You are on page 1of 31

Journal of Management Studies 48:3 May 2011

doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6486.2010.00983.x

JMS
Research in Cognition and Strategy: Reflections on
Two Decades of Progress and a Look to the Future

joms_983

665..695

Sarah Kaplan
University of Toronto
abstract This review of cognition in strategic management research takes as its starting
point the appreciation of the seminal paper, Competitive groups as cognitive communities:
the case of Scottish knitwear manufacturers, by Porac, Thomas and Baden-Fuller on cognitive
categorization of competition, published in the Journal of Management Studies only 20 years ago.
In this paper, I reflect on the context in which their paper emerged, the impact it has had, and
the future paths that research on cognition in strategy might take. In doing so, I highlight the
challenges associated with establishing cognition as a legitimate factor in strategic management
(alongside the traditional explanations of capabilities and incentives) and of showing the causal
relationship between cognition and strategic outcomes. Subsequent work in cognition explored
the dynamic relationship between cognition, capabilities, and incentives, and, in process
models of framing, linked cognition with political action. Rather than managerial cognition
becoming its own independent field, cognitive concepts have diffused throughout work in
many different managerial fields, leading to a proliferation of terms, concepts, and approaches.
I conclude by exploring some of the paths that research in cognition and strategy is taking in
the present day particularly those involving studies of the construction of markets and
categories, each of which are themes that the work by Porac, Thomas and Baden-Fuller
brought to our attention.

INTRODUCTION
Just over 20 years ago, Joe Porac, Howard Thomas and Charles Baden-Fuller published
their article on Competitive groups as cognitive communities: the case of Scottish
knitwear manufacturers (Porac et al., 1989) in a special issue of the Journal of Management
Studies focused on Managerial Thinking in Business Environments. This paper proved
to be an important contributor to the movement towards acknowledging the role of
managerial cognition in shaping strategic outcomes and other organizational processes.
The authors showed in their analysis of Scottish knitwear firms that rivalry was a
cognitive construction based on the set of firms identified by managers as competitors.
Address for reprints: Sarah Kaplan, University of Toronto, Rotman School, 105 St. George Street, Room 537,
Toronto, ON, M5S 3E6, Canada (sarah.kaplan@rotman.utoronto.ca).
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and Society for the Advancement of Management
Studies. Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford, OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street,
Malden, MA 02148, USA.

666

S. Kaplan

Their work broke with the strategic management field by suggesting that categorization
processes played an important role in competition and that the environments in which
firms operated were endogenous to the interpretations and actions of the managers in
firms themselves.
In this paper, I analyse the impact of their publication on the field of strategic
management and explore some paths that research in this arena has taken since the
paper was published. Note that this article is accompanied in this issue of JMS by a piece
by Porac et al. (2011), who reflect on the conversations in which their work has been
engaged and provide a nice complement to the analysis I provide here. For my part,
I highlight the challenges in establishing the legitimacy of cognitive explanations
both in creating valid measures and in showing the power of these theories alongside the
traditional explanations in strategy associated with capabilities and incentives. In discussing the subsequent extensions of their ideas over the past 20 years, I show that the
most powerful works sought to integrate cognition with these other explanations into
contingent models of strategic management. Throughout the discussion I interrogate the
methods used in cognitive research on strategy, showing what kinds of insights can be
achieved using various methodological approaches. I close with some thoughts about
promising future paths of research that build on the primary insights of Porac, Thomas
and Baden-Fuller, focusing specifically on the resurgence of interest in categorization
and the emergence of new fields.
COGNITIVE COMMUNITIES IN SCOTTISH KNITWEAR PRODUCTION
This section explores the Porac, Thomas and Baden-Fuller paper in context. I identify
the broad trends in which it fell in order to situate its impact subsequent to publication.
I also highlight not only the stream of papers in which the Porac et al. paper moved but
also its contribution to the broader development of the field through, for example, the
creation of an interest group in the Academy of Management and book series on
the topic. An examination of the numerous citations to the paper shows that its reach has
been very broad to the point that it has been transformed into a pro forma citation in the
area of managerial cognition.
Antecedents to Porac et al. and the JMS Special Issue on
Managerial Thinking
The Porac et al. paper is most usefully seen in the context of several different movements
taking place in management research in general and strategy scholarship more specifically. As Porac et al. noted in their introduction, the early 1980s had seen an increase in
interest in the interpretive side of organizations (Barley, 1983, 1986; Bartunek, 1984;
Daft and Weick, 1984; Kiesler and Sproull, 1982; Sims and Gioia, 1984; Weick, 1979),
which was beginning to be incorporated into questions of strategic management (Dutton
and Jackson, 1987; Huff, 1982; Prahalad and Bettis, 1986; Smircich and Stubbart, 1985;
Spender, 1980, 1989; Walsh and Fahey, 1986). This movement had followed on the
heels of a groundswell of interest in cognition in the field of social psychology (see Fiske
and Taylor, 1984, for a review) as well as an important development in the social studies
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

667

of technology to see technologies and environments as constructed through actors


interactions in the marketplace (Bijker et al., 1987). These two trends pointed to the role
of interpretation at the individual and field levels, so it seems only natural that studies of
strategy would take these ideas on board at the firm level of analysis.[1]
The emerging research on cognition in organizations made two related moves.
The first was to address the question of organizational response to their environments,
which had been dominant in the discourse of organizations as least since Lawrence and
Lorschs (1967) classic. The cognitive perspective suggested that the environment is not
purely exogenous and therefore organizational response to the environment is mediated
by the interpretations made of that environment by managers. Cognitive researchers
pointed out that this is particularly true because of the uncertainties and complexities of
the environment that decision-makers faced. Some argued, drawing on the work of
Simon and March (March and Simon, 1958; Simon, 1947), that the need for interpretation comes from the cognitive limits of managers who therefore use simplifying heuristics to make sense of the world. Others suggested that the environment is fraught with
Knightian (Knight, 1921/1965) uncertainties, that could not be resolved, even with
more analytical capacity, because the range of outcomes is unknown and probabilities
could not be assigned to them. In either case, the effect was to endogenize the environment by suggesting that cognitive frames are the means by which managers could
make sense of the environment, and such sensemaking shapes strategic choice and action
(Daft and Weick, 1984).
The second move was to bring the focus back to the actions of managers after the late
1970s when theories that privileged position and situation such as population ecology
(Hannan and Freeman, 1977), transaction cost economics (Williamson, 1975), and
resource dependence (Pfeffer and Salancik, 1978) were introduced. The cognitive
approach suggested that structural features do not determine outcomes but rather that
organizations only act through the choices and actions of the managers within them.
These two moves were tied together in a reaction to the rational actor model of utility
maximization in explaining strategic choice and action. As Charles Stubbart (1989,
p. 326) argued in his piece in the same JMS special issue, In economics, axioms provide
scant comfort to managers who are uncertain about their preferences, unsure about
their economic alternatives, and baffled as to which strategy represents the marginal
utility-maximizing choice.
Reflecting this increasing interest in cognition, a group of scholars organized a
conference at Boston University in 1987 on managerial thinking, and it is from these
contributions that Porac and Thomas, as editors of the special issue of JMS, drew
representative papers. Stubbart (1989) provided an overview of cognitive theories, suggesting that managerial interpretation is implicit in any strategic management theory
that is based in analysis of internal or external factors. The remaining papers were
empirical and descriptive, examining the differences in competitors perceptions following environmental jolts (Gronhaug and Falkenberg, 1989), the changing cognitive maps
of managers in one firm over time (Fahey and Narayanan, 1989), dimensions managers
use to sort issues (Dutton et al., 1989), and the mental maps of managers regarding
diversification opportunities (Ginsberg, 1989), in addition to the analysis of cognitive
strategic groups within an industry by Porac, Thomas and Baden-Fuller.
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

668

S. Kaplan

The Porac et al. paper mobilized these emerging ideas about organizational cognition
to address a challenge in the strategic management literature posed by asymmetries
within industries. There was an emerging critique (driven in part by Howard Thomas) of
the tendency (escalated by the publication of Michael Porters 1980 book on competitive
strategy and industry analysis) to focus strategic analysis at the industry level. Thomas
and others suggested that such a high-level grouping omitted asymmetries within industries that could explain outcomes, and they defined strategic groups as an intermediate
structure between firms and industries (McGee and Thomas, 1986). The goal was to
explain strategic behaviour better by focusing on intra-industry structures. Much of the
subsequent research was aimed at developing better analytical techniques for identifying
these groups within industries. Porac, Thomas and Baden-Fuller (1989, p. 413) turned
away from this task, stating that such definitions were analytical abstractions. Instead,
they based their theorizing and analysis on the idea that managers categorizations of
their competition enacted the strategic grouping. Rather than being purely material, the
groups were constructed of both material and cognitive dynamics.
Contributions of Porac et al. (1989)
To develop these ideas, the authors drew on data from their examination of Scottish
knitwear manufacturers. They identified 17 such firms, which appeared from the outside
to be following similar focused strategies Porters (1980) term by selling premium
quality, expensive garments through specialist distribution channels to a limited number
of high income consumers (Porac et al., 1989, p. 404). No other firms outside of this
predetermined set were examined. Of these 17 firms, the authors interviewed managers
from 35 per cent (six firms) and complemented this primary data collection with archival
information about the types of products, production processes, sales volume, location,
and beliefs of each.
Using methods to elicit cognitive taxonomies[2] of the industry, they found that the
managers they interviewed had a hierarchical categorization of their competitive space,
for example, starting broadly with textiles, then narrowing to knitwear (vs. hosiery
and lace), then full-fashioned classic (vs. fashion which had its own subsets of fully
fashioned and cut and sew), and finally high quality (vs. the rest). They found that
most managers thought only of other Scottish knitwear manufacturers as their competition. Firms from other parts of the UK and other countries, even if they produced
fully-fashioned knitwear at similar price points, were seen as being in different businesses
or only somewhat competitors (Porac et al., 1989, pp. 4078). The authors concluded
that the set of beliefs about which firms are capable of producing a certain kind of sweater
defined the strategic group because managers in these manufacturers only saw these
related firms as potentially posing competitive threats.
They further argued that the definition of the competitive group was self-reinforcing
because the managers drew on data from a fairly limited set of sources. These managers
tended not to do systematic market research but instead consulted their existing agents,
design consultants, and the shop owners who sold their products. As a result, their own
network acts as an information filter by narrowing the range of market data to that
which is of immediate concern to the core participants (Porac et al., 1989, p. 409). Porac
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

