You are on page 1of 99

Introduction to Learning Theories 1

INTRODUCTION

TO

LEARNING THEORIES

by

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.

University of Houston

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 2

Copyright 2012 Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D., 6011 Bayonne, Spring, TX 77389. All rights

reserved. Published in the United States of America. This publication is protected by

copyright, and permission should be obtained from the author/publisher prior to any

prohibited reproduction, storage in a retrieval system, or transmission in any form or by any

means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or likewise. To obtain permission to

use material from this work, please submit a written request to Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D. at the

address given above.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 3

CONTENTS

CHAPTER 1: THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF LEARNING .............. 8

DEFINING AND STUDYING LEARNING ............................... 9

Defining Learning ........................................................................................ 9

The Goals of the Scientific Study of Learning ...................................................... 10

Describing Learning .................................................................................. 10

Explaining Learning .................................................................................. 11

Predicting Learning .................................................................................. 11

Influencing Learning ................................................................................. 12

METATHEORETICAL ISSUES......................................... 12

Epistemology ............................................................................................ 13

Learning Theory Families .............................................................................. 16

Behaviorism ........................................................................................... 16

Cognitivism............................................................................................ 16

Constructivism........................................................................................ 18

Evaluating the Usefulness of Learning Theories .................................................... 19

A PREVIEW OF LEARNING THEORIES .............................. 20

CHAPTER 2: CONDITIONING THEORIES ........................... 23

CLASSICAL CONDITIONING.......................................... 23

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 4

The Elements of Classical Conditioning ............................................................. 25

Influences on Classical Conditioning ................................................................. 27

EMOTIONAL CONDITIONING AND THE LAW OF EFFECT........ 28

John Watson and Emotional Conditioning ........................................................... 28

E. L. Thorndike and the Law of Effect ............................................................... 29

OPERANT CONDITIONING ........................................... 31

Reinforcers .............................................................................................. 31

Positive and Negative Reinforcers ................................................................. 31

Primary and Secondary Reinforcers ............................................................... 32

The Process of Reinforcement ........................................................................ 33

Operant Conditioning Outcomes ...................................................................... 35

Schedules of Reinforcement .......................................................................... 36

Punishment .............................................................................................. 37

Escape and Avoidance Learning ...................................................................... 39

EVALUATION OF CONDITIONING THEORIES ...................... 40

Systematic Desensitization ............................................................................ 41

Behavior Modification .................................................................................. 42

CHAPTER 3: INFORMATION PROCESSING THEORY .............. 46

THE ATKINSON-SHIFFRIN MODEL OF MEMORY .................. 47

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 5

Sensory Memory ......................................................................................... 48

Short-Term and Working Memory..................................................................... 48

Long-Term Memory ..................................................................................... 51

Explicit and Implicit Memory ....................................................................... 52

Episodic and Semantic Memory .................................................................... 52

REMEMBERING ........................................................ 53

Remembering as Retrieval............................................................................. 54

Remembering as Reconstruction ..................................................................... 54

Schemas ............................................................................................... 55

Networks .............................................................................................. 56

FORGETTING.......................................................... 57

Context Effects ......................................................................................... 58

The Curve of Forgetting ............................................................................... 59

Other Types of Forgetting ............................................................................. 60

EVALUATION OF INFORMATION PROCESSING THEORY ........ 62

CHAPTER 4: COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT ......................... 65

THE DEVELOPING BRAIN ............................................ 66

PIAGETS THEORY OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT .............. 68

Piagets Great Discovery: Four Stages of Cognitive Development ............................... 68

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 6

The Sensorimotor Stage ............................................................................. 69

The Preoperational Stage ........................................................................... 69

The Concrete Operational Stage ................................................................... 70

The Formal Operational Stage ..................................................................... 72

Piagets Explanation of Cognitive Development ................................................... 74

Schemes. .............................................................................................. 74

Stages as Schemes ................................................................................... 75

Influences on Progression through the Stages ................................................... 76

THE INFORMATION-PROCESSING APPROACH TO COGNITIVE

DEVELOPMENT ....................................................... 78

Information Processing Efficiency .................................................................... 78

Processing Speed ..................................................................................... 78

Short-term Memory Capacity ....................................................................... 79

Working Memory Efficiency ......................................................................... 80

Automaticity .......................................................................................... 81

Expertise. ............................................................................................. 81

Metacognition ........................................................................................... 82

Selective Attention. ................................................................................. 82

Cognitive Monitoring................................................................................. 83

Metamemory. ......................................................................................... 83

Neo-Piagetian Theories of Cognitive Development ................................................ 84

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 7

VYGOTSKYS SOCIOCULTURAL THEORY.......................... 85

EVALUATION OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENTAL THEORY ...... 86

CHAPTER 5: SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY ........................ 91

LEARNING THROUGH IMITATION .................................. 92

Early Theories of Imitative Learning ................................................................. 92

Banduras Approach to Learning through Imitation ................................................ 93

LEARNING THROUGH MODELING .................................. 95

What and How We Learn from Models ............................................................... 95

Influences on Modeling................................................................................. 96

EVALUATION OF SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY ................... 97

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 8

THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF

LEARNING

DEFINING AND STUDYING LEARNING

METATHEORETICAL ISSUES

A PREVIEW OF LEARNING THEORIES

SUMMARY

KEY TERMS, CONCEPTS, AND PEOPLE

You probably use the word learning fairly often, but have you ever taken the time to develop

a precise definition of it? If you did so, you would probably find that your general beliefs

about human nature play a large role in your definition. In this regard, you would have a great

deal in common with the approach that philosophers take to defining learning. That is, you

would use your general beliefs as premises from which to derive propositions about the nature

of learning. Educators typically approach the definition of learning in a similar manner, but

the beliefs they work from come from exposure to theories of instruction, their own teaching

experiences, as well as their general beliefs about human nature. In contrast to both

philosophers and educators, psychologists view learning as a natural, law-governed process

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 9

that researchers can describe, explain, test, and apply using the steps of the scientific

method. Nevertheless, the scientific study of learning has its roots in the philosophical

approach and, as you will see, cannot be separated from it. Whether we are interested in the

philosophy or the science of learning, a working definition of it must be our starting point.

DEFINING AND STUDYING LEARNING

Defining Learning

Simply put, learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior or knowledge that is caused

by experience. However, it is important to differentiate between learning and other kinds of

change. The distinguishing characteristic of learning is that it results exclusively from

experience. Think about a child learning to recite the alphabet. If she had never heard a

teacher, parent, or television character reciting the alphabet, she would never have learned

to do so herself.

In contrast to learning, maturation results from an inborn genetic plan. For example,

we often say that a baby "learns" to walk, but this statement violates the definition of

learning because it does not result exclusively from experience. All healthy human infants

living in reasonably supportive environments walk sooner or later, because humans are

genetically programmed to walk. Therefore, an infants change in status from non-walker to

walker is due to maturation rather than learning.

Development involves changes that arise from interactions between learning and

maturation. Consider the case of language. Some aspects of language development are built

in to the brain and depend on its maturation, but it also depends on the presence of language

in the environment. A child who grows up in a language-less environment will not develop

language. Likewise, a child who has an organic condition that interferes with brain

development may never develop language no matter how much of it is present in his

environment.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 10

The Goals of the Scientific Study of Learning

As you read earlier, psychologists apply the scientific method to the study of learning. Its

steps include observation, theory development, and hypothesis testing. In addition,

psychologists seek to apply scientific findings to problems outside the tightly controlled world

of the laboratory. Thus, psychologists who study learning seek to

Describe learning outcomes as accurately as possible (observation)

Explain why learning outcomes occur (theory development)

Predict learning outcomes and test their predictions (hypothesis testing)

Influence learning outcomes (application)

Educators are usually more concerned with the fourth goal, influencing learning

outcomes, than the other three. As a result, they sometimes studies of learning to be of no

value if they do not immediately lead to solutions to instructional problems. However,

effective instruction begins with reliable and valid ideas about how learning occurs, just as

the development of a vaccine begins with an understanding of the virus it is designed to

protect against. Thus, describing, explaining, and predicting learning outcomes are

indispensable to the development of instructional strategies that can influence learning.

Describing Learning: Some types of learning are easily observed and, as a result, can be

described in fairly straightforward ways. For instance, think about a child who is learning to

identify the letters of the alphabet. We can describe her proficiency in a number of ways

based on direct observation of her behavior. We could say that she can name 13 letters, 50%

of the alphabet, or list the specific letters she can name consistently.

However, there are some types of learning that are difficult or impossible to observe

directly. These types of learning are known as constructs, psychological variables that cannot

be directly observed but can be inferred from behavior. To describe a construct, a researcher

must develop an operational definition, an observable behavior that is a reasonable

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 11

representation if the construct. For example, student performance on standardized tests

often serves as an operational definition of the construct of reading achievement.

To be useful to learning theorists and researchers, operational definitions must have

reliability, that is, they must yield consistent scores. They must also have validity. In other

words, an operational definition must measure what it purports to measure. For example, a

reading test must be distinguishable from a general intelligence test to be valid.

Explaining Learning: Explaining learning requires the development of learning theories,

statements that account for relationships among observations of learning behavior. It is

important to note that learning theories, like other scientific theories, are not simply opinions

that one can accept or reject on the basis of consistency with ones personal beliefs,

experiences with teaching and learning, or intuition about human nature. Instead, they are

statements that explain specific empirical observations. As such, they must be tested

empirically and determined to have support or lack support rather than judged to be

true or false. This is the essential difference between a philosophy of learning and a

learning theory. To be viable, a philosophys conclusions must flow logically from its

premises. For a theory to remain viable, the hypotheses derived from it must survive the

scrutiny of empirical testing.

Predicting Learning: Theories yield hypotheses, testable predictions of how changes in one

or more variables affect learning outcomes. Thus, the purpose of the prediction phase of the

scientific study of learning is to subject theory-based hypotheses to empirical testing so that

the theory can be accepted, rejected, or modified accordingly. For example, you will learn in

Chapter 2 that Ivan Pavlovs research assistants observed hungry dogs in Pavlovs laboratory

salivating when the assistants teased the dogs with food. Building on that observation, Pavlov

developed a theory of conditioning, a type of learning in which behaviors are thought of as

responses to stimuli. His theory generated a number of testable hypotheses, most of which

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 12

were confirmed by his research. These successful studies, in turn, led to the establishment of

principles of conditioning. Principles are predictions that have been supported by research so

many times that they are regarded as reliable nearly 100% of the time. Therefore, we can say

with confidence that Pavlovs conditioning theory has empirical support due to its having

yielded testable hypotheses the examination of which has led to the establishment of reliable

principles of learning. We may not like conditioning theory. We may not think it explains

anything important about learning, but we must concede that it is supported by empirical

evidence and has led to the development of principles of learning. As you can see, it would be

inappropriate to say I agree with Pavlov or I disagree with Pavlov on the question of

empirical support for his theory.

Influencing Learning: Once reliable principles of learning have been established, we can use

them to develop theories of instruction. For example, classroom management theories that

emphasize the notion that the degree to which students feel emotionally comfortable in a

classroom depends on the nature of the stimuli that are present in the environment are

largely based on principles of classical condition. Such theories assume that cues such as

furniture that is more common to homes than to classrooms (e.g., a rocking chair, scatter

rugs, throw pillows) can help students feel welcome and metaphorically at home.

METATHEORETICAL ISSUES

Metatheoretical issues involve questions that apply to all learning theories. For example, as

you learned earlier, learning theories, like all psychological theories, have their roots in

philosophy. Thus, comparing and contrasting the philosophical bases of learning theories

addresses a metatheoretical issue. Grouping theories according to shared characteristics and

comparing their relative usefulness to the scientific study of learning address two other

important metatheoretical issues.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 13

Epistemology

Every learning theory makes assumptions about epistemology, the branch of philosophy that

addresses the question "Where does knowledge come from?" For this reason, psychologists

often compare learning theories in terms of their underlying epistemologies. Such

comparisons reveal why some theories focus on factors that are external to the learner, such

as instruction, and other focus on internal psychological variables, such as memory function.

Theories that are based on empiricist epistemologies assume that the causes of

learning are external to the learner. For example, the statement Instruction causes

learning is consistent with empiricist epistemology. Learning theories that are derived from

rationalist epistemologies assume that the causes of learning are internal to the learner. A

statement such as Default learning structures that are genetically wired into the human

brain determine learning reflects an underlying epistemology that is rationalist in nature.

Theories with an organismic epistemological foundation assume that interactions among

factors that are both external and internal to the learner cause learning. Thus, the organismic

approach gives rise to propositions that are more complex than those that are based on

empiricist or rationalist epistemology. For instance, an organismicist would probably agree

that The combined effects of instruction, brain maturation, prior learning, stage of cognitive

development, and learners transitory emotional states cause learning.

Epistemologies can be thought of as ways of answering three important questions

about learning:

Are learners active or passive in the learning process?

Which is more important to learning, nature or nurture?

Is learning quantitative are qualitative?

An epistemologys answer to the first question determines its location on the

activity/passivity dimension. That is, some argue that learners are active and at least

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 14

somewhat in control of the learning process, while others depict learners as passive and at

the mercy of the environment. Epistemologies vary on the nature/nurture dimension in

accordance with their answers to the second question. This dimension focuses on the

interplay between factors that are biological, which include inherited individual and species-

specific differences, and those that are environmental, such as instruction. Nature

collectively refers to biological factors, while nurture collectively refers to environmental

ones. Answers to the third question represent varying positions on the

quantitative/qualitative dimension, an epistemologys assumptions about the degree to

which learning is quantitative (sometimes called continuous) or qualitative (sometimes called

qualitative). Epistemologies that take the quantitative approach assume that each learning

experience simply adds to those that can before. As a result, such theories characterize

movement from point A (no knowledge) to point B (knowledge) as happening in a straight

line, somewhat like walking up a ramp. Epistemologies that assume learning to be qualitative

assume that some instances of learning modify, rather than add to, previously learned

behaviors and ideas. Consequently, these epistemologies characterize movement from point A

(no knowledge) to Point B (knowledge) as happening in a series of leaps or a pattern of

apparent progressions and regressions, somewhat like walking up a staircase or hiking up a

switchback trail to get to the top of a mountain.

