You are on page 1of 15

Journal of Applied Psychology

1978, Vol. 63, No. 5, 533-547

An Opponent Process Theory of Job Satisfaction


Frank J. Landy
Pennsylvania State University

The role of job satisfaction in research and theory in the area of industrial and
organizational psychology is considered. Job satisfaction seems to occupy a
position as the hedonic or affective component in theories of motivation. In
spite of its importance, little theory is available for understanding the affective
state represented by the concept of job satisfaction. Opponent process theory
is suggested as a reasonable deductive statement for a consideration of the
phenomenon of satisfaction. The theory proposes that every excursion from
hedonic neutrality is accompanied by an attempt to bring the excursion back
within "normal" limits. This return to normal levels is accomplished via an
opponent process. The theory further suggests that the opponent process grows
in strength with use. The theory is applied to some current questions regarding
the relationship between job satisfaction and work motivation. The major
parameters of the theory are represented by a series of research hypotheses
and corollaries.

Job satisfaction is a popular concept in strongly negative one. The position that the
industrial and organizational psychology. The individual occupies depends on both internal
reasons for its popularity have been well and external variables. Job-related stimuli
documented in Locke's (1976) recent chapter comprise a class of these variables. Thus, it is
in the Handbook of Industrial and Organiza- assumed that, at least in part, a person's
tional Psychology (Dunnette, 1976). At various emotional state is affected by interactions with
times, the concept has been a dependent the work environment. It is normally this
variable, an independent variable, a covariate, portion of general hedonic or affective variance
and a moderator variable. It has been linked that is referred to as job satisfaction.
to productivity, motivation, absenteeism and Locke (1976) defines job satisfaction as
tardiness, accidents, mental health, physical "a pleasurable or positive emotional state
health, and general life satisfaction. Fifty resulting from the appraisal of one's job or job
years of research have attempted to document experiences" (p. 1300). This definition high-
the relationship between an individual's lights one of the major questions addressed
feelings about his or her job and that individ- in job satisfaction research: Under what
ual's behavior. conditions does a positive or negative state arise?
There does seem to be one common theme The answer to this question has implications
that pervades the research on job satisfaction. for theories of work motivation. At a basic
An affective state is implied. It is assumed that level, all models of work motivation imply
at any specific point in time, an individual that individuals will expend energy in main-
occupies a point on a continuum that ranges taining or increasing pleasurable experiences;
from a strongly positive emotional state to a conversely, energy will be expended in min-
imizing or decreasing unpleasant experiences.
Thus, the affective reaction of an individual
The author is grateful to Mary Dunnette, Jim Farr,
John Hall, Jeanne Herman, and to the late Don to work-related stimuli (either positive or
Trurabo for comments on an earlier version of this negative) is an indication of the potential
manuscript. power that those stimuli have for affecting
Requests for reprints should be sent to Frank J.
Landy, Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania the individual's behavior.
State University, University Park, Pennsylvania 16802. As an illustration of the role of job satisfac-
Copyright 1978 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0021-9010/78/6305-0533$00.75

533
534 FRANK J. LANDY

tion in current theories of work motivation, activities related to the reduction of this
consider Maslow's (1943) need hierarchy tension. The role of satisfaction in equity
theory. In this framework, unfulfilled lower theory is less clear. The most reasonable
needs represent a deficiency in the individual; interpretation of available theoretical discus-
this deficiency is experienced as discomfort by sion and empirical research is that satisfaction
the individual. Presumably, if an individual is the absence of dissatisfaction. There has
were asked a question concerning well-being been little attention devoted to the phenom-
while "under the influence" of this deficiency, enon of the positive end of the hedonic contin-
the answer would imply some dissatisfaction. uum by researchers using the equity paradigm.
The theory proposes that the individual will Finally, in the Porter and Lawler (1968)
engage in actions that will diminish this version of Vroom's (1964) VIE model, satisfac-
discomfort. The same basic mechanism seems tion is hypothesized to be a derivative variable,
to be implied in the fulfillment of upper level depending for its value on the match between
needs. Once lower level needs are fulfilled, the expected and obtained rewards. Satisfaction,
degree to which upper levels needs are fulfilled in turn, has a general excitatory or inhibitory
assumes major importance for determining effect on future estimations made by the
the degree of satisfaction of dissatisfaction person concerning the value of promised
that an individual experiences. Once again, rewards. By virtue of the capacity to remember
the theory implies that if an individual were pleasant and unpleasant experiences and
asked questions concerning his or her well- relate these experiences to specific contexts
being, the answers would reflect the degree to and stimuli, the individual is able to anticipate
which the individual perceived these upper future hedonic experiences and choose behav-
level needs as being met, and concomitantly, ioral alternatives accordingly.
the degree to which the individual would Even though the work motivation theories
engage in activities aimed at fulfilling these presented above are only a sample of a much
needs. Thus, in need hierarchy theory, states larger domain, they are representative of the
of satisfaction-dissatisfaction precede directed implied role of job satisfaction in the area of
behavior. Additionally, since there is no firm work motivation. The following general prin-
continuum underlying the ordering of Maslow's ciples would hold for most of these theories:
needs in the hierarchy, one might infer that (a) The hedonic state of an individual can
satisfaction at one level functions as a release be located on a continuum from positive
mechanism for dissatisfaction at the next (pleasant) to negative (unpleasant), and (b)
higher level. there are systematic interactions between the
In equity theory, dissatisfaction is an emotional state of an individual and work-
unpleasant aftereffect of discordant cognitions. related variables. These work-related variables
As such, this dissatisfaction represents a source may either be temporally antecedent to the
of tension to be reduced, and the organism emotional state (e.g., reward policies of the
expends energy in service of this reduction. organization) or temporally consequent to the
The discordant cognitions are the result of emotional state (e.g., absenteeism or tardiness).
individuals' comparisons of their own inputs In the motivation theories presented above,
and outcomes to the inputs and outcomes of job satisfaction had clear emotional overtones.
significant others. In that sense, equity theory It represented a psychophysiological state of
emphasizes the role of social stimuli in general pleasure or displeasure. This psychophysiolog-
hedonic states. Nevertheless, the implicit ical state had implications for future action
assumption that individuals will engage in choices by the individual. Thus, it is surprising
activities to reduce tension or discomfort is that there have been no well-articulated
similar to the one proposed by Maslow. theories that include hedonic components in
Research in equity theory has attempted to accompanying postulates or corollaries. Most
show that individuals with discordant cogni- research attention has been directed toward
tions report dissatisfactions; this dissatisfac- identifying the environmental elements or
tion is thought to represent felt tension; it conditions capable of producing positive or
is proposed that individuals will engage in negative emotional states. Taylor (1947)
OPPONENT PROCESS 535

