You are on page 1of 7

95

A Kinematic Study of Contextual Effects on Reaching


Performance in Persons With and Without Stroke:
Influences of Object Availability
Ching-yi Wu, ScD, Catherine A. Trombly, ScD, Keh-chung Lin, ScD, Linda Tickle-Degnen, PhD
ABSTRACT. Wu C-Y, Trombly CA, Lin K-C, Tickle- cine and the American Academy of Physical Medicine and
Degnen L. A kinematic study of contextual effects on reaching Rehabilitation
performance in persons with and without stroke: influences of
object availability. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2000;81:95-101.
Objective: To examine the effects of context on reaching A SSESSMENT OF movement performance often involves
recording the end of range of motion or the number of
repetitions. Recently, sophisticated systems have been devel-
performance in neurologically impaired and intact populations.
Context was varied by the presence or absence of objects used oped that quantify or document kinematics in three dimensions
to complete a task. throughout complex movement. Movement kinematics de-
Design: A counterbalanced repeated-measures design. scribe the spatiotemporal characteristics of movement (for
Setting: A motor control laboratory in a university setting. example, displacement and the derivatives of displacement,
Participants: Fourteen persons with stroke and 25 neurologi- acceleration and jerk), which may tap subtle aspects of quality
cally intact adults. of performance on motor tasks.1 A complete kinematic descrip-
Interventions: Each participant was tested under two condi- tion of a movement may indicate the neural commands
tions: the presence of the object, in which the participant reached responsible for this movement2 and may be used to infer the
forward with the impaired arm (or corresponding arm) to scoop organization of the movement.3 Therefore, use of kinematic
coins off the table into the other hand; and the absence of the object, analysis is a possible way of providing information on how
in which the participant reached forward to the place where the movement is organized or can be modified in persons who have
coins would be placed in the condition of object present. suffered stroke.4,5 One possible way to modify motor perfor-
Main Outcome Measures: Kinematic variables of move- mance in patients after stroke is to vary the performance
ment time, total displacement, peak velocity, percentage of context. The importance of context in determining the perfor-
reach where peak velocity occurs, and movement units (derived mance or behavior of any individual6 has been increasingly
from acceleration data) for reaching tasks. addressed in the fields of neurologic rehabilitation7 and move-
Results: The condition of using real objects elicited kinemati- ment science.8,9 Despite this emphasis of context, the potential
cally better performance of reaching movements than the contribution of contextual features in evaluation and interven-
condition of performing movements without relevant objects tion relative to task performance has not been well under-
present. Better performance was reflected by shorter movement stood.10 The need for research in this area has been called for.10
time, less total displacement, higher peak velocity, greater Rogoff11 has identified three aspects of context: the task
percentage of reach where peak velocity occurs, and fewer materials, the meaningfulness of the goal of the activity, and the
movement units. social situation in which the task is embedded. The task
Conclusion: The results of this study showed that the materials within the context are the most obvious, and thus the
condition of object present elicited better performance of move- most likely to be given consideration when contextual effects
ments represented by kinematic variables than the condition of on performance are discussed.12,13 There is some evidence that
object absent. The clinical implication is that the use of real and the presence of objects facilitates better motor performance
functional objects might be an effective way of facilitating efficient, than the absence of objects in neurologically intact14 and
smooth, and coordinated movement with the impaired arm in impaired15 people. Further knowledge generated from clinical
persons with stroke. This study, however, should be replicated populations may have direct implications for the evaluation and
and extended to confirm the validity of its findings and to allow therapy of movement disorders. The rationale for studying
for generalization in various functional activities. neurologically intact subjects is to provide a basis for establish-
Key Words: Kinematics; Stroke; Movements; Rehabilita- ing normal patterns in task performance under the contexts of
tion. object present and absent. This investigation aims at studying
r 2000 by the American Congress of Rehabilitation Medi- whether object availability would influence movement perfor-
mance in able-bodied adults and persons with cerebral vascular
accident, which is the leading cause of disability among adults,
From Department of Occupational Therapy, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan (Dr. particularly older adults, in the United States.16
Wu), and the School of Occupational Therapy, National Taiwan University, Taipei (Dr.
Lin), Taiwan; and the Department of Occupational Therapy, Boston University,
Newell and Valvano9 stressed that action is a result of the
Boston, MA (Drs. Trombly, Tickle-Degnen). interaction of persons with their everyday environment (ie,
Submitted for publication January 13, 1999. Accepted in revised form May 6, 1999. performance context). A person has to perceive useful informa-
Supported in part by the American Occupational Therapy Foundation. tion from the context so that the control of behavior can be
No commercial party having a direct financial interest in the results of the research
supporting this article has or will confer a benefit upon the authors or upon any
organized and adjusted to the possibilities for action that are
organization with which the authors are associated. supported by the context.17 Presence of objects allows the actor
Reprints requests to Dr. Keh-chung Lin, School of Occupational Therapy, College to perceive sensory information about the layout of the perfor-
of Medicine, National Taiwan University, 7 Chung-shan South Road, Taipei 100, mance context, ones own body, as well as the relation between
Taiwan.
the hand and the context,18 and perceptual information such as
r 2000 by the American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine and the American the functional usefulness of the object8 and the goal suggested
Academy of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
0003-9993/00/8101-5415$3.00/0 by the objects. That is, objects existing in a performance context

