You are on page 1of 10

Journal ofhttp://jad.sagepub.

com/
Attention Disorders

Reaction Time and Attention: Toward a New Standard in the Assessment of ADHD? A Pilot Study
Gabriel G. De la Torre, Juan M. Barroso, José León-Carrión, José M. Mestre and Rocío Guil Bozal
Journal of Attention Disorders published online 26 December 2012
DOI: 10.1177/1087054712466440

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://jad.sagepub.com/content/early/2012/12/21/1087054712466440.citation

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

Additional services and information for Journal of Attention Disorders can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://jad.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://jad.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> OnlineFirst Version of Record - Dec 26, 2012

What is This?

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at CADIZ UNIV on January 10, 2013


466440
rnal of Attention DisordersG. De la Torre et al.
© 2012 SAGE Publications

Reprints and permission:


JADXXX10.1177/1087054712466440Jou

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Articles
Journal of Attention Disorders

Reaction Time and Attention:  Toward


XX(X) 1­–9
© 2012 SAGE Publications
Reprints and permission:

a New Standard in the Assessment of sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav


DOI: 10.1177/1087054712466440
http://jad.sagepub.com
ADHD? A Pilot Study

Gabriel G. De la Torre1, Juan M. Barroso2, José León-Carrión2,


Jose M. Mestre1, and Rocío Guil Bozal1

Abstract
This pilot study shows results of an experiment comparing reaction times (RTs) and attentional performance between
an ADHD group of 30 children and 30 controls, both Spanish speaking. The experiment was carried out using the Seville
computerized neuropsychological battery (SNB). Objective: This study had two goals: One was to test sensitivity of
SNB for attention deficits in ADHD and the second was to detect differences in RTs between ADHD and controls.
Possible explanations and implications of such differences are also discussed. Method: SNB computerized system was
used to assess RTs and accuracy, and alternate forms of continuous performance task were used. Results: Results showed
high sensitivity of some of the SNB tests, especially cancellation tests. RTs were significantly different between groups.
Conclusion: SNB represents a helpful tool for detection of attention deficits, and RT indices represent the most significant
variable in differentiation of both groups studied. (J. of Att. Dis. 2012; XX(X) 1-XX)

Keywords
ADHD, response inhibition, computer attention training, time perspective

Introduction age and developmental stage of the child before being


considered as clinically significant (American Psychiatric
Multiple investigators have evaluated differences in neuro- Association, 2000).
psychological test performance among cultural and ethnic Studies of the cognitive profiles generally present in
minorities (Ardila, 1995; Manly et al., 1998; Wong, Strickland, cases of ADHD indicate poor performance across multiple
Fletcher-Janzen, Ardila, & Reynolds, 2000). Yet to date, com- executive functions and other domains (Barkley, 1997;
paratively few assessment measures have been developed for, Kuntsi, McLoughlin, & Asherson, 2006; Willcutt, Doyle,
adapted to, or normalized among historically underrepre- Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, 2005). A recent, detailed
sented populations. The relative dearth of culturally fair tests investigation confirmed earlier observations that the largest
is of particular concern for non-U.S.-born-Spanish-speaking differences between children with ADHD and control chil-
persons such as Hispanics. Hispanics are one of the most dren across several commonly used cognitive tasks occur in
rapidly growing ethnic groups in the United States, with one the domain of reaction time (RT) variability (Klein,
in four Americans expected to be Hispanic by 2050. Some Wendling, Huettner, Ruder, & Peper, 2006). The proposed
important work has been done by specialists to develop better underlying causes of RT variability in ADHD continue to be
approaches to Spanish-speaking patients (Judd et al., 2009). debated (Bellgrove, Hawi, Kirley, Gill, & Robertson, 2005;
According to the National Academy of Neuropsychology, it is Castellanos et al., 2005; Castellanos & Tannock, 2002;
preferable for neuropsychologists to provide services directly Kuntsi et al., 2006). ADHD is also linked to performance
in the language of evaluation rather than working through deficits in inhibitory control tasks (Willcutt et al., 2005).
interpreters and translators (American Psychological
Association, 1993, 2002). 1
University of Cádiz, Puerto Real, Spain
2
ADHD is currently one of the most frequent causes of University of Seville, Spain
neuropsychological consultation; this is also true in Hispanics
Corresponding Author:
and the Spanish-speaking population. ADHD is character- Gabriel G. De la Torre, University of Cádiz, Facultad Ciencias de la
ized by a persistent pattern of inattention or hyperactivity– Educación, Campus Puerto Real, 11519 Spain
impulsivity. These features must be inappropriate for the Email: gabriel.delatorre@uca.es

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at CADIZ UNIV on January 10, 2013


2 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)

