You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Consumer Research, Inc.

Sex Typing and Consumer Behavior: A Test of Gender Schema Theory


Author(s): Bernd H. Schmitt, France Leclerc and Laurette Dub-Rioux
Source: Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 15, No. 1 (Jun., 1988), pp. 122-128
Published by: The University of Chicago Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2489178 .
Accessed: 24/11/2014 01:04
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

The University of Chicago Press and Journal of Consumer Research, Inc. are collaborating with JSTOR to
digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Consumer Research.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 202.84.40.92 on Mon, 24 Nov 2014 01:04:47 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Sex Typing and Consumer Behavior: A Test


of Gender Schema Theory
BERND H. SCHMITT*
FRANCE LECLERC
LAURETTEDUBE-RIOUX
Using the context of advertising, packaging, and consumer choice, three experiments were conducted to test the predictions of gender schema theory that gender-schematic (sex-typed) individuals and gender-aschematic (non-sex-typed) individuals differ in the way they process and evaluate information related to gender.
Results indicated strong sex effects, but the predicted sex-type effects were not
obtained.

basic claim that sex-typed individuals are more gender-schematic than non-sex-typed individuals (see
Bem 1985 for a review). For example, to test whether
gender-schematic individuals, as identified by the
Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI; Bem 1974), spontaneously encode and organize information on the basis of the sex-linked associations that constitute the
gender schema, subjects were asked to remember
words presented in random order (Bem 1981). These
words included animal names, verbs, and articles of
clothing; one-third of each category had been judged
by undergraduatesas masculine (e.g., gorilla, hurling,
trousers),one-third as feminine (e.g., butterfly,blushing, bikini), and one-third as neutral (e.g., ant, stepping, sweater). In their recall of the items, genderschematic subjects were more likely than genderaschematic subjects to cluster the words on the basis
of gender, i.e., after having recalled a masculine item
to recall another masculine item and after having recalled a feminine item to recall another feminine
item. A related study measured subjects' response latencies when asked whether a series of attributes was
self-descriptive (Bem 1981). Gender-schematic subjects were faster than gender-aschematic subjects
when endorsing sex-appropriate attributes (e.g.,
"forceful" and "dominant" for male subjects and
"gentle" and "warm" for female subjects) and when
rejecting sex-inappropriate attributes. Finally, gender-schematic subjectswere more likely than genderaschematic subjects to choose sex-appropriateactivities (e.g., using a drill if they are male and knitting
and ironing if they are female) and to avoid sex-inappropriatebehavior. Moreover, while performing sexinappropriate activities, gender-schematic individuals feel significantlyworse than gender-aschematicindividuals (Bem and Lenney 1976).
Recently, several studies have tested but failed to

ender serves as an important social category in


all cultures, and each culture has developed a
rich network of associations that surround its concepts of "maleness" and "femaleness." However, individuals within a culture may differ from one another in the degree to which they utilize cultural
definitions of masculinity and femininity as standards against which they perceive, categorize, and
evaluate gender-related information. According to
Bem's (1981, 1985) gender schema theory, sex-typed
individuals tend to encode,and organize incoming information in terms of a gender schema, using the traditional bipolar masculinity/femininity dimension as
the organizing principle. Non-sex-typed individuals,
however,whenever feasible, use other, nongender-related dimensions to organize information and are
thus less likely than sex-typed individuals to engage
in gender-schematicprocessing. Moreover, sex-typed
individuals are concerned about keeping their behavior consistent with the culture's definitions of gender
appropriateness;non-sex-typed individuals, in contrast, are more likely to cross the traditional boundaries of masculinity and femininity.
Several studies have tested gender schema theory's
G

* Bernd H. Schmitt is a Ph.D. candidate in the Department of


Psychology, France Leclerc is a Ph.D. candidate in the Johnson
Graduate School of Management, and Laurette Dube-Rioux is a
Ph.D. candidate in the School of Hotel Administration, all at Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853. The order of the second and
third authors was determined by the flip of a coin. The authors wish
to thank Sandra L. Bem, Pradeep Kakkar, J. Edward Russo, and
three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this article; the authors also wish to thank Isabelle
Rougemont for her invaluable help in conducting Experiments 1
and 2, and Laurel Sgan and Deanna Silver for preparing the stimuli
and running the subjects in Experiment 3. A longer version of this
article may be obtained by writing to the first author, who will be
at the Columbia University Graduate School of Business, New
York, NY 10027, as of the 1988 fall semester.

confirm

vender schema

theorv

in the area of con-

122
?) JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH * Vol. 15 * June 1988