implied that monetary rewards mediated these to recognize that attention has been paid
emotional states in workers. The Hawthorne almost exclusively to the conditions antecedent
researchers replaced monetary rewards with to that affective state (Lawler, 1973). Little
social rewards and incentives (Roethlisberger or no attention has been paid to the character-
& Dixon, 1939). Schaffer (1953) introduced istics of the state itself or to the intra-individual
the notion of individual differences in the past history of that state. For example, few
salience of rewards. Maslow (1943) suggested researchers in the area would quarrel with the
five sources of satisfaction and dissatisfaction. proposition that the perception of constant
Herzberg, Mausner, and Snyderman (1959) levels of work-related stimuli (such as pay,
proposed two general sources, and Alderfer co-workers, or challenge) changes systemat-
(1969) suggested three classes of potential ically over time. Yet no current theories of
rewards. Even though they were not clearly job satisfaction suggest any mechanisms for
"content theorists," the Cornell researchers understanding this change. A valuable theory
(Smith, Kendall, & Hulin, 1969) implied five of hedonic states and their effects on behavior
sources of job satisfaction. Locke (1976) should be able to deal with these systematic
proposes that feelings of well-being or satisfac- and gradual changes in hedonic states over
tion derive from a system of values rather than time. There have been some attempts to deal
from needs. While needs are innate, values with these changes in the general experimental
are assumed to be learned. It is these values paradigm as adaptation (Helson, 1964) or
that determine the individual's actual choices habituation (Groves & Thompson, 1970). In
and emotional reactions. Thus, values are the industrial and organizational area, mech-
thought to be the determiners of hedonic states. anisms like arousal or activation have been
In addition to content theories of satisfac- invoked to deal with perceptions of stimulus
tion, emphasizing the source of the emotional change (Duffy, 1962; McGrath, 1970; Scott,
state, there have been various process theories 1966). Nevertheless, the issue of satisfaction
of satisfaction, suggesting how various reac- is seldom explicitly addressed in these con-
tions to environmental stimuli combine to siderations. As I have indicated above, the
produce the state called satisfaction or hedonic state represented by the term satisfac-
dissatisfaction. Early theorists implied a single tion has some rather important implications
variable, linear model. Schaffer (1953) made for more general theories of industrial behavior.
some radical changes in that model. He Consequently, theories of satisfaction should
suggested multiple variables with weights more clearly address the characteristics of the
determined by need strength. This might be state itself, not solely the antecedents of this
thought of as the discrepancy model (Lawler, state.
1973) and is characteristic of the work of The present article is an attempt to adapt
many satisfaction researchers (Katzell, 1964; a general theory of derived motivation
Likert, 1961; Morse, 1953). Herzberg et al. (Solomon & Corbit, 1973) to a specific con-
(1959) suggested a radically different process sideration of the phenomenon of job satisfac-
model proposing that satisfaction and dissatis- tion. In this adaptation, specific emphasis
faction were not on a single continuum and will be placed on affective arousal, main-
that there were limits to the effects of increased tenance, and decay as separate processes. It is
"rewards," yielding asymptotic curves. Recent hoped that by decomposing the phenomenon
work by more cognitively oriented theorists of job satisfaction-dissatisfaction and reducing
has generally followed a discrepancy framework it to simpler process components, the flexibility
in which rewards are thought to be linearly and utility of the concept will be broadened.
and additively related to the general emotional
state on a satisfaction-dissatisfaction con-
tinuum. An Opponent Process Theory of
Job Satisfaction
If one accepts the proposition that job
satisfaction represents some affective state In several recent articles, Solomon (Hoffman
that is an important component of most & Solomon, 1974; Solomon & Corbit, 1973,
theories of work motivation, it is distressing 1974) proposes that many emotional or hedonic
536 FRANK J. LANDY

Primary Process shortly thereafter, the opponent process


decays as well. Figure 2 presents the net
effect of stimulus onset and termination and
the joint operation of the primary and oppo-
nent processes. Thus, Figure 2 represents
_ ittr&shold the behavioral manifestation of the opposing
Hedonic _^ processes.
Neutrality \. Solomon suggests that this type of model is
most parsimonious for explaining a number of
hedonic phenomena. In support of the model,
Opponent Process he presents data from both infrahuman and
human studies of "emotionality." These
studies include examinations of imprinting
I STIMULUS I behavior in young ducklings, responses of
onset termination dogs to varying amounts of electrical shock,
reactions of parachutists to free-fall conditions,
Figure 1. Underlying opponent processes after few responses to the loss of a loved one, cyclic
stimulus presentations.
variations in opiate usage, and cyclic variations
characteristic of cigarette addiction. He points
states that can be commonly observed in
out that when individuals are stimulated
humans seem to follow a regular pattern of
above some level, they do not return imme-
change. This pattern of change suggests that diately to some "base" level when the source
there is a two-phase underlying process govern- of stimulation is removed. Instead, they tend
ing manifest emotional behavior. The first to "overshoot" the base level in a direction
stage is excitatory and commences with opposite to that produced by the original
stimulus presentation. This is called the stimulation. Only gradually do they return to
primary process. The second state is inhibitory base level. This overshoot phenomenon can
(at least initially) and is instigated by the be seen both physiologically and behaviorally.
excitatory or primary state. The second stage Solomon stresses the capacity of the opponent
is called the opponent process. Figure 1 process mechanism to explain systematic
graphically depicts the interaction of the two changes in hedonic responses over time. It
processes. Stimulus onset is shown at the is this capacity that makes the theory poten-
bottom of the figure. The upper portion of the tially useful in understanding the phenomenon
figure shows the effect of the stimulus onset of job satisfaction. There are a number of
on the primary process and the sequential theoretical postulates that must be presented
effect of the primary process on the opponent
process when a hedonic threshold is exceeded.
Solomon proposes that the inhibitory or
opponent process seems to function as a
control on the level of stimulation. The model
implies that there are limits to a departure Hedonic
from hedonic neutrality that are acceptable to
the organism. The interaction of the primary Neutrality
and opponent processes is thought to be
governed by neural mechanisms that place
limits on levels of excitation. Thus, the
operation of the mechanism is straightforward:
(a) the organism is excited by external stimula-
tion; (b) when excitation exceeds critical STIMULUS!
levels, an opposing inhibitory process com-
mences to bring excitation within normal or onset termination
acceptable levels; (c) when external stimulation Figure 2. Manifest hedonic response after few stimulus
disappears, the primary process ceases, and presentations.
OPPONENT PROCESS 537

