You are on page 1of 26

| DPP16~1!

440 1026 02013004 11:29 am REVISED PROOF |

Development and Psychopathology 16 ~2004!, 43–68


Copyright © 2004 Cambridge University Press
Printed in the United States of America
DOI: 10.10170S0954579404040404

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child


relationship in the toddler years:
Antecedents and consequences

NICHD EARLY CHILD CARE RESEARCH NETWORK

Abstract
The purpose of this study was to examine child, maternal, and family antecedents of children’s early affect
dysregulation within the mother–child relationship and later cognitive and socioemotional correlates of affect
dysregulation. Children’s affect dysregulation at 24 and 36 months was defined in the context of mother–child
interactions in semistructured play and toy cleanup. Dyads were classified as dysregulated at each age based on high
negative affect. Affect dysregulation was associated with less maternal sensitivity and stimulation, more maternal
depressive symptoms, and lower family income over the first 36 months of life. Children with early negative mood,
lower Bayley Mental Development Index scores and insecure-avoidant ~15 months! or insecure-resistant attachment
classifications ~36 months! were more likely to be in an affect-dysregulated group. Controlling for family and child
variables, affect-dysregulated children had more problematic cognitive, social, and behavioral outcomes at 54
months, kindergarten, and first grade. The findings are discussed in terms of the early role played by parents in
assisting children with affect regulation, the reciprocal nature of parent–child interactions, and the contribution of
affect regulation to children’s later cognitive, social, and behavioral competence.

The first 3 years of life are an important time ars increasingly argue that self-regulation in
in the development of a child’s emotional and infancy is best conceptualized as a quality of
behavioral self-regulation ~Thompson, 1998!. the infant–caregiver relationship, rather than
Although the literature often refers to self- as a characteristic of the infant alone. Sroufe
regulation as a characteristic of the child, schol- ~1996! noted that the role of the caregiver in

This study is directed by a Steering Committee and sup- Wellesley College; Kathleen McCartney, Harvard Univer-
ported by NICHD through a cooperative agreement ~U10!, sity; Marion O’Brien, University of Kansas; Margaret Tre-
which calls for scientific collaboration between the grant- sch Owen, University of Texas, Dallas; Chris Payne,
ees and the NICHD staff. Participating investigators, listed University of North Carolina, Greensboro; Deborah
in alphabetical order, are Virginia Allhusen, University of Phillips, National Research Council, Washington, DC;
California, Irvine; Jay Belsky, University of London; Cath- Robert Pianta, University of Virginia; Wendy Robeson,
ryn L. Booth, University of Washington; Robert Bradley, Wellesley College; Susan Spieker, University of Wash-
University of Arkansas, Little Rock; Celia A. Brownell, ington; Deborah Lowe Vandell, University of Wisconsin,
University of Pittsburgh; Margaret Burchinal, University Madison; and Marsha Weinraub, Temple University. The
of North Carolina, Chapel Hill; Susan B. Campbell, Uni- authors express appreciation to the study coordinators at
versity of Pittsburgh; K. Alison Clarke–Stewart, Univer- each site who supervised the data collection, the research
sity of California, Irvine; Martha Cox, University of North assistants who collected the data, and especially the fam-
Carolina, Chapel Hill; Sarah L. Friedman, NICHD, Be- ilies and child care providers who welcomed the authors
thesda, Maryland; Kathyrn Hirsh–Pasek, Temple Univer- into their homes and workplaces and cooperated will-
sity; Aletha Huston, University of Texas, Austin; Elizabeth ingly with repeated requests for information.
Jaeger, Temple University; Jean F. Kelly, University of Address correspondence and reprint requests to:
Washington; Bonnie Knoke, Research Triangle Institute, NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, NICHD,
Research Triangle, North Carolina; Nancy Marshall, 6100 Executive Blvd., 4A01, Rockville, MD 20852.

43
| DPP16~1! 440 2026 02013004 11:29 am REVISED PROOF |

44 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

the infant’s regulation of emotion is so funda- izations of children’s early problem behavior
mental that researchers have used terms such is whether and in what contexts they are un-
as “co-regulation” ~Fogel, 1993! and “mutual able to control negative affect ~Thompson,
regulation” ~Tronick, 1989!. These terms de- 1994, 1998!, and thus, for example, show ex-
scribe the process whereby the child’s self- treme levels of anger and defiance, or affect
regulation arises out of the patterns of arousal dysregulation, in interactions with their care-
and affect modulation repeatedly experienced givers. Children’s inability to control negative
in early caregiving relationships. affect in early interactions with their caregiv-
Both the goodness of fit model of Thomas ers may forecast continuing difficulties with
and Chess ~1977! and attachment theory affective regulation across multiple contexts.
~Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Emotions and emotion regulation are increas-
Bowlby, 1968; Carlson & Sroufe, 1995! em- ingly conceptualized as functional in all of the
phasize the importance of the child’s relation- child’s transactions with the world ~Saarni,
ship with the primary caregiver as central to Mumme, & Campos, 1998!, including the
understanding the developmental processes physical world, and development of emotion
leading from early affective arousal and atten- regulation entails coordinating and regulating
tional control to later functioning. Thomas and affective responding across multiple domains
Chess ~1977! and Sameroff and Chandler ~Dodge, 1989!. Hence, problems in early af-
~1975! argued that the caregiver–child rela- fect regulation may disrupt optimal cognitive
tionship emerges as a function of ongoing development. Furthermore, as children enter
transactions in which each member of the dyad school, both their adjustment and their achieve-
changes the other’s behaviors and expec- ment depend in part on their ability to manage
tations in an ongoing feedback loop. The the emotional and regulatory demands of the
quality of the dyadic relationship over time, classroom. This may include coping with bore-
including children’s mood, compliance with dom, frustration, or anxiety, as well as dealing
maternal requests, and ability to use affect to with the interpersonal requirements of the
communicate security needs, appears to be an classroom setting, including impulse control
important component of individual differ- and regulation of interpersonal affect. Be-
ences in children’s ability to regulate negative cause the roots of emotional and interpersonal
affect and may have long-term implications. regulation lie in the caregiver–child dyad ~Carl-
During the first 3 years of life, children are son & Sroufe, 1995; Kopp, 1989; Sroufe,
navigating several successive stage-salient 1996!, affect dysregulation in this context may
tasks related to self-regulation. These include be a precursor of both socioemotional and cog-
establishing physiological regulation, syn- nitive outcomes.
chronizing dyadic interaction, forming at- In the current report we use measures col-
tachments to caregivers, learning to manage lected as part of the NICHD Study of Early
impulses, establishing autonomy, and internal- Child Care to identify aspects of affect dys-
izing norms and standards for social inter- regulation that take into account the child’s
action ~Cicchetti, Toth, Bush, & Gillespie, behavior in relation to mother across different
1988; Kopp, 1982, 1989; Sroufe, 1979!. Most interactive contexts. These contexts include
children negotiate these developmental tasks minimally stressful semistructured play ses-
relatively smoothly, although some are seen sions and a potentially more stressful but
as showing brief periods of disruptive or shorter clean up after play episodes. We de-
noncompliant behavior ~Campbell, 2002!. fine children’s affect dysregulation in the con-
However, for a small group of children these text of the mother–child relationship at 24 and
difficult transitions may escalate into nega- 36 months with these measures and then ex-
tive, coercive exchanges with caregivers that amine the developmental antecedents and
are seen as a potential precursor of later be- sequelae of varying degrees of affect dysreg-
havior problems ~Bates, Pettit, & Dodge, 1995; ulation. More specifically, we examine how
Campbell, 2002; Campbell, Shaw, & Gilliom, well earlier aspects of the mother–child rela-
2000!. A common theme in most conceptual- tionship and family context predict affect dys-
| DPP16~1! 440 3026 02013004 11:29 am REVISED PROOF |

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in toddlers 45

regulation in the second and third years, and a temperament perspective and the search for
how well affect dysregulation in the mother– individual difference parameters that appear
child relationship predicts later child function- to predict later socioemotional functioning and
ing in the social and cognitive domains. Most to differentiate between well-adjusted and
existing work addresses either antecedents or poorly adjusted children ~Bates, Maslin, &
sequelae of affect regulation, but seldom both. Frankel, 1985; Kochanska, 1995; Rothbart &
And few studies have operationalized affect Bates, 1998!. In general, studies of stable, bi-
regulation dyadically despite the strong theo- ologically based individual differences in in-
retical grounds for doing so. fants and toddlers have emphasized reactivity
We adopted a relationship-based approach and self-regulation that reflect variations in
in defining affect dysregulation because it ap- irritability, impulsivity, resistance to control,
pears to capture a potentially important and fearfulness, and attentional processes ~Roth-
developmentally salient aspect of the child’s bart & Bates, 1998!. A growing body of evi-
emerging self-control. Children’s negative af- dence indicates that high levels of negative
fect when it occurs in the context of the affect, impulsivity, and irritability in infancy
caregiver–child relationship may be an espe- and toddlerhood predict later behavior prob-
cially important marker of potential problems lems ~Campbell et al., 2000; Keenan, Shaw,
with affect regulation. This is because the care- Delliquadri, Giovannelli, & Walsh, 1998; San-
giver is usually expected to be a source of sup- son, Oberklaid, Pedlow, & Prior, 1991!. How-
port in situations in which children have ever, this relationship is not always clear or
difficulty managing their own negative emo- direct ~e.g., Bates, Pettit, Dodge, & Ridge,
tions. Strong or persistent negative affect in the 1998; Belsky, Hsieh & Crnic, 1998!. Not all
context of the caregiver–child relationship may difficult infants evidence behavior problems
reflect a lack of mutual cooperation, harmony, in the preschool years, and there are often com-
or support as toddlers and preschoolers grap- plex interactions between child characteristics
ple with stage-salient issues such as establish- and aspects of parental behavior that together
ing autonomy and learning to cooperate with predict which difficult children will and will
others. Affect dysregulation in the caregiver– not show problem behavior at later ages. Pre-
child relationship may partly reflect the qual- diction from early temperamental characteris-
ity of maternal scaffolding and limit setting tics to later functioning requires complex
during a developmental period when young chil- transactional models that take into account the
dren still depend on adults to help them control child’s reactivity and regulation as well as par-
arousal and negative emotion, despite their de- enting behavior.
sire to become independent. Thus, caregivers Several recent studies provide support for
of young children need to strike a balance in this transactional perspective by demonstrat-
responding to their toddlers’ competing de- ing that temperamental characteristics of the
mands for autonomy and support. Some care- child and features of maternal parenting style
givers may be better able to help their young interact to predict outcomes. Belsky, Wood-
children learn to use and manage potent af- worth, and Crnic ~1996! characterized toddler
fects, whereas others may be less skilled. The boys and their families as having difficulties
children of less skillful caregivers may be more with the “terrible twos” on the basis of both
angry and frustrated and use affect less ef- children’s defiance in response to parental con-
fectively within the relationship. From this trol attempts and parents’ use of negative con-
perspective, then, young children’s ability to trol. In roughly 20% of the sample, parent–
regulate negative emotion may be seen as child dyads engaged in escalating negative
emerging from the early caregiver–child rela- control attempts across the second year. Moth-
tionship, evident in child mood and behavior ers, fathers, and caregivers rated boys in this
as well as qualities of the caregiver–child rela- group higher on externalizing symptoms at
tionship, including attachment security. age 3. Follow-up of this sample revealed an
Interest in young children’s affect regula- interaction between child characteristics and
tion derives from several perspectives. One is maternal behavior in predicting later behavior
| DPP16~1! 440 4026 02013004 11:29 am REVISED PROOF |

