You are on page 1of 11

Thinking Skills and Creativity 8 (2013) 3444

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Thinking Skills and Creativity


journal homepage: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/tsc

Moods, emotions and creative thinking: A framework for teaching


Douglas P. Newton
Durham University, School of Education, Leazes Road, Durham DH1 1TA, UK

a r t i c l e

i n f o

Article history:
Received 19 March 2012
Received in revised form 28 May 2012
Accepted 30 May 2012
Available online 30 June 2012

Keywords:
Emotioncreativity interaction
Teaching framework

a b s t r a c t
When planning and teaching, attention is generally given to cognition while the effect
of mood and emotion on cognition is ignored. But students are not emotionless thinkers
and the effect can make a difference to their thought. This is particularly evident when
attempting to foster creative thinking. This article draws on research to describe aspects of
creative thought and problem-solving, moods and emotions, and some of their interactions.
It uses these to construct a framework to help teachers of students at all levels plan for,
think about, manage and mediate creative thinking in classrooms. The framework takes into
account the ow of changing moods and emotions as tasks progress and accommodates
individual thinking and collaborative group work. Implications for practice and teacher
training are considered.
2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction
Thinking has a variety of goals and some are highly valued for what they can do for the thinker and for others (Craft,
1999; Pink, 2005; Shaheen, 2010; Sharp & Le Mtais, 2000). Creative thought is one of these (Newton, 2012a). Commonly,
emotions are seen as primitive responses which can impede thought (Ragozinno, Km, Hassert, Minniti, & Kiang, 2003)
but creative thought, particularly in the arts, tends to be the exception with high creativity being popularly linked with
emotional excess (Averil, Chon, & Hahn, 2001; Gino, Agote, Miron-Spektor, & Todorova, 2009; Russ, 1996; Sternberg, 1985).
However, the subtle and complex interaction between emotions and creative thought is common and needs no excess.
Nevertheless, it has tended to be ignored by those whose work bears on teaching and learning (Adler & Obstfeld, 2007;
Hargreaves, 2000). Immordino-Yang and Damasio (2007: 3) contend that this is a serious omission as, we feel, therefore we
learn. A teachers success in fostering creative thought could be greater if the teacher gave some attention to the feeling
involved in learning. This account describes some of the interactions between moods, emotions and creative thought and
draws out some implications for the teacher. It begins with a short description of creativity then outlines emotions, moods
and related states. These are brought together in an account of some studies of their interaction which are then used to
suggest a framework to support the development of creative thinking skills.
2. Creativity
Popularly, creativity is seen as something you either have or do not have and, if you have it, it reveals itself spontaneously
and inexplicably (Sternberg, 2006). Like other thought, creativity rests on unconscious processes but it is neither mystical
nor ineffable (Osborne, 2003). Carruthers (2002: 226) has pointed out that anyone who is imagining how things could be
other than they are will be thinking creatively. Nevertheless, creative behaviours are complex, drawing partly on an ability

Tel.: +1 0191 3348353; fax: +1 0191 33488311.


E-mail address: D.P.Newton@durham.ac.uk
1871-1871/$ see front matter 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2012.05.006

D.P. Newton / Thinking Skills and Creativity 8 (2013) 3444

35

to associate or generate combinations of previously unconnected ideas to produce something more or less novel or original,
at least to that person (Nassif, 2004). Novelty alone is not sufcient; it must be associated with appropriateness, plausibility,
tness-for-purpose or rightness-of-t in the eyes of others (Amabile, 1996; Csikszentmihalyi, 1996; Newton, 2010, 2012a;
Siegesmund, 1998). Achieving these ends involves conscious and unconscious decisions on both the micro and macro scale
(Sternberg, 2006). For instance, when painting, Matisse appeared to work in a rapid, continuous manner but slow-motion
lm shows he hesitated before each brush stroke, presumably as he decided what to do (Merleau-Ponty, 1964). Novelty and
appropriateness may be achieved in a variety of ways but some may be particularly clever, elegant well-crafted, simple or
economical (OQuin & Besemer, 1989), commonly seen as a desirable quality which Glck, Ernst, and Unger (2002) have
called impression. The precise terms used depend on the domain. In science, for instance, explanations of the world are
fairly tightly constrained by scientic plausibility and parsimony. Generating a scientic explanation is often called problem
solving. In art, on the other hand, the problem is much less constrained and, while a representation must have a rightnessof-t and be rewarding in some way, the process is rarely described as problem solving, although it could be cast in that
form. Similarly, the weight afforded each attribute can vary with the domain. For instance, artists may value originality of
subject more than rightness-of-t while many architects value appropriateness (for them, function) more than originality
(Glck et al., 2002; Newton & Donkin, 2011). Cropley (2001) and Craft (2008) also add the desirability of wise and ethical
creativity.
While some product or solution may be novel, what gives rise to it is a process. Perlovsky and Levine (2010) argue
that this has its distant origins in the evolution of the ability to represent the world mentally and to manipulate these
representations to t changing circumstances. Some forty thousand years ago, this ability blossomed as humans, perhaps
with the help of language, exploited the creative process in their art and technology to make it one of the most striking
features of the human species (Carruthers, 2002: 226). On this basis, most people have the potential to be creative but it
may not be equally distributed or well-developed (Boden, 2004). Governments in the West, and increasingly elsewhere,
see economic advantage in workplace creativity (see e.g., Sharp & Le Mtais, 2000). To gain that advantage, teachers are
widely urged to foster creativity across the curriculum (see e.g., DfES, 2003; Milne, 2007; Newton & Newton, 2010; Park,
Soo-Young, Oliver, & Cramond, 2006; QCA, 2005; Tan, 2000). At the same time, a creative, non-conformist disposition is
seen as encouraging democratic attitudes, at least as they are dened by UNESCO (2006). But, creativity can be suppressed
or stied by educational practices which avoid and discourage it, or by cultures which value other abilities more (Averil
et al., 2001; Craft, 2002; Garner, 2007). At the same time, novelty, appropriateness, elegance and their relative importance
may be different in different cultures (Al-Karasneh & Saleh, 2010; Niu, 2009; Sofowara, 2007). For instance, the Confucian
tradition has generally given more weight to appropriateness than to novelty (Averil et al., 2001). With an increasingly global
economy and shrinking world, such differences can matter in education as various cultures come together for learning and
trade. However, they should not be taken to imply that one cultural group is innately less creative than another or that there
are no differences in creative dispositions within an otherwise homogenous group.
Not everyone sees widespread creativity as something which is good (Cropley, Cropley, Kaufman, & Runco, 2010). Osborne
(2003), for instance, believes a profusion of novel products will make life unstable, unsettling and socially disturbing. On
the other hand, fostering a creative disposition can also be of direct, personal benet to learners insofar as it equips them to
cope successfully with new situations and solve problems in short, it gives them a life skill (Newton, 2012a). This, alone,
would be a worthwhile outcome. However, teachers conceptions of creativity tend to be narrow and lack differentiation
and specicity. While most recognise that novelty is an attribute, they can be unclear about other attributes and the variety
of creative opportunities in specic curriculum contexts (Newton, 2012b).

