You are on page 1of 14

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2000, 33, 181194 NUMBER 2 (SUMMER 2000)

SKILL ACQUISITION IN THE IMPLEMENTATION OF


FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS METHODOLOGY
BRIAN A. IWATA, MICHELE D. WALLACE, SUNGWOO KAHNG,
JANA S. LINDBERG, EILEEN M. ROSCOE, JULIET CONNERS,
GREGORY P. HANLEY, RACHEL H. THOMPSON, AND APRIL S. WORSDELL
THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

Functional analysis methodology is a powerful assessment tool for identifying contingen-


cies that maintain a wide range of behavior disorders and for developing effective treat-
ment programs. Nevertheless, concerns have been raised about the feasibility of con-
ducting functional analyses in typical service settings. In this study, we examined the issue
of skill acquisition in implementing functional analyses by evaluating an instructional
program designed to establish a basic set of competencies. Eleven undergraduate students
enrolled in a laboratory course in applied behavior analysis served as participants. Their
performance was assessed during scripted simulations in which they played the roles of
therapists who conducted functional analyses and trained graduate students played the
roles of clients who emitted self-injurious and destructive behaviors. To approximate
conditions under which an individual might conduct an assessment with limited prior
training, participants read a brief set of materials prior to conducting baseline sessions.
A multiple baseline design was used to assess the effects of training, which consisted of
reading additional materials, watching a videotaped simulation demonstrating correct
procedural implementation, passing a written quiz, and receiving feedback on perfor-
mance during sessions. Results showed that participants scored a relatively high percentage
of correct therapist responses during baseline, and that all achieved an accuracy level of
95% or higher following training that lasted about 2 hr. These results suggest that basic
skills for conducting functional analyses can be acquired quickly by individuals who have
relatively little clinical experience.
DESCRIPTORS: assessment, functional analysis, staff training

Assessment procedures that identify the ior disorders (e.g., Individuals with Disabil-
functional characteristics of behavior are ities Education Act, 1975/1997). Thus,
powerful tools for identifying sources of re- functional approaches to behavioral assess-
inforcement that maintain behavior disor- ment have come to be regarded as best prac-
ders and for developing subsequent treat- tice in both clinical research and application.
ment programs. In addition to having gen- Many techniques have been subsumed
erated a great deal of interest in the research under the general label functional assessment,
literature (e.g., see special issue of the Journal including indirect methods such as inter-
of Applied Behavior Analysis, Vol. 27, No. 2, views and questionnaires, descriptive analy-
1994), these approaches to assessment have ses via direct observation, and systematic
been mandated in recent court decisions and manipulations of environmental conditions
legislation on behalf of persons with devel- (for reviews, see Iwata, Vollmer, & Zarcone,
opmental disabilities who have severe behav- 1990; Mace, Lalli, & Lalli, 1991). The most
precise methods for identifying behavioral
This research was supported in part by a grant from
the Florida Department of Children and Families. function are derived from procedures that
SungWoo Kahng is now at the Kennedy Krieger In- are commonly used in applied behavior anal-
stitute, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine. ysis and are characterized by objective mea-
Reprints may be obtained from Brian Iwata, Psy-
chology Department, The University of Florida, sures of ongoing behavior taken under mul-
Gainesville, Florida 32611. tiple test and control conditions, in which

