You are on page 1of 8

Current Directions in Psychological

Science
http://cdp.sagepub.com/

Cognitive Strategy Variations During Aging


Patrick Lemaire
Current Directions in Psychological Science 2010 19: 363
DOI: 10.1177/0963721410390354
The online version of this article can be found at:
http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/19/6/363

Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Association for Psychological Science

Additional services and information for Current Directions in Psychological Science can be found at:
Email Alerts: http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts
Subscriptions: http://cdp.sagepub.com/subscriptions
Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav
Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> Version of Record - Dec 14, 2010


What is This?

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com by Cristina Trifan on October 27, 2013

Cognitive Strategy Variations


During Aging

Current Directions in Psychological


Science
19(6) 363-369
The Author(s) 2010
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0963721410390354
http://cdps.sagepub.com

Patrick Lemaire
Universite de Provence and Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique

Abstract
The last two decades of research in cognitive aging have seen a shift from simply describing age-related changes in cognitive
performance to determining the mechanisms underlying these changes. Recent findings on variations in the use of cognitive
strategies during aging further our understanding of how these changes in performance occur during adulthood. Data show
age-related differences in strategy repertoire, strategy distribution, strategy execution, and strategy selection. I illustrate these
findings in cognitive domains as varied as episodic memory, working memory, reasoning, decision making, problem solving, and
language. I discuss how strategic variations are best studied both conceptually and methodologically and how investigating
strategic variations helps us make significant progress in the study of cognitive aging. As I also show in this article, whichever the
cognitive domain being studied, there are no restrictions that would prevent us from adopting a strategy perspective.
Keywords
cognitive aging, strategies, problem solving

Research in cognitive aging has two fundamental goals. First,


researchers want to know how cognition changes with age.
Aging often has negative effects on cognitive performance;
sometimes has no effects; and, fairly rarely, has positive effects
(Craik & Salthouse, 2007). The second goal of research in
cognitive aging is to understand the mechanisms underlying
age-related changes and stability in cognitive performance.
To discover these mechanisms, researchers have tested two
types of factors: so-called quantitative and qualitative factors.
Quantitative factors include such parameters as processing
resources (e.g., processing speed, working memory capacities).
Qualitative factors include factors like different cognitive
strategies used in problem solving.
A strategy perspective offers a great window to understanding cognitive aging and is not incompatible with quantitative
approaches. It helps to better describe and explain (in mechanistic terms) age-related differences and similarities and how
changes occur during adulthood. It enables us to understand
how people of different ages think and how (some) older adults
can compensate in order to moderate the effects of aging on
cognition. Recent data in a wide variety of cognitive domains
have shown individual variations in cognitive strategies during
aging. Age differences have been found in each strategy dimension involved in cognitive performance identified by Lemaire
and Siegler (1995). Young adults differ from older adults in
strategy repertoire (or which strategies they use to accomplish

a cognitive task), strategy distribution (or how often they use


each strategy), strategy execution (or how fast and accurate
they are with each strategy), and in strategy selection (or how
they choose among strategies on a given problem).

Strategic Variations During Aging


Age-related changes in strategy repertoire
Many theoretical and empirical works on aging assume that
young adults and older adults approach cognitive tasks the
same way. However, this is often not the case.
Strategy repertoires in different domains in which agerelated differences have been found are listed in Table 1. For
example, Dunlosky and Hertzog (2001) found that adults over
60 years old were less likely to spontaneously use effective
mediators (e.g., making a mental image or generating a sentence) for encoding paired associates than were adults in their
twenties. As another example, Lemaire and Arnaud (2008)
asked participants to solve two-digit addition problems and
assessed (via verbal protocols) the strategies participants used
Corresponding Author:
Patrick Lemaire, Universite de Provence & CNRS, Case D, 3 Place Victor
Hugo, 13331 Marseille, France
E-mail: patrick.lemaire@univ-provence.fr