3. Emotions, moods, dispositions and traits


Emotions are mental states arising from personal evaluations of the world which prompt a readiness to act in support
of well-being (Damasio, 2000; Oatley & Jenkins, 1996). Episodes of emotion, like fear at the sight of a snake, are generally
produced by a specic event, person or object and have a relatively short duration ranging from seconds to hours. Such
emotions can produce rapid responses without recourse to conscious thought but with evident survival value (Frijda, 2004;
Lahikainen, Kirmanen, Kraav, & Taimalu, 2003; LeDoux, 1989, 2000; Plutchik, 1980). Moods, however, generally refer to
low intensity diffuse states which can last from hours to months. Isen (1993) suggested that moods have a self-regulatory
function which provides information about ones general state of being relative to the world (see also, Hullett, 2005). Some
states, like depression and anxiety, can be very long lasting and may be described as dispositions or traits (e.g., George & Zhou,
2002; Madjar & Oldham, 2002; Oatley & Jenkins, 1996). People often learn that certain kinds of events generally threaten
or promote their well-being. This knowledge can produce a more or less sustained state of readiness to respond to events
in a particular way (Frijda, 1986). For instance, most know that examinations matter so feel anxious as one approaches. In
effect, emotions provide information about the environment and, in particular, can reect the perceived nature of a task
in its context (Frijda, 1988; Schwartz & Clore, 2003). Of course, the intensity of the emotion varies from person to person,
as does how well each copes with it. For some, it can be disabling and generate long-term academic problems (Richards
& Hadwin, 2011). Feelings refer only to the private experience of mental states and cannot be observed by someone else
(Damasio, 2000). Emotions, moods, dispositions and traits, however, are wider and include effects which might be noticed
by, for instance, a teacher.

36

D.P. Newton / Thinking Skills and Creativity 8 (2013) 3444

Although these mental states may be distinguished by duration, it is a loose distinction. While moods can be more
enduring than emotions, Lazarus (1991) has pointed out that some moods are eeting and readily displaced as the situation
changes. This has particular relevance for the creative or problem solving process which extends over time. During such an
event, emotions and moods can develop and change in what has come to be called the stream or ow of affective experience
(e.g., Vandekerckhove & Panskepp, 2009; Watson, 2000). Emotions, moods, dispositions and traits can all shape creative
thought (Mumford, 2003) but, in the normal course of classroom events, strong emotions may be less evident than moods,
dispositions and traits in the stream of affective experience and it is moods that are more susceptible to change in that ow
of experience.
Affective experience can be classied in various ways. A long-standing division has been according to their hedonic
tone, that is, their association with pleasant or unpleasant feelings. Happiness and joy, for instance, have pleasant, positive hedonic tones; sadness and depression have unpleasant, negative hedonic tones. It is also possible to be in a neutral
state which, nevertheless, may have consequences for thought (Diener & Lucas, 2000). Moods and emotions have also been
divided into those which activate and those which deactivate. Thus, happiness and anger are activating while serenity
and sadness are deactivating. A third division is into those which prompt approach and those which prompt avoidance,
often referred to as promotion and prevention focused moods, respectively. Happiness, sadness and frustration, for example, tend to produce approach while serenity, fear and anxiety produce avoidance. This means that each mood can be
more or less pleasant, more or less activating and more or less directing at the same time (Baas, De Dreu, & Nijstad,
2008).
Responses to moods and emotions (or, at least, their expression), vary with people and culture. Those in some Eastern
societies, for instance, have been described as reserved (Chon, Kim, & Ryoo, 2000; Hahn, Lee, & Ashton, 1999) but so have
some in the West at certain times and we should not assume that such differences are permanent or universal within a
particular society. As Averil et al. (2001) have pointed out, the prevailing norms of a society and the process of enculturation
shape peoples behaviour and these vary with time and place.
4. Moods and creative thought interacting
Plato talked of the rational mind struggling to master the emotions, like a charioteer with an unruly horse, while Aristotle put the emotions to use in persuasive argument (Kristjnsson, 2007; Sokolon, 2006). Care is needed, however, as
, commonly translated as emotion, may not be entirely synonymous with what emotion means to us (Konstan,

2006). Aristotle, for instance, did not include happiness amongst his emotions and Rosenwein (2010) illustrates similar difculties in understanding emotional expression in history. For example, love of a leader in medieval times can
mean that most agree with the leader. Consequently, Rosenwein describes history as a succession of emotional communities, each with more or less overlapping understandings of emotion. Nevertheless, interpretations of the past inuence
those who inherit it. Descartes in the seventeenth century cut rmly between reason and passion (Damasio, 1994). This
helped to shape the ethos of what followed so, for instance, Thomas Jefferson saw American independence as demonstrating that reason alone is what counts (Lehrer, 2009). Kant (1785/2002) took this to its limit and made morality a
matter of logic. Not all agreed. Hume (1739/1978) argued that passions have the prime role in directing affairs, although
they may be modied by reason. Deutscher (2011) has demonstrated that neither Humes nor Kants views are entirely
tenable in their extreme forms and that reason and emotion are not separable. Lehrer (2009, 20) goes further: If it
werent for our emotions, reason wouldnt exist at all. This is not to say that emotion has been ignored in education.
For instance, while Blooms 1956 taxonomy of educational objectives began with the cognitive domain, the affective domain
was added later but the interaction of the domains was of little concern at that time. Anderson and Krathwohls revision of the taxonomy acknowledged the involvement of affect but is conned to the cognitive domain (Moseley et al.,
2005).
4.1. Task context
Adler and Obstfeld (2007) consider moods and emotions to be central to motivation and whether there will even be
creative engagement with a task. They point to the importance of how the task is framed, enjoyment and interest being
more relevant for those in positive moods while task importance and the need for a high quality outcomes being more
signicant for those in negative moods. For those in a positive mood, satisfaction tends to come from the tasks intrinsic
interest, agreeableness and the generation of many solutions. At the outset, happy participants tend to look for opportunities
for these sources of satisfaction. If these are not evident, they alter the task to supply them (Hirt, Devers, & McCrea, 2008).
For those in a negative mood, satisfaction is in the recognition and reward they believe will come from success (e.g., Gasper,
2004; George & Zhou, 2002; Kaufman, 2003; Kaufman & Vosberg, 1997; Vosberg, 1998). At the same time, participants are
often subject to a controlling regime. If this is one of coercion, obligation, punishment, comparison with better performers,
and ne control of action, there is likely to be stress, anxiety and stied creativity (Adler & Obstfeld, 2007; Avey et al.,
2011). Substituting autonomy for constraint tends to encourage creative behaviours probably because of the freedom it
gives to imagination and the exploration of ideas (Hon, 2012). Perhaps counter-intuitively, offering high quality products as
exemplars tends to reduce novelty and stimulate copying, particularly for those who are inclined to be adventurous with
ideas, like those in a positive mood. Rook and van Knippenberg (2011) suggest that pointing out the potential gains of a good

D.P. Newton / Thinking Skills and Creativity 8 (2013) 3444

37

idea with the help of weak ideas, products or solutions could encourage more adventurous thought. Nevertheless, although
careful meta-analyses of many studies conrm such ndings, there are enough exceptions to indicate that this is not the full
story (e.g., Baas et al., 2008; Davis, 2009).

