You are on page 1of 25

From the Neuron to the World and Back: The Poetics of the Neuromolecular Gaze in Bart Koubaas Het

gebied van Nevski and James Camerons Avatar1


Stephan Besser, University of Amsterdam

Abstract This article explores the poetics of the neuromolecular gaze in Bart Koubaas 2007 novel Het gebied van Nevski and James Camerons 2009 film Avatar. It considers the novel and the film as artistic reflections of contemporary neuroculture and argues that they both employ their central metaphors the brain as battlefield and the brain as network in such a way that the same basic principles seem to return on various levels of biological and social existence: the micro-level of nerve cells and synapses, the meso-level of individual brains and human beings and the macro-level of historical events and socio-political interaction. In doing so, the novel and the film both suggest a natural continuity between the neurobiological and the social that closely resembles manifestations of the neuromolecular gaze (Rose/Abi-Rached) in contemporary science and neuroculture at large. Significantly, however, these poetic extrapolations of neuromolecular knowledge to personal identity and society do not lead to a homogeneous and reductionist view of the cerebral subject (F. Vidal) but to quite diverse, ambiguous and contradictory interpretations of what it means to be ones brain. Keywords: Brain, Neuroculture, Darwinism, Network, Metaphor, Cerebral Subject, Poetics of Knowledge, Literature and Science

Humanoid Neurons
One of the key passages of Bart Koubaas 2007 novel Het gebied van Nevski depicts a microscopic battle scene. The theatre of war is situated within the brain of the

43

stephan besser

narrator of the text, a Flemish writer living in the city of Ghent, and observed via an advanced neuroimaging technique that his doctors use to study a newly discovered area in his cerebrum. Zooming in on this so-called Nevskys area, the neurologists witness a violent battle of nerve cells (neurons) that seem to encode competing thoughts and memories in the writers brain. They see how strong nerve cells start to swing and cluster together in a very specific rhythm, thus forming new groups and alliances while other neurons are cast out. When combat is over, the victorious majority of cells makes a treaty with the strongest memory that is then reported to other areas of the brain. Neurons killed in action are buried and collaborators, spies and informants immediately executed.2 This anthropomorphic description establishes the fictitious Nevskys area as an important hot spot in the writers creative brain and metaphorically links the events in its neural structures to a historical conflict and its film adaptation by Sergei Eisenstein that plays a crucial role in the novel, namely the famous Battle on the Ice between the army of Alexander Nevsky, the prince of Novgorod, and the Teutonic Knights on Lake Peipus in 1242 (Fig. 1). A quite different but yet curiously similar transfiguration of humanoids and nerve cells occurs in another product of contemporary neuroculture, James Camerons science fiction film Avatar from 2009. In two scenes towards the end of the film we see large groups of Navi, the tall and blue-skinned humanoid inhabitants of Pandora, gather around their sacred Tree of Souls in an attempt to transfer the minds of two human beings to Navi bodies. The participants in the ceremony sit down, connect their long braids to a network of fibres on the ground of the forest and start to move their upper bodies synchronously to the rhythm of a spiritual chant (Fig. 2). When the camera pans out and shows the gathering in its entirety, the network of interconnected Navi resembles a giant cerebral cortex.3 The individual Navi appear as single nerve cells humanoid neurons within a large neural tissue whose nervous and spiritual energy is expected to save the earthlings. Hence, just like Koubaas novel, the film stages a figurative conflation of neurons and neuronal groups with human(oid) beings and forms of social interaction. In this article, I will read Het gebied van Nevskis and Avatars representational strategies in the context of contemporary neuroculture, i.e. the interdiscursive fascination with the brain as the source and biological substrate of human conduct and identity. More specifically, I will analyze the novel and the film as artistic modifications of and responses to a scientific style of thought that sociologists of medicine Joelle M. Abi-Rached and Nikolas Rose have termed the neuromolecular gaze. This style of thought is based on the assumption that any understanding of the workings of the brain and mental processes must start at the molecular level of nerve cells and then scale up4 to the higher levels of neural networks, cerebral regions and the brain as a whole. I will argue that by creating systems of analogy that visually and linguistically link the micro-level of

44

from the neuron to the world and back

neurons and synapses to the meso-level of individual brains and the macro-level of social interaction, Avatar and Het gebied van Nevski perform their own versions of the neuromolecular gaze. They thereby employ what I suggest to call a poetics of the neuromolecular gaze, i.e. representational and associative strategies that suggest a natural continuity between the cellular, the cerebral and the social. In this way they participate in what has been called the seductive allure5 that contemporary neuroscience exerts on the popular imagination by suggestive and reductionist forms of reasoning that claim direct relations between social processes, mental states and their alleged neural correlates. These strategies certainly call for discursive analysis and ideological critique; they do not, however, necessarily lead to a homogenous and hegemonic neuro world-view, as some critics of the present neuro-hype fear. Avatar and Het gebied van Nevski are cases in point: because they use different tropes to scale up from the neuronal to the social the Darwinian trope of the brain as a battlefield and the connectionist trope of the brain as a network they also project quite different visions of what a neuromolecular view of personal identity and social relations could mean.

Figure 1 Sergei Eisenstein, Alexander Nevsky (1938)

45

stephan besser

Figure 2 Gathering at the Tree of Souls (James Cameron, Avatar)

The Neuromolecular Gaze and the Cultural Dissemination of the Brain


The concept of the neuromolecular gaze has been introduced by Joelle M. AbiRached and Nikolas Rose in order to describe an epistemological shift that led to the emergence of the modern neurosciences in the 1960s and still is dominant today. Inspired by Michel Foucaults notion of the clinical gaze and Ludwik Flecks concept of the style of thought, the neuromolecular gaze comprises, but is not limited to, visual technologies and forms of representation. Like Foucaults clinical gaze, the neuromolecular gaze is a dissecting and anatomizing gaze, one that literally and figuratively penetrates the body/brain in order to gain knowledge of the processes inside, for instance by the use of neuroimaging techniques. But in Rose and Abi-Racheds definition the term gaze also more broadly refers to a new discourse in the sciences of the brain and the nervous system6 that emerged in the second half of the twentieth century, i.e. a specific research perspective and vision of the brain7 in general. Therefore, I suggest to regard the tropes, metaphors and figures of thought that support this vision and facilitate its cultural circulation as an integral part of the neuromolecular gaze as well.