364

Lemaire

Table 1. Strategies Used (or Not) by Young and Older People to (a) Solve Two-Digit Addition Problems (Lemaire & Arnaud, 2008), (b) Verify
Arithmetic Inequalities (Duverne & Lemaire, 2004), (c) Encode Pairs of Words in Episodic Memory (Dunlsoky & Hertzog, 2001), and (d) Search
Information in Decision-Making Tasks (Johnson, 1990).
Two-digit addition
problem-solving task
(e.g., 12 46)
Rounding the first operand down
(10 46) 2
Rounding the second operand down
(12 40) 6
Rounding both operands down
(10 40) (2 6)
Columnar retrieval
(2 6) (10 40)
Rounding the first operand up
(20 46) 8
Rounding the second operand up
(12 50) 4
Rounding both operands up
(20 50) 8 4
Borrowing units
18 40
Retrieving
58

Arithmetic problem
verification task

Episodic memory:
Paired-associate recall task

Information search strategies in


decision making

Exhaustive-verification strategy
Encoding the problem,
searching for correct solution
in memory, comparing this
solution with the proposed
solution, making a true/false
decision, and responding
(e.g., 8  5 < 41. True? False?)
Approximate verification strategy
Encoding the problem, judging
the plausibility of the proposed
answer, making a true/false
decision, and responding
(e.g., 8  5 < 47. True? False?)

Interactive mental imagery


strategy
Making a mental image that
relates the two words of each
pair (e.g., for glasswater,
imagining someone drinking
a glass of water)
Sentence-generation strategy
Making a sentence with both
words of each pair (e.g., for
glasswater, giving the
sentence The boy drinks a
glass of water)
Rote repetition strategy
Mentally repeating both words
of each pair (e.g., glasswater/
glasswater/glasswater)
Other or no strategy

Compensatory attribute comparison


strategy
Comparing multiple attributes
for each alternative (e.g., price,
comfort for a Honda or a
Volkswagen)
Noncompensatory alternative
comparison strategy
Comparing multiple alternatives for each attribute (e.g.,
comparing Honda and Volkswagen on price only)

on each trial. It was found that young individuals used a larger


number of strategies than did older individuals. Similarly, participants performance in Duverne and Lemaires (2004) study
suggest that young adults used both the exhaustive and approximate verification strategies to verify arithmetic inequalities,
whereas older adults used only the exhaustive verification strategy (a result confirmed with event-related potentials, ERP, data
by El Yagoubi, Lemaire, & Besson, 2005). For example, to
determine whether 5 7 < 13 is correct or incorrect, they used
an exhaustive verification strategy consisting in retrieving the
correct solution (12) in memory and comparing that to the proposed solution (13). To verify whether 5 7 < 19, they do not
need to retrieve the correct answer in memory; they can quickly
reject the proposed answer, as it is too far from the correct
answer and is, thus, too implausible. When Johnson (1990)
asked participants to search information about different alternative cars (attributes such as purchase pricing or resale value)
in order to decide which car to buy, she found that young adults
used the so-called compensatory strategy (e.g., taking both into
account price and comfort) and older adults the noncompensatory strategy (e.g. focusing on one feature such as price). These
studies show that, whenever possible, assessing which strategy
is used on each item is the most appropriate way to determine
whether young and older individuals differ in how they accomplish a given task.

Age-related changes in strategy distributions


When young and older adults use the same strategies, they
may differ in how often they select each available strategy.
Age-related differences both in how many participants use each

available strategy and in how often each participant chooses


each strategy have been found.
Examples of domains in which age-related differences in
strategy distribution have been found are shown in Figure 1.
For example, Cohen and Faulkner (1983) found different numbers of participants using each available strategy in a rotatedfigure task. In this task, participants are presented with one
of eight versions of a human figure and are required to respond
left or right according to which hand of the figure is holding a
ball. Four strategies are available: rotation (inverted figures are
converted to an upright version by a mental rotation of 180 in
the two-dimensional picture plane), flip (inverted figures are
mentally flipped through the third dimension around the horizontal axis), body reference (people use their own body as a
reference and mentally align themselves to correspond with the
position of the figure), and the rule strategy (the individual formulates a rule such as for inverted views, the left hand is on the
right). More young participants used the rule strategy, more
older adults used the rotation and body-reference strategies, and
only young adults used the flip strategy. Hartley and Anderson
(1983) reported similar age differences in the number of participants using different inductive reasoning strategies, with more
young adults using the optimal strategy and only older adults
selecting the nonoptimal strategy.
Problem-by-problem assessments of strategies have also
revealed age-related differences in strategy distributions.
For example, in a study on numerosity estimation (i.e., participants are asked to estimate the number of dots briefly presented
on a computer screen), verbal protocols and eye movement
recordings on each trial revealed that participants used several
strategies (Gandini, Lemaire, & Dufau, 2008). Some strategies