4.2. Thinking on task


Positive moods have often been found to promote creative thought and problem solving (e.g., Fredrickson, 2004; Hirt
et al., 2008; Isen, Daubman, & Nowicki, 1987; Isen, Johnson, Mertz, & Robinson, 1985; Kaufman, 2003; Vosberg, 1998). One
explanation is that such moods are consistent with a situation that is benign, safe and open to unguarded exploration,
experimentation and play. Attention is broad and thinking exible and divergent and the number and quality of ideas is
greater. Positive moods also reduce concerns about being judged, something which can suppress creativity. These positive
mood effects, however, tend to decline as time passes and they can predispose people to accept less than optimum solutions
(e.g., Clore, Gaspar, & Garvin, 2001; Fiedler, 1988; Fredrickson, 2001; George & Zhou, 2002; Grawitch & Munz, 2005; Kaufman,
2003; Schwartz, 2001; Schwartz & Clore, 2003).
Negative moods are not necessarily inimical to creative thought; they can help to produce solutions in complex problem
solving situations but thinking strategies are often different from those induced by positive moods. For instance, those in a
negative mood have been found to give more attention to collecting information to use in solving a problem than those in
a positive mood (Spering, Wagener, & Funke, 2005). In some circumstance, a negative mood can produce greater creativity
than positive moods. The mood induced by social rejection, for example, has been found to increase the creativity of those
engaged in artwork (Akinola & Mendes, 2008). Negative moods tend to produce cautious, focused, close attention; thinking
becomes convergent, analytical, careful, systematic, persistent and concerned with detail and can produce well thought-out
ideas (e.g., Gasper, 2004; George & Zhou, 2002; Fredrickson, 2004; Kaufman, 2003; Martin & Stoner, 1996; Schwartz, 2001;
Schwartz & Clore, 2003). In other words, there is the potential for creative success in both moods but the character of that
success may be different (Spering et al., 2005).
Some effects of moods are explained by other theories. According to the level of activation view, the capacity for complex
thought increases as the level of mental arousal increases but only up to a point. After that, more intense arousal takes mental
resources away from the task to attend to the source of arousal or its effects. At some optimum level, which may be different
for different people, the potential for creative thought is greatest (De Dreu, Baas, & Nijstad, 2008), an effect which is also
notably evident with children (Russ & Grossman-McKee, 1990). A skilful analysis by Baas et al. (2008), however, found that
the effects of mood on creativity were explained better by combinations of theories. In particular, the interplay of activation
and self-regulation can predict many of the observed effects. For example, happiness is both activating and promotion
focused and so favours an engagement with the task which could be productive, probably aided by happinesss ability to
widen attention and increase exibility of thought. Sadness, on the other hand, is promotion focused but deactivating so
does not favour an engagement with the task. Anxiety, while being activating, is prevention focused and induces avoidance
so also fails to support creativity.

4.3. Promoting productive thinking


The creative situation, however, is not constant. Amongst other things, there are times for generating ideas, for evaluating
ideas, for adjusting and rening ideas, for testing ideas, and for starting again. Whatever the mood at the outset, it may change
in a stream of affective experience. At times, the mood may not produce the kind of thinking best suited to the current needs
(e.g., Krper, Fay, Lindberg, & Meinel, 2011). For instance, successfully generating promising solutions to a problem may
be exhilarating but those now need careful, detailed evaluation. Similarly, a failed idea may be depressing but now new
ideas are needed. Some people can change their moods themselves (Averil et al., 2001; Phelps, 2006). In the absence of
successful self-regulation, a team leader or supervisor may try to manage events to sustain productive thought. When
creativity is expected in the workplace, being treated with kindness, respect and dignity (what George (2007) referred to
as interactional justice) coupled with an acceptance that creativity is never certain, encourages expansive thought and risk
taking. Supervisors who show interactional justice and are receptive to ideas encourage others to make the most of their
thinking, whether moods are positive or negative (George, 2007). George also found that developmental feedback could
help people make the most of their current mode of thought: those in a positive mood are encouraged to engage in expansive
thinking and to generate ideas, those in a negative mood are encouraged to use their systematic, bottom-up thinking to make
progress.
Complex as this is, it may not be the full story. Adler and Obstfeld (2007) have pointed out that research may be biased
towards studies of successful creativity and away from failed creativity. At present, there is also a greater interest in experiments on creative thinking in the workplace than in the classroom. This means that care is needed in extending such ndings
to other contexts. While they could be very useful for informing the teaching of older students (particularly those learning
in authentic, real-world situations), care is needed when applying them to younger learners who may not think or respond
in entirely the same way. Nevertheless, there are good indications that childrens responses broadly reect those of adults
(Rader & Hughes, 2005).

38

D.P. Newton / Thinking Skills and Creativity 8 (2013) 3444

Fig. 1. A framework for thinking about fostering creativity and problem solving in teaching and learning contexts.

5. Some lessons for the teacher


Various strategies have been used to support the creative process ranging from collections of useful ideas, such as that of
Bowkett (2007), to systematic tools like the Theory of Inventive Problem Solving (TRIZ; Altshuller, 2000). Between these is
de Bonos Cognitive Research Trust (CoRT) programme with its well-known Six Thinking Hats strategy (e.g., de Bono, 1995).
This directs attention to particular kinds of thought seen as needed at a given time. For instance, Time to wear a White Hat
indicates that more information needs to be collected to inform further thought. The Red Hat refers directly to affect and
allows the thinker to express feelings about ideas without the need to justify them. de Bono (1987) believes that emotions
change the brains chemistry so that a different brain is doing the thinking, thereby bringing diversity to thought aimed at
solving a problem. At times, this could be true but the stream of affective experience may also produce an emotion which
closes down productive thought. Studies of CoRTs thinking tools show they are often enjoyed although their effectiveness
is less clear except, perhaps, for learners with Special Educational Needs (Moseley et al., 2005). Less well-known is Hursons
(2008) Productive Thinking by Design, a sequence of steps through the design process, each offering tools and techniques.
Here, emotion (as discontent with the present) is seen only as a motivator. Strategies for supporting creativity could benet
from a consideration of the stream of affect. Fig. 1 depicts the typical components of creative activity in this way.
5.1. A framework for thinking about creativity and problem solving
The institution and the teacher generally establish a regime to facilitate learning, represented in Fig. 1 by the allencompassing learning situation. This can vary widely but reects, amongst other things, the institutions goals and the
teachers beliefs about learning and how to achieve it. Regimes may, for instance, be relaxed or coercive or both at different
times. Given that creativity does not thrive on anxiety, fear of failure, pressure for quick solutions, micro-management, or a
xation with information acquisition, oppressive or coercive regimes may reduce opportunities for creative thought (Adler
& Obstfeld, 2007).
Within the learning situation, the teacher often selects and sets the task. Creativity tasks in the classroom are rarely like
those of laboratory investigations which may, for instance, ask people to create as many origami bird models as possible (e.g.,
Fischer et al., 2007). Instead, they are usually embedded in a curriculum context with success measured by what counts as
novelty, appropriateness and elegance in that context. Teachers need to know what these mean in each domain they teach.
Each learner brings various mental attributes to the task, including knowledge, know-how, capacities, habits, dispositions,
attitudes, values and emotions and moods. Teachers are usually very aware of the moods generated in previous lessons:
students may arrive discontented, bored and frustrated from dull lessons or immoderately elated after a badly managed
lesson. Such moods affect attention, perception and the recall and use of knowledge and know-how. Subsequent engagement
with the task produces its own stream of affective experience which interacts with thought. For instance, problem nding
(as opposed to having a problem supplied by the teacher) and solving can generate intense emotional experiences, possibly
due to the amount of personal investment (Ritchie, Shore, LaBanca, & Newman, 2011). Even very young children nd and
solve problems. One child wanted to write that she was four-and-a-half years old. She could write the four but not the
half but she solved the problem by depicting a half of the gure four next to the rst four and was proud of her solution
(Worthington & Carruthers, 2003). Fig. 1 does not mark off matters of affect sharply from matters of cognition to emphasise
that cognition may not be without affect and its very elements are often be coloured by it (Heyward, 2010; Willis, 2007).

