You are on page 1of 5

LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

1
PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSES/RULES

From last lecture:

tick [I'ik] [I']
stick [sIik] [I]
later [|eror] [r]
kitten [ki?n] [?]
/t/


[] [] [t] [t]
V__V V__ [n] #__V elsewhere


The phoneme (=mental object) /I/ is
realized/pronounced by four different
allophones (=sounds/phones) in English: [r],
[?], [I'], [I].
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

2
RULE: EXAMPLE 1

RULE: In English, the phoneme /t/ is
pronounced as:
[] between two vowels when the preceding
vowel carries the word stress
/I/ [r] / V__V
[] between a vowel and the alveolar nasal
consonant [n]
/I/ [?] / V__ [n]
[t] at the beginning of a word when
followed by a vowel
/I/ [I'] / #__V
[t] elsewhere
/I/ [I] elsewhere


LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

3
RULE: EXAMPLE 2
Pam [p'n] __ [n] Pat [p'I] __ [I]
bean [bin] __ [n] beat [biI] __ [I]
broom [brun] __ [n] bruise [bruz] __ [z]
long [la q] __ [q] lock [lak] __ [l]
/v/ in English speakers minds

[v] [v] what English speakers utters
___ [n]/[n]/[q] elsewhere

natural (they are all nasal consonants)
class

NATURAL CLASS
A group of language sounds that shares one or more
articulatory properties, to the exclusion of all the other
sounds.

Conclusion
A nasal vowel and its non-nasal equivalent are allophones
(=sound realizations) of the same phoneme in English.
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

4
But in French

laid [lc] ugly lin [lc] flax

las [la] tired lent [la] slow

lot [lo] prize long [lo] long


/c/ /c/ /a/ /a/ /o/ /o/ in French speakers minds

[c] [c] [a] [a] [o] [o] what French speakers utters



Conclusion
A nasal vowel and its non-nasal equivalent are allophones
(=sound realizations) of different phonemes in French.

LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

5
We can conceive of the use of allophones as
the result of rules which represent various
phonological processes:

PHONEMIC FORM
(abstract sequence of phonemes/mental
objects in the mind)

Phonological Processes/Rules

PHONETIC FORM
(sequence of context-appropriate
allophones/sounds that are actually
pronounced)
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

6
RULE: EXAMPLE 3
SPELLING IN THE MIND PRONOUNCED
I can ask /aj kn sk/ > [aj kn sk]
I can play /aj kn ple/ > [aj km ple]
I can sing /aj kn si/ > [aj kn si]
I can go /aj kn gow/ > [aj k gow]
I can bake /aj kn bek/ > [aj km bek]
I can count /aj kn kawnt/> [aj k kawnt]
RULE: In English, the phoneme /n/ is
pronounced (at least, for most speakers in
casual speech at a normal speed) as:
[m] before a bilabial consonant ([p], [b])
[] before a velar consonant ([k], [g])
[n] elsewhere
[The phonetic transcriptions above are slightly different (nothing crucial!) from
those in your assigned extra phonology reading, but they are consistent with
your textbook.]
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

7
NATURAL CLASSES

What happens with /n/ also happens with /t/ and
/d/:
hat trick /ht trik/ > [ht trik]
hit batsman /hit btsmon/ > [hip btsmon]
night class /najt kls/ >[najk kls]
bad dream /bd drim/ >[bd drim]
head band /hd bnd/ >[hb bnd]
bad guy /bd gaj/ >[b gaj]

We say that the phonemes /t/, /d/ and /n/
form a natural class in English because their
most common allophones (=the allophones
that occur elsewhere, that is [t], [d], [n]
respectively) share one or more phonetic
properties, which no other sounds of English
shares: [t], [d] and [n] are all the alveolar
stops (oral or nasal) that are found in
English.
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

8
RULE:
EXAMPLE 3 - GENERALIZED

RULE: In English, the natural class of
phonemes that are (pronounced in most
environments as) nasal or non-nasal alveolar
stops are pronounced as:
labial stops before a bilabial consonant
velar stops before a velar consonant
alveolar stops elsewhere

- Chance in place of articulation
- No change in manner of articulation
- No change in voicing
- No change in nasality
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

9
TYPES OF
PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSES/RULES

Theres a limited number of phonological
processes used in languages.
We can represent these phonological
processes as rules using a formal
notation.
Some of these processes/rules are
obligatory (they apply whenever the right
environment occurs) some other are
optional (they may apply or not when the
right environment occurs) and may
depend on factors like how fast or how
formal one speaks (see examples below).
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

10
ASSIMILATION
Assimilation is the phonological process according
to which if a phoneme (mental object) in a language
is realized by two or more allophones (sounds), then
the allophone that is chosen is the one that is more
similar to a close or adjacent sound. Assimilation
could involve voicing, place of articulation, manner
of articulation, tongue height, lip rounding, etc.