This content downloaded from 202.84.40.92 on Mon, 24 Nov 2014 01:04:47 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

SEX TYPING AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR

sumerbehavior. However, many of these studies were


iimited in scope, and some were flawed by methodological and conceptual shortcomings. Most studies
(e.g., Allison et al. 1980; Coughlin and O'Connor
1985; Gentry and Doering 1977; Gentry, Doering,
and O'Brien 1978) tested only one aspect of marketing (e.g., product choice or product perceptions) or
used only one measure of advertising effectiveness
(e.g., ad recall or purchase intent). Some studies did
not use the BSRI in determining individuals' sex type
(e.g., Gentryand Doering 1977;Gentry, Doering, and
O'Brien 1978; Gentry and Haley 1984).' Most importantly, the hypotheses of some studies are not derivable from gender schema theory. McIntyreet al. contended that they provided partial support for gender
schema theory because "emphasis on traditional
roles and on sexist products may . . . produce a com-

mercialthat is extremely salient, and offensive to people with a liberal gender schema" (1986, p. 190)-a
type of schema that does not exist in Bem's theory.
Similarly,Kahle and Homer hypothesized, on the basis of gender schema theory as they claimed, that
"males ought to value food more based on the 'size of
the kill' (e.g., cattle are harderto kill than chickens),
whereas the nutritional aspects of food ought to be
more important for women, who have historically
planned menus and prepared food" (1985, p. 243).
This hypothesis cannot be derived from gender
schema theory, because the theory does not make
claims about how role assignimientsat differenttimes
of human history influence the selection of lunch
items today. In sum, it is not clear whether gender
schema theory has been given a fair test in consumer
research.
This article's three experiments were designed to
test the theory extensively. In designing the three experiments, we tried to adhere as closely as possible to
experimental paradigms employed in previous psychological research that revealed positive results for
gender schema theory.
In Experiment 1, subjects judged jeans advertisements that depicted a man and a woman either in a
sex-role conformist or in a sex-role nonconformist
way. In a second part of the experiment, subjectsprovided appealratingsfor fragranceflacons (two masculine, two feminine). We predicted that gender-schematic subjects, compared to gender-aschematicindividuals, would show a more positive attitude toward
sex-role conformist advertisements than toward sexrole nonconformist advertisements. Additionally,
gender-schematic individuals should show stronger

' Bem (1985) has not excluded the use of other sex role inventories in a test of gender schema theory. However, Kelly, Furman,
and Young (1978) have shown that a large proportion of subjects
may be classified into differentsex-type categories, depending on
the inventoryused.

123

preferences than gender-aschematic individuals for


sex-appropriatefragranceflacons.
In Experiment 2, subjects were shown the names of
18 magazines (one-third masculine, one-third feminine, one-third neutral). We predicted that genderschematic individuals would be more likely than
gender-aschematic individuals to remember sex-appropriate magazines and, in their recall of the magazines, to cluster them on the basis of gender. After the
recall task, subjects were asked whether they would
be interested in readingeach magazine. Gender-schematic subjects should be more likely than genderaschematic subjects to choose sex-role conformist
magazines. Moreover, gender-schematic subjects
should be faster than gender-aschematic subjects
when making sex-role conformist choices and slower
when making sex-role nonconformist choices.
Experiment 3 was designed to test whether the two
groups would differ from one another in their memory for sex-role conformist and sex-role nonconformist advertisements. We predicted that gender-schematic individuals would be more likely to remember
sex-role conformist advertisements than sex-role
nonconformist advertisements. Gender-aschematic
individuals, however, should be less likely to show
such a differentialrecall.2

EXPERIMENT 1

Method
Experiment 1 was presented as a study on advertising and packaging, and 151 students from an undergraduatemarketingcourse at Cornell University participated. At the end of the study, subjects completed
the BSRI, which was used to determine subjects' sex
types.3 The sample contained 39 androgynous sub2
Recent researchin social cognition has shown that people are
often better at rememberingschema-consistentinformation (Taylor and Crocker 1981). The results of some studies, however, are
inconsistent with this prediction. For example, based on research
by Hastie and Kumar (1979), we would predict that gender-schematic individualswould be betterat rememberingsex-rolenonconformist rather than sex-role conformist advertisements,but gender-aschematicindividualswould be less likely to show this differential recall. In either case, we would predict differencesbetween
gender-schematicand gender-aschematicindividuals.
3 The BSRI is a paper-and-penciltest that asks subjects to indicate the extent to which each of 60 attributesdescribeshimself or
herself.The BSRIconsists of two 20-item scales that reflectAmerican culture's definition of masculinity (e.g., "self-reliant,""independent") and of femininity (e.g., "affectionate,""tender"), and
20 neutralitems (e.g., "happy,""sincere").In variouspsychometric analyses, Bem (1974) demonstratedthat the masculinity (M)
and femininity(F) scalesareorthogonal,that the BSRIis internally
consistent (coefficientalpha > 0.8) and test-retestreliable(r > 0.9),
and that high scores do not reflect a general tendency to respond
in a socially desirabledirection. Recently, Qualls (1987) provided
evidence forthe BSRI'sreliability(alpha> 0.85) and for its convergent and discriminantvalidity.
Each respondent'scomposite M and F scores were calculated,

