You are on page 1of 14

Child Development, March/April 2001, Volume 72, Number 2, Pages 625638

Early TeacherChild Relationships and the Trajectory of


Childrens School Outcomes through Eighth Grade
Bridget K. Hamre and Robert C. Pianta

This study followed a sample of 179 children from kindergarten through eighth grade to examine the extent to
which kindergarten teachers perceptions of their relationships with students predict a range of school out-
comes. Kindergarten teachers rated childrens behavior and the quality of the teacherchild relationship. Follow-
up data from rst through eighth grade were organized by epoch and included academic grades, standardized
test scores, work-habit ratings, and discipline records. Relational Negativity in kindergarten, marked by con-
ict and dependency, was related to academic and behavioral outcomes through eighth grade, particularly for
children with high levels of behavior problems in kindergarten and for boys generally. These associations re-
mained signicant after controlling for gender, ethnicity, cognitive ability, and behavior ratings. The results
have implications for theories of the determinants of school success, the role of adultchild relationships in de-
velopment, and a range of early intervention and prevention efforts.

INTRODUCTION unique working models of the social world based on


their early experiences in relationships with adult care-
Childrens abilities to form and maintain styles and
givers (Bowlby, 1982; Bretherton, 1990; Main & Hesse,
strategies of coping with the social environment in the
1990; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985) that in turn help
early school years are important factors in establishing
shape early development and inuence childrens
a trajectory of academic and behavioral performance
school experiences. For example, children with secure
(Entwisle & Hayduk, 1988; Lynch & Cicchetti, 1997; Pi-
attachments to their primary caregiver engage more ac-
anta, Steinberg, & Rollins, 1995). Those children who
tively in peer interaction and exploration of the school
are able to successfully navigate early social environ-
environment, have higher self-esteem, show a greater
ments in school get off to a better start and continue to
capacity for forming friendships, are more popular
prot from their social knowledge and experience as
with peers, and demonstrate less negative emotion and
they progress through elementary and middle school.
hostile aggression than do their insecurely attached
Markers of classroom social adjustment, including
peers (Park & Waters, 1989; Sroufe, 1983; Sroufe, 1988;
emotional regulation, school liking, peer competence,
Verschueren, Marcoen, & Schoefs, 1996). Children with
engagement with the school environment, and self-
insecure attachments are at risk for lower social compe-
control are linked to childrens success in school (Birch
tence and self-esteem. They tend to show elevated lev-
& Ladd, 1997; Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1996; Wentzel,
els of aggressive and hostile behavior and elicit less
1996). Furthermore, it is evident that, at least in the pri-
contact and more anger from preschool teachers than
mary grades, classroom social adjustment is inuenced
do securely attached children (Cohn, 1990; Sroufe, 1983;
by adultchild relationships, including teacherchild
Sroufe, 1988). Thus, early relationships with adults play
relationships (see Pianta, 1999). Thus, we suspect that
an important role in the formation of social competen-
qualities of the teacherchild relationship, even early in
cies that often translate into positive adjustment in ele-
a childs school career, can forecast later problems and
mentary school classrooms.
successes. The present article examines the extent to
Once children enter school, relationships with non-
which kindergarten teachers relationships with their
parental adults, specically childteacher relationships,
students, as perceived by the teacher, are associated
become increasingly important to classroom adjustment
with childrens academic and behavioral outcomes
(Birch & Ladd, 1997; Greenberg, Speltz, & Deklyen,
through eighth grade and are, in part, implicated in
1993; Howes, Hamilton, & Matheson, 1994; Howes &
the early schooling processes that give rise to well-
Matheson, 1992; Lynch & Cicchetti, 1992; Pianta, 1992a;
documented stability in childrens academic and be-
Pianta & Nimetz, 1991; Pianta et al., 1995). Teachers
havioral performance (Entwisle & Hayduk, 1988;
Kowaleski-Jones & Duncan, 1999).
The work of attachment theorists provides substan- 2001 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
tial evidence to support the idea that children develop All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2001/7202-0020
626 Child Development

play an important role in shaping childrens experi- Cicchetti, 1992), as well as to constructs like control and
ence in school. Beyond the traditional role of teaching warmth observed in parentchild interactions (Pianta,
academic skills, they are responsible for regulating Nimetz, & Bennett, 1997).
activity level, communication, and contact with peers The majority of research on teacherchild relation-
(Howes & Hamilton, 1993; Howes, Matheson, & ships and school outcomes in the early elementary
Hamilton, 1994; Pianta, 1997). Teachers also provide years has focused on childrens socioemotional and be-
behavioral support and teach coping skills to children havioral adaptation. Birch and Ladd (1997) found
(Doll, 1996). that teacherchild conict correlated positively with
From the teachers perspective, strong, positive re- school avoidance and negatively with school liking,
lationships with students can provide motivation to self-directedness, and cooperation in the classroom.
spend extra time and energy promoting childrens Over time, relationships characterized by high conict
success. In contrast, teacherchild relationships char- were associated with a decline in childrens prosocial
acterized by conict may lead to frequent attempts behavior as well as gains in peer-perceived aggressive
to control childrens behavior and thus hinder efforts to behavior (Birch & Ladd, 1998). Furthermore, teacher re-
promote a positive school environment for them. This ports of conict in a relationship are correlated with in-
may be one reason why negative teacherchild rela- creases in childrens problem behaviors and decreases
tionships are related to efforts to exclude children in competence behaviors over time (Pianta et al., 1995).
from the classroom (Pianta et al., 1995). When the teacherchild relationship is characterized
From a childs perspective, positive relationships by closeness, children show higher levels of overall
with teachers may protect against the poor school school adjustment relative to peers scoring at the lower
performance associated with an unsupportive home end of the closeness dimension (Birch & Ladd, 1997; Pi-
environment (Cicchetti & Lynch, 1993). For example, anta et al., 1995). Additionally, Birch and Ladd (1997)
in a sample of maltreated and nonmaltreated chil- found that high levels of child dependence on the
dren, Lynch and Cicchetti (1992) concluded that as a teacher correlated with school adjustment difculties,
result of their experience with parents, maltreated including more negative school attitudes and less posi-
children may express a greater desire for closeness to tive engagement with the school environment. Exces-
nonparental adults compared with nonmaltreated chil- sively dependent children are also more likely to be so-
dren. Just as teachers are likely to put more effort into cially withdrawn and aggressive with peers (Howes et
children with whom they have a positive relation- al., 1994). Overall, these ndings indicate that teacher
ship, children who trust and like teachers may be child relationships are important correlates of childrens
more motivated to succeed. Although there is some social adaptation through second grade.
evidence to suggest that children feel their relation- Academic outcomes have also been investigated in
ships with teachers become less positive as they get association with teacherchild relationships. Birch and
older (Lynch & Cicchetti, 1997), recent data from a Ladd (1997) found correlations between the quality of
large national survey indicate that, even in adoles- teacherchild relationships and academic performance,
cence, relationships with teachers are one of the single as assessed by the Metropolitan Readiness Test. Speci-
most common resources for children and may oper- cally, both closeness and dependency contributed to
ate as a protective factor against risk for a range of performance of visual and language skills in kindergar-
problem outcomes (Resnick et al., 1998). ten. Pianta and colleagues (1995) found that kindergar-
Thus, from both teachers and childrens perspec- ten students for whom academic failure or special edu-
tives, the emotional connection between adults and cation referral were predicted, but who were not
children in schools is an important factor in childrens actually retained or referred, had more positive relation-
school performance. Through the early elementary ships with their kindergarten teachers than did students
years there is substantial evidence supporting the link at similar levels of risk who were retained or referred.
between the quality of teacherchild relationships and Furthermore, improvements in teacherchild relation-
childrens adaptation. Teacher self-report measures re- ships in kindergarten have been associated with more
veal three dimensions of teacherchild relationships: positive adjustment for children at the end of rst grade
conict, closeness, and dependency (Ladd & Burgess, than that predicted solely on the basis of early kinder-
1999; Pianta, 1994; Pianta & Steinberg, 1992). These di- garten adjustment (Pianta & Nimetz, 1991). These nd-
mensions are consistent across child age, ethnicity, and ings indicate that childrens abilities to form warm,
socioeconomic status (Saft & Pianta, 2000), are stable trusting, and low-conict relationships with teachers in
from kindergarten to second grade (Birch & Ladd, the early elementary years are salient markers of chil-
1997; Pianta et al., 1995), and map conceptually to chil- drens adaptation to the social environment and, as
drens reports of relationships with teachers (Lynch & such, may forecast academic success, at least through
Hamre and Pianta 627