To better understand the quantitative/qualitative distinction, consider two examples

from child development. First, although children sometimes have growth spurts, the nature of

height itself never changes; children just get more of it as they get older. Thus, changes in

height are quantitative or continuous, because height is height no matter how tall the child

is. We can accurately describe it in terms of inches or centimeters in individuals of all ages

and heights.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 15

In contrast to height, puberty is a qualitative change, because it is a change in kind.

Prior to puberty, children are incapable of reproduction. During puberty, they acquire the

capacity to reproduce. When puberty is complete, children, who are now renamed

adolescents, are equal to adults in their capacity for reproduction. We do not say that an

adolescent or adult has more reproductive capacity than a child, because the difference is

not a quantitative one as the word more implies. The difference is qualitative in that

prepubescent children cannot reproduce, and post-pubescent adolescents and adults can

reproduce. Puberty results in changes in the nature of the body itself; organs and glands gain

capacities that they lacked before it occurred.

Some epistemologies imply that learning is akin to childrens height gains from year to

year. From this perspective, people accumulate learning experiences as time goes on, and

their knowledge at any given point in time can be thought of as the sum of all of their

learning experiences up until then. These epistemologies lie at the quantitative end of the

continuum. By contrast, other epistemologies are based on the idea that some learning events

are more like puberty than height gains. That is, some learning events actually change the

ways in which individuals learn and in how they think about what they already know. These

epistemologies lie at the qualitative end of the continuum. Moreover, stages are the hallmark

of theories that are associated with epistemologies that emphasize the qualitative aspects of

learning.

Another example, one drawn from language development, may help to clarify the

distinction between quantitative and qualitative change. Think about the various ways in

which childrens vocabularies change as they get older. One such change is that children learn

new words as they get older. This is a quantitative change. That is, ten-year-olds know more

words than four-year-olds do, and sixteen-year-olds know more words than ten-year-olds do.

But childrens understanding of the interconnections among word meanings also changes as

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 16

they get older. For instance, at some point, children observe that the words bicycle and

motorcycle have the root word cycle in common. When they realize that cycle refers to the

circular nature of wheels, they can use that knowledge to infer that there must be something

circular about the water cycle when they encounter it in science class. The construction of

meaningful links among bits of knowledge that can be used to acquire new knowledge through

inference is a qualitative change.

Learning Theory Families

Learning theories are grouped into three broad families according to their epistemological

assumptions.

Behaviorism. In a seminal article published in 1904, psychologist John B. Watson coined the

term behaviorism to denote an approach to studying learning that emphasizes observable

behavior. The epistemological assumptions of Watsons behaviorism fit within the empiricist

tradition. According to the behaviorists, learning happens when passive learners are acted

upon by active agents in the environment. Consequently, theories in the behaviorist family

emphasize nurture over nature. Likewise, they view learning as comprised almost exclusively

of quantitative change. That is, a learners behavior at any given time is the sum of all prior

learning experiences. Similarly, behaviorists discount or deny the existence of thinking and

emotion, both of which they view as learned responses to environmental stimuli.

Cognitivism. In contrast to behaviorism, cognitivism emphasizes unobservable cognitive

processes. These processes include attention, perception, reasoning, memory, decision

making, and problem solving. For cognitivists, learning happens when the mind takes in

information and transforms it. As such, cognitivism fits within the rationalist epistemological

tradition. The information provided to the mind by the environment matters, but, for

cognitivists, it is what the mind does with the information that really counts. For instance,

early cognitivists such as Kurt Koffka, Wolfgang Kohler, and Max Wertheimer discovered a

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 17

number of important cognitive phenomena, the Gestalt principles of perception. The term

Gestalt implies that the mind tends to impose the best possible form on sensory input. Three

of these principles are illustrated in Figure 1.1. The principle of proximity causes us to

perceive the two sets of plus signs differently. We do not see them as two sets of 54 plus

signs. Instead, we view those on the left as two rows and those on the right as three columns.

We perceive the form of the plus signs, not their number. The principle of similarity causes

us to view the collection of plus signs and ampersands on the left as consisting of columns and

the ones on the right as consisting of rows. The principle of closure causes us to say that the

two figures in the third row are polygons when, in fact, they are not. Gestaltists would say

that we impose our mental template of a triangle on the lines in the left cell and that of a

rectangle on the one in the cell on the right because that is the best match our minds can

make between stored knowledge and incomplete sensory information. In effect, we mentally

close the lines in the two figures because we perceive that they should be closed rather than

open according to the standards of perceptual wholeness that Gestaltists believed are wired

in to our brains.

Figure 1.1
Gestalt Principles of Perception

Principle Columns or Rows?

+ + + + + + + + +
+ + + + + + + + +
+ + + + + + + + +
+ + + + + + + + +
+ + + + + + + + +
+ + + + + + + + +
Proximity + + + + + + + + +
+ + + + + + + + +
+ + + + + + + + +
+ + + + + + + + +
+ + + + + + + + +
+ + + + + + + + +

+ & + & + & + & + + + + + + + + + +


+ & + & + & + & + & & & & & & & &
Similarity + & + & + & + & + + + + + + + + + +
+ & + & + & + & + & & & & & & & &

Polygon or Not?

Closure

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 18

The Gestaltists further argued that insight, the aha experience that often happens

when we focus on a problem and suddenly arrive at a solution, works similarly. The senses

provide our minds with information about the elements of the problem. But it is our mind

that perceives these elements and the solution that satisfactorily unites them as a whole that

we can produce by acting on the elements in a certain way.

Clearly, the cognitivists learner is an active processor of information rather than a

passive recipient of environmental influence. Because both the information and the cognitive

processes applied to it contribute to learning, cognitivists tend to emphasize interactions

between nature (i.e., the brains hard-wired information processors) and nurture (i.e.,

information provided by the environment). They also view learning as involving both

quantitative and qualitative change.

Constructivism. Cognitivists typically argue that principles such as proximity, similarity,

closure, and insight are wired in to the brain. In philosophical language, they are a priori

constraints on information processing. In contrast, advocates for constructivism argue that

the learners mind builds most of the internal structures that it uses to interpret sensory

information on its own, although they acknowledge the existence of some a priori constraints.

As such, constructivism stands with cognitivism in opposition to behaviorism in that it views

the learner as an active agent in her own learning, emphasizes interactions between nature

and nurture, and sees both quantitative and qualitative change at work in the learning

process. Where the two orientations differ is in their relative emphasis on a priori constraints

and qualitative change. For constructivists, the number of experience-based, self-constructed

understandings far exceeds the number of a priori constraints. For this reason, as we noted

earlier, cognitivism fits within the rationalist epistemological tradition, while constructivism

is more consistent with the organismic approach. Moreover, constructivists place a great deal

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 19

more emphasis on qualitative than on quantitative change. Thus, theories within the

constructivist family typically describe change in terms of stages, while cognitivists do not.

Before moving on, you should be aware that the usage of the term constructivist in

theories of instruction varies considerably from its usage in learning theories. Typically,

constructivist instructional methods allow students experiment with learning materials, often

in collaboration with peers, in the hopes that such experimentation will lead them to

construct skills or knowledge (as opposed to objectivist models that emphasize acquisition

of information). In contrast, constructivist learning theories do not argue that learning

depends on opportunities for experimentation or on collaboration with others. It can happen

in such contexts or as a result of direct, objectivist-oriented instruction. The key distinction is

that, according to constructivist learning theory, learners construct their own knowledge no

matter what kind of instruction they are exposed to. Thus, research that supports

constructivist learning theories should not be construed as supportive of constructivist

teaching methods.

Evaluating the Usefulness of Learning Theories

In addition to comparing theories in terms of their epistemologies and grouping them into

theoretical families, we can evaluate learning theories in terms of their usefulness.

Generally, there are four questions that address a learning theorys usefulness:

Does it have heuristic value, that is, does it generate debate and research?

Does it explain the basic facts of learning?

Does it generate testable predictions?

Does it lead to practical strategies that are relevant to real-world learning and

instruction?

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 20

You will see these four questions again after you take a brief look at the theories you will

read about in the coming chapters. For now, just keep them in mind as you read the next

section.

A PREVIEW OF LEARNING THEORIES

Table 1.1 summarizes the epistemological assumptions and theoretical families of each of the

theories you will read about in the remaining chapters of this text. The table also includes a

preview of the mechanisms posited by each theory to explain learning. For example, in

Piaget's cognitive-developmental theory, the primary mechanisms are assimilation,

accommodation, and equilibration of schemes. This means that Piaget's explanations of

learning focus on schemes (internal cognitive structures) and how they change with

experience (assimilation, accommodation, and equilibration).

Table 1.1
Overview of Learning Theories
Theoretical Explanations of
Epistemological Questions
Family Learning
Behaviorism
Active/Passive Nature/Nurture Quantitative/ Mechanisms of
Cognitivism
Qualitative
Constructivism Change
Theory
With which
Is the learner Which is more Is learning
theoretical
active or passive important to the quantitative or
family is the What causes learning?
in the learning learning process, qualitative in
theory most
process? nature or nurture? nature?
compatible?

Classical Passive Nurture Quantitative Behaviorism Association of stimuli


Conditioning

Reinforcement,
Operant Passive Nurture Quantitative Behaviorism
punishment
Conditioning
Characteristics of the IP
Information Active Both Both Cognitivism system and to-be-
Processing learned information

Piagets Assimilation,
Constructivism
Cognitive Active Both Qualitative accommodation,
(with stages)
Developmental equilibration of schemes
Theory
Behaviorism
Social-Cognitive Active Nurture Quantitative Constructivism Modeling; self-efficacy
Theory (no stages)

As you might guess from looking at the table, the change mechanisms that each theory

proposes arise from its epistemological assumptions. For example, classical conditioning

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 21

theory views learners as passive, more influenced by nurture than nature, and emphasizes

quantitative change. As a result, the mechanisms of learning it proposes are located in the

environment.

In each of the chapters that follow, you will read an in-depth examination of each

theorys assumptions and proposed learning mechanisms. You will also learn about the

relative usefulness of the theories this text covers. For now, the main thing you need to know

about these theories is that, for the most part, all of them get high ratings on the first

criterion listed above. They all have heuristic value in that they have led to a great deal of

debate and research. That is why these theories form the core of advanced learning theory

courses. It is also why they continue to exert a great deal of influence on the scientific study

of learning and on theories of instruction. Thus, in the chapters that follow, you will read

about how well each theory addresses the three remaining criteria of usefulness

comprehensiveness, testability, and applicability to real-world learning and instruction.

SUMMARY

In this chapter, you have learned how psychologists define learning and the goals that guide

research that examines the learning process. You have also become familiar with the criteria

that psychologists use to compare and evaluate theories. These include the metatheoretical

issues of epistemology, theoretical family classification, and usefulness. In future chapters we

will return to these issues in the context of in-depth examinations of five major learning

theories: classical conditioning, operant conditioning, information processing, cognitive-

developmental theory, and social-cognitive theory.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 22

KEY TERMS, CONCEPTS, AND PEOPLE

learning
maturation
development
construct
operational definition
reliability
validity
theories
hypotheses
Ivan Pavlov
principles
metatheoretical issues
epistemology
empiricist
rationalist
organismic
activity/passivity dimension
nature/nurture dimension
quantitative/qualitative dimension
John B. Watson
behaviorism
cognitivism
Kurt Koffka
Wolfgang Kohler
Max Wertheimer
Gestalt principles of perception
insight
constructivism
a priori constraints
heuristic value

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 23

CONDITIONING THEORIES

CLASSICAL CONDITIONING

EMOTIONAL CONDITIONING AND THE LAW OF EFFECT

OPERANT CONDITIONING

EVALUATION OF CONDITIONING THEORIES

SUMMARY

KEY TERMS, CONCEPTS, AND PEOPLE

You should recall from Chapter 1 that epistemological assumptions of the conditioning

theories flow from these theorists belief that learners are passive and that learning is caused

by the action of the environment on the learner. Thus, they place far more emphasis on

nurture than on nature. Likewise, conditioning theorists dismiss the notion that there are

stages, or any kind of qualitative change, in learning. They argue that stage-like behavior is

the result of an accumulation of learning experiences.

CLASSICAL CONDITIONING

Classical conditioning is a type of learning in which an organism acquires the tendency to

exhibit a natural behavior in response to an unnatural stimulus. It happens as a result of the

pairing of natural with neutral stimuli.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 24

Pavlovs Classic Experiment

Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (1849-1936) is credited with discovering the principles of

classical conditioning. In the early years of a research career that would endure until just a

few days before Pavlov died at the age of 86, his primary research interests were in the

physiology of digestion and circulation. Pavlov developed a number of research techniques

and devices that won him the esteem of his colleagues. Moreover, in 1904, he received a

Nobel Prize in physiology and medicine for his studies examining the nerves of the heart.

It was in the context of Pavlovs studies of the digestive system that he and his

research team accidentally discovered the existence of conditioned reflexes, automatic

behaviors that an organism exhibits in response to environmental signals, in 1901. Based on

what was known about digestion at the time, scientists believed that animals would salivate

only when the salivary glands in their mouths came into direct contact with food. However,

one of Pavlovs assistants found that the dogs that were being used in the teams experiments

salivated whenever they were teased, as he called it, with food. That is, the dogs would

drool at the sight and smell of food, although not as heavily as they did when food was

actually placed in their mouths. Pavlov hypothesized that the dogs drooling was a

conditioned reflex and, together with his research assistants, he devised a series of

experiments to test this hypothesis.

In the best known of these experiments, researchers rang a bell before they fed the

dogs. They repeated the pairing of the bell and the food several times. Next, they rang the

bell but withheld the food to see if the ringing of the bell alone would cause the dogs to

salivate. As Pavlovs hypothesis predicted, the dogs salivated at the sound of the bell.