46 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

problems ~Belsky et al., 1998!. Maternal neg- pliance and defiance characteristic of the
ative affect and intrusiveness in the second terrible twos, have tended to emphasize child
and third years of life were more strongly pre- characteristics ~Braungart–Rieker & Stifter,
dictive of later behavior problems among irri- 1996; Calkins, 1994! that are then examined
table than nonirritable toddlers. Toddlers who as a function of different parenting strategies.
were more positive in mood were less suscep- Three recent studies, however, have specifi-
tible to the influence of negative maternal be- cally examined dyadic regulation of affect as
havior. Shaw, Keenan, and Vondra ~1994! also a predictor of early behavior problems ~Den-
showed that negative and irritable children with ham, Workman, Cole, Weissbrod, Kendziora,
less flexible or sensitive mothers were more & Zahn–Waxler, 2000; Leadbeater, Bishop, &
likely to have difficulties controlling their be- Raver, 1996; Martin, Clements, & Crnic,
havior and affect than irritable toddlers with 2001!. In all three studies, when parent–child
more responsive and supportive mothers. Con- dyads were characterized by negativity, anger,
versely, other research suggests that positive and conflict when children were 2 years old,
parenting interacts with children’s irritability the children were more likely to exhibit dis-
or fearfulness to predict positive outcomes ruptive, externalizing behavior problems later
~Pettit & Bates, 1989; Kochanska, 1995!. A as preschoolers.
growing body of work is thus emerging to sup- In summary, affect regulation is a construct
port the idea that child characteristics inter- that generally encompasses behaviors and strat-
act with aspects of parenting to predict early egies that children use to control and commu-
signs of behavior problems, especially exter- nicate affect and arousal, especially negative
nalizing problems reflecting poor emotional affect ~Calkins, 1994; Kopp, 1989; Thomp-
control, or to protect irritable children from son, 1994, 1998!. Affect regulation is increas-
potentially negative outcomes ~e.g., Bates et al., ingly conceptualized as an important early
1998; Kochanska, 1995!. emerging skill with implications for both
Attachment theorists also consider the qual- social and cognitive development ~Calkins,
ity of the attachment relationship as having im- 1994!. This is because both constructive so-
plications for how well children learn to manage cial interactions and the ability to learn from
negative affect ~Carlson & Sroufe, 1995!. The the environment are likely to be facilitated
experience of a satisfying, reciprocal relation- when young children can modulate attention,
ship that meets the child’s needs for comfort and arousal, and negative affect to engage in re-
security leads the child to value the relation- warding and enriching social and cognitive ac-
ship and become more amenable to socializa- tivities. Conversely, children who are highly
tion efforts. For example, Kochanska ~1995, aroused and unable to modulate negative emo-
1997! suggested that caregiver–child mutual- tions such as anger and frustration are likely
ity and cooperation, partly reflected in secure to engage in fewer positive social interactions
attachment, may render some children more sus- with either adults or peers. They also may have
ceptible to the internalization of parental val- a more difficult time focusing attention in ways
ues and expectations, as well as more willing that enhance the acquisition of cognitive and
to comply with maternal requests. Thus, chil- language skills ~Rothbart & Bates, 1998; Roth-
dren should be better able to manage arousal bart, Posner, & Hershey, 1995!.
and negative affect in the context of a secure Based on this review of the importance of
attachment relationship. In contrast, insecurely the caregiver–child relationship context for
attached children, who presumably have less children’s emerging emotion regulation, we de-
harmonious relationships with their mothers, fined children’s affect dysregulation on the
may be less easily socialized and more likely to basis of child behavior observed in two age-
engage in negative behavior with their mothers appropriate, interactional contexts with mother
even in relatively undemanding situations that at 24 and 36 months. These ages were chosen
require cooperation and shared goals. because they represent a period when chil-
Studies of individual differences in chil- dren’s relationship with mother has stabilized
dren’s affect regulation skills, especially com- into a predictable dyadic partnership and when
| DPP16~1! 440 5026 02013004 11:29 am REVISED PROOF |

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in toddlers 47

the challenge of balancing needs for auton- and first grade as a function of earlier affect
omy and dependence is at its peak. We se- dysregulation. We expected that children who
lected situations that differed in the degree of showed no affect dysregulation would func-
challenge to the dyad. However, they both rely tion most competently on outcome measures.
on dyadic cooperation, mutuality, and a sense Although our primary focus on the anteced-
of shared goals. The situations included an un- ents of affect dysregulation in the mother–
demanding, semistructured play interaction and child relationship was on child and family
a potentially more emotionally charged com- characteristics, we also examined whether the
pliance task in which mothers requested that quantity or quality of child care experienced
children clean up a set of appealing toys with by children accounted for any associations be-
which they had been playing. At each age, we tween affect dysregulation and other child0
defined affect dysregulation in the mother– family variables, including children’s outcomes
child relationship on the basis of the child’s at 54 months and school age.
observed negative affect during play with
mother, or child negative affect and defiance
Method
during cleanup. Because the mother–child play
situation was undemanding and positive for
Participants
most dyads, child negative affect in this set-
ting was considered an important indicator of Participants in the NICHD Study of Early Child
poor control of emotion directed at mother, Care were recruited from hospitals near the
and possibly an indicator of a more generally following locations throughout 1991: Little
negative mother–child relationship. During toy Rock, Arkansas; Irvine, California; Lawrence,
cleanup, although some degree of resistance Kansas; Boston, Massachusetts; Philadelphia,
may be normative ~Crockenberg & Litman, Pennsylvania; Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania;
1990; NICHD Early Child Care Research Net- Charlottesville, Virginia; Morganton, North
work, 1998!, high levels of defiance and neg- Carolina; Seattle, Washington; and Madison,
ative affect in this context also signal a poorly Wisconsin. Potential participants were se-
regulated mother–child relationship in which lected from among 8,986 mothers giving birth
conflict over disparate goals overshadows mu- during selected 24-hr sampling periods to de-
tual cooperation and harmony. We expected termine their eligibility for the study. Subjects
that most children would show no signs of were excluded from the sample if ~a! the
affect dysregulation in the mother–child rela- mother was under 18, ~b! the mother was not
tionship at either age. When affect dysregula- conversant in English, ~c! the family planned
tion was observed, we expected that it would to move, ~d! the child was hospitalized for
be associated with earlier and concurrent as- more than 7 days after birth or had obvious
pects of child behavior and family context. In disabilities, ~e! the mother had a known or an
particular, we expected that children who ev- acknowledged substance-abuse problem, or ~f !
idenced no affect dysregulation would have the family lived at considerable distance from
families that could more easily support the de- the site or in a location that posed a danger to
velopmental needs of young children and that home visitors. Of those contacted in the initial
this would be reflected in greater demographic sampling periods, 5,265 met the eligibility cri-
and psychosocial resources ~maternal educa- teria for the study and agreed to be contacted
tion, financial resources, maternal mood, and after their return home from the hospital. A
maternal language skills! and more sensitive subset of this group was selected in accor-
and stimulating maternal caregiving. These dance with a conditionally random sampling
children would be more likely to have been plan.
characterized as less difficult in infancy, more When the infants were 1 month old, a total
advanced cognitively, and securely attached of 1,364 families with healthy newborns were
at 15 months. enrolled in the study. The recruited families
We also examined children’s cognitive and came from a wide range of socioeconomic and
social outcomes at 54 months, kindergarten, sociocultural backgrounds, and included 24%
| DPP16~1! 440 6026 02013004 11:29 am REVISED PROOF |

48 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

ethnic-minority children, 11% low-education tioned in the analyses. We first present mea-
mothers ~less than high school education!, and sures used to create affect-dysregulation
14% single-parent mothers. ~Note that these variables at 24 and 36 months, and then
percentages are not mutually exclusive.! The describe how different affect-dysregulation
recruited families did not differ significantly groups were constructed. Next we discuss the
from other families eligible to participate on child and family variables used to describe the
major demographic variables, except that the characteristics of the affect-dysregulation
mothers in the study were slightly ~4%! more groups at 24 and 36 months, including vari-
likely to report that they planned to be em- ables that will be used as covariates in the
ployed compared with the nonparticipating analyses of subsequent child social and cog-
mothers. Of the mothers recruited, 53% nitive outcomes at 54 months, kindergarten,
planned to work full time ~.30 hr0week!, 23% and Grade 1. Finally, we describe measures
part time ~10–30 hr0week!, and 24% did not used to assess social and cognitive outcomes
intend to be employed during the child’s first at 54 months, kindergarten, and Grade 1.
year.
The sample was not designed to be nation-
Affect dysregulation
ally representative. However, we compared the
demographic characteristics of the sample with Affect dysregulation was assessed during two
those of people living in the same census tracts laboratory playroom observations of mother–
and nationally. The sample was similar to fam- child interaction, meant to index aspects of
ilies in the census tract records and the nation their relationship at 24 and 36 months: a toy
as a whole on key demographic variables cleanup activity and a semistructured mother–
~household income and ethnicity!. child play interaction.

Lab cleanup. At the end of a 15-min toy play


Overview of data collection
period which opened the lab sessions at 24
Data for this report were collected from the and 36 months, the visit coordinator handed
time the child was 1 month of age through the mother containers for the toys and in-
first grade. When the child was 1 month of structed her to have the child participate in
age, basic demographic information on the picking up the toys; no other directions were
child and family was gathered and mothers given to mother ~see NICHD Early Child Care
completed questionnaires during a home Research Network, 1998, for details!. Clean
visit. Data on child-care usage were collected up lasted no more than 5 min. Child behaviors
through phone calls at 3-month intervals, up coded on a 5-point scale from 1 5 not at all
to age 3 years and at 4-month intervals there- characteristic to 5 5 very characteristic were:
after, and in face to face contacts with moth- compliance, assertive noncompliance, defiant
ers at 1, 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months and noncompliance, passive noncompliance, pos-
Grade 1. Data were gathered in the laboratory itive affect, and negative affect. Only the de-
and in the home on children’s behavioral func- fiant noncompliance and negative affect scores
tioning, mother’s psychological functioning, were used. A rating of $3 on both of these
mother–child interaction, and the quality of scales at one age point was required for a child
the home environment when children were 6, to be assigned to the affect-dysregulation group
15, 24, 36, and 54 months of age, and in the at that age. The intraclass correlation ~Winer,
spring of Grade 1. Teachers also completed 1971! of intercoder reliabilities of the defiant
questionnaires about the child in kindergarten noncompliant and negative affect scales were
and Grade 1. .71 and .81 at 24 months, and .83 and .81 at 36
months.
Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for
Overview of measures
the selected lab cleanup ratings. Means, and
This presentation of measures is organized to standard deviations for the defiant noncompli-
reflect the ways in which the variables func- ance and negative affect ratings are presented,
| DPP16~1! 440 7026 02013004 11:29 am REVISED PROOF |

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in toddlers 49

Table 1. Measures of dyadic affect dysregulation

Child Age

24 Months 36 Months

Lab Clean Up

Defiance M ~SD! 1.13 ~0.51! 1.16 ~0.54!


Low ~ratings of 1–2! n 989 ~97%! 979 ~97%!
High ~ratings of 3–5! n 29 ~3%! 31 ~3%!
Negative affect M ~SD! 1.15 ~0.51! 1.20 ~0.56!
Low ~ratings of 1–2! n 994 ~98%! 971 ~96%!
High ~ratings of 3–5! n 24 ~2%! 39 ~4%!

Mother–Child Interaction

24-month negative affect M ~SD! 1.43 ~0.70!


Low ~ratings of 1–2! n 926 ~91%!
High ~ratings of 3– 4! n 95 ~9%!
36-month negative affect M ~SD! 1.66 ~1.08!
Low ~ratings of 1–3! n 953 ~94%!
High ~rating of 4–7! n 62 ~6%!