669

et al. further argued that not only do transactional networks filter the information that
manufacturers receive, but also beliefs about the various network elements shape strategic choices. They outlined a set of causal beliefs about the customer (e.g. the top 5 per
cent of wage earners are unaffected by economic trends), competitors (e.g. Scottish
companies are not good a fashion design), suppliers (Scottish spinners are the finest in
the world), and retailers (minimum order quantity is one) that influenced a general
strategy pursued by the firms in this competitive group.
These causal beliefs similar to Spenders (1980, 1989) notion of industry recipe or the
concept of industry macrocultures developed slightly later by Abrahamson and Fombrun
(1994) are associated with a set of actions such as purchasing from a certain set of
suppliers, using a specific type of equipment, working with certain designers, distributing
through agents, selling at flexible quantities, and targeting a narrow set of consumers.
The pattern of transactions in the market was therefore, they claimed, stabilized by the
interplay between mental models and strategic choices. However, because Porac et al.s
data is essentially cross-sectional, such causal inferences can only be seen as suggestive.
The most important idea that emerged from their analysis involved the connection
between competitive groups and individual firm strategic behaviour. They suggest that
the link between group-level and firm-level dynamics is the shared mental model of
strategy makers. More importantly, they argued that such mental models are the basis of
coordinated behaviour in some competitive settings. Cognitive oligopolies exist because
competitors define each other as such. Managers do this because of their cognitive limits,
which create the need for a definition of the competitive set that identifies a small enough
set of firms to be monitored. What is powerful about this argument is that it reverses
the causality proposed by economists, suggesting that interfirm monitoring and
co-ordination create rather than result from oligopolistic situations (Porac et al., 1989,
p. 413). Their model endogenizes rivalry rather than treating it as an immutable external
force. More generally, the paper should be seen as a contribution to a growing literature
that placed cognitive dynamics at the centre of strategy formulation and strategic
outcomes.
Subsequent Impact of Porac et al.
The management field was slow to pick up on the potential of the ideas proposed in
the article. As demonstrated in Figure 1, for the first five or six years, Porac et al. did not
draw much attention (as was the case with all of the papers from the special issue).
However, in the mid-1990s, citations to this work grew quite substantially, achieving 226
in the ISI citation index by the end of 2009 (and emerging as the third most cited paper
published by JMS).[3] While all of the other papers in the special issue also received an
important increase in citations in 199495, attention subsequently waned for the rest and
no other article from that issue attracted as many citations.
Not surprisingly, an examination of the pattern of papers that have cited the work by
Porac et al. shows that it has been picked up mostly in journals related to strategic
management (see Figure 2). On the other hand, general management and organizational
theory journals as well as journals specializing in particular domains such as sports
management and health care are also represented. We also see substantial representation
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

670

S. Kaplan

Figure 1. Trends in citations to Porac et al.


Source: ISI Web of Knowledge.

Figure 2. Distribution of journals in which works that cite Porac et al. have been published
Source: ISI Web of Knowledge.
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

671

of journals from both sides of the Atlantic. Conceivably, because this paper
is co-authored by top scholars based in both the USA (Porac and Thomas) and the
UK (Baden-Fuller), the article has been noticed by authors in a range of geographic
locations.
Interestingly, their most powerful insight concerning cognitive oligopolies seems not to
have been addressed as prominently. Karl Weick (Weick, 1995; Weick and Roberts,
1993) drew on the idea in some of his works on sensemaking, but otherwise it has
appeared in only a few other publications, and often outside of the field of management
(e.g. marketing or engineering).[4] Indeed, as shown in Table I, only one third of the
papers that cite Porac et al. are even focused on the broader topics of strategic groups or
rivalry. Most of these papers were published in the mid- to late-1990s. More recently, the
paper seems to be cited in a generic way, as a study that looked at cognitions connection
to strategy (including strategy formulation) or even more broadly at cognition in organizations. Smaller groups of citations included in the other category are made by papers
focusing on topics such as legitimacy and organizational identity. Thus, while Porac,
Thomas and Baden-Fullers paper has indeed been well cited, over time the citations
seem to have taken on the character of pro forma recognitions of a foundational paper in
the literature on cognition and organizations rather than specific links to ideas in their

Table I. Themes in citations to Porac et al.


Cognition and
strategic groups
1990
1991
1992
1993
1994
1995
1996
1997
1998
1999
2000
2001
2002
2003
2004
2005
2006
2007
2008
2009
Total

Strategic groups
(general)

Rivalry

2
3
2
2
1
3
1
3
3
1
1
1
1
2
1
28

Cognition and
strategy

Cognition
(general)

Agglomeration/
clusters/networks

2
1
3
1
2
2
2
1
2

2
3
4
1
2
3
2
1
1

2
1
2
4
24

1
2
1
3
27

2
1
2
5
1
2
5
4
2
4
4
3
1
6
4
47

1
2
1
1
1
1
4
1
2
3
1
4
2
4
1
7
11
47

Other

Total

3
0
2
8
7
8
7
14
9
12
14
18
14
13
14
16
14
14
16
23
226

1
1
1

3
1
2
4
2
1
2
17

3
1
1
3
5
4
1
3
6
3
1
2
36

Source: ISI Web of Knowledge. Coding by the author based on titles and abstracts of 226 papers citing Porac et al. to the
end of 2009.
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

672

S. Kaplan

work. One exception to this conclusion is an emerging trend in the 2000s to cite Porac
et al. in studies of business agglomeration, clusters and networks that are not related to
cognition per se. This suggests a re-reading of the paper in light of new research agendas
in strategic management.

LOOKING BACK AT 20 YEARS OF RESEARCH ON COGNITION


AND STRATEGY
The special issue in JMS on managerial thinking was part of a flurry of activity related
to cognition in the late 1980s and early 1990s. In 1989, Marlene Fiol and Bill Starbuck
founded the Managerial and Organizational Cognition (MOC) Interest Group within
the Academy of Management and its membership grew rapidly in the early years,
achieving about 800 members by 1994 (and becoming a full division of the Academy in
1999).[5] Anne Huff published her foundational edited volume, Mapping Strategic Thought
(Huff, 1990), which included both a series of empirical studies as well as several chapters
highlighting specific methods for eliciting and measuring cognition. The JAI Press
launched a series on Advances in Managerial Cognition and Organizational Information Processing
in 1994, which produced six volumes yearly through 1999, edited by many of the people
whose work appeared in the 1989 JMS special issue.
This burst of interest, particularly in the field of strategic management, led to rapid
growth in the number of management articles using cognition as a key construct,
moving from an average of 6 per year in the late 1980s, to 17 per year in the early
1990s, and 29 per year in the late 1990s (see Figure 3 for a count of the number of
articles in a selected list of management journals). However, at this point, growth
appears to have stalemated, both in MOC membership as well as in publications
focused on cognition.
In the remainder of this section, I will evaluate the progress made in the study of
cognition in strategic management and, in doing so, suggest some explanations for these
trends. Rather than provide a comprehensive review of the literature, I will explore a few
exemplars of different kinds of empirical research on cognition in strategy to illustrate key
moves scholars have made, important progress to date, and future directions for research
(see Table II for a classification).[6] In order to avoid engaging in any pro forma citing
behaviour myself, I will go into some detail on the papers I cite, describing their empirical
contexts, methods, and findings.
In exploring what each of these papers do, I demonstrate how progress
has been achieved in the field. I find that the field emerged and solidified through
a process of establishing a proof of concept, in other words, creating and validating
measures of cognition that could be shown to be distinctive from other constructs.
Later, research moved to testing the accuracy of managers cognition using these validated constructs and, more importantly, showing how cognition could matter for
strategic outcomes. In the more recent history of the field, scholars have now moved
away from looking at cognition in isolation and towards an examination of how cognition interacts with other organizational factors in dynamic models of organizational
action.
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Example papers:
empirical and
simulations

Looking at organizational dynamics


Cognition, capabilities and
incentives, case studies:
Dougherty (1992)
Garud and Rappa (1994)
Tripsas and Gavetti (2000)
Laamanen and Wallin (2009)
Cognition, capabilities and
incentives, large samples:
Kaplan (2008b)
Eggers and Kaplan (2009)
Internal cognitive dynamics in
organizations (case studies):
Gioia and Chittipeddi (1991)
Markoczy (2001)
Szulanski et al. (2004)
Rouleau (2005)
Kaplan (2008a)

Establishing the link with outcomes


Survey:
Thomas et al. (1993)
Ginsberg and Venkatraman
(1995)
Plambeck and Weber (2009)
Archival data (annual reports),
case studies:
Huff et al. (1990)
Barr et al. (1992)
Barr (1998)
Archival data (annual reports),
larger samples:
Osborne et al. (2001)
Kaplan et al. (2003)
Cho and Hambrick (2006)

Assessing accuracy
Survey:
Sutcliffe (1994)
Baum and Lant (2003)
Simulation:
Gavetti and Levinthal (2000)

* Only selected empirical and simulation papers included here (not theory or review pieces).

Huff (1990)
Milliken (1990)
Barr et al. (1992)
Reger and Huff (1993)
Porac and Thomas (1994)
Porac et al. (1995)
Lant and Baum (1995)
Reger and Palmer (1996)
Peteraf and Shanley (1997)
Markoczy (1997)

Establishing proof of concept

Phases

Table II. Phases in research on cognition and strategy after 1989 (selected papers)*

Cognition and Strategy


673

2011 The Author


Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

674

S. Kaplan

Figure 3. Articles appearing in selected management journals with the term cognition mentioned
(total = 586)
Note: Papers with the keyword cognit* in the following journals: Academy of Management Journal, Academy of
Management Review, Administrative Science Quarterly, Strategic Management Journal, Organization Science, Management
Science, Journal of Management Studies, Organization Studies, Human Relations, Advances in Strategic Management.
Source: ISI Web of Knowledge.

