You are on page 1of 3

Behavioural Processes 79 (2008) 1927

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behavioural Processes
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc

Chimpanzees fail to plan in an exchange task but


succeed in a tool-using procedure
V. Dufour a,b, , E.H.M. Sterck b,c
a
Ethology Research, Animal Science Department, Biomedical Primate Research Centre, Lange Kleiweg 139, 2288 GJ Rijswijk, The Netherlands
b
Behavioural Biology, Utrecht University, Kruijt Gebouw Room O 211, Padualaan 8, 3584 CH Utrecht, The Netherlands
c
School of Psychology, University of St. Andrews, St. Marys College, South Street, St. Andrews, Fife, Scotland KY16 9JP, United Kingdom

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Planning has long been considered a uniquely human capacity. Lately, however, it has been shown that apes
Received 28 November 2007 and a corvid species act now to derive a material future benet. Since primates are highly social animals
Received in revised form 29 March 2008 and their sociality is considered a strong selective force that resulted in complex cognitive capacities,
Accepted 11 April 2008
planning is also expected in social situations. Unfortunately, prompting from social partners cannot be
excluded in a social setting. Therefore, we controlled for this factor by testing the capacity to plan in
Keywords:
chimpanzees using an exchange paradigm, that involves both a material and a social component, and a
Planning
tool-use paradigm, similar to the one used on two other ape species. All chimpanzees failed to plan in
Tool-use
Exchange task
the exchange task, but three individuals showed planning behavior in the tool-use task. Our methods
Cooperation controlled for the fact that chimpanzees were not prompted by the visibility of the reward at the moment
Chimpanzees of planning and also could not repeat a previously acquired routine. The best interpretation for our results
is that chimpanzees can plan. However, planning was limited to the situation where the action to attain
the future benet only depended on a chimpanzees own behavior.
2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Whether humans are the sole species that evolved episodic of breakfast the next morning (Raby et al., 2007). Also primates
memory, the capacity to mentally travel in time, is currently sub- show future-oriented behavior. Monkeys going to distant food sites
ject of a heated debate (Schwartz and Evans, 2001; Clayton et al., choose the appropriate route minutes in advance (Cebus apella,
2003a,b; Roberts, 2002; Suddendorf and Busby, 2003a,b). Many Janson, 1998). Captive chimpanzees spontaneously save tokens,
researchers (Roberts, 2002; Suddendorf and Busby, 2003a) claim indicating anticipation of a delayed opportunity to exchange them
that animals have no recollections of personal events and con- (Sousa and Matsuzawa, 2001). Recent work (Dufour et al., 2007;
sequently are not able to plan for future needs. Instead, it has Beran and Evans, 2006) shows that chimpanzees can postpone an
been suggested that animals are stuck in time (Roberts, 2002). expected reward for a time scale measured in minutes, not seconds.
Animals generally favor the immediate reward (Richards et al., In both common chimpanzees and bonobos, individuals trained to
1997; Tobin et al., 1996), when exposed to one small immediate use symbols were able to announce intended action and travel des-
and one larger but delayed reward. Silverberg et al. (1998) noted tination plans (Menzel et al., 2002). However, only one study has yet
that chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) were unable to stock fruits in experimentally shown that apes performed future-oriented behav-
advance, thus exhibiting temporal myopia the inability to antic- ior that was not driven by current need, a necessary condition for
ipate a future need . Yet in a non-primate species, Clayton and planning (Mulcahy and Call, 2006). In this study, orangutans (Pongo
Dickinson (1998) showed that scrub jays (Aphelocoma californica), pygmaeus) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) collected, saved and trans-
a food hoarding bird species, possess episodic-like memory of a ported tools to use them later. Individuals of both species could plan
past event, since they integrate information on what-where-when for a predictable event 1 h, and even one night, ahead. It has, how-
to recover cached food (Clayton et al., 2001). Moreover, scrub jays ever, been argued that prompting, the food was visible at the time
react to the potential of future pilfering by reaching the food (Dally of tool collection, or learning from the rst task to solve the sec-
et al., 2006) and future planning was suggested as an explanation ond task may also explain these results (Suddendorf and Corballis,
for food caching behavior in the evening before a predictable lack 2007).
Advanced cognitive capacities, such as future planning, may
have evolved in primates to cope with a complex environment.
Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 7954 989813; fax: +44 1334 463042. This complexity may concern their social environment (Byrne and
E-mail address: vmd1@st-andrews.ac.uk (V. Dufour). Whiten, 1988), extractive foraging requiring tool use (Reader and

0376-6357/$ see front matter 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2008.04.003
20 V. Dufour, E.H.M. Sterck / Behavioural Processes 79 (2008) 1927