Note: The criteria for high negative affect in the mother–child interactions differ
over age because the ratings of interactions could range from 1 to 4 at 24 months
and from 1 to 7 at 36 months.

along with the proportion of children display- sive negative affect in interaction with their
ing low and high levels of negativity. Rela- mother. At 24 months, extensive negative af-
tively few children showed high levels of both fect was indicated by scores of 3– 4, and at
defiance and negative affect. As shown in 36 months by scores of 4–7. Table 1 shows
Table 2, only 17 children were high in both the mean and standard deviation of negative
defiance and negative affect during toy cleanup affect at each of the two ages and the propor-
at 24 months, whereas 7 children showed high tion of children classified as displaying exten-
levels of negative affect alone and 12 children sive negativity during interactions with their
showed high levels of defiance alone. Only 21 mothers. As shown in this table, 95 children
children showed high levels of both defiance met this criterion at 24 months and 62 at 36
and negative affect during toy cleanup at 36 months.
months. Children were classified as dysregulated
if they showed high levels of negative affect
Mother–child interaction. Mother–child inter- during interaction with the mother or if they
action was videotaped in semistructured 15- showed high levels of both defiance and
min play observations at both ages ~see NICHD negative affect during the toy cleanup. The
Early Child Care Research Network, 1999 for bottom half of Table 2 displays the num-
details!. At 24 months child negative mood was bers of children meeting these criteria at both
rated on a 4-point scale.At 36 months, child neg- ages. As the table shows, a total of 105 chil-
ativity, which paralleled the 24-month rating of dren were classified as dysregulated at 24
child negative mood, was rated on a 7-point months because they displayed high levels of
scale. Winer ~1971! intraclass correlations, as- negative affect during both interaction and
sessing interrater reliability for this scale, were cleanup ~n 5 6!, in interaction alone ~n 5
.88 and .85 for 24 and 36 months, respectively. 89!, or during cleanup alone ~n 5 10!. At 36
Children were assigned to the affect- months, 77 children were classified as
dysregulation group if they displayed exten- dysregulated.
| DPP16~1! 440 8026 02013004 11:29 am REVISED PROOF |

50 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

Table 2. Number of children displaying outcome variables; child gender was added as
affect dysregulation a covariate because of its association with the
outcome measures.!
During Lab Cleanup Maternal education was represented by five
24 Months 36 Months
levels as determined at 1 month: 1 5 less than
high school, 2 5 high school graduate, 3 5
Negative Affect Negative Affect some college, 4 5 college graduate, and 5 5
graduate education.
Defiance No Yes No Yes Maternal vocabulary was assessed with the
No ~n! 982 7 961 18 Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test— Revised
Yes ~n! 12 17 10 21 ~PPVT-R; Dunn & Dunn, 1981!. The PPVT
assesses verbal intelligence operationalized as
Mother–Child Interactions receptive knowledge of vocabulary. Scores are
24 Months 36 Months standardized with a mean of 100 and standard
deviation of 15. Scores are moderately corre-
Negative Affect Negative Affect lated with standard measures of verbal intelli-
gence such as the WAIS Vocabulary subscale
Lab Cleanup No Yes No Yes
~Weschler, 1981!.
No ~n) 911 89 925 57 Maternal personality was assessed using
Yes ~n) 10 6 18 2 three scales of the NEO Personality Inventory
~NEO PI; Costa & McCrae, 1985! obtained at
Note: The children who were high on both Negative Af- 1 month. The three NEO scales were neuroti-
fect and Defiance were classified as dysregulated during
lab cleanup; children considered dysregulated by either cism, the extent to which the mother indicated
criterion were classified as dysregulated. she is anxious, hostile, and depressed ~re-
versed!; extraversion, the extent to which she
is sociable, fun-loving, and optimistic; and
agreeableness, the extent to which she is trust-
ing, helpful, and forgiving. Thus, higher scores
Family and child characteristics
indicated better adjustment.
The following variables were used as family Maternal depressive symptoms were mea-
and child predictors or correlates of affect dys- sured using the Center for Epidemiological
regulation in univariate analyses ~Table 3!: ma- Studies Depression Scale ~CES-D; Radloff,
ternal education, vocabulary, personality, and 1977! administered at each home visit ~1, 6,
depressive symptoms; partner in the home; in- 15, 24, 36, and 54 months, and Grade 1!. Cron-
come; caregiving quality; child gender and eth- bach’s alpha for the CES-D across the data
nicity ~Black, Hispanic, other, or White!; collection points ranged from .88 to .91. The
6-month negative mood; 6-month tempera- depression scores had moderately strong cor-
ment; 15-month cognitive functioning; 15-, 24-, relations over time ~.46 to .58!.
and 36-month attachment; and amount and Partner in the home was reported by moth-
quality of child care. Variables significantly ers at each interview, and the proportion of
associated with affect dysregulation at the time a partner was in residence was computed
univariate level were included in a single multi- for each family from 6 to 54 months.
variate analysis predicting to dysregulation sta- Income was the income0needs ratio com-
tus ~Table 4! and were also covariates in the puted from maternal interview items col-
analyses predicting child outcomes ~Table 5!. lected at each home visit. Family income,
~Exceptions were child variables from assess- excluding AFDC, was divided by the appro-
ments after age 6 months, to avoid overcon- priate poverty threshold, determined by the
trolling for child outcomes; and child care year in which the income was earned and by
quality, because it was not available for the household size. This variable was averaged
full sample. Site was a covariate, as it was across 0 –54 months to create an average
complexly associated with both predictor and income0needs ratio.
| DPP16~1! 440 9026 02013004 11:29 am REVISED PROOF |

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in toddlers 51

Table 3. Comparisons on selected family and child variables for affect-dysregulated groups

Dysregulated 24 Months Dysregulated 36 Months

No Yes No Yes
N 5 917 N 5 106 N 5 942 N 5 81

Maternal Characteristics

Maternal education a 14.6 ~2.4! 13.7 ~2.4! 14.5 ~2.4! 13.9 ~2.6!
Maternal receptive vocabulary b,c 100.3 ~18.3! 93.7 ~18.5! 100.2 ~18.2! 92.2 ~19.6!
Maternal agreeableness at 6 months b 46.7 ~5.2! 44.8 ~4.7! 46.6 ~5.1! 45.4 ~5.9!
Maternal extraversion at 6 months 42.6 ~5.8! 41.3 ~5.9! 42.5 ~5.8! 41.8 ~5.7!
Maternal neuroticism at 6 months d 29.5 ~7.0! 31.3 ~7.1! 29.6 ~7.0! 30.6 ~7.4!
Maternal depression ~CESD score! c,d
6 months 8.6 ~7.9! 11.3 ~9.5! 8.6 ~7.8! 11.5 ~10.5!
15 months 8.8 ~8.0! 10.0 ~8.7! 8.8 ~8.0! 10.3 ~9.2!
24 months 9.1 ~8.3! 12.0 ~10.3! 9.1 ~8.2! 11.9 ~11.6!
36 months 8.9 ~8.1! 12.0 ~10.7! 8.9 ~8.1! 12.6 ~11.3!

Family Characteristics

Partner 6–54 months d,e 0.87 ~0.28! 0.75 ~0.38! 0.87 ~0.28! 0.76 ~0.39!
Income0poverty threshold a
6 months 3.9 ~3.2! 3.1 ~2.4! 3.9 ~3.1! 3.2 ~3.3!
15 months 3.9 ~3.3! 3.0 ~2.4! 3.8 ~3.2! 3.4 ~3.1!
24 months 3.9 ~3.0! 2.9 ~2.4! 3.8 ~2.9! 3.7 ~3.8!
36 months 3.8 ~3.1! 2.8 ~2.6! 3.7 ~3.0! 3.3 ~3.8!
Maternal caregiving a, f,g
6 months 0.09 ~0.80! 20.31 ~0.90! 0.07 ~0.79! 20.27 ~1.01!
15 months 0.10 ~0.78! 20.28 ~0.86! 0.08 ~0.78! 20.20 ~0.96!
24 months 0.14 ~0.74! 20.68 ~0.99! 0.09 ~0.79! 20.37 ~0.95!
36 months 0.10 ~0.77! 20.48 ~1.07! 0.11 ~0.77! 20.76 ~1.06!

Child Characteristics

Male gender ~%! 50 57 51 47


Child ethnicity ~%!
Black d,e 10 20 11 21
Hispanic 6 7 6 6
Other 4 6 4 3
White d,e 80 68 80 70
Negative mood SI 6 months b,d 1.41 ~0.68! 1.64 ~0.87! 1.41 ~0.69! 1.63 ~0.82!
Carey temperament 6 months 3.16 ~0.40! 3.21 ~0.44! 3.16 ~0.39! 3.19 ~0.50!
Bayley MDI 15 months a,e 110.2 ~13.6! 100.5 ~13.9! 109.6 ~13.9! 104.7 ~14.1!
Attachment SS 15 months ~%!
Aa 12 31 13 19
Ba 64 45 63 53
C 9 9 9 8
D0U 15 14 15 19
Q-security 24 months b,c 0.31 ~0.20! 0.21 ~0.20! 0.30 ~0.20! 0.21 ~0.20!
SS 36 months ~%!
A 5 4 5 4
B 64 48 64 46
C b,c 16 25 16 28
D0U b 15 23 16 23
Hours of care0week
6 months 23.5 ~21.4! 23.4 ~21.1! 23.8 ~21.6! 19.8 ~17.9!
15 months 25.1 ~21.7! 25.4 ~18.6! 25.4 ~21.4! 22.7 ~20.6!
24 months 25.4 ~21.0! 28.2 ~21.0! 25.7 ~21.1! 25.5 ~20.1!
36 months 27.0 ~21.3! 27.5 ~21.3! 27.2 ~21.2! 24.5 ~20.2!
54 months 30.6 ~18.8! 33.7 ~20.4! 30.9 ~18.8! 31.3 ~21.8!
| DPP16~1! 440 10026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

52 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

Table 3 ~cont.!

Dysregulated 24 Months Dysregulated 36 Months

No Yes No Yes
N 5 917 N 5 106 N 5 942 N 5 81

Child Characteristics ~cont.!

Quality ~ORCE composite rating! c,d


6 months 3.0 ~0.6! 3.0 ~0.5! 3.0 ~0.6! 3.0 ~0.5!
15 months 3.0 ~0.6! 2.9 ~0.6! 3.0 ~0.6! 2.8 ~0.5!
24 months 2.8 ~0.6! 2.7 ~0.6! 2.8 ~0.6! 2.6 ~0.6!
36 months 2.8 ~0.5! 2.7 ~0.6! 2.8 ~0.5! 2.6 ~0.5!
54 months 3.0 ~0.6! 2.9 ~0.6! 3.0 ~0.6! 2.9 ~0.6!

Note: N, maximum number listed; sample sizes vary depending on the outcome examined. The site is included as a
covariate in all analyses. All values except percentages are means ~standard deviations!.
a 24-month dysregulated groups different at p , .001.
b 24-month dysregulated groups different at p , .01.
c 36-month dysregulated groups different at p , .01.
d 24-month dysregulated groups different at p , .05.
e 36-month dysregulated groups different at p , .05.
f 36-month dysregulated groups different at p , .001.
g Mean levels of maternal caregiving decrease over time for both 24- and 36-month dysregulated groups ~ p , .001!.

Mother–child interaction during play at as the sum of 45 items at 6 and 15 months, and
home at 6 months, and in a laboratory play- as the sum of 55 items at 36 and 54 months,
room at 15, and 24 months, was rated on 4-point with higher values denoting higher levels of
global rating scales developed for this study; maternal responsiveness, child stimulation, and
7-point rating scales were used for ratings of support to the child. All observers attended
36 and 54 month–month laboratory interactions. centralized training sessions before collecting
A maternal sensitivity composite composed of the data and were required to maintain relia-
the sum of sensitivity to nondistress, instrusive- bility by matching a master coder on 90% of
ness ~reverse scored!, and positive regard was items. The Infant0Toddler version was admin-
formed at 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months ~see istered at 6 and 15 months, and the Early Child-
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, hood version was administered at 36 and 54
1999, for a discussion of the adjustment to con- months. Cronbach’s alpha for the total score
vert all ratings to the same scale!. Intercoder was .76 at 6 months, .80 at 15 months, .87 at
reliability was calculated as the intraclass cor- 36 months, and .82 at 54 months. Correlations
relation ~Winer, 1971!. For the maternal sensi- across time range from .48 to .70 ~ p , .0001!.
tivity composites the intraclass correlations were A composite measure of quality of mater-
.87, .83, .84, .84, and .84 for 6, 15, 24, 36, and nal caregiving was created at 6, 15, 24, 36,
54 months, respectively. Cronbach’s alpha for and 54 months from the mean of the standard-
the composites ranged from .70 to .84. ized maternal sensitivity composite plus the
The Home Observation for Measurement standardized HOME total score.
of the Environment ~HOME; Caldwell & Bra- Infant negative mood was the 6-month
dley, 1984! assesses overall quality of the phys- 4-point child negative mood rating from vid-
ical and social resources available to a child eotaped mother–child interaction in the home
within the home. The HOME consists of both ~see NICHD Early Child Care Research Net-
direct observation and a semistructured inter- work, 1999, for details!. The intraclass corre-
view with the mother. The focus is on the child lation for this scale was .69, indicating adequate
as a recipient of stimulation from objects, interrater reliability.
events, and interactions occurring in the fam- Infant temperament was based on fifty-five
ily surroundings. The total score is computed 6-point items from the Infant Temperament
| DPP16~1! 440 11026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in toddlers 53

Table 4. Logistic regressions predicting affect-dysregulation group


from child and family characteristics

24-Month Dys. 36-Month Dys.