Pavlovs team repeated the study many times with a number of variations. In addition, over

the next few decades, they conducted hundreds of experiments that enabled them to identify

the elements of and influences upon the process of classical conditioning.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 25

The Elements of Classical Conditioning

Classical conditioning begins with a reflex, an involuntary behavior that an organism exhibits

when it encounters the natural stimulus to which it is biologically linked. For instance, your

pancreas reflexively excretes insulin when your blood sugar rises, because the blood sugar-

insulin stimulus-reflex bond is part of your bodys genetic programming. Likewise, you move

your head out of the way when a flying object, such as a ball, approaches it. The stimulus is

the approaching object, and the reflex is moving your head.

Pavlovs research and thousands of studies by others that followed it revealed a key

principle about the relationship between natural stimuli and reflexes. When a neutral

stimulus, one that does not have a reflex connected to it, consistently precedes a natural

stimulus, it eventually acquires the power to elicit the reflex on its own. For instance, in

Pavlovs classic experiment, the bell was the neutral stimulus. Because it was consistently

paired with the natural stimulus for the salivation reflex (food), the bell ultimately acquired

the same signal value as the natural stimulus. That is, it became a signal that prompted the

dogs salivary glands to produce saliva. As a result, the dogs developed two variations on the

salivation reflex. When they salivated in response to food being placed in their mouths,

salivation was classified as a natural reflex. But when they salivated in response to the bell,

the salivation reflex was classified as a learned reflex.

Learning researchers apply specific terms to the various components of the classical

conditioning process. The natural stimulus is called the unconditioned stimulus

(unconditioned = unlearned), and the natural reflex is called the unconditioned response.

The neutral stimulus, after it acquires the ability to elicit the reflex, is called the

conditioned stimulus (conditioned = learned), and the learned reflex is called the

conditioned response. The diagram below illustrates the application of these terms to

Pavlovs experiment.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 26

Figure 2.1
Pavlovs Classic Study

Unconditioned Stimulus

Unconditioned Response

Neutral Stimulus Unconditioned Stimulus


Unconditioned Response

Conditioned Stimulus Conditioned Response

Interestingly, once a stimulus functions independently as a trigger for a reflex, it can

play the same role in the conditioning process that the natural stimulus played in the original

series of events. This process is known as higher-order conditioning, the process through

which a new neutral stimulus acquires the capacity to elicit a conditioned response as a result

of being paired with a conditioned stimulus. For instance, after Pavlovs dogs learned to

salivate in response to a bell, he was able to produce higher-order conditioning by pairing the

bell with a variety of neutral stimuli. If he sounded a buzzer prior to ringing the bell, the

dogs salivation reflex eventually became a conditioned response that they exhibited

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 27

whenever the buzzer was sounded. Thus, the buzzer became a new conditioned stimulus with

the same capacity to elicit the salivation reflex as the bell and as the food with which the

bell was originally paired.

Influences on Classical Conditioning

Pairing a neutral stimulus with a natural stimulus doesnt always lead to classical

conditioning. For one thing, a neutral stimulus must precede or occur at the same time as the

natural stimulus. Presenting the neutral stimulus after the natural stimulus doesnt work. In

addition, the delay between the neutral stimulus and the natural stimulus must be so brief

that no other neutral stimulus can occur between them.

Furthermore, classical conditioning isnt permanent. If a conditioned stimulus occurs

many times without being followed by the natural stimulus, it will eventually lose the ability

to elicit the reflex, a phenomenon known as extinction. Oddly, too, conditioned responses

sometimes disappear unexpectedly, and the organism reverts to its natural behavior. This

process is known as spontaneous recovery. Classical conditioning may also be context-

specific. That is, an organism may exhibit a conditioned response in connection with a

conditioned stimulus only in the setting in which the conditioned and natural stimulus were

originally paired.

A classically conditioned response may be exhibited in either a narrow or a broad way.

In discrimination, a conditioned response occurs only when the conditioned stimulus itself is

present. For example, a dog who learns to salivate in response to a bell that sounds the

musical tone C exhibits discrimination if it salivates only when a bell that sounds the tone C is

rung and does not respond to other tones. By contrast, in generalization, a conditioned

response occurs when the specific conditioned stimulus is present and when other stimuli that

are similar to it are present. For example, if a dog conditioned with a C-tone bell salivates

when a D-tone or B-tone bell is rung, generalization has occurred.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 28

Finally, biological predispositions, genetic tendencies to acquire or resist acquiring

classically conditioned responses to specific stimuli, influence classical conditioning.

Researchers have found that organisms rapidly acquire classically conditioned responses that

involve stimuli that are real threats to survival. For example, chimpanzees rapidly acquire

classically conditioned fear responses to any neutral stimulus that precedes the introduction

of a snake into the environment (Mineka & Oehlberg, 2008; World).

EMOTIONAL CONDITIONING AND THE LAW OF EFFECT

Two early conditioning theorists, John Watson and E. L. Thorndike, put forward ideas and

empirical evidence that advanced psychologists understanding of human and animal learning.

Both amplified and extended Pavlovs findings to construct more comprehensive descriptions

of learning. Thus, Watsons work on emotional conditioning and Thorndikes studies of trial-

and-error learning served as the foundation of modern learning theories.

John Watson and Emotional Conditioning

As you learned in Chapter 1, John Watson (1878-1958) coined the term behaviorism, the

perspective that explains learning exclusively in terms of the influences of environmental

agents on organisms. Moreover, he argued that virtually all variations in human outcomes are

attributable to classical conditioning. He focused particularly on emotional outcomes such as

learned fears. In search of evidence for his arguments, Watson carried out one of the best

known and most controversial experiments in the history of psychology.

Watson and his assistant, Rosalie Raynor, placed an 11-month-old infant named Albert

in a room with a white rat. The child curiously observed the rat and attempted to play with

it. Importantly, he exhibited no fear whatsoever. As you probably know, infants are naturally

afraid of loud noises. Typically they startle and cry fearfully when they are exposed to sudden

loud noises such as gunfire. Watson and Raynor wanted to show that they could use Alberts

natural fear of loud noises to condition him to fear the white rat. Consequently, they placed a

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 29

suspended steel bar behind Alberts back where he could not see them strike the bar with a

hammer. Following Pavlovs procedures, they presented the rat and then banged the steel

bar. Predictably, Albert soon startled and cried whenever he saw the rat. Moreover, he

generalized the conditioned fear response to other white, furry objects. These included his

mothers fur coat and a Santa Claus beard that Watson himself donned to test the extent of

Alberts generalization of the conditioned fear response.

Watson argued that the Little Albert experiment, as his study came to be known,

and others like it proved his hypothesis that all human learning is attributable to classical

conditioning. Few psychologists today would agree, and virtually all of them would find fault

with the ethics of Watsons experiment. However, Watson is credited with being the first

behavioral scientist to demonstrate that emotional responses are subject to classical

conditioning.

E. L. Thorndike and the Law of Effect

Like Watson, pioneering learning theorist E. L. Thorndike (1874-1949) argued that

psychologists should focus their studies on observable behavior rather than on internal mental

states. However, Thorndike deviated from behaviorist orthodoxy in his belief that hereditary

differences across individuals contributed to learning outcomes. Consequently, his research

career included two distinctive strands. First, he conducted rigorous laboratory studies of

both animal and human learning. Second, he developed standardized psychological tests to

measure human variations in traits such as personality and intelligence that enabled him to

compare similarities in these traits among twins, non-twin siblings, parents and children, and

unrelated individuals. Thus, Thorndike is often called the father of educational psychology.

Thorndike developed a distinctive learning theory based on studies in which he

confined cats in boxes like the one in Figure 2.2. He placed a fish outside the door of the box

and observed as the cats determined how to operate a lever, cord, or other device to open

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 30

the door to get hold of the fish. Thorndike observed the cats engaging in a process he called

trial and accidental success, rather than trial and error, as they attempted to figure out

how to operate the opening mechanism of each type of door he presented to them. As they

worked on opening the doors, the cats abandoned behaviors that did not produce results and

repeated those that moved them closer to solving the problem of getting the door open.

Figure 2.2
Thorndikes Puzzle Box

Thorndike proposed that the cats repetition of successful actions and discontinuation

of unsuccessful ones constitute the law of exercise, the tendency of repeated behaviors to

strengthen bonds between sensory perceptions (such as seeing a cord attached to a puzzle

box door) and actions (such as pulling on the cord to see if it opens the door). Moreover, he

proposed a learning principle that he called the law of effect, the idea that the behavioral

repertoire of an organism consists of stamped-in and stamped-out responses. Stamped-in

responses are those that the organism acquires because they produce satisfying outcomes.

Stamped-out responses are those that the organism abandons because they produce annoying

outcomes.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 31

OPERANT CONDITIONING

The ideas and discoveries of Pavlov, Watson, and Thorndike were instrumental to the work of

B. F. Skinner. As you may recall, classical conditioning deals with reflexive responses, such as

the way that you move your head when you think an object is going to hit you in the face. By

contrast, Skinners work addressed changes in non-reflexive behaviors, which he called

operants, and the ways in which they change as a result of the consequences they produce, a

process he called operant conditioning. He referred to the relationship between an operant

and a consequence as a contingency, meaning that the consequence is contingent upon the

exhibition of the behavior. Operants typically occur randomly with no plan or purpose behind

them. However, Skinner argued that after operants are changed by the actions of

consequences upon them, operants become learned behaviors that organisms use to

manipulate their environments. In other words, organisms use operantly conditioned

behaviors to produce desirable consequences and avoid undesirable ones. For this reason,

operant conditioning is often characterized as instrumental learning, meaning that the

behaviors acquired through operant conditioning become purposeful once they are learned.

Reinforcers

The effects of consequences on operants, not their characteristics, determine how we classify

them. For instance, you often hear people refer to pleasant consequences as reinforcers

regardless of their influence on behavior. Technically, though, a reinforcer is any

consequence, pleasant or otherwise, that increases the frequency of the behavior it follows.

Thus, material rewards such as stickers are not reinforcers unless they increase the frequency

of the behavior on which they are contingent.

Positive and Negative Reinforcers. Skinner described two effects that operants have on the

environments in which they occur. He noted that operants can either cause a new stimulus to

appear in the environment, or they can cause an already present stimulus to disappear. He

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 32

referred to these effects as positive and negative effects on consequences. Many people

equate positive with good and negative with bad. However, in operant conditioning, the

mathematical connotations of these terms are the relevant ones. Thus, a positive reinforcer

is a consequence that involves the addition of a stimulus that increases behavior, while a

negative reinforcer is a consequence that involves the subtraction of a stimulus that

increases behavior.

Primary and Secondary Reinforcers. Before leaving the discussion of reinforcers, we should

also consider the distinction between primary or secondary reinforcers. Primary reinforcers

satisfy biological needs. Note that they can be used either as positive or negative reinforcers.

For example, Brussels sprouts satisfy the biological need for food, but many people find their

taste to be aversive. Imagine a parent saying, Because you finished all of your green beans,

you dont have to eat your Brussels sprouts. If the behavior of green-bean-eating increases,

then the avoidance of Brussels sprouts has served as a negative reinforcer. By contrast,

imagine a parent saying, Because your finished all of your green beans, Im going to give you

some ice cream. If the target behavior increases, the ice cream has served as a positive

reinforcer.

Secondary reinforcers are learned through association with primary reinforcers (e.g.,

money can buy food) or because they have social value. Again, these can function as either

positive or negative reinforcers. For instance, winning $100 positively reinforces the behavior

of buying lottery tickets in a winner who buys tickets more frequently afterward. The money

is a positive reinforcer because it was added and it increased behavior. By contrast, paying a

$100 late fee negatively reinforces the behavior of making mortgage payments on time in a

late-payer who pays on time more frequently afterward. The late fee is a negative reinforcer

because its removal led to an increase in behavior. By exhibiting the behavior of paying on

time, the person avoids the aversive outcome of having to pay late fees in the future.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 33

The Process of Reinforcement

As noted earlier, a reinforcer is a consequence that increases behavior. Typically, we use the

term reinforcement to refer to the entire series of events that leads to an increase in

behavior in response to a consequence. Positive reinforcement is a sequence of events in

which an added consequence, usually a pleasant one, increases the frequency of an operant.

Negative reinforcement is a sequence of events in which a subtracted consequence, typically

an aversive one, increases the frequency of an operant. To put it a bit differently, three

things happen in the process of reinforcement: (1) an operant (non-reflexive behavior)

happens; (2) a consequence is either added or subtracted (the positive or negative part,

respectively) by an agent in the environment or as a natural consequence of the operants

impact on the environment; and (3) the frequency of the operant increases (the

reinforcement part), decreases, or remains unchanged. Lets consider a few examples.

Positive reinforcement: Johnnys room is a mess. Johnny cleans his room.

Mom gives Johnny a Snickers bar. Johnny cleans his room more frequently

thereafter. (agent adds a consequence)

Negative reinforcement: Johnnys room is a mess. Mom yells at Johnny.

Johnny cleans his room. Mom stops yelling at him. Johnny cleans his room more

frequently thereafter. (agent removes a consequence)

Positive reinforcement: Johnnys room is a mess. Johnny cleans his room.

Johnny enjoys the feeling of accomplishment he experiences as a result.

Johnny cleans his room more frequently thereafter. (natural consequence

added)

Negative reinforcement: Johnnys room is a mess. Johnny notices an

unpleasant order indicating the presence of spoiled food hidden somewhere in

the mess. Johnny cleans his room in order to locate the source of the odor. He

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 34

finds a two-week old sandwich under his bed and throws it away. He notices

that the unpleasant odor has ceased. Johnny cleans his room more frequently

in the future. (natural consequence removed)

Lets break down these examples in terms of the three things that happen in the

reinforcement process. First, the operant is the same in all four examples: Johnny cleans his

room. Second, consider the variations in the consequences across the four examples. In the

first example, the consequence, the Snickers bar, is added by Johnnys mom, an agent. In the

second example, the consequence, Moms yelling, is removed by an agent, Mom herself. In

the third, the consequence, Johnnys sense of accomplishment, is added but not by an agent.