the lower grades, consistent with Entwisles (Entwisle & who are able to form strong and positive relationships
Hayduk, 1988) argument on the importance of social ad- with teachers may be more likely to have occasional
aptation in early elementary school. misbehavior overlooked due to teachers understand-
Much less is known about associations between ing of their individual experiences and needs, com-
teacherchild relationships and childrens outcomes pared with children who have more difculty forming
in upper elementary and middle school. Despite the strong relationships with teachers and are therefore
suggestion that teacherchild relationships may be more isolated and less well known. Grades and test
more important to elementary school students than to scores, on the other hand, are more objective mea-
middle school students (Lynch & Cicchetti, 1997), it sures and are less likely to be inuenced by teacher
was expected that, as Entwisle and Hayduk (1988) perceptions, although they are nonetheless open to
suggest, childrens adaptation to the social context of the inuence of the quality of the teacherchild rela-
the early school years, as measured by the quality of tionship. Based on previous work demonstrating the
teacherchild relationships, would account for a sig- negative impact of conict on childrens outcomes,
nicant portion of childrens school performance in however (Amato & Keith, 1991; Campbell, 1994; Cum-
upper elementary and middle school. Given the high mings, Iannotti, & Zahn-Waxler, 1985), it was expected
level of stability in childrens academic and behav- that teachers experience of conict in relationships
ioral performance, the associations between early so- with children would be a particularly strong indicator
cial adjustment and later performance were thought of childrens later academic and behavioral difculties.
to be largely mediated by early performance. This An additional goal of this study was to investigate
study examined these hypotheses by following a sub- possible moderators of the association between early
sample of children originally assessed at school entry to social adjustment and later academic and behavioral
determine the extent to which kindergarten teachers re- performance. Child gender, ethnicity, verbal abilities,
lationships with children, as perceived by the teachers, and early behavioral problems are consistently identi-
would predict academic and behavioral outcomes in ed as reliable predictors of later school problems (e.g.,
lower elementary (grades 14), upper elementary Pianta & McCoy, 1997; Tramontana, Hooper, & Selzer,
(grades 5 and 6), and middle school (grades 7 and 8). 1988). Children at risk of school failure may have the
Specically, outcomes included language arts and most to gain, or lose, through their ability to adapt to
math grades, standardized test scores, work-habit the social environment of the classroom. Therefore, it
marks, and disciplinary records. It was hypothesized was expected that the association between early
that childrens ability to form and maintain styles teacherchild relationships and later school perfor-
of coping with the social environment in early years of mance would be strongest for those children who were
schooling, as measured by kindergarten teachers per- at the highest risk for academic and behavioral prob-
ceptions of relationships, would maintain moderate lems. Specically, it was hypothesized that a greater
associations with outcome measures through eighth portion of the variance in the performance of boys, Af-
grade, and would contribute uniquely to their predic- rican American students, children with low verbal
tion after controlling for early measures of child intelli- abilities, and/or children with signicant early behav-
gence and behavior. It was anticipated that by upper ioral problems would be predicted by kindergarten
elementary and middle school, these hypothesized as- teachers ratings of relationship quality.
sociations would be mediated by childrens perfor-
mance in lower elementary school.
With regard to the two outcome domains assessed in METHODS
this study, it was expected that kindergarten teachers
Participants
perceptions of the quality of teacherchild relation-
ships would be more strongly associated with behav- The sample of 179 children was a subset of an origi-
ioral than with academic outcomes for several rea- nal group of 436 children consisting of the entire enter-
sons. First, the quality of teacherchild relationships ing kindergarten class of 19881989 in a small city
is, in part, a measure of social adjustment, and thus school district. The subsample included only those
behavioral outcomes are more proximal to this predic- children who remained in the district through eighth
tor. Second, the behavioral outcomes assessed in this grade. There were no differences between the subsam-
study were inuenced directly by teachers perceptions ple and the rest of the group in mothers level of edu-
of children. Work-habit scores reected teachers sub- cation, IQ scores, or gender, p  .05. There was, how-
jective experience of childrens social adjustment and ever, a higher percentage of African American students
discipline infractions were typically instigated by in the subsample than in the group eliminated from the
teachers responses to student misbehavior. Children original sample (40% versus 28%), 2(1, N  327) 
628 Child Development