Recent meta-analyses indicated that poor performance administered in Spain. The SNB computerized system
among ADHD sufferers on the stop task, which involves the serves as an interface for the completion of various neuro-
suppression of an ongoing response, is not caused by real psychological tests, such as the attention cancellation and
differences in inhibition of stopping between the ADHD conditioned cancellation tests, Stroop test, Tower of Hanoi
and control groups but reflects differences in the groups’ test, and Luria Memory Scale. For this study, we used all of
mean RTs in response to “go” stimuli (Alderson, Rapport, those tests except the Luria Memory Scale.
& Koefler, 2007; Lijffijt, Kenemans, Verbaten, & Van, Overall, the first goal was to evaluate a new, easy-to-
2005). In clinical practice, the continuous performance test administer, brief, sensitive, comprehensive, computerized
(CPT; Conners, 1995) is the most frequently used tool for tool for the assessment of attention in children with ADHD.
assessing children with ADHD (DuPaul, Anastopoulos, The secondary goal was to check whether participants
Shelton, Guevremont, & Metevia, 1992). The CPT has would show different performance on a task requiring
been widely used in child psychopathology research, responses only when the target is on-screen than displayed
including studies on children at risk of schizophrenia in continuous response–attention tasks on the traditional
(Cornblatt & Erlenmeyer-Kimling, 1985), children with CPT. Although our main objective was to test the sensitivity
learning disabilities (Swanson et al., 1991), and other dis- of the cancellation tests contained in the SNB, we also
orders. The CPT has various forms, but all instruct the administered the Stroop and Tower of Hanoi tests for com-
child to follow a sequence of stimuli visually or by hearing; parison. We hypothesized that this tool would be suffi-
participants are required to respond continuously accord- ciently sensitive to detect ADHD attention deficits, as it was
ing to a certain predefined goal and inhibit responses in the previously proven able to detect symptoms of attention
presence of various target stimuli. The indices studied most deficits in brain injury patients. We also consider the atten-
frequently in CPT paradigms are omissions and errors, and tion tasks included in the SNB as constituting a better
more recently, RT. approach than more traditional CPT tasks. The cancellation
As stated above, not many common tests used to assess attention test (León-Carrión, 1998) consists of a classic let-
attention in U.S. children are available for Spanish-speaking ter cancellation task in which the participant has to press a
children or have been appropriately normed for this popula- button whenever it appears on the screen during presenta-
tion, in either the United States or Latin America. Not many tion of the target stimulus, which in this case was the letter
tests of attention are available for Spanish-speaking people. O. This test has similarities with the CPT (Conners, 1995),
The CPT has not been translated into Spanish, nor have nor- although in the latter, the goal is to inhibit unwanted
mative CPT data been collected from Spanish-speaking responses and sustain the delivery or rate of objective
people to date. Thus, the need for specific tests for Spanish- responses to those stimuli. The CPT assesses the mecha-
speaking people in Spain, Latin America, and the United nisms of sustained attention over time and the dependence
States should represent an important goal in current neuro- of the ability to inhibit impulsive responses or errors on
psychological assessment research. There is a steadily preparation time between target stimuli. In the SNB test,
emerging literature relating to clinical and technical issues participants need to focus on the targets and press the key
in cross-cultural neuropsychological testing (Cofresi & only when they appear on the screen. Each attention subtest
Gorman, 2004; Ferraro, 2002; Fletcher-Janzen, Strickland, of the SNB is 5 min long by default, but the program per-
& Reynolds, 2000; Hambleton, Merenda, & Spielberger, mits the possibility of manipulating every experimental
2005; Hambleton & Patsula, 1999; Nell, 2000; Pontón & parameter, including total task time, the duration of expo-
León-Carrion, 2001). Test developers and publishers are sure, and the intertask interval.
encouraged to identify in test manuals and advertising Second, we wanted to investigate the differences in RTs
whether their test adaptations and translations conform to between ADHD children and controls. As SNB attention
the International Test Commission’s (2002) Test Adaptation tests differ from the common CPT task, we allowed par-
Guidelines. ticipants more time between targets to prepare to respond
We had two objectives in conducting this pilot study. to add a prospective quality to the task; therefore, we
First, we wanted to test the sensitivity of the Seville com- expected to see differences in RT not only between the two
puterized neuropsychological battery (SNB) to detect atten- groups but also between the two attention tests (simple
tion deficits in children with ADHD as compared with and conditioned). Electrophysiological data indicate that
controls. Previous research has tested the SNB among brain difficulties with inhibitory control in ADHD are accompa-
injury patients (León-Carrion, 1998, 1996) and children nied by altered processes of response preparation and exe-
with neurodevelopmental disorders such as spina bifida cution in children (Banaschewski et al., 2004; Brandeis
(De la Torre, 2003); its attention tasks are verified to have et al., 1998; Kuntsi, Wood, Van der Meere, & Asherson,
good sensitivity. The SNB is a computerized neuropsycho- 2009; Pliszka, Liotti, & Woldorff, 2000); this may indicate
logical battery for which normative data have been col- a more general state- or response-regulation problem
lected in the Spanish-speaking population, and the test is (Banaschewski et al., 2004).