46

from the neuron to the world and back

Hence, I propose to combine Rose and Abi-Racheds epistemological concept with insights from the current research into the poetic dimension of neuroscientific knowledge and its cultural dissemination.8 In addition, I will argue that the general disposition to invest the neuromolecular level of nerve cells, neurotransmitters and synapses with specific meaning and relevance when it comes to the explanation and imagination of higher order phenomena such as memory and subjectivity is not limited to scientific discourses. Instead, in this broader definition the general willingness to relate all kinds of social, cultural and biological phenomena to a neuromolecular ground level can be seen as an important structuring element of contemporary neuroculture at large. According to Rose and Abi-Rached, the neuromolecular gaze is both inherently interdisciplinary and profoundly reductionist.9 Its interdisciplinarity is the result of a growing awareness of scientists since the 1950s that their efforts in the fields of molecular biology, neuroanatomy, neurology, chemistry, behavioural psychology and other psy-disciplines could only lead to a comprehensive understanding of the human mind and brain if they were brought together in a crossdisciplinary research agenda.10 This also entailed a new conception of the object of research itself: The brain was now conceived of as a complex biological system that needed to be dissected and studied from different levels of analysis cellular, morphological, developmental, behavioral, physiological etc. and in an integrated manner what was termed a synthetic approach.11 This new integrative approach was institutionalized in research programs and organizations such as the International Brain Research Foundation (established in 1961) and the Neurosciences Research Program at MIT (1962), whose founder Francis O. Schmitt coined the term neuroscience in the same year.12 The general orientation and rhetoric of this approach can still be found in the self-descriptions of research institutions today. The website of the research priority area Brain and Cognition at the University of Amsterdam, for instance, describes the programs activities as covering the entire spectrum from nerve cell to social behavior.13 According to Rose and Abi-Rached, this cross-disciplinary approach is nevertheless reductionist because it privileges the molecular level as the ultimate anchorage point to which all the higher order phenomena of consciousness, cognition and behaviour must be retraced in order to produce valid scientific explanations.14 Entities such as the human organism and indeed the human subject are seen as dissectible, reducible to traits, behaviours, cells, genes, brain processes (like vision or consciousness), to atomic elements: neuromolecular parts that could be dissected out and studied separately from the whole.15 This means that even fields as psychology, psychiatry and the study of human memory were increasingly dominated by a reductionist molecular method,16 at least until the growing acceptance for the notion of neuroplasticity in the 2000s started to act as something of a counterweight to such reductionism.17 As an example of the reductionist approach, Abi-Rached and Rose point

47

stephan besser

to the work of Nobel laureate Eric Kandel on the neurobiology of memory and his maxim that the brain should be studied one cell at a time.18 While avoiding any alarmist rhetoric about an alleged colonization of society by the neurosciences, Abi-Rached and Rose warn against the premature and unwise extrapolation to society of the modest and always uncertain and correctible knowledge of the laboratory sciences.19 In their view, the translational imperative under which neuroscientists operate today i.e. the pressure on and willingness of researchers to quickly translate their laboratory findings into real-world situations can lead to reckless extrapolations.20 Animal experiments, for instance, are presented as providing insights into psychiatric disorders in humans,21 plasticity research is invoked as neurological legitimation of a neoliberal economic order22 and neurochemical findings are translated into pharmacological solutions to psychological problems. While pointing to these dangers, Rose and Abi-Rached nevertheless take a nuanced position in the current debate about the social and political consequences of the neuro-boom. Not surprisingly, the tentacular growth23 of the neurocultural universe has spawned various forms of criticism. As philosopher Jan Slaby pointed out, it is probably harder these days to give examples of cultural, social and academic fields that are not affected by the so-called neuro revolution24 than to name fields in which claims about the relevance or even revolutionary impact of neuroscientific findings abound; newly emerging disciplines such as neuromarketing or neurophilosophy are cases in point.25 Slaby and others have therefore called for the critical contextualization and analysis of the truth claims of contemporary neuroscience and their ideological implications.26 In a related vein, psychologists Paolo Legrenzi and Carlo Umilt have attacked the biologism of contemporary neuromania and its reductionist approach to the mind-brain relationship.27 In the Netherlands, the enormous success of popular neuroscience books such as Dick Swaabs bestselling Wij zijn ons brein and Victor Lammes De vrije wil bestaat niet has been countered by psychologist Jan Derksens appeal to free psychology28 from the imperialist encroachment of the brain myth and Douwe Draaismas rejection of Swaabs hubristic transgression of disciplinary boundaries.29 Historian of science Fernando Vidal, one of the most outspoken critics of a cultural hegemony of the neuro, has likewise criticized the neurosciences tendency to boast their power to radically alter the view of the human.30 According to Vidal, contemporary neurocultural discourses propagate a notion of the self that is dominated by the ideology of brainhood, i.e. the idee of being, rather than simply having, a brain and thus the reducibility of the self to one organ. Vidal terms this conception of selfhood the cerebral subject and argues that it emphasizes interiority, autonomous choice and individualism at the expense of social bonds and contexts.31 Similar and more specific criticisms of the cerebralization of personhood have been voiced with regard to the ontological impact of neuropsychiatric disease entities and classifications on

48

from the neuron to the world and back

the patients sense of self,32 the links of neurochemical conceptions of personhood with contemporary biopolitics and the interests of the pharmaceutical industry,33 the mobilization of the plastic brain in neoliberal discourses of flexibility, employability and self-management34 and the neurosexist essentialization of gender identities.35 Although Rose and Abi-Rached share some of these concerns, they do not believe that personhood has or will simply become brainhood. In their view, the cerebral subject is just one of many forms of self-fashioning that are presently employed and mixed in different contexts and departments of life.36 Moreover, they contend that neurobiological conceptions of personhood themselves are more diverse than the critics seem to acknowledge and do not portray humans as mere puppets of their brains or neuronal man as isolated, individualized and asocial.37 Picking up on this argument, I will argue that Avatar and Het gebied van Nevski precisely point to this inherent diversity of a neuro world view and even amplify it by their use of seductive analogies. On the one hand, the film and the novel show that the neuromolecular gaze is not merely a scientific style of thought but in its broader definition as a disposition to relate the molecular to the human and the social forms an important element of contemporary neuroculture at large. On the other hand, the specific calibrations of the neuromolecular gaze in Avatar and Het gebied van Nevski produce quite different notions of what it means to be ones brain. Hence, by devising their own integrated multilevel approaches38 to the brain and human existence, the novel and the film illuminate the frictions and ambiguities within this gaze as well.

Het gebied van Nevski: The Brain as Battlefield


Bart Koubaas Het gebied van Nevski tells the story of an arduous recovery after a severe case of brain damage. The novel begins with the description of a near-fatal accident during a visit that the narrator, an anonymous Flemish writer, pays to Lake Peipus, the site of the historical battle between Alexander Nevsky and his army against the Teutonic Knights in 1242. A fervid admirer of the historical Nevsky as well as Sergei Eisensteins 1938 drama film Alexander Nevksy, the writer adventurously strolls around the frozen surface of the lake. He sinks through the ice and sustains a specific form of expressive aphasia that manifests itself in difficulties to produce grammatically correct and coherent sentences. His neurologist professor Van Nieuwenhuyze advises the narrator to keep a diary to order his thinking and rehearse his linguistic skills. This diary or brain research,39 as the narrator terms it, is basically identical to the text of the novel itself and is characterized by a highly associative and aphasic style of writing full of neologisms, wordplay and logical short circuits. As the narrator himself puts it: I write fluently, but the sentences are not always meaningful for someone with a healthy brain.40

