You are on page 1of 203

The American Academy of Political and Social Science

c/o Fels Center of Government, University of Pennsylvania, 3814 Walnut Street,


Philadelphia, PA 19104; (215) 746-6500; (215) 898-1202 (fax); www.1891.org

Board of Directors
LAWRENCE W. SHERMAN, President
KATHLEEN HALL JAMIESON, Chair
ELIJAH ANDERSON SARA MILLER MCCUNE
STEPHEN B. BURBANK MARY ANN MEYERS
HENRY LOUIS GATES, JR. KLAUS NAUD
FREDERICK HELDRING LOUIS H. POLLAK
RICHARD D. LAMBERT JAROSLAV PELIKAN
JANICE FANNING MADDEN

Editors, THE ANNALS


ROBERT PEARSON, Executive Editor
RICHARD D. LAMBERT, Editor Emeritus
ALAN W. HESTON, Editor
DARLENE WOLTMAN, Business Manager
JENNIFER WARREN, Managing Editor

Origin and Purpose. The Academy was organized December 14, 1889, to promote the progress of po-
litical and social science, especially through publications and meetings. The Academy does not take
sides in controverted questions, but seeks to gather and present reliable information to assist the public
in forming an intelligent and accurate judgment.
Meetings. The Academy occasionally holds a meeting in the spring extending over two days.
Publications. THE ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science is the bimonthly
publication of The Academy. Each issue contains articles on some prominent social or political problem,
written at the invitation of the editors. Also, monographs are published from time to time, numbers of
which are distributed to pertinent professional organizations. These volumes constitute important ref-
erence works on the topics with which they deal, and they are extensively cited by authorities through-
out the United States and abroad. The papers presented at the meetings of The Academy are included
in THE ANNALS.
Membership. Each member of The Academy receives THE ANNALS and may attend the meetings of
The Academy. Membership is open only to individuals. Annual dues: $65.00 for the regular paperbound
edition (clothbound, $100.00). For members outside the U.S.A., add $24.00 for shipping of your subscrip-
tion. Members may also purchase single issues of THE ANNALS for $20.00 each (clothbound, $28.00).
Subscriptions. THE ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science (ISSN
0002-7162) is published six times annuallyin January, March, May, July, September, and November
by Sage Publications, 2455 Teller Road, Thousand Oaks, CA 91320. Telephone: (800) 818-SAGE (7243)
and (805) 499-9774; FAX/Order line: (805) 499-0871. Copyright 2002 by the American Academy of Po-
litical and Social Science. Institutions may subscribe to THE ANNALS at the annual rate: $420.00
(clothbound, $475.00). Add $24.00 per year for subscriptions outside the U.S.A. Institutional rates for
single issues: $81.00 each (clothbound, $91.00).
Periodicals postage paid at Thousand Oaks, California, and at additional mailing offices.
Single issues of THE ANNALS may be obtained by individuals who are not members of The Academy
for $32.00 each (clothbound, $42.00). Single issues of THE ANNALS have proven to be excellent supple-
mentary texts for classroom use. Direct inquiries regarding adoptions to THE ANNALS c/o Sage Publi-
cations (address below).
All correspondence concerning membership in The Academy, dues renewals, inquiries about member-
ship status, and/or purchase of single issues of THE ANNALS should be sent to THE ANNALS c/o Sage
Publications, 2455 Teller Road, Thousand Oaks, CA 91320. Telephone: (800) 818-SAGE (7243) and
(805) 499-9774; FAX/Order line: (805) 499-0871. Please note that orders under $30 must be prepaid.
Sage affiliates in London and India will assist institutional subscribers abroad with regard to orders,
claims, and inquiries for both subscriptions and single issues.

Printed on recycled, acid-free paper


THE ANNALS
2002 by The American Academy of Political and Social Science

All rights reserved. No part of this volume may be reproduced or utilized in any form or by any
means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information
storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher. All inquiries
for reproduction or permission should be sent to Sage Publications, 2455 Teller Road, Thousand
Oaks, CA 91320.
Editorial Office: Fels Center of Government, University of Pennsylvania, 3814 Walnut
Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6197.
For information about membership* (individuals only) and subscriptions (institutions),
address:
SAGE PUBLICATIONS
2455 Teller Road
Thousand Oaks, CA 91320
Sage Production Staff: BARBARA CORRIGAN, SCOTT SPRINGER, and ROSE TYLAK

From India and South Asia, From Europe, the Middle East,
write to: and Africa, write to:
SAGE PUBLICATIONS INDIA Pvt. Ltd SAGE PUBLICATIONS LTD
P.O. Box 4215 6 Bonhill Street
New Delhi 110 048 London EC2A 4PU
INDIA UNITED KINGDOM

*Please note that members of The Academy receive THE ANNALS with their membership.
International Standard Serial Number ISSN 0002-7162
International Standard Book Number ISBN 0-7619-2744-1 (Vol. 582, 2002 paper)
International Standard Book Number ISBN 0-7619-2743-3 (Vol. 582, 2002 cloth)
Manufactured in the United States of America. First printing, July 2002.

The articles appearing in THE ANNALS are abstracted or indexed in Academic Abstracts,
Academic Search, America: History and Life, Asia Pacific Database, Book Review Index, CAB
Abstracts Database, Central Asia: Abstracts & Index, Communication Abstracts, Corporate
ResourceNET, Criminal Justice Abstracts, Current Citations Express, Current Contents:
Social & Behavioral Sciences, e-JEL, EconLit, Expanded Academic Index, Guide to Social
Science & Religion in Periodical Literature, Health Business FullTEXT, HealthSTAR
FullTEXT, Historical Abstracts, International Bibliography of the Social Sciences,
International Political Science Abstracts, ISI Basic Social Sciences Index, Journal of
Economic Literature on CD, LEXIS-NEXIS, MasterFILE FullTEXT, Middle East: Abstracts &
Index, North Africa: Abstracts & Index, PAIS International, Periodical Abstracts, Political
Science Abstracts, Sage Public Administration Abstracts, Social Science Source, Social
Sciences Citation Index, Social Sciences Index Full Text, Social Services Abstracts, Social
Work Abstracts, Sociological Abstracts, Southeast Asia: Abstracts & Index, Standard
Periodical Directory (SPD), TOPICsearch, Wilson OmniFile V, and Wilson Social Sciences
Index/Abstracts, and are available on microfilm from University Microfilms, Ann Arbor,
Michigan.

Information about membership rates, institutional subscriptions, and back issue prices may
be found on the facing page.
Advertising. Current rates and specifications may be obtained by writing to THE ANNALS
Advertising and Promotion Manager at the Thousand Oaks office (address above).
Claims. Claims for undelivered copies must be made no later than six months following
month of publication. The publisher will supply missing copies when losses have been sus-
tained in transit and when the reserve stock will permit.
Change of Address. Six weeks advance notice must be given when notifying of change of ad-
dress to ensure proper identification. Please specify name of journal. POSTMASTER: Send
address changes to: THE ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science,
c/o Sage Publications, 2455 Teller Road, Thousand Oaks, CA 91320.
CONTENTS

PREFACE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . David P. Farrington


and Brandon C. Welsh 8
PART I: METHODS AND PERSPECTIVES
MEETING THE CHALLENGES OF
EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY:
THE CAMPBELL COLLABORATION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Anthony Petrosino,
Robert F. Boruch,
Haluk Soydan,
Lorna Duggan,
and Julio Sanchez-Meca 14
THE CAMPBELL COLLABORATION
CRIME AND JUSTICE GROUP . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . David P. Farrington
and Anthony Petrosino 35
DOES RESEARCH DESIGN AFFECT STUDY
OUTCOMES IN CRIMINAL JUSTICE? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . David Weisburd,
Cynthia M. Lum,
and Anthony Petrosino 50
META-ANALYTIC METHODS
FOR CRIMINOLOGY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . David B. Wilson 71
PART II: RESEARCH FINDINGS FROM PREVENTION
AND INTERVENTION STUDIES
EARLY PARENT TRAINING TO PREVENT
DISRUPTIVE BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS
AND DELINQUENCY IN CHILDREN . . . . . . . . . . . . . Odette Bernazzani,
Catherine Ct,
and Richard E. Tremblay 90
THE EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS
POLICING ON CRIME . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Anthony A. Braga 104
EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL BOOT
CAMPS ON OFFENDING. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Doris Layton MacKenzie,
David B. Wilson,
and Suzanne B. Kider 126
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL PROGRAMS
FOR OFFENDERS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Mark W. Lipsey,
Gabrielle L. Chapman,
and Nana A. Landenberger 144
PART III: FUTURE DIRECTIONS
TOWARD AN EVIDENCE-BASED APPROACH
TO PREVENTING CRIME . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Brandon C. Welsh
and David P. Farrington 158
BOOK DEPARTMENT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
INDEX. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 203
BOOK DEPARTMENT CONTENTS

INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS AND POLITICS


BROOKER, PAUL. Non-Democratic
Regimes. Paul C. Sondrol. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
DOBROWOLSKY, ALEXANDRA. The Politics
of Pragmatism. Women, Representation, and
Constitutionalism in Canada. Naomi Black . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 175
FLERAS, AUGIE and PAUL SPOONLEY.
Recalling Aotearoa: Indigenous Politics and
Ethnic Relations in New Zealand. Terry Wotherspoon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 176
LAMMERS, WILLIAM W. and MICHAEL A. GENOVESE.
The Presidency and Domestic Policy:
Comparing Leadership Styles, FDR to Clinton. Lance T. LeLoup. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 177
MACMANUS, SUSAN A. Targeting Senior Voters:
Campaign Outreach to Elders and Others with
Special Needs. Robert E. Denton, Jr. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 179

AFRICA, ASIA, AND LATIN AMERICA


KOLST, PL. Political Construction Sites:
Nation-Building in Russia and the
Post-Soviet States. Ray Taras . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 179
LOMNITZ, LARISSA ADLER and ANA MELNICK.
Chiles Political Culture and Parties, An
Anthropological Explanation. Michael Fleet . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181
RONIGER, LUIS and MARIO SZNAJDER.
The Legacy of Human Rights Violations
in the Southern Cone, Argentina, Chile
and Uruguay. Cesar N. Caviedes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 182
SWAINE, MICHAEL D. and ASHLEY J. TELLIS.
Interpreting Chinas Grand Strategy:
Past, Present and Future. Brantly Womack . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 183
VERSTIQUE, BERNARDINO. Michoacn and Eden:
Vasco de Quiroga and the Evangelization of
Western Mexico. William B. Taylor . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 185

EUROPE
GLOVER, JONATHAN. Humanity:
A Moral History of the Twentieth
Century. Brian VanDeMark . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 186
GONEN, JAY Y. The Roots of Nazi Psychology:
Hitlers Utopian Barbarism. Vamik D. Volkan. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 187
QUINN, FREDERICK. The French
Overseas Empire. G. Wesley Johnson . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 188
UNITED STATES
ALTSCHULER, GLENN and STUART BLUMIN.
Rude Republic: Americans and Their Politics in
the Nineteenth Century. Jean H. Baker . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 189
DIGGENS, JOHN PATRICK. On Hallowed Ground:
Abraham Lincoln and the Foundations of
American History. Barry Schwartz. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 190
FISHER, LOUIS. Congressional Abdication on
War & Spending. Charles Tiefer. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 192
STARK, ANDREW. Conflict of Interest in American
Public Life. Glenn Harlan Reynolds . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 193
STEPHAN, ALEXANDER. Communazis:
FBI Surveillance of German Emigre
Writers. Bernard F. Dick . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 194
SUMMERS, MARK WAHLGREN. Rum,
Romanism, & Rebellion: The Making
of a President, 1884. Samuel T. McSeveney . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 195
WEBBER, MICHAEL J. New Deal Fat Cats:
Campaign Finances and the Democratic
Party in 1936. Marc Dollinger. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 196

SOCIOLOGY
BAYOR, RONALD H. Race and the
Shaping of Twentieth Century
Atlanta. Clarence N. Stone . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 197
BROWDER, LAURA. Slippery Characters:
Ethnic Impersonators and American
Identities. S. Elizabeth Bird . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 198
COSTA, DORA L. The Evolution of Retirement:
An American Economic History,
1880-1990. Jon R. Moen . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 199

ECONOMICS
FISKE, EWARD B. and HELEN F. LADD.
When Schools Compete: A Cautionary
Tale. John F. Witte . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 201
THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY
PREFACE

PREFACE

The main aims of this special issue of The Annals are to examine the
systematic review method and to report on some of its contributions to evi-
dence-based crime prevention. The main title of this issue, What Works in
Preventing Crime? signals our primary interest in identifying those inter-
ventions that are effective in preventing crime and offending and that ulti-
mately may lead to more effective crime prevention policy and practice. The
issues subtitle, Systematic Reviews of Experimental and Quasi-Experimen-
tal Research, signals our interest in using the most rigorous methods of
research synthesis and only the highest-quality research designs to evaluate
the effectiveness of criminological interventions.
Systematic reviews have received increased attention in recent years in
the social sciences generally and in criminology and criminal justice specifi-
cally. This is part of the broader interest in evidence-based policy and practice
in public services (Davies, Nutley, and Smith 2000) and evidence-based crime
prevention (Sherman et al. 1997, forthcoming).
At the forefront of the development of systematic reviews is the newly
formed Campbell Collaboration. Named after the influential experimental
psychologist Donald T. Campbell (1969), this was set up for the purpose of pre-
paring, maintaining, and disseminating evidence-based research on the
effects of interventions in the fields of education, social welfare, and crime
and justice. Its Crime and Justice Group aims to prepare and maintain sys-
tematic reviews of criminological interventions and to make them accessible
electronically to scholars, practitioners, policy makers, and the general pub-
lic. The present work, although not officially carried out under the auspices of
the Campbell Collaboration, represents an important contribution to its
Crime and Justice Group, and the four systematic reviews reported here are
undergoing review by the Campbell Collaboration; it is hoped that they will
be approved and disseminated as Campbell reviews in due course.
This issue of The Annals introduces the path-breaking work of the Camp-
bell Collaboration and its Crime and Justice Group, examines key method-
ological issues facing systematic reviews of criminological interventions,
reports on four original systematic reviews of the effects of different interven-
tions on crime and offending, and makes progress toward an evidence-based
approach to preventing crime and offending. Throughout this issue, crime
prevention is defined as any program or policy that causes a lower number of
crimes to occur in the future than would have occurred without that program
or policy.
This special issue originated with the 2001 Jerry Lee Crime Prevention
Symposium, a 2-day conference on systematic reviews of criminological inter-
ventions, held in early April at the University of Maryland, College Park, and
at the U.S. capitol building in Washington, D.C. Convened by the University of
Marylands Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice and sponsored

8
PREFACE 9

1
by the Jerry Lee Foundation, the conference brought together leading
researchers in the fields of crime prevention, experimental criminology, and
research synthesis. The conference also benefited from the participation of
directors of governmental and nongovernmental research units, criminal jus-
tice policy makers, and journalists specializing in crime and justice. Five
countries were represented: Australia, Canada, Israel, the United Kingdom,
and the United States. Papers on substantive issues relating to methods and
findings of systematic reviews of criminological interventions were presented
and discussed. Subsequently the papers were revised in light of editorial com-
ments, and they are now presented here.

METHODS AND PERSPECTIVES

Four articles examine methods of conducting systematic reviews of crimi-


nological interventions. Important in each of these articles, and to this special
issue as a whole, is the need for systematic reviews to include only those stud-
ies with the highest quality research designs to evaluate the impact of pre-
vention programs on crime and offending. In the case of criminology and crim-
inal justice, this means experimental (randomized and nonrandomized) and
quasi-experimental designs. Ideally we would have been able to limit studies
in systematic reviews to only those that used randomized experimental
designs, as this is the most convincing method of evaluating crime prevention
programs (Farrington 1983). However, for systematic reviews of criminologi-
cal interventions, this is rarely feasible, largely because there is not a long
history of experimental evaluations in criminology and this would result in
the complete exclusion of area-level studies (as opposed to individual-level
studies), such as the effects of closed circuit television and many types of com-
munity crime prevention programs. In area-level studies, the best and most
feasible design usually involves before and after measures of crime in compa-
rable experimental and control conditions, together with statistical control of
extraneous variables.
In the first article, by Anthony Petrosino, Robert Boruch, Haluk Soydan,
Lorna Duggan, and Julio Sanchez-Meca, the authors discuss the growing
interest in and challenges facing an evidence-based approach to the develop-
ment of public policy and practice. Some of these challenges, which take the
form of political and policy efforts to maintain the status quo (see Davies,
Nutley, and Smith 1999) as well as the quality of the evaluation evidence, can
be overcome through the use of systematic reviews.
In the second article, by David Farrington and Anthony Petrosino, the
authors describe in detail the methodology of systematic reviews to investi-
gate the effectiveness of interventions to prevent crime and criminal offend-
ing. Also delineated are the many challenges facing the Campbell Collabora-
tion Crime and Justice Group. In the context of discussing the most important
of these challengesthe criterion of methodological quality to be used for
10 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

including studiesthe authors review some of the theoretical underpinnings


of evaluation methodology.
In light of the need for systematic reviews of criminological interventions
to include studies with different high-quality research designs, instead of
being limited to studies with only randomized experimental designs, an
important question is, Does the research design affect study outcomes? This
is the focus of the third article by David Weisburd, Cynthia Lum, and
Anthony Petrosino. The authors begin with a review of the methods literature
on this topic and report on key studies in medicine and the social sciences that
have investigated the subject of research designs affecting study outcomes.
For their studythe first of its kind focused specifically on criminological and
criminal justice interventionsthe authors draw on a recent, encyclopedic
review of the crime prevention evidence that was commissioned by the
National Institute of Justice and carried out by Lawrence Sherman and his
colleagues (1997). Weisburd and his colleagues find that there is a moderate
inverse relationship between research design and study outcomes. The main
implication of this finding, despite the authors noting that their work is pre-
liminary and hence that it should be interpreted with caution, is that the find-
ings of systematic reviews may be biased by the inclusion of nonrandomized
studies.
In the final article in this section, David Wilson reviews the methodology of
the quantitative data synthesis technique of meta-analysis (the statistical
analysis of the results of prior research studies), examines some of its
strengths and limitations as a method for assessing the effectiveness of crimi-
nological interventions, and discusses the importance of its use in systematic
reviews. As noted in the article by David Farrington and Anthony Petrosino
and elsewhere (for example, Farrington and Petrosino 2000; Farrington,
Petrosino, and Welsh 2001), quantitative techniques (for example, meta-anal-
ysis) should be used, when appropriate and feasible, in summarizing results
as part of systematic reviews. The appropriateness and feasibility of doing a
meta-analysis as part of a systematic review depend, in large part, on the
need to have a reasonable number of intervention studies (from which effect
sizes can be calculated) and to have studies that are sufficiently similar to be
grouped together. These points notwithstanding, the inclusion of a meta-
analysis in a systematic review (when done with full integrity, of course) has
the capacity to produce the most rigorous summary assessment of the effec-
tiveness of a criminological intervention based on a number of evaluation
studies.

RESEARCH FINDINGS FROM PREVENTION


AND INTERVENTION STUDIES

The systematic review and the meta-analytic review are the most rigorous
methods for assessing the effectiveness of criminological interventions and
2
have the most to offer to evidence-based crime prevention.
PREFACE 11

Systematic reviews use rigorous methods for locating, appraising, and syn-
thesizing evidence from prior evaluation studies, and they are reported with
the same level of detail that characterizes high-quality reports of original
research. They have explicit objectives, explicit criteria for including or
excluding studies, extensive searches for eligible evaluation studies from
around the world, careful extraction and coding of key features of studies, and
a structured and detailed report of the methods and conclusions of the review.
All of this contributes greatly to the ease of their replication by other
researchers.
Four articles report on systematic reviews of the effects of different inter-
ventions in preventing crime or offending: parent training and support pro-
grams, hot spots policing, correctional boot camps, and cognitive-behavioral
programs. Each follows as closely as possible the methodology for conducting
systematic reviews that has been specified by the Campbell Collaboration.
In the first article of this section, Odette Bernazzani, Catherine Ct, and
Richard Tremblay report on a systematic review of parent training and sup-
port before age 3. Seven studies are reviewed, and effectiveness is assessed on
the outcome measures of disruptive behavior (for example, opposition to
adults, truancy, aggression) and delinquency. The authors find that the evi-
dence on effectiveness is mixed: three studies report some beneficial effects
(with one reporting some harmful effects), and the other four studies report
no evidence of effectiveness. The authors call for caution in interpreting the
results (for example, due to modest effect sizes of the beneficial studies) and
recommend further intervention studies in these areas.
In the second article, Anthony Braga reviews the effects of hot spots polic-
ing to reduce crime. This form of policing involves the targeting of police
enforcement measures in high-crime areas. Nine studies are reviewed, and
effectiveness is assessed on the outcome measures of crime and disorder.
Braga also examines the effects of the studies on the displacement of crime
(an unintended increase in crime, for example, in a different location) and the
diffusion of crime control benefits (an unintended decrease in crime in a dif-
ferent location). He finds evidence that targeted police actions can prevent
crime and disorder in hot spots, that displacement is rare, and that some pro-
grams produce unintended crime prevention benefits.
The third article, by Doris MacKenzie, David Wilson, and Suzanne Kider,
reports on a systematic reviewincorporating meta-analytic techniquesof
the effects of correctional boot camps (otherwise known as shock or intensive
incarceration) on offending. Forty-four studies are reviewed, and effective-
ness is assessed according to recidivism. The systematic review reveals var-
ied effects on recidivism: 9 studies report beneficial effects, 8 report harmful
effects, and 27 report no effects. Overall the meta-analysis finds evidence of
no effect. MacKenzie and her colleagues conclude that boot camps are an inef-
fective correctional treatment to reduce future offending.
In the final article in this section, Mark Lipsey, Gabrielle Chapman, and
Nana Landenberger report on a systematic reviewincorporating
12 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

meta-analytic techniquesof cognitive-behavioral programs for offenders.


Fourteen of the highest quality studies with outcome measures of recidivism
are reviewed. Lipsey and his colleagues find strong evidence that cognitive-
behavioral programs are effective in reducing recidivism.

FUTURE DIRECTIONS

In the final article in this collection, we bring together the main conclu-
sions from the individual articles and identify priorities for moving toward an
evidence-based approach to preventing crime.
The Campbell Collaboration Crime and Justice Group has begun the
important task of preparing systematic reviews of the effectiveness of a wide
range of criminological interventions and will soon be in a position to make
them accessible to researchers, policy makers, practitioners, and the general
public. We argue that alongside the Campbell effort, a program of research of
new crime prevention and intervention experiments and quasi-experiments
must be initiated. These need to be evaluated using the most rigorous
research designs, involve large samples, have long-term follow-up periods,
and include cost-benefit or cost-effectiveness analyses. These new studies
should be initiated in many Western countries.
It is a well-known fact that having convincing research evidence and hav-
ing it influence policy and practice are two very different matters. How to
overcome some of the misconceived political and policy barriers to get more of
what works in preventing crime into policy and practice is by no means an
easy task, but fortunately it has received some attention in various academic
disciplines, criminology included.
In the final analysis, a great deal of work needs to be doneby researchers,
policy makers, practitioners, and politicians (in no order of importance)to
achieve the well-intentioned yet lofty goal of using the highest-quality scien-
tific evidence in the development of public policy and practice for the preven-
tion of crime. We view this special issue of The Annals as an important step
toward this goal. Of course, should it spur academic interest, encourage more
systematic reviews, inspire further innovation among policy makers and
practitioners, and ignite the interest of politicians, these too will be important
effects.

DAVID P. FARRINGTON
BRANDON C. WELSH

Notes
1. We wish to personally thank Jerry Lee not only for his sponsorship of this important event
but also for his unwavering support of an evidence-based approach to preventing crime and
building safer communities. We are grateful to Lawrence Sherman for helping to organize this
event (with David Farrington) and to Charles Wellford and his staff (especially Cynthia
Mewborn) for their top-rate coordination and administration of the conference. Special thanks
PREFACE 13

also to Senator Barbara Mikulski and the Consortium of Social Science Associations for their as-
sistance.
2. We discuss here only the systematic review method because systematic reviews employ
meta-analysis when appropriate and possible.

References
Campbell, Donald T. 1969. Reforms as Experiments. American Psychologist 24:409-
29.
Davies, Huw T. O., Sandra M. Nutley, and Peter C. Smith. 1999. Editorial: What
Works? The Role of Evidence in Public Sector Policy and Practice. Public Money &
Management 19:3-5.
Davies, Huw T. O., Sandra M. Nutley, and Peter C. Smith, eds. 2000. What Works?
Evidence-Based Policy and Practice in Public Services. Bristol, UK: Policy Press.
Farrington, David P. 1983. Randomized Experiments on Crime and Justice. In Crime
and Justice: An Annual Review of Research. Vol. 4, ed. Michael Tonry and Norval
Morris. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Farrington, David P. and Anthony Petrosino. 2000. Systematic Reviews of Criminolog-
ical Interventions: The Campbell Collaboration Crime and Justice Group. Interna-
tional Annals of Criminology 38:49-66.
Farrington, David P., Anthony Petrosino, and Brandon C. Welsh. 2001. Systematic Re-
views and Cost-Benefit Analyses of Correctional Interventions. The Prison Journal
81:338-58.
Sherman, Lawrence W., David P. Farrington, Brandon C. Welsh, and Doris Layton
MacKenzie, eds. Forthcoming. Evidence-Based Crime Prevention. London:
Routledge.
Sherman, Lawrence W., Denise C. Gottfredson, Doris Layton MacKenzie, John E. Eck,
Peter Reuter, and Shawn D. Bushway. 1997. Preventing Crime: What Works, What
Doesnt, Whats Promising. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, National
Institute of Justice.
ANNALS, AAPSS, 578, November 2001

THE ANNALS OF
CHALLENGES OFTHE
EVIDENCE-BASED
AMERICAN ACADEMY
POLICY

Meeting the Challenges


of Evidence-Based Policy:
The Campbell Collaboration

By ANTHONY PETROSINO, ROBERT F. BORUCH, HALUK SOYDAN,


LORNA DUGGAN, and JULIO SANCHEZ-MECA

ABSTRACT: Evidence-based policy has much to recommend it, but it


also faces significant challenges. These challenges reside not only in
the dilemmas faced by policy makers but also in the quality of the
evaluation evidence. Some of these problems are most effectively ad-
dressed by rigorous syntheses of the literature known as systematic
reviews. Other problems remain, including the range of quality in
systematic reviews and their general failure to be updated in light of
new evidence or disseminated beyond the research community. Based
on the precedent established in health care by the international
Cochrane Collaboration, the newly formed Campbell Collaboration
will prepare, maintain, and make accessible systematic reviews of re-
search on the effects of social and educational interventions. Through
mechanisms such as rigorous quality control, electronic publication,
and worldwide coverage of the literature, the Campbell Collaboration
seeks to meet challenges posed by evidence-based policy.

Anthony Petrosino is a research fellow at the Center of Evaluation, American Acad-


emy of Arts and Sciences and coordinator for the Campbell Crime and Justice Group.
Robert F. Boruch is University Trustee Professor at the University of Pennsylvania
Graduate School of Education and cochair of the Campbell Collaboration Steering
Group.
Haluk Soydan is the director of the Center for Evaluation at the Center for Evalua-
tion, Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare and a cochair of the Campbell Col-
laboration Steering Group.
Lorna Duggan is a forensic psychiatrist in the United Kingdom and reviewer for the
Cochrane Collaboration.
Julio Sanchez-Meca is a psychology professor at the University of Murcia in Spain
and the director of the Unit for Meta-Analysis.

NOTE: We appreciate the assistance of Maureen Matkovich and colleagues at the National
Criminal Justice Reference Service who supplied the data in Figure 1. The first authors work
was supported by grants from the Mellon Foundation to the Center for Evaluation and from the
Home Office to Cambridge University. Thanks to Brandon C. Welsh for his comments on earlier
drafts of the article.

14
CHALLENGES OF EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY 15

D ONALD Campbell (1969) was an


influential psychologist who
wrote persuasively about the need
they trust were not the product of
advocacy?
Donald Campbell unfortunately
for governments to take evaluation did not live long enough to bear wit-
evidence into account in decisions ness to the creation of the interna-
about social programs. He also recog- tional collaboration named in his
nized, however, the limitations of the honor that ambitiously attempts to
evidence-based approach and the address some of the challenges posed
fact that government officials would by evidence-based policy. The Camp-
be faced with a number of political di- bell Collaboration was created to pre-
lemmas that confined their use of re- pare, update, and disseminate sys-
search. The limits of evidence-based tematic reviews of evidence on what
policy and practice, however, reside works relevant to social and educa-
not only in the political pressures tional intervention (see http://
faced by decision makers when im- campbell.gse.upenn.edu). The target
plementing laws and administrative audience will include decision
directives or determining budgets; makers at all levels of government,
they also reside in problems with the practitioners, citizens, media, and
research evidence. researchers.
This article begins with a discus-
Questions such as, What works to
sion of the rationale for the Campbell
reduce crime in communities? are
Collaboration. We then describe
not easily answered. The studies that
the precedent established by the
bear on these questions are often
Cochrane Collaboration in health care.
scattered across different fields and
This is followed by an overview of the
written in different languages, are
advent and early progress of the
sometimes disseminated in obscure
Campbell Collaboration. We con-
or inaccessible outlets, and can be of
clude with the promise of the Camp-
such questionable quality that inter-
bell Collaboration in meeting the
pretation is risky at best. How can challenges posed by evidence-based
policy and practice be informed, if not policy.
persuaded, by such a fragmented
knowledge base comprising evalu-
ative studies that range in quality? RATIONALE

Which study, or set of studies, if any


at all, ought to be used to influence Surge of interest in
policy? What methods ought to be evidence-based policy
used to appraise and analyze a set of There are many influences on
separate studies bearing on the same decisions or beliefs about what ought
question? And how can the findings to be done to address problems like
be disseminated in such a way that crime, illiteracy, and unemployment.
the very people Donald Campbell Influential factors include ideology,
cared aboutthe decision makers in politics, costs, ethics, social back-
government and elsewherereceive ground, clinical experience, expert
findings from these analyses that opinion, and anecdote (for example,
16 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Lipton 1992). The evidence-based Gibbon (1999), MacDonald (1999),


approach stresses moving beyond and Sheldon and Chilvers (2000),
these factors to also consider the among others, articulated views
results of scientific studies. Although about evidence-based education and
few writers have articulated the social welfare.
deterministic view that the term A more persuasive indicator that
evidence-based suggests, it is clear evidence-based policy is having some
that the vast majority of writers impact is initiatives undertaken by
argue that decision makers need to governments since the late 1990s.
at the very leastbe aware of the Whether due to growing pragmatism
research evidence that bears on poli- or pressures for accountability on
cies under consideration (for exam- how public funds are spent, the
ple, Davies, Nutley, and Smith 2000). evidence-based approach is begin-
Certainly the implicit or explicit goal ning to take root. For example, the
of research-funding agencies has United Kingdom is promoting
always been to influence policy evidence-based policy in medicine
through science (Weiss and Petrosino and the social sectors vigorously (for
1999), and there have always been example, Davies, Nutley, and Smith
individuals who have articulated the 2000; Wiles 2001). In 1997, its
need for an evidence-based approach Labour government was elected
(for example, Fischer 1978). But using the slogan, What counts is
there has been a surge of interest, what works (Davies, Nutley, and
particularly in the 1990s, in argu- Smith 2000). The 1998 U.K. Crime
ments for research-, science-, or evi- Reduction Programme was greatly
dence-based policy (for example, influenced by both the University of
Amann 2000; Boruch, Petrosino, and Maryland report to Congress on
Chalmers 1999; Nutley and Davies crime prevention (Sherman et al.
1999; Wiles 2001). 1997) and the Home Offices own syn-
One indirect gauge of this surge is theses (Nuttall, Goldblatt, and Lewis
the amount of academic writing on 1998). In Sweden, the National
the topic. For example, in Shermans Board of Health and Welfare was
(1999) argument for evidence-based commissioned by the government to
policing, decisions about where to draft a program for advancing
target police strategies would be knowledge in the social services to
based on epidemiological data about ensure they are evidence based
the nature and scope of problems. (National Board of Health and Wel-
The kinds of interventions employed, fare 2001).
and how long they were kept in place, In the United States, the Govern-
would be guided by careful eval- ment Performance and Review Act of
uative studies, preferably random- 1993 was implemented to hold fed-
iz ed field trials. Cu l l en an d eral agencies responsible for identi-
Gendreau (2000) and MacKenzie fying measurable objectives and
(2000) are among those who made reaching them. This has led to the
similar arguments about correc- development of performance indica-
tional treatment. Davies (1999), Fitz- tors to assess whether there is value
CHALLENGES OF EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY 17

added by agencies. Th e 1998 Coinciding with fragmentation,


reauthorization of the Safe and Drug evaluation studies are not regularly
Free Schools and Communities Act published in academic journals or in
required that programs funded outlets that are readily accessible.
under the law be research based. The Instead a large percentage of
news media now commonly ask why evaluative research resides in what
police are not using research-based Sechrest, White, and Brown (1979)
eyewitness identification techniques called the fugitive literature. These
(Gawande 2001) or why schools use are government reports, disserta-
ineffective drug prevention pro- tions and masters theses, conference
grams (for example, Cohn 2001). papers, technical documents, and
All of these signs seem to indicate other literature that is difficult to
more than a passing interest in evi- obtain. Lipsey (1992), in his review of
dence-based policy. As Boruch (1997) delinquency prevention and treat-
noted, different policy questions ment studies, found 4 in 10 were
require different types of scientific reported in this literature. Although
evidence. To implement the most some may argue that unpublished
effective interventions to ameliorate studies are of lesser quality because
problems, careful evaluations are they have not been subjected to blind
needed. An evidence-based approach peer review as journal articles are,
to what works therefore requires this is an empirical question worthy
that these evaluations be gathered, of investigation. Such an assertion,
appraised, and analyzed and that the at the very least, ignores the high-
results be made accessible to influ- quality evaluations done by private
ence relevant decisions whenever research firms. Evaluators in such
appropriate and possible. entities do not have organizational
incentives to publish in peer-
Challenges to evidence-based reviewed journals, as professors or
policy: Evaluation studies university-based researchers do.
If evidence-based policy requires Relevant studies are not reported
that we cull prior evaluation studies, solely within the confines of the
researchers face significant chal- United States or other English-
lenges in doing so. For one, the rele- speaking nations. Recently the
vant evaluations are not tidily Kellogg Foundation supported an
reported in a single source that we international project that has identi-
can consult. Instead they are scat- fied more than 30 national evalua-
tered across different academic tion societies, including those in
fields. For example, medical, psycho- Brazil, Ghana, Korea, Sri Lanka,
logical, educational, and economic Thailand, and Zimbabwe (see http://
researchers more routinely include home.wmis.net/~russon/icce/
crime measures as dependent vari- eorg.htm). One argument is that it is
ables in their studies (for example, not important to consider evalua-
Greenberg and Shroder 1997). These tions conducted outside of ones
evaluations are as relevant as those jurisdiction because the cultural con-
reported in justice journals. text will be very different. This is
18 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

FIGURE 1
CUMULATIVE GROWTH OF EVALUATION STUDIES:
NATIONAL CRIMINAL JUSTICE REFERENCE SERVICE DATABASE

20000
18000
16000
14000
12000
10000
8000
6000
4000
2000
0
1970- 1975- 1978- 1980- 1985- 1990- 1995-
1974 1977 1979 1984 1989 1994 2000
SOURCE: The National Criminal Justice Reference Service (www.ncjrs.org).

another assertion worthy of empiri- Another challenge to gathering


cal test. Harlen (1997) noted that evaluative studies is that there is no
many in education believe evaluative finite time by which the production of
studies and findings from different this evidence stops. Research, includ-
jurisdictions are not relevant to each ing evaluation, is cumulatively
other. This ignores the reality that increasing (Boruch, Petrosino, and
interventions are widely dissemi- Chalmers 1999). An example is pro-
nated across jurisdiction without vided in Figure 1. Consider the
concern for context. For example, the cumulative growth of studies in-
officer-led drug prevention program dexed either as evaluation or as
known as D.A.R.E. (Drug Abuse evaluative study by the National
Resistance Education) is now in 44 Criminal Justice Reference Service
nations (Weiss and Petrosino 1999). for its database of abstracts. The data
Harlen (1997) suggested that we underscore the challenge faced in
must investigate the role of context coping with the burgeoning evalua-
across these evaluations. This is tion literature.
most effectively done through rigor- It would be good to identify and
ous research reviews. Such reviews, acquire all relevant evaluations and
however, are difficult with interna- keep abreast of new studies as they
tional literature without translation become available. It would be even
capabilities. better if all evaluations were of
CHALLENGES OF EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY 19

similar methodological quality and evaluations (for example, Bailey


came to the same conclusion about 1966; Kirby 1954; Lipton, Martinson,
the effectiveness of the intervention. and Wilks 1975; Logan 1972; Witmer
Unfortunately not all evaluations and Tufts 1954). Although a few of
are created equal. The results across these earlier syntheses were remark-
studies of the same intervention will ably exhaustive, the science of
often differ, and sometimes those dif- reviewing that developed in the
ferences will be related to the quality 1970s focused attention on the meth-
of the methods used (see Weisburd, ods used in reviews of research.
Lum, and Petrosino 2001). This high-
Methods for analyzing separate
lights the importance of appraising
but similar studies have a century of
evaluation studies for methodologi-
experience (Chalmers, Hedges, and
cal quality.
Cooper in press), but it was not until
Challenges to evidence-based the 1970s that reviews became scru-
policy: Reviewing methods tinized like primary reports of sur-
veys and experiments. This was
But what is the best way to draw ironic, as some of the most influential
upon these existing evaluation stud- and widely cited articles across fields
ies to understand what works and were literature reviews (Chalmers,
develop evidence-based policy? Cer- Hedges, and Cooper in press). Begin-
tainly, relying on one or a few studies ning in the 1970s, not only were the
when others are available is very traditional reviews of evaluations
risky because it ignores evidence. For under attack, but the modern statis-
example, relying on one study if five tical foundation for meta-analysis or
relevant studies have been com-
quantitative analysis of study
pleted means that we ignore 80 per-
results was also being developed (for
cent of the evidence (Cook et al.
example, Glass, McGaw, and Smith
1992). It is true that the one study we
1981; Hedges and Olkin 1985).
pick may be representative of all the
Research confirmed that traditional
other studies, but as mentioned pre-
reviews, in which researchers make
viously, studies in an area often con-
flict rather than converge. Evalua- relative judgments about what
tion studies themselves are part of a works by using some unknown and
sampling distribution and may differ inexplicit process of reasoning, were
because of chance probability. Until fraught with potential for bias (Coo-
we do a reasonable job of collecting per and Hedges 1994). Quinsey
those other studies, an assertion of (1983) underscored how such bias
convergence based on an inadequate could affect conclusions about
sampling of studies is unsupported. research following his review of
Criminologists have generally research on sex offender treatment
understood the problem of drawing effects: The difference in recidivism
conclusions from incomplete evi- across these studies is truly remark-
dence and have a half centurys expe- able; clearly by selectively contem-
rience in conducting broad surveys of plating the various studies, one can
the literature to identify relevant conclude anything one wants (101).
20 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

One major problem noted with effects have often been interpreted as
regard to traditional reviews was statistically insignificant and there-
their lack of explicitness about the fore as treatment failures.
methods used, such as why certain
studies were included, the search Systematic reviews
methods used, and how the studies
were analyzed. This includes the cri- Evidence-based policy requires
teria used to judge whether an inter- overcoming these and other prob-
vention was effective or not. Consider lems with the evaluation studies and
the debate over the conclusions in the methods for reviewing them. There is
Lipton, Martinson, and Wilks (1975) consensus among those who advo-
summary of more than 200 correc- cate evidence-based policy that sys-
tional program evaluations, briskly tematic reviews are an important
reported first by Martinson (1974). tool in this process (Davies 1999;
Despite finding that nearly half of Nutley, Davies, and Tilley 2000).
the evaluations reported in Martin- In systematic reviews, researchers
sons article had at least one statisti- attempt to gather relevant evalua-
cally significant finding in favor of tive studies, critically appraise them,
treatment, his overall conclusions and come to judgments about what
were gloomy about the prospects of works using explicit, transparent,
correctional intervention. The crite- state-of-the-art methods. Systematic
rion for success was not readily reviews will include detail about
apparent, but it must have been each stage of the decision process,
strict (Palmer 1975). including the question that guided
Thes e earlier revie w s, l i k e the review, the criteria for studies to
Martinsons (1974), were also prob- be included, and the methods used to
lematic because they seemed to rely search for and screen evaluation
on statistical significance as the cri- reports. It will also detail how analy-
terion for judging whether an inter- ses were done and how conclusions
vention was successful. This later were reached.
proved to be problematic, as research The foremost advantage of sys-
showed that statistical significance tematic reviews is that when done
is the function not only of the size of well and with full integrity, they pro-
the treatment effect but of method- vide the most reliable and compre-
ological factors such as sample size hensive statement about what
(for example, Lipsey 1990). For exam- works. Such a final statement, after
ple, large and meaningful effects s i f t i n g t h rou g h t h e av ai l abl e
reported in studies with small sam- research, may be, We know little or
ples would be statistically insignifi- nothingproceed with caution.
cant; the investigator and traditional This can guide funding agencies and
reviewer would consider the finding researchers toward an agenda for a
evidence that treatment did not suc- new generation of evaluation studies.
ceed. Given that most social science This can also include feedback to
research uses small samples, moder- funding agencies where additional
ate and important intervention process, implementation, and theory-
CHALLENGES OF EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY 21

driven studies would be critical to psychotherapy, class size, and school


implement. funding. Palmer (1994) noted that
Systematic reviews, therefore, are meta-analyses like Lipseys (1992)
reviews in which rigorous methods helped to counter the prevailing pes-
are employed regardless of whether simism about the efficacy of correc-
meta-analysis is undertaken to sum- tional treatment generated by ear-
marize, analyze, and combine study lier reviews.
findings. When meta-analysis is
used, however, estimates of the aver- Challenges to evidence-
age impact across studies, as well as based policy: Current
how much variation there is and why, systematic reviews
can be provided. Meta-analysis can There seems to be growing conver-
generate clues as to why some gence among researchers and others
programs are more effective in some that systematic reviews are a critical
settings and others are not. Meta- tool for evidence-based policy (for
analysis is also critical in ruling out example, Nutley, Davies, and Tilley
the play of chance when combining 2000). This is reflected in the deci-
results (Hedges and Olkin 1985). sion by the United Kingdoms most
Systematic reviews have other prestigious social science funding
byproducts. They can reconcile dif- agency, the Economic and Social
ferences between studies. Because Research Council, to support an
each study document is scrutinized, evidence-based policy and practice
systematic reviews can underscore initiative featuring systematic
deficiencies in report writing and reviews (see http://www.esrc.ac.uk/
lead to better systems for collecting EBPesrcUKcentre.htm). On closer
data required by reviewers. Reviews scrutiny, however, we find that there
also ensure that relevant evalua- are some challenges to the use of sys-
tionswhich may have been ignored tematic reviews in evidence-based
and long forgottenare eternally policy as they are currently done.
utilized. It is satisfying to investiga- One problem is that there is often
tors to find their studies still consid- a lack of transparency in the review
ered 20 years or more after comple- process. Completed syntheses are
tion (Petrosino forthcoming). g en eral l y s u bmi t t ed t o peer-
Systematic reviews have been reviewed journals, long after the
influential. This is understandable, research question has been deter-
as Weiss (1978) predicted that policy mined and the methods selected.
makers would find good syntheses of Except for rare occasions in which
research compelling because they reviewers submit a grant proposal
would reconcile conflicting studies for funding, researchers do not a pri-
when possible and provide a compre- ori describe why they are doing the
hensive resource for their aides to review and what methods they will
consult. Hunt (1997) discussed how employ. Without transparent pro-
the results from meta-analysis con- cesses from beginning to end, ex post
tradicted conclusions in earlier tradi- facto decisions that can influence a
tional reviews, in areas such as review and slant it knowingly or
22 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

unknowingly toward one conclusion interests of individual researchers.


or another are possible. This is espe- For example, in criminology, offender
cially important in persuading policy treatment has been a controversial
makers who want to be sure that and popular topic and the target of
research is not the product of slick most systematic review activity
advocacy. (Petrosino 2000). Other important
Because there is no uniform qual- areas for review such as police train-
ity control process, systematic ing, services for crime victims, court
reviews, like evaluations, range on a backlog interventions, and so on have
continuum of quality. In some cases, been inadequately covered. Evi-
the quality is due to the methods dence-based policy requires that
employed. Some reviewers may use evaluations in these areas be synthe-
meta-analytic methods but inade- sized, even if they are less relevant to
quately describe their decision pro- longstanding criminological debates.
cess. Other reviews may detail ex- Even if reviews did cover many
haustive search processes but then more questions than they currently
use questionable methods for analy- do, they are often not disseminated in
sis. It is difficult for even the discern- such ways that decision makers and
ing reader to know how trustworthy the public can get them. All too often,
the findings are from a particular reviews are published by academics
review. In other cases, the quality of in peer-reviewed journals, outlets
the review is due to the way it is that are not consulted by policy mak-
reported. Sometimes the nature of ers. In fact, decision makers often get
the outlet dictates how explicit and their information about research
transparent the reviewers can be. from news media, which can selec-
Some reviews, particularly those pre- tively cover only a few of the thou-
pared for academic print journals sands of evaluative studies relevant
with concerns about page lengths, to crime and justice reported each
are briskly written. Dissertations year (Weiss and Singer 1988). Tyden
and technical reports are usually (1996) wrote that publishing an aca-
very detailed but are less accessible demic paper to disseminate to policy
to readers. Systematic reviews may makers was akin to shooting it over a
all contain Materials and Methods wall, blindly, into water. The path to
sections, but the level of detail in utilization by decision makers was
each may vary depending on the dis- haphazard at best.
semination outlet. To examine dissemination further,
Policy makers and other inter- we analyzed the citations for 302
ested users of research have a wide meta-analyses reported by Lipsey
range of questions about what works and Wilson (1993) of psychological
to w hich they want an s w ers. and educational treatment studies.
Although funding agencies will spon- Nearly two-thirds listed in the refer-
sor reviews at times to meet these ence section were published in aca-
information needs, reviews are gen- demic journals. These were scattered
erally conducted because of the across 93 journals during the years
CHALLENGES OF EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY 23

covered (1977-1991). Only the Some journals also have lengthy lag
Review of Educational Research pub- times between submission and publi-
lished an average of one review or cation, delaying the dissemination of
more per year. Unless researchers evidence even further.
were using other unknown mecha-
nisms such as oral briefings and
THE COCHRANE
internal memos to communicate to COLLABORATION
decision makers, it seems very
unlikely that this evidence got into Are there ways of overcoming
the hands of anyone other than challenges to using systematic
research specialists working in nar- reviews in evidence-based policy? A
row areas. precedent for doing so was estab-
Most systematic reviews also tend lished in the health care field. Archie
to be one-off exercises, conducted Cochrane was a noted epidemiologist
only as funding, interest, or time per- who wrote persuasively about the
mits. Rarely are they updated to take need for medical practitioners to take
into account new studies that are rel- scientific evidence into account in
evant to the review, a challenge that their practice. Cochrane (1972)
is more significant given the cumula- lamented the fact that although ran-
tive growth of evaluation reports domized trials had shown some prac-
highlighted in Figure 1. Yet years tices to be effective and others harm-
may go by before an investigator pur- ful, clinical practitioners and medical
sues funding to update an existing schools were ignoring the informa-
review. The methodology and statis- tion. He later (Cochrane 1979) won-
tical foundation for meta-analysis is dered why the medical sciences had
still rapidly evolving, with improved not yet organized all relevant trials
techniques and new software being into subspecialties so that decision
developed to solve data problems. It makers could take such evidence into
is rare to find reviews that take into account. A protg of Cochrane, an
account these new techniques, con- obstetrician turned researcher
ducting analyses to determine if named Iain Chalmers, soon identi-
results using different methods fied and reviewed randomized trials
converge. relevant to childbirth and prenatal
Some reviewers publish in print interventions (see www.cochrane.
journals, an inefficient method for org).
disseminating reviews. Because In the early 1990s, the U.K.
print journals find them too costly, National Health Service (NHS),
cases in which reviewers take into under the direction of Sir Michael
account cogent criticisms by others Peckham, initiated the Research and
and conduct reanalysis are rarely Development Programme with the
reported. Unlike medical journals, goal of establishing an evidence-
criminological journals do not have a based resource for health care.
strong tradition in routinely printing Because of the success of their earlier
letters to the editor that respond to project on childbirth and pregnancy
criticisms with additional analyses. studies, Chalmers and his colleagues
24 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

were asked to extend this effort to all which the intended topic area seems
areas of health care intervention. appropriate. Once a title for the pro-
The U.K. Cochrane Centre was estab- posed review is agreed on, it is circu-
lished, with core funding from the lated to ensure that no other similar
NHS, to begin the work. It soon reviews are being prepared by
became clear that the amount of reviewers from other CRGs. Reduc-
work far surpassed the capacity of ing overlap and duplication is a cru-
one center or one nation to take into cial goal for the Cochrane Collabora-
account. In 1993, in honor of his men- tion, as scarce resources must be
tor, Chalmers and his colleagues used judiciously. Reviews are needed
launched the international Cochrane in so many areas of health care that
Collaboration to help people make wide coverage is a priority. Once the
well-informed decisions about title is agreed on, the reviewers must
healthcare by preparing, maintain- then submit a protocol for the review.
ing and promoting the accessibility of The protocol is a detailed plan that
systematic reviews of the effects of spells out a priori the question to be
healthcare interventions. In just 8 answered, the background to the
years, the Cochrane Collaboration i s s u e, an d t h e met h ods t o be
has been able to organize thousands employed. The protocol then goes
of individuals worldwide to contrib- through a round or two of criticism by
ute to its work. Much more informa- the CRG editors. It must conform to a
tion about the Cochrane Collabora- certain template to facilitate elec-
tion can be found at its Web site, t ron i c pu bl i cat i on u s i n g t h e
w w w.cochrane. org. B u t t h e Cochrane Collaborations software,
Cochrane Collaboration, in a very Review Manager, or RevMan. Once
brief time, established a number of the protocol is approved, it is pub-
mechanisms to address challenges to lished in the next edition of the quar-
using systematic reviews in evi- terly electronic publication, the
dence-based health care policy. Cochrane Library, and made avail-
For example, collaborative review able to all subscribers for comment
groups (CRGs) are responsible for and criticism. The editorial board
the core work of systematic review- must decide which criticisms should
ing. CRGs are international net- be taken into account.
works of individuals interested in The reviewers then prepare the
particular health areas such as review according to the protocol.
breast cancer, epilepsy, injuries, and Although deviation from the plan is
stroke. Each CRG has an editorial sometimes necessary, the protocol
board, generally comprising persons forces a prospective, transparent pro-
with scientific or practical expertise cess. Post hoc changes are readily
in the area, who are responsible for detected, and analyses can be done to
quality control of protocols (plans) determine if they altered findings.
and completed drafts of reviews. After the reviewers conduct the
It is useful to examine how a review and write up a draft, it too is
Cochrane review is prepared. First, submitted to the CRG editorial
individuals approach the CRG in board. Once the review draft is
CHALLENGES OF EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY 25

completed, the editors critique it make sure that decisions in reviews,


again. It is also sent to external read- such as setting eligibility criteria, be
ers, including researchers as well as informed as much as possible by evi-
practitioners and patients. This dence. The Cochrane Collaboration is
round of criticism is designed to also facilitated by 15 centers around
improve the methods in the review the world; they promote the interests
and to ensure that the final review is of the collaboration within host coun-
written as accessibly as possible to a tries, train people in doing system-
nonresearch audience, including atic reviews, and identify potential
health care patients, providers, and collaborators and end users. Finally
citiz ens. For each com pl et ed Cochrane fields and networks, such
Cochrane review, the Cochrane Con- as the Cochrane Consumer Network,
sumer Network crafts a one-page focus on dimensions of health care
synopsis written accessibly for other than health problems and work
patients and other consumers and to ensure that their priorities are
posts the synopses at its Web site (see reflected in systematic reviews.
http://www.cochrane.org/cochrane/ The main product of the Cochrane
consumer.htm). Once the review is Collaboration is the Cochrane
approved, it is published in the next Library. This electronic publication
issue of the Cochrane Library and is updated quarterly and made avail-
again made available for external able via the World Wide Web or
criticism by subscribers. Again the through CD-ROMs mailed to sub-
editors and the reviewers have to scribers. The January 2001 issue con-
determine which of these criticisms tained 1000 completed reviews and
ought to be taken into account in a 832 protocols (or plans for a review)
subsequent review update. Cochrane in one central location using the
reviews must be updated every 2 same format. Wolf (2000) noted that
years, to take into account new stud- the uniformity allows the reader to
ies meeting eligibility criteria. understand and find all of the neces-
Another important mechanism for sary information in each review, fa-
the Cochrane Collaboration is the cilitating training and use. Another
methods groups. These are interna- important feature of the Cochrane
tional networks of individuals who Library is the Cochrane Controlled
conduct systematic reviews focused Trials Register (CCTR). The CCTR
on the methods used in systematic has more than a quarter million cita-
reviews and primary studies. For tions to randomized trials relevant to
example, a methods group might col- health care, an important resource in
lect all systematic reviews in which assisting reviewers find studies so
randomized trials are compared to they can prepare and maintain their
nonrandomized trials. In this review, reviews.
they would seek to determine if there Empirical studies have reported
is a consistent relationship between that Cochrane syntheses are more
the reporting of random assignment rigorous than non-Cochrane system-
and results. Their objective is to atic reviews and meta-analyses pub-
26 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

lished in medical journals. For exam- ing an unbiased, single source for evi-
ple, Jadad and his colleagues (1998) dence and producing reviews,
found that Cochrane reviews pro- abstracts, and synopses for different
vided more detail, were more likely to audiences, they facilitate utilization.
test for methodological effects, were
less likely to be restricted by lan-
THE CAMPBELL
guage barriers, and were updated COLLABORATION
more than print journal reviews. The
Cochrane Library is quickly becom- With the success of the Cochrane
ing recognized as the best single Collaboration, the same type of orga-
source of evidence on the effective- nization was soon suggested for re-
ness of health care interventions viewing social and educational eval-
(Egger and Davey-Smith 1998). uations. Adrian Smith (1996),
Reviews by the Cochrane Collabora- president of the Royal Statistical So-
tion are frequently used to generate ciety, issued a challenge when he
and support guidelines by govern- said,
ment agencies such as the National
Institute for Clinical Excellence (for As ordinary citizens . . . we are, through
example, see Chalmers, Hedges, and the media, confronted daily with contro-
Cooper in press). In 1999, the U.S. versy and debate across a whole spec-
National Institutes of Health made trum of public policy issues. Obvious topi-
the Cochrane Library available to all cal examples include educationwhat
16000 of its employees. It is now does work in the classroom?and penal
accessible by all doctors in Brazil, the policywhat is effective in reducing
reoffending? Perhaps there is an opportu-
U.K. NHS, and all U.K. universities
nity . . . to launch a campaign directed at
(Mark Starr, personal communica-
developing analogues to the Cochrane
tion, 2001). Finally the queen recog- Collaboration, to provide suitable evi-
nized Iain Chalmers for his efforts dence bases in other areas besides medi-
with the United Kingdoms greatest cine [emphasis added]. (378)
honor: knighthood!
Thus the Cochrane Collaboration A number of individuals across
has been able to meet many of the different fields and professions orga-
challenges posed by evidence-based nized and met to determine how best
policy in health care. By requiring to meet this challenge. Several ex-
detailed protocols, the Cochrane Col- ploratory meetings were held during
laboration addresses the lack of 1999, including two headed by the
transparency in most systematic School of Public Policy at University
reviews of research. Through rigor- CollegeLondon, and one organized
ous quality control, they produce in Stockholm by the National Board
commendable reviews. By publishing of Health and Welfare. These meet-
electronically, dissemination is ings, which included researchers and
quickened, and the ability to update members of the policy and practice
and correct the reviews in light of communities, provided evidence that
new evidence is realized. By provid- the development of an infrastructure
CHALLENGES OF EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY 27

similar to Cochranes for social and TABLE 1


educational intervention including PRINCIPLES OF THE
criminal justice should be vigorously CAMPBELL COLLABORATION
pursued (Davies, Petrosino, and
Collaborating by fostering open communica-
Chalmers 1999; www.ucl.ac.uk/spp/ tion, cooperation, and transparency
publications/campbell.htm). Building on the enthusiasm of individuals by
involving and supporting people of different
Early days and progress skills and backgrounds
Avoiding unnecessary duplication by coordinat-
The Campbell Collaboration was ing and maximizing economy of effort
officially inaugurated in February Minimizing bias by maximizing scientific rigor,
assuring broad participation, and avoiding
2000 at a meeting in Philadelphia,
conflicts of interest
with more than 80 individuals from Keeping current by ensuring that systematic
12 nations participating. The Camp- reviews are kept up to date through
bell Collaboration was founded on incorporation of new evidence
nine principles developed first by the Ensuring relevance by promoting reviews that
use outcomes that matter to people making
Cochrane Collaboration (see Table 1).
choices
At the February 2000 inaugural Promoting access by widely disseminating the
meeting, it was agreed that the head- collaborations products and taking
quarters (secretariat) should reside advantage of strategic alliances
at the University of Pennsylvania. Ensuring quality by inviting critical comment,
applying advances in methodology, and
An international eight-member
developing systems for quality improvement
steering group was officially desig- Continuing to renew by updating reviews,
nated to guide its early development. editorial processes, and key functions and
The first three Campbell coordi- by engaging new collaborators
nating groups (similar to Cochranes SOURCE: C2 Steering Group (2001).
CRGs) were created to facilitate sys-
tematic reviews in their areas: edu-
cation, social welfare, and crime and employment programs within the
justice. The Campbell Education welfare system. The early progress of
Coordinating Group is focused on the Campbell Crime and Justice
developing protocols and reviews in Coordinating Group is described
the following critical areas: truancy, el s ew h ere i n t h i s i s s u e ( s ee
mathematics learning, science learn- Farrington and Petrosino 2001).
ing, information technology learning, The Campbell Collaboration
work-related learning and transfer- Methods Group was developed to
able skills, assessment and learning, increase the precision of Campbell
comprehensive school reform, school reviews by conducting reviews to
leadership and management, profes- investigate the role of methodologi-
sional education, and economics and cal and statistical procedures used in
education. The Campbell Social Wel- systematic reviews, as well as char-
fare Coordinating Group has also acteristics in original studies (see
organized itself into several areas: http:// web.missouri.edu/~c2method).
social work, transportation, housing, Three methods subgroups were cre-
social casework with certain ethnic ated during the past year, including
clientele, child w elfar e, an d statistics, quasi-experiments, and
28 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

FIGURE 2
C2-SPECTR

6000

Criminology
5000 Education
Psychology
Social
No of reports of trials

4000

3000

2000

1000

0
1950 1955 1960 1965 1970 1975 1980 1985 1990 1995
Year

SOURCE: Petrosino et al. (2000).

process and implementation sub- Figure 2 shows, preliminary work


groups. In conjunction with the toward C2-SPECTR has already
Campbell secretariat and the coordi- identified more than 10000 citations
nating groups, the methods group to randomized or possibly ran-
has taken the lead in developing a domized trials (Petrosino et al. 2000),
preliminary quality control process and this has now been augmented
based on the Cochrane model (see with new additions to the data file.
Appendix A). The Campbell Commu- Like the CCTR in health care, C2-
nication and Dissemination Group SPECTR should serve as a produc-
will develop best practice in translat- tive resource and facilitate prepara-
ing results to a variety of end users, tion and maintenance of reviews.
including policy makers, practitio- Plans to build a complimentary data-
ners, media, and the public. base of nonrandomized evaluations
To facilitate the work of reviewers, are being discussed.
the Campbell Collaboration Social, During 2000, Campbell and
Psychological, Educational and Cochrane groups mutually partici-
Criminological Trials Register (C2- pated in the NHS Wider Public
SPECTR) is in development. As Health Project (see www.york.ac.uk/
CHALLENGES OF EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY 29

crd/publications/wphp.htm), an unbiased and technically sound as


attempt to collate evidence from sys- possible. Appendix B provides a pre-
tematic reviews relevant to the liminary model of stages in a Camp-
United Kingdoms intended health bell review.
policies. As the NHS now considers
education, social welfare, and crimi-
CONCLUSION
nal justice directly or indirectly
related to public health, Campbell Donald Campbell articulated an
groups were involved (for example, evidence-based approach before the
Petrosino 2000). Several hundred methods of systematic reviewing and
systematicor possibly system- meta-analysis became staples of
aticreviews were identified in empirical inquiry. Still we think he
these areas and can now be used to would be pleased with the efforts of
help us map the terrain and identify hundreds of individuals who are
target areas where high-quality working worldwide to advance the
reviews are needed. international collaboration that
Funding from the Ministry of bears his name.
Social Affairs of Denmark has been The challenges of evidence-based
acquired to establish a Campbell policy are many. We think the Camp-
Center in Copenhagen to facilitate bell Collaboration will help to meet
efforts in the Nordic reg i on . some of them. For example, with rig-
Resources have also been secured to orous quality control and protocols,
create the Meta-Analysis Unit at the the Campbell Collaboration will
University of Murcia in Spain (see attempt to produce the same level of
http://www.um.es/sip/unidadma1. transparency with unbiased reviews
html), the first step in developing a for which the Cochrane Collabora-
Campbell Center for Mediterranean tion is lauded. Through electronic
nations. Objectives of these centers publication of reviews in a single
include facilitating reviews through source (that is, Campbell Library), it
training, identifying end users and will attempt to extend beyond com-
collaborators, and promoting dissem- municating with researchers and
ination and utilization. facilitate dissemination and utiliza-
These are just a few of the many tion by decision makers and ordinary
developments in the early days of the citizens. Maintaining reviews, taking
Campbell Collaboration. Although into account evidence worldwide, and
the collaboration anticipates creat- preparing reviews using the best sci-
ing an electronic publication that ence available should enhance the
will make available C2-SPECTR and use of Campbell reviews.
other helpful resources, its critical Systematic reviews are certainly
product will be high-quality system- an important tool for evidence-based
atic reviews. For the Campbell Col- policy, but like Campbell before us,
laboration to achieve the kind of suc- we do not wish to zealously oversell
cess Cochrane has obtained in the scientific evidence. Systematic
health care field, it will have to reviews will not resolve all enduring
ensure that these reviews are as political and academic conflicts, nor
30 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

will they oftenif at allprovide Editorial team (including a method-


neat and tidy prescriptions to deci- ologist and external reviewer[s]) reviews
the protocol
sion makers on what they ought to

do. In their proper role in evidence- Editorial team submits the protocol to
based policy, they will enlighten by the Campbell Database
explicitly revealing what is known
from scientific evidence and what is Reviewer completes the literature
not. They will also generate more searching, quality assessment of primary
questions to be resolved. One unan- studies, data extraction, and analysis
ticipated benefit in the short life of
the Campbell Collaboration is the Reviewer writes and submits a draft
review to primary editor
sustained forum it provides for dis-

cussions about evaluation design Reviewer receives and incorporates
and how to engage people from feedback from the editorial team
around the world.
Criminology is a noble profession Reviewer submits a second draft
because it aims to reduce the misery
Primary editor obtains external re-
stemming from crime and injustice.
views of second draft
To the extent that this collaboration

can fulfill Don Campbells vision of Reviewer incorporates feedback from
assisting people in making well- external reviewer
informed decisions, it will help crimi-
nologists stay true to criminologys Reviewer submits final review
original and noble intent.
Review is published
SOURCE: C2 Steering Group (2001).
APPENDIX A
A FLOW CHART OF THE STEPS
IN THE CAMPBELL REVIEW APPENDIX B
QUALITY CONTROL PROCESS STAGES OF A CAMPBELL
SYSTEMATIC REVIEW