~n 5 919! ~n 5 904!

Maternal
Education 0.05 ~0.07! 0.15 ~0.08!
Vocabulary 20.00 ~0.01! 20.01 ~0.01!
Agreeableness 20.02 ~0.03! 0.01 ~0.03!
Neuroticism 0.00 ~0.02! 20.02 ~0.03!
Depression 6–24036 months 20.01 ~0.02! 0.03 ~0.03!
Partner in home 6–24036 months 20.07 ~0.33! 0.22 ~0.37!
Income0poverty 6–24036 months 20.00 ~0.06! 0.09 ~0.06!
Maternal caregiving 6–24036 months 20.89 ~0.25!*** 20.94 ~0.29!**
Child ethnicity ns ns
Black–White 20.54 ~0.34! 0.18 ~0.43!
Hispanic–White 0.27 ~0.40! 0.67 ~0.48!
Other–White 0.03 ~0.50! 21.05 ~0.81!
Negative mood 6 months 0.24 ~0.15! 0.16 ~0.17!
Bayley MDI 15 months 20.04 ~0.01!*** 20.01 ~0.01!
Attachment 15 months ** ns
A–B 0.76 ~0.23!** 0.08 ~0.20!
C–B 20.20 ~0.32! 20.15 ~0.38!
D–B 20.24 ~0.26! 0.20 ~0.28!
Attachment security 24 months 21.10 ~0.62! 20.77 ~0.72!
Attachment 36 months ns
A–B 20.04 ~0.50!
C–B 0.42 ~0.28!
D–B 20.22 ~0.31!
Site * ns

Note: Dys., dysregulated. The coefficients for the site are not listed. All values are B and
~standard errors!.
**p , .01. ***p , .001.

Questionnaire completed by mothers at 6 Infant–mother attachment security was as-


months ~Medoff–Cooper, Carey, & McDevitt, sessed at 15, 24, and 36 months. At 15 months
1993!. The composite measure reflected diffi- the Ainsworth et al. ~1978! Strange Situation
cult temperament and was created by calculat- procedure was conducted. Videotapes of all
ing the mean of the nonmissing items with Strange Situations were coded in a central lo-
appropriate reversal of scales, so that numer- cation and attachment security was evaluated
ically large scores consistently reflected a more by a team of three coders blind to child-care
“difficult” temperament. Cronbach’s alpha was status and other information about the child
.81. Higher scores reflect a more negative dis- and family. These coders double-coded 1,201
position: that is, a child seen as more intense, Strange Situation assessments using the stan-
less positive in mood, and less adaptable to dard attachment classifications of secure ~B!,
daily routines. insecure-avoidant ~A!, insecure-resistant ~C!,
Children’s cognitive functioning was as- disorganized ~D!, and unclassifiable ~U!. The
sessed using the original Bayley Scales of In- group viewed disagreements and a code was
fant Development ~Bayley, 1969! at 15 months assigned by consensus. Across all coder pairs,
during lab visits. The Mental Development In- before conferencing, agreement with the
dex ~MDI! represents overall cognitive level. 5-category classification system was 83%
Scores are standardized so that the mean is ~k 5 .69!. In this report the D and U classifi-
100 with a standard deviation of 15. cations are combined.
|
DPP16~1! 440
Table 5. Affect dysregulation and child outcomes from 54 months to first grade
24-Month Dysregulation 36-Month Dysregulation

Time Interaction Time Interaction

12026
Main Effect Adj. M Main Effect Adj. M

F d F 54 G0 G1 F d F 54 G0 G1

02013004
Cognitive0academic outcomes
Sample size
Not dys. 875 848 897 872
Dys. 99 97 77 73

11:30 am
Math ~WJ Applied Problems! a

6.48* 0.25* 3.03 11.48*** 0.37*** 0.22


Not dys. 427.2 472.6 427.4 472.5
Dys. 424.9 467.4 421.5 467.5

Reading ~WJ Letter–Word Identification! a

REVISED PROOF
54

3.31 0.17 2.21 4.06* 0.21* 1.18


Not dys. 373.3 455.0 373.6 454.7
Dys. 371.4 449.3 367.6 451.8

Phonological Knowledge ~WJ Incomplete Words! a

4.76* 0.19* 2.32 10.27** .32** .00


Not dys. 464.4 486.6 464.4 486.8
Dys. 460.7 485.6 460.4 482.7
Impulsivity ~CPT errors of commission log transformed!
7.62* 20.25* 0.12 4.32* 20.19 0.56
Not dys. 1.91 1.29 1.92 1.31
Dys. 2.19 1.52 2.18 1.45
Sustained attention ~CPT errors of omission log transformed!
0.61 20.07 0.02 2.78 20.17 0.64
Not dys. 1.94 0.77 1.94 0.77
Dys. 2.01 0.82 2.11 0.85
Mother rating of social skills
Sample size
Not dys. 881 857 847 895 879 871
Dys. 101 100 97 78 78 72

|
|
DPP16~1! 440
CPRS, closeness
3.95* 0.18* 0.82 2.17 0.13 0.11

13026
Not dys. 4.61 4.67 4.76 4.60 4.66 4.76
Dys. 4.54 4.61 4.73 4.57 4.62 4.71
CBCL, externalizing
4.52* 20.19* 0.35 0.60 20.10 2.12

02013004
Not dys. 51.3 50.2 48.5 51.3 50.3 48.7
Dys. 52.8 52.0 50.6 52.8 51.1 48.6
CBCL, internalizing
1.89 0.12 0.02 0.00 0 1.38
Not dys. 46.8 47.4 48.3 46.9 47.5 48.5
Dys. 48.0 48.5 49.3 47.5 47.6 47.8

11:30 am
SSRS, social skills
10.82** 0.30** 5.55* 1.20 0.11 1.29
Not Dys. 99.1 101.7 105.7 99.0 101.5 105.2
Dys. 96.7 97.9 99.8 96.6 99.8 104.7
Teacher rating of social skills
Sample size
Not dys. 588 822 820 602 843 843

REVISED PROOF
Dys. 66 96 97 52 75 73
TCRS, closeness
0.16 0.04 0.04 0.04 0.03 0.00
55

Not dys. 4.18 4.22 4.28 4.19 4.22 4.28


Dys. 4.21 4.24 4.30 4.20 4.23 4.29
TRF, externalizing
14.75*** 20.20*** .70 0.86 20.09 0.63
Not dys. 49.6 49.8 50.0 49.8 50.0 50.4
Dys. 51.9 52.5 53.4 51.1 50.8 50.5
TRF–Internalizing
0.71 1.12 1.35 0.12 0.00 0.00
Not dys. 49.2 48.8 48.2 49.2 48.8 48.3
Dys. 48.9 49.3 49.8 49.4 49.1 48.6

SSRS–Social Skills

5.87* 0.22* 0.04 5.49 .24* 1.03


Not dys. 103.6 103.9 104.0 103.5 103.8 104.2
Dys. 101.0 101.0 101.0 102.0 100.9 99.3

Note: 54, 54 months; G0, kindergarten; G1, first grade; dys., dysregulated; WJ, Woodcock–Johnson. Covariates: site, mother’s education, PPVT vocabulary,
caregiving ~maternal sensitivity 1 HomeTotal!, depressive symptoms, income, partner in the home; child gender, ethnicity, negative mood at 6 months, age at
assessment, and all interactions with age at assessment.
a Raush scores were used in the longitudinal analyses of Woodcock–Johnson subscales.

*p , .05. **p , .01. ***p , .001.

|
| DPP16~1! 440 14026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

56 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

Infant–mother attachment security was as- arrangements, scored as mean hours per week
sessed at 24 months using the Attachment during the periods 1– 6 months, 7–15 months,
Q-set. Two-hour home visits were conducted 16–24 months, 25–36 months, and 37–54
during which trained observers made notes of months. Children who experienced no routine
child behavior that was naturally occurring or nonmaternal care received a score of zero.
in response to semistructured situations intro- Quality of child care was assessed during
duced by the observer. After the visit the ob- two half-day child care visits at 6, 15, 24, and
servers used the Attachment Q-Sort ~AQS! to 36 months, and one half-day visit at 54 months,
describe security with mother ~Vaughn & Wa- using observational methods developed for this
ters, 1990; Waters & Deane, 1985!. The result- study ~Observational Record of the Caregiv-
ing profile is correlated with the profile of a ing Environment or ORCE; see NICHD Early
prototypically secure child, as determined by Child Care Network, 1996; in press!. Positive
experts in the field, and the correlation is the caregiving was generated at 6, 15, and 24
child’s Q-security score. Research assistants months by summing five qualitative ratings of
were tested on five videotaped observations the care received by the focus child, made at
scored by experts Drs. Posada and Gao. The the end of four observation cycles: sensitivity0
Posada–Gao consensus score was the “gold responsiveness to child’s nondistress expres-
standard” for this test. Their reliability agree- sions, positive regard, stimulation of cognitive
ment across the five test tapes, before confer- development, detachment ~reversed!, and flat
encing to consensus, was .77, with a security affect ~reversed!. At 36 and 54 months, two
score difference of .04. Research assistants additional ratings, fostering exploration and
were certified to collect data if the median intrusiveness ~reversed!, were added to the
correlation of their Q-security scores with the composite. The Cronbach’s alphas were .89,
gold standard Q-security scores was .55, and .88, .86, .82, and .79 at 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54
no correlation was below .40. Across the data months, respectively. Interobserver reliability
collection period within-site reliability was as- estimates ~Pearson correlations! for positive
sessed. Across all research assistants at all sites, caregiving ratings were .94, .86, .81, .80, and
this correlation was .73, with a security score .94 ~videotapes!, and .90, .89, .89, .90, and .98
difference of .06. ~live! at each age.
A modified Strange Situation procedure
based on recommendations by Cassidy and
Child outcome measures
Marvin and the MacArthur Working Group
on Attachment ~1992! was used to assess at- The set of child outcome variables included in
tachment classifications at 36 months see the analyses was derived from measures ob-
~NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, tained during laboratory, home, and child care
2001, for details!. The MacArthur coding sys- or school visits. The criteria used to determine
tem classifies preschoolers as secure ~B! or the final set of outcomes included coverage of
insecure-avoidant ~A!, insecure-resistant ~C!, major developmental domains ~cognitive0
and insecure-controlling0insecure other ~D!. academic and social!, good psychometric
A team of three coders conducted the coding. properties, and representation of important con-
Reliability was calculated based on 867 ran- structs. We also sought to limit the set to a
domly paired cases. Disagreements were dis- small number to permit us to construct a rela-
cussed and a consensus code was assigned. tively coherent picture of the antecedents and
Intercoder agreement ~before conferencing! on sequelae of early affect dysregulation. The
the 4-category A B C D classifications was child outcomes were measured over time: some
75.7% ~k 5 .58!. at 54 months and first grade and others at 54
months, kindergarten, and first grade.
Child care variables
Cognitive0academic outcomes. Three sub-
Amount of child care was the sum of all hours scales of the Woodcock Johnson Achievement
reported by mothers in all nonmaternal care Battery, Letter–Word Identification, Incom-
| DPP16~1! 440 15026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in toddlers 57