It is the natural consequence of Johnnys having worked to accomplish a goal. In the fourth,

the consequence, discontinuation of the spoiled food odor, is removed as a natural

consequence of Johnnys having cleaned his room and found the old sandwich. Finally, notice

that the outcome is the same in all four examples: Johnny cleans his room more frequently in

the future. However, in the first and third examples, Johnny cleans his room more frequently

in order to get something pleasant. In the second and fourth, he cleans his room to avoid

something unpleasant.

You may have noticed that something else is the same across the four examples. The

state of Johnnys room is the context in which the reinforcement process occurs in all of

them. In operant conditioning terms, Johnnys messy room is an antecedent condition, a

feature of the environment that is present before the operant occurs. According to Skinner,

antecedent conditions trigger learned patterns of behavior based on a learners expectations

as to what will happen when a specific behavior is exhibited. As a result, antecedent

conditions become discriminative stimuli, signals that tell learners what kinds of

consequences are likely to ensue if they exhibit specific behaviors. In the first example,

whenever Johnnys room is messy, he will clean it in anticipation of getting a Snickers bar or

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 35

another treat. In the third, he will clean his room in anticipation of feeling good about the

fruits of his labor. But notice that the antecedent conditions in the second and fourth

examples are a bit more complex. In the second, the room is messy and Mom is yelling. As a

result, Johnnys learned pattern of room cleaning behavior may depend on the presence of

both a messy room and a yelling mother. In the fourth example, the room is messy and there

is an odor of spoiled food present. Thus, Johnnys learned pattern of room cleaning behavior

may depend on the presence of both a messy room and an unpleasant odor.

Operant Conditioning Outcomes

The tendency of human beings to apply previously learned operant-reinforcer connections to

new situations extends the influence of operant learning beyond the confines of a single

conditioning episode. For instance, students tend to generalize the reinforcers associated

with one assistant principal to all of them. Generalization is the process of responding to an

entire category of reinforcers rather than to a single reinforcer, or source of reinforcement,

within the category. Thanks to generalization, students dont have to relearn how to behave

in the presence of an assistant principal every time they encounter a new one. Nevertheless,

students also learn the subtle differences in behavior across the people they know, including

assistant principals. As a result, they adapt their behavior accordingly, or discriminate, when

they are in the presence of an administrator whom they know. Discrimination is the tendency

to respond differently to individual reinforcers, or sources of reinforcement, within a

category.

The same principles of generalization and discrimination apply to students

interactions with teachers. In addition, through generalization, some students learn to

respond to grades as a category of reinforcers. These students regard all grades as important,

put forth effort to obtain good grades and avoid bad ones, and are conscientious about grade-

related details. By contrast, some students exhibit discrimination when they respond

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 36

effortfully to some grades and carelessly to others based on the characteristics of the grades

themselves. For example, discriminating students respond in an effortful way only to

assignments that represent major grades.

Extinction happens when behaviors that are contingent on reinforcers disappear when

those reinforcers are no longer available. For example, students may read large numbers of

books when teachers offer tokens or other types of rewards. As soon as the reward scheme is

removed, however, their reading rates drop.

Because of the tendency of operantly conditioned behaviors to become extinct, some

psychologists argue that operant conditioning is not true learning. (Think back to the

definition of learning as a permanent change.) This assertion is supported by another

phenomenon known as instinctive drift, or the tendency of an organism to revert to

unlearned behavior. Still, both animals and humans sometimes show spontaneous recovery,

the sudden return of a long-absent conditioned behavior.

Schedules of Reinforcement

Contingencies between reinforces and operants are often described in terms of schedules of

reinforcement. Some schedules link behaviors directly to reinforcers, ratio schedules, while

others depend on the passage of time, interval schedules. Fixed ratio schedules and fixed

interval schedules apply reinforcers in the same way at all times. For instance, a fixed ratio

schedule might reinforce every other behavior the organisms exhibits, while a fixed interval

schedule might administer reinforcement every five minutes. Variable ratio schedules apply

reinforcers inconsistently. The goal of variable schedules is to prevent organisms from

predicting when they will receive a reinforcer.

Variations in schedules are strongly linked to variations in acquisition and extinction

rates. For instance, a continuous schedule of reinforcement, a special type of fixed ratio

schedule in which every behavior is reinforced, usually results in a rapid acquisition rate.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 37

However, it is also linked to a rapid rate of extinction. By contrast, variable ratio schedules

produce what is known as the partial reinforcement effect, the tendency of behaviors to be

learned slowly but to be extremely resistant to extinction. For example, when teachers grade

homework randomly, students typically display this effect. They do not know which

assignments will be graded, so they increase the overall amount of homework they do.

Fixed interval schedules produce the scalloping effect, or the tendency to exhibit high

rates of behavior just before reinforcers are administered. For instance, before direct

deposit, nobody was absent from work on payday. Alternatively, variable interval schedules

can induce superstitious behavior, because the organism does not know which behaviors are

linked to the reinforcers. Thus, the organism tends to repeat all of the behaviors that

occurred in the interval between reinforcers. For this reason, variable interval schedules are

useful for training complex sets of behaviors such as appropriate classroom behavior. This is

so because such schedules reinforce learners for exhibiting a number of specific behaviors

that collectively constitute a system of behavior that is appropriate to a specific situation.

Punishment

A punisher is any consequence that reduces the probability of an operant. So, it is the

opposite of a reinforcer. Remember, we dont define consequences in terms of their

characteristics, but with regard to their effects on behavior. Thus, a pleasant or desirable

consequence can be a punishment if it causes a reduction in behavior. Like positive

reinforcement, positive punishment involves an added consequence, usually one that is

aversive, but unlike positive reinforcement, it involves a decrease in the frequency of a

behavior. Think about a situation in which a cat owner wants to teach his pet to stop clawing

the furniture. Following a friends suggestion, the cat owner zaps the cat with a squirt gun

every time he starts to claw. As a result, the cat stops clawing the furniture, and we can

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 38

conclude that positive punishment has occurred. The behavior, furniture clawing, decreased

because of an added stimulus, the water gun zap.

Similarly, negative punishment involves a subtracted consequence, usually one that is

pleasant, followed by a decrease in the frequency of a behavior. For instance, grounding or

taking privileges away from children is a form of negative punishment if it results in a

decrease in the behavior that caused the consequence. Likewise, time out is a negative

punishment because it removes the child from a setting in which reinforcers are available.

Table 2.1 summarizes the definitions of positive and negative reinforcement and positive and

negative punishment.

Table 2.1
Reinforcement and Punishment

Positive Negative

Positive Reinforcement Negative Reinforcement


Reinforcement
Increased behavior Increased behavior
Added consequence Removed consequence

Positive Punishment Negative Punishment


Punishment
Decreased behavior Decreased behavior
Added consequence Removed consequence

Some people confuse positive punishment with negative reinforcement because both

usually involve aversive stimuli. Remember, punishment causes a decrease in behavior, while

reinforcement causes an increase in behavior. But here is what makes negative

reinforcement and positive punishment more complex than they might appear to be. A single

consequence can be a positive punisher and a negative reinforcer at the same time. Consider

this example. A child runs out the front door of his home, putting himself in danger. As a

result, the parent yells at him. The child then stops and returns to the safety of the house.

The parents yelling punished the behavior of running outside but also reinforced the

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 39

behavior of returning to the house. How? The yelling was added to the behavior of running out

(positive punishment) but it was removed when the child returned to the house (negative

reinforcement). Similarly, you read earlier that late fees negatively reinforce the behavior of

paying on time. They also positively punish the behavior of paying late. Timely payments

increase in frequency because they enable the payer to avoid late fees (negative

reinforcement). Late payments decrease in frequency because they elicit late fees (positive

punishment).

Escape and Avoidance Learning

Aversive stimuli are associated with escape learning and avoidance learning. Escape learning

involves, quite literally, escaping from an aversive stimulus that is already present. For

example, when you were a teenager, you probably learned to look remorseful whenever your

parents lectured you. Sad facial expressions, you learned, caused a lecture to be shorter than

it would be if you exhibited defensive behaviors such as talking back. Avoidance learning

involves exhibiting a behavior in order to avoid the administration of an aversive. Many

children learn to read their parents and teachers nonverbal cues, such as tone of voice, to

know when they have reached the point when some kind of aversive is about to happen (often

referred to colloquially as pushing buttons). When that point is reached, the child exhibits

a behavior she knows the parent or teacher wants her to, thus avoiding the aversive.

Escape and avoidance learning are adaptive in nature. However, another kind of

learning associated with aversives, learned helplessness, is not. Learned helplessness

develops when an organism has been exposed to aversives from which escape or avoidance

are impossible. The classic learned helplessness experiment involved dogs who were strapped

down and shocked. As a result, they allowed themselves to endure electric shocks even when

they werent strapped down. Some observers describe the passive behavior of many of

todays students as the result of learned helpless. They have failed so many times, and

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 40

believe their failure to be outside their own control, that they have given up trying. Those

who are resilient often turn to other sources of reinforcement. In some cases, these sources

of reinforcement only make matters worse (gangs, drugs, low-achieving peer groups),

although they may make the students feel better about their situation. Other students exhibit

learned helplessness by becoming depressed and withdrawn.

EVALUATION OF CONDITIONING THEORIES

Recall from Chapter 1 that learning theorists use four criteria to evaluate the usefulness of a

theory. As you read in Chapter 1, all of the theories discussed in this text have demonstrated

their heuristic value. Consequently, evaluation of each theory focuses on the remaining three

questions that are relevant to a learning theorys usefulness:

Does it explain the basic facts of learning?

Does it generate testable predictions?

Does it lead to practical strategies that are relevant to real-world learning and

instruction?

Thus, the first question we need to ask about conditioning theories concerns the degree to

which they provide comprehensive explanations of real-world behaviors. Despite claims by

conditioning theorists to the contrary, most learning theorists believe that it is difficult, if not

impossible, to reduce every behavior to an inborn stimulus-response connection as would be

required if classical conditioning is to satisfactorily explain real-world learning. Likewise,

contrary to the predictions of operant conditioning theory, organisms often exhibit behavior

that has not been reinforced. Similarly, in the case of human learning, it is quite common for

people to display behaviors to which aversive stimuli have been repeatedly applied by

environmental agents. These shortcomings of the conditioning theories lead most learning

theorists to conclude that, while both classical and operant theory explain important aspects

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 41

of learning, something more is required if we are to truly understand behavior change in the

real world.

Second, we must consider whether conditioning theories generate testable

hypotheses. The answer to this question is a clear yes. There is no doubt that both classical

and operant conditioning are capable of generating a wide variety of hypotheses. Moreover,

most hypotheses derive from the conditioning theories lend themselves to the experimental

method and, especially, to laboratory experimentation in which researchers can control all

relevant variables.

Finally, classical and operant conditioning theories provide us with a number of

techniques, such as systematic desensitization and behavior modification, that are useful for

changing behavior and enhancing peoples lives.

Systematic Desensitization

Systematic desensitization is a therapeutic technique that is used to reduce or eliminate

both natural and learned anxieties and fears through increasing levels of exposure to feared

stimuli (Wolpe, 1958, 1973). Such fears include workplace issues, such as zoo workers need

to gain control over the natural anxieties that are evoked by working with dangerous animals.

In addition, systematic desensitization helps individuals overcome life-limiting anxieties such

as fear of exposure to germs, fear of leaving home, and the fear of food that is experienced

by many individuals with eating disorders.

The first step that a therapist using systematic desensitization takes with a client is to

identify the stimuli that trigger the clients fearful response. For instance, consider a client

who is so afraid of dogs that he wont go outside his house. It might seem that the trigger

stimulus is dogs, and that the process of systematic desensitization would involve exposing

the client to dogs. However, in this clients case, the trigger that is preventing him from

leaving home isnt actually a dog. It is the thought of encountering a dog. Consequently, the

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 42

first step in the therapists plan would be to help the client become comfortable with

thinking about encounters with dogs. Ultimately, the plan would involve exposing the client

to dogs, probably by gradually increasing amounts of exposure time and decreasing distances

from dogs, but there would be a number of intermediate steps between the thinking step and

the dog-exposure steps. The intermediate steps might include having the client watch videos

of dogs, for instance.

A complex fear and the sub-fears that contribute to it are usually collectively referred

to as a hierarchy of fears. A successful systematic desensitization plan typically identifies

and addresses all of the steps in the hierarchy. Everyday fears of this type for which

systematic desensitization can be effective include test anxiety, injection anxiety, and fear of

flying.

Behavior Modification

Behavior modification is an agents systematic application of the principles of operant

conditioning to the task of changing an organisms behavior. It is especially helpful for

enabling individuals with autism, mental retardation, and psychological disorders to adapt to

their environments. Moreover, educators frequently use it to change the behavior of students

who do not have such disabilities. Businesses, too, employ the principles of operant

conditioning to address problems such as absenteeism. Health professionals also find that

behavior modification is useful in working with patients who need to stop smoking, lose

weight, exercise more, or make other health-related behavior changes. In addition, the

change agent in the behavior modification equation can be oneself, that is, individuals can

use behavior modification to change their own behavior.

Professionals who specialize in using behavior modification to alter individuals

problematic behaviors are known as applied behavior analysts in educational settings and

behavioral therapists in clinical settings. The first step in a behavior modification plan is to

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 43

identify the behavior that needs changing, or target behavior. The next step is to determine

the frequency of the target behavior through careful observation. Next, analysts or therapists

use their knowledge of schedules of reinforcement to devise a plan for using reinforcement

(and sometimes punishment) to change the behavior. The implementation phase comes next

when the plan is actually put into action. In the evaluation phase, the analyst or therapist

determines whether the plan has been successful.