5.34, p  .05. Parents consented to all aspects of the composite, which consists of conduct, learning, and
data collection. Information was also obtained from shy/anxious problems, was used. Internal consistency
childrens kindergarten teachers (n  26) and school reliabilities on the Behavior Problems composite ex-
records through eighth grade. ceed .90 (Hightower et al., 1986). The TCRS has been
The nal sample contained 91 boys and 88 girls. Of employed as an indicator of classroom adjustment,
these students, 108 (60%) were White and the remaining correlating moderately with other behavior checklists,
71 (40%) were African American. Mothers education, as well as grades and performance on standardized
which was only available for 147 of the cases, was dis- tests (Trickett, McBride-Chang, & Putman, 1994).
tributed as follows: graduate work, 8%; college gradu- Student Teacher Relationship Scale. Kindergarten
ate, 14%; some college, 16%; high school graduate, 35%; teachers also completed the StudentTeacher Rela-
some high school, 22%; and no high school, 5%. tionship Scale (STRS; Pianta, 1992b) in May of that
The kindergarten teachers in the sample had an av- school year. It is a 28-item rating scale, using a Likert-
erage of 10.3 years of teaching experience (range  0 type format, designed to assess teachers perceptions
20). There were two African American kindergarten of their relationship with a particular student. Kinder-
teachers; the remainder were White. garten teachers completed a separate STRS for each of
the children in their classrooms. The scale was admin-
istered at the end of the school year to ensure that
Procedure
teachers had the longest time possible in which to form
All subjects were administered a school screening an impression of their relationship with each child. The
battery as they entered kindergarten. Among the items on this scale were based on a previous 16-item
screening measures were tests of cognitive develop- version (Pianta & Nimetz, 1991) developed from at-
ment. Teacher questionnaires were administered during tachment theory, the attachment Q-set (Waters &
May of the kindergarten year. Extensive longitudinal ac- Deane, 1985), and a review of literature on teacher
ademic and behavior data were collected from school child interactions. The items were written to assess a
records, including math and language arts grades, stan- teachers feelings and beliefs about her relationship
dardized test scores, work-habit marks, and disciplin- with a student, and her feelings and beliefs about the
ary records. Children attended three schools during students behavior toward her. This scale has been
the time period over which data were collected: lower used extensively in studies of preschool- and elemen-
elementary (kindergarten through fourth grade); up- tary-age children (e.g., Birch & Ladd, 1997, 1998;
per elementary (fth and sixth grades); and middle Howes & Hamilton, 1992; Howes & Richie, 1999).
school (seventh and eighth grades). Data were col- In an earlier study consisting of 436 children, a three-
lected in the spring of the childrens eighth-grade year, factor solution accounted for 60% of the total vari-
so outcomes for eighth grade include only records ance. These three factors were labeled Conict, Close-
from the rst semester. ness, and Dependency. The Conict scale accounted
Cognitive development. An estimate of cognitive de- for 30.4% of the variance and contained such items as
velopment at kindergarten entry was obtained through This child easily becomes angry at me and This
administration of the vocabulary subtest of the Stan- child and I always seem to be struggling with each
ford-Binet Intelligence ScaleRevised, Fourth Edition other. The coefcient  was .93 for the 12 items with
(SB-FE; Thorndike, Hagen, & Sattler, 1986). The rst 14 loadings of .40 or greater on this factor. The Closeness
items on this test require the child to name or give the factor accounted for 14.1% of the variance. It contained
most pertinent detail of a picture. The remaining items items such as I share an affectionate, warm relation-
ask the child for an oral explanation of each word. Ac- ship with this child, If upset, this child will seek com-
cording to the test manual, vocabulary is a reliable sub- fort from me, and This child spontaneously shares
test, r  .87, and correlates highly with the composite information about him/herself. The  for the 11-item
score, r  .81. Closeness scale was .86. The four-item Dependency
TeacherChild Rating Scale. The TeacherChild Rating factor accounted for 4.3% of the variance and in-
Scale (TCRS; Hightower et al., 1986) was collected from cluded items such as This child is overly dependent
kindergarten teachers in May of that school year. It is a on me and This child reacts strongly to separation
38-item teacher-reported rating scale of childrens from me. The coefcient  for this scale was .68.
classroom behavior. The items load on seven factor- In accordance with hypotheses regarding the im-
based subscales: conduct problems, learning problems, portance of the negative components of the teacher
shy/anxious problems, frustration tolerance, work child relationship in predicting later outcomes, the
habits, assertive social skills, and peer sociability. For Conict and Dependency scores were added together
the purposes of this study, the Behavior Problems to form a Relational Negativity score.
Hamre and Pianta 629

Academic performance. Grades were collected each the type of school attended by the children. With the
year, in rst through eighth grades. In second grade, exception of disciplinary infractions, the composites
math and language arts grades were recorded dichoto- were created by taking the average of scores for each
mously as either On/Above Level (2) or Below year they were available. In calculating composite math
Level (1). In rst grade and third through eighth and reading grades for lower elementary school,
grades, students received standard letter grades (A  second-grade scores were not included because they
5; B  4; C  3; D  2; F  1) in math and language arts. were represented dichotomously rather than by letter
Iowa Test of Basic Skills. School personnel routinely grades and included very little variance. To produce
administered the Iowa Test of Basic Skills (ITBS; more normal distributions for the total number of disci-
Hieronymus & Hoover, 1978). Students took the exam plinary infractions, a log transformation was completed.
in a group format each year in the spring of second Our rst step in the data analysis was to examine
through fth grades. The ITBS is a nationally normed bivariate associations between kindergarten teachers
achievement test measuring prociency in the areas of ratings of the quality of their relationship with each
word analysis, vocabulary, reading, language, work child and the childs academic and behavioral per-
study, mathematics, listening, social studies, science, formance through eighth grade. Pearson product-
and writing skills (Hambleton, Hieronymus, & Hoover, moment correlations were calculated between the
1987). Standard scores for the comprehensive total Closeness, Conict, and Dependency scales of the STRS
score were used. The ITBS has internal consistency and and math and language arts grade composites, stan-
equivalent-form reliabilities in the mid-.80s to low .90s dardized test scores, work-habit marks, and total
for the area and comprehensive scores (Salvia & Yssel- number of disciplinary infractions for lower and up-
dyke, 1985). per elementary and middle school.
Work habits. Work-habit behaviors were recorded Next, a hierarchical regression procedure was com-
by teachers in each grade after kindergarten (18). pleted for each outcome measure to assess the extent to
Teachers reports of predetermined positive and neg- which teacher ratings of the quality of their relation-
ative descriptors were tallied from report cards. Only ship with each child in kindergarten contributed
behaviors that pertained to the childs work habits in uniquely, beyond covariates, as well as the extent to
math and language arts were included in these tallies. which this association was mediated by performance
Although these reports varied by grade, they gener- in that area during the previous epoch (e.g., early ele-
ally focused on behaviors such as listening, participa- mentary). In the rst step of each regression, a set of
tion, compliance, cooperation, and study habits. covariates from the kindergarten year was entered into
Disciplinary record. The school district maintained the model, including verbal IQ scores, gender, ethnic-
detailed records on each students disciplinary history. ity, and teacher ratings of child behavior (TCRS). Rela-
Type of infraction and disciplinary action taken were re- tional Negativity scores from the STRS completed in
corded. There was a wide variety of possible infractions spring of the kindergarten year were entered in the sec-
ranging from less severe (e.g., unexcused absence) to ond step. In the third step, interactions between the
very severe (e.g., use of weapon or sale of drugs). Of the childteacher relationship and each of the covariates
1,930 total disciplinary infractions recorded for this sam- were entered separately; these were only retained if
ple between rst and eighth grade, the most frequent they added signicantly to the model. In the last step,
were deance of school authority (24%), classroom dis- childrens previous performance on the dependent
ruption (21%), inammatory actions (13%), disruption variable was entered when applicable to test the medi-
of the educational process (12%), ghting (6%), unex- ation hypotheses. In the nal model, standardized 
cused absences (6%), disruptive behavior on the school values were used to interpret the relative contribution
bus (5%), and abusive language (4%). Variables used in of each predictor. Finally, an identical model was used
this study included the total number of disciplinary in- in a logistic regression to predict probability of suspen-
fractions students had in each grade and whether or not sion in rst through eighth grade.
they were ever suspended.
RESULTS
Data Analysis
Because of our hypotheses regarding gender differ-
Children attended three types of schools. In grades ences, as well as signicant differences between boys
K through 4, they attended lower elementary school; in and girls in both predictor and outcome variables that
grades 5 and 6, upper elementary school; and in grades were revealed through a series of ANOVAs, all de-
7 and 8, middle school. All outcome data were com- scriptive and correlational information is presented
posited within these three epochs corresponding to by gender. Descriptive information on predictor and
630 Child Development