Reviewer defines the research ques-


tion or topic 1. Formulate review questions
2. Define inclusion and exclusion crite-
Reviewer submits the title and ex- ria
pected completion date to the relevant Participants
area coordinator Interventions and comparisons
Outcomes
Coordinator and committee chair as- Study designs and methodological
sign project to a primary editor quality
3. Locate studies; develop search strat-
Reviewer begins the literature search egy considering the following sources:
with assistance from the primary editor C2-SPECTR
Electronic databases and trials reg-
Reviewer develops the review protocol isters not covered by C2-SPECTR
Checking of reference lists
CHALLENGES OF EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY 31

Hand searching of key journals References


Personal communication with ex-
perts in the field Amann, Ron. 2000. Foreword. In What
4. Select studies Works? Evidence-Based Policy and
Have eligibility checked by more Practice in Public Services, ed. Huw T.
than one observer O. Davies, Sandra Nutley, and Peter C.
Develop strategy to resolve dis- Smith. Bristol, UK: Policy Press.
agreements Bailey, William C. 1966. Correctional
Keep log of excluded studies, with Outcome: An Evaluation of 100 Re-
reasons for exclusions ports. Journal of Criminal Law, Crim-
5. Assess study quality inology and Police Science 57:153-60.
Consider assessment by more than Boruch, Robert F. 1997. Randomized Ex-
one observer periments for Planning and Evalua-
Use simple checklists rather than tion: A Practical Guide. Thousand
quality scales Oaks, CA: Sage.
Assess handling of attrition Boruch, Robert F., Anthony Petrosino,
Consider blinding assessors to au- and Iain Chalmers. 1999. The Camp-
thors, institutions, and journals bell Collaboration: A Proposal for Mul-
Assess randomization and power tinational, Continuous and System-
6. Extract data atic Reviews of Evidence. In Pro-
Design and pilot data extraction ceedings of the International Meeting
form on Systematic Reviews of the Effects of
Consider data extraction by more
Social and Educational Interventions
than one extractor
July 15-16, ed. Philip Davies, Anthony
Consider blinding of extractors to
Petrosino, and Iain Chalmers. London:
authors, institutions, and journals
University CollegeLondon, School of
7. Analyze and present results
Public Policy.
Tabulate results from individual
Campbell, Donald T. 1969. Reforms as
studies
Experiments. American Psychologist
Examine plots
Explore possible sources of hetero- 24:409-29.
geneity Chalmers, Iain, Larry V. Hedges, and
Consider meta-analysis of all trials Harris Cooper. In press. A Brief His-
or subgroups of trials tory of Research Synthesis. Evalua-
Perform sensitivity analyses, exam- tion & the Health Professions.
ine funnel plots Cochrane, Archie L. 1972. Effectiveness
Make list of excluded studies avail- and Efficiency. Random Reflections on
able to interested readers Health Services. London: Nuffield Pro-
Examine process/implementation vincial Hospitals Trust.
of interventions . 1979. 1931-1971: A Critical Re-
8. Interpret results view, with Particular Reference to the
Consider limitations, including pub- Medical Profession. In Medicines for
lication and related biases the Year 2000. London: Office of
Consider strength of evidence Health Economics.
Consider applicability Cohn, Jason. 2001. Drug Education: The
Consider statistical power Triumph of Bad Science. Rolling Stone
Consider economic implications 869:41-42, 96.
Consider implications for future re- Cook, Thomas D., Harris Cooper, David S.
search Cordray, Heidi Hartmann, Larry V.
SOURCE: C2 Steering Group (2001). Hedges, Richard J. Light, Thomas A.
32 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Louis, and Frederick Mosteller, eds. Gawande, Atul. 2001. Under Suspicion.
1992. Meta-Analysis for Explanation. The Fugitive Science of Criminal Jus-
New York: Russell Sage. tice. New Yorker, 8 Jan., 50-53.
Cooper, Harris C. and Larry V. Hedges, Glass, Gene V., Barry McGaw, and Mary L.
eds. 1994. The Handbook of Research Smith. 1981. Meta-Analysis in Social
Synthesis. New York: Russell Sage. Research. London: Sage.
C2 Steering Group. 2001. C2: Concept, Greenberg, David and Mark Shroder.
1997. The Digest of Social Experi-
Status, Plans. Overarching Grant Pro-
ments. 2d ed. Washington, DC: Urban
posal. Philadelphia: Campbell Collab-
Institute Press.
oration Secretariat.
Harlen, Wynne. 1997. Educational Re-
Cullen, Francis T. and Paul Gendreau.
search and Educational Reform. In
2000. Assessing Correctional Rehabil- The Role of Research in Mature Educa-
itation: Policy, Practice, and Prospects. tional Systems, ed. Seamus Hegarty.
In Policies, Processes, and Decisions of Slough, UK: National Foundation for
the Criminal Justice System, Criminal Educational Research.
Justice 2000, vol. 3, ed. Julie Horney. Hedges, Larry V. and Ingram Olkin. 1985.
Washington, DC: National Institute of Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis.
Justice. New York: Academic Press.
Davies, Huw T. O., Sandra M. Nutley, and Hunt, Morton. 1997. The Story of Meta-
Peter C. Smith, eds. 2000. What Analysis. New York: Russell Sage.
Works? Evidence-Based Policy in Pub- Jadad, Alejandor R., Deborah J. Cook,
lic Services. Bristol, UK: Policy Press. Alison Jones, Terry P. Klassen, Peter
Davies, Phillip. 1999. What Is Evidence- Tugwell, Michael Moher, and David
Based Education? British Journal of Moher. 1998. Methodology and Re-
Educational Studies 47:108-21. ports of Systematic Reviews and
Davies, Philip, Anthony Petrosino, and Meta-Analyses: A Comparison of
Iain Chalmers, eds. 1999. Proceedings Cochrane Reviews with Articles Pub-
of the International Meeting on Sys- lished in Paper-Based Journals. Jour-
tematic Reviews of the Effects of Social nal of the American Medical Associa-
and Educational Interventions, July tion 280:278-80.
15-16. London: University College Kirby, Bernard C. 1954. Measuring Ef-
London, School of Public Policy. fects of Criminals and Delinquents.
Sociology and Social Research 38:368-
Egger, Matthias and George Davey-
74.
Smith. 1998. Bias in Location and Se-
Lipsey, Mark W. 1990. Design Sensitivity:
lection of Studies. British Medical
Statistical Power for Experimental Re-
Journal 316:61-66.
search. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Farrington, David P. and Anthony
. 1992. Juvenile Delinquency
Petrosino. 2001. The Campbell Collab-
Treatment: A Meta-Analytic Inquiry
oration Crime and Justice Group. An-
into the Variability of Effects. In Meta-
nals of the American Academy of Polit- Analysis for Explanation, ed. Thomas D.
ical and Social Science 578:35-49. Cook, Harris Cooper, David S. Cordray,
Fischer, Joel. 1978. Does Anything Work? Heidi Hartmann, Larry V. Hedges,
Journal of Social Service Research Richard J. Light, Thomas A. Louis, and
1:215-43. Frederick Mosteller. New York:
Fitz-Gibbon, Carol. 1999. Education: The Russell Sage.
High Potential Not Yet Realized. Pub- Lipsey, Mark W. and David B. Wilson.
lic Money & Management 19:33-40. 1993. The Efficacy of Psychological,
CHALLENGES OF EVIDENCE-BASED POLICY 33

Educational and Behavioral Treat- Palmer, Ted. 1975. Martinson Revisited.


ment: Confirmation from Meta- Journal of Research in Crime and De-
Analysis. American Psychologist linquency 12:133-52.
48:1181-209. . 1994. A Profile of Correctional Ef-
Lipton, Douglas S. 1992. How to Maxi- fectiveness and New Directions for Re-
mize Utilization of Evaluation Re- search. Albany: State University of
search by Policymakers. Annals of the New York Press.
American Academy of Political and Petrosino, Anthony. 2000. Crime, Drugs
Social Science 521:175-88. and Alcohol. In Evidence from System-
Lipton, Douglas S., Robert Martinson, atic Reviews of Research Relevant to
and Judith Wilks. 1975. The Effective- Implementing the Wider Public
Health Agenda, Contributors to the
ness of Correctional Treatment. New
Cochrane Collaboration and the
York: Praeger.
Campbell Collaboration. York, UK:
Logan, Charles H. 1972. Evaluation Re- NHS Centre for Reviews and Dissemi-
search in Crime and Delinquency: A nation.
Reappraisal. Journal of Criminal
. Forthcoming. What Works to Re-
Law, Criminology and Police Science
duce Offending? A Systematic Review
63:378-87.
of 300 Randomized Field Trials in
MacDonald, Geraldine. 1999. Evidence- Crime Reduction. New York: Oxford
Based Social Care: Wheels Off the University Press.
Runway? Public Money & Manage- Petrosino, Anthony, Robert F. Boruch,
ment 19:25-32. Cath Rounding, Steve McDonald, and
MacKenzie, Doris Layton. 2000. Evi- Iain Chalmers. 2000. The Campbell
dence-Based Corrections: Identifying Collaboration Social, Psychological,
What Works. Crime & Delinquency Educational and Criminological
46:457-71. Trials Register (C2-SPECTR) to Facil-
Martinson, Robert. 1974. What Works? itate the Preparation and Mainte-
Questions and Answers About Prison nance of Systematic Reviews of Social
Reform. Public Interest 10:22-54. and Educational Interventions. Eval-
National Board of Health and Welfare. uation Research in Education 14:293-
307.
2001. A Programme of National Sup-
port for the Advancement of Knowl- Quinsey, Vernon L. 1983. Prediction of
edge in the Social Services. Stockholm: Recidivism and the Evaluation of
Author. Treatment Programs for Sexual Of-
fenders. In Sexual Aggression and the
Nutley, Sandra and Huw T. O. Davies.
Law, ed. Simon N. Verdun-Jones and
1999. The Fall and Rise of Evidence in A. A. Keltner. Burnaby, Canada: Si-
Criminal Justice. Public Money & mon Fraser University Criminology
Management 19:47-54. Research Centre.
Nutley, Sandra, Huw T. O. Davies, and Sechrest, Lee, Susan O. White, and
Nick Tilley. 2000. Letter to the Editor. Elizabeth D. Brown, eds. 1979. The Re-
Public Money & Management 20:3-6. habilitation of Criminal Offenders:
Nuttall, Christopher, Peter Goldblatt, Problems and Prospects. Washington,
and Chris Lewis, eds. 1998. Reducing DC: National Academy of Sciences.
Offending: An Assessment of Research Sheldon, Brian and Roy Chilvers. 2000.
Evidence on Ways of Dealing with Of- Evidence-Based Social Care: A Study
fending Behaviour. London: Home Of- of Prospects and Problems. Dorset,
fice. UK: Russell House.
34 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Sherman, Lawrence W. 1999. Evidence- L. E. Lynn. Washington, DC: National


Based Policing. In Ideas in American Academy of Sciences.
Policing. Washington, DC: Police Weiss, Carol Hirschon and Anthony
Foundation. Petrosino. 1999. Improving the Use of
Sherman, Lawrence W., Denise C. Research: The Case of D.A.R.E. Un-
Gottfredson, Doris Layton MacKen- published grant proposal to the Rob-
zie, John E. Eck, Peter Reuter, and ert Wood Johnson Foundation, Sub-
Shawn D. Bushway. 1997. Preventing stance Abuse Policy Research Pro-
Crime: What Works, What Doesnt, gram. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Grad-
Whats Promising. Washington, DC: uate School of Education.
U.S. Department of Justice, National Weiss, Carol Hirschon and Eleanor
Institute of Justice. Singer. 1988. The Reporting of Social
Smith, Adrian. 1996. Mad Cows and Ec- Science in the National Media. New
stasy: Chance and Choice in an Evi- York: Russell Sage.
dence-Based Society. Journal of the Wiles, Paul. 2001. Criminology in the
Royal Statistical Society 159:367-83. 21st Century: Public Good or Private
Tyden, Thomas. 1996. The Contribution Interest? Paper presented at the meet-
of Longitudinal Studies for Under- ing of the Australian and New Zea-
standing Science Communication and land Society of Criminology, Univer-
Research Utilization. Science Commu- sity of Melbourne, Australia, Feb.
nication 18:29. Witmer, Helen L. and Edith Tufts. 1954.
Weisburd, David, Cynthia M. Lum, and The Effectiveness of Delinquency Pre-
Anthony Petrosino. 2001. Does Re- vention Programs. Washington, DC:
search Design Affect Study Out- U.S. Department of Health, Education
comes? Findings from the Maryland and Welfare, Social Security Adminis-
Report Criminal Justice Sample. An- tration, Childrens Bureau.
nals of the American Academy of Polit- Wolf, Frederic M. 2000. Lessons to be
ical and Social Science 578:50-70. Learned from Evidence-Based Medi-
Weiss, Carol Hirschon. 1978. Improving cine: Practice and Promise of Evi-
the Linkage Between Social Research dence-Based Medicine and Evidence-
and Public Policy. In Knowledge and Based Education. Medical Teacher
Policy: The Uncertain Connection, ed. 22:251-59.
ANNALS, AAPSS, 578, November 2001

THE ANNALS
CAMPBELL COLLABORATION
OF THE AMERICAN
CRIME
ACADEMY
AND JUSTICE GROUP

The Campbell Collaboration


Crime and Justice Group

By DAVID P. FARRINGTON and ANTHONY PETROSINO

ABSTRACT: Systematic reviews use rigorous methods for locating,


appraising, and synthesizing evidence from prior evaluation studies.
They have explicit objectives, explicit criteria for including or exclud-
ing studies, and a structured and detailed report. The Campbell Col-
laboration Crime and Justice Group aims to prepare and maintain
systematic reviews of criminological interventions and to make them
accessible electronically to scholars, practitioners, policy makers, and
the general public. The major challenges include setting methodolog-
ical criteria for including studies in reviews, securing continued fund-
ing, academics needing publications in scholarly journals, and coping
with the volume of work needed to maintain high standards, includ-
ing refereeing proposals and final reviews and dealing with corre-
spondence and unsolicited proposals. The aim of making the best
knowledge about the effectiveness of criminological interventions im-
mediately available to everyone is ambitious and very important.

David P. Farrington is Professor of Psychological Criminology at the University of


Cambridge and Jerry Lee Research Professor of Criminology at the Department of
Criminology and Criminal Justice, University of Maryland.
Anthony Petrosino is a research fellow at the Center for Evaluation, Initiative for
Children Program at the American Academy of Arts and Sciences and a research asso-
ciate at Harvard University. He is also the coordinator of the Campbell Crime and Jus-
tice Coordinating Group.

NOTE: We are very grateful to the Home Office and to Paul Wiles, its director of research and
statistics, for supporting the work of the Campbell Crime and Justice Coordinating Group. An-
thony Petrosino was also supported by a Mellon Foundation grant to the Center for Evaluation,
Initiative for Children Program.

35
36 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

W HAT works to reduce crime?


How should offenders be dealt
with so that they do not reoffend?
collaboration, the Campbell Crime
and Justice Group will ensure that
relevant evaluation studies con-
What methods of preventing crime ducted all over the world will be
are most cost-effective? These are all taken into account in its systematic
questions to which citizens, as well as reviews and that the evidence from
government officials, policy makers, such reviews will be made accessible
practitioners, researchers, teachers, globally through language transla-
and the news media, deserve good tion and worldwide dissemination.
answers. All such persons should
have ready access to the most rigor-
CHARACTERISTICS OF
ous and up-to-date evidence on the SYSTEMATIC REVIEWS
effects of interventions designed to
reduce crime and offending. The What are systematic reviews?
best evidence on what works should These are reviews that use rigorous
be quickly accessible to those who methods for locating, appraising, and
need it. synthesizing evidence from prior
In recent years, a number of sys- evaluation studies. They contain
tematic reviews and meta-analyses Methods and Results sections and
have been carried out to discover are reported with the same level of
what works to reduce crime. This detail that characterizes high-qual-
article describes systematic reviews ity reports of original research. Other
and then discusses a newly formed features of systematic reviews in-
international network that will facil- clude the following:
itate the production and accessibility
of rigorous and continually updated 1. Objectives are explicit. The ra-
evidence on what works in crime and tionale for conducting the review is
justice: the Campbell Collaboration made clear.
Crime and Justice Group. This net- 2. Eligibility criteria are explicit.
work of researchers, policy makers, The reviewers specify in detail why
practitioners, and others from they included certain studies and re-
around the world will collaborate in jected others. What was the mini-
preparing systematic reviews of mum level of methodological quality?
high-quality research on the effects Did they consider only a particular
of criminological interventions. type of evaluation design such as
These systematic reviews will be randomized experiments? Did the
maintained and updated in light of studies have to include a certain type
new studies, cogent criticisms, or new of participant such as children or
methodological developments. They adults? What types of interventions
will be made readily accessible were included? What kinds of out-
through the use of electronic publish- come data had to be reported in the
ing, the Internet and the World Wide studies? All criteria or rules used in
Web, and traditional publishing selecting eligible studies should be
methods. Through international explicitly stated in the final report.
CAMPBELL COLLABORATION CRIME AND JUSTICE GROUP 37

3. The search for studies is de- in analyzing results. Although there


signed to reduce potential bias. There is still some confusion about the
are many potential ways in which meaning of these terms, it is useful to
bias can compromise the results of a distinguish between a systematic re-
review. The reviewers must explicitly view and a meta-analysis. A meta-
state how they conducted their analysis involves the statistical or
search of potential studies to reduce quantitative analysis of the results of
such bias. How did they try to locate prior research studies. Since it in-
studies reported outside scientific volves the statistical summary of
journals? How did they try to locate data (for example, effect sizes), it re-
studies in foreign languages? All bib- quires a reasonable number of inter-
liographic databases that were vention studies that are sufficiently
searched should be made explicit so similar to be grouped together. For
that potential gaps in coverage can example, there may be little point in
be identified. reporting a mean effect size based on
4. Each study is screened accord- a very small number of studies. Nev-
ing to eligibility criteria, with exclu- ertheless quantitative methods can
sions justified. The searches will un- be very important in helping the re-
doubtedly locate many citations to viewer determine the average effect
and abstracts of potentially relevant of a particular intervention.
studies. Each of the reports of these A systematic review may or may
potentially relevant studies must be not include a meta-analysis. For
screened to determine if it meets the example, a reviewer may find only a
eligibility criteria for the review. A few studies meeting the eligibility
full listing of all excluded studies and criteria. Those studies may differ just
the justifications for exclusion enough in the operational definition
should be made available to readers. of the intervention or in the way they
5. The most complete data possi- were conducted to make formal meta-
ble are assembled. The systematic re- analysis inappropriate and poten-
viewer will generally try to obtain all tially misleading. Another possibility
relevant evaluations meeting the eli- is that a researcher may carry out a
gibility criteria. In addition, all data meta-analysis adequately but use
relevant to the objectives of the re- inexplicit and potentially biased
view should be carefully extracted methods in conducting the search for
from each eligible report and coded relevant studies. In this case, the
and computerized. Sometimes origi- review would not be systematic.
nal study documents lack important 7. The report is structured and
information. When possible, the sys- detailed. The final report of a system-
tematic reviewer will attempt to ob- atic review is structured and detailed
tain this data from the authors of the so that the reader can understand
original report. each phase of the research, the deci-
6. Quantitative techniques are sions that were made, and the conclu-
used, when appropriate and possible, sions that were reached.
38 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

SYSTEMATIC REVIEWS The meta-analysis carried out by


IN CRIMINOLOGY
Andrews and his colleagues (1990)
has been particularly influential in
Petrosino (2000) was commis- North America. They classified cor-
sioned by the U.K. National Health rectional treatment as appropriate
Service to identify, retrieve, and sum- or inappropriate; appropriate treat-
mariz e s ys tematic rev i ew s of ment involved delivery of service to
research on the effects of interven- higher risk cases, targeting of
tions relevant to crime and drug or criminogenic needs, and use of tech-
alcohol abuse. He used a variety of niques (for example, cognitive-
techniques to locate rel ev an t behavioral) matched to the needs of
reviews. He conducted electronic clients. They found that appropriate
searches of 18 bibliographic data- treatment was effective, while crimi-
bases and the World Wide Web, nal sanctions and inappropriate
searched for citations from the refer- techniques had negative effects (that
ence lists of existing reviews, and is, the controls did better). Among
contacted researchers for their own agencies influenced by this research
reviews or those of others in their was the U.S. National Institute of
files. Especially useful was his scru- Corrections, in its technical advice to
tiny of reviews already gathered and jurisdictions implementing correc-
analyzed by other researchers such tional programs.
as Lipsey and Wilson (1993), Lsel Similarly, Redondo, Sanchez-
(1995), and Palmer (1994). Meca, and Garrido (1999) in Spain
Petrosino (2000) located 205 cita- identified 32 European studies
tions to systematic or possibly sys- between 1980 and 1991 that tested
tematic reviews of research on the the effects of some treatment on sub-
effects of interventions relevant to sequent criminal offending. They
crime, drugs, or alcohol. These reported an average 12 percent
reviews were categorized into the fol- reduction in recidivism when com-
lowing groups: preventive interven- pared to a quasi-experimental com-
tions for crime, drugs, or alcohol (84); parison group or the pretest period.
programs designed to treat or control Interestingly no randomized experi-
crime, drugs, or alcohol (95); improv- ments were included in their sample
ing the criminal or mental health of 32 studies. Nonetheless their over-
systems (12); interventions for crime all results are typical of most of the
victims (12); and reviews addressing meta-analyses reported to date in
more than one of these areas (2). As showing that correctional interven-
many as 72 of the prevention/control tion is generally effective in reducing
reviews were relevant to what works reoffending. Furthermore they and
in corrections, which was by far the most other reviewers (for example,
most common area. Consequently, we Antonowicz and Ross 1994; Lipsey
will concentrate on this area in very and Wilson 1998) concluded that the
briefly discussing the recent history most effective type of correctional
and influence of systematic reviews treatment was cognitive-behavioral
in criminology. skills training.
CAMPBELL COLLABORATION CRIME AND JUSTICE GROUP 39

THE CAMPBELL ensure that relevant evaluation


COLLABORATION
studies conducted around the world
will be taken into account in its sys-
The success of the Cochrane Col-
tematic reviews and that evidence
laboration in reviewing health care
from such reviews will be made
interventions stimulated interna-
accessible globally through language
tional interest in establishing a simi-
translation and worldwide dissemi-
lar infrastructure for conducting sys-
nation. Consumers of Campbell Col-
tematic reviews of research on the
laboration products should include
effects of social and educational
the general public, practitioners,
interventions. Following several
funding organizations, professional
exploratory meetings in London and
associations, policy makers, and
elsewhere, the Campbell Collabora-
teachers and their students. The
tion (named after the psychologist
reviews will be nonpartisan and
Donald T. Campbell) was officially
objective, using methods that are
founded at a meeting of more than 80
defensible and explicit.
persons from 12 countries in Phila-
delphia in February 2000. Professor
Robert Boruch of the University of
Pennsylvania was appointed chair of THE CAMPBELL CRIME AND
JUSTICE COORDINATING
the Campbell Collabor at i on s
GROUP (CJCG)
Steering Group. More information on
the Steering Group, as well as on the
background and progress of the At that February 2000 meeting,
Campbell Collaboration, can be t h e C ampbel l C ol l aborat i on
found at http://www.campbell.gse. appointed its CJCG to coordinate the
upenn.edu. work of the Crime and Justice Group.
Following the example of the The broad mission of the CJCG is to
Cochrane Collaboration, the Camp- oversee the preparation, mainte-
bell Collaboration will prepare rigor- nance, and accessibility of systematic
ous and systematic reviews of high- reviews of research on the effects of
quality evidence about what works. criminological and criminal justice
Recognizing that evidence is chang- interventions. The main emphasis is
ing all the time, these reviews will be on reviews of interventions designed
updated on a periodic basis, taking to prevent or reduce crime or delin-
account of new studies, cogent criti- quency. Broadly, the CJCG will
cisms, and methodological advances. include systematic reviews of
The Campbell Collaboration will go research on the effects of interven-
beyond traditional dissemination in tions delivered by the courts, police,
scientific journals and will use a vari- probation or parole agencies, prisons,
ety of methods including the World and community groups; for more
Wide Web, to promote rapid access to information about the Campbell
evidence of all interested persons Crime and Justice Group, see
and not just the research community. Farrington and Petrosino (2000).
Through international network- Persons who contribute system-
ing, the Campbell Collaboration will atic reviews to the Campbell Collabo-
40 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

ration must agree to the following re- 8. A commitment to make the re-
quirements: view available to broader audiences
than readerships of peer-reviewed
academic journals through electronic
1. A commitment to conduct up-
publication and dissemination into
dates of the systematic review to in-
policy, practice, and media outlets.
corporate new evidence, respond to
criticisms, or use more advanced
methods, on a regular basis (for ex- The CJCG consists of 14 members
ample, every 2 years). from 10 countries: David P. Farring-
2. A commitment to undergo a ton (United Kingdom, chair), Ulla V.
rigorous editorial review process B on des on ( D en mark ) , Vi cen t e
from not only researchers but also Garrido (Spain), Peter Grabosky
policy makers, practitioners, and citi- (Australia), Jerry Lee (United
zens to ensure that the review meets States), Mark W. Lipsey (United
high scientific standards and is also States), Friedrich Lsel (Germany),
written to be understandable to Joan McCord (United States), An-
nonacademic audiences. thony Petrosino (United States),
3. A commitment to maintain Lawrence W. Sherman (United
transparent and open review pro- States), Chuen-Jim Sheu (Taiwan),
cesses so that users can comment Richard E. Tremblay (Canada),
and criticize each stage of the review, Hiroshi Tsutomi (Japan), and David
from its proposal through to its com- L. Weisburd (Israel). Anthony
pletion. Petrosino was appointed part-time
4. A commitment to use the most coordinator for the CJCG, and Joan
rigorous search methods available to McCord was appointed to liaise with
ensure that all relevant studies are the Campbell Collaboration Steering
considered for inclusion or exclusion Group. It was also decided that the
and not just those reported in easily institutional base for the Crime and
accessible journals and books. Justice Group should be the Jerry
5. A commitment to cover litera- Lee Center of Criminology at the Fels
ture from around the world and not Center of Government, University of
just the English-speaking world. Pennsylvania, under the direction of
6. A commitment to code and Lawrence Sherman. Since the Brit-
computerize key features of each ish government in general and the
evaluation study reviewed (so that Home Office in particular are in-
anyone accessing the review can or- creasingly committed to evidence-
ganize the studies according to such based policy and practice, David
features as sample size, design, and Farrington applied to the Home Of-
effect size). fice for funding and obtained support
7. A commitment to explicitly re- for the first 2 years of the CJCG, be-
port the final review so that readers ginning in April 2000.
can understand decisions made at To encourage international partic-
each stage, justifications for those de- ipation, the first meeting of the
cisions, and how conclusions were CJCG was held in Paris in May 2000,
reached. coinciding with meetings of the
CAMPBELL COLLABORATION CRIME AND JUSTICE GROUP 41

International Society of Criminology need for quick results to demonstrate


(ISC). It was hoped that ISC mem- the value of this effort. In general, the
bers, who are drawn from all over the aim was to select a narrow topic for
world, would help this venture, espe- each review, where there was likely
cially since Lawrence Sherman is the to be only a small number of high-
current president of the ISC. At the quality evaluationsof the order of
Paris meeting, it was decided that 20 rather than hundreds. Together,
systematic reviews should be solic- the reviews would cover a wide range
ited on 15 key topics, examining the of criminological interests.
effects on crime of the following: Potential reviewers were asked to
restorative justice, parent education complete proposals for their reviews
programs, child skills training, juve- specifying the following: the back-
nile curfews, juvenile boot camps, ground to the reviews (hypotheses
electronic monitoring, cognitive- tested, operational definitions of
behavioral programs for offenders, independent and dependent vari-
faith-based programs for prisoners, ables, interventions); objectives of
length of imprisonment, community the reviews; strategies for searching
service orders, treatment of psycho- the literature; selection criteria for
paths, closed-circuit television, including or excluding studies; and
improved street lighting, neighbor- strategies for data extraction, coding,
hood watch, and hot spots policing. and analysis. This was modeled on
The CJCG decided initially to take the protocol for Cochrane reviews;
a proactive approach in soliciting the Campbell Collaboration devel-
reviews rather than a reactive oped its own standards for a proposal
approach (responding to unsolicited in January 2001.
proposals) for four main reasons. The Potential reviewers were also sent
first was to ensure that early reviews a draft checklist for data extraction
targeted areas that were especially suggesting that they code and com-
important to policy, practice, or puterize the following topics for each
research. At the first meeting of the study: principal investigators; full
CJCG, we benefited from a great deal citations to all evaluation reports;
of advice from Paul Wiles of the funding; publication dates; design of
Home Office. The second was to con- the study; characteristics of experi-
trol the amount of work and flow of mental units (for example, age and
correspondence in light of the limited sex of individuals, prior crime rates
resources available to deal with it. of areas); sample size, hypotheses,
We were worried about being over- and interventions; implementation
whelmed by correspondence and e- details; how independent and extra-
mail. The third was to establish the neous variables were controlled so
early reputation of the Crime and that it was possible to disentangle
Justice Group by getting well-known the impact of the intervention; who
researchers to do the first reviews. were the program delivery person-
The fourth was to ensure that some nel; what were the control conditions
reviews were completed in a timely (since it was rarely possible to have a
fashion. We were conscious of the truly untreated control group); who
42 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

knows what about the intervention proposals. These people are being
(since double blind trials were ideal); kept informed about all develop-
measurement of dependent variables ments. An informal advisory board of
(for example, official records of crime 30 persons has also been created. The
or self-reports of offending); before members of the board have been
and after measures of crime or asked to suggest topics for review
offending; and follow-up period after and to identify funding opportuni-
the intervention. ties. The Jerry Lee Crime Prevention
Symposium in April 2001, titled Sys-
tematic Reviews of Criminological
SUBSEQUENT DEVELOPMENTS
Interventions, can be regarded as the
official launching meeting of the
The CJCG met for the second time
Crime and Justice Group.
in San Francisco in November 2000,
The next meeting of the CJCG was
coinciding with the American Society
in Paris in May 2001. The work of the
of Criminology meetings. At this
Crime and Justice Group will be pub-
meeting, more topics for review were
licized at the American Society of
proposed, including repeat victimiza-
Criminology meeting in Atlanta,
tion programs; treatment of sex
Georgia, in November 2001. Plans
offenders; domestic violence inter-
include having a panel session on
ventions; reentry programs for incar-
systematic reviews and also a work-
cerated offenders; sports/recreation
shop (sponsored by the Division of
based programs; aftercare treatment
Sentencing and Corrections) provid-
for juvenile offenders; drug courts;
ing training on systematic reviews
prison-based drug treatment; inter-
and meta-analyses. There are also
ventions for serious, violent youth;
plans to have the next Jerry Lee
and interventions for gun violence.
Crime Prevention Symposium, titled
Anthony Petrosino constructed a
Systematic Reviews of Criminologi-
brochure for the Crime and Justice
cal Interventions, in April 2002 and
Group, which is being used to
to publish the proceedings of this
respond to enquiries and to circulate
conference in the first 2003 issue of
at meetings. A Web site for the Crime
The Annals.
and Justice Group has been con-
structed by John Myrtle and Peter
Levan of the Australian Institute of KEY CHALLENGES
Criminology. It has been decided to
house a central registry of studies at Several major challenges were
the Fels Center of Government in identified. The first (perhaps most
Philadelphia. This requires the important and controversial) is what
development of a core coding system criterion of methodological quality
for every study. should be set for including evalua-
More than 120 people from 16 tion studies in systematic reviews.
countries have written to express Several CJCG members argued that
interest in the Crime and Justice only randomized experiments should
Group, many of whom wish to help in be reviewed since these were able to
conducting reviews or reviewing demonstrate effects most convincingly
CAMPBELL COLLABORATION CRIME AND JUSTICE GROUP 43

(with highest internal validity). sideration in systematic reviews of


However, setting the gold standard of criminological interventions for the
randomized experiments would inev- Campbell Collaboration Crime and
itably exclude almost all evaluations Justice Group will be influenced by
based on areas (as opposed to indi- the methodological quality of the
viduals), covering the effects of inter- studies. Criteria for inclusion and
ventions such as neighborhood watch standards for methodological quality
or closed-circuit television. Also, have yet to be recommended by the
when David Farrington, Anthony Campbell Collaboration Methods
Petrosino, and Lawrence Sherman Group, whose conclusions are likely
made a presentation to the Commit- to be influential for all Campbell
tee of Directors of Criminological reviews.
Institutes in November 2000, some of Internal validity is maximized (or
those present were concerned that conversely, threats to internal valid-
the methodological standard should ity are minimized) in a randomized
not be set so high that important experiment. The randomization
evaluations were excluded. Directors maximizes the chance that experi-
said that they needed the best avail- mental units (for example, persons or
able evidence and that conclusions areas) receiving the intervention are
saying we know nothing because all comparable in all respects to those
studies are flawed were very units (controls) that do not receive
unhelpful. the intervention. Of course, random-
Intervention studies differ in ized experiments are only the gold
methodological quality. Two of the standard if implemented with full
most important features of method- integrity. To the extent that there are
ological quality are internal and implementation problems (for exam-
external validity (Cook and Camp- ple, problems of maintaining random
bell 1979). Internal validity refers to assignment, differential attrition,
how well the study unambiguously crossover between control and exper-
demonstrates that an intervention imental conditions), internal validity
(for example, restorative justice) had could be reduced in them. It is
an effect on an outcome (for example, expected that reviewers will code
offending). External validity refers to such implementation problems and
how well the effect of an intervention will attempt to answer questions
on an outcome is generalizable or such as which types of project man-
replicable in different conditions: dif- agement techniques are most likely
ferent operational definitions of the to avoid implementation problems.
intervention and various outcomes, Other things being equal, an inter-
different persons, different environ- vention study in which experimental
ments, and so on. Each reviewer is and control units are matched or sta-
ultimately responsible for determin- tistically equated (for example, using
ing criteria for inclusion or exclusion a prediction score) prior to the inter-
of studies in a systematic review. vention has less internal validity
However, it is expected that the selec- than a randomized experiment. An
tion of studies for detailed con- intervention study with no control
44 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

group has even less internal validity internal validity because of its fail-
since it fails to address many threats ure to tackle the threats mentioned
to internal validity (for example, his- above. An interrupted time series
tory, that is, the idea that changes in study is more convincing than a one-
the outcome might be attributable to group pre-post design, especially if
events other than the intervention; an experimental time series is com-
maturation or the continuation of pared with a control time series.
preexisting trends; testing or instru- It is expected that persons con-
mentation effects; and regression to ducting systematic reviews of inter-
the mean). ventions will select studies with high
Different considerations apply to internal validity for detailed consid-
intervention studies based on areas eration and inclusion in systematic
compared to intervention studies reviews. However, studies with lower
based on individuals. Randomized internal validity may also be listed in
experiments can rarely be carried less detail in the review, together
out to evaluate area-based interven- with summary information about
tions. It is rarely feasible to allocate them (for example, sample size,
at random a sufficiently large num- design, effect size). This will permit
ber of areas (for example, at least others to argue that these studies
100) to experimental or control con- should have been included in the
ditions in order to equate these con- expected dialogue between reviewers
ditions on all possible extraneous and others that will be facilitated by
variables before the intervention the electronic publication of system-
(within the limits of statistical atic reviews. The plan is that reviews
fluctuation). should be continually updated in
In area-based studies, internal light of new studies, relevant criti-
validity is usually maximized by hav- cisms, and methodological develop-
ing before and after measures of an ments. This updating may well
outcome (for example, crime) in com- change the studies that are included
parable experimental and control or excluded.
areas. Even better, the effect of an External validity will be mea-
intervention on crime can be investi- sured according to the extent to
gated after controlling (for example, which the selected studies report
in a regression equation) not only for similar results. It is important to
prior crime but also for other factors determine how well interventions do
that influence crime. Another possi- or do not work within different con-
bility is to match two areas and then texts or boundary conditions. Other
to choose one at random to be the types of validity (for example, con-
experimental area. Of course, several struct validity, statistical conclusion
pairs of areas would be better than validity) will also be summarized by
only one pair. An intervention study the reviewers for each study, who
with no comparable control area also may try to draw conclusions
(merely measuring crime before and about what is the active ingredient
after an intervention, as in a one- of an intervention or about causal
group pre-post design) has low pathways between interventions and
CAMPBELL COLLABORATION CRIME AND JUSTICE GROUP 45

outcomes. In addition, reviewers One of the main aims of conduct-


should consider how well the pro- ing a systematic review is to draw
gram is described in detail and conclusions about the best available
whether there is information about knowledge about the effects of a
the reliability and validity of out- criminological intervention, possibly
come measures. accompanied by methodological
The statement that reviewers will caveats or health warnings. To the
select studies with high internal extent that existing research is inad-
validity does not imply that review- equate, in internal validity or in any
ers will select only randomized other feature, this should be stated
experiments. This might possibly be by the reviewer, and recommenda-
the case for an intervention where tions should be made about needed
there are many randomized experi- research in the future. It is hoped
ments (for example, cognitive-behav- that systematic reviews will identify
ioral skills training). However, ran- gaps in knowledge and key questions
domized experiments to evaluate that need addressing and also that
criminological interventions are rel- they will lead to increased attention
atively uncommon. If reviews were to methodological issues and to an
restricted to randomized experi- improvement in the methodological
ments, they would be relevant to only quality of intervention research in
a small fraction of the key questions criminology.
for policy and practice in criminology. The second challenge is funding.
Where there are few randomized The very welcome Home Office fund-
experiments, it is expected that ing covers the salary of the part-time
reviewers will select both random- coordinator and some CJCG meeting
ized and nonrandomized studies for expenses but not the cost of reviews.
inclusion in detailed reviews, possi- The Jerry Lee Crime Prevention Pro-
bly reporting different types of gram generously sponsored the 2001
research design in different sections conference. Without funding, it was
of the review. thought likely that reviewers would
For area-based interventions, it is not give Campbell reviews high pri-
unlikely that reviewers will be able ority. Already one potential reviewer
to review any randomized experi- has responded that he thinks the sys-
ments. In this case, it is expected that tematic review is very important and
reviewers will select the highest would love to do it but cannot without
quality quasi-experimental inter- resources. It is hoped that further
vention studies for detailed consider- funding will be received from other
ation, such as those with before and sources, including the U.S. National
after measures of the outcome in Institute of Justice and the Smith-
experimental and control areas. Richardson Foundation, and that the
However, they may also provide a Canadian Department of Justice will
summary list of before and after fund Canadian reviewers. Funding is
studies with noncomparable or no needed not only for reviews and
control areas in their systematic meeting expenses but also for refer-
review. eeing proposals and completed
46 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

reviews, administrative and organi- afford to wait for these topics to be


zational support, translation costs, resolved by the Campbell Collabora-
and setting up a registry of studies. tion. There is also the problem that
Of course, efforts are being made con- similar topics are being reviewed by
currently to obtain funding for the t h e C och ran e D ev el opmen t al ,
larger Campbell Collaboration, and Psychosocial and Learning Problems
some of this funding may yet trickle Group, such as the treatment of sex
down to the Crime and Justice offenders. We decided to go ahead
Group. with our own reviews and to see how
A third challenge, related to incen- they differed from Cochrane reviews,
tives for reviewers, is the worry that which focus more on medical and
academics will not give Campbell health care issues.
reviews high priority if they do not The fifth challenge is how the
produce publications. Academics CJCG can cope with the work needed
need publications in scholarly jour- to maintain the high quality of sys-
nals to get tenure, promotion, and tematic reviews. This involves refer-
fame. It was therefore suggested that eeing both proposals and final ver-
reviewers be encouraged to complete sions of reviews, as well as building
not only a detailed electronic Camp- up the network and infrastructure of
bell review but also an (inevitably the Crime and Justice Group.
less detailed) review for a scholarly Already, organizing the CJCG has
journal. Of course, this raises prob- generated an enormous amount of
lems of copyright and duplication day-to-day work for both of us, as all
that need to be resolved. Journal edi- the different activities have built up.
tors may be reluctant to publish sys- This is why we decided to move for-
tematic reviews if a more detailed ward initially with a relatively small
version is available on the Web. On number of reviews and to learn from
the other hand, many journals are experience. We are concerned that
now being published on the Web. It is large-scale publicity, inviting people
hoped that in due course Campbell to volunteer to do particular reviews,
reviews on the Web will have suffi- might open the floodgates and
cient prestige themselves to carry unleash a monster that we cannot
weight in tenure and promotion cope with. However, we have to open
decisions. things up and move to unsolicited
A fourth challenge is the coordina- review proposals and wider partici-
tion of reviews, both with other parts pation by the research community as
of the Campbell Collaboration and soon as possible.
with the Cochrane Collaboration. The sixth challenge is how to regu-
Anthony Petrosino met with repre- larize the CJCG, which was essen-
sentatives of the Campbell Collabo- tially appointed on an ad hoc basis
ration Methods Group in July 2000 to for three years (until August 2003).
discuss standards for proposals and The overall Campbell Collaboration
other methodological issues. The Steering Committee needs to work
Crime and Justice Group has moved out procedures for appointing and
forward very quickly; we could not replacing CJCG members and the
CAMPBELL COLLABORATION CRIME AND JUSTICE GROUP 47

chair, ensuring that the CJCG recommendations. These would be


includes a wide variety of expertise useful for funding agencies, but some
and countries and that it has the members of the CJCG feel that sys-
capacity to supervise reviews and tematic reviews should be restricted
reviewers effectively. to scientific conclusions. An issue for
A seventh challenge is that the the future is how to ensure that sys-
people who are most knowledgeable tematic reviews are understandable
and motivated to carry out a system- to a wide audience. This will probably
atic review are often those who have involve soliciting comments on
worked on the particular topic, but reviews from members of the public
arguably they are not unbiased and as well as from policy makers and
may have a stake in the conclusions. practitioners. So far, we have focused
One way of dealing with this chal- on reviews of interventions designed
lenge is to encourage collaborations to reduce crime and offending. In the
between reviewers who have and future, we might aim to commission
have not previously worked on a par- reviews on interventions designed to
ticular topic. Also, there is a need for improve the management or opera-
workshops to train people in how to tions of the criminal justice system.
do systematic reviews. Also, we could expand our remit to
There are other issues to be systematic reviews of the strength of
resolved. For example, most of the relationships between particular
persons who have so far been asked risk factors and offending or of tests
to do systematic reviews are located of particular criminological theories.
in the United States. It is important However, it might be better to estab-
to solicit more reviews and more con- lish new crime and justice commit-
tributions to reviews (possibly tees to cover both of these topics.
involving translation) from persons
in other countries. To build up a reg- CONCLUSION
istry of studies, it is necessary to
agree on a common core of items to be It is hoped that systematic
coded and computerized in all reviews will improve on previous
reviews. Another issue is how far all influential comprehensive reviews of
reviewers should be encouraged to criminological interventions by the
carry out a meta-analysis or other U.S. National Academy of Sciences
quantitative summary of conclu- and others (for example, Nuttall,
sions. Ideally cost-benefit informa- Goldblatt, and Lewis 1998; Sherman
tion should be included in all reviews, et al. 1997) in a number of ways. Sys-
where available. Ideally the CJCG tematic reviews describe their meth-
should develop a master plan (a sys- ods more explicitly, search the litera-
tematic classification and organiza- ture more extensively and more
tion of all possible intervention topics internationally, and provide more
for review) and aim to fill the gaps information about criteria for inclu-
with solicited reviews. sion or exclusion and about sources
Another issue is how far Campbell searched. They include more detailed
reviews should include policy tables and codings of studies. They
48 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

are subject to rigorous quality con- References


trol, are revised in light of cogent crit- Andrews, Donald A., Ivan Zinger, Robert D.
icisms, are regularly updated, and Hoge, James Bonta, Paul Gendreau,
are speedily disseminated. Best of and Francis T. Cullen. 1990. Does Cor-
all, systematic reviews are immedi- r e c t i o n a l T r e a t m e n t Wo r k ? A
ately available electronically to Clinically Relevant and Psycho-
everyone. They should be used in the logically Informed Meta-analysis.
future by reviewers for the U.S. Criminology 28:369-404.
Antonowicz, Daniel H. and Robert R.
National Academy of Sciences and
Ross. 1994. Essential Components of
other agencies. Successful Rehabilitation Programs
The Campbell Collaboration for Offenders. International Journal
CJCG has made enormous progress of Offender Therapy and Comparative
in a very short time. We are proud to Criminology 38:97-104.
have received the first Donald Camp- Cook, Thomas D. and Donald T. Camp-
bell. 1979. Quasi-Experimentation:
bell Award from the Campbell Col-
Data and Analysis Issues for Field Set-
laboration in 2000 to recognize our tings. Chicago: Rand McNally.
outstanding progress. Nevertheless, Farrington, David P. and Anthony
we anticipate that the majority of Petrosino. 2000. Systematic Reviews
reviews of criminological interven- of Criminological Interventions: The
tions will always be carried out out- Campbell Collaboration Crime and
side the Campbell Collaboration. By Justice Group. International Annals of
setting and maintaining high stan- Criminology 38:49-66.
Lipsey, Mark W. and David B. Wilson.
dards, we hope that Campbell
1993. The Efficacy of Psychological,
reviews will become recognized as Educational, and Behavioral Treat-
the best available. We also hope that ment: Confirmation from Meta-
reviewers outside the Campbell Col- Analysis. American Psychologist
laboration will be influenced to carry 48:1181-209.
out systematic reviews and that the . 1998. Effective Intervention for
greater use of systematic reviews Serious Juvenile Offenders: A Synthe-
will lead to improvements in the sis of Research. In Serious and Violent
Juvenile Offenders: Risk Factors and
reporting of intervention studies.
Successful Interventions, ed. Rolf
While the work ahead is daunting, Loeber and David P. Farrington. Thou-
systematic reviews of criminological sand Oaks, CA: Sage.
interventions have enormous poten- Lsel, Friedrich. 1995. Increasing Con-
tial both to advance knowledge and sensus in the Evaluation of Offender
to make policy and practice more Rehabilitation? Lessons from Recent
effective in the future. The aim of Research Syntheses. Psychology,
making the best knowledge about the Crime and Law 2:19-39.
Nuttall, Christopher, Peter Goldblatt,
effectiveness of interventions to
and Chris Lewis, eds. 1998. Reducing
reduce crime immediately available Offending: An Assessment of Research
to everyone is ambitious and very Evidence on Ways of Dealing with Of-
important and would benefit every- fending Behaviour. London: Home Of-
one in all countries. fice.
CAMPBELL COLLABORATION CRIME AND JUSTICE GROUP 49

Palmer, Ted. 1994. A Profile of Correc- Redondo, Santiago, Julio Sanchez-Meca,


tional Effectiveness and New Direc- and Vicente Garrido. 1999. The Influ-
tions for Research. Albany: State Uni- ence of Treatment Programmes on the
versity of New York Press. Recidivism of Juvenile and Adult Of-
Petrosino, Anthony. 2000. Crime, Drugs fenders: A European Meta-Analytic
and Alcohol. In Evidence from System- Review. Psychology, Crime and Law
atic Reviews of Research Relevant to 5:251-78.
Implementing the Wider Public Sherman, Lawrence W., Denise C.
Health Agenda, Contributors to the Gottfredson, Doris Layton MacKen-
Cochrane Collaboration and the zie, John E. Eck, Peter Reuter, and
Campbell Collaboration. York, UK: Shawn D. Bushway. 1997. Preventing
NHS Centre for Reviews and Dissemi- Crime: What Works, What Doesnt,
nation. Retrieved August 2000 from Whats Promising. Washington, DC:
http://www.york.ac.uk/inst/crd/wph. U.S. Department of Justice, National
htm. Institute of Justice.
ANNALS, AAPSS, 578, November 2001

THE ANNALS
RESEARCH DESIGN
OF THE
AND
AMERICAN
STUDY OUTCOMES
ACADEMY

Does Research Design


Affect Study Outcomes
in Criminal Justice?

By DAVID WEISBURD, CYNTHIA M. LUM,


and ANTHONY PETROSINO

ABSTRACT: Does the type of research design used in a crime and jus-
tice study influence its conclusions? Scholars agree in theory that
randomized experimental studies have higher internal validity than
do nonrandomized studies. But there is not consensus regarding the
costs of using nonrandomized studies in coming to conclusions re-
garding criminal justice interventions. To examine these issues, the
authors look at the relationship between research design and study
outcomes in a broad review of research evidence on crime and justice
commissioned by the National Institute of Justice. Their findings
suggest that design does have a systematic effect on outcomes in
criminal justice studies. The weaker a design, indicated by internal
validity, the more likely a study is to report a result in favor of treat-
ment and the less likely it is to report a harmful effect of treatment.
Even when comparing randomized studies with strong quasi-experi-
mental research designs, systematic and statistically significant dif-
ferences are observed.

David Weisburd is a senior research fellow in the Department of Criminology and


Criminal Justice at the University of Maryland and a professor of criminology at the
Hebrew University Law School in Jerusalem.
Cynthia M. Lum is a doctoral student in the Department of Criminology and Crimi-
nal Justice at the University of Maryland.
Anthony Petrosino is a research fellow at the Center for Evaluation, Initiative for
Children Program at the American Academy of Arts and Sciences and a research asso-
ciate at Harvard University. He is also the coordinator of the Campbell Crime and Jus-
tice Coordinating Group.

NOTE: We are indebted to a number of colleagues for helpful comments in preparing this arti-
cle. We especially want to thank Iain Chalmers, John Eck, David Farrington, Denise Gottfredson,
Doris MacKenzie, Joan McCord, Lawrence Sherman, Brandon Welsh, Charles Wellford, and
David Wilson.

50
RESEARCH DESIGN AND STUDY OUTCOMES 51

T HERE is a growing consensus


among scholars, practitioners,
and policy makers that crime control
www.cochrane.org.) The Cochrane
Library is now widely recognized as
the single best source of evidence on
practices and policies should be the effectiveness of health care and
rooted as much as possible in scien- medical treatments and has played
tific research (Cullen and Gendreau an important part in the advance-
2000; MacKenzie 2000; Sherman ment of evidence-based medicine
1998). This is reflected in the steady (Egger and Smith 1998). More
growth in interest in evaluation of recently, social scientists following
criminal justice programs and prac- the Cochrane model established the
tices in the United States and the Campbell Collaboration for develop-
United Kingdom over the past de- ing systematic reviews of research
cade and by large increases in crimi- evidence in the area of social and
nal justice funding for research dur- educational interventions (see
ing this period (Visher and Weisburd Boruch, Petrosino, and Chalmers
1998). Increasing support for re- 1999). In recognition of the growing
search and evaluation in criminal importance of evidence-based poli-
justice may be seen as part of a more cies in criminal justice, the Campbell
general trend toward utilization of Collaboration commissioned a coor-
scientific research for establishing dinating group to deal with crime
rational and effective practices and and justice issues. This group began
policies. This trend is perhaps most with the goal of providing the best
prominent in the health professions, evidence on what works in crime
where the idea of evidence-based and justice through the develop-
medicine has gained strong govern- ment of systematic reviews of
ment and professional support research on the effects of crime and
(Millenson 1997; Zuger 1997), justice interventions (Farrington
though the evidence-based paradigm and Petrosino 2001 [this issue]).
is also developing in other fields (see In the Cochrane Collaboration,
Nutley and Davies 1999; Davies, and in medical research in general,
Nutley, and Smith 2000). clinical trials that randomize partici-
A central component of the move- pants to treatment and control or
ment toward evidence-based practice comparison groups are considered
and policy is reliance on systematic more reliable than studies that do
review of prior research and evalua- not employ randomization. And the
tion (Davies 1999). Such review recognition that experimental
allows policy makers and practitio- designs form the gold standard for
ners to identify what programs and drawing conclusions about the
practices are most effective and in effects of treatments or programs is
which contexts. The Cochrane Col- not restricted to medicine. There is
laboration, for example, seeks to pre- broad agreement among social and
pare, maintain, and make accessible behavioral scientists that random-
systematic reviews of research on the ized experiments provide the best
effects of health care interventions method for drawing causal infer-
(see Chalmers and Altman 1995; ences between treatments and
52 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

programs and their outcomes (for would be relevant to only a small fraction
example, see Boruch, Snyder, and of the key questions for policy and prac-
DeMoya 2000; Campbell and Boruch tice in criminology. Where there are few
randomized experiments, it is expected
1975; Farrington 1983; Feder, Jolin,
that reviewers will select both random-
and Feyerherm 2000). Indeed, a task
ized and non-randomized studies for in-
force convened by the Board of Scien- clusion in detailed reviews. (3)
tific Affairs of the American Psycho-
logical Association to look into statis-
tical methods concluded that for In this article we examine a cen-
research involving causal inferences, tral question relevant both to the
the assignments of units to levels of Campbell Collaboration crime and
the causal variable is critical. Ran- justice effort and to the more general
dom assignment (not to be confused emphasis on developing evidence-
with random selection) allows for the based practice in criminal justice:
strongest possible causal inferences Does the type of research design used
free of extraneous assumptions in a crime and justice study influence
(Wilkinson and Task Force on Statis- the conclusions that are reached? As-
tical Inference 1999). suming that experimental designs
While reliance on experimental are the gold standard for evaluating
studies in drawing conclusions about practices and policies, it is important
treatment outcomes has become to ask what price we pay in including
common in the development of evi- other types of studies in our reviews
dence-based medicine, the Campbell of what works in crime and justice.
Collaboration Crime and Justice Co- Are we likely to overestimate or un-
ordinating Group has concluded that derestimate the positive effects of
it is unrealistic at this time to restrict treatment? Or conversely, might we
systematic reviews on the effects of expect that the use of well-designed
interventions relevant to crime and nonrandomized studies will lead to
justice to experimental studies. In about the same conclusions as we
developing its Standards for Inclu- would gain from randomized experi-
sion of Studies in Systematic Reviews mental evaluations?
(Farrington 2000), the group notes To examine these issues, we look
that it does not require that review- at the relationship between research
ers select only randomized experi- design and study outcomes in a broad
ments: review of research evidence on crime
and justice commissioned by the
National Institute of Justice. Gen-
This might possibly be the case for an in-
erally referred to as the Maryland
tervention where there are many ran-
domized experiments (e.g. cognitive-be-
Report because it was developed in
havioral skills training). However, the Department of Criminology and
randomized experiments to evaluate Criminal Justice at the University of
criminological interventions are rela- Maryland at College Park, the study
tively uncommon. If reviews were re- was published under the title Pre-
stricted to randomized experiments, they venting Crime: What Works, What
RESEARCH DESIGN AND STUDY OUTCOMES 53

Doesnt, Whats Promising (Sherman treatment program on recidivism. If


et al. 1997). The Maryland Report at the end of the evaluation the
provides an unusual opportunity for researcher can present study results
assessing the impact of study design and confidently assert that the
on study outcomes in crime and jus- effects of treatment have been iso-
tice both because it sought to be com- lated from other confounding causes,
prehensive in identifying available the internal validity of the study is
research and because the principal high. But if the researcher has been
investigators of the study devoted unable to ensure that other factors
specific attention to the nature of the such as the seriousness of prior
research designs of the studies records or the social status of offend-
included. Below we detail the meth- ers have been disentangled from the
ods we used to examine how study influence of treatment, he or she
design affects study outcomes in must note that the effects observed
crime and justice research and report for treatment may be due to such con-
on our main findings. We turn first, founding causes. In this case internal
however, to a discussion of why ran- validity is low.
domized experiments as contrasted In randomized experimental stud-
with quasi-experimental and non- ies, internal validity is developed
experimental research designs are through the process of random allo-
generally considered a gold standard cation of the units of treatment or
for making causal inferences. We also intervention to experimental and
examine what prior research sug- control or comparison groups. This
gests regarding the questions we means that the researcher has ran-
raise. domized other factors besides treat-
ment itself, since there is no system-
WHY ARE RANDOMIZED
atic bias that brings one type of
EXPERIMENTS CONSIDERED subject into the treatment group and
THE GOLD STANDARD? another into the control or compari-
son group. Although the groups are
The key to understanding the not necessarily the same on every
strength of experimental research characteristicindeed, simply by
designs is found in what scholars chance, there are likely to be differ-
refer to as the internal validity of a encessuch differences can be
study. A research design in which the assumed to be distributed randomly
effects of treatment or intervention and are part and parcel of the sto-
can be clearly distinguished from chastic processes taken into account
other effects has high internal valid- in statistical tests. Random alloca-
ity. A research design in which the tion thus allows the researcher to
effects of treatment are confounded assume that the only systematic dif-
with other factors is one in which ferences between the treatment and
there is low internal validity. For comparison groups are found in the
example, suppose a researcher seeks treatments or interventions that are
to assess the effects of a specific drug applied. When the study is complete,
54 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

1
the researcher can argue with confi- comparisons made. However, in non-
dence that if a difference has been experimental studies, statistical con-
observed between treatment and trols are the primary method applied
comparison groups, it is likely the in attempts to increase the level of a
result of the treatment itself (since studys internal validity. In this case,
randomization has isolated the treat- multivariate statistical methods are
ment effect from other possible used to isolate the effects of treat-
causes). ment from that of other causes. This
In nonrandomized studies, two deman ds of cou rs e t h at t h e
methods may be used for isolating researcher clearly identify and mea-
treatment or program effects. Quasi- sure all other factors that may
experiments, like randomized exper- threaten the internal validity of the
iments, rely on the design of a study outcomes. Only if all such fac-
research study to isolate the effects of tors are included in the multivariate
treatment. Using matching or other models estimated can the researcher
methods in an attempt to establish be confident that the effects of treat-
equivalence between groups, quasi- ment that have been reported are not
experiments mimic experimental confounded with other causes.
designs in that they attempt to rule In theory, the three methods
out competing causes by identifying described here are equally valid for
groups that are similar except in the solving the problem of isolating
nature of the treatment that they treatment or program effects. Each
receive in the study. Importantly, can ensure high internal validity
however, quasi-experiments do not when applied correctly. In practice,
randomize out the effects of other however, as Feder and Boruch (2000)
causes as is the case in randomized note, there is little disagreement
experimental designs; rather they that experiments provide a superior
seek to maximize the equivalence method for assessing the effective-
between the units studied through ness of a given intervention (292).
matching or other methods. Threats Randomization, according to Kunz
to internal validity in quasi-experi- and Oxman (1998), is the only
mental studies derive from the fact means of controlling for unknown and
that it is seldom possible to find or to unmeasured differences between
create treatment and control groups comparison groups as well as those
that are not systematically different that are known and measured
in one respect or another. (1185). While random allocation
Nonexperimental studies rely pri- itself ensures high internal validity
marily on statistical techniques to in experimental research, for quasi-
distinguish the effects of the inter- experimental and nonexperimental
vention or treatment from other con- research designs, unknown and
founding causes. In practice, quasi- unmeasured causes are generally
experimental studies often rely as seen as representing significant
well on statistical approaches to potential threats to the internal
2
increase the equivalence of the validity of the comparisons made.
RESEARCH DESIGN AND STUDY OUTCOMES 55

