You are on page 1of 6

Obviation of Opioid Withdrawal

Syndrome by Concomitant Administration


of Naltrexone in Microgram Doses:
Two Psychonautic Bioassays^

Jonathan Ott*

Abstract—Two psychonautic bioassays (self-experiments) in stepwise and abrupt cessation of long-


term daily oral ingestion habits of 800 mg of codeine phosphate are presented. Concomitant
administration of minute doses (about 0.5 meg) of the opioid antagonist naltrexone with each dose of
codeine was found in both cases to obviate the expected opioid withdrawal syndrome, resulting in
asymptomatic and uneventful transitions from physical opioid dependency states to exogenous opioid-
free metabolism. These experiments are analyzed in the context of a conjectured, rapid, iterative
reduction and complete elimination of opioid tolerance, once acquired. It was found that
coadministration of naltrexone with codeine phosphate obviated the development of both tolerance
and physical dependency over several months of four daily oral doses of 200 mg, allowing abrupt
("cold turkey"), asymptomatic and uneventful withdrawal. This points the way to the biochemical
substrate of opioid tolerance itself, and shows that this can easily and inexpensively be blocked,
even over months of iterative oral administration of substantial doses of opioid analgesics. Finally, it
suggests the opioid withdrawal syndrome is directly related to the physiology of opioid tolerance,
and can be prevented by blocking tolerance itself. Even when tolerance has been acquired, this can
be reduced stepwise over a matter of days, with no symptoms of opioid withdrawal syndrome.

Keywords—addiction, codeine, naltrexone, opioids, tolerance, withdrawal

Like progressive toleratice to their atialgesic atid eu- prevent the development of opioid tolerance, and also to
phoric effects, the well-ktiowti opioid withdrawal sytidrome attenuate the allied withdrawal syndrome, a phenomenon I
is thought to be ati ineluctable sequel to cessatioti of ha- recently dubbed "anti-Mithridatism"' (Ott 1997). Examples
bitual use of opioids, at high etiough dosage and for of anti-Mithridatism are the use of dizocilpine, also known
sufficient duration. However, several classes of compotinds as MK-801 (Trujillo & Akil 1991), and proglumide
are known in animal ttiodels to ameliorate and even to (Watkins, Kinscheck & Mayer 1984). Based likewise on
tThe author is indebted to Richard Heffem for valuable advice and animal models, it has lately been reported that nano- and
consultation. picomolar doses of naltrexone, coadministered with opio-
•Partner, Entheobotanica, Solothum, Switzerland. ids, could not merely enhance their analgesic potency, but
Please address correspondence and reprint requests to Jonathan Ott,
Entheobotanica, Kronengasse, 11, Solothum CH-4502, Switzerland. attenuate the withdrawal syndrome occasioned by their

Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 101 Volume 38(1), March 2006