This content downloaded from 202.84.40.92 on Mon, 24 Nov 2014 01:04:47 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

124

jects (21 males, 18 females), 37 undifferentiatedsubjects (18 males, 19 females), and 56 traditionally sextyped subjects (26 males, 30 females). According to
Bem (1985), androgynous and undifferentiated subjects are gender-aschematic, and traditionally sextyped subjects are gender-schematic. Because gender
schema theory does not make specific predictions
about cross-sex-typed individuals (Bem 1985, p.
195), they were excluded from subsequent analyses.
In the first part of Experiment 1, subjects saw 12
jeans advertisements. Jeans were selected as a product category because they are popular among college
students and because they qualify as a neutral product (Gentry, Doering, and O'Brien 1978). Each advertisement consisted of a male or female describing
himself or herselfwith a masculine statement (e.g., "I
seek out positions of authority."), a feminine statement (e.g., "I try to get others to notice the way I
dress."), or a neutral statement (e.g., "I admire free,
spontaneous people."). The person in the picture was
always the same male or female. Each subject saw
each person in the advertisement in connection with
two masculine, two feminine, and two neutral statements, resultingin 12 advertisements.The masculine
and feminine statements in the advertisements were
selected from the ANDRO PRF, a sex role inventory
that includes self-descriptive statements of the Personality ResearchForm (PRF); items of the PRF that
are not part of the ANDRO PRF and that had been
found to be non-sex-typed (Berzins, Welling, and
Wetter 1978) were selected as neutral statements. The
order of presentation of the 12 advertisements was
randomized across subjects. Using seven-point Likert-type scales, subjects indicated for each of the 12
advertisementshow much they liked the type of person portrayedin the advertisement, how similar they
thought they were to that type of person, and how
effective the advertisement might be in convincing
people to buy the product. This part of the experiment took the form of a 2 (sex of subject) X 3 (sex
type) X 2 (sex of person in the advertisement) X 3
(type of statement) experimentaldesign. The firsttwo
factorswere between-subjectsvariables,and the latter
factorswere within-subjectsvariables.
The stimuli of the second partof Experiment 1 were
professionally rendered drawings of four fragrance
flacons of differentshapes. Subjects indicated on sevand the M and F medians were computed. The median of the M
scale was 103 and the median of the F scale was 98. Following Bem
( 1985), a median split procedurewas used to divide the sampleinto
four groups:those scoring above the median on both scales were
categorized as androgynous;those scoring below the median on
both scales were categorized as undifferentiated;those scoring
above the median on the sex-congruentscale and below the median
on the sex-incongruentscale were categorizedas traditionallysextyped; those scoring below the median on the sex-congruentscale
and above the median on the sex-incongruentscale were categorized as cross-sex-typed.

en-point scales how appealing they would find each


flacon if they were to purchase a fragrancefor their
own use, for a male friend, and for a female friend.
The four flacons were selected on the basis of a pretest; 20 pretestsubjectshad ratedtwo flacons as "masculine" and two flacons as "feminine." The shapes of
the two masculine flacons were rectangular and the
shapes of the feminine flacons were round and more
refined. The second part took the form of a 2 (sex) X 3
(sex type) X 4 (flacon shape) design. The presentation
order of "flacon shape," the within-subjects variable,
was counterbalanced.