D.P. Newton / Thinking Skills and Creativity 8 (2013) 3444

1. Know creativity

39

2. Recognise
emotions and moods

3. Construct a Frame

4. Consider the Situation

5. Think about the Learners

6. Negotiate the Stream

Fig. 2. Working with moods and emotions while fostering creative thinking.

There may be individual and group creativity (indicated in Fig. 1 by overlaid learners). In group work, happy people
are generally more cooperative than unhappy people (Hom & Arbuckle, 1988). Of course, that mood may change as the
stream of affective experience takes a different course. The moods of group members also interact. In particular, the mood
of a dominant member can spread to others and can suppress or support creative thought. Apathy, for example, can be
contagious and adversely affect the quality of creative outcomes, if any are produced at all (Hood, 2008). Group composition
is also important and some children are unhappy if they must always work in a group. There are times when people feel
that we could be more productive working alone.
5.2. Working with the stream of affect
The creative process involves various kinds of thinking and can be untidy. To compound this, each thinking hat, to use
de Bonos expression, may be worn in a variety of moods thereby multiplying the thinking conditions many times over. In
other words, what a teacher faces is potentially much more diverse than is generally recognised. How might the teacher
handle this? Two possibilities suggest themselves. The rst is to take a broad view and simply make the most of whatever
the stream of affect brings. The second is to attempt a sensitive management of the stream. Both, however, may benet from
some preliminary preparation. In particular:
5.2.1. Know creativity
What counts as creativity depends on the context or domain, such as history or science or art. Some notions of creativity
are vague, narrow and awed and could adversely affect what a teacher provides and supports (see, e.g., Newton, 2012b).
While, in general, we might look for novelty, appropriateness and elegance, a teacher needs to be more specic and know
what these mean in a given context. They also need to know what does not count as subject-specic creativity. For instance,
a teacher may need to know that opportunities for scientic creativity include generating explanations of the world and
constructing practical tests of them. At the same time, the teacher should know that following instructions to make a Solar
System model probably involves little creativity, at least as dened here (Newton & Newton, 2009). Similarly, in Art, a teacher
should know that splashing paint around in new ways is not enough, there must be some rightness-of t (Newton & Donkin,
2011).
5.2.2. Recognise emotions and moods
Most of us can identify powerful emotions like anger, fear and elation. Moods, however, can be more difcult. They tend
to reveal themselves through body language and the tone of verbal responses. Some mood watching may help a teacher
develop an awareness of moods and their avour amongst their students. Mature students are often able and willing to
disclose how they feel but young children are less likely to be able to label their moods. To begin with, limiting attention to
hedonic tone (e.g., feeling good, feeling low) simplies things. Judgments of intensity could be added later (e.g., feeling very
good, feeling a little low).
Planning and practice draw on this preliminary preparation. Fig. 2 shows this schematically.
5.2.3. Construct a frame
How a task is framed is likely to shape how students feel about it. Interest underpins a lot of positive emotion and can
broaden thinking and prepare learners for generating ideas (Willis, 2007). Interest stems from, for instance, self-relevance,