plete Words, and Applied Problems, to assess X appeared on the screen and to refrain from
reading, phonological knowledge, and math, pressing when any other letter appeared. Be-
respectively, were administered during lab vis- fore administration, children’s letter knowl-
its at 54 months and Grade 1. All subscales edge was assessed. The task lasted 8 min and
have a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 30 s.
15. In addition, Raush scores are available Sustained attention was assessed by the
for longitudinal analyses. Raush scores are number of errors of omission. Errors of omis-
not standardized by age, and so true develop- sion occurred when children failed to press
mental change can be charted by increasing the button in response to the appearance of the
scores. Scores on the three subtests were cor- target stimulus. Therefore, children with greater
related in our sample at 54 months ~r 5 .38– ability to sustain attention had fewer lapses in
.59, p , .001! and first grade ~r 5 .34–.56, attention and made fewer errors of omission.
p , .001!. Impulsivity was reflected in the number of er-
Sustained attention and impulsivity were rors of commission made by the child—that
measured using the Continuous Performance is, incorrect button-press responses to nontar-
Task ~CPT; Rosvold, Mirsky, Sarason, Bran- get stimuli. Children with more incorrect re-
some, & Beck, 1956!. This task was admin- sponses to nontargets were considered more
istered to each child individually toward the impulsive than were children with fewer in-
end of a 2.5-hr laboratory visit at the 54 correct responses.
months and first-grade assessments. At 54 Measures of sustained attention and impul-
months, with an experimenter in the room, sivity derived from the CPT have adequate
the child was seated at a table in front of a test–retest reliability ~r 5 .65–.74!, based on
2-in. square screen and a red button. Dot- data from a sample of 138 nonreferred boys
matrix pictures of 10 familiar objects ~e.g., ~Halperin, Sharma, Greenblatt, & Schwartz,
butterfly, fish, flower! were generated by a 1991!. Children’s performance on the CPT
computer and presented on the screen. The has high construct validity as a measure of
child was asked to press the button “as fast attention ~Halperin et al., 1991!, and adequate
as you can” each time a target stimulus ~a predictive validity ~e.g., Barkley, 1994; Bark-
chair! appeared on the screen. A total of 220 ley, Brodzinsky, & DuPaul, 1992; Campbell,
stimuli were presented in 22 blocks. The tar- Pierce, March, Ewing, & Szumowski, 1994!.
get stimulus was randomly presented within Performance on these two measures of atten-
each block and appeared twice within each tion is correlated at r 5 .24 and r 5 .48 ~ p ,
block for a total of 44 presentations, leaving .0001! at 54 months and first grade in the cur-
176 presentations of nontarget stimuli. Each rent sample. Cross age correlations are r 5 .26
stimulus appeared on the screen for 500 ms and .19 ~ p , .0001! for sustained attention
at 1500-ms interstimulus intervals. The child’s and impulsivity, respectively.
task was twofold: ~a! to press the red button
as soon as he or she saw the image of the Social and behavioral outcomes. Mothers and
chair on the screen, and ~b! to refrain from teachers rated children’s social skills using their
pressing the button at the appearance of other respective forms of the Social Skills scale from
nontarget stimuli. The task took 7 min and the Social Skills Rating System ~Gresham &
20 s. Elliott, 1990!, rating how often a social behav-
A more challenging version of this task ior occurs on a 3-point scale ~0 5 never, 1 5
was used at first grade. Children were seated sometimes, 2 5 very often!. The teacher So-
in front of a computer monitor and letter stim- cial Skills Scale contains three subscales:
uli were randomly presented in 30 blocks, 10 Cooperation, Assertion, and Self-control. Co-
stimuli per block. The target stimulus, the let- operation includes behaviors such as paying
ter X, appeared twice in each block. The stim- attention to the teacher’s instruction and put-
ulus duration was 200 ms and the interstimulus ting away work materials properly. Assertion
interval was 1500 ms. The child was in- includes initiating appropriate behaviors, such
structed to press the red button whenever an as starting conversations with peers, introduc-
| DPP16~1! 440 16026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

58 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

ing oneself, and volunteering to help peers with Analysis and Results
classroom tasks. Self-control includes behav-
iors that are effective in conflict situations, Of the 1,364 families enrolled in the study,
such as responding to teasing or peer pressure 1,023 were included in the analyses in this
appropriately, receiving criticisms well, and report; 341 were excluded because of missing
controlling temper. The maternal version in- data on the dysregulation, covariates, or out-
cludes an additional Responsibility subscale. come variables. The included and excluded
Responsibility includes behaviors that demon- families do not differ significantly on child’s
strate ability to communicate with adults and gender or dysregulation classification at 24 or
exhibiting regard for property or work. For 36 months. Mothers who were excluded and
the elementary-level forms, Cronbach’s a 5 included in these analyses also did not differ
.87 and .94 for the total mother and teacher on maternal depression scores at any age, or
Social Skills scales, respectively. The total maternal neurotism or extroversion scores ob-
score was used in these analyses. The validity tained at 1 month. The two groups were also
~content, criterion, and construct! of the SSRS comparable on income at 15, 24, 36, and 54
is documented extensively in Gresham and El- months and at Grade 1. However, families with
liot ~1990!. missing outcome data were more likely to in-
The Child Behavior Checklist ~CBCL; clude a child who was Black, x 2 ~3, N 5
Achenbach, 1991a! and Teacher Report Form 1,364! 5 15.7, p , .001, and a mother rated as
~TRF; Achenbach 1991b! are slightly differ- less agreeable on the personality inventory, t
ent versions of a widely used measure to ~1,270! 5 23.21, p , .001, with less maternal
assess the social competence and problem be- education, t ~1,361! 5 25.87, p , .001, and
havior of children 4–18 years. This family of with lower maternal vocabulary scores on the
measures has been standardized on large sam- PPVT, t ~1,165! 5 22.91, p , .01.
ples of children in the United States and The results are presented in three sections.
abroad. A series of behaviors ~about 100 items First, children in the two affect-dysregulation
per version! are rated on 3-point scales from groups at 24 and 36 months ~0 5 not dysreg-
0 ~not true of the child ! to 2 ~very true of the ulated, 1 5 dysregulated ! are compared for
child !. Broad band scales of internalizing and concordance over time. Second, they are com-
externalizing problems are derived from a pared on relevant child and family character-
computerized scoring program. Mothers and istics, including some that were measured
teachers completed the CBCL and TRF at 54 longitudinally from 6 to 36 months. Third, the
months, kindergarten, and Grade 1. two affect-dysregulation groups are compared
The Child–Parent Relationship Scale on the child outcomes, collected longitudi-
~CPRS; Pianta, 1994! and Student–Teacher nally from 54 months to Grade 1, adjusting
Relationship Scale ~STRS; Pianta, 1992! are for a variety of child, family, and child care
30-item, 5-point Likert-type scales designed variables.
to assess the adult’s perception of the quality
of the relationship between the child and the Concordance in affect-dysregulation
adult. The Closeness scale was used in these groups over time
analyses. The 54-month version had 11 items
and the later versions had 8 items. The total Children classified as dysregulated at 24
score was the mean across items. Higher scores months were substantially more likely to be
indicated the respondent reported a warmer, classified as dysregulated at 36 months, x 2
closer relationship with the child. The alpha ~1, N 5 1,023! 5 44.75, p , .001, but there
values ranged from .64 to .69 for the parent- was no extensive overlap across age. As shown
completed measure and from .83 to .85 for in Table 6, about 84% of the sample did not
the teacher-completed form. The CPRS and show affect dysregulation at either age. Of the
STRS were completed by caregivers or teach- 161 children classified as dysregulated at ei-
ers and mothers, respectively, at 54 months, ther age, 26 children were classified as dys-
kindergarten, and Grade 1. regulated at both ages.
| DPP16~1! 440 17026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in toddlers 59

Table 6. Agreement between affect- F ~1, 1,006! 5 9.68, p , .01, and at 36 months,
dyregulation classifications at 24 d 5 .35; F ~1, 1,006! 5 8.66, p , .01!. The
and 36 months mothers of children dysregulated at 24 months
but not 36 months also reported poorer psy-
36-Month chological adjustment, lower agreeableness,
Dysregulation
24-Month d 5 .34; F ~1, 993! 5 10.3, p , .01, and higher
Dysregulation No Yes neuroticism, d 5 25, F ~1, 993! 5 5 .62, p ,
.05. The dysregulation groups did not differ
No 862 ~84%! 55 ~5%!
Yes 80 ~8%! 26 ~3%!
on extraversion. In addition, dysregulated chil-
dren were less likely to have mothers with part-
Note: The difference in numbers between Table 2 and this ners in the household at 24 months, d 5 .39,
table is because a few children have missing data on the F ~1, 1,011! 5 13.8, p , .001, and 36 months,
lab cleanup or mother–child interaction and so meet cri-
teria based only on one or the other. The values are the
d 5 .26, F ~1, 1,011! 5 4.78, p , .05, than
number of subjects. those children who were not dysregulated.
x 2 ~1, n 5 1,023! 5 44.75, p , .001. Significant differences among the dysreg-
ulation groups emerged on many of the child
characteristics. No gender differences emerged.
More African American and fewer White chil-
dren were in the dysregulated groups at 24
Comparing affect-dysregulation groups
months, x 2 ~3, N 5 1,023! 5 10.2, p 5 .017,
on child and family characteristics
and at 36 months, x 2 ~3, N 5 1,023! 5 8.58,
First, we compared the dysregulated groups p 5 .036!. Children tended to show more neg-
on selected family and child variables. Analy- ative affect in interactions with their mothers
ses of variance compared families on all mea- when they were 6 months old if dysregulated
sures collected at a single age, and repeated at 24, d 5 .28, F ~1, 905! 5 7 .11, p , . 01, or
measures analyses compared families on all 36 months, d 5 .24, F ~1, 905! 5 4.07, p 5
measures collected longitudinally. Table 3 lists .04!, although there were no significant differ-
the means and standard deviations or propor- ences in mothers’ reports of temperament
tions for these variables. Both sets of analyses ratings at 6 months of age. Children also per-
asked whether the two dysregulation groups formed more poorly on the Bayley at 15 months
~0, 1! at 24 months and the two dysregulation than their nondysregulated peers if they were
groups at 36 months differed in analyses that dysregulated at 24 months, d 5 .68, F ~1,
adjusted for site. These analyses were con- 976! 5 39.9, p , .001, or at 36 months, d 5
ducted separately for each of the selected char- .25, F ~1, 976! 5 4.07, p , .05. Finally, the
acteristics. Both effect sizes and test statistics dysregulated children also differed on at-
are reported below when significant group dif- tachment. Compared with nondysregulated
ferences were detected. Effect sizes were com- children, children who were classified as dys-
puted as the difference between the adjusted regulated at 24 months were less likely than
means for the two dysregulation groups di- nondysregulated children to have been se-
vided by the pooled standard deviation, the curely attached at 15 months and they were
square root of the residual error variance. more likely to have been classified as insecure-
The first set of analyses compared the ma- avoidant, x 2 ~3, N 5 996! 5 27.1, p , .001,
ternal and family characteristics that were based on analyses of individual cells using EX-
collected only once. Many of them differed ACON ~Bergman & El-Khouri, 1987, 1998!.
according to the child’s dysregulation status. However, children in the dysregulated group
Mothers of children dysregulated at 24 months, at 24 months were more likely than other chil-
but not 36 months, reported less education, dren to be classified as insecure-resistant or
d 5 .38; F ~1, 1,011! 5 12.8, p , .001. Ma- insecure-controlling0insecure-other at 36
ternal vocabulary scores on the PPVT were months, x 2 ~3, N 5 1,003! 5 12.1, p , .01,
significantly lower among children classified based on analyses of individual cells. In addi-
as dysregulated at 24 months, d 5 .33; tion, children who were classified as dysreg-
| DPP16~1! 440 18026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