Laland, 2001) or a combination of the two (Reader and Laland, a food reward could be obtained from a human partner upon the
2002). Anecdotal evidence on chimpanzees suggests that they are return of a specic object at a specic time of the day. To succeed in
able to plan for future material needs. Wild chimpanzees travel this task, chimpanzees rst had to collect earlier in the day a specic
over hundreds of meters in dense vegetation to pick up and trans- category of object among three available for a limited time inter-
port tools necessary to crack nuts (Boesch and Boesch, 1984). val, and return with it in the test compartment where the exchange
Chimpanzees may also plan for future social needs. They seem to activity would take place with the human partner.
maintain a high dominance rank through tactical social maneu-
vering (de Waal, 1982). Moreover, chimpanzees that groom in the 1.1. Methods
morning have a higher chance to obtain a share of food from
their grooming partner in the afternoon (de Waal, 1997). However, 1.1.1. Subjects
interpreting these observations as evidence of planning remains Two groups of chimpanzees from the Biomedical Primate
speculative since other, lower-level, explanations are possible. For Research Centres colony participated to this study. They were
example, chimpanzees tool transport may result from the cur- housed in a similar enclosure with the same facilities and daily rou-
rent motivational state of the individual (Suddendorf and Corballis, tine. Group 1 was composed of two males and three females aged
1997). Moreover, in a cooperative context chimpanzees fail to show from 19 to 23 years. Group 2 was composed of two females and four
other regarding behavior (Jensen et al., 2006; Silk et al., 2005) or males aged from 12 to 15 years. Each group was housed in its com-
to understand cues givens by others who communicate coopera- munal room (a 40 m2 room with elevated sitting devices) to which
tive intentions (Hare and Tomasello, 2004). As suggested by Byne individual feeding compartments were permanently connected.
(2007), cooperation observed in chimpanzees may not result from a These compartments were used as individual testing compart-
calculation of future benets (planning), but from their inclination ments. One individual in group 1 never learned how to use objects
to favor those with whom cooperation has been successful before. and was therefore not included in the study. The study was designed
Herrmann and Tomasello (2006) have shown that chimpanzees to provide enrichment without disturbing the daily routine of the
express more cognitive skills when in a competitive context rather groups. Participants were given the choice to come or not and where
than in a cooperative one: chimpanzees deduce from a commu- always rewarded with a small treat at the end of the session, what-
nicative gesture from a competitor toward a bucket that the food ever their level of participation to the task. Both groups had never
must be hidden there. A similar gesture made by a cooperative part- been observed for ethological purposes before. They had never been
ner with the intent to communicate the location of the food is not trained for or involved in cognitive tasks. The chimpanzees were not
understood. Cognitive skills in a cooperative context may be con- reported to have been trained to exchange with humans before.
strained by the high level of competition in this species (Hare and
Tomasello, 2004; Hare et al., 2007). If this is the case, planning for 1.1.2. Training
the future involving cooperation may be a more challenging task Prior to the tasks, the chimpanzees were all invited to enter their
than planning for a future activity where success only depends on individual compartment, a familiar procedure. For the exchange
ones own behavior. task, chimpanzees were trained to return an object to the human
We aimed to test the capacity to plan in chimpanzees. Most tests partner to obtain a food reward. The object to trade was a color-
of planning in animals concern a future material benet and do not ful plastic straw that was easy to insert through the front of the
involve uncertainty related to other factors such as a cooperative compartment and was unknown to all subjects. The human part-
partner. In the present study we tested for chimpanzees planning ner (V.D.) proceeded as follows: she begged the chimpanzee with
capacities involving material benets based on a cooperative inter- the palm up presented in front of her/him, holding a peanut in the
action (Part 1), and planning capacities involving material benets other hand, and asked verbally for the return of the token (terug
only (Part 2). the Dutch equivalent for back) 12 times in a row. After a successful
exchange, the chimpanzee was given 15 s to eat the peanut. Chim-
panzees learned to give the straw back with a mean of 4.7 sessions
1. Study 1: planning to selectively collect items for later of 12 trials each (range 117 sessions). Once all individuals mas-
exchange tered the exchange task, this task was given by the partner as a
daily activity for 20 consecutive daily sessions at 13:10 every after-
To be qualied as planning, a behavior must be performed in noon. This insured predictability of the activity and also insured
the present without being driven by current need (Tulving, 1983) that the human partner was associated with the exchange task.
or by current cues to future benets (Suddendorf, 2006). Tulving The human partner had at the start of this study been known to
(2005) proposed the spoon-test to test for planning in animals. In the chimpanzees for half a year and she was only involved in neu-
this test, a girl was disappointed because she could not get desert tral or positive interactions with the animals. The chimpanzees had
at a party since she did not bring her own spoon. The crucial test known the keepers from at least 5 years.
for planning capacities is to check whether this memory of disap- To determine whether the chimpanzees planned for the
pointment will lead her to bring a spoon to the next party. In this exchange task at hand or were interested in just any object, other
study, we devised an analogue of the spoon test (Tulving, 2005), to objects were present as distracters at the time of testing. These
assess chimpanzees capacity to plan for a future benet involving objects, like the straws used in the exchange activity, were also
a cooperative interaction. Unfortunately, purely social interactions associated with a desired food, a specic function, and a specic
cannot be used to test future directed behavior, since the antici- time of the day. The chimpanzees were provided a natural wooden
pated future event will always involve other individuals and direct branch that was to be used in a dipping task. The chimpanzees
reactions to the others cannot be ruled out. Therefore, we tested were trained to dip it in a box full of honey that was held in front of
chimpanzees capacities in an exchange paradigm, where the coop- them by a keeper. Since the dipping task was part of their weekly
erative individual was represented by a human partner. While the enrichment, only 3 sessions of 3 min per day and per individual
human partner could be considered as a social tool, failure may be were required to insure that all the individuals performed the task.
considered from the subjects point of view as a failure in the part- The third object consisted in a sharp wooden stick that was used in
ner behavior, and not his own. From this point of view it involves a a picking task. The chimpanzees were trained to retrieve, by pick-
social component that the subject cannot control. In this paradigm, ing into them, orange pieces from a plastic box held in front of the
ID Title Pages

2427736 Chimpanzees fail to plan in an exchange task but succeed in a tool-using procedure 9

http://fulltext.study/article/2427736

http://FullText.Study

You might also like