49

stephan besser

Within the expanding network of contemporary Dutch neuroculture Koubaas novel forms a particularly thick knot of intertextual references. They include numerous allusions to the history of brain research and neurological localization theories41 as well as references to more recent developments such as discussions about mirror neurons and the notion of the empathic brain,42 new neuroimaging techniques, studies of brain plasticity in songbirds43 and philosopher Daniel Dennetts metaphor of the Cartesian theater.44 By orchestrating and narrativizing these references, the novel explores the implications of a neuromaterialistic notion of identity. It seems to arrive at a conclusion that pre-formulates the catch-phrase in the title of Dick Swaabs bestseller when the writer opens one of his last entries with the remark: Ik ben mijn hersenen.45 This insight and its consequences, however, are not presented as an ultimate truth of the cerebral subject. Instead, the novel constantly oscillates between a serious, knowledgeable engagement with neuroscientific theories and the parodic exaggeration and playful subversion of their philosophical implications. The most important intertextual reference in Het gebied van Nevski in this regard concerns a neuroscientific theory that is known as neural Darwinism or theory of neuronal group selection. One of its best-known representatives, American neurophysiologist William H. Calvin, is repeatedly quoted in the novel with the notion that the brain encodes memories in spatial patterns of nerve cells that are frozen in time, a sheet of music waiting for a pianist.46 This conception forms an important element of Calvins theory of Darwinian thought,47 according to which the natural selection of neuronal groups in the brain represents the biological substrate of processes of remembering and forgetting. This hypothesis in turn is heavily and explicitly indebted to the theory of neural Darwinism that biologist and Nobel laureate Gerard Edelman proposed in his 1987 study Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection. In this book and its popular scientific counterpart Bright Air, Brilliant Fire (1992), Edelman controversially applies the principles of Darwinist population thinking to the workings of the brain. He argues that we can only truly understand memory and cognition if we conceive of the brain not as a static storage device or computer but as dynamically organized into cellular populations [] the structure and function of which are selected by different means during development and behavior.48 According to Edelman, these groups of strongly connected neurons constantly compete for electrochemical stimulation. This competition results in a selective strengthening and weakening of synaptic connections and neuronal groups through processes of cell division and cell death during growth and development of an organism (developmental selection), through behaviour and learning during an individuals life (experiental selection) and the massively parallel second-order signaling between neuronal groups (reentry).49 It is this particular piece of neuromolecular knowledge that serves to anchor and orient the neuromolecular gaze in Het gebied van Nevski. It provides material

50

from the neuron to the world and back

evidence for the novels central trope of the brain as battlefield which in turn forms the framework for the playful superimposition of various layers of Darwinist imagery, ranging from the submicroscopic realm of cells and synapses over the medium level of individual brains up to the macro-scale of historical events and social interaction. On the cellular level, Koubaa employs the trope of the battle by comparing individual neurons with tiny soldiers in the writers head and stating that on average one hundred thousand [of them] fall in battle daily.50 This analogy of cells and soldiers recalls alchemist theories of the homunculus and recasts them in a neural Darwinist fashion. In accordance with the theory of neuronal group selection, the formation of thoughts and memories in the writers brain is described as an evolutionary process and struggle for life in which dreams, memories and ideas pitch into each other like samurais.51 This evolutionary principle also seems to be at work when professor Van Nieuwenhuyze struggles to recall the title of Eisensteins Alexander Nevksy because the thought of another historical Alexander Alexander the Great overgrows and suffocates52 the sought-after piece of information in his brain. Since the narrator believes that he literally is his brain, the notion of the battle also directly concerns his conception of identity and sense of self. To him, this self appears as an ever changing pattern of thoughts and memories and thus synaptic connections that constantly compete, copy and transform themselves.53 In addition, the notion of battle is also applied to the text of the diary itself everything you read has fought in my brain for the space on these pages54 and to literary works in general that appear as the result of a Darwinian evolutionary process55 of adaptation to specific cultural environments and product of sexual selection: Language is the tail that the writer shows off.56 Importantly, this metonymic shift from biological matter nerve cells and neuronal groups to text and literature is facilitated by another trope that carries strong associations with evolutionary theory, namely the metaphor of the four letters of the alphabet of the genetic code.57 Once again musing about the bio-ontological status of his own writing, the narrator reasons: If I indeed am nothing else than the four letters A, C, G, and T, the combination of which makes up the machine language of my construction, then it must be possible to construct all my memories [] from them as well; then my fingers [on the computer keyboard, SB] must be able to play the song of our existence.58 Finally, Koubaa also deploys the trope of the battle on the supra-individual level of historical events and human sociality. As mentioned before, the emphasis here lies on the world-historical battle between the Teutonic Knights and Alexander Nevskys motley army of peasants, fishermen and crafts people59 (who in turn are likened to the words on the pages of his diary with which the narrator fights his aphasia). In addition, there is a sustained comparison of the street map of the city of Ghent with the topography of the human brain in which the search

51

stephan besser

for the stolen panel The Just Judges of the famous Ghent altarpiece by Jan and Hubert Van Eyck merges with the scientific quest for the location of morality in the human brain.60 The social Darwinist and racist implications of the novels central trope also briefly come to the fore when the narrator ruminates that the nationalistic motto Eigen Volk Eerst (Put your own people first) is not actually a political slogan but the expression of an age-old human characteristic in dealing with strangers.61 It is essential to point out that in the text of the novel itself the various instantiations of the battle trope are not as neatly separated as my rather schematic synopsis suggests; instead, they constantly overlap, intersect and resonate with each other. Still, the cellular or neuromolecular level provides the most important point of reference for this complex arrangement of analogies that resembles a dizzying hall of mirrors in which battles of various kinds and on various levels of existence reflect, deform and thwart each other. Abounding with logical fallacies, false syllogisms and fantastic associations, the text employs and exposes the reductionism inherent in the notion that all these battles express the same basic biological principle. It is precisely because of this double movement that the poetics of the neuromolecular gaze in Koubaas novel can shed an interesting light on popular and neuroscientific attempts to integrate the various levels of biological and social existence into a coherent Darwinist view of human existence.62 In contemporary neuroscience itself, for instance, it is still hotly debated whether Edelmans neuronal groups can actually be regarded as clearly definable units of selection that are able to replicate themselves, as a properly Darwinian process of evolution would imply. Because of such open questions, molecular biologist and Nobel Prize laureate Francis Crick has famously suggested that Edelmans idiosyncratic interpretation of evolutionary theory should be called neural Edelmanism instead of neural Darwinism.63 (Interestingly, Edelman himself has occasionally described neural Darwinism as a metaphor,64 suggesting that these processes are similar rather than the same.) This discussion also directly relates to another problem of neural Darwinism that is addressed in Koubaas novel, namely the relation of genes and nerve cells in theories of the brain. Significantly, Edelman himself opposes any genetic determinism and rejects the notion that the circuitry of neuronal groups could be strictly programmed by a molecular code.65 Instead, Edelmans focus on the cellular level implies an emphasis on epigenetic processes of selection and reentrant signaling, resulting in a dynamic conception of neural connectivity that is in many regards akin to more recent theories of neuroplasticity. Therefore, his view of the brain and human subjectivity is remarkably different from other, equally Darwinist approaches that instead focus on the genetic basis of the wiring of neural networks.