Proof of Concept
The first phase of work in cognition and strategy aimed to create legitimacy for the line
of inquiry and to establish that shared mental models existed. In other words, they sought
to generate a proof of concept that would demonstrate the usefulness of the domain for
research into strategy and organizations. Following the JMS special issue on managerial
thinking and other related work in the 1980s, scholars fleshed out methods for representing cognitive frames and demonstrated their presence in multiple empirical settings.
Much of the research in this period examined the content of these frames. Reger and
Huff (1993) employed the repertory grid technique (which uses interviews to elicit
informants perspectives on similarities between various entities) to generate perceived
groupings of banks in the Chicago area. They found evidence for three different cognitive strategic groups that were widely recognized by actors in the industry and that were
also associated with performance differences across groups. Porac and Thomas followed
up the Scottish knitwear study with an examination of retailers in the Midwest of the
United States (Porac and Thomas, 1994). They showed that managers based their
categorization of competitors on a hierarchical understanding of the product offerings
(from the general category of retail, to food retailer to grocery stores). Porac and Thomas
(along with Fiona Wilson, Douglas Paton, and Alaina Kanfer) also revisited the Scottish
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

675

knitwear setting in a more systematic study of a larger number of competitors (including


interviews at 20 different firms and 89 questionnaires) (Porac et al., 1995). In it, they
established, as they did in their JMS paper, that rivalry occurs as managers in firms
define other firms as rivals (a socially constructed notion of rivalry). They also showed
that an industry recipe of the attributes for categorizing competition developed over
time.
Beyond demonstrating the presence of cognitive understandings of rivalry, several
studies examined the degree to which these cognitive frames substantively reflected
rivalry. For example, Porac et al. (1995) showed that the identification of rivals was often
asymmetric primarily due to the high visibility of larger firms (where small firms were
more likely to designate large firms as rivals than the reverse). Lant and Baum (1995)
used interviews with managers of Manhattan hotels in a competitor group mapping
exercise to show that size also mattered for classification. Managers of larger hotels
categorized competitors over a wider range of prices than did those of small hotels
(perhaps because large hotels can offer a wider range of room rates). Peteraf and Shanley
(1997) built on these findings to suggest that only some cognitive strategic groups
mattered. Those with strong identities were ones that received enough managerial focus
to affect organizational action, while groups with weak identities did not function in any
meaningful way. Reger and Palmer (1996) suggested that managers categorizations
evolved at different rates in dynamic industries, depending on the type of cognitive
processing (automatic or controlled) in which they engaged.
Other studies began to expand the question of interpretations of rivalry to cognition
about other aspects of the environment. Milliken (1990) used surveys of managers in
liberal arts colleges to examine their perceptions of an important environmental change,
and concluded that managers assessments of organizational characteristics affected their
perception of uncertainty in the environment. Fiol (1990) used an analysis of letters to
shareholders of ten chemical companies to show that their perceptions of the external
constraints on their actions varied remarkably despite being in similar market conditions.
One crucial aspect of these studies was that they created and validated direct measures
of cognition rather than using proxies such as those based in managers demographic
characteristics. Much work on top management teams had used demographic measures
to capture cognitive constructs such as receptivity to change or the willingness to
take risks (Finkelstein and Hambrick, 1990; Hambrick and Mason, 1984; Norburn and
Birley, 1988; Wiersema and Bantel, 1992). Research in managerial cognition began to
suggest that demographics were but weak approximations of cognition, and therefore
more direct measures needed to be used (Barr et al., 1992; Markoczy, 1997). The
view was that demographics are important indicators of sources of cognition they
can reasonably be seen to represent managers knowledge accumulations from prior
experiences but they cannot, by definition, capture context-specific interpretations.
The mid-1990s marked a turning point in the managerial cognition field. Among
other punctuation points, James Meindl, Charles Stubbart, and Joe Porac edited a
special issue on managerial cognition in Organization Science (see their introduction,
Meindl et al., 1994); the MOC division hosted a discussion (between Anne Huff, Joe
Porac, and Jim Walsh) on the future of Managerial and Organizational Cognition at the
Academy of Management Annual Meeting in 1994; Jim Walsh published his exhaustive
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

676

S. Kaplan

review of the field, also in Organization Science (Walsh, 1995); and Gerard Hodgkinson
(1997) reviewed the cognitive strategic groups literature for Human Relations.
In the main, these interventions came down in the same place. They all acknowledged
the tremendous progress in describing different aspects of cognition and in identifying
various methods for eliciting cognitive frames. Having done so, they first advocated for
a refinement of the constructs and methods to study cognition. Meindl et al. (1994)
pushed for a narrowing to the most promising concepts and methodologies. Hodgkinson
(1997) promoted a move away from small-scale exploratory investigations towards
larger-scale theory testing approaches. The MOC panel discussants[7] noted that there
was increasing tolerance between those who advocated more focus on qualitative
approaches that could add richness and those who wanted the field to move more in the
direction of quantitative studies. Walsh (1995) specifically encouraged an assessment of
the accuracy of cognitive frames against the environments they represent. The challenge
for this, he noted, was in locating the objective reality in what many believed was a
socially constructed world. As I discuss in the next section, scholars have subsequently
addressed this challenge in a few different ways.
However, these scholars ultimately were less interested in accuracy than in the effect
of cognitive frames on outcomes. Walsh quoted Weick (1990, pp. 687): Accuracy is
nice, but not necessary, and the reason is that organizations generate action which
creates its own substitutes for accuracy and learning . . . the truth of the map lies in the
action and in the conditions of use. To pursue this line of thinking, Walsh recommended
a moratorium on research whose only aim is to describe the content of a particular
knowledge structure (p. 303). The more than 25 studies that he identified (a few of which
are described above) went a long way to legitimizing the pursuit of studies in managerial
cognition, but he (as well as Hodgkinson, Meindl, the MOC panel, and others) urged
scholars to take up where these studies had left off and begin to examine the effects of
cognitive frames on outcomes. As Meindl et al. (1994, p. 293) prognosticated, In the
future, the most important studies will clearly show linkages between cognition, behavior, and organizational outcomes.
Walsh went further to say that the ultimate objective was to understand the process
explanations that undergird these effects (p. 306). He noted however that these dynamic
analyses could only happen after adequate study of the accuracy of cognitive frames and
the correlations between cognition and action. It is therefore to these next phases of
managerial cognition research that I turn in the sections that follow.
Assessing the Accuracy of Managers Cognitive Frames
Research appears to have responded to the agenda articulated by Walsh (1995) and
others. One stream of research built from the notion of bounded rationality (March and
Simon, 1958), acknowledging that cognitive frames existed but seeking to understand
how accurately managers views represented the underlying competitive landscape.
From this standpoint, any inaccuracies were due to the kinds of biases in perception
that have been identified by cognitive psychologists (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979;
Kahneman et al., 1994). A theory paper by Zajac and Bazerman (1991) suggested that
blind spots (inaccuracies) in perception could lead to problematic outcomes for firms
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

677

and industries. Others followed with both empirical studies (using surveys) and analytical
models to examine the sources and effects of such inaccuracies.
A main empirical method of studying accuracy was the use of survey data. In early
work, Sutcliffe (1994) used surveys to identify the degree to which managers accurately
perceived environmental instability and munificence, comparing perceptions to standardized measures of these factors from industry segment financial data. She also
identified top team characteristics such as work history diversity, team tenure, centralization, and scanning behaviour that were associated with inaccuracy of perceptions.
Later, Baum and Lant (2003) compared Manhattan hotel managers categorization of
their competitors (also from survey data) with an analytical model of rivalry based on
material factors in the industry (size, location, and price). They solved the problem of
identifying the information environment that cognitive frames are meant to represent by
using what they call the objectiveempirical basis of competition derived from detailed
analyses of rivalry in the industry (Baum and Mezias, 1992). They did find that the
managers maps of competition were closely associated with the important material
factors shaping competition, though managers appeared to underestimate the importance of size similarity and overestimate the importance of location similarity. As a result,
they found that managers made both Type I errors (categorized some hotels as competitors that were not predicted by their analytical model of similarity) and Type II errors
(failed to categorize a competitor that the model predicted would be a rival). These errors
were attributed to experience (the least and most experienced managers made the most
errors), and this was compounded for the least experienced managers by information
overload and the organizations deviations from performance expectations.
Some simulation models were also developed to address problems of accuracy. Gavetti
and Levinthal (2000) used computer simulations to model cognitive frames as lowdimensional representations of more complex landscapes. They solved the problem of
identifying the underlying environment by generating its structure themselves in their
computer simulation. They described cognition as a forward-looking process based on an
actors beliefs about the links between choices and actions. The assumption was that the
more accurate the cognitive frame (or mental model) from which these beliefs were
derived, the better the outcomes. In their model, the actors beliefs were not fully accurate,
and therefore different cognitive frames were better or worse at driving the actor to a
performance peak in the landscape. They first showed that actors experiential learning
influenced changes in the cognitive frames and that these changes affected further choices
and actions to respond to their understanding of the landscape. While they based their
construction of the cognitive frames on an objectively known landscape (much as Baum
and Lant started from the basis of a known set of dimensions predicting rivalry), what is
most interesting about their analysis is the demonstration through computer simulations
of the ways that different frames shaped subsequent paths of search and action.[8]
Connecting Cognition to Strategic Outcomes
This links to another stream of research that was less concerned with accuracy and more
concerned with the impact that these representations might have on outcomes. It was not
enough, researchers noted, to say that managers had cognitive frames (though, of course,
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

678

S. Kaplan

the proof of concept was an important first step); the real goal was to show that these
interpretations could be connected to organizational actions and outcomes. Much of the
challenge here was methodological. Could measures of cognition be found that would
allow for longitudinal studies that would enable analysis of causality? And, ultimately,
could these be compared to and combined with other factors previously identified as
contributing to strategic outcomes?
In Porac et al. and other early work on cognition in strategy, the authors found
cross-sectional correlations between the cognitive frames of managers and the patterns of
strategic choices and actions in the firms and industries. Another early study (Thomas
et al., 1993) used scenario-based surveys of hospital managers to determine their scanning and interpreting propensities and correlated these with their hospital performance
measures. The problem with these observations is that it is difficult, especially in the
cross-section, to attribute any form of causality. It is possible that cognitive frames direct
subsequent strategic choice and action, but based on the evidence in these studies, it is
equally plausible that frames simply capture a pattern of actions within the industry and
are therefore epiphenomenal.
In order to establish causal relationships, later work moved towards longitudinal
studies in which multiple firms could be compared. The challenge of this approach,
however, was in finding sources of data that allow these comparisons to take place
longitudinally across firms. Interviews were useful in capturing details of cognitive
frames at one point in time, but were not as useful in capturing longitudinal perspectives because of the risk of retrospective bias. Another method for gathering data on
managers cognition was to conduct surveys. Such surveys proved useful in understanding such factors as the urgency, understandability, and manageability of environmental change as Ginsberg and Venkatraman (1995) showed in their analysis of tax
preparation firms response to the introduction of electronic filing of tax returns but
because of their cross-sectional nature suffered from the lack of ability to identify the
direction of the relationship between cognition and outcomes. An exception is research
by Plambeck and Weber (2009), who conducted a two-stage survey examining the
relationship between German CEOs ambivalence about the enlargement of the European Union and subsequent organizational actions. Yet, getting a reasonable response
rate (Plambeck and Weber got about 30 per cent at each stage) is difficult across
multiple stages, and therefore surveys are not well suited for examining causality over
longer time periods.
Following initial work by Bowman (1982), one solution that many scholars have settled
on is the use of the Letter to Shareholders in a companys Annual Report to Shareholders
(Abrahamson and Hambrick, 1997; Barr, 1998; Barr et al., 1992; Cho and Hambrick,
2006; Clapham and Schwenk, 1991; Eggers and Kaplan, 2009; Kaplan, 2008b; Kaplan
et al., 2003; Nadkarni and Barr, 2008). They are documents that capture managers
views contemporaneously. In this way, they are superior to interviews in which managers
likely engage in retrospective reconstruction of events to fit more neatly with the understanding about how things have subsequently evolved. They are also available for all
(publicly traded) firms over time. Though many critiques of this source of information on
managerial cognition can be (and have been!) made, it remains an important option
for scholars who are seeking ways to examine cognition in a consistent manner across
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