365

Number of Participants

Strategic Variations During Aging

6
5

Young
Older

4
3
2
1
0

Number of Participants

Rule

Rotation

Percent Use

35
30
25
20
15
10
5
0

Flip

25
Young
Older

20
15
10
5
0
Optimal

Body Reference

Suboptimal

Nonoptimal

Decomposition/
Recomposition

Approximate Counting

Young
Older

Anchoring

Estimation

Exact Counting

Fig. 1. Age-related differences in strategy distributions used by young and older participants. Panel a shows the number of participants using
rule, rotation, body reference, and flip strategies in a rotated-figure task (data from Cohen & Faulkner, 1983); panel b shows the mean number of
participants using optimal, suboptimal, and nonoptimal strategies in an inductive reasoning task (data from Hartley & Anderson, 1983); and panel
c shows participants mean percent use of five numerosity estimation strategies (anchoring, perceptual estimation, decomposition/recomposition, approximate counting, and exact counting; data from Gandini, Lemaire, & Dufau, 2008).

(anchoring and perceptual estimation) were used equally often


by both age groups, whereas the groups differed in their use of
other strategies (decomposition/recomposition, approximate
counting, and exact counting).

Age-related changes in strategy execution


To assess age-related differences in strategy execution, Siegler and Lemaire (1997) proposed using the choice/no-choice
method, in which participants in one condition are instructed
to use a given strategy on all items and in another condition to
choose among available strategies. As all participants use all
available strategies on all items, strategy performance and age
differences in strategy performance are not contaminated by

strategy repertoire, distributions, and selection (i.e., a fast


strategy could be used more often than a slow strategy by
young adults and equally often by older adults, thereby slowing down older adults). Several studies have found true age
differences and similarities in strategy performance when
other strategy dimensions are controlled (Fig. 2).
For example, Mata, Schooler, and Rieskamp (2007)
asked participants to forage for fish in a virtual landscape
and to decide when to move between ponds so as to maximize the number of fish caught. They found that older
adults performed more poorly than young adults when
executing the incremental strategy (i.e., deciding on an initial staying time at a pond and then increasing it with each
capture).

366

Lemaire

Young
Older

Short Travel
Time Environment

Accuracy Rates

Young
Older

70
65
60
55
Interactive Imagery

5000

IndividualInteractive
Imagery

Young
Older

4000
3000
2000
1000
0
Perceptual Estimation

80
75

6000

Long Travel
Time Environment

Verification Times (in ms)

Solution Latencies (in ms)

100
90
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0

Mean Number of Captures

Anchoring

2500
Young
Older

2000
1500
1000
500
0
Linguistic

Pictorial

Fig. 2. Age-related differences in strategy execution for young and older participants. Panel a shows mean number of fish caught in two environments in which participants were instructed to use one of two incremental strategies to accomplish a foraging task (data from Mata, Schooler,
& Rieskamp, 2007); panel b shows mean solution times to accomplish numerosity estimation tasks using perceptual estimation or anchoring
strategies (data from Gandini, Lemaire, Anton, & Nazarian, 2008); panel c shows mean number of correctly recognized pairs of words in an
associative recognition test, using interactive imagery versus individual interactive imagery (data from Patterson & Hertzog, 2010); panel d
shows mean verification times to determine whether sentences are consistent with patterns in a sentence verification task, using linguistic versus
pictorial strategies (data from Cohen & Faulkner, 1983).

In some tasks, some strategies yield equal levels of performance in young and older adults, whereas other strategies yield
different levels of performance between age groups. For example, Gandini, Lemaire, Anton, & Nazarian (2008) asked participants to find the approximate number of dots in dot
collections briefly displayed on a computer screen. Older
adults were slower than young adults while executing the
anchoring strategy (i.e., counting groups of dots and adding the
number of groups before estimating the remaining dots), but
both age groups were equally fast when executing the perceptual strategy (i.e., scanning the whole pattern or a subset of
dots, searching a corresponding numerosity in memory, and
adding or subtracting small amounts to this retrieved numerosity before stating a response). Gandini, Lemaire, Anton, &
Nazarian (2008) have found different brain networks underlying execution of each strategy in young and older adults.
Similarly, Patterson and Hertzog (2010) found that young
adults obtained better performance than older adults did in an
associative recognition test while using an interactive imagery
strategy (i.e., forming an interactive image for the two words in