40

D.P. Newton / Thinking Skills and Creativity 8 (2013) 3444

curiosity, mysteries, and challenges which develop competence (Astleitner, 2000; Newton, 1990). The effect is even stronger
with children (Baas et al., 2008). Allowing freedom of thought and action also fosters creative thinking as in project work
when the exploration of a topic is given to the student (Ritchie et al., 2011).
5.2.4. Consider the situation
Over time, students tend to develop feelings about a subject or teacher which they bring with them and which colour
their thinking. In the relatively recent past, some teenagers saw school music as boring and useless. They would arrive
feeling resentful and truculent. Unsurprisingly, the typical music teacher was often a martinet, short on temper and quick
on the pre-emptive strike (Ross, 1975: 185). This, of course reinforced the negative moods of the students. Older students
will also bring moods with them. Roy (2007, 221) described her terrifying experience of high school practical science in
Canada. She felt she was expected to nd the right answer and the sooner the better. The learning situation generated such
powerful, negative emotions that it seriously impeded learning and made her dread the next session. The teacher did nothing
to alleviate it. Creativity does not thrive on anxiety, fear of failure or pressure for quick answers (Adler & Obstfeld, 2007).
Teachers acquire habitual ways of organising learning activities and should consider if they are likely to inhibit creativity.
5.2.5. Think about the learners
Each learner brings emotions and moods. If the students arrive discontented, bored and frustrated or immoderately
elated, it can affect productive thought. Teachers may resort to coercion and suppression to ensure compliance but produce
a situation inimical to creative thinking. Learners also respond in different ways. How young children view and interact with
the world changes as they mature and this may materially affect their creative disposition. For instance, children play and this
can exercise divergent thinking and generate positive moods. While they may be imaginative and generate lots of unusual
ideas, these are likely to be concrete and often, from an adults point of view, inappropriate. At the same time, children
can have poor self-control and may be more readily deected from a task by moods and emotions. More controversially,
some argue that brain development affects creative behaviours in teenagers, who, they argue, lack empathy, are impulsive,
prone to risk taking, and seek autonomy and sensation in attempts to activate strong emotions (Giedd, 2008; Payne, 2010;
Sercombe, 2010). Overly strong emotions are obstacles to task-related attention, exible and divergent thinking. On the
other hand, a craving for autonomy could be used to advantage in project work. Whether or not these stem from brain
development, or lack of it, matters less than that the teacher recognises these tendencies in their practices. Recognising
moods and allowing for differences can, therefore, be useful. Teachers should also consider how their own dispositions are
perceived by others. Moods are infectious so a teacher can present a demeanour which makes moods worse.
At times, a teacher will organise students into groups. Being able to collaborate in problem solving is a useful skill which
can benet from a positive mood as this fosters cooperation. Negative moods, however, are contagious, particularly if evident
in a dominant student (Johnson, 2008). To compound this, a disengaged student can stimulate responses in a teacher which
increase disengagement (Skinner & Belmont, 1993; Sutton & Wheatley, 2003). One tactic would be to join the group and, in
effect, become the dominant member for a while. Some students may also need to learn how to work in a group, showing
consideration for others, listening and thinking about what is suggested.
Intense emotions tend to interact adversely with cognition. Shaping moods needs moderation to avoid such emotions.
Someone who frequently exhibits uncontrollable emotions may need specialist help. Tasks which threaten to expose low
ability or sensitivities publicly, can generate anxiety and fear which impede creativity and, instead, generate coping, selfimage preserving strategies (Thompson & Perry, 2005). Asking students to write a poem and read it to the class could lead
to task avoidance, prevarication, or deliberately writing nonsense in order to avoid the risk of ridicule. Such responses can
be avoided with forethought.
5.2.6. Negotiating the stream
The creative process can benet from particular states of mind. At some points, a free and easy feeling may help to
generate ideas; at others, serious consideration of alternatives may produce the best outcome. Ways of working with the
stream of affect could vary from going with the ow to controlling it. Going with the ow means accepting the prevailing
mood; after all, moods do not last forever (although they may outlast a lesson). Doing nothing failed for Roy (2007) but it need
not be like that. Some teachers intuitively compensate for adverse interactions between thinking and moods. Roy pointed
to another teacher who accepted mistakes as normal and patiently provided stress-free time for more expansive thought.
Similarly, faced with students who are somewhat low, a teacher going with the ow could emphasise the seriousness of
the task and the importance of the outcome. With students who are more positive, the teacher may have to focus attention
on the quality of their more prolic ideas. When ideas are thin on the ground, the teacher might hint at other possibilities.
At the other extreme is the production of particular moods in learners using, say, emotive pictures. Deliberately inducing
moods in this way could be risky in the classroom; it tends to be conned to experimental studies where there is effective
provision for mood repair afterwards. Sometimes, however, emotions and moods are the intended outcome of learning and
Immordino-Yang (2011) describes the use of stories which evoke emotions like sadness, compassion and pity and enable
discussion of such feelings in their context.
Between these are strategies like those which people use themselves to control their moods, consciously or otherwise.
Even children as young as 5 or 6 years show awareness of some of them (Davis et al., 2010) although children may not
always use them well and can make emotions like anxiety worse by their actions (Carthy, Horesh, Apter, & Gross, 2010).

D.P. Newton / Thinking Skills and Creativity 8 (2013) 3444

41

It seems reasonable to help students use such strategies to manage their moods so that they gain from the opportunities
provided. Particularly effective for changing negative to positive moods are behavioural strategies which involve physical
activity, such as a short period of exercise, tidying, non-competitive play. Relaxation through, for instance, deep breathing,
relaxing the body, having a short break, or taking two minutes of quiet time, can have the same effect. Even acting as if you
are in the desired mood can be useful. Thought alone can also be effective as when thinking of something more congenial,
thinking about a past or future happy event (What will you do next weekend? How will you feel?). Distraction from an
unsupportive mood also has a useful effect and music can be used in that way (see, e.g., Augustine & Hemenover, 2009;
Carthy et al., 2010; Davis et al., 2010; Parkinson & Totterdell, 1999; Schnall & Laird, 2003; van Goethem & Sloboda, 2011).
But even helpful moods do not last forever and may need to be refreshed by, for instance, a short break or change of activity,
a (moderate) use of humour or simply suggesting, Lets relax for a couple of minutes. Some of these will be more acceptable
in some contexts than others but teachers should not forget their own powers of persuasion through modellingmoods are
catching (Bettencourt, Gillett, Gall, & Hull, 1983; Mackintosh & Mathews, 2003; Willis, 2007).
Such strategies should not be applied indiscriminately. If the prevailing mood is already appropriate, they can alter it for
the worse (Augustine & Hemenover, 2009). Perhaps counter-intuitively, announcing incentives for good performance tends
to narrow thinking which may be useful for weighing options but can close down idea generation (De Dreu, Nijstad, & Baas,
2011). Urging students to suppress, inhibit or vent a mood is ineffective in changing a mood (Augustine & Hemenover, 2009).
Some students, however, can regulate their own emotions so that drawing their attention to the need for a mood change
can work (Forgas & Vargas, 2000). In other words, supportive feedback need not be conned to matters of cognition. Older
students, collectively, may be able to manage moods themselves. For example, undergraduates learning about the nature
of science were given pictures of large rocks on the at, lifeless oor of a dry lake bed in Death Valley, California. Tracks
stretched away from the rocks the rocks had moved. What caused it? After some very frivolous suggestions, one student
said, OK, seriously! Now in a moderately cheerful mood, they began to suggest plausible explanations. When these were
collected for evaluation, the mood spontaneously became serious and the thinking analytical.
6. Concluding considerations
Teachers are widely expected to foster creative thinking in their students. This needs an understanding of creativity in
specic curriculum contexts, how it can be affected by moods and what they might do about it. But thought and mood are
not the only things which affect creativity; the quality of support, resources, sources of distraction and personalities also
matter. When dealing with moods, however, there may need to be a consideration of ethical matters. In particular, is it right
to manipulate moods? Probably few would argue against helping someone feel happy but most would pause at deliberately
making someone sad, particularly when that is a trusting child. Social rejection, for instance, can promote artistic creativity
but it is generally not an acceptable strategy in a classroom. However, Without attempting to alter the childs basic emotional
character, educators may select learning activities that maximise educational progress (Levinson, 1999: 206). Nevertheless,
a teacher needs to guard against inducing excessive emotional or lasting negative moods in learners. At the same time, we
should not assume that mental peculiarity is inimical to creative thought. Popularly, creativity tends to be associated with
mental disturbance, epitomized by the artist, Van Gogh. There seems to be some foundation for the belief. Studies in various
(Western) countries have found that those with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and depression are over-represented in
artistic occupations and creative activities. It is suggested that creativity gives these people a feeling of well-being (Galvez,
Thommi, & Ghaemi, 2009; Kyaga, Lichtenstein, Boman, Hultman, & Lngstrm, 2011). Children with bipolar disorder also
tend towards higher levels of creativity (Simeonova, Chang, Strong, & Ketter, 2005). Severe emotional disabilities, however,
are generally better left to experts.
If teachers are to mediate and manage task engagement with reference to emotions, it would help if they are able to
recognise emotions and moods but can they do it? Recognising emotions and moods in others is a part of what some call
emotional intelligence. Goleman (1995) is of the view that expert teachers have it. There is evidence that the ability to
recognise some emotions in facial expressions is at least partly innate but develops and widens with experience. There can
be very high levels of recognition of emotions such as anger, fear, sadness and surprise from facial expression and vocal tone
(Keltner, Ekman, Gonzago, & Beer, 2003; Scherer, Johnstone, & Klasmeyer, 2003). The ability varies, however, from person
to person and is generally better when observing those who are known well and from familiar cultures and socio-economic
groups (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2003; Elfenbein, Marsh, & Ambady, 2002). This suggests that teachers will have some ability to
recognise emotions in their students, particularly if they know them well. However, moods can be milder and more diffuse
than such emotions and could be more difcult to recognise. DMello, Taylor, Davidson, and Graesser (2008), in seeking to
make their electronic AutoTutor responsive to the users emotions, tested two accomplished teachers abilities to recognise
the emotions of such users and did not nd them to be reliable. These teachers, however, were only two in number, they were
not familiar with the users and no vocal clues to moods were reported. It was found subsequently that providing training
with Ekmans Facial Action Coding System (Ekman & Friesen, 2003) greatly improved emotional recognition (Graesser et al.,
2006). This suggests that teachers are likely to have some ability at emotion and mood recognition, particularly when working
with students they know. Even when the ability is not strong, it may be open to development through practice and training.
Given that moods and emotions are central to thinking and action and give them particular forms, they should not be
ignored by teachers attempting to foster thinking skills. Teachers need to consider their potential effects in their planning,
in the learning atmosphere they provide, and in how they negotiate the ow of affective experience. Rarely mentioned