outcome measures is presented in Table 1. ANOVAs High levels of perceived conict between a teacher
revealed that kindergarten teachers reported closer and her male students in kindergarten, however,
and less conictual relationships with girls than with were related to poorer math and reading grades in
boys. Girls and boys had equivalent test scores in lower each epoch. Despite a few signicant correlations be-
and upper elementary school, but girls consistently had tween teacher ratings of the quality of their relation-
higher letter grades as well as fewer discipline infrac- ships with girls and later academic performance, the
tions. Girls received signicantly more positive work- associations were generally weaker than for the boys
habit marks in lower elementary school than did boys, in the sample.
but the differences between boys and girls work habits Due to the infrequency of disciplinary infractions in
in upper elementary and middle school were not signif- lower elementary school, positive work-habit marks
icant. There were moderate correlations between kin- served as the best indicator of early behavioral adjust-
dergarten behavior ratings (TCRS Behavior Problems ment. In upper elementary and middle school the vari-
composite) and ratings of the quality of the teacher ability, seriousness, and expected repercussions of dis-
child relationship in kindergarten (STRS): Conict scale: ciplinary infractions made them particularly interesting
r(178) .63, p  .01; Closeness scale: r(178)  .45, p  indicators of later behavioral functioning. Both boys
.01; and Dependency scale: r(178)  .26, p  .01. and girls who were reported to have high levels of con-
Correlations between the STRS factor scores and stu- ict with their kindergarten teachers tended to have
dent grade and test performance by epoch revealed that fewer positive work-habit marks in elementary school
teachers perceptions of high conict and dependency and more discipline infractions in upper elementary
were signicantly related to poor academic outcomes school (see Table 3). These associations remained sig-
for boys throughout the period from rst through nicant for boys in middle school.
eighth grade for which scores were available (see Table An interesting pattern was evident in examining the
2). For boys, teacher ratings of dependency in kinder- associations between teacher-perceived closeness and
garten were more consistently related to academic out- dependency in kindergarten and later behavior out-
comes in elementary school than in middle school. comes. Boys viewed as dependent in kindergarten re-

Table 1 Mean Scores and SDs on Kindergarten Predictor Variables and Outcome Composites by Epoch and Gender

Boys Girls

M SD M SD F value

Kindergarten predictor variables


Verbal IQ 51.08 7.22 49.85 6.99 1.32
TCRS Behavior Problems 25.06 9.02 22.66 7.86 3.55
STRS
Closeness 41.99 7.21 45.05 6.51 8.83**
Conflict 24.27 9.86 21.02 9.45 5.07*
Dependency 7.96 3.17 7.82 2.91 .09
Outcome composites (grade levels)
Math and reading grades
Lower elementary (1, 34) 7.97 1.36 8.35 1.22 3.69
Upper elementary (56) 7.11 2.03 7.77 1.63 5.62*
Middle school (78) 6.26 2.03 6.98 2.04 5.41*
ITBS scores (grade level)
Lower elementary (24) 108.53 14.30 109.12 12.28 .08
Upper elementary (5) 129.40 20.45 131.16 15.78 .36
No. of positive work habits (grade level)
Lower elementary (14) 8.64 2.04 9.76 1.15 20.27**
Upper elementary (56) 1.98 2.11 2.49 2.01 2.68
Middle school (7) 1.10 2.28 1.86 2.85 3.63
No. of disciplinary infractions (grade level)
Lower elementary (14) .26 .84 .05 .24 5.13*
Upper elementary (56) 2.91 4.86 .74 1.87 15.33**
Middle school (78) 6.28 9.34 1.98 4.24 15.42**

Note: ITBS  Iowa Test of Basic Skills; STRS  StudentTeacher Relationship Scale; TCRS  TeacherChild Rating Scale.
* p  .05; ** p  .01.
Hamre and Pianta 631

Table 2 Correlations between STRS Factor Scores and Aca- Table 3 Correlations between STRS Factor Scores and Behavioral
demic Outcomes by Epoch and Gender Outcomes by Epoch and Gender

Lower Upper Middle Lower Upper Middle


Elementary Elementary School Elementary Elementary School
(Grades 14) (Grades 56) (Grades 78) (Grades 14) (Grades 56) (Grades 78)

Math and Language Arts Grades Positive Work-Habit Ratings


Closeness Closeness
Boys .05 .11 .17 Boys .12 .02 .15
Girls .19 .10 .15 Girls .32** .07 .09
Conflict Conflict
Boys .29** .26** .22* Boys .39** .21 .24*
Girls .22** .21* .14 Girls .50** .15 .20
Dependency Dependency
Boys .30** .16 .18 Boys .32** .09 .19
Girls .15 .02 .01 Girls .18 .04 .04
ITBS Total No. of Disciplinary Infractionsa
Closeness Closeness
Boys .01 .05 Boys .08 .03
Girls .07 .12 Girls .23* .16
Conflict Conflict
Boys .29** .37** Boys .43** .35**
Girls .17 .21 Girls .26* .16
Dependency Dependency
Boys .25* .29* Boys .25* .29**
Girls .09 .15 Girls .04 .02

Note: ITBS (Iowa Test of Basic Skills) scores were not available for Note: Due to the infrequency of discipline infractions in lower ele-
rst and sixth through eighth grades. Math and language arts mentary school, correlations for that epoch were not completed.
grade composites for lower elementary school are included for Log transformations were completed on the total number of disci-
rst, third, and fourth grades; second grade scores were not avail- pline infractions in upper elementary and middle school to produce
able. Middle school math and language arts grade composites in- more normal distributions of those variables. STRS  Student
clude grades through the rst half of eighth grade. STRS  Teacher Relationship Scale.
StudentTeacher Relationship Scale. a Because disciplinary infractions were not prevalent in lower ele-

* p  .05; ** p  .01. mentary school, the correlational data is not given here.
* p  .05; ** p  .01.

ceived fewer positive work-habit marks from teachers


in lower elementary school and continued to have be- garten teachers perceptions of Relational Negativity
havioral difculties in upper elementary and middle with a child, beyond measures of verbal intelligence
school, as evidenced by higher rates of discipline prob- and teacher-rated classroom problem behavior. The re-
lems. Teachers ratings of girls dependency in kinder- sults of these analyses are presented in Table 4. The
garten were not signicantly related to any later be- amount of variance explained by each successive block
havioral outcome measures. Girls who had a close of predictors is indicated in the tables by the change in
relationship with their kindergarten teacher, however, R2. The standardized  coefcients presented in this
tended to have more positive work habits in lower ele- table represent the relative signicance of variables
mentary school, as well as fewer disciplinary problems within each block and were the values produced in the
in upper elementary school. In contrast, kindergarten nal analysis, with all variables in the model.
teachers perceptions of closeness in their relationships Results predicting language arts and math grades in
with boys were not related to boys behavioral adjust- lower elementary school demonstrated that after cova-
ment in elementary or middle school. riates, Relational Negativity accounted for a small but
To test hypotheses regarding the association be- signicant proportion of variance. Interactions be-
tween negative components of the teacherchild rela- tween Relational Negativity and each of the other pre-
tionship and later school outcomes, the Relational dictors were not signicant; the results were therefore
Negativity variable, a composite of Conict and De- not included in the table. By upper elementary and
pendency, was used in the regression models. The rst middle school, Relational Negativity did not explain a
two sets of regression analyses examined academic signicant amount of variance in math and language
outcomes and assessed the predictive power of kinder- arts grades, beyond the covariates. Teacher-rated be-
632 Child Development

Table 4 Regression Analyses: Predicting Academic Outcomes in Lower Elementary, Upper Elementary, and Middle School from
Teacher Ratings of Negativity in Relationships with Children and Earlier Performance

Math and Language Arts Grade Composites ITBS

Lower Upper Middle Lower Upper


Elementary Elementary School Elementary Elementary
(n  178) (n  177) (n  173) (n  165) (n  149)

Block R2  R2  R2  R2  R2 

1. Covariates .22** .33** .26** .36** .40**


Gender .16* .13* .14* .07 .06
Ethnicity .15 .19** .15* .23** .00
Verbal IQ .31** .20** .20* .44** .10*
TCRS Behavior Problems composite .06 .04 .05 .12 .04
2. Relational Negativity (STRS) .03** .23** .01 .01 .00 .01 .03* .20* .03* .05
3. Lower elementary school performance .12** .41** .10** .37** .42** .84**
Total R2 .25 .45 .36 .39 .85