52

from the neuron to the world and back

An interesting case in point is Dick Swaabs aforementioned popular neuroscience bestseller Wij zijn ons brein. On the one hand, Swaabs book is decidedly and even provocatively Darwinist in many regards: Several of the chapters are preceded by mottos taken from the writings of Charles Darwin himself, there is a substantial chapter on the evolutionary history of the human brain in which Swaab explains the genetic determination of human behaviour and cultural practices66 and a consistent emphasis on the importance of genetics and the prenatal wiring of the brain in the uterus: We are born with brains that have been made unique by a combination of our genetic background and the programming during our development in the womb. These influences determine to a significant extent our personal character traits, talents and limitations.67 Correspondingly, Swaab depicts the influence of social and cultural environments on an individuals brain after birth as almost negligible; the only changes in adult brains that he discusses concern degenerative disorders such as Alzheimers and Parkinsons disease. Importantly, it is exactly this inclination towards genetic determinism that makes Swaabs book not an example of neural Darwinism, even though its author occasionally and not quite convincingly refers to Edelmans notion in support of his own approach.68 While Swaab sees brain plasticity as limited to the early developmental stages, Edelman insists that the competition and selection of neural groups do not stop in adults and regards the cellular dynamics of neural maps as a continuing process throughout the life of an individual organism.69 For the same reason Edelman unambiguously rejects all computer metaphors of the brain because they suggest a hardwired nervous system whereas Swaab explicitly embraces them.70 In contemporary neuroculture this distinction between gene-centred and neuron-centred approaches translates into rather different conceptions of neuroidentity: deterministic and neurocalvinist views such as the one popularized by Swaab on the one hand and flexible and plastic conceptions of the synaptic self71 on the other. By combining both options in a way that resembles the confusing poly-perspectivism of a cubist painting, Koubaas novel points to the diversity of possible extrapolations (Abi-Rached and Rose) of neuromolecular knowledge in present day neuroculture.

Avatar: The brain as network


This picture becomes even more complex if one takes into consideration that the metaphor of the battle is just one of many possibilities to construct a meaningful continuity between neurons, humans and society at large. In James Camerons Avatar this function is fulfilled by a trope that is at least equally important in contemporary neuroculture, namely the notion of the network. Accordingly, the calibration of the neuroscientific gaze in Camerons film significantly di-

53

stephan besser

verges from Koubaas novel and produces quite different meaning effects, interdiscursive links and extrapolations of neuroscientific knowledge to human subjectivity. In addition, the specific cinematic mediality of the film, enhanced by stereoscopic 3D technology, facilitates the construction of visual analogies in a way that cannot be achieved in literary texts. Avatar is set in the mid-twenty-second century on Pandora, a habitable moon of the gas giant Polyphemus in the Alpha Centauri star system. After the depletion of Earths natural resources, the Resources Development Agency (RDA) mines on Pandora for a valuable mineral called unobtanium. The film tells the story of the increasingly violent conflict between the Navi, the native inhabitants of Pandora, and the armed forces that protect the mining activities of the RDA. It is the task of a research team lead by xenobotanist Dr Grace Augustine to further explore Pandoras biosphere and persuade the Navi to leave their territory so that even larger areas of Pandora can be exploited by the RDA. Augustine and her staff, however, become increasingly critical of the destruction of the settlement areas of the Navi and their culture and eventually join their resistance against the RDA security forces. After an epic battle in which Eywa, the mother goddess of the Navi, intervenes by sending giant dinosaur-like creatures to the rescue of her children, the balance of life is restored and the earthlings are expelled from Pandora. Significantly, the escalation of this increasingly violent conflict is contrasted with an entirely different escalation in the literal sense of an increase and development of successive stages of the network trope in the film, starting on the neurocellular level and leading to the eventual revelation halfway through the film that the biosphere of Pandora in its entirety is a actually giant super-brain. This second escalation begins with what can be regarded as the establishing shots of the neuromolecular gaze in Avatar during an extensive laboratory scene at the beginning of the film. We see how the paraplegic ex-marine Jake Sully, a new member of Dr Augustines research team, is prepared for the first connection with his avatar, a genetically engineered and neurologically operated replica of the body of a Navi. The visual aesthetics of this scene are dominated by hightech laboratory equipment, transparent computer screens and the imagery of brain scans flashing up in green, yellow and white. While the scientists attempt to synchronize Jakes neural activity with that of the avatar Their nervous systems must be in tune one of the neuroimaging devices zooms in on the neural pathways and the microstructure of the synaptic connections in Sullys brain (Fig. 3). When Jakes mind is actually connected to its new host, the camera eye itself seems to be transported through a tunnel-like neural pathway into the avatars head.72 With its neuronal imagery thus firmly anchored on the cellular level, the film successively adds new layers and dimensions of network images that, as film scholar Patricia Pisters has pointed out, at every level reproduce our neural

54

from the neuron to the world and back

Figure 3 Scanning the brain, seeing networks (James Cameron, Avatar) system and stretch out in all directions like an overflowing brain themselves.73 The most conspicuous instantiation of the network trope at the meso-level of humanoid and animal organisms is a psychobiological linking procedure called the bond (tsaheylu). This bond is established when the Navi connect their long queues hair-like extensions of their nervous systems to other organisms such as the horse-like or dragon-shaped creatures of Pandora they use as means of transport. The form and look of the queues are clearly modelled on visual representations of nerve cells, with the braid resembling the elongated cell body (soma) and the fringy endings bearing a close likeness to dendrites, the branched projections of nerve cells named after the Greek term for tree (dendron). On an even more comprehensive bio-spiritual level, the entire biosphere of Pandora, dominated by lush and dense jungles, gradually turns out to form a giant nervous network. On this level, it is mainly the entanglement of lianas and branches, air roots, trees and climbing plants that visually produces the similarity to actual neural networks as first presented in the laboratory scene. The film here reiterates a long tradition of comparing nerve cells to trees and forests that goes back at least to the Spanish neuroscientist Ramn y Cajal (1852-1934), a pioneer of the cellular approach in neuroanatomy. Ramn y Cajal famously spoke of the cellular forest of the brain and compared the cerebrum to a tree whose foliage spreads out74 in order to emphasize the ability of brain cells to respond to the environment by growing in new directions.75 This aspect also comes to the fore in Gerard Edelmans frequent use of botanical imagery, for instance when he talks about branches of axons as arbors,76 dendritic trees77 or the aborization78 of nerve fibres.

55

stephan besser

Figure 4 The floating mountains of Pandora (James Cameron, Avatar) In Avatar, however, this linguistic and visual analogy is not only repeated but pushed to a new level by the effective employment of 3D filming technology. This technique visually enhances the representation of the Pandora jungle as a three-dimensional space that resembles the spatiality of an actual brain as a three-dimensional object: the kind of object the viewers have seen in the holographic projections of brains and neural pathways at the beginning of the film. The potential of these new cinematic possibilities is fully and spectacularly played out when Augustines research team explores the legendary floating mountains of Pandora by helicopter. Consisting of huge rocks and lumps of earth suspended in mid-air and connected by lianas and other plants, these floating mountains visually resemble clusters of neurons (ganglions) and give a new dimension to the spatial experience of Pandora as a giant brain (Fig. 4).79 The spectacular flight scenes and nose dives of the camera eye in Avatar therefore are more than just effective means of an action film to create thrill and excitement, they have an epistemological dimension as well: they visually superimpose neuronal and planetary spaces and thus contribute to the films attempt to show the microscopic in the global and the giant in the small. After this extensive audiovisual elaboration of the network trope it hardly comes as a surprise when Grace Augustine eventually introduces this notion on a linguistic level as well. In a heated conversation with Parker Selfridge, the head administrator of the RDA on Pandora, she explains:

56

from the neuron to the world and back

What we think we know is that there is some kind of electrochemical communication between the roots of the trees, like the synapses between neurons. And each tree has ten-to-the-fourth-connections to the trees around it. And there are ten-to-the twelfth trees on Pandora. [] Its more connections than the human brain. Get it? Its a network. Its a global network and the Navi can access it. They can upload and download data. Memories.80 Eventually, this network proves to be a global autopoetic system, (dis-)embodied in the great Mother Eywa, that is able to regain homeostasis after disturbance from outside, in Gaia style. An appropriately postcolonial reading of the film could conclude that it is not so much the empire but the system/network that strikes back when Eywa sends her help after Jake Sullys distress call via the fibre optical branches of the Tree of Souls. Despite the films holistic approach, however, the use of the network as a passe-partout analogy for all forms and levels of life is in no less problematic than the universal neuro-Darwinism exposed in Koubaas novel. In an enthusiastic review of Camerons film, American biologist and science writer Carol Yoon praised Avatar for artistically recreating what is the heart of biology: the naked, heart-stopping wonder of really seeing the living world.81 One should add that the film combines this fascination with the living world with constant references to contemporary discourses of the global brain and the internet as an emerging central nervous system for humanity.82 By suggesting a natural continuity between biological and technological networks, Avatar glosses over important differences between them. In this regard, the films use of the network analogy recalls the attempts of the connectionist movement in cognitive science to project the properties of neuronal tissues on artificial neural networks and vice versa. As Douwe Draaisma has shown, these cross-mappings made researchers overlook the fact that the parallels were actually minimal or superficial: unlike the individual units of neural networks, actual neurons exhibit great mutual differences in construction and function; moreover, their activity is modulated by chemical processes under hormonal control.83 More recently, Nicholas Carr has pointed out that representations of the internet as a global nervous network are fundamentally mistaken because they fail to see the profound differences between hyperlinks and the organic richness and sensitivity84 of synapses. By employing a universal networkism, Avatar participates in the current naturalization of new communication technologies and contributes to depictions of the network society and network economy as natural outcomes of processes of globalization. Along this line of criticism, media scholar Ken Hillis has convincingly pointed to the fetishization of new digital technologies and the ideological conflation of biology, spirituality and technology in the film: Avatars core politics are animated by its depiction of an idealized future society predicated on a carbon-based, biological network of networks operationalized through the meta-

57

stephan besser

physical logic of World Soul.85 As an example, one could cite the films seamless integration of the flashy laboratory aesthetics of brain scans and holographic projections with the glossy and beaming representation of the Pandorian jungle and its fibre optical elements. This visual, narrative and epistemic integration of nature, neurons and society in Avatar is the result of a specific calibration of the neuromolecular gaze on the basis of analogy: a comparison that highlights and constructs similarities but also obscures and blocks out fundamental differences between source and target domain.86 Yet, although the poetics of the neuromolecular gaze in Avatar are far less critical and self-reflexive than in Koubaas novel, the film does not simply subscribe to a reductionist notion of the cerebral subject. Instead, it seems to privilege a conception of identity that is informed by the so-called 4AE approach in contemporary neuroscience and philosophy. This acronym refers to a wide range of approaches that study the mind as an embodied, embedded, enacted, extended and affective phenomenon.87 They include Andy Clarks concepts of the extended mind and the dynamic loops between world and brain as well as Francisco Varela, Evan Thompson and Eleanor Roschs anti-dualist theory of embodied and enactive cognition and, by extension, Bruno Latours Actor-Network Theory.88 Combining allusions to several of these approaches, Avatar can be read as an imagination of situated subjectivity89 that is in touch with what Rose and AbiRached describe as theories of the social brain that locate neural processes firmly in the dimension of time, development, and transactions with a milieu.90 In the narrative and visual language of the film this environmental approach to subjectivity also implies a turning away from genetics that, on a symbolic level, goes much further than for instance Gerald Edelmans. After the destruction of their Hometree, a giant arboreal structure that through the spiral structure at its core is closely associated with genetics, the Navi move on to the fibre optical Tree of Souls and, literally, to a networked and epigenetic existence.

Conclusion
I have argued that Avatar en Het gebied van Nevski can be read as examples of what I suggest to call the poetics of the neuromolecular gaze: the projection of neuromolecular imagery onto other levels of biological and social existence by means of analogy. The film and the novel implement these poetics in quite different ways: They employ different tropes the brain as battlefield and the brain as network as basis for the projection of analogies and use different representational techniques and strategies for creating them (verbal and visual association, neologisms, wordplay, editing, lighting, etc.). In doing that, both the film and the novel also deviate from a unidirectional bottom-up approach that regards the molecular level as an ultimate site of truth and knowledge. Het gebied van Nevksi, for instance, constantly shuffles back and forth between different levels of exis-

58

from the neuron to the world and back

tence, resulting rather in a profound epistemological disorientation than in the affirmation of a solid molecular ground level of being. Nevertheless, the novel assigns a certain poetic privilege to this level by using it as its most important reservoir of imagery. Avatar in turn can be seen to at least point to a mutual enfolding of microscopic and cosmic perspectives that renders any epistemic or poetic privilege of molecular processes questionable.91 At the same time, the steady narrative unfolding of the network trope from the micro-level of synapses and neural pathways to the networked global brain of Pandora orders these perspectives in a specific way and suggests a particular relevance of the molecular dimension as a general point of reference for everything that is to follow in the films tale of conflict and equilibration. It would take further research to determine the scope of the neuromolecular gaze in contemporary culture and to investigate the influence of popular imageries and artistic representations of the brain on neuroscience itself.92 Here, I would like to conclude that precisely because the film and the novel give in to the neurocultural lure of translations93 in different ways they also show that the fascination with the neuronal matter of the brain does not necessarily lead to a reductionist world-view or conception of the cerebral subject. Whereas Fernando Vidal, Francisco Ortega and other critics of contemporary neuroculture tend to identify the cerebral subject with a more or less stable and univocal ideology of brainhood, the novel and the film point to quite diverse answers to the question what it means to be ones brain. Avatar even manages to combine a neuromolecular perspective with a vision of situated and embodied subjectivity that neurocritics Jan Slaby and Suparna Choudhury credit for standing in sharp contrast94 to the notion of the cerebral subject. It seems that the seductive allure of contemporary neuroscience not only resides in the simplicity and the reductionism of the explanations it offers but also in their ambiguity, diversity and poetic potential. In the process of its cultural dissemination, the neuromolecular imagination itself is lured into different directions by the various tropes, metaphors and analogies that it employs in order to gain cultural relevance and to move from the lab into the world.

Notes
1. I would like to thank the members of the research group Neuroaesthetics and Neurocultures at the University of Amsterdam for their challenging, insightful and inspiring comments on an earlier version of this text. In het gebied kunnen we niet alleen herinneringen zien concurreren, en ik zeg wel degelijk zien omdat het professor Duchamps eindelijk gelukt is om deze in beeld om te zetten, maar ook, en dat is verbluffend, kunnen we in datzelfde gebied vaststellen hoe op een heel specifiek ritme combinaties ontstaan van sterke elkaar aantrekkende cellen en hoe andere worden verstoten. We nemen aan dat die zich vormende meerderheid van cellen een verdrag sluit met de sterkste herinnering om dat daarna

2.