Results
Did gender-schematicand gender-aschematicsubjects differ in the way that they responded to the sextyped items of the PRF that were used as self-descriptive statements in the jeans advertisements?To provide such a manipulation check, subjects' responses
on the similarity measure were analyzed by a 2 (sex
of subject) X 3 (sex type) X 2 (sex of person in the
ad) X 3 (type of statement) ANOVA, which revealed a
significant three-way interaction of sex, sex typing,
and type of statement (F(6,270) = 76.92, p < 0.0001).
Although the schematic group, composed of traditionally sex-typed subjects, was sensitive to the sextyping of the masculine and feminine statements, the
gender-aschematicgroup, composed 'of androgynous
and undifferentiatedsubjects, did not respond differently to masculine and feminine statements. Specifically, schematic males identified more easily with
people in the advertisements who used masculine
ratherthan feminine statements (M = 3.99 versus M
= 2.82, Bonferroni-adjustedp < 0.0001), and schematic females identified more easily with people who
used feminine rather than masculine statements (M
= 4.1 1 versus M = 3.46, p < 0.0001). Aschematic subjects' similarity ratings, however, did not differ significantly for the masculine and feminine statements.
Thus, subjects responded differentlyto the sex-typed
statements in the advertisementsas a function of their
sex type: gender-schematic subjects, but not genderaschematic subjects, were sensitive to the sex-typing
of the statements and responded accordingly.
Yet, did gender-schematic subjects have a more
positive attitude toward sex-role conformist advertisements comparedto sex-role nonconformist advertisements, and did gender-aschematicsubjectsjudge
both advertisements similarly? To test this prediction, a composite score was calculated for all sex-role
conformist advertisements (i.e., advertisements in
which the male gave a masculine self-description and
the female gave a feminine self-description), for all
sex-role nonconformist advertisements (i.e., advertisements that showed the man's picture with a feminine statement and the woman's picture with a masculine statement), and for all neutral advertisements

This content downloaded from 202.84.40.92 on Mon, 24 Nov 2014 01:04:47 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

125

SEX TYPING AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR

(i.e., advertisements containing neutral statements).


All composite scores were divided by the number of
advertisementsto transformthe scores to the original
seven-point scale measurement unit. Subsequently, a
2 (sex) X 3 (sex type) X 3 (type of advertisement)MANOVA was conducted on the two attitude measures:
liking of the type of person portrayedin the advertisement and judged effectiveness of the advertisement.
The MANOVA revealed only one significanteffect:
a main effect for "type of advertisement" (Wilks's X
= 0.675-, F(4,498) = 27.06, p < 0.0001). The effect

was also significant in the subsequent ANOVAs conducted separately on the two dependent variables
(F(2,124) = 46.03,p<0.0001

andF(2,124)

= 25.78,

p < 0.0001). Neutral advertisementswere liked better


and judged to be more effective than sex-role conformist and sex-role nonconformist advertisements
(M = 4.52 versus M = 3.80 and M = 3.89 for liking;
M = 3.79 versus M = 3.14 and M = 2.63 for effectiveness). However, the interaction of sex-type and
"type of advertisement,"predictedby gender schema
theory, was not significant in the overall MANOVA
or in the univariate ANOVAs (Fs < 1.00). Thus, the
advertisement part of Experiment 1 did not provide
support for gender schema theory. Although traditionally sex-typed subjects, in comparison to androgynous and undifferentiatedsubjects, noticed the sextyping of the advertisements, it had no impact on
their attitudes toward the advertisements.
Next, subjects' preference ratings, provided in the
second part of Experiment 1, were analyzed by a 2
(sex) X 3 (sex type) X 4 (flacon shape) MANOVA on
the three dependent variablesthat measured how appealing subjects found a particular fragrance flacon
for their own use, for a male friend, and for a female
friend.The MANOVA revealeda significantsex main
effect (X = 0.857, F(3,124) = 6.90, p < 0.001), a significant sex type effect (X = 0.875, F(6,248) = 2.85, p

< 0.05), and a significant effect for flacon shape (X


= 0.262, F(9,9 10) = 74.20, p < 0.001). In addition,

there was a significant interaction of sex and flacon


shape (X = 0.487, F(9,9 10) = 34.79, p < 0.000 1). Be-

cause the interaction of sex type and flacon shape was


not significant (p > 0.25), gender schema theory's
predictions could not be supported.
The main effect of sex type was due to the androgynous subjects, who, independent of flacon shape,
found all the flacons more appealing than did the undifferentiatedand traditionally sex-typed subjects (p
< 0.05). The sex effect was due to females' tendency
to find the fragranceflacons more appealing than the
males did. The significant main effect of flacon shape
and the significantinteraction of sex and flacon shape
were further explored by ANOVAs conducted on
each measure and three subsequent orthogonal contrasts. In the fi-rstcontrast, the two masculine shapes
were comparedwith the two feminine shapes and the
other two contrasts tested differenceswithin the mas-