Note: Standardized  coefcients represent the relative signicance of variables within each block and were the values produced in the
nal analysis, with all variables included in the model. ITBS  Iowa Test of Basic Skills; TCRS  TeacherChild Rating Scale; STRS 
StudentTeacher Relationship Scale.
* p  .05; ** p  .01.

havior problems (TCRS Behavior Problems composite) than lower elementary performance, as the mediating
did not predict childrens math and language arts variable, which was used in the rest of the models.
grades either before or after Relational Negativity was After controlling for gender, ethnicity, verbal IQ,
entered into these models. and behavior problem ratings (TCRS Behavior Prob-
Analyses predicting composite ITBS scores revealed lems composite), children with more negative rela-
similar ndings. The Relational Negativity scores ac- tionships with their kindergarten teachers tended to
counted for a small, but signicant, portion of the vari- have fewer positive work-habit marks in lower ele-
ance in ITBS test scores in lower elementary school, be- mentary school than did their peers with more posi-
yond the covariates (see Table 4). As in the analysis of tive relationships.
grades, interactions between teacher-child negativity An analysis of interaction terms lent partial sup-
and each of the other predictors were not signicant. port to the hypothesis that relationship quality may
Relational Negativity, however, continued to predict be more important in predicting outcomes for stu-
unique variance in standardized test scores in upper el- dents at higher risk of later problems than for those
ementary school, above and beyond the covariates. The children at low risk. For children in the top third of
addition of lower elementary school performance as a kindergarten teacher behavior problem ratings, Rela-
predictor revealed that the variance in upper elemen- tional Negativity was a strong predictor of work-
tary school standardized test scores explained by kin- habit marks in lower elementary school, r(178) 
dergarten teachers ratings of their relationships with .52, p  .001. The association between Relational
children was mediated by childrens earlier perfor- Negativity and work-habit ratings was not signicant
mance on the ITBS. As in the grade analyses, kindergar- for those children with fewer kindergarten behavior
ten teachers ratings of child behavior did not account problems. These results were also obtained control-
for a signicant portion of variance in ITBS scores. ling for the covariates. Examination of standardized 
In a series of regression analyses predicting behav- coefcients in the nal models revealed that the prob-
ioral outcomes through eighth grade, Relational Nega- lem behavior ratings dropped out as signicant pre-
tivity contributed unique variance into upper elemen- dictors of work habits when Relational Negativity
tary and middle school. These results are presented in was added to each model.
Table 5 and are organized similarly to those described Although there was not a main effect for Relational
above. Because discipline infractions were not preva- Negativity in predicting work-habit ratings in upper
lent in lower elementary school, they were not used as elementary or middle school, the interaction between
an outcome measure. Therefore, the test for mediation Relational Negativity and kindergarten teacher-rated
in predicting childrens disciplinary performance uti- behavior problems remained signicant in upper ele-
lized upper elementary discipline infractions, rather mentary school. Once again, there was only a signi-
Hamre and Pianta 633

Table 5 Regression Analyses: Predicting Behavioral Outcomes in Lower Elementary, Upper Elementary, and Middle School from
Teacher Ratings of Negativity in Relationships with Children and Earlier Performance

Positive Work Habits No. of Disciplinary Infractionsa

Lower Upper Middle Upper Middle


Elementary Elementary School Elementary School
(n  178) (n  175) (n  170) (n  179) (n  178)

Block R2  R2  R2  R2  R2 

1. Covariates .30** .18** .20** .29** .30**


Gender .30** .04 .11 .01 .01
Ethnicity .16* .11 .11 .19 .10
Verbal IQ .20* .22* .23* .17 .17**
TCRS Behavior Problems composite .50 .24 .34 .39 .13
2. Relational Negativity (STRS) .05** .34 .00 .30 .00 .30 .03* .06 .01 .01
3. STRS interactions .04** .03* .00 .05** .04*
STRS TCRS 1.11** .41 .45 .91** .17
STRS Gender .48* .10
4. Performance in previous epoch .11** .42** .01 .13 .27** .66**
Total R2 .39 .32 .21 .37 .62

Note: Standardized  coefcients represent the relative signicance of variables within each block and were the values produced in the
nal analyses, with all variables included in the model. Earlier performance reects childrens performance in lower elementary school for
work-habit analyses and their upper elementary performance for predicting later disciplinary infractions. STRS  StudentTeacher Rela-
tionship Scale; TCRS  TeacherChild Rating Scale.
a Because disciplinary infractions were not prevalent in lower elementary school, they were not used as an outcome measure.

* p  .05; ** p  .01.

cant relationship between teacher-rated Relational previous analyses, the association between Relational
Negativity and work-habit ratings for those children Negativity scores and disciplinary infractions in mid-
in the top third of teacher-rated behavior problems, dle school was mediated by performance in the previ-
r(178)  .34, p  .01. When lower elementary school ous epoch, in this case childrens disciplinary record
work habits were entered into the model predicting up- from upper elementary school.
per elementary work habits, however, the Relational Finally, a logistic regression was completed to pre-
Negativity interaction was no longer signicant, indi- dict the likelihood of suspension at any time during
cating that the signicant association between Rela- grades K8, using the model presented above. Seventy-
tional Negativity and upper elementary performance seven of the students (43%) were suspended at least
was mediated by lower elementary school performance. once between kindergarten and eighth grade. There
Relational Negativity in kindergarten also ac- was a good model t on the basis of the covariates
counted for a signicant proportion of variance in the alone, 2(4, N  176)  48.45, p  .001. The model was
number of disciplinary infractions accumulated by improved signicantly with the addition of Relational
children in upper elementary school, beyond the con- Negativity, 2(1, N  176)  4.09, p  .05, and the inter-
tribution of all covariates (see Table 5). Consistent with actions between Relational Negativity and two of the
hypotheses and the ndings for work-habit outcomes, covariates, verbal IQ and kindergarten teacher-rated
follow-up analyses revealed that the association be- behavior problems, 2(2, N  176)  13.71, p  .01.
tween Relational Negativity in kindergarten and later Further examination revealed that, as in earlier
disciplinary problems was strongest for those children analyses, having high levels of Relational Negativity
with the most behavior problems in kindergarten. Ad- with a kindergarten teacher was a more inuential
ditionally, there was a signicant interaction between predictor of later outcomes for children in the top
Relational Negativity and gender in predicting the to- third of kindergarten behavior problem ratings. Thus,
tal number of disciplinary infractions in upper elemen- for children who were in the bottom two thirds of be-
tary school. Kindergarten teachers ratings of the qual- havior problem ratings in kindergarten, suspended
ity of the relationship were more strongly associated and nonsuspended children had similar levels of Rela-
with boys upper elementary school disciplinary per- tional Negativity in kindergarten. However, there was a
formance than with that of the girls in the sample. As in signicant difference between suspended and nonsus-
634 Child Development