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at CADIZ UNIV on January 10, 2013


G. De la Torre et al. 3

The primacy of the inhibition deficit in ADHD has been only when the target stimuli appear on the screen. The target
further studied using event rates and incentive manipula- is set as the letter “O.” The conditioned cancellation test
tions. In two studies, an apparent response-inhibition deficit (León-Carrión, 1998) is very similar, but the participant is
in ADHD “disappeared” after the introduction of incentives instead asked to press the key upon seeing the letter “O”
(Konrad, Gauggel, Manz, & Scholl, 2000; Slusarek, Velling, only if it has been preceded by the letter “X,” thus adding a
Bunk, & Eggers, 2001). However, those results have not complication to the original experiment. Both tests were 5
been consistently replicated, and in general, study results min long in this study. In both tests, the duration of stimulus
appear highly sensitive to task parameters (Luman, exposure and the interstimulus interval were 1 s. Both tests
Oosterlaan, & Sergeant, 2005). Using manipulations of provided information on a series of variables, such as RT,
event rates, some studies report no inhibition deficit in omissions, errors, and correct responses, in an explanatory
ADHD under any event rate, whereas other studies find dif- set of screens at the end of the test. A total of 50 target stim-
ferences between ADHD sufferers and controls only in slow uli appeared on the screen during each test.
or fast conditions (van der Meere, 2002; Wiersema, Roeyers, Tower of Hanoi/Seville test of SNB. This test, which is
Van, & Baeyerns, 2006). The effects of such manipulations included in the SNB, measures problem-solving skills,
on performance, when measured by the stop and go-/no-go learning, and reasoning. Specifically, the Tower of Hanoi/
tasks, also vary across studies, emphasizing the sensitivity Seville test (León-Carrión, 1998) is specially designed for
of ADHD performance to task parameters and the differen- the assessment of executive function. In this test, partici-
tial sensitivity of different aspects of task performance to pants are presented with three numbered bars and three col-
the same task manipulations (Kuntsi et al., 2009). ored disks of different sizes. The goal is to build the tower
or pyramid that appears on the leftmost bar, except that it
needs to be moved to the rightmost bar. The number of
Method pieces varies according to the needs of the test type, ranging
Participants from one to five discs. To move the discs, the participant
can press one of three numbered keys on the computer key-
Sixty Spanish participants aged between 9 and 14 years board. The participant has to keep pressing the buttons
were included in this study, 30 of whom were healthy con- alternately according to the rules provided prior to the start
trols and 30 of whom had been diagnosed with ADHD of the test. There are two different administration forms for
(ADHD group: 15 male and 15 female individuals, mean this test: A and B. In Form A, participants are not informed
age = 12.75 years, SD = 1.98; control group = 13 male and of the rule stating the impossibility of placing a larger disk
17 female individuals, mean age = 12.85 years, SD = 1.75). on top of a smaller one. In Form B, participants are informed
Homogeneity was tested via the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test of all the rules, including those regarding movements that
(p ≤ .05). are disallowed by the program. The computer makes a noise
The healthy controls had no history of either neurologi- if participants try to perform one of these illegal move-
cal or psychiatric disorders. In the ADHD group, all partici- ments, and participants are instructed at the outset that a
pants had been diagnosed by school psychologists or sound made by the computer signifies that they have
clinical neuropsychologists on the basis of neuropsycho- attempted an illegal move. This option is helpful for seeing
logical and behavioral assessments. All children in this whether participants are able to determine the logic of disal-
group had diagnoses of ADHD–combined type according to lowed movements for themselves. This provides especially
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders interesting options for the detection of disexecutive symp-
(4th ed., text rev.; DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric tomatology, inflexibility, and reiterative behavior. At the
Association, 2000) diagnostic criteria. To avoid circularity, end of the test run, the system offers complete information
the specialists who determined the diagnoses of patients on task performance, including the average response time,
included in the analysis were blind to the patients’ perfor- total number of movements, total number of incorrect
mance on the SNB. movements, total number of correct movements, total time
taken to complete the task, and so on. In addition, the sys-
tem displays the commission numbers of three types of
Materials errors: Type I error, when the participant tries to move a
Both groups were evaluated with a series of neuropsycho- disk from an empty bar; Type II error, when the participant
logical subtests included in the SNB; all had been standard- tries to move a disk from one bar to the same one; and Type
ized and normalized among the Spanish-speaking population. III error, when the participant tries to place a larger disk on
top of a smaller one (Figure 1).
SNB Subtests Stroop test of SNB. The Stroop test (León-Carrión,
Cancellation test and conditioned cancellation test of SNB. 1998) of neurocognitive interference (Stroop, 1935) mea-
In the cancellation test, participants have to press the key sures the error or delay in naming the displayed color of a

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at CADIZ UNIV on January 10, 2013