181
182 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

antecedent and consequent events are clearly while interacting with a client, and it could
prescribed and are arranged in such a way as be argued that staff who lack this skill would
to identify functional relations between en- also be unable to implement most behavioral
vironment and behavior. These latter meth- interventions with any degree of consistency.
ods are commonly referred to as experi- Logical arguments aside, little is known
mental analysis or functional analysis about the extent to which (or the speed with
methodologies, and their utility has been which) staff can acquire a basic set of skills
demonstrated repeatedly over the past 15 for conducting functional analyses. Staff
years. training has rarely been a topic of empirical
Nevertheless, concerns have been raised investigation in research on the assessment
about the feasibility of conducting function- and treatment of behavior disorders because
al analyses in typical service settings. The a high degree of competence is usually a pre-
most commonly cited limitations of func- requisite for conducting a study. In other
tional analyses are the fact that they may be words, implementation errors in clinical re-
lengthy (Applegate, Matson, & Cherry, search are typically minimized as unwanted
1999; Durand & Crimmins, 1988; Pyles, sources of risk to both clients and staff as
Riordan, & Bailey, 1997; Sturmey, 1994) well as potential sources of experimental
and that their implementation may require confounding and, as a result, have not been
a considerable amount of training and clin- examined systematically as dependent vari-
ical expertise (Crawford, Brockel, Schauss, ables (see Shore, Iwata, Vollmer, Lerman, &
& Miltenberger, 1992; Durand & Crim- Zarcone, 1995, as an exception).
mins, 1988; Spreat & Connelly, 1996; Stur- In this study, we evaluated the effects of
mey, 1994). Although conducting a func- a training program designed to establish ba-
tional analysis is a more lengthy process than sic competence in conducting functional
is administering a questionnaire, it is not analyses. Of particular interest were (a) the
necessarily any more time consuming than extent to which untrained individuals could
is performing a descriptive analysis (Lerman implement a functional analysis with mini-
& Iwata, 1993). Furthermore, recent re- mal instruction, and (b) the amount of
search has yielded several ways for increasing training that was necessary to produce a high
the efficiency of functional analyses by con- degree of technical accuracy.
ducting either fewer (Northup et al., 1991)
or briefer (Wallace & Iwata, 1999) sessions.
METHOD
By contrast, the issue of procedural com-
plexity has not been addressed systematically. Participants and Setting
Thus, suggestions that many staff members Therapists. Although staffing arrange-
in positions to develop such programs lack ments vary across settings, the entry-level in-
the professional training to conduct such dividual responsible for conducting assess-
complex analyses (Spreat & Connelly, ments, developing service plans, and imple-
1996, p. 528) remain largely unanswered. menting interventions is usually the teacher
A logical basis for routinely teaching staff or the qualified mental retardation profes-
how to conduct functional analyses can be sional with a BA degree. Therefore, we se-
found through consideration of the skills lected as therapists upper-level undergradu-
needed to implement assessment and treat- ate students who were representative of the
ment programs. A functional analysis re- types of staff who might be expected to con-
quires the ability to deliver a prescribed se- duct functional analyses. The participants
quence of antecedent and consequent events were 11 students (10 women and 1 man)
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS METHODOLOGY 183

who were enrolled in an applied behavior orders (see Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, &
analysis laboratory course. All participants Richman, 1982/1994). A fourth condi-
were junior or senior psychology majors who tionalonewas deleted because it did not
had one prior course in behavior analysis but require the presence of a therapist. Correct
no practicum experience in behavior analysis implementation of the assessment conditions
and no experience in the use of functional involved the delivery and removal of pre-
analysis methodology. The study was con- scribed antecedent and consequent events by
ducted at the beginning of the term, before a therapist, as indicated below. (Note that
participants had the opportunity to observe condition descriptions depict ideal respons-
ongoing assessment (functional analysis) or es, which were not necessarily observed dur-
treatment sessions. Although participants ing baseline.)
were required to complete this project as During the attention condition, the client
part of their course activities, their perfor- was given free access to several leisure items
mance in the study did not count toward throughout the session. The therapist ig-
their course grades. All participants provided nored the client throughout the session, ex-
written informed consent for the use of their cept to deliver attention in the form of state-
data as part of a research project. ments of concern (e.g., Stop, dont do that;
Clients. In order to collect data on the youll hurt yourself ) and brief physical con-
performance of therapists who were relative- tact (e.g., a pat on the back) following each
ly untrained, it was necessary to create a con- occurrence of the target behavior. During
text for conducting functional analyses that
the demand condition, the therapist pre-
did not place either therapists or actual cli-
sented learning trials to the client through-
ents who had severe behavior problems at
out the session, which were initiated at 30-
risk. Therefore, 8 graduate students, all of
s intervals using a three-prompt sequence
whom had extensive experience conducting
(verbal instruction, instruction plus demon-
functional analyses, played the roles of cli-
ents throughout the study. stration, instruction plus physical prompt).
Settings. Training sessions for therapists The therapist delivered praise if the client
were conducted either in a university class- complied, continued the prompting se-
room or in various therapy rooms of a day- quence if the client did not comply, and ter-
treatment program located on the grounds minated the trial (by removing the task ma-
of a state residential facility for persons with terials and turning away from the client until
developmental disabilities, where most of the the next trial) if the client exhibited a target
students laboratory work was conducted. All behavior at any time during the trial. During
functional analysis sessions were conducted the play condition, the client had free access
at the day program. to several leisure items throughout the ses-
sion. The therapist delivered attention on a
Functional Analysis Conditions fixed-time (FT) 30-s schedule throughout
The experimental task involved imple- the session, responded to any client-initiated
mentation of procedures that typically com- appropriate social interactions, and ignored
prise a functional analysis. Three assessment all occurrences of inappropriate behavior
conditions (attention, demand, and play) (target and nontarget). If the client was en-
were selected as representative of those com- gaged in inappropriate behavior when the
monly used in functional analyses involving FT schedule called for the delivery of atten-
the manipulation of antecedent and conse- tion, attention was delayed until inappro-
quent events associated with behavior dis- priate behavior had ceased for 5 s.
184 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