When applied behavior analysts and behavior therapists need to induce a person to

learn behaviors that she is not exhibiting at all, they often use of the principles of shaping,

the process in which an agent breaks down a complex target behavior into intermediate steps

to which reinforcers are applied. For instance, suppose an applied behavior analyst is takes on

the challenge of helping a kindergartener who never sits down to remain seated and engaged

in class work. She would begin by reinforcing him for staying near his seat. Next, she would

reinforce him for actually sitting down, no matter how briefly. Next, she would gradually

extend the amount of time between the administration of the reinforcer and the time the

child first sat down. The next step would probably be to reinforce any evidence of

engagement with class work while he is seated, perhaps behaviors as simple as picking up a

pencil or looking at a book or worksheet. In this way, the analyst would help the child build a

repertoire of small behaviors that, together, constitute the complex behavior of engaging in

class work while seated.

Animal trainers also use shaping techniques. Moreover, because of their familiarity

with the principles of extinction and instinctive drift, experienced trainers know that there

really is no such thing as a trained wild animal that wont eventually revert to its natural

behavior. Consequently, most behavior modification regimes for animals include a

maintenance reinforcement plan that is designed to reduce the chances of extinction and

instinctive drift. Behavior analysts and therapists who work with humans typically do likewise,

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 44

especially those who work with clients who have autism, mental retardation, and/or serious

psychological disorders.

SUMMARY

In this chapter you have learned that the conditioning theories have given teachers,

therapists, and others a number of helpful techniques for changing behavior. Consequently,

psychologists view the conditioning theories as meeting the practical application criterion for

useful theories. Likewise, the conditioning theories are quite amenable to experimentation.

Thus, within the limited sphere of reflexive behavior and non-reflexive behavior that is

changed as a result of consequences, the conditioning theories do a good job of explaining

learning. However, as we will see in future chapters, we must look beyond the conditioning

theories if we want to construct a truly comprehensive account of learning.

KEY TERMS, CONCEPTS, AND PEOPLE

classical conditioning
Ivan Pavlov
conditioned reflexes
reflex
natural stimulus
neutral stimulus
unconditioned stimulus
unconditioned response
conditioned stimulus
conditioned response
higher-order conditioning
extinction (in classical conditioning)
spontaneous recovery (in classical conditioning)
discrimination (in classical conditioning)
generalization (in classical conditioning)
biological predispositions
behaviorism
John Watson
Rosalie Raynor
Little Albert
E. L. Thorndike
trial and accidental success
law of exercise
law of effect
operants
operant conditioning
instrumental learning
reinforcer

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 45

primary reinforcer
secondary reinforce
positive reinforcer
negative reinforcer
reinforcement
positive reinforcement
negative reinforcement
antecedent condition
discriminative stimuli
schedules of reinforcement
ratio schedules
interval schedules
fixed ratio schedules
fixed interval schedules
continuous schedule
partial reinforcement effect
scalloping effect
superstitious behavior
generalization (in operant conditioning)
discrimination (in operant conditioning)
extinction (in operant conditioning)
instinctive drift
spontaneous recovery (in operant conditioning)
punisher
positive punishment
negative punishment
escape learning
avoidance learning
learned helplessness
systematic desensitization
hierarchy of fears
behavior modification
target behavior
implementation phase
shaping
maintenance reinforcement

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 46

INFORMATION PROCESSING

THEORY

THE ATKINSON-SHIFFRIN MODEL OF MEMORY

REMEMBERING

FORGETTING

EVALUATION OF INFORMATION PROCESSING THEORY

SUMMARY

KEY TERMS, CONCEPTS, AND PEOPLE

In Chapter 1 you learned that information processing (IP) theory is consistent with

rationalist epistemology because it posits that an internal characteristic, the IP system,

constrains how input from the environment is interpreted and learned. For instance, the IP

system is limited in how much information it can take in at one time. Therefore, when

information exceeds that amount, some is lost. Furthermore, IP theorists assume that the

mind processes information in ways that are similar to those used by computers. Thus, these

theorists often talk about the roles of hardware (e.g., neural structures) and software (e.g.,

learning strategies) in memory and other cognitive processes. In addition, like all cognitive

theories, IP theory emphasizes the interactive effects of nature and nurture rather than

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 47

attributing learning to one or the other. Moreover, it asserts that learning involves both

quantitative and qualitative change. While, in general, IP theorists do not propose that

learning occurs in stages, they recognize that there are some learning outcomes in which

change takes place in a stage-like fashion.

THE ATKINSON-SHIFFRIN MODEL OF MEMORY

Information processing theory is not a single grand theory that explains all learning.

Instead, it is a general perspective that informs micro-theories, theories that explain specific

learning outcomes. Thus, an IP micro-theory that explains the acquisition of decoding skills

might be quite different from one that explains the development of reading comprehension.

What the two micro-theories would have in common is that each would be framed in terms of

a general model of cognitive functioning that has its roots in a three-store model of human

memory proposed by Richard Atkinson and Richard Shiffrin (1968) that has informed IP

research for the past several decades (see Figure 3.1).

Figure 3.1
The Atkinson-Shiffrin Model of Memory

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 48

Sensory Memory

As you can see in Figure 3.1, incoming information enters the system through the sensory

memory, a temporary store that briefly holds visual, auditory, and other sensory information.

Sensory memory holds bits of visual information, icons, for only to of a second. By

contrast, bits of auditory information, echoes, are held for 2 seconds or more. Thus, the

human IP system is somewhat biased in favor of auditory information. This lack of balance

may be due to the fact that language is such an important means of communication for

humans.

The capacity of sensory memory is quite large. However, most information is lost

unless it is selected for further processing. The information selection process is more

commonly known as attention. It is influenced by a number of variables. First, characteristics

of information such as its novelty, familiarity, intensity, and personal relevance increase the

chances that the IP system will attend to it. Second, because auditory information is stored

longer than visual information, it is more likely to be attended to. Information that is

attended to moves on from sensory memory to the next component of the IP system.

Short-Term and Working Memory

The central, organizing feature of the Atkinson-Shiffrin model of memory is the short-term

memory (STM). On average, the STM can hold 7 bits of information, with typical adults

varying in STM capacity from 5 to 9 bits. Moreover, the maximum storage time for information

in STM is about 30 seconds.

The IP system can overcome the limited capacity of STM through automaticity, the

tendency of frequently used or well-rehearsed information to become accessible to STM

without taking up processing space. We experience automaticity in everyday life when we

find ourselves thinking about something other than what we are doing when we are

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 49

performing a routine task such as driving to work or doing household chores. Our IP systems

automatically access the information we need to perform the tasks, and, as a result, STM

space is freed up for processing other information.

Chunking, reducing the amount of information to be remembered by grouping

individual items into blocks of information, is another means of expanding STM capacity. For

example, if we attempted to remember a telephone number by memorizing ten individual

digits, we would exceed the capacity of short-term memory. Instead, we group the individual

digits in phone numbers into three chunks, XXX-XXX-XXXX. As a result, phone numbers are

processed as three bits of information rather than ten.

In addition, STM processing time can be extended indefinitely through the application

of memory strategies, cognitive techniques that enhance the memorability of information

(see Table 3.1). For instance, simply mentally repeating information stored in STM increases

the amount of time it stays in the system, and linking new information to previously acquired

knowledge increases the chances that the new information will move on to the IP systems

permanent storage system.

Table 3.1
Memory Strategies

Strategy Definition and Everyday Memory Example

Repeating information until it is no longer needed or until it can be easily retrieved from long-term
memory
Rehearsal
Example: Repeating the list of items you need at the grocery store until you have memorized it

Sorting information into categories for storage in long-term memory


Organization
Example: Arranging a grocery list according to categories such as meats, produce, dairy, and so on

Relating new and previously learned information in meaningful ways


Elaboration
Example: Using recipes stored in long-term memory to remember the items on a grocery list

Creating non-meaningful links between new and previously learned information


Mnemonics
Example: Using the acronym SALT to remember a grocery list that includes sausage, asparagus,
linguine, and tomatoes.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 50

The Atkinson-Shiffrin model implies that the effectiveness of the entire IP system

depends on the efficiency of the STM. For this reason, IP theorists often refer to the

processing demands of cognitive tasks to explain differences in individuals performance on

them. For instance, think back to when you took the GRE. At least some of your STM capacity

was probably devoted to coping with anxiety. At times, you may have felt that there was a

wall between your thoughts and the knowledge you needed to answer the test questions, but

you were confident that the information was stored in your long-term memory. In these

instances, the anxiety was taking up so much STM space, that there was none left over for

retrieving information from long-term memory. Likewise, algebra students who have not

automatized basic math facts and the relationships among them (i.e., 6 x 4 = 24 is

meaningfully related to 24/6 = 4) have difficulty with complex problems. This is because their

STM capacity is consumed by simple arithmetic calculations. Moreover, they cannot apply

what they know about number relationships to variables (i.e., a x b = c is meaningfully

related to c/a = b). Importantly, too, providing such students with calculators is not helpful,

because calculators increase rather than decrease processing demands. That is, all of the

behaviors, physical and cognitive, involved in operating the calculator add to the processing

demands of the task. Similarly, readers who cannot automatically decode written words into

spoken words have little or no STM capacity available to comprehend what they read. Thus,

instructing such students in the use of comprehension strategies is not likely to lead to

improvements in reading proficiency. The students may learn the strategies, but their lack of

automatic decoding skills will prevent them from applying them, again, because of the

limited space available in STM. In fact, comprehension strategies add to the processing

demands of reading tasks in much the same way that calculators compound the demands

associated with solving algebra problems.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 51

Some IP theorists, most notably British psychologist Allan Baddeley, argue that STM is

actually a multi-component system that is more accurately described by the term working

memory (Baddeley, 2009). Literally speaking, working memory is the sub-system of the IP

system where information is worked on to increase its comprehensibility, memorability,

and applicability to intellectual and practical problems. Consequently, IP theorists often use

STM to refer to the baseline capacity of the IP systems temporary information store and

working memory or WM to refer to the functions and processes associated with STM.

Baddeley (2009) proposes that WM has four components. The central executive is the

WM systems master component that coordinates the functions of its three sub-systems.

The first of these sub-systems is the phonological loop, the sub-system of WM that verbally

codes information. The second is the visuospatial sketch pad in which information is

represented visually. The third is the episodic buffer whose task is to impose order on

information and to link verbal and visual codes to create multi-dimensional representations of

it. For instance, when you are reading a novel, the episodic buffer keeps a running chronology

of the storys events and links the images your mind forms of characters, events, and settings

to the words of the novel itself and to words that your WM retrieves from long-term memory

to interpret the story. As you might guess, the working memory cannot function

independently of the systems permanent store of information. In fact, the more relevant

knowledge the IP system has on hand for processing new information, the more efficiently the

WM functions.

Long-Term Memory

Long-term memory (LTM) is where the IP system permanently stores information. It is

constantly reorganizing information and compressing it to make room for more. To accomplish

these tasks, it tends to organize information into categories that progress from broad to

narrow.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 52

Explicit and Implicit Memory. The two broadest categories of information in LTM are explicit

memory and implicit memory. The easy way to remember what explicit memories are is to

note that they are declarative (from the root word to declare), that is, you can describe them

quite well and communicate them precisely to others using only words. By contrast, implicit

memories are nondeclarative, meaning that they cannot be effectively communicated in

words. For example, think about answering these two questions:

What is the capital of Texas?

How do you ride a bicycle?

Could you answer both questions equally well using only words? Answering the first question

requires nothing more than verbal knowledge. By contrast, words alone cannot answer the

second effectively. They must be accompanied by a physical demonstration. Consequently, IP

theorists would say that the first question taps explicit memory, while the second draws on

implicit memory.

Episodic and Semantic Memory. There are two subsystems in explicit memory, episodic and

semantic memory. Episodic memory includes memories of events, while semantic memory

contains general information. These two subsystems do not function independently. We need

semantic memory to help us interpret events. Likewise, some events provide us with

information that expands semantic memory. For example, suppose you went on a vacation to

Hawaii. You would store your vacation memories in episodic memory in verbal codes that tap

into the vocabulary stored in your semantic memory. These codes would enable you to

describe your vacation to others. At the same time, it is likely that you would pick up bits of

information on your trip, such as the fact that Oahu is the third largest of the Hawaiian

Islands, that you would store in semantic memory. You would be able to retrieve your

knowledge of the relative sizes of the islands without retrieving memories of your vacation.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 53

Interestingly, semantic memory usually does not code information in terms of its

associated episodic context. That is, typically, we do not remember anything about the time,

place, or context in which we acquired a given bit of semantic information unless there is

something out of the ordinary about them (such as learning them in the context of a Hawaiian

vacation). Recollection of the context in which we acquire memories are called source

memories. The tendency to fail to store source memories strongly influences the accuracy of

eyewitness testimony. Witnesses frequently confuse having actually seen something with

having been told by others what they saw. In addition, lack of source memories renders

eyewitness accounts vulnerable to the misinformation effect, a phenomenon in which an

investigators questions contain information that a witness unknowingly incorporates into her

memory of the event.

When semantic memories are strongly tied to episodic memories that were formed in

the context of a striking emotional experience, they are often referred to as flashbulb

memories. For example, your episodic memory probably includes a recollection of how you

learned of the terrorist attacks of September 11, 2001. The context in which you learned of

the attacks, your source memory, is stored in episodic memory. Because of the emotions

surrounding the event, the IP system maintained a link between your episodic and semantic

memories of the attacks. Over time, however, as you learned more about the attacks and

failed to store source memories for the additional information you have acquired, the link

between your initial learning of the attacks and the facts you learned at that time has

weakened. That is, due to lack of source memories, your IP system has incorporated later-

learned information into your flashbulb memory. As a result, at this point, you probably have

difficulty remembering which facts you learned from the original source and which you

learned later.

REMEMBERING

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 54

The Atkinson-Shiffrin model provides a good descriptive overview of the structures of the IP

system. The process of remembering involves the functions of these structures and how they

interact when we attempt to bring information to mind.

Remembering as Retrieval

Retrieval is the process of calling up information that has been stored in LTM. There are

various types of retrieval, some of which are more difficult than others. Recognition involves

matching new stimuli to those that are stored in LTM. It requires nothing more than the

realization that a given stimulus has or has not been encountered in the past. Recognition is

the remembering process that requires the least amount of mental effort.