INTERNAL VALIDITY reached from the literature is that


AND STUDY OUTCOMES
IN PRIOR REVIEWS
there is not a consistent bias that
results from use of nonrandomized
While there is general agreement research designs. At the same time, a
that experimental studies are more few studies suggest that differences,
likely to ensure high internal valid- in whatever direction, will be small-
ity than are quasi-experimental or est when nonrandomized studies are
nonexperimental studies, it is diffi- well designed and implemented.
cult to specify at the outset the effects Kunz and Oxman (1998), for
that this will have on study out- example, using studies drawn from
comes. On one hand, it can be the Cochrane database, found vary-
assumed that weaker internal valid- ing results when analyzing 18 meta-
ity is likely to lead to biases in assess- analyses (incorporating 1211 clinical
ment of the effects of treatments or trials) in the field of health care. Of
interventions. However, the direction these 18 systematic reviews, 4 found
of that bias in any particular study is randomized and higher-quality
3
likely to depend on factors related to studies to give higher estimates of
the specific character of the research effects than nonrandomized and
that is conducted. For example, if lower-quality studies, and 8 reviews
nonrandomized studies do not found randomized or high-quality
account for important confounding studies to produce lower estimates of
causes that are positively related to effect sizes than nonrandomized or
treatment, they may on average lower-quality studies. Five other
overestimate program outcomes. reviews found little or inconclusive
However, if such unmeasured causes differences between different types
are negatively related to treatment, of research designs, and in one
nonrandomized studies would be review, low-quality studies were
expected to underestimate program found to be more likely to report find-
outcomes. Heinsman and Shadish ings of harmful effects of treatments.
(1996) suggested that whatever the Mixed results are also found in
differences in research design, if systematic reviews in the social sci-
nonrandomized and randomized ences. Some reviews suggest that
studies are equally well designed and nonrandomized studies will on aver-
implemented (and thus internal age underestimate program effects.
validity is maximized in each), there For example, Heinsman and Shadish
should be little difference in the esti- (1996) looked at four meta-analyses
mates gained. Much of what is known that focused on interventions in four
empirically about these questions is different areas: drug use, effects of
drawn from reviews in such fields as coaching on Scholastic Aptitude Test
medicine, psychology, economics, and performance, ability grouping of
education (for example, see Burtless pupils in secondary schools, and
1995; Hedges 2000; Kunz and Oxman psychosocial interventions for
1998; Lipsey and Wilson 1993). Fol- postsurgery outcomes. Included in
lowing, what one would expect in the- their analysis were 98 published and
ory, a general conclusion that can be unpublished studies. As a whole,
56 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

randomized experiments were found between randomized and nonran-


to yield larger effect sizes than stud- domized studies decreased when
ies where randomization was not confounding variables related to the
used. In contrast, Friedlander and quality of the design of the study
Robins (2001), in a review of social were included.
welfare programs, found that non- Works that specifically address
experimental statistical approaches the relationship between study
often yielded estimates larger than design and study outcomes are
those gained in randomized studies scarce in criminal justice. In turn,
(see also Cox, Davidson, and Bynum assessment of this relationship is
1995; LaLonde 1986). most often not a central focus of the
In a large-scale meta-analysis reviews developed, and reviewers
examining the efficacy of psychologi- generally examine a specific criminal
cal, educational, and behavioral justice area, most often corrections
treatment, Lipsey and Wilson (1993) (for example, see Bailey 1966; Mac-
suggested that conclusions reached Kenzie and Hickman 1998; White-
on the basis of nonrandomized stud- head and Lab 1989). Results of these
ies are not likely to strongly bias con- studies provide little guidance for
clusions regarding treatment or pro- specifying a general relationship
gram effects. Although studies between study design and study out-
varied greatly in both directions as to comes for criminal justice research.
whether nonrandomized designs In an early review of 100 reports of
overestimated or underestimated correctional treatment between 1940
effects as compared with randomized and 1960, for example, Bailey (1966)
designs, no consistent bias in either found that research design had little
direction was detected. Lipsey and effect on the claimed success of treat-
Wilson, however, did find a notable ment, though he noted a slight posi-
difference between studies that tive relationship between the rigor
employed a control/comparison of the design and study outcome.
design and those that used one-group Logan (1972), who also reviewed cor-
pre and post designs. The latter stud- rectional treatment programs, found
ies produced consistently higher esti- a slight negative correlation between
mates of treatment effects. study design and claimed success.
Support for the view that stronger Recent studies are no more conclu-
nonrandomized studies are likely to sive. Wilson, Gallagher, and Mac-
provide results similar to random- Kenzie (2000), in a meta-analysis of
ized experimental designs is pro- corrections-based education, voca-
vided by Shadish and Ragsdale tion, and work programs, found that
(1996). In a review of 100 studies of run-of-the-mill quasi-experimental
marital or family psychotherapy, studies produced larger effects than
they found overall that randomized did randomized experiments. How-
experiments yielded significantly ever, such studies also produced
larger weighted average effect sizes larger effects than did low-quality
than nonequivalent control group designs that clearly lacked compara-
designs. Nonetheless, the difference bility among groups. In a review of
RESEARCH DESIGN AND STUDY OUTCOMES 57

165 school-based prevention pro- over a yearlong period between 1996


grams, Whitehead and Lab (1989) and 1997. The report attempted to
found little difference in the size of identify all available research rele-
effects in randomized and non- vant to crime prevention in seven
randomized studies. Interestingly broad areas: communities, families,
how ever, they reported t h at schools, labor markets, places, polic-
nonrandomized studies were much ing, and criminal justice (correc-
less likely to report a backfire effect tions). Studies chosen for inclusion in
whereby treatment was found to the Maryland Report met minimal
exacerbate rather than ameliorate methodological requirements.5
the problem examined. In contrast, a Though the Maryland Report did
more recent review by Wilson, not examine the relationship be-
Gottfredson, and Najaka (in press) tween study design and study out-
found overall that nonrandomized comes, it did define the quality of the
studies yielded results on average methods used to evaluate the
significantly lower than randomized strength of the evidence provided
experiments results, even account- through a scientific methods scale
ing for a series of other design char- (SMS). This SMS was coded with
acteristics (including the overall numbers 1 through 5, with 5 being
quality of the implementation of the the strongest scientific evidence
study). However, it should be noted (Sherman et al. 1997, 2.18). Overall,
that many of these studies did not studies higher on the scale have
include delinquency measures, and higher internal validity, and studies
schools rather than individuals were with lower scores have lower internal
often the unit of random allocation.4 validity. The 5-point scale was
broadly defined in the Maryland Re-
port (Sherman et al. 1997) as follows:
THE STUDY

We sought to define the influence 1: Correlation between a crime


of research design on study outcomes prevention program and a mea-
across a large group of studies repre- sure of crime or crime risk fac-
senting the different types of tors.
research design as well as a broad 2: Temporal sequence between the
array of criminal justice areas. The program and the crime or risk
most comprehensive source we could outcome clearly observed, or a
identify for this purpose has come to comparison group present with-
be known as the Maryland Report out the demonstrated compara-
(Sherman et al. 1997). The Maryland bility to the treatment group.
Report was commissioned by the 3: A comparison between two or
National Institute of Justice to iden- more units of analysis, one with
tify what works, what doesnt, and and one without the program.
whats promising in preventing 4: Comparison between multiple
crime. It was conducted at the Uni- units with and without the pro-
versity of Marylands Department of gram, controlling for other fac-
Criminology and Criminal Justice tors, or a non-equivalent com-
58 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

parison group has only minor reviewed in the report. Importantly,


differences evident. in many cases it was not possible to
5: Random assignment and analy- code such information because the
sis of comparable units to pro- original study authors did not pro-
gram and comparison groups. vide the specific details necessary for
(2.18-2.19) calculating standardized effect coef-
ficients. But the approach used by
A score of 5 on this scale suggests a the Maryland investigators also
randomized experimental design, reflected a broader philosophical
and a score of 1 a nonexperimental decision that emphasized the bottom
approach. Scores of 3 and 4 may be line of what was known about the
associated with quasi-experimental effects of crime and justice interven-
designs, with 4 distinguished from 3 tions. In criminal justice, the out-
by a greater concern with control for come of a study is often considered
threats to internal validity. A score of more important than the effect size
2 represents a stronger nonexperi- noted. This is the case in good part
mental design or a weaker quasi-ex- because there are often only a very
perimental approach. However, the small number of studies that exam-
overall rating given to a study could ine a specific type of treatment or
be affected by other design criteria intervention. In addition, policy deci-
such as response rate, attrition, use sions are made not on the basis of a
of statistical tests, and statistical review of the effect sizes that are
power. It is impossible to tell from the reported but rather on whether one
Maryland Report how much influ- or a small group of studies suggests
ence such factors had on each studys that the treatment or intervention
rating. However, correspondence works.
with four of the main study investi- From the data available in the
gators suggests that adjustments Maryland Report, we developed an
based on these other factors were un- overall measure of study outcomes
common and generally would result that we call the investigator reported
in an SMS decrease or increase of result (IRR). The IRR was created as
only one level. an ordinal scale with three values: 1,
Although the Maryland Report 0, and 1, reflecting whether a study
included a measure of study design, concluded that the treatment or in-
it did not contain a standardized tervention worked, had no detected
measure of study outcome. Most effect, or led to a backfire effect. It is
prior reviews have relied on stan- defined by what is reported in the ta-
dardized effect measures as a crite- bles of the Maryland Report and is
rion for studying the relationship coded as follows:6
between design type and study find-
ings. Although in some of the area 1: The program or treatment is re-
reviews in the Maryland Report, ported to have had an intended
standardized effect sizes were calcu- positive effect for the criminal
lated for specific studies, this was not justice system or society. Out-
the case for the bulk of the studies comes in this case supported
RESEARCH DESIGN AND STUDY OUTCOMES 59

the position that interventions gators to support the specific conclu-


or treatments lead to reduc- sions reached in each study. In turn,
tions in crime, recidivism, or re- the tables in the Maryland Report
lated measures. 7 often do not note whether specific
0: The program treatment was re- studies employed statistical tests of
ported to have no detected ef- significance. Accordingly, in review-
fect, or the effect was reported ing the Maryland Report studies, we
as not statistically significant. cannot assess whether the presence
1: The program or treatment had or absence of such tests influences
an unintended backfire effect our conclusions. Later in our article
for the criminal justice system we reexamine our results, taking into
or society. Outcomes in this case account statistical significance in the
supported the position that in- context of a more recent review in the
terventions or treatments were corrections area that was modeled on
harmful and lead to increases the Maryland Report.
in crime, recidivism, or related Finally, as we noted earlier, most
measures.8 systematic reviews of study out-
comes have come to use standardized
This scale provides an overall effect size as a criterion. While we
measure of the conclusions reached think that the IRR scale is useful for
by investigators in the studies that gaining an understanding of the rela-
were reviewed in the Maryland Re- tionship between research design
port. However, we think it is impor- and reported study conclusions, we
tant to note at the outset some spe- recognize that a different set of con-
cific features of the methodology clusions might have been reached
used that may affect the findings we had we focused on standardized
gain using this approach. Perhaps effect sizes. Again, we use the correc-
most significant is the fact that tions review referred to above to
Maryland reviewers generally relied assess how our conclusions might
on the reported conclusions of inves- have differed if we had focused on
tigators unless there was obvious ev- standardized effect sizes rather than
idence to the contrary.9 This ap- the IRR scale.
proach led us to term the scale the We coded the Scientific Methods
investigator reported result and rein- Scale and the IRR directly from the
forces the fact that we examine the tables reported in Preventing Crime:
impacts of study design on what in- What Works, What Doesnt, Whats
vestigators report rather than on the Promising (Sherman et al. 1997). We
actual outcomes of the studies exam- do not include all of the studies in the
ined. Maryland Report in our review. First,
While the Maryland reviewers given our interest in the area of crim-
examined tests of statistical signifi- inal justice, we excluded studies that
cance in coming to conclusions about did not have a crime or delinquency
which programs or treatments ou t come meas u re. S econ d, w e
work,10 they did not require that sta- excluded studies that did not provide
tistical tests be reported by investi- an SMS score (a feature of some
60 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 1 TABLE 2
STUDIES CATEGORIZED BY SMS STUDIES CATEGORIZED BY THE IRR

Studies Studies
SMS n Percentage IRR n Percentage

1 10 3 1 34 11
2 94 31 0 76 25
3 130 42 1 198 64
4 28 9
5 46 15 Total 308 100

Total 308 100

justice studies reviewed in the Mary-


land Report produced a reported
tables in the community and family result in the direction of success for
sections of the report). Finally, we the treatment or intervention exam-
excluded the school-based area from ined. This result is very much at odds
review because only selected studies with reviews conducted in earlier
were reported in tables.11 All other decades that suggested that most
studies reviewed in the Maryland interventions had little effect on
Report were included, which resulted crime or related problems (for
in a sample of 308 studies. Tables 1 example, see Lipton, Martinson, and
and 2 display the breakdown of these Wilks 1975; Logan 1972; Martinson
12
studies by SMS and IRR. 1974). At the same time, a number
As is apparent from Table 1, there of the studies examined, about 1 in
is wide variability in the nature of 10, reported a backfire effect for
the research methods used in the treatment or intervention.
studies that are reviewed. About 15
percent were coded in the highest
RELATING STUDY DESIGN
SMS category, which demands a ran- AND STUDY OUTCOMES
domized experimental design. Only
10 studies included were coded in the In Tables 3 and 4 we present our
lowest SMS category, though almost basic findings regarding the relation-
a third fall in category 2. The largest ship between study design and study
category is score 3, which required outcomes in the Maryland Report
simply a comparison between two sample. Table 3 provides mean IRR
units of analysis, one with and one outcome scores across the five SMS
without treatment. About 1 in 10 design categories. While the mean
cases were coded as 4, suggesting a IRR scores in this case present a sim-
quasi-experimental study with ple method for examining the results,
strong attention to creating equiva- we also provide an overall statistical
lence between the groups studied. measure of correlation, Tau-c (and
The most striking observation the associated significance level),
that is drawn from Table 2 is that which is more appropriate for data of
almost two-thirds of the crime and this type. In Table 4 we provide the
RESEARCH DESIGN AND STUDY OUTCOMES 61

TABLE 3 in terms of internal validity as mea-


MEAN IRR SCORES sured by the SMS, the less likely is a
ACROSS SMS CATEGORIES study to conclude that the interven-
tion or treatment worked. The
Standard
SMS Mean n Deviation weaker the method, the less likely
the study is to conclude that the
1 .80 10 .42
intervention or treatment backfired.
2 .66 94 .63
3 .56 130 .67 While 8 of the 10 studies in the
4 .39 28 .83 lowest SMS category and 74 percent
5 .22 46 .70 of those in category 2 show a treat-
ment impact in the desired direction,
Total .53 308 .69
this was true for only 37 percent of
NOTE: Tau-c = .181. p < .001. the randomized experiments in cate-
gory 5. Only in the case of backfire
outcomes in categories 4 and 5 does
cross-tabulation of IRR and SMS the table not follow our basic find-
scores. This presentation of the ings, and this departure is small.
results allows us to examine more Overall the relationship observed in
carefully the nature of the relation- the table is statistically significant at
ship both in terms of outcomes in the the .005 level.
expected treatment direction and
outcomes that may be classified as Comparing the highest-quality
backfire effects. nonrandomized studies with
Overall Tables 3 and 4 suggest randomized experiments
that there is a linear inverse rela-
As noted earlier, some scholars
tionship between the SMS and the
argue that higher-quality nonran-
IRR. The mean IRR score decreases
domized studies are likely to have
with each increase in step in the SMS
outcomes similar to outcomes of ran-
score (see Table 3). While fully
domized evaluations. This hypothe-
nonexperimental designs have a
sis is not supported by our data. In
mean IRR score of .80, randomized
Table 5 we combine quasi-experi-
experiments have a mean of only .22.
mental studies in SMS categories 3
The run of the mill quasi-experimen- and 4 and compare them with ran-
tal designs represented in category 3 domized experimental studies placed
have a mean IRR score of .56, while in SMS category 5. Again we find a
the strongest quasi experiments (cat- statistically significant negative
egory 4) have a mean of .39. The over- relationship (p < .01). While 37 per-
all correlation between study design cent of the level 5 experimental stud-
and study outcomes is moderate and ies show a treatment effect in the
negative (.18), and the relationship desired direction, this was true for 65
is statistically significant at the .001 percent of the quasi-experimental
level. studies.
Looking at the cross-tabulation of Even if we examine only the high-
SMS and IRR scores, our findings are est-quality quasi-experimental stud-
reinforced. The stronger the method ies as represented by category 4 and
62 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 4
CROSS-TABULATION OF SMS AND IRR

SMS
1 2 3 4 5
IRR n Percentage n Percentage n Percentage n Percentage n Percentage

1 0 0 8 9 13 10 6 21 7 15
0 2 20 16 17 31 24 5 18 22 48
1 8 80 70 74 86 66 17 61 17 37

Total 10 100 94 100 130 100 28 100 46 100


NOTE: Chi-square = 25.487 with 8 df (p < .005).

TABLE 5 TABLE 6
COMPARING QUASI-EXPERIMENTAL COMPARING HIGH-QUALITY QUASI-
STUDIES (SMS = 3 OR 4) WITH EXPERIMENTAL DESIGNS (SMS = 4)
RANDOMIZED EXPERIMENTS (SMS = 5) WITH RANDOMIZED DESIGNS (SMS = 5)

SMS SMS
3 or 4 5 4 5
IRR n Percentage n Percentage IRR n Percentage n Percentage
1 19 12 7 15 1 6 21 7 15
0 36 23 22 48 0 5 18 22 48
1 103 65 17 37 1 17 61 17 37

Total 158 100 46 100 Total 28 100 46 100


NOTE: Chi-square = 12.971 with 2 df (p < NOTE: Chi-square = 6.805 with 2 df (p < .05).
.01).

compare these to the randomized this was true for only 37 percent of
studies included in category 5, the the randomized studies in category 5.
relationship between study out- Accordingly, even when comparing
comes and study design remains sta- those nonrandomized studies with
tistically significant at the .05 level the highest internal validity with
(see Table 6). There is little difference randomized experiments, we find
between the two groups in the pro- significant differences in terms of
portion of backfire o u t comes reported study outcomes.
reported; however, there remains a
very large gap between the propor- Taking into account tests
tion of SMS category 4 and SMS cate- of statistical significance
gory 5 studies that report an outcome It might be argued that had we
in the direction of treatment effec- used a criterion of statistical signifi-
tiveness. While 61 percent of the cat- cance, the overall findings would not
egory 4 SMS studies reported a posi- have been consistent with the analy-
tive treatment or intervention effect, ses reported above. While we cannot
RESEARCH DESIGN AND STUDY OUTCOMES 63

examine this question in the context TABLE 7


of the Maryland Report, since statis- RELATING SMS AND IRR ONLY FOR
tical significance is generally not STUDIES IN MACKENZIE AND HICKMAN
(1998) THAT INCLUDE TESTS OF
reported in the tables or the text of
STATISTICAL SIGNIFICANCE
the report, we can review this con-
cern in the context of a more recent Scale A Scale B
review conducted in the corrections SMS Mean n Mean n
area by one of the Maryland investi-
1 0 0
gators, which uses a similar method- 2 0.83 24 1.46 24
ology and reports Maryland SMS 3 0.62 26 1.04 26
(see MacKenzie and Hickman 1998). 4 0.36 11 0.64 11
MacKenzie and Hickman (1998) 5 0.00 7 0.14 7
examined 101 studies in their
Total .59 68 1.03 68
1998 review of what works in correc-
NOTE: Tau-c for scale A = .285 (p < .005).
tions, of which 68 are reported to Tau-c for scale B = .311 (p < .005).
have included tests of statistical
significance.
Developing the IRR score for each study design and study outcomes is
of MacKenzie and Hickmans (1998) negative and statistically significant
studies proved more complex than ( p < . 005) i rres pect i v e of t h e
the coding done for the Maryland approach we used to define the IRR
Report. MacKenzie and Hickman outcome scale (see Table 7). Using
reported all of the studies results, scale A, the correlation observed is
sometimes breaking up results by .29, while using scale B, the observed
gender, employment, treatment mix, correlation is .31.
or criminal history, to list a few exam-
ples. Rather than count each result Comparing effect size
as a separate study, we developed two and IRR score results
different methods that followed dif-
ferent assumptions for coding the It might be argued that our overall
IRR index. findings are related to specific char-
The first simply notes whether acteristics of the IRR scale rather
any significant findings were found than the underlying relationship
supporting a treatment effect and between study design and study out-
codes a backfire effect when there are comes. We could not test this ques-
statistically significant negative tion directly using the Maryland
findings with no positive treatment Report data because, as noted earlier,
13
effects (scale A). The second (scale standardized effect sizes were not
B) is more complex and gives weight consistently recorded in the report.
14
to each result in each study. However, MacKenzie and Hickman
Taking this approach, our findings (1998) did report standardized effect
analyzing the MacK enzie and size coefficients, and thus we are able
Hickman (1998) data follow those to reexamine this question in the
reported when analyzing the Mary- context of corrections-based criminal
land Report. The correlation between justice studies.
64 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Using the average standardized TABLE 8


effect size reported for each study RELATING AVERAGE EFFECT SIZE
reviewed by MacKenzie and Hick- AND SMS FOR STUDIES IN
MACKENZIE AND HICKMAN (1988)
man (1998) for the entire sample
(including studies where statistical Effect Size Available
significance is not reported), the from the Entire Sample
results follow those gained from SMS Mean n
relating IRR and SMS scores using
1 0
the Maryland Report sample (see 2 .29 39
Table 8). Again the correlation 3 .23 30
between SMS and study outcomes is 4 .19 13
negative; in this case the correlation 5 .00 7
is about .30. The observed relation-
Total .23 89
ship is also statistically significant at Missing values 12
the .005 level. Accordingly, these
NOTE: Correlation (r) = .296 (p < .005).
findings suggest that our observa-
tion of a negative relationship
between study design and study out- IRR index that was drawn from the
comes in the Maryland Report sam- Maryland Report. We found our
ple is not an artifact of the particular results to be consistent using both
codings of the IRR scale. methods. Studies that were defined
as including designs with higher
DISCUSSION internal validity were likely to report
smaller effect sizes than studies with
Our review of the Maryland designs associated with lower inter-
Report Studies suggests that in crim- nal validity.
inal justice, there is a moderate Prior reviews of the relationship
inverse relationship between the between study design and study out-
quality of a research design, defined comes do not predict our findings.
in terms of internal validity, and the Indeed, as we noted earlier, the main
outcomes reported in a study. This lesson that can be drawn from prior
relationship continues to be observed research is that the impact of study
even when comparing the highest- design is very much dependant on
quality nonrandomized studies with the characteristics of the particular
randomized experiments. Using a area or studies that are reviewed. In
related database concentrating only theory as well, there is no reason to
on the corrections area, we also found assume that there will be a system-
that our findings are consistent atic type of bias in studies with lower
when taking into account only stud- internal validity. What can be said
ies that employed statistical tests of simply is that such studies, all else
significance. Finally, using the same being equal, are likely to provide
database, we were able to examine biased findings as compared with
whether our results would have dif- results drawn from randomized
fered had we used standardized experimental designs. Why then do
effect size measures rather than the we find in reviewing a broad group of
RESEARCH DESIGN AND STUDY OUTCOMES 65

crime and justice studies what measures. Of these, 9 were random-


appears to be a systematic relation- ized experiments, 4 were quasi-
ship between study design and study experimental trials, and 7 were fully
outcomes? nonexperimental studies. Petrosino,
One possible explanation for our Petrosino, and Buehler reported on
findings is that they are simply an the randomized experimental trials
artifact of combining a large number in their Campbell Collaboration
of studies drawn from many different review. They concluded that Scared
areas of criminal justice. Indeed, Straight and related programs do not
there are generally very few studies evidence any benefit in terms of
that examine a very specific type of recidivism and actually increase sub-
treatment or intervention in the sequent delinquency. However, a
Maryland Report. And it may be that very different picture of the effective-
were we able to explore the impacts ness of these programs is drawn from
of study design on study outcomes for our review of the quasi-experimental
specific types of treatments or inter- and nonexperimental studies. Over-
ventions, we would find patterns dif- all, these studies, in contrast to the
ferent from the aggregate ones experimental evaluations, suggest
reported here. We think it is likely that Scared Straight programs not
that for specific areas of treatment or only are not harmful but are more
specific types of studies in criminal likely than not to produce a crime
justice, the relationship between prevention benefit.
study design and study outcomes will We believe that our findings, how-
differ from those we observe. None- ever preliminary, point to the possi-
theless, review of this question in the bility of an overall positive bias in
context of one specific type of treat- nonrandomized criminal justice
ment examined by the Campbell Col- studies. This bias may in part reflect
laboration (where there was a sub- a number of other factors that we
stantial enough number of ran- could not control for in our data, for
domized and nonrandomized studies example, publication bias or differen-
for comparison) points to the salience tial attrition rates across designs
of our overall conclusions even (see Shadish and Ragsdale 1996).
within specific treatment areas (see However, we think that a more gen-
Petrosino, Petrosino, and Buehler eral explanation for our findings is
2001). We think this example is par- likely to be found in the norms of
ticularly important because it sug- cri mi n al j u s t i ce res earch an d
gests the potential confusion that practice.
might result from drawing conclu- Such norms are particularly
sions from nonrandomized studies. important in the development of non-
Relying on a systematic review randomized studies. Randomized
conducted by Petrosino, Petrosino, experiments provide little freedom to
and Buehler (2001) on Scared the researcher in defining equiva-
Straight and other kids-visit pro- lence between treatment and com-
grams, we identified 20 programs parison groups. Equivalence in ran-
that included crime-related outcome domized experiments is defined
66 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

simply through the process of ran- argue just the opposite. The inflexi-
domization. However, nonran- bility of randomized experimental
domized studies demand much designs has sometimes been seen as
insight and knowledge in the devel- a barrier to development of effective
opment of comparable groups of sub- theory and practice in criminology
jects. Not only must the researcher (for example, see Clarke and Cornish
understand the factors that influ- 1972; Eck 2001; Pawson and Tilley,
ence treatment so that he or she can 1997). Here it is argued that in a field
prevent confounding in the study in which we still know little about the
results, but such factors must be root causes and processes that
measured and then controlled for underlie phenomena we seek to
through some statistical or practical influence, randomized studies may
procedure. not allow investigators the freedom
It may be that such manipulation to carefully explore how treatments
is particularly difficult in criminal or programs influence their intended
justice study. Criminal justice practi- subjects. While this argument has
tioners may not be as strongly social- merit in specific circumstances, espe-
ized to the idea of experimentation as cially in exploratory analyses of
are practitioners in other fields like problems and treatments, we think
medicine. And in this context, it may our data suggest that it can lead in
be that a subtle form of creaming in more developed areas of our field to
which the cases considered most significant misinterpretation and
amenable to intervention are placed confusion.
in the intervention group is common.
In specific areas of criminal justice, CONCLUSION
such creaming may be exacerbated
by self-selection of subjects who are We asked at the outset of our arti-
motivated toward rehabilitation. cle whether the type of research
Nonrandomized designs, even in rel- design used in criminal justice influ-
atively rigorous quasi-experimental ences the conclusions that are
studies, may be unable to compen- reached. Our findings, based on the
sate or control for why a person is Maryland Report, suggest that
considered amenable and placed in design does matter and that its effect
the intervention group. Matching on in criminal justice study is system-
traditional control variables like age atic. The weaker a design, as indi-
and race, in turn, might not identify cated by internal validity, the more
the subtle components that make likely was a study to report a result in
individuals amenable to treatment favor of treatment and the less likely
and thus more likely to be placed in it was to report a harmful effect of
intervention or treatment categories. treatment. Even when comparing
Of course, we have so far assumed studies defined as randomized
that nonrandomized studies are designs in the Maryland Report with
biased in their overestimation of pro- strong quasi-experimental research
gram effects. Some scholars might designs, systematic and statistically
RESEARCH DESIGN AND STUDY OUTCOMES 67

significant differences were ob- 4. Moreover, it may be that the finding of


higher standardized effects sizes for random-
served. Though our study should
ized studies in this review was due to school-
bscores e seen only as a preliminary level as opposed to individual-level assign-
step in understanding how research ment. When only those studies that include a
design affects study outcomes in delinquency outcome are examined, a larger
criminal justice, it suggests that sys- effect is found when school rather than stu-
dent is the unit of analysis (Denise Gott-
tematic reviews of what works in
fredson, personal communication, 2001).
criminal justice may be strongly 5. As the following Scientific Methods
bias ed w hen incl u di n g Scale illustrates, the lowest acceptable type of
nonrandomized studies. In efforts evaluation for inclusion in the Maryland Re-
such as those being developed by the port is a simple correlation between a crime
prevention program and a measure of crime or
Campbell Collaboration, such poten-
crime risk factors. Thus studies that were de-
tial biases should be taken into scriptive or contained only process measures
account in coming to conclusions were excluded.
about the effects of interventions. 6. There were also (although rarely) stud-
ies in the Maryland Report that reported two
findings in opposite directions. For instance, in
Notes Sherman and colleagues (1997) section on
specific deterrence (8.18-8.19), studies of ar-
1. Statistical adjustments for random rest for domestic violence had positive results
group differences are sometimes employed in for employed offenders and backfire results for
experimental studies as well. nonemployed offenders. In these isolated
2. We should note that we have assumed cases, the study was coded twice with the same
so far that external validity (the degree to scientific methods scores and each of the in-
which it can be inferred that outcomes apply to vestigator-reported result scores (of 1 and 1)
the populations that are the focus of treat- separately.
ment) is held constant in these comparisons. 7. For studies examining the absence of a
Some scholars argue that experimental stud- program (such as a police strike) where social
ies are likely to have lower external validity conditions worsened or crime increased, this
because it is often difficult to identify institu- would be coded as 1.
tions that are willing to randomize partici- 8. For studies examining the absence of a
pants. Clearly, where randomized designs program (such as a police strike) where social
have lower external validity, the assumption conditions improved or crime decreased, this
that they are to be preferred to nonrandomized would be coded as 1.
studies is challenged. 9. Only in the school-based area was there
3. Kunz and Oxman (1998) not only com- a specific criterion for assessing the investiga-
pared randomized and nonrandomized stud- tors conclusions. As noted below, however, the
ies but also adequately and inadequately con- school-based studies are excluded from our re-
cealed randomized trials and high-quality view for other reasons.
versus low-quality studies. Generally, high- 10. For example, the authors of the Mary-
quality randomized studies included ade- land Report noted in discussing criteria for de-
quately concealed allocation, while lower- ciding which programs work, These are pro-
quality randomized trails were inadequately grams that we are reasonably certain of
concealed. In addition, the general terms high- preventing crime or reducing risk factors for
quality trials and low-quality trials indicate a crime in the kinds of social contexts in which
difference where the specific effect of random- they have been evaluated, and for which the
ization or allocation concealment could not be findings should be generalizable to similar set-
separated from the effect of other methodologi- tings in other places and times. Programs
cal manoeuvres such as double blinding coded as working by this definition must have
(Kunz and Oxman 1998, 1185). at least two level 3 evaluations with statistical
68 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

significance tests showing effectiveness and significant results were present) but no
the preponderance of all available evidence statistically significant results supporting the
supporting the same conclusion (Sherman effectiveness of treatment.
et al. 1997, 2-20).
11. It is the case that many of the studies in
this area would have been excluded anyway References
since they often did not have a crime or delin-
quency outcome measure (but rather exam- Bailey, Walter C. 1966. Correctional Out-
ined early risk factors for crime and delin- come: An Evaluation of 100 Reports.
quency). Journal of Criminal Law, Criminology
12. While the Maryland Report is consis- and Police Science 57:153-60.
tent with other recent reviews that also point Boruch, Robert F., Anthony Petrosino,
to greater success in criminal justice interven- and Iain Chalmers. 1999. The Camp-
tions during the past 20 years (for example, see
bell Collaboration: A Proposal for Sys-
Poyner 1993; Visher and Weisburd 1998;
tematic, Multi-National, and Continu-
Weisburd 1997), we think the very high per-
centage of studies showing a treatment impact ous Reviews of Evidence. Background
is likely influenced by publication bias. The paper for the meeting at University
high rate of positive findings is also likely in- CollegeLondon, School of Public Pol-
fluenced by the general weaknesses of the icy, July.
study designs employed. This is suggested by Boruch, Robert F., Brook Snyder, and
our findings reported later: that the weaker a Dorothy DeMoya. 2000. The Impor-
research design in terms of internal validity, tance of Randomized Field Trials.
the more likely is the study to report a positive Crime & Delinquency 46:156-80.
treatment outcome.
Burtless, Gary. 1995. The Case for Ran-
13. The coding scheme for scale A was as
follows. A value of 1 indicates that the study
domized Field Trials in Economic and
had any statistically significant findings sup- Policy Research. Journal of Economic
porting a positive treatment effect, even if Perspectives 9:63-84.
findings included results that were not signifi- Campbell, Donald P. and Robert F.
cant or had negative or backfire findings. A Boruch. 1975. Making the Case for
value of 0 indicates that the study had only Randomized Assignment to Treatments
nonsignificant findings. A value of 1 indicates by Considering the Alternatives: Six
that the study had only statistically signifi- Ways in Which Quasi-Experimental
cant negative or backfire findings or statisti-
Evaluations in Compensatory Educa-
cally significant negative findings with other
tion Tend to Underestimate Effects. In
nonsignificant results.
14. Scale B was created according to the fol- Evaluation and Experiment: Some
lowing rules. A value of 2 indicates that the Critical Issues in Assessing Social
study had only or mostly statistically signifi- Programs, ed. Carl Bennett and
cant findings supporting a treatment effect Arthur Lumsdaine. New York: Aca-
(more than 50 percent) when including all re- demic Press.
sults, even nonsignificant ones. A value of 1 in- Chalmers, Iain and Douglas G. Altman.
dicates that the study had some statistically 1995. Systematic Reviews. London:
significant findings supporting a treatment ef- British Medical Journal Press.
fect (50 percent or less, counting both positive
Clarke, Ronald V. and Derek B. Cornish.
significant and nonsignificant results) even if
the nonsignificant results outnumbered the
1972. The Control Trial in Institu-
positive statistically significant results. A tional Research: Paradigm or Pitfall
value of 0 indicates that no statistically signifi- for Penal Evaluators? London: HMSO.
cant findings were reported. A value of 1 indi- Cox, Stephen M., William S. Davidson,
cates that the study evidenced statistically and Timothy S. Bynum. 1995. A Meta-
significant backfire effects (even if non- Analytic Assessment of Delinquency-
RESEARCH DESIGN AND STUDY OUTCOMES 69

Related Outcomes of Alternative Edu- Feder, Lynette, Annette Jolin, and


cation Programs. Crime & Delin- William Feyerherm. 2000. Lessons
quency 41:219-34. from Two Randomized Experiments
Cullen, Francis T. and Paul Gendreau. in Criminal Justice Settings. Crime &
2000. Assessing Correctional Rehabil- Delinquency 46:380-400.
itation: Policy, Practice, and Prospects. Friedlander, Daniel and Philip K. Robins.
In Policies, Processes, and Decisions of 2001. Evaluating Program Evalua-
the Criminal Justice System: Criminal tions: New Evidence on Commonly Used
Justice 3, ed. Julie Horney. Washing- Non-Experimental Methods. Ameri-
ton, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, can Economic Review 85:923-37.
National Institute of Justice. Hedges, Larry V. 2000. Using Converging
Davies, Huw T. O., Sandra Nutley, and Evidence in Policy Formation: The
Peter C. Smith. 2000. What Works: Evi- Case of Class Size Research. Evalua-
dence-Based Policy and Practice in tion and Research in Education
Public Services. London: Policy Press. 14:193-205.
Davies, Philip. 1999. What Is Evidence- Heinsman, Donna T. and William R.
Based Education? British Journal of Shadish. 1996. Assignment Methods
Educational Studies 47:108-21. in Experimentation: When Do
Eck, John. 2001. Learning from Experi- Nonrandomized Experiments Ap-
ence in Problem Oriented Policing and proximate Answers from Randomized
Crime Prevention: The Positive Func- Experiments? Psychological Methods
tions of Weak Evaluations and the 1:154-69.
Negative Functions of Strong Ones. Kunz, Regina and Andy Oxman. 1998.
Unpublished manuscript. The Unpredictability Paradox: Re-
Egger, Matthias and G. Davey Smith. view of Empirical Comparisons of
1998. Bias in Location and Selection of Randomized and Non-Randomized
Studies. British Medical Journal Clinical Trials. British Medical Jour-
316:61-66. nal 317:1185-90.
Farrington, David P. 1983. Randomized LaLonde, Robert J. 1986. Evaluating the
Experiments in Crime and Justice. In Econometric Evaluations of Training
Crime and Justice: An Annual Review Programs with Experimental Data.
of Research, ed. Norval Morris and Mi- American Economic Review 76:604-20.
chael Tonry. Chicago: University of Lipsey, Mark W. and David B. Wilson.
Chicago Press. 1993. The Efficacy of Psychological,
. 2000. Standards for Inclusion of Educational, and Behavioral Treat-
Studies in Systematic Reviews. Dis- ment: Confirmation from Meta-Analy-
cussion paper for the Campbell Col- sis. American Psychologist 48:1181-
laboration Crime and Justice Coordi- 209.
nating Group. Lipton, Douglas S., Robert M. Martinson,
Farrington, David P. and Anthony and Judith Wilks. 1975. The Effective-
Petrosino. 2001. The Campbell Collab- ness of Correctional Treatment: A Sur-
oration Crime and Justice Group. An- vey of Treatment Evaluation Studies.
nals of the American Academy of Polit- New York: Praeger.
ical and Social Science 578:35-49. Logan, Charles H. 1972. Evaluation Re-
Feder, Lynette and Robert F. Boruch. search in Crime and DelinquencyA
2000. The Need for Experiments in Reappraisal. Journal of Criminal
Criminal Justice Settings. Crime & Law, Criminology and Police Science
Delinquency 46:291-94. 63:378-87.
70 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

MacKenzie, Doris L. 2000. Evidence- Sherman, Lawrence W., Denise C.


based Corrections: Identifying What Gottfredson, Doris Layton MacKen-
Works. Crime & Delinquency 46:457-71. zie, John E. Eck, Peter Reuter, and
MacKenzie, Doris L. and Laura J. Shawn D. Bushway. 1997. Preventing
Hickman. 1998. What Works in Correc- Crime: What Works, What Doesnt,
tions (Report submitted to the State of Whats Promising. Washington, DC:
Washington Legislature Joint Audit U.S. Department of Justice, National
and Review Committee). College Park: Institute of Justice.
University of Maryland. Visher, Christy A. and David Weisburd.
Martinson, Robert. 1974. What Works? 1998. Identifying What Works: Recent
Questions and Answers About Prison Trends in Crime Prevention. Crime,
Reform. Public Interest 35:22-54. Law and Social Change 28:223-42.
Millenson, Michael L. 1997. Demanding Weisburd, David. 1997. Reorienting
Medical Excellence: Doctors and Ac- Crime Prevention Research and Pol-
countability in the Information Age. icy: From the Causes of Criminality to
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. the Context of Crime (Research Report
Nutley, Sandra and Huw T. O. Davies. NIJ 16504). Washington, DC: U.S. De-
1999. The Fall and Rise of Evidence in partment of Justice, National Insti-
Criminal Justice. Public Money & tute of Justice.
Management 19:47-54. Whitehead, John T. and Steven P. Lab.
Pawson, Ray and Nick Tilley. 1997. Real- 1989. A Meta-Analysis of Juvenile
istic Evaluation. London: Sage. Correctional Treatment. Journal of
Petrosino, Anthony, Carolyn Petrosino, Research in Crime and Delinquency
and John Buehler. 2001. Pilot Test: 26:276-95.
The Effects of Scared Straight and Wilkinson, Leland and Task Force on Sta-
Other Juvenile Awareness Pro- tistical Inference. 1999. Statistical
grams on Delinquency. Unpublished Methods in Psychology Journals:
manuscript. Guidelines and Explanations. Ameri-
Poyner, Barry. 1993. What Works in can Psychologist 54:594-604.
Crime Prevention: An Overview of Wilson, David B., Catherine A. Gallagher,
Evaluations. In Crime Prevention Doris L. MacKenzie. 2000. A Meta-
Studies. Vol. 1, ed. Ronald V. Clarke. Analysis of Corrections-Based Educa-
Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press. tion, Vocation, and Work Programs for
Shadish, William R. and Kevin Ragsdale. Adult Offenders. Journal of Research
1996. Random Versus Nonrandom As- in Crime and Delinquency 37:347-68.
signment in Controlled Experiments: Wilson, David B., Denise C. Gottfredson,
Do You Get the Same Answer? Jour- and Stacy S. Najaka. In Press. School-
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- Based Prevention of Problem Behav-
chology 64:1290-305. iors: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of
Sherman, Lawrence W. 1998. Evidence- Quantitative Criminology.
Based Policing. In Ideas in American Zuger, Abigail. 1997. New Way of Doc-
Policing. Washington, DC: Police toring: By the Book. New York Times,
Foundation. 16 Dec.
ANNALS, AAPSS, 578, November 2001

THE ANNALS OF THE


META-ANALYTIC METHODS
AMERICAN
FOR CRIMINOLOGY
ACADEMY

Meta-Analytic Methods
for Criminology

By DAVID B. WILSON

ABSTRACT: Meta-analysis was designed to synthesize empirical re-


lationships across studies, such as the effects of a specific crime pre-
vention intervention on criminal offending behavior. Meta-analysis
focuses on the size and direction of effects across studies, examining
the consistency of effects and the relationship between study features
and observed effects. The findings from meta-analysis not only reveal
robust empirical relationships but also identify existing weaknesses
in the knowledge base. Furthermore, meta-analytic results can easily
be translated into summary statistics useful for informing public pol-
icy regarding effective crime prevention efforts.

David B. Wilson is an assistant professor of the administration of justice at George


Mason University. His research interests include program evaluation research method-
ology, meta-analysis, crime and general problem behavior prevention programs, and ju-
venile delinquency intervention effectiveness.

NOTE: This work was supported by the Jerry Lee Foundation.

71
72 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

I MAGINE you are given the task of


synthesizing what is currently
known about the effectiveness of cor-
typhoid fever (Pearson 1904). His
method involved computing the cor-
relation between inoculation and
rectional boot camps for reducing mortality within each study and then
future criminal behavior among ju- averaging the correlations across
venile and adult offenders. An ex- studies, producing a composite corre-
haustive search for all relevant eval- lation. By todays standards, this was
uations of boot camp programs a meta-analysis, although the term
compared with more traditional was not introduced until the 1970s
forms of punishment and rehabilita- (Glass 1976).
tion identifies 29 unique studies. The The logical framework of meta-
findings from these studies range analysis is based on the assumption
from large positive to large negative that the averaging of findings across
statistically significant effects. To studies will produce a more valid
complicate matters, the studies vary estimate of the effect of interest
in the evaluation methods used, in- than that of any individual study.
cluding the definition of recidivism Typically, the finding from any indi-
(for example, rearrest, reconviction, vidual study is imprecise due to sam-
and reinstitutionalization), offender pling error. Thus some studies of a
populations, and program character- specific phenomenon, such as the
istics. How will you meaningfully effectiveness of correctional boot
make sense of this array of informa- camps, will overestimate and others
tion? will underestimate the size of the
The statistical methods of meta- true effect. Instability in observed
analysis were designed specifically effects due to sampling error is an
to address this situation. Meta-anal- assumption at the core of statistical
ysis represents a statistical and sys- inference testing, such as a t test
tematic approach to reviewing between an intervention and com-
research findings across multiple parison condition. Averaging across
independent studies. As such, meta- studies is analogous to averaging
analyses are systematic reviews across individuals within a single
(Petrosino et al. 2001 [this issue]). study or averaging across multiple
However, not all criminological inter- test items.
vention research literatures can be For a collection of pure replica-
successfully meta-analyzed, and tions, the logic behind meta-analysis
thus not all systematic reviews will is indisputable if one accepts the
use the statistical methods of meta- logic and assumptions of the stan-
analysis. dard statistical practices of the social
The basic idea behind meta-analy- and medical sciences. Meta-analysis
sis dates back almost 100 years and as it is applied in criminology and the
is simple. Karl Pearson, the devel- other social sciences extends this
oper of the Pearson product-moment logic to collections of studies that are
correlation coefficient, synthesized conceptual replications, that is, stud-
the findings from multiple studies of ies that examine the same relation-
the effectiveness of inoculation for ship of interest but differ from one
META-ANALYTIC METHODS FOR CRIMINOLOGY 73

another in other respects, such as the it is objective and systematic, yet


research design or elements of the simple. Furthermore it upholds the
intervention. long-standing tradition in the social
Conceptual replications are assumed sciences of allowing the statistical
to be estimating the same fundamen- significance test to be the arbiter of
tal relationship, despite differences the validity of a scientific hypothesis.
in methodology and other substan- The intuitive appeal of the vote
tive features. This variability in count obscures its weaknesses. First,
study features can be viewed as a the vote count fails to account for the
strength, however, because a synthe- differential precision of the studies
sis of conceptual replications can being reviewed. Larger studies, all
show that a relationship is observed else being equal, provide more pre-
across a range of methodological and cise estimates of the relationship of
substantive variability. Unlike sam- interest and thus should be given
pling error, however, errors in esti- greater weight in a review.
mates of the relationship of interest Second, the vote count fails to rec-
that arise from poor study design will ognize the fundamental asymmetry
not necessarily cancel out as a result of the statistical significance test. A
of aggregation. Therefore the meta- statistically significant finding is a
analyst must carefully assess the strong conclusion, whereas a statisti-
influence of methodological variation cally nonsignificant (null) finding is a
on observed effects (Wilson and weak conclusion. In the vote-count
Lipsey, in press). review, null findings are typically
interpreted as evidence that the rela-
WHY META-ANALYSIS? tionship of interest does not exist (for
example, the intervention is not
Meta-analysis is not the only effective). This is an incorrect inter-
method of synthesizing or reviewing pretation. Failure to reject a null
results across studies. Other ap- hypothesis is not support for the
proaches include the narrative and null, merely suspended judgment.
vote-count review. The narrative Enough null findings in the same
review relies on a researchers ability direction are evidence that the null is
to digest the array of findings across false. This possibility was recognized
studies and arrive at a pronounce- by Fisher (1944), a strong proponent
ment regarding the evidence for or of significance testing.
against a hypothesis using some Third, the vote count ignores the
unknown and unknowable (that is, size of the observed effects. By focus-
subjective) mental calculus. ing on statistical significance, and
The vote-count method imposes not the size and direction of the
discipline on this process by tallying effect, a study with a small but statis-
the number of studies with statisti- tically significant effect would be
cally significant findings in favor of viewed as evidence favoring the hy-
the hypothesis and the number con- pothesis, and a study with a large
trary to the hypothesis (null find- nonsignificant effect would be
ings). This approach is appealing, for viewed as evidence against the
74 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

hypothesis. Both studies provide evi- As a method, meta-analysis


dence that the relationship is non- includes all of the essential features
zero, although the strength of that of a systematic review (see Petrosino
evidence is weak in one of the studies. et al. 2001), including an exhaustive
The benefits of a null hypothesis sta- search for all relevant studies (pub-
tistical significance test for inter- lished or not), explicit inclusion and
preting a finding from an individual exclusion criteria, and a coding pro-
study do not translate into benefits tocol for extracting data from the
when evaluating a collection of studies. The distinctive feature of
related studies. meta-analysis is the application of
Furthermore a counterintuitive statistical techniques to the analysis
feature of the vote-count method is of the study findings, where study
that the likelihood of arriving at an findings are encoded on a common
incorrect conclusion increases as the metric. The section below presents
number of s tudies on a t opi c an overview of the analytic methods
increases, if the typical statistical of meta-analysis. Several articles in
power of the studies in that area is this issue (MacKenzie, Wilson, and
low. This is a common situation in Kider 2001 [this issue]; Lipsey, Chap-
criminology. For example, Lipsey and man, and Landenberger 2001 [this
colleagues (1985) estimated that the issue]) provide examples of meta-
typical power of evaluations of juve- analytic methods. This article con-
nile delinquency interventions was cludes with a discussion of the
less than .50. A vote-count review of strengths and weaknesses of meta-
that literature is sure to yield mis- analysis and guidance on when not to
leading conclusions. use meta-analysis.
Meta-analysis avoids the pitfalls
of the vote-count method by focusing A FRAMEWORK FOR
on the size and direction of effects META-ANALYSIS
across studies, not whether the indi-
vidual effects were statistically sig- A defining feature of meta-analy-
nificant. The latter largely depends sis is the effect size, that is, any index
on the sample size of the study. Fur- of the effect of interest that is compa-
thermore focusing on the size and rable across studies. The effect size
direction of the effect makes better might index the effects of a treat-
use of the data available in the pri- ment group relative to a comparison
mary studies, providing a mecha- group or the relationship between
nism for analyzing differences across two observed variables, such as gen-
studies and drawing inferences der and mathematical achievement
about the likely size of the true popu- or attachment to parents and delin-
lation effect of interest. The statisti- quent behavior. In the analysis of
cal methods of meta-analysis allow meta-analytic data, the effect size is
for an assessment of both the consis- the dependent variable.
tency of findings across studies and The need for an effect size places
the relationship of study features restrictions on what research can be
with variability in effects. meta-analyzed. The collection of
META-ANALYTIC METHODS FOR CRIMINOLOGY 75

studies of interest to the reviewer has been argued that the correlation
must examine the same basic rela- coefficient is the ideal effect size
tionship, even if at a broad level of (Rosenthal 1991). However, the stan-
abstraction. At the broad end of the dardized mean difference and odds
continuum would be a group of stud- ratio effect sizes have distinct statis-
ies examining the effects of school- tical advantages over the correlation
based prevention programs on delin- coefficient for intervention research
quent behavior. At the narrow end of and are more natural indices of pro-
the continuum would be a set of repli- gram effects.
cations of a study on the effects of the
drug DepoProvea on the perpetra- Standardized
tion of sexual offenses. The research mean difference
designs of a collection of studies
would all need to be sufficiently simi- The standardized mean differ-
lar such that a comparable effect size ence, d, represents the effect of an
could be computed from each. Thus intervention as the difference
between the intervention and com-
most meta-analyses of intervention
parison group means on the depend-
studies will stipulate that eligible
ent variable of interest, standardized
studies use a comparison group
by the pooled within-groups stan-
design.
dard deviation. Thus findings based
The specific effect size index used on different operationalizations of
in a given meta-analysis will depend the dependent variable of interest
on the nature of the research being (for example, delinquency) are stan-
synthesized. Commonly used effect dardized to a common metric: stan-
size indices for intervention research dard deviation units for the popula-
are the standardized mean differ- tion. An advantage of d is that it can
ence, odds ratio, and correlation coef- be computed from a wide range of
ficient. The standardized mean dif- statistical data, including means and
ferencetype effect size is well suited standard deviations, t tests, F tests,
to two group comparison studies (for correlation coefficients, and 2 2 con-
example, a treatment versus a com- tingency tables (see Lipsey and Wil-
parison condition) with continuous son 2001). Although conceptualized
or dichotomous dependent measures. as the difference between two groups
The odds ratio is well suited to these on a continuous dependent variable,
same research domains with the d can also be computed from dichoto-
exception that the dependent mea- mous data.
sures must be dichotomous, such as
whether the participants recidivated
Odds ratio
within 12 months of leaving the pro-
gram. The correlation coefficient can The odds ratio, o, represents the
be applied to the broadest range of effect of an intervention as the odds
research designs, including all of a favorable (or unfavorable) out-
designs for which standardized mean come for the intervention group rela-
difference and odds ratio effect sizes tive to the comparison group. It is
can be computed. Because of this, it used when the outcome is measured
76 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

dichotomously, such as is common in cussion of other alternatives, see


medicine and criminology. The odds Lipsey and Wilson 2001).
ratio is easy to compute from either
the raw frequencies of a 2 2 contin- ANALYSIS OF
gency table or the proportions of suc- META-ANALYTIC DATA
cesses or failures in each condition.
As a ratio of two odds, a value of 1 A typical meta-analysis extracts
indicates an equal likelihood of a suc- one or more effect sizes per study and
cessful outcome, whereas values codes a variety of study characteris-
between 1 and 0 indicate a negative tics to represent the important sub-
effect and values greater than 1 indi- stantive and methodological differ-
cate a positive effect. Unlike the cor- ences across studies. Before analysis
relation coefficient, the odds ratio is of the data, statistical transforma-
unaffected by differential base rates tions and adjustments may need to
(the marginal distribution) for the be applied to the effect size. If multi-
outcome acros s s tudi es ( s ee ple effect sizes were extracted per
Farrington and Loeber 2000), thus study, then a method of including
eliminating a potential source of only a single effect size per study (or
effect variability across studies. sample within a study) per analysis
will need to be adopted. The analysis
Correlation coefficient of effect size data typically examines
the central tendency of the effect size
The correlation coefficient is a
distribution and the consistency of
widely used and widely understood
effects across studies. Additional
statistic within the social sciences. It
analyses test for the ability of study
can be used to represent the relation-
features to explain inconsistencies in
ship between two dichotomous vari-
effects across studies. Meta-analytic
ables, a dichotomous and a continu-
methods for performing these analy-
ous variable, and two continuous
ses are summarized below.
variables. The correlation coefficient
has a distinct disadvantage, however,
Transformations
when one or both of the variables on
and adjustments
which it is based are dichotomous
(Farrington and Loeber 2000). For There are standard adjustments
example, the correlation coefficient is and transformations that are rou-
restricted to less than +1 in absolute tinely applied to effect sizes, and
value if the percentage of partici- optional adjustments may be applied
pants in the intervention and com- depending on the purpose of the
parison conditions is not split fifty- meta-analysis. For example, Hedges
fifty. Thus it is recommended that it (1982; Hedges and Olkin 1985)
only be used for meta-analyses of showed that the standardized mean
correlational research and that difference effect size is positively
meta-analyses of intervention stud- biased when based on a small sam-
ies use either the standardized mean ple; that is, it is too large in absolute
difference, the odds ratio, or a more value, and the bias increases as sam-
specialized effect size (for a dis- ple size decreases. The size of bias is
META-ANALYTIC METHODS FOR CRIMINOLOGY 77

very modest for all but very small studies, such as reliability and valid-
sample sizes, but the adjustment is ity coefficients. The logic of these
easy to perform and routinely done adjustments is to estimate what
when using d as the effect size index would have been observed under
(for formulas, see the appendix). more ideal research conditions.
When using the odds ratio, one These adjustments, while common in
encounters a complication that is meta-analyses of measurement
also easily rectified. The odds ratio is generalizability studies, are rarely
asymmetric, with negative relation- used in meta-analyses of interven-
ships represented as values between tion research. If they are used, it is
0 and 1 and positive relationships recommended that a sensitivity
represented as values between 1 and analysis be performed to assess the
infinity. This complicates analysis. effect the adjustments have on the
Fortunately, the natural logarithm of results.
the odds ratio is symmetric about 0
with a well-defined standard error. Statistical independence
The importance of the latter is dis- among effect sizes
cussed below. Thus, for purposes of A complication with effect size
analysis, the odds ratio is trans- data is the often numerous effect
formed into the logged odds ratio. sizes of interest available from each
Results can be transformed back into study. Effect sizes that are based on
odds ratios for purposes of interpre- the same sample of individuals (or
tation using the antilogarithm. other units of analysis, such as city
Similarly the correlation coeffi- blocks and so forth) are statistically
cient has a distributional shape that dependent, that is, correlated with
is less than ideal for purposes of com- each other. Meta-analytic analysis
puting averages. Furthermore the assumes that each data point (effect
standard error is asymmetric, partic- size in this case) is statistically inde-
1
ularly as the correlation approaches pendent of all other data points.
1 or +1. This is easily solved by Thus we can include only one effect
applying Fishers Zr transformation, size per sample in any given analysis.
which normalizes the correlation and An independent set of effect sizes can
results in a standard error that is be obtained through several strate-
remarkably simple. As with the odds gies. First, each major outcome con-
ratio, final results can be trans- struct of interest can, and should, be
formed back into correlation coeffi- analyzed separately. For example,
cients for interpretative purposes. effect sizes representing employ-
Hunter and Schmidt (1990) pro- ment success should be analyzed sep-
posed adjusting effect sizes for mea- arately from those representing
surement unreliability and invalid- criminal behavior. Second, multiple
ity, range restriction, and artificial effect sizes within each outcome con-
dichotomization. These adjustments, struct can be averaged to produce one
however, depend on information that effect size per study or sample within
is rarely reported for outcome mea- a study. Alternatively, a meta-ana-
sures in crime and justice evaluation lyst may choose a single effect size
78 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

based on an explicit criterion. That is, the overall mean effect size, com-
the meta-analyst may prefer rearrest puted as a weighted mean, weighting
data over reinstitutionalization data by the inverse variance weight. A z
if the former are available. Finally, test can be performed to assess
the meta-analyst may randomly whether the mean effect size is sta-
select among those effect sizes that tistically greater than (or less than)
are of interest to a given analysis. 0, and a confidence interval can be
Note that several analyses can be constructed around the mean effect
performed, each with a different set size. Both statistics rely on the stan-
of independent effect sizes. dard error of the mean effect size,
computed from the sum of the
The inverse variance weight weights. Thus both the precision and
number of the individual effect sizes
An additional complication of influence the precision of the mean
meta-analytic data is the differential effect size. (For equations, see the
precision in effect sizes across stud- appendix.)
ies. Effect sizes based on large sam-
The mean effect size is meaningful
ples, all other things being equal, are
only if the effects are consistent
more precise than effect sizes based
across studies, that is, statistically
on small samples. A simple solution
homogeneous. If the effects are
to this problem would be to weight
highly heterogeneous, then a single
each effect size by its sample size.
overall mean effect size does not ade-
Hedges (1982) showed, however, that
qu at el y repres en t t h e ef f ect s
the optimal weight is based on the
observed by the collection of studies.
variance (squared standard error) of
In meta-analysis, consistency in
each effect size. This is intuitively
effects is assessed with the homoge-
appealing as well, for the standard
neity statistic Q. A statistically sig-
error is a statistical expression of the
n i f i can t Q i n di cat es t h at t h e
precision of parameter, such as an
observed variability in effect sizes
effect size. The smaller the standard
exceeds statistical expectations
error, the more precise is the effect
regarding the variability that would
size. Thus, in all meta-analytic anal-
be observed across pure replications,
yses, weights are computed from the
that is, if the collection of studies
inverse of the squared standard error
were indeed estimating a common
of the effect size. This is called the
population effect size. A statistically
inverse variance weight method.
nonsignificant Q suggests that the
Equations for the inverse variance
variability in effects across studies is
weight for each of the three effect size
no greater than expected due to sam-
indices discussed above are pre-
pling error.
sented in the appendix.
A heterogeneous distribution (a
The mean effect size significant Q) is often the desired
and related statistics outcome of a homogeneity analysis.
Heterogeneity justifies the explora-
A starting point for the analysis of tion of the relationship between study
effect size data is the computation of features and effects, an important
META-ANALYTIC METHODS FOR CRIMINOLOGY 79

aspect of meta-analysis. The analytic As with the overall distribution,


approaches available to the meta- the residual distribution of effects
analyst for examining between study within categories may be homoge-
effects are an analysis of mean effect neous or heterogeneous. This is
sizes by a categorical study feature, tested with the Q within statistic (see
analogous to a one-way ANOVA, and the appendix). A homogeneous Q
a meta-analytic regression analysis within indicates that the categorical
approach. Both approaches rely on variable explained the excess vari-
inverse variance weighting, and both ability detected by the overall homo-
can be implemented under the geneity test. In this case, the categor-
assumptions of a fixed- or random- ical variable provides an explanation
effects model. The assumptions of for the variability in effects across
these models will be discussed below. studies. Alternatively, additional
sources of variability in effects exist
Categorical analysis if the Q within is significant.
of effect sizes: The The computation of the analog to
analog to the ANOVA the ANOVA can be tedious. Macros
that work with existing statistical
The analog to the ANOVA-type
software packages exist for perform-
analysis is used to examine the rela-
ing this analysis (for example, Lipsey
tionship between a single categorical
and Wilson 2001; Wang and Bush-
variable, such as treatment type or
man 1998). BioStat (2000) has cre-
research method, and effect size.
ated a meta-analysis program that
There may be as few as two catego-
among other features performs the
ries, in which case the analysis is con-
analog to the ANOVA analysis.
ceptually similar to a t test, or many
categories. A separate mean effect
Meta-analytic
size and associated statistics, such as
regression analysis
a z test and confidence interval, are
computed for each category of the The analog to the ANOVA is lim-
variable of interest. To test whether ited to a single categorical variable. A
the mean effect sizes differ across more flexible and general analytic
categories, a Q between groups is cal- strategy for assessing the relation-
culated (see the appendix). Although ship between study features and
this statistic is distributed as a chi- effect size is regression analysis.
square, it is interpreted in the same Regression analysis can incorporate
fashion as an F from a one-way multiple independent variables
ANOVA. A significant Q between (study features) in a single analysis,
groups indicates that the variability including continuous variables and
in the mean effect sizes across cate- categorical variables (via dummy
gories is greater than expected due to coding). The differences between
sampling error. Thus the category is ordinary least squares regression
related to effect size. Examination of and meta-analytic regression are the
confidence intervals provides evi- weighting by the inverse variance
dence of the source of the important and a modification to the standard
difference(s). error of the regression coefficients,
80 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

necessitating the use of specialized Fixed and random


software (for example, Lipsey and effects models
Wilson 2001; Wang and Bushman
1998). As with the analog to the The statistical model presented
ANOVA, two Q values are calculated above assumes that the collection of
as part of meta-analytic regression: a effect sizes being analyzed is esti-
Q for the model and a Q for the resid- mating a common population effect
ual or error variance. The former is a size. In statistical terms, this is a
test of the predictive ability of the fixed-effects model. Stated differ-
study features in explaining between- ently, a fixed-effects model assumes
studies variability in effects. The that each effect size differs from the
regression model accounts for signifi- true population effect size solely due
cant variability in the effect size dis- to subject-level sampling error. Each
tribution if the Q for the model is sig- observed effect size is viewed as an
nificant. As with the Q within for the imperfect estimate of the true, single
analog to the ANOVA, a significant Q population effect for the intervention
for the error variance indicates that of interest. This provides the theoret-
excess variability remains in the ical basis for incorporating the stan-
effects across studies after account- dard error of the effect size (an esti-
ing for the variability explained by mate of subject-level sampling error)
the regression model. That is, the into the analysis as the inverse vari-
residual distribution in effect sizes is ance weight.
heterogeneous. This assumption is restrictive and
Recognizing the correlational likely to be untenable in many syn-
nature of the above analyses of the theses of criminological intervention
relationship between study features research where studies of a common
and effect size is critical. Study fea- research hypothesis differ on many
tures are often correlated with one dimensions, some of which are likely
another and, as such, a moderating to be related to effect size. Thus each
relationship may be the result of con- effect size has variability (that is,
founded between-studies features. instability) due to subject-level sam-
For example, the mean effect size for pling error and study-level variabil-
treatment type A may be higher than ity. The random-effects model
the mean effect size for treatment assumes that at least some portion of
type B. The studies examining treat- the study-level variability is unex-
ment type B, however, may have used plained by the study features
a less sensitive measure of the out- included in the statistical models of
come construct, thus confounding effect size. These study differences
treatment type with characteristics may simply be unmeasured, or they
of the dependent variable. Multi- may be unmeasurable. In both cases,
variate analyses can help assess the each effect size is assumed to esti-
interrelationships between study mate a true population effect size for
features, but these analyses cannot that study, and the collection of true
account for unmeasured study population effect sizes represents a
characteristics. random distribution of effects. In
META-ANALYTIC METHODS FOR CRIMINOLOGY 81

statistical terms, this is a random- effect size per study for any given
effects model. analysis may also affect the meta-
Methods for estimating random- analytic findings. For example, in the
effects models in meta-analysis are boot camp systematic review by Mac-
well developed. The basic method Kenzie, Wilson, and Kider (2001), the
involves modifying the definition of analyses were performed on a single
the inverse variance weight such effect size selected from each study
that it incorporates both the subject- based on a set of decision rules. A sen-
and study-level estimates of instabil- sitivity analysis showed that using a
ity. The inverse variance weight is composite of all recidivism effect
thus based on both the standard sizes produced the same results, bol-
error of the effect size and an esti- stering the authors confidence in the
mate of the variability in the distri- findings. Third, if the meta-analysis
bution of population effects. The lat- has included methodologically weak
ter is computed from the observed studies, analyses examining the rela-
distribution of effects. Random- tionship between method features
effects models are more conservative and observed effects are essential.
than fixed-effects models. Confi-
dence intervals will be larger, and Illustration: Cognitive-
regression coefficients that were sta- behavioral programs
tistically significant under a fixed- for sex offenders
effects model may no longer be signif-
To illustrate the methods outlined
icant under a random-effects model.
above, I have selected a subset of
It is recommended that meta-analy-
studies included in a meta-analysis
ses of criminological literatures use a
of sex offender programs (Gallagher,
random-effects model of analysis
Wilson, and MacKenzie no date).
unless a clear justification to do oth-
Presented below are the programs
erwise exists.
based on cognitive-behavioral princi-
ples. Studies were included if they
Sensitivity analysis
used a comparison group design and
A final analytic issue is the sensi- the comparison received either no
tivity of the results to unusual study treatment or non-sex-offender-spe-
effects and decisions made by the cific treatment. Studies also had to
meta-analyst. First, it is wise to report a measure of sex offense recid-
examine the influence of outliers in ivism at some point following termi-
the distribution of effect sizes and nation of the program.
the distribution of inverse variance A total of 13 studies met the eligi-
weights. A modest effect size outlier bility criteria for this meta-analysis.
with a large weight can drive an The recidivism data were dichoto-
analysis. Rerunning an important mous and as such, the odds ratio was
analysis with and without highly selected as the effect size index. The
influential studies can help verify odds ratio and 95 percent confidence
that the observed result is not solely interval for these 13 studies are pre-
a function of a single unusual study. sented in Figure 1. Visual inspection
Second, the method of selecting one of these odds ratios shows a distinct
82 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