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 81, January 2000


96 A KINEMATIC STUDY OF CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS, Wu

may facilitate information processing, alluded to above, to METHODS


perform a reaching movement.19-21 If the object is absent, the
visual and perceptual information is deprived, the environmen- Participants
tally defined goal is absent, and the movement must be Fourteen persons with stroke (9 men, 5 women, aged 39 to 84
organized by attending to the process of moving. It is expected years, mean age 61.79 yrs) and 25 age-matched neurologically
that functional use of an object within a performance context intact adults (8 men, 17 women, aged 37 to 81 years, mean age
may facilitate better or more organized performance of move- 63.80 yrs) volunteered for this study. All participants signed
ment than movement performed within an objectless context informed consent forms approved by the Institutional Review
(eg, movement for exercise). Board. All participants were right-handed. The able-bodied
The notion that using an object during a functional task may adults included in this study had no prior history of cerebral
facilitate better performance of movement than a performance disease by self-report. The persons with stroke were able to
context without objects or objectless exercise has been tested by understand and respond to directions given by the experimenter
cumulative studies. The vast majority of the studies used and demonstrated movement of the proximal part of the
traditional measures (eg, the number of movement repetitions, impaired arm. The stroke group consisted of eight persons with
range of motion, duration of movements) to compare the motor left hemisphere damage, five persons with right hemisphere
performance under the conditions of object present versus damage, and one person with both hemispheres damaged. The
duration postonset ranged from 5 to 174.5 months. The
object absent in neurologically intact22-26 and neurologically
information on the date of onset of the stroke and the site of
impaired populations.27,28 Collectively, the results of these stroke was obtained from patients self-report. Demographic
studies showed a positive effect in support of functional use of and clinical characteristics of persons with stroke are listed in
an object for enhancing movement performance in neurologi- table 1.
cally intact and impaired populations. Clinical measures of spasticity, joint sense, and visuospatial
Scant studies have utilized computerized measures to com- neglect were administered to the stroke patients to enhance
pare the kinematic variables of movements performed with the interpretation of results. The modified Ashworth scale39 used to
presence or absence of objects in neurologically intact14,23 and measure spasticity involves manually moving a limb through
impaired15 populations. Given the paucity of studies that used the range of motion to passively stretch specific muscle groups.
kinematic analysis to investigate movement performance under Except for subjects 2 and 8, spasticity was negligible in this
various contextual conditions in normal adults, there is need for sample (table 1).
further study. Leo and Soderberg40 suggested that the sensory awareness of
The reaching movement, a complicated multijoint movement a stroke patients impaired limb is highly associated with the
directed to a defined point in space,29 is the major action of the ability to combine synergies into normal voluntary movements.
arm to bring the hand into interaction with the environment.30 The patient was blindfolded for administration of the Per-
Reaching is, thus, an important movement to study in persons ception of Joint Position Sense Test and asked to move the
who have suffered stroke. The reaching movement was charac- index finger of the nonimpaired upper extremity to touch a
terized by kinematic variables, including movement time, total small piece of tape affixed to the radial styloid of the impaired
displacement, the amplitude of peak velocity, percentage of upper extremity, which was moved into each of six combina-
movement where peak velocity occurs, and the number of tions of shoulder, elbow, and forearm placements. Three
patients (nos. 5, 7, 13) exhibited severe sensory awareness
movement units. Movement time refers to the time for the
impairment (table 1).
execution of the reaching movement. The path of the hand in The line bisection test was used to test the presence of
three-dimensional (3-D) space is described by total displace- visuospatial neglect. This test consisted of three horizontal
ment in x, y, and z coordinates. Once the movement starts, for black lines of 12, 18, and 24cm in length, each of which was
example, reaching for the target, the arm generally accelerates presented separately on a sheet of legal-size paper.41 Patients 5
toward the target and then decelerates at each point of direction and 13 were found to have left neglect (table 1).
change or correction of trajectory.29 One acceleration and one
deceleration phase comprise a movement unit. The peak
Materials and Instrumentation
velocity corresponds to the changeover from the acceleration to
the deceleration phase.31,32 If the movement is well controlled, Coins, including dimes and quarters, were used for the target
the motor program may not rely heavily on feedback loops to objects in the condition of object present.
correct the ongoing movement. Consequently, the path of the The OPTOTRAK/3020 3-D active infrared motion analysis
movement will be more direct (ie, less total displacement),33 the systema was used to track the movement of the infrared
time for execution of movement will be shorter (ie, shorter light-emitting diodes (LEDs) attached to the limb as it moved in
3-D. Two infrared LEDs were used in this study: one was
movement time),33,34 the movement will be faster (ie, higher
attached over the head of the fifth metacarpal of the hand of the
peak velocity)14 and smoother (ie, fewer movement units),35,36 subject; the other was placed directly in front of the location of
and the percentage of reach where peak velocity occurs will be the target object and used as a reference LED. The two LEDs
greater.37,38 Greater percentage of reach where peak velocity were tracked simultaneously by three infrared sensors (cam-
occurs indicates less dependence on on-line feedback for eras) permanently mounted in a 5-foot long housing. This
movement correction. instrument was placed approximately 2m from the subject
Our hypothesis is that the condition of object present could during recording.
elicit better performance of movements than that of object The OPTOTRAK is a precalibrated and noncontact system
absent in both the able-bodied and the stroke populations. capable of recording high-velocity motion in three dimensions
Enhanced performance of movements would be reflected with 0.1-mm accuracy at a distance of 2.5m. The LEDs were
kinematically by shorter movement time, less total displace- fired sequentially at 2,000Hz by the central controller unit via a
ment, higher peak velocity, greater percentage of reach where strober. As the subject moved, the instantaneous position of
peak velocity occurs, and fewer movement units. each LED was digitized at a sampling rate of 100Hz. Any