59

stephan besser
bekend te maken. [...] De cellen die geen deel uitmaken van dit verdrag, ruimen het puin en begraven de gesneuvelden waarna ze verslag uitbrengen in andere gebieden. Collaborateurs, spionnen en informanten worden onverbiddelijk gexecuteerd. B. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski (Amsterdam: Querido, 2007), p. 85. All translations are mine unless indicated otherwise. Avatar (James Cameron, USA/UK, 2009), 01:54:00-01:56:00 and 02:27:30-02:28:45. N. Rose and J. M. Abi-Rached, Neuro: The New Brain Sciences and the Management of the Mind (Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press, 2013), p. 229. Rose and AbiRached roughly identify the neuromolecular level with the communication along and between neurons (p. 43). J. Slaby, Steps towards a Critical Neuroscience, in Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 9 (2010), 397; C. Fine, Delusions of Gender: How Our Minds, Society, and Neurosexism Create Difference (New York: W.W. Norton & Company, 2010), p. 171. J.M. Abi-Rached and N. Rose, The Birth of the Neuromolecular Gaze, in History of the Human Sciences 23 (2010), 11-36 (p. 12, p. 17). Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, p. 38. See for instance D. Draaisma, Metaphors of Memory: A History of Ideas about the Mind, trans. by Paul Vincent (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000); C. Borck, Toys are Us: Models and Metaphors in Brain Research, in Critical Neuroscience: A Handbook of the Social Cultural Contexts of Neuroscience, ed. by S. Choudhury and J. Slaby (Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell, 2012), pp. 113-34. Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, p. 10. Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, pp. 25-8, pp. 41-7. Abi-Rached and Rose, Birth of the Neuromolecular Gaze, 11-36 (p. 24). Abi-Rached and Rose, Birth of the Neuromolecular Gaze, 11-36 (p. 20). Zo dekt het onderzoek het volledige spectrum van hersencel tot sociaal gedrag af. Brain and Cognition: Zwaartepunt. http://www.uva.nl/onderzoek/onderzoek-aan-de-uva/ zwaartepunten/content/brain-and-cognition/brain-and-cognition.html [last accessed April 10, 2013]. Abi-Rached and Rose, Birth of the Neuromolecular Gaze, 11-36 (p.11, p. 13). Also see Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, pp. 9-11, pp. 41-51. Abi-Rached and Rose, Birth of the Neuromolecular Gaze, 11-36 (p. 24). Abi-Rached and Rose, Birth of the Neuromolecular Gaze, 11-36 (p. 11). Also see Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, pp. 11-2. Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, p. 51. For a basic definition of the concept Abi-Rached and Rose cite the Wikipedia entry on Neuroplasticity (December 2010) that describes the phenomenon as the ability of the human brain to change as a result of ones experience (Neuro, p. 48). Abi-Rached and Rose, Birth of the Neuromolecular Gaze, 11-36 (p. 24); Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, p. 45. See E. Kandel, In Search of Memory: The Emergence of a New Science of Mind (New York / London: W.W. Norton & Company, 2007), p. 55. Abi-Rached and Rose, The Birth of the Neuromolecular Gaze, 11-36 (p. 32). Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, p. 228. Cf. Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, pp. 82-109.

3. 4.

5.

6. 7. 8.

9. 10. 11. 12. 13.

14. 15. 16. 17.

18.

19. 20. 21.

60

from the neuron to the world and back


22. Cf. C. Malabou, What Should We Do With Our Brain?, trans. by Sebastian Rand (New York: Fordham University Press, 2008), pp. 40-46 and V. Pitts-Taylor, The Plastic Brain: Neoliberalism and the Neuronal Self, Health 14.6 (2010), 635-52. 23. F. Vidal and F. Ortega, Approaching the Neurocultural Spectrum: An Introduction, in Neurocultures: Glimpses into an Expanding Universe, ed. by F. Ortega and F. Vidal (New York: Peter Lang, 2011), p. 18. 24. Z. Lynch with B. Laursen, The Neuro Revolution: How Brain Science is Changing our World (New York: St. Martins Press, 2009). 25. J. Slaby, Perspektiven einer kritischen Philosophie der Neurowissenschaften, Deutsche Zeitschrift fr Philosophie 59.3 (2011), 375-390 (p. 375). 26. See the essays collected in S. Choudhury and J. Slaby (eds), Critical Neuroscience: A Handbook of the Social and Cultural Contexts of Neurosciene (Chichester/Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 2012). 27. P. Legrenzi and C. Umilt, Neuromania: On the Limits of Brain Science (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2011). 28. J. Derksen, Bevrijd de psychologie uit de greep van de hersenmythe (Amsterdam: Bert Bakker, 2012). 29. D. Draaisma, Its all right, Im a doctor. Grensoverschrijding in de wetenschap, in De Academische Boekengids 88 (September 2011), 10-2. 30. Vidal and Ortega, Approaching the Neurocultural Spectrum, p. 8. 31. F. Vidal, Brainhood, Anthropological Figure of Modernity, in History of the Human Sciences 22.1 (2009), 5-36 (p.5, p. 6, p. 7). See also Vidal and Ortega, Approaching the Neurocultural Spectrum, pp. 15-9; S. Choudhury and J. Slaby, Introduction: Critical Neuroscience Between Lifeworld and Laboratory, in Critical Neuroscience, ed. by Choudhury and Slaby, pp. 7-12. 32. See for example J. Slaby Steps towards a Critical Neuroscience, 397-416 (pp. 400-16); S. Cohn, Disrupting Images: Neuroscientific Representations in the Lives of Psychiatric Patients, in Critical Neuroscience, ed. by Choudhury and Slaby (2012), pp. 179-94. 33. N. Rose, Neurochemical Selves, Society 41.1 (2003), 46-59. 34. Pitts-Taylor, The Plastic Brain: Neoliberalism and the Neuronal Self, pp. 639-46. 35. Fine, Delusions of Gender. 36. Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, pp. 219-22. Vidal and Ortega likewise acknowledge that the cerebral subject should not be regarded as a monolithic and hegemonic notion of personhood, since in actual human experience and identity formation it constantly interacts with other notions of the self. They nevertheless present brainhood itself as a more or less stable category (Approaching the Neurocultural Spectrum, pp. 17-8). 37. Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, p. 226. 38. Abi-Rached and Rose, Birth of the Neuromolecular Gaze, 11-36 (p. 13). 39. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 18. 40. Ik [...] schrijf vloeiend, maar de zinnen hebben niet altijd betekenis voor iemand met gezonde hersenen. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 15. 41. Of particular importance in this regard is Jan Verplaetses study Het morele brein. Een geschiedenis over de plaats van de moraal in onze hersenen (Antwerpen: Garant, 2007) that forms the blueprint for a subplot of the novel. 42. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 88.