EXAMPLE: English vowel nasalization
(obligatory rule)
Phonemes (mental objects) that are pronounced as
vowels have two allophones (sounds) in English: a
nasal one and a non-nasal one. The nasal allophone
is produced right before a nasal consonant
Pam /pm/ > [p'm]
Pat /pt/ > [p't]
[For completeness, I also added the aspiration of the initial stop
consonant, which is an example of strengthening (see below)]

LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

11
DISSIMILATION
Dissimilation is the phonological process
according to which a phoneme (mental object) is
realized by the allophone (sound) of another
phoneme that is more dissimilar (in place or
manner of articulation) to a close or adjacent
sound:

amphitheater
/ mfitr / >[mpitr]
[f] and []: same manner of art. (fricative)
[p] and []: different manner of art. (stop and
fricative)

etcetera
/ctscIoro/ >[ckscIoro]
[I] and [s]: same place of art. (alveolar)
[k] and [s]: different place of art. (velar and
alveolar)

LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

12
INSERTION
Insertion is the phonological process
according to which a phone that is not
present at the phonemic level (=as a
phoneme/mental object) is added to the
phonetic form (=actual pronunciation) of a
word.

EXAMPLE: In English, it is possible (but not
obligatory!) to insert a stop between a nasal
consonant and [s] (optional rule):
hamster /hmstr/ >[hmpstr]
prince /prins/ >[prints]
presence /przns/ >[prznts]
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

13
DELETION
Deletion is the phonological process
according to which a phoneme (mental
object) that is present in the phonemic level
(mental level) is not pronounced, that is it is
not associated to any allophone (sound) at
the phonetic level.

EXAMPLE: English phonemes that would be
pronounced as unstressed vowels may be
(optionally!) deleted if found between an
obstruent (stop, fricative, affricate) and a
liquid ([l], [r]) (optional rule):
police /plis/ > [plis]
believe /bliv/ > [bliv]
parade /pred/ > [pred]
terrific /trifik/ > [trifik]
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

14
WEAKENING
Weakening is the phonological process
according to which a phoneme is realized by
a weaker allophone (=shorter or produced
with more open constriction).
EXAMPLE: flapping
In many varieties of English, a phoneme that would
elsewhere be pronounced as an alveolar oral stop is
realized as a flap [] (briefly hitting the alveolar
ridge with the tongue tip), if it is pronounced
between two vowels the first of which is stressed:
atom [ron]
Italy [iroli]

Stress matters! Compare:
atomic [oIonik], not *[oronik]
Italian [oIl]on], not *[orl]on]

LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

15


Why did this student get the wrong class
material?
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

16
STRENGTHENING
Strengthening is the phonological process
according to which a phoneme is realized by
a stronger allophone (=longer or produced
with less open constriction).

EXAMPLE: aspiration of voiceless stops in
word initial position in English is a form of
strengthening:
tip /tip/ > [t'ip]
pet /pt/ > [p't]
cat /kt/ > [k't]

Notice: /t/, /p/, k/ form a natural class in English
since their most common allophones ([t], [p], and
[k], respectively) are all and the only voiceless
stops in English.
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

17
PROSODIC OR SUPRASEGMENTAL
FEATURES

Features of phones/sounds that are over and
above (=independent from) manner and place
of articulation and from voicing:
1) length
2) stress
3) pitch and tone


LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

18
1. Length
Consonants and vowels can have different length
(=duration of their pronunciation).
bean [bi:n] The pronunciation of [i] in bean lasts a bit longer than [i]
bin [bin]
[:] is the IPA symbol that means long.
In some languages (but not in English), length is
contrastive (=there are minimal pairs that only
differ from the length of one sound):
Examples of contrastive length with vowels:
[biru] building J APANESE
[bi:ru] beer
Examples of contrastive length with consonants:
[Iapan] I kill FINNISH
[Iap:an] I meet
[pala] shovel ITALIAN
[pal:a] ball
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

19
2. Stress
In some languages, certain syllables are louder,
slightly higher in pitch (see below), and
somewhat longer in duration than other syllable
in the word. They are stressed syllables.
incite [insa]I]
insight [insa]I]
digest [da]dcsI]
digest [da]dcsI]
[ is the IPA symbol that precedes the syllable(s) of a word that
are stressed]
Change in stress can trigger vowel and
consonant changes in English:
photograph [lorogrl]
photography [loIagroli]
Italy [iroli]
Italian [oIl]on]
LIGN 101 Lectures7 Phonology Caponigro

20
3. Pitch and tone
The pitch depends on how fast the vocal folds
vibrate: the faster they vibrate, the higher the
pitch. If the larynx is small, like in women and
children, the shorter vocal folds vibrate faster
and the pitch is higher.
There are languages in which a change in the
pitch on a syllable is contrastive, that is it
produces two or more words with different
meaning. These languages are called tone
languages. Most languages are tone languages,
for instance, Mandarin Chinese, Burmese
(spoken in Burma), Thai, Nupe (spoken in
Nigeria).
Mandarin Chinese
[ma] high mother
[ma] high rising hemp
[ma] low falling rising horse
[ma] high falling scold

You might also like