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 81, January 2000


A KINEMATIC STUDY OF CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS, Wu 97

Table 1: Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Stroke Subjects


No. of Months Side of Ashworth Sensory Bias Index on
Subject Age Sex Poststroke Lesion Score Awareness Score Line Bisection

1 69 M 27.8 Left 7.5 10 .020


2 39 M 46 Right 19 12 .015
3 64 F 40.3 Right 0 14 .0042
4 51 M 11 Both 0 12 .012
5 66 M NA Right 0 6 .12
6 54 F 69* Left 0 14 .0097
7 51 M 5 Left 3 8 .015
8 45 M 174.5 Right 44.5 18 .0093
9 83 M 5 Right 0 12 .02
10 84 M 50 Left 2 14 .0167
11 61 M 58.3 Left 0 18 .00517
12 80 F 13 Left 0 18 .0583
13 46 F 98 Left 8 6 .0584
14 72 F 15 Left 0 16 .0458
The Ashworth score ranks muscle spasticity of the upper extremity.39 The score can range from 0 (normal tone) to 4 (rigidity) for each motion.
Fourteen upper extremity motions were examined in this test. The Sensory Awareness Score detects the patients awareness of joint position of
the impaired limb. Six combinations of shoulder, elbow, and forearm placement were tested. The score ranges from 6 (difficulty in finding the
tape attached to the impaired hand in each of the six positions) to 18 (direct and accurate movements to touch the tape in the six positions).40
The Bias Index on Line Bisection indicates the degree of bisection bias from center. The index can range from 1 to 1, with 0 representing no
bias toward either side, and with a positive value indicating a rightward bias. A bias index of .055 or more indicates left neglect.41
Abbreviation: NA, not available.
* This patient had three separate strokes; 69mo is the time since the first stroke.
Results are not substantially altered by inclusion of subject 8, who exhibited greater spasticity than other subjects.