Assessment Simulations Table 1


Sample Scenario for the Attention Condition
We developed a series of scripts that spec-
ified the occurrence of various client behav- Time Scripted client behavior
iors during 5-min simulated assessment ses- (minutes:
seconds) Response class Topography
sions representing each of the three condi-
tions described above. The behaviors exhib- 0:16 SIB Hit self
0:18 SIB Hit self
ited by clients included (a) self-injurious 0:30 SIB Hit self
behavior (SIB), which was the target behav- 0:43 SIB Hit self
ior during the functional analysis and con- 0:56 SIB Hit self
1:21 Disruption Kick wall
sisted of striking an arm, leg, or part of the 1:50 Appropriate play Put toy in therapists
torso with the hand; (b) disruption, a non- hand
target problem behavior, which consisted of 2:01 SIB Hit self
2:12 SIB Hit self
kicking a wall; kicking, throwing, or knock- 2:22 SIB Hit self
ing over furniture; or jumping up and down 2:33 SIB Hit self
while screaming; (c) appropriate play, which 2:44 Social initiation Ask Can we play?
2:58 SIB Hit self
consisted of either manipulation of leisure 3:35 Disruption Throw chair
materials in a nondestructive manner or at- 4:02 SIB Hit self
tempts to hand the materials to the thera- 4:12 SIB Hit self
pist; (d) compliance with an instruction giv- 4:23 SIB Hit self
4:35 SIB Hit self
en by the therapist; and (e) appropriate ini- 4:45 Appropriate play Tug therapists sleeve
tiation of social interaction, which consisted with toy in hand
of asking the therapist to play or tugging on 4:56 SIB Hit self
the therapists sleeve. Fifteen scenarios were
created, five each for the attention, demand,
directive to perform a task, delivered either
and play conditions. Each scenario con-
with or without a supplemental prompt.
tained the same number and distribution of These two behaviors generated a larger num-
client behaviors; however, both the sequence ber of scoring categories because both could
and temporal occurrence of these behaviors be initiated or terminated as either anteced-
throughout a session were varied from one ent or consequent events with respect to cli-
scenario to the next. For example, each sce- ent behavior. Therapist behaviors were
nario called for 15 occurrences of SIB, which scored as either correct or incorrect based on
occurred at different times across scenarios. their occurrence, nonoccurrence, or termi-
A sample scenario for an attention session is nation relative to either a prescribed tem-
provided in Table 1. poral sequence or the occurrence (or non-
occurrence) of client behavior. For example,
Target Therapist Behaviors
the delivery of an antecedent event (e.g.,
The behaviors of primary interest were noncontingent attention during the play
the performances of the undergraduate stu- condition or instructions during the demand
dents, who played the roles of therapists condition) was scored as correct if it oc-
while interacting with clients during the curred at the appropriate time or as incorrect
simulated functional analyses. The designat- if it did not occur. The delivery of conse-
ed therapist behaviors consisted of (a) atten- quences (e.g., contingent attention during
tion, defined as any social interaction, in- the attention condition or termination of a
cluding reprimands, unrelated to a specific trial during the demand condition) was
task; and (b) instruction, defined as a verbal scored as correct if it followed a designated
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS METHODOLOGY 185

client behavior within 5 s or as incorrect if Participants were given the method sec-
it did not occur following a target client be- tion of the Iwata et al. (1982/1994) article
havior. Finally, the nondelivery of conse- and were asked to read it several days prior
quences (e.g., the absence of attention for to initiating the collection of baseline data
appropriate behavior during the attention (the exact number of days varied from 1 to
condition) was scored as correct if it did not 5 because of scheduling differences). On the
occur or as incorrect if it did occur. first day of baseline, participants were given
an additional 30 min to study the materials,
Response Measurement and Reliability after which they were asked to serve as ther-
All sessions lasted for 5 min and were vid- apists while conducting functional analysis
eotaped and scored later by graduate stu- sessions (attention, demand, and play con-
dents, who recorded occurrences of therapist ditions) with graduate students playing the
and client behaviors using a 10-s partial-in- roles of clients. Prior to each baseline ses-
terval recording procedure. Client behaviors sion, participants were told which type of
were scored during all sessions, and compar- assessment condition they were to conduct
isons of these data with sequences specified but were given no further instructions. Par-
in the scripts provided a measure of proce- ticipants received no feedback on their per-
dural fidelity; these comparisons always ex- formance following baseline sessions.
ceeded 90% accuracy. A second observer
scored 26% of the videotapes to collect in- Training
dependent data on the occurrence of thera- Training was conducted by one or more
pist behaviors. Observers records were com- graduate students in two phases. The first
pared on an interval-by-interval basis, and phase was conducted in a group format in a
percentage agreement scores were calculated university classroom. Participants were first
by dividing the number of scoring agree- given written descriptions and outlines of
ments (for the occurrence or nonoccurrence the assessment conditions to read. The de-
of a behavior) by the number of agreements scriptions were brief summaries based on the
plus disagreements, and multiplying by Iwata et al. (1982/1994) article that high-
100%. The mean agreement score for ther- lighted salient components: the purpose of a
apist behaviors was 93.9% (range, 86.0% to condition, target behaviors, and how to con-
100%). duct a session. Descriptions used for each
condition are contained in Appendix A, and
Baseline their corresponding outlines are contained in
Although exposure to any training during Appendix B. A graduate student then re-
baseline would preclude the assessment of viewed the key components of each condi-
naive performance, we presumed that such tion and showed a videotaped simulation of
performance would be uninformative from a each condition, in which one graduate stu-
practical standpoint because it is unlikely dent played the role of therapist while an-
that anyone would conduct a functional other played the role of client. Following this
analysis with no information whatsoever. exercise, participants took a 20-item written
Thus, the baseline was constructed to ap- quiz containing simple factual questions
proximate a situation in which a therapist about the assessment process (a sample quiz
who was attempting to conduct a functional can be found in Appendix C). If a partici-
analysis had access to published material but pant scored below 90% correct on the quiz,
received no formal instruction, assistance, or a graduate student reviewed the quiz answers
feedback. with the participant, showed the videotape
186 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