61

stephan besser
43. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 80. 44. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 78; see D. Dennett, Consciousness Explained (London: Penguin, 1993), pp. 113-5, pp. 137-44. 45. I am my brain. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 123. 46. W. Calvin, How to Think What No One Has ever Thought before, in How Things Are: A Science Tool-Kit for the Mind, ed. by J. Brockman and K. Matson (New York: William Morrow, 1995), p. 161. Explicit references to Calvin and the content of this chapter in Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 24 ([] dat herinneringen in de tijd bevroren ruimtelijke patronen zijn, een vel bladmuziek dat op een pianist ligt te wachten) and p. 126. 47. Calvin, How to Think, p. 159. 48. G. Edelman, Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection (New York: Basic Books, 1987), pp. 4-5. 49. Edelman, Neural Darwinism, pp. 4-8. Also see G. Edelman, Bright Air, Brilliant Fire: On the Matter of the Mind (London: Penguin, 1994), pp. 80-98. 50. Ik sluit mijn ogen en ontwaar een wirwar van dansende kleuren waartussen minuscule soldaten oplichten; Dagelijks sneuvelen er gemiddeld honderdduizend. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 124, p. 123. 51. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 44, 72; also see p. 54 and p. 114. 52. Steeds weer kwam ik by Alexander de Grote uit, alsof hij zich in mijn hersenen als onkruid had vermenigvuldigd en de andere Alexander overwoekerde en verstikte. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 46. 53. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 44. 54. [A]lles wat u leest heeft in mijn brein gevochten voor de ruimte op deze bladzijden. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 110. This remark is addressed to professor Van Nieuwenhuyze. 55. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 52. 56. [...] literatuur [is] een seksueel gedreven soort afwijking van de evolutie! De taal is de staart waarmee de schrijver pronkt [...]. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 99. 57. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 66, p. 113. 58. En als ik niet meer ben dan de vier letters A, C, G, en T, waarvan de combinatie de machinetaal voor mijn constructie vormt, dan moeten mijn herinneringen en die van de stad daar ook mee kunnen worden opgebouwd, dan moeten mijn vingers het lied dat wij zijn kunnen spelen. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 66. 59. Woorden die als een samengeraapt leger van boeren, vissers en ambachtslieden de opmars van het kwaad op de ijsvlakte tot staan brengen. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 26. 60. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, pp. 32-6, pp. 43-4. 61. [...] Eigen Volk Eerst, een politieke slogan die er geen is omdat hij een oeroude sociaal menselijke eigenschap verwoordt [...]. Koubaa, Het gebied van Nevski, p. 93. 62. For a recent critical overview of these attempts see H. Rose and S. Rose, Darwin and After, New Left Review 63 (2010), 91-113. 63. F. Crick, Neural Edelmanism, Trends in Neuroscience 12 (1989), 240-8. For a recent defense and elaboration of Edelmans view see C. Fernando, R. Goldstein and E.

62

from the neuron to the world and back


Szathmry, The Neuronal Replicator Hypothesis, Neural Computation 22 (2010), 280957. G. Edelman, Neural Darwinism: Selection and Reentrant Signaling in Higher Brain Function, Neuron 10 (1993), 115-27 (p. 119). Edelman, Neural Darwinism: Selection and Reentrant Signaling, 115-27 (p. 117). See also Edelman, Bright Air, p. 83 and Joseph LeDoux, Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are (New York: Viking Penguin, 2003), p. 72. D. Swaab, Wij zijn ons brein: Van baarmoeder tot Alzheimer (Amsterdam: Contact, 2010), pp. 433-46. Wij komen ter wereld met hersenen die door een combinatie van onze genetische achtergrond en de programmering gedurende de ontwikkeling in de baarmoeder uniek zijn geworden en waar onze karaktereigenschappen, talenten en beperkingen al voor een belangrijk deel in zijn vastgelegd. Swaab, Wij zijn ons brein, p. 447. The way the brains systems function determines which cells and connections will remain to exist. The brain cells struggle for life is called neuronal Darwinism. [] Multiple factors influence the brain in the early development stages. They decide, later in life, how we experience ourselves. D. Swaab, We Are Our Brain: Brains, Consciousness and Faith Neurobiological Aspects, Academy Magazine, Fall 2005, p. 4. Edelman, Neural Darwinism, p. 126. Edelman, Bright Air, pp. 25-8; Swaab, Wij zijn ons brein, p. 27, p. 38, p. 51, p. 382. J. Ledoux, Synaptic Self; see also M. Sitskoorn, Het maakbare brein. Gebruik je hersens en wordt wie je wilt zijn (Amsterdam: Bert Bakker, 2008) and J.B. Arden, Rewire Your Brain: Think Your Way to a Better Life (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, 2010). Avatar, 0:13:30-0:16:00. P. Pisters, The Neuro-Image: A Deleuzian Film-Philosophy for Digital Screen Culture (Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 2012), p. 181. S. Ramn y Cajal, Precepts and Counsels on Scientific Investigation, ed. by C.B. Courville, trans. by J. M. Snchez Prez (Mountain View, CA: Pacific Press, 1953), pp. 147-8. L. Otis, Membranes: Metaphors of Invasion in Nineteenth-Century Literature, Science, and Politics (Baltimore / London: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1999), p. 85. Also see Kandel, In Search of Memory, pp. 61-65. To be sure, Ramn y Cajal himself actually rejected the contemporary notion that brain cells formed a continuous reticulum (network). Edelman, Bright Air, p. 24. Edelman, Neural Darwinism, p. 170. Edelman, Bright Air, p. 27; Edelman, Neural Darwinism, pp. 133-6. Avatar, 0:53:00-0:54:18, 1:02:32-1:05:30. Avatar, 1:28:00. C.K. Yoon, Luminous 3D Jungle is a Biologists Dream, New York Times, 18 January 2010. http://www.nytimes.com/2010/01/19/science/19essay.html [last accessed 8 April 2013]. F. Heylighen, Conceptions of a Global Brain: A Historical Review, p. 1. To be published in From Big Bang to Global Civilization: A Big History Anthology, ed. by B. Rodriguez, L. Grinin and A. Korotayev (University of California Press). For a brief history of the notion of the global brain see C. Gere, Brains-in-vats, Giant Brains and World

64. 65.

66. 67.

68.

69. 70. 71.

72. 73. 74. 75.

76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81.

82.

63

stephan besser
Brains: The Brain Metaphor in Digital Culture, Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 35 (2004), 351-66. Draaisma, Metaphors of Memory, p. 201. For Draaismas discussion of connectionism also see pp. 185-211 in general. N. Carr, The Shallows: How the Internet is Changing the Way We Think, Read and Remember (London: Atlantic Books, 2010). K. Hillis, From Capital to Karma: James Camerons Avatar, Postmodern Culture 19.3 (2009) http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/postmodern_culture/v019/19.3.hillis.html. See N.L. Stepan, Race and Gender: The Role of Analogy in Science, in Feminsim and Science, ed. by E. Fox Keller and H. Longino (Oxford / New York: Oxford University Press, 1996), pp.126-32. Choudhury and Slaby, Introduction, p. 10. Latour himself has pointed to this similarity in his An Attempt at a Compositionist Manifesto, New Literary History 41 (2010), 471-2. Choudhury and Slaby, Introduction, p. 10. Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, p. 23; also see pp. 216-9. In her analysis of Darren Aronofskys film The Fountain (US, 2006), Patricia Pisters has described this kind of omnidirectional Deleuzian enfoldings as an important characteristic of the neuro-image in contemporary cinema (Pisters, Neuro-Image, pp. 186214). It would be interesting to further investigate the relation of the neuro-imagine in screen culture to the poetics of the neuromolecular gaze that I attempt to describe in this article. For the context of genetics such a project has been undertaken in S. Anker and D. Nelkin, The Molecular Gaze: Art in the Genetic Age (New York: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press, 2004). Rose and Abi-Rached, Neuro, p. 148. Choudhury and Slaby, Introduction, p. 10.

83. 84. 85. 86.

87. 88. 89. 90. 91.

92.

93. 94.

