You are on page 1of 6

Personality and Individual Differences 52 (2012) 218223

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Adolescent bullying and personality: An adaptive approach


Angela S. Book a, Anthony A. Volk b,, Ashley Hosker a
a
Department of Psychology, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada L2S 3A1
b
Department of Child and Youth Studies, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada L2S 3A1

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: From an evolutionary perspective, bullying behavior may be viewed as adaptive in nature. Moreover, as
Received 8 July 2011 bullies may utilize both prosocial and aggressive means to achieve desired goals, they likely exhibit
Received in revised form 16 October 2011 specic personality traits that allow for this bistrategic approach to survival. Therefore, after accounting
Accepted 20 October 2011
for general aggression levels, bullying should be negatively associated with personality traits such as
Available online 10 November 2011
fairness and modesty (HonestyHumility), but unrelated to traits such as forgiveness and tolerance
(Agreeableness). Additionally, the intentional nature of the behavior suggests that bullying should be
Keywords:
positively associated with instrumental, but not reactive, aggression. A sample of 310 adolescents
Bullying
Personality
completed measures of bullying, personality, and instrumental/reactive aggression. Results supported
Adolescents the hypotheses and are interpreted from an adaptive perspective.
HEXACO 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Evolution

1. Introduction engage in antisocial behaviors (Berger, 2007). In fact, bullies appear


to be better off than average adolescents with regard to mental
Bullying behavior is dened as an imbalance of power between health (Ireland, 2005; Juvonen, Graham, & Schuster, 2003; Volk
two individuals, where the stronger individual repeatedly causes et al., 2006), physical health (Wolke, Woods, Bloomeld, & Karstadt,
harm to the weaker individual (Olweus, 1993). Adolescent bullying 2001), popularity (de Bruyn, Cillessen, & Wissink, 2010; Juvonen
is a signicant international problem (Pepler & Craig, 2008) with as et al., 2003), and social skills, including theory of mind, cognitive
many as 100600 million adolescents directly involved with empathy, leadership, social competence, and self-efcacy (Caravita,
bullying worldwide, each year (Volk, Craig, Boyce, & King, 2006). Di Blasio, & Salmivalli, 2009; Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham, 1999;
Moreover, bullying has been document by anthropologists studying Vaillancourt, Hymel, & McDougall, 2003). As in other animals,
modern hunter-gatherers (e.g., Briggs, 1970; Turnbull, 1972) as well bullying appears to be sexually adaptive as bullies start dating at a
as historians documenting past cultures (Cunningham, 2005; younger age, are more active with members of the opposite sex,
Hsiung, 2005). Given its tremendous ubiquity across different times, report greater dating/mating opportunities, and are more likely to
cultures, and geographies, some researchers have suggested that be in a dating relationship (Connolly, Pepler, Craig, & Taradash,
bullying may be (in part) an evolved adaptation (Kolbert & Crothers, 2000). Furthermore, in situations of scarce resources, bullying
2003; Volk, Camilleri, Dane, & Marini, in press). may also be adaptive by increasing physical resources (Turnbull,
1972). Taken together, these data suggest that bullying is a plausible
1.1. Bullying as an adaptation adaptation in human adolescents (Volk et al., in press).
A necessary, but not sufcient, condition for an adaptation is
The idea that bullying is, at least in part, the result of an evolved that it is linked to genes. In a cohort of over 1000 10 year-old twins
adaptation is supported by evidence beyond its ubiquity. Bullying is genetic differences accounted for 61% of the variation in bullying
widespread amongst social animals ranging from sh (Alcock, 1988) (Ball et al., 2008). While it has yet to be determined exactly which
to chickens (Masure & Allee, 1934) to chimpanzees (Goodall, 1986) genes are involved in bullying, one promising category of genes
where it is adaptive because it promotes access to physical, social, that inuence bullying are those associated with personality (Volk
and/or sexual resources. Contrary to popular stereotypes, and unlike et al., in press). A number of studies have shown that many aspects
victims (Hawker & Boulton, 2000) or bully-victims (Mynard & of personality have linkages with specic genes (e.g., DRD4 and
Joseph, 1997), adolescent bullies do not appear to suffer many aggression, Schmidt, Fox, Rubin, Hu, & Hamer, 2002; DRD4 and
adverse effects from bullying beyond a heightened propensity to extraversion or 5-HTTPLR and neuroticism, Ebstein, 2006). These
genes, and likely numerous others that have not been described,
Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 905 688 5550; fax: +1 905 641 2509. are plausible candidates for bullying given the signicant connec-
E-mail address: tvolk@brocku.ca (A.A. Volk). tion between bullying and personality (Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001;