in order to fully understand how the proposi- that changes as a function of stimulus presenta-
tions relate to hedonic change. tion.
The first major postulate is that there are It is important to note that the two processes
central nervous system mechanisms that are inextricably bound when some threshold
function in such a manner as to reduce all level of stimulation is exceeded. In this case,
variations beyond some normal excursions the opponent process is referred to as a " slave"
from hedonic neutrality. Such mechanisms process. Although the opponent process as-
must assume the role of a hypothetical con- sumptions do not deny the propositions of
struct at this point. Nevertheless, it is no arousal theorists, the theory does imply that
less reasonable than other similar constructs once some critical level of arousal is attained,
such as "activation" or "arousal" levels monotonicity of hedonic responsiveness dis-
(Duffy, 1962; Scott, 1966). appears.
The second postulate of the theory is that Although Solomon suggests that the oppo-
these mechanisms work identically for positive nent process mechanism operates identically
and negative stimuli. As far as central nervous for both positive and negative primary
system responses are concerned, both positive stimulation, there is one basic difference
and negative variations from hedonic neutral- between the positive and negative primary
ity are equally threatening. Once again, this process with respect to the behavior of
assumption is similar to the inverted-U individuals. A negative primary process is
hypothesis of the arousal models (Duffy, 1962). produced by an aversive stimulus. At least
The overarousal end of the continuum implies initially, individuals are unlikely to engage
that too much stimulation is counterproductive in actions voluntarily that would lead to
to the organism regardless of whether the aversive stimulus onset. This would continue
source is thought to be pleasant or unpleasant to be true until the opponent process (opposite
on a normative basis. in sign, thus, positive) grew in strength
The third postulate is that, at least initially, sufficient to suppress the aversive effects of
both the primary response to the stimulus the primary stimulus and produce a pleasant
(primary process) and the mechanism attempt- decay period following stimulus termination.
ing to control this primary process (the This is not true with primary positive states.
opponent process) are automatic rather than Individuals will engage in activities necessary
learned. to produce the positive primary states vol-
The fourth postulate is that the process of untarily. As the opponent process grows in
stimulus reduction begins at the point at strength (a negative opponent process, in
which some hedonic threshold is exceeded. this case), individuals will increasingly engage
This stimulus reduction or opponent process in activities that will cause the reoccurrence
might be thought of as a negative feedback of the pleasant primary state and the termina-
loop or dampening mechanism. Further, it is tion of the unpleasant opponent state. Thus,
assumed that the opponent process is somewhat there is some asymmetry to the positive and
sluggish in decay. Thus, while the primary negative primary and opponent states. This
affective response system decays quickly after asymmetry will be important in applying the
external stimulation is terminated, the oppo- opponent process theory to the concept of
nent process disappears more slowly. job satisfaction.
The final postulate is, by far, the most Even though the focus of the present article
crucial. It is assumed that the opponent is not on the content aspects of job satisfaction
process is strengthened with use and weakened (i.e., exactly which factors "cause" satisfaction
with disuse. It is this final assumption that or dissatisfaction) but rather on the process
lays the groundwork for explaining systematic aspects (i.e., the operations which govern the
change in hedonic response levels to constant emergence, maintenance, and decay of job-
stimuli. A corollary of this proposition is related emotional states), some attention
that the primary process or response to should be given to how job-related stimuli
stimulation never changes in intensity or might be categorized as positive or negative on
reactivity. It is only the opponent process an a priori basis. Such a categorization is
538 FRANK J. LANDY

Primary Process in Figure 1. Figure 4 presents the manifest


hedonic response after many stimulus presenta-
tions. Once again, notice the dramatic dif-
ference between the two manifest hedonic
responses represented by Figures 2 and 4.
Notice particularly the effect of the termination
Hedonic or weakening of stimulation after many
stimulus presentations. If the original stimulus
Neutrality would be considered "positive," the aftereffect
would be negative. If the original stimulus
were aversive, the aftereffect would be positive.
Opponent Process Based on the assumption that the opponent
process grows in strength and that speed of
initiation is directly proportional to its use,
[STIMULUS! we would predict that after many stimulations,
onset an individual has a much more modest hedonic
termination response to stimulus presentation but a much
Figure 3. Underlying opponent processes after many more dramatic and opposite response to
stimulus presentations.
stimulus termination.
Since his original statement of the theory,
important in avoiding tautologies similar to
Solomon (Star & Solomon, Note 1) has
those that hindered the behaviorists in their
investigated conditions surrounding the de-
attempts to define reinforcement. There are velopment and decay of opponent processes.
several alternatives that might be chosen for Of major importance was a recent finding
an initial attempt at categorization. One might that the opponent process seemed to increase
choose the operant paradigm by defining in strength only when the interstimulus-
positive stimuli as those that increase the interval period was less than the decay period
probability of a class of responses when the for the opponent hedonic response. This
presentation of these stimuli is made contingent suggests the operation of classically conditioned
upon a response of that class. Most content mechanisms in opponent process operation.
theories of satisfaction presume such para- There is a good deal of work to be done on
metric information (e.g., two-factor theory; the basic propositions of the model. For
the Cornell studies) or ignore the implied example, Solomon suggests that there may be
circularity (e.g., pleasant working conditions primary processes that have no correspond-
are satisfying). The asymmetry of primary ing opponent processes (Solomon & Corbit,
hedonic states under conditions of pleasurable 1974). It is more likely that the speed with
and aversive initial stimulation demands which opponent processes develop, and their
rigorous and independent definitions of the ultimate strength, is related to the degree to
properties of these stimuli. The operant para- which stimulus termination can be identified
digm peresents one avenue for resolution.
It was indicated above that the theory
proposes that the opponent process grows in Hedonic L
strength with use and decreases in strength
with disuse. This implies that the manifest Neutrality
hedonic response of an individual after
few stimulations will be different than that
for the same individual after many stimula-
tions. Figure 2 depicted the manifest hedonic
response level after few stimulations. In
Figure 3, hypothetical primary and opponent ISTIMULUSl
processes are shown after many stimulations. onset termination
Notice the increase in strength of the opponent Figure 4. Manifest hedonic response after many
process compared to the strength of the process stimulus presentations.
OPPONENT PROCESS 539