again, and administered another quiz. This functional analysis conditions) prior to re-
phase of training was complete when all par- ceiving training.
ticipants scored 90% correct or higher on a
quiz. All participants achieved a passing
score on either their first or second quiz, and RESULTS
all training activities in this phase were com- Figure 1 shows participants performance
pleted in approximately 1.5 hr. during the simulated functional analysis ses-
Following the classroom training, partici- sions, expressed as the percentage of correct
pants again conducted functional analysis therapist responses. Baseline performances
sessions as in baseline, with two exceptions. were generally high (M 5 69.9%; range of
These two changes from baseline procedures individual means, 50.0% to 89.5%), al-
represented the second phase of training, though the data reflect a great deal of vari-
which was conducted at the day program. ability. In addition, the performances of sev-
First, participants were allowed to bring the eral participants, especially Carrie, Cindee,
outline into a session, which they could use and Ricki, showed noticeable upward trends
as a cue. Second, they were given feedback during baseline. All participants improved
on their performance by a graduate student following training (M 5 97.5%; range of
immediately following each set of three ses- individual means, 92.1% to 100%) and
sions (one session of each condition). If a completed the study after conducting the
participant scored less than 95% correct re- minimum number of posttraining sessions.
sponses during a session, the graduate stu- A comparison of individual performances
dent replayed the videotape of that session across conditions showed that none of their
while pointing out both correct and incor- baseline means exceeded 90%, whereas all of
rect aspects of the participants performance. their posttraining means exceeded 95%,
This sequence continued until a participant with the exception Karen, whose posttrain-
completed two consecutive sessions of each ing mean was 92.1% because of one low
of the three conditions (excluding the first score (her first demand condition following
posttraining session of each condition) at or training). Across all participants, 33 of the
above 95% accuracy. The total duration of 51 baseline scores were below 80%; by con-
trast, 69 of the 99 posttraining scores were
training in this second phase (including
100%.
feedback and repeat viewing of videotapes
but excluding actual session time) varied
across participants from 10 min to 30 min. DISCUSSION
Experimental Design It has been suggested that the skills re-
quired to conduct functional analyses of be-
Participants conducted simulated func- havior disorders exceed those of staff who are
tional analysis sessions, as described above, typically responsible for performing assess-
in a fixed repeating sequence (attention, ments and, furthermore, that the training
play, demand). The effects of training were necessary to develop such skills may be ex-
evaluated in a multiple baseline across sub- cessive. We examined these issues in the
jects design. Five participants conducted present study by observing the performance
three sessions (one set of functional analysis of undergraduate students playing the roles
conditions) under baseline conditions prior of therapists while conducting simulated
to receiving training; the remaining 6 par- functional analyses with graduate students
ticipants conducted six sessions (two sets of playing the roles of clients who exhibited
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS METHODOLOGY 187

Figure 1. Percentage of correct therapist responses during simulated functional analysis sessions across base-
line and posttraining conditions.

problem behaviors. After reading excerpts of sponses during baseline sessions. Subse-
previously published research describing one quently, all participants achieved a 95% ac-
method for conducting a functional analysis, curacy criterion after receiving about 2 hr of
participants averaged about 70% correct re- training, which consisted of reading addi-
188 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