0191-8869/$ - see front matter 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.028
A.S. Book et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 52 (2012) 218223 219

Eysenck, 2006). This makes a study of personality and bullying potentially differentiate adolescents who are selectively aggressive
highly relevant from an adaptive perspective. from those who are generally aggressive.
A signicant body of research has shown that children and ado-
lescents who are aggressive can enjoy signicant adaptive benets
1.2. Bullying and personality
if they selectively employ both aggression and prosociality to ob-
tain their goals (Hawley, Little, & Rodkin, 2007). Hawley has la-
Over the last two decades bullying, as a general eld, has at-
beled these children as bistrategic controllers (Hawley, 2002,
tracted a signicant amount of research attention (Berger, 2007).
2003) as they are able to employ two different strategies, one pro-
However, relatively little research has directly studied the link
social, one aggressive, to obtain control over desired resources. In
between personality and bullying. Olweus (1993) outlined the
support of relating Hawleys theory to bullying is research showing
typical personality of bullies as being tolerant of violence, impul-
that bullies are selective in their aggression, attacking easy victims
sive, and un empathic. Studies using the Eysenck Personality
and/or those who cant hurt the bullys social standing (Dijkstra,
Inventory-Junior reported heightened levels of psychoticism and
Lindenberg, & Veenstra, 2008; Veenstra, Lindenberg, Munniksma,
modest increases in extraversion and neuroticism amongst bullies
& Dijkstra, 2010). Bullies also appear to selectively employ their
(Connolly & OMoore, 2003; Mynard & Joseph, 1997; Slee & Rigby,
aggression to maintain/create strategic alliances with desired ado-
1993).
lescents (Pellegrini & Long, 2002). The HEXACO offers the ideal
Italian studies of bullying and Big Five personality revealed that
measure to test whether bullies personalities match the pattern
children who bullied tended to show a similar pattern of low
of bistrategic control or whether they are generally aggressive. If
Friendliness (Agreeableness) and higher Emotional Instability
bullies were the latter, we expect HonestyHumility and Agree-
(Neuroticism; Menesini, Camodeca, & Nocentini, 2010; Tani,
ableness to be equally important predictors of bullying. If however,
Greenman, Schneider, & Fregoso, 2003). A study amongst American
bullying was a targeted behavior, we predict that bullying would
children again found a negative correlation with Agreeableness,
be signicantly negatively related to HonestyHumility, but con-
but no relationship with Neuroticism and a signicant negative
trary to studies using the Big Five, there would not be a signicant
relationship with Conscientiousness (Bollmer, Harris, & Milich,
relationship with Agreeableness, particularly after aggressive
2006). Scholte and colleagues (2005) found that Undercontrollers
behaviors were controlled for. Given our belief that bullying may
(moderate to high scores on extraversion, low scores on Agreeable-
be adaptive, we predict that any relationship between Agreeable-
ness and Conscientiousness) were more likely to bully other chil-
ness and bullying will be nullied by the inclusion of measures
dren. Bullying has also been linked to moderately higher levels of
of general aggression as this would leave Agreeableness as a
callous-unemotional (CU) traits that include lack of guilt, lack of
measure of sociability and capacity for reactive anger that is not
empathy, poor affect, and use of another for personal gain (Barry
confounded by a bullies targeted aggression towards their victims.
et al., 2000; Viding, Simmonds, Petrides, & Frederickson, 2009).
Thus we predict that only instrumental aggression will be predic-
Thus the ndings for bullying and personality are few, scattered
tive of bullying. If bullying is meant to be a deliberate, targeted
in their measures, and largely atheoretical. We therefore propose
attempt at gaining resources in a manner similar to bistrategic
studying personality and bullying using an adaptive theoretical
aggressive children, then instrumental, and not reactive,
viewpoint to explicitly shed light on why certain personality fac-
aggression should be the predominant predictor.
tors, and not others, are related to bullying. Ideally, this would in-
volve using a personality scale that has an explicitly adaptive
theoretical underpinning. As such, we chose to use the HEXACO 2. Method
(Ashton & Lee, 2007) to study the relationship between bullying
and personality. 2.1. Participants