4 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)

previous work (De la Torre, 2002, 2003) on an adaptation of


an assessment of attention proposed by Mirsky Pascualvaca,
Duncan, and French (1999).
We performed a descriptive data contrast for two inde-
pendent samples between the two groups (ADHD patients
and controls). As dependent variables, we used quantita-
tive variables corresponding to different indices measured
by the neuropsychological tests used (attention cancella-
tion test of SNB: correct responses, erroneous responses,
and RT; conditioned cancellation test of SNB: correct
responses, erroneous responses, and RT; Tower of Hanoi/
Seville test of SNB: total time, average time, total moves,
correct moves, and incorrect moves; Stroop test of SNB:
subtest results for word-reading and color-naming tests).
All hypotheses were tested using alphas of p ≤ .05. The
analysis was carried out using the SPSS statistical package
Figure 1. Tower of Hanoi (SNB) for Windows. Before any further analysis, it was verified
that all quantitative variables met the criteria for normal
distribution established by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov and
Anderson–Darling tests.
word whose meaning denotes a different color than the
color in which it is displayed on the screen. This occurs
due to mutual interference between different dimensions Results
or stimulus characteristics, which make it difficult to As shown in Table 1, participants in the ADHD group per-
ignore the informational conflict while performing the formed worse on the cancellation and conditioned cancella-
task, even though the conflicting information is irrelevant tion tests of SNB than controls except in terms of the RT
to the task. The SNB includes a version of this task in index of the latter (t = −1.400, p = .195). Participants with
which the participant must recognize and verbally respond ADHD had poorer results than controls in terms of all indi-
to the target stimuli; the evaluator then presses one of ces of the cancellation tests of the SNB, with fewer correct
three keys on the keyboard according to the verbaliza- answers (t = 2.535, p = .014), more errors (t = −2.883, p =
tions of the participant. .017), and higher average RTs (t = −1.318, p = .007).
Control participants showed higher numbers of correct
answers on the SNB than ADHD sufferers (t = 3.398, p =
Method .015). The results of post hoc tests showed medium-to-high
The participating children came from private and public effect sizes (Cohen’s d) for these t tests. Interestingly, chil-
schools in Spain. All of the children in the ADHD group dren with ADHD had improved RTs on the conditioned
had completed the first phase of screening for identification cancellation test, which always followed the simple cancel-
and diagnosis of ADHD, which was conducted by inter- lation test. Similar variations were shown by controls and
viewing their parents and teachers and administering the ADHD patients in terms of this index, making their RTs
ADHD Albor-COHS questionnaire (Garcia & Magaz, much more similar in this second cancellation test.
2000). In some cases, a previous comprehensive psycho- In the Tower of Hanoi/Seville test, no differences were
logical or educational evaluation confirming the presence found between participant groups in terms of any of its indi-
of ADHD already existed. Later, neuropsychological con- ces. We found no significant differences on the Stroop test
sultations in private medical centers preceded neuropsy- between the two groups in terms of correct answers during
chological assessments conducted by administering the word reading (t = 1.860, p = .069), but we did find differ-
same tests as were used with the control group (listed ences in terms of correct answers made during color naming
above). In some cases, the evaluation was carried out across (t = 2.913, p = .005). Children with ADHD made fewer cor-
two to three sessions separated by 1 week. rect responses than controls on this subtest.
The children in the control group had no academic or In general, the ADHD group had poorer results than con-
medical problems; their performance and development trols on those indices corresponding to most of the four
were adequate considering their ages and academic levels. main elements of attention in Mirsky et al.’s (1999) multidi-
All of them participated in the study voluntarily, and their mensional model. We can also discern worse performance
parents provided informed consent. Each test and its atten- by the ADHD group on factors related to both inhibitory
tional component were paired according to the results of mechanisms (such as working memory) and consolidation

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at CADIZ UNIV on January 10, 2013


G. De la Torre et al. 5

Table 1. Results for SNB Indices in ADHD and Controls Including Cohen’s d Values

Controls ADHD t test

  M SD M SD t p d
Attention cancellation test 48.00 2.56 45.70 5.66 2.535 .014* 0.523
corrects
Attention cancellation test errors 1.66 2.12 8.60 0.08 −2.883 .017* −1.678
Attention cancellation test 0.49 0.02 0.51 0.08 −1.318 .007* −0.342
reaction time
Condition attention test corrects 49.29 0.46 47.33 3.03 3.398 .015* 0.901
Condition attention test errors 0.30 0.48 2.63 3.52 −3.433 .018* −0.927
Condition attention test reaction 0.43 0.07 0.44 0.06 −1.400 .195 −0.153
time
Tower of Hanoi total time 194.88 111.83 194.76 167.43 −0.245 .808 <0.001
Tower of Hanoi mean time 8.88 3.87 8.80 5.22 −0.435 .666 0.017
Tower of Hanoi total correct 14.37 5.80 15.75 6.65 −0.189 .851 −0.221
movements
Tower of Hanoi total erroneous 9.12 4.79 9.10 5.89 −0.032 .975 0.003
movements
Stroop word reading 98.12 2.31 95.50 5.43 1.860 .069 0.778
Stroop color naming 100 0 95.50 5.02 2.913 .005* 1.267
*significant difference for a ≤ .05.