FIGURE 1
ODDS RATIO AND 95 PERCENT CONFIDENCE INTERVAL FOR EACH OF
THE 13 COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL SEX OFFENDER EVALUATION STUDIES

Author(s) N Favors Comparison Favors Intervention


Borduin, Henggeler, Blaske & Stein (N = 16)
McGrath, Hoke & Vojtisek (N = 103)
Hildebran & Pithers (N = 90)
Marhsall, Eccles & Barbaree (N = 38)
Studer, Reddon, Roper & Estrada (N = 220)
Nicholaichuk, Gordon, Andre & Gu (N = 579)
Gordon & Nicholaichuk (N = 206)
Guarino & Kimball (N = 75)
Marques, Day, Nelson, & West (N = 229)
Huot (N = 224)
Gordon & Nicholaichuk (N = 1248)
Song & Lieb (N = 278)
Nicholaichuk (N = 65)
Overall Mean Odds-Ratio

.02 .1 .50 1 5 25 200


Odds-Ratio

NOTE: Sources of programs are available from the author.

positive trend, with 12 of the 13 stud- related to study features, Q = 21.99,


ies observing lower recidivism rates df = 12, p < .05.
(and hence odds ratios greater than This collection of studies differed
1) for the sex offender treatment con- in many ways, both in the research
dition than the comparison condi- methods used and the specifics of the
tion. The sole study with a negative sex offender treatment program.
effect (an odds ratio between 0 and 1) Many of these 13 studies evaluated a
had a large confidence interval that cognitive-behavioral approach called
extended well into the positive range relapse prevention. Relapse preven-
and was from a study of poor method- tion programs may be more (or less)
ological quality. effective than other cognitive-behav-
ioral programs. To explore this, the
The weighted mean odds ratio for
mean effect size for relapse preven-
this collection of 13 studies was 2.33, tion and other cognitive-behavioral
and the 95 percent confidence inter- programs was calculated (2.41 and
val was 1.57 to 3.42. The z test indi- 1.73, respectively). Also calculated
cates that this odds ratio was statis- were the Q between and Q within.
tically significant at conventional The Q between was 0.87, p > .05, indi-
levels, z = 4.26, p < .001. This collec- cating that the observed difference
tion of studies supports the conclu- between these two means was not
sion that cognitive-behavioral pro- statistically significant. The Q
grams for sex offenders reduce the within was statistically significant,
risk of a sexual reoffense. The homo- QWITHIN = 21.12, df = 11, p = .03, indi-
geneity statistic was significant, cating that significant variability
indicating that the findings are not acros s g rou ps remai n ed af t er
consistent across studies and may be accounting for treatment type.
META-ANALYTIC METHODS FOR CRIMINOLOGY 83

A regression analysis was per- from a practical or clinical perspec-


formed to test whether the differen- tive. That is, is the effect significant
tial lengths of follow-up across stud- in the everyday meaning of that
ies and the different definitions of word? Meta-analysts are confronted
recidivism could account for the het- with the same problem. What is the
erogeneity. The regression coefficient practical significance of an observed
for whether the recidivism was mea- mean effect size? A common ap-
sured at least five years posttreat- proach to addressing this problem is
ment was statistically significant the translation of the effect size into
and positive, B = 1.58, p = .01, sug- a success rate differential for the
gesting that studies with longer fol- intervention and comparison condi-
low-up periods observed larger dif- tions, such as using the binomial
ferences in the rates of sexual effect size display (Rosenthal and
offending between the treated and Rubin 1983). For example, a stan-
nontreated groups. The effects of sex dardized mean difference effect size
offender programs may increase over of .40 is equivalent to a success rate
time, or the length of follow-up was differential of 20 percent (that is, 40
related to an unmeasured program percent recidivism in the interven-
characteristic that led to greater tion condition and 60 percent recidi-
effectiveness. The regression coeffi- vism in the comparison condition). If
cient for whether the recidivism mea- the audience for the meta-analysis is
sure was an indicator of arrest or not familiar with standardized mean
reconviction was also statistically difference effect sizes, then the suc-
significant, B = 1.25, p = .04, suggest- cess rate differential provides a use-
ing that arrest may be a more sensi- ful method of understanding the
tive measure of the program effects. practical significance of the observed
Significant variability in the effect findings.
size distribution was accounted for The odds ratio has a natural inter-
by this regression model, QMODEL = pretation without transformation:
7.05, df = 3, p = .03. Furthermore the the odds ratio is the odds of a success-
Q associated with the residual vari- ful outcome in the treated condition
ability in effect sizes was not statisti- relative to the comparison condition.
cally significant, QRESIDUAL = 14.9, df = Thinking about odds is, however, odd
10, p = .13, indicating that the resid-
for all but the more mathematically
ual variability in effects is not
inclined. As with the standardized
greater than would be expected due
mean difference, a mean odds ratio
to sampling error.
can be translated into percentages of
successes (or failures). This transla-
INTERPRETATION OF tion requires fixing the failure rate
META-ANALYTIC FINDINGS for one of the conditions. For exam-
ple, if we assume a 50 percent recidi-
A researcher who finds a statisti- vism rate for the comparison condi-
cally significant effect is presented tion, then an odds ratio of 1.5
with the difficult task of deciding translates into a recidivism rate of 40
whether the effect is meaningful percent in the treatment condition.
84 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Presenting the results of a meta- applied to a small number of similar


analysis of odds ratios as percent- studies.
ages provides a means of assessing As a practitioner of meta-analysis,
the magnitude of the observed pro- I see few justified disadvantages to
gram effects. the use of meta-analysis. This does
not mean that meta-analysis does
not have its disadvantages. On the
ADVANTAGES AND DISADVANTAGES
OF META-ANALYSIS practical side, meta-analysis is far
more time-consuming than tradi-
Meta-analysis has several distinct tional forms of review and requires a
advantages over alternative forms of moderate level of statistical sophisti-
reviewing empirical research. As a cation. Meta-analysis also simplifies
systematic method of review, meta- the findings of the individual studies,
analysis is replicable by independent often representing each study as a
researchers. The methods are single effect size and a small set of
explicit and open to the scrutiny of descriptor variables. Complex pat-
other scholars, who may question the terns of effects often found in individ-
inclusion and exclusion criteria and ual studies do not lend themselves to
critique the variables used to exam- synthesis, such as the results from
ine between-studies differences. This individual growth-curve modeling.
can lead to productive debates and To accommodate this, a reviewer may
competing analyses of the meta-ana- wish to augment a meta-analytic
lytic data. In addition, meta-analysis review with narrative descriptions of
makes efficient use of the informa- important studies and interesting
tion contained in the primary stud- study-level findings obscured in the
ies. Focusing on the direction and meta-analytic synthesis. Finally, the
magnitude of the findings across methods of meta-analysis cannot
studies using a common statistical overcome weaknesses in the primary
benchmark allows for the explora- studies. If the research base that
tion of relationships between study examines the hypothesis of interest
features of effects that would not oth- is methodologically weak, then the
erwise be observable. The statistical findings from the meta-analysis will
methods of meta-analysis help guard also be weak. In these situations,
against interpreting the dispersion meta-analysis creates a solid founda-
in results as meaningful when it can tion for the next generation of studies
just as easily be explained as sam- by clearly identifying the weak-
pling error. Finally, meta-analysis nesses of the current knowledge base
can handle a much larger number of on a given issue.
studies than could effectively be
summarized with alternative meth- WHEN NOT TO DO META-ANALYSIS
ods. There is no theoretical limit to
the number of studies that can be Meta-analysis is the preferred
incorporated into a single meta-anal- method of systematically reviewing a
ysis, yet as a method it can also be collection of empirical studies
META-ANALYTIC METHODS FOR CRIMINOLOGY 85

examining a common research analyzed. Finally, meta-analysis


hypothesis. However, meta-analysis does not address broad theoretical
is not appropriate for the synthesis of issues that may be important to a
all empirical research literatures. debate regarding the value of various
First, meta-analysis cannot be used crime prevention efforts. Meta-anal-
when a common effect size index can- ysis is designed to synthesize the evi-
not be computed across the studies of dence regarding the strength of a
interest. For example, the appropri- relationship across distinct research
ate effect size for area studies (that studies. This is a very specific task
is, studies that have a geographic that may be imbedded in a larger
area as the unit of analysis) is cur- scholarly endeavor.
rently being discussed among mem-
bers of the Campbell Collaboration.
Second, the research designs across a CONCLUSIONS
collection of studies examining the
relationship of interest may be too Systematic reviews approach the
disparate for meaningful synthesis. task of summarizing findings of a col-
For example, studies with different lection of research studies as a
units of analysis cannot be readily research task. As a method of sys-
meta-analyzed unless sufficient data tematic reviewing, meta-analysis
are presented to compute an effect takes this a step further by quantify-
size at a common level of analysis. ing the direction and magnitude of
Studies with fundamentally differ- the findings of interest across studies
ent research designs, such as one- and uses specialized statistical
group longitudinal studies and com- methods to analyze the relationship
parison group studies also should not between findings and study features.
be combined in the same meta-analy- Properly executed, meta-analysis
sis. Third, the research question provides a firm foundation for future
for a meta-analysis may involve research. That is, empirical relation-
a multivariate relatio n s h i p. ships that are well established and
Although methods have been devel- areas that are underresearched or
oped for meta-analyzing multi- that have equivocal findings are
variate research studies (for exam- identified through the meta-analytic
ple, Becker 1992; Becker 1996; process. In addition, meta-analysis
Premack and Hunter 1988), these provides a defensible strategy for
methods have rarely been applied summarizing crime prevention and
and are still not well developed. It is intervention efforts for informing
unlikely that the more elaborate public policy. Although the methods
research designs will ever easily lend are technical, the findings can be
themselves to synthesis. Thus some translated into summary statistics
research questions addressed by pri- readily understandable by non
mary studies are not easily meta- social science researchers.
86 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

APPENDIX
EQUATIONS FOR THE CALCULATION OF EFFECT
SIZES AND META-ANALYTIC SUMMARY STATISTICS

No. Equation Notes

Common effect size indices


X1 X2
(1) d = Standardized mean difference effect size; X1 is the
s pooled mean of the intervention condition; X2 is the mean of
the comparison condition; and spooled is the pooled
within-groups standard deviation
ad
(2) o = Odds ratio effect size; a and c are the number of
bc successful outcomes in the intervention and
comparison conditions, and b and d are the number
of failures in the intervention and comparison
conditions (based on a 2 2 contingency table)
(3) r = r Correlation coefficient effect size; r is the Pearson
product-moment correlation coefficient between the
two variables of interest

Common transformations of effect size


3
(4) d = 1 d Small sample size bias correction; d is the standardized
4N 9
mean difference effect size and N is the total sample
size
(5) lor = log(o) Log transformation of the odds ratio
1+ r
(6) z =.5 log Fishers transformation of the correlation effect size
1 r
lor
(7) o = e Logged odds ratio (lor) transformed into an odds ratio
(o); e is the constant 2.7183
e 1 2z
(8) r = Transforms the effect size z from equation 6 back into a
e 2z + 1 correlation; e is the constant 2.7183

Fixed effects model inverse variance weights


n + n2 d2
(9) v d = 1 + The variance for the standardized mean difference; n1
n1n2 2(n1 + n2 ) and n2 are the sample sizes for the intervention and
comparison conditions
1 1 1 1
(10) v lor = + + + The variance for the logged odds ratio; a, b, c, and d
a b c d are the cell frequencies of a 2 2 contingency table
1
(11) v z = The variance for the Fishers transformed correlation
N 3 coefficient; N is the total sample size
1
(12) w = The inverse variance weight; v is the inverse variance
v from equation 9, 10, or 11

Mean effect size and related statistics

(13) ES = (ES w ) Weighted mean effect size, where ES is the effect size
w index (equations 4, 5, or 6) and w is the inverse
variance weight (equation 12)
META-ANALYTIC METHODS FOR CRIMINOLOGY 87

APPENDIX Continued

No. Equation Notes

1
(14) se ES = The standard error of the mean effect size
w
ES
(15) z = A z test; tests whether ES is statistically greater than or
se ES less than 0
(16) LowerCI = ES 1.96se ES Lower bound of the 95 percent confidence interval

(17) UpperCI = ES + 1.96se ES Upper bound of the 95 percent confidence interval

Homogeneity test Q
( (ES )
2
w)
(18) Q = (ES 2
w) Homogeneity test Q; distributed as a chi-square,
w degrees of freedom equals the number of effect
sizes less 1

Random effects variance component and weight


Q (k 1)
(19) V = The random effects variance component; the random
w w2 effects variance component has a more complex form
w when used as part of the analog to the ANOVA or
regression models
1
(20) w = The random effects inverse variance weight, where v is
v + v defined as in equations 9 through 11

Analog to the ANOVA

( (ES )
2
wj
(ES
j
(21) Q j = 2
w ) Q between groups; where j is 1 to the number of
w
j j
j categories for the independent variable; distributed
as a chi-square with j 1 degrees of freedom
(22) QW = Q QB Q within groups; where Q is the overall homogeneity
statistics defined in equation 18 and QB is defined in
equation 21; distributed as a chi-square with the
number of effect sizes minus the number of categories
in the independent variable as the degrees of freedom

Meta-analytic regression analysis

(23) Use specialized software For example, SAS, SPSS, or Stata macros by Lipsey
and Wilson (2001); SAS macros by Wang and
Bushman (1998)
88 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Note Larry V. Hedges. New York: Russell


Sage.
1. Methods have been developed for han- Hedges, Larry V. 1982. Estimating Effect
dling dependent effect sizes in a single analy-
Size from a Series of Independent Ex-
sis, but these methods are beyond the scope of
this article. (For details, see Gleser and Olkin
periments. Psychological Bulletin 92:
1994; Kalaian and Raudenbush 1996.) 490-99.
Hedges, Larry V. and Ingram Olkin. 1985.
Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis.
References Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Hunter, John E. and Frank L. Schmidt.
Becker, Betsy J. 1992. Models of Science 1990. Methods of Meta-Analysis: Cor-
Achievement: Forces Affecting Perfor- recting Error and Bias in Research
mance in School Science. In Meta- Findings. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
analysis for Explanation: A Casebook, Kalaian, H. A. and Stephen W. Rauden-
ed. Thomas D. Cook, Harris Cooper, bush. 1996. A Multivariate Mixed Lin-
David S. Cordray, Heidi Hartmann, ear Model For Meta-Analysis. Psycho-
Larry V. Hedges, Richard J. Light, logical Methods 1:227-35.
Thomas A. Louis, and Frederick Lipsey, Mark W., Gabrielle L. Chapman,
Mosteller. New York: Russell Sage. and Nana A. Landenberger. 2001. Cog-
Becker, G. 1996. The Meta-Aanalysis of nitive-Behavioral Programs for Of-
Factor Analyses: An Illustration fenders. Annals of the American Acad-
Based on the Cumulation of Correla- emy of Political and Social Science
tion Matrices. Psychological Methods 578:144-157.
1:341-53. Lipsey, Mark W., Scott Crosse, J. Dunkle,
BioStat. 2000. Comprehensive Meta- J. Pollard, and G. Stobart. 1985. Evalu-
Analysis (Software Program, Version ation: The State of the Art and the
1.0.9). Englewood, NJ: BioStat. Avail- Sorry State of the Science. New Direc-
able: www.metaanalysis.com. tions for Program Evaluation 27:7-28.
Farrington, David P. and Rolf Loeber. Lipsey, Mark W. and David B. Wilson.
2000. Some Benefits of Dichot- 2001. Practical Meta-Analysis. Thou-
omization in Psychiatric and Crimino- sand Oaks, CA: Sage.
logical Research. Criminal Behaviour MacKenzie, Doris Layton, David B. Wil-
and Mental Health 10:100-122. son, and Suzanne B. Kider. 2001. Ef-
Fisher, Ronald A. 1944. Statistical fects of Correctional Boot Camps on
Methods for Research Workers. 9th ed. Offending. Annals of the American
London: Oliver and Boyd. Academy of Political and Social Sci-
Gallagher, Catherine A., David B. Wilson, ence 578:126-143.
and Doris Layton MacKenzie. N.d. A Pearson, Karl. 1904. Report on Certain
Meta-Analysis of the Effectiveness of Enteric Fever Inoculation Statistics.
Sexual Offender Treatment Pro- British Medical Journal 3:1243-46.
grams. Unpublished manuscript, Uni- Quoted in Morton Hunt, How Science
versity of Maryland at College Park. Takes Stock: The Story of Meta-
Glass, Gene V. 1976. Primary, Secondary Analysis (New York: Russell Sage,
and Meta-Analysis of Research. Edu- 1997).
cational Researcher 5:3-8. Petrosino, Anthony, Robert F. Boruch,
Gleser, Leon J. and Ingram Olkin. 1994. Haluk Soydan, Lorna Duggan, and
Stochastically Dependent Effect Julio Sanchez-Meca. 2001. Meeting
Sizes. In The Handbook of Research the Challenges of Evidence-Based
Synthesis, ed. Harris Cooper and Policy: The Campbell Collaboration.
META-ANALYTIC METHODS FOR CRIMINOLOGY 89

Annals of the American Academy of fect. Journal of Educational Psychol-


Political and Social Science 578:14-34. ogy 74:166-69.
Premack, Steven L. and John E. Hunter. Wang, Morgan C. and Brad J. Bushman.
1988. Individual Unionization De- 1998. Integrating Results Through
cisions. Psychological Bulletin 103: Meta-Analytic Review Using SAS
223-34. Software. Cary, NC: SAS Institute.
Rosenthal, Robert. 1991. Meta-Analytic Wilson, David B. and Mark W. Lipsey. In
Procedures for Social Research. Ap- press. The Role of Method in Treat-
plied Social Research Methods Series. ment Effect Estimates: Evidence from
Vol. 6. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Psychological, Behavioral, and Educa-
Rosenthal, Robert and Donald B. Rubin. tional Treatment Intervention Meta-
1983. A Simple, General Purpose Dis- Analyses. Psychological Methods.
play of Magnitude of Experimental Ef-
ANNALS, AAPSS, 578, November 2001

THE ANNALS
EARLY PARENT
OFTRAINING
THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Early Parent Training to Prevent


Disruptive Behavior Problems
and Delinquency in Children

By ODETTE BERNAZZANI, CATHERINE CT,


and RICHARD E. TREMBLAY

ABSTRACT: Early forms of disruptive behaviors in children often


leading to juvenile delinquency are associated with poor parenting
skills. Thus early intervention programs targeting parenting skills
may have an important impact on disruptive behaviors in children.
The objective of this review was to assess the impact of early
parenting and home visitation programs on behavior problems and
delinquency in children. Selected trials were identified using elec-
tronic databases and relevant reviews. The following selection crite-
ria were used: (1) the intervention involved the provision of parent
training to families with a child under age 3, and (2) the design was a
randomized or quasi-experimental trial. Overall, of the seven trials
identified, only three reported some beneficial effects on disruptive
behavior or delinquency. Due to the limited number of adequately de-
signed studies, caution is recommended in the interpretation of avail-
able results. Numerous well-designed early prevention experiments
specifically targeting disruptive behaviors and delinquency should
be initiated.

Odette Bernazzani, M.D., Ph.D., is an associate professor in the Department of Psy-


chiatry, University of Montreal. Her funded research concerns the intergenerational
transmission of emotional problems related to childhood neglect and abuse.
Catherine Ct is a Ph.D. student in the Department of Psychology, University of
Montreal. She has been awarded a research scholarship from the St-Justine Hospital
Research Center and the Fonds pour la Formation de Chercheurs et lAide la Recher-
che (FCAR) agency.
Richard E. Tremblay, Ph.D., FRSC, is a professor in the Departments of Psychiatry
and Psychology, University of Montreal. He has recently received the Canada Research
Chair in Child Development. He has published more than 200 papers in the field of
child development.

NOTE: We thank the following agencies for financial support: Canadian Institute for Ad-
vanced Research, FCAR, Fonds de la Recherche en Sant de Qubec, Molson Foundation, Social
Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, and St-Justine Hospital Research Centre.

90
EARLY PARENT TRAINING 91

D ISRUPTIVE behavior can be de-


fined as an array of behavior
problems including opposition to
and even the following generation
(White et al. 1990; Farrington 1995;
Fergusson and Horwood 1998;
adults, hyperactivity, stealing, lying, Serbin et al. 1998; Frick and Loney
truancy, extreme noncompliance, ag- 1999; Loeber 2001; Ct et al. in
gression, physical cruelty to people press). Prevention appears a worthy
and animals, and destructive and goal as treatment programs have
sexually coercive behaviors (Ameri- shown a modest impact (Chamber-
can Psychiatric Association 1994; lain 1999; Kavale, Forness, and
Quay and Hogan 1999a). Opposi- Walker 1999). The developmental
tional Defiant Disorder, Conduct Dis- trajectories of disruptive behaviors
order, and Attention Deficit Hyperac- are a major reason to argue for very
tivity Disorder are the diagnostic early prevention. There is good evi-
categories most often used in the psy- dence that chronic disruptive behav-
chiatric field to refer to children pre- ior leading to serious delinquency
senting severe disruptive behavior appears during early childhood
patterns. Although epidemiological (Moffitt et al. 1996; Broidy, Nagin,
studies in this area face important and Tremblay 1999; Nagin and
measurement problems and are lim- Tremblay 1999). There is also evi-
ited by sample size (Lahey et al. dence to suggest that children with
1999), it has been suggested that the di s ru pt i v e beh av i or probl ems
three forms of disruptive behaviors become increasingly resistant to
account for up to two-thirds of all change with age despite treatment
childhood and adolescent psychiatric efforts (Kazdin 1985; Frick and
disorders (Quay and Hogan 1999a). Loney 1999; Tremblay 2000). All
Most children manifest disruptive these considerations underscore the
behaviors during early childhood and need for early preventive programs
show a gradual decline in frequency targeting high-risk families.
w ith age (Broidy, Nagin , an d D u ri n g t h e pas t 40 y ears,
Tremblay 1999; Lahey et al. 1999; parenting programs have been
Nagin and Tremblay 1999; Tremblay offered in a variety of settings and to
2000; McCord, Widom, and Crowell a variety of families. Many of these
2001). The term delinquent behav- programs have targeted families
ior refers to disruptive behaviors with school-age disruptive children
sanctioned by the law. Age of the (Patterson 1982; Webster-Stratton,
child who performs a disruptive be- Kolpacoff, and Hollinsworth 1988;
havior is generally a key factor in de- Kazdin, Siegel, and Bass 1992;
ciding whether the behavior is, or is Tremblay et al. 1995; Hawkins et al.
not, sanctioned by the law (McCord, 1999). Parenting interventions as
Widom, and Crowell 2001). early as pregnancy have recently
Longitudinal studies have shown been stimulated by the evidence of
that there are long-term conse- reduced delinquent behavior in ado-
quences of disruptive behavior disor- lescents of poorly educated mothers
ders for the individual as well as his who received a home visitation pro-
or her family, friends, community, gram during pregnancy and the first
92 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

two years following birth (Olds et al. effects from other effects including
1998). These home visitation pro- developmental effects.
grams aim at a wide range of out-
comes including maternal physical Types of participants
and psychosocial health, parenting
The review was limited to families
skills, and childrens psychosocial
with a child under age 3 at the start
development and physical health.
of the intervention to ensure that the
The long-term impact on delin-
interventions were provided early in
quency of intensive home visitation
the childs life. However, no limits
during a period of more than two
were set concerning the childs age at
years supports the hypothesis that
the end of the intervention. In addi-
quality of family environment during
tion, selected interventions could tar-
the early years is a key to delin-
get either the general population
quency prevention (Patterson, Reid,
(universal intervention) or a high-
and Dishion 1992; Yoshikawa 1994;
risk group (selective intervention).
McCord, Widom, and Crowell 2001;
Nagin and Tremblay 2001). Early
Types of intervention
parenting interventions generally
postulate that quality of parent-child Studies were eligible for this
relations will facilitate learning of review when parent training or sup-
control over impulsive, oppositional, port was a major component of the
and aggressive behavior, thus intervention, although not necessar-
reducing disruptive behavior and its ily the only one.
long-term negative impact on social
integration. Types of outcomes
This review aims to address The original aim of the review was
whether early parenting and home to assess the impact of the interven-
visitation programs are effective in tions on the childrens delinquent
preventing behavior problems and behavior. However, since we found
delinquency in children. only one study assessing delin-
quency, we used a broader scope in
our review and selected studies with
CRITERIA FOR CONSIDERING
outcome measures of disruptive
STUDIES FOR THIS REVIEW
behaviors. These assessments
included self-reported delinquency;
Types of studies self-, parent-, or teacher-rated mea-
sures of disruptive behavior; and
Only studies employing random observer-rated assessments of dis-
assignment or quasi-experimental ruptive behavior in the classroom.
(preintervention and postinter-
vention assessments and adequate SEARCH STRATEGY FOR
control groups ) des ign s w ere IDENTIFICATION OF STUDIES
included. Studies lacking control
groups were excluded as they cannot Our starting point for searching
help differentiate intervention through the literature was two
EARLY PARENT TRAINING 93

previous reviews. The first (Mrazek METHOD OF THE REVIEW


and Brown 1999) reviewed psycho-
social interventions during the pre- Data collection, quality
school years that were designed to assessment, and data analysis
enhance child development accord- Titles and abstracts of studies
ing to a wide variety of outcomes. The identified through our searches were
s econd review (Tre mbl ay, reviewed to determine whether they
LeMarquand, and Vitaro 1999) met inclusion criteria. Trials were
focused on programs targeting fami- selected for methodological quality
lies of preadolescents for the preven- using the criteria suggested by
tion of disruptive behavior. Mrazek and Brown (1999). These
To identify additional relevant tri- authors have extensively reviewed
als potentially overlooked by, or pub- outcomes in psychosocial prevention
lished since, these reviews, two major and early intervention in young chil-
electronic databases were searched: dren. They have developed an instru-
PsychINFO and MEDLINE (1967 to ment called the Threats to Trial
2001). These specific databases were Integrity Score (TTIS) that allows
the most relevant considering the one to measure the quality of the
topic of our review. A wide search design of a controlled trial, whether
strategy was used to ensure that rel- it be randomized or not. This scale
evant studies were not missed. assesses the potential threat regard-
Hence the search terms excluded ing 10 dimensions of quality design
study design and reflected a wide age on a 4-point scale including null or
group. Our selection criteria regard- minimal risk (N or 0), low risk (L or
1), moderate risk (M or 2), and high
ing study design and age of partici-
risk (H or 3). Scores for each of the 10
pants were applied later during the
dimensions are combined in a
systematic review of the abstracts
weighted fashion to obtain a global
yielded by the search. The search
score (for additional information, see
terms used were parent training
Mrazek and Brown 1999). The
and childhood or pre-school and
authors then categorized this ordinal
delinquency or conduct disorder scale into a five-level Trial Quality
or antisocial behavior or aggres- Grade. Each trial was classified as a
sion or physical aggression or 1- to 5-star design. The 5-star designs
behavior problems. Finally, two were the highest-scoring trials based
other major sources of information on TTIS score (about 5 percent). The
were searched, the Cochrane Library 4-star designs were among the top
and the Future of Children publica- quarter of trials; the 3-star designs
tions, as well as all the potentially were in the second quartile, and so
relevant review articles identified forth. Mrazek and Brown suggested
during the search (Gomby et al. 1993; concentrating on trials with 5-star
Yoshikawa 1995; Vitaro, De Civita, and 4-star designs as they are clearly
and Pagani 1996; Culross 1999; well-designed studies with sufficient
Gomby, Culross, and Behrman 1999; design integrity. Mrazek and Brown
Barlow and Coren 2001). identified 165 prevention studies
94 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

with preschool children, but only 34 interventions (that is, they targeted
met the 4- or 5-star classification. Of high-risk groups, mostly socially dis-
the 34 studies, a total of 6 trials met advantaged families or, in one study,
our inclusion criteria. Three addi- premature babies). Boys and girls
tional trials were identified in were included in all studies. Two
Tremblay, LeMarquand, and Vitaro studies targeted minority groups:
(1999), but they were not kept in our African Americans and Mexican
review as they did not meet the 4- Americans. The latter study was the
star design criteria of Mrazek and only one that did not attempt to
Brown. obtain a representative population
The PsychINFO search yielded sample due to major recruitment
151 new abstracts, none of which challenges. While it can be argued
were included in the review. Most of that nearly all studies tried to
them were excluded because they involve families, in practice, most
targeted older children. Others were studies intervened mainly with
excluded for methodological reasons, mothers.
mostly because of the absence of a In total, 7917 families were ran-
control group. Search i n g t h e domized to receive parent training or
Cochrane Library and the Future of to be in a control group. One study
Children publications generated an had more than 4000 participants
additional four reviews that provided involving 21 sites, two had more than
information about one trial that had 1000 participants, three had more
not already been identified and met than 300, and one had 125. Attrition
our criteria. rates varied greatly from one study
Thus seven trials met our criteria. to another, ranging from 20 to 67 per-
The data were summarized using cent. Sample numbers relevant to
effect sizes but were not combined in our review varied from 117 to more
a meta-analysis due to the small than 2000 (exact number was not
number of studies and the presence available for St-Pierre and Layzer
of substantial heterogeneity among 1999).
them.
Intervention characteristics
RESULTS Four interventions began when
the child was 12 months old or youn-
Sample characteristics ger (see Table 2). All four continued
beyond age 2, up to age 3, 5, or 6. Two
All seven trials were randomized trials began during the prenatal
controlled experiments (see Table 1). period, and both continued up to 2
All but two were conducted in the years. Finally, one trial began when
United States; one was done in Aus- children were 24 months old and
tralia and another in Bermuda. Two ended when they were about 4 years.
interventions targeted the general Overall duration of interventions
population (universal preventive ranged from more than 2 to 6 years.
interventions) while the remaining Length of follow-up ranged from
five were selective preventive immediate end of intervention to 13
EARLY PARENT TRAINING 95

TABLE 1
SAMPLE CHARACTERISTICS OF STUDIES INCLUDED IN THE REVIEW
a
Study Target Population Country Final N

Cullen (1976) Universal Australia 246


Johnson and Low-income Mexican American families United States 139
Breckenridge (1982) Selective
Johnson and Walker
(1987)
Kitzman et al. (1997) Pregnant women with at least two of the United States 743
following characteristics: unmarried,
less than 12 years of education,
unemployed
Most participants were African American
Selective
McCarton et al. (1997) Low-birth-weight premature infants United States 874
Selective
Olds et al. (1986, 1998) Women who were young (younger than United States 323
19 years), unmarried, or of low
socioeconomic status
Selective
Scarr and McCartney All families with a 2-year-old child in a Bermuda 117
(1988) Bermudian parish
Universal
St-Pierre and Layzer Families with incomes below the United States More than
(1999) poverty level 2,000
Selective (exact
number not
available)
NOTES: Universal = a universal preventive intervention: an intervention that targets the general
population. Selective = a selective preventive intervention: an intervention that targets high-risk
groups.
a. Sample number related to outcomes examined in this review.

years following the end. The longest surveillance, free transportation, or


follow-up was for the Elmira project annual contact by the secretary of
(Olds et al. 1998). Nearly all studies the study.
(six) involved intensive home visita-
tion. Half of these had additional Outcome characteristics
intervention components, either par-
ticipation in a child development cen- Overall, results concerning the
ter or participation in parent groups. effectiveness of parent training in
One study involved a clinic-based the prevention of behavior problems
interview conducted with mothers by in children were mixed (see Table 3).
a general practitioner. In all but one Four studies reported no evidence of
study (Scarr and McCartney 1988), effectiveness, two reported beneficial
control groups were offered a effects, and one study reported
nonintensive follow-up including mainly beneficial effects with some
screening procedures, pediatric harmful effects. Of the studies with
96 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 2
INTERVENTION CHARACTERISTICS OF STUDIES INCLUDED IN THE REVIEW

Average Age Intervention


at Start of Period
Study Intervention (childs age) Type of Intervention

Cullen (1976) 3 months Up to 6 years Clinic-based interview with general


practitioner
Johnson and 12 months 1 to 3 years Home visits, family workshops, and
Breckenridge (1982) child development center
Johnson and Walker
(1987)
Kitzman et al. (1997) Gestational age: Prenatal to 2 years Home visits
16.5 weeks
McCarton et al. (1997) 7 weeks Up to 3 years Home visits, parent groups, child
development center
Olds et al. (1986, Gestational age: Prenatal to 2 years Home visits
1998) 25 weeks
Scarr and McCartney 24 months 2 to 4 years Home visits
(1988)
St-Pierre and Layzer Not available Younger than Home visits, child development
(1999) 1 year to 5 years center

significant results, which provided cognitive development and physical


sufficient data to calculate an effect health. The childs age at evaluation
size, the treatment effect ranged varied greatly from one study to the
from 0.25 to 1.05 (calculations from other, ranging from 2 to 15 years.
Mrazek and Brown 1999). All of the Only two studies reported differen-
studies, with the exception of one tial effects according to gender, but
(Scarr and McCartne y 1988) , both girls and boys benefited from
included mothers reports of disrup- the interventions.
tive behavior. Two studies also Only one study (Olds et al. 1998)
included teachers or schools reports evaluated the effectiveness of home
(Johnson and Walker 1987; Olds visitation and parent training on
et al. 1998), and one study used self- delinquent behaviors. Although not
reported delinquency (Olds et al. initially designed with the aim of
1998). Only two of the seven trials preventing delinquency, the Elmira
were designed to target specifically project reported beneficial effects
behavior problems, the Houston Par- with respect to childrens delinquent
ent-Child Development Center Pro- behavior 13 years after the end of the
gram (Johnson and Breckenridge intervention (age 15). However, the
1982; Johnson and Walker 1987) and beneficial effect of the intervention
the Brusselton study (Cullen 1976). concerned a subgroup of children of
Most studies looked at behavior poor, young, and unmarried women
problems among a wide range of only (n = 68). The intervention was
other outcomes, for ex ampl e, an intensive nurse home-visiting
EARLY PARENT TRAINING 97

TABLE 3
OUTCOME FINDINGS OF THE STUDIES INCLUDED IN THE REVIEW

Direction of
Study Outcome Effect Sizea p Value Outcome

Cullen (1976) At age 6


Mothers report Beneficial
Talked loudly < 0.25 < 0.05 T
Hit or struck others < 0.25 < 0.05 T
< 0.35 < 0.05 G
Exaggerated/told lies < 0.35 < 0.05 G
Harmful
Late for school > 0.42 < 0.001 T
> 0.48 < 0.01 B
Johnson and At age 5.3
Breckenridge Mothers report Beneficial
(1982) Behavior assessment
Johnson and Destructive 1.05 < 0.01 B
Walker (1987) High activity 0.55 < 0.05 B
At age 5.5
Teachers report Beneficial
Classroom Behavior Inventory
Hostility Scale 0.46 0.01 T
0.66 0.01 B
Behavior problems
Disrupts 0.42, 0.53 0.019, 0.038 T, B
Obstinate 0.48, 0.61 0.007, 0.018 T, B
Restless 0.47, 0.70 0.008, 0.007 T, B
Fights 0.46, 0.68 0.01, 0.008 T, B
Impulsive 0.58, 0.54 0.025, 0.03 B, G
Kitzman et al. At age 2
(1997) Mothers report
Child Behavior Checklist ns
McCarton et al. At age 8
(1997) Mothers report
Child Behavior Checklist ns
Behavior Profile ns
Olds et al. At age 15
b
(1986, 1998) Childs report Beneficial
Running away NAc 0.003 Beneficial
Arrests NA 0.03 Beneficial
Convictions, probation
violations NA < 0.001 Beneficial
Number of sex partners NA 0.003 Beneficial
Days having consumed
alcohol NA 0.03 Beneficial
Minor antisocial acts ns
Major delinquent acts ns
Externalizing problems ns
Acting out problems ns

(continued)
98 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 3 Continued

Direction of
Study Outcome Effect Sizea p Value Outcome

Incidence of times stopped


by police ns
Alcohol impairment ns
Days using drugs ns
Parents report
Similar scales ns
Schools report
Incidence of short- or long-
term school suspensions ns
Scarr and At age 45 months
McCartney Blind examiner
(1988) Childhood Personality Scale ns
Infant Behavior Record ns
St-Pierre and At ages 3, 4, and 5
Layzer (1999) Mothers report
Child Behavior Checklist ns
Total score
Externalizing score
Internalizing score
NOTES: T = total sample. B = boys. G = girls. ns = not significant.
a. Effect size calculations are taken from Mrazek and Brown (1999). They can be either negative or
positive, and their interpretation depends on the way the outcome measure is coded.
b. The beneficial outcomes concerned only the subgroup of children of poor unmarried women.
c. Insufficient data were provided to calculate an effect size.

program that started early during available for this review. In addition,
the pregnancy of high-risk women overall results were mixed: four stud-
and continued during the 2 years ies reported no evidence of effective-
after birth. The nurses promoted sev- ness, two reported beneficial effects,
eral aspects of maternal functioning and one reported mainly beneficial
and well-being including competent effects with some harmful effects.
care of the children. The nurses com- The latter effects, however, con-
pleted an average of 9 visits during cerned one specific item only: late for
pregnancy and 23 visits from birth to school. Studies varied greatly from
the childs second year (Olds et al. one another on various aspects,
1997). including outcome measures, childs
age at evaluation, the nature and
DISCUSSION duration of the intervention, and
sample size. Studies reporting bene-
A very limited number of well- ficial effects showed no specific pat-
designed studies including both terns allowing distinction from the
early interventions and outcomes other studies. In this context, it is
related to disruptive behaviors were impossible to make a definitive
EARLY PARENT TRAINING 99

statement as to whether early parent poor, young, and unmarried women.


training and support is effective in Several aspects of maternal func-
preventing disruptive behaviors in tioning were promoted in addition to
children and delinquency during the competent care of the child,
adolescence. Thus caution is sug- including maternal personal devel-
gested in the interpretation of the opment and positive health behav-
existing studies. iors. In addition, an important focus
Similar caution has already been was put on the involvement of other
expressed with regard to home visit- family members and people in the
ing programs, which provide an social network.
important amount of parent train- On the other hand, the Brusselton
ing. Some authors have argued that project in Australia (Cullen 1976)
home visits are a necessary but was significantly different in nature
insufficient component of programs and intensity. Only 20- to 30-minute
seeking to help families and young counseling sessions were provided by
children (Weiss 1993). More recently, the same general practitioner to all
a major review of six home visiting mothers living in a rural community.
models that were being, or had been, Four sessions were provided during
implemented nationally in the the first 2 years of life, followed by
United States concluded that results two sessions per year for the next 4
regarding the effectiveness of home years. Hence, although significantly
visiting for a wide range of outcomes less intensive, the duration of the
were quite modest, at the most Brusselton intervention was 3 times
(Gomby, Culross, and Behrman longer than the duration of the
1999). Elmira intervention. The progress of
Several factors can contribute to the child formed the basis of each
these overall disappointing results interview in the Brusselton trial.
(for an excellent review of these fac- Mothers were encouraged to accept
tors, see Gomby, Culross, and themselves as they were and to
Behrman 1999; St-Pierre and Layzer, reflect on, and eventually modify,
1999). The heterogeneity in the defi- their child-rearing practices. Finally,
nition of parent training and the the third study, showing beneficial
absence of evidence regarding which effects on disruptive behaviorsthe
components of parent training are Houston, Texas, project (Johnson and
most effective appear most relevant Breckenridge 1982)targeted low-
to our own review. The three trials income Mexican American families
reporting beneficial results varied and combined several intervention
greatly with regard to the nature of compon en t s t h at emph as i z ed
the intervention. The Elmira project parenting skills: home visits, family
in New York State (Olds et al. 1998), workshops, and the participation in a
an intensive nurse home-visitation child development center. Fathers
program that emphasized parental were strongly encouraged to partici-
development and was provided dur- pate. This heterogeneity in the small
ing the first 2 years of the childs life, number of studies showing beneficial
had a significant effect on children of effects underscores the fact that little
100 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

information is available to guide CONCLUSIONS


intervention programs when they
choose to target parent education. As Caution is suggested in the inter-
St-Pierre and Layzer (1999) pointed pretation of findings of research on
out, the field of parent education tar- the effectiveness of early parent
geting young families seems to suffer training for the prevention of disrup-
from a lack of evidence about which tive behavior problems in children
intervention components are most and juvenile delinquency due to
important, which parents are more three important considerations: (1)
likely to benefit from the interven- there is a limited number of ade-
tion, how long it should last, and quately designed studies; (2) results
whether parent training should be of the well-designed studies avail-
combined with other intervention able are mixed and, where positive,
types. often modest in magnitude; and (3)
It is of interest to note that the very few studies (two of seven) were
Brusselton and Houston studies specifically designed to prevent dis-
were the only two initially designed ruptive behaviors in children. Since
to prevent behavior disorders, and there is good evidence from longitu-
both reported beneficial effects. This, dinal studies that disruptive behav-
perhaps, highlights the relevance of ior starts during the preschool years
developing specific models for the and often leads to juvenile delin-
prevention of behavior problems quency, there is clearly a need for
rather than using general models to numerous trials testing different
improve a wide range of maternal types of early interventions specifi-
and child outcomes. In their review of cally designed for the prevention of
major American home-visiting pro- disruptive behavior problems and
grams targeting broad outcomes, juvenile delinquency.
Gomby, Culross, and Behrman (1999)
advocated a more modest view of the
References
potential of home-visiting programs.
In addition, they strongly recom- American Psychiatric Association. 1994.
mended the use of new models to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
improve the overall effectiveness of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington,
home-visiting programs. We believe DC: Author.
this recommendation is especially Barlow, Jane and Esther Coren. 2001.
relevant for interventions targeting Parent-Training for Improving Mater-
the prevention of childrens disrup- nal Psychosocial Health. The Coch-
tive behavior problems and delin- rane Library [online] 2.
Broidy, Lisa, Daniel Nagin, and Richard E.
quency. Without any doubt, many
Tremblay. 1999. The Linkage of Tra-
additional studies are required in
jectories of Childhood Externalizing
order to identify the characteristics Behaviors to Later Violent and Nonvi-
of early parent training and support olent Delinquency. Paper presented at
programs that can prevent the devel- the biennial meeting of the Society for
opment of disruptive behavior disor- Research in Child Development, Albu-
ders and delinquency. querque, NM, Apr.
EARLY PARENT TRAINING 101

Chamberlain, Patricia. 1999. Residential ysis and Recommendations. Future of


Care for Children and Adolescents Children 3:6-22.
with Oppositional Defiant Disorder Hawkins, J. David, Richard F. Catalano,
and Conduct Disorder. In Handbook of Richard Kosterman, Robert Abbott,
Disruptive Behavior Disorders, ed. and Karl G. Hill. 1999. Preventing Ad-
Herbert C. Quay and Anne E. Hogan. olescent Health-Risk Behaviors by
New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Strengthening Protection During
Ct, Sylvana, Mark Zoccolillo, Richard Childhood. Archives of Pediatrics and
E. Tremblay, Daniel Nagin, and Frank Adolescent Medicine 153:226-34.
Vitaro. In press. Predicting Girls Con- Jo h n s o n , D a l e L . a n d Ja m e s N.
duct Disorder in Adolescence from Breckenridge. 1982. The Houston
Childhood Trajectories of Disruptive Parent-Child Development Center
Behaviors. Journal of the American and the Primary Prevention of Behav-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy- ior Problems in Young Children.
chiatry. American Journal of Community Psy-
Cullen, Kevin J. 1976. A Six-Year chology 10:305-16.
Controlled Trial of Prevention of Johnson, Dale L. and Todd T. Walker.
Childrens Behavior Disorders. Jour- 1987. Primary Prevention of Behavior
nal of Pediatrics 88:662-66. Problems in Mexican-American
Culross, Patti L. 1999. Summary of Home Children. American Journal of Com-
Visiting Program Evaluation Out- munity Psychology 15:375-95.
comes. Future of Children 9:195-223. Kavale, Kenneth. A., Steven R. Forness,
Farrington, David P. 1995. The Develop- and Hill M. Walker. 1999. Interven-
ment of Offending and Antisocial Be- tions for Oppositional Defiant Disor-
havior from Childhood: Key Findings der and Conduct Disorder in the
from the Cambridge Study in Delin- Schools. In Handbook of Disruptive
quent Development. Journal of Child Behavior Disorders, ed. Herbert C.
Psychology and Psychiatry 36:929-64. Quay and Anne E. Hogan. New York:
Fergusson, David M. and L. John Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Horwood. 1998. Early Conduct Prob- Kazdin, Alan E. 1985. Treatment of Anti-
lems and Later Life Opportunities. social Behavior in Children and Ado-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- lescents. Homewood, IL: Dorsey.
chiatry 39:1097-108. Kazdin, Allan E., Todd C. Siegel, and
Frick, Paul J. and Bryan R. Loney. 1999. Debra Bass. 1992. Cognitive Problem-
Outcomes of Children and Adoles- Solving Skills Training and Parent
cents with Oppositional Defiant Dis- Management Training in the Treat-
order and Conduct Disorder. In Hand- ment of Antisocial Behavior in
book of Disruptive Behavior Disorders, Children. Journal of Consulting and
ed. Herbert C. Quay and Anne E. Ho- Clinical Psychology 60:733-47.
gan. New York: Kluwer Academic/Ple- Kitzman, Harriet, David L. Olds, Charles R.
num. Henderson, Carole Hanks, Robert
Gomby, Deanna S., Patti L. Culross, and Cole, Robert Tatelbaum, Kenneth M.
Richard E. Behrman. 1999. Home McConnochie, Kimberly Sidora, Den-
Visiting: Recent Program Evalua- nis W. Luckey, David Shaver, Kay
tionsAnalysis and Recommenda- E n g e l h a r d t , D av i d Ja m e s, a n d
tions. Future of Children 9:4-26. Kathryn Barnard. 1997. Effect of Pre-
Gomby, Deanna S., Carol S. Larson, Eu- natal and Infancy Home Visitation by
gene M. Lewit, and Richard E. Nurses on Pregnancy Outcomes,
Behrman. 1993. Home Visiting: Anal- Childhood Injuries and Repeated
102 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Childbearing. Journal of the American and Non-Violent Juvenile Delin-


Medical Association 278:644-52. quency. Child Development 70:1181-
Lahey, Benjamin B., Terri L. Miller, 96.
Rachel A. Gordon, and Anne W. Riley. . 2001. Parental and Early Child-
1999. Developmental Epidemiology of hood Predictors of Persistent Physical
the Disruptive Behavior Disorders. In Aggression in Boys from Kindergar-
Handbook of Disruptive Behavior Dis- ten to High School. Archives of General
orders, ed. Herbert C. Quay and Anne E. Psychiatry 58:389-94.
Hogan. New York: Kluwer Academic/ Olds, David L., Charles R. Henderson,
Plenum. Robert Chamberlin, and Robert
Loeber, Rolf. 2001. Developmental As- Tatelbaum. 1986. Preventing Child
pects of Juvenile Homicide. Paper pre- Abuse and Neglect: A Randomized
sented at the biennial meeting of the Trial of Nurse Home Visitation. Pedi-
Society for Research in Child Develop- atrics 78:65-78.
ment, Minneapolis, MN, Apr. Olds, David L., Charles R. Henderson,
McCarton, Cecilia M., Jeanne Brooks- Robert Cole, John Eckenrode, Harriet
Gunn, Ina F. Wallace, Charles R. Kitzman, Dennis Luckey, Lisa Pettitt,
Bauer, Forrest C. Bennet, Judy C. Kimberly Sidora, Pamela Morris, and
Bernbaum, Sue Broyles, Patrick H. Jane Powers. 1998. Long-Term Effects
Casey, Marie C. McCormick, David T. o f N u r s e H o m e Vi s i t a t i o n o n
Scott, Jon Tyson, James Tonascia, and Childrens Criminal and Antisocial
Curtis L. Meinert. 1997. Results at Behavior: 15-year Follow-Up of a Ran-
Age 8 Years of Early Intervention for domized Controlled Trial. Journal of
Low-Birth-Weight Premature Infants: the American Medical Association
The Infant Health and Development 280:1238-44.
Program. Journal of the American
Olds, David L., Harriet Kitzman, Robert
Medical Association 277:126-32.
Cole, and JoAnn Robinson. 1997. The-
McCord, Joan, Cathy Spatz Widom, and oretical and Empirical Foundations of
Nancy E. Crowell. 2001. Juvenile a Program of Home Visitation for
Crime, Juvenile Justice. Washington, Pregnant Women and Parents of
DC: National Academy Press. Young Children. Journal of Commu-
Moffitt, Terrie E., Avshalom Caspi, Nigel nity Psychology 25:9-25.
Dickson, Phil S. Silva, and Warren
Patterson, Gerald R. 1982. Coercive Fam-
Stanton. 1996. Childhood-onset Ver-
ily Process. Eugene, OR: Castalia.
sus Adolescent-Onset Antisocial
Conduct Problems in Males: Natural Patterson, Gerald R., John B. Reid, and
History from Ages 3 to 18 Years. Devel- Thomas J. Dishion. 1992. Antisocial
opment & Psychopathology 8: 399-424. Boys. Eugene, OR: Castalia.
Mrazek, Patricia J. and C. Henricks Quay, Herbert C. and Anne E. Hogan.
Brown. 1999. An Evidenced-Based Lit- 1999a. Preface. In Handbook of Dis-
erature Review Regarding Outcomes ruptive Behavior Disorders, ed. Her-
in Psychosocial Prevention and Early bert C. Quay and Anne E. Hogan. New
Intervention in Young Children. York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Toronto, Canada: Invest in Kids , eds. 1999b. Handbook of Disrup-
Foundation. tive Behavior Disorders. New York:
Nagin, Daniel and Richard E. Tremblay. Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
1999. Trajectories of Boys Physical St-Pierre, Robert G. and Jean I. Layzer.
Aggression, Opposition, and Hyperac- 1999. Using Home Visits for Multiple
tivity on the Path to Physically Violent Purposes: The Comprehensive Child
EARLY PARENT TRAINING 103

Development Program. Future of orders, ed. Herbert C. Quay and Anne E.


Children 9:134-51. Hogan. New York: Kluwer Academic/
Scarr, Sandra and Kathleen McCartney. Plenum.
1988. Far from Home: An Experimen- Vitaro, Frank, Mirella De Civita, and
tal Evaluation of the Mother-Child Linda Pagani. 1996. The Impact of Re-
Home Program in Bermuda. Child De- search-Based Prevention Programs
velopment 59:531-43. on Childrens Disruptive Behavior.
Serbin, Lisa A., Jessica M. Cooperman, Exceptionality Education Canada
Patrica L. Peters, Pascale M. Lehoux, 5:105-35.
Dale M. Stack, and Alex E. Schwartz- We b s t e r- S t r a t t o n , C a r o l y n , M a r y
man. 1998. Intergenerational Trans- Kolpacoff, and Terri Hollinsworth.
fer of Psychosocial Risk in Women 1988. Self-Administered Videotape
with Childhood Histories of Aggres- Therapy for Families with Conduct-
sion, Withdrawal, or Aggression and problem Children: Comparison with
Withdrawal. Developmental Psychol- Two Cost-Effective Treatments and a
ogy 34:1246-62. Control Group. Journal of Consulting
Tremblay, Richard E. 2000. The Develop- and Clinical Psychology 56:558-66.
ment of Aggressive Behaviour During Weiss, Heather B. 1993. Home Visits:
Childhood: What Have We Learned in Necessary but Not Sufficient. Future
the Past Century? International Jour- of Children 3:113-28.
nal of Behavioral Development White, Jennifer L., Terrie E. Moffitt,
24:129-41. Felton Earls, Lee Robins, and Phil A.
Tremblay, Richard E., Linda Kurtz, Silva. 1990. How Early Can We Tell?
Louise C. Msse, Frank Vitaro, and Predictors of Childhood Conduct Dis-
Robert O. Pihl. 1995. A Bimodal Pre- order and Adolescent Delinquency.
ventive Intervention for Disruptive Criminology 28:507-33.
Kindergarten Boys: Its Impact Yoshikawa, Hirokazu. 1994. Prevention
Through Mid-Adolescence. Journal of as Cumulative Protection: Effects of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology Early Family Support and Education
63:560-68. on Chronic Delinquency and Its Risks.
Tremblay, Richard E., David LeMar- Psychological Bulletin 115:28-54.
quand, and Frank Vitaro. 1999. The . 1995. Long-Term Effects of Early
Prevention of Oppositional Defiant Childhood Programs on Social Out-
Disorder and Conduct Disorder. In comes and Delinquency. Future of
Handbook of Disruptive Behavior Dis- Children 5:51-75.
ANNALS, AAPSS, 578, November 2001

THE ANNALS
EFFECTS OF HOT
OF THE
SPOTS
AMERICAN
POLICING
ACADEMY
ON CRIME

The Effects of Hot Spots


Policing on Crime

By ANTHONY A. BRAGA

ABSTRACT: In recent years, researchers have argued that police ac-


tions should be focused on high-risk crime places rather than spread
thinly across the urban landscape. This review examines the avail-
able evaluation evidence on the effects of concentrating police en-
forcement efforts on crime hot spots. Five randomized experiments
and four nonequivalent control group quasi-experiments were identi-
fied. The findings of these evaluations suggest that focused police ac-
tions can prevent crime and disorder in crime hot spots. These studies
also suggest that focused police actions at specific locations do not
necessarily result in crime displacement. Unintended crime preven-
tion benefits were also associated with the hot spots policing pro-
grams. Although these evaluations reveal that these programs work
in preventing crime, additional research is needed to unravel other
important policy-relevant issues such as community reaction to fo-
cused police enforcement efforts.

Anthony A. Braga is a senior research associate in the Program in Criminal Justice


Policy and Management of the Malcolm Wiener Center for Social Policy at Harvard
Universitys John F. Kennedy School of Government and a visiting fellow at the U.S. Na-
tional Institute of Justice.

NOTE: The author would like to thank Phyllis Schultze at Rutgers Universitys Criminal Jus-
tice Library for her valuable assistance in completing this review.