tional materials, watching a videotape, pass- functional analyses, we postponed these ac-
ing a quiz, and receiving feedback on their tivities as long as possible at the beginning
performance. These results suggest that staff of the term to insure that sources of infor-
with academic preparation equivalent to that mation (e.g., additional readings, extended
of a BA degree can quickly acquire a basic observation of sessions) other than those de-
set of skills for conducting functional anal- scribed as training in this study would not
yses. influence the data. Nevertheless, the baseline
The fact that our participants exhibited a trends represent a weakness in the study and
high percentage of correct responses during raise the possibility that some participants
baseline deserves further comment. First, be- eventually may have achieved the posttrain-
cause their baseline performance was not ing performance criterion with no additional
completely naive, it was probably much instruction. All of these factors suggest that
higher than that of individuals who would participants did reasonably well in their ini-
attempt to conduct a functional analysis tial attempts to conduct sessions with min-
with absolutely no prior instruction. As not- imal exposure to information (access to read-
ed earlier, however, we felt that truly naive ing material) and that they may have im-
performances would provide highly unrep- proved further with practice.
resentative samples of behavior likely to be The training procedures used in this study
exhibited by staff who actually attempt to were neither novel nor complex. All of the
conduct functional analyses. Second, the instructional components, including written
amount of information participants received materials, video simulations, quizzes, and
prior to conducting baseline sessions was performance feedback, are common features
limited to brief exposure to a single source of many staff training curricula (Reid, Par-
(Iwata et al., 1982/1994). We selected the sons, & Green, 1989) and, with the excep-
source because it is frequently cited in the tion of the quizzes, bear close resemblance
literature and because it serves as the basis to training programs implemented with
for functional analyses conducted in our paraprofessional staff such as direct-care ser-
program. In reality, it is likely that staff who vice providers (e.g., Ducharme & Feldman,
attempt to initiate assessments based on 1992) and respite-care workers (e.g., Neef,
functional analysis methodology would do Trachtenberg, Loeb, & Sterner, 1991). In
so only after having read more extensively. addition, graduate students prepared all of
In spite of this limited exposure, participants the training materials used in this study and
performed rather well during the baseline conducted all of the training sessions. Thus,
sessions. Third, the performance of several the level and extent of training provided in
participants showed increasing trends during this study were probably representative of
baseline. We predicted (somewhat inaccu- those available in many service settings that
rately) that participants baseline perfor- employ behavior analysts with advanced de-
mance would be uniformly poor and would grees.
not improve; therefore, we arbitrarily set the Although the participants in this study ac-
baseline lengths at the beginning of the quired a very high degree of proficiency in
study. It would have been preferable to con- conducting functional analysis sessions, the
tinue taking baseline data until trends sta- findings are limited in two significant re-
bilized, but this was not possible from a spects. First, all performance was assessed
practical standpoint. Because the partici- under simulated conditions. Although re-
pants laboratory responsibilities included sults from several studies suggest that staff
observing and, in some cases, conducting training conducted under simulated condi-
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS METHODOLOGY 189

tions generalizes to clinical situations (e.g., it is possible to establish levels of compe-


Ducharme & Feldman, 1992; Jones & Ei- tence that are comparable to those reported
mers, 1975; Neef et al., 1991), the extent to here in less than half a day. The consultative
which our participants could adequately nature of this work precluded an experimen-
conduct functional analyses with actual cli- tal arrangement, so the collection of con-
ents immediately following training is un- trolled data in clinical contexts should be
known. We did not attempt to measure per- considered an important area for future
formance under actual clinical conditions for work.
two reasons. First, unlike other research on The second limitation in the present
staff training, in which participants (train- study was that training focused on a circum-
ees) were actual service providers or in which scribed set of skills. It is important to note
interventions involved teaching adaptive be- that the implementation of functional anal-
havior to clients, the participants in this ysis methodology under actual clinical con-
study had virtually no prior experience ditions may require professional judgments
working with any clinical population, and related to data interpretation, modification
the actual clients all exhibited behavior of assessment conditions to identify idiosyn-
problems (i.e., SIB or aggression) that placed cratic maintaining variables, risk manage-
either them or therapists at risk. Therefore, ment, and so forth. Although these addi-
the use of performance probes with actual tional skills were not examined in the pres-
clients immediately after training was ent study, to the extent that they can be de-
deemed unacceptable. Second, although a scribed objectively, it is likely that they could
number of participants eventually did con- be taught in component fashion using meth-
duct functional analysis sessions during the ods similar to those employed here (e.g., see
course of their laboratory work, several in- the recent work of Hagopian et al., 1997,
tervening educational experiences (e.g., read- on interpretation of functional analysis out-
ing a series of related articles, collecting pri- comes). Of course, the full benefit of adopt-
mary and reliability data from assessment ing functional analysis methodology is real-
and treatment sessions, conducting prefer- ized when it is placed within the context of
ence assessments with clients) had transpired a program that integrates empirical ap-
since the completion of training provided in proaches to individual assessment with ad-
this study, making it difficult to identify the vanced therapeutic technology, and an anal-
necessary components required to produce ysis of the skills required to develop such a
competence under clinical conditions. As an program extends far beyond the scope of any
extension of the work described in this single study.
study, however, each of the authors has par- Given these limitations, the present results
ticipated in the training of therapists (in- are nevertheless encouraging in light of re-
cluding paraprofessional staff ) in residential cent suggestions that functional analysis
or school programs for persons with devel- methodology is too complicated for use un-
opmental disabilities. The instructional pro- der typical clinical conditions. Most service
gram has usually been conducted in a group settings to which clients with severe behavior
context and has included the textual, role- disorders are referred employ one or more
play, videotape, and performance feedback therapists with graduate training that in-
components used in the present study, with cludes course work in behavior analysis. The
an additional component consisting of dem- procedures and data presented here suggest
onstration and practice with actual clients. that these therapists should be sufficiently
Using these procedures, we have found that skilled to train both themselves and others
190 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