of the anterior and posterior attentional systems in the had clearly worse performance on this test. The results of
model of attention proposed by Posner and Petersen (Posner the attention tests of the SNB are directly related with the
& Petersen, 1990; Posner & Dehaene, 1994). results of other tests traditionally used in neuropsychologi-
cal assessment of children with ADHD, for example, the
CPT (Losier, McGrath, & Klein, 1996; Willcutt et al.,
Discussion and Conclusion 2005). This poor performance could be explained by the
In this study, participants with ADHD committed more attention deficits described by different elements of
errors and gave fewer correct responses on all neuropsy- Mirsky’s multidimensional attention model (Mirsky,
chological tests used except for the Tower of Hanoi/Seville Anthony, Duncan, Ahem, & Kellam, 1991). In terms of
test of the SNB. focus/execution element of Attention, Mirsky’s model
Regarding our first hypotheses and goal, we conclude that would explain sustained attentional problems in our ADHD
the cancellation tests contained in the SNB, although differ- group, but no differences were found in our study between
ent from traditional CPT tests, showed sensitivity to the ADHD and controls for the alternate/shift element.
attention deficits of children with ADHD. The attention tests Decrement in vigilance was shown to progress throughout
contained in the SNB permitted participants with ADHD to the test, as related work has previously shown (Stins et al.,
prepare their responses and requested that they be sufficiently 2005; Swaab-Barneveld et al., 2000; Wilding, 2005;).
alert to execute the motor task of pressing a key on a key- We obtained some discrete evidence with regard to our
board only when a specific target appears on a screen. This second hypothesis. As mentioned above, the CPT requires
method represents a good method of performing research on that the participant continuously press a key on the computer
RT, attention, and inhibitory processes. We believe that these keyboard and inhibit that response when the target appears
types of cancellation tests, in combination with other specific on the screen. In our study, we used the SNB cancellation
neuropsychological tests and multivariate functional models test to introduce an important variable to this task:
of attention, may represent an effective research approach to Participants only need to press the key when the target
the clinical and experimental neuropsychological assessment appears, giving them more time to prepare and execute their
of patients with attention problems (León-Carrión et al., responses. However, children with ADHD nevertheless
1998). Attention tests contained in the computerized SNB showed higher error rates and lower rates of correct responses
can greatly aid in the detection of attention deficits in Spanish- than controls. It is particularly interesting to note that in the
speaking children with ADHD. conditioned attention test, these effects were significant in
Especially sensitive were the cancellation tests of the the same directions, but the RTs were lower in this test. This
SNB, which revealed significant performance differences performance profile indicates that errors and impulsivity are
between the two groups; participants in the ADHD group independent of preparation and readiness to process target

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at CADIZ UNIV on January 10, 2013


6 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)

stimuli in children with ADHD. Performance is dependent those obtained in a previous study of different subtypes of
on the nature of the task itself and is a function of time on the ADHD (Nigg, Blaskey, Huag-Pollock, & Rappley, 2002); in
millisecond scale. As previously mentioned in the that study, patients with the same ADHD subtype constitut-
“Introduction” section, this may indicate more general state- ing our sample (the combined subtype) obtained poorer
or response-regulation problems (Banaschewski et al., 2003; results on these two tests. Performance in the Tower test was
2004). In our study, although children with ADHD obtained very similar across both groups in terms of the measured
similar RTs to controls, they continued to make more errors indices (total time, mean completion time, total movements,
and fewer correct answers compared with controls, espe- and erroneous movements). One possible explanation for
cially in terms of RT on both cancellation tests. Despite the these results is the low sensitivity of this task in comparison
two factors that helped children with ADHD to improve with the others included in the SNB. As we explained in the