104
EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON CRIME 105

P LACE-ORIENTED crime pre-


vention strategies have begun to
occupy a central role in police crime
science views that the police make
only minimal contributions to crime
prevention relative to more powerful
prevention research and policy (Eck social institutions like the family and
and Weisburd 1995). This idea devel- labor markets (as discussed in
oped from the hot spots of crime per- Sherman 1997). A number of well-
spective, which suggests that crime known empirical studies on basic
does not occur evenly across urban police crime control strategiesran-
landscapes; rather, it is concentrated dom patrol, rapid response, and crim-
in relatively small places that gener- inal investigationsupport the
ate more than half of all criminal assertion that police can do little to
events (Pierce, Spaar, and Briggs prevent crime (Kelling et al. 1974;
1988; Sherman, Gartin, and Buerger Spelman and Brown 1984; Green-
1989; Weisburd, Maher, and Sher- wood, Chaiken, and Petersilia 1977).
man 1992). Even within the most crime- However, based on new research evi-
ridden neighborhoods, crime clusters dence, many crime prevention schol-
at a few discrete locations, and other ars suggest the ability of the police to
areas are relatively crime free (Sher- prevent crime may have more to do
man, Gartin, and Buerger 1989). A with how well they are focused on
number of researchers have argued specific crime risk factors rather
that many crime problems could be than how well they randomly patrol
reduced more efficiently if police offi- large areas, rapidly respond to calls
cers focused their attention on these for service, and make large numbers
deviant places (Sherman 1995; of reactive arrests (Sherman 1997;
Weisburd 1997). Three complemen- Clarke 1992; Goldstein 1990; Wilson
tary perspectives on crime theoreti- and Kelling 1982). As such, police
cally support these observations on should focus their actions on the
the uneven distribution of deviance: places, times, and people who pose
rational choice, routine activities, the highest risks to public safety
and environmental criminology (Cor- rather than dilute their crime pre-
nish and Clarke 1986; Cohen and vention potency by spreading them
Felson 1979; Brantingham and thinly across the urban landscape.
Brantingham 1991). By preventing This review examines the available
victims and offenders from converg- evaluation evidence on one type of
ing in space and time, police can re- risk-focused policing to prevent
duce crime. A growing body of re- crime: concentrating police enforce-
search evidence suggests that ment efforts in high-risk places
focused police interventions, such as where crime is concentrated, or hot
directed patrols, proactive arrests, spots policing.
and problem solving, can produce sig-
nificant crime prevention gains at SYSTEMATIC REVIEW OF
high-crime hot spots (Sherman 1997). HOT SPOTS POLICING STUDIES
These new perspectives on the
ability of the police to prevent crime This study reviews and synthe-
contrast with conventional social sizes existing published and non-
106 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

published empirical evidence on the All studies where police interven-


effects of focused police enforcement tions were focused on places smaller
interventions at crime hot spots and than a neighborhood, community, or
provides a systematic assessment of police beat were considered. The
the preventive value of these pro- units of analysis in eligible studies
grams. In keeping with the conven- did not have to be hot spots or high-
tions established by the systematic activity crime places. However, the
reviews methods literature, the police interventions had to be specifi-
stages of this review and the criteria cally targeted at hot spots within
used to select eligible studies are these larger area units.
described below.
Types of interventions
Types of studies
The interventions used to control
This review was limited to studies crime hot spots were limited to police
that used a no-treatment control enforcement efforts. Suitable police
group design involving before and enforcement efforts included tradi-
after measures. In eligible studies, tional tactics such as directed patrol
the no-treatment control group expe- and heightened levels of traffic
rienced routine police interventions enforcement as well as alternative
(that is, regular levels of random strategies such as aggressive disor-
patrol, ad hoc investigations, and the der enforcement and problem-ori-
like). Crime places that received the ented policing (POP) interventions
focused police interventions were with limited situational responses
compared to places that experienced and limited engagement of the pub-
routine levels of traditional police lic. To be considered for this review,
service. The comparison group study POP initiatives had to engage pri-
designs had to be either experimen- marily traditional policing tactics
tal or quasi-experimental (nonran- such as law enforcement actions,
domized) (Campbell and Stanley informal counseling and cautioning,
1966; Cook and Campbell 1979). and referrals to other agencies.1 POP
programs that involved multiple
Types of areas interventions implemented by other
To be included in this review, the stakeholders, such as community
focus of police interventions in the members, business owners, or
evaluations had to be crime hot spots residential managers, were not
or crime places. As John Eck (1997) considered.
suggested,
Types of outcome measures
a place is a very small area reserved for a
narrow range of functions, often con-
Eligible studies had to measure
trolled by a single owner, and separated the effects of the police intervention
from the surrounding area. . . . Examples on officially recorded levels of crime
of places include stores, homes, apart- at the places. Appropriate measures
ment buildings, street corners, subway of crime could include crime incident
stations, and airports. (7.1) reports, citizen emergency calls for
EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON CRIME 107

service, or arrest data. Other out- 1. Searches of online databases


comes measures such as surveys, (see below);
interviews, and systematic observa- 2. Searches of narrative and em-
tions of physical and social changes pirical reviews of literature that ex-
at places used by eligible studies amine the effectiveness of police in-
were included in the assessment of terventions on crime hot spots;
program effectiveness. Particular 3. Searches of bibliographies of
attention was paid to studies that police crime prevention efforts and
measured crime displacement effects place-oriented crime prevention pro-
and diffusion of crime control benefit grams; and
effects. The value of policing strate- 4. Contacts with leading re-
gies focused on specific locations has searchers.
been questioned by the threat of
crime displacement. That is, efforts The following 10 databases were
aimed at reducing specific crime at a searched:
place will simply cause criminal
activity to move elsewhere, be com- 1. Criminal Justice Periodical
mitted in another way, or even be Index;
manifested as another type of crime, 2. Sociological Abstracts;
3. Social Science Abstracts;
thus negating any crime control
4. Arts and Humanities Search;
gains (Reppetto 1976). More recently,
5. Criminal Justice Abstracts;
academics have observed that crime
6. National Criminal Justice
prevention programs may result in
Reference Service Abstracts;
the complete opposite of displace-
7. Educational Resources Infor-
mentthat crime control benefits
mation Clearinghouse;
were greater than expected and spill
8. Legal Resource Index;
over into places beyond the target
9. Dissertation Abstracts; and
areas (Clarke and Weisburd 1994).
10. Government Publications Of-
The quality of the methodologies fice Monthly Catalog.
used to measure displacement and
diffusion effects, as well as the types The following terms were used to
of displacement examined, was search the 10 databases listed above:
assessed.
1. Hot spot;
2. Crime place;
Search strategies for
3. Crime clusters;
identification of studies
4. Crime displacement;
5. Place-oriented interventions;
All published and unpublished
6. High crime areas;
studies, including those not written
7. High crime locations; and
in the English language, were consid-
8. Targeted policing.
ered for this review. To identify stud-
ies meeting the criteria of this review,
the following four search strategies In addition, two existing registers
were used: of randomized controlled trials were
108 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

consulted. These included (1) the 1. Minneapolis Repeat Call Ad-


Registry of Randomized Experiments dress Policing (RECAP) Program (Sher-
in Criminal Sanctions, 1950-1983 man, Buerger, and Gartin 1989);
(Weisburd, Sherman, and Petrosino 2. Minneapolis Hot Spots Patrol
1990), and (2) the Social, Psychologi- Program (Sherman and Weisburd
cal, Educational, and Criminological 1995);
Trials Register, or SPECTR, being de- 3. Jersey City Drug Markets
veloped by the U.K. Cochrane Centre Analysis Program (DMAP) (Weis-
and the University of Pennsylvania burd and Green 1995);
(Petrosino et al. in press). 4. Jersey City POP at Violent
Places Project (Braga et al. 1999);
5. St. Louis POP in Three Drug
Selection of studies
Market Locations Study (Hope
1994);
The four search strategies led to
6. Kansas City Crack House Po-
the identification of 588 distinct ab-
lice Raids Program (Sherman and
stracts. The text of each abstract was
Rogan 1995a);
screened carefully to identify poten-
7. Kansas City Gun Project
tially eligible studies, per the criteria
(Sherman and Rogan 1995b);
described above. The screening pro-
8. Houston Targeted Beat Pro-
cess yielded 43 distinct abstracts
gram (Caeti 1999); and
that identified 18 potentially eligible
9. Beenleigh Calls for Service
evaluation studies.2 The full-text re-
Project (Criminal Justice Commis-
ports, journal articles, and books for
sion 1998).
these 43 abstracts were acquired and
carefully assessed to determine
Characteristics related to the
whether the interventions involved
methodological quality of the nine se-
focused police enforcement efforts at
lected studies were extracted from
crime hot spots and whether the
the full-text journal articles and re-
studies used randomized experimen-
ports. These characteristics included
tal or nonrandomized quasi-experi-
the definition criteria used to iden-
mental designs. Of the 18 studies, 9
tify crime hot spots, the quality of an-
were excluded from this review be-
alytic methods to evaluate program
cause the focused policing interven-
outcomes, the measurement of dis-
tions were applied uniformly across
placement, any violation of random-
areas much larger than specific high-
ization procedures, case attrition
crime locations (see, for example,
from the study, and any subversion of
Caulkins, Larson, and Rich 1993;
the experiment by participants.
Novak et al. 1999)3 and/or the treat-
When appropriate and possible, the
ment was not composed of primarily
role of these methodological factors
police-initiated enforcement tactics
on the observed empirical results
(see, for example, Green Mazerolle,
was noted. Since there were only nine
Price, and Roehl 2000; Eck and
studies selected, this review was con-
Wartell 1996). The nine studies in-
ducted as a structured qualitative
cluded in this review were the follow-
ing:
EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON CRIME 109

exercise; no quantitative analyses evaluated the effects of increased lev-


were conducted. els of preventive patrol on crime
(Sherman and Weisburd 1995, 634).
Characteristics of The Jersey City DMAP and the Kan-
selected studies sas City Crack House Police Raids
The nine evaluations were con- Program evaluated the effects of
ducted in five large cities in the well-planned crackdowns on street-
United States and one suburb in level drug markets and court autho-
Australia. Research teams involving rized raids on crack houses, respec-
either Lawrence W. Sherman or tively (Weisburd and Green 1995,
David L. Weisburd conducted six of 718; Sherman and Rogan 1995a, 766-
the nine evaluations. The treatments 67).
used to prevent crime at hot spots fell Five of the selected studies used
into three broad categories: enforce- randomized experimental designs,
ment POP interventions, directed and four used nonequivalent control
and aggressive patrol programs, and group quasi-experimental designs.
police crackdowns and raids (see All randomized experiments and one
Table 1). The effects of POP initia- quasi-experiment, the St. Louis POP
tives comprising mostly traditional study, used crime hot spots as the
tactics with limited situational unit of analysis. The remaining three
responses were evaluated in the Min- quasi-experiments evaluated the
neapolis RECAP Program, Jersey aggregate beat-level effects of
City POP at Violent Places Study, St. focused police interventions at hot
Louis POP at Drug Market Locations spots within targeted beats. With the
Study, and Beenleigh Calls for Ser- exception of the Minneapolis RECAP
vice Project (Buerger 1994, 6-7; ex peri men t , t h e ex peri men t al
Braga et al. 1999, 554; Criminal Jus- designs used more sophisticated
tice Commission 1998, 28). The eval- methodologies to identify crime hot
uation of the Houston Targeted Beat spots. The Minneapolis Hot Spots
Program examined the effects of Patrol, Jersey City DMAP, and Jer-
three types of treatments applied in sey City POP at Violent Places exper-
different target areas; these inter- iments used the most sophisticated
ventions included high-visibility methods to identify hot spots. In gen-
patrol, zero tolerance disorder polic- eral, the research teams defined hot
ing, and enforcement POP (Caeti spot areas by mapping official police
1999, 246-50). The Kansas City Gun call data to identify high volume
Project examined the gun violence street address clusters and intersec-
prevention effects of proactive patrol tion areas, ensured that these loca-
and intensive enforcement of fire- tions had stable numbers of calls
arms laws via safety frisks during over time, and considered qualitative
traffic stops, plain view searches and i n di cat ors s u ch as pol i ce an d
seizures, and searches incident to researcher observations to define hot
arrests on other charges (Sherman spot boundaries (Sherman and
and Rogan 1995b, 681). The Minne- Weisburd 1995, 630-32; Weisburd
apolis Hot Spots Patrol Program and Green 1995, 713-15; Braga et al.
110 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 1
HOT SPOTS POLICING EXPERIMENTS AND QUASI-EXPERIMENTS
a
Study Treatment Hot Spot Definition Research Design

Minneapolis POP interventions com- Addresses ranked by Randomized experiment;


(MN) prising mostly tradi- frequency of citizen control and treatment
RECAP tional enforcement tac- calls for service groups were each
Program tics with some divided into randomly allocated
(Sherman, situational responses commercial and 125 commercial and
Buerger, One-year intervention residential lists; 125 residential
and Gartin period the top 250 addresses
1989) Integrity of treatment commercial and top Differences in the
threatened by large 250 residential number of calls to each
caseloads that out- addresses were address from a
stripped the resources included in the baseline year to the
the RECAP unit could experiment experimental year were
bring to bear compared between
RECAP and control
groups

Minneapolis Uniformed police patrol; One-hundred-ten hot Randomized experiment;


(MN) Hot experimental group, on spots comprising control and treatment
Spots Patrol average, experienced address clusters groups were each
Program twice as much patrol that experienced randomly allocated
(Sherman presence high volumes of 55 hot spots within
and One-year intervention citizen calls for statistical blocks
Weisburd period service, had stable Differences of differ-
1995) Breakdown in the numbers of calls for ences between citizen
treatment noted during over 2 years, and calls in baseline and
the summer months were visually experimental years,
proximate comparing control and
experimental groups

Jersey City Well-planned crack- Fifty-six drug hot Randomized experiment;


(NJ) DMAP downs followed by spot areas control and treatment
(Weisburd preventive patrol to identified based on groups were each
and Green maintain crime control ranking intersection randomly allocated
1995) gains areas with high 28 drug hot spots
Fifteen-month levels of drug- within statistical blocks
intervention period related calls and Differences of
Slow progress at narcotics arrests, differences between
treatment places types of drugs sold, citizen calls during
caused intervention police perceptions 7-month pretest and
time period to be of drug areas, and posttest periods,
extended by 3 months offender movement comparing control and
patterns experimental groups
EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON CRIME 111

TABLE 1 Continued
a
Study Treatment Hot Spot Definition Research Design

Jersey City POP interventions Twenty-four violent Randomized experiment;


(NJ) POP comprising mostly crime places identi- 24 places were
at Violent aggressive disorder fied based on rank- matched into like pairs
Places enforcement tactics ing intersection based on simple quan-
Project with some situational areas with high titative and qualitative
(Braga et al. responses levels of assault analyses; control and
1999) Sixteen-month and robbery calls treatment groups were
intervention period and incidents as each randomly
Initial slow progress at well as police and allocated 12 places
places caused by researcher within matched pairs
resistance of officers to perceptions of Differences of differ-
implement intervention violent areas ences between a num-
ber of indicators during
6-month pretest and
posttest periods, com-
paring control and
experimental groups

St. Louis POP interventions Subjective selection Quasi-experiment with


(MO) POP comprising mostly of POP efforts nonequivalent control
in Three traditional enforcement made at three hot group; changes in
Drug Market tactics with some spot locations citizen calls at hot spot
Locations situational responses comprising specific addresses location
Study Nine-month intervention addresses associ- were compared to
(Hope period ated with street- changes in calls at
1994) No threats to the integrity level drug sales other addresses on the
of the treatment block as well as other
reported blocks in surrounding
areas
Simple trend analyses
including 12-month
preintervention and
6-month post-
intervention periods
Kansas City Court-authorized raids Two hundred seven Randomized experiment;
(MO) Crack on crack houses blocks with at least raids were randomly
House conducted by five calls for allocated to 104 blocks
Police uniformed police service in the and were conducted at
Raids officers 30 days preceding 98 of those sites; the
Program Intervention period was an undercover drug other 109 blocks did not
(Sherman the day of the raid buy; sample was receive raids
and Rogan All but seven cases restricted to raids Differences of differences
1995a) received randomly on the inside of analytic design; pre-
assigned treatment as residences where post time periods were
assigned a drug buy was 30 days before and
No threats to the made that was after raid for experimen-
integrity of the eligible for a search tal blocks and 30 days
treatment reported warrant before and after con-
trolled buy at treatment
block for control blocks

(continued)
112 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 1 Continued
a
Study Treatment Hot Spot Definition Research Design

Kansas City Intensive enforcement of Eight-by-ten-block Quasi-experiment with


(MO) Gun laws against illegally target beat nonequivalent control
Project carrying concealed selected by federal group; target beat
(Sherman firearms via safety officials for Weed matched to a control
and Rogan frisks during traffic and Seed grant beat with nearly identi-
1995b) stops, plain view, and Enforcement actions cal levels of drive-by
searches incident to targeted at hot shootings
arrest on other charges spots in beat Difference of means
Twenty-nineweek identified by comparing weekly gun
intervention period computer analyses crimes between
No threats to the intervention period and
integrity of the treat- 29-week pretest period
ment reported; two Time series analyses of
phases of patrols weekly gun crimes for
reported due to shifts 52-week before-after
in grant funding period
Analysis of variance
models with one extra
pre year and post year
to examine changes in
homicides and drive-by
shootings for both patrol
phases

Houston (TX) Patrol initiative designed Seven highest crime Quasi-experiment with
Targeted to reduce index crimes beats were nonequivalent control
Beat in seven beats selected for this groups; target beats
Program Three beats used high- program were matched to
(Caeti 1999) visibility patrol at hot Enforcement actions noncontiguous
spots targeted at hot comparison beats
Three beats used zero spots in beats through cluster analysis
tolerance policing at identified by and correlations of
hot spots computer analyses census data
One beat used a POP Difference of means in
approach comprising reported crime were
mostly traditional used to evaluate
tactics to control hot program effects for
spots 3-year preintervention
Two-year intervention and 2-year intervention
period period
Three high-visibility
patrol beats managed
by one substation
Experienced police
resistance to the
program
EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON CRIME 113

TABLE 1 Continued
a
Study Treatment Hot Spot Definition Research Design

Beenleigh POP interventions com- Two groups of 10 Quasi-experiment with


(Australia) prising mostly tradi- addresses that nonequivalent control
Calls for tional enforcement tac- experienced the group: Beenleigh, a
Service tics with some highest volume of lower-income suburb
Project situational responses calls during sepa- with a population of
(Criminal Six-month intervention rate 6-month 40,000, was matched to
Justice period periods similar Brown Plains
Commis- No threats to the integ- suburb
sion 1998) rity of the treatment Simple time series analy-
reported ses of total monthly
calls for service in 5-
month pretest, 6-month
intervention, and 3-
month posttest periods
Nineteen pre-post, no
control case studies
a. The control group in each study received routine levels of traditional police enforcement tactics.

1999, 549-50). The Kansas City interventions within larger targeted


Crack House Raid experiment areas. The high-activity addresses
focused on blocks that had at least evaluated in the St. Louis POP quasi-
five calls for service in the month pre- ex peri men t w ere s u bj ect i v el y
ceding an undercover drug buy made selected after a researcher searched
on the inside of a residence (Sherman for candidate cases within the St.
and Rogan 1995a, 767). The remain- Louis Police Department (Hope 1994,
ing studies used less refined meth- 10).
ods. Simple ranking procedures to
identify high-volume addresses Effects of hot spots policing pro-
based on numbers of citizen calls for grams on crime and disorder
service were used to define specific
locations for focused police interven- Noteworthy crime reductions
tions in the Minneapolis RECAP were reported in seven of the nine
experiment (Sherman, Buerger, and selected studies (see Table 2). The
Gartin 1989, 4-5) and the Beenleigh strongest crime control gains were
quasi-experiment (Criminal Justice reported in the Jersey City POP at
Commission 1998, 9). In the Kansas Violent Places experiment and the
City Gun quasi-experiment (Sher- Kansas City Gun Project quasi-
man and Rogan 1995b, 678) and the experiment. In the Jersey City POP
Houston Targeted Beat quasi-experi- experiment, the enforcement POP
ment (Caeti 1999, 248-50), simple strategy resulted in statistically sig-
computer analyses of call and inci- nificant reductions in total calls for
dent data were used to focus police service and total crime incidents, as
114 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

well as varying reductions in all sub- service, ranging from 6 percent to 13


categories of crime types, in the percent, in treatment places relative
treatment violent crime hot spots rel- to control places (Sherman and
ative to controls (Braga et al. 1999, Weisburd 1995, 643). Moreover, sys-
562-63). Analyses of systematic tematic observations of the hot spots
observation data collected during the suggested that disorder was only half
pretest and posttest periods revealed as prevalent in treatment hot spots
that social disorder was alleviated at as compared to control hot spots
10 of 11 treatment places relative to (Sherman and Weisburd 1995, 643).
4
controls (Braga et al. 1999, 564). The Jersey City DMAP experiment
Nonexperimental systematic obser- suggested that well-planned crack-
vation data collected pretest and downs followed by patrol mainte-
posttest at treatment places sug- nance resulted in significant reduc-
gested that physical disorder was tions in disorder calls for service at
alleviated at 10 of 11 treatment the treatment drug hot spots relative
5
places (Braga et al. 1999, 564). Pre- to controls (Weisburd and Green
test and posttest interviews with key 1995, 723-26). Similarly, the St. Louis
community members suggested that POP quasi-experiment found that
community perceptions of places the enforcement POP strategy was
improved at 7 of 12 treatment places associated with varying degrees of
(Braga 1997, 235-36). Proactive reductions in total calls for service at
patrols focused on firearm recoveries all three high-activity drug locations;
in the Kansas City quasi-experiment these reductions were greater than
resulted in a statistically significant any reductions observed in other
65 percent increase in gun seizures blocks and intersections in the sur-
and a statistically significant 49 per- rounding areas (Hope 1994, 17, 21,
cent decrease in gun crimes in the 26). The Kansas City Crack House
target beat area; gun seizures and Raid experiment reported modest
gun crimes in the comparison beat decreases in citizen calls for service
area did not significantly change and crime offenses at treatment
(Sherman and Rogan 1995b, 684). A blocks relative to controls that
separate nonequivalent control decayed within two weeks of the
group quasi-experiment examined raids (Sherman and Rogan 1995a,
community reaction to the Kansas 770-76).
City intervention and found that the The results of the Houston Tar-
community strongly supported the geted Beat quasi-experiment must
intensive patrols and perceived an be interpreted with caution. The key
improvement in the quality of life in analytic measures of effectiveness
the treatment neighborhood (Shaw were comparisons of pretest and
1995). posttest differences (as measured by
The Minneapolis Hot Spots Patrol t tests) in reported crime incidents at
experiment revealed that roughly treatment beats relative to control
doubling the level of patrol in crime beats (Caeti 1999, 319-22). However,
hot spots resulted in modest, but sig- the research did not examine the dif-
nificant, reductions in total calls for ferences of differences between
EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON CRIME 115

TABLE 2
RESULTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING EVALUATIONS

Displacement/
Study Crime Outcomes Other Outcomes Diffusion

Minneapolis No statistically significant None Not measured


(MN) differences in the
RECAP Pro- prevalence of citizen calls
gram for service
(Sherman,
Buerger, and
Gartin 1989)

Minneapolis Modest, but statistically Systematic observa- Not measured


(MN) Hot significant, reductions in tions of crime and
Spots Patrol total crime calls for service disorder were half
Program ranging from 6 percent to as prevalent in
(Sherman 13 percent experimental as in
and control hot spots
Weisburd
1995)

Jersey City Statistically significant None Examined displace-


(NJ) DMAP reductions in disorder calls ment and diffu-
(Weisburd for service in treatment drug sion effects in
and Green markets relative to control two-block catch-
1995) drug markets ment areas sur-
rounding the
treatment and
control drug
places and repli-
cated the drug
market identifica-
tion process
Little evidence of
displacement;
analyses suggest
modest diffusion
of benefits

(continued)

treatment and control areas. As such, Reported significant reductions in


the quasi-experimental analyses did t reat men t beat s rel at i v e t o
not directly measure whether nonsignificant decreases and any
observed changes in treatment beats increases in reported crime can be
were significantly different from interpreted with some confidence.
observed changes in control beats. However, conclusions that the
116 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 2 Continued

Displacement/
Study Crime Outcomes Other Outcomes Diffusion

Jersey City Statistically significant reduc- Observation data Examined


(NJ) POP at tions in total calls for service revealed that social displacement and
Violent and total crime incidents disorder was diffusion effects in
Places Pro- All crime categories experi- alleviated at 10 of two-block
ject (Braga enced varying reductions; 11 treatment catchment areas
et al. 1999) statistically significant places relative to surrounding the
reductions in street fight control places treatment and
calls, property calls, narcot- Nonexperimental control drug
ics calls, robbery incidents, observation data places
and property crime revealed that Little evidence of
incidents physical disorder immediate spatial
was alleviated at 10 displacement or
of 11 treatment diffusion
places
Nonexperimental
interviews with key
community
members in target
locations suggest
no noteworthy
improvements in
citizen perceptions
of places

St. Louis (MO) All three drug locations expe- None Compared trends in
POP in rienced varying reductions calls at targeted
Three Drug in total calls addresses to
Market Regression analysis suggests trends in calls at
Locations that reductions on blocks other addresses
Study (Hope where drug locations were on same block
1994) located were greater than Location 1signifi-
other blocks and intersec- cant displacement
tions in surrounding areas into surrounding
addresses; loca-
tion 2no dis-
placement or dif-
fusion; location
3no displace-
ment or diffusion
EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON CRIME 117

TABLE 2 Continued

Displacement/
Study Crime Outcomes Other Outcomes Diffusion

Kansas City Modest decreases in citizen None Not measured


(MO) Crack calls and offense reports
House that decayed in 2 weeks
Police Raids
Program
(Sherman
and Rogan
1995a)

Kansas City Sixty-five-percent increase in Separate pre-post Displacement tests


(MO) Gun guns seized by the police; quasi-experiment using pre-post dif-
Project 49-percent decrease in gun surveying citizen ference in means
(Sherman crimes opinions of the and Auto Regres-
and Rogan Kansas City Gun sive Integrated
1995b) Project suggests Moving Average
citizens were time series analy-
aware of the pro- ses were con-
ject, generally ducted in seven
supported the contiguous beats
intensive No significant dis-
approach, and placement into
perceived an specific beats; two
improvement in beats showed sig-
the quality of life in nificant reductions
treatment in gun crimes
neighborhood

Houston (TX) Aggregated experimental None Simple pre-post


Targeted beats experienced signifi- analyses of
Beat Pro- cant reductions in auto theft, reported crimes in
gram (Caeti total part 1 index crimes, beats contiguous
1999) and total part 1 suppressible to treatment beats
(robbery, burglary, auto No evidence of sig-
theft) index crimes relative nificant displace-
to aggregate control beats ment; contiguous
Three zero-tolerance beats beats surrounding
experienced mixed results; three target areas
certain reported crimes (problem-solving
decreased in particular beat, two zero-
beats tolerance beats)
Three high-visibility beats experienced pos-
experienced reductions in a sible diffusion of
wide variety of index crimes benefits in particu-
Problem-solving beat experi- lar reported
enced no significant crimes
decrease relative to control
beat
(continued)
118 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 2 Continued

Displacement/
Study Crime Outcomes Other Outcomes Diffusion

Beenleigh No noteworthy differences in None Not measured


(Australia) total number of calls
Calls for between Beenleigh and
Service Pro- Brown Plains areas
ject (Crimi- Noteworthy reductions in calls
nal Justice reported by
Commission nonexperimental pre-post
1998) impact assessments in 16 of
the 19 case studies

program did not work in treatment was implemented to control hot spots
beats with reported significant crime did not experience noteworthy
reductions relative to control beats decreases relative to a control beat.
with significant crime reductions The limits of the analytic framework
were not justified. It is completely preclude conclusions that certain
possible that the observed significant types of policing strategies may be
reductions in the treatment beats more effective in preventing crime in
were significantly greater than the hot spots. Nevertheless, the results of
significant reductions in control this study can be broadly taken to
beats. support the position that focused
Given these caveats, the Houston police enforcement efforts can be
Targeted Beat quasi-experiment sug- effective in reducing crime at hot
gests that the aggregated treatment spots.
beats experienced significant reduc- The Beenleigh Calls for Service
tions in auto theft, total part 1 index quasi-experiment found no notewor-
crimes, and total part 1 patrol-sup- thy differences in the total number of
pressible crimes (robbery, burglary, calls in the town of Beenleigh rela-
and auto theft) relative to aggregated tive to the matched town of Brown
control beats. The three treatment Plains (Criminal Justice Commis-
beats where zero-tolerance aggres- sion 1998, 25). However, simple
sive disorder policing was used to nonexperimental pre-post compari-
control hot spots experienced mixed sons found noteworthy reductions in
reductions in part 1 crimes relative total citizen calls for service in 16 of
to control beats; the three treatment 19 case studies included in the
beats where high-visibility directed report. The research team concluded
patrol was used to control hot spots that the POP strategy enjoyed some
experienced reductions in a wide success in reducing calls for service
variety of part 1 crimes relative to at the targeted locations, but due to
control beats; the one treatment beat the small scale of the project and lim-
where an enforcement POP strategy itations of the research design, these
EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON CRIME 119

crime prevention gains were not this resulted in low levels of treat-
large enough to be detected at the ment during the early months of both
aggregate town level (Criminal Jus- experiments. In the Jersey City
tice Commission 1998, 28). DMAP experiment, this situation
The Minneapolis RECAP experi- was remedied by providing a detailed
ment showed no statistically signifi- crackdown schedule to the narcotics
cant differences in the prevalence of squad commander and extending the
citizen calls for service at addresses experiment from 12 to 15 months.
that received the POP treatment as This problem was remedied in the
compared to control addresses Jersey City POP experiment by
(Sherman, Buerger, and Gartin 1989, changing the leadership of the POP
21). These results were probably due unit, developing an implementation
to the assignment of too many cases accountability system, and providing
to the RECAP unit, thus outstripping additional training in the POP
the amount of resources and atten- approach as well as through other
tion the police officers provided to smaller adjustments.
each address (Buerger 1993). More- The patrol treatment in the Min-
over, the simple randomization pro- neapolis Hot Spots experiment
cedure led to the placing of some of (Sherman and Weisburd 1995, 638-
the highest-event addresses into the 39) was disrupted during summer
treatment group; this led to high months due to a peak in the overall
variability between the treatment calls for service received by the Min-
and control groups and low statisti- neapolis Police Department and a
cal power. Although the overall find- shortage of officers due to vacations;
ings suggest that the RECAP pro- this situation was further compli-
gram was not effective in preventing cated by changes in the computerized
crime, a case study analysis revealed calls for service system implemented
that several addresses experienced in the fall. The changes in the calls
dramatic reductions in total calls for for service system and the disappear-
service (Buerger 1992). ance of differences in patrol dosage
Beyond the RECAP experiment, between treatment and control hot
only three other studies reported spots during summer months were
potential threats to the internal addressed by conducting separate
validity of the research designs. The outcome analyses using different
Jersey City DMAP experiment intervention time periods; there were
(Weisburd and Green 1995, 721) and no substantive differences in the out-
Jersey City POP at Violent Places comes of the experiment across the
experiment (Braga 1997, 107-42) different time periods. Of course,
reported instances where the treat- these implementation problems are
ments were threatened by subver- not unique to these experiments;
sion by the participants. The officers many well-known criminal justice
charged with preventing crime at the field experiments have experienced
treatment hot spots were resistant to and successfully dealt with method-
participating in the programs, and ological difficulties.6
120 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Displacement and the differences of differences in citi-


diffusion effects zen calls for service in two-block
catchment areas surrounding treat-
Five studies examined whether fo-
ment and control hot spot areas. The
cused police efforts were associated
Jersey City POP at Violent Places
with crime displacement or diffusion
experiment found little evidence of
of crime control benefits (see Table 2).
displacement in the catchment areas
Prior to a discussion of the research
and reported significant decreases in
findings, it must be noted that it is
total calls for service and disorder
very difficult to detect displacement
calls for service in the catchment
effects because the potential mani-
areas.7 The Jersey City DMAP exper-
festations of displacement are quite
iment found significant decreases in
diverse. As Barr and Pease (1990)
public morals calls for service and
suggested,
narcotics calls for service in treat-
ment catchment areas relative to
if, in truth, displacement is complete,
controls. The Jersey City DMAP
some displaced crime will fall outside the
areas and types of crime being studied or experiment also replicated the drug
be so dispersed as to be masked by back- market identification process and
ground variation. . . . No research study, found six new drug hot spots within
however massive, is likely to resolve the two blocks of the treatment locations.
issue. (293) This result suggests that some mod-
est displacement may have occurred,
Diffusion effects are likely to be as but it could not be determined
difficult to assess. All five studies whether these new drug hot spots
were limited to examining immedi- were the result of experimental
ate spatial displacement and diffu- squad actions or control squad
sion effects, that is, whether focused actions or if they would have devel-
police efforts in targeted areas re- oped naturally without any enforce-
sulted in crimes moving around the ment efforts (Weisburd and Green
corner or whether these proximate 1995, 730-31).
areas experienced unintended crime The Kansas City Gun quasi-exper-
control benefits. iment used before and after differ-
None of the five studies reported ence of means tests and Auto Regres-
substantial immediate spatial dis- sive Integrated Moving Average time
placement of crime into areas sur- series analyses to examine whether
rounding the targeted locations. Four gun crimes were displaced into seven
studies suggested possible diffusion beats contiguous to the target beat.
effects associated with the focused None of the contiguous beats showed
police interventions. The two Jersey significant increases in gun crime,
City experiments used the most and two of the contiguous beats
sophisticated methodologies to mea- reported significant decreases in gun
sure immediate spatial displacement crimes. The Houston Targeted Beat
and diffusion effects. In both experi- quasi-experiment examined dis-
ments, the research teams examined placement and diffusion effects by
EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON CRIME 121

conducting simple pre-post compari- prevention efforts do not inevitably


sons of reported part 1 index crimes lead to the displacement of crime
in beats contiguous to the treatment problems (Clarke and Weisburd
beats. The analyses revealed no over- 1994; Hesseling 1994; Eck 1993);
all evidence of displacement, and rather, when displacement was mea-
contiguous beats surrounding three sured, it was quite limited, and often,
targeted beats (one POP beat and unintended crime prevention bene-
two zero tolerance beats) experi- fits were associated with the hot
enced possible diffusion effects as spots policing programs.
several types of reported index Unfortunately, the results of this
crimes decreased notably. The St. review provide criminal justice pol-
Louis POP at Drug Locations quasi- icy makers and practitioners with lit-
experiment assessed displacement tle insight on what types of policing
effects by comparing trends in calls strategies are most preferable in con-
for service at targeted addresses to trolling crime hot spots. Clearly, the
nontargeted addresses on the same enforcement-oriented strategies
block. Significant increases in calls reviewed here work in preventing
for service at nontargeted addresses crime. We do not know, however,
on the same block were reported in which enforcement strategies are
only one of the three analyses. The more effective in preventing crime
primary cause of the observed dis- and under what circumstances cer-
placement was a shift in drug sales tain strategies are more appropriate.
from a targeted apartment building This review also offers little insight
to a similar nontargeted apartment on the effectiveness of enforcement
building on the same block. tactics relative to other broader-
based community problem-solving
CONCLUSION policing programs (see, for example,
Skogan and Hartnett 1997). This
The results of this systematic small body of evaluation research
review support the assertion that does not unravel the important ques-
focusing police efforts at high-activ- tion of whether enforcement-ori-
ity crime places can be used to good ented programs result in long-term
effect in preventing crime. Seven of crime reductions in hot spot areas.
nine experimental and quasi-experi- Research suggests that a variety of
mental evaluations reported note- situational factors cause crime to
worthy crime and disorder reduc- cluster at particular places (Eck and
tions. Methodological problems in Weisburd 1995). Proactive patrols,
the research and evaluation designs raids, and crackdowns do not specifi-
probably accounted for the lack of cally address the site features and
crime prevention gains in the Minne- facilities that cause specific locations
apolis RECAP and Beenleigh stud- to generate high volumes of crime.
ies. This review also supports the With the exception of the POP pro-
growing body of research evidence grams with limited situational inter-
that suggests that focused crime ventions, the place-oriented inter-
122 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

ventions in this review consisted of Notes


uniform tactics applied across heter- 1. For a discussion of enforcement and
ogeneous places. Perhaps a greater situational problem-oriented policing strate-
focus on changing these criminogenic gies, see Eck (1992).
situational characteristics would 2. There were multiple distinct abstracts
identifying the same study. For example, a doc-
result in longer-lasting crime reduc-
toral dissertation leading to a journal article
tions at crime places. would generate two distinct abstracts describ-
Beyond thinking about the rela- ing the same evaluation.
tive crime prevention value of these 3. A replication of the Kansas City Gun
Project was not included in this review be-
programs, we need to know more
cause the interventions tested did not focus
about community rea ct i on t o specifically on hot spots within the targeted
increased levels of police enforce- beats (McGarrell and Chermak 2000).
ment action. The results of the Kan- 4. One case was excluded from these analy-
sas City Gun quasi-experiment sug- ses because the observational data were inap-
propriately collected (Braga et al. 1999, 564).
gest that residents of communities
5. One case was excluded from these analy-
suffering from high rates of gun vio- ses because it did not have any physical disor-
lence welcome intensive police der in the pretest and posttest periods (Braga
efforts against guns (Shaw 1995). et al. 1999, 564).
However, some observers question 6. The landmark Kansas City Preventive
Patrol Experiment had to be stopped and re-
the fairness and intrusiveness of started three times before it was implemented
such approaches and caution that properly; the patrol officers did not respect the
street searches, especially of young boundaries of the treatment and control areas
men and minorities, look like police (Kelling et al. 1974). Likewise, the design of
harassment (Moore 1980; Kleck the Minneapolis Spouse Abuse Experiment
was modified to a quasi-experiment when ran-
1991). In New York City, although the domization could not be achieved because offi-
gun-oriented policing strategies of cers chose to arrest certain offenders on a
the New York Police Department nonrandom basis (Berk, Smyth, and Sherman
(NYPD) have been credited with a 1988).
decrease in gun homicides (see, for 7. Property crime incidents increased sig-
nificantly while property crime calls for ser-
example, Fagan, Zimring, and Kim vice did not significantly change in the treat-
1998), the aggressive policing tactics ments catchment areas relative to controls.
of the NYPD have been criticized as The research team viewed this result as an ar-
resulting in increased citizen com- tifact of the experiment rather than a substan-
tive finding (Braga et al. 1999, 567-69).
plaints about police misconduct and
abuse of force (Greene 1999). We
need to know more about the appro- References
priate ways to implement increased
enforcement programs in a manner Barr, Robert and Ken Pease. 1990. Crime
Placement, Displacement, and Deflec-
that will not undermine the legiti-
tion. In Crime and Justice: A Review of
macy of the police in the communities
Research. Vol. 12, ed. Michael Tonry
they serve. Future evaluations of hot and Norval Morris. Chicago: Univer-
spots policing initiatives that engage sity of Chicago Press.
enforcement tactics need to focus Berk, Richard, Gordon Smyth, and Law-
closely on the reaction of the commu- rence W. Sherman. 1988. When Ran-
nity to these programs. dom Assignment Fails: Some Lessons
EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON CRIME 123

from the Minneapolis Spouse Abuse Studies. Albany, NY: Harrow and
Experiment. Journal of Quantitative Heston.
Criminology 4:209-23. Clarke, Ronald V. and David L. Weisburd.
Braga, Anthony A. 1997. Solving Violent 1994. Diffusion of Crime Control Ben-
Crime Problems: An Evaluation of the efits: Observations on the Reverse of
Jersey City Police Departments Pilot Displacement. In Crime Prevention
Program to Control Violent Places. Studies. Vol. 2, ed. Ronald V. Clarke.
Ph.D. diss., Rutgers University, NJ. Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press.
Braga, Anthony A., David L. Weisburd, Cohen, Lawrence E. and Marcus Felson.
El i n Wa r i ng, L or r a i ne G r e e n 1979. Social Change and Crime Rate
Mazerolle, William Spelman, and Trends: A Routine Activity Approach.
Francis Gajewski. 1999. Problem-Ori- American Sociological Review 44:588-
ented Policing in Violent Crime 605.
Places: A Randomized Controlled Ex- Cook, Thomas D. and Donald T. Camp-
periment. Criminology 37:541-80. bell. 1979. Quasi-Experimentation:
B r a nt i ng ha m , Pa ul a nd Pa t r i c i a Design and Analysis Issues for Field
Brantingham, eds. 1991. Environmen- Settings. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
tal Criminology. 2nd ed. Prospect Cornish, Derek and Ronald V. Clarke,
Heights, IL: Waveland Press. eds. 1986. The Reasoning Criminal:
Buerger, Michael, ed. 1992. The Crime Rational Choice Perspectives on Of-
Prevention Casebook: Securing High fending. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Crime Locations. Washington, DC: Criminal Justice Commission. 1998.
Crime Control Institute. Beenleigh Calls for Service Project:
Evaluation Report. Brisbane, Austra-
. 1993. Convincing the Recalci-
lia: Author.
trant: Reexamining the Minneapolis
Eck, John E. 1992. Alternative Futures
RECAP Experiment. Ph.D. diss.,
for Policing. In Police Innovation and
Rutgers University, NJ.
Control of the Police, ed. David L.
. 1994. The Problems of Problem Weisburd and Craig Uchida. New
Solving: Resistance, Interdepen- York: Springer-Verlag.
dencies, and Conflicting Interests.
. 1993. The Threat of Crime Dis-
American Journal of Police 13:1-36.
placement. Criminal Justice Abstracts
Caeti, Tory. 1999. Houstons Targeted 25:527-46.
Beat Program: A Quasi-Experimental . 1997. Preventing Crime at Places.
Test of Police Patrol Strategies. Ph.D. In Preventing Crime: What Works,
diss., Sam Houston State University, What Doesnt, Whats Promising, ed.
TX. Lawrence W. Sherman, Denise C.
Campbell, Donald T. and Julian Stanley. Gottfredson, Doris Layton MacKen-
1966. Experimental and Quasi- zie, John E. Eck, Peter Reuter, and
Experimental Designs for Research. Shawn D. Bushway. Washington, DC:
Chicago: Rand McNally. U.S. Department of Justice, National
Caulkins, Jonathan P., Richard C. Institute of Justice.
Larson, and Thomas F. Rich. 1993. Ge- Eck, John E. and Julie Wartell. 1996. Re-
ographys Impact on the Success of Fo- ducing Crime and Drug Dealing by
cused Local Drug Enforcement Opera- Improving Place Management: A Ran-
tions. Socio-Economic Planning domized Experiment (Final report to
Sciences 27:119-30. the San Diego Police Department).
Clarke, Ronald V., ed. 1992. Situational Washington, DC: Crime Control Insti-
Crime Prevention: Successful Case tute.
124 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Eck, John E. and David L. Weisburd. trol. Washington, DC: Brookings Insti-
1995. Crime Places in Crime Theory. tution.
In Crime and Place, ed. John E. Eck Moore, Mark H. 1980. The Police and
and David L. Weisburd. Monsey, NY: Weapons Offenses. Annals of the
Criminal Justice Press. American Academy of Political and
Fagan, Jeffrey, Franklin E. Zimring, and Social Science 452:22-32.
June Kim. 1998. Declining Homicide Novak, Kenneth J., Jennifer Hartman,
in New York City: A Tale of Two Alexander Holsinger, and Michael
Trends. Journal of Criminal Law and Turner. 1999. The Effects of Aggres-
Criminology 88:1277-324. sive Policing of Disorder on Serious
Goldstein, Herman. 1990. Problem- Crime. Policing: An International
Oriented Policing. Philadelphia: Tem- Journal of Police Strategies and Man-
ple University Press. agement 22:171-90.
Green Mazerolle, Lorraine, James F. Petrosino, Anthony, Robert Boruch, Cath
Price, and Jan Roehl. 2000. Civil Rem- Rounding, Steve McDonald, and Iain
edies and Drug Control: A Random- Chalmers. In press. Assembling a So-
ized Field Trial in Oakland, Califor- cial, Psychological, Educational, and
nia. Evaluation Review 24:212-41. Criminological Trials Register
(SPECTR). Evaluation Research in
Greene, Judith A. 1999. Zero Tolerance: A
Education.
Case Study of Police Practices and Pol-
Pierce, Glenn L., Susan Spaar, and
icies in New York City. Crime & Delin-
LeBaron Briggs. 1988. The Character
quency 45:171-81.
of Police Work: Strategic and Tactical
Greenwood, Peter, Jan Chaiken, and Joan
Implications. Boston: Northeastern
Petersilia. 1977. The Investigation
University, Center for Applied Social
Process. Lexington, MA: Lexington
Research.
Books.
Reppetto, Thomas. 1976. Crime Preven-
Hesseling, Rene. 1994. Displacement: A tion and the Displacement Phenome-
Review of the Empirical Literature. In non. Crime & Delinquency 22:166-77.
Crime Prevention Studies. Vol. 3, ed. Shaw, James. 1995. Community Policing
Ronald V. Clarke. Monsey, NY: Crimi- Against Guns: Public Opinion of the
nal Justice Press. Kansas City Gun Experiment. Justice
Hope, Timothy. 1994. Problem-Oriented Quarterly 12:695-710.
Policing and Drug Market Locations: Sherman, Lawrence W. 1995. The Police.
Three Case Studies. In Crime Preven- In Crime, ed. James Q. Wilson and
tion Studies. Vol. 2, ed. Ronald V. Joan Petersilia. San Francisco: ICS
Clarke. Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press.
Press. . 1997. Policing for Crime Preven-
Kelling, George, Anthony Pate, Duane tion. In Preventing Crime: What
Dickman, and Charles Brown. 1974. Works, What Doesnt, Whats Prom-
The Kansas City Preventive Patrol Ex- ising, ed. Lawrence W. Sherman,
periment: A Technical Report. Wash- Denise C. Gottfredson, Doris Layton
ington, DC: Police Foundation. MacKenzie, John E. Eck, Peter Reuter,
Kleck, Gary. 1991. Point Blank: Guns and and Shawn D. Bushway. Washington,
Violence in America. New York: Aldine DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Na-
de Gruyter. tional Institute of Justice.
M c G a r r el l , Ed m und a n d S t e v e n Sherman, Lawrence W., Michael Buerger,
Chermak. 2000. Targeting Firearms and Patrick Gartin. 1989. Repeat Call
Violence Through Directed Police Pa- Address Policing: The Minneapolis
EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON CRIME 125

RECAP Experiment (Final report to Serious Crime. Washington, DC: Gov-


the National Institute of Justice). ernment Printing Office.
Washington, DC: Crime Control Insti- Weisburd, David L. 1997. Reorienting
tute. Criminal Justice Research and Policy:
Sherman, Lawrence W., Patrick Gartin, From the Causes of Criminality to the
and Michael Buerger. 1989. Hot Spots Context of Crime. Washington, DC:
of Predatory Crime: Routine Activities U.S. Department of Justice, National
and the Criminology of Place. Crimi- Institute of Justice.
nology 27:27-56. Weisburd, David L. and Lorraine Green.
Sherman, Lawrence W. and Dennis 1995. Policing Drug Hot Spots: The
Rogan. 1995a. Deterrent Effects of Po- Jersey City Drug Market Analysis Ex-
lice Raids on Crack Houses: A Ran- periment. Justice Quarterly 12:711-
domized Controlled Experiment. Jus- 36.
tice Quarterly 12:755-82. Weisburd, David L., Lisa Maher, and
Lawrence W. Sherman. 1992. Con-
. 1995b. Effects of Gun Seizures on
trasting Crime General and Crime
Gun Violence: Hot Spots Patrol in
Specific Theory: The Case of Hot Spots
Kansas City. Justice Quarterly 12:673-
of Crime. Advances in Criminological
94.
Theory 4:45-69.
Sherman, Lawrence W. and David L. Weisburd, David L., Lawrence W.
Weisburd. 1995. General Deterrent Sherman, and Anthony J. Petrosino.
Effects of Police Patrol in Crime Hot 1990. Registry of Randomized Experi-
Spots: A Randomized, Controlled ments in Criminal Sanctions, 1950-
Trial. Justice Quarterly 12:625-48. 1983. Los Altos, CA: Sociometrics Cor-
Skogan, Wesley G. and Susan M. poration, Data Holdings of the Na-
Hartnett. 1997. Community Policing, tional Institute of Justice.
Chicago Style. New York: Oxford Uni- Wilson, James Q. and George Kelling.
versity Press. 1982. Broken Windows: The Police
Spelman, William and Dale Brown. 1984. and Neighborhood Safety. Atlantic
Calling the Police: Citizen Reporting of Monthly Mar.:29-38.
ANNALS, AAPSS, 578, November 2001

THE ANNALS
EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL
OF THE AMERICAN
BOOT
ACADEMY
CAMPS ON OFFENDING

Effects of Correctional
Boot Camps on Offending

By DORIS LAYTON MACKENZIE, DAVID B. WILSON,


and SUZANNE B. KIDER

ABSTRACT: A systematic review incorporating meta-analytic tech-


niques of correctional boot camps studies was conducted. An inten-
sive search identified 771 documents of which 144 were deemed po-
tentially relevant, located, and evaluated for eligibility. In 37
documents, 29 studies were judged eligible for inclusion in the sys-
tematic review. The 29 studies resulted in 44 samples providing the
primary unit of analysis. Quasi-experimental and experimental stud-
ies evaluated a residential program with a militaristic environment
and compared the recidivism of participants to a comparison group
receiving another correctional sanction. In 9 studies, boot camp par-
ticipants had lower recidivism than did comparison groups; in 8, com-
parison groups had lower recidivism; and in the remaining studies, no
significant differences were found. A meta-analysis found no overall
significant differences in recidivism between boot camp participants
and comparison samples. Further analyses indicated the results can-
not be explained by differences in study methodology, offender char-
acteristics, or boot camp program components.

Doris Layton MacKenzie is the director of the Evaluation Research Group and a pro-
fessor in the Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice, University of Maryland.
Her research interests include evaluation of correctional programs, correctional alter-
natives, correctional boot camps, self-report criminal activities, and offender behavior.
David B. Wilson is an assistant professor of the administration of justice at George
Mason University. His research interests include program evaluation research method-
ology, meta-analysis, crime and general problem behavior prevention programs, and ju-
venile delinquency intervention effectiveness.
Suzanne B. Kider is a masters student in the Department of Criminology and Crimi-
nal Justice at the University of Maryland. Her main research interests include correc-
tions and probation and parole reentry.

126
EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL BOOT CAMPS ON OFFENDING 127

C ORRECTIONAL boot camps,


also called shock or intensive in-
carceration, are short-term incarcer-
programming, while others focus on
discipline and rigorous physical
training. Programs also differ in
ation programs modeled after basic whether they are designed to be
training in the military (MacKenzie alternatives to probation or to prison.
and Parent 1992; MacKenzie and In some jurisdictions judges sen-
Hebert 1996). Participants are re- tence participants to the camps; in
quired to follow a rigorous daily others, participants are identified by
schedule of activities including drill department of corrections personnel
and ceremony and physical training. from those serving terms of incarcer-
They rise early each morning and are ation. Another difference among pro-
kept busy most of the day. Correc- grams is whether the residential
tional officers are given military ti- phase is followed by an aftercare or
tles, and participants are required to reentry program designed to assist
use these titles when addressing the participants with adjustment to
staff. Staff and inmates are required the community.
to wear uniforms. Punishment for Correctional boot camps were first
misbehavior is immediate and swift opened in adult correctional systems
and usually involves some type of in the United States in 1983, in Geor-
physical activity like push-ups. Fre- gia and Oklahoma. Since that time
quently, groups of inmates enter the they have rapidly grown, first within
boot camps as squads or platoons. adult correctional systems and later
There is often an elaborate intake in juvenile corrections. Today, correc-
ceremony where inmates are imme- tional boot camps exist in federal,
diately required to follow the rules, state, and local juvenile and adult
respond to staff in an appropriate jurisdictions in the United States.
way, stand at attention, and have Juvenile boot camps developed later
their heads shaved. Many programs than the adult camps. However, dur-
have graduation ceremonies for ing the 1990s camps for juveniles
those who successfully complete the rapidly developed, and by 2000, 70
program. Frequently, family mem- juvenile camps had been opened in
bers and others from the outside pub- the United States (see the Koch
lic attend the graduation ceremonies. Crime Institute Web site at www.kci.
While there are some basic simi- org). The camps for adjudicated juve-
larities among the correctional boot niles differ somewhat from the adult
camps, the programs differ greatly in camps. In juvenile camps, less
other aspects (MacKenzie and emphasis is placed on hard labor, and
Hebert 1996). For example, the as required by law, the camps offer
camps differ in the amount of focus academic education. Juvenile camps
given to the physical training and are also apt to provide more thera-
hard labor aspects of the program peutic components. However, in
versus therapeutic programming many other aspects the juvenile
such as academic education, drug camps are similar to adult camps
treatment, or cognitive skills. Some with rigorous intake procedures,
camps emphasize the therapeutic shaved heads, drill and ceremony,
128 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

physical training, immediate physi- protect the public, rehabilitate the


cal punishment for misbehavior (for offenders, reduce costs, and lower
example, push-ups), and graduation recidivism (Gowdy 1996). Thus,
ceremonies. except for reducing the costs of cor-
Despite their continuing popular- rections, all of the major goals are
ity, correctional boot camps remain associated in some way with reduc-
controversial. Primarily, the debate ing the criminal activities of partici-
involves questions about the impact pants. Sufficient time has now
of the camps on the adjustment and elapsed since the beginning of these
behavior of participants while they camps so that a body of research
are in residence and after they are examining the impact of the camps
released. According to advocates, the on the recidivism of participants has
atmosphere of the camps is condu- been produced. This systematic
cive to positive growth and change review is designed to examine this
(Clark and Aziz 1996; MacKenzie research in order to draw conclusions
and Hebert 1996). In contrast, critics regarding what is currently known
argue that many of the components about the effectiveness of correc-
of the camps are in direct opposition tional boot camps in reducing
to the type of relationships and sup- recidivism.
portive conditions that are needed
for quality therapeutic programming METHOD
(Andrews et al. 1990; Gendreau, Lit-
tle, and Goggin 1996; Morash and
Rucker 1990; Sechrest 1989). Search strategy and
eligibility criteria
Research examining the effective-
ness of the correctional boot camps The scope of this review was
has focused on various potential experimental and quasi-experimen-
impacts of the camps. Some have tal evaluations that examined boot
examined whether the camps change camp and boot camplike programs
participants attitudes, attachments for juvenile and adult offenders. To
to the community, or impulsivity be eligible to be included in the
(MacKenzie et al. 2001; MacKenzie review a study had to (1) examine a
and Shaw 1990; MacKenzie and residential program that incorpo-
Souryal 1995). Others have exam- rated a militaristic environment (the
ined the impact of the camps on the programs were called by various
need for prison bed space (MacKen- names such as boot camp, shock
zie and Piquero 1994; MacKenzie incarceration, and intensive incar-
and Parent 1991). However, the ceration); (2) include a comparison
research receiving the most interest group that received either commu-
appears to be that examining the nity supervision (for example, proba-
impact of the camps on recidivism tion) or incarceration in an alterna-
(MacKenzie 1997). tive facility such as jail, prison, or
According to a survey of state cor- juvenile residential facility; (3)
rectional officials, the major goals of include participants who were con-
the camps are to deter future crime, victed or adjudicated; and (4) report a
EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL BOOT CAMPS ON OFFENDING 129

postprogram measure of criminal documents (see references). The


behavior, such as arrest or conviction majority of these studies were state
(the measure may be based on official or federal technical reports (n = 22).
records or self-report and may be Only 9 of these studies were pub-
reported on a dichotomous or contin- lished in peer-reviewed journals. One
uous scale). The comparison group in study was conducted in Canada, and
a quasi-experimental design had to another study was conducted in Eng-
be selected to be reasonably similar land. The remaining studies evalu-
to the experimental group; thus any ated boot camp programs in the
study that compared the experimen- United States.
tal group to a general national or
state sample was eliminated from Data collection
the study. Furthermore the study eli- and analysis
gibility criteria eliminated quasi- The coding protocol developed for
experimental designs that only com- the synthesis allowed for the coding
pared program dropouts to program of multiple samples from a single
completers. study (distinct evaluations reported
The strategies used to identify all in a single report, different cohorts or
studies, published or otherwise, that data reported for males and females
met these criteria included a key- separately). This resulted in 44 dis-
word search of computerized data- tinct samples, and these samples rep-
bases and contact with authors work- resent the primary unit of analysis
ing in this area. The following for this systematic review. The cod-
databases were searched: Criminal ing protocol also allowed for the cod-
Justice Periodical Index, Disserta- ing of multiple indicators of criminal
tion Abstracts Online, Government involvement, such as arrest, convic-
Publications Office Monthly Catalog, tion, and technical violation, mea-
Government Publications Reference sured at multiple time points follow-
File, National Criminal Justice Ref- ing release from the program. A copy
erence Service, PsychINFO, Sociolog- of the coding protocol can be obtained
ical Abstracts, Social SciSearch, and from the authors. All studies were
U.S. Political Science Documents. double coded, and any discrepancies
The keywords used were boot in the coding between the two coders
camp(s), intensive incarceration, were resolved.
and shock incarceration. Several of The protocol captured aspects of
the searched databases indexed the research design, including meth-
unpublished works. This identified odological quality, characteristics of
771 unique documents. Review of the the boot camp program, comparison
titles and abstracts suggested that group condition, study participants,
152 might meet the above criteria or outcome measures, and direction and
were relevant review articles that magnitude of the observed effects.
might contain additional references. The primary effect of interest was
Of these 152, 144 were obtained and recidivism or a return to criminal
evaluated for eligibility, resulting in activity on the part of the offender
29 eligible studies reported in 37 after leaving the program. Recid-
130 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

ivism data were reported dichoto- recidivism are reported in the follow-
mously across all studies and were ing analyses.
based on official records, generally
reflected as arrest, reconviction, or
RESULTS
reinstitutionalization. As such, the
natural index of effectiveness is the The distribution of recidivism
odds ratio (see Fleiss, 1994) and was effects across the 44 boot camp ver-
the index of effect (see below). The sus comparison group samples is
mean odds ratio and homogeneity of shown in Figure 1. Each row of this
effects across studies was computed forest plot represents a distinct sam-
using the inverse variance weight ple, identified by the label in the left
method. A random-effects model was column. The recidivism odds ratio
assumed, and the random-effects (effect size) is represented by the
variance component was estimated small diamonds, and the line spans
using the methods outlined by the 95 percent confidence interval
Dersimonian and Laird (1986) and around the odds ratio. The samples
Raudenbush (1994). The computa- are sorted with the largest positive
tions were performed using macros effect at the top and the smallest neg-
written by the second author that are ative effect (odds ratios between 1
available for use with SAS, SPSS, and 0) on the bottom. At the very bot-
and Stata (Lipsey and Wilson 2001). tom of the plot is the overall random-
A total of 155 recidivism effect effects mean odds ratio.
sizes were extracted from the stud- The effects across these studies
ies. Recidivism effects that reflected ranged from large reductions to large
technical violations only were increases in the risk of recidivating
excluded from the analyses reported for the boot camp participants rela-
below, reducing the set of effect sizes tive to the comparison groups. The
to 142. The recidivism effects were overall mean odds ratio was 1.02 (95
examined in two ways. First, multi- percent confidence interval of 0.90 to
ple recidivism effects from a single 1.17), indicating an almost equal
study and sample were averaged odds of recidivating between the boot
prior to analysis, producing a set of camp and comparison groups, on
44 recidivism effect sizes for the average. Thus there appears to be no
analysis. The second set of analyses relationship between program par-
used arrest as the measure of recidi- ticipation (boot camp or comparison)
vism if it was available; if not, and recidivism. The equivalent recid-
reconvictions were used as the mea- ivism rates for the average boot camp
sure, and if neither of these was and comparison group, given this
available, reinstitutionalizations overall odds ratio, would be 49.4 per-
were used. The results from the two cent for the boot camp and 50 percent
methods of measuring recidivism for the comparison condition. This is
were compared and did not yield any a small difference by most any stan-
substantive differences in the dard. Thus, overall, the evidence sug-
results. Therefore, results based on gests that boot camps do not reduce
the second method of measuring the risk of recidivism relative to
EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL BOOT CAMPS ON OFFENDING 131

FIGURE 1
A FOREST PLOT SHOWING THE RECIDIVISM ODDS RATIOS AND
95 PERCENT CONFIDENCE INTERVAL FOR EACH STUDY AND
SAMPLE AND THE OVERALL MEAN ODDS RATIO

Favors Comparison Favors Bootcamp


Fl. Dept. of JJ (Martin Co.), 1997
Farrington, et al., 2000
Fl. Dept. of JJ (Polk Co., Boys), 1997
Jones (FY97), 1998
Jones (FY94-95), 1998
Mackenzie & Souryal (Illinois), 1994
Mackenzie & Souryal (Louisiana), 1994
Jones (FY91-93), 1998
Mackenzie & Souryal (Florida), 1994
Jones (FY96), 1998
Marcus-Mendoza (Men), 1995
Flowers, Carr, & Ruback 1991
Fl. Dept. of JJ (Leon Co.), 1996
Mackenzie & Souryal (Oklahoma), 1994
T3 Associates, 2000
Mackenzie & Souryal (New York), 1994
Peters (Mobile, AL), 1996b
Camp & Sandhu, 1995
Zhang, 2000
Mackenzie & Souryal (S.C., New), 1994
Jones, 1996
Zhang, 2000
NY DCS (88-96 Releases), 2000
Marcus-Mendoza (Women), 1995
Farrington, et al., 2000
Harer & Klein-Saffran, 1996
Kempinem & Kurlychek, 2001
Austin, Jones, & Bolyard, 1993
Burns & Vito, 1995
Peters (Denver, CO), 1996a
Fl. Dept. of JJ (Bay Co.), 1997
Mackenzie, et al. 1997
CA Dept. of the Youth Authority, 1997
NY DCS (96-97 Releases), 2000
NY DCS (97-98 Releases), 2000
Fl. Dept. of JJ (Pinellas Co.), 1996
Fl. Dept. of JJ (Manatee Co.), 1996
Boyles, Bokenkamp, & Madura, 1996
Mackenzie & Souryal (S.C., Old), 1994
Fl. Dept. of JJ (Polk Co., Girls), 1997
Jones, 1997
Thomas & Peters, 1996
Wright & Mays, 1998
Mackenzie & Souryal (Georgia), 1994
Overall Mean Odds-Ratio

.1 .25 .50 .75 1 2 5 10


Odds-Ratio

other existing criminal justice sys- between the boot camp samples and
tem forms of punishment and reha- the comparisons.
bilitation. From the forest plot, it is The distribution of odds ratios was
also evident that 9 studies observed a highly heterogeneous, Q = 464.6, df =
statistically significant positive ben- 43, p < .0001, suggesting the pres-
efit of boot camps, whereas 8 studies ence of moderators of the effects,
observed a statistically significant either methodological or substantive,
positive benefit of the comparison such as the nature of the boot camp
condition. The remaining 27 studies program and comparison conditions
found no significant differences and the types of offenders served.
132 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 1
CROSS-TABULATION OF QUALITATIVE METHODOLOGICAL
QUALITY SCORE AND OTHER METHOD DESCRIPTORS (N = 44)

Qualitative Methodological Quality Score


Method Variable 4 (n = 19) 3 (n = 17) 2 (n = 8)

Randomly assigned participants to conditions


Yes 4 (21) 1 (6) 0 (0)
No 15 (79) 16 (94) 8(100)
Used group-level matching**
Yes 14 (74) 5 (29) 1 (13)
No 5 (26) 12 (71) 7 (87)
Prospective research design**
Yes 17 (89) 9 (53) 6 (75)
No 2 (11) 8 (47) 2 (25)
Used statistical controls in analyses**
Yes 13 (68) 3 (18) 1 (13)
No 6 (32) 14 (82) 7 (87)
Boot camp dropouts in analysis**
Yes 9 (47) 9 (53) 0 (0)
No 10 (53) 8 (47) 8(100)
Overall attrition apparent
Yes 3 (16) 2 (12) 1 (12)
No 16 (84) 15 (88) 7 (88)
Differential attrition apparent
Yes 3 (16) 3 (18) 2 (25)
No 16 (84) 14 (82) 6 (75)
NOTE: Percentages are in parentheses.
**p < .05, based on a chi-square test.

Possible moderating effects are developed by Sherman and col-


explored below. leagues (1997) and has five levels of
methodological rigor. The lowest
Methodological characteristics level of methodological quality was
of the studies excluded from this synthesis and
reflects studies without a compari-
Any conclusion regarding the
son group. The highest level of meth-
effectiveness (or ineffectiveness, as
odological rigor (level 5) represents
the data suggest) of boot camps rela-
randomized designs that are not
tive to more traditional correctional
approaches in reducing the risk of compromised through attrition or
recidivism is valid only if the meth- other common problems in carrying
odological quality of this collection of out a randomized evaluation study.
studies is sufficiently high. Table 1 As can be seen in Table 1, none of
displays the frequency of studies the five randomized evaluations
with various methodological charac- included in this synthesis were
teristics by our qualitative method- granted a method quality score of 5.
ological rating scale. This scale was This was generally because the stud-
EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL BOOT CAMPS ON OFFENDING 133

ies had high attrition or excluded different from each other (for exam-
program dropouts from the recidi- ple, confidence interval includes 1).
vism analysis, creating a potential In contrast to studies that did not use
threat from selection bias. Thus statistical controls in the analysis of
there were no evaluations of the recidivism outcomes, studies that
effectiveness of boot camps that were used controls observed smaller
free from methodological blemishes. effects that were negative in direc-
That said, however, many of the stud- tion. Once again, neither category
ies (19 of 44, or 43 percent) were differed significantly from the null
judged to be methodologically solid hypothesis. All other methodological
(method score of 4). These studies variables were unrelated to the
were generally the higher-quality observed odds ratios.
quasi-experimental designs that
either carefully selected the compari- Offender characteristics
son group so as to maximize similar- across studies
ity with the boot camp group (for There was generally little infor-
example, selecting boot camp eligible mation regarding the characteristics
offenders and matching the groups of the offenders in the studies. For 11
on demographic characteristics) or of the 44 samples, the authors did not
used statistical controls in the analy- indicate the gender, although it is
sis of recidivism effects. Only 8 of the reasonable to assume that in these
44 evaluations (18 percent) were cases the samples were all male.
judged to be of poor methodological Only 3 of the 44 samples were all
quality. female, and the mean odds ratio for
To assess the robustness of the these samples was 1.06 and statisti-
general finding of no effect, a sepa- cally nonsignificant. This mean odds
rate mean odds ratio was computed ratio is roughly the same as that for
for each category of the different the overall sample. Four samples
methodological variables (see Table were mixed gender, although they
2). The mean effect size was slightly were predominantly male (equal to
lower for the studies judged to be of or greater than 80 percent). Thus
overall higher methodological qual- there are insufficient data to ade-
ity, although the trend was statisti- quately explore whether boot camps
cally nonsignificant. Studies that are differentially effective for males
used a prospective research design and females, as some theorists have
had observed larger positive effects hypothesized (Morash and Rucker
(although not significantly different 1990).
from a null odds ratio of 1) than did All samples were successfully
retrospective designs. That is, while classified as either juvenile or adult.
the mean odds ratio of prospective The adult samples were typically
and retrospective designs are signifi- young adults and in some cases
cantly different from each other, nei- included at least a small percentage
ther design produces an odds ratio of juveniles who were adjudicated as
that suggests that the experimental adults. As shown in Table 3, the mean
and control samples are significantly odds ratio for the studies evaluating
134 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 2
MEAN ODDS RATIO AND 95 PERCENT CONFIDENCE
INTERVAL BY METHOD VARIABLES (N = 44)

95 Percent
Confidence Interval
Mean
Method Variable Odds Ratio Lower Upper ka

Qualitative methodological quality score


Random assignment, not degraded 0
High-quality quasi-experiment 0.92 0.73 1.15 19
Standard quasi-experiment 1.07 0.85 1.34 17
Poor-quality quasi-experiment 1.15 0.84 1.59 8
Randomly assigned participants to conditions
Yes 0.75 0.48 1.17 5
No 1.06 0.91 1.24 39
Used group-level matching
Yes 1.11 0.88 1.40 20
No 0.97 0.80 1.17 24
Prospective research design**
Yes 1.13 0.95 1.34 32
No 0.83 0.65 1.06 12
Used statistical controls in analyses**
Yes 0.85 0.68 1.07 17
No 1.14 0.96 1.37 27
Boot camp dropouts in analysis
Yes 1.03 0.82 1.28 18
No 1.02 0.83 1.24 26
No overall attrition apparent
Yes 1.06 0.91 1.24 39
No 0.72 0.46 1.14 5
No differential attrition apparent
Yes 1.03 0.87 1.21 36
No 0.96 0.67 1.41 8
a. k = number of samples included in analysis.
**p < .05.

the effectiveness of juvenile boot Juvenile detention facilities are more


camps was lower than that of the likely, although not guaranteed, to
studies evaluating adult (often have a greater emphasis on rehabili-
young adult) boot camps, although tation than their adult counterparts.
this difference was not statistically Unfortunately, the availability of
significant. This difference may rehabilitative treatment within the
reflect a difference in the typical com- compari s on f aci l i t i es was n ot
parison group for juveniles relative reported by the primary studies.
to adults. Traditional juvenile deten- The racial/ethnic makeup of the
tion facilities are qualitatively differ- of f en der popu l at i on s an d t h e
ent from adult prison or adult proba- offender risk level were often unre-
tion, the common comparison groups ported, with no information available
for the studies of adult boot camps. for 9 of the 44 samples (20 percent).
EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL BOOT CAMPS ON OFFENDING 135

TABLE 3
MEAN ODDS RATIO AND 95 PERCENT CONFIDENCE
INTERVAL BY OFFENDER CHARACTERISTICS (N = 44)

95 Percent
Confidence Interval
Mean
Offender Characteristic Odds Ratio Lower Upper ka

Age group of offender


Juvenile 0.88 0.68 1.14 16
Adult 1.09 0.92 1.30 28
Offender type
Juveniles
Nonviolent/nonperson crimes 0.92 0.61 1.38 4
Mixed (violent and nonviolent) crimes 0.85 0.65 1.11 12
Adults
Nonviolent/nonperson crimes 1.17 0.92 1.50 13
Mixed (violent and nonviolent) crimes 1.01 0.79 1.31 15
a. k = number of samples included in analysis.