culine and the feminine shapes. Not surprisingly,for


their own use, male subjectsselected masculine rather
than feminine shapes, and female subjects preferred
feminine rather than masculine shapes. For male
friends, both sexes selected the masculine shapes over
the feminine shapes, and for female friends, both
sexes selected the feminine shapes over the masculine
shapes. All these mean differenceswere significant at
p < 0.0001. In addition to these strong differencesbetween masculine and feminine flacons, subjects also
perceiveddifferenceswithin the two flacon categories.
For example, one of the two masculine flacons was
consistently judged to be more appealingfor men and
less appealing for women than the other masculine
flacon. Interestingly enough, this difference emerged
only on some measures (i.e., own use, males judging
use for male friend, and females judging use for female friend), but not on others (i.e., females judging
use for male friend, and males judging use for female
friend). The difference between the two stimuli was
thus quite subtle, yet the effect was not mediated by
subjects' sex types.
Finally, analyses were performed on all the dependent variables of Experiment 1, including only those
subjects who scored in the upper and lower quartiles
of the M and F scale. This extreme group analysis revealed exactly the same pattern of results found in the
analyses reportedpreviously.

EXPERIMENT 2
Method
Forty-six subjects (20 males, 26 females) from the
sample of Experiment 1 volunteered to participate in
Experiment 2, which was conducted about a week
later. The responses of cross-sex-typed subjects and
the responsesof three subjectswho failed to follow the
instructions were excluded from subsequentanalyses.
The sample consisted of 17 gender-schematicsubjects
(seven males, 10 females) and 22 gender-aschematic
subjects (nine males, 13 females).
Eighteen popular American magazines were selected as stimuli for the experiment. The magazines
were classified as masculine (e.g., Car & Driver,
Sports Illustrated), feminine (e.g., Cosmopolitan,
Family Circle), or neutral (e.g., Newsweek, Tennis)
on the basis of two criteria:(1) the ratio of male versus
female readership according to the 1986 marketing
report of the Simmons Market Bureau, Inc., and
(2) the magazine pretest ratings of 20 undergraduates
who made their judgments on seven-point scales
ranging from "predominantly read by men" to "predominantly read by women"-the midpoint was labeled "equal number of male and female readership."
Magazines that, according to the Simmons report,
were predominantly (>70 percent) read by men (or
by women) and received extreme ratingsin the pretest

This content downloaded from 202.84.40.92 on Mon, 24 Nov 2014 01:04:47 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

126

THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

were categorized as masculine (or feminine); magazines were categorized as neutral if they had an approximately equal number (between 40 percent and
60 percent) of male and female readershipand if they
received ratings around the midpoint of the scale in
the pretest.
Subjects were run individually. They were first
asked to sit in front of an Apple McIntosh computer
and to read the following instructions presented on
the computer screen: "Imagine you are waiting in a
doctor's office and the following magazines are on a
table." At this point, the names of the magazineswere
presentedon the screen, one afterthe other in random
order, for a duration of 500 ms (interstimulus interval: 500 ms). After having been exposed to 18 magazines, subjects were asked to enter the names of the
magazinesthey could remember.
Next, subjects were shown each stimulus again.
They were asked to press a yes key if they would be
interestedin readingthe magazine and a no key if they
would not. The yes key was always assigned to a subject's dominant hand. Subjectswere unawarethat the
computer programtimed their responses.

Results
Free Recall of Magazines. Gender-schematic and
gender-aschematicsubjects displayed no differences
in terms of the number of ;magazines they recalled
within each magazine-type category. A 2 (sex) X 2
(sex type) MANOVA, conducted on the percentageof
masculine, feminine, and neutral magazines recalled,
revealed a significant sex effect (Wilks's X = 0.651,
F(2,32) = 8.59, p < 0.001); the main effect of sex typing and the interaction of sex and sex typing, predicted by gender schema theory, were not significant
(Fs < 1.00). In subsequent ANOVAs, the sex effect
was significant for the masculine magazines (F( 1,38)
= 9.94, p < 0.01) and for the feminine magazines
(F(1,38) = 16.85, p < 0.001), but not for the neutral
magazines (F < 1.00). Males remembered significantly more masculine magazines than females did
(M = 41.40 percent versus 28.86 percent), and females rememberedsignificantly more feminine magazines than males did (M = 37.70 percent versus
22.41 percent). There was no differencein the relative
number of neutral magazines that subjects recalled.
Clustering in Recall. Although gender-schematic
and gender-aschematicsubjects did not differin their
free recall of sex-typed magazines, they might nonetheless have remembered the magazines in different
orders. Specifically, gender-schematic individuals
should be more likely than gender-aschematic individuals to recall magazines by gender, i.e., to recall in
sequences of masculine and feminine magazines. To
test this prediction, we calculated an index of gender