A total of 310 adolescents (121 boys, 189 girls) between the


1.3. Current study
ages of 10 and 18 (M = 13.6, SD = 3.2) involved in extracurricular
athletic (e.g., hockey, gymnastics) or youth clubs (e.g., church
The HEXACO is an evolutionarily-informed model of personality
youth groups, Pathnders/Guides) from across Southern Ontario
that extends the Big Five model of personality by adding a sixth
participated in the present study. The sample was generally White
factor: HonestyHumility (Ashton & Lee, 2007). The HEXACO
(85%; 13% Asian; 2% Black) and middle-class.
model includes HonestyHumility, Emotionality, Extraversion,
Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness to Experience
(Ashton & Lee, 2001). Extraversion, Conscientiousness, and Open- 2.2. Measures
ness are very similar to the standard Big Five representations of
these traits (Ashton & Lee, 2001; Digman, 1990). The additional Participants were asked to give information on demographics
factor of HonestyHumility is characterized by truthfulness, (as discussed above), followed by questionnaires pertaining to so-
fairness, sincerity, modesty, and lack of greed. Agreeableness is cial relationships in school and their primary organization or ath-
characterized by tolerance, forgiveness, and low aggression; note letic group (the latter were used for a concurrent study of
that the low pole of HEXACO Agreeableness includes anger-related athletes personal relationships).
traits that are associated with Big Five Neuroticism. Emotionality
includes traits typically associated with Neuroticism in the Big Five 2.2.1. Bullying
model, such as anxiety, fearfulness, and emotional reactivity, but Participants lled out a bullying questionnaire (Volk & Lagzdins,
also adds sentimentality and dependence. With regards to study- 2009) asking how often in the last school term they had taken the
ing antisocial behaviors, the HEXACO offers a signicantly better role as a bully in terms of racial/ethnic, physical, verbal, indirect, or
t with the antisocial Dark Triad personality traits as compared sexual bullying at school. For example, participants were asked
to the Big Five (Lee & Ashton, 2005). Whats more, the HEXACO questions such as, In school, how often have you made fun of
differentiates between a willingness to exploit others (Honesty someone much weaker or less popular because of their religion
Humility) and a tolerance and forgiveness of others (Agreeable- or race last term?, with answers ranging in frequency from one
ness; Ashton & Lee, 2007). This is a critical distinction that differs (that hasnt happened) to ve (several times a week). A total score
from the general Agreeableness factor of the Big Five as it can was created for bullying by totaling the values for each form of
220 A.S. Book et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 52 (2012) 218223