their poorer performance, the differences continued to be “Method” section, this task is a modification of the Tower of
significant between the two groups. Current research on this Hanoi task called Tower of Hanoi/Seville test, which has
factor offers revealing results. A time-processing deficit has been adapted for use on computers. Children with ADHD
been proposed as a neuropsychological candidate endophe- had more difficulties understanding the mechanics and
notype for ADHD (Valko et al., 2010). We observed in our instructions given on this test than controls; this represents a
study that RTs may vary with learning effects and condi- limitation of our study, and further research needs to be con-
tioned tasks; these improve performance in ADHD sufferers, ducted on the sensitivity and specificity of this test.
but not to normal levels. This suggests that children with
ADHD have deficits in prospective time-task processing Declaration of Conflicting Interests
similar to those observed in cases of prospective memory The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
problems. It is well known that the rostral prefrontal cortex to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article
supports a cognitive system that reflects either stimulus-
oriented (SO) or stimulus-independent (SI) attending. This Funding
part of the brain has also been shown to play a crucial role in The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
the maintenance and realization of delayed intentions. for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: The
Previous research has shown the same types of deficits publication of this work received financial support from Spanish
observed in this study on time-dependent computer tasks Government Science and Industry grant AYA201018573.
among children with ADHD (Zinke et al., 2010). The rostral
prefrontal cortex and its relationship with ADHD have also References
been studied using neuroimaging techniques. These neuro- Alderson, R. M., Rapport, M. D., & Koefler, M. J. (2007). Atten-
imaging studies showed decreased activation in the left ros- tion deficit/hyperactivity disorder and behavioural inhibition:
tral medial frontal cortex during the go-/no-go task and A meta-analytic review of the stop-signal paradigm. Journal
decreased activation in the bilateral prefrontal, temporal, of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 745-758.
and right parietal lobes during the switch task among chil- Aman, C. J., Roberts, R. J., Jr., & Pennington, B. F. (1998). A
dren with ADHD (Smith, Taylor, Brammer, Toone, & Rubia, neuropsychological examination of the underlying deficit in
2006). Other studies have also investigated the roles of ADHD: The frontal lobe versus right parietal lobe theories.
abnormalities of the rostral prefrontal cortex in developmen- Developmental Psychology. 34, 956-969.
tal disorders on a task-by-task basis (Gilbert, Bird, Brindley, American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statisti-
Frith, & Burgess, 2008; Schulz et al., 2004). cal manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washing-
The SNB and, specifically, its simple and conditioned ton, DC: Author.
cancellation tests were especially sensitive to the detection American Psychological Association. (1993). Guidelines for pro-
of attentional problems in children with ADHD. The SNB viders of psychological services to ethnic, linguistic, and cul-
has been previously used and tested in Spain among brain turally diverse populations. American Psychologist, 48, 45-48.
injury and spina bifida patients (De la Torre, 2003; León- American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical principles
Carrión et al., 1998), and adaptations of it have been created of psychologists and code of conduct. Retrieved from http://
in Mexico and Brazil (Bittencourt-Chastinet, Morais, www.apa.org/ethics/code2002.html
Solovieva, & Quintanar-Rojas, 2012). Ardila, A. (1995). Directions of research in cross-cultural neuro-
Although some studies have shown poor performance on psychology. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsy-
the Tower of Hanoi/Tower of London (Aman, Roberts, & chology, 17, 143-150.
Pennington, 1998; Paule et al., 2000) and Stroop tests Banaschewski, T., Brandeis, D., Heinrich, H., Albrecht, B.,
(Paule et al., 2000; Seidman, Biederman, Faraone, Weber, Brunner, E., & Rothenberger, A. (2004). Questioning inhibi-
& Ouellette, 1997) in populations with ADHD, the results tory control as the specific deficit of ADHD—Evidence from
of our study did not show those trends, except for one index brain electrical activity. Journal of Neural Transmission, 111,
of the Stroop test. This pattern of results also differs from 841-864.