679

firms and over time (Duriau et al., 2007).[9] It was the development of the Letter to
Shareholders, and specifically word counts of themes within them, as a legitimated
measure of managerial cognition that made it possible to proceed with longitudinal
studies connecting cognition to action.
In small sample studies, Barr and colleagues (Barr, 1998; Barr et al., 1992) used
Letters to Shareholders to map links between cognition and action using a causal
mapping approach (Huff et al., 1990). In an early study, Barr et al. (1992) examined a
matched pair of railroad companies and found that simply noticing new environmental
conditions was not sufficient for change. Instead, managers had to make explicit links
to the organizations strategy and then change those links as environmental conditions
changed. Barrs (1998) careful examination of the evolution of managers maps of
the environment in six pharmaceutical firms tracked how each firms executives
interpreted an important change in the environment (increased regulation of pharmaceutical research and production). Through a qualitative comparison, she found
that different firms manifested different interpretations of the event (for example,
some seeing it as a major shift in the industry, others linking it only to a change in
the expense structure of the firm) and that these interpretations changed over time.
Importantly, she found that the firms did not undertake strategic responses to the event
until interpretations had evolved to the point of recognizing a substantial impact on
performance.
To move to larger samples, causal mapping approaches have proven too cumbersome,[10] and scholars began to focus on CEO attention as captured by the count of
words in the documents. Osborne et al. (2001) also examined pharmaceutical firms, this
time 400 of them. They used words in the Letters to Shareholders to identify different
themes (for example, the terms international, alliances, foreign exchange, risk, and
licensing, defined a theme they labelled overseas expansion) addressed by the executives of the different firms. They found that various firms grouped together according to
the themes they pursued and that these cognitive strategic groups were closely related to
the strategic groups identified based on performance measures (which they drew from
Cool and Schendels (1987) analysis of the strategic groups of these same firms). More
importantly, they also found statistically significant relationships between the cognitive
groups as defined in the initial time period, strategic actions in subsequent periods (e.g.
facilities expansions or introduction of new product lines), and later reports of the success
or failure of those activities.
In their study of deregulation in the airline industry, Cho and Hambrick (2006)
extended this line of thinking by showing how changes in the top management teams
focus of attention (from an engineering orientation to an entrepreneurial one) were
associated with subsequent changes in strategic actions (such as choices about the
number of cities served, the number of planes in the fleet, the level of passenger service,
and expenditures on advertising and sales). Their more unique contribution was in
identifying a series of antecedents to changes in top management team attention, such as
changes in the average industry tenure of the team, changes in the type of functional
experiences, and increases in the heterogeneity of the team.
The challenge of a number of these studies that attempted to associate cognition
with strategic outcomes is that they did not control meaningfully for the alternative
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

680

S. Kaplan

explanations of strategic action that exist in the strategic management literature. Without
these controls, one can raise the reasonable concern that measures of cognition may
simply be epiphenomena of actions that the firm might reasonably take based on its
interests or its capabilities. To address these concerns, I, along with Rebecca Henderson
and Fiona Murray, examined the response of 15 pharmaceutical firms to the emergence
of biotechnology over a period of 20 years using biotechnology-related words in the
Letter to Shareholders to proxy for managerial attention to this new technology (Kaplan
et al., 2003). The study found that efforts to get patents and publish scientific papers in
this realm were associated with prior changes in managerial attention. This paper
responded to scepticism about the connection between cognition and performance, by
first verifying the relationship even when including a series of measures for plausible
alternative explanations for differences in firms responses (e.g. scale, prior related experience (capabilities), market position, competitive actions, isomorphic pressures, and
financial performance); and second, ruling out reverse causality between organizational
actions and subsequent attention to the issues.
This phase of research in cognition and strategy made strides in connecting cognition
to outcomes of interest in strategic management. This progress came through innovations in measurement and through careful attention to alternative explanations of organizational action already well established in the strategic management literature. This
research completed the primary research agenda established by the early scholars of
cognition and strategy by showing not only that managerial cognitive frames exist (proof
of concept) but that they matter.
Building Dynamic Models of Cognition in Strategy Making
What happened next? If one looks at the membership of the Academy of Managements
Managerial and Organizational Cognition Division, one would not be particularly
hopeful about the subsequent momentum of research on managerial cognition in general
nor cognition in strategy specifically. Its membership stalemated in the late 1990s and
grew only slightly (and more slowly than the overall Academys membership growth) in
the 2000s.[11] What is apparent, however, is that cognition began to spread throughout
the different areas of managerial scholarship. Indeed, in the Academy of Management
Programme in 2009, of the 96 sessions including the words cognition or cognitive, only
22 were associated with the MOC division. Interestingly, the next most highly associated
division was Business, Policy and Strategy (with 16 sessions) (see Figure 4). This trend was
presaged by another series of MOC-sponsored discussions at the 1999 Academy of
Management Annual Meeting in which one of the main themes (particularly championed by Anne Huff and William Starbuck) was that of the pluralistic, bridging nature
of cognition.[12] These scholars recognized that many researchers interested in cognition
might have their primary intellectual homes in other divisions, such as strategy, organizational behaviour, or elsewhere.
Thus, contrary to the notion that research in managerial cognition may have stalemated after the burst of activity from the late 1980s to the late 1990s, what we see is a
diffusion of ideas about cognition throughout strategy and management scholarship.
Looking at papers that have cited the Porac et al. article, we see that different aspects of
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

681

Figure 4. Division affiliations of sessions in the Academy of Management Annual Meeting 2009 including
the term cognition (total sessions = 96)
Note: Not including Interactive Paper Sessions and Caucuses for which division affiliations are not stated.
Sum of divisions mentioned is greater than 96 because symposia can list multiple affiliations.
Source: Academy of Management online programme, 2009 Annual Meeting.
MH = Management history
AAT = All academy theme
MOC = Managerial and organizational cognition
BPS = Business policy and strategy
MSR = Management spirituality and religion
CAR = Careers
OB = Organizational behaviour
CM = Conflict management
OCIS = Organizational communication and
CMS = Critical management studies
information systems
ENT = Entrepreneurship
OMT = Organizational and management theory
GDO = Gender and diversity in organizations
ONE = Organizations and the natural environment
HCM = Healthcare management
PNP = Public and non-profit
HR = Human resources
RM = Research methods
ODC = Organization development and change
SIM = Social issues in management
IM = International management
TIM = Technology and innovation management
MC = Management consulting
MED = Management education and development

cognition have been picked up by different scholars. The concept of cognitive strategic
groups was connected to ideas about mental models, shared knowledge, social learning,
technological frames, etc. (see Table III).[13] That the Managerial and Organizational
Cognition division in the Academy of Management has not grown substantially may
actually be a sign of the success of the pioneers of this field. Cognition and all of
the concepts related to interpretive processes can be seen as a sort of general purpose
technology (Bresnahan and Trajtenberg, 1995) that has become an essential part
of the theorizing in many different fields of management, including, or especially,
strategy.
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

682

S. Kaplan

Table III. Cognition-related, author-supplied keywords for 226 articles citing Porac et al.

analogy(ies)
aspiration levels
attention
automatic and controlled
information processing
blind spots
casual path analysis
causal maps
cognition
cognitive approach
cognitive bias
cognitive complexity
cognitive construction
cognitive diversity
cognitive factions
cognitive frames
cognitive groups
cognitive mapping
cognitive maps
cognitive structures

collective strategy frames


domestic mindsets
egocentrism
environmental interpretation
frames
frames of reference
framing
heuristics and biases
information exchange
information processing
information sharing
insight
issue selling
judgment
knowledge base
knowledge structures
knowledge transfer
managerial cognition
measuring knowledge
mental maps

mental models
organizational image and identity
organizational knowledge
overconfidence
overlapping classification
perception
personal construct theory
problem representation
repertory grid (technique)
sensemaking
shared cognition
shared understanding
signalling
similarity judgments
social cognition(s)
social learning
sociocognitive resources
strategic cognition
technological frames

Source: ISI Web of Knowledge. Coding by the author based on the author-supplied keywords of 226 papers citing Porac
et al. to the end of 2009.

It is therefore not possible in the space available to trace the paths that managerial
cognition research has taken, even just in the field of strategy. Instead, in this section, I
will highlight a few significant directions (admittedly those biased by my own research
interests) that researchers took as they more fully integrated cognition into the field of
strategic management (with apologies to all those scholars whose work I have omitted for
reasons of space).
Having accomplished the objective of finding systematic associations between cognitive frames and strategic outcomes, a set of scholars have turned to what Walsh (1995)
had described as the ultimate objective of managerial cognition research, that of understanding the processes and practices underlying these effects. In their 1989 piece, Porac,
Thomas and Baden-Fuller took as an assumption a set of processes that connect managers identification of cognitive strategic groups to strategic outcomes in which individuals attend to cues in the environment, interpret the meaning of such cues, and then
externalize these interpretations via concrete activities (p. 398). These views built on
earlier theoretical work by Daft and Weick (1984) and Kiesler and Sproull (1982) who
proposed that strategic action is influenced by how managers notice and interpret change
and translate those perspectives into strategic choices.
In the 2000s, scholars returned to these earlier models with an aim to develop a richer
empirical understanding of the ways in which cognition shapes action. One fruitful area
for these explorations was in understanding how organizations respond to changes in
their environments, be they regulatory changes, new technologies, or market crises.
While some organizations are able to adapt, many are subject to strong inertial forces,
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