bullying. This bullying scale has ve items with a reliability coef- 3.2. Univariate correlations
cient of a = .82. Further support for the cohesive structure of the
bullying scale comes from a principal components analysis (unro- Next we calculated the zero-order correlations between the
tated). Only one eigenvalue exceeded 1.0, and the scree plot sup- variables (see Table 2). Bullying was signicantly negatively corre-
ported the one component solution. Component loadings ranged lated with HonestyHumility, Emotionality, Agreeableness, and
from .45 to .81 for the ve items. Conscientiousness traits. Bullying was signicantly positively
related to both instrumental and reactive aggression. Honesty
2.2.2. Personality Humility had signicant negative correlations with both dimen-
Participants provided self-reports on the 100-item version of sions of aggression, while Agreeableness appeared to be more
the HEXACO Personality InventoryRevised (HEXACO-PI-R; e.g., strongly correlated with reactive aggression.
Lee & Ashton, 2004). Each item uses a ve-point response scale
(1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). The HEXACO-PI-R con- 3.3. Bullying hierarchical linear regression
tains six broad factor-level scales, each of which subsumes four fa-
cet-level scales. (An additional facet scale, Altruism versus The relationships of the independent variables with bullying
Antagonism, was designed to load on several factors). The 100 item were analyzed using a 3-step hierarchical linear regression. The
HEXACO-PI-R was obtained from www.hexaco.org (Ashton & Lee, rst step included age and sex, the second included the HEXACO,
2008). The reliability coefcients for the HEXACO-PI-R in this study and the measures of aggression were entered on the third step to
ranged from a = .73.78. determine whether they altered any of the relationships between
bullying and the HEXACO items. The results of the regression can
2.2.3. Aggression be seen in Table 3. The regression model explained just over a
Aggression was measured using the instrumental and reactive quarter of the variance for bullying (26.0%). Being a boy and being
aggression scales development by Little and colleagues (2003). older were both positive predictors of bullying. HonestyHumility
Each form of aggression asked 12 questions using a four-point re- and Agreeableness were signicant negative predictors in step two,
sponse scale (1 = not at all true to 4 = completely true). The reliabil- but HonestyHumility remained the only signicant personality
ity coefcients for instrumental aggression was a = .89 while for factor once the two measures of aggression were added. As pre-
reactive aggression a = .92. A sample item from the proactive scale dicted, instrumental aggression signicantly predicted bullying,
was I often hit, kick, or punch others to get what I want, while a but reactive aggression did not (although its p = .06).
sample item from the reactive scale was If others have angered
me, I often hit, kick or punch them. 4. Discussion

2.3. Procedure We examined the relationship between adolescents bullying


and a measure of personality while controlling for general aggres-
Local extracurricular organizations were contacted through sion. As predicted, we found signicant relationships with bullying
existing connections with the researchers and through email solic- for both our personality measure of HonestyHumility as well as
itations. Adult supervisors were briefed and asked to provide writ- the measure of instrumental aggression. Also as predicted, in the
ten consent to approach their adolescent participants. Researchers nal step of the regression both Agreeableness and reactive aggres-
then visited participating clubs to brief participants about the sion were not signicant predictors of adolescent bullying. We sug-
study and its methods. To reduce participant bias, participants gest that these results are consistent with the hypothesis that
were told it was a study of peer relationships. Participants were bullying is adaptive. They demonstrate that bullies can engage in
give two envelopes to bring home. The rst envelope contained a targeted aggression while still maintaining the capacity for
parental letter of information and consent. The second envelope supportive friendships, unlike popular stereotypes of bullies that
contained a participant letter of assent and the questionnaires, depict them as maladaptive aggressors (e.g., Smokowski & Kopasz,
which they completed in private, at a time of their choosing. Both 2005).
parental consent and participant assent were required. Parents
were asked to not discuss the study prior to its completion to avoid 4.1. Bullying and personality
biasing the adolescents answers. Participants were protected from
any personal liability associated with their answers and participa- At the univariate level, adolescents bullying was correlated
tion was voluntary with no penalty for withdrawing. with a number of personality traits. Similar to previous studies,
At a predetermined date, the participants returned their forms Agreeableness, Emotionality, and Conscientiousness were all sig-
and received a verbal debrieng. After this debrieng, participants nicantly negatively correlated with bullying (Bollmer et al.,
were asked to complete a second assent form because of the 2006; Tani et al., 2003). This offers condence about the construct
incomplete initial brieng. The participants then received $20 for validity of our results as they broadly replicate what other studies
their participation. These methods were approved by the Brock have found and conrm the importance of personality traits in
University Research Ethics Board. understanding bullying. However, our results go beyond those of
previous studies. The largest personality correlate of bullying was
3. Results HonestyHumility, which ts with its hypothesized role in predict-
ing antisocial behavior (Lee & Ashton, 2005). The importance of
3.1. Descriptive data HonestyHumility is reminiscent of the signicant correlation
between Callous-Unemotional traits and bullying (Viding et al.,
We calculated the means for age, bullying, the HEXACO, and 2009).
aggression for adolescent boys and girls. A one-way ANOVA re- The predictive relationship of sex and age in our regression
vealed that boys had signicantly higher bullying and instrumental agree with previous literature given that boys are well known to
aggression scores than girls, while girls had signicantly higher bully more than girls on average, while bullying peaks mid-way
scores on HonestyHumility, Emotionality, and Openness (see through adolescence, so one might well expect a modest correla-
Table 1). tion with age across our sample (Volk et al., 2006). What is perhaps
A.S. Book et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 52 (2012) 218223 221