42

D.P. Newton / Thinking Skills and Creativity 8 (2013) 3444

in teacher training, or only tied to class control, moods and emotions should be more evident in learning how to teach
and to foster creative thinking. At present, a pedagogical vocabulary for discussing mood/emotion/cognition interaction
is undeveloped and is needed. Studies which explore and test teaching strategies for the effective management of this
interaction are also needed. For those whose pre-service training is apprentice-like and entirely or largely school-based,
becoming aware of the need and acquiring the vocabulary, knowledge and skills could be difcult when practices do not
already exist in schools. In other words, this is another instance of the need for a kind of teacher training where introducing
new knowledge and practices in education is made possible, as it can be in research-informed training in a university.

References
Adler, P. S., & Obstfeld, D. (2007). The role of affect in creative projects and exploratory search. Industrial and Corporate Change, 16(1), 1950.
Akinola, M., & Mendes, W. B. (2008). The dark side of creativity: Biological vulnerability and negative emotions lead to greater artistic creativity. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(12), 16771686.
Al-Karasneh, S. M., & Saleh, A. M. (2010). Islamic perspectives of creativity. Procedia Social and Behavioural Sciences, 2, 412426.
Altshuller, G. S. (2000). The innovation algorithm: TRIZ, systematic innovation and technical creativity. Worcester, MA: Technical Innovation Center.
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context boulder. West View Press.
Astleitner, H. (2000). Designing emotionally sound instruction: The FEASP approach. Instructional Science, 28, 169198.
Augustine, A. A., & Hemenover, S. H. (2009). On the relative effectiveness of affect regulation strategies: A meta-analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 23(6),
277303.
Averil, J. R., Chon, K. K., & Hahn, D. W. (2001). Emotions and creativity, East and West. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 4, 165183.
Avey, J. B., Luthans, S. T., Sweetman, D., & Peterson, C. (2011). Impact of employees character strengths of wisdom on stress and creative performance.
Human Resource Management Journal, <http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j. 1748-8583.2010.00157.x/full> (accessed 19.03.12)
Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus?
Psychological Bulletin, 134(6), 779806.
Bettencourt, E. M., Gillett, M. H., Gall, M. D., & Hull, R. E. (1983). Effects of teacher enthusiasm training on students on-task behaviour and achievement.
American Educational Research Journal, 20(3), 435450.
Boden, M. A. (2004). The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms. London: Routledge.
Bowkett, S. (2007). 100+ ideas for teaching creativity. London: Continuum.
Carthy, T., Horesh, N., Apter, A., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Patterns of emotional reactivity and regulation in children with anxiety disorders. Journal of Pyschopathology & Behavioral Assessment, 32, 2336.
Carruthers, P. (2002). Human creativity. British Journal of the Philosophy of Science, 53, 225249.
Chon, K. K., Kim, K. H., & Ryoo, J. B. (2000). Experience and expression of anger in Korea America. Korean Journal of Rehabilitation Psychology, 7, 6175.
Clore, G., Gaspar, K., & Garvin, E. (2001). Affect as information. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 121141). Mahwah: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Craft, A. (1999). Creative development in the early years. The Curriculum Journal, 10, 135150.
Craft, A. (2002). Creativity and early years education. London: Continuum.
Craft, A. (2008). Nurturing creativity, wisdom, and trusteeship in education. In A. Craft, H. Gardner, & G. Claxton (Eds.), Creativity, wisdom and trusteeship
(pp. 115). Thousand Oaks: Corwin.
Cropley, A. J. (2001). Creativity in education and learning. London: RoutledgeFalmer.
Cropley, D. H., Cropley, A. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Runco, M. A. (2010). The dark side of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: HarperCollins.
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: Penguin.
Damasio, A. R. (2000). A second chance for emotion. In R. D. Lane, & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 1223). Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Davis, E. L., Levine, L. J., Lench, H. C., & Quas, J. A. (2010). Metacognitive emotion regulation: Childrens awareness that changing thoughts and goals can
alleviate negative emotions. Emotion, 10(4), 498510.
Davis, M. A. (2009). Understanding the relationship between mood and creativity: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
108, 2538.
de Bono, E. (1987). Letters to thinkers. Key Porter Books: New York.
de Bono, E. (1995). Serious creativity. The Journal of Quality and Participation, 18(5), 1218.
De Dreu, C. K. W., Baas, M., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation level in the moodcreativity link. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
94(5), 739756.
De Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., & Baas, M. (2011). Behavioral activation links to creativity because of increased cognitive exibility. Social Psychological &
Personality Science, 2, 7280.
Deutscher, M. (2011). Strong of reason. Parrhesia, 13, 7995.
DMello, S., Taylor, R., Davidson, K., & Graesser, A. (2008). Self versus teacher judgments of learner emotions during a tutoring session with AutoTutor.
Intelligent Tutoring Systems, Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 5091, 918.
DfES (Department for Education and Skills). (2003). Excellence and enjoyment. London: DfES.
Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (2000). Subjective emotional well-being. In M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 325337). New York:
Guilford.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (2003). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. Cambridge, MA: Malor.
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003). Universals and cultural differences in recognizing emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(5), 159164.
Elfenbein, H. A., Marsh, A. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). Emotional intelligence and the recognition of emotion from facial expressions. In L. F. Barrett, & P.
Salovey (Eds.), The wisdom of feelings: Processes underlying emotional intelligence (pp. 3759). New York: Guilford.
Fiedler, K. (1988). Emotion and mood, cognitive style and behavior regulation. In K. Fielder, & J. P. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition and social behavior (pp.
100119). Toronto: Hogrefe.
Fischer, K. W., Daniel, D. B., Immordino-Yang, M. H., Stern, E., Battro, A., & Koizumi, H. (2007). Why mind, brain, and education? Why now? Mind, Brain and
Education, 1(1), 12.
Forgas, J. P., & Vargas, P. T. (2000). The effects of mood on social judgment and reasoning. In M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions
(pp. 350367). New York: Guilford.
Fredrickson., B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218226.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 359,
13671377.
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43(5), 349358.