683

and scholars have found cognitive explanations to be particularly powerful in understanding these differences in outcomes. The research to explore these dynamics took two
major forms: the first was to place cognitive explanations in the context of the two other
major sets of explanations pursued in research on strategy, those of incentives and
capabilities; and the second was to look inside the organization to uncover the daily
practices that connect cognitive frames to outcomes.
Cognition, capabilities, and incentives. In the field of strategy, substantial research efforts have
been devoted to understanding differences in organizations responses to change, and the
explanations have been grounded in differences in either incentives or capabilities (only
a very few citations to which are provided below). Economists have argued that differences in responses to changes such as new technologies can be explained as rational
responses to differential incentives (Gans and Stern, 2000; Henderson, 1993; Zucker and
Darby, 1996). Researchers drawing on the Resource Based View and behavioural
theories of the firm argue that firms often have difficulty adapting to change because
of the path dependence associated with initial endowments (Cockburn et al., 2000;
Leonard-Barton, 1992), unless the firms capabilities happen to be pre-adaptive
(Cattani, 2005; Klepper and Simons, 2000) or firms possess dynamic managerial
capabilities that allow resources to be reconfigured (Helfat, 1997; Teece et al., 1997).
A series of recent studies mainly rich qualitative inquiries have sought to add
cognitive explanations to these discussions. Tripsas and Gavetti (2000) used their analysis
of Polaroids failure to transition from analogue to digital imaging technologies to
highlight the paradox of Polaroids early development of technical capabilities but
ultimate failure to be competitive in the digital camera market. They suggested that this
paradox can be explained by managerial cognitive frames that placed primacy on
technical excellence and modelled economic success on a razor/razor blade model for
making money that turned out to be inappropriate in the digital world. They demonstrated that the presence or absence of capabilities was not an adequate explanation of
organizational inertia and that cognition was a central contributor to outcomes. This
analysis amplified earlier findings by Dougherty (1992) whose study of new product
development processes showed that departmental thought worlds and organizational
product routines interacted to inhibit the acquisition of new technology and market
knowledge and by Garud and Rappa (1994) from their study of the development of
cochlear implants which highlighted the intimate relationship between understandings
of a technology, the organizational routines developed for evaluating those technologies,
and the strategic choices of firms to invest in one technology or another.
This empirical research connected to a set of theoretical developments about the
relationships between capabilities, incentives, and cognition. In outlining his attentionbased view of the firm, Ocasio (1997) argued that one set of the contextual factors
shaping the allocation of attention in the organization is what he calls the rules of the
game the routines and incentive systems that structure the process by which interpretations are made. Routines can be understood to have both cognitive and motivational
dimensions they embed both an understanding of how things should be done and what
gets rewarded (Cohendet and Llerena, 2003; Coriat and Dosi, 1998; Dosi et al., 2003;
Kaplan and Henderson, 2005). As a result, these routines and economic forces may
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

684

S. Kaplan

reinforce attention traps ( Johnson and Hoopes, 2003, p. 1058) that cause managers to
miss seeing change. These various streams of work all imply that cognition, capabilities,
and incentives are interconnected factors shaping organizational outcomes.
Laamanen and Wallin (2009) built on these insights in a comparative case study
analysis of three software firms to show how capability development paths paralleled
cognitive paths of managers, where shifts in how managers thought about the business
enabled the organization to build needed capabilities. Indeed, it was the ability of
managers to decide which capability bottleneck to focus on that made adaptation to
changing conditions possible.
My colleague J. P. Eggers and I (Eggers and Kaplan, 2009; Kaplan, 2008b) explored
these dynamics in larger samples to examine how CEO cognition interacts with organizational capabilities and incentives in shaping strategic outcomes. These studies of
communication technology firms responses to the emergence of fibre optics showed that
the effects of cognition, incentives, and capabilities are related. Regarding choices about
the direction of R&D (Kaplan, 2008b), changes in managerial attention had an important main effect and this effect was intensified if firms did not have prior related
competences in the optical arena or customer-related incentives to act. This implies that
having the appropriate capabilities or strong incentives to act may improve a firms
chance for responding to environmental change, but they alone may not be enough to
instigate the change. Echoing Tripsas and Gavettis (2000) findings, even when a firm has
the correct technical capabilities, it may be unable to respond effectively to a new
technology if managers beliefs are not correctly aligned with the opportunity. Similarly,
when incentives to act are low, cognition will have an increasingly substantial effect on
outcomes.
Regarding subsequent launching of optical products (Eggers and Kaplan, 2009), the
results suggest that the effect of the CEOs cognition differs depending on the focus of
their attention accelerating entry when attention is directed to the emerging technology
and slowing response when attention is directed to the existing technology. This implies
that firms with the same underlying organizational capabilities and incentives might
move in different directions if the different CEOs attention is either myopic or farsighted. It is therefore not necessarily organizational capabilities or incentives associated
with the existing technologies that hold firms back, but rather greater attention to these
technologies by managers that lead to inertia. Thus, this work indicates that understanding the interaction between cognition, capabilities, and incentives ends up being essential
to understanding strategic outcomes in organizations.
Internal cognitive dynamics in organizations. Another stream of research that picked up on the
process assumptions in Porac et al. has focused on the internal cognitive dynamics in
organizations that produce the relationships between cognition and strategic outcomes
described above. This followed the advocacy by several managerial cognition scholars to
move research beyond purely content issues in order to focus on cognitive processes
(Bogner and Barr, 2000; Elsbach et al., 2005; Lant, 2002). These scholars argued that an
understanding of cognitive mechanisms in such processes as strategy making requires
engagement with the situated and interactive nature of cognition. They suggested that it
is risky to reify the organization as a cognizer and that scholars should endeavour to
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

685

understand how a collective frame and ultimately a collective decision might emerge
through actors interactions (Garud and Karne, 2001; Spender, 1998; Weick and
Roberts, 1993) (for the most recent update of these ideas, see Garud et al., 2010). Weick
(1995) was clearly inspired by the Scottish knitwear study as he devoted several pages to
a discussion of it in his 1995 book on sensemaking, and it was this portrayal of the
interactive process of interpretation that provoked questions in the field of strategic
management about static treatments of cognition to that date.
Some scholars have responded to this challenge with in-depth case studies of strategy
making inside organizations. The models developed from these various case studies (a
few of which are described below) depart from the way cognition has traditionally been
construed in the managerial literature. Instead of a static representation of cognition in
which actors are depicted as constrained by rigid mental models, these studies of the
process of strategy making portray cognition as a dynamic, purposive, and politicallycharged process of meaning construction.
These studies have mainly been published in the past decade, but one early study in
this regard was that by Gioia and Chittipeddi (1991) who examined an organizational
change process inside a university. Using interviews and participant observations, they
found that the CEO of the university and other senior managers were engaged in
efforts, first, to make sense of the strategic change based on ideas from their backgrounds,
the history of the university, and information from key stakeholders; and, second, to
provide a vision to others (what they call sensegiving). This analysis demonstrated that
interpretation was both an individual and a social process. Individuals need social
interaction in order to understand and interpret their environments but they also use
these social interactions to shape the interpretations of others. Thus, sensegiving is
intertwined with ones own sensemaking efforts.
Rouleau (2005) explored these dynamics further in her study of middle managers in a
top-of-the-line clothing company. She demonstrated that these middle managers had a
crucial role as both interpreters and sellers of strategic change within the organization
and, crucially, to outsiders such as clients. Similarly, Markoczy (2001) looked at how
consensus in beliefs was achieved in three different Hungarian companies recently
acquired by Anglo-Saxon firms. Using causal mapping techniques, she showed that
consensus building was driven not by top management but by an interest group of
organizational members who would benefit from the strategic change.
Szulanski et al. (2004) conducted case studies of the strategic decision-making processes in three firms dealing with another kind of strategic change, the emergence of the
internet. By looking inside these firms, they were able to make links between organizational dynamics and strategic outcomes. They found that the type and breadth of
cognitive frames as well as the timing of efforts to develop these frames affected both the
degree to which each firm responded to the technical change and the success of those
strategic choices. They argued that successful managers in organizations take deliberate
steps to interpret change (technical or other) and that default frames based on past
individual or organizational experience can lead to poor outcomes.
Inspired by these ideas, I conducted an ethnographic case study of strategy making
inside one (communications technology) organization facing a radical change (the
collapse of the fibre optics market in the early 2000s) (Kaplan, 2008a). To interpret these
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

686

S. Kaplan

data, I drew on research about social movements to develop a model of framing contests
inside the organization. This model described the ways that actors translate their own
cognitive frames into a predominant collective frame as the process of strategy making
unfolds. I showed that when cognitive frames were not congruent, different groups
attempted to make their own frame resonate with others and therefore mobilize action
in their favour. Those actors who most skilfully engaged in these practices shaped the
frame that prevailed in the organization. This paper modelled framing in strategy
making as both highly contested and connected to the political pursuit of interests. The
focal point of the political process is the collective effort of meaning making, such that the
pursuit of interests can only be understood alongside the frames actors use to interpret
those interests. Making strategy under uncertainty can therefore be comprehended as a
product of contests over which frame should guide the understanding of an ambiguous
environment and of choices about how to respond to it.
This series of studies on framing shows that cognition and the political pursuit of
interests are intertwined. From this perspective, frames are an ex ante part of the political
process that produces decisions, defining the nature of the problems around which
political jockeying will take place. As a result, these papers brought framing and interpretation into research on strategy processes, which has traditionally emphasized only
the political side of the equation (Eisenhardt and Bourgeois, 1988; Pettigrew, 1973). The
papers described above offer a glimmer of the possibilities created by the examination of
interpretive processes inside organizations. Much potential remains in this stream of
research.

CONCLUSION LOOKING FORWARD


Where to next? The answer, of course, is many places. As I have sketched out above,
cognitive constructs have diffused widely both in strategy and more broadly in organizational analysis. Scholarship on cognition in strategic management over the past 20
years has put behind us questions about whether cognition matters for outcomes. It has
also steered us away from the analysis of accuracy and bias in perception these are not
central or even perhaps appropriate questions in a context where interpretative processes
have been shown to endogenize organizational and market structures.[14] Instead, I
suggest that, in addition to further studies of cognitive dynamics inside organizations as
discussed above, there are three avenues that Porac, Thomas and Baden-Fuller opened
up which are experiencing a resurgence of interest.

Endogenous Environments
The first stream of research builds on the insight from Porac et al. that the environment
is endogenous to the actions of organizations. They showed that rivalry in the Scottish
knitwear industry was not preordained but rather shaped by how managers in each of the
firms defined who their rivals were and what criteria upon which rivalry would take
place. More recently, studies have identified and analysed other dimensions of the
environment such as relative reputations of firms, the legitimacy of new practices,
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

687

industry velocity, and the understanding of value that are produced through the
interpretative processes of the actors in those environments.
Rindova and Fombruns (1999) study of IBMs evolution as a company found that the
reputation of the firm was constructed through the sociocognitive dynamics of interactions between the firm, its constituents, and its rivals. Furthermore, the changing reputation affected the firms ability to gain access to key competitive resources. As a result,
the interpretations of the various players produced the terrain upon which competition
took place. Kennedy and Fiss (2009) found that the diffusion of TQM practices
depended not only on desires for economic performance or legitimacy as classic models
of adoption would suggest but crucially on whether such practices were perceived as
posing a threat or an opportunity.
Similarly, Nadkarni and Narayanan (2007) found in their study of aircraft and semiconductor firms that the very pace of industry change was constructed by different firms
interpretations about the degree of industry velocity. High- or low-velocity conditions
were shaped by the assumptions of managers and then reinforced through their social
networks and the feedback from their own strategic actions. Further, Fiona Murray and
I (Kaplan and Murray, 2010) found in our analysis of the evolution of the biotechnology
industry that the notion of what was of value, and therefore what technological directions
should be pursued, was a product of the interpretations of a wide range of actors in the
field. These studies suggest that environments have multiple dimensions rivals, reputations, velocity, value and that these dimensions are not given characteristics of the
field but rather constructed through the interpretations of the actors who make up the
field.
Categories and Their Emergence
The second stream of research returns to the idea of categorization. The Scottish
knitwear study showed how the categorization of different competitors as direct rivals
affected the strategic choices and actions of firms. They claimed associations between
categorization and performance. More recent work has picked up on these ideas and has
sought to nail down this relationship. Porac returned to the issue of categorization with
co-author Jos Antonio Rosa (Rosa and Porac, 2002) to demonstrate that in the motorcycle industry categorizations had different levels of persistence depending on the source
ideas for the categorizations themselves, with those coming from scientific authority, for
example, having greater persistence, and those coming from perceptible properties of the
product having lower persistence. Zuckerman (1999) showed that stock prices suffer for
firms that are not easily classified into an industry sector by analysts. Hsu et al. (2009)
found that products (films or items for sale on eBay) that span many categories are less
likely to attract customers than products where the categorization is clear. In addition,
they show that firms that span multiple categories have difficulty in developing capabilities to do well in any one specific category.
The logical follow on question is of course how these categories emerge in the first
place. Researchers have begun to explore this topic, identifying key antecedents to
category creation and the ways in which new categories emerge and are legitimated, with
the media and high status or powerful actors playing central roles. In early studies in this
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