In contrast, recall, requires a person to search her memory and retrieve a specific

piece of information. Clearly, recall is far more difficult than recognition. No doubt you have

had personal experience with a memory task that involves both recognition and recall. Have

you ever run into a person whom you know you have met (recognition) but whose name you

cannot remember (recall)?

Recall is easier when retrieval cues are present. For instance, in the I-know-I-should-

know-this-persons-name situation above, the person might trigger recall of her name by

mentioning the context in which you met her. Alternatively, she might jump-start your

memory by mentioning a person whom both of you know.

The process of relearning, in which you reprocess previously learned information, also

involves retrieval cues. However, in this case, the information itself serves as its own

retrieval cue. For example, suppose you are reading Chapter 2 for a second time because you

cant quite recall all of the details regarding classical conditioning. Often, just reading the

first sentence or two of a paragraph will cause you to recall its contents more effectively than

you could prior to rereading.

Remembering as Reconstruction

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 55

In most cases, effectively remembering information requires retrieval as well as

reconstruction, the building up of complete, but not always accurate, memories from

fragmentary recollections. Reconstruction typically results from an integration of new and

previously learned information. Two LTM information structures, schemas and networks,

strongly influence the process of reconstruction.

Schemas. In the course of his studies of cultural influences on memory, early memory

researcher Sir Frederick Bartlett suggested that information in long-term memory is organized

into schemas, generalizable aspects of specific experiences and content domains that are

stored in semantic memory. The IP system deploys schemas to comprehend and filter new

information and to reorganize old information. For instance, our fast food restaurant

schema includes paper-wrapped food and plastic utensils, while tablecloths and china are

part of our fancy table service restaurant schema. Cues such as the presence or absence of

a drive-through help us determine which schema is most applicable to a specific restaurant

regardless of whether we have ever eaten there. Consequently, we can use these two

schemas to make a decision about whether a given restaurant matches our desire for a

specific type of food, fits with whatever time constraints we are dealing with, and the

amount of money we want to spend.

The construction of schemas happens automatically as we acquire and use

information. Schemas are vital to comprehension and memory because both are

reconstructive processes. That is, memory does not function like a tape recorder. Instead, it

functions like an interpreter who summarizes some of a speakers statements in order to

expedite communication between the speaker and the audience for whom she is interpreting.

This strategy is efficient, but it may also distort the speakers intended message. Similarly, if

you witness a traffic accident that involves a stop sign, you create a mental summary of the

event using both what you actually saw and the general knowledge stored in your stop-sign

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 56

schema (i.e., cars are supposed to stop at them; there are rules about who goes first at a 4-

way stop, etc.). As a result, your mental summary of the event may not faithfully represent

what you actually saw.

Schemas also play a crucial role in the development and deployment of domain-

specific knowledge. For instance, what a person sees when he watches a football game

depends on how much he knows about the game. People who know little about football see

two groups of eleven players lined up opposite one another. By contrast, football coaches,

players, and others with a great deal of knowledge about the game mentally separate the

eleven players on each side into smaller groups of players. The ways in which these smaller

groups are arranged and the directions in which they move when the ball is snapped provide

clues that only those with large stores of relevant information can decipher about the plays

that offenses run and the strategies that defenses use to stop them. Thus, an important

principle derived from information processing research is that quantitative changes in

knowledge generate qualitative changes in the ways in which knowledge is processed. That is,

the more specific knowledge you have about a topic, the better able you are to think about it

in meaningful ways. Thus, meaningful thinking is not a content-free skill. Instead, it is the

natural by-product of accumulated information.

Although most schemas are experience-based, research suggests that a small number

of them may actually be inborn. For instance, infants seem to be born with a schema for what

a face is supposed to look like. They also appear to be born with schemas for categorizing

speech sounds. The story schemathe expectation that all stories include characters, setting,

plot, and a conclusionmay also be inborn.

Networks. Networks are similar to schemas but are semantic in nature. For instance, table

and chair are embedded in a network because they are semantically related. As a result,

when you process information about tables, you also think of chairs, usually without being

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 57

aware of it. Research that involves asking people to recall whether specific words occurred in

sentences they have heard or read illustrates the ways in which networks influence memory.

In these studies, researchers expose participants to sentences such as the girl sat down at

the table and ask whether the word chair occurred in the sentence. About half of

participants will say yes. Such findings suggest that participants are actually inferring (a form

of reconstruction) rather than remembering and that their inferences are based on

expectations that are generated by networks of meaning stored in their LTMs. The inferring

process goes something like this:

Chairs belong with tables. Sitting down at a table usually requires a chair.

Therefore, it is reasonable to infer that the word chair was in the sentence. On

the other hand, the table might be a coffee table, in which case no chair

would be required. In addition, it is acceptable to say sat down at the table

without explicitly using the word chair, so its reasonable to infer that the

word chair was not in the sentence.

Of course, all of this thinking takes place in a fraction of a second, and such results

show that the IP system stores the gist, or meaning, of the language we hear and read

rather than the specific words it contains. Nevertheless, attempting to distinguish

between knowing facts and thinking about facts results in a false dichotomy.

Information such as what a chair is, what a table is, and how the two are related must

be stored in LTM for a person to be able to think about them in a meaningful way and

to create a memory of the gist of a sentence about a person sitting down at a table.

FORGETTING

As you have seen, some of the features of the IP system can lead to distortions that affect the

process of remembering. Similar, but distinctive, processes are at work in the process of

forgetting, the inability to retrieve information from memory that was previously accessible.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 58

Context Effects

You learned earlier that the source memories are usually not stored along with semantic

memories. There are some circumstances, however, in which the link between a memory and

the context in which it was acquired are so strong that the information cannot be recalled

unless the original context is reproduced. Such context effects were discovered by early 20th

century psychologists, but they were best documented in a classic study by Duncan Godden

and Allan Baddeley (1975).

Participants in the Godden and Baddeley study memorized words while submerged in

ten feet of water. When asked to recall the words on land, they performed poorly. However,

when asked to recall the words under water, they performed much better. Godden and

Baddeley also had the participants memorize words on land and try to recall them

underwater. Not surprisingly, their findings mirrored those of the underwater studies. That is,

the participants recalled words learned on land best when they were on land and worse when

they were underwater.

Physiological states also exert context effects on memory. For example, research

shows that participants who acquire information while under the influence of alcohol do not

recall the information accurately when sober (Goodwin et al., 1969). However, when they

consume alcohol, they are capable of remembering the information much more effectively.

Research on context effects suggests a possible explanation for episodes of forgetting.

When a person cannot remember something, it may be that the information was encoded

along with some feature of the context in which it was learned. Indeed, most people have

had the experience of forgetting something and remembering it upon returning to the

location in which they last thought of it. Similarly, test-takers may remember answers to

exam questions by visualizing the circumstances in which they studied. Moreover, if the test-

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 59

takers levels of anxiety during studying and testing are about the same, then so-called test

anxiety works to the test-takers advantage.

The Curve of Forgetting

Historians of psychology credit Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850-1909) with carrying out the first

systematic studies of remembering and forgetting. Ebbinghaus reasoned that meaning affects

our ability to remember stimuli and that, in order to arrive at a pure measure of memory,

it would be necessary to use meaningless stimuli. As a result, he chose to use nonsense

syllables as to-be-remembered stimuli.

In the first phase of Ebbinghauss studies, he memorized lists of nonsense syllables,

repeating the lists until he could recall them perfectly. He made careful records of the

amount of time required to learn each list. In the next phase, he measured how much of each

list he could recall after varying amounts of time passed without having practiced it. Finally,

he relearned each list. Afterwards, he compared the amount of time required to learn each

list originally to the time it took to relearn it. Figure 3.2 illustrates the curve of forgetting

that Ebbinghaus documented (Ebbinghaus, 1885). As you can see from the red line in the

graph, forgetting occurs at a rapid rate for two days or so and then levels off.

Figure 3.2
Ebbinghauss Forgetting Curve

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 60

Ebbinghaus also discovered the serial position effect, the tendency to forget

information in the middle of a list more easily than items at the beginning of end. The serial

position effect comes into play any time information is stored in memory in a specific order.

Therefore, to reduce the chances of this type of forgetting, the order in which information is

rehearsed should be varied unless the order itself has meaning.

Other Types of Forgetting

There are several types of forgetting other than those associated with context effects, the

curve of forgetting, and the serial position effect. Encoding failure, the failure to effectively

store information in LTM, is perhaps the most common of them. Typically, encoding failure

occurs when we store only the gist of new information in LTM without encoding the details.

This gives us the feeling that we have stored the information in memory when we really have

stored only its general theme and a few supporting details. Encoding failure is particularly

common in text learning, the kind of learning you are doing right now. When we read, we

tend to store generalizations rather than details. As a result, when we are asked to recall

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 61

details, as might happen on a quiz, we think we should know them but cannot recall them.

The fact is that we cant recall them because we never put them in LTM to begin with.

Another common type of forgetting is interference, information loss that is due to the

limited capacity of working memory. For example, suppose you have forgotten where you put

your car keys. You attempt to retrace your steps back to the point where you took the keys

out of your cars ignition. Unfortunately, you find yourself able to remember a number of

specific eventssetting a grocery bag on the kitchen counter, letting the dog out, leafing

through the mail, and so onbut are unable to recall where you left your keys. Most likely,

you have forgotten where you put your keys because your working memory was processing

information about these other events at the time you let go of the keys. The information you

were processing at the time interfered with your working memorys ability to process

information about the location of your keys.

Motivated forgetting is information loss that is due to the emotional features of the

information. Suppose you wake up one morning and suddenly remember that you have a

dentists appointment but forgot to request a day off from work. Oh well, you think, Ill

just have to cancel the appointment. Its at least possible that you forgot the appointment

because you feel anxious about going to the dentist and are relieved that you now have an

excuse to put it off.

Have you ever taken a test and suddenly remembered the answer to one of the

questions after you turned in your exam? If so, you have experienced retrieval failure, the

failure to retrieve information that is stored in LTM in a context in which the information is

necessary. Retrieval failure happens for a variety of reasons. For example, the way in which

the information is stored in LTM may not match the features of the situation in which it is

needed. This can happen in a situation in which the wording of test questions varies from that

of a textbook. The textbook might say 73% of adolescents, while the test question says

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 62

about three-fourths of adolescents. Whatever the cause of retrieval failure, it is often

associated with the tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) phenomenon, confidence in the fact that

needed information is stored in LTM even though it cannot be recalled.

Finally, the neurological underpinnings of memory are responsible for another type of

forgetting, consolidation failure. For instance, people who lose consciousness typically

cannot recall events that immediately preceded the loss of consciousness. This happens

because the physiology of unconsciousness interfered with the physiological processes that

must occur in order for the brain to form a memory. More commonly, consolidation failure

occurs because of sleep deprivation. Neuroscientists have learned that memories are

consolidated during phases of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep that occur as a normal part

every nights sleep. When the amount of time devoted to sleep decreases, so does the

number of REM sleep phases. As a result, individuals who get less than optimum amounts of

sleep are unable to consolidate some of the memories that their IP systems process during the

day.

EVALUATION OF INFORMATION PROCESSING THEORY

Like all of the theories discussed in this text, information processing theory has had a sizeable

impact on the scientific study of learning. The theorys heuristic value is not in doubt.

However, to determine its usefulness, assessment of its value with regard to these questions

is required:

Does it explain the basic facts of learning?

Does it generate testable predictions?

Does it lead to practical strategies that are relevant to real-world learning and

instruction?

The explanatory and hypothesis-generating power of information processing theory are

impressive. There are few cognitive events that cannot be described and studied effectively

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 63

by combining the features of the Atkinson-Shiffrin model of the IP system with Baddeleys

model of working memory. Where IP theory sometimes falls short, however, is in the transfer

of the research findings it has generated from the controlled world of the laboratory to the

messy world of real-world learning contexts. Nevertheless, IP researchers have addressed

this problem by applying their theoretical models and research methods to the study of

everyday and academic cognitive functioning. As a result, the theory is now accumulating a

great deal of evidence on everyday and academic cognition that stands alongside its much

larger body of laboratory-based findings. These studies have revealed that perhaps the

greatest strength of the IP model is its capacity for generating hypotheses that can address a

seemingly infinite array of questions about cognition. For these reasons, the IP approach is

arguably the most influential perspective in learning theory today.

Despite the clear scientific value of IP theory, it is also necessary to ask whether the

theory is capable of producing instructional strategies that influence learning. On this

criterion, IP theory also receives high marks, not so much because of strategies that derive

directly from the theory but more so for its capacity for providing empirical support for the

effectiveness of one strategy over another.

SUMMARY

In this chapter, you have learned how the information processing system stores and loses

memories. A key point was that remembering involves both retrieval and reconstruction.

Consequently, task-relevant knowledge stored in long-term memory is vital to the functioning

of the entire system. Moreover, the functional efficiency of the working memory strongly

affects memory processes.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 64

KEY TERMS, CONCEPTS, AND PEOPLE

micro-theories
Richard Atkinson
Richard Shiffrin
sensory memory
icons
echoes
attention
short-term memory (STM)
automaticity
chunking
memory strategies
Allan Baddeley
working memory (WM)
central executive
phonological loop
visual-spatial sketch pad
episodic buffer
long-term memory (LTM)
explicit memory
implicit memory
semantic memory
episodic memory
source memories
flashbulb memories
retrieval
recognition
recall
relearning
reconstruction
Sir Frederick Bartlett
schemas
networks
forgetting
context effects
Duncan Gooden
state-dependent memory
Hermann Ebbinghaus
curve of forgetting
serial position effect
encoding failure
interference
motivated forgetting
retrieval failure
tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) phenomenon
consolidation failure

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 65

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

THE DEVELOPING BRAIN

PIAGETS THEORY OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

THE INFORMATION-PROCESSING APPROACH TO COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

VYGOTSKYS THEORY OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

EVALUATION OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENTAL THEORY

SUMMARY

KEY TERMS, CONCEPTS, AND PEOPLE

You should remember from Chapter 1 that learning is a relatively permanent change in

behavior or knowledge. Learning stands in contrast to maturationchange due to an inborn,

genetic, species-specific, planand developmentchange that results from an interaction of

learning and maturation. Consequently, theories of development are broader than theories of

learning. In addition, as you learned in Chapter 1, theories of cognitive development, age-

associated changes in intellectual skills and knowledge, are consistent with organismic

epistemology. They assume that learners are active contributors to these changes and that

nature and nurture are interactive influences. Moreover, theories of cognitive development

emphasize qualitative change and, as a result, typically include stages. Because all theories

of cognitive development assume that age-related change arises from an interaction between

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 66

nature and nurture, specifically the maturation of the brain and environmental demands on

its functions, it is important for you to become acquainted with some of the features of the

developing brain before delving into theories of cognitive development.