pended childrens Relational Negativity scores within marked by conict and overdependency, emerged as
the group of children with the most kindergarten behav- a signicant predictor of a wide range of academic
ior problems, F(1, 60)  5.23, p  .05. Kindergarten and behavioral outcomes, even when controlling for
teachers of suspended children within this group re- other early indicators of these outcomes. This nding
ported more negative relationships than did teachers of is consistent with a series of studies (Birch & Ladd,
nonsuspended students. 1997; Howes et al., 1994; Pianta & Nimetz, 1991; Pi-
In contrast to the hypotheses, Relational Negativity anta et al., 1995).
was a more inuential predictor of suspension out- Particularly for boys, kindergarten teachers per-
comes for children with above-average verbal IQ ceptions of conict and overdependency were signif-
scores than for those with below-average IQ scores. icantly correlated with academic outcomes through-
The majority of children who had IQ scores below the out elementary school and into middle school. This
mean (58%) were suspended regardless of their kin- nding linking social processes in kindergarten with
dergarten teachers perceptions about the level of neg- later academic competence is consistent with other
ativity in the teacherchild relationship. There was no work (Birch & Ladd, 1997; Pianta & Nimetz, 1991; Pi-
signicant difference between the level of Relational anta et al., 1995) and suggests that the quality of
Negativity in kindergarten for suspended and non- childteacher relationships may reect the extent to
suspended children in this low-IQ group, F(1, 89)  which children are able to engage the instructional re-
1.48, p  .23. Within the group of children with above- sources present in classrooms (Entwisle & Hayduk,
average IQ, however, those who were suspended 1988). In the present study, Relational Negativity pre-
(30%) tended to have more negativity in their relation- dicted standardized test scores in upper elementary
ship with kindergarten teachers than did their non- school, but, as hypothesized, this association was me-
suspended peers, F(1, 87)  24.74, p  .001. In the nal diated by childrens performance on these tests in
logistic regression model, 49 of the 77 suspended stu- lower elementary school, again suggesting the impor-
dents (63.64%) were correctly identied. tance of social processes in the early school years. It is
also interesting to note that there were no signicant
associations between teacher-rated behavior and
DISCUSSION
either measure of academic outcome, indicating that
The results suggest that early teacherchild relation- negativity in the teacherchild relationship was a
ships, as experienced and described in kindergarten more salient marker of the socioemotional processes
by teachers, are unique predictors of academic and involved in academic performance than was a
behavioral outcomes in early elementary school, with teacher-rated measure of the childs misbehavior.
mediated effects through eighth grade. Kindergarten As hypothesized, this study suggests that the qual-
teachers reports of negativity in relation to students ity of teacherchild relationships is a stronger predic-
uniquely predicted student grades, standardized test tor of behavioral than of academic outcomes. Rela-
scores, and work habits through lower elementary tional Negativity accounted for greater increments of
school. Relational Negativity continued to uniquely explained variance for the behavioral outcomes than
predict behavioral outcomes into upper elementary for the academic outcomes. Although previous work
and middle school, particularly for those students at established associations between teacherchild rela-
greatest risk of behavior difcultiesspecically tionships and socioemotional and behavioral func-
those with early behavior problems and boys gener- tioning through second grade (Birch & Ladd, 1997;
ally. However, further tests revealed that the associa- Howes et al., 1994; Pianta et al., 1995), the present
tions between Relational Negativity and behavior study provides evidence of prediction for much
outcomes in upper elementary and middle school longer term behavioral outcomes. Zero-order correla-
were mediated by childrens earlier performance in tions suggested that negative relational styles appear
those domains. The results have implications for the- to be especially strong predictors of subsequent disci-
ories of the determinants of school success, the role of plinary problems for boys. In regressions controlling
adultchild relationships in development, and a for main effects of gender, ethnicity, verbal cognitive
range of early intervention and prevention efforts. abilities, and problem behaviors, teacherchild Rela-
As suggested by Entwisle and Hayduk (1988), this tional Negativity added signicantly to the predic-
study provides evidence that beyond cognitive func- tion of disciplinary performance in upper elementary
tioning and classroom behavior, childrens ability to school and middle school, although the ndings for
form relationships with their teachers forecasts later middle school outcomes suggest that this association
academic and behavioral adjustment in school. More was moderated by other factors, as discussed below.
specically, negativity in teacherchild relationships, It is important to note that these ndings do not per-
Hamre and Pianta 635

tain simply to an accumulation of minor infractions, tionship quality and later outcomes based on matches
as Relational Negativity scores also added to the pre- between teacher and child ethnicity. There is some ev-
diction of suspension, a relatively severe penalty. idence, however, to suggest that this is an area that
In support of the literature suggesting that ethnic- deserves attention in future work, as ethnic match be-
ity, gender, and verbal abilities play a role in increas- tween teacher and child appears to be associated with
ing a childs risk for problems in school (e.g., Pianta & more positive relationships (Saft & Pianta, 2000).
McCoy, 1997; Tramontana et al., 1988), within this In contrast to hypotheses, Relational Negativity
sample, boys, African American students, and chil- was a better predictor of suspension for children with
dren with low verbal intelligence scores in kindergar- above-average than for those with below-average
ten were more likely to have academic and behavioral verbal IQ scores. This unexpected nding may be re-
problems through eighth grade. Most of these associ- lated, in part, to the fact that so many children with
ations between risk factors and outcomes in upper el- below-average verbal IQ scores were suspended at
ementary and middle school were mediated by chil- some point between rst and eighth grade, making it
drens standing on corresponding outcomes in lower difcult for Relational Negativity in kindergarten to
elementary school. have the sensitivity necessary to distinguish between
As noted earlier, however, one goal of this study suspended and nonsuspended children. This nding
was to examine the hypothesis that teachers relation- also provides evidence that, even for children for
ships with children may be particularly important in whom generally positive school performance is pre-
predicting outcomes for children at the highest risk of dicted, the kindergarten teacherchild relationship is
school failure, following the general hypothesis that an important indicator of early social adaptation and
vulnerable children will be more inuenced by experi- a marker of later behavioral outcomes.
ence in school contexts (see Entwisle & Alexander, The results of this study have important implica-
1998). The results lend some support to this hypothe- tions for early intervention efforts. Lochman and col-
sis. Although there was no evidence of moderation in leagues (1995) suggest that the most useful markers for
the analyses of academic outcomes, in the models pre- prevention programs are those that measure processes
dicting behavior outcomes (work-habit ratings, disci- amenable to intervention. These markers may measure
plinary infractions, and chance of suspension), the inter- processes that directly cause negative outcomes, or
action between teachers ratings of problem behavior in they may indicate complex processes that mediate the
kindergarten and their ratings of Relational Negativity development of problem behaviors (Cichetti & Rich-
was consistently signicant. Follow-up analyses sug- ters, 1993; Rutter, 1990). The current study provides
gested that the level of negativity in childrens relation- strong, preliminary evidence that the quality of teacher
ships with kindergarten teachers was a stronger predic- child relationships is one such early marker of later ac-
tor of work-habit ratings and disciplinary infractions ademic and behavioral difculties, available as early as
for the children that teachers also rated as having the kindergarten. Preventive intervention programs that
highest levels of problem behavior. Children rated as build supportive teacherchild relationships for all
having fewer behavior problems in kindergarten did children, as well as interventions targeted at improv-
well irrespective of the level of negativity in their rela- ing specic teacherchild relationships (Pianta, 1999),
tionship with their kindergarten teacher. The results may hold promise for enhancing school outcomes by
suggest that those children who, despite signicant be- means of their inuence on the social aspects of school-
havior problems, were able to develop relationships ing emphasized by Entwisle and Hayduk (1988) and
with kindergarten teachers marked by low levels of others (e.g., Birch & Ladd, 1997; Howes et al., 1994; Pi-
negativity, were in turn more likely to avoid future be- anta et al., 1995). Additionally, the results of this study
havioral difculties than were their peers who had high suggest possible means for identifying the targets of
negativity ratings. these intervention efforts. Children who not only have
The interaction between Relational Negativity in poor relationships with their teachers but also have
kindergarten and ethnicity was not a signicant pre- signicant teacher-rated behavior problems in the
dictor of any of the outcomes measured in this study, early years of schooling appear to be a particular group
despite the fact that the teachers within this sample that may benet.
tended to report higher levels of negativity in their re- It is possible to speculate briey on processes that
lationships with African American students than in account for why the relationships that children de-
their relationships with White students. Because all velop with their teachers early in school forecast such
but two of the kindergarten teachers in this sample a wide range of long-term school outcomes. Recent
were White, it was not possible to examine differ- observational studies of kindergarten classrooms in-
ences in the association between teacherchild rela- dicate that teachers sensitive behavior toward chil-
636 Child Development