688

S. Kaplan

arena, Joe Porac, along with a number of authors, found that in the case of the
introduction of minivans into the motor vehicle market (Rosa et al., 1999) or different
classes of motorcycles (Rosa and Porac, 2002), new categories were negotiated between
producers and users through the media. Kennedys (2005, 2008) studies of the emergence of the computer workstation market showed that new market categories need to be
legitimized, and media coverage of new firms is a crucial means for doing so.
Rao et al.s (2003, 2005) analysis of French chefs showed that, as a new category
emerged (nouvelle cuisine relative to classical cuisine), chefs self-categorizations changed
based on the status of the proponents of the new category and the defections by peers into
the new category. Furthermore, Raos related study with Lounsbury (Lounsbury and
Rao, 2004) on the mutual fund industry emphasized that these actors can exercise their
power to either preserve the existing structure of categories or change them. The
research highlighted above delves further into the ideas set up by Porac et al. that the
interpretations of actors embedded in these contexts shape the contexts themselves in an
iterative process. However, there is much more to learn about the process of emergence,
and cognition will certainly have a central role in the theories still to be developed.
Reciprocal Dynamics between Cognition, Materiality, and Interests
Porac et al.s project in 1989 was to move away from purely material definitions of
strategic groups and show that they were constructed of both material and cognitive
dynamics. However, because their emphasis was on introducing cognition to models
which had previously omitted it, they did less work at the time to show how the cognitive
and material interacted. Recent research digs into these reciprocal dynamics between
categories, material products, and the interests of actors in the creation and stabilization
of categories (Kaplan and Tripsas, 2008).
Pontikes (2008) suggested, in her analysis of new categories in the software industry,
that there is an intertwining between the nature of a new technology, the character of
existing categories, and the creation of new categories. She found that new technologies
are seeds for defining new categories but that a new category will only emerge if existing
categories are not flexible enough to accommodate the technology. Weber et al. (2008)
showed, in a study of the emergence of a market for grass-fed beef, that a grass roots
social movement solved challenges in creating new markets by stimulating production,
creating a system of exchange, and crucially, generating a collective identity and shared
understanding of what the market would be.
Fleischer (2009) tied the shaping of categories directly to the interests of focal actors by
looking at the forces shaping brokerage firm rating systems of different companies. She
found that where brokerage firms had conflicts of interest (in that they were engaged in
underwriting activities associated with firms they were also rating), the ambiguity of the
categorizations increased. The definition of categories was not only related to technical
features of the companies themselves, but was shaped by the interests of those constructing the categories. Benner and Tripsas (forthcoming) showed, in the context of digital
cameras, that firms prior industry experience shaped their beliefs about what form the
new technology would take, but as they gained experience with particular features of the
technology, the influence of those previously shared frames declined. These studies begin
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

689

to unpack the complex relationship between technologies, categories, and actors


interests as they emerge and co-evolve.
Conclusion
The research highlighted above delves further into the ideas set up by Porac et al. that
the interpretations of actors embedded in these contexts shape the contexts themselves in
an iterative process. The essential questions that researchers interested in cognition and
strategy can pursue today are: Where do categories come from? How can we account for
the relationship between cognition, materiality, and actors interests in understanding
how categories emerge? How do organizational strategies shape categories and how do
categories shape organizations and their strategies?
It is clear from this review of research in cognition and strategy over the past 20 years
that one cannot advocate using a single construct in approaching these questions. I use
the language of category in the research questions above only because it has become a
recently popular label for interpretive constructs, but one could easily substitute in
cognitive frame or schema or technological frame or many other terms to those
questions and they would be equally valid.
Neither can I advocate a single method. Some of the questions will only be answered
with detailed qualitative studies of internal organizational dynamics, but others will be
amenable to many different approaches already developed in the field. One of the most
recent trends, driven by increased electronic availability of texts and new sophistication
in computational methods, is textual analysis of themes and relationships. The field is
moving rapidly beyond the simple word counts that made cognitive constructs tractable
in the past. Further, while not covered in any detail here, methods developed in cognitive
psychology, both behavioural experiments and cutting edge brain-scanning (fMRI) techniques being exploited fairly aggressively in the marketing and decision sciences literature, may also bring new insights to the field. Despite 20 years of scholarship in cognition
and strategy, there is still much to know and thus a thousand flowers [of constructs and
methods] blooming is very much in order.
Joe Porac, Howard Thomas and Charles Baden-Fuller, along with a number of other
pioneering scholars in the 1980s, set us on a path to explore the role of cognition in
strategy. This path has turned into many paths, only some of which I have highlighted
in this appreciation of the impact of their work. Cognitive explanations have gained
legitimacy and have been analysed alongside more traditional explanatory factors in
strategic management research. Scholars have established and legitimized various measures of cognitive constructs. Managerial cognition as a topic has diffused throughout the
Academy, especially in the field of strategy. Thus, the scholars of the 1980s can claim
substantial victories, and they can also greet with satisfaction new streams of research
that build on the ideas born more than 20 years ago.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The author would like to thank Timothy Vogus for data from the Academy of Management Managerial
and Organizational Cognition Division archives; Joel Baum, Joep Cornelissen, Anne Fleischer, Sucheta
Nadkarni, and Davide Ravasi for helpful comments; and Keyvan Vakili for research assistance.
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

690

S. Kaplan

NOTES
[1] The other important link here is to images of institutional isomorphism (DiMaggio and Powell, 1983),
which, while not specifically focused on cognition, did suggest that factors other than rational economic
choice of actors were at play in shaping firm strategic choices and action.
[2] This is a concept that Porac and Thomas had been developing in a series of conference papers which
resulted in a theory building paper in the Academy of Management Review in 1990 (Porac and Thomas,
1990).
[3] These statistics are based on searches of the ISI Web of Knowledge (28 January 2010). According to
Google Scholar, the article has been cited 541 times, but these data are difficult to analyse at a granular
level because Google does not provide keywords and abstracts systematically. So the analyses presented
here draw from the ISI results alone, and are therefore only a partial representation of the intellectual
landscape in which the Porac et al. paper sits.
[4] This information is based on searches of the ISI Web of Knowledge and Google Scholar (28 January
2010).
[5] I wish to thank Professor Timothy Vogel, the Archivist of the Managerial and Organizational Cognition Division, for providing membership statistics.
[6] For reasons of both interest and space, I focus this analysis on a selection of empirical papers. However,
it should be noted that there have been many interesting theoretical papers proposing new ways of
theorizing about cognition and strategy (such as Ginsberg, 1994; Milliken and Lant, 1991; Ocasio,
1997) that should be considered in a fuller appreciation of the development of the field.
[7] As summarized by Frances Milliken in the MOC interest group newsletter, November 1994.
[8] Gavetti and Levinthal, as well as Rivkin pursued this line of thinking in a series of subsequent papers
contrasting cognition with rational search and analysis (Gavetti and Rivkin, 2007; Gavetti et al.,
2005). These perspectives see cognition as an effect of bounded rationality (March and Simon, 1958),
treating it mainly as a computational problem.
[9] First, the Letter to Shareholders can meaningfully be interpreted as representing CEO attention to
specific issues. Unlike the rest of the Annual Report, which is often written by the investor relations
department of a firm, the Letter to Shareholders is signed by the Chief Executive Officer and is at least
closely reviewed if not specifically written by him/her (Cho and Hambrick, 2006; Kohut and Segars,
1992). In my own research using Letters to Shareholders (Eggers and Kaplan, 2009), we saw clear signs
that changes in CEOs had an impact on the style, length, and content of the Letters. For example, the
majority of the CEO changes covered in our study were associated with an at least 20 per cent change
in the average number of words used in the Letter. We also noticed significant shifts in tone and
approach used by new CEOs, even if the CEO had been a member of the senior leadership team for
quite some time prior to assuming the top job. Such changes are signals that each CEO put his or her
imprint on the Letter to Shareholders. It is also unlikely that the CEO would engage in an excessive
amount of misdirection in the Letter because fiduciary requirements make it hard for the CEO to
suppress important discussions (Abrahamson and Park, 1994). Finally, research has shown that the
themes emphasized in the Annual Report mirror closely the discussions in internal strategic planning
documents (though the positive or negative spin on the issues may differ) (DAveni and MacMillan,
1990; Fiol, 1995).
[10] Note that Nadkarni and Barr (2008) expanded the causal mapping approach to 24 firms in their
analysis of the effect of perceptions of industry velocity and the speed of response to environmental
changes in four industries.
[11] The MOC division grew at about 2.6 per cent per annum from the mid-1990s to 2008 (slightly higher
at 4.4 per cent from 2003 to 2008), but the Academy of Management overall grew 4.9 per cent p.a.
since the mid-1990s and 5.3 per cent from 2003 to 2008 (data from the MOC Division archivist,
Timothy Vogel).
[12] As previewed in the June 1999 newsletter of the MOC division announcing the session.
[13] In his 1995 review of the field, Jim Walsh identified 77 different terms used in the managerial cognition
field, from knowledge structures, to frames, to beliefs, etc. This pluralism of terms connects in part
to the different disciplines from which people have approached managerial thinking. It also explains a
good deal of the diffusion of the field in more recent years: paths pursued by different scholars have
been diverse. Advocacy in the 1990s (described above) to narrow the number of concepts used in
managerial cognition has not been heeded.
[14] Note that, in research on individual choice and action in behavioural economics and finance, there is
a huge literature examining accuracy and bias that is too large to summarize with any subtlety here.
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

691

This primarily experimental work has made important psychological contributions to understanding
why neoclassical economic models of behaviour are not fully representative of the dynamics we observe
in markets. However, they are less salient in understanding the organizational dynamics associated with
strategic management that is the subject of Porac et al.s (1989) paper and my own review of the related
literature here.