to implement a number of the experimental Neef, N. A., Trachtenberg, S., Loeb, J., & Sterner, K.
(1991). Video-based training of respite care pro-
approaches to behavioral assessment that viders: An interactional analysis of presentation
comprise functional analysis methodology. format. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24,
473486.
Northup, J., Wacker, D., Sasso, G., Steege, M., Cig-
REFERENCES rand, K., Cook, J., & DeRaad, A. (1991). A brief
functional analysis of aggressive and alternative
Applegate, H., Matson, J. L., & Cherry, K. E. (1999). behavior in an outclinic setting. Journal of Applied
An evaluation of functional variables affecting se- Behavior Analysis, 24, 509522.
vere problem behaviors in adults with mental re- Pyles, D. A. M., Riordan, M. M., & Bailey, J. S.
tardation by using the Questions about Behavioral (1997). The stereotypy analysis: An instrument
Function Scale (QBAF). Research in Developmenal for examining environmental variables associated
Disabilities, 20, 229237. with differential rates of stereotypic behavior. Re-
Crawford, J., Brockel, B., Schauss, S., & Miltenberger,
search in Developmental Disabilities, 18, 1138.
R. G. (1992). A comparison of methods for the
Reid, D. H., Parsons, M. B., & Green, C. W. (1989).
functional assessment of stereotypic behavior.
Staff management in human services: Behavioral re-
Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe
Handicaps, 17, 7786. search and application. Springfield, IL: Charles C.
Ducharme, J. M., & Feldman, M. A. (1992). Com- Thomas.
parison of staff training strategies to promote gen- Shore, B. A., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Lerman, D.
eralized teaching skills. Journal of Applied Behavior C., & Zarcone, J. R. (1995). Pyramidal staff
Analysis, 25, 165179. training in the extension of treatment for severe
Durand, V. M., & Crimmins, D. B. (1988). Identi- behavior disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior
fying the variables maintaining self-injurious be- Analysis, 28, 323332.
havior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- Spreat, S., & Connelly, L. (1996). Reliability analysis
orders, 18, 99117. of the Motivation Assessment Scale. American
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., Journal on Mental Retardation, 100, 528532.
Owen-DeSchryver, J., Iwata, B. A., & Wacker, D. Sturmey, P. (1994). Assessing the functions of aber-
P. (1997). Toward the development of structured rant behaviors: A review of psychometric instru-
criteria for interpretation of functional analysis ments. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-
data. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, orders, 24, 293304.
313326. Wallace, M. D., & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Effects of
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act, 20 U.S.C. session duration on functional analysis outcomes.
1400 et seq. (1975, as amended, 1997). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 175183.
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K.
E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a func- Received October 8, 1999
tional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Be- Final acceptance February 23, 2000
havior Analysis, 27, 197209. (Reprinted from Action Editor, David P. Wacker
Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Dis-
abilities, 2, 320, 1982)
Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., & Zarcone, J. R. (1990).
The experimental (functional) analysis of behavior
disorders: Methodology, applications, and limita-
APPENDIX A
tions. In A. C. Repp & N. N. Singh (Eds.), Per-
spectives on the use of nonaversive and aversive in- PROCEDURAL DESCRIPTIONS OF
terventions for persons with developmental disabili- ASSESSMENT CONDITIONS
ties (pp. 301330). Sycamore, IL: Sycamore.
Jones, F. H., & Eimers, R. C. (1975). Role playing Attention Condition
to train elementary teachers to use a classroom Purpose
management skill package. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 8, 421433. This condition is designed to determine
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1993). Descriptive whether the target behavior is maintained by
and experimental analyses of variables maintaining
self-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior contingent attention delivered by a therapist.
Analysis, 26, 293319. The condition involves remaining in a room
Mace, F. C., Lalli, J. S., & Lalli, E. P. (1991). Func- with a client and ignoring all client behavior,
tional analysis and treatment of aberrant behavior.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 12, 155 except for the target behavior, which is fol-
180. lowed by attention.
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS METHODOLOGY 191