For an additional 8 samples, only the drug abuse counseling, vocational


percentage of African Americans was education, and aftercare transition
reported. Thus roughly half of the assistance, the greater the likelihood
samples had complete racial/ethnic that boot camps will have positive
makeup information. In general, benefits relative to alternative cor-
African Americans were the predom- rectional approaches, such as prison
inant racial group, representing and probation. To assess this issue,
roughly 52 percent of the samples we coded whether the evaluation
reporting this information. Cauca- report described the boot camp pro-
sians represented 23 percent of the gram as providing various rehabili-
24 samples, and Hispanics repre- tative programs listed in Table 4.
sented roughly 9 percent of the 21 Mean odd ratios were computed sep-
samples reporting these data. The arately for juvenile and adult
data did not lend themselves to an programs.
analysis of the relationship between The only program characteristic
racial/ethnic makeup of the samples that showed a strong relationship to
and the observed odds ratios. the effectiveness of the boot camp
programs was the presence of an
Programmatic differences aftercare treatment component for
across studies the adult programs. The 11 odds
ratios for boot camps with an after-
Boot camps vary in the emphasis care component versus comparison
placed on rehabilitative treatment group contrasts had a mean of 1.46
relative to physical exercise and mili- with a 95 percent confidence interval
tary drill and ceremony. It has been that did not include 1, indicating a
speculated that the greater the statistically significant positive
emphasis on treatments, such as effect. This evidence suggests that
136 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 4
MEAN ODDS RATIO AND 95 PERCENT CONFIDENCE INTERVAL BY
PROGRAM CHARACTERISTICS (JUVENILES n = 16, ADULTS n = 28)

95 Percent
Confidence Interval
Program Characteristic Mean Odds Ratio Lower Upper ka

Aftercare treatment component


Juveniles
Yes 0.88 0.70 1.12 14
No 0.79 0.44 1.43 2
Adults***
Yes 1.46*** 1.14 1.87 11
No 0.89 0.72 1.10 17
Academic education
Juveniles
Yes 0.88 0.68 1.14 16
No 0
Adults
Yes 1.13 0.93 1.38 24
No 0.86 0.51 1.43 4
Vocational education
Juveniles
Yes 0.98 0.62 1.55 3
No 0.84 0.66 1.08 13
Adults*
Yes 0.82 0.56 1.20 6
No 1.17* 0.97 1.43 22
Drug treatment
Juveniles
Yes 0.90 0.70 1.15 12
No 0.78 0.49 1.24 4
Adults
Yes 1.08 0.88 1.33 22
No 1.12 0.73 1.72 6
Counseling (group and individual)
Juveniles
Yes 0.91 0.70 1.17 10
No 0.79 0.52 1.18 6
Adults
Yes 1.17 0.95 1.44 21
No 0.85 0.58 1.26 7
Manual labor
Juveniles
Yes 1.03 0.73 1.44 7
No* 0.79 0.61 1.02 9
Adults
Yes 1.07 0.88 1.31 24
No 1.22 0.73 2.04 4
a. k = number of samples included in analysis.
*p .10. ***p .01.
EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL BOOT CAMPS ON OFFENDING 137

aftercare may be important in reduc- after adjusting for methods features.


ing the risk of recidivism, at least for The counterintuitive finding regard-
adult samples. ing vocational education was not
A counterintuitive finding is the robust to method difference; that is, it
negative relationship between voca- was statistically nonsignificant once
tional education and odds ratio for conditioned on method features. This
the adult samples. Study samples reinforces our hunch that this find-
with vocational education had a ing was the result of a confounding of
lower mean odds ratio than did those study features and not due to any
without. The number of boot camp negative effects of vocational edu-
programs with vocational education cation. No new significant study
was small, however, raising the pos- characteristics emerged in the multi-
sibility that this relationship is con- variate analyses.
founded with other study differences.
DISCUSSION
Multivariate analysis of effect
AND CONCLUSION
size and study characteristics
The simple univariate analyses of In our overall meta-analysis of
the relationships between odds recidivism, we found no differences
ratios and study characteristics do between the boot camp and compari-
not take into account the possible son samples. Our analysis predicts
confounding of study features. To that if the comparison samples recid-
assess this possibility, a mixed- ivism is estimated to be 50 percent,
effects regression model (see Lipsey the boot camp samples recidivism
and Wilson 2001; Raudenbush 1994) would be estimated to be 49.4 per-
was estimated, regressing the logged cent, or only 0.6 percent lower. When
odds ratios onto study features. The the individual studies were exam-
basic model included the major ined, no significant differences were
methodological features, accounting found between the boot camp sam-
for significant variability in odds ples and the comparisons in the
ratios across studies, R2 = .28, Q = majority of the studies. In only 17
16.19, df = 7, p = .02. Significant vari- samples out of the total of 44, a signif-
ability remained, however, after icant difference between the experi-
accounting for methodological differ- mental and control samples was
ences. Building on this basic methods found; approximately half favored
model, separate regression analyses the boot camp while the remaining
were run for each major program favored the comparisons. Thus, by
characteristic shown in Table 4. whatever criteria are used, there is
Because of the possibility of an inter- no evidence that the boot camps
action between program characteris- reduce recidivism.
tics and offender age, these models The results of this systematic
were run separately for juveniles and review and meta-analysis will be dis-
adults. The finding of a positive bene- appointing for many people. Advo-
fit from aftercare for the adult offend- cates of the programs expect them to
ers remained statistically significant successfully reduce the future
138 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

criminal activities of adults and juve- and (2) whether the participants
niles. Critics argue that the pro- were limited to those convicted
grams are poorly conceived as thera- or adjudicated for nonviolent/
peutic interventions, they will not nonperson crimes or mixed violent
reduce recidivism, and they may and nonviolent crimes. Again we
actually have the opposite effect by found no evidence that differences in
increasing criminal activities. Our these characteristics explained the
results do not support either side of differences in the results.
this argument because we found no We were able to code and analyze
differences in recidivism between the the possible impact of six program
44 boot camp samples and the com- characteristics, including whether
parisons. Correctional boot camps the boot camps had aftercare, aca-
are neither as good as the advocates demic education, vocational educa-
assert nor as bad as the critics tion, drug treatment, counseling, or
hypothesize. manual labor components. It is
An examination of the forest plot important to note that this informa-
of the individual studies (see Figure tion was limited to general informa-
1) and our analysis of the data dem- tion about the characteristics of the
onstrated large differences in the programs. We assume the quality
studies in terms of the effect of boot and intensity of the programs dif-
camps. Some studies found boot fered greatly. From our knowledge of
camp participants did better than the boot camps we know that some
the comparisons, and others found programs consider Narcotics Anony-
comparison samples did better. For mous or Alcoholics Anonymous meet-
this reason, we explored whether the ings drug treatment, whereas others
differences among studies could be provide a more intensive drug treat-
attributed to the methods or design ment experience using a Therapeutic
of the studies or to characteristics of Communitytype model. We did not
the programs or individual partici- have enough information to code
pants. In our examination of the such differences. Almost no informa-
methodological variables, we did not tion was given about what happened
find any evidence that differences in to the comparison samples. The
the results of studies could be potential impact of these differences
explained by the study methodology. on recidivism cannot be overlooked.
Our examination of the offender When we examined the impact of
characteristics was disappointing program characteristics, the only dif-
because very few studies reported ferences we found were for adult
sufficient information to enable us to studies and, after controlling for
code and analyze the possible impact methodological differences, the only
of these characteristics on study out- difference was for boot camps that
comes. Few studies even reported on included an aftercare component. In
the gender of the samples. The only other words, whereas the odds ratios
variables we could examine were (1) differed for boot camps with and
whether the studies focused on adult without aftercare, in neither case did
offenders or adjudicated juveniles, the boot camp samples differ
EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL BOOT CAMPS ON OFFENDING 139

significantly from the comparisons. jurisdictions that also provide such


While the recidivism of releasees treatment to those in the comparison
from boot camps with aftercare dif- programs within the jurisdiction.
fered from the recidivism of releasees Thus, in terms of the type of treat-
from boot camps without aftercare, ment or therapy that has been shown
there were no significant differences to be effective, correctional programs
in recidivism between boot camp within the same jurisdictions will be
releasees and comparisons for either similar. The boot camps may only dif-
type of boot camp (for example, with fer from other correctional programs
or without aftercare). Thus we were in the same jurisdiction in the mili-
unable to identify any characteristic tary aspects and not in therapy and
of the methods, offenders, or pro- treatment. It seems likely that the
grams that would explain differences therapy and treatment are the
in results of the studies. important components in reducing
Why dont boot camps reduce recidivism. Therefore, since boot
recidivism when compared to other camps and other correctional pro-
correctional alternatives? In our grams provide similar therapy and
opinion, one possible reason boot treatment, the impact on recidivism
camps are not any more or less effec- will be similar.
tive than other alternatives is
The research demonstrates that
because they may offer no more ther-
there are no differences in recidivism
apy or treatment than the alterna-
when boot camp samples are com-
tives. That is, boot camps by them-
pared to those who receive other cor-
selves have little to offer as far as
rectional sanctions. In our opinion,
moving offenders away from criminal
this can be interpreted to show that a
activities. Sufficient research cur-
military atmosphere in a correc-
rently exists to demonstrate that
appropriate correctional treatment tional setting is not effective in
with particular characteristics can reducing recidivism. However, many
be effective in changing offenders questions remain. It would be partic-
(Andrews and Bonta 1998; Gendreau ularly valuable to have more infor-
and Ross 1987; Lipsey 1992). Some mation about the characteristics of
boot camps incorporate this type of the participants, and the components
treatment and therapy into the of the programs, both for the boot
regime of the camps, while others do camps and for the comparisons. From
not. Similarly, some comparison these studies, we were able to code
facilities or programs provide such very little of this information. We
treatment. Almost all studies com- anticipate that programs with more
pared offenders or juveniles in boot treatment and therapy will be more
camps to others in correctional pro- successful in reducing recidivism.
grams within the same jurisdictions. The question is whether this would
We hypothesize that there are simi- explain some of the differences in
larities within jurisdictions such results across studies. Future
that boot camps with therapy and research would greatly benefit by
treatment w ill be locat ed i n increasing the amount of detailed
140 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

information about the programs and Eight States. Criminology


the participants. 33:327-57.
MacKenzie, Doris Layton, James
W. Shaw, and Voncile B. Gowdy.
1990. Evaluation of Shock In-
APPENDIX
carceration in Louisiana, Execu-
SECONDARY SOURCES USED
tive Summary. Washington, DC:
IN THE META-ANALYSIS
U.S. Department of Justice, Na-
tional Institute of Justice.
1. Burns and Vito (1995) 7. Peters (1996a), Peters (1996b), and
Burns, Jerald C. 1994. A Compara- Thomas and Peters (1996)
tive Analysis of the Alabama Peters, Michael, David Thomas,
Department of Corrections Boot and Christopher Zamberlan.
Camp Program. Unpublished 1997. Boot Camps for Juvenile
Ph.D. diss., University of Ala- Offenders: Program Summary.
bama, Tuscaloosa. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department
2. State of New York Department of of Justice, National Institute of
Correctional Services (2000) Justice.
Courtright, Kevin E. 1991. An
NOTE: Secondary sources are shown after
Overview and Evaluation of
the primary sources included in the reference
Shock Incarceration in New
list.
York State. Unpublished mas-
ters thesis, Mercyhurst College,
Erie, PA. References
3. Marcus-Mendoza (1995)
Andrews, Donald A. and James Bonta.
Holley, Philip D. and David E.
1998. The Psychology of Criminal Con-
Wright. 1995. Oklahomas Regi-
duct. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson.
mented Inmate Discipline Pro-
Andrews, Donald A., Ivan Zinger, Robert
gram for Males: Its Impact on
D. Hoge, James Bonta, Paul Gendreau,
Recidivism. Journal of the
and Frances T. Cullen. 1990. Does Cor-
Oklahoma Criminal Justice Re-
r e c t i o n a l T r e a t m e n t Wo r k ? A
search Consortium 2:58-70.
Clinically Relevant and Psycho-
4. Kempinem and Kurlychek (2001) logically Informed Meta-Analysis.
Kempinem, Cynthia and Mark Criminology 28:369-404.
Motivans. 1998. Who Goes to *Austin, James, Michael Jones, and Me-
Pennsylvanias Boot Camp? Pa- lissa Bolyard. 1993. Assessing the Im-
per presented at the meeting of pact of a County Operated Boot Camp:
the American Society of Crimi- Evaluation of the Los Angeles County
nology, Washington, DC, Nov. Regimented Inmate Diversion Pro-
5. Harer and Klein-Saffran (1996) gram (NCJRS document reproduction
Klein-Saffran, Jody. 1991. Shock service no. 154401). San Francisco:
I nc a r c er a ti on, B u r e a u o f National Council on Crime and Delin-
Prisons Style. Research Forum quency.
1:1-9.
6. MacKenzie and Souryal (1994)
*References marked with an asterisk indi-
MacK enzie, Doris L., Robert
cate studies included in the meta-analysis. (See
Brame, David McDowall, and appendix for secondary sources used in the
Claire Souryal. 1995. Boot meta-analysis showing primary resource.)
Camp Prisons and Recidivism in
EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL BOOT CAMPS ON OFFENDING 141

*Boyles, Cecilia E., Eric Bokenkamp, and ment Boot Camp: A Follow-Up Study
William Madura. 1996. Evaluation of of the First Five Platoons. Tallahassee:
the Colorado Juvenile Regimented Florida Department of Juvenile Jus-
Training Program. Golden: Colorado tice, Bureau of Data and Research.
Department of Human Services, Divi- *. 1996b. Manatee County Sheriffs
sion of Youth Corrections. Boot Camp: A Follow-Up Study of the
*Burns, Jerald C. and Gennaro F. Vito. First Four Platoons. Tallahassee:
1995. An Impact Analysis of the Ala- Florida Department of Juvenile Jus-
bama Boot Camp Program. Federal tice, Bureau of Data and Research.
Probation 59:63-67. *. 1996c. Pinellas County Boot
*California Department of the Youth Au- Camp: A Follow-Up Study of the First
thority. 1997. LEAD: A Boot Camp and Five Platoons (research rep. no. 33).
Intensive Parole Program; the Final Tallahassee: Florida Department of
Impact Evaluation (Report to the Cali- Juvenile Justice, Bureau of Data and
fornia Legislature). Sacramento: Au- Research.
thor.
*. 1997a. Bay County Sheriffs Of-
*Camp, David A. and Harjit S. Sandhu.
fice Boot Camp: A Follow-Up Study of
1995. Evaluation of Female Offender
the First Seven Platoons (research rep.
Regimented Treatment Program
no. 44). Bay County: Florida Depart-
(FORT). Journal of the Oklahoma
ment of Juvenile Justice, Bureau of
Criminal Justice Research Consor-
Data and Research.
tium 2:50-57.
*. 1997b. Martin County Sheriffs
Clark, Cheryl L. and David W. Aziz. 1996.
Office Boot Camp: A Follow-Up of the
Shock Incarceration in New York
First Four Platoons (research rep. no.
State: Philosophy, Results, and Limi-
43). Martin County: Florida Depart-
tations. In Correctional Boot Camps:
ment of Juvenile Justice, Bureau of
A Tough Intermediate Sanction, ed.
Data and Research.
Doris Layton MacKenzie and Eugene
E. Hebert. Washington, DC: U.S. De- *. 1997c. Polk County Juvenile
partment of Justice, National Insti- Boot Camp: A Follow-Up Study of the
tute of Justice. First Four Platoons. Tallahassee:
Dersimonian, Rebecca and Nan Laird. Florida Department of Juvenile Jus-
1986. Meta-Analysis in Clinical tice, Bureau of Data and Research.
Trials. Controlled Clinical Trials *. 1997d. Polk County Juvenile
7:177-88. Boot CampFemale Program: A Fol-
*Farrington, David P., G. Hancock, M. low-Up Study of the First Seven Pla-
Livingston, Kate Painter, and G. Towl. toons. Polk County: Florida Depart-
2000. Evaluation of Intensive Regimes ment of Juvenile Justice, Bureau of
for Young Offenders (Home Office re- Data and Research.
search findings). London: Home Office *Flowers, Gerald T., Timothy S. Carr, and
Research, Development and Statistics R. Barry Ruback. 1991. Special Alter-
Directorate. native Incarceration Evaluation. At-
Fleiss, Joseph L. 1994. Measures of Effect lanta: Georgia Department of Correc-
Size for Categorical Data. In The tions.
Handbook of Research Synthesis, ed. Gendreau, Paul, Tracy Little, and Claire
Harris Cooper and Larry V. Hedges. E. Goggin. 1996. A Meta-Analysis of
New York: Russell Sage. the Predictors of Adult Offender Re-
*Florida Department of Juvenile Justice. cidivism: What Works! Criminology
1996a. Leon County Sheriffs Depart- 34:575-607.
142 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Gendreau, Paul and Robert R. Ross. 1987. venting Crime: What Works, What
Revivication of Rehabilitation: Evi- Doesnt, Whats Promising, ed. Law-
dence from the 1980s. Justice Quar- r e n c e W. S h e r m a n , D e n i s e C.
terly 4:349-408. Gottfredson, Doris Layton MacKen-
Gowdy, Voncile B. 1996. Historical Per- zie, John Eck, Peter Reuter, and
spective. In Correctional Boot Camps: Shawn Bushway. Washington, DC:
A Tough Intermediate Sanction, ed. U.S. Department of Justice, National
Doris Layton MacKenzie and Eugene Institute of Justice.
E. Hebert. Washington, DC: U.S. De- MacKenzie, Doris Layton and Eugene E.
partment of Justice, National Insti- Hebert, eds. 1996. Correctional Boot
tute of Justice. Camps: A Tough Intermediate Sanc-
*Harer, Miles D. and Jody Klein-Saffran. tion. Washington, DC: U.S. Depart-
1996. An Evaluation of the Federal ment of Justice, National Institute of
Bureau of Prisons Lewisburg Inten- Justice.
sive Confinement Center. Unpub- MacKenzie, Doris Layton and Dale G.
lished manuscript, Federal Bureau of Parent. 1991. Shock Incarceration and
Prisons, Research and Evaluation, Prison Crowding in Louisiana. Jour-
Washington, DC. nal of Criminal Justice 19:225-37.
*Jones, Mark. 1996. Do Boot Camp Grad- MacKenzie, Doris L. and Dale G. Parent.
uates Make Better Probationers? 1992. Boot Camp Prisons for Young
Journal of Crime and Justice 19:1-14. Offenders. In Smart Sentencing: The
*. 1997. Is Less Better? Boot Camp, Emergence of Intermediate Sanctions,
Regular Probation and Rearrest in ed. James M. Byrne, Arthur J. Lurigio,
North Carolina. American Journal of and Joan Petersilia. Newbury Park,
Criminal Justice 21:147-61. CA: Sage.
*Jones, Robert J. 1998. Annual Report to MacKenzie, Doris Layton and Alex
the Governor and the General Assem- Piquero. 1994. The Impact of Shock In-
bly: Impact Incarceration Program. carceration Programs on Prison
Springfield: Illinois Department of Crowding. Crime & Delinquency
Corrections. 40:222-49.
*Kempinem, Cynthia A. and Megan C. MacKenzie, Doris Layton and James W.
Kurlychek. 2001. Pennsylvanias Moti- Shaw. 1990. Inmate Adjustment and
vational Boot Camp (2000 Report to Change During Shock Incarceration.
the Legislature). Quehanna: Pennsyl- Justice Quarterly 7:125-50.
vania Commission on Sentencing. *MacKenzie, Doris L. and Claire Souryal.
Lipsey, Mark. 1992. Juvenile Delin- 1994. Multi-Site Evaluation of Shock
quency Treatment: A Meta-Analytic Incarceration: Executive Summary.
Inquiry into the Variability of Effects. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
In Meta-Analysis for Explanation: A Justice, National Institute of Justice.
Casebook, ed. Thomas Cook, Harris . 1995. Inmate Attitude Change
Cooper, David S. Cordray, Heidi During Incarceration: A Comparison
Hartmann, Larry V. Hedges, Richard of Boot Camp with Traditional Prison.
J. Light, Thomas A. Louis, and Freder- Justice Quarterly 12:325-54.
ick Mosteller. New York: Russell Sage. *MacKenzie, Doris L., Claire Souryal,
Lipsey, Mark W. and David B. Wilson. Miriam Sealock, and Mohammed Bin
2001. Practical Meta-Analysis. Thou- Kashem. 1997. Outcome Study of the
sand Oaks, CA: Sage. Sergeant Henry Johnson Youth Lead-
MacKenzie, Doris L. 1997. Criminal Jus- ership Academy (YLA). Washington,
tice and Crime Prevention. In Pre- DC: University of Maryland, U.S. De-
EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL BOOT CAMPS ON OFFENDING 143

partment of Justice, National Insti- Sherman, Lawerence W., Denise C.


tute of Justice. Gottfredson, Doris Layton MacKen-
MacKenzie, Doris L., David B. Wilson, zie, John Eck, Peter Reuter, and
Gaylene S. Armstrong, and Angela R. Shawn Bushway. 1997. Preventing
Gover. 2001. The Impact of Boot Crime: What Works, What Doesnt,
Camps and Traditional Institutions Whats Promising. Washington, DC:
on Juvenile Residents: Perception, Ad- U.S. Department of Justice, National
justment, and Change. Journal of Re- Institute of Justice.
search in Crime and Delinquency *State of New York Department of Cor-
38:279-313. rectional Services, Division of Parole.
*Marcus-Mendoza, Susan T. 1995. Pre- 2000. The Twelfth Annual Shock Leg-
liminary Investigation of Oklahomas islative Report (Shock Incarceration
Shock Incarceration Program. Jour- and Shock Parole Supervision). Al-
nal of the Oklahoma Criminal Justice bany, NY: Division of Parole.
Research Consortium 2:44-49. *Thomas, David and Michael Peters.
Morash, Merry and Lila Rucker. 1990. A 1996. Evaluation of the Impact of Boot
Critical Look at the Idea of Boot Camp Camps for Juvenile Offenders: Cleve-
as a Correctional Reform. Crime & De- land Interim Report. Fairfax, VA: U.S.
linquency 36:204-22. Department of Justice, Office of Juve-
*Peters, Michael. 1996a. Evaluation of nile Justice and Delinquency Preven-
the Impact of Boot Camps for Juvenile tion.
Offenders: Denver Interim Report. *T3 Associates Training and Consulting.
Fairfax, VA: U.S. Department of Jus- 2000. Project Turnaround Outcome
tice, Office of Juvenile Justice and De- Evaluation. Final Report. Ottawa,
linquency Prevention. Canada: Author.
*. 1996b. Evaluation of the Impact *Wright, Dionne T. and G. Larry Mays.
of Boot Camps for Juvenile Offenders: 1998. Correctional Boot Camps, Atti-
Mobile Interim Report. Fairfax, VA: tudes, and Recidivism: The Oklahoma
U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Experience. Journal of Offender Reha-
Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Pre- bilitation 28:71-87.
vention. *Zhang, Sheldon X. 2000. An Evaluation
Raudenbush, Stephen W. 1994. Random of the Los Angeles County Juvenile
Effects Models. In The Handbook of Drug Treatment Boot Camp (Final Re-
Research Synthesis, ed. Harris Cooper port). Washington, DC: U.S. Depart-
and Larry V. Hedges. New York: Rus- ment of Justice, National Institute of
sell Sage. Justice.
Sechrest, Dale D. 1989. Prison Boot
Camps Do Not Measure Up. Federal
Probation 53:15-20.
ANNALS, AAPSS, 578, November 2001

THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN


COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL PROGRAMS
ACADEMY
FOR OFFENDERS

Cognitive-Behavioral
Programs for Offenders

By MARK W. LIPSEY, GABRIELLE L. CHAPMAN,


and NANA A. LANDENBERGER

ABSTRACT: A systematic review using meta-analytic techniques


was conducted with 14 studies selected to provide the best evidence
on the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral programs for reducing
the reoffense recidivism of criminal offenders. The results indicated
that, overall, cognitive-behavioral programs are effective, and the
best of them are capable of producing sizable reductions in recidi-
vism. Many of the available studies, however, investigate research-
oriented demonstration programs; the effects found for routine prac-
tical programs were notably smaller. Moreover, the research coverage
of both juvenile and adult programs in institutional and
noninstitutional settings is uneven and leaves troublesome gaps in
the evidence.

Mark W. Lipsey is a professor of public policy at Vanderbilt University and a


codirector of the Center for Evaluation Research and Methodology at the Vanderbilt In-
stitute for Public Policy Studies.
Gabrielle L. Chapman is the director of planning and research at the Tennessee De-
partment of Correction and a doctoral candidate in sociology at Vanderbilt University.
Nana A. Landenberger is a doctoral candidate in clinical psychology at Vanderbilt
University with several years experience providing cognitive-behavioral treatment to
adult offenders.

144
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL PROGRAMS FOR OFFENDERS 145

O NE of the notable characteris-


tics of chronic offenders is dis-
torted cognitionself-justificatory
instance, CBTs may train offenders
to monitor their patterns of auto-
matic thoughts to situations in which
thinking, misinterpretation of social they tend to react with violence. Vari-
cues, deficient moral reasoning, ous techniques are rehearsed for
schemas of dominance and entitle- assessing the validity of those
ment, and the like (Beck 1999; Dodge thoughts and substituting accurate
1993; Walters 1990; Walters and interpretations for biased ones.
White 1989; Yochelson and Samenow Often role-play or practice in real sit-
1976). Offenders with such distorted uations is used to help consolidate
thinking may react to essentially be- new ways of coping with situations
nign situations as if they were that tend to prompt criminal behav-
threatening, for example, be predis- ior. CBTs may focus on managing
posed to perceive comments others anger, assuming personal responsi-
make about them as disrespectful or bility for behavior (for example, chal-
attacking. They may hold conceptu- lenging offenders tendency to excuse
alizations of themselves, others, and their behavior by blaming the victim,
the world that justify antisocial be- society, or other circumstances
havior, for example, nobody can be beyond their control), taking a moral
trusted, everyone is against me, or and empathetic perspective on inter-
society doesnt give me a chance. personal behavior (for example, vic-
Their behavior may be guided by dys- tim impact awareness), solving prob-
functional assumptions and rules lems, developing life skills, setting
about how one should behave, for ex- goals, or any combination of these
ample, you have to punish people for themes. A relapse prevention compo-
messing with you or they wont re- nent is also often included, which
spect you, you have to rebel against teaches offenders strategies for
authority or they will break you. avoiding or deescalating the pre-
And they may have deficient cogni- cursors to offending behavior (for
tive skills for long-term planning, example, high-risk situations, places,
problem solving, and decision mak- associates, or maladaptive coping
ing that contribute to maladaptive responses).
and rigid behavior. Prototypical examples of CBT pro-
Cognitive-behavioral treatments grams for offenders include the Rea-
(CBTs) for offenders are designed to soning and Rehabilitation program
correct these dysfunctional and (Ross and Fabiano 1985), Moral
criminogenic thinking patterns. Reconation Therapy (Little and Rob-
They employ systematic training inson 1986), and Aggression Replace-
regimens aimed at creating cognitive ment Training (Goldstein and Glick
restructuring and flexible cognitive 1987). The Reasoning and Rehabili-
skills such that offenders develop tation program is organized around a
more adaptive patterns of reasoning series of exercises (for example, criti-
and reacting in situations that trig- cal thinking, social perspective tak-
ger their criminal behavior. For ing) that focus on
146 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

modifying the impulsive, egocentric, il- offenders than less-structured pro-


logical and rigid thinking of the offenders grams (for example, Andrews et al.
and teaching them to stop and think be-
1990; Lipsey and Wilson 1998; Lsel
fore acting, to consider the consequences
of their behaviour, to conceptualize alter-
and Kferl 1989; Palmer 1994;
native ways of responding to interper- R edon do, S an ch ez - M eca, an d
sonal problems and to consider the im- Garrido 1999). Cognitive-behavioral
pact of their behaviour on other people, interventions appear to be especially
particularly their victims. (Ross, Fabiano, effective among such programs. A
and Ewles 1988, 31) recent meta-analysis of group-ori-
ented cognitive behavioral programs
Moral Reconation Therapy is for offenders examined 20 studies of
based on Kohlbergs stages of moral varying levels of methodological
development and uses a series of quality and concluded that CBT was
group and workbook exercises de- effective for reducing criminal
signed to raise the moral reasoning behavior among offenders (Wilson,
level of offenders stepwise through Allen, and MacKenzie 2000). Nearly
16 graded moral and cognitive stages all of the studies showed positive
(Finn 1998). Aggression Replace- effects (though not necessarily sta-
ment Training is structured around tistically significant), and represen-
a curriculum comprising three com- tative CBT programs were found to
ponentsSkillstreaming, Anger
reduce recidivism by 20 percent to 30
Control Training, and Moral Edu-
percent compared to untreated con-
cation (Goldstein and Glick 1994).
trol groups.
Skillstreaming teaches 50 prosocial
behaviors through modeling and role Wilson, Allen, and MacKenzies
playing; Anger Control Training in- (2000) meta-analysis was restricted
structs offenders in self-control by to programs delivered in groups, but
having them record anger-arousing this is a minor limitation since virtu-
experiences, identify triggers and ally all such programs for offenders
cues, and then apply anger control are conducted in group formats. A
techniques; Moral Education exposes more interesting aspect of this meta-
offenders to Kohlberg-type moral di- analysis is the range of offenders rep-
lemmas in a discussion format aimed resented in the studies it included
at advancing the level of moral some applied CBT programs to gen-
reasoning. eral samples of offenders; others
treated only specialized types of
offenders, for example, sex offenders,
THE EFFECTIVENESS
OF CBT FOR OFFENDERS drug offenders, driving under the
influence cases, or batterers. Much of
Several reviews and meta-analy- the variability in effects found across
ses have found that structured, direc- studies may have been due to differ-
tive, skill-oriented programs are gen- ences in the responses of these differ-
erally more effective in reducing the ent types of offenders, though there
subsequent reoffense rates of were too few studies in any one
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL PROGRAMS FOR OFFENDERS 147

category for Wilson, Allen, and Mac- convincingly tests this promising
Kenzie to closely examine this factor. program approach.
The only other meta-analysis of In this article, we present another
the effectiveness of CBT programs on meta-analysis of research on the
the reoffense rates of offenders of effects of cognitive-behavioral pro-
which we are aware is Pearson and grams on the reoffense rates of
his colleagues (no date). This meta- offenders. To provide a clear view of
analysis included 69 research stud- the best evidence available on the
ies but covered both behavioral (for effectiveness of this specific treat-
example, contingency contracting, ment modality, we have restricted
token economy) and cognitive-behav- the studies eligible for inclusion in
ioral programs. Pearson and his col- the analysis in four important ways.
leagues found that the cognitive- First, we have selected only studies
behavioral programs were more with experimental or strong quasi-
effective in reducing recidivism than experimental designs so that the
the behavioral ones were, with a most methodologically credible evi-
mean recidivism reduction for dence is represented. Second, to
treated groups of about 30 percent. better isolate the distinctive feature
Moreover studies of higher method- of CBT, we have adopted a relatively
ological quality showed the largest narrow definition of CBT that
effect sizes. The criteria for identify- requires the intervention to focus
centrally on cognitive change. Third,
ing cognitive-behavioral programs in
we have included only studies that
this meta-analysis were rather
applied CBT to samples of the gen-
broad, however. They included not
eral of f en der popu l at i on an d
only interventions directed specifi-
excluded those using samples of spe-
cally toward altering cognitions but
cialized offenders. While the issue of
also social skills training and prob-
the effectiveness of CBT for different
lem-solving programs for which cog-
types of offenders is important, the
nitive change was not the main focus.
broader issue is whether CBT can be
These two meta-analyses provide used with good results in routine cor-
strong indications of the effective- rectional practice with typical mixed
ness of CBT for reducing the recidi- samples. Last, we have focused the
vism of offenders. Both, however, analysis exclusively on reoffense
encompass considerable diversity recidivism as an outcome variable.
within their scope, including a range Though disciplinary infractions
of offender types, quality of study within a correctional institution,
design, and (especially in Pearson technical parole violations, and other
et al. no date) variations in what is such outcomes may be proxies
counted as a CBT. Against this back- for criminal behavior, the effects of
ground, it seems wise to confirm the CBT on subsequent criminal behav-
effectiveness of CBTs for offenders ior is best shown by results on
with an analysis of the set of avail- such direct measures as rearrest and
able studies that most directly and reconviction.
148 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

METHOD senting the contrast between the


subsequent criminal behavior of
The criteria used to select the treated versus untreated offenders.
studies for this meta-analysis are as
follows. Research methods. The study uses
a design in which participants are as-
Interventions. The treatment un- signed to intervention and control
der investigation is directed toward conditions either randomly or with a
changing offenders distorted or dys- nonrandom procedure that does not
functional cognitions or toward involve manifest differential selec-
teaching new cognitive skills in areas tion. That is, if nonrandom assign-
where offenders have deficits. The ment is used, the selection procedure
therapeutic techniques consist of must not involve treatment-related
specific, relatively structured learn- differentiation (for example, volun-
ing experiences designed to affect teers versus nonvolunteers, treat-
such cognitive processes as inter- ment completers versus dropouts),
preting social cues, reasoning about and the groups must be matched ex-
right and wrong behavior, and mak- plicitly or implicitly on key demo-
ing decisions about appropriate be- graphic variables and/or prior
havior. When the program also offense histories or evidence must be
includes other elements, the cogni- provided that demonstrates initial
tive-behavioral component is cen- equivalence between the groups.
trally featured rather than being Control groups can represent pla-
only a secondary component. cebo, waiting list, no-treatment, or
treatment-as-usual conditions.
Participants. The recipients of the
intervention are criminal offenders, Other considerations. Both pub-
either juvenile (ages 12 to 21) or lished and unpublished studies are
adult, who are treated while on pro- eligible, but only studies from Eng-
bation, incarcerated, or in aftercare/ lish-speaking countries that are re-
parole. They are drawn from a gen- ported in English are included.
eral offender population and are not
selected for, or restricted to, a specific Search strategy for
type of offense (for example, sex of- identification of studies
fenses, driving under the influence) Candidate study reports were
or problem behavior (for example, located first through the authors
drug use). existing delinquency intervention
database (Lipsey 1992) and the refer-
Outcome measures. The study re- ences in recent meta-analyses and
ports subsequent delinquent or crim- research reviews covering cognitive-
inal offenses as an outcome variable behavioral interventions. Additional
and presents quantitative data or studies were then sought through
statistical information that permits computer searches of a wide range of
computation or reasonable estima- bibliographic databases (for example,
tion of an effect size statistic repre- Psychological Abstracts, National
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL PROGRAMS FOR OFFENDERS 149

Criminal Justice Reference Service, odds ratio could be imputed, using


Sociological Abstracts, Dissertation the median control group recidivism
Abstracts International, Educa- base rate from the other studies.
tional Resources Information Clear-
inghouse, and Medline) and exami-
RESULTS
nation of the bibliographies of
studies retrieved for screening.
Fourteen studies that met the eli-
Copies of promising study reports
gibility criteria were located and
were obtained, and determination of
coded. We consider this a work in
their eligibility for the meta-analysis
progress and are continuing to
was made by one of the authors.
search for additional studies, but we
are confident that our search to this
Coding procedures
point has been reasonably thorough.
Each study judged eligible for The characteristics of these studies
inclusion in the meta-analysis was are summarized in Table 1. As Table
coded into a database by one of the 1 shows, all of these studies were con-
authors. The coding scheme recorded ducted in the United States or Can-
general information about the study ada and were published in 1985 or
report (for example, date and type of later. The interventions were con-
publication) and a range of specific ducted as demonstration programs
characteristics of the intervention, for half the studies and, in the other
the participating offenders, and the half, constituted ongoing programs
study methods (Table 1 shows some in a criminal justice context. All the
of the coding categories used). treatments were provided in group
The most critical portion of the format, and nearly two-thirds took
coding dealt with the findings place in custodial institutions. The
reported by the studies on reoffense intervention typically lasted from 11
recidivism outcomes. In all but one to 20 weeks, meeting one to two times
instance, those outcomes were per week, for a total of 10 hours or
reported as the simple proportion or less average weekly contact time.
percentage of offenders in each The offenders who participated in
research group who recidivated. This the interventions were about evenly
information was coded in odds ratios divided between juvenile groups
representing the odds of recidivating with a mean age of 15 to 18 years and
among the treatment group relative adult groups with a mean age in the
to the odds among the control group. 20 to 30 years range. The group com-
For dichotomous outcomes, the odds position was usually all or mostly
ratio provides an effect size statistic male and white or mixed race
for meta-analysis that has favorable (though in half the studies informa-
statistical properties and yields tion on ethnicity was not provided).
readily interpretable results (Lipsey Of the 14 studies, 8 used a random-
and Wilson 2001). The one study assignment design, with the remain-
(Walters 1999) that did not report ing ones involving mainly matched or
recidivism outcomes as proportions waiting list controls, and most of the
provided information from which an study samples consisted of 100 or
150 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 1 TABLE 1 Continued


CHARACTERISTICS OF THE STUDIES
Number Proportion
INCLUDED IN THE META-ANALYSIS
Characteristic of Studies of Studies
Number Proportion
Characteristic of Studies of Studies Gender mix
All female 1 0.07
Study reports Mostly male 5 0.36
Type of publication All male 8 0.57
Journal article, Ethnic mix
book chapter 8 0.57 Most/all Anglo 3 0.21
Book 2 0.14 Most/all black 1 0.07
Technical report, thesis 4 0.29 Mixed, none
Date of publication predominates 3 0.21
1985-1990 5 0.36 Cannot tell 7 0.50
1991-2000 9 0.64 Research methods
Country of origin Design
United States 9 0.64 Randomized 8 0.57
Canada 5 0.36 Nonrandom 6 0.43
Treatment program Control condition
Circumstances of Treatment as usual 14 1.00
implementation Other 0 0.00
Demonstration program 7 0.50 Sample size (treatment
Ongoing practical plus control)
program 7 0.50 20-45 3 0.21
Setting 55-100 6 0.43
Correctional institution 9 0.64 100-300 4 0.29
Probation or parole 5 0.36 > 2,000 1 0.07
Treatment format Outcome variable
Individual 0 0.00 Rearrest 8 0.57
Group 14 1.00 Reconviction 6 0.43
Treatment duration Weeks posttreatment
5-10 weeks 4 0.29 when measured
11-20 weeks 8 0.57 13-26 6 0.43
> 20 weeks 2 0.14 30-52 3 0.21
Total hours of treatment > 52 5 0.36
5-15 3 0.21
30-40 4 0.29
60-80 4 0.29
> 100 3 0.21 fewer offenders in the treatment and
Frequency of sessions control groups combined. The control
1-2 times per week 7 0.50
2-4 times per week 2 0.14
condition in all studies was treat-
Daily 5 0.36 ment as usual, that is, regular proba-
Hours per week tion, parole, or prison as received by
1-2 6 0.43 the intervention participants, but
3-10 6 0.43 without the intervention program.
> 10 2 0.14
The reoffense recidivism outcome
Offenders in treatment variables selected for effect size cod-
samples
Mean age
ing were either rearrest or recon-
15-18 7 0.50 viction and, most commonly, were
20-30 6 0.43 measured 13 to 16 weeks posttreat-
> 30 1 0.07
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL PROGRAMS FOR OFFENDERS 151

FIGURE 1
LOGGED ODDS RATIOS FOR RECIDIVISM EFFECTS

Ross et al. 1988

Goldstein & Glick 1994

Kownacki 1995

Goldstein et al. 1989

Leeman et al. 1993

Bottcher 1985

Shivrattan 1988

Guerra & Slaby 1990

Porporino & Robinson 1995

Finn 1998

Robinson 1994

Robinson 1995

Walters 1999

Bonta et al. 2000

Grand Mean

Intervention has Control has


lower recidivism lower recidivism

-4.0 -3.0 -2.0 -1.0 0.0 1.0 2.0

Log of Odds Ratio with 95% Confidence Interval

ment, though about one-third were ivism among treated offenders rela-
measured more than a year after tive to the odds among offenders in
treatment was completed. the control group, where the odds are
defined as the number recidivating
Intervention effects di v i ded by t h e n u mber n ot
recidivating. Thus an odds ratio of
Figure 1 shows the logged odds .50 means that the ratio of recidivists
ratios and their confidence intervals to nonrecidivists among treated
for the treatment versus control offenders was half that of the control
group differences in reoffense recidi- group. The logged odds ratio is more
vism across the 14 studies. The odds convenient for statistical computa-
ratio represents the odds of recid- tions and takes negative values
152 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

when the treatment group recid- the control groups (that is, 26 percent
ivates less than the control does, pos- versus 38 percent recidivating in the
itive values when the control respective groups). Thus only about
recidivates less than the treatment two-thirds as many offenders receiv-
group does, and zero when recidivism ing CBT recidivated as did offenders
rates are the same for both groups. in the control groups.
Figure 1 shows that with one The grand mean odds ratio and
slight exception, the odds ratios from treatment versus control recidivism
these 14 studies all indicate lower rates, however, do not take into
recidivism for the offenders receiving account the variation in treatment
CBT than for those in the control effects across the 14 studies. A Q test
groups with whom they were com- of the homogeneity of the logged odds
pared. As the confidence intervals ratios (Lipsey and Wilson 2001;
reveal, however, only a few of these Shadish and Haddock 1994) showed
odds ratios are statistically signifi- that even with only 14 studies, the
cant (that is, the confidence interval variation in findings was statisti-
around the logged odds ratio does not cally significant. Adjusting the
1
include zero). The weighted mean weighting for the one large sample
odds ratio across all 14 studies, com- reduced the heterogeneity but still
puted as .66 (log = .42), however, left it marginally significant (p = .07).
takes advantage of the statistical Before drawing any conclusions
power of the full set of studies and is about the overall effectiveness of
statistically significant (p < .05). CBT from these studies, therefore,
Of the 14 studies, 1 (Robinson some investigation is warranted of
1995) had an exceptionally large the potential sources of this variabil-
sample size (more than 2000; next ity in findings across studies.
largest was less than 300) and thus One potentially important moder-
exercised a disproportionate influ- ator of effect sizes is the type of
ence on the weighted mean odds research design. Although only
ratio. With the weight on the odds the strongest quasi-experimental
ratio from that study scaled back to designs were eligible for inclusion in
match the next highest, the weighted the meta-analysis, they still might be
mean across studies was .55 (log = sufficiently biased to yield findings
.59), which gives a more generally systematically different from those
representative value for this collec- resulting from randomized experi-
tion of study findings. This value ments. This did not prove to be the
indicates that on average, the odds of case, however. The mean odds ratio
recidivating for offenders receiving from the six nonrandomized designs
CBT programs were almost half (.62) was not significantly different,
those for offenders in the control Q(1) = .75, p = .39, from the mean for
groups who did not receive CBT. the eight randomized designs (.50).
More specifically, the weighted mean Of the remaining coded variables
recidivism rate for the intervention dealing with important study char-
groups was .26 compared with .38 for acteristics, exploratory analysis
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL PROGRAMS FOR OFFENDERS 153

suggested that as many as half a offenders on probation or parole (that


dozen might be significantly associ- is, not incarcerated). These studies
ated with effect size. Some of these tended to measure short-term recidi-
were correlated among themselves, vism outcomes (6 months or less
however, making simple bivariate posttreatment) and use relatively
breakdowns potentially misleading. small samples. The recidivism effects
With only 14 studies, on the other they reported were quite substantial.
hand, there was little scope for Offenders receiving CBT in these
multivariate analysis. To generate studies had a mean recidivism rate of
some insight about the study charac- .15 compared to .52 for the controls.
teristics most closely associated with The four studies yielding in-
larger and smaller intervention between effects on recidivism were
effects, therefore, a simple categori- also demonstration programs, with
cal procedure was used. The 14 stud- one exception, but these were applied
ies were sorted by effect size and to juvenile offenders in institutional
divided into the groups with the settings. They tended to examine lon-
highest, middle, and lowest effect
ger-term recidivism (more than 6
sizes. The dividing points were set
months posttreatment) and use
inductively at gaps in the effect size
larger samples. The mean recidivism
distribution and with an eye on con-
rate for the offenders receiving CBT
trasts in the study characteristics
in these studies was .37 compared to
between groups. The coded study
.53 among offenders in the corre-
characteristics (shown in Table 1)
that were not already binary items sponding control groups.
were then dichotomized at the The six studies that produced the
median or the nearest break in the smallest effects were ongoing
respective distribution. Next, a chi- practical programs for which the
square test was applied to the simple researcher was primarily the evalua-
cross-tabulation of each study char- tor. These programs used CBT with
acteristic and the three effect size adult offenders in correctional insti-
groupings for the 14 studies. tutions (with one exception). They
Table 2 shows the study character- also tended to examine longer-term
istics that were significantly associ- recidivism outcomes and use larger
ated with the grouping of studies by samples. Most of these studies were
effect size. The characteristics most reported since 1995, whereas most in
distinctive of each grouping are the other two categories were pub-
shown in boldface. Table 2 also lished before then. The recidivism
reports the mean odds ratios and rates for the offenders in the control
treatment versus control group groups in these studies were notably
recidivism rates for each group of lower than for the other two catego-
studies. ries of studies, with a mean of .31.
The four studies that reported the The treatment groups still showed a
largest effects of CBT on recidivism lower recidivism rate than their
were demonstration programs set up respective control groups, but at .26
by researchers and applied to it was not very much lower.
154 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 2
CHARACTERISTICS OF STUDIES WITH DIFFERENT
MAGNITUDES OF RECIDIVISM EFFECTS

Treatment Program Offender Research Method Study Report


Type n Type n Type n Type n

Studies showing largest effects (n = 4); Mean odds ratio = .16 (log = 1.85);
Weighted mean recidivism rate; Treatment groups = .15; Control groups = .52
Demonstration 4 Juveniles 2 Posttest < 6 months 4 Published before 1995 3
Practical 0 Adults 2 Posttest 6 months 0 Published in or after 1995 1

Institutional 0 Sample N < 75 4


Probation/parole 4 Sample N > 75 0

Studies showing in-between effects (n = 4); Mean odds ratio = .41 (log = .89);
Weighted mean recidivism rate; Treatment groups = .37; Control groups = .53
Demonstration 3 Juveniles 4 Posttest < 6 months 1 Published before 1995 4
Practical 1 Adults 0 Posttest > 6 months 3 Published in or after 1995 0

Institutional 4 Sample N < 75 1


Probation/parole 0 Sample N > 75 3

Studies showing smallest effects (N = 6); Mean odds ratio = .76 (log = .28);
Weighted mean recidivism rate; Treatment groups = .26; Control groups = .31
Demonstration 0 Juveniles 1 Posttest < 6 months 1 Published before 1995 1
Practical 6 Adults 5 Posttest > 6 months 5 Published in or after 1995 5

Institutional 5 Sample N < 75 1


Probation/parole 1 Sample N > 75 5
NOTE: Boldface entries identify the characteristics most distinctive of the respective category of
studies.

CONCLUSION control groups with which they were


compared. Moreover, the most effec-
This meta-analysis encompasses tive programs reduced recidivism
only a small set of studies, but they rates to about one-third of the rate
were selected by criteria that make for untreated controls.
them the best available evidence The meta-analysis findings are
about the effectiveness of CBT pro- not all good news for use of CBT in
grams for reducing the reoffense rehabilitation programs for offend-
recidivism of criminal offenders. ers, however. Most of the large recidi-
Taken as a group, these studies show vism reductions shown in the
rather clearly that CBT is indeed an research studies were produced by
effective interventiontreated demonstration programs set up by
offenders on average recidivated at a researchers for the limited time
rate of about two-thirds that of the required to mount an evaluation of
offenders in the treatment-as-usual their effects. Such programs involve
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL PROGRAMS FOR OFFENDERS 155

levels of training for treatment per- programs of CBT delivered to adults


sonnel, adherence to treatment pro- in jail or prison are available, it
tocols, and strength and fidelity of would be premature to conclude that
implementation that are not likely to this setting or offender population
be typical of routine practice in crimi- is either more or less conducive to
nal justice settings. The effects found effective treatment than the others
in the six evaluation studies of ongo- that have been studied with such
ing CBT programs implemented by programs.
correctional institutions clustered at The most promising findings from
the bottom of the effect size distribu- the research reviewed here apply to
tion, and though positive, they were juvenile offenders. Demonstration
quite modest. programs with juveniles both on pro-
It is encouraging, on one hand, bation/parole and in custodial insti-
that practical programs imple- tutions produced sizeable reductions
mented on a more or less routine in recidivism, with treated offenders
basis in correctional facilities can showing only one-third to two-thirds
produce measurable, even if modest, the recidivism rates of untreated
recidivism reductions. Such effects, controls. However, no research stud-
multiplied over the volume of offend- ies of practical programs using CBT
ers to whom CBT programs can be with juvenile offenders were found
administered in correctional facili- that met the methodological stan-
ties, may well be worthwhile, for dards of this meta-analysis. The very
instance, in cost-benefit terms positive results found in these dem-
(though none of the studies reviewed onstration programs, therefore, have
here provided cost data). On the not yet been confirmed with research
other hand, the gap between the studies of CBT administered to juve-
small effects of the practical pro- nile offenders in routine juvenile jus-
grams and the substantial effects of tice circumstances. In short, the best
the demonstration programs sug- available research evidence supports
gests that a great deal of improve- the concept of CBT as an effective
ment may be possible in the adminis- intervention for offenders, at least for
tration of effective CBT programs by juvenile offenders. Whether that con-
criminal justice agencies. At the cept can be translated into effective
same time, it must be noted that the routine practice, however, is an open
relatively large effects achieved by question.
demonstration programs came from
studies conducted in institutional
settings with juvenile offenders or
studies with adult and juvenile Note
offenders on probation or parole.
These differences raise a question as 1. All statistical calculations were con-
to whether the strong results from ducted with the logged odds ratios weighted by
the inverse of their conditional variances to re-
the demonstration programs can be flect the differential statistical precision stem-
generalized to incarcerated adults. ming from their different sample sizes (Lipsey
Until studies of demonstration and Wilson 2001; Shadish and Haddock 1994).
156 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

References *Guerra, Nancy G. and Ronald G. Slaby.


1990. Cognitive Mediators of Aggres-
Andrews, Don A., Ivan Zinger, Robert D. sion in Adolescent Offenders: 2. Inter-
Hoge, James Bonta, Paul Gendreau, vention. Developmental Psychology
and Francis T. Cullen. 1990. Does Cor- 26:269-77.
r ec t i ona l T r ea tm ent Wo r k ? A *Kownacki, Richard J. 1995. The Effec-
Clinically-Relevant and Psycho- tiveness of a Brief Cognitive-
logically Informed Meta-Analysis. Behavioral Program on the Reduction
Criminology 28:369-404. of Antisocial Behaviour in High-Risk
Beck, Aaron T. 1999. Prisoners of Hate: Adult Probationers in a Texas Com-
The Cognitive Basis of Anger, Hostil- munity. In Thinking Straight: The
i t y, a nd Vi ol e nc e. N e w Yo r k : Reasoning and Rehabilitation Pro-
HarperCollins. gram for Delinquency Prevention and
Offender Rehabilitation, ed. Robert R.
*Bonta, James, Suzanne Wallace-
Ross and Roslynn D. Ross. Ottawa,
Capretta, and Jennifer Rooney. 2000.
Canada: Air Training and Publica-
A Quasi-Experimental Evaluation of
tions.
an Intensive Rehabilitation Supervi-
*Leeman, Leonard W., John C. Gibbs, and
sion Program. Criminal Justice and
Dick Fuller. 1993. Evaluation of a
Behavior 27:312-29.
Multi-Component Group Treatment
*Bottcher, Jean. 1985. The Athena Pro- Program for Juvenile Delinquents.
gram: An Evaluation of a Girls Treat- Aggressive Behavior 19:281-92.
ment Program at the Fresno County Lipsey, Mark W. 1992. Juvenile Delin-
Probation Departments Juvenile quency Treatment: A Meta-Analytic
Hall. Sacramento: California Youth Inquiry into the Variability of Effects.
Authority. In Meta-Analysis for Explanation: A
Dodge, Kenneth A. 1993. Social-Cogni- Casebook, ed. Thomas D. Cook, Harris
tive Mechanisms in the Development Cooper, David S. Cordray, Heidi
of Conduct Disorder and Depression. Hartmann, Larry V. Hedges, Richard
Annual Review of Psychology 44:559- J. Light, Thomas A. Louis, and Freder-
83. ick Mosteller. Newbury Park, CA:
*Finn, Peter. 1998. The Delaware Depart- Sage.
ment of Correction Life Skills Pro- Lipsey, Mark W. and David B. Wilson.
gram. Washington, DC: U.S. Depart- 1998. Effective Intervention for Seri-
ment of Justice, Office of Justice Pro- ous Juvenile Offenders: A Synthesis of
grams. Research. In Serious and Violent Juve-
Goldstein, Arnold P. and Barry Glick. nile Offenders: Risk Factors and Suc-
1 9 8 7 . A g g r e s s i on R e p l a c e m e n t cessful Interventions, ed. Rolf Loeber
Training: A Comprehensive Interven- and David P. Farrington. Thousand
tion for Aggressive Youth. Champaign, Oaks, CA: Sage.
IL: Research Press. . 2001. Practical Meta-Analysis.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
*. 1994. The Prosocial Gang: Im-
Little, Gregory L. and Kenneth D. Robin-
plementing Aggression Replacement
son. 1986. Juvenile MRT: How to Es-
Training. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
cape Your Prison. Memphis, TN: Eagle
*Goldstein, Arnold P., Barry Glick, Mary
Wing Books.
J. Irwin, Claudia Pask-McCartney,
and Ibrahim Rubama. 1989. Reducing
Delinquency: Intervention in the Com- *References marked with an asterisk indi-
munity. 1st ed. New York: Pergamon. cate studies included in the meta-analysis.
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL PROGRAMS FOR OFFENDERS 157

Lsel, Friedrich and Peter Kferl. 1989. 55(08):2582A. (University Microfilms


Evaluation Research on Correctional No. 95-02199)
Treatment in West Germany: A Meta- Ross, Robert R. and Elizabeth A. Fabiano.
Analysis. In Criminal Behavior and 1985. Time to Think: A Cognitive
the Justice System: Psychological Per- Model of Delinquency Prevention and
spectives, ed. Hermann Wegener, Offender Rehabilitation. Johnson City,
Friedrich Lsel, and Jochen Haisch. TN: Institute of Social Sciences and
New York: Springer-Verlag. Arts.
Palmer, Ted. 1994. A Profile of Correc- *Ross, Robert R., Elizabeth A. Fabiano,
tional Effectiveness and New Direc- and Crystal D. Ewles. 1988. Reasoning
tions for Research. Albany: State Uni- and Rehabilitation. International
versity of New York Press. Journal of Offender Therapy and
Pearson, Frank S., Douglas S. Lipton, Comparative Criminology 32:29-35.
Charles M. Cleland, and Dorline S. Shadish, William R. and C. Keith Had-
Yee. N.d. The Effects of Behavioral/ dock. 1994. Combining Estimates of
Cognitive-Behavioral Programs on Effect Size. In The Handbook of Re-
Recidivism. Unpublished manuscript, search Synthesis, ed. Harris Cooper
National Development and Research and Larry V. Hedges. Thousand Oaks,
Institutes, Inc., New York. CA: Sage.
*Porporino, Frank J. and David Robin-
*Shivrattan, Jacob L. 1988. Social
son. 1995. An Evaluation of the Rea-
Interactional Training and Incarcer-
soning and Rehabilitation Program
ated Juvenile Delinquents. Canadian
with Canadian Federal Offenders. In
Journal of Criminology 30:145-63.
Thinking Straight: The Reasoning
Walters, Glenn D. 1990. The Criminal
and Rehabilitation Program for Delin-
Lifestyle: Patterns of Serious Criminal
quency Prevention and Offender Reha-
Conduct. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
bilitation, ed. Robert R. Ross and
Roslynn D. Ross. Ottawa, Canada: Air *. 1999. Short-Term Outcome of
Training and Publications. Inmates Participating in the Lifestyle
Redondo, Santiago, Julio Sanchez-Meca, Change Program. Criminal Justice
and Vicente Garrido. 1999. The Influ- and Behavior 26:322-37.
ence of Treatment Programmes on the Walters, Glenn D. and Thomas W. White.
Recidivism of Juvenile and Adult Of- 1989. The Thinking Criminal: A Cog-
fenders: An European Meta-Analytic nitive Model of Lifestyle Criminality.
Review. Psychology, Crime, and Law Criminal Justice Research Bulletin
5:251-78. 4:1-10.
*Robinson, David. 1995. The Impact of Wilson, David B., Leana C. Allen, and
Cognitive Skills Training on Post- Doris L. MacKenzie. 2000. A Quantita-
Release Recidivism Among Canadian tive Review of Structured, Group-
Federal Offenders. Ottawa: Correc- Oriented, Cognitive-Behavioral Pro-
tional Service of Canada. grams for Offenders. Unpublished
*Robinson, Sue C. 1994. Implementation manuscript, University of Maryland,
of the Cognitive Model of Offender Re- College Park.
habilitation and Delinquency Preven- Yochelson, Samuel and Stanton E.
tion (Cognitive Skills Training). Ph.D. Samenow. 1976. The Criminal Person-
diss., University of Utah. Abstract in ality. Vol I, A Profile for Change. New
Dissertation Abstracts International York: Aronson.
ANNALS, AAPSS, 578, November 2001

THEEVIDENCE-BASED
AN ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN
APPROACHACADEMY
TO PREVENTING CRIME

Toward an Evidence-Based
Approach to Preventing Crime

By BRANDON C. WELSH and DAVID P. FARRINGTON

ABSTRACT: This article brings together the main conclusions from


the previous articles in this issue and identifies priorities for moving
toward an evidence-based approach to preventing crime. The Camp-
bell Collaboration Crime and Justice Group has begun the important
task of preparing systematic reviews of the effectiveness of a wide
range of criminological interventions. Alongside the Campbell initia-
tive, a program of research of new crime prevention and intervention
experiments and quasi-experiments needs to be launched. Efforts
must also be made to confront the challenges of getting research evi-
dence into policy and practice. Here, political and policy consider-
ations need to be faced.

Brandon C. Welsh is an assistant professor in the Department of Criminal Justice,


University of MassachusettsLowell.
David P. Farrington is Professor of Psychological Criminology at the Institute of
Criminology, University of Cambridge and Jerry Lee Research Professor of Crimi-
nology at the Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice, University of Mary-
land.