by the organism. Consequently, we are more opponent process theory has both methodolog-
likely to find well-developed opponent proc- ical and theoretical implications for the area
esses in situations in which stimulus presenta- of job satisfaction. In the next section, some
tion follows cyclic parameters closely. Examples of these implications will be introduced.
of such situations would be invariant task-
specific cycle time, fixed reward intervals,
and other consistent temporal parameters Measurement Implications
governing individual-environment interactions.
If the opponent process theory is a reason-
It will be necessary to conduct basic investiga-
able process structure for understanding an
tions in the area of contexts that are more
individual's response to work-related stimuli,
or less conducive to the development of
there are a number of serious implications for
opponent processes. In addition, basic research
the ways in which that hedonic response is
must be conducted to determine the properties
measured. The most serious of these implica-
of several parameters of the model, for example,
tions is the role of temporal variables in the
initiation time, response intensity, and decay
measurement of satisfaction. From an inspec-
time for primary and opponent processes.
tion of Figure 4, it should be clear that a
Nevertheless, the model provides some ex-
response to a satisfaction question will vary
planatory mechanisms that are crucial for
depending upon whether the question is asked
understanding the role of hedonic states in
prior to stimulus onset, prior to stimulus
theories of satisfaction. In particular, the
termination, or after stimulus termination.
theory provides an excellent framework for
Further, the intensity of the response will
understanding the affective responses of
depend on whether the stimulus has been
workers to work-related stimuli as well as a
encountered often or seldom. Finally, the
description of how those responses change as a
direction of the satisfaction response (posi-
function of repeated stimulus presentation.
tive-negative) will depend on the interaction
Opponent process theory describes a manifest between the direction of the primary state
emotional state (e.g., job satisfaction-dissatis-
(positive-negative) and the point on the
faction) as the resolution of two opposing
hedonic continuum when the response is
hedonic processes. Further, it suggests a
provided (stimulus on-stimulus off).
mechanism for understanding how stimuli in
A second major measurement implication is
the work environment lose their value.
that correlational within-subject designs pur-
The theory is not particularly parsimonious porting to address stability of satisfaction are
compared to other process theories such as
inappropriate. The opponent process model
Schaffer's (1953) or Lawler's (1973). Never-
would suggest that while correlatons between
theless, when the simpler theories attempt to
responses on two separate occasions may be
explain why two individuals respond differently high, absolute levels of satisfaction can vary
to the same job conditions, their arguments
dramatically as a function of the number of
often become circular (e.g., "individual dif- stimulus presentations between the first and
ferences in the salience of various rewards second measurements. In addition, low correla-
must be taken into account as well . . .")• tions over time may be a result of unequal
In such cases, parsimony is a vice not a virtue. numbers of stimulus presentations across
We are asked to accept individual differences individuals rather than inherent structural
as an explanation rather than an admission of flaws in the instrument used to measure
ignorance. In the area of job satisfaction, the
satisfaction.
opponent process theory implies the opposite A third measurement implication has to do
of parsimony, reductionism. It implies that
with the role of reward salience, valence, or
previous process theories have stopped short importance. It is common in satisfaction
of efficient explanation. To be sure, there studies to ask a subject how important or
will be individual difference parameters in salient a particular reward is. A number
the opponent process model as well. Some of representing the degree of that importance is
these will be introduced in a later section. then used as a multiplier of some kind in a
Nevertheless, it should be clear that the "need strength-need satisfaction" paradigm
540 FRANK J. LANDY

(e.g., Schaffer, 1953; Vroom, 1964). The Theoretical Implications


opponent process theory suggests that the
importance an individual attaches to a In a recent examination of the current
potential reward will be affected by both the state of work motivation theory, Campbell
decay period of the last presentation of that and Pritchard (1976) identify the role of
reward and the number of previous presenta- reinforcement as crucial in experimentally
tions of that reward. An examination of Figure grounded theories of work motivation. This
4 might make this point clearer. If relative would be particularly true for unadorned
importance of a particular reward is assessed associationist and reinforcement positions but
by means of self-report measures prior to would also have implications for need and
stimulus termination, it is likely to be con- drive theorists as well. The basic question is
sidered less important than after the individual "What makes a reinforcer reinforcing?" While
returns to base or prereward level; if impor- opponent process theory does not purport to
tance is measured after stimulus termination answer the content question implied, it does
but prior to return to base or prereward level, deal more effectively with a corollary to that
the factor will likely be considered very question than other theories, that is, Under
important. In fact, reported satisfaction what conditions will reinforcers increase and
with the element should be identical to decrease in strength. Campbell and Pritchard
reported importance of the element. This suggest that the mechanism of reinforcement
is exactly what has been demonstrated in might be found in the central nervous system,
previous research (Ewen, 1967; Locke, 1976). citing the work of Miller (1957) and Olds
In a similar manner, individuals who have (1962), among others. They also imply that the
experienced many previous stimulus presenta- work of the "activation" and arousal-level
tions should assign relatively less importance theorists leads to an examination of physio-
to stimuli prior to stimulus termination than logical changes as reinforcements in and of
individuals with fewer previous stimulus themselves. If that were to be the case, the
presentations. Such a mechanism was suggested opponent process theory would assume content
originally by Maslow (1943) in an attempt aspects as well. If one assumes (a) that job
to build in individual differences parameters to satisfaction (regardless of how it is operational-
his concept of a need hierarchy. ized) represents a point on a hedonic con-
There are certainly many other measurement tinuum, (b) that hedonic level covaries with
implications. Those mentioned above rep- reinforcement, and (c) that reinforcement has
resent a sample of a larger domain. Neverthe- implications for directed energy, then the
less, even this small sample suggests that opponent process theory would seem to repre-
(a) satisfaction reponses should be gathered sent a move in the direction suggested by
within individuals across occasions, (b) analyses Campbell and Prichard.
The relationship between the opponent
of change over time should include both level
process theory and the construct of arousal is
and shape changes, and (c) verbal report of
hard to ignore. Both imply some optimal level
satisfaction and/or importance responses must of stimulation, and both imply that beyond
be examined for systematic temporal hedonic that level both "positive" and "negative"
effects. A corollary to this third suggestion is stimulation are equally counterproductive.
that physiological, psychophysiological, and Nevertheless, opponent process theory is
behavioral indicators of hedonic level or able to extend the construct of arousal or
state be examined for estimates of satisfaction. activation by suggesting a mechanism for
While the physiological and psychophysiolog- understanding shifting base arousal levels.
ical effects of satisfaction and dissatisfaction Berlyne (1967) has suggested that progress
have been considered by researchers in the in understanding the impact of arousal on
recent past (Frankenhauser, 1974; Kahn, manifest behavior can be made by considering
1973), few researchers have considered anchor- the interaction of arousal and reinforcement.
ing satisfaction-dissatisfaction in physiolog- These mechanisms and interaction processes
ical or psycholophysiological networks. are well represented in the primary and
OPPONENT PROCESS 541