Table 1
Mean Scores and sex differences for adolescent participants (N = 310).

Variable $ Mean $ SD # Mesn # SD F(1, 289)


Age 14.45 1.84 14.05 1.47 3.78
Bullying
Total bullying .99 1.69 2.1 2.93 16.81**
HEXACO
HonestyHumility 3.41 .60 3.05 .53 28.10**
Emotionality 3.49 .54 2.86 .49 98.10**
Extraversion 3.52 .53 3.57 .49 .75
Agreeableness 3.04 .51 3.10 .53 1.12
Conscientiousness 3.27 .57 3.16 .56 2.68
Openness 3.18 .60 2.97 .53 8.14**
Aggression
Reactive 23.37 6.42 24.73 7.19 2.78
Instrumental 15.48 4.70 17.98 7.23 12.82**
**
p < .01.

Table 2
Correlations between bullying, aggression, and personality traits in adolescents (N P 287).

H E X A C O Reactive Instrumental
Bully .36* .11* .05 .24* .18** .08 .39** .40**
H .28** .02 .31** .21** .21* .43** .46**
E .06 .04 .04 .15** .07 .11*
X .21** .14** .14** .09 .04
A .20** .10* .44** .27**
C .25** .25** .19**
O .18** .15**
Reactive .63**
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.