dren (e.g., responding in a timely fashion, anticipa- conict and dependency, appear to be especially im-
tion of student needs and emotions), as well as the portant in the prediction of later school outcomes.
frequency of teacher feedback to the child, provide This study also suggests that boys and girls may ben-
strong supports for childrens academic and social et differently from particular aspects of teacher
competence in the classroom setting (Bryant, Clifford, child relationships; boys with low levels of conict
& Feinberg, 1991; Meyer, Waldrop, Hastings, & Linn, and dependency with kindergarten teachers had bet-
1993; Pianta, Laparo, Payne, Cox, & Bradley, 2000) ter long-term outcomes, whereas girls with close rela-
across a wide range of child characteristics. Thus, the tionships in kindergarten had fewer behavioral dif-
extent to which children can access the instructional culties in later years. Lastly, these ndings provide
and socialization resources of the classroom environ- evidence that children with signicant behavior prob-
ment may be in part predicated on teacherchild in- lems in the early years of school, who are nevertheless
teractions and, over time, relationships, a possibility able to form relationships with teachers that are low
that is consistent with Entwisle and Hayduks (1988) in conict and dependency, may be less likely to have
view of the classroom as a social context. continuing behavior problems compared with their
One of the most notable limitations of this study was peers who, despite similar levels of behavior prob-
the absence of repeated measures of the teacherchild lems, are less able to adapt to the social environment
relationship as children progressed through school. Fu- of the classroom. Although the ndings of this study
ture work in this area would benet greatly from such were correlational, not causal, it is clear that the pro-
data. Additionally, the amount of variance in school out- cesses related to the development and inuences of
comes explained by kindergarten measures of teacher teacherchild relationships are important compo-
child relationships in this study was relatively small. nents of childrens success in school. These relational
The regression models constituted relatively strict tests, processes can be considered an essential component
however, controlling for cognitive ability and behavior of the school environment, and may provide a useful
ratings completed by the same teacher who rated rela- focus for research, intervention, and prevention in
tionship quality, at the same time. Furthermore, regres- pathways of risk and school outcomes.
sion models predicted outcomes over an 8-year period.
Thus, although the overall amount of variance ex-
plained is small, the signicant ndings indicate that ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
attention to the impact of teacherchild relationship The work reported herein was supported, in part, un-
quality is warranted. der the Educational Research and Development Cen-
As others have articulated (e.g., Birch and Ladd, ters Program, PR/Award Number R307A60004, as
1998), understanding of teacherchild relationships administered by the Ofce of Educational Research
and their inuence on child development also re- and Improvement, U.S. Department of Education.
quires a focus on childrens perceptions of relation- However, the contents do not necessarily represent
ships with teachers. This study did not consider how the positions or policies of the National Institute on
childrens attitudes toward their teachers affected Early Childhood Development and Education, the
their school experiences. Birchs (1997) examination Ofce of Educational Research and Improvement, or
of childrens perceptions of the relationship reports the U.S. Department of Education, and endorsement
correlations with teachers ratings ranging from .29 to by the federal government should not be assumed.
.71, suggesting a fair degree of concordance in per-
ceptions. Recently, Essex and Armstrong (1999) have
explored young childrens perceptions of their rela- ADDRESSES AND AFFILIATIONS
tionships with teachers by using a puppet interview
format. Additionally, Lynch and Cicchetti (1992) have Corresponding author: Robert C. Pianta, University of
demonstrated that childrens perceptions, particu- Virginia, P.O. Box 800784, Charlottesville, VA 22904-
larly as they get older, are valuable indicators of rela- 9784; e-mail: rcp4p@virginia.edu. Bridget K. Hamre is
tionship quality. Thus, future research should attempt also at the University of Virginia.
to include input from children.
In summary, these ndings suggest that the associ-
ation between the quality of early teacherchild rela- REFERENCES
tionships and later school performance can be both Amato, P. R., & Keith, B. (1991). Parental divorce and the
strong and persistent. The association is apparent in well-being of children: A meta-analysis. Psychological
both academic and social spheres of school perfor- Bulletin, 110, 2646.
mance. Negative relational styles, marked by high Birch, S. H. (1997, April). Childrens nominations and ratings of
Hamre and Pianta 637