Ott Obviation of Opioid Withdrawai Syndrome

withdrawal from dependent animals. This follows the in- ETHICS OF SELF-EXPERIMENTION
triguing discovery that the m-opioid receptors that mediate WITH PSYCHOACTIVE DRUGS
analgesia exist as two types on nociceptive neurons (pain-
sensitive neurons): apart from the prototypical inhibitory In peer-review of this article, the issue of ethics was
|i-receptors, there is a much smaller population of excita- raised: specifically regarding the fact that this research "was
tory ^.-receptors, opioid binding to which produces not conducted under the auspices of an Institutional Re-
hyperalgesia (Crain & Shen 1996). In this innovative tech- view Board" (IRB), nor a physician's supervision. In a prior
nique, which is currently undergoing clinical trials by Pain article in this journal, I have already addressed the ethical
Therapeutics, Inc. (using MorViva® and OxyTrex®),^ dimensions of purportedly "objective" pharmacological
minute doses of the potent opioid antagonist naltrexone (ca. research with animals, as opposed to "subjective" human
1: 100,000 in relation to opioid doses, expressed as molar self-experimentation (Ott 2001). As far as the IRB is con-
equivalents of base) apparently block preferentially these cerned, such does not apply here, insofar as the research
hyperalgesic, excitatory m-receptors, and in effect "unmask" was not conducted at any institution, nor involved any sub-
the true analgesic potency of opioids. ject other than the experimenter.
Being a long-time (ca. 25 years) daily user of medici-
nal opiates (chiefly morphine sulfate peroral, up to 300 mg/ EXPERIMENT 1: MAY-JUNE 2001
day, and codeine phosphate peroral, up to 1.6 g/day), I am
well familiar with the opioid withdrawal syndrome, best I am the subject, then aged 52 and in good health. I
detailed in human studies by the U.S. Public Health Ser- was taking daily 800 mg codeine phosphate peroral as four
vice at the erstwbile federal "narcotics farm" prison in 200 mg doses (Perduretas®), and had maintained tbis habit
Lexington, Kentucky (Kolb & Himmelsbach 1938; Small for over a year, at times taking 1.0-1.6 g codeine phos-
et al. 1938). When I learned of this biphasic, mixed analge- phate daily. I resolved to reduce my habitual dose stepwise
sic/hyperalgesic activity of opioids in animals, I at once over 14 days, with tbree plateaus (where a given dose is
resolved to ascertain whether it were operative in human repeated a second day). I followed a valuable practice: each
beings. Using pharmaceutical codeine phosphate of known daily dose was taken a balf-bour later in tbe day, that is,
dosage and unmixed with aspirin or other drugs, and stan- 24.5 hours elapsed between doses (25 hours is probably
dard solutions of pharmaceutical naltrexone hydrochloride better). As bas been known since De Quincey's day, it is
(Antaxone®, which allowed precise oral or sublingual ap- unproblematic to reduce one's dose by one-balf, and so
plication of 0.5 meg doses, expressed as naltrexone base), I tbe day before commencing, I simply prescinded (or elimi-
began their concomitant administration. At roughly a 1: nated) two of my customary four doses, thus taking 400
100,000 (as molar equivalents) ratio of naltrexone base to mg. Table 1 shows tbe reduction schedule. Each codeine
codeine base, peroral, there was approximately a 30% in- dose was accompanied by 0.5 meg naltrexone base, ex-
crease in potency, as assessed by subjective (euphoric) cepting Day 1, wben the dose was 1.0 meg.
effects, in contrast to crude tests for analgesia in common I experienced only tbe slightest withdrawal symptoms
use by pharmacologists, despite the fact that I was then tak- on Day 1 (neck and shoulder tension starting 1.5 hours
ing 800 mg/day codeine phosphate peroral, divided into four before dose time) and on Day 2 (similar tension two bours
doses of 200 mg each. I assert that an experienced opioid before dose time, and slight goose flesh 0.5 hours prior).
habitue, who knows dosage amounts precisely, is able to This was likely due to an overly abrupt initial reduction in
make rather good subjective assessments of potency—a dosage. Apart from tbis, I passed a completely asymptom-
methadone-maintainee, for instance, would know soon atic and uneventful witbdrawal, in the course of wbich I
enough if a reduced or enhanced dose had mistakenly been ate, slept and worked normally. Althougb tbere was some
administered. So dramatic and incontrovertible was this "clock-watcbing" up to Day 3, on Day 9 I forgot tbe time
potentiation, that I at once reasoned it should follow that a and took my dose an hour late. Following merely 36 hours
25% reduction in codeine dosage cum naltrexone would be of total abstinence (having been asymptomatic for 13 days
perceived as being equipotent with my habitual dose of and after 10 days at doses beneatb 10% of the habitual),
codeine neat. This proved to be a palpable hit: it was in- on tbe morning of Day 15 and again that evening, I took
deed equipotent. This suggested that the biochemical two 120 mg doses of codeine phosphate, wbich of course
substrate of opioid tolerance (which remains as inscrutable gave good euphoric effects. Then on Day 16, 24 bours af-
as ever, despite exhaustive research) is amenable to phar- ter tbe second dose of codeine, I conducted a
macological readjustment in real time, as it were. It followed naltrexone-challenge test, by taking 25 mg naltrexone per-
inexorably, then, that in principle an opioid habitud could oral, wbicb would bave precipitated withdrawal symptoms
stepwise rachet-down tolerance; indeed, reduce the dose to bad I somehow been mistaken in my appraisals, or had the
zero, absent the opioid withdrawal syndrome. This also prompt resumption of codeine intake for one day reestab-
proved to be true, as will be evident from my two self- lished physical dependence. Althougb I could indeed
experiments. perceive the "anti-opioid" nature of naltrexone, absolutely

Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 102 Volume 38(1), March 2006


Ott Obviation of Opioid Withdrawal Syndrome

TABLE 1
Codeine-Withdrawal Reduction-Schedule (Experiment 1)

Day Dose Total Reduction % Reduction

0 400 mg 50% 50%

1 160 mg 60% 60%


2 144 mg 64% 10%
3 120mg 70% 17%
4 120mg — — Week 1: 800 mg
5 96 mg 76% 20%
6 80 mg 80% 17%
7 80 mg — —

8 64 mg 84% 20%
9 48 mg 88% 25%
10 48 mg — —
11 40 mg 90% 17% Week 2: 256 mg
12 32 mg 92% 20%
13 24 mg 94% 25%
14 Omg 100% —