Target Behavior ically block the hitting response, but do not


The target behavior being assessed con- physically restrain the client. The general
sists of self-hitting, defined as one body part idea is to express concern, briefly interrupt
(arm, hand, etc.) striking against another the behavior, and calm the client. Do not
body part. shout at the client and do not handle the
client roughly.
How to Conduct a Session 6. After a target behavior occurs and you
have responded as indicated above (Step 5),
1. Begin a session by directing the client
resume ignoring the client until another tar-
toward the leisure materials that are present
get behavior occurs or until the session is
in the room. Tell the client that he or she over.
should play with the toys while you do some
work. Demand (Escape) Condition
2. After issuing the initial instruction, Purpose
move away from the client, sit in another This condition is designed to determine
chair, read or do some paperwork (or pre- whether the target behavior is maintained by
tend to do so), and completely ignore all escape from task demands. The condition
behaviors exhibited by the client except as involves presenting a series of instructional
noted below. trials to a client. Compliance produces
3. If the target behavior does not occur praise, noncompliance produces a series of
during the session, you will ignore the client prompts, and occurrence of the target be-
for the entire session. Someone will inform havior immediately terminates the trial.
you when the session is over.
4. If any behaviors other than the target Target Behavior
behavior occur, ignore these also. Examples The target behavior being assessed con-
include appropriate behaviors (e.g., playing sists of self-hitting, defined as one body part
with the toys, smiling at you, or any at- (arm, hand, etc.) striking against another
tempts to talk to you or to interact with you body part.
in an appropriate manner) and inappropriate
behaviors other than self-hitting (e.g., How to Conduct a Session
screaming, throwing materials, running 1. Begin a session with you and the client
around the room, aggression, etc.). seated at a table. Using the materials that are
5. The only time you will attend to the available, you will implement a series of tri-
client is when he or she engages in self-hit- als to teach the client to perform a task. The
ting. If the client exhibits the target behavior task selected for this simulation is putting
of self-hitting at any time during the session, blocks in a bucket.
do the following: (a) Go over to the client 2. Activate a stopwatch at the beginning
and verbally express concern and disapprov- of the session. At the beginning of every 30-
al. For example, you could say something s interval (starting at 0), you will initiate an
like, Stop that, youre going to hurt your- instructional trial. Thus, there will be ap-
self, [Name], you shouldnt hit yourself; proximately 10 trials during a 5-min session.
play with your toys, [Name], I dont want Begin each training trial with the bucket and
you to do that; youre going to get hurt, or a block on the table in front of the client.
something similar. (b) While you express The sequence to be used during each trial is
concern, briefly touch the clients arm, place as follows: (a) First deliver a clear instruction
your hand on the clients shoulder, or phys- to the client, such as [Name], put the block
192 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

in the bucket. If the client performs the (arm, hand, etc.) striking against another
response within 5 s (count to 5 slowly to body part.
determine this), or at least begins to initiate
the response during that time, deliver praise How to Conduct a Session
(e.g., say nice job, thats great, good, 1. Begin a session by activating a stop-
etc.) when the client has finished. (b) If the watch and directing the client toward the
client does not perform the response within leisure materials that are present in the
5 s, repeat the instruction and simultaneous- room. You may say something like, Here
ly demonstrate the response (i.e., you put a are some nice toys; why dont you play with
block in the bucket). If, following this dem- them for a while? or Would you like to
onstration, the client performs the response play with these toys? (as you hand one to
in 5 s, deliver praise as noted above. (c) If the client), or anything similar.
the client does not perform the response 2. At least once every 30 s, deliver some
within 5 s of your demonstration, repeat the form of attention to the client. For example,
instruction again and simultaneously provide you can tell the client that he or she is play-
physical assistance. That is, use your hands ing nicely, ask if he or she is having fun, and
to help the client pick up the block and put so forth. You can also hand the client an-
it in the bucket. Do not deliver praise if you other toy, pat the client briefly on the shoul-
used physical assistance. (d) If, at any time der, or smile at the client. The general idea
during this sequence, the client emits the is to provide some type of friendly, nonde-
target behavior (self-hitting), immediately manding interaction (lasting about 5 s) at
terminate the trial. Remove the materials 30-s intervals.
from the table, turn away from the client, 3. If the client attempts to interact with
and ignore the client until it is time to begin you appropriately (e.g., asks for something,
a new trial. (e) If the client emits other in- hands you a toy, etc.), reciprocate.
appropriate behaviors (screaming, throwing 4. If the client emits any form of inap-
things, aggression, etc.), continue with the propriate behavior, including the target be-
sequence; do not terminate the trial when havior, do not deliver attention.
these responses occur. 5. If the target behavior occurs precisely
3. Repeat the above sequence after 30 s at the end of a 30-s interval (just as you are
have elapsed since the trial began, and con- about to deliver attention), do not deliver
tinue until the session is over. attention. Instead, wait until the behavior
has stopped for 5 s, then deliver attention.
Play Condition
Purpose
APPENDIX B
This is designed to be a general control
condition, in which no demands are placed OUTLINE OF ASSESSMENT CONDITIONS
on the client, continuous access to leisure Attention Condition
materials is available, and attention is deliv- 1. Instruct client to play with toys; then
ered frequently independent of the clients ignore.
behavior. 2. Client emits appropriate behavior: Ig-
nore.
Target Behavior 3. Client emits inappropriate behavior
The target behavior being assessed con- other than the target: Ignore.
sists of self-hitting, defined as one body part 4. Client emits target behavior (hits self ):
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS METHODOLOGY 193