Table 3 bullying amongst adolescents. Our multivariate results conrmed


Hierarchical linear regression for adolescent personality, aggression, and bullying our hypothesis that the most critical multivariate predictor is not
(N = 310). Agreeableness, but HonestyHumility. They also emphasize the
Variable B SE B B D r2 Model r2 unique advantage of using the HEXACO as opposed to the Big Five
** model, as HonestyHumility predicted bullying above and beyond
Step 1 .107 .107**
Age .31 .08 .23** Agreeableness. Agreeableness was a signicant univariate predic-
Sex 1.24 .27 .26** tor but it was no longer a signicant predictor when measures of
Step 2 .107** .214** general aggression were entered. Rather, its correlations show it
Age .24 .08 .18**
to be a better predictor of anger/reactive aggression than planned
Sex .98 .30 .21**
HonestyHumility .99 .24 .25**
proactive aggression. When one considers the personality of
Emotionality .17 .24 .05 aggressive individuals, a measure of aggression (like the Big Five
Extraversion .01 .25 .00 or HEXACO Agreeableness) is almost certainly going to be con-
Agreeableness .56 .26 .12* founded with general aggression. In other words, it is no surprise
Conscientiousness .33 .23 .08
that aggressive bullies show aggressive personality traits. What is
Openness .07 .23 .02
Step 3 .046** .260** perhaps surprising is that when aggressive behavior is controlled
Age .19 .07 .14** for, bullies appear to be as agreeable as other adolescents and that
Sex .86 .30 .18** agreeableness is more strongly related to reactive rather than pro-
HonestyHumility .60 .25 .16** active aggression. This is crucially important as it could allow bul-
Emotionality .13 .23 .03
Extraversion .01 .24 .00
lies to maintain adaptive close relationships with their allies while
Agreeableness .29 .27 .07 at the same time using aggression to adaptively target victims in
Conscientiousness .23 .22 .06 order to gain control over the physical or social resources they
Openness .14 .22 .04 desire.
Reactive aggression .05 .03 .14
The selective nature of bullies aggression is reinforced by our
Instrumental aggression .06 .03 .16*
ndings that instrumental aggression was a signicant multivari-
*
p < .05. ate predictor of bullying while reactive aggression was not. Despite
**
p < .01.
a trend towards reactive aggression being a signicant predictor, it
nevertheless appeared to be a weaker multivariate predictor than
instrumental aggression. Indeed, the two signicant (non-demo-
most encouraging is that we were able to obtain a large effect, graphic) predictors in our nal regression, HonestyHumility and
explaining a quarter of the variance in adolescents bullying using instrumental aggression, make a strong case for considering bul-
a simple regression model based on age, sex, a measure of person- lies behavior as being deliberate and targeted. We appreciate that
ality, and two complimentary measures of aggression. This high- this study does not explicitly measure whether these traits are evo-
lights the potential utility of personality traits in predicting lutionarily adaptive as we lack appropriate outcome measures
222 A.S. Book et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 52 (2012) 218223