THE DEVELOPING BRAIN

You may remember from high school biology that the brain consists of two types of cells. The

first of these, neurons, are responsible for brain functions. Figure 4.1 depicts the parts of the

neuron and their functions. The second type of cell, glial cells, support neurons and give the

brain its shape. The brain attains its full complement of neurons in the early weeks of

prenatal development. Thus, the weight and size of the human brain increase with age not as

the result of the development of new neurons, but as the result of the growth of neurons.

Figure 4.1
The Neuron

Dendrites: Cell body extensions that receive messages from the next neuron

Cell body: Contains the nucleus and carries out vital function such as respiration

Axon: Tail-like cell body extension that carries messages to the next neuron

Nerve ending (synapse): Junction of one neuron with another

Neuronal growth spurts are one of the features of brain maturation that influence

cognitive development. These spurts involve changes in neuronal size and the number of

interconnections among neurons. Each spurt is followed by a period of pruning in which

inefficient or infrequently used neuronal connections are destroyed. These growth/pruning

cycles affect different areas of the brain at different ages. Those that are relevant to

cognitive development occur in in the cerebral cortex, the convoluted outer covering of the

brain that is the locus of cognition. Such spurts may be responsible for stage-like changes in

cognitive functioning, because each spurt influences brain functioning differently. Table 4.1

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 67

lists some of the major spurt/pruning cycles and their effects on cognitive functions. Note

that brain growth continues well into adulthood. Thus, some of the key structures in cognitive

functioning do not reach maturity until long after most individuals have completed their

formal schooling.

Table 4.1
Spurt/Pruning Cycles in Brain Development

Age Functions Affected

20 months Goal-oriented planning

4-6 First-language fluency, predicting outcomes

6-8 Writing, drawing, emotional control, association of sensory and cognitive functions

10-12 Logic, planning, memory

Spatial perception, integration of sensory, language, and motor functions, conscious control and
13-15
organization of thought processes, risk assessment

17-20 Logic, planning, risk assessment

Myelination, the development of an insulating layer of fat on neuronal axons, is

another key maturational process that underlies cognitive development. It is largely

responsible for the speed at which impulses travel from one neuron to the next. Like brain

growth, myelination occurs in spurts. One of the earliest structures to be myelinated is the

hippocampus, the structure that is primarily responsible for memory formation. Its neurons

are nearly fully myelinated by age 3. By contrast, the association areas, neuronal

connections that link sensory, motor, and cognitive functions, in the brains frontal lobes are

not fully myelinated until age 12. The neurons of the reticular formation, the brain structure

that controls attention, are fully myelinated much later, typically around age 25.

Lateralization is the process through which brain functions become relegated to the

right or left hemisphere of the cerebral cortex. Language functions are lateralized to the left

side of the brain in almost all humans prior to birth. However, the process of lateralization is

highly dependent upon development of the corpus callosum, the membrane that connects

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 68

the two hemispheres and allows for communication between them. As a result, most

functions that require lateralization to attain optimal efficiency are not lateralized until after

birth. Early childhood, approximately ages two to six years, is the period of life during which

the corpus callosum grows and matures most profoundly. Consequently, lateralization tends

to be complete in most children by age 8 or so. The latest lateralizing function is spatial

perception, the capacity for processing information about locations and spatial relationships

among objects. Differences between 8-year-olds and younger children in the ability to use

and generate maps as well as their performance in activities that involve flying objects (e.g.,

balls, Frisbees) illustrate the impact of lateralization on spatial perception.

PIAGETS THEORY OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

Swiss philosopher Jean Piaget (1896-1980) began his science career while still in grade school.

He learned the methods of the emerging field of natural history and applied them to his

observations of plant and animal life in the woods and mountains that surrounded his boyhood

home in Neuchtel, Switzerland. By the age of 10, he had published his first scientific paper,

and at 15, he was offered a research position at a prestigious museum in Paris. Piaget

graduated from the University of Geneva with a Ph.D. in zoology at the age of 21 and began

to pursue a research agenda that focused on the evolution of human intelligence. While

administering intelligence tests in Alfred Binets lab in Paris, Piaget observed childrens

illogical responses to the questions he posed and became more interested in their reasoning

than in whether their answers were correct. As a result, when he joined the faculty of the

University of Geneva, Piaget began to study childrens reasoning and produced a set of

findings that continue to spur research, debate, and theorizing.

Piagets Great Discovery: Four Stages of Cognitive Development

Recall from Chapter 1 that the first goal of the scientific study of learning is to produce

accurate descriptions of change. Piaget combined the meticulous observational methods of

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 69

the early 20th centurys natural historians with the response-dependent questioning methods

of the psychoanalysts to explore childrens and teens responses to a variety of problems. The

description of age-related changes in reasoning that emerged from these studies has proven

to be one of the most accurate and most frequently replicated research findings in the entire

field of psychology. Piaget argued that his findings were best thought of as representing four

universal, hierarchical stages of cognitive development. Each stage features a unique set of

intellectual tools and a few key skills that must be developed before moving on to the next

stage.

The Sensorimotor Stage. In Piagets sensorimotor stage, infants use their senses and motor

actions to make sense of the world. At about eight months of age, they develop object

permanence, the understanding that objects continue to exist even when they cannot be

seen, felt, or heard. Over the next year or so, a number of key intellectual tools emerge from

this rudimentary concept. These tools include:

Mental representation: The ability to think about non-present objects (Example:

Searching for a desired object or person)

Means-end behavior: The ability to formulate and carry out goal-based plans

(Example: Using a chair to climb on the kitchen counter to get a cookie)

Pretend play: The use of one object to represent another (Examples: Pretending

that a block is a car, imaginary role play)

The Preoperational Stage. Between the ages of 2 and 6, children continue to use their

senses and motor actions to explore the world, but they also have a firm grasp of the

semiotic function, the understanding that symbols can represent objects, people, and ideas

and that these symbols can be used for both thinking and communicating. Thus, words,

images, and pretend play are the primary intellectual tools children use in Piagets

preoperational stage. As a result, preschoolers are much more cognitively sophisticated than

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 70

infants are. However, their thinking includes a number of important logical flaws. These flaws

include:

Egocentrism: The belief that others see the world from the childs point of view

(Example: Using pronouns without antecedents in recounting an event. The child

knows to whom he is referring when he says she, therefore, listeners should as

well.)

Centration: The tendency to process information in terms of single variables

(Example: The size of a coin determines its value relative to other coins.)

Appearance/reality confusion: The reliance on appearance as the primary

indicator of the characteristics of a substance (Example: Living things movie;

therefore a leaf comes to life when the wind blows it across the yard.)

Transductive reasoning: Reasoning based on the belief that two events that occur

close together in time are causally related (Example: A magician says

abracadabra before pulling a rabbit out of a hat; therefore, the word

abracadabra caused the rabbit to come out of the hat.)

As preschoolers accumulate experiences in which they act on and observe the physical and

social worlds, these flaws gradually decline, paving the way for the emergence of the next

stage of development.

The Concrete Operational Stage. In the concrete operational stage, 6- to 12-year-olds

develop a powerful set of intellectual tools that allow them to achieve adult-level mastery of

some of the most important features of the physical and social worlds. These tools emerge

gradually and typically co-exist with the declining, but still influential, logical flaws of the

preoperational stage. One of the key features of concrete operational thinking is

conservation, the understanding that appearance does not alter the characteristics of a

substance. For example, in one of Piagets classic problems, children are asked whether a ball

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 71

of clay that has been rolled into a sausage shape contains more or less clay than it did when it

was in a ball. Children exhibit conservation by correctly observing that the amount of clay is

the same regardless of its shape and exhibiting the intellectual tools below in the reasons

they give for reaching the conclusion that the amount of clay did not change:

Reversibility: The ability to use backwards thinking to solve problems; the primary

intellectual tool developed in the concrete operational stage (Example: If you roll

it into a sausage shape, you can just make it into a ball again to see that its still

the same amount.)

Inductive reasoning: Recognizing patterns and using them to formulate rules

(Example: You didnt add any clay or take any away. Thats why its still the same

amount.)

Decentration and compensation: The ability to use multiple variables to process

information (Example: The ball looks like it has more clay because its taller than

the sausage, but you have to take into account that the sausage is wider than the

ball.)

In addition to conservation and the intellectual tools that underlie it, children in the concrete

operational stage also develop an understanding of class inclusion, the reasoning that

underlies hierarchical classification systems such as the familiar class-order-family-genus-

species scheme that is used to classify living things. In addition, they acquire the capacity for

seriation, the ability to arrange a large number of objects in size order. Younger children can

order three to five objects according to size. By contrast, by the end of the concrete

operational stage, children have developed a highly flexibility set of skills for using magnitude

as an organizing principle.

Piaget used the term concrete when in the name of this stage because he concluded

that its purpose was to enable children to develop an accurate mental model of the world. As

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 72

such, the cognitive milestones of this stage involve abstract ideas that can be represented in

the physical world. For example, children develop an understanding of the associative

property ([(a + b) + c] = [a + (b + c)]) during this stage. It can be stated as a rule (abstract) or

demonstrated with objects (concrete). Thus, concrete operational reasoning involves a great

deal of abstract thinking, but the content of the concrete operational thinkers abstractions is

limited to objects, people, and ideas that can be easily represented in the physical world.

Because of this limitation, children in the concrete operational stage do not function well

when asked to solve problems that involve deductive reasoning, reasoning from a premise to

a valid conclusion. As a result, when a concrete operational child is asked to think creatively

in response to what if? questions, he typically copies the world as he knows it instead of

proposing new possibilities. Thus, one of the signs indicating that a child has just about

completed the concrete operational stage is his emerging capacity for deductive thinking that

renders him capable of developing simple, informal theories about academic subject matter

as well as important events in his own life (e.g., What might happen if I skip a step when

doing this kind of math problem? or How will my life be different if I make the basketball

team?)

The Formal Operational Stage. Thanks to the solid grasp of reality that emerges from the

concrete operational stage, children enter the formal operational stage around age 12 or so

with a rapidly emerging cognitive tool, hypothetico-deductive thinking, a form of deductive

reasoning in which the premise is hypothetical, that is, factually untrue. Hypothetico-

deductive thinking is the reasoning that underlies scientific inquiry and, as a result, it endows

teens with a capacity for abstraction that goes far beyond that of the concrete operational

stage. Equipped with hypothetico-deductive thinking, the adolescent is no longer tied to

abstractions that have concrete referents in the physical and social worlds. Thus, she enters a

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 73

new world in which she can both generate and test hypotheses in the inner world of her mind

and adjust her outer behavior accordingly.

However, the very cognitive power that makes hypothetico-deductive thinking such a

monumental cognitive advance generates a number of intellectual vulnerabilities. Changes in

these vulnerabilities that occur over the ten to fifteen years that follow the first appearance

of formal operational thought constitute the difference between adolescent and adult formal

operational thinking. The cognitive vulnerabilities exhibited by teens that gradually diminish

in the early adult years include:

Possibilities unconstrained by probabilities: Believing anything is possible with

minimal regard for pre-existing limitations (Example: Even though my progress

report average is 32, I might be able to pass geometry if I get 100s on all the daily

quizzes for the rest of the six-weeks.)

Nave idealism: Proposing simple, usually excessively optimistic, solutions for

complex problems (Example: If all the rich countries shared their money with the

poor countries, there wouldnt be any more poor countries.)

Adolescent egocentrism: Believing that others are equally interested in and

concerned about ones strengths and weaknesses as one is himself (Example:

Everybodys looking at me funny today. They must be thinking that the haircut I

got yesterday looks weird.)

Personal fable: Unrealistic optimism or pessimism along with poor causal reasoning

about ones autobiography (Examples: My parents divorced when I was 3. Thats

why I dont have a girlfriend. or He hasnt called in three weeks, but I know

were meant to be together. Ive already picked out my wedding dress.)

Imaginary audience: Mentally rehearsing and adjusting behavior for an imagined

peer audience (Example: I cant go to the movie with my Mom. Some of my

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 74

friends might be there. What will they think if they see me at a movie with my

Mom?)

Unique invulnerability: Believing that others are more vulnerable to risks than

oneself (Examples: It doesnt matter that much if I start smoking now. Im young

and healthy so I can quit later and avoid getting cancer. or I know I should be

using protection, but I just dont see myself as a teen mom. I dont think

pregnancy is in my future.)

Ages and Stages. The ages that Piaget found to be associated with each stage are averages

for children and teenagers in industrialized societies. Thus, there is some degree of individual

variation. In addition, there are no periods of true stage stability, no plateaus, in other

words. A 6- to 12-year-old, for instance, is best thought of as being in the process of

developing concrete operations. She wont show formal operational thought until her

concrete operational skills have fully emerged. Thus, the sequence of stages is universal, but

the ages at which individuals achieve each of them can vary.

Piagets Explanation of Cognitive Development

In studies whose publication dates span nearly a century, researchers have replicated Piagets

observations of childrens and teens thinking across a wide variety of cultural settings. Thus,

there is wide agreement among developmentalists regarding Piagets description of cognitive

development. However, the theory that he proposed to explain his findings has been the

subject of much debate.