60 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

ulated at 36 months were also more likely to lated at 24 months, d 5 .32, F ~1, 1,011! 5
be insecure-resistant at 36 months, x 2 ~3, N 5 11.4, p , .001, but not at 36 months. Change
1,003! 5 12.7, p , .01. Children classified as over time in income was not related to either
dysregulated at 24 months also had lower 24- dysregulation group. Compared with the other
month Q-security scores, F ~1, 1,005! 5 14.83, children, dysregulated children at 24 months
p , .01, as did children classified as dysreg- experienced less sensitive and stimulating
ulated at 36 months, F ~1, 1,005! 5 7.71, p , maternal caregiving over time, d 5 .64, F ~1,
.01. Overall, therefore, children who were clas- 1,011! 5 51.3, p , .001, and their mothers
sified as dysregulated at either 24 or 36 months became less sensitive and stimulating be-
also were less likely to have secure attach- tween 15 and 36 months, F ~3, 1,011! 5 20.1,
ments to their mothers both earlier and con- p , .001. For example, the dyregulation groups
currently, although the specific patterns varied became more discrepant, increasing from an
somewhat over time. effect size of 0.50 at 15 months to an effect
Next, we asked whether children’s classi- size of 1.0 at 24 months. Similarly, dysregu-
fications as affect-dysregulated showed dif- lated children at 36 months also experienced
ferent patterns of associations over time on less sensitive and stimulating maternal care-
repeated assessments of maternal caregiving, giving over time, d 5 47; F ~1, 1,011! 5 22.1,
maternal depression, family income, and p , . 001, and more decline in caregiving qual-
amount and quality of child care experiences. ity over time, F ~3, 1,011! 5 23.7, p , .001,
Repeated measures analyses of variance were than children who were not dysregulated at 36
conducted in which the affect-dysregulation months. Although the amount of nonmaternal
groups at 24 and 36 months were the predic- care did not differ between the dysregulation
tors of interest and site was included as a co- groups at either age, children considered dys-
variate. Repeated assessments of each measure regulated at 24 months, d 5 .20; F ~1, 772! 5
were collected across four waves of data col- 4.93, p , .05, and at 36 months, d 5 .29; F ~3,
lection from 6 to 36 months. Table 3 lists the 772! 5 7.88, p , .01, were observed to be in
means on the repeated assessments for chil- lower quality child care. Change in child care
dren classified as dysregulated or not dysreg- quality over time was not related to dysregu-
ulated at the two ages. The repeated measures lation group.
analyses simultaneously tested whether the Finally, we conducted logistic regression
groups differed on the average level of a given analyses to determine the independent contri-
outcome over time and whether they showed bution of each of the significant family and
different patterns of change between 6 and 36 child characteristics, controlling for the ef-
months on that outcome. A general linear mixed fects of all other variables in the model, in
model approach was used so that we could predicting whether the child was classified as
include children who might be missing data at dysregulated at each age. As shown in Table 4,
a particular age ~e.g., child care quality at ages the logistic regression included maternal edu-
at which the child was in exclusive maternal cation, vocabulary, agreeableness, extraver-
care!. sion, and depressive symptoms; partner in the
Children classified as dysregulated dif- home; income; and caregiving quality; and
fered from other children in four of the five child’s ethnicity ~Black, Hispanic, other, or
repeated measures analyses. More maternal de- White!, 6-month negative mood, 15-month
pressive symptoms were reported over time cognitive functioning, 15- and 36-month at-
by mothers of children in the 24-month dys- tachment classifications, and 24-month
regulated group, d 5 .28, F ~1, 1,011! 5 11.4, Q-security. Child care quality was excluded
p , .001, and the 36-month dysregulated because children not in child care had missing
group, d 5 . 27, F ~1, 1,101! 5 8.0, p , .01, data. The repeated assessments were averaged
but no differences in patterns of change over from 6 to 24 months when predicting 24-
time were observed in either comparison. month dysregulation status, and from 6 and 36
Lower family incomes were reported over time months when predicting 36-month dysregula-
by mothers of children classified as dysregu- tion status. Only three variables showed sig-
| DPP16~1! 440 19026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in toddlers 61

nificant independent prediction, and one of Cognitive0academic outcomes. Children clas-


them emerged as a significant predictor of both sified as dysregulated at 24 or 36 months scored
24- and 36-month dysregulation classifica- higher on impulsivity and lower on all mea-
tions. Children whose mothers were less sen- sures of academic achievement. As can be seen
sitive and stimulating had children who were under the cognitive0academic outcomes in
significantly more likely to be classified as Table 5, children who displayed affect dysreg-
dysregulated at 24 months ~B 5 2.89, SE 5 ulation at 24 months, F ~1, 978! 5 6.47, p ,
.25, p , .001! and 36 months ~B 5 2.94, .05, and at 36 months, F ~1, 978! 5 11.48, p ,
SE 5 .29, p , .01!. In addition, children were .001, scored lower on a standardized math test
significantly more likely to be classified as on average over time but did not show pat-
dysregulated at 24 months if they had lower terns of gains over time different from those
Bayley MDI cognitive scores ~B 5 2.04, SE 5 of other children. The children dysregulated
.01, p , .001! or were classified as avoidant at 24 months scored on average 2.3 points
versus secure at 15 months ~B 5 .76, SE 5 lower at 54 months and 5.2 points lower in
.23, p , .001!. first grade, resulting in an averaged over time
effect size of d 5 .25. The children dysregu-
lated at 36 months scored on average 5.9 points
Child outcomes lower at 54 months and 5.0 points lower in
first grade, resulting in an averaged over time
We conducted repeated measures analyses of effect size of d 5 .37.
variance to determine the extent to which chil- A similar pattern of results obtained for the
dren classified as dysregulated at either 24 or other academic outcomes. Dysregulated chil-
36 months exhibited more negative outcomes dren at 24 months scored lower than other
during preschool and the early elementary children on phonological knowledge, d 5 .19;
school years. The repeated measures analyses F ~1, 978! 5 4.76, p , .05, and higher on im-
were conducted using a general linear mixed pulsivity, d 5 .25; F ~1, 967! 5 7.62, p , .01.
model approach ~Singer, 1998! in which re- Similarly, dysregulated children at 36 months
peated assessments of predictors as well as scored lower than other children on reading,
repeated assessments of outcomes were in- d 5 .21; F ~1, 978! 5 4.06, p , .05, and pho-
cluded. The models included site; maternal ed- nological knowledge, d 5 .32; F ~1, 978! 5
ucation, verbal ability, and concurrent measures 10.27, p , .01, and higher on impulsivity, d 5
of income, partner in the home, depressive .19; F ~1, 967! 5 4.32, p , .05. None of these
symptoms, and caregiving; and the child’s eth- analyses indicated that change over time on
nicity, sex, and negative mood rating at 6 these cognitive measures was related to dys-
months age and all interactions with age. The regulation status at either age.
results of these analyses are reported in Table 5.
This table lists the F statistics for main effect Social and behavioral outcomes: Mother
tests comparing the children dysregulated at report. Affect dysregulation at 24 but not 36
24 months with the other children in the first months was related to maternal ratings of so-
column and the associated effect size in the cial skills between 54 months and the end of
second column. The effect size is computed as first grade. Children in the dysregulation group
the difference between the adjusted mean for at 24 months were rated as lower in social
the two groups divided by the estimated stan- skills on three of the four mother-reported out-
dard deviation ~square root of the estimated comes. Their mothers reported less closeness
residual variance!. The third column lists the in the mother–child relationship, d 5 .18; F ~1,
F statistics for the test of the Age 3 Dysregu- 981! 5 3.95, p , .05, more externalizing prob-
lation interaction and the next three columns lems, d 5 2.19; F ~1, 981! 5 4.52, p , .05,
list the adjusted means at each age for each and fewer social skills, d 5 .30; F ~1, 981! 5
group. The same values are listed in the 7th 10.82, p , .01, than the other children. In the
through 12th columns with respect to dysreg- analysis of mother-reported social skills, an
ulation status at 36 months. Age3Dysregulation interaction, F ~1, 1,845!5
| DPP16~1! 440 20026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

62 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

5.55, p , .05, emerged because mothers re- tion capacities in young children. Children
ported fewer gains over time in social skills characterized as affect-dysregulated in the cur-
for the dysregulated children than for the other rent study displayed marked negative affect
children. Children in the dysregulated group during low stress play with their mothers, and
at 36 months did not differ significantly from some children also exhibited high levels of
the other children on any of these maternal defiance and other forms of negative affect
ratings of social skills. when asked by their mothers to clean up toys
after playing in the lab. In other words, as
Social and behavioral outcomes: Teacher early as age 2 or 3, some mother–child dyads
report. Teachers rated the social skills of chil- had developed relationships that appeared to
dren in the dysregulated groups at 24 or 36 be affectively negative, sometimes coercive,
months as more problematic on two of the four and relatively ineffective during times of low
measures. Teachers reported that children in and moderate stress.
the dysregulated group at 24 months had more About 16% of the sample of more than
externalizing problems, d 5 2.20; F ~1, 953! 5 1,000 children was identified as affect-
14.75, p , .001, and fewer social skills, d 5 dysregulated at either 24 or 36 months. The
.22; F ~1, 949! 5 5.87, p , .05, than the other children in affectively dysregulated relation-
children. Children in the dysregulated group ships with their mothers differed in important
at 36 months were also rated by their teacher ways from children not so identified. They
as showing fewer social skills, d 5 .24, came from families that were more likely to
F ~1, 940! 5 5.49, p , .05, than the other be African American, with fewer father fig-
children. Teachers reported no differences in ures and mothers who had less education, lower
the levels of internalizing symptoms, in their verbal ability, and poorer psychological adjust-
feelings of closeness to the study child, or in ment. The children themselves had lower Bay-
change over time on any measure for children ley MDI scores at 15 months. In addition, their
who did or did not show affect dysregulation mothers were less sensitive and stimulating
in interactions with their mothers at 24 or 36 during both semistructured play and during nat-
months. urally occurring interactions at home during
the first years of these children’s lives. How-
ever, when all of these predictors were con-
Discussion
sidered in a multivariate analysis, only the
In this study we have conceptualized the con- mother’s sensitivity and stimulation and the
struct of children’s affect dysregulation in the child’s cognitive development and insecure-
early years at the dyadic level rather than at the avoidant attachment classification at 15 months
individual child level. Our measures of affect were associated with affect-dysregulation sta-
dysregulation represent the child’s responses to tus at 24 or 36 months. Children classified as
mother during dyadic interactions that varied affect-dysregulated thus experienced and ex-
in their emotional demands. The child’s affect pressed negative affect in the context of par-
and affect regulation as we have assessed them enting relationships that were less emotionally
are thus a product of the mother–child dyad, but supportive overall, and they may have had
the mother’s behavior, per se, was not included fewer cognitive resources to assist in strategic
in the formation of the affect-dysregulation emotion regulation and to elicit more positive
groups. Based on their negative affect with caregiving.
their mothers, we have characterized some chil- There were several associations between at-
dren as showing “affect-dysregulation in the tachment, which was assessed in different ways
context of the mother–child relationship” in at 15, 24, and 36 months, and affect dysregu-
recognition of the central role of parents in lation at both 24 and 36 months. Children who
assisting children with affect regulation in the were insecure-avoidant at 15 months, but not
first few years of life, and the bidirectional insecure-resistant or insecure-disorganized
nature of parent–child interactions, including children at that age, were more likely to be
those which scaffold emerging affect regula- classified in the affect-dysregulated group at
| DPP16~1! 440 21026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in toddlers 63

24 months. This finding is interesting because 1989; Thompson, 1994!, it is perhaps unsur-
avoidance is often characterized by emotional prising that the developmental outcomes in
overcontrol, or a minimization of negative af- later preschool and the early school years were
fect when the attachment system is activated much less positive for children in the affect-
in the presence of the mother. However, some dysregulation groups. Children who were dys-
avoidant infants ~in the A2 subcategory! do regulated as toddlers or young preschoolers
show fussiness in the Strange Situation, and were less cognitively and socially competent
Ainsworth, Bell, and Stayton ~1972! showed at 54 months, kindergarten, and Grade 1, and
that avoidant infants are angry with their moth- were more likely to be rated higher on behav-
ers at home and ambivalent about contact with ior problems than were never dysregulated
her, fussing and crying when they are put down. children. Notably, this was true even after con-
Thus, avoidant children who inhibit the ex- trolling for earlier and concurrent family and
pression of attachment behavior when their at- child characteristics that were associated with
tachment systems are activated do behave in affective dysregulation, including maternal ed-
more negative ways with their mothers under ucation, vocabulary, depressive symptoms,
some conditions. The results of this study also partner in the home, income, and caregiving
support the findings by others that avoidant quality, and child ethnicity and 6-month neg-
infants are undercontrolled in other situations ative mood. Thus, early affect dysregulation
in later childhood ~Egeland, Pianta, & O’Brien, in the context of the mother–child relation-
1993; Munson, McMahon, & Spieker, 2001; ship apparently is associated with continuing
Renken, Egeland, Marvinney, Mangelsdorf, & problems in both cognitive and social devel-
Sroufe, 1989!. The lack of association with opment through preschool and into the early
disorganized attachment classifications is in- school years.
teresting. However, this classification is het- Mothers of affectively dysregulated chil-
erogeneous. We know little about how children dren reported feeling less closeness with their
whose Strange Situation behavior is classified children in later years, compared with moth-
as disorganized manage negative affect in in- ers of children who were not dysregulated. In
teraction with the mother in less stressful sit- contrast, teachers did not report differential
uations. It is possible, for instance, that when feelings of closeness across the groups of chil-
their attachment systems are not activated they dren although they did report differences in
tend to inhibit the expression of negative af- social skills and behavior problems. Perhaps
fect in the presence of their mother ~i.e., a this pattern of results reflects teacher profes-
pattern opposite that observed in infants clas- sionalism. It will be interesting to test in fu-
sified as avoidant!. A shift in the type of in- ture analyses whether observations of the
secure attachment associated with affect quality of student–teacher interactions sup-
dysregulation is already detectable by 36 port or contradict teacher reports of the qual-
months, when the association is with insecure- ity of the student–teacher relationship.
resistant attachment for both 24- and 36- The findings from this study converge with
month affect dysregulation groups. Crittenden those of three smaller studies that also re-
~1992! predicted such a shift to insecure- ported links between negativity and conflict
resistance in the preschool years. Our findings in the mother–child relationship during the tod-
are consistent with her speculation that more dler years and subsequent behavior problems
preschoolers will begin to use the more coer- ~Denham et al., 2000; Leadbeater et al., 1996;
cive strategies that are available to them as a Martin et al., 2001!. However, the large sam-
result of their increasing cognitive and linguis- ple size and measurement strategies of the
tic competence. NICHD Study of Early Child Care have per-
Given the importance of affect regulation mitted us to control for a number of possible
for the acquisition of competence in multiple confounding factors and to extend the find-
domains ~Campos, Campos, & Barrett, 1989; ings from previous research in several impor-
Campos, Mumme, Kermoian, & Campos 1994; tant ways: ~a! we included multiple measures
Dodge, 1989; Garber & Dodge, 1991; Kopp, of affect dysregulation in different contexts of
| DPP16~1! 440 22026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