clustering (Bem 1981).4 Contrary to the predictions


made on the basis of gender schema theory, more gender clustering was found in the recall of genderaschematic females than in the recall of gender-schematic females (M = 30.6 percent versus M = 15.5 per-

cent). This difference was statistically significant,


(F(1,21) = 4.58, p < 0.05). Schematic and aschematic
males showed no difference in terms of clustering (F
< 1.00).

Magazine Choice. A 2 (sex) X 2 (sex type) X 3 (magazine type) ANOVA was conducted on the number of
magazines subjectsindicated they might be interested
in reading. Magazine type and the interaction of sex
and magazine type were significant (F(2,66) = 16.00,
p < 0.0001 and F(2,66) = 19.84, p < 0.0001). Other
effects, including the interaction of sex type and magazine type, were not significant. Pairwise comparisons using Bonferroni'sprocedurerevealed that male
subjects were more likely to choose masculine and
neutral magazines rather than feminine magazines
(M = 3.73 and M = 3.87 versus M = 1.00; p

<

0.05),

and female subjectswanted to read feminine and neutral magazines rather than masculine magazines (M
= 3.21 and M = 3.83 versus M = 1.88;p < 0.05).
Response Latencies. The previous analysis indicated that sex type did not affect the overall tendency
to make sex-role conformist choices. However,
were gender-schematic subjects faster than genderaschematic subjects in making sex-role conformist
choices (e.g., females saying "no" to a masculine
magazine and "yes" to a feminine magazine) and
slower in making sex-role nonconformist choices
(e.g., females saying "yes" to a masculine magazine
and "no" to a feminine magazine)?Results indicated
that this was not the case. Instead, female subjects
tended to respond faster than male subjects. In the
two 2 (sex) X 2 (sex type) ANOVAs conducted on sexrole conformist and sex-role nonconformist responses, this sex effect was the only effect that approached significance (p = 0.12 and p = 0.07).

EXPERIMENT 3
Method
Out of 312 undergraduatesfrom an introductory
psychology class who completed the BSRI, 60 students were recruited to participate in Experiment 3
on the basis of their BSRI scores. Thirty subjects (15
males, 15 females) were gender-schematic and 30
subjects (15 males, 15 females) were gender-aschematic. The responses of one male subject had to be
eliminated because he did not complete the memory
test.
4 The index is based on a simple formulaby Shuell (1969), which
has been frequentlyused in researchon categoryclustering.

This content downloaded from 202.84.40.92 on Mon, 24 Nov 2014 01:04:47 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

SEX TYPING AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR

Subjects were told that they would be shown 30


slides of advertisementsthat might appear in a local,
college student newspaper. Each slide would be
shown for five seconds and would show a college student reading a magazine. In half of the slides, the
magazine reader was a male college student; in the
other half of the slides, the readerwas a female college
student. The factor "sex of reader"was crossed with
the factor "type of magazine." One-thirdof the magazines were masculine, one-third feminine, and onethird neutral. The order of the slides was randomized
across subjects. The design was a 2 (sex of subject)
X 2 (seqxtype) X 2 (sex of reader) X 3 (type of magazine) factorial design.
Nongender-relatedinformation was also systematically varied in the slides to test whether, as gender
schema theory would predict, possible memory
effects are specific to gender-related information.
Specifically,in half of the slides, the readerwas standing, and, in the other half of the slides, s/he was sitting; half of the readershad black hair, and the other
half had blonde hair; half of the slides contained a
window in the background, and the other half contained no window.
After watching the slides, subjects were asked for
each magazine: (1) whether the person reading the
magazine was male or female, (2) whether s/he was
sitting or standing, (3) whether s/he had black or
blonde hair, and (4) whether or not a window was in
the background.