Express concern paired with brief physical just as you are about to deliver attention:
contact. Wait until the behavior has stopped for 5 s.
Demand (Escape) Condition APPENDIX C
1. Activate stop watch and begin trials. SAMPLE QUIZ
2. First instruction (prompt): Instruct cli-
1. Which assessment condition (attention,
ent to put block in bucket.
demand, alone, play) is considered the con-
(a) Client complies: Deliver praise.
trol condition for the other three conditions?
(b) Client emits target behavior (hits self ):
2. In which assessment condition or con-
Withdraw materials and turn away until
next trial. ditions does the client have access to leisure
(c) Client emits any other behavior: Contin- items?
ue sequence. 3. During all conditions, what should you
(d) Client emits no response: Go to second do if the client engages in a disruptive or
prompt. aggressive behavior (e.g., tips over a table or
3. Second prompt: Repeat instruction and tries to kick the therapist) that is not a target
demonstrate. behavior during the functional analysis?
(a) Client complies: Deliver praise. 4. What should you do if a client becomes
(b) Client emits target behavior (hits self ): injured during a session?
Withdraw materials and turn away until 5. How do you begin an attention session
next trial. (what do you say and do)?
(c) Client emits any other behavior: Contin- 6. When do you deliver attention to the
ue sequence. client during the attention condition?
(d) Client emits no response: Go to third 7. Give two examples of what you might
prompt. say or do when delivering attention during
4. Third prompt: Repeat instruction and the attention condition.
physically guide. 8. What should you do if the client asks
(a) Do not deliver praise. a question or requests help during the atten-
(b) Client emits target behavior (hits self ): tion condition?
Withdraw materials and turn away until 9. How often do you deliver attention to
next trial. the client during the play condition?
(c) Client emits any other behavior: Contin- 10. Give two examples of what you might
ue sequence. say or do when delivering attention during
5. Begin a new trial when the 30-s inter- the play condition.
val has ended. 11. What should you do if the client en-
gages in the target behavior (SIB) during the
Play Condition play condition just as you are about to de-
1. Activate stopwatch and direct client to- liver attention?
ward toys. 12. What should you do if the client asks
2. Deliver attention at least once every you a question during the play condition
30 s. when you are not scheduled to deliver atten-
3. Client initiates appropriate social inter- tion?
action with therapist: Deliver attention. 13. What should you do if the client en-
4. Client emits any inappropriate behav- gages in disruptive behavior (e.g., knocks over
ior: Do not deliver attention. furniture, throws objects, etc.) that is not a
5. Client emits inappropriate behavior target behavior during the play condition?
194 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

14. How often should you initiate train- 18. What should you do if the client en-
ing trials during the demand condition? gages in the target behavior (SIB) while you
15. If the task during the demand con- are trying to get the client to work on a
dition is putting a puzzle piece into a puzzle, puzzle during the demand condition?
what should you say when initiating a trial? 19. Should you praise the client during
16. If, during the demand condition, the the demand condition if you had to physi-
client does not respond to your first prompt cally guide the client to complete the task?
within 5 s, what should you do? 20. What should you do if the client asks
17. If, during the demand condition, the for help completing the assigned task during
client does not respond to your second the demand condition?
prompt within 5 s, what should you do?

STUDY QUESTIONS

1. What are the three general methods used for conducting functional assessments? Which
method is considered superior, and what is its main advantage?

2. What are the most commonly cited limitations of functional analysis methodology, and
which of these was addressed in the current study?

3. What were the authors rationales for their selection of therapists and clients, and for the
procedure they used to assess therapist performance?

4. The behaviors of primary interest were those of the therapist. What target responses were
scored, and what criteria were used to determine whether these responses were correct or
incorrect?

5. Briefly describe the baseline procedures. Why was performance not assessed under completely
naive conditions?

6. What were the major components of the training program, and what was the total duration
of training?

7. Describe the participants performance in baseline and posttraining. What aspect of the data
may have diminished the demonstration of experimental control? In discussing this limita-
tion, what did the authors suggest about the likelihood that participants might have achieved
high performance levels without any training?

8. What additional skills (not addressed in this study) may be important to the successful
implementation of functional analyses?

Questions prepared by Gregory Hanley and Rachel Thompson, The University of Florida

You might also like