occluded points were flagged and the data linearly interpolated her hand on the switch and then said go to indicate start of a
for the occluded points if a total of occluded points was 5% or trial. The timing of the go signal was randomized.
less of the data for one trial. One percent of the trials were Each participant received two experimental conditions and
excluded because the occluded points were greater than 5%. performed 10 trials for each condition. The two experimental
The raw data were converted to 3-D coordinates by use of a conditions are described as follows.
direct linear transformation algorithm. The presence of the object. One quarter and one dime were
The start and the end of the reach event were determined placed in front of the participant. The participant was instructed
through the use of one switch and the reference LED. Before to reach forward to scoop the coins off the table into the other
initiating movement, the participants hand rested on a switch hand. After the participant scooped the coins, he or she could
6.3cm in diameter. The beginning of movement was recorded keep the money.
when the hand moved off the switch. The end of movement was The absence of the object. The participant was instructed
determined to be the time when the distance between the to reach forward to the spot where the coins should be placed in
reference LED and the LED attached to the hand of the the condition of object present and then bring his or her arm
participant was minimal. back to the starting point. The participant was instructed to
rotate the forearm into midposition while reaching forward,
Design which was similar to the end position of reaching to the coins in
A counterbalanced repeated-measures design was used. Each the condition of object present. The spot was indicated by the
incoming patient was randomly assigned to one of the two experimenter before the beginning of this condition. There was
sequences: AB and BA, where A and B represent the conditions no visual cue of the spot. The purpose of reaching forward,
of object present and object absent, respectively. rotating the forearm, and bringing the hand back is to try to keep
the biomechanical constraints the same across the two condi-
Procedures tions.
Each person with stroke reached out with the impaired arm,
and the matched neurologically intact adult reached out with the Data Reduction
same arm as that used by the person with stroke. Some persons The 3-D data were filtered using a second order Butterworth
with stroke were matched with more than one nonimpaired filter, with forward and backward pass, at a frequency of 5Hz.
adult. The patient with both hemisphere lesions used his left The displacement data were then processed using a custom
hand because he reported that his right arm could perform tasks written program to provide information on the velocity and
normally. Consequently, six patients and 14 nonimpaired sub- acceleration reaching performance.
jects used their right arms to perform the tasks; eight patients
and 11 controls used their left arms. Data Analysis
Each participant sat on a chair 45.1cm high in front of a table Two 2 2 mixed (ie, one between-factor and one repeated-
73.7cm high in the same testing room. Before the start of the factor) analyses of variance (ANOVA) were used to test the a
experiment, the participant was asked to place the hand on the priori hypothesis for the able-bodied adult participants and
switch located on the table directly in front of the arm. The those with stroke. The between factor was the sequence and the
target object was placed 16.5cm away from the body in line repeated factor (or the within factor) was the order. The
with the participants midsagittal plane. The experimenter said treatment effect, which was essentially a test of the differences
ready to the participant to remind the participant to rest his or in the kinematic variables for the two experimental conditions,

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 81, January 2000


98 A KINEMATIC STUDY OF CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS, Wu

was determined by testing the interaction effect of sequence Table 3: Sequence Order Mixed ANOVA on Kinematic Variables
with order. This practice allowed the use of a more precise error for Persons With Stroke
term by removing the confounding effects of sequence and Sum of Mean
order.42 df Squares Square F p
The data were analyzed using the SAS computer package.b Movement time
The effect size r was calculated for each dependent variable Between 13
using the procedures described by Rosenthal and Rosnow43 to Sequence 1 .0448 .0448 .08 .7854
