: A socioanalytic perspective. Journal of Ap-
Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 21–50. plied Psychology, 88, 100 –112.
Crant, J. M. (1995). The Proactive Personality Scale and objective job Hogan, J., & Ones, D. S. (1997). Conscientiousness and integrity at work.
performance among real estate agents. Journal of Applied Psychology, In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality
80, 532–537. psychology (pp. 849 – 870). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Cravens, D. W., & Woodruff, R. B. (1973). An approach for determining Hogan, J., & Roberts, B. W. (1996). Issues and non-issues in the fidelity–
criteria of sales performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 57, 242– bandwidth trade-off. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 17, 627– 637.
247. Hough, L. M., Oswald, F. L., & Ployhart, R. E. (2001). Determinants,
Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure “change”: Or detection and amelioration of adverse impact in personnel selection
should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74, 68 – 80. procedures: Issues, evidence and lessons learned. International Journal
Davison, M. L., Kwak, N., Seo, Y. S., & Choi, J. (2002). Using hierarchical of Selection and Assessment, 9, 152–194.
linear models to examine moderator effects: Person-by-organization Howard, A. (1995). The changing nature of work. San Francisco: Jossey-
interactions. Organizational Research Methods, 5, 231–254. Bass.
Deadrick, D. L., Bennett, N., & Russell, C. J. (1997). Using hierarchical Hulin, C. L., Henry, R. A., & Noon, S. L. (1990). Adding a dimension:
852 THORESEN, BRADLEY, BLIESE, AND THORESEN

Time as a factor in the generalizability and predictive relationships. agement. Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management,
Psychological Bulletin, 107, 328 –340. 13, 153–200.
Hunter, J. E., & Hunter, R. F. (1984). Validity and utility of alternative Mount, M. K., Barrick, M. R., & Stewart, G. L. (1998). Five-factor model
predictors of job performance. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 72–98. of personality and performance in jobs involving interpersonal interac-
Hurtz, G. M., & Donovan, J. J. (2000). Personality and job performance: tions. Human Performance, 11, 145–165.
The Big Five revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 869 – 879. Muckler, F. A. (1992). Selecting performance measures: “Objective” ver-
Jacobs, R., Hofmann, D. A., & Kriska, S. D. (1990). Performance and sus “subjective” measurement. Human Factors, 34, 441– 445.
seniority. Human Performance, 3, 107–121. Murphy, K. R. (1989). Is the relationship between cognitive ability and job
Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluation performance stable over time? Human Performance, 2, 183–200.
traits—self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emo- Murphy, K. R. (1994). Potential effects of banding as a function of test
tional stability—with job satisfaction and job performance: A meta- reliability. Personnel Psychology, 47, 477– 495.
analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 80 –92. Murphy, K. R., Osten, K., & Myors, B. (1995). Modeling the effects of
Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J. E., & Thoresen, C. J. (2002). Are measures banding in personnel selection. Personnel Psychology, 48, 61– 83.
of self-esteem, neuroticism, locus of control, and generalized self- Ones, D. S., & Viswesvaran, C. (1996). Bandwidth-fidelity dilemma in
efficacy indicators of a common core construct? Journal of Personality personality measurement for personnel selection. Journal of Organiza-
and Social Psychology, 83, 693–710. tional Behavior, 17, 609 – 626.
Judge, T. A., Martocchio, J. J., & Thoresen, C. J. (1997). Five-factor model Ones, D. S., & Viswesvaran, C. (1997). Personality determinants in the
of personality and employee absence. Journal of Applied Psychology, prediction of aspects of expatriate job success. In A. Zeynep (Ed.), New
82, 745–755. approaches to employee management: Vol. 4. Expatriate management:
Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Pucik, V., & Welbourne, T. M. (1999). Theory and research (pp. 63–92). Stamford, CT : JAI Press.
Managerial coping with organizational change: A dispositional perspec- Ones, D. S., & Viswesvaran, C. (1999). Relative importance of personality
tive. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 107–122. dimensions for expatriate selection: A policy capturing study. Human
Keil, C. T., & Cortina, J. M. (2001). Degradation of validity over time: A Performance, 12, 275–294.
test and extension of Ackerman’s model. Psychological Bulletin, 127, Patterson, F. (2001). Developments in work psychology: Emerging issues
and future trends. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol-
673– 697.
ogy, 74, 381–390.
Lance, C. E., Vandenberg, R. J., & Self, R. M. (2000). Latent growth
Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and
models of individual change: The case of newcomer adjustment. Orga-
S-PLUS. New York: Springer-Verlag.
nizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 83, 107–140.
Ployhart, R. E., & Hakel, M. D. (1998). The substantive nature of perfor-
LePine, J. A., Colquitt, J. A., & Erez, A. (2000). Adaptability to changing
mance variability: Predicting interindividual differences in intraindi-
task contexts: Effects of general cognitive ability, conscientiousness, and
vidual performance. Personnel Psychology, 51, 859 –901.
openness to experience. Personnel Psychology, 53, 563–593.
Pulakos, E. D., Arad, S., Donovan, M. A., & Plamondon, K. E. (2000).
Lucas, H. C., Jr., Weinberg, C. B., & Clowes, K. W. (1975). Sales response
Adaptability in the workplace: Development of a taxonomy of adaptive
as a function of territorial potential and sales representative workload.
performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 612– 624.
Journal of Marketing Research, 12, 298 –305.
Quiñones, M. A., Ford, J. K., & Teachout, M. S. (1995). The relationship
Macan, T. H. (1994). Time management: Test of a process model. Journal
between work experience and job performance. Personnel Psychology,
of Applied Psychology, 79, 381–391.
48, 887–910.
Mattell, M. S., & Jacoby, J. (1971). Is there an optimal number of
Raudenbush, S. W. (2001). Comparing personal trajectories and drawing
alternatives for Likert scale items? Educational and Psychological Mea-
causal inferences from longitudinal data. Annual Review of Psychology,
surement, 31, 657– 674. 52, 501–525.
McDaniel, M. A., Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1988a). Job experience Raudenbush, S. W., & Xiao-Feng, L. (2001). Effects of study duration,
correlates of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 327– frequency of observations, and sample size on power in studies of group
330. differences in polynomial change. Psychological Methods, 6, 387– 401.
McDaniel, M. A., Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1988b). A meta-analysis Russell, C. J. (2001). A longitudinal study of top-level executive perfor-
of the validity of methods for rating training and experience in personnel mance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 560 –573.
selection. Personnel Psychology, 41, 283–314. Ryans, A. B., & Weinberg, C. B. (1979). Territory sales response. Journal
McManus, M. A., & Brown, S. H. (1995). Adjusting sales results measures of Marketing Research, 16, 453– 465.
for use as criteria. Personnel Psychology, 48, 391– 400. Sackett, P. R., & DeVore, C. J. (2001). Counterproductive behaviors at
Mischel, W. (1977). The interaction of person and situation. In D. Mag- work. In N. Anderson, D. S. Ones, H. K. Sinangil, & C. Viswesvaran
nusson & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Personality and the crossroads: Current (Eds.), Handbook of industrial, work, & organizational psychology
issues of interactional psychology (pp. 333–352). Hillsdale, NJ: (Vol. 1, pp. 145–164). London: Sage.
Erlbaum. Salgado, J. F. (1997). The five factor model of personality and job perfor-
Mitchell, T. R., & James, L. R. (2001). Building better theory: Time and mance in the European Community. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82,
the specification of when things happen. Academy of Management 30 – 43.
Review, 26, 530 –547. Salgado, J. F. (2002). The Big Five personality dimensions and counter-
Moon, H. (2001). The two faces of conscientiousness: Duty and achieve- productive behaviors. International Journal of Selection and Assess-
ment striving in escalation of commitment dilemmas. Journal of Applied ment, 10, 117–125.
Psychology, 86, 535–540. Schmidt, F. L. (1991). Why all banding procedures are logically flawed.
Moon, H., Hollenbeck, J. R., Humphrey, S. E., & Maue, B. (2003). The Human Performance, 4, 265–277.
tripartite model of neuroticism and the suppression of depression and Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection
anxiety within an escalation of commitment dilemma. Journal of Per- methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications
sonality, 71, 347–368. from 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262–
Mount, M. K., & Barrick, M. R. (1995). The Big Five personality dimen- 274.
sions: Implications for research and practice in human resources man- Schmidt, F. L., Hunter, J. E., & Outerbridge, A. N. (1986). Impact of job
PERSONALITY AND PERFORMANCE TRAJECTORIES 853