Results
Subjects' recall for nongender-relatedcharacteristics in each type of advertisement was assessed to test
whether gender-schematic and gender-aschematic
subjects differedin their overall memory capacity. A
MANOVA and three 2 (sex of subject) X 2 (sex type)
X 2 (sex of reader) X 3 (type of magazine) ANOVAs
revealedno sex-type effects.
However, gender-schematic and gender-aschematic subjects also did not differ in their memory for
gender-related information. In the 2 X 2 X 2 X 3
ANOVA conducted on the relative percentage of
male and female readers that subjects correctly remembered, sex type was not significant as a main
effect or in any interaction (all ps > 0.25). Instead,
the main effect of sex of reader (F(1,55) = 16.20, p

127

shown a feminine magazine, ratherthan a masculine


or neutral magazine (M = 4.05 versus M 3.77 and M

= 3.81). Moreover, an exploration of the interaction


of sex of reader and type of magazine indicated that
when a male was shown in the advertisements, subjects best remembered he was male when he read a
masculine or neutral magazine ratherthan a feminine
magazine (M = 81.4 percent and M = 84.4 percent

versus M = 72.2 percent); a female was best remembered when she read a feminine magazine ratherthan
a masculine or neutral magazine (M = 77.2 percent
versus M = 70 percent and M = 71.6 percent). That

is, all subjects, independent of their own sex and sex


type, rememberedthe sex of the readerin the sex-role
conformist slides better than the sex of the reader in
the sex-role nonconformist slides.

GENERAL DISCUSSION
To examine the relevance of gender schema theory
to the study of consumer behavior, we conducted
three experiments that tested predictions related to
advertising, packaging, consumer choice, and consumer memory. The experiments used three different
product categories(jeans, fragrances,and magazines)
and several dependent measures (attitude measures,
preference judgments, memory measures, choice
measures, and response latencies). We thus provided
a good opportunity for positive results to emerge;
however, we did not find any support for gender
schema theory.5
Although we took great care to minimize methodological shortcomings, our null effects may be partly
due to our sex-type measure or to the stimuli used in
the studies. Specifically, the BSRI and Bem's categorization procedure have recently been criticized
(Spence 1984). Paulhus (1987) has arguedthat the orthogonal M and F scales of the BSRI are inappropriate measures of the concept of gender schematism.
Another possible explanation for some of our null results may be that in some of our experiments we employed highly sex-typed stimuli (e.g., the flacons in
Experiment 1 and the magazines in Experiments 2
and 3) that may be poorly suited to detect subtle sextype differences.However, it is unclear why it should
be easier in principle to obtain sex effects ratherthan
sex-type effects. For example, some of the stimuli that

< 0.001), the interaction of sex of readerand type of


magazine (F(2,1 10) = 5.74, p < 0.01), and the inter-

action of sex of subject and type of magazine


(F(2,1 10) = 5.54, p < 0.01) were significant. Pairwise
comparisons using Bonferroni's procedure revealed
that males were more likely to remembercorrectlythe
sex of the reader when they were shown masculine
and neutral rather than feminine magazines (M
= 3.80 and M = 3.95 versus M = 3.42). Female sub-

jects remembered the sex of the reader better when

5 BesidesBem'sgenderschematheory, Markus'sself-schematheory also makes predictionsabout individual differencesin the processingof gender-relatedinformation(Markuset al. 1982). The two
theoriesdifferabout who should be consideredas gender-schematic
and gender-aschematic.Self-schematheoryproposesthat androgynous individuals are gender-bischematic(i.e., they process both
masculineand feminine informationschematically),and traditionally sex-typedsubjectsare either masculine or feminine schematic.
The only truly gender-aschematicindividuals, according to selfschematheory, are the undifferentiated.Additionalanalysesof our
data, however,also revealedno supportfor self-schematheory.

This content downloaded from 202.84.40.92 on Mon, 24 Nov 2014 01:04:47 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

128

THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Bem (1981) used in her clustering study were highly


sex-typed(i.e., clothing articles),but she reportedsextype effects without obtaining sex effects. Moreover,
in the fragrance part of Experiment 1, besides the
strong differences between the masculine and feminine flacons, we also obtained subtle effects, but even
these subtle effectswere not mediated by sex type. Finally, in the clustering measure in Experiment 2, we
did find a sex-type effect, but it was inconsistent with
the clainmsof the theory. Therefore, it seems unlikely
that our null results can be attributedto the nature of
our stimuli.
Rather, the results in this article suggestthat a person's sex type may not be a powerful individual
differencevariable in many contexts relevant to consumer behavior. Thus, it might be inappropriatetoday to employ the typology suggested by gender
schema theory as part of a marketing strategy or
to incorporate gender schematism as an explanatory
construct in theoretical models of consumer behavior.
[Received June 198 7. Revised December 198 7.1