(such as increased dating partners). Nevertheless, we note that our Connolly, J., Pepler, D., Craig, W., & Taradash, A. (2000). Dating experiences of bullies
in early adolescence. Child Maltreatment, 5, 299310.
results are consistent with the evolutionarily-adaptive model of
Cunningham, H. (2005). Children and childhood in western society since 1500 (2nd
bullying mentioned above and in other papers (Kolbert & Crothers, ed.). Toronto: Pearson-Longman.
2003; Volk et al., in press). de Bruyn, E. H., Cillessen, A. H., & Wissink, I. B. (2010). Associations of peer
Whether or not bullying is adaptive for bullies, the literature acceptance and perceived popularity with bullying and victimization in early
adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 30, 543566.
leaves no doubt that it is extremely maladaptive for victims Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the Five-Factor Model.
(Hawker & Boulton, 2000). Our ndings should be interpreted as Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417440.
an attempt to better understand bullying in order to try and Dijkstra, J. K., Lindenberg, S., & Veenstra, R. (2008). Beyond the class norm: Bullying
behavior of popular adolescents and its relation to peer acceptance and
prevent bullying. For example, working with (instead of against) rejection. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 12891299.
bullies high HonestyHumility scores by providing them with Ebstein, R. P. (2006). The molecular genetic architecture of human personality:
more effective, but prosocial, alternatives that achieve their goals. Beyond self-report questionnaires. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 427445.
Eysenck, H. J. (2006). The biological basis of personality. New Brunswick, NJ:
We believe that such an informed approach is better than contin- Transaction.
uing to stereotypically mislabel bullying as maladaptive behavior Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
conducted by indiscriminately angry and aggressive individuals Press.
Hawker, D. S., & Boulton, M. J. (2000). Twenty years research on peer victimization
(e.g., Smokowski & Kopasz, 2005). and psychosocial maladjustment: A meta-analytical view of cross-sectional
studies. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 441455.
Hawley, P. H. (2002). Social dominance and prosocial and coercive strategies of
4.1.1. Limitations and future research resource control in preschoolers. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
A limitation of the current study was that it was based on infor- 26, 167176.
mation obtained via self-report measures. However, previous liter- Hawley, P. H. (2003). Strategies of control, aggression and morality in preschoolers:
An evolutionary perspectives. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85,
ature has shown that self-report is a valid method for measuring 213235.
bullying (Pellegrini & Bartini, 2000) and personality (Lee & Ashton, Hawley, P. H., Little, T. D., & Rodkin, P. C. (Eds.). (2007). Aggression and adaptation.
2004), allowing us to have condence in our data. Another limita- Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Helson, R., Kwan, V. S., John, O. P., & Jones, C. (2002). The growing evidence for
tion is that our sample was relatively homogeneous in terms of
personality change in adulthood: Findings from research with personality
race and SES. Third, the difference between the regression effect inventories. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 287306.
sizes of reactive and instrumental aggression were small, and thus Hsiung, P. C. (2005). A Tender Voyage: Children and Childhood in Late Imperial China.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
some caution should be placed on related conclusions. Finally, our
Ireland, J. L. (2005). Psychological health and bullying behavior among adolescent
conclusions are based on cross-sectional correlational data, which prisoners: A study of young and juvenile offenders. Journal of Adolescent, 36,
limits our ability to draw causal conclusions. 236243.
Future studies should therefore seek to replicate the current Juvonen, J., Graham, S., & Schuster, M. A. (2003). Bullying among young adolescents:
The strong, the weak, and the troubled. Pediatrics, 112, 12311237.
ndings using longitudinal methods to study bullying and person- Kolbert, J. B., & Crothers, L. (2003). Bullying and evolutionary psychology: The
ality. This might reveal key changes that occur as both bullying dominance hierarchy among students and implications for school personnel.
(Volk et al., 2006) and personality (Helson, Kwan, John, & Jones, Journal of School Violence, 2, 7391.
Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO personality
2002) generally undergo changes during adolescence. Our results inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(2), 329358.
suggest that adolescent bullies possess personality traits and Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2005). Psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and narcissism in
behavioral tendencies that are well suited towards engaging in the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Individual
Differences, 38, 15711582.
an adaptive, bistrategic mode of behavior whereby they can bully Little, T. D., Jones, S. M., Henrich, C. C., & Hawley, P. H. (2003). Disentangling the
victims, but can also be friendly towards allies. Finally, the value whys from the whats of aggressive behavior. International Journal of
of personality, and in particular the HEXACO, as a predictor of bul- Behavioral Development, 27, 122133.
Masure, R. H., & Allee, W. C. (1934). The social order in ocks of the common
lying strongly suggests that future bullying research and/or inter-
chicken and the pigeon. The Auk, 51, 306327.
ventions should seriously consider incorporating measures of Menesini, E., Camodeca, M., & Nocentini, A. (2010). Bullying among siblings: The
personality such as the HEXACO. role of personality and relational variables. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 28, 921939.
Mynard, H., & Joseph, S. (1997). Bully/victim problems and their association with
References Eysencks personality dimensions in 8 to 13-year olds. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 67, 5154.
Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school: What we know and what we can do. NY: Wiley-
Alcock, J. (1988). Animal behavior (4th ed). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
Blackwell.
Ashton, M., & Lee, K. (2001). A theoretical basis for the dimensions of personality.
Pellegrini, A. D., & Bartini, M. (2000). An empirical comparison of methods of sample
European Journal of Personality, 15, 327353.
aggression and victimization in school settings. Journal of Educational
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of
Psychology, 92(2), 360366.
the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology
Pellegrini, A. D., & Long, J. D. (2002). A longitudinal study of bullying, dominance,
Review, 11, 150166.
and victimization during the transition from primary school through secondary
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2008). The prediction of HonestyHumility-related criteria
school. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 259280.
by the HEXACO and Five-Factor models of personality. Journal of Research in
Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (Eds.). (2008). Understanding and addressing bullying: An
Personality, 42, 12161228.
international perspective. Bloomington, IN: Author House.
Ball, H. A., Arsenault, L., Taylor, A., Maughan, B., Caspi, A., & Moftt, T. E. (2008).
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., Hu, S., & Hamer, D. H. (2002). Molecular
Genetic inuences on victims, bullies, and bully-victims in childhood. The
genetics of shyness and aggression in preschoolers. Personality and Individual
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 104112.
Differences, 33, 227238.
Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J., DeShazo, T. M., McCoy, M. G., Ellis, M., & Loney, B. R. (2000).
Scholte, R. H., van Lieshout, C. F., de Wit, C. A., & van Aken, M. A. (2005). Adolescent
The importance of callous-unemotional traits for extending the concept of
personality types and subtypes and their psychosocial adjustment. Merrill-
psychopathy to children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 335340.
Palmer Quarterly, 51, 258286.
Berger, K. S. (2007). Update on bullying at school: Science forgotten? Developmental
Slee, P. T., & Rigby, K. (1993). The relationship of Eysencks personality factors and
Review, 27, 90126.
self-esteem to bully-victim behaviour in Australian schoolboys. Personality and
Bollmer, J. M., Harris, M. J., & Milich, R. (2006). Reactions to bullying and peer
Individual Differences, 14, 371373.
victimization: Narratives, physiological arousal, and personality. Journal of
Smokowski, P. R., & Kopasz, K. H. (2005). Bullying in school: An overview of types,
Research in Personality, 40, 803828.
effects, family characteristics, and intervention strategies. Children & Schools, 27,
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, and personality. Behavior
101110.
Genetics, 31, 243273.
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999). Bullying and theory of mind: A
Briggs, J. L. (1970). Never in anger. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
critique of the social skills decit view of anti-social behaviour. Social
Caravita, S. C. S., Di Blasio, P., & Salmivalli, C. (2009). Unique and interactive effects
Development, 8, 117127.
of empathy and social status on involvement in bullying. Social Development, 18,
Tani, F., Greenman, P. S., Schneider, B. H., & Fregoso, M. (2003). Bullying and the Big
140163.
Five: A study of childhood personality and participant roles in bullying
Connolly, I., & OMoore, M. (2003). Personality and family relations of children who
incidents. School Psychology International, 24, 131146.
bully. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 559567.
A.S. Book et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 52 (2012) 218223 223