opposing processes suggested by Solomon. In would deduce that the challenge represents a
this light, arousal occupies the role of a hypo- "stimulus on" condition and meeting the
thetical construct while opponent process challenge or overcoming the obstacle represents
theory helps to define that construct operation- stimulus termination. Further, if the "stimulus
ally and to suggest theoretical postulates and off" condition produces a pleasurable state,
their concomitant corollaries. then the stimulus on state must be opposite in
Campbell and Pritchard (1976) suggest that sign or unpleasant. If this were the case, one
there is sufficient evidence available to should be able to demonstrate that individuals
conclude that the "overall judgment about the initially require some urging to accept increased
job (job satisfaction) is made up of two, challenge and responsibility since the primary
sub-general factors corresponding roughly state is negative and the opponent state is
to the intrinsic vs. extrinsic breakdown initially weak. This further suggests that
originally indentified by Herzberg" (p. 103). individuals will initially attempt to avoid
Thus, independent of Herzberg's propositions, challenge when possible rather than meeting
a review of the available literature suggests it and overcoming obstacles to success. After
that there are at least two distinct classes of repeated presentations of challenge or in-
job-related factors that have the capacity to creased responsibility followed by successful
effect manifest behavioral patterns of workers. strategies for meeting that challenge, the
Several researchers have suggested that challenge itself should become less aversive
these two classes of factors have differential and the hedonic state following successful
main effects as well as interactive effects on meeting of that challenge should become more
manifest behavior (deCharms, 1968; Deci, 1972, pleasurable. These predictions would follow
1975; Herzberg et al., 1959). Opponent process directly from the proposition that the opponent
theory would also suggest differential main process grows in strength with use.
effects, although interactive effects would not If we were to return briefly to a consideration
be as clear. Let us assume that the presentation of the effect of repeated presentations of
of extrinsic factors generally results in a extrinsic factors on hedonic state, we would
positive primary state. The presentation of observe the converse of the effect of intrinsic
money, the opportunity to interact with factors such as challenge. The repeated
co-workers, a promotion, etc., are much more presentation of the extrinsic factor should
likely to result in positive hedonic states than result in diminishing levels of pleasure. The pre-
negative ones in a random sample of subjects. sentation of these factors would be considered
That is not to say that every individual will the stimulus on condition. The absence of these
be equally pleased with the every extrinsic factors would be considered the stimulus off
element, but that in a normative sense, the condition, and the theory would predict that
presentation of stimulus elements comprising increasing levels of displeasure would be
the class "extrinsic" will most likely yield associated with these stimulus elements in
pleasant hedonic states. direct proportion to the number of stimulus
Intrinsic factors are represented by such presentations. Thus, after extended periods
terms as responsibility, challenge, demand, etc. of time, elements that originally had power
A number of theories (Herzberg et al., 1959; over behavioral choice by virtue of their
Locke," 1965; McClelland, 1961) suggest that presence now have control only by virtue of
it is not the challenge itself that produces a their actual or threatened absence.
pleasurable state, but successfully meeting In summary, opponent process theory
that challenge. In a recent consideration of the predicts the affective reactions among experi-
relationship of challenge or increased respon- enced workers to intrinsic and extrinsic factors
sibility to job satisfaction, Locke (1976) on the job; intrinsic factors have the capacity
proposed that "these challenges must be to yield neutral hedonic states in their presence
successfully overcome for the individual to and positive hedonic aftereffects; extrinsic
experience pleasure" (p. 1320). If one were factors produce neutral hedonic states in
to invoke the propositions of the opponent their presence and negative hedonic aftereffects.
process theory to describe the phenomenon, one A simpler way of stating this relationship
542 FRANK J. LANDY