demonstrate the degree to which performance context exerts an Error 12 6.935 .578
influence on reaching kinematics. The effect size measures are Within 14
free of sample-size influence and can determine the degree to Order 1 .0277 .0277 .32 .5840
which the phenomenon under study is manifested.44 According Order Sequence 1 .996 .996 11.40 .0028
to Cohen, a large effect is represented by an r of at least .50, a Error 12 1.048 .0873
moderate effect by .30, and a small effect by .10.44 Total displacement
Between 13
RESULTS
Sequence 1 28079.26 28079.26 3.16 .1010
Error 12 106782.29 8898.52
Analyses of the Effects of Experimental Conditions
Within 14
Table 2 presents the means of the kinematic variables Order 1 1.108 1.108 0.00 .9802
associated with each testing condition for the stroke patients Order Sequence 1 22056.84 22056.84 12.82 .0019
and the control adults. Except for the variable of peak velocity, Error 12 20643.35 1720.28
patterns of the average performance are consistent with the a Peak velocity
priori hypotheses for both participant groups, that is, the Between 13
condition of object present produced shorter movement time, Sequence 1 44748.81 44748.81 .92 .3568
less total displacement, greater percentage of reach where peak Error 12 584785.12 48732.09
velocity occurs, and fewer movement units than the condition Within 14
of object absent. Order 1 21552.87 21552.87 3.05 .1063
Table 3 displays the sources of variance in the 2 2 mixed Order Sequence 1 2567.82 2567.82 .36 .2791
ANOVA for each kinematic variable for the stroke group. Error 12 84845.27 7070.44
Results of the order by sequence analyses showed significant Percentage of reach where
and large object presence effects for movement time (F(1, peak velocity occurs
12) 11.40, r .70, p .0028), for total displacement (F(1, Between 13
12) 12.82, r .72, p .0019), for percentage of reach Sequence 1 444.65 444.65 3.27 .0957
where peak velocity occurs (F(1, 12) 15.12, r .75, Error 12 1631.99 136.00
p .0011), and for movement units (F(1, 12) 9.05, r .66, Within 14
p .0055). A nonsignificant and small effect was found in the Order 1 5.49 5.49 .12 .7365
variable of peak velocity (F(1, 12) .36, r .17, p .2791). Order Sequence 1 699.80 699.80 15.12 .0011
Table 4 reveals the sources of variance in the 2 2 mixed Error 12 555.51 46.29
ANOVA in the outcome measures for the neurologically intact Movement units
group. Order by sequence results showed significant and large Between 13
object presence effects for movement time (F(1, 23) 160.16, Sequence 1 1.06 1.06 .14 .7140
r .94, p .00005), for total displacement (F(1, 23) 65.70, Error 12 90.36 7.53
r .86, p .00005), and for percentage of reach where peak Within 14
velocity occurs (F(1, 23) 168.44, r .94, p .00005). A Order 1 .84 .84 .59 .4581
marginally significant and moderate effect was found in the Order Sequence 1 12.93 12.93 9.05 .0055
variable of movement units (F(1, 23) 2.86, r .33, Error 12 17.15 1.43
Order refers to the order of administration of the treatment. For
Table 2: Means (Standard Deviations) for the Kinematic Variables example, in the sequence of AB, A (the condition of object present)
for Each Condition was administered first and took the first order, B (the condition of
object absent) was given next and assumed the second order.
Condition
Kinematic Variables Object Present Object Absent
p .0522) and a nonsignificant and modest effect in the
Stroke
variable of peak velocity (F(1, 23) .21, r .095, p .3249).
Movement time (sec) 1.04 (.44) 1.42 (.65)
As shown in table 4, there were significant sequence effects
Total displacement (mm) 292.39 (74.12) 348.52 (80.42)
in movement time and in total displacement, and a significant
Peak velocity (mm/sec) 544.14 (171.57) 563.30 (164.84)
order effect in movement time. Despite the significant sequence and
Percentage of reach where peak
order effects, significant effects of condition (order by sequence)
velocity occurs (%) 39.50 (10.58) 29.50 (9.54)
were obtained after removing the order and the sequence effects
Movement units 2.63 (1.67) 3.99 (2.37)
and was, therefore, not confounded by these effects.
Able-Bodied
Movement time (sec) .47 (.11) .75 (.15) Further Analyses
Total displacement (mm) 203.89 (41.69) 267.26 (52.25)
To provide better understanding of the nature of participant
Peak velocity (mm/sec) 687.51 (154.52) 700.27 (147.05)
performance, the relationships between kinematic profiles and
Percentage of reach where peak
several factors were examined independent of the testing
velocity occurs (%) 50.54 (5.78) 34.90 (6.84)
conditions. The examinations involved correlation between
Movement units .95 (.57) 1.64 (.39)
kinematic variables and clinical tests (ie, the modified Ashworth