REFERENCES
Abrahamson, E. and Fombrun, C. J. (1994). Macrocultures determinants and consequences. Academy of
Management Review, 19, 72855.
Abrahamson, E. and Hambrick, D. C. (1997). Attentional homogeneity in industries: the effect of discretion. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18, 51332.
Abrahamson, E. and Park, C. (1994). Concealment of negative organizational outcomes: an agency theory
perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 130234.
Barley, S. R. (1983). Semiotics and the study of occupational and organizational cultures. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 28, 393.
Barley, S. R. (1986). Technology as an occasion for structuring evidence from observations of CT scanners
and the social-order of radiology departments. Administrative Science Quarterly, 31, 78108.
Barr, P. S. (1998). Adapting to unfamiliar environmental events: a look at the evolution of interpretation and
its role in strategic change. Organization Science, 9, 64469.
Barr, P. S., Stimpert, J. L. and Huff, A. S. (1992). Cognitive change, strategic action, and organizational
renewal. Strategic Management Journal, 13, Special Issue, 1536.
Bartunek, J. M. (1984). Changing interpretive schemes and organizational restructuring: the example of a
religious order. Administrative Science Quarterly, 29, 35572.
Baum, J. A. C. and Lant, T. K. (2003). Hits and misses: managers (mis)categorization of competitors in the
Manhattan hotel industry. Geography and Strategy, 20, 11956.
Baum, J. A. C. and Mezias, S. J. (1992). Localized competition and organizational failure in the Manhattan
hotel industry, 18981990. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37, 580604.
Benner, M. J. and Tripsas, M. (forthcoming). The influence of prior industry affiliation on framing in
nascent industries: the evolution of digital cameras. Strategic Management Journal.
Bijker, W. E., Hughes, T. P. and Pinch, T. J. (1987). The Social Construction of Technological Systems: New Directions
in the Sociology and History of Technology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bogner, W. C. and Barr, P. S. (2000). Making sense in hypercompetitive environments: a cognitive
explanation for the persistence of high velocity competition. Organization Science, 11, 21226.
Bowman, E. H. (1982). Risk seeking by troubled firms. Sloan Management Review, 23, 3342.
Bresnahan, T. F. and Trajtenberg, M. (1995). General purpose technologies: engines of growth?. Journal
of Econometrics, 65, 83108.
Cattani, G. (2005). Preadaptation, firm heterogeneity, and technological performance: a study on the
evolution of fiber optics, 19701995. Organization Science, 16, 56380.
Cho, T. S. and Hambrick, D. C. (2006). Attention as the mediator between top management team
characteristics and strategic change: the case of airline deregulation. Organization Science, 17, 45369.
Clapham, S. E. and Schwenk, C. R. (1991). Self-serving attributions, managerial cognition, and company
performance. Strategic Management Journal, 12, 21929.
Cockburn, I. M., Henderson, R. M. and Stern, S. (2000). Untangling the origins of competitive advantage.
Strategic Management Journal, 21, 112345.
Cohendet, P. and Llerena, P. (2003). Routines and incentives: the role of communities in the firm. Industrial
and Corporate Change, 12, 27197.
Cool, K. O. and Schendel, D. (1987). Strategic group formation and performance the case of the
United-States pharmaceutical-industry, 19631982. Management Science, 33, 110224.
Coriat, B. and Dosi, G. (1998). Learning how to govern and learning how to solve problems: on the
co-evolution of competences, conflicts and organizational routines. In Chandler, A. D., Hagstrom,
P. and Solvell, O. (Eds), The Dynamic Firm: The Role of Technology, Strategy, Organization and Regions. Oxford
and New York: Oxford University Press, 10333.
Daft, R. L. and Weick, K. E. (1984). Toward a model of organizations as interpretation systems. Academy
of Management Review, 9, 28495.
DAveni, R. A. and MacMillan, I. C. (1990). Crisis and the content of managerial communications: a study
of the focus of attention of top managers in surviving and failing firms. Administrative Science Quarterly, 35,
63457.
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

692

S. Kaplan

DiMaggio, P. J. and Powell, W. W. (1983). The iron cage revisited institutional isomorphism and collective
rationality in organizational fields. American Sociological Review, 48, 14760.
Dosi, G., Levinthal, D. A. and Marengo, L. (2003). Bridging contested terrain: linking incentivebased and learning perspectives on organizational evolution. Industrial and Corporate Change, 12,
41336.
Dougherty, D. (1992). Interpretive barriers to successful product innovation in large firms. Organization
Science, 3, 179202.
Duriau, V. J., Reger, R. K. and Pfarrer, M. D. (2007). A content analysis of the content analysis literature
in organization studies: research themes, data sources, and methodological refinements. Organizational
Research Methods, 10, 534.
Dutton, J. E. and Jackson, S. E. (1987). Categorizing strategic issues: links to organizational action. Academy
of Management Review, 12, 7690.
Dutton, J. E., Walton, E. J. and Abrahamson, E. (1989). Important dimensions of strategic issues: separating
the wheat from the chaff. Journal of Management Studies, 26, 37996.
Eggers, J. P. and Kaplan, S. (2009). Cognition and renewal: comparing CEO and organizational effects on
incumbent adaptation to technical change. Organization Science, 20, 46177.
Eisenhardt, K. M. and Bourgeois, L. J. III (1988). Politics of strategic decision making in high-velocity
environments: toward a midrange theory. Academy of Management Journal, 31, 73770.
Elsbach, K. D., Barr, P. S. and Hargadon, A. B. (2005). Identifying situated cognition in organizations.
Organization Science, 16, 42233.
Fahey, L. and Narayanan, V. K. (1989). Linking changes in revealed causal maps and environmental
change: an empirical study. Journal of Management Studies, 26, 36178.
Finkelstein, S. and Hambrick, D. C. (1990). Top-management-team tenure and organizational outcomes:
the moderating role of managerial discretion. Administrative Science Quarterly, 35, 484503.
Fiol, C. M. (1990). Explaining strategic alliance in the chemical industry. In Huff, A. S. (Ed.), Mapping
Strategic Thought. Chichester, New York: John Wiley and Sons, 22749.
Fiol, C. M. (1995). Corporate communications: comparing executives private and public statements.
Academy of Management Journal, 38, 52236.
Fiske, S. T. and Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social Cognition. New York: Random House.
Fleischer, A. (2009). Ambiguity and the equity of rating systems: United States brokerage firms, 19952000.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 54, 55574.
Gans, J. S. and Stern, S. (2000). Incumbency and R&D incentives: licensing the gale of creative destruction.
Journal of Economics and Management Strategy, 9, 485511.
Garud, R. and Karne, P. (2001). Path Dependence and Creation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Garud, R. and Rappa, M. A. (1994). A socio-cognitive model of technology evolution: the case of cochlear
implants. Organization Science, 5, 34462.
Garud, R., Kumaraswamy, A. and Karne, P. (2010). Path dependence or path creation?. Journal of
Management Studies, 47, 76074.
Gavetti, G. and Levinthal, D. (2000). Looking forward and looking backward: cognitive and experiential
search. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45, 11337.
Gavetti, G. and Rivkin, J. W. (2007). On the origin of strategy: action and cognition over time. Organization
Science, 18, 42039.
Gavetti, G., Levinthal, D. A. and Rivkin, J. W. (2005). Strategy making in novel and complex worlds: the
power of analogy. Strategic Management Journal, 26, 691712.
Ginsberg, A. (1989). Construing the business portfolio a cognitive model of diversification. Journal of
Management Studies, 26, 41738.
Ginsberg, A. (1994). Minding the competition: from mapping to mastery. Strategic Management Journal, 15,
Special Issue, 15374.
Ginsberg, A. and Venkatraman, N. (1995). Institutional initiatives for technological-change from issue
interpretation to strategic choice. Organization Studies, 16, 42548.
Gioia, D. A. and Chittipeddi, K. (1991). Sensemaking and sensegiving in strategic change initiation. Strategic
Management Journal, 12, 433.
Gronhaug, K. and Falkenberg, J. S. (1989). Exploring strategy perceptions in changing environments.
Journal of Management Studies, 26, 34959.
Hambrick, D. C. and Mason, P. A. (1984). Upper echelons: the organization as a reflection of its top
managers. Academy of Management Review, 9, 193206.
Hannan, M. T. and Freeman, J. (1977). The population ecology of organizations. American Journal of
Sociology, 82, 92964.
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

693

Helfat, C. E. (1997). Know-how and asset complementarity and dynamic capability accumulation: the case
of R&D. Strategic Management Journal, 18, 33960.
Henderson, R. M. (1993). Underinvestment and incompetence as responses to radical innovation: evidence
from the photolithographic alignment equipment industry. RAND Journal of Economics, 24, 24870.
Hodgkinson, G. P. (1997). The cognitive analysis of competitive structures: a review and critique. Human
Relations, 50, 62554.
Hsu, G., Kocak, O. and Hannan, M. T. (2009). Multiple category memberships in markets: an integrative
theory and two empirical tests. American Sociological Review, 74, 15069.
Huff, A. S. (1982). Industry influences on strategy reformulation. Strategic Management Journal, 3, 11931.
Huff, A. S. (1990). Mapping Strategic Thought. Chichester, New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Huff, A. S., Narapareddy, V. and Fletcher, K. E. (1990). Coding the causal association of concepts. In Huff,
A. S. (Ed.), Mapping Strategic Thought. Chichester, New York: John Wiley and Sons, 31126.
Johnson, D. R. and Hoopes, D. G. (2003). Managerial cognition, sunk costs, and the evolution of industry
structure. Strategic Management Journal, 24, 105768.
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decisions under risk. Econometrica, 47,
26391.
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P. and Tversky, A. (1994). Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, 13th edition.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kaplan, S. (2008a). Framing contests: strategy making under uncertainty. Organization Science, 19, 72952.
Kaplan, S. (2008b). Cognition, capabilities, and incentives: assessing firm response to the fiber-optic
revolution. Academy of Management Journal, 51, 67295.
Kaplan, S. and Henderson, R. (2005). Inertia and incentives: bridging organizational economics and
organizational theory. Organization Science, 16, 50921.
Kaplan, S. and Murray, F. (2010). Entrepreneurship and the construction of value in biotechnology.
Research on the Sociology of Organizations. Technology and Organization: Essays in Honour of Joan Woodward, 29,
10949.
Kaplan, S. and Tripsas, M. (2008). Thinking about technology: applying a cognitive lens to technical
change. Research Policy, 37, 790805.
Kaplan, S., Murray, F. and Henderson, R. M. (2003). Discontinuities and senior management: assessing the
role of recognition in pharmaceutical firm response to biotechnology. Industrial and Corporate Change, 12,
20333.
Kennedy, M. T. (2005). Behind the one-way mirror: refraction in the construction of product market
categories. Poetics, 33, 20126.
Kennedy, M. T. (2008). Getting counted: markets, media, and reality. American Sociological Review, 73,
27095.
Kennedy, M. T. and Fiss, P. C. (2009). Institutionalization, framing, and diffusion: the logic of TQM
adoption and implementation decisions among US hospitals. Academy of Management Journal, 52, 897
918.
Kiesler, S. and Sproull, L. (1982). Managerial response to changing environments: perspectives on problem
sensing from social cognition. Administrative Science Quarterly, 27, 54870.
Klepper, S. and Simons, K. L. (2000). Dominance by birthright: entry of prior radio producers and
competitive ramifications in the U.S. television receiver industry. Strategic Management Journal, 21,
9971016.
Knight, F. H. (1921/1965). Risk, Uncertainty and Profit. New York: Harper & Row (first published in 1921,
Hart, Schaffner & Marx).
Kohut, G. F. and Segars, A. H. (1992). The presidents letter to stockholders: an examination of corporate
communication strategy. Journal of Business Communication, 29, 721.
Laamanen, T. and Wallin, J. (2009). Cognitive dynamics of capability development paths. Journal of
Management Studies, 46, 95081.
Lant, T. K. (2002). Organizational cognition and interpretation. In Baum, J. A. C. (Ed.), Companion to
Organizations. Malden, MA: Blackwell, 34462.
Lant, T. K. and Baum, J. (1995). Cognitive sources of socially constructed competitive groups: examples
from the Manhattan hotel industry. In Scott, W. R. and Christensen, S. (Eds), The Institutional Construction of Organization: International and Longitudinal Studies. Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 1538.
Lawrence, P. R. and Lorsch, J. W. (1967). Organization and Environment; Managing Differentiation and Integration.
Boston, MA: Division of Research, Graduate School of Business Administration, Harvard University.
Leonard-Barton, D. (1992). Core capabilities and core rigidities: a paradox in managing new product
development. Strategic Management Journal, 13, Special Issue, 11125.
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