might be that extrinsic factors can produce the new job is high and prompt performance
dissatisfaction after their termination and feedback imperative.
intrinsic factors can produce satisfaction after An extension of the consideration of job
their termination. If an experienced worker enrichment is an examination of the mirror
were asked to recall pleasant work-related image, that is, boring tasks. Boredom and
situations, these situations would involve dissatisfaction with the work itself are treated
intrinsic factors (e.g., having overcome a synonymously, and both are thought to be a
difficult challenge); if the same worker were result of lack of mental challenge (Locke,
asked to recall unpleasant work-related situa- 1976). In spite of the importance of the
tions, these situations would involve extrinsic phenomenon of boredom for understanding
factors (e.g., the withdrawal of environmental workers' reactions to jobs, there are few
attributes normally seen as pleasurable). Thus, theories that provide any insights into how
opponent process theory would lead one to tasks become boring. Activation and arousal
deduce the principles that form the basis of theory (Duffy, 1962; Scott, 1966) suggest
Herzberg's inductive theory of job satsifaction. that the particular stimulus element falls
If one considers that challenge produces a below some optimal level, which results in
negative hedonic state that is replaced with underarousal, a negative hedonic state. Never-
a positive hedonic state when the challenge is theless, it is commonplace to note that these
met, the implications for feedback in the work stimulus elements become "boring" over time.
setting are rather dramatic. In most work Helson (1964) has suggested that this increas-
settings, individuals require outside confirma- ing impotence of stimuli to arouse the orga-
tion that a challenge has been met. The most nism is a result of some neural mechanism
obvious source for this feedback is the super- represented by the hypothetical construct of
visor. Thus, if individuals are provided with "adaptation level." The operations of hedonic
challenge yet never or seldom told when they excitation, maintenance, and decay are con-
have successfully met the challenge (or made siderably clearer in the opponent process
progress toward meeting it), one would expect theory than in the adaptation or arousal
constant levels of negative tension. Feedback level frameworks. This is due in part to the
indicating that a challenge has been met is fact that both arousal and adaptation proposi-
necessary for the " termination" of the stimulus tions are more directly concerned with action
represented by the challenge. In this frame- than emotion.
work, feedback is a necessary but not sufficient Opponent process theory requires one to
condition for producing positive hedonic view the issue of boredom as a more general
states. The sufficient condition is controlled phenomenon than currently implied by extant
by the number of previous presentations of literature. The issue becomes one of the
challenge. habituation level of an organism relative to
Mechanisms such as those described above various classes of stimuli. Thus, one can be
might also help account for the effects of job just as easily "bored" with levels of pay as
enrichment. If the enriched job represents one can with tasks required by a particular
increased levels of challenge, initial reactions job. In this framework, job enrichment might
of workers should be negative to varying be thought of as an instance of stimulus
degrees (depending on the degree of perceived sensitization; boring tasks would be an
change between the old levels of challenge and instance of habituation. In this framework, the
the new levels). As the "new" job is learned adjective boring might be applied to any one
and the challenges successfully met, resistance of a number of stimulus elements that char-
and negative responses should decrease and acterize the job, not just the tasks that define
satisfaction with mastery of the new job that job.
should increase. The degree to which the job When the concept of boredom is extended
is enriched should have implications for in this way, there are few theories of job
resulting hedonic states. If the enriched job satisfaction capable of explaining the phenom-
represents dramatically increased levels of enon. Groves and Thompson (1970) have
challenge, the potential for dissatisfaction with suggested that a model of habituation must
OPPONENT PROCESS 543

include at least the following parameters: earlier, Locke (1976) implies that achieving
time course and recovery, effect of repeated goals produces pleasure, and opponent process
series, effect of stimulus frequency, effect of theory would imply that setting goals may
stimulus intensity, and stimulus generalization have negative hedonic effects. Thus, early in
gradients. With the exception of the final a goal-setting "career," individuals will ac-
parameter, opponent process theory meets the tively resist goal setting and receive minimal
requirements. In their consideration of habit- amounts of pleasure from achieving those
uation, Groves and Thompson suggest that goals. As experience with goal setting and
habituation and stimulus frequency are expo- goal attainment increases, resistance should
nentially related. Thus, opponent process decrease (since the primary state is reduced by
theory would seem uniquely suited for a the increasingly powerful opponent state)
detailed decomposition of the phenomenon and derived pleasure from goal attainment
of boredom and an extension of the concept should increase (since the aftereffect rep-
to cover stimulus elements other than job resented by the opponent state becomes
tasks. increasingly stronger and decays more slowly).
There are some clear differences between Thus, opponent process theory provides
the approach that activation theorists (Scott, goal-setting propositions with a mechanism
1966) take to decompose the hedonic phenom- for describing some affective history. This
enon accompanying boredom and that pro- affective history, in turn, represents an in-
posed by opponent process theory. The dividual differences parameter affecting the
activation position would be that all departures likelihood that an individual will accept a
from some idiographic "normal" level would hard goal and be satisfied with its achievement.
be noxious to the organism and that the
organism would attempt to escape from that
state and ultimately avoid that state by Parametric Investigations
controlling levels of stimulation. Opponent
process theory would separate manifest hedonic It should be obvious that the first major
behavior from latent states represented by the obstacle to be overcome in the application of
primary and opponent processes. Thus, individ- opponent process theory to the study of
uals would be capable of both positive and job statisfaction is a specification of the
negative affective reactions toward stimuli parameters of the model. An examination of
that exceed some threshold. A good deal of Figures 1-4 is sufficient to identify most of
recent physiological research (Frankenhauser, them.
1974) on stress suggests that there are "hidden" The first major question concerns the
costs to the organism for overarousal. This definition of a "stimulus." The work environ-
research suggests that individuals are not ment is rich with stimuli, and one must be
always capable of recognizing the harmful able to decompose that environment into
effects or costs of manifestly pleasant ex- main effects and interactions. Opponent proc-
periences. This line of research clearly implies ess theory demands such dec&mposition to
that not all suprathreshold arousal is con- a greater degree than other approaches to
sidered noxious by the organism, contrary to satisfaction. The content theories of job satis-
the propositions of activation theory. faction ask us to accept a limited number of
Finally, the propositions of opponent process homogeneous stimulus elements. Neverthe-
theory suggest some modifications of Locke's less, both the opponent process theory and
goal-setting theory of work motivation and job alternative approaches face this obstable
satisfaction (Locke & Bryan, 1969; Locke, equally. A reasonable strategy for an examina-
Cartledge, & Knerr, 1970). A major proposition tion of this question might be borrowed from
of Locke's approach is that individuals who the experimentalists. The concept of stimulus
accept hard goals expend greater energy and control (Terrace, 1966) might be a useful
have the capacity to experience greater strategy for addressing this issue. In such a
satisfaction than individuals who accept paradigm, the organism's ability to dis-
easier goals or no goals at all. As indicated criminate among stimuli is of central concern.
544 FRANK J. LANDY