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 81, January 2000


A KINEMATIC STUDY OF CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS, Wu 99

Table 4: Sequence Order Mixed ANOVA on Kinematic Variables time, less total displacement, higher peak velocity, greater
for Neurologically Intact Participants percentage of reach where peak velocity occurs, and less
Sum of Mean movement units than the stroke group.
Source df Squares Square F p Effects of laterality of brain damage. The results showed
Movement time moderate, though nonsignificant, effects on total displacement
Between 24 (t(11) 1.32, r .37, p .2130) and percentage of reach
Sequence 1 .2826 .2826 17.11 .0004 where peak velocity occurs (t(11) 1.27, r .36, p .2310).
Error 23 .3798 .0165 Persons with left brain lesions engendered longer total displace-
Within 25 ment and less percentage of reach where peak velocity occurs
Order 1 .0339 .0339 5.41 .0292 than those with right brain lesions.
Order Sequence 1 1.004 1.004 160.16 .00005
Error 23 .144 .00627 DISCUSSION
Total displacement
The findings of this study agree with those of previous
Between 24
research14,15 showing that movements performed under the
Sequence 1 28347.33 28347.33 10.87 .0032
condition of object present were more efficient (shorter move-
Error 23 59970.86 2607.43
ment time), straighter (less total displacement), more pro-
Within 25
grammed (greater percentage of reach where peak velocity
Order 1 1162.74 1162.74 1.51 .2322
occurs), and smoother (fewer movement units) than those
Order Sequence 1 50734.29 50734.29 65.70 .00005
performed under the context without object present. These
Error 23 17760.07 772.18
findings are in accord with the view that providing concrete
Peak velocity
sensory (eg, the presence of the object) and functional informa-
Between 24
tion on object use would enhance performance of functional
Sequence 1 259.457 259.457 .01 .9356
movements. When the object was presented as the end goal of
Error 23 894675.394 38898.93
reaching, the person could scale the spatial relationships
Within 25
between the person and the object without making an extra
Order 1 6236.71 6236.71 .75 .3948
effort to imagine the target location. The scaling of this spatial
Order Sequence 1 1755.61 1755.61 .21 .3249
Error 23 190780.51 8294.80
relationships without the need to imagine the target location
Percentage of reach
may lead to better integrity of the planning of the executive
where peak
neuromuscular system to perform the object-related task,
velocity occurs
compared with the condition of object absent. Therefore, the
Between 24
movement performed was more efficient (ie, less movement
Sequence 1 2.34 2.34 .04 .8494
time) and straighter (ie, less total displacement).45 A complemen-
Error 23 1461.52 63.54
tary explanation for the more efficient and straighter movement
Within 25
under the condition of object present pertains to monetary
Order 1 47.61 47.61 2.65 .1170
rewards. When the objects (ie, coins) were present, the subject
Order Sequence 1 3022.46 3022.46 168.44 .00005
reached for, scooped, and then kept the coins. Rewarding the
Error 23 412.72 17.94
subject with objects of sociocultural value may have motivated
Movement units
the person to perform the task in a more efficient way. This
Between 24
rewarding effect warrants further scrutiny in future research.
Sequence 1 10.75 10.75 3.07 .0930
There are two possible explanations for the results that the
Error 23 80.47 3.50
person performed more programmed and smoother (or more
Within 25
continuous) reaching movements when the target object existed
Order 1 3.33 3.33 1.52 .2302
within the context than when no object was present. The first
Order Sequence 1 6.27 6.27 2.86 .0522
was described earlier, that is, when the target position of a
Error 23 50.46 2.19
reaching movement was decided by presence of a real object,
the participant acquired clear visual information on the object
before performing the task, which facilitated the scaling of the
spatial and temporal characteristics of movements in advance.
scale and the Perception of Joint Position Sense Test) in stroke Therefore, the degree of reliance on external feedback decreased
patients, the differences in movement kinematics between the during task performance; the deceleration phase is shorter (ie,
control and the stroke groups, and the differences in perfor- greater percentage of reach where peak velocity occurs) when the
mance by persons with left and right hemisphere lesions. object is present versus absent. Second, the condition of object
Correlation with clinical tests. A moderate correlation present involved a functional and familiar task that might have been
between movement time and the score of the Perception of the performed numerous times during the participants daily life. The
Joint Position Sense test (r .40, p .1570) was found. As movement of forward reach to scoop coins might thus be more
the awareness of joint position sense decreased, the time for the centrally preprogrammed than the movement involving only
execution of movement increased. reaching forward to a particular location.
Differences between the control and the stroke groups. The beneficial effects of the presence of the object on
The group effects are significant and moderate to large for movement kinematics have practical implications for rehabilita-
movement time (t(13.8) 4.41, r .76, p .0006), total tive treatment of the persons with stroke. Tasks with the use of
displacement (t(18.3) 4.03, r .69, p .0008), peak ve- real objects may improve movement performance of persons
locity (t(37) 2.93, r .43, p .0058), percentage of reach with stroke by enhancing the quality of their movements; that
where peak velocity occurs (t(18.7) 3.13, r .59, p .0056), is, movements may be performed more efficiently, smoothly,
and movement units (t(37) 3.84, r .53, p .0005). The and directly. In addition, use of activities that involve objects of
control group performed movements with shorter movement rewarding values may motivate the patient to accomplish the