694

S. Kaplan

Lounsbury, M. and Rao, H. (2004). Sources of durability and change in market classifications: a study of the
reconstitution of product categories in the American mutual fund industry, 19441985. Social Forces,
82, 96999.
March, J. G. and Simon, H. A. (1958). Organizations. New York: Wiley.
Markoczy, L. (1997). Measuring beliefs: accept no substitutes. Academy of Management Journal, 40, 122842.
Markoczy, L. (2001). Consensus formation during strategic change. Strategic Management Journal, 22, 1013
31.
McGee, J. and Thomas, H. (1986). Strategic groups: theory, research and taxonomy. Strategic Management
Journal, 7, 141.
Meindl, J. R., Stubbart, C. and Porac, J. F. (1994). Cognition within and between organizations: five key
questions. Organization Science, 5, 28993.
Milliken, F. J. (1990). Perceiving and interpreting environmental-change an examination of college
administrators interpretation of changing demographics. Academy of Management Journal, 33, 4263.
Milliken, F. J. and Lant, T. K. (1991). The effect of an organizations recent performance history on strategic
persistence and change: the role of managerial interpretations. Advances in Strategic Management, 7,
12956.
Nadkarni, S. and Barr, P. S. (2008). Environmental context, managerial cognition, and strategic action: an
integrated view. Strategic Management Journal, 29, 1395427.
Nadkarni, S. and Narayanan, V. K. (2007). The evolution of collective strategy frames in high- and
low-velocity industries. Organization Science, 18, 688710.
Norburn, D. and Birley, S. (1988). The top management team and corporate performance. Strategic
Management Journal, 9, 22537.
Ocasio, W. (1997). Towards an attention-based view of the firm. Strategic Management Journal, 18, 187206.
Osborne, J. D., Stubbart, C. I. and Ramaprasad, A. (2001). Strategic groups and competitive enactment: a
study of dynamic relationships between mental models and performance. Strategic Management Journal,
22, 43554.
Peteraf, M. and Shanley, M. (1997). Getting to know you: a theory of strategic group identity. Strategic
Management Journal, 18, Special Issue, 16586.
Pettigrew, A. M. (1973). The Politics of Organizational Decision-Making. London: Tavistock.
Pfeffer, J. and Salancik, G. R. (1978). The External Control of Organizations: A Resource Dependence Perspective.
New York: Harper & Row.
Plambeck, N. and Weber, K. (2009). CEO ambivalence and responses to strategic issues. Organization
Science, 20, 9931010.
Pontikes, E. G. (2008). Fitting In or Starting New? An Analysis of Invention, Constraint, and the Emergence of New
Categories in the Software Industry. Working Paper, University of Chicago.
Porac, J. and Thomas, H. (1990). Taxonomic mental models in competitor definition. Academy of Management
Review, 15, 22440.
Porac, J. F. and Thomas, H. (1994). Cognitive categorization and subjective rivalry among retailers in a
small city. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 5466.
Porac, J. F., Thomas, H. and Baden-Fuller, C. (1989). Competitive groups as cognitive communities:
the case of Scottish knitwear manufacturers. Journal of Management Studies, 26, 397416.
Porac, J. F., Thomas, H., Wilson, F., Paton, D. and Kanfer, A. (1995). Rivalry and the industry model of
Scottish knitwear producers. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 20327.
Porac, J. F., Thomas, H. and Baden-Fuller, C. (2011). Competitive groups as cognitive communities: the
case of Scottish knitwear manufacturers revisited. Journal of Management Studies, in press.
Porter, M. E. (1980). Competitive Strategy: Techniques for Analyzing Industries and Competitors. New York: Free Press.
Prahalad, C. K. and Bettis, R. A. (1986). The dominant logic: a new linkage between diversity and
performance. Strategic Management Journal, 7, 485501.
Rao, H., Monin, P. and Durand, R. (2003). Institutional change in Toque Ville: nouvelle cuisine as an
identity movement in French gastronomy. American Journal of Sociology, 108, 795843.
Rao, H., Monin, P. and Durand, R. (2005). Border crossing: bricolage and the erosion of categorical
boundaries in French gastronomy. American Sociological Review, 70, 96891.
Reger, R. K. and Huff, A. S. (1993). Strategic groups: a cognitive perspective. Strategic Management Journal,
14, 10323.
Reger, R. K. and Palmer, T. B. (1996). Managerial categorization of competitors: using old maps to
navigate new environments. Organization Science, 7, 2239.
Rindova, V. P. and Fombrun, C. J. (1999). Constructing competitive advantage: the role of firm-constituent
interactions. Strategic Management Journal, 20, 691710.
2011 The Author
Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

Cognition and Strategy

695

Rosa, J. A. and Porac, J. F. (2002). Categorization bases and their influence on product category knowledge
structures. Psychology and Marketing, 19, 50331.
Rosa, J. A., Porac, J. F., Runser-Spanjol, J. and Saxon, M. S. (1999). Sociocognitive dynamics in a product
market. Journal of Marketing, 63, 6477.
Rouleau, L. (2005). Micro-practices of strategic sensemaking and sensegiving: how middle managers
interpret and sell change every day. Journal of Management Studies, 42, 141341.
Simon, H. A. (1947). Administrative Behavior; A Study of Decision-Making Processes in Administrative Organization.
New York: Macmillan.
Sims, H. P. and Gioia, D. A. (1984). Performance failure executive response to self-serving bias. Business
Horizons, 27, 6471.
Smircich, L. and Stubbart, C. (1985). Strategic management in an enacted world. Academy of Management
Review, 10, 72436.
Spender, J. C. (1980). Strategy-Making in Business. PhD Thesis, Manchester Business School, Manchester, UK.
Spender, J. C. (1989). Industry Recipes: An Enquiry into the Nature and Sources of Managerial Judgement. Oxford,
New York: Blackwell.
Spender, J. C. (1998). The dynamics of individual and organizational knowledge. In Eden, C. and Spender,
J. C. (Eds), Managerial and Organizational Cognition. London: Sage, 1329.
Stubbart, C. I. (1989). Managerial cognition: a missing link in strategic management research. Journal of
Management Studies, 26, 32547.
Sutcliffe, K. M. (1994). What executives notice: accurate perceptions in top management teams. Academy of
Management Journal, 37, 1360.
Szulanski, G., Doz, Y. and Ovetzky, Y. (2004). Incumbents framing: three established companies respond
to the internet. Advances in Strategic Management: Business Strategy Over the Industry Life Cycle, 21, 77106.
Teece, D. J., Pisano, G. and Shuen, A. (1997). Dynamic capabilities and strategic management. Strategic
Management Journal, 18, 50933.
Thomas, J. B., Clark, S. M. and Gioia, D. A. (1993). Strategic sensemaking and organizational performance: linkages among scanning, interpretation, action, and outcomes. Academy of Management Journal,
36, 23970.
Tripsas, M. and Gavetti, G. (2000). Capabilities, cognition, and inertia: evidence from digital imaging.
Strategic Management Journal, 21, 114761.
Walsh, J. P. (1995). Managerial and organizational cognition: notes from a trip down memory lane.
Organization Science, 6, 280321.
Walsh, J. P. and Fahey, L. (1986). The role of negotiated belief structures in strategy making. Journal of
Management, 12, 32538.
Weber, K., Heinze, K. L. and DeSoucey, M. (2008). Forage for thought: mobilizing codes in the movement
for grass-fed meat and dairy products. Administrative Science Quarterly, 53, 52967.
Weick, K. E. (1979). The Social Psychology of Organizing, 2nd edition. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Weick, K. E. (1990). Cartographic myths in organizations. In Huff, A. S. (Ed.), Mapping Strategic Thought.
Chichester, New York: John Wiley and Sons, 111.
Weick, K. E. (1995). Sensemaking in Organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Weick, K. E. and Roberts, K. H. (1993). Collective mind in organizations: heedful interrelating on flight
decks. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 35781.
Wiersema, M. F. and Bantel, K. A. (1992). Top management team demography and corporate strategic
change. Academy of Management Journal, 35, 91121.
Williamson, O. E. (1975). Markets and Hierarchies, Analysis and Antitrust Implications: A Study in the Economics of
Internal Organization. New York: Free Press.
Zajac, E. J. and Bazerman, M. H. (1991). Blind spots in industry and competitor analysis implications of
interfirm (mis)perceptions for strategic decisions. Academy of Management Review, 16, 3756.
Zucker, L. G. and Darby, M. R. (1996). Costly information: firm transformations, exit, or persistent failure.
American Behavioral Scientist, 39, 95974.
Zuckerman, E. W. (1999). The categorical imperative: securities analysts and the illegitimacy discount.
American Journal of Sociology, 104, 1398438.

2011 The Author


Journal of Management Studies 2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and
Society for the Advancement of Management Studies

You might also like