Bibliography
Abi-Rached, Joelle M. and Rose, Nikolas, The Birth of the Neuromolecular Gaze, History of the Human Sciences 23 (2010), 11-36. Arden, Joseph B., Rewire Your Brain: Think Your Way to a Better Life (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, 2010). Anker, Suzanne, and Dorothy Nelkin, The Molecular Gaze: Art in the Genetic Age (New York: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press, 2004). Borck, Cornelius, Toys are Us: Models and Metaphors in Brain Research, in Critical Neuroscience: A Handbook of the Social Cultural Contexts of Neuroscience, ed. by Suparna Choudhury and Jan Slaby (Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell, 2012), pp. 113-34. Brain and Cognition: Zwaartepunt, http://www.uva.nl/onderzoek/onderzoek-aan-de-uva/ zwaartepunten/content/brain-and-cognition/brain-and-cognition.html [last accessed on 10 April 2013].

64

from the neuron to the world and back Calvin, William H. How to Think What No One Has Ever Thought Before, in How Things Are: A Science Tool-Kit for the Mind, ed. by John Brockman and Katinka Matson (New York: William Morrow, 1995), pp. 151-63. Cameron, James, Avatar (USA/UK, 2009). Carr, Nicholas, The Shallows: How the Internet is Changing the Way We Think, Read and Remember (London: Atlantic Books, 2010). Choudhury, Suparna, and Slaby, Jan, Introduction: Critical Neuroscience Between Lifeworld and Laboratory, in Critical Neuroscience: A Handbook of the Social and Cultural Contexts of Neurosciene, ed. by Suparna Choudhury and Jan Slaby (Chichester/Oxford: WileyBlackwell, 2012), pp. 7-26. Choudhury, Suparna, and Slaby, Jan (eds), Critical Neuroscience: A Handbook of the Social and Cultural Contexts of Neuroscience (Chichester/Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 2012). Cohn, Simon, Disrupting Images: Neuroscientific Representations in the Lives of Psychiatric Patients, in Critical Neuroscience, ed. by Choudhury and Slaby (2012), pp. 179-94. Crick, Francis, Neural Edelmanism, Trends in Neuroscience 12 (1989), 240-8. Dennett, Daniel C., Consciousness Explained (London: Penguin, 1993). Derksen, Jan, Bevrijd de psychologie uit de greep van de hersenmythe (Amsterdam: Bert Bakker, 2012). Draaisma, Douwe, Metaphors of Memory: A History of Ideas about the Mind, trans. by Paul Vincent (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000). Draaisma, Douwe, Its all right, Im a doctor. Grensoverschrijding in de wetenschap, De Academische Boekengids 88 (September 2011). Edelman, Gerald M. Neural Darwinism: Selection and Reentrant Signaling in Higher Brain Function, Neuron 10 (1993), 115-27. Edelman, Gerald M., Bright Air, Brilliant Fire: On the Matter of the Mind (London: Penguin, 1994). Edelman, Gerald M., Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection (New York: Basic Books, 1987). Fernando, Christine, Goldstein, Richard, and Szathmry, Ers, The Neuronal Replicator Hypothesis, Neural Computation 22 (2010), 2809-57. Fine, Cordelia, Delusions of Gender: How Our Minds, Society, and Neurosexism Create Difference (New York: W.W. Norton & Company, 2010). Gere, Charlie, Brains-in-vats, Giant Brains and World Brains: The Brain Metaphor in Digital Culture, Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 35 (2004), 351-66. Heylighen, Francis, Conceptions of a Global Brain: A Historical Review. To be published in From Big Bang to Global Civilization: A Big History Anthology, ed. by B. Rodriguez, L. Grinin and A. Korotayev (University of California Press). Hillis, Ken, From Capital to Karma: James Camerons Avatar, Postmodern Culture 19.3 (2009), http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/postmodern_culture/v019/19.3.hillis.html (electronic journal). Kandel, Eric R., In Search of Memory: The Emergence of a New Science of Mind (New York / London: W.W. Norton & Company, 2007). Koubaa, Bart, Het gebied van Nevski (Amsterdam: Querido, 2007).

65

stephan besser Lamme, Victor, De vrije wil bestaat niet. Over wie er echt de baas is in het brein (Amsterdam: Bert Bakker, 2010). Latour, Bruno, An Attempt at a Compositionist Manifesto, New Literary History 41 (2010), 471-2. LeDoux, Joseph, Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are (New York: Viking Penguin, 2003). Legrenzi, Paola, and Umilt, Carlo, Neuromania: On the Limits of Brain Science (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2011). Lynch, Zack, with Laursen, B., The Neuro Revolution: How Brain Science is Changing our World (New York: St. Martins Press, 2009). Malabou, Catherine, What Should We Do With Our Brain?, trans. by Sebastian Rand (New York: Fordham University Press, 2008). Ortega, Francisco, and Vidal, Fernando (eds), Neurocultures: Glimpses into an Expanding Universe (New York: Peter Lang, 2011). Otis, Laura, Membranes: Metaphors of Invasion in Nineteenth-Century Literature, Science and Politics (Baltimore / London: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1999). Pisters, Patricia, The Neuro-Image: A Deleuzian Film-Philosophy for Digital Screen Culture (Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 2012). Pitts-Taylor, Victoria, The Plastic Brain: Neoliberalism and the Neuronal Self, Health 14.6 (2010), 635-52. Ramn y Cajal, Santiago, Precepts and Counsels on Scientific Investigation, ed. by C.B. Courville, trans. by J.M. Snchez Prez (Mountain View, CA: Pacific Press, 1953). Rose, Hilary, and Rose, Steven, Darwin and After, New Left Review 63 (2010), 91-113. Rose, Nikolas, and Abi-Rached, Joelle M., Neuro: The New Brain Sciences and the Management of the Mind (Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press, 2013). Rose, Nikolas, Neurochemical Selves, Society 41.1 (2003), 46-59. Sitskoorn, Margriet, Het maakbare brein. Gebruik je hersens en word wie je wilt zijn (Amsterdam: Bert Bakker, 2008). Slaby, Jan, Perspektiven einer kritischen Philosophie der Neurowissenschaften, Deutsche Zeitschrift fr Philosophie 59.3 (2011), 375-390. Slaby, Jan, Steps towards a Critical Neuroscience, in Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 9 (2010), 397-416. Stepan, Nancy Leys, Race and Gender: The Role of Analogy in Science, in Feminism and Science, ed. by Evelyn Fox Keller and Helen E. Longino (Oxford / New York: Oxford University Press, 1996), pp. 121-36. Swaab, Dick, We Are Our Brain: Brains, Consciousness and Faith Neurobiological Aspect, Academy Magazine, Fall 2005, pp. 4-10. Swaab, Dick, Wij zijn ons brein. Van baarmoeder tot Alzheimer (Amsterdam: Contact, 2010). Verplaetse, Jan, Het morele brein. Een geschiedenis over de plaats van de moral in onze hersenen (Antwerpen: Garant, 2007). Vidal, Fernando, and Ortega, Francisco, Approaching the Neurocultural Spectrum: An Introduction, in Neurocultures: Glimpses into an Expanding Universe, ed. by Francisco Ortega and Fernando Vidal (New York: Peter Lang, 2011), pp. 7-28. Vidal, Fernando, Brainhood, Anthropological Figure of Modernity, in History of the Human Sciences 22.1 (2009), 5-36.

66

from the neuron to the world and back Yoon, Carol K., Luminous 3D Jungle is a Biologists Dream, New York Times, 18 January 2010. http://www.nytimes.com/2010/01/19/science/19essay.html [last accessed 8 April 2013].

67

You might also like