The experimental designs resemble those strategies might be easily extended to plot the
associated with constructing generalization "power" of various work-related stimuli. This
gradients for various stimulus elements. Opera- would provide a preliminary specification of
tionally, one might attempt to determine the threshold parameter of the model.
the independence of stimulus elements in terms A third parameter of importance is the
of the response characteristics of the organism. decay functions of both the primary and
If the response levels to various stimulus opponent states. When the stimulus is ter-
elements is relatively flat, we might conclude minated, it is unlikely that the primary state
that the stimulus elements are not distinct disappears simultaneously. There is bound to
from the point of view of the respondent. On be some latency or decay. The same is true,
the other hand, if the generalization gradients and to a greater extent, of the opponent
are steep, we would conclude that the stimulus state. In the simplest terms, the question is
elements are perceived as independent by the "How long does a stimulus last?" The experi-
individual. Historically, researchers in the mental paradigm for studying such a question
area of the job satisfaction have proposed is one of extinction. Since longitudinal studies
stimulus groupings on the basis of the verbal of satisfaction have been so infrequent, we
responses across individuals. These responses know little or nothing about the decay func-
have traditionally been factor or cluster tions of stimulation as a molar concept, let
analyzed and functional generalization gra- alone the decay of two separate functions such
dients assumed. This is an imprecise paradigm as those represented by the primary and
for determining stimulus properties. The opponent states in opponent process theory.
stimulus generalization or stimulus control Nevertheless, it is reasonable to propose that
paradigm is an alternative approach to the a decay parameter interacts with stimulus
problem that may be of some value. frequency and intensity.
A second question relates to the "acceptable" A fourth parameter is suggested by some
levels of neural stimulation. Opponent process recent work done by Solomon (Starr &
theory proposes that when some threshold is Solomon, Note 1). He suggests that the
exceeded, the opponent process is activated. interstimulus interval is the key to the increas-
The definition of this acceptable-unacceptable ing strength and decreasing latency of the
excursion from neutrality must be considerably opponent process. Specifically, in laboratory
tightened. Once again, this is not a problem studies it has been found that if the stimulus
peculiar to industrial and organizational of interest is presented before the opponent
psychologists. In some senses, this parameter state of the previous stimulus presentation
is tied to the question of when a stimulus is disappears or decays, the opponent state will
"present." Experimental psychology has dealt grow in strength and decrease in latency.
with variations of this question in the just Conversely, if the next stimulus presentation
noticeable differences paradigm. To some conies after the decay or disappearance of the
extent, we are describing a process whereby previous opponent state, the opponent state
the organism "notices" that stimulus intensity will decrease in strength. This suggests a
has changed. The question becomes one of classical conditioning mechanism connecting
identifying the level of stimulus change the primary and opponent states. This condi-
necessary for the perception of change by tioning paradigm might be useful for examining
the organism. Weber's law and Steven's power the effect of interstimulus intervals. Intui-
functions (Stevens, 1957) are operational tively, the concept of interstimulus intervals
definitions of such changes for various modal- is an appealing one for work-related phenomena
ities. The concept of just noticeable differences such as reward periods, job cycles, etc., but
is at the heart of equity theory (Adams, 1965; there has been little or no systematic investiga-
Jaques, 1961). In addition, there have been tion of this parameter.
sporadic attempts to use classic psychophysical Although there are other, less obvious,
operations to plot the effect of increased parameters that might be suggested by theory,
stimulus intensity against satisfaction in the the major ones have been presented above.
work setting (Zedeck & Smith, 1968). These These parameters should be important for
OPPONENT PROCESS 545

any theory of job satisfaction, yet they have degree that it improves our understanding or
not been directly addressed by those consider- prediction.
ing the question of job satisfaction. The
advantage of opponent process theory is that
it provides a reason to investigate those Suggested Research
parameters as well as a model for placing them The immediate research questions suggested
in coherent relationships with each other. by the opponent process theory are parametric
These conceptual applications of opponent in nature. As such, research generated by these
process theory to satisfaction and motivation questions should prove useful to all theory
in work settings are intended to demonstrate building in the area of job satisfaction. Below,
the value of considering a new paradigm in several of these questions have been formally
the study of job satisfaction. The kinds of stated as general hypotheses with specific
questions that opponent process theory raises corollaries:
concerning the phenomenon of job satisfaction 1. There is a systematic recruitment, main-
have not often been asked in published tenance, and decay function that can be
research. Nevertheless, they are appropriate used to describe the hedonic response of an
and even crucial questions if we accept the individual over time to the presentation of a
proposition that the concept of job satisfaction particular stimulus element, (a) After the
is intended to represent some hedonic state. presentation of a reward, an individual's
Most current approaches to job satisfaction satisfaction with that reward will systemat-
(Lawler, 1973) imply that it is a cognitive ically change over time, eventually returning
outcome with simple hedonic consequences. to prereward levels of satisfaction with that
Opponent process theory requires one to reward, (b) Individuals will report both
consider the nature and consequences of that satisfaction and dissatisfaction with constant
hedonic state in a more direct and complex levels of a particular reward at differing
manner. points in time.
To be sure, the opponent process approach 2. Repeated stimulus presentation affects
is problematic in any setting, but even more the manifest response of an individual to a
so in attempting to explain the field behavior particular stimulus element, (a) The greater
of humans rather than the laboratory behavior the number of times that a particular reward
in infrahumans. As mentioned earlier, one of or punishment has been presented, the longer
the most difficult problems in the application it will take for an individual to return to
of the theory to the concept of job satisfaction prereward or punishment level, (b) The
is the notion of stimulus onset and stimulus greater the number of times a reward has been
termination. Since humans have the well- presented, the less the satisfaction of the
developed capacity to abstract, form concepts, individual with each succeeding presentation,
and deal with the real world symbolically, one (c) The greater the number of times a reward
cannot easily control, or even measure, or punishment has been presented, the greater
temporal stimulus properties. In addition, will be the effect of terminating that reward or
there are multiple primary and opponent punishment on job satisfaction of individuals.
processes operating simultaneously in the real 3. The shorter the interval between succes-
world. The manner in which these multiple sive presentations of stimulus elements, the
hedonic systems interact becomes a central stronger the opposing response to the primary
question for the researcher. The intricacy and state generated by those stimulus elements,
complexity of opponent process theory suggests (a) Rewards or punishments that are presented
that it is not terribly parsimonious when frequently will have smaller immediate effects
compared to more compact approaches. Never- on job satisfaction than those presented
theless, the parsimony of past approaches infrequently.
to job satisfaction has not proven useful in 4. Goal-directed activity produces negative
incorporating the concept of satisfaction into primary states and positive opponent states,
the broader question of motivation. Parsimony (a) Individuals will report less satisfaction
in theory building is only desirable to the with a particular task before meeting a goal
546 FRANK J. LANDY