experience and ability on job knowledge, work sample performance, and Swan, J. E., Bowers, M. R., & Richardson, L. D. (1998). Customer trust in
supervisory ratings of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, the salesperson: An integrative review and meta-analysis of the empir-
71, 432– 439. ical literature. Journal of Business Research, 44, 93–107.
Schmidt, F. L., Hunter, J. E., Outerbridge, A. N., & Goff, S. (1988). Joint Tesluk, P. E., & Jacobs, R. R. (1998). Toward an integrated model of work
relation of experience and ability with job performance: Test of three experience. Personnel Psychology, 51, 321–355.
hypotheses. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 46 –57. Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., & Rothstein, M. (1991). Personality measures
Schneider, R. J., Hough, L. M., & Dunnette, M. D. (1996). Broadsided by as predictors of job performance: A meta-analytic review. Personnel
broad traits: How to sink science in five dimensions or less. Journal of Psychology, 44, 703–742.
Organizational Behavior, 17, 639 – 655. Tupes, E. C., & Christal, R. E. (1992). Recurrent personality factors based
Schneider, V., Chung, B., & Yusko, K. P. (1993). Service climate for on trait ratings. Journal of Personality, 60, 225–251.
service quality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 197– Vinchur, A. J., Schippmann, J. S., Switzer, F. S., III, & Roth, P. L. (1998).
200. A meta-analytic review of predictors of job performance for salespeople.
Snijders, T. A. B., & Bosker, R. J. (1994). Modeled variance in two-level Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 586 –597.
models. Sociological Methods and Research, 22, 342–363. Watson, D., & Hubbard, B. (1996). Adaptational style and dispositional
Snijders, T. A. B., & Bosker, R. J. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An intro- structure: Coping in the context of the five-factor model. Journal of
duction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage. Personality, 64, 737–774.
Spector, P. E., Zapf, D., Chen, P. Y., & Frese, M. (2000). Why negative Willett, J. B., & Sayer, A. G. (1994). Using covariance structure analysis
affectivity should not be controlled in job stress research: Don’t throw to detect correlates and predictors of individual change over time.
out the baby with the bath water. Journal of Organizational Behavior, Psychological Bulletin, 116, 363–381.
21, 79 –95. Witt, L. A., Burke, L. A., Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (2002). The
Steele-Johnson, D., Osburn, H. G., & Pieper, K. F. (2000). A review and interactive effects of conscientiousness and agreeableness on job per-
formance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 164 –169.
extension of current models of dynamic criteria. International Model of
Selection and Assessment, 8, 110 –136.
Stewart, G. L. (1999). Trait bandwidth and stages of job performance: Received August 9, 2002
Assessing differential effects for conscientiousness and its subtraits. Revision received November 10, 2003
Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 959 –968. Accepted November 19, 2003 䡲

You might also like