158
AN EVIDENCE-BASED APPROACH TO PREVENTING CRIME 159

U NFORTUNATELY, crime pre-


vention today as in the past has
a tendency to be driven more by rhet-
of police objectives (Sherman 1998,
6). This is equally applicable to the
larger field of crime prevention,
oric than reality (Visher and whether it be for police, courts, or cor-
Weisburd 1998, 238). However, effec- rections or for alternative, noncrim-
tive public policy and practice need to inal justice approaches such as vio-
be based on scientific evidence. This lence prevention programs in schools
is an approach that has garnered or gang intervention programs in
much support in medicine (Millenson communities. As noted by Petrosino
1997) and other fields dedicated to (2000), an evidence-based approach
the betterment of society. This is not, requires that the results of rigorous
how ever, the practice u s u al l y evaluation be rationally integrated
adopted in criminology or criminal into decisions about interventions by
justice. Anecdotal evidence, program policymakers and practitioners
favorites of the month, and political alike (635).
ideology seemingly drive much of the This final article brings together
crime policy agenda. As a result, we the main conclusions of the eight pre-
are left with a patchwork of pro- vious articles in this special issue of
grams that are of unknown potential The Annals and identifies priorities
in preventing crime. Crime preven- for moving toward an evidence-based
tion programs may or may not work approach to preventing crime. It is
or worse yet may produce harmful or divided into four parts. In the first
iatrogenic results. We are not sug- part, we examine the main review
gesting that the public is being inten- methods for assessing what works in
tionally misled by lawmakers and light of their (potential) contribution
policy makers who are funding pro- to evidence-based crime prevention.
grams with no scientific evidence of In the second part, we review key fea-
effectiveness but rather that law- tures of impact evaluation research,
makers and policy makers are shirk- focusing on the highest-quality
ing their responsibility to the taxpay- research designs (experimental and
ing public by not funding only those quasi-experimental designs); discuss
programs with evidence of effective- the relationship between research
ness in preventing crime. design and program effects; and
In writing on the subject of evi- make some recommendations for
dence-based policing, Sherman improving the state of evaluation
(1998) noted that most police prac- research in criminology. In the third
tice, like medical practice, is still part, we briefly summarize the main
shaped by local custom, opinions, findings and discuss policy implica-
theories, and subjective impressions tions of the four systematic reviews
(6). But evidence-based policing reported in this special issue, and in
challenges those principles of deci- the final part, we discuss a number of
sion making and creates systematic important political and policy consid-
feedback to provide continuous qual- erations that influence an evidence-
ity improvement in the achievement based approach to preventing crime.
160 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

ASSESSING WHAT WORKS early childhood interventions that


have measured effects on criminal
Efforts to assess whether a partic- activity (see Farrington and Welsh
ular crime prevention strategy (for 1999).
example, developmental, criminal Narrative reviews of the literature
justice), intervention modality (for quite often include many studies
example, parent training), or some and may be very comprehensive.
other subset of crime prevention pro- Their main drawback, however, is
grams worksthat is, has a reducing researcher bias. This bias, whether
effect on crime (or alternatively, if it intentional or not, typically starts
is promising, has no effect, or is right from the beginning with a less
harmful)can take many different than rigorous methodology for
forms. Some of these are the single searching for studies. More often
study method, the narrative review than not, the researcher will limit his
method, the vote-count method, or her search to academic sources or
the meta-analytic review, and the ev en s el f - s el ect s t u di es t o be
systematic review. Each of these is included, based on the researchers
discussed in light of its (potential) familiarity with them. This can lead
contribution to evidence-based crime to an incorrect interpretation of the
prevention (Sherman et al . interventions effects on crime; for
forthcoming). example, what should have been pre-
Not only is the single study sented as a positive effect is instead
method self-explanatory, but its reported as an uncertain effect (that
limitationsin comparison with the is, unclear evidence of an effect).
other methodsare blatantly evi- The vote-count method adds a
dent. Here, a single evaluation study, quantitative element to the narra-
usually of high quality methodologi- tive review, by considering statistical
cally (for example, randomized con- significance. In essence, this method
trolled experiment), is used to repre- tallies up the number of studies
s ent a body of res ea rch on a with statistically significant findings
particular type of intervention. The in favor of the hypothesis and the
well-known Perry Preschool program number contrary to the hypothesis
(Schweinhart, Barnes, and Weikart (null findings) (Wilson 2001 [this
1993) has long been used by advo- issue], 73).
cates of early childhood intervention A more comprehensive vote-count
to show the beneficial results this method was developed by Sherman
type of intervention can have on and his colleagues (1997) to help
delinquency and later offending. them draw conclusions about what
Despite its beneficial results, as well works, what does not work, what is
as findings from cost-benefit analy- promising, and what is unknown in
ses that showed that it returned to preventing crime (see our appendix)
society savings far in excess of the in seven major institutional settings:
costs to run the program (see Barnett families, communities, schools, labor
1996; Greenwood et al. 2001), it is by markets, places (for example, urban
no means representative of other centers, homes), police agencies, and
AN EVIDENCE-BASED APPROACH TO PREVENTING CRIME 161

courts and corrections. In addition to evidence-based crime prevention.


statistical significance, their vote- Systematic reviews use rigorous
count method integrated a scientific methods for locating, appraising, and
methods scale that was based on the synthesizing evidence from prior
work of Cook and Campbell (1979) evaluation studies, and they are
and described research designs that reported with the same level of detail
are most effective in eliminating that characterizes high-quality
threats to internal validity (that is, reports of original research. System-
alternative plausible explanations of atic reviews, according to Johnson
observed effects). As shown in our and his colleagues (2000), essen-
appendix, level 3 studies (presence of tially take an epidemiological look at
before and after crime measures in the methodology and results sections
comparable experimental and con- of a specific population of studies to
trol conditions) were used as the min- reach a research-based consensus on
imum cutoff point for inclusion of a given study topic (35). They have
studies. This decision was based explicit objectives, explicit criteria
largely on the existing state of scien- for including or excluding studies,
tific knowledge. Their vote-count extensive searches for eligible evalu-
method also took into account the ation studies from across the world,
preponderance of evidence of the careful extraction and coding of key
crime prevention program type (for features of studies, and a structured
example, home visitation for disad- and detailed report of the methods
vantaged mothers, closed circuit tele- and conclusions of the review. All
vision in town centers) in the rele- of this contributes greatly to the
1
vant setting being investigated. ease of their replication by other
One of the main strengths of this researchers.
vote-count method is that it goes The foremost advantage of sys-
beyond the use of statistical signifi- tematic reviews, as noted in the arti-
cance in drawing conclusions about cle by Anthony Petrosino and his col-
what works or does not work. It also leagues (2001 [this issue]), is that
has great utility as part of meta-ana- when done well and with full integ-
lytic and systematic reviews. How- rity, they provide the most reliable
ever, some of the limitations of the and comprehensive statement about
vote-count method include not w h at w ork s ( 20) . S y s t emat i c
accounting for the influence of larger reviews are not, however, without
studies and not taking account of the their limitations, although these lim-
size of the observed effects. (For a itations or challenges appear to be
more complete discussion of the vote- more closely linked with administra-
count methods limitations, see tive and dissemination issues (see
David Wilsons [2001] article.) the article by Petrosino et al. 2001).
The systematic review and the The challenges that face the sub-
meta-analytic review are the most stance of systematic reviews include
rigorous methods for assessing the the transparency of the process (for
effectiveness of criminological inter- example, the need to state the rea-
ventions and have the most to offer to sons studies were included or
162 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

excluded) and the need to reconcile tioners, policy makers, and the gen-
differences in coding of study charac- eral public.
teristics and outcomes by multiple A meta-analysis involves the sta-
researchers (that is, interrater tistical or quantitative analysis of
reliability). the results of prior research studies.
As noted in the article by David Since it involves the statistical sum-
Farrington and Anthony Petrosino mary of data (for example, effect
(2001 [this issue]), in systematic sizes), it requires a reasonable num-
reviews, quantitative techniques ber of intervention studies that are
are used, when appropriate and pos- sufficiently similar to be grouped
sible, in analyzing results (37). The together. For example, there may be
use of quantitative techniques such little point in reporting a mean effect
as meta-analysis may not be suitable size based on a very small number of
due to a small number of studies, het- studies. Nevertheless, quantitative
erogeneity across studies, and differ- methods can be very important in
ent units of analysis of the studies helping the reviewer determine the
(that is, a mix of area- and individual- average effect of a particular
based studies). Another instance in intervention.
which a meta-analysis does not equal David Wilsons (2001) article
a systematic review is when the for- reviews meta-analysis in the context
mer fails to conform to the methodol- of criminological interventions. He
ogy of systematic review, for example, identifies a number of strengths and
using biased search strategies to limitations in the use of meta-analy-
locate relevant studies. (For other sit- sis. Among the strengths, he lists its
uations in which a meta-analysis transparent naturethe explication
of its methods and the studies
should not be performed, see David
involvedwhich makes it easily rep-
Wilsons [2001] article.)
licated by other researchers and its
High-quality systematic reviews ability to handle a very large number
are the core ingredient of the newly of studies that may be overwhelming
created Campbell Collaboration. for other review methods, and he
Named after the influential experi- notes that the statistical methods of
mental psychologist Donald T. Camp- meta-analysis help guard against
bell, the Campbell Collaboration was interpreting the dispersion in results
set up for the purpose of preparing, as meaningful when it can just as
maintaining, and disseminating evi- easily be explained as sampling
dence-based research on the effects error (84). Limitations of meta-anal-
of interventions in the three fields of ysis include, on a practical side, its
education, social welfare, and crime time-consuming nature and its in-
and justice. The Campbell Collabora- ability to synthesize complex pat-
tion Crime and Justice Group aims to terns of effects often found in individ-
prepare and maintain systematic ual studies (Wilson 2001, 84).
reviews of criminological interven- Overall, as David Wilson notes,
tions and to make them accessible meta-analysis provides a defensible
electronically to scholars, practi- strategy for summarizing crime
AN EVIDENCE-BASED APPROACH TO PREVENTING CRIME 163

prevention and intervention efforts exclusion of area-based interven-


for informing public policy (85). tions (for example, closed circuit
One issue that is central to each of television and many types of commu-
the types of review methods dis- nity crime prevention programs) (see
cussed above (and discussed in all of Hope 1995; Welsh and Hoshi forth-
the articles in this special issue of coming). This part reviews key fea-
The Annals) is the importance of tures of impact evaluation research,
including only the highest-quality discusses the relationship between
studies, that is, those studies that research design and program effects,
used the most methodologically rig- and makes some recommendations
orous evaluation designs (experi- for improving the state of evaluation
mental and quasi-experimental research in criminology.
designs) to assess criminological out- Intervention studies differ in
comes. We take up this important met h odol og i cal qu al i t y ( s ee
issue next. Farrington and Petrosino 2001). Two
of the most important features of
methodological quality are internal
EXPERIMENTAL AND
QUASI-EXPERIMENTAL and external validity (Cook and
EVALUATION DESIGNS Campbell 1979). Internal validity
refers to how well the study unam-
The subtitle of this special issue of biguously demonstrates that an
The Annals makes clear the types of intervention (for example, parent
evaluation research that are of inter- training) had an effect on an outcome
est to the discussion of systematic (for example, offending). External
reviews reported throughout. Our validity refers to how well the effect
interest in the use of experimental of an intervention on an outcome is
(randomized and nonrandomized) generalizable or replicable in differ-
and quasi-experimental research ent conditions: different operational
designs in evaluating criminological definitions of the intervention and
interventions is largely threefold. various outcomes, different persons,
First, they represent the most meth- different environments, and so on.
odologically rigorous approaches to It might perhaps be argued that
evaluating the impact of the inde- the gold standard design should be
pendent variable (the program) on the randomized experiment, which is
the dependent variable (crime or the most convincing method of evalu-
offending). Second, in the field of ating crime prevention programs
criminology, unlike in medicine, for (Farrington 1983). The key feature of
example, there is not a long history of randomized experiments is that the
randomized controlled experiments, experimental and control groups are
and by limiting systematic reviews to equated before the experimental
this type of evaluation design there intervention on all possible extrane-
would be very little to say (for most ous variables. Hence any subsequent
intervention modalities). Third, lim- differences between them must be
iting systematic reviews to random- attributable to the intervention. The
ized experiments would mean the randomized experiment, however, is
164 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

the most convincing method of evalu- control of extraneous variables. This


ation only if it is implemented with is an example of a quasi-experimen-
full integrity. To the extent that there tal evaluation design. Even better,
are implementation problems (for the effect of an intervention on crime
example, problems of maintaining can be investigated after controlling
random assignment, differential (for example, in a regression equa-
attrition, cross over between control tion) not only for prior crime but also
and experimental conditions), inter- for other factors that influence crime.
nal validity could be reduced in it. Another possibility is to match two
Another important feature of the areas and then to choose one at ran-
randomized experiment is that a suf- dom to be the experimental area. Of
ficiently large number of units needs course, several pairs of areas would
to be randomly assigned to ensure be better than only one pair. These
that the treatment group is equiva- are the best ways of dealing with
lent to the comparison group on all threats to internal validity when
extraneous variables (within the lim- random assignment of units to exper-
its of statistical fluctuation). As a imental and control conditions can-
rule of thumb, at least 50 units in not be done.
each category are needed (Farring- Regarding systematic reviews in-
ton 1997). This number is relatively cluding studies with different high-
easy to achieve with individuals but quality research designs, instead of
very difficult to achieve with larger being limited to studies with only
units such as communities, schools, randomized experimental designs,
and classrooms (see below). an important question needs to be
Other things being equal, an inter- asked: Does the type of research de-
vention study in which experimental sign affect study outcomes? This is
and control units are matched or the subject of the article by David
statistically equated (for example, Weisburd, Cynthia Lum, and An-
using a prediction score) prior to thony Petrosino (2001 [this issue]),
interventionwhat is called a which is further elaborated by the
nonrandomized experimenthas following:
less internal validity than a random-
ized experiment. An intervention
study with no control group has even Assuming that experimental designs are
less internal validity since it fails to the gold standard for evaluating prac-
address many threats to internal tices and policies, it is important to ask
validity, such as history, maturation, what price we pay in including other
regression to the mean, and testing types of studies in our reviews of what
or instrumentation effects (Cook and works in crime and justice. Are we likely
Campbell 1979). to overestimate or underestimate the
positive effects of treatment? Or con-
In area-based studies, the best and versely, might we expect that the use of
most feasible design usually involves well-designed nonrandomized studies
before and after measures in compa- will lead to about the same conclusions as
rable experimental and control con- we would gain from randomized experi-
ditions, together with statistical mental evaluations? (52)
AN EVIDENCE-BASED APPROACH TO PREVENTING CRIME 165

Both medical and social science used for including (and excluding)
(criminal justice included) litera- studies is perhaps the most impor-
tures on the subject of research de- tant and controversial (42) in con-
sign affecting study outcomes are ducting systematic reviews. How
mixed, with some systematic reviews high to set the bar of methodological
reporting that nonrandomized stud- rigor as part of a review of the litera-
ies produce results with significantly ture, systematic review method or
larger effect sizes than randomized otherwise, is a question that all
studies while other reviews report researchers face. (For a brief discus-
the opposite. Using a sample of crimi- sion of this issue in the context of the
nal justice studies with outcome vote-count review method, see the
measures of crime (N = 308) from the note 1 and MacKenzie 2000.) We sup-
University of Maryland report port the inclusion of intervention
(Sherman et al. 1997), Weisburd, studies that use experimental or
Lum, and Petrosino (2001) find a quasi-experimental designs in sys-
moderate inverse relationship be- tematic reviews. Systematic reviews
tween the quality of the research de- of criminological interventions con-
sign, defined in terms of internal va- ducted under the name of the Camp-
lidity, and the outcomes reported in a bell Collaboration should, however,
study (64). A number of different set as a minimum for inclusion stud-
analyses (for example, highest-qual- ies with before and after measures in
ity nonrandomized versus random- comparable experimental and con-
ized experimental studies, excluding trol conditions, together with statis-
quasi-experimental studies) confirm tical control of extraneous variables.
the results. The main implication of Of course, for certain intervention
this finding, despite the authors not- modalities like multisystematic
ing that their work is preliminary therapy for chronic and violent
and hence that the findings should be young offenders and cognitive-
interpreted with caution, is that the behavioral skills training for offend-
findings of systematic reviews may ers, it will be possible for researchers
be bias ed by the inclus i on of to use only randomized experiments
nonrandomized studies. More re- because of the comparatively large
search is needed on this subject, ex- number of experimental evaluations
amining different intervention mo- in these areas.
dalities (for example, paren t M ore h i g h - qu al i t y res earch
training, hot spots policing) and set- designs of this sort and, of course,
tings in which crime prevention randomized experiments are needed
takes place (for example, families, in criminology. Experiments and
communities), as well as examining quasi-experiments should have large
if research design affects study out- samples, long follow-up periods, and
comes differently for individual- and follow-up interviews (Farrington
area-level studies. 1999). As noted above, sample size
In their article, Farrington and is particularly important for both
Petrosino (2001) note that the crite- individual- and area-based studies.
rion of methodological quality that is Long-term follow-ups are needed to
166 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

assess how long effects persist after here, of course, is fostering high-
the intervention ends. This informa- quality researchusing experimen-
tion may point to the need for booster tal and quasi-experimental evalua-
sessions. Long follow-ups are a rarity tion designson the effects of inter-
in criminological interventions and ventions. Another important process,
should be a top priority of funding likely (but disappointingly) to be
agencies. even more important than the
Research is needed to identify the research product, which we take up
active ingredients of successful (and in the next section, is the political
promising) crime prevention pro- and policy considerations that are
grams (Farrington 2000). Many pro- made about what evidence gets used
grams are multimodal, making it dif- and what does not.
ficult to isolate the independent Systematic reviews are the most
effects of different components. comprehensive method to assess the
Future experiments that attempt to effectiveness of crime prevention
disentangle the effects of different measures and, in an evidence-based
elements of the most successful pro- society, they would be the source that
grams are needed. Although not a governments would turn to for help
specific focus of this special issue, it is in the development of policy. This
also important that programs issue of The Annals reports on four
include, as part of the original systematic reviews of different crimi-
research design, provision for an eco- nological interventions. Each follows
nomic analysiseither a cost-benefit as closely as possible the methodol-
or cost-effectiveness analysisto ogy for conducting systematic
allow for an assessment of the eco- reviews as advocated by the Camp-
nomic efficiency of the program (see bell Collaboration, although it is
Welsh and Farrington 2000; Farring- important to note that none of these
ton, Petrosino, and Welsh 2001; reviews (or their research protocols)
Welsh, Farrington, and Sherman has yet been approved by the Camp-
2001). bell Collaboration. In this part we
briefly summarize the main findings
and discuss policy implications of
USING WHAT WORKS AND
STOPPING USING WHAT DOES these four systematic reviews.
NOT AND WHAT IS HARMFUL In the systematic review of parent
training and support before age 3 by
In an evidence-based society, gov- Odette Bernazzani, Catherine Ct,
ernment crime prevention policy and and Richard Tremblay (2001 [this
local practice would be based on issue]), seven studies were included,
interventions with demonstrated and effectiveness was assessed on
effectiveness in preventing crime the outcome measures of disruptive
and offending. Equally important, behavior (for example, opposition to
governments would put an end to adults, truancy, aggression) and
those interventions that do not work delinquency. The authors found that
and, more important, to those that three of the studies reported some
are harmful or iatrogenic. The key beneficial effects on disruptive
AN EVIDENCE-BASED APPROACH TO PREVENTING CRIME 167

behavior or delinquency (one also between boot camp participants and


reported some harmful effects), while their control group counterparts. On
the remaining four studies reported the basis of the evidence, the authors
no evidence of effectiveness. The concluded that boot camps do not
authors call for caution in interpret- reduce the risk of recidivism relative
ing the results (for example, due to to other existing criminal justice sys-
modest effect sizes of the beneficial tem forms of punishment and reha-
studies) and recommend further bilitation (131).
intervention studies in these areas. In the systematic review of cogni-
In Anthony Bragas (2001 [this tive-behavioral programs for offend-
issue]) systematic review of hot spots ers by Mark Lipsey, Gabrielle Chap-
policingtargeting of police enforce- man, and Nana Landenberger (2001
ment meas ures in high- cri me [this issue]), the authors bring
areasnine studies were included, together 14 of the highest-quality
and effectiveness was assessed on studies with outcome measures of
the outcome measures of crime and recidivism. The authors find that all
disorder. Findings suggested that but 1 of the studies reports beneficial
targeted police actions can prevent effects on recidivism, and a meta-
crime and disorder in hot spots. analysis reveals that the grand mean
Braga also investigated the effects of effect size across all 14 studies is sta-
the studies on the displacement of tistically significant. The authors
crime and the diffusion of crime con- also investigate the effectiveness of
trol benefits and found that evidence cognitive-behavioral treatment on
of displacement was rare and some recidivism for those studies carried
programs produced unintended out as research-oriented demon-
crime prevention benefits in areas
stration programs (144) (n = 8) and
that did not receive the treatment.
those carried out as part of ongoing
Braga concluded that results of this
criminal justice practice (n = 6). Find-
systematic review support the asser-
ings from the meta-analysis show
tion that focusing police efforts at
that the former find the largest recid-
high-activity crime places can be
ivism reductions of the total sample
used to good effect in preventing
of studies.
crime (121).
When viewed together, these four
In the systematic review of correc-
systematic reviews suggest three
tional boot camps by Doris MacKen-
main courses of action:
zie, David Wilson, and Suzanne
Kider (2001 [this issue]), 44 studies
were included, and effectiveness was 1. Increase funding of police ini-
assessed on recidivism. The authors tiatives targeted at crime hot spots
found beneficial effects on recidivism (to reduce crime and disorder) and
for 9 studies, harmful effects for 8 cognitive-behavioral programs for of-
studies, and no effects for the remain- fenders (to reduce recidivism);
ing 27 studies. A meta-analysis 2. Stop funding of correctional
found there to be no overall signifi- boot camps designed to reduce recidi-
cant differences in recidivism (126) vism; and
168 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

3. Initiate a program of research extensive knowledge base on these


to test the effects of parent training types of interventions. Information
and support programs during early on the effects on delinquency from
childhood on disruptive behavior and these new trials will of course take
delinquency. some time to become available. How-
ever, updates of this systematic
Acting on this evidence could ar- review may produce more informa-
guably make a difference to prevent- tion on delinquency in the short
ing crime, both now and in the long term. This is because, of the seven
run. In the first place, police enforce- studies included in the present sys-
ment of street-level crime and correc- tematic review, a number are longi-
tional treatment account for a large tudinal trials and are approaching
share of criminal justice expenditure the age of the participants (or the
on preventing future offending in the participants children) when delin-
community, and increasing the use of quency can be measured.
hot spots policing and cognitive-be-
havioral treatment for offenders as POLITICAL AND POLICY
part of the overall police and correc- CONSIDERATIONS
tional programming activities, re-
spectively, could produce impressive In any discussion about imple-
effects on crime. Such a shift in police menting new crime prevention pro-
enforcement efforts has been shown grams, expanding existing ones, or
to be a promising approach to pre- putting an end to ineffective or harm-
venting crime (Eck and Maguire ful ones, political and policy consider-
2000). ations are dominant. In fact, it is all
Second, the elimination of boot too common that the strength of the
camp programs from correctional programmatic evidence under con-
practice would free up public dollars sideration for developing policy
that could be used on interventions becomes secondary to the political
with proven effectiveness in reduc- and policy considerations of the day.
ing recidivism. The ending of boot Certainly, some of these consider-
camp programs would also have the ations are important. Other govern-
positive benefit of eliminating one ment priorities such as military
source of harmful or iatrogenic defense spending, environmental
effects among correctional practice, protection, and lower costs of pre-
which is an important aim in itself scription drugs for seniors are com-
(see Dishion, McCord, and Poulin peting for scarce public resources.
1999). National polls may show that the
Third, future demonstration trials public is more concerned with mat-
that test the effects of parent support ters of public policy other than crime
and training during early childhood prevention or community safety.
on disruptive behavior and delin- Other political considerations that
quency offer to contribute to a more are more g erman e t o cri me
AN EVIDENCE-BASED APPROACH TO PREVENTING CRIME 169

prevention include the perception on 3. If the environment of the pro-


the part of politicians that they are gram is relatively stable; and
being soft on crime by supporting 4. When the program is in a cri-
noncriminal justice crime preven- sis or paralysis, and nobody knows
tion efforts, as well as the short time what to do. Here the decision maker
horizons of politicians (Tonry and may turn to evaluation. (24-25)
Farrington 1995), which makes pro-
grams that show results only in the In commenting on evaluation re-
longer run less appealing to politi- search on gun control policy (equally
cians who are trying to get elected applicable to crime prevention policy
every few years. in general), Rosenfeld (2000) illumi-
How to overcome some of the mis- nated one of the hopeful outcomes of
conceived political and policy barri- an evidence-based approach to policy
ers in order to get more of what works development: although political con-
in preventing crime into policy and siderations will always play a promi-
practice is by no means an easy task, nent role in policy development, poli-
but fortunately it has received some tics that has to contend with the
results of good science should pro-
attention in criminology (for exam-
duce better policy than politics based
ple, Cullen forthcoming), public man-
on poor science or none at all (616).
agement (for example, Nutley and
Davies 2000; Nutley, Davies, and Til-
ley 2000), public health care (for APPENDIX
example, Millenson 1997; Halladay STUDY INCLUSION CRITERIA
and Bero 2000), and evaluation sci- FOR UNIVERSITY OF MARYLAND
ence (for example, Weiss 1998), to REPORT
name a few of the academic disci-
plines that have been investigating The scientific methods scale ranks
the subject. evaluation studies from 1 = weakest to 5 =
According to Weiss (1998), getting highest on overall internal validity:
research evidence to be utilized by
decision makers may be facilitated 1. Correlational evidence (low offending
under any one of the following four correlates with the program at a sin-
gle point in time);
scenarios:
2. No statistical control for selection bias
but some kind of comparison (for ex-
ample, program group compared with
1. If the implications of the find- nonequivalent control group; program
ings are relatively non-controversial, group measured before and after in-
neither provoking rifts in the organi- tervention, with no control group);
zation nor running into conflicting 3. Moderate statistical control (for exam-
interests; ple, program group compared with
comparable control group, including
2. If the changes that are im-
pre-post and experimental-control
plied are within the programs exist- comparisons);
ing repertoire and are relatively 4. Strong statistical control (for example,
small-scale; program group compared with control
170 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

group, with control of extraneous in- Note


fluences on the outcome, by matching,
prediction scores, or statistical con- 1. Here, Sherman and his colleagues (1997)
trols); and were faced with a dilemma in deciding how
5. Randomized experiment: units as- high the threshold of scientific evidence
signed at random to program and con- should be set for determining program effec-
trol groups prior to intervention. tiveness. A very conservative approach might
require at least two level 5 studies (that is, ran-
domized controlled experiments) showing that
What works. These are programs that
a program is effective (or ineffective), with the
the authors (Sherman et al. 1997) were preponderance of the remaining evidence in
reasonably certain prevent crime or re- favor of the same conclusion. Employing a
duce risk factors for crime in the kinds of threshold that high, however, would have left
social contexts in which they have been very little to say about crime prevention, based
evaluated and for which the findings can on the existing science. There was a clear
be generalized to similar settings in other tradeoff between the level of certainty in the
places and times. For a program to be answers that can be given about program ef-
classified as working, there must be a fectiveness and the level of useful information
that can be gleaned from the available science.
minimum of two level 3 studies with sig-
Sherman and his colleagues took the middle
nificance tests demonstrating effective- road between reaching very few conclusions
ness and the preponderance of evidence with great certainty and reaching very many
in support of the same conclusion. conclusions with very little certainty. This is a
dilemma that also faces other social science
What does not work. These are pro- disciplines such as education and social work.
grams that the authors were reasonably
certain fail to prevent crime or reduce
risk factors for crime, using the identical References
scientific criteria used for deciding what Barnett, W. Steven. 1996. Lives in the
works. For the classification of not work- Balance: Age-27 Benefit-Cost Analysis
ing, there must be a minimum of two level of the High/Scope Perry Preschool
3 studies with significance tests showing Program. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope
ineffectiveness and the preponderance of Press.
evidence in the same direction. Bernazzani, Odette, Catherine Ct, and
Richard E. Tremblay. 2001. Early Par-
What is promising. These are pro- ent Training to Prevent Disruptive
grams for which the level of certainty Behavior Problems and Delinquency
from available evidence is too low to sup- in Children. Annals of the American
port generalizable conclusions but for Academy of Political and Social Sci-
which there is some empirical basis for ence 578:90-103.
predicting that further research could Braga, Anthony A. 2001. Effects of Hot
support such conclusions. For the classifi- Spots Policing on Crime. Annals of the
cation of promising, at least one level 3 American Academy of Political and
study is required with significance tests Social Science 578:104-125.
showing effectiveness and the prepon- Cook, Thomas D. and Donald T. Camp-
derance of evidence in support of the bell. 1979. Quasi-Experimentation:
same conclusion. Design and Analysis Issues for Field
What is unknown. Any program not Settings. Chicago: Rand McNally.
classified in one of the three above cate- Cullen, Francis T. Forthcoming. Rehabili-
gories is considered to have unknown ef- tation and Treatment Programs. In
fects. Crime and Public Policy. 2d ed., ed.
AN EVIDENCE-BASED APPROACH TO PREVENTING CRIME 171

James Q. Wilson and Joan Petersilia. Using Family-Based Interventions.


San Francisco: Institute of Contempo- Children and Society 13:287-303.
rary Studies Press. Greenwood, Peter W., Lynn A. Karoly, Su-
Dishion, Thomas J., Joan McCord, and san S. Everingham, Jill Houb, M.
Franois Poulin. 1999. When Inter- Rebecca Kilburn, C. Peter Rydell, Mat-
ventions Harm: Peer Groups and thew Sanders, and James Chiesa.
Problem Behavior. American Psychol- 2001. Estimating the Costs and Bene-
ogist 54:755-64. fits of Early Childhood Interventions:
Eck, John E. and Edward R. Maguire. Nurse Home Visits and the Perry Pre-
2000. Have Changes in Policing Re- school. In Costs and Benefits of Pre-
duced Violent Crime? An Assessment venting Crime, ed. Brandon C. Welsh,
of the Evidence. In The Crime Drop in David P. Farrington, and Lawrence W.
America, ed. Alfred Blumstein and Sherman. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Joel Wallman. New York: Cambridge Halladay, Mark and Lisa Bero. 2000. Im-
University Press. plementing Evidence-Based Practice
Farrington, David P. 1983. Randomized in Health Care. Public Money & Man-
Experiments on Crime and Justice. In agement 20:43-50.
Crime and Justice: An Annual Review Hope, Tim. 1995. Community Crime Pre-
of Research. Vol. 4, ed. Michael Tonry vention. In Building a Safer Society:
and Norval Morris. Chicago: Univer- Strategic Approaches to Crime Preven-
sity of Chicago Press. tion. Vol. 19, Crime and Justice: A Re-
. 1997. Evaluating a Community view of Research, ed. Michael Tonry
Crime Prevention Program. Evalua- and David P. Farrington. Chicago:
tion 3:157-73. University of Chicago Press.
. 1999. A Criminological Research Johnson, Byron R., Spencer De Li, David
Agenda for the Next Millennium. In- B. Larson, and Michael McCullough.
ternational Journal of Offender Ther- 2000. A Systematic Review of the Reli-
apy and Comparative Criminology giosity and Delinquency Literature: A
43:154-67. Research Note. Journal of Contempo-
. 2000. Explaining and Preventing rary Criminal Justice 16:32-52.
Crime: The Globalization of Knowl- Lipsey, Mark W., Gabrielle L. Chapman,
edgeThe American Society of Crimi- and Nana A. Landenberger. 2001. Cog-
nology 1999 Presidential Address. nitive-Behavioral Programs for Of-
Criminology 38:1-24. fenders. Annals of the American Acad-
Farrington, David P. and Anthony emy of Political and Social Science
Petrosino. 2001. The Campbell Collab- 578:144-157.
oration Crime and Justice Group. An- MacKenzie, Doris Layton. 2000. Evi-
nals of the American Academy of Polit- dence-Based Corrections: Identifying
ical and Social Science 578:35-49. What Works. Crime & Delinquency
Farrington, David P., Anthony Petrosino, 46:457-71.
and Brandon C. Welsh. 2001. System- MacKenzie, Doris Layton, David B. Wil-
atic Reviews and Cost-Benefit Analy- son, and Suzanne B. Kider. 2001. Ef-
ses of Correctional Interventions. The fects of Correctional Boot Camps on
Prison Journal 81:338-58. Offending. Annals of the American
Farrington, David P. and Brandon C. Academy of Political and Social Sci-
Welsh. 1999. Delinquency Prevention ence 578:126-143.
172 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Millenson, Michael L. 1997. Demanding Crime: What Works, What Doesnt,


Medical Excellence: Doctors and Ac- Whats Promising. Washington, DC:
countability in the Information Age. U.S. Department of Justice, National
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Institute of Justice.
Nutley, Sandra and Huw T. O. Davies. Tonry, Michael and David P. Farrington.
2000. Making a Reality of Evidence- 1995. Strategic Approaches to Crime
Based Practice: Some Lessons from Prevention. In Building a Safer Soci-
the Diffusion of Innovations. Public ety: Strategic Approaches to Crime
Money & Management 20:35-42. Prevention. Vol. 19, Crime and Justice:
Nutley, Sandra, Huw T. O. Davies, and A Review of Research, ed. Michael
Nick Tilley. 2000. Editorial: Getting Tonry and David P. Farrington. Chi-
Research into Practice. Public Money & cago: University of Chicago Press.
Management 20:3-6. Visher, Christy A. and David Weisburd.
Petrosino, Anthony. 2000. How Can We 1998. Identifying What Works: Recent
Respond Effectively to Juvenile Trends in Crime Prevention Strat-
Crime? Pediatrics 105:635-37. egies. Crime, Law and Social Change
Petrosino, Anthony, Robert F. Boruch, 28:223-42.
Haluk Soydan, Lorna Duggan, and Weisburd, David, Cynthia M. Lum, and
Julio Sanchez-Meca. 2001. Meeting Anthony Petrosino. 2001. Does Re-
the Challenges of Evidence-Based Pol- search Design Affect Study Out-
icy: The Campbell Collaboration. An- comes? Findings from the Maryland
nals of the American Academy of Polit- Report Criminal Justice Sample. An-
ical and Social Science 578:14-34. nals of the American Academy of Polit-
Rosenfeld, Richard. 2000. Tracing the ical and Social Science 578:50-70.
Brady Acts Connection with Homi- Weiss, Carol H. 1998. Have We Learned
cide and Suicide Trends. Journal of Anything New About the Use of Eval-
the American Medical Association uation? American Journal of Evalua-
284:616-18. tion 19:21-33.
Schweinhart, Lawrence J., Helen V. We l s h , B r a n d o n C. a n d D av i d P.
Barnes, and David P. Weikart. 1993. Farrington. 2000. Monetary Costs and
Significant Benefits: The High/Scope Benefits of Crime Prevention Pro-
Perry Preschool Study Through Age grams. In Crime and Justice: A Review
27. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press. of Research. Vol. 27, ed. Michael Tonry.
Sherman, Lawrence W. 1998. Evidence- Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Based Policing. In Ideas in American Welsh, Brandon C., David P. Farrington,
Policing. Washington, DC: Police and Lawrence W. Sherman. 2001. Im-
Foundation. proving Confidence in What Works
S her m a n, L aw r enc e W. , D av i d P. and Saves Money in Preventing
Farrington, Brandon C. Welsh, and Crime: Priorities for Research. In
Doris Layton MacKenzie, eds. Forth- Costs and Benefits of Preventing
coming. Evidence-Based Crime Pre- Crime, ed. Brandon C. Welsh, David P.
vention. London: Routledge. Fa r r i n g t o n , a n d L aw r e n c e W.
Sherman, Lawrence W., Denise C. Sherman. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Gottfredson, Doris Layton MacKen- Welsh, Brandon C. and Akemi Hoshi.
zie, John E. Eck, Peter Reuter, and Forthcoming. Communities and
Shawn D. Bushway. 1997. Preventing Crime Prevention. In Evidence-Based
AN EVIDENCE-BASED APPROACH TO PREVENTING CRIME 173

Crime Prevention, ed. Lawrence W. Wilson, David B. 2001. Meta-Analytic


Sherman, David P. Farrington, Methods for Criminology. Annals of
Brandon C. Welsh, and Doris Layton the American Academy of Political and
MacKenzie. London: Routledge. Social Science 578:71-89.
Book Department
INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS AND POLITICS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
AFRICA, ASIA, AND LATIN AMERICA . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 179
EUROPE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 186
UNITED STATES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 189
SOCIOLOGY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 197
ECONOMICS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 201

THE ANNALS
BOOK DEPARTMENT
OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS clearly about important differences


AND POLITICS between totalitarianism and authoritari-
anism. Elements common to most taxon-
BROOKER, PAUL. 2000. Non-Demo- omies include the role of the supreme
cratic Regimes. Pp. viii, 288. New York: leader, the nature and ideology (if any) of
St. Martins. Paperbound, $21.95. the official party (if such a party exists),
the degree of political terror, social mobi-
Despite advances in governance, the lization, and societal penetration.
world remains characterized by a host The core of the book adopts a thematic
and variety of tyrannical and oppressive approach analyzing the most common
regimes. Today the concepts of authori- form of dictatorships in modern times:
tarian and totalitarian are routinely and military and party dictatorships.
indiscriminately employed by commen- Brookers careful schema brings out the
tators interested in all-inclusive generic novel aspects of modern nondemocracies
labels to various autocracies, despotism, relating to political control and social or-
dictatorships, satraps, or tyrannies. Paul ganization and expertly clarifies these
Brookers book provides a current and concepts by comparative cross-national
thoughtful examination of modern non- analysis. Included in this are case studies
democratic regimes that in their various of Chiles General Pinochet, Paraguays
forms, dominated the twentieth century Alfredo Stroessner, and Iraqs Saddam
and continue to thrive in the twenty-first Hussein, who transformed theirs into
century. personalist dictatorships; also consid-
Two opening chapters effectively draw ered is how dictatorships make and im-
the reader in, examining how classic plement policies and whether they are
and leading theorists have sought to ex- effective in promoting economic growth.
plain and categorize nondemocracies. Penultimate chapters examine the
Brookers careful and thorough literature challenge of democratization and how
review illustrates how totalitarianism some modern semidemocracies/semi-
emerged as an analytic concept during dictatorships (such as Mexicos PRI/
the World War II as a generation of social Presidentialist regime, or Perus Albert
scientists attempted to understand the Fujimori) have sought to disguise them-
distinctive characteristics of the Hitler, selves as democratic regimes. Brookers
Stalin, and (to a lesser extent) Mussolini concluding chapter cautions current, con-
regimes. Examining the works of ventional wisdom that democratization
Hannah Arendt, Carl J. Friedrich, is inevitable everywhere and that dicta-
Michael Curtis, Juan Linz, Amos Perl- torships are headed for the historical
mutter, Leonard Shapiro, and others, scrap heap. Democratically disguised,
Brooker shows how and why various sys- bureaucratic semidictatorships of Mex-
tems of classification help scholars think icos or Perus ilk, rather than blatant mil-

174
BOOK DEPARTMENT 175

itary regimes, are probably the future Amendment would have provided for the
models. United States. This was in 1982; the rele-
Brookers book is a well-documented, vant sections of the Canadian Charter of
sobering analysis of the oldest and most Rights would have been significantly
common form of government: non- weaker without the intervention of
democracy. His book also has important women activists. A second attempt at
foreign policy implications for the United constitutional revision, the decentraliz-
States in its relationships with a plethora ing, Quebec-focused Meech Lake Accord,
of less than democratic systems around failed to achieve the necessary provincial
the world. This is an excellent book for ratification in 1987, partly because of op-
upper-division or graduate courses in po- position by womens groups. Then, in
litical science. 1992, a national referendum rejected a
PAUL C. SONDROL packet of constitutional amendments
University of Colorado that would, among other things, have
Colorado Springs subordinated womens rights to commu-
nal claims. Canadas largest national
womens organization, the National Ac-
tion Committee on the Status of Women,
was conspicuous among the many oppo-
DOBROWOLSKY, ALEXANDRA. 2000. nents of the Charlottetown Accord; other
Th e Po l i t i c s o f P r a g m a t i s m . women and womens groups were among
Women, Representation, and Constitu- the prominent supporters.
tionalism in Canada. Pp. xii, 320. Don The role of women activists in the first
Mills, Canada: Oxford University of these episodes has already been stud-
Press. Paperbound, Canada$24.95. ied at some length, but Dobrowolsky has
The Politics of Pragmatism recounts been able to add previously unpublished
the activism of national womens groups material from interviews and private ar-
in Canada as they attempted to influence chives. Womens political responses to the
the series of constitutional changes pro- Meech Lake Accord have had almost no
posed in the years 1980 to 1992. These attention, and their organized involve-
proposals, which first fascinated and ment with the campaign to defeat the
then appalled even the mavens of Cana- Charlottetown Accord has had little
dian politics, are hardly familiar to any- more, so most of the relevant, detailed
one else. Even fewer are aware of the role discussion in this book is new. Dobro-
played by women and womens groups in wolsky shows that as we might expect,
the process. As Alexandra Dobrowolsky there has been continuing attention to
is able to demonstrate, in Canada as else- the constitutional process from national
where, womens groups are now active, womens groups. She also interestingly
continuously, in all forms of conventional demonstrates how media and politicians
and unconventional politics. If anything, have lumbered womens activism with la-
the books focus on national groups (as bels that would delegitimize its political
well as the identity of those interviewed) role: the womens movement is only a spe-
understates the amount and variety of cial interest group (therefore elitist and
feminist activism across Canada. unrepresentative) or a social movement
Dobrowolskys topic includes one of (therefore with no political or policy
the icons of contemporary Canadian fem- standing).
inism, the successful enshrinement in As its title and subtitle suggest, The
the Canadian constitution of equality Politics of Pragmatism is inclined to draw
rights superior to what the Equal Rights rather sweeping theoretical conclusions
176 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

from its case study material. Certainly, a as global forces and internal challenges
more experienced scholar would not be so produce competing claims for entitle-
surprised to learn of excellent efforts by ments, recognition, and resources. Few
womens groupsand less excellent re- states are exempt from such tensions.
sults. Nor would she feel obliged to refute However, crises related to sovereignty
the many monocausal theories that ab- pose a special significance for nations
stract from the complex realities of politi- that are struggling to acknowledge and
cal action. For example, it is hardly redefine their relationships with indige-
necessary to prove that womens organi- nous populations. Augie Fleras and Paul
zations in Canada have, in spite of mis- Spoonley, in an insightful and provoca-
givings, worked with and through tive recent book, highlight critical dimen-
political parties. In addition, the knowl- sions of these challenges for New Zealand
edgeable reader will look at The Politics (Aotearoa) and its relations with Maori.
of Pragmatism and remember what the Fleras and Spoonley present a con-
protagonists are like as individuals. Tact vincing case for a reconstituted vision of
and self-protection together probably dic- New Zealand through multiculturalism
tated the absence of such a personal di- with a bi-national framework, which ac-
mension, but it is important for the study knowledges in substantive ways the need
of politics in a large country with a small to create effective spheres of Maori self-
population of activists. government within a nation constituted
Feminism is not extinct, and the by increasing diversity. While their posi-
womens movement has not been ren- tion is rooted in an understanding of de-
dered obsolete; these facts make a differ-
tails specific to the history, composition,
ence for mainstream politics. This book is
and dynamics of New Zealand society,
therefore to be recommended to those in-
their analysis carries implications that
terested in constitutional politics as well
extend even beyond the most obvious ref-
as that more obvious audience whose con-
erents to comparable societies like the
cern is the involvement of women with
United States, Canada, and Australia in
politics.
which recent changes in immigration
NAOMI BLACK
patterns are layered on a white settler co-
York University lonialism imposed over long-standing
Toronto forms of social organization among indig-
Canada enous peoples.
Mount Saint Vincent University Fleras and Spoonley begin with a re-
Halifax thinking of New Zealand history from a
Canada Maori perspective to highlight contend-
ing visions that accompany efforts to re-
alize a meaningful postcolonial state.
They do not define the issue by whether
there is any legitimacy to claims to
FLERAS, AUGIE and PAUL SPOON-
indigenous self-determination, since
LEY. 1999. Recalling Aotearoa: Indig-
indigenous rights are recognized and
enous Politics and Ethnic Relations in
guaranteed in a legal and historical
New Zealand. Pp. xiv, 288. Auckland,
framework; they define an issue of dis-
NZ: Oxford University Press. $29.95.
agreement over the nature, scope, and
The meaning and nature of nation- specific manifestations of Maori sover-
states, national identity, and social cohe- eignty in relation to the sovereignty of
sion are becoming increasingly contested the nation-state. The British crowns rec-
BOOK DEPARTMENT 177

ognition of indigenous sovereignty (for- The book is not without problems. A


malized in the 1840 Treaty of Waitangi), more direct and integrated discussion of
even if asserted to accomplish immediate the key issues, especially in the early
colonial objectives to exercise dominion chapters, would make the work more ac-
over land, labor, and resources, has had cessible to an extended readership (espe-
the contradictory impact of leaving indig- cially those with a limited knowledge of
enous people and their societies vulnera- New Zealand issues) to whom the book
ble to marginalization and subordination should be made available. A more focused
at the same time as they are ensured a analysis of socioeconomic conditions, in-
special status as distinct collectivities ternal population differentiation, and
whose rights and identities derive from key institutional domains that charac-
their entitlements as original occupants terize Maori-state relations, comparable
of the land. The true challenges are em- to that devoted in chapter 6 to the Tagata
bedded along the pathways leading to re- PasifikaNew Zealanders with South
alization of meaningful forms of Maori Pacific originswould also be benefi-
sovereignty within contemporary politi- cial. Nonetheless, Recalling Aotearoa is
cal economies. an important book with respect to both
A major strength of the book lies in the its explicit focus on the politics of
authors willingness to embrace and dis- indigeneity in New Zealand and its wider
cuss key contradictions or paradoxes im- contributions to an understanding of ten-
plicated in efforts to negotiate indigenous sions and issues related to sovereignty,
rights, sovereignty, and national identi- diversity, and identity.
ties. These include extended discussions TERRY WOTHERSPOON
of relations among individual and collec-
tive rights, socially cohesive and divisive University of Saskatchewan
forces, colonialism and postcolonial resis- Saskatoon
tance, internal differentiation within dis- Canada
tinct social groups, and the politics of
multiculturalism and ethnic identity. In
particular, Fleras and Spoonley integrate
the analysis of indigenous politics with LAMMERS, WILLIAM W. and MI-
an understanding of the material and CHAEL A. GENOVESE. 2000. The
ideational circumstances within which Presidency and Domestic Policy: Com-
they are played out. Whereas academic paring Leadership Styles, FDR to
literature and policy frameworks alike Clinton. Pp. xii, 383.
tend either to subsume consideration of
indigenous peoples under broader atten- Could it be that Roosevelt, Johnson,
tion to race and ethnic relations or to iso- or Reagan were actually underachiev-
late the analysis of issues relevant to ers? William Lammers and Michael
indigenous people from those that per- Genovese ask in the early pages of their
tain to immigrants and racialized minor- work. Their question is posed not so much
ities, the authors here emphasize the to buck conventional wisdom but rather
complex dynamics involved in strategies to make the point that presidents should
to ensure social cohesion amid competing be evaluated and compared based on the
pressures generated by racial and multi- relative climate of opportunities that
cultural diversity, indigenous rights and they faced. To do this, the authors divide
sovereignty, broader state objectives, and the presidents since Franklin D. Roose-
global challenges. velt into three categories: Roosevelt,
178 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Johnson, and Reagan are categorized as overview. Particularly important is the


high-opportunity presidents; Truman, emphasis on policy outcomes and results
Eisenhower, and Kennedy are classified as a major component of comparing presi-
as moderate-opportunity presidents; and dential effectiveness in policy making.
Nixon, Carter, Bush, and Clinton are con- The last chapter compares the accom-
sidered low-opportunity presidents. The plishments and the effectiveness of the
major part of the book consists of an indi- presidents since Franklin D. Roosevelt. It
vidual chapter on each of these presi- is both the best chapter of the book and
dents, assessing their effectiveness in the one most subject to dispute as presi-
domestic policy making given their rela- dents are ranked within their opportu-
tive opportunity for leadership. nity level. Among the high-opportunity
Chapter 1 outlines Lammers and presidents, Roosevelt is ranked first, fol-
Genoveses strategy for assessing presi- lowed by Johnson, with Reagan last. Of
dents. Four dimensions of presidential the moderate-opportunity presidents,
activity are compared: approaches to ad- Truman is first, Eisenhower second, and
visory processes and decision making, ad- Kennedy third. Among the four low-op-
ministrative strategies, public leadership, portunity presidents, Nixon is rated
and congressional leadership. Although highest, Clinton second, and Carter and
many of the indicators of presidential Bush are tied for last. Many readers may
skills and dimensions of the political en- dispute these conclusions or even the cat-
vironment are not new, they are assem- egorization of certain presidents. For ex-
bled in an innovative way. Lammers and ample, one could argue that Clintons
Genovese effectively summarize helpful opportunity levels were closer to Eisen-
influences and major limitations facing howers than to Bushs. Nixons high
the last 10 presidents. ranking among low-opportunity presi-
Chapters 2 through 10 examine each dents is also questionable, given that his
presidency, beginning with Roosevelt and rather substantial legislative record was
the high-opportunity presidents and end- largely pushed by Congress.
ing with Clinton and the low-opportunity Studies of individual presidencies are
presidents. Each chapter combines a often detailed and rich but usually too id-
wide-ranging array of information on iosyncratic for meaningful comparisons.
each chief executive in addition to the fac- Conversely, all presidential typologies
tors listed above: personal characteris- oversimplify to some extent, and that is
tics, career path, personality traits, policy the case here. Overall, however, this book
views, major challenges, major legisla- largely succeeds and is a fine addition to
tive enactments, and success with Con- the literature on the presidency. Perhaps
gress. Looking at this sweeping array of no other book covers as much ground on
factors for 10 presidents while keeping twentieth-century presidents and the
this book to less than 400 pages necessi- policy achievements of their administra-
tates a certain degree of superficiality in tions in a truly comparative fashion. As
coverage, particularly for the taste of explained in the preface, this book is pri-
presidential scholars. For example, marily the work of William Lammers,
rarely is more than a page devoted to who submitted a draft of the manuscript
such complex questions as psychological shortly before his death. Michael
factors and presidential personality. For Genovese, his former student and col-
students and those new to the presidency league, saw the manuscript through to
literature, however, it is an excellent publication. It is a fine tribute to the
BOOK DEPARTMENT 179

scholarship and broad perspective of transportation to the polls, ballot access,


Lammers and is recommended to and alternative voting arrangements.
readers. There is another chapter full of dos and
LANCE T. LELOUP donts relative to telephone interviews,
focus groups, campaign literature, televi-
Washington State University
sion ads, campaigning among disabled
Pullman
senior voters, and how to make voting
easier and more secure.
There are seven chapters in the book
with nearly 100 tables and figures. More
MACMANUS, SUSAN A. 2000. Targeting specifically, the book describes the grow-
Senior Voters: Campaign Outreach to ing size and influence of senior voters, re-
Elders and Others with Special Needs. ports on national survey results on what
Pp. xxi, 225. Lanham, MD: Rowman & seniors think about a variety of social is-
Littlefield. $59.00. Paperbound, sues, and identifies the best ways to tar-
$21.95. get and communicate with senior
citizens. Thus one feature that makes
Susan MacManus provides a compre- this book rather unique is the combina-
hensive analysis of the importance, role, tion of descriptive and prescriptive mate-
and influence of senior citizens in Ameri- rial. It provides both an in-depth analysis
can politics. Political candidates and con- of the attitudes, opinions, and political
sultants have long recognized the behaviors of senior voters and a descrip-
importance of senior Americans. Indeed, tive guide on how to target them.
they vote, they contribute funds, they In light of the 2000 presidential elec-
stay informed of issues, and they voice tion and the problems with voting in
their opinions. In the next two decades, many jurisdictions of Florida, Targeting
the baby boomers will continue to con- Senior Voters: Campaign Outreach to El-
tribute to the graying of America. By ders and Others with Special Needs
2030, those older than 65 will comprise should be mandatory reading for all elec-
more than 20 percent of the population. tion officials. In addition, this book would
Thus anyone who wishes to seek an elec- be a valuable resource guide for political
toral office or work as staff members for candidates, office holders, consultants,
politicians, political parties, or special in- special interests groups, and members of
terest groups must target senior citizens the media.
in terms of message and issues.
ROBERT E. DENTON, JR.
The volume is a comprehensive re-
source guide to understanding senior cit- Virginia Polytechnic Institute and
izens by including original survey State University
information on how to talk with them, Blacksburg
how they view important issues, and how
to help them maximize civic participa-
tion. The analysis goes beyond simple
techniques of pandering to full apprecia- AFRICA, ASIA, AND
tion of the values and contributions elder LATIN AMERICA
Americans may provide the political pro-
cess. For example, there is a full chapter KOLST, PL. 2000. Political Construc-
dedicated to the issue of helping senior tion Sites: Nation-Building in Russia
citizens register and vote. MacManus ad- and the Post-Soviet States. Pp. xi, 308.
dresses such topics as registration site Boulder, CO: Westview. $65.00. Paper-
accessibility, registering the disabled, bound, $24.00.
180 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

The first thing that should be noted Bukovina, the Donbass, and much stud-
about this book is that it is primarily his- ied Crimea and Galicia). There is slightly
torical and descriptive. Despite the in- more than one page on Chechnya and six
triguing conceptualization that is pages on Tatarstan. Even a specialist on
contained in the title, University of Oslo the Baltic region would appreciate
professor Pl Kolst is primarily con- Kolsts study of the Latgale province of
cerned with the background conditions Latvia.
that gave former Soviet republics the his- Another refreshing feature of this
toric opportunity to become nation build- book is its Scandinavian perspective.
ers. His survey is more far reaching than Whether it is invoking the pioneering
comparable studies published in recent work of Norwegian Stein Rokkan, citing
years. The countries examined include the research results of not-well-known
Russia (the Russian diasporas found in Danish and Swedish academics, or draw-
the other new independent states are ing comparisons to the unique status of
treated in a separate chapter), Latvia, Finlands land islands, the North Amer-
Ukr a i ne, Bel a r us, Mol d o v a , a n d ican reader will be enriched by exposure
Kazakhstan. A decade after Soviet disin- to new ideas. Indeed Kolsts work is just
tegration, few authors are brave enough one example of the high-quality scholar-
to draw comparisons between so many ship produced in recent years by Scandi-
and such diverse former Soviet republics. navian academics studying former Soviet
To be sure, Kolst is writing for under- republics. The anecdotal information
graduate students and a general reader- that Kolst relates on national airlines,
ship. As an introductory text, this book postage stamps, soccer matches, and
has much to recommend it. Instead of Olympic athletes in the new states
simply a sequence of case studies of select should not be treated as trivia. Rather
new states, it begins with useful over- they confirm his extensive knowledge
views of such topics as nation building, about the societies he writes about. More-
construction of historiography, and reli- over he demonstrates the degree to which
gious pathways. Certainly a more robust nation building has been micromanaged
chapter on theorizing about nation build- in these new countries.
ing and social integration would be desir- The last chapter of Political Construc-
able. As it is, the attention given to a tion Sites attempts to test certain theo-
handful of experts (Walker Connor, Karl r i e s o f n a t i o n b u i l d i n g ( s u ch a s
Deutsch, Ernest Gellner, and Arend consociationalism and ethnic democracy)
Lijphart are the featured scholars) does in light of Kolsts empirical findings
not do credit to this exceptionally fertile about the new states. Kolst concludes,
field. rightly, that not all of them will be suc-
Kolsts major achievement is to alert cessful in nation building. But he infers,
the reader to the diversity between and confusingly, that there is good reason to
within the new states. His accounts of mi- expect that at least some of them will join
norities in the new countries are particu- the international community of nation-
larly informative and also idiosyncratic. states. Even if they may qualify only as
He piques the interest of the reader with quasi-states today, that does not mean
references to the ethnic complexities of they are doomed to remain in this cate-
Moldova (for example, the recently estab- gory forever. The contemporary interna-
lished Gagauz Yeri territory) and of tional system already comprises quasi
Ukraine (Transcarpathia, Northern states from several continents. This unof-
BOOK DEPARTMENT 181

ficial status does not affect their legal sta- tional developments, and ultimate suc-
tus. Nor do quasi states consist only of cesses and failures are conditioned. To
failed nation builders. describe these networks, and to trace
RAY TARAS their ongoing impacts, they draw on the
abundant secondary literature on Chil-
Tulane University
New Orleans ean parties and politics and on open-
Louisiana ended interviews of leaders, militants,
and members of both parties that they
conducted during the 1980s and early
1990s. On these bases, they describe in of-
ten rich detail the origins, early develop-
LOMNITZ, LARISSA ADLER and ANA ment, and coming of age of these parties
MELNICK. 2000. Chiles Political Cul- and nicely capture the flavor of their re-
ture and Parties, An Anthropological spective projects and their contributions
Explanation. Pp. ix, 161. Notre Dame, to pre-1973 Chilean politics, although in
IN, University of Notre Dame Press. each instance they largely reinforce the
$26.00. Paperbound, $14.00. images or portraits that the existing lit-
erature provides of these parties.
This is an English-language version of
It is not clear to me, however, that
a study published in Chile in 1998 by the
Lomnitz and Melnick really test their ap-
Fondo de Cultura Economica. The au-
proach or show its superior explanatory
thors, one a naturalized Mexican citizen
power (vis--vis alternative perspectives)
who lived in Chile for many years, and
in dealing with either Chilean parties
the other a Chilean, both teach at
generally or their evolution beyond 1973.
Universidad Nacional Autnoma de
They argue that contrasting Radical and
Mxico but frequently work on Chilean
Christian Democratic identities have en-
topics. In this essay/study, they analyze
dured over the years thanks to the con-
two political parties, the avowedly secu-
lar and entirely middle-class Radical tinuing vitality of underlying social
party, and the multiclass and largely networks, the ongoing appeal of values
Catholic Christian Democratic party, and symbols associated with their re-
from the vantage points of their respec- spective subcultures, and their success-
tive political cultures. In their view, polit- ful and not so successful electoral and
ical parties cannot be understood political experiences. They further con-
independent of the subjective attitudes of tend that horizontally linked networks
their leaders, militants, and rank-and- continue to provide the basis for internal
file members and supporters. But they do tendencies and factions within the par-
not believe that such attitudes are ade- ties and that they enable party members
quately captured by survey data alone to restrain or hold their party leaders
and opt for studying the formative expe- more accountable in Chile than in coun-
riences and relationships that in their tries like Mexico. But I do not believe that
view give them meaning and legitimacy. Lomnitz and Melnick look carefully
Lomnitz and Melnick view party cul- enough at either the distant or the not-so-
tures as emerging from formative social distant past to establish connections
networks (which are clubs, regions, among social networks, party tendencies,
schools, and family settings) to which and party life. Although claiming to ex-
leaders and activists are initially at- plain key party choices (of candidates, al-
tracted or exposed, and by which their liances, or platforms) at crucial junc-
subsequent initiatives, decisions, fac- tures, they do not look closely enough at
182 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

any of these events, at the changing con- perous, socially progressive, and Euro-
texts in which they take place, or at the peanized republics in Latin America.
other factors that may or may not have During the 1970s, these characteristics
played important roles. And it is there- were supplemented by an unflattering
fore difficult to compare their assertions one: all three were governed by authori-
or conclusions with those reached by tarian regimes that exercised repression
other analysts. as a preferred tool of political control and
In fact, aside from asserting that fac- social intimidation.
tions or tendencies do exist and are based This book recounts the events leading
on underlying social networks, Lomnitz to the installation of dictatorial rule and
and Melnick tend to treat these parties as the subsequent use of repression against
monoliths. In the case of the PDC, for ex- political activists and dissenters. When
ample, Lomnitz and Melnick emphasize authoritarian rule ended (1983 in Argen-
its strong identification with the progres- tina, 1985 in Uruguay, and 1989 in Chile)
sive social Christian tradition but fail to these nations entered a period of democ-
note that both the meaning and content ratization during which societies and po-
of this social Christian tradition were litical elites had to deal with the
vigorously debated from the late 1940s on restoration of civil government and si-
(when a second generation of activists en- multaneously engage in a critical exami-
tered the Falange), and rival internal ten- nation of what occurred in the past. To
dencies have attempted to fuse social different degrees, in each of these coun-
Christianity with things as dissimilar as tries the politics of reconciliation had to
neocapitalist and development (Frei balance the search for truth and justice
Montalva), purist independence from with the urgent need to establish viable
both Left and Right (Castillo), and Chris- democratic regimes. Public agendas had
tian socialism in alliance with the tradi- to incorporate a necessary commitment
tional left (Silva, Chonchol, and others) toward human rights protection, but for
as early as the PDCs 1959 founding con- the sake of keeping the state functioning,
gress. a compromise had to be established be-
MICHAEL FLEET tween memory and oblivion. Obviously,
Marquette University this threw into question the primacy that
Milwaukee the human rights discourse held in the
Wisconsin public affairs and private lives of individ-
uals. Not dealing with the crimes of the
past and allowing for impunity disturbed
the sensitivities of many who held that
collective morality was an integral part of
RO NI G ER, L UI S a nd M A R I O
the national soul. On the other hand, the
SZNAJDER. 1999. The Legacy of Hu-
majority sentiment was that indulging in
man Rights Violations in the Southern
recriminations or fueling revenges did
Cone, Argentina, Chile and Uruguay.
little to appease the national social con-
Oxford Studies in Democratization.
science.
Pp. xvi, 367. Oxford, UK: Oxford Uni-
Although all three countries share
versity Press. $82.00.
many common sociocultural traits, their
Argentina, Chile, and Uruguay, the historical and political paths diverge con-
three temperate countries of South siderably, so it is erroneous to assume
America, were considered the most pros- that repression and expiation ran similar
BOOK DEPARTMENT 183

courses in each of them. Argentina was tation of the sad events that occurred in
shaken most by both processes, as lack of the recent history of Argentina, Chile,
accountability and an attitude of disre- and Uruguay. The book is structured in a
spect for the powerless nurtured a defi- way that permits a clear understanding
ance and cynicism that made recognition of the issues involved, and the abundant
of past atrocities more difficult and con- background information substantiates
trition less frequent. Consequently, the every aspect of the redemocratization
healing process has taken longer and has and reconciliation processes. Moreover,
been more bitter than in its sister repub- in the discussion of the transgressions
lics. Uruguays use of violent repression committed, Roniger and Sznajder invoke
during the dictatorship years (1972- enlightening interpretations that come
1985) was less encompassing, and the from current theories explaining authori-
subsequent reconciliation was carried tarianism, political abuse, and violations
out in a spirit of compromise. The concil- of civil rights. Such frameworks allow for
iatory and pragmatic character of Uru- a theory-informed view of the issues at
guayans permitted the resumption of play in the Southern Cone during these
political dialogue, and redemocratization troubled years. In this light, the abuses
proceeded without the antagonistic tones do not appear as unique South American
found in Argentina. Developments in configurations but figure as recurrent
Chile followed a different course from themes in contemporary history from
those of the Platine countries. The dicta- which, it is hoped, general lessons can be
torship of General Augusto Pinochet was drawn.
accompanied by an economic develop- CESAR N. CAVIEDES
ment not mirrored in the other two coun- University of Florida
tries, which contributed to a greater Gainesville
acceptance of military rule by the rich
and middle-class segments. The poor and
the Left suffered so deeply that their for-
giveness of authoritarian abuses has
been more difficult to achieve. The legal-
SWAINE, MICHAEL D. and ASHLEY J.
istic character of Chileans and their
TELLIS. 2000. Interpreting Chinas
belief that institutions and public actors
Grand Strategy: Past, Present and Fu-
are accountable for their actions ex-
ture. Pp. xx, 283. Santa Monica, CA:
plains the ongoing search for justice and
RAND. $35.00. Paperbound, $20.00.
punishment.
One can dispute the propriety of using Of the many large unknowns facing
the term legacy of human rights viola- the United States in the new century,
tions, referenced in the title, when refer- China is perhaps the most tangible,
ring to the postauthoritarian political though not the most predictable. Inter-
events in Argentina, Chile, and Uruguay. preting Chinas Grand Strategy is a com-
Even Roniger and Sznajder find difficul- prehensive attempt by two senior RAND
ties in specifying what is meant by this Corporation analysts to fit Chinapast,
expression that certainly represents an present, and futureinto a general pat-
unpleasant bequest to the countries in tern of predictability. The result is not
question. Despite the improper use of the convincing, but it is the best-argued pes-
term legacy, this work is a well-docu- simistic estimate of Chinas security tra-
mented account and a credible interpre- jectory.
184 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Swaine and Tellis take structural re- through catastrophe or internal dissen-
alism as their analytic approach, arguing sion is considered and dismissed. The
that China, like other nations, wants to possibility that the global context of a fu-
maximize its interests and power, and ture powerful China will constrain it to
therefore its strategic posture is set by its be cooperative is considered and rebutted
situation of relative power. Since power is in some detail. We are left with an asser-
ultimately a function of a states capabil- tive China, one whose interests are more
ity to coerce other states, the argument concentrated on its periphery but whose
is relevant to current American policy to- memories of imperial glory and confi-
ward China, and since Chinas capacity is dence of new strength lead it to view the
likely to continue to grow in both absolute existing global order as an old one.
and relative terms, it will be even more Any book of this scope requires gener-
critical for the United States after 2020. alizations and judgments that can be dis-
Despite their expectation that China will puted, but two deeper questions should
become more assertive as it approaches be raised. First, the book is not an inter-
great power capacity, Swaine and Tellis pretation of Chinas grand strategy, if
recommend a long-term policy of engage- strategy involves intent, and by interpre-
ment rather than one of preemptive con- tation we mean the effort to understand
tainment. anothers intent. In contrast to such clas-
The book begins with a lengthy discus- sics by RAND alumni as Allen Whitings
sion of Chinas history, arguing that Chinas Calculus of Deterrence or Mel
China has not been reluctant to use vio- Gurtovs China and Southeast Asia,
lence in the past. Chinas wars were usu- there is little careful analysis of Chinese
ally on its periphery, and they were often diplomacy here. Indeed, the five princi-
limited and defensive, but there were ples of peaceful coexistence, Chinas ma-
many of them. The second major part of jor diplomatic mantra since 1954, is not
the book concerns Chinas strategic pos- mentioned. The book argues that China
ture since Deng Xiaopings reform era be- is not an exception from the predictable
gan in 1979. Swaine and Tellis describe a behavior of power-sensitive states, and
foreign policy that is based on economic therefore its grand strategy is a question
progress, requires peaceful international of capacity, not intent.
relations, and does not unduly concen- A second problem is that while the
trate resources on the military. This book is well researched and presents
peaceful posture is explained as a enough data to make its argument plau-
calculative strategy, one based on the sible, China contains sufficient ambigu-
fact that China is currently weak as a ous and contradictory data that a
modern military power and therefore its contrary argument could also be made
current purposes are best served by non- plausible. In such a situation an analyst
military means. Moreover, economic must be careful not to ride a tangent too
growth is a necessary prerequisite of far. For example, Swaine and Tellis say
global military prominence, so Chinas that if all the territories claimed, occu-
shortest path to great power status is pied, or directly controlled by China since
through peaceful expansion of its econ- its unification in the 3rd century BC were
omy for the next two decades. matched against its current physical
The final third of the book discusses holdings, the presently disputed territo-
possible future trajectories for Chinas ries would fade into insignificance. True,
security strategy. The possibility that but not true enough. After all, cozily
China will eliminate itself as contender within the same time frame the same
BOOK DEPARTMENT 185

could be said of Greece, Italy, Great Brit- a ch i e v e j u s t w h a t h e i n t e n d e d .