64 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

mother–child interaction that embodied differ- months. It emerged later for children classi-
ent kinds of emotional demands or challenges; fied as dysregulated at 36 months. For both
~b! we identified several demographic, mater- groups, mothers of dysregulated toddlers were
nal, and family antecedents and concurrent less sensitive and stimulating beginning in
correlates of affect dysregulation; ~c! our de- infancy. Thus, infants of such mothers experi-
velopmental outcome measures included not enced higher rates and0or intensities of nega-
only behavior problems, but also more gen- tive affect in the very relationship on which
eral measures of social and cognitive compe- they depend for reducing and managing their
tence, and were obtained using multiple forms negative affect, and they would receive less
of assessment and multiple informants; and sensitive responsiveness and scaffolding to help
~d! we followed the children longitudinally them learn to manage their negative emotions
through preschool and into the school years as independently. Amplified during the emotion-
demands for affective and behavioral self- ally difficult transitions of the toddler years,
regulation steadily increased. such dyads may be more likely to establish a
The effects reported here are notable for pattern of mutual negativity and coercion ~Pat-
their longevity and for their breadth. The con- terson, DeBaryshe, & Ramsey, 1989! that be-
vergence across very different assessments of comes progressively more difficult to escape.
competence as well as across informants makes With less effective socialization of attention
us confident in concluding that affect dysreg- control ~Raver, 1996; Raver, Blackburn, Ban-
ulation in the early years constitutes a sig- croft, & Torp, 1999! and compliance ~Kochan-
nificant risk for later social and cognitive ska, 1994; Londerville & Main, 1981! in such
development. What might account for these dyads, in addition to increasingly negative
effects enduring into the preschool and early affect, children’s cognitive and achievement-
school years? Interestingly, dysregulated tod- related outcomes as well as their social com-
dlers and preschoolers had not been identified petence could be compromised; deficits could
by their mothers as more temperamentally dif- easily cumulate and further exacerbate the dif-
ficult as infants than were children who exhib- ficult mother–child relationship.
ited no dysregulation. However, these children As appealing as such an account may be, it
were more likely than their nondysregulated fails to tell the whole story. Among mothers
counterparts to become affectively negative who were in the lowest quartile of maternal
with their mothers during play interactions as caregiving from 6 through 36 months, only
early as 6 months of age. We cannot rule out 24% of their infants were in the affect-
the possibility that negativity was characteris- dysregulated group at 24 months, and only 16%
tic of these children early on, which could ex- were in the affect-dysregulated group at 36
plain their difficulties with negative emotions months. This suggests the still more complex
in the mother–child relationship at 24 months possibility that the roots of affect dysregula-
~Mangelsdorf, Gunnar, Krestenbaum, Lang, & tion are truly dyadic—that is, that some moth-
Andreas, 1990!. However, it is equally likely ers and infants are a poor affective fit for one
that the early negativity was a child response another ~Crockenberg & Litman, 1990; Kopp,
to the less sensitive and stimulating maternal 1989; Mangelsdorf et al., 1990; Seifer, Schiller,
caregiving that they received at every age. Just Sameroff, Resnick, & Riordan, 1996!. The ef-
as Ainsworth et al. ~1978! concluded that du- fects of such mismatches may not be readily
ration of infant crying in the first quarter of apparent until the second or third year of life
life reflected differences in maternal respon- when the young child’s emotions are both
siveness to crying, it is possible that some quickly differentiating and intensifying ~Cam-
mothers induced or elicited negative affect in pos et al., 1994; Lewis, 1993!, when demands
their infants, thereby creating an emotionally for self-regulation increase ~Kopp, 1989;
dysregulated relationship by either 24 or 36 Thompson, 1994!, and when autonomy bids
months. Higher infant negativity with moth- emerge as issues for children and for their par-
ers was observed by 6 months for the children ents ~Belsky et al., 1998; Crockenberg & Lit-
who would be in the dysregulated group at 24 man, 1990!. Under these conditions, dyads that
| DPP16~1! 440 23026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in toddlers 65

have not effectively negotiated the affect reg- ginning in infancy, and not on assessments of
ulation challenges of infancy may be particu- infants or mothers alone.
larly susceptible to affect dysregulation in the Despite the strength of our findings, an im-
toddler and early preschool years. This sus- portant feature of this study limits our conclu-
ceptibility is perhaps especially likely to be sions. Although we have assessed children’s
expressed in adverse ecological conditions that affect dysregulation in a dyadic context, we
place additional psychological stresses on fam- have not evaluated mothers’ contributions to
ilies, and make it more difficult for mothers to the dyadic interactions. We do not know for
support and manage their children’s changing which dyads the negative affect in interaction
emotional needs. Such a possibility is consis- was mutual, thus reflecting a true coercive cy-
tent with our findings that children were dis- cle, and for which dyads it was one-sided on
proportionately more likely to be affect- the part of the child only. Thus, we are unable
dysregulated when they were growing up in to examine the dyadic processes that contrib-
poverty with mothers who were poorly edu- ute to affect dysregulation in mother–child re-
cated and depressed. lationships or to identify which particular
There are several implications of the find- qualities within the dyadic relationship are most
ings of this study for research on affect reg- critical to effective or ineffective affect regu-
ulation. First, there are implications for lation. The finding that affect dysregulation in
conceptualizing affect regulation in the early the mother–child dyad at 24 months predicted
years. Although several scholars have hypoth- more broadly to later outcomes than did 36-
esized that affect regulation occurs in the con- month affect dysregulation further suggests the
text of the parent–child dyad during infancy need for research to discern which features of
~Kopp, 1989; Thompson, 1994; Tronick, mother–child interaction at age two are dis-
1989!, the current study is one of the first to rupted in affectively dysregulated dyads, and
operationalize early affect dysregulation in ex- how they differ from disruptions at age 3. Per-
plicitly dyadic terms and is the largest and haps the requirements for a good fit between
most complex. The findings that dysregu- mother and child vary somewhat at the two
lated toddlers and preschoolers differ with re- ages, so that some dyads are particularly at
spect to both antecedent and concurrent risk for dysregulation at particular develop-
correlates as well as social and cognitive se- mental stages.
quelae several years later confirm the value This study suggests several additional ave-
of observing and studying early affect regu- nues for future investigation. First, although
lation in a dyadic context. Similarly, there we found a significant association between
are implications for predictive relationships affect-dysregulation at 24 and 36 months, in
among early child, maternal, and environmen- fact most children were classified as affec-
tal characteristics and later developmental out- tively dysregulated at a single age. Either af-
come. The findings of the current study fect dysregulation trajectories are variable or
suggest that it is perhaps not so much child our measurement is still imprecise. We did not
or even maternal affective characteristics that find that using continuous measures of dysreg-
are most predictive, but relationship qualities ulation made significant differences in our find-
to which both partners contribute and which ings, but finer grained observations might make
are best captured at the dyadic level. By ob- a difference. Second, although we assessed af-
serving child affect with the mother in un- fect dysregulation in the toddler years, we have
demanding play settings and more demanding not examined later features of self-regulation
compliance settings, we have gained a unique such as resistance to temptation, delay of
perspective on the extent to which the mother– gratification, sustained attention, and affect
child relationship serves as an effective or an regulation strategies that emerge during the
ineffective regulator of the child’s negative preschool years. These may be important me-
emotions across contexts. This account sug- diators of the longitudinal associations be-
gests that future researchers should focus on tween early affect regulation and later cognitive
dyadic measures of affective dysregulation be- and social development. Third, we have pri-
| DPP16~1! 440 24026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

66 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

marily considered underregulated rather than social and cognitive development. These data
overregulated emotion. We found few associ- also may have implications for prevention and
ations between early dysregulation and later intervention. For example, they suggest the
internalizing behavior, perhaps because we did importance of intervening early in poorly reg-
not look for early indicators of overregula- ulated mother–toddler relationships to help
tion. It is interesting to speculate that children mothers learn to calm and scaffold toddlers
whose emotions are consistently checked in who are upset and difficult to console, and to
their relationships with mothers during the nor- support sensitive responsiveness in mothers
mative emotional upheavals of the toddler years of young children who may be at risk for
may also be at risk for later deficits in func- behavioral or learning problems. Because the
tioning, perhaps especially in the socioemo- sample is being followed into later primary
tional domain. grades, we will be able to identify longer
In conclusion, we are encouraged by the term developmental trajectories for children
intriguing and important findings of the cur- dysregulated as toddlers and preschoolers,
rent investigation and the potential value of permitting us to determine both how stable
the construct of affect dysregulation for un- and pervasive these effects are, and whether
derstanding associations between emotions in they are amplified or reduced by schooling
the parent–child relationship and children’s experiences.

References
Achenbach, T. M. ~1991a!. Manual for the Child Behav- externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 34,
ior Checklist04-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: 982–995.
University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry. Bayley, N. ~1969!. Bayley Scales of Infant Development.
Achenbach, T. M. ~1991b!. Manual for the Teacher’s Re- San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
port Form and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: Univer- Belsky, J., Woodworth, S., & Crnic, K. ~1996!. Troubled
sity of Vermont Department of Psychiatry. family interaction during toddlerhood. Development
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. ~1972!. and Psychopathology, 8, 477– 495.
Individual differences in the development of some at- Belsky, J., Hsieh, K., & Crnic, K. ~1998!. Mothering, fa-
tachment behaviors. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 18, thering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys
123–143. externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years:
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. Differential susceptibility to rearing experience? De-
~1978!. Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. velopment and Psychopathology, 10, 301–320.
Barkley, R. A. ~1994!. The assessment of attention in chil- Bergman, L. R., & El-Khouri, B. ~1987!. EXACON: A
dren. In G. Reid Lyon ~Ed.!, Frames of reference for Fortran 77 program for the exact analysis of single
the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on cells in a contingency table. Educational and Psycho-
measurement issues ~pp. 69–102!. Baltimore, MD: Paul logical Measurement, 47, 155–161.
H. Brookes. Bergman, L. R., & El-Khouri, B. M. ~1998!. SLEIPNER:
Barkley, R. A., Grodzinsky, G. M., & DuPaul, G. J. ~1992!. A statistical package for pattern-oriented analyses,
Frontal lobe functions in attention deficit disorder with V 2.0. Stockholm: Stockholm University, Department
and without hyperactivity: A review and research re- of Psychology.
port. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, Bowlby, J. S. ~1968!. Attachment and loss: Vol. I. Attach-
163–188. ment. New York: Basic Books.
Bates, J. E., Maslin, C. A., & Frankel, K. A. ~1985!. At- Braungart-Rieker, J. M., & Stifter, C. A. ~1996!. Infants’
tachment security, mother–child interaction, and tem- responses to frustrating situations: Continuity and
perament as predictors of behavior problem ratings at change in reactivity and regulation. Child Develop-
age three years. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters ~Eds.!, ment, 67, 1767–1779.
Growing points in attachment theory and research. Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. ~1984!. Home obser-
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child De- vation for the measurement of the environment. Little
velopment, 50~Serial No. 209!, 167–193. Rock, AR: University of Arkansas at Little Rock.
Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Dodge, K. A. ~1995!. Family Calkins, S. D. ~1994!. Origins and outcomes of individual
and child factors in stability and change in children’s differences in emotion regulation. In N. A. Fox ~Ed.!,
aggressiveness in elementary school. In J. McCord The development of emotion regulation. Biological
~Ed.!, Coercion and punishment in long-term perspec- and behavioral considerations. Monographs of the So-
tives ~pp. 124–138!. New York: Cambridge Univer- ciety for Research in Child Development, 59~Serial
sity Press. No. 240!, 53–72.
Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Ridge, B. ~1998!. Campbell, S. B. ~2002!. Behavior problems in preschool
The interaction of temperamental resistance to con- children: Developmental and clinical issues ~2nd edi-
trol and restrictive parenting in the development of tion!. New York: Guilford Press.
| DPP16~1! 440 25026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