D.P. Newton / Thinking Skills and Creativity 8 (2013) 3444

43

Frijda, N. H. (2004). Emotions and action. In A. S. R. Manstead, N. Frijda, & A. Fischer (Eds.), Feelings and emotions (pp. 158173). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Galvez, J. F., Thommi, S., & Ghaemi, S. N. (2009). Positive aspects of mental illness: A review of bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 128, 185190.
Garner, R. (2007). Schools must do more for creativity. The Independent, 31(October), 12.
Gasper, K. (2004). Do you see what I see? Affect and visual information processing. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 405421.
George, J. M. (2007). Dual tuning in a supportive context. The Academy of Management Journal, 50(3), 605622.
George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2002). Understanding when bad moods foster creativity and good ones dont: The role of context and clarity of feelings. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 87(4), 687697.
Giedd, J. N. (2008). The teen brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42(4), 335343.
Gino, F., Agote, L., Miron-Spektor, E., & Todorova, G. (2009). First get your feet wet: The effects of learning from direct and indirect experience on team
creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 111, 102115.
Glck, J., Ernst, R., & Unger, F. (2002). How creatives dene creativity: Denitions reect different types of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 14(1),
5567.
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
Graesser, A., McDaniel, B., Chipman, P., Witherspoon, A., DMello, S., & Gholson, B. (2006). Detection of emotions during learning with AutoTutor. In R. Son
(Ed.), Proceedings of the 28th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 285290). Mahweh: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Grawitch, M. J., & Munz, D. C. (2005). Individual and group affect in problem-solving workgroups. In C. E. J. Hartel, W. J. Zerbe, & N. M. Ashkenasy (Eds.),
Emotions in organizational behavior (pp. 119188). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hahn, D-W., Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (1999). A factor analysis of the most frequently used Korean personality trait adjectives. European Journal of Personality,
13(4), 261282.
Hargreaves, A. (2000). Mixed emotions: Teachers perceptions of their interactions with students. Teaching & Teacher Education, 16, 811826.
Heyward, P. (2010). Emotional engagement through drama. International Journal of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education, 22(2), 197203.
Hirt, E. R., Devers, E. E., & McCrea, S. M. (2008). I want to be creative: Exploring the role of hedonic contingency theory in the positive mood-cognitive
exibility link. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 214230.
Hom, H. L., & Arbuckle, B. (1988). Mood induction effects upon goal setting and performance in young children. Motivation and Emotion, 12, 113122.
Hon, A. H. Y. (2012). Shaping environments conducive to creativity: The role of intrinsic motivation. Cornell Hospitality Quarterly, 53(1), 5364.
Hood, P. (2008). What do we teachers need to know to enhance our creativity? Education 3-13, 36(2), 139151.
Hullett, C. R. (2005). The impact of mood on persuasion: A meta-analysis. Communication Research, 32(4), 423442.
Hume, D. (1739/1978). Treatise of human nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hurson, T. (2008). Think better. New York: McGraw Hill.
Immordino-Yang, M. H. (2011). Me, my self and you: Neuropyschological relations between social emotions, self-awareness and morality. Emotion Review,
3(3), 313.
Immordino-Yang, M. H., & Damasio, A. (2007). We feel, therefore we learn. Mind, Brain and Education, 1(1), 310.
Isen, A. M. (1993). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 261277). New York: Guilford.
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 52(6),
11221131.
Isen, A. M., Johnson, M. M. S., Mertz, E., & Robinson, G. F. (1985). The inuence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations. Journal of Personality
& Social Psychology, 48(6), 14131426.
Johnson, S. K. (2008). I second that emotion: Effects of emotional contagion, and affect at work on leader and follower outcomes. The Leadership Quarterly,
19(1), 119.
Kant, E. (1785/2002). Critique of practical reason. Cambridge: Hackett.
Kaufman, G. (2003). Expand the moodcreativity equation. Creativity Research Journal, 15(23), 131135.
Kaufman, G., & Vosberg, S. K. (1997). Paradoxical mood effects on creative problem solving. Cognition and Emotion, 11(2), 151170.
Keltner, D., Ekman, P., Gonzago, G. C., & Beer, J. (2003). Facial expression of emotion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of
affective sciences (pp. 415432). Oxford University Press: Oxford.
Konstan, D. (2006). The emotions of the Ancient Greeks. Toronto: University of Toronto.
Kristjnsson, K. (2007). Aristotle, emotions and education. Aldershot: Ashgate.
Krper, M., Fay, D., Lindberg, T., & Meinel, C. (2011). Interrelations between motivation, creativity and emotions in design thinking processesAn empirical
study based on regulatory focus theory. In T. Taura, & Y. Nagai (Eds.), Design creativity 2010 (pp. 97104). London: Springer-Verlag.
Kyaga, S., Lichtenstein, P., Boman, M., Hultman, C., & Lngstrm, N. (2011). Creativity and mental disorder: A family study of 300 000 people with severe
mental disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 199, 373379.
Lahikainen, A. R., Kirmanen, T., Kraav, I., & Taimalu, M. (2003). Studying fears in young children. Childhood, 10, 83104.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotions and adaptation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
LeDoux, J. E. (1989). Cognitiveemotional interactions in the brain. Cognition and Emotion, 3(4), 267289.
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155184.
Lehrer, J. (2009). The decisive moment. Edinburgh: Canongate.
Levinson, M. (1999). The demands of a liberal education. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Mackintosh, B., & Mathews, A. (2003). Dont look now: Attentional avoidance of emotionally valenced cues. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 623646.
Madjar, N., & Oldham, G. R. (2002). Preliminary tasks and creative performance on a subsequent task. Creativity Research Journal, 14(2), 239251.
Martin, L., & Stoner, P. (1996). Mood as input. In L. Martin, & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling (pp. 279301). Hillsdale: LEA.
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1964). Signs. Evanston: North-western University Press.
Milne, I. (2007). Childrens science. Primary Science Review, 100, 3334.
Moseley, D., Baumeld, V., Elliott, J., Gregson, M., Miller, J., & Newton, D. P. (2005). Frameworks for thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Mumford, M. D. (2003). Where have we been, where are we going: Taking stock in creativity research. Creativity Research Journal, 15, 107120.
Nassif C. (2004). Investigating creativity with the MMPI-2. Doctoral thesis. Dakota: University of Dakota. Dissertation Abstracts International, 65(04B),
21362280.
Newton, D. P. (1990). Making science education relevant. London: Kogan Page.
Newton, D. P. (2010). Assessing the creativity of scientic explanations in elementary science. Research in Science and technological Education, 28(3), 187201.
Newton, D. P. (2012). Teaching for understanding. London: Routledge.
Newton, D. P., & Donkin, H. (2011). Some notions of artistic creativity amongst history of art students acquired through incidental learning. International
Journal of Education through Art, 7(3), 283298.
Newton, D. P., & Newton, L. D. (2009). Some student teachers conceptions of creativity in school science. Research in Science & Technological Education,
27(1), 4560.
Newton, D. P., & Newton, L. D. (2010). What teachers see as creative incidents in elementary science lessons. International Journal of Science Education,
32(15), 19892005.
Newton, L. D. (2012). Creativity for a new curriculum: 5-11. London: Routledge.
Niu, W. (2009). Development of creativity research in Chinese societies. In J. C. Kaufman, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The international handbook of creativity (pp.
374394). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding emotions. Oxford: Blackwell.