ain, the Iroquois, and Peru. Verstiques Quiroga was no soul mate of
BRANTLY WOMACK Bartolom de las Casas, the energetic
protector of the Indians who regarded na-
University of Virginia
tive peoples of Mesoamerica as fully the
Charlottesville
equals of Spaniards (arguably their supe-
riors, he thought, given Spaniards bes-
tial behavior in the conquest). Rather,
Quiroga saw them as crude, ignorant peo-
VERSTIQUE, BERNARDINO. 2000. ple who resembled untamed animals in
Michoacn and Eden: Vasco de their behavior. Teachable, perhaps, but
Quiroga and the Evangelization of definitely in need of teaching and radical
Western Mexico. Pp. xviii, 194. Austin: transformation. Only in his conflicts with
University of Texas Press. $40.00. Spanish rivals did Quiroga begin to see
Paperbound, $19.95. Purhpecha as a potentially active citi-
zenry. Even his Santa Fe de la Laguna
Utopia is an alluring but dangerous
idea that, in its heyday since 1789, often community project was ultimately a
slopes steeply into authoritarian night- form of coercion.
mares. Now Mexicos best-loved utopian The strength of this book is in
experiment under Vasco de Quiroga, Verstiques own balanced, humane vi-
Michoacns first bishop during the sec- sion, his interest in Purhpecha culture,
ond third of the sixteenth century, is re- and his aims to bring a more historical
ceiving more skeptical attention. Quiroga and the question of religious
Quiroga is resurfacing as an authoritar- change in Michoacn after the conquest
ian zealot more than the benign, idealis- to a general audience. He concludes that
tic bishop long celebrated as the apostle there was, in fact, a social and religious
of the New World. In this new critical transformation of native society, but not a
spirit, James Krippner-Martnez has ex- neat conversion to Christianity and Eu-
amined how views of Tata Vasco have ropean ways of living.
been shaped by the times since the eigh- Unfortunately, we are given too little
teenth century, and Fernando Gmez has solid information about Quirogas actions
given us Quiroga the letrado (expert in and their results or Purhpecha partici-
law), a Foucaldian hospital administra- pation in and resistance to his plans and
tor who combined monastic rigor with a view of conversion to judge whether this
chillingly letter-of-the-law conception of view is more than plausible. Half of the
a Christian republic. very short text up to the epilogue is given
Michoacn and Eden offers a more over to background discussions of Span-
balanced and ample vision of Bishop ish and Purhpecha history, geography,
Quiroga within this new criticism, a vi- and religion before Spaniards reached
sion that springs from Verstiques Michoacn, little of which is put to any di-
grounding in the Chicago history of reli- rect use in the 50 or so pages that actually
gions school, with a dash of liberation address Quiroga and his New World
theology added. Quiroga emerges here as Eden. Verstique gives Quiroga a some-
the agent of colonialism he undoubtedly what different twist than did Benedict
wascapable of the actions of a benevo- Warren in his earlier pioneering research
lent pastor bent on protecting native on Quiroga and the early colonial history
Purhpecha people from abusive Spanish of Michoacn, but he adds virtually noth-
encomenderos, but substituting his own ing new to Warrens information. That
authoritarian practices and unable to Purhpechas may have viewed their forced
186 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

removal into hospital-villages as a cir- the (im)moral history of the twentieth


cumstance in which they could create a century. Glover tackles the central and
native network that would preserve their troublesome question of modern mans
religious and cultural autonomy seems inhumanity to his fellow man by using
likely, but again, there is practically noth- ethics to pose questions of history and by
ing in the book to establish this view. exploring those parts of human nature
WILLIAM B. TAYLOR that are relevant to ethicsall in the
hope that by understanding more about
University of California
our world and ourselves, we can create a
Berkeley
world with less misery and perhaps pre-
vent the long nightmare of the twentieth
century from spilling over into the
twenty-first.
EUROPE Glover gives us a detailed catalog of
the horrors of the last centurya re-
GLOVER, JONATHAN. 2000. Humanity: minder of the evil of which humans are
A Moral History of the Twentieth Cen- capable. But he does more than this. He
tury. Pp. xiv, 464. New Haven, CT: Yale also explores what produced this unprec-
University Press. $27.95. edented brutality. Mankind has always
The chief business of twentieth-cen- had a capacity for cruelty. He quotes
tury philosophy, wrote R. G. Colling- Dostoyevskys character Ivan Kara-
wood, is to reckon with twentieth- mazov: no animal could ever be so cruel
century history. And what a bloody cen- as a man, so artfully, so artistically cruel.
tury it wasalmost beyond imagination. The atrocities of the twentieth century,
The flower of a generation of European he argues, resulted from coupling this ca-
youth was wasted in the muddy trenches pacity for cruelty with modern technol-
of Flanders during World War I. British ogys terribly efficient machinery of
and American bombing of Germany and destruction and death. To Glover, the les-
Japanese cities during World War II led son is clear: contemporary societies must
to the death of hundreds of thousands of resuscitate morality from the ground up
noncombatants. The victims of Stalins by placing greater emphasis on sympa-
tyranny numbered in the millions. The thy and respect for other humans; this is
number of Maos victims was greater. And what prevents cruelty between people.
Pol Pot killed a higher proportion of his The task is especially urgent because of
own population than either Stalin or the vast and terrifying efficiency with
Mao. More recently, there was the terri- which all jobs of murder can now be car-
ble bloodbath in Rwanda. Yet even after ried out. People must be persuaded to
taking stock of all of these brutalities, to think and act differently toward one an-
ponder Hitlers campaign of genocide otherthere is no real and sustainable
against the Jews of Europe is to look into alternative. It is too late to stop the tech-
the very heart of darkness. No explana- nology, he points out, so it is to the psy-
tion or apology is needed for giving the chology we should now turn.
unprecedented inhumanity of the twenti- Glover identifies several reasons for
eth century a central place in recent his- hope: we possess the moral resources to
tory. fight inhumanity, we can nurture the
This is what Jonathan Glover, director moral defenses that restrain cruelty, and
of the Center of Medical Law and Ethics we can remember. The latter may be the
at Kings College in London, does in his easiest and the most important. In a
sustained and thoughtful mediation on speech to SS camp guards going to the ex-
BOOK DEPARTMENT 187

termination camps of Poland in the their hurt and humiliation, the public
1940s, Hitler told them to kill men, turned to old myths from German cul-
women, and children without pitytheir ture. Such myths helped them to explain,
acts would be forgotten. Who remembers for example, their indigestible defeat in
now the massacres of the Armenians? he World War I and their having been
sneered. There is a chilling similarity be- stabbed in the back by evil doers, that is,
tween Hitlers words and Stalins com- Jews. The German people reactivated a
ment while signing death warrants group fantasy of rebuilding a Holy Ro-
during the Great Purge of the 1930s: man Empire of the German nation (a con-
Whos going to remember all this riff-raff cept that requires a closer look).
in ten or twenty years time? No one. Gonen rightly tells us that Hitler was
BRIAN VANDEMARK a borrower of ideas. He states that Hitler
United States Naval Academy borrowed ideas on the psychology of
Annapolis crowds from the French Gustave Le Bon
Maryland who was a racist himself and was ad-
mired by the fascist leadership in Italy.
While there is no proof that Hitler him-
self read Le Bon, the Nazi literature sug-
gests that Joseph Goebbels was familiar
GONEN, JAY Y. 2000. The Roots of Nazi with Le Bons writings. Interestingly,
Psychology: Hitlers Utopian Barba- Gonen does not focus on cronies of Hitler
rism. Pp. 224. Lexington: University of such as Goebbels. Perhaps future studies
Kentucky Press. $25.00. will explore the importance of the psy-
After the collapse of Yugoslavia, the chology between a leader and his or her
way in which Serbian leaders Slobodan entourage as a factor in the success of
Milosevic and Radovan Karadzic revived propaganda campaigns. Nevertheless,
historical traumas and enflamed malig- Gonen should be complimented on his il-
nant nationalism has increased contem- lustration of how a leaders internal
porary scholars interest in the relation- needs dovetail with the primitive desires
ship between such leaders and their and fears of the masses. Leaders, even ab-
millions of followers. How is political pro- solute dictators like Hitler, do not exist in
paganda related to the psychology of the a vacuum. The psychology of both sides,
masses? Nazi Germany still remains an leaders and followers, needs to be taken
arena for further exploration of this ques- into account for a comprehensive view of
tion. In The Roots of Nazi Psychology: Hit- history.
lers Utopian Barbarism, Jay Gonen At the end of his book, Gonen looks at
focuses on Nazi ideology to illustrate the present-day Germany and notes, for ex-
contact points between the Fhrer and ample, that the reunification of East and
the German people. Adolf Hitler often West Germany did not bring back old
used the repetition of short slogans to dreams of malignant nationalism. This
feed his followers Nazi ideology, includ- makes him optimistic that Germans ac-
ing ideas on race, the Jewish threat, liv- knowledgment of responsibility for the
ing space, world domination, the folkish past will secure a future that is neither
state, the femininity of masses, and the utopian nor barbaric. This reviewer, as a
omnipotence of the leader. Hitler was clinician, would point out that it has
able to sell this ideology in part because, taken Jewish people several decades to
after being traumatized during World fully recognize the transgenerational
War I, Germans were looking for an om- transmission of trauma and its influence
nipotent savior. As a reaction against on the second and third generations,
188 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

whether adaptive or pathological. Ac- establishment and expansion of the new


cordingly, the trauma that the German empire, its vicissitudes from 1900 to
people experienced in the Nazi period 1945, and finally its demise during decol-
and its influence on the second and third onization with the 1962 loss of Algeria.
generations likewise needs to be opened One is struck by how much of the
up further. Despite the social, legal, and French experience falls in the shadow of
intellectual safeguards against recreat- Great Britain. The nineteenth century
ing malignant nationalism and anti- escalated what Walter Dorn called the
Semitism, there are hints that the influ- competition for empire, and in 1763, the
ence of the Third Reich continues be- French lost Canada and India. In the
neath the surface. Clinical studies of nineteenth century, however, the French
Nazi skinheads and German individuals outflanked the other powers interested in
in psychoanalysis whose ancestors were the Far East by taking Indochina and
Nazis tell us a great deal about how the making a comeback in North and Black
relationship between the Fhrer and his Africa to rival Britain. Quinn points out
people is still being played out at the un- the irony of the Third Republic of libert,
conscious level. galit, and fraternit presiding over an
VAMIK D. VOLKAN empire whose subjects had few rights.
University of Virginia Quinn emphasizes that France could
Charlottesville never hope to match Britain, since its
navy and merchant marine were always
inferior; since France was primarily a
land power, politics always favored the
Continent; and finally, the fact is that the
QUINN, FREDERICK. 2000. The French
French body politic, despite enthusiastic
O v e rs e a s Em p i r e. P p. x x , 3 3 6 .
visits to colonial expositions, was never
Westport, CT: Praeger. $65.00.
much interested in the colonies. Algeria,
This book is an excellent example of a enshrined in the myth of being a part of
genre all too rare in our day: a broad his- mainland France, was the most tragic
torical canvas painted by a scholar-diplo- chapter of all and provides a sober ending
mat, who has served in the places he to five centuries of imperial rule.
writes about. In this instance, North Af- Why, then, did France play an interna-
rica, Black Africa, West Indies, and Viet- tional game of expansion? Except for Al-
namthe most important places in the geria (and early Canada) there were no
old French overseas empire. settler colonies (prompting Bismarcks
Because of his service, Quinn is com- remark, France has colonies but no colo-
pelled to ask, how did this far-flung em- nists); French commerce was never
pire come about? His plan is ambitious: to greatly involved in overseas trade as
take us on a tour of five centuries of compared to Britain. Was it the Napole-
French expansion and contraction, cul- onic quest for glory? Or was it (at a later
minating in the twentieth century when stage) national prestige? It is here that
the empire reached its apogee. He gives a Quinn abandons ship: we are waiting for
well-balanced, thoughtful account, start- a grand conclusion, an analytical over-
ing with the fishing empire, followed by view of what all of this activity meant,
the traders empire, the eighteenth-cen- but he prefers to bail out. I hope that
tury duel with Britain, and the nine- Quinn will publish such an overview,
teenth-century advent of the military which he is well qualified to write, in a
and missionaries. He then focuses on the journal article in the near future.
BOOK DEPARTMENT 189

The strengths of the volume are its missing in purely political narratives.
comprehensiveness, which makes it use- And his quip from Casablanca, the
ful for teachers and students, and its at- Claude Rains line, of the French to
tention to detail. Its broad scope fills a round up the usual suspects, adds a mar-
need to have an up-to-date narrative of velous light touch to the proceedings.
the French empire; while imperial stud- G. WESLEY JOHNSON
ies are out of fashion in some quarters,
Brigham Young University
they are essential if we are to understand
Provo
the modern postidependence world. Utah
Quinn is at his best when he brings per-
sonal insights to bear on his material,
such as his arriving in Vietnam to be
given a library of Victor Hugos works
hidden since colonial days. UNITED STATES
One problem for Quinn is that of
sources. Generally, it is a fully docu- ALTSCHULER, GLENN and STUART
mented work. He was well advised to pe- BLUMIN. 2000. Rude Republic: Amer-
ruse the most relevant French sources, icans and Their Politics in the Nine-
which he has done, but in the process, his teenth Century. Pp. xii, 316. Princeton,
notes show a lack of familiarity with NJ: Princeton University Press.
works by British and American scholars $35.00.
of the French empire. For instance, the In the 1960s political historians devel-
African materials do not reflect the rele- oped what has become an enduring para-
vant works of Crowder, Wallerstein, digm of nineteenth-century American
Morgenthau, Austen, Foltz, and others. history. In its elemental form this inter-
Part of the problem is that his French ex- pretation holds that politics after 1840
perts themselves often do not cite the was pervasive, interesting, and accessi-
English-speaking sources, which Quinn ble to large numbers of Americans who
needed to pick up. displayed their commitment to the pro-
One of the empires most important cess by voting in the frequent elections
legacies of empire in our century was the that mark this period, by joining the po-
formation of political parties, which from litical parties that became institutional-
the 1920s onward played a capital role in ized, and by participating in the
the decolonization process. This subject campaign rituals that grew to large-scale
area, as well as the evolution of elites celebrations such as parades and pole
(who eventually took over the empire), raisings. Politics, in the clich that now
needed fuller coverage. We learn about summarizes this period, was in the air
Bourguiba and Ho Chi Minh but not everywhere, as the American electorate
much about Blaise Diagne or Ferhat created a golden age of politics.
Abbas. Glenn Altschuler and Stuart Blumin,
Last, more copyediting was needed in their remarkable study The Rude Re-
misspellings of Henri Brunschwig, spahi, public: Americans and Their Politics in
Lamine Guye, Ousmane Sembne, and the Nineteenth Century, challenge this
so forth mar an otherwise literate and ruling wisdom. Theirs is not so much a
readable text. On the positive side, heads-on confrontation as it is a flanking
Quinns sections on the literary aspect of operation that seeks to demonstrate the
colonial life add a sensitive touch often variability and different levels of political
190 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

activity among nineteenth-century But newspapers, so famously useful


Americans. Even to the most active and for the argument that Americans were
interested, politics was only part of life, interested in politics, offer only insider
and at that not always the most impor- information in the Altschuler-Blumin
tant part. model. In a back-breaking and eye-
Among their insights is that the evi- straining use of sources, Altschuler and
dence of the so-called party period of Blumin look at the world beyond politics
American politics has come from insiders through fiction, diaries, and iconography,
who glorify their own political engage- the former written by those who were
ment, which is by no means representa- outsiders with varying interest in the
tive of their fellow Democrats and world inhabited by the partisans whom
Republicans. In fact, outsiders ranged we have heretofore taken as typical
from the apathetic to the intermittently Americans.
interested (mostly during presidential Altschuler and Blumin pursue their
elections and during the Civil War) to the hypothesis through the nineteenth cen-
silents about whom even the research of tury. In this thick description specialists
Altschuler and Blumin reveals little to will take issue with some of their conclu-
the engaged disbelief of skeptical Ameri- sions. For example, they argue that be-
cans who considered politics as they fore the Civil War slavery was not in
might a Barnum circus. Neglected by the itself a matter of national policy, and in
practitioners of the New Political His- their demotion of voting after the war
tory whose application of statistics was they entirely neglect the agonizing ef-
much heralded 30 years ago, write forts of women to obtain an instrumental
Altschuler and Blumin, was any sus- right. Still the range of this book, its so-
tained analysis of the nature and depth of phisticated use of sources, and its evi-
popular political engagement. dence is striking. Inevitably The Rude
Altschuler and Blumin provide such Republic points to a comparison with to-
an analysis. Using representative towns days politics. While we may be comforted
in various sections of the United States, that things have not declined as much as
which are changed over time because of we may think, still this important, su-
demographic transformations, they have perbly researched and argued book may
constructed a list of those men mentioned make us wonder whether our democracy
by newspapers as engaged in some form was ever a government by the people.
of public activity. The numbers involved JEAN H. BAKER
in this sample are not paltry; for example, Goucher College
their data include thousands of activists Baltimore
whom they track for political and civic in- Maryland
volvement and whom they use to deter-
mine the social structure of political
activism. For Augusta, Georgia, before
the war they find 866 Augustans, al-
DIGGENS, JOHN PATRICK. 2000. On
though most of these did not participate
Hallowed Ground: Abraham Lincoln
in the nominating system of conventions,
and the Foundations of American His-
which takes in The Rude Republic quite a
tory. New Haven, CT: Yale University
beating in terms of interest. Cross-refer-
Press. No price.
ence to the U.S. census provides a social
profile of the elite nature of partisan lead- At the core of American society resides
ers, at least before the war. a body of liberal ideals that has realized
BOOK DEPARTMENT 191

itself in every generation and in every clusion by rejecting liberalism. A long


historical era. The present generation of strand of African American writing, from
historians, distinguished by its obsession Frederick Douglass and Booker T. Wash-
with the sins of Americas past, convinced ington to Thomas Sowell and Shelby
that the function of any dominant ideol- Steele, all influenced by Locke, sees the
ogy is to exclude minorities, and blind to marketplace as the mechanism of social
anything about which Americans agree, integration. On the other side, W.E.B. Du
have utterly distorted American history. Bois, Martin Luther King, Jr., Cornel
Such is John Diggenss claim. West, and Henry L. Gates seek to inte-
Diggens summons the example of grate African Americans into the main-
Abraham Lincoln to illustrate his point. stream of life through the political arena.
The real Abraham Lincoln was not the Diggenss stand on this issue leaves noth-
man now admired by politically correct ing to the imagination: one cannot bask
television documentaries and their Left- vicariously in the glow of victimhood, cel-
looking talking heads. The real Lincoln ebrate the politics of difference, and ex-
was the ultimate capitalist. Embracing pect to keep up with history.
the liberalism of the founders, Lincoln be-
lieved in the morality of the free market Those with the will to work and the ambition
and the sanctity of property. John Locke, to prosper have always made up the liberal
one of the founders ideological sages, de- consensusa consensus that includes women,
laborers, blacks and other ethnic minorities,
fined property as the lynchpin of the good
all those who believe in the gospel of work and
society. When man mixes his labor with try to live for conviction as well as comfort. Call
his soil, Locke declared, the product must them Lincolns heirs. (291)
be his to possess. Owners and workers
alike, from the very creation of the Amer- Diggenss book is distinguished by its
ican state, believed that the right to pro- relentless and effective criticism of multi-
duce and accumulate property is a culturalism, poststructuralism, and
natural right subsisting independently of other imbecilities of the academic Left,
the consent of others. For Lincoln, the but it does not warrant its subtitle: Abra-
nation is worth fighting for, to secure ham Lincoln and the Foundations of
such an inestimable jewel. Lincoln never American History. Readers who think
studied Locke closely, but Locke was part they are buying a book about Lincoln will
of the political atmosphere in which he be very disappointed, for only a small por-
lived and breathed. Unqualified recogni- tion of its pages actually deals with him.
tion of the will to work and to succeed in Lincoln is Diggenss vehicle for an analy-
the race of life oriented Lincolns life be- sis of liberalism in American intellectual
cause it oriented Americas political cul- history. Historians, in this regard, will
ture. And when Lincoln invoked liberty find plenty to criticize, including
to all he meant not only political rights Diggenss overestimating Locke by gloss-
but, above all, enterprise, and industry to ing over Montesquieu and Blackstone,
all. The very purpose of life, Lincoln be- and his ignoring Lincolns realization
lieved, is to improve ones condition. that northerners fear of freed slaves
The right to accumulate property and glutting the labor market superceded
influence comprises the liberal consen- Lockean ideals. Diggenss argument,
sus that contemporary historians deny. however, withstands such details. True,
African Americans, more than any other he says nothing new about the cultural
group, Diggens stresses, have been ex- Left, but his commentary on Lincolns
cluded from the liberal consensus, but values, however brief, is fresh and provoc-
many black spokesmen retard their in- ative. In truth, John Locke, mentioned
192 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

rarely by American historians, suffused for scrutinizing the military operations


Lincolns life and Americas history. The by President Bush as to Panama and
time to bring liberalism back into the Iraq, and Clinton as to Somalia, Haiti,
study of Lincoln and his America is now. Bosnia, Iraq redux, and Yugoslavia
On Hallowed Ground is a good start. (Kosovo). To illustrate how recent Con-
BARRY SCHWARTZ gresses let presidents make war deci-
University of Georgia sions, Fisher marshals fresh and
Athens thought-provoking evidence, from Pro-
fessor Ezra Y. Siff s 1999 study of the
Tonkin Gulf Resolution in the Vietnam
War, to how the Clinton administration
quietly reprogrammedthat is, spent
FISHER, LOUIS. 2000. Congressional without Congress fully votinga billion
Abdication on War & Spending. Pp. xv, previously appropriated dollars in 1996
220. College Station: Texas A&M Uni- to finance the Bosnia commitment.
versity Press. $34.95. Paperbound, Next, as to the budget process, Con-
$17.95. gress had largely kept the power of the
Louis Fisher, senior specialist in the purse in its own hands from the framers
Congressional Research Service and dis- time to the 1960s. However, Fisher finds
tinguished author of dozens of scholarly that congressional enactment of the Bud-
works on interbranch relations, probes get Act of 1974 as a riposte to President
more profoundly than ever two of his Nixons impoundments relinquished
prime subjects: war powers and the bud- more legislative power than it recovered.
get process. Partly, Congressional Abdi- The now-centralized budget process it-
cation takes war and budget decision self strengthened presidents in the wres-
making from the framers to the eve of the tling for budget control, from President
new administration, reviewing against Reagans 1981 Omnibus Act with his do-
the backdrop of history, recent events mestic agenda aboard, through the
such as the military interventions in Yu- Gramm-Rudman-Hollings Law. Again,
goslavia and the Supreme Courts line the book builds on pre-1990s prologue to
item veto decision. Most interestingly, explore its abdication thesis, this time to
Fisher expounds and defends a challeng- the self-straitjacketing spending caps of
ing thesis: that for all the vital need of de- the 1990s and the enactment of the Line
mocracy for a vigorous legislature and Item Veto Act as an ultimate abdication
institutional checks, Congress of late has of spirit and institutional self-respect
conducted an abject surrender of legisla- eliciting Supreme Court invalidation in
tive prerogatives to the president. 1998.
As to war powers, the framers in- Fisher writes with a lively nonparti-
tended only to empower the sacrifice- san style, his well-footnoted critiques
wary Congress, not the martially inclined lambasting Presidents Bush and Clinton
president, to engage the nation in com- and Democratic and Republican Con-
bat. Until 1950, Congress declared, ex- gresses alike, the former pair for stealing
pressly authorized, or at least impliedly or at least receiving stolen power, the lat-
ratified virtually all major military ac- ter for cravenly surrendering it. The
tions. The Korean War, Vietnam, and the reader will enjoy his occasional first-per-
initiatives of President Reagan (for ex- son accounts, such as a few of his 37 (!) ap-
ample, Iran-Contra) marked a surge in pearances at congressional hearings
presidential war making. Fishers survey where he often first field tests warnings
of these serves primarily as the prologue about (then-)impending outrages.
BOOK DEPARTMENT 193

Scholars and students of interbranch Starks book provides an in-depth sur-


relations, with interests quickened in vey of the many different varieties of con-
2001 by the first inauguration in a half flict-of-interest regulation, continually
century of a unified Republican govern- returning to a single theme: that the re-
ment, will find in Congressional Abdica- placement of laws regulating mala in se
tion a lucid work providing deep (those acts that are wrong in themselves)
perspective on real-time controversies. with regulations of acts innocent in
CHARLES TIEFER themselves but forbidden for prophylac-
tic reasons and thus classed as mala
University of Baltimore
prohibita, has created a dilemma. As
Chevy Chase
Stark notes, the problem with conflict-
Maryland
of-interest law is that it has become a
moral stigmatizer when, in reality, it is
just law.
Nonetheless, Stark is not an enemy of
conflict-of-interest law. His attitude
STARK, ANDREW. 2000. Conflict of In- might be better described as that of a con-
terest in American Public Life. Pp. x, cerned friend: because Stark believes
331. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer- that it is important to regulate conflicts
sity Press. $49.95. of interest, he wishes to see such regula-
From Whitewater and Hillary Clin- tion kept within its proper scope, where it
tons cattle futures to the makeup of the might do good. Such pragmatic modesty
Food and Drug Administrations advisory is, unfortunately, rare in scholarship
panels, issues of governmental ethics these days.
have been steady front-page news for Starks defense of regulations based
years. Add to this the growing list of cor- on preventing an appearance of impro-
porate ethics consultants, think tanks priety (or, perhaps more accurately, on
and advocacy groups devoted to ethical maintaining an appearance of propriety)
issues of various kinds, and even the oc- is more problematic. The essence of an
casional efforts to force the adoption of a appearance violation, of course, is that it
scholarly code of ethics for academics, is not actually wrong but that it might
and it would seem that we must live in look bad, at least to the ill informed.
the most ethical age in history. Yet this is Stark conscientiously raises the point
hardly the way most people feel. that the public deserves a share of the
One reason for this, notes Andrew blame here (if citizens bothered to know
Stark in his new book Conflict of Interest more, they would attend less to appear-
in American Public Life, is that we have ances and more to substance) but ulti-
been experiencing a sort of ethical mately favors a strict-liability approach
bracket creep. We have raised our stan- in such cases. One might wonder (in fact,
dards from those of subjective ill intent to I do wonder) whether this approach, with
create a vast array of prophylactic laws its inevitable injustices and absurdities
designed to forbid any circumstances in practice, really does anything to pro-
that might lead to inner conflict. Yet such mote public confidence, but Stark sets
rules cover so much conduct that viola- forth the best possible case for his posi-
tions are sure to be found, regardless of tion.
the intent of those they cover. The end re- Appropriately enough, the book does
sult, as others have pointed out, is that not end with a checklist of cut-and-dried
the regulation of ethics becomes a game approaches for ending problems with
for technicians, drained of moral content. governmental ethics for all time. The
194 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

problem, as Stark frankly admits, is as dependently, that emigre = intellectual =


much one of culture as of legality: so long left wing (at least) = Communist/fellow
as we want governmental officials to pos- traveler/dupe/nationalist/maybe-not-
sess great authority and wide discretion, commie-but-probably-un-American.
and so long as we find the notion of deep Never mind that neither Communist
inquiry into individuals thoughts and Party USA members nor the emigres,
motivations repellently totalitarian, we most of whom were not Communists, con-
will be forced to enact objective regula- stituted a threat to the internal security
tions that will often be a poor fit with in- of the United States. In theory they did.
dividual cases. Though this conclusion In a perverse way, the theory was not
will not satisfy those who yearn for sim- all that farfetched, although it was mis-
ple (or at least simplistic) solutions, it is applied. The reasoning was not so much
correct. In Conflict of Interest in Ameri- simple as simplistic: since Communism
can Public Life, Stark has given us a clear was a German invention, and the two
and insightful guide to the features, greatest threats to democracy were the
flaws, and purposes of todays political German brand of fascism (Nazism) and
ethics laws. Communism, there was virtually no dif-
GLENN HARLAN REYNOLDS ference between them. Thus Herbert J.
University of Tennessee Hoover called Communists Red fas-
Knoxville cists, even though through the 1940s
and 1950s, Fascism and Communism
were considered antonymsthe differ-
ence being that ideologically, the former
was right wing; the latter left wing. Al-
S T EP H AN, AL EX AND E R . 2 0 0 0 . though it took leftists a long time to real-
Communazis: FBI Surveillance of ize it, there was no difference between the
German Emigre Writers. Trans. Jan extreme Right and the extreme Left, each
van Heurck. Pp. xxi, 384. New Haven, having its own way of enforcing confor-
CT: Yale University Press. $29.95. mity and settling scores.
The title is somewhat misleading What is ultimately saddening about
since Stephan deals not only with the FBI Stephans painstaking study, enriched by
but also with two other organizations excerpts from files that Stephan has ac-
equally interested in the activities of ex- quired through the Freedom of Informa-
patriates during World War II and the tion Act (which in itself makes his book
1950s: the Office of Strategic Services invaluable), is the amount of time and
(OSS) (before it was phased out of exis- money wasted on investigations that re-
tence and evolved into the CIA) and the veal more about the investigators than
INS. Stephans is a sobering study even to the investigatedfor example, the FBIs
those familiar with the extent to which fear that Thomas Mann might become a
government agencies were obsessed with member of the Free Germany Commit-
the minutiae of the lives that under ordi- tee, which was perceived as comprising
nary circumstances, would never have Stalinist Germans. The FBI assumed
merited such scrutiny. But these were not that emigres cherished a dream of a Ger-
ordinary times; Emma Lazaruss wel- many guided by Social Democrats (trans-
come to the huddled masses yearning to late: Socialists or worse) but not by the
breathe free was given lip service during principles of American democracy. And
World War II and the McCarthy era, why were the sexual propensities of
when the great triumviratethe FBI, Manns son, Klaus, of such concern except
OSS, and INSdecided, more or less in- that the equation had been extended to
BOOK DEPARTMENT 195

include homosexuals, so that any expatri- SUMMERS, MARK WAHLGREN. 2000.


ate gay is a potential (or actual) subver- Rum, Romanism, & Rebellion: The
sive. All that is missing is a pink Making of a President, 1884. Pp. xv,
triangular stamp on the file transcript. 377. Chapel Hill: University of North
Although most of the book is devoted Carolina Press. $55.00. Paperbound,
to the more colorful and famous Los An- $19.95.
geles community (Thomas Mann, Bertolt
In Rum, Romanism, & Rebellion,
Brecht, Franz Werfel, Erich Maria Re-
Mark Wahlgren Summers, author of two
marque, Emil Ludwig), there are chap-
earlier estimable books, deals with more
ters on the New York and Mexico
than The Making of a President, 1884; in-
expatriates. Especially instructive is
deed, as he himself notes, his study
Stephans reminder that Mexico was
sprawls across the map and over eight
quite liberal in its immigration policies
years. James G. Blaine (Republican) and
and welcomed those who supported the
Grover Cleveland (Democrat) are not
Spanish republic during the Spanish civil
nominated for president until chapters 8
war. One of the most fascinating of the
and 9, respectively, this after coverage of
Mexican emigres was Bodo Uhse, a Com-
gilded age political culture, campaigns,
munist writer and Spanish civil war vet- and elections; the disputed presidential
eran. Although Stephan does not election of 1876 and its aftermath; the
mention Lillian Hellman, it should be Democrats ruthless consolidation of
noted that her prophetically antifascist power in the South; Republican party fac-
play, Watch on the Rhine (1941), deals tionalism; cultural issues (Protestant-
with a family that has returned from Ger- Catholic conflict and prohibition); the
many to Washington, DC, via Mexico. The tariff issue; and the revival of Democratic
husbands politics are never specified ex- fortunes before 1884.
cept as antifascist, but he fought in Spain The political campaigns of 1884 are
and is about as far to the Left as a charac- treated exhaustively by Summers, in-
ter could be in a Broadway play. Interest- cluding state contests held prior to the
ingly, the younger sons name is Bodo, a presidential canvass. Minor parties re-
rather unusual one (the other children ceive their due, as do other groups of sig-
are called Joshua and Babette). Given nificance, particularly (though by no
Hellmans politics (supposedly she be- means exclusively) in New York State,
longed to the Communist Party for a very which would decide the outcome of the
short time) and the prodigious research national electionMugwumps, Irish pol-
she did for the play, the choice of name iticians, and Irish nationalists. Cam-
was not accidental. paign episodes with which readers are
Communazis is the kind of book that likely to be familiarscandals, most fa-
adds to our knowledge and illuminates mously Grover Clevelands fathering a
what we already know. If I had not known child out of wedlock; the Reverent Sam-
I was reading a translation, I would have uel Burchards alliterative but impolitic
congratulated the author on writing such indictment of the Democratic Party as
readable prose. Instead, I must commend the party of rum, Romanism, and rebel-
Jan van Heurck for a translation that lion at a large ministerial rally for
does not sound like one. Blaine; and the controversial fund-rais-
BERNARD F. DICK ing dinner for Blaine (Belshazzars
Feast) at Delmonicos restauranttake
Fairleigh Dickinson University on new significance as Summers care-
Teaneck fully places them in their contexts. Like
New Jersey earlier chapters, those on the unfolding
196 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

events of the presidential election year eral presentation (p. 300, Table 10), rank-
rest on diligent and remarkably wide re- ing the states in which Blaine fared worst
search in manuscript collections, news- and indicating his Gain/Loss over 1880,
papers, and magazines of opinion. somehow generally confuses 1876 to
Summerss familiarity with relevant 1884 percentage point shifts with those of
scholarship, unpublished as well as pub- 1880 to 1884. Certainly James A. Gar-
lished, further strengthens his study. field carried neither Louisiana nor South
In the end, Summers concludes that Carolina. (A finaland idiosyncratic
Clevelands election marked a signifi- concern: I wonder whether the cotton
cant moment in Gilded Age politics in fields of Iowa were not fields of dreams).
several waysreal issues were at stake My caveats notwithstanding, I strongly
in 1884; politics remained the main- endorse Rum, Romanism, & Rebellion:
stream politicians game; the demon- The Making of a President, 1884.
strated need of the parties for campaign SAMUEL T. MCSEVENEY
funds would influence their subsequent Vanderbilt University
behavior. Likewise, the Democrats Nashville
tightening grip on the South and recap- Tennessee
ture of the presidency meant that the Re-
publicans would seek to strengthen
further their position in the North by
sharpening their protective tariff rheto-
ric and by seeking admission of Western WEBBER, MICHAEL J. 2000. New Deal
states. Summerss arguments merit close Fat Cats: Campaign Finances and the
attention by political historians, particu- Democratic Party in 1936. Pp. xiii, 208.
larly those of us who have stressed the New York: Fordham University Press.
significance of developments and elec- $39.95. Paperbound, $19.95.
tions during the 1890s and perhaps failed The rise and fall of Franklin D. Roose-
to appreciate fully the importance of velts New Deal coalition has captured
those of the previous decade. the imagination of generations of histori-
Rum, Romanism, & Rebellion stands ans, political scientists, and sociologists.
as a fine work of historical analysis, but it For most, the story begins with FDRs im-
is not without limitations and weak- pressive victory in the 1932 general elec-
nesses. Summers writes well, very well, tion and continues with the Democratic
but occasionally he strikes me as trying presidents skill in solidifying his broad-
too hard for effect. He is a master of de- based coalition in the 1936, 1940, and
tail, but some of that detail could have 1944 electoral contests.
been deleted without serious loss, indeed In New Deal Fat Cats, Michael Webber,
perhaps with benefit. The generally high chair of the Department of Sociology at
standard of writing to the contrary not- the University of San Francisco, offers a
withstanding, a handful of sentences con- powerful and convincing social scientific
tain gaffes that should have been caught. analysis of one of the centurys most im-
Summers appears more comfortable ana- portant elections. Noting an abundance
lyzing the behavior of politicians and of literature on class conflict and the rise
other public figures than analyzing that of labor unions, Webber focuses his study
of the electorate. Quantitative election of the 1936 election on the interactions
analyses are generally adequate, no between business, organized labor, and
more; for the most part they focus on se- political parties. The guiding thread of
lected districts rather than illuminate this study, Webber argues, is the as-
broader patterns in the period. One gen- sumption that campaign finance contri-
BOOK DEPARTMENT 197

butions can be a reliable empirical the activist role taken by the federal gov-
indicator of the political preferences of ernment since the inception of the New
people following their real material inter- Deal.
ests. Campaign donations, he explains, New Deal Fat Cats offers a powerful
act as an important tracer element and convincing mix of social scientific
marking the tracks of business and labor data and historical narrative. Webbers
influence in the political system. ability to translate the theoretical to the
In an eight-chapter book, Webber of- real makes this an excellent book for both
fers a historiographic overview of New the specialist and the interested ob-
Deal writings in both the social sciences server. His opening chapter surveying ex-
and humanities fields, reviews the role of isting New Deal scholarship was
business between 1932 and 1936, dis- especially useful and informative.
cusses the mass-consumption sector and Webbers use of secondary sources from
Democratic party finances, evaluates the across disciplinary lines also demon-
investment theory of politics, offers a fas- strated great attention to detail and re-
cinating and novel investigation of the spect for competing methodological
1936 Democratic convention book soft approaches.
money scandal, points out important dif- MARC DOLLINGER
ferences among business leaders in New Princeton University
York City and the South, and closes with New Jersey
a study of FDR and the unions.
For Webber, the story of business and
the 1936 election reaffirms the impor-
tance of FDRs far-reaching political co-
alition. If there is a lesson to be learned SOCIOLOGY
from the New Deal, Webber explains, it
is that the Democratic party successfully BAYOR, RONALD H. [1996] 2000. Race
mobilized groups outside the main- and the Shaping of Twentieth Century
Atlanta. Pp. xvi, 334. Chapel Hill: Uni-
stream of the American power structure
versity of North Carolina Press.
to bring about far-reaching changes in
Paperbound, $19.95.
how government responded to the needs
of the American people. The publication of a paperback edition
Webber, in one of his most important of Ronald H. Bayors book on race in
observations, discovered strong pro-FDR twentieth-century Atlanta is a welcome
sentiment among business leaders from event. Bayor is a careful scholar who
New York City, the South, and the alcohol writes with clarity and insight, and he de-
industry. Avoiding a common tendency to serves a wide reading.
blur all business leaders together, Historians, journalists, and social sci-
Webber showed how Jews in New York entists have perhaps studied and written
City, southern Democrats, and opponents about Atlanta more than any other city in
of prohibition dissented from the old-line the South. Site of Booker T. Washingtons
Protestant corporate elite. In this sense, famous speech at the Cotton States and
marginalized business leaders mirrored International Exposition, and just a few
the political attitudes of the larger elec- years later scene of a particularly violent
torate. The presidential election of riot precipitated by inflammatory anti-
1936, as Webber notes in his conclusion, black newspaper rhetoric, birthplace of
was, in many respects, a referendum on Martin Luther King, Jr., a one-time cen-
198 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

ter of Klan activity, headquarters for of land-use policy and a continuing drive
many progressive organizations seeking by whites to distance their commercial
to disestablish the Jim Crow system and and residential lives from African Ameri-
diminish its legacy, and the only South- cans remained a substantial force
ern city whose mayor testified in favor of throughout the second half of the twenti-
the public accommodations section of the eth century.
1964 Civil Rights Act, Atlanta has a ra- In mid-century, when the Civil Rights
cial history with twists and turns un- movement challenged the racial order of
matched by any other city in the nation. the South, apologists for the status quo
In the second half of the twentieth cen- often spoke of that order as resting on lo-
tury, Atlanta, with its widely celebrated cal custom. Historian Ronald Bayor sets
biracial coalition, was known as the city the record straight. The status quo of the
too busy too hate. Yet, as Bayor shows, midtwentieth century rested on a policy
such a simple slogan fails to capture the past constructed by a multitude of gov-
complexity of Atlanta in that period, and ernment actions. Once constructed, as
it overlooks the weight of the citys racial subsequent events demonstrated, that
past. Bayors book provides needed depth past could not be undone by a simple shift
to the story of how racially motivated pol- of political power and a wave of policy de-
icy actions taken at an earlier time re- cisions based in that shift. Contemporary
sound through the growth and change of conservatives who complain that African
a city that has often prided itself on rising Americans look too much to past griev-
to new heights out of the ashes of its past. ances reveal a superficial grasp of history.
Georgia is a deep South state, and for the They would do well to read Ronald
first half of the twentieth century there Bayors book and reflect on its lessons.
were few checks on white disregard for There is much that a deeper understand-
and antagonism toward the interests of ing of history can teach.
Atlantas black citizens. Black Atlanta CLARENCE N. STONE
proved adept at making use of limited re- University of Maryland
sources, but in harsh circumstances College Park
adeptness can only carry a group so far.
Focusing on public policy, Bayor shows
how large a part the drive to establish
and maintain a Jim Crow system played
in the shaping of the city, not just in the BROWDER, LAURA. 2000. Slippery
first half of the twentieth century, but Characters: Ethnic Impersonators and
American Identities. Pp. xii, 312. Cha-
through that early legacy in the second
pel Hill: University of North Carolina
half as well. The biracial coalition was
Press. $49.95. Paperbound, $18.95.
not so much a thorough triumph of en-
lightenment as a new arena in which At- An elderly WASP man poses as a La-
lantas African American community tino youth, a colored Southern janitor
could ply its efforts to end old practices goes native as a Cherokee Indian, and the
and cope with their consequences. To be daughter of a Welsh Baptist mother cre-
sure, electoral enfranchisement and ates an identity as an orthodox Jew.
other federal civil rights actions came to These are only three of the many ethnic
play a major part in giving black impersonators whose lives are discussed
Atlantans a larger body of resources with by Laura Browder in this intriguing
which to pursue their own attempt to rise study.
from the ashes, in their case the ashes of Browders guiding premise is that the
oppression. Nevertheless, early patterns autobiographical narrative is a key genre
BOOK DEPARTMENT 199

in American culture, which has been used Lance, and carved out a successful career
for a variety of personal and political pur- as an authentic Indian, starring in both
poses. She argues, convincingly, that pop- fictional and ethnographic films. In ear-
ular autobiographies have come to claim lier chapters, Browder carefully de-
a moral authority, as the power of indi- scribes how Indian representations in
vidual testimony can help reshape public theatre, museums, and Wild West shows
thinking and public discourse, in this laid the groundwork for the mlange of
case about race and ethnicity. Browder imagery that came to define Indian iden-
traces these autobiographies from white- tity and on which both Carter and Long
authored slave narratives to contempo- traded.
rary accounts of individuals passing from Turning to black identity, Browder
one ethnic category to another. traces the use of fictional slave narra-
An irony that unites them all is that tives by abolitionists to make the case for
autobiographies by impersonators were the essential humanity of African Ameri-
often greeted with greater public enthu- cans. Yet abolitionists also perpetuated
siasm than those by real members of the the stereotypes: unwilling to trust the
groups concerned. A case in point is the slaves to tell their own stories, these writ-
now-notorious 1976 memoir, The Educa- ers created slave personae who were dis-
tion of Little Tree, written under a pseud- gusted by black bodies . . . and unable to
onym by Forrest Carter, a former Ku see the truth about slavery without the
Klux Klansman and speechwriter for Al- intercession of the abolitionists. Later,
abama Governor George Wallace. In the she discusses the development of black-
persona of the Cherokee Little Tree, face entertainers, and describes how
Carter wrote of being raised by saintly, groups like the Symbionese Liberation
spiritual grandparents before enduring Army took on blacker-than-thou perso-
torment in a brutal orphanage and fi- nae in an attempt to demonstrate their
nally being rescued. The book fit perfectly radical authenticity in the 1970s.
with the new age mood of the time and be- Throughout, Browder makes a good
came a huge best seller. As Browder ex- case for the primacy of the personal nar-
plains, Little Tree was a fantasy about rative in American cultural history and
Indian spirituality, and it is also a fan- explores the complicated and ever-chang-
tasy perfectly attuned to an American ing notions of race and ethnicity. She
strains her argument when she includes
public well versed in the rhetoric of self-
a chapter on hoboes, asserting that pov-
actualization and the recovery move-
erty behaves as an ethnicity, and I would
ment. In the wake of the success of
have liked to see a discussion of a major
Carlos Castanedas equally spurious
autobiographical genre, the Indian cap-
Yacqui medicine man Don Juan (whom
tivity narrative, but these are quite mi-
Browder does not mention), Little Tree
nor flaws in an innovative and engaging
epitomized the noble savage image, and
book.
the cruel irony is that Carter succeeded
S. ELIZABETH BIRD
in his impersonation by trading on his
deep knowledge of racial and ethnic ste- University of South Florida
reotypes. Tampa
While Carter used his impersonation
to escape his racist past, Sylvester Long
became an Indian to escape his racial
identity as a colored man in the 1930s
South. In a series of calculated steps, COSTA, DORA L. 1998. The Evolution of
Long became Chief Buffalo Child Long Retirement: An American Economic
200 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

History, 1880-1990. Pp. xiii, 234. Chi- has decreased over time, or as economists
cago: University of Chicago Press. would put it, the elasticity of demand for
$40.00. Paperbound, $19.00. retirement with respect to income has de-
creased. Costa concludes that rising in-
Dora Costas book on retirement is come was important perhaps up until
very much in the cliometric tradition, 1940, after which other factors became
containing much evidence and statistical more important in the labor force with-
analysis. The big issue she tackles is ex- drawal decision. This result is important
plaining the well-documented with- for academic researchers, but it is also
drawal from the labor force of men 65 and very important for policy makers who
older in the United States, a trend that will be increasingly faced with a growing
started in the late nineteenth century. number of retirees as the baby boom gen-
The key factors she examines are rising eration ages. An example of such a policy
real income, improvements in health, and issue is, How much of the retired popula-
changes in the structure of occupations.
tion should social security retirement
It is based in part on several of her previ-
benefits cover? Has income, and indirect
ously published papers combined with
saving, become high enough that some re-
some new material that covers the politi-
strictions on coverage or even means
cal history of retirement and speculates
testing become sensible? Likewise, what
about future patterns of retirement.
would the impact of partial privatization
While it is filled with charts, graphs, and
of social security be on labor force activ-
econometrics, her story is still quite ac-
ity?
cessible to the careful, noneconomist
Given the scope of Costas subject,
reader.
The core of her evidence comes from a some questions arise. Equating declining
large sample of the military pension re- labor force participation rates with in-
cords of Union Civil War veterans. She creasing retirement rates is troublesome,
supplements these samples with samples as retirement has strong voluntary con-
drawn from the 1900 and 1910 Public notations to it, while being out of the la-
Use Microdata Samples of the Decennial bor force can include withdrawal owing to
Censuses. The Union Army Veteran disability, injury, or unemployment. I also
Sample allows the observation of how in- suspect that labor force activity of older
creases in real income, a pure income ef- men may start increasing as the rela-
fect, affected the labor force behavior of tively smaller supply of labor coming
veterans. As the sample of veterans is from the cohort following the baby boom
representative of the general population, generation puts pressure on the labor
Costa is able to show that the income market, increasing real wages. Perhaps
from the pension was large enough to in- the substitution effect, as economists put
duce many older men to withdraw from it, will induce older men to stay in the la-
the labor force and that until recently, ris- bor force longer now that health issues
ing real income was central in reducing are less of an issue than they were earlier
the labor force participation of men 65 in the twentieth century. This book, nev-
and older. ertheless, provides the basis from which
Rising real income has been key in most economic analysis of retirement
spurring the withdrawal from the labor should proceed.
force, particularly before social security JON R. MOEN
and other government pensions were put
in place. The sensitivity to changes in in- University of Mississippi
come on the part of older men, however, University
BOOK DEPARTMENT 201

ECONOMICS The reforms they describe, to use


American parlance, amount to a combi-
FISKE, EWARD B. and HELEN F. nation of open enrollment and extreme
LADD. When Schools Compete: A Cau- site-based management. These reforms
tionary Tale. Pp. xvii, 342. Washing- moved the delivery of education in New
ton, DC: Brookings Institution. Zealand an enormous distance from the
$47.95. prior attendance-area, Wellington-run
system in which principals needed per-
This book provides a detailed history
mission to buy pencils.
and analysis of one of the broadest and
A great deal of information in the book
most radical reforms in education decen-
will be useful to education reformers in
tralization in the world. The New Zea-
other countries. First, the effects of open
land reforms may be of even more
enrollment were as one would anticipate.
interest than are similar reforms in
There was dramatic shift of students
Great Britain, Chile, or the United States
from lower to higher socioeconomic
because they occurred throughout the
schools. And Pakeha (European descen-
country and they transformed a system
dants) were more likely to take advan-
that was extremely centralized before the
reforms began in 1989. Thus the degree of tage of this movement than Maori or
change appears to be truly revolutionary. Pacific Island minorities. The derivative
Written by a husband and wife team results were more racial isolation and
combining the theoretical and statistical more segregated schools by class and
skills of an economist (Ladd) with the his- race. Thus one of the downsides of choice,
torical and qualitative eye of a journalist echoed in Scotland, England, and Chile,
(Fiske), When Schools Compete is thor- is the tendency of parents and schools to
ough and precise yet reads extremely select what each sees as better options.
well. The reader obtains the degree of This is exactly what market models
background, historical, and context ma- would predict, and the inequality that re-
terial to understand thoroughly the New sults is also easy to anticipate.
Zealand experience. The statistical pre- The real question is whether that
sentation is simple and descriptive, with same competition will also improve the
more complex presentations being re- lot of all schools, by forcing bad ones to get
served for academic journals. Thus the better. Here, unfortunately, the Fiske-
book should have broad appeal for both Ladd book can tell us very little. There
classrooms and interested general audi- was no real effort in New Zealand to sys-
ences. tematically study this phenomenon.
Roughly the first third of the book There is nothing like value-added stan-
presents the context, history, assump- dardized testing available, and the re-
tions, and logic of reforms in New Zea- searchers are forced to use student
land. It alone is worth the price of the movement to try to distinguish success-
book. Fiske and Ladd focus on three ful from unsuccessful schools. This
philosophical currents behind the re- obviously confounds the two critical
forms: democratic-populism, manage- questions concerning choice, and thus the
rial-business, and new-right-market effects of competition on student learning
currents. This combination leads Fiske are essentially unknown.
and Ladd to believe that New Zealand Finally, a fascinating feature of the
thus became a laboratory for testing the New Zealand reforms is the battle over
extent to which principles deduced from teacher salaries and assignments. Fiske
the economic marketplace are relevant to and Ladd realize this is crucial and de-
the delivery of education. vote considerable space to proposed
202 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

teacher salary block grants to schools. Soviet states are going through the pro-
This would have changed the practice of cess of devolving education responsibili-
assigning and paying teachers directly ties to local and school levels. Some
from the national Ministry of Education. variant of the New Zealand model may
The idea was to allow schools to use these well allow these states to continue fund-
and operating grants in a flexible fash- ing schools on a national level while de-
ion. Unions strongly resisted, and after volving authority and responsibility to
10 years and continuous sweetening of localities and schools. Policy makers in
the pot, there still were a number of those and other countries would be well
schools not participating in this funding advised to read this wonderful book.
scheme. The model and implementation JOHN F. WITTE
problems are important because many
University of Wisconsin
education systems in former Eastern and
Madison
THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY
INDEX

INDEX

Backfire effect, 57, 58-61 Crime and Justice Coordinating Group


BERNAZZANI, ODETTE, CATHERINE (Campbell Crime and Justice Coordi-
CT, and RICHARD E. TREMBLAY, nating Group, 39-48, 79
Early Parent Training to Prevent Dis- funding, 45-46
ruptive Behavior Problems and Delin- membership in, 40-41, 47
quency In Children, 90-103 methological quality requirements, 42-45
Boot camps, see Correctional boot camps procedures, 41
BORUCH, ROBERT F., see PETROSINO, AN- publications, 45
THONY, coauthor requirements of participants, 40
BRAGA, ANTHONY A., The Effects of Hot review process, 30-31
Spots Policing on Crime, 104-125 Web site, 42
See also Campbell Collaboration
Campbell Collaboration, 26-31, 39-48
CAMPBELL COLLABORATION CRIME DOES RESEARCH DESIGN AFFECT
A N D J U S T I C E G R O U P, T H E , STUDY OUTCOMES IN CRIMINAL
Farrington, David P. and Anthony JUSTICE? David Weisburd, Cynthia M.
Petrosino, 35-49 Lum, and Anthony Petrosino, 50-70
Campbell Collaboration Social, Psychological, DUGGAN, LORNA, see PETROSINO, AN-
Educational and Criminological Trials THONY, coauthor
Register (C2-SPECTR), 28-29
CBTs, see Cognitive-behavioral treatments EARLY PARENT TRAINING TO PREVENT
CHAPMAN, GABRIELLE L., see LIPSEY, DISRUPTIVE BEHAVIOR PROB-
MARK W., coauthor LEMS AND DELINQUENCY IN
characteristics of studies, 108-113 CHILDREN, Odette Bernazzani,
Cochrane Collaboration, 39, 46 Catherine Ct, and Richard E.
Cochrane Consumer Network, 25 Tremblay, 90-103
Cochrane Controlled Trials Register (CCTR), Effect size, 74-87
25 calculation of, 86-87
Cochrane Library, 25-26, 51 Effect size, indexes of
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL PROGRAMS categorical analysis of effect, 79
FOR OFFENDERS, Mark W. Lipsey, odds ratio, 75-76, 83-84
Gabrielle L. Chapman, and Nana A. regression analysis, 79, 83
Landenberger, 144-157 standardized mean difference, 75
Cognitive-behavioral programs for sex offend- EFFECTS OF CORRECTIONAL BOOT
ers, 81-83 CAMPS ON OFFENDING, Doris
Cognitive-behavioral treatments (CBTs) for Layton MacKenzie, David B. Wilson,
offenders, systematic review of, 145-155 and Suzanne B. Kider, 126-143
Collaboration Methods group, 27-28 EFFECTS OF HOT SPOTS POLICING ON
Communication and Dissemination Group, CRIME, THE, Anthony A. Braga, 104-
28 125
Education Coordinating Group, 27 Evidence-based policy, 15-23, 159-170
Social Welfare Coordinating Group, 27 challenges to, 17-20
Web site, 24, 39 legislation and, 16-17
See also Crime and Justice Coordinating on cognitive-behavioral treatments for of-
Group fenders, 154-155, 167-168
Collaborative review groups (CRGs), 24-26 on correctional boot camps, 137-140, 167-
Correctional boot camps, systematic review of, 168
127-140 on hot spots policing, 105, 121-122, 167-168
implications of, 137-140 on parent training, 121-122, 166-167
method, 128-130 systematic reviews and, 20-23
results, 130-137 See also Systematic reviews
search strategies for studies, 128-129 External validity, see Validity, external
COTE Ct, CATHERINE, see BERNAZZANI, FARRINGTON, DAVID P., see WELSH,
ODETTE, coauthor BRANDON C., coauthor

203
204 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

FARRINGTON, DAVID P., and ANTHONY PETROSINO, ANTHONY, see FARRINGTON,


PETROSINO, The Campbell Collabora- DAVID P., coauthor; WEISBURD,
tion Crime and Justice Group, 35-49 DAVID, coauthor
PETROSINO, ANTHONY, ROBERT F.
Hot spots policing, systematic review of, 105- BORUCH, HALUK SOYDAN, LORNA
122 DUGGAN, and JULIO SANCHEZ-
characteristics of studies, 108-113 MECA, Meeting the Challenges of Evi-
criteria for studies selected, 106-107 dence-Based Policy: The Campbell Col-
implications, 105-106, 121-122, 167-168 laboration, 15-34
Place-oriented crime prevention, see Hot spots
Internal validity, see Validity, internal policing
Investigator reported results (IRRs), 58-59 Public policy, see Evidence-based policy; Sys-
tematic reviews
KIDER, SUZANNE B., see M AC KENZIE,
DORIS LAYTON, coauthor Randomized studies, value of, 51-52
Research design, 51-67
LANDENBERGER, NANA A., see LIPSEY, effect size, 74-87
MARK W., coauthor impact on study conclusions, 52-67
LIPSEY, MARK W., GABRIELLE L. CHAP- measures of design quality, 93-94
MAN, and NANA A. LANDENBERGER, nonrandomized, 54, 65-67
Cognitive-Behavioral Programs for Of- randomized, 53-54, 64-67
fenders, 144-157 ROBERT F. BORUCH, see PETROSINO, AN-
LORNA DUGGAN, see PETROSINO, AN- THONY, coauthor
THONY, coauthor
LUM, CYNTHIA M., see Weisburd, David, co- SANCHEZ-MECA, JULIO, see PETROSINO,
author ANTHONY, coauthor
Single-study method, 160
MACKENZIE, DORIS LAYTON, DAVID B. SOYDAN, HALUK, see PETROSINO, AN-
WILSON, and SUZANNE B. KIDER, THONY, coauthor
Effects of Correctional Boot Camps on Systematic reviews, 21-23, 36-48, 161-162
Offending, 126-143 characteristics, 36-37
Maryland Report, 52-53, 57-67 in criminology, 38
MEETING THE CHALLENGES OF EVI- meta-analysis and, 37
DENCE-BASED POLICY: THE CAMP- publication of, 22-23, 48
BELL COLLABORATION, Anthony rationale for, 15
Petrosino, Robert F. Boruch, Haluk See also Campbell Collaboration; Evi-
Soydan, Lorna Duggan, and Julio dence-based policy; Research design
Sanchez-Meca, 15-34
Meta-analysis, 21, 37, 72-87 TOWARD AN EVIDENCE-BASED AP-
product-moment correlation as, 72 PROACH TO PREVENTING CRIME,
purpose of, 72-74 Brandon C. Welsh and David P.
vs. vote-count method, 73-74 Farrington, 158-173
META-ANALYTIC METHODS FOR CRIMI- TREMBLAY, RICHARD E., see BERNAZZANI,
NOLOGY, David B. Wilson, 71-89 ODETTE, coauthor
Methodological quality
external validity, 43-45 Validity
internal validity, 43-45 external, 44-45
internal, 43-44, 52-57
Parent training, systematic review of, 90-100 See also Research design
criteria for studies selected, 91 Vote-count method, 73-74, 160-161
implications, 98-100, 121-122, 166-168
method of review, 93-94 WEISBURD, DAVID, CYNTHIA M. LUM, and
results, 94-98 ANTHONY PETROSINO, Does Re-
search strategy for studies, 92-93 search Design Affect Study Outcomes
Pearson, Karl, 72 in Criminal Justice? 50-70
INDEX 205

WELSH, BRANDON C., and DAVID P. WILSON, DAVID B., see MACKENZIE, DORIS
FARRINGTON, Toward an Evidence- LAYTON, coauthor
Based Approach to Preventing Crime, WILSON, DAVID B., Meta-Analytic Methods
158-173 for Criminology, 71-89

You might also like