15 240 mg — —

16 Naltrexone (25 mg) Challenge

no withdrawal symptoms supervened. Out of curiosity, over I might add parenthetically that throughout the course
the next 72 hours, I made six "codeine-challenges" of of four daily doses of 200 mg codeine phosphate, solid
naltrexone. Doses of 160 mg codeine phosphate at two, euphoric effects followed each by some 30 minutes (the
three, 24, 38, 64 and 72 hours post-naltrexone were taken. pills were crushed in a mortar and partially dissolved in
Not until 64 hours was any opioid effect perceived, per- hot water), suggesting tolerance was not developing. On
haps 50% attenuated. At 72 hours, naltrexone blockade of the other hand, absent naltrexone, this is normal in my ex-
m-receptors had ceased, which agreed with my calculations perience—despite development of progressive tolerance,
based on data showing an approximate four-hour tissue half- a controlled user who knows dose precisely can stabilize at
life for naltrexone. a mid-level dose, experience pleasant, but not overwhelm-
ing, opioid euphoria, and maintain that situation indefinitely.
EXPERIMENT 2: JUNE 2002 It is worth noting that exaggerated attention is focused on
uncontrolled users with no idea of dosage, nor indeed pu-
I again maintained a habit of 800 mg/day codeine phos- rity, much less identity, of their opioids. Meanwhile, the
phate peroral for several months, always administering each United States, with roughly 4% ofthe world's population,
200 mg dose of codeine with 1.0 meg naltrexone orally or in 1994 consumed 700 tons or 50% of the world's licit
sublingually. Given the above self-experiment, it seemed opium production, which is to say 13 times world per capita
lo me possible that not only might I develop no tolerance consumption (International Narcotics Control Board, cf.
to codeine-effects, but that such a habit could abruptly be Marnell 1995). Most of this raw material for opiate phar-
terminated without occasioning any withdrawal syndrome. maceuticals is converted to codeine, much simply extracted
In this case, I merely dropped my daily dose to 200 mg and purified as morphine salts, some transformed to po-
over three days by the simple expedient of daily prescinding tent, artificial opiate derivatives like oxycodone and
one of my habitual doses. On the fourth day and thence- oxymorphone. Now, 700 tons of opium (even at a conser-
forth until this writing two months later, I abstained vative 10% morphine), represents 70 tons of morphine (or
completely from codeine or other opioid analgesic. Al- 70,000,000 g). Assuming a daily oral habit of 100 mg
though I felt weary on my first opiate-free day, I slept (which I can attest certainly leads to physical dependency;
normally and experienced not the slightest trace of any vide also Burroughs 1959, 1953), 36.5 g per year would
opioid withdrawal syndrome. maintain one "addict," and 70 tons in principle could

Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 103 Volume 38(1). March 2006