each pair) but found no age differences when both groups


executed the individual interactive imagery strategy (i.e., first
forming an image of each word, then forming an interactive
image for the words in the pair). Cohen and Faulkner (1983)
found larger age-related differences in a sentencepicture verification task when participants executed a linguistic strategy (i.e.,
describing the picture with a sentence and comparing the two sentences) than when they executed a pictorial strategy. These data
illustrate a more general and interesting point: When we investigate cognitive aging with a strategy perspective, we can find true
age differences in participants performance but also true age
similarities even within the same domain or the same task.

Age-related differences in strategy selection


Even when young and older adults use the same strategies, use
available strategies equally often, and execute strategies
equally efficiently, age-related differences in participants
performance can be found. How participants choose among
strategies may vary with age. Previous research has found that

Strategic Variations During Aging

Young

1.6

Older

1.4
1.2
1

0.8
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
Hard Items

Percent Use of the Best Strategy

Easy Items

Percent Use of the Best Strategy

1.8

Mean Number of Items Retrieved

367

80
Young

70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
SmallUnit Problems
Young

70

Older

LargeUnit Problems

Older

60
50
40
30
20
10
0

No Emphasis

Emphasis

Fig. 3. Age-related differences in strategy selection. Panel a shows mean number of retrieves on easy and hard problems in a synthetic arithmetic problem-solving task (data from Onyper, Hoyer, & Cerella, 2008); panel b shows mean percent use of the best strategy on small-unit (e.g.,
31  82) and large-unit (e.g., 27  69) problems in a computational estimation task (data from Lemaire, Arnaud, & Lecacheur, 2004); panel c
shows mean percent use of the best strategy in a computational estimation task when participants were instructed to try to be most accurate
versus no accuracy emphasis (data from Lemaire et al., 2004)

young and older adults differ in which strategies they select on


problems, in selecting the best strategy for a problem, or in
changing their strategies as a function of situation parameters
or relative strategy efficacy (Fig. 3).
As an example, Lemaire, Arnaud, and Lecacheur (2004)
asked young and older adults to find approximate products for
arithmetic problems like 46  52. For each problem, participants were asked to choose the best among two strategies
(i.e., the strategy that yields the most accurate product): rounding up and rounding down. In the rounding up strategy, participants rounded both operands up to the nearest 10 (e.g., 50 
60). In the rounding down strategy, participants rounded both
operands down (e.g., 40  50). One nice feature of this task
is that it is easy to know for each problem which is the best
strategy: The rounding down strategy yields the best product
for problems like 51  62, whereas the rounding up strategy
is best for problems like 57  69. Young adults selected the
best strategy on 65% of the problems, significantly more often
than did older adults (57%). Young adults were able to select
the best strategy on both small-unit problems (e.g., 41  74)
and large-unit problems (e.g., 57  38), whereas older adults
selected the best strategy only on small-unit problems.

Moreover, young adults were more able than older adults to


improve their strategy selection under accuracy pressure (see
Onyper, Hoyer, & Cerella, 2008, for similar findings in skill
acquisition).

Mechanisms of Strategic Changes


During Aging
Why do young and older adults differ in strategy repertoire,
distribution, execution, and selection? Several recent studies
suggest that such age differences in strategies may result from
age-related decreases in processing resources. For example,
regarding episodic memory, Bouazzaoui et al. (2010) recently
found that 82% of age-related variance in mental memory
strategy use was accounted for by executive functions. Duverne
and Lemaire (2004) found that 70% of age-related variance in
strategy use in arithmetic problem-solving tasks was mediated
by processing speed. Whether decrease in all processing
resources (processing speed, working memory, inhibition,
executive control) or only in a subset is responsible for
age-related changes in strategies is unknown. Duverne and
Lemaire (2004) found that processing speed was the predictor