44

D.P. Newton / Thinking Skills and Creativity 8 (2013) 3444

OQuin, K., & Besemer, S. P. (1989). The development, reliability and validity of the Revised Creative Product Semantic Scale. Creativity Research Journal, 2,
268279.
Osborne, T. (2003). Against creativity: A philistine rant. Economy and Society, 32(4), 507525.
Park, S., Soo-Young, L., Oliver, S., & Cramond, B. (2006). Changes in Korean science teachers perceptions of creativity and science teaching after participating
in an overseas professional development program. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 17(1), 3764.
Parkinson, B., & Totterdell, P. (1999). Classifying affect-regulation strategies. Cognition and Emotion, 13(3), 277303.
Payne, M. A. (2010). Use-it-or-lose-it? Interrogating an educational message from the teen brain research. Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 35(5).
Article 6, http://ro.ecu.au/ajte/vol35/iss5/6 (accessed 19.03.12)
Perlovsky, L. I., & Levine, S. D. (2010). Drive for creativity. In The 2010 International Joint Conference on Neural Networks 1823 July, Barcelona.
Phelps, E. A. (2006). Emotion and cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 2753.
Pink, D. H. (2005). A whole new mind. New York: Penguin.
Plutchik, R. (1980). A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In R. Plutchik, & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory & research: Theories of emotion
(pp. 333). New York: Academic Press.
QCA (Qualications and Curriculum Authority). (2005). Creativity: Find it, promote it!. London: QCA.
Rader, N., & Hughes, E. (2005). The inuence of affective state on the performance of a block design task in 6 and 7-year-old children. Cognition and Emotion,
19(1), 143150.
Richards, H. J., & Hadwin, J. A. (2011). An exploration of the relationship between trait anxiety and school attendance in young people. School Mental Health,
3(4), 236244.
Ritchie, K. C., Shore, B. M., LaBanca, F., & Newman, A. J. (2011). The impact of emotions on divergent thinking processes. Mind, Brain and Education, 5(1),
211225.
Ragozinno, M. E., Km, J., Hassert, D., Minniti, N., & Kiang, C. (2003). The contribution of the rat prelimbicinfralimbic areas to different forms of task switching.
Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 10541065.
Rook, L., & van Knippenberg, D. (2011). Creativity and imitation: Effects of regulatory focus and creative exemplar quality. Creativity Research Journal, 23(4),
346356.
Rosenwein, B. H. (2010). Thinking historically and medieval emotions. History Compass, 8(8), 828842.
Ross, M. (1975). Whats wrong with school music. British Journal of Music Education, 12, 185201.
Roy, S. (2007). Creativity, complexity and physics education. In Proceedings of the 2007 complexity science and educational research conference Vancouver,
BC, February 1820, (pp. 217224).
Russ, S. W. (1996). Development of creative processes in children. New Directions for Child Development, 72, 3142.
Russ, S., & Grossman-McKee, A. (1990). Affective expression in childrens fantasy play. Journal of Personality Assessment, 54, 756771.
Scherer, K. R., Johnstone, T., & Klasmeyer, G. (2003). Vocal expression of emotion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of
affective sciences (pp. 433456). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Schnall, S., & Laird, J. (2003). Keep smiling. Cognition and Emotion, 17(5), 787797.
Schwartz, N. (2001). Feelings as information. In L. Martin, & G. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition (pp. 159176). Mahweh: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Schwartz, N., & Clore, C. L. (2003). Mood, misattribution and judgments of well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 513523.
Sercombe, H. (2010). The gift and the trap: Working the teen brain into our concept of youth. Journal of Adolescent Research, 25(1), 3147.
Shaheen, R. (2010). Creativity and education. Creative Education, 1(3), 166169.
Sharp, C., & Le Mtais. (2000). The arts, creativity and cultural education: An international perspective. London: Qualications and Curriculum Authority.
Siegesmund, R. (1998). Why do we teach art today? Studies in Art Education, 39(3), 197214.
Simeonova, D. I., Chang, K. D., Strong, C., & Ketter, T. A. (2005). Creativity in familial bipolar disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 39, 623631.
Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(4), 571581.
Sofowara, O. A. (2007). The use of educational cartoons and comics in enhancing creativity in primary school pupils in Ile-ife, Osun State, Nigeria. Journal
of Applied Sciences Research, 3, 913920.
Sokolon, M. K. (2006). Political emotions: Aristotle and the symphony of reason and emotion. DeKalb: Northern Illinois University Press.
Spering, M., Wagener, D., & Funke, J. (2005). The role of emotions in complex problem-solving. Cognition and Emotion, 19(8), 12521261.
Sternberg, R. (1985). Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity and wisdom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 607627.
Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The nature of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 18(1), 8798.
Sutton, R. E., & Wheatley, K. F. (2003). Teachers emotions and teaching: A review of the literature and directions for future research. Educational Psychology
Review, 15(4), 327358.
Tan, A.-G. (2000). A review of the study of creativity in Singapore. Journal of Creative Behavior, 34(4), 259284.
Thompson, T., & Perry, Z. (2005). Is the poor performance of self-worth protective students linked with social comparison goals? Educational Psychology,
25(5), 471490.
UNESCO (United Nations Educational, Scientic and Cultural Organization). (2006). Road maps for arts education. Lisbon: UNESCO.
Vandekerckhove, M., & Panskepp, J. (2009). The ow of anoetic to noetic and autonoetic consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(4), 10181028.
van Goethem, A., & Sloboda, J. (2011). The functions of music for affect. Musicae Scienticiae, 15(2), 208228.
Vosberg, S. K. (1998). Mood and the quantity and quality of ideas. Creativity Research Journal, 11(4), 315324.
Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. Guilford: New York.
Willis, J. (2007). The neuroscience of joyful education. Educational Leadership, 64, 15.
Worthington, M., & Carruthers, E. (2003). Childrens mathematics: Making marks, making meaning. London: Paul Chapman.

You might also like