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 81, January 2000


100 A KINEMATIC STUDY OF CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS, Wu

task goal in a more efficient manner. Further efficacy studies are objects still hold for other functional tasks? Would the effects
needed to corroborate the effects observed in this study. found in this study be obtained when samples of other stroke
Concerning peak velocity, the two experimental conditions patients or different clinical populations are employed?
produced similar amplitudes of peak velocity. This suggests that Finally, this study discussed the kinematic nature of move-
the variable of peak velocity is not a sensitive outcome variable ment disorders following cerebral vascular accident. Descrip-
for detecting the effects of certain contextual features. tions of the abnormal kinematics observed in persons with
In comparison with neurologically intact controls, the per- stroke and the different kinematics in left versus right hemi-
sons with stroke generated movements that were less efficient sphere damage provides us with a better understanding of the
(longer movement time), less direct (greater total displace- characteristics of reaching movements.
ment), less smooth (more movement units), and slower (lower
peak velocity), and used more feedback for correcting the
ongoing movements (lower percentage of reach where peak References
velocity occurs). This poorer performance might result from the 1. Lin K-C, Wu C-Y, Tickle-Degnen L, Coster W. Enhancing
disturbance of proprioceptive sensation.4 The data from this occupational performance through occupationally embedded exer-
cise: a meta-analytic review. Occup Ther J Res 1997;17:25-47.
study showed that increased impairment of joint position 2. Jeannerod M. The neural and behavioral organization of goal-
sensation moderately correlated with decreased level of move- directed movements. New York: Oxford University Press; 1988.
ment performance. This evidence suggests that persons with 3. Platz T, Denzler P, Kaden B, Mauritz KH. Motor learning after
impaired proprioceptive sensation may not be able to efficiently recovery from hemiparesis. Neuropsychologia 1994;32:1209-23.
use past experience about the relationships of sensory input to 4. Trombly CA. Observations of improvement of reaching in five
motor outcomes and, thus, have difficulty in directly and subjects with left hemiparesis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
efficiently reaching for the end target. The poorer performance 1993;56:40-5.
by the persons with stroke also might be explained by the lack 5. Trombly CA. Deficits of reaching in subjects with left hemiparesis:
of appropriate force generation and control.5 According to Wing a pilot study. Am J Occup Ther 1992;46:887-97.
and colleagues,46 increased peak velocity is associated with 6. Letts L, Law M, Rigby P, Cooper B, Stewart D, Strong S.
Person-environment assessments in occupational therapy. Am J
greater force generation. The persons with stroke generated Occup Ther 1994;48:608-18.
lower peak velocity than the controls, suggesting the patients 7. Mulder T. Current topics in motor control: implications for
might have the problem of force generation. Mattingley and rehabilitation. In: Greenwood R, Barnes MP, McMillan TM, Ward
associates47 suggested that the high proportion of movement CD, editors. Neurological rehabilitation. Edinburgh: Churchill
time spent in the deceleration phase by the persons with stroke Livingstone; 1993. p. 125-34.
implied poor force control that required more submovements. 8. Charlton JL. Motor control considerations for assessment and
In this study, when compared to normal performance, the rehabilitation of movement disorders. In: Summers JJ, editor.
persons with stroke revealed a lower percentage of reach where Approaches to the study of motor control and learning. New York:
peak velocity occurs, that is, prolonged deceleration time, Elsevier; 1992. p. 441-67.
9. Newell KM, Valvano J. Therapeutic intervention as a constraint in
which might stem from loss of force control. Moreover, poor learning and relearning movement skills. Scand J Occup Ther
force control may lead to more distinct changes in movement 1998;5:51-7.
direction, which resulted in more movement units. 10. Dunn W, Brown C, McGuigan A. The ecology of human perfor-
Persons with left hemisphere damage demonstrated less mance: a framework for considering the effect of context. Am J
direct and less programmed movement than those with right Occup Ther 1994;48:595-607.
hemisphere damage. These findings suggested that persons with 11. Rogoff B. Integrating context and cognitive development. Adv
left hemisphere lesions were less able to make use of visual and Dev Psychol 1982;2:125-70.
proprioceptive information to update and modify the trajectory 12. Christiansen C. Occupational therapy: intervention for life perfor-
of the reach than persons with right hemisphere damage mance. In: Christiansen C, Baum C, editors. Occupational therapy:
overcoming human performance deficits. Thorofare (NJ): Slack;
possibly because the left hemisphere is responsible for motor 1991. p. 2-43.
programming and for translating external information into 13. Carr JH, Shepherd RB. Motor relearning programme for stroke.
internal coordinates.48,49 Once the left hemisphere is damaged, 2nd ed. Oxford (UK): William Heinemann Medical Books; 1994.
the ability to translate external information into internal coordi- 14. Wu C-Y, Trombly CA, Lin K-C. The relationship between
nates is diminished. Therefore, movement performance of the occupational form and occupational performance: a kinematic
persons with left-hemisphere damage is worse than that of right perspective. Am J Occup Ther 1994;48:679-87.
hemisphere damage. The results also suggest that damage to the 15. Trombly CA, Wu C-Y. Effect of rehabilitation tasks on organiza-
left hemisphere is more detrimental to the performance of tion of movement post stroke. Am J Occup Ther 1999;53:333-44.
functional tasks with the right arm than right hemisphere 16. Sullivan LW. Striking out stroke in America. Be stroke smart:
damage is to the performance of tasks with the left arm. National Stroke Association Newsletter 1992;9:1, 3.
17. Oudejans RRD, Michaels CF, Bakker FC, Dolne MA. The
CONCLUSION relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of
catchableness of fly balls. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform
One of the unique contributions of this study was the use of 1996;22:879-91.
both nonimpaired controls and persons with stroke to study 18. Lee DN. The function of vision. In: Pick HL, Salzman E, editors.
contextual effects on kinematic characteristics. The results we Modes of perceiving and processing information. Hillsdale (NJ):
obtained corroborate the findings of previous research14 that the Lawrence Erlbaum; 1978. p. 159-69.
presence of an object for functional use enhances the perfor- 19. Clifton RK, Rochat P, Litovsky RY, Perris EE. Object representa-
mance of functional reach, relative to the absence of an object. tion guides infants reaching in the dark. J Exp Psychol: Hum
The clinical implication is that the use of real objects might be Percept Perform 1991;17:323-9.
20. Klapp ST, Porter-Graham KA, Hoifjeld AR. The relation of
an effective way of facilitating efficient, smooth, and coordi- perception and motor action: ideomotor compatibility and interfer-
nated movement with the impaired arm in persons with stroke. ence in divided attention. J Motor Behav 1991;23:155-62.
This study, however, should be replicated and extended to 21. Neumann O. Direct parameter specification and the concept of
confirm the validity of its findings, and to allow for generaliza- perception. Special issue: Domains of mental functioning: at-
tion. For example, would the beneficial effects of the use of real tempts at a synthesis. Psychol Res 1990;52:207-15.