368
of age-related differences in arithmetic problem-solving strategies. Taconnat et al. (2009) found that executive functions were
the main mediator of age-related differences in memory strategies. Whether strategic changes with age are the result of
changes in all processing resources or just some (and in that
case, which ones) may depend on several parameters, such as
cognitive domain or how (and which) processing resources are
tested. Future research will determine this.
How can declines in processing resources with age lead to
strategic changes? Regarding changes in strategy repertoire,
there are two possibilities. First, it could be that older adults use
fewer strategies than young adults do because they know fewer
strategies and have never used strategies that young adults know.
Second, it could be that the same strategies are available in both
age groups but that older individuals restrict the set of strategies
they use in a given task. Lemaire and Arnauds (2008) data are
consistent with the second hypothesis. At the group level, the
same set of nine addition problem-solving strategies was found
in both older and younger adults. However, older individuals
used fewer strategies than young adults did. Decreased processing resources with age might lead older adults to restrict the set
of strategies. Indeed, fewer resources makes it harder to maintain
all strategies in working memory and harder to select a given
strategy among more (compared to fewer) strategies.
Regarding strategy selection, decreased processing
resources may lead older adults to select the best strategy on
each problem less often than do young adults. This could happen via different processes. Fewer resources makes it more difficult to encode problem features efficiently in order to choose
the most appropriate strategy for each problem. Moreover,
decreased efficiency of executive control processes reduces
cognitive flexibility for switching from one strategy to another
on each problem. Such decreased flexibility prevents older
adults from inhibiting a just-executed strategy on a given problem and to activate the most appropriate strategy on the next
problem. Lemaire et al. (2004) found that older adults were
as good as young adults at selecting the best strategy on each
item when they were instructed to only select the best strategy
for each item and not to execute it. However, when asked to
both select and execute strategies on the same items in the same
tasks, older adults strategy choices were much poorer.
Decreased processing resources might lead to less efficient
strategy execution because executing strategies, especially
those involving more numerous and more complex processes,
require cognitive resources to be successfully executed. In all
cognitive domains, older adults obtain poorer performance than
young adults when they are asked to execute harder strategies.
However, if participants are asked to execute easier strategies,
age-related differences are much smaller and even sometimes
nonexistent. Finally, changes in strategy distribution may also
be influenced by changes in cognitive resources with age. With
fewer resources, older adults tend to use the easiest strategies
more often, even if those strategies do not yield the best performance. For example, Bouazzaoui et al. (2010) and many others
have found that, in contrast to young adults, older adults use
less consuming external memory strategies (e.g., making notes

Lemaire
on paper) more often than more consuming internal (i.e., mental)
strategies.

Conclusions and Future Directions


Four main strategic changes occur during aging: decrease in the
size of strategy repertoire, increases and decreases in frequency
of use of strategies, less efficient execution of strategies, and
poorer choices among strategies. Age differences in strategies
tend to be largest in most difficult tasks, domains, or conditions. Change mechanisms leading to these strategic changes
include, among others, reduced processing resources.
In addition to its usefulness for furthering our understanding
of cognitive aging, the strategy perspective helps us better
understand how older adults compensate for age-related
declines in human cognition. Neuroimaging studies have found
that comparable performance in young and older adults are
associated with neural compensations during aging, as older
adults often recruit brain regions not typically recruited by
younger adults (Cabeza, Nyberg, & Park, 2005). Behavioral
studies point to functional compensations (i.e., older adults
select strategies they can more easily and accurately execute)
and suggest that age-related differences in neural activations
may result from functional compensations (e.g., Gandini,
Lemaire, Anton, et al., 2008; Logan, Sanders, Snyder, Morris,
& Buckner, 2002), an issue that future studies will more systematically address.
Strategic variations during aging have been more investigated
in some domains than in others. The strategy perspective has
mostly been adopted to investigate normal aging. Some recent
studies (e.g., Arnaud, Lemaire, Allen, & Michel, 2008) suggest
that it is a fruitful perspective to understand pathological aging,
as patients with Alzheimers disease undergo very important
strategic changes. Whichever the cognitive domain and whichever the cognitive status of the age group, I see no restrictions
that would prevent researchers from adopting a strategy perspective. The benefits of such a perspective for furthering our understanding of cognitive aging outweigh the costs (in relative time
and trouble) of assessing strategic variations.
Recommended Reading
Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., & Park, D.C. (2005). (See References). An
overview of how changes in the brain produce changes in human
cognition.
Craik, F.I.M., & Salthouse, T.A. (2007). (See References). An up-todate, state-of-the-art book on aging and cognition.
Lemaire, P. (2005). Strategic aspects of human cognition: Implications for understanding human reasoning. In M. Roberts (Ed.),
Cognitive strategies and human reasoning (pp. 1129). London,
England: Psychology Press. A detailed discussion of strategic
aspects of human cognition.
Park, D.C., & Schwarz, N. (2000). Cognitive aging: A primer. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. A comprehensive and accessible
overview of cognitive aging.