REFERENCES
Allison, Neil K., Linda L. Golden, Gary M. Millet, and
Donna Coogan (1980), "Sex-typed Product Images:
The Effects of Sex, Sex Role Self Concept, and MeasurementImplications,"in Advancesin ConsumerResearch, Vol. 7, ed. JerryC. Olson, Ann Arbor, MI: Association for ConsumerResearch,604-609.
Bem, SandraL. (1974), "The Measurementof Psychological Androgyny," Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology,42 (April), 155-162.
(1981), "Gender Schema Theory: A Cognitive Account of Sex Typing,"PsychologicalReview, 88 (July),
354-364.
(1985), "Androgynyand Gender Schema Theory:A
Conceptual and Empirical Integration," in Nebraska
Symposiumon Motivation:Psychologyand Gender,ed.
Theo B. Sonderegger,Lincoln, NE: University of NebraskaPress, 179-226.
and Ellen Lenney (1976), "Sex Typing and the
Avoidance of Cross-SexBehavior,"Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33 (January),48-54.
Berzins, Juris I., MarthaA. Welling, and Robert E. Wetter
(1978), "A New Measureof Psychological Androgyny
Based on the Personality Research Form," Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46 (February),
126-138.
Coughlin, Maureenand Paul J. O'Connor(1985), "Gender
Role Portrayalsin Advertising:An Individual Differences Analysis," in Advances in ConsumerResearch,
Vol. 12, eds. Elizabeth C. Hirschman and Morris B.
Holbrook, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 238-241.
Gentry,JamesW. and MildredDoering (1977), "Masculin-

ity-Feminity Related to Consumer Choice," in Contemporary Marketing Thought, eds. Barnett A.


Greenberg and Danny N. Bellinger, Chicago, IL:
American MarketingAssociation, 423-427.
, Mildred Doering, and TerrenceV. O'Brien (1978),
"Masculinity and Femininity Factors in Product Perception and Self Image,"in Advancesin ConsumerResearch, Vol. 5, ed. H. Keith Hunt, Provo, UT: Association for ConsumerResearch,326-332.
and Debra A. Haley (1984), "Gender Schema Theory as a Predictor of Ad Recall," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 11, ed. Thomas C. Kinnear,
Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 259264.
Hastie, Reid and Purohit A. Kumar (1979), "Person Memory: Personality Traits as Organizing Principles in
Memoryfor Behaviors,"Journalof Personalityand Social Psychology, 37 (January),25-30.
Kahle, Lynn R. and Pamela Homer (1985), "Androgyny
and Midday Mastication: Do Real Men Eat Quiche?"
in Advancesin ConsumerResearch,Vol. 12, eds. Elizabeth C. Hirschman and Morris B. Holbrook, Provo,
UT: Association for Consumer Research,242-246.
Kelly, Jeffrey A., Wyndol Furman, and Veronica Young
(1978), "Problems Associated with the Typological
Measurement of Sex Roles and Androgyny," Journal
of Consultingand Clinical Psychology,46 (December),
1574- 1576.
Markus,Hazel, Marie Crane, Stan Bernstein, and Michael
Siladi (1982), "Self-Schema and Gender," Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 42 (January), 3850.
McIntyre, Pat, Harmon H. Hosch, Richard J. Harris, and
D. Wayne Norvell (1986), "Effectsof Sex and Attitudes
TowardWomen on the Processingof Television Commercials," Psychology and Marketing, 3 (Summer),
181-190.
Paulhus, Delroy L. (1987), "The Effectsof Group Selection
on Correlations and Factor Patterns in Sex Role Research,"Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
53 (August), 314-317.
Qualls, William J. (1987), "Household Decision Behavior:
The Impactof Husbands'and Wives' Sex Role Orientation," Journal of ConsumerResearch, 14 (September),
264-279.
Shuell, Thomas (1969), "Clustering and Organization in
Free Recall," Psychological Bulletin, 72 (December),
353-374.
Spence, Janet T. (1984), "Masculinity, Femininity and
Gender-Related Traits: A Conceptual Analysis and
Critique of CurrentResearch," in Progress in Experimental PersonalityResearch:Normal Personality Processes, Vol. 13, eds. BrendanA. Malerand WinfriedB.
Maler,New York: Academic Press, 1-97.
Taylor, Susan E. and Jennifer Crocker(1981), "Schematic
Basesof Social InformationProcessing,"in Social Cognition: The Ontario Symposium, Vol. 1, eds. E. Tory
Higgins et al., Hillsdale, NJ: LawrenceErlbaumAssociates, 89-134.

This content downloaded from 202.84.40.92 on Mon, 24 Nov 2014 01:04:47 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like