Ott Obviation of Opioid Withdrawal Syndrome

support nearly two million such habits. This is certainly a disease, pursuant to ordinary understanding and use of the
higher number than the total population of heroin users, term, irrespective of the possibility of medical manage-
and surely there exist at least as many medicinal opioid ment of drug habits.
"addicts" as "junkies," whether by medical necessity or The nature and intensity ofthe opioid withdrawal syn-
personal choice. drome, as well as its relative importance in sustaining
COMMENTARY habituation, has been exaggerated wildly in films (e.g.. The
Man with the Golden Arm, Monkey on my Back) and lit-
The two experiments of opioid withdrawal described erature (notably William Burroughs' Junkie and Naked
here—completely novel in my long experience with such— Lunch). Burroughs' "algebra of need" is a satisfactory lit-
may well represent the first reports ever of truly erary device, but as mathematics does not add up to real
asymptomatic withdrawal of opioids following long-estab- numbers. The physical withdrawal syndrome is not the sine
lished habituation to significant and multiple daily doses. qua non of addiction. Perhaps it is more fruitful to view
While of course no comparison can be made to intravenous opioid habituation as a sort of religious faith. Having once
injection of street heroin in terms of analgesic potency in had sufficient experience, the opioid habitue comes to ac-
human beings, 800 mg of codeine phosphate peroral is quire a confirmed belief in an obdurate dogma of
equivalent to 240 mg of morphine sulfate or 120 mg of diminishing dose-response; supported by hard physical
oxycodone hydrochloride peroral (Beers & Berkow 1999). evidence (felt in the bones), as well as eyewitness-testi-
What is being discussed here is the obviation, not of opioid mony (what all the experts and fellow users say). Having
habituation per se, but of aspects of tolerance and physical twice or thrice experienced total withdrawal from a state
dependence, heretofore thought to follow like night, the day of physical dependency on opioids, there is likewise in-
of cessation of an opioid habit of such dosage-level and culcated in the user a fervent belief in the inevitability of
duration as to constitute so-called "addiction." The term this pay-for-play purgatory: the more you take, the better
"addiction" is used so loosely and tendentiously, and has you feel; and the longer you feel better, the worse you'll
been subject to so many redefinitions and qualifications, as feel when you stop. These confirmed beliefs in inflexible
to render it too imprecise for scientific use. The physical dogmas, at times attested and reconfirmed every single day,
withdrawal syndrome was once held to be the sine qua non are in my view keystones in the psychological arch but-
of addiction (Small et al. 1938) until a paradigm-shift oc- tressing opioid habituation. As Burroughs put it: "no
curred in the 1980s. Whereas prior to this, negative good . . . no bueno . . . hustling myself (Burroughs 1959:
reinforcement (i.e., avoidance of the pain of withdrawal) 213). Physical dependence on opioids and the allied with-
had been held to be the primary motor of addiction, posi- drawal syndrome are real and physiological, but
tive reinforcement (rewarding effects) came to displace it, psychological or psychophysical factors are at least as
and today the drugs considered to have the highest "addic- important in the long-term maintenance of opioid habitua-
tive liability" are the stimulants, notably cocaine, tion.
amphetamines and nicotine, the withdrawal of which from Banishing the spectre ofthe dread opioid withdrawal
habitues occasions no physical withdrawal syndrome of syndrome is in itself a major advance, but the real signifi-
note. Despite politically-inspired efforts to conjure a cance of these findings, at least for me, as a trained scientific
cocainic withdrawal syndrome, in my long experience and researcher who approaches these matters in that spirit, is
based on observations and interviews of more than 100 this: in a flash, as it were, a single ingestion-experiment
habitues, nothing resembling the opioid withdrawal syn- (reducing the dose by 25%, with no reduction in intensity
drome accrues on cessation of habitual use, irrespective of of effects) can suffice convincingly to refute perhaps years
its duration and dosage-levels. The same is true for nico- of daily evidence and supportive testimony for the dopers'
tine, even after prolonged sublingual use exceeding 100 mg/ dogma of diminishing dose-response; as, in turn, a single
day of nicotine free-base (this will be the subject of another experience of asymptomatic and uneventful withdrawal can
report). "Mcofine-habituation" is often used carelessly to render the heretofore inevitable Sword of Damocles emi-
describe "tobacco-smoking habituation." The so-called "to- nently forgettable. This is the crux of the case, but its
bacco-smoking withdrawal-syndrome" does not appear to meaning to a given habitu6 is a matter of conjecture. In
be based on physical dependency on nicotine (present at my case, these dramatic refutations at once and forever
low, nonpsychoactive levels in commercial tobacco; ca. 1.0 altered my personal psychology regarding opioid habitua-
mg/cigarette). To obviate the physical withdrawal syndrome tion, something now not worthy of much personal thought
of opioids, while it perforce removes negative reinforce- or care. For another, however, a new dogma of play-and-
ment aspects of the habit, nonetheless leaves intact any don't-pay might simply conduce to increased indulgence.
positive reinforcement factors. As such, it should not be Moreover, what is being discussed here is oral inges-
mistakenly regarded as a cure for opioid habituation. Such tion of opioids, in which the euphoric reward is some 30
does not exist, nor likely ever will. Addiction is a synonym to 60 minutes removed from the act of ingestion. The closer
for devotion, and devotion to a habit is in any case hardly a is the temporal connection between ingestion-behavior and

Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 104 Volume 38(1). March 2006