Strategic Variations During Aging


Acknowledgments
I would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful
comments on a previous version of this manuscript.

Funding
Research reported in this article was supported by grants from Agence
Nationale de la Recherche (Grants #: ANR-06-BLAN-0241-01; ANRBLAN07-1_196867).

References
Arnaud, L., Lemaire, P., Allen, P., & Michel, B.-F. (2008). Strategic
aspects of young, healthy older adults and Alzheimer patients
arithmetic performance. Cortex, 44, 119130.
Bouazzaoui, B., Isingrini, M., Fay, S., Angel, L., Vanneste, S., &
Clarys, D., et al. (2010). Aging and self-reported internal and external strategy uses: The role of executive functioning. Acta Psychologica, 135, 5966.
Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., & Park, D.C. (2005). Cognitive neuroscience
of aging: Linking cognitive and cerebral aging. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Cohen, G., & Faulkner, D. (1983). Age differences in performance on
two information-processing tasks: Strategy selection and processing efficiency. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 447454.
Craik, F.I.M., & Salthouse, T.A. (2007). The handbook of aging and
cognition (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dunlosky, J., & Hertzog, C. (2001). Measuring strategy production during associative learning: The relative utility of concurrent versus retrospective reports. Memory & Cognition, 29,
247253.
Duverne, S., & Lemaire, P. (2004). Age-related differences in
arithmetic problem-verification strategies. Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences,
59, 135142.
El Yagoubi, R., Lemaire, P., & Besson, M. (2005). Effects of aging on
arithmetic problem-solving: An event-related brain potential study.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 3750.
Gandini, D., Lemaire, P., Anton, J.L., & Nazarian, B. (2008).
Neural correlates of approximate quantification strategies in
young and older adults: An fMRI study. Brain Research,
1246, 144157.

369
Gandini, D., Lemaire, P., & Dufau, S. (2008). Older and young adults
strategies in approximate quantification. Acta Psychologica, 129,
175189.
Hartley, A.A., & Anderson, J.W. (1983). Task complexity, problem
representation, and problem-solving performance by younger and
older adults. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 7277.
Johnson, M.M.S. (1990). Age differences in decision making: A process methodology for examining strategic information processing.
Journal of Gerontology, 45, 7578.
Lemaire, P., & Arnaud, L. (2008). Young and older adults strategies in complex arithmetic. American Journal of Psychology,
121, 116.
Lemaire, P., Arnaud, L., & Lecacheur, M. (2004). Adults agerelated differences in adaptivity of strategy choices: Evidence
from computational estimation. Psychology and Aging, 10,
467481.
Lemaire, P., & Siegler, R.S. (1995). Four aspects of strategic change:
Contributions to childrens learning of multiplication. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 8397.
Logan, J.M., Sanders, A.L., Snyder, A.Z., Morris, J.C., &
Buckner, R.L. (2002). Under-recruitment and nonselective recruitment: Dissociable neural mechanisms associated with aging. Neuron, 33, 827840.
Mata, R., Schooler, L.J., & Rieskamp, J. (2007). The aging decision
maker: Cognitive aging and the adaptive selection of decision
strategies. Psychology and Aging, 22, 796810.
Onyper, S.V., Hoyer, W. J., & Cerella, J. (2008). Effects of item difficulty on the retrieval of solutions during cognitive skill acquisition: Age differences. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition,
15, 358383.
Patterson, M.M., & Hertzog, C. (2010). The effects of age in four
alternative forced-choice item and associative recognition tasks.
Psychology and Aging, 25, 235238.
Siegler, R.S., & Lemaire, P. (1997). Older and younger adults strategy choices in multiplication: Testing predictions of ASCM via the
choice/no-choice method. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 126, 7192.
Taconnat, L., Raz, N., Tocze, C., Bouazzaoui, B., Sauzeon, H.,
Fay, S., et al. (2009). Aging and organization strategies in free
recall: The role of cognitive flexibility. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 347365.

You might also like