teacherchild relationships: Measure development and impli- Hieronymus, A. N., & Hoover, H. D. (1978). Iowa Test of Basic
cations for education practice. Poster session presented at Skills, Forms G and H. Chicago: Riverside Publishing
the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Company.
Development, Washington, DC. Hightower, A. D., Work, W. C., Cowen, E. L., Lotyczewski,
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacherchild rela- B. S., Spinnell, A. P., Guare, J. C., & Rohrbeck, C. A. (1986).
tionship and childrens early school adjustment. Journal The TeacherChild Rating Scale: A brief objective mea-
of School Psychology, 35, 6179. sure of elementary childrens school problem behaviors
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1998). Childrens interpersonal and competencies. School Psychology Review, 15, 393409.
behaviors and the teacherchild relationship. Develop- Howes, C., & Hamilton, C. E. (1992). Child relationships
mental Psychology, 34, 934946. with child care teachers: Stability and concordance with
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment parental attachments. Child Development, 63, 859866.
(Rev. ed.). New York: Basic. Howes, C., & Hamilton, C. E. (1993). The changing experi-
Bretherton, I. (1990). Open communication and internal ence of child care: Changes in teachers and in teacher
working models: Their role in the development of attach- child relationships and childrens social competence
ment relationships. In R. A. Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska with peers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 1532.
Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 36. Socioemotional develop- Howes, C., Hamilton, C. E., & Matheson, C. C. (1994). Chil-
ment. (pp. 57113). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. drens relationships with peers: Differential associations
Bryant, D., Clifford, R., & Feinberg, E. (1991). Best practices with aspects of the teacherchild relationship. Child De-
for beginners: Developmental appropriateness in kinder- velopment, 65, 253263.
garten. American Educational Research Journal, 28, 783803. Howes, C., & Matheson, C. C. (1992). Contextual constraints
Campbell, S. B. (1994). Hard-to-manage preschool boys: Ex- on the concordance of motherchild and teacherchild
ternalizing behavior, social competence, and family con- relationships. In R. C. Pianta (Ed.), Beyond the parent: The
text at two-year follow-up. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- role of other adults in childrens lives: New directions for child
chology, 22, 147167. development (pp. 2540). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Cicchetti, D., & Lynch, M. (1993). Toward an ecological/ Howes, C., Matheson, C. C., & Hamilton, C. E. (1994). Ma-
transactional model of community violence and child ternal, teacher, and child care history correlates of chil-
maltreatment: Consequences for childrens development. drens relationships with peers. Child Development, 65,
Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 56, 96118. 264273.
Cicchetti, D., & Richters, J. E. (1993). Developmental consid- Howes, C., & Richie, S. (1999). Attachment organizations in
erations in the investigation of conduct disorder. Devel- children with difcult life circumstances. Development
opment and Psychopathology, 5, 331344. and Psychopathology, 11, 251268.
Cohn, D. A. (1990). Childmother attachment of six-year- Kochenderfer, B. J., & Ladd, G. W. (1996). Peer victimiza-
olds and social competence at school. Child Development, tion: Cause or consequence of school maladjustment?
61, 152162. Child Development, 67, 13051317.
Cummings, E. M., Iannotti, R. J., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1985). Kowaleski-Jones, L., & Duncan, G. J. (1999). The structure of
The inuence of conict between adults on the emotion achievement and behavior across middle childhood.
and aggression of young children. Developmental Psy- Child Development, 70, 930943.
chology, 21, 495507. Ladd, G. W., & Burgess, K. B. (1999). Charting the relation-
Doll, B. (1996). Children without friends: Implications for ship trajectories of aggressive, withdrawn, and aggres-
practice and policy. School Psychology Review, 25, 165183. sive/withdrawn children during early grade school.
Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1998). Facilitating the Child Development, 70, 910929.
transition to rst grade: The nature of transition and re- Lochman, J. E., & The Conduct Problems Prevention Re-
search on factors affecting it. Elementary School Journal, search Group. (1995). Screening of child behavior prob-
98, 351364. lems for prevention programs at school entry. Journal of
Entwisle, D. R., & Hayduk, L. A. (1988). Lasting effects of el- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 549559.
ementary school. Sociology of Education, 61, 147159. Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1992). Maltreated childrens re-
Essex, M. J., & Armstrong, J. M. (1999, March). The impor- ports of relatedness to their teachers. In R. C. Pianta
tance of assessing childrens self-perceptions in understanding (Ed.), Beyond the parent: The role of other adults in childrens
the transition to school. Paper presented at the biennial lives: New directions for child development (pp. 81108).
meeting of the Society of Research in Child Develop- San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
ment, Albuquerque, NM. Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Childrens relationships
Greenberg, M. T., Speltz, M. L., & Deklyen, M. (1993). The with adults and peers: An examination of elementary
role of attachment in the early development of disrup- and junior high school students. Journal of School Psychol-
tive behavior disorders. Development and Psychopathol- ogy, 35, 8199.
ogy, 5, 191213. Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents unresolved traumatic
Hambleton, R. K., Hieronymous, A. N., & Hoover, H. D. experiences are related to infant disorganized attach-
(1987). Iowa Test of Basic Skills, Forms G and H. In D. J. ment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental
Keyser & R. C. Sweetland (Eds.), Test critiques (Vol. VI, pp. behavior the linking mechanism? In M. T. Greenberg, D.
277286). Kansas City, KS: Test Corporation of America. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the
638 Child Development

preschool years (pp. 161182). Chicago: University of Chi- Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Bauman, K. E.,
cago Press. Harris, K. M., Jones, J., Tabor, J., Beuhring, T., Sieving, R. E.,
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in in- Shew, M., Ireland, M., Bearinger, L. H., & Udry, J. R.
fancy, childhood, & adulthood: A move to the level of (1998). Protecting adolescents from harm: Findings from
the representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health.
Growing points in attachment theory and research (pp. In R. E. Muuss & H. D. Porton (Eds.), Adolescent behavior
66104). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child De- and society: A book of readings (5th ed., pp. 376395). New
velopment, 50(12, Serial No. 209). York: McGraw-Hill.
Meyer, L. A., Waldrop, J. L., Hastings, C. N., & Linn, R. L. Rutter, M. (1990). Psychosocial resilience and protective
1993. Effects of ability and settings on kindergartners mechanisms. In J. E. Rolf & A. S. Matsen (Eds.), Risk and
reading performance. Journal of Educational Research, 86, protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp.
142160. 181214). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Park, K. A., & Waters, E. (1989). Security of attachment and Saft, E., & Pianta, R. C. (2000). A descriptive study of the Student
preschool friendships. Child Development, 60, 10761081. Teacher Relationship Scale with preschoolers. Unpublished doc-
Pianta, R. C. (1992a). Beyond the parent: The role of other adults toral dissertation, University of Virginia, Charlottesville.
in childrens lives: New directions for child development. San Salvia, J., & Ysseldyke, J. E. (1985). Assessment in special and
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. remedial education (3rd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifin.
Pianta, R. C. (1992b). The Student Teacher Relationship Scale. Sroufe, L. A. (1983). Infantcaregiver attachment and pat-
University of Virginia, Charlottesville. terns of adaptation in preschool: The roots of maladap-
Pianta, R. C. (1994). Patterns of relationships between chil- tation and competence. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Minne-
dren and kindergarten teachers. Journal of School Psychol- sota Symposium in Child Psychology: development and policy
ogy, 32, 1531. concerning children with special needs (Vol. 16, pp. 4181).
Pianta, R. C. (1997). Adultchild relationship processes and Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
early schooling. Early Education and Development, 8, 1126. Sroufe, L. A. (1988). The role of infantcaregiver attachment
Pianta, R. C. (1999). Enhancing relationships between children in development. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clin-
and teachers. Washington, DC: American Psychological ical implications of attachment (pp. 1838). Hillsdale, NJ:
Association. Erlbaum.
Pianta, R. C., La Paro, K. M., Payne, C., Cox, M. J., & Brad- Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). Guide
ley, R. (2000). Observed quality of the kindergarten classroom for administering and scoring the Stanford-Binet Intelligence
environment: Description and relations with teacher, family, Scale: Fourth edition. Chicago: Riverside.
and school characteristics and child outcomes. Unpublished Tramontana, M. G., Hooper, S. R., & Selzer, S. C. (1988). Re-
manuscript. search on the preschool prediction of later academic
Pianta, R. C., & McCoy, S. J. (1997). The rst day of school: achievement: A review. Developmental Review, 8, 89146.
The predictive validity of early school screening. Journal Trickett, P. K., McBride-Chang, C., & Putman, F. W. (1994). The
of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18, 122. classroom performance and behavior of sexually abused
Pianta, R. C., & Nimetz, S. L. (1991). Relationships between females. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 183194.
children and teachers: Associations with classroom and Verschueren, K., Marcoen, A., & Schoefs, V. (1996). The internal
home behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychol- working model of the self, attachment, and competence in
ogy, 12, 379393. ve-year-olds. Child Development, 5, 24932511.
Pianta, R. C., Nimetz, S. L., & Bennett, E. (1997). Mother Waters, E., & Deane, K. E. (1985). Dening and assessing in-
child relationships, teacherchild relationships and ad- dividual differences in attachment relationships: Q-
justment in preschool and kindergarten. Early Childhood methodology and the organization of behavior in infants
Research Quarterly, 12, 263280. and early childhood. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.),
Pianta, R. C., & Steinberg, M. S. (1992). Teacherchild rela- Growing points of attachment theory and research (pp.
tionships and the process of adjusting to school. New Di- 4165). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child De-
rections for Child Development, 57, 6180. velopment, 50(1, Serial No. 209).
Pianta, R. C., Steinberg, M. S., & Rollins, L. B. (1995). The Wentzel, K. (1996, April). Effective teachers are like good par-
rst two years of school: Teacherchild relationships ents: Understanding motivation and classroom behavior.
and deections in childrens classroom adjustment. De- Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American
velopment and Psychopathology, 7, 295312. Educational Research Association, New York, NY.

You might also like