than after meeting a goal, (b) Individuals Campbell, J. P., & Pritchard, R. D. Motivation theory
will report less satisfaction with difficult in industrial and organizational psychology. In
M. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and
tasks than with easy tasks prior to meeting organizational psychology. Chicago: Rand McNally,
a task goal, (c) Individuals will report greater 1976.
satisfaction with difficult tasks than with easy deCharms, R. Personal causation: The internal affective
tasks after meeting a task goal, (d) Individuals determinants of behavior. New York: Academic
Press, 1968.
with a history of goal setting will report less
Deci, E. L. The effects of contingent and non-contingent
dissatisfaction during goal-seeking periods rewards and controls on intrinsic motivation.
than individuals with no history of goal setting. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance,
These are just a few of the hypotheses that 1972, 8, 217-229.
might be posed on the basis of the opponent Deci, E. L. Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum,
1975.
process theory and its application to the Duffy, E. Activation and behavior. New York: Wiley,
phenomenon of job satisfaction. There are 1962.
many other, less obvious, implications of the Dunnette, M. Handbook of industrial and organizational
theory. For example, stimulus intensity seems Psychology. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1976.
less critical than stimulus frequency in Ewen, R. B. Weighting components of job satisfaction.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 1967, 51, 68-73.
understanding hedonic response level; it may Frankenhauser, M. Overstimulation: a threat to the
make little difference for hedonic states if quality of life. In, Man in the communications system
rewards are contingent or noncontingent upon of the future. Stockholm: Swedish Cabinet Office,
some particular behavior; avoidance and Secretariat for Future Studies, 1974.
Groves, P. M., & Thompson, R. F. Habituation: A
escape mechanisms are as crucial for under- dual process theory. Psychological Review, 1970,
standing in industrial behavior as are approach 77, 419-450.
mechanisms. Kelson, H. Adaptation level theory: An experimental and
In summary, the questions posed by a systematic approach to behavior. New York: Harper
consideration of opponent process theory and & Row, 1964.
Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., & Snyderman, B. B. The
its applicability to job satisfaction are ques- motivation to work. New York: Wiley, 1959.
tions that are crucial for any reasonable Hoffman, H. S., & Solomon, R. L. The opponent
theory of job satisfaction. Opponent process process theory of motivation: III. Some affective
theory represents the most well-developed dynamics in imprinting. Learning and Motivation,
1974, 5, 149-164.
deductive research currently available. As Jaques, E. Equitable payment. New York: Wiley, 1961.
such, it might be efficiently interlaced with Kahn, R. L. Conflict, ambiguity, and overload: Three
current content theories of job satisfaction to elements in job stress. Occupational Mental Health,
yield a more reasonable decomposition of the 1973, 3, 2-9.
satisfaction phenomenon. Katzell, R. Personal values, job satisfaction, and job
behavior. In H. Borow (Ed.), Man in a world at work.
Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1964.
Lawler, E. E. Motivation in work organizations.
Reference Note Monterey, Calif.: Brooks/Cole, 1973.
Likert, R. New patterns in management. New York:
1. Starr, M. D., & Solomon, R. L. Imprinting with
brief exposures: Factors affecting the development of McGraw-Hill, 1961.
separation distress in ducklings. Paper presented Locke, E. The relationship of task success to task
at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Associa- liking and satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology,
tion, New York, April 1976. 1965, 49, 379-385.
Locke, E. A. The nature and causes of job satisfaction.
In M. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and
References organizational psychology. Chicago: Rand McNally,
1976.
Adams, J. S. Injustice in social exchange. In L. Berko- Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. F. The directing function of
witz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology goals in task performance. Organizational Behavior
(Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press, 1965. and Human Performance, 1969, 4, 35-42.
Alderfer, C. P. An empirical test of a new theory of Locke, E. A., Cartledge, N., & Knerr, C. S. Studies of
human needs. Organizational Behavior and Human the relationship between goal setting, satisfaction,
Performance, 1969, 4, 142-175. and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human
Berlyne, D. E. Arousal and reinforcement. In D. Levine Performance, 1970, 5, 135-139.
(Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 15). Maslow, A. A theory of human motivation. Psycholog-
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1967. ' ical Review, 1943, 50, 370-396.
OPPONENT PROCESS 547
McClelland, D. C. The achieving society. Princeton, Smith, P. C., Kendall, L. M., & Hulin, C. L. The
N.J.: Van Nostrand, 1961. measurement of satisfaction in work and retirement.
McGrath, J. Social and psychological factors in stress. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1969.
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1970, Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. An opponent process
Miller, N. E. Experiments on motivation: Studies theory of motivation: II. Cigarette addiction.
combining psychological, physiological, and phar- Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1973, 81, 158-171.
macological techniques. Science, 19S7, 126', 1271- Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. An opponent process
1278. theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect.
Morse, N. C. Satisfactions in the white collar job. Ann Psychological Review, 1974,81, 119-145.
Arbor: University of Michigan Survey Research Stevens, S. S. On the psychophysical law. Psychological
Center, 1953. Review, 1957, 64, 153-181.
Olds, J. Hypothalamic substrates of reward. Physiolog- Taylor, F. W. Principles of scientific management.
ical Review, 1962, 42, 554-604. New York: Harper & Row, 1947.
Porter, L. W., & Lawler, E. E. Managerial attitudes and Terrace, H. S. Stimulus control. In W. H. Honig (Ed.),
performance. Homewood, 111.: Irwin-Dorsey, 1968. Operant behavior: areas of research and application.
Roethlisberger, F. J., & Dixon, W. J. Management and New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966.
the worker. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Vroom, V. Work and motivation. New York: Wiley,
Press, 1939. 1964.
Schaffer, R. H. Job satisfaction as related to need Zedeck, S., & Smith, P. C. A psychophysical determina-
satisfaction in work. Psychological Monographs, tion of equitable payment. Journal of Applied
1953, <J7(14, Whole No. 304). Psychology, 1968, 52, 343-347.
Scott, W. E. Activation theory and task design.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance,
1966, ;, 3-30. Received November 28, 1977 •

You might also like