Affect dysregulation in the mother–child relationship in toddlers 67

Campbell, S. B., Pierce, E. W., March, C. L., Ewing, L. J., Halperin, J. M., Sharma, V., Greenblatt, E., & Schwartz,
& Szumowski, E. K. ~1994!. Hard-to-manage pre- S. T. ~1991!. Assessment of the continuous perfor-
school boys: Symptomatic behavior across contexts mance test: Reliability and validity in a nonreferred
and time. Child Development, 65, 836–851. sample. Psychological Assessment: Journal of Con-
Campbell, S. B., Shaw, D. S., & Gilliom, M. ~2000!. Early sulting and Clinical Psychology, 3, 603– 608.
externalizing behavior problems: Toddlers and pre- Keenan, K., Shaw, D., Delliquadri, E., Giovannelli, J.,
schoolers at risk for later maladjustment. Develop- Walsh, B. ~1998!. Evidence for the continuity of early
ment and Psychopathology, 12, 467– 488. problem behaviors: Application of a developmental
Campos, J., Campos, R., & Barrett, K. ~1989!. Emergent model. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26,
themes in the study of emotional development and 441– 452.
emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 25, Kochanska, G. ~1994!. Beyond cognition: Expanding the
394– 402. search for the early roots of internalization and con-
Campos, J., Mumme, D., Kermoian, R., & Campos, R. science. Developmental Psychology, 30, 20–22.
~1994!. A functionalist perspective on the nature of Kochanska, G. ~1995!. Children’s temperament, mothers’
emotion. In N. Fox ~Ed.!, the development of emotion discipline, and security of attachment: Multiple path-
regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations. ways to emerging internalization. Child Develop-
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child De- ment, 66, 597– 615.
velopment, 59~203, Serial No. 240!. Kochanska, G. ~1997!. Mutually responsive orientation
Carlson, E. A., & Sroufe, L. A. ~1995!. Contributions of between mothers and their young children: Implica-
attachment theory to developmental psychopathol- tions for early socialization. Child Development, 68,
ogy. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen ~Eds.!, Manual of 94–112.
developmental psychopathology, Vol. I. Theory and Kopp, C. B. ~1982!. Antecedents of self-regulation: A de-
methods ~pp. 581– 617!. New York: Wiley. velopmental perspective. Developmental Psychology,
Cassidy, J., Marvin, R. S., & The MacArthur Attachment 18, 199–214.
Working Group. ~1992!. Attachment organization in Kopp, C. B. ~1989!. Regulation of distress and negative
preschool children: Procedures and coding manual. emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psy-
Unpublished coding manual, Pennsylvania State chology, 25, 343–354.
University. Leadbeater, B., Bishop, S. & Raver, C. ~1996!. Quality of
Cicchetti, D., Toth, S., Bush, M. A., & Gillespie, J. F. mother–toddler interactions, maternal depressive symp-
~1988!. Stage salient issues: A transactional model of toms, and behavior problems in preschoolers of ado-
intervention. New Directions for Child Development, lescent mothers. Developmental Psychology, 32,
39, 123–145. 280–288.
Costa, P., & McCrae, R. ~1985! The NEO Personality Lewis, M. ~1993!. The emergence of human emotions. In
Inventory Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assess- M. Lewis & J. Havilland ~Eds.!, Handbook of emo-
ment Resources. tions ~pp. 223–235!. New York: Guilford Press.
Crittenden, P. M. ~1992!. Quality of attachment in the Londerville, S., & Main, M. ~1981!. Security of attach-
preschool years. Development and Psychopathology, ment, compliance and maternal training methods in
4, 209–241. the second year of life. Developmental Psychology,
Crockenberg, S., & Litman, C. ~1990!. Autonomy as com- 17, 289–299.
petence in 2-year-olds: Maternal correlates of child Mangelsdorf, S., Gunnar, M., Krestenbaum, R., Lang, S.,
defiance, compliance, and self-assertion. Developmen- & Andreas, D. ~1990!. Infant proneness-to-distress tem-
tal Psychology, 26, 961–971. perament, maternal personality, and mother–infant at-
Denham, S., Workman, E., Cole, P, Weissbrod, C., tachment: Associations and goodness of fit. Child
Kendziora, K., & Zahn–Waxler, C. ~2000!. Prediction Development, 61, 820–831.
of externalizing behavior problems from early to mid- Martin, S., Clements, M., & Crnic, K. ~2001!. Emotional
dle childhood: The role of parental socialization and regulation in mother–toddler dyads: Links to chil-
emotion expression. Development and Psychopathol- dren’s internalizing and externalizing behavior prob-
ogy, 12, 23– 45. lems. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the
Dodge, K. ~1989!. Coordinating responses to aversive Society for Research in Child Development, Minne-
stimuli: Introduction to special section on the devel- apolis, MN.
opment of emotion regulation. Developmental Psy- Medoff–Cooper, B., Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C.
chology, 25, 339–342. ~1993!. Early Infancy Temperament Questionnaire.
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. ~1981!. Peabody Picture Journal of Developmental and Behavior Pediatrics,
Vocabulary Test—Revised. Circle Pines, MN: Ameri- 14, 230–235.
can Guidance Service. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. ~1996!. Char-
Egeland, B., Pianta, R., & O’Brien, M. A. ~1993!. Mater- acteristics of infant child care: Factors contributing to
nal intrusiveness in infancy and child maladaptation positive caregiving. Early Childhood Research Quar-
in early school years. Development and Psychopathol- terly, 11, 269–306.
ogy, 5, 359–370. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. ~1998!. Early
Fogel, A. ~1993!. Developing through relationships. Chi- child care and self-control, compliance, and problem
cago: University of Chicago Press. behavior at 24 and 36 months. Child Development,
Garber, J., & Dodge, K. ~Eds.!. ~1991!. The development 69, 1145–1170.
of emotion regulation and dysregulation. Cambridge: NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. ~1999!.
Cambridge University Press. Child care and mother–child interaction in the first
Gresham, F., & Elliot, S. ~1990!. The social skills rating three years of life. Developmental Psychology, 35,
system. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. 1399–1413.
| DPP16~1! 440 26026 02013004 11:30 am REVISED PROOF |

68 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. ~2001!. Child derstanding. In W. Damon ~Series Ed.! & N. Eisenberg
care and family predictors of preschool attachment ~Vol. Ed.!, Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. So-
and stability from infancy. Developmental Psychol- cial, emotional, and personality development ~5th ed.,
ogy, 37, 847–862. pp. 237–309!. New York: Wiley.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. ~in press!. Sameroff, A., & Chandler, M. ~1975!. Reproductive risk
The NICHD study of early child care: Contexts of and the continuum of caretaking casualty. In F. Horo-
development and developmental outcomes over the witz ~Ed.!, Review of child development research ~Vol.
first seven years of life. In J. Brooks–Gunn & L. J. 4, pp. 187–244!. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Berlin ~Eds.!, Young children’s education, health, and Sanson, A., Oberklaid, F., Pedlow, R., & Prior, M. ~1991!.
development: Profile and Synthesis Project report. Risk indicators: Assessment of infancy predictors of
Washington, DC: Department of Education. preschool behavioral maladjustment. Journal of Child
Patterson, G., Baryshe, B., & Ramsey, E. ~1989!. A de- Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 609– 626.
velopmental perspective on antisocial behavior. Amer- Seifer, R., Schiller, M., Sameroff, A., Resnick, S., & Ri-
ican Psychologist, 44, 329–335. ordan, K. ~1996!. Attachment, maternal sensitivity, and
Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. ~1989!. Family interaction infant temperament during the first year of life. De-
patterns and children’s behavior problems from in- velopmental Psychology, 32, 12–25.
fancy to age 4 years. Developmental Psychology, 25, Shaw, D. S., Keenan, K., & Vondra, J. I. ~1994!. Devel-
413– 420. opmental precursors of externalizing behavior: Ages
Pianta, R. ~1992!. The Student–Teacher Relationship Scale. 1 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 30, 355–364.
Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia. Singer, J. ~1998!. Using SAS PROC MIXED to fit multi-
Pianta, R. ~1994!. The Child–Parent Relationship Scale. level models, hierarchical models, and individual
Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia. growth models. Journal of Educational and Behav-
Radloff, L. ~1977!. The CES-D Scale: A self-report de- ioral Statistics, 24, 323–355.
pression scale for research in the general population. Sroufe, L. A. ~1979!. The coherence of individual de-
Journal of Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, velopment: Early care, attachment, and subsequent
385– 401. developmental issues. American Psychologist, 34,
Raver, C. ~1996!. Success at catching and keeping tod- 834–841.
dler’s attention: An examination of joint attention Sroufe, L. A. ~1996!. Emotional development. Cam-
among low-income mothers and their 2-year-olds. bridge: Cambridge University Press.
Early Development and Parenting, 5, 225–236. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. ~1977!. Temperament and devel-
Raver, C., Blackburn, E., Bancroft, M., & Torp, N. ~1999!. opment. New York: Brunner0Mazel.
Relations between effective emotional self-regulation, Thompson, R. A. ~1994!. Emotion regulation: A theme in
attentional control, and low-income preschoolers’ so- search of a definition. Monographs of the Society for
cial competence with peers. Early Education and De- Research in Child Development, 59~Serial No. 240!,
velopment, 10, 333–350. 25–52.
Renken, B., Egeland, B., Marvinney, D., Mangelsdorf, Thompson, R. A. ~1998!. Early sociopersonality devel-
S., & Sroufe, L. A. ~1989!. Early childhood ante- opment. In W. Damon ~Series Ed.! & N. Eisenberg
cedents of aggression and passive-withdrawal in ~Vol. Ed.!, Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. So-
early elementary school. Journal of Personality, 57, cial, emotional, and personality development ~pp. 25–
257–281. 104!. New York: Wiley.
Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome, E. D. Tronick, E. ~1989!. Emotions and emotional communica-
Jr., & Beck, L. H. ~1956!. A continuous performance tion in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119.
test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychol- Vaughn, B. E., & Waters, E. ~1990!. Attachment behavior
ogy, 20, 343–350. at home and in the laboratory: Q-sort observations
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. ~1998!. Temperament. In and Strange Situation classifications of one-year-
W. Damon ~Ed.! & N. Eisenberg ~Vol. Ed.!, Hand- olds. Child Development, 61, 1865–1973.
book of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, Waters, E., & Deane, K. ~1985!. Defining and assessing
and personality development ~5th ed., pp. 105–176!. individual differences in attachment relationships:
New York: Wiley. Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in
Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Hershey, K. L. ~1995!. infancy and early childhood. Monographs for the So-
Temperament, attention, and developmental psycho- ciety for Research in Child Development, 41– 65.
pathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen ~Eds.!, Man- Weschler, D. ~1981!. Manual for the Weschler Adult In-
ual of developmental psychopathology, Vol. 1: Theory telligence Scale—Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psycho-
and methods ~pp. 315–340!. New York: Wiley. logical Corporation.
Saarni, C., Mumme, D.L., & Campos, J. J. ~1998!. Emo- Winer, B. J. ~1971!. Statistical principles in experimental
tional development: Action, cummunication, and un- design ~2nd ed.!. New York: McGraw–Hill.

You might also like