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at CADIZ UNIV on January 10, 2013


G. De la Torre et al. 7

Banaschewski, T., Brandeis, D., Heinrich, H., Albrecht, B., Revista Española de Neuropsicología [Spanish Journal of
Woerner, W., Brunner, E., & Rothenberger, A. (2003). Asso- Neuropsychology], 5, 177-193.
ciation of ADHD and conduct disorder—Brain electrical evi- DuPaul, G., Anastopoulos, A. D., Shelton, T. L., Guevremont, D. C.,
dence for the existence of a distinct subtype. Journal of Child & Metevia, L. (1992). Multi-method assessment of attention-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 356-376. deficit hyperactivity disorder: The diagnostic utility of
Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New clinic-based tests. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21,
York, NY: Guilford. 294-402.
Bellgrove, M. A., Hawi, Z., Kirley, A., Gill, M., & Robertson, I. H. Ferraro, R. (Ed.). (2002). Minority and cross-cultural aspects of
(2005). Dissecting the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder neuropsychological assessment. Royersford, PA: Swets &
(ADHD) phenotype: Sustained attention, response variabil- Zeitlinger.
ity and spatial attentional asymmetries in relation to dopa- Fletcher-Janzen, E., Strickland, T. L., & Reynolds, C. R. (Eds.).
mine transporter (DAT1) genotype. Neuropsychologia, 43, (2000). Handbook of cross-cultural neuropsychology. New
1847-1857. York, NY: Kluwer/Plenum.
Bittencourt-Chastinet, J., Morais, C., Solovieva, Y., & Quintanar- García Pérez, E. M., & Magaz, A. (2000). Escalas Magallanes
Rojas, L. (2012). Propuesta de adaptación de la prueba de de áreas de conductas problema [The Magallanes Scales for
abordaje luriano “evaluación neuropsicológica infantil Behavioral Problems]. Bilbao, Spain: EACP.
Puebla-Sevilla” para el idioma portugués [Proposal for adap- Gilbert, S. J., Bird, G., Brindley, R., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P.
tation of the Lurian approach “ Neuropsychological Child W. (2008). Atypical recruitment of medial prefrontal cortex in
Assessment Puebla-Seville” for the Portuguese language.] autism spectrum disorders: An fMRI study of two executive
MAGIS–Revista Internacional de Investigación en Edu- function tasks. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2281-2291.
cación, 4(9), 669-683. Hambleton, R. K., Merenda, P. F., & Spielberg, C. D. (Eds.).
Brandeis, D., van Leeuwen, T. H., Rubia, K., Vitacco, D., Steger, J., (2005). Adapting educational and psychological tests for
Pascual-Marqui, R. D., & Steinhausen, H. C. (1998). Neu- cross-cultural assessment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
roelectric mapping reveals precursor of stop failures in chil- Hambleton, R. K., & Patsula, L. (1999). Increasing the validity of
dren with attention deficits. Behavioral Brain Research, 94, adapted tests: Myths to be avoided and guidelines for improv-
111-125. ing test adaptation practices. Applied Testing Technology Jour-
Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Scheres, A., Di Martino, A., nal, 1, 1-12. Retrieved from http://www.testpublishers.org/
Hyde, C., & Walters, J. R. (2005). Varieties of attention- journal-of-applied-testing-technology
deficit/hyperactivity disorder-related intra individual variabil- International Test Commission. (2002). International Test Com-
ity. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1416-1423. mission test adaptation guidelines. Retrieved from www.intes-
Castellanos, F. X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention- tcom.org
deficits/hyperactivity disorder: The search for endophenotypes. Judd, T., Capetillo, D., Carrión-Baralt, J., Mármol, L., Miguel-
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 617-628. Montes, L. S., & Navarrete, M. G. NAN Policy and Planning
Cofresi, N. I., & Gorman, A. A. (2004). Testing and assessment Committee. (2009). Professional considerations for improving
issues with Spanish-English bilingual Latinos. Journal of the Neuriopsychological evaluation of Hispanics: A National
Counseling & Development, 82, 99-106. Academy of Neuropsychology Education paper. Archives of
Conners, C. K. (1995). Conners’ continuous performance test Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 127-135.
computer program 3.0: User’s manual. North Tonwanda, NY: Klein, C., Wendling, K., Huettner, P., Ruder, H., & Peper, M.
Multi-Health Systems. (2006). Intra-subjects variability in attention-deficit hyperac-
Cornblatt, B. A., & Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. (1985). Global atten- tivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 1088-1097.
tional deviance as a marker of risk for schizophrenia: Specific- Konrad, K., Gauggel, S., Manz, A., & Scholl, M. (2000). Lack
ity and predictive validity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, of inhibition: A motivational deficit in children with attention/
94, 470-486. hyperactivity disorder and children with traumatic brain
De la Torre, G. G. (2002). El Modelo Funcional de Atención en injury. Child Neuropsychology, 6, 286-296.
Neuropsicología [The Functional Model of Attention in Neu- Kuntsi, J., McLoughlin, G., & Asherson, P. (2006). Attention
ropsychology]. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuromolecular Medicine, 8,
[Journal of General and Applied Psychology], 1(55), 113-121. 461-484.
De la Torre, G. G. (2003). Evaluación de la Atención mediante Kuntsi, J., Wood, A. C., Van der Meere, J., & Asherson, P. (2009).
el Test de Cancelación Simple y Cancelación Condicionada Why cognitive performance in ADHD may not reveal true
(Batería Neuropsicológica Sevilla®) en niños con Trastorno potential: Findings from a large population-based sample.
por Déficit de Atención e Hiperactividad (TDAH) [Assess- Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 15,
ment of Attention using the Simple and Conditioned Cancel- 570-579.
lation Tests (Seville Neuropsychological Battery®) in children León-Carrión, J. (1996). The attentional system in brain injury sur-
with Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)]. vivors. International Journal of Neurosciences, 85, 231-236.