Turnbull, C. M. (1972). The mountain people. NY: Touchstone. Evolutionary Perspectives on Violence, Homicide, and War. NY: Oxford
Vaillancourt, T., Hymel, S., & McDougall, P. (2003). Bullying is power: Implications University Press.
for school-based intervention strategies. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 19, Volk, A., Craig, W., Boyce, W., & King, M. (2006). Adolescent risk correlates of
157176. bullying and different types of victimization. International Journal of Adolescent
Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Munniksma, A., & Dijkstra, J. K. (2010). The complex Medicine and Health, 18, 375386.
relation between bullying, victimization, acceptance, and rejection: Giving Volk, A. A., & Lagzdins, L. (2009). Bullying and victimization among adolescent girl
special attention to status, affection, and sex differences. Child Development, 81, athletes. Athletic Insight, 11, 1225.
480486. Wolke, D., Woods, S., Bloomeld, L., & Karstadt, L. (2001). Bullying involvement in
Viding, E., Simmonds, E., Petrides, K. V., & Frederickson, N. (2009). The contribution primary school and common health problems. Archives of Disease in Childhood,
of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems to bullying in early 85, 197201.
adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 471481.
Volk, A., Camilleri, J., Dane, A., & Marini, Z. (in press). If, when, and why bullying is
adaptive. In Shackelford, T., Shackelford, V. (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of

You might also like