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 81, January 2000


A KINEMATIC STUDY OF CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS, Wu 101

22. Bloch MW, Smith DA, Nelson DL. Heart rate, activity, duration, 36. Brooks VB, Watts SL. Adaptive programming of arm movement. J
and affect in added-purpose versus single-purpose jumping activi- Motor Behav 1988;2:117-32.
ties. Am J Occup Ther 1989;43:25-30. 37. Marteniuk RG, MacKenzie CL, Jeannerod M, Athenes S, Dugas C.
23. DeKuiper WP, Nelson DL, White BE. Materials-based occupation Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. Can J Psychol
versus imagery-based occupation versus rote exercise: a replica- 1987;41:365-78.
tion and extension. Occup Ther J Res 1993;13:183-97. 38. Nagasaki H. Asymmetric velocity and acceleration profiles of
24. Kircher MA. Motivation as a factor of perceived exertion in human arm movements. Exp Brain Res 1989;74:319-26.
purposeful versus nonpurposeful activity. Am J Occup Ther 39. Bohannon RW, Smith MD. Interrater reliability of a modified
1984;38:165-70. Ashworth scale of muscle spasticity. Phys Ther 1987;67:206-7.
25. Lang EM, Nelson DL, Bush MA. Comparison of performance in 40. Leo K, Soderberg G. Relationship between perception of joint
materials-based occupation, imagery-based occupation, and rote position sense and limb synergies in patients with hemiplegia.
exercise in nursing home residents. Am J Occup Ther 1992;46: Phys Ther 1981;61:1433-7.
607-11. 41. Lin K-C, Cermak S, Kinsbourne M, Trombly CA. Effects of
26. Zimmerer-Branum S, Nelson DL. Occupationally embedded exer- cueing on line bisection in post-stroke patients with unilateral
cise versus rote exercise: A choice between occupational forms by neglect. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1996;2:404-11.
elderly nursing home residents. Am J Occup Ther 1995;49:397-
42. Rosenthal R, Rosnow RL. Essentials of behavioral research:
404.
methods and data analysis. 2nd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill
27. Hsieh C-L, Nelson DL, Smith DA, Peterson CQ. A comparison of
Publishing Company; 1991.
performance in added-purpose occupations and rote exercise for
dynamic standing balance in persons with hemiplegia. Am J Occup 43. Rosenthal R, Rosnow RL. Contrast analysis: focused comparisons
Ther 1996;50:10-6. in the analysis of variance. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge Univer-
28. Sidaway B, Sekiya H, Fairweather M. Movement variability as a sity Press; 1985.
function of accuracy demand in programmed serial aiming re- 44. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd
sponses. J Motor Behav 1995;27:67-76. ed. Hillsdale (NJ): Erlbaum; 1988.
29. Georgopoulos AP. On reaching. Annu Rev Neurosci 1986;9: 45. Sage HG. Motor learning and control: a neuropsychological
147-70. approach. Dubuque (IA): Wm C Brown; 1984.
30. Shepherd RB. Physiotherapy in paediatrics. 3rd ed. Oxford: 46. Wing AM, Lough S, Turton A, Fraser C, Jenner JR. Recovery of
Butterworth-Heinemann Ltd; 1995. elbow function in voluntary positioning of the hand following
31. Thelen E, Skala KD, Kelso JAS. The dynamic nature of early hemiplegia due to stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1990;53:
coordination: Evidence from bilateral leg movements in young 126-34.
infants. Dev Psychol 1987;3:179-86. 47. Mattingley JB, Phillips JG, Gradshaw JL. Impairments of move-
32. Wing AM, Miller E. Research note: peak velocity timing invari- ment execution in unilateral neglect: a kinematic analysis of
ance. Psychol Res 1984;46;121-7. directional bradykinesia. Neuropsychologia 1994;32:1111-34.
33. Ada L, ODwyer NJ, Neilson PD. Improvement in kinematic 48. Haaland KY, Delaney HD. Motor deficits after left or right
characteristics and coordination following stroke quantified by hemisphere damage due to stroke or tumor. Neuropsychologia
linear systems analysis. Hum Movement Sci 1993;12:137-53. 1981;17:17-27.
34. Kluzik J, Fetters L, Coryell J. Quantification of control: a 49. Kimura D, Archibald Y. Motor functions of the left hemisphere.
preliminary study of effects of neurodevelopmental treatment on Brain 1974;100:527-42.
reaching in children with spastic cerebral palsy. Phys Ther
1990;70:65-76.
35. Brooks VB, Kennedy PR, Ross HG. Movement programming Suppliers
depends on understanding of behavioral requirements. Physiol a. Northern Digital, Inc., Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3V2, Canada.
Behav 1983;31:561-3. b. SAS Institute Inc., Cory, NC 27513-2414.

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 81, January 2000

You might also like