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at CADIZ UNIV on January 10, 2013


8 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)

León-Carrión, J. (1998). Batería Neuropsicología Sevilla [Seville Schulz, K. P., Fan, J., Tang, C. Y., Newcord, J. H., Buchsbaum,
Neuropsychological Battery. Madrid]. Madrid, Spain: TEA. M. S., & Cheung, A. M. (2004). Response inhibition in ado-
León-Carrión, J., Alarcón, J. C., Revuelta, M., Murillo-Cabezas, F., lescents diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disor-
Domínguez-Roldán, M., Domínguez-Morales, M. R., & der during childhood: An event-related fMRI study. American
Forastero, P. (1998). Executive functioning as outcome in Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1650-1657.
patients alter traumatic brain injury. International Journal of Seidman, L. J., Biederman, J., Faraone, V., Weber, W., & Ouellette, C.
Neuroscience, 94, 75-83. (1997). Toward defining a neuropsychology of attention-
Lijffijt, M., Kenemans, J. L., Verbaten, M. N., & van, E. H. (2005). deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Performance of children and
A meta analytic review of stopping performance in attention- adolescents from a large clinically referred sample. Journal of
deficit hyperactivity disorder: Deficit inhibitory motor con- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 150-160.
trol? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 216-222. Slusarek, M., Velling, S., Bunk, D., & Eggers, C. (2001). Moti-
Losier, B. J., McGrath, P., & Klein, R. M. (1996). Error patterns on vational effects on inhibitory control in children with ADHD.
the continuous performance test in non-medicated and medi- Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy-
cated samples of children with and without ADHD: A meta- chiatry, 40, 355-363.
analytic review. Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry and Smith, A. B., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Toone, B., & Rubia, K.
Allied Disciplines, 37, 971-987. (2006). Task-specific hypoactivation in prefrontal and tem-
Luman, M., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). The impact poroparietal brain regions during motor inhibition and task
of reinforcement contingencies on ADHD: A review and theo- switching in medication-naive children and adolescents with
retical appraisal. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 183-213. attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of
Manly, J. J., Miller, S. W., Heaton, R. K., Byrd, D., Reilly, J., & Psychiatry, 163, 1044-1051.
Velasquez, R. J. The HIV Neurobehavioral Research Cen- Stins, J. F., Tollenaar, M. S., Slaats-Willemse, D. I., Buitelaar, J. K.,
ter (HNRC) Group. (1998). The effect of African-American Swaab-Barneveld, H., Verhulst, F. C., & Boomsma, D. I.
acculturation on neuroopsychological test performance in nor- (2005). Sustained attention and executive functioning perfor-
mal and HIV-positive individuals. Journal of the International mance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Neu-
Neuropsychological Society, 4, 291-302. ropsychology, 11, 285-294.
Mirsky, A. F., Anthony, B. J., Duncan, C. C., Ahem, M. B., & Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reac-
Kellam, S. G. (1991). Analysis of the elements of attention: A neu- tions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.
ropsychological approach. Neuropsychology Review, 109-145. Swaab-Barneveld, H., de Sonneville, L., Cohen-Kettenis, P.,
Mirsky, A. F., Pascualvaca, D. M., Duncan, C. C., & French, L. M. Gielen, A., Buitelaar, J., & Van Engeland, H. (2000). Visual
(1999). A model of attention and its relation to ADHD. Men- sustained attention in a child psychiatric population. Journal
tal Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Reviews, 3, 169-176. 39, 651-659.
Nell, V. (2000). Cross-cultural neuropsychological assessment: Swanson, J. M., Posner, M. I., Potkin, S., Bonforte, S.,
Theory and practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Youpa, D., & Cantwell, D. (1991). Activating tasks for the
Nigg, J. T., Blaskey, L., Huang-Pollock, C., & Rappley, M. D. study of visual–spatial attention in ADHD children: A cog-
(2002). Neuropsychological executive functions and ADHD nitive anatomical approach. Journal of Child Neurology,
DSM-IV subtypes. Journal of the American Academy of Child 6, S119-S127.
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 59-66. Valko, L., Schneider, G., Doehnert, M., Muller, U., Brandeis, D.,
Paule, M. G., Rowland, A. S., Ferguson, S. A., Chelonis, J. J., Steinhausen, H. C., & Drechsler, R. (2010). Time processing
Tannock, R., Swanson, J. M., & Castellanos, F. X. (2000). Atten- in children and adults with ADHD. Journal of Neural Trans-
tion deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Characteristics, interventions, mission, 117, 1213-1228.
and models. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 22, 631-651. Van der Meere, J. J. (2002). The role of attention. In S. Sandberg (Ed.),
Pliszka, S. R., Liotti, M., & Woldorff, M. G. (2000). Inhibi- Hyperactivity disorders of childhood (2nd ed., pp. 162-213). Cam-
tory control in children with attention-deficit/hyperactiv- bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
ity disorder: Event-related potentials identify the processing Wiersema, R., van der, M. J., Roeyers, H., Van, C. R., & Baeyerns, D.
component and timing of an impaired right-frontal response- (2006). Event rate and event related potentials in ADHD.
inhibition mechanism. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 238-246. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Dis-
Pontón, M. O., & León-Carrion, J. (Eds.). (2001). Neuropsychol- ciplines, 47, 560-567.
ogy and the Hispanic patient: A clinical handbook. Mahwah, Wilding, J. (2005). Is attention impaired in ADHD? British Jour-
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. nal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 487-505.
Posner, M. I., & Dehaene, S. (1994). Attentional networks. Trends Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., &
in Neuroscience, 17, 75-79. Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function
Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-
the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42. analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336-1346.

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at CADIZ UNIV on January 10, 2013


G. De la Torre et al. 9

Wong, T. M., Strickland, T. L., Fletcher-Janzen, E., Ardila, A., & has extensive research experience on brain injury and neuropsy-
Reynolds, C. R. (2000). Theoretical and practical issues in the chological aspects.
neuropsychological assessment and treatment of culturally
dissimilar patients. In E. Fletcher-Janzen, T. L. Strickland & Jose León-Carrión is Full Professor of Psychology and Director
C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural Neuropsy- of the Human Neuropsychology Laboratory at the Department of
chology (pp. 3-18). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic. Experimental Psychology of University of Seville. He has been
Zinke, K., Altgassen, M., Mackinlay, R. J., Rizzo, P., Drechsler, R., Vice-president of the International Brain Injury Association
& Kliegel, M. (2010). Time-based prospective memory per- (IBIA) and member of numerous international committees on the
formance and time-monitoring in children with ADHD. Child topic.
Neuropsychology, 16, 338-349.
Jose M. Mestre is Full Professor of Psychology at the Department
Bios of Psychology of the University of Cádiz in Spain. He is specialist
Gabriel G. De la Torre is Associate Professor of Psychology at on Emotional Intelligence and belongs to several international
the Department of Psychology of University of Cádiz in Spain. He committees on this area.
teaches Attention and Perception and Neuropsychology of Brain
Injury. He does research on ADHD, attention and Spina Bifida Rocio Guil Bozal is Cathedra Professor of Psychology at
cognitive aspects and human factors. the Department of Psychology of the University of Cádiz in
Spain. She is the director of the emotional Intelligence
Juan M. Barroso is Full professor of Psychology at Department Laboratory and she specialized in Social Psychology and Social
of Experimental Psychology of University of Seville in Spain. He Neuroscience.

Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at CADIZ UNIV on January 10, 2013

You might also like