o« Obviation of Opioid Withdrawal Syndrome

associated reward, the more habituating is that behavior such as setting and maintaining pain thresholds. Since
(recall one can not become addicted to drugs per se, but physical tolerance can be acquired from a single opioid
rather to the habit of ingesting them; until the twentieth dose, it follows that a single dose could in principle dimin-
century, the substantive "addict" did not exist in English, ish it. Certainly it is remarkable that one can reduce an
and "addiction" was used with linguistic precision, viz., opioid habit to one-eighth its former level over five days,
addiction to a habit of ingesting one or another drug, or to while experiencing opioid euphoria from each successively
another behavior). In the case of intravenous drug injec- diminished dose. This is a dramatic first I'd have thought
tion, this reward delay is but a minute or so; following next to impossible prior to having experienced it.
inhalation of alkaloidal free-base vapors, about 30 seconds
merely. In this latter circumstance, ingestion-act and re- NOTES
lated reward become virtually contemporaneous, insofar
as one begins to feel the rewarding "rush" even before ex- 1. Anti-Mithridatism: Mithridates VI of Pontus (120-
haling. To inhale vapors of free-base cocaine is directly to 63 BC), together with his infamous physician Kratevas, on
confront crystalline craving. It is unclear, pending further the basis of human experimentation upon condemned con-
investigation, whether picomolar naltrexone combined with victs, who were poisoned, developed an all-purpose antidote
injected or vaporized and inhaled opioids can block and to poisoning, which was named the mithridatium or mith-
reverse tolerance or obviate the withdrawal syndrome, as ridate. This was a type of theriac, and it happens that opium
is the case with oral ingestion (although the animal research was a chief, and later practically the sole, ingredient in
on which the present report is based involved induced de- theriacs. Whereas a theriac was designed to prevent illness
pendency via injected morphine). (perhaps as an immunostimulant), repeated small doses of
One final point is more than significant. During the toxins in the mithridatium were supposed to establish a pro-
stepwise dose reduction in Experiment 1, very satisfactory gressive immunity to their toxicity.
euphoric effects were experienced for the first five days, 2. Pain Therapeutics, Inc.; 250, E. Grand Street, Suite
although at the end of that period, my daily dose stood at 70; San Francisco, CA, 94080. The company has already
96 mg, or less than one-eighth of my habitual dosage! This released preliminary data on animal testing of these two
is unheard of in stepwise withdrawal. Significantly, that products: MorViva® in two formulations, both designated
dose, around 100 mg, constitutes the lowest dose that had PTI-555 (morphine sulfate 3 mg/kg + naltrexone 0.3 ng/
sufficed to produce euphoric effects for me, when I had kg; and morphine sulfate 3 mg/kg + naltrexone 3 ng/kg)
first used opioids some 30 years ago. This is to be expected, and OxyTrex® likewise in two formulations, both desig-
were one indeed undoing and readjusting stepwise the bio- nated PTI-801 (oxycodone 0.1 mg/kg + naltrexone 1 pg/
chemical substrate of opioid tolerance, which appears to kg; oxycodone 0.1 mg/kg + naltrexone 1 ng/kg). Measur-
be the case. While astonishing at the time, on subsequent ing tail-flick latency in female mice, MorViva® showed
reflection this did not surprise me overmuch. There is ev- stronger analgesia and greater duration than 3 mg/kg mor-
ery reason to anticipate such rapid homeostatic plasticity phine sulfate neat; whereas OxyTrex® showed stronger
in an exquisitely sensitive and primary neural control analgesia than 0.1 mg/kg oxycodone neat.
mechanism, which mediates vital physiological parameters.

REFERENCES

Beers, M.H. & Berkow, R. (Eds.) 1999. The Merck Manual of Diagnosis Ott, J. 1997. Pharmacophilia or the Natural Paradises. Vashon,
and Therapy. Whitehouse Station, New Jersey: Merck Research Washington: Natural Products Co.
Laboratories. Small, L.F.; Eddy, N.B.; Mosettig, E. & Himmelsbach, C.K. 1938. Studies
Burroughs, W.S. 1959. Naked Lunch. New York: Grove Press. on Drug Addiction, with Special Reference to Chemical Structure
Burroughs, W.S. 1953. Junkie. New York: Ace Books. of Opium Derivatives and Allied Synthetic Substances and their
Crain, S.M. & Shen, K.-F. 1996. United States Patent No. 5,580,876; Physiological Action. Supplement No. 138 to the Public Health
issued 3 December. Supported by an extensive bibliography. Reports. Washington, D.C: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Kolb. L. & Himmelsbach, C.K. 1938. Clinical studies ofdrug addiction. Trujillo, K.A. & Akil, A. 1991. Inhibition of morphine tolerance and
III. A critical review of withdrawal treatments with methods of dependence by the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801. Science
evaluating abstinence syndromes. American Journal of Psychiatry 251:85-7.
94: 759-97. Watkins, L.R.; Kinscheck, I.B. & Mayer, D.J. 1984. Potentiation of opiate
Mamell, T. (Ed.) 1995. Drug Identification Bible, Second Edition. Denver, analgesia and apparent reversal of morphine tolerance by
Colorado: Drug Identification Bible. proglumide. Science 224: 395-6.
Ott, J. 2001. Pharmafiopo-psychonautics: Human intranasal, sublingual,
intrarectal, pulmonary and oral pharmacology of bufotenine.
Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 33: 273-81.

Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 105 Volume 38(1), March 2(X)6

You might also like