Schemes. The basic cognitive unit of change in Piagets theory is the scheme, a French word

that is best translated as blueprint. Schemes are sometimes referred to as action plans. Their

purpose is to guide behavior in the presence of specific cues. They are the products of

organization, a cognitive process that extracts generalizable information from specific

experiences. For example, a toddler who plays with tennis balls develops a scheme that she

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 75

applies to all objects that resemble tennis balls. Consequently, when she encounters a

tangerine for the first time, she treats it as if it has the same properties as a tennis ball. This

is because the process of organization has extracted a scheme, an action plan, from her

experiences with tennis balls. The cues that trigger the scheme are the similarities between

tangerines and tennis balls. Both are round, for example, and the physical actions that enable

an individual to pick up and hold on to a tennis ball also work for tangerines.

Schemes change through adaptation. Adaptation involves two processes, assimilation

and accommodation. Assimilation occurs when an individual applies an existing scheme to a

new experience. So, the toddler mentioned earlier assimilates a tangerine to her tennis ball

scheme when she picks up the piece of fruit and holds it as she would a ball. But she is likely

to discover fairly quickly that there are important differences between tangerines and tennis

balls. One, obviously, is that tangerines dont bounce. Consequently, when the toddler

applies the throwing part of her tennis ball scheme to the tangerine, she will receive some

information that her ball scheme cannot deal with. As a result, she will have to change the

tennis ball scheme such that it excludes fruit. The process of changing a scheme to fit new

information is called accommodation.

When schemes fit reality, that is, when the toddler is clear about the difference

between balls and round fruits and demonstrates this understanding behaviorally, the

schemes are said to have completed the process of equilibration, a state in which

assimilation and accommodation are in balance. Schemes must be exercised to achieve

equilibration. In other words, the toddler has to throw everything in her world that resembles

a tennis ballplums, peaches, nectarines, Christmas tree ornaments, knick-knacks at

Grandmas housein order to equilibrate her tennis ball scheme.

Stages as Schemes. According to Piaget, each stage is a network of schemes that are

qualitatively distinct from those of the other stages. Moreover, the schemes of each stage

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 76

must reach equilibration before those of the next stage can emerge. Consequently, for

Piaget, an infant in the sensorimotor stage is in the process of developing sensorimotor

schemes. As soon as a sufficient number of sensorimotor schemes reach equilibration, the

symbolic schemes of the preoperational stage begin to emerge. When these are equilibrated,

children enter the concrete operational stage and begin developing rule-based schemes that

represent logical connections among objects, people, and ideas in the physical world. When

these rule-based schemes are equilibrated, the hypothetico-deductive schemes of the formal

operational stage make their appearance. Thus, adults should be thought of as having four

types of equilibrated schemes (the four stages), teens as having three, children as having

two, and infants as having one.

Influences on Progression through the Stages. As we noted earlier, the ages at which

individuals attain each stage vary from one person to another. Piaget proposed that four

factors are responsible for age variations. He described each factor as necessary but not

sufficient for cognitive development. That is, no single factor is responsible for cognitive

development. All of them are necessary.

First, maturation of the central nervous system constrains cognitive development.

That is, Piaget argued that each stage depends on the development of one or more

mechanisms in the brain and cannot develop until those mechanisms reach maturity. Clearly,

children mature at different rates, and these rates are reflected in cognitive development as

well as in other behavioral domains. Furthermore, factors such as illness and malnutrition

that adversely affect maturation also hinder cognitive development.

Second, according to Piaget, social transmission, or information that we get from

others through instruction, modeling, and interaction affects the rate of cognitive

development. Formal schooling is one important source of social transmission. As a result,

children who do not receive formal schooling due to poverty or customs exhibit slower rates

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 77

of progression through Piagets stages than those who go to school. However, the quality of

social transmission is equally as important. Thus, children who attend poor quality

educational programs also develop more slowly than peers in high quality programs. The same

is true for access to adults.

Third, children require experience that includes opportunities to act on the world and

observe the results. Fourth, equilibration, which Piaget described as the cognitive systems

tendency to improve its schemes until they achieve the best fit possible with reality, is

required for cognitive development. Experience and equilibration are interdependent. Think

back to the example of the toddler and the tangerine. The environment provided her with the

tangerine. Her cognitive system supplied the scheme for acting on it. Because she had the

opportunity to apply her tennis ball scheme to the tangerine and observe the outcome, her

cognitive system was able to accommodate the tennis ball scheme appropriately and

construct a new scheme for the tangerine.

Citing Piagets stages of cognitive development and his writings on the four factors

that influence progression through them, some educators have interpreted his work to mean

that instruction in a given skill or concept should not occur until the childs mind is ready

for it. Piaget did not agree. He argued that cognitive development is dependent upon the

degree to which the environment challenges childrens schemes and urged educators to

approach instruction with this principle in mind. However, he also pointed out that

instruction that taps childrens reasoning abilities may be more or less efficient depending on

its timing relative to students cognitive developmental status. In other words, second-

graders might require several weeks of instruction in a logic-based skill or concept (e.g.,

place value) that third-graders can master in a few days. Balancing the interplay between

challenging childrens schemes and planning for instructional efficiency is the essence of

useful classroom applications of Piagets theory of cognitive development.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 78

THE INFORMATION-PROCESSING APPROACH TO COGNITIVE

DEVELOPMENT

In Chapter 3 you learned that the model of the information processing (IP) system that

researchers use to study cognitive functioning includes three components: sensory memory,

short-term/working memory, and long-term memory. The IP system functions in children in

the same way that it does in adults. However, there are several age-related variables that

cause the IP systems of children to function much less efficiently than those of adults do.

Thus, some developmentalists have proposed that these variables do a better job of

explaining the patterns of cognitive development that Piagets observations brought to light

than his own theory does.

Information Processing Efficiency

The efficiency of the IP system is strongly related to chronological age. Thus, IP theorists

argue that individuals performance on Piagets tasks improves with age because of increases

in the efficiency of the IP system. These increases result from improvements in processing

speed, short-term memory capacity, working memory functioning, automaticity, and

expertise.

Processing Speed. As neurons become myelinated, the amount of time required for an

impulse to travel from one point to another in the brain declines. Behaviorally speaking, this

means that, given the same knowledge, an older child can access it more rapidly than a

younger child can. For example, if a 6-year-old and a 10-year-old are shown photos of

everyday objects and asked to name them as quickly as possible, the 10-year-old will

outperform the 6-year-old. The reason for this age difference is that more of the neurons in

the 10-year-olds brain are myelinated, so he processes information more quickly. As a result,

he can retrieve the names of the objectives from memory more rapidly. The more rapidly an

individual can retrieve information from memory, the more efficiently his IP system works.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 79

Short-term Memory Capacity. The baseline capacity of short-term memory increases a great

deal as children and teens get older. Digit span tasks are typically used as operational

definitions of the baseline STM capacity. Table 4.2 lists average digit span test scores from

ages 5 to 16 years. As you can see in the table, STM capacity increases a great deal across the

elementary and secondary school years. Moreover, the average child does not reach adult

capacity (7 +/- 2 bits of information) until the age of 16. As you read in Chapter 3, short-term

memory capacity contributes to the efficiency of the IP system because it limits the amount

of information that can be processed at one time. So, increased capacity means decreased

limitations and, as a result, increased efficiency.

Table 4.2
Age and Digit Span

Age Average Digit Span

5 3.5
6 4.1
7 4.4
8 4.8
9 5.2
10 5.6
11 5.6
12 6.2
13 6.2
14 6.2
15 6.7
16 7

Another way of looking at the impact of STM capacity on the efficiency of the IP

system is to relate it to a simple memory task such as memorizing a list of words. If the list

includes six words, memorizing it will be a single learning task for the typical 16-year-old. Her

STM capacity allows her to process all six words at once. In contrast, the typical 5-year-old

will have to break it down into two learning tasks, because she can only process three words

at a time.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 80

Working Memory Efficiency. Most memory tasks, especially those associated with academic

learning, are far more demanding than memorizing a list of words. As a result, they tap the IP

systems working memory capacity, that is, the coordinated efforts of the WMs central

executive, phonological loop, visuospatial sketchpad, and episodic buffer, all of which you

read about in Chapter 3. Childrens working memories are far more limited than simple

measures of baseline STM, such as digit span tests, suggest. This is because each component

of the WM depends on the development and myelination of specific brain structures.

Moreover, maintaining information in WM requires selective attention, which, in turn,

requires inhibition of competing information, what we call in everyday language

distractions. As you read earlier in this chapter, selective attention depends on myelination

of the reticular formation, a milestone that is not attained until the mid-twenties.

To get additional insight into age differences in working memory, think of the common

task of retracing your steps to find a lost object. As you work backwards, you have to store

the outcome of the first step and an explanation as to why it failed to lead you to the object

somewhere in working memory. You might verbalize it and store it in your phonological loop,

or you might visualize it and store it in your visuospatial sketchpad. Either way, you have to

keep rehearsing it or you will forget that the missing object wasnt in the first place you

looked. As you move from step to step, you have to keep rehearsing the results of all the

prior steps at the same time. Consequently, as you probably know from personal experience,

retracing your steps to find something taxes the working memory even in adults, especially

when you factor in the strain that anxiety about having lost something exerts on working

memory efficiency. Furthermore, as you would probably predict, this kind of strategy quickly

overloads a childs working memory. As a result, children either abandon the search after the

first couple of steps or they needlessly return to the first step time after time. Similarly, if

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 81

decoding and/or comprehending a text overwhelms a students working memory capacity, she

will not be able to remember it.

Automaticity. As you read in Chapter 3, automaticity is the chief means by which the IP

system overcomes the limitations of short-term memory. It can be achieved at any age simply

by repeatedly rehearsing and retrieving a memory. Obviously, as children and teens mature,

they develop automaticity for more and more bits of information. With growing automaticity

comes increased efficiency in the IP system, particularly in working memory.

Unfortunately, automaticity is often disparaged as rote memorization and

condemned by educators. However, it is absolutely essential to the efficiency of the IP

system. There are many studies showing, for instance, that automaticity of letter-sound

connections is essential for beginning readers because it allows them to devote working

memory capacity to connecting written words to their spoken equivalents. When a child uses

her automatized decoding skills to read a text that contains familiar words, concepts, and

grammatical structures, comprehension happens automatically as well. When both decoding

and comprehension function automatically, the chances that the child will be able to

remember, answer questions about, and meaningfully process a text (e.g., identify the main

idea) greatly improve thanks to the increased efficiency of WM.

Expertise. As you have just read, comprehension occurs automatically when children read

texts comprised of familiar words, concepts, and grammatical structures. Collectively, the

relevant knowledge we tap into when performing a cognitive task is called expertise. As

individuals go through life, the amount of information they have stored in long-term memory

grows. As a result, no matter what kind of cognitive task is involved, the chances are quite

good that an adult will have more expertise than a teenager, who, in turn, will have more

expertise than a child. Consequently, age-related increases in expertise help to explain age

differences in IP efficiency.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 82

Clearly, however, regardless of age, wide differences in expertise exist across

individuals. Thus, individual differences in expertise help to explain individual differences in

IP efficiency. Moreover, studies that compare children who are expert chess players to adults

who know little about the game show that, when children possess more expertise than adults

do for specific memory tasks, children tend to outperform adults. These findings suggest that

task-relevant knowledge helps the IP system overcome limitations that are associated with

brain maturation. This is a very important principle, because expertise is highly dependent

upon experience. In other words, unlike maturation, the development of expertise can be

very strongly enhanced by teachers, parents, and agents in the environment. This is so

because any experience that helps a child develop a rich base of knowledge has the potential

to improve the efficiency of the IP system. Moreover, a childs IP system will function most

efficiently when performing memory tasks within the content domains with which he has the

greatest amount of expertise.

Metacognition

As children get older, the amount of knowledge they accumulate about the nature of

knowledge itself and the process of acquiring it increases dramatically. Metacognition,

thinking about thinking, develops along with this knowledge base. The development of

metacognitive knowledge and the ability to intentionally and effortfully apply it to the inner

workings of the IP system contributes to the overall efficiency of the system. Thus, IP

theorists argue that children may perform poorly on Piagets problems because they focus on

irrelevant features of them, fail to monitor what is going on in their minds as they attempt to

solve them, or lack the ability to apply memory strategies that may be helpful.

Selective Attention. Suppose a teacher who normally writes the days agenda on the left side

of the board (from the students perspective) decides for some reason to record it on the

right side one day. As an adult learner, you would notice the change but would also recognize

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 83

that it is irrelevant to the content of the agenda. As a result, you would attend to the content

rather than the placement of the agenda. By contrast, children are likely to pay more

attention to the unusual placement of the agenda than to its content. Psychologists use the

term selective attention to refer to the capacity to focus on the most important and relevant

aspects of an information array. Due to the slow rate of myelination in the reticular

formation, children are far less able than teens to sort out irrelevant from relevant

information. And teens are far less able than adults to do so. As a result, both children and

teens often expend attentional resources on processing information that is irrelevant to the

task at hand.

Cognitive Monitoring. Have you ever been reading a text and suddenly stop yourself because

you realize that you dont understand what you are reading? Most likely, you went back to the

beginning of the text and reread it. Such episodes result from adults capacity for cognitive

monitoring, the ability to track the progress and effectiveness of information processing as it

is happening. Children are less able than adults to monitor their thoughts in this way primarily

because of age differences in WM efficiency.

Metamemory. An individuals knowledge about memory strategies and how they can be used

to enhance memory functioning is called metamemory. Childrens knowledge of the memory

strategies you learned about in Chapter 3 and their ability to use them independently

improves with age (see Table 4.3). However, due to limitations on childrens working

memories, they are often unable to deploy strategies effectively. To use a memory strategy,

there must be sufficient WM capacity available for both the to-be-remembered information

and the strategy itself. Consequently, in describing childrens approach to memory strategies,

IP theorists often differentiate between production deficiencies, the inability to apply a

strategy unless reminded to do so, and utilization deficiencies, the ineffective use of a

known strategy due to WM limitations.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 84

Table 4.3
Age and Independent Use of Memory Strategies

Strategy Definitions and Academic Learning Examples Age

Repeating information until it is no longer needed or until it can be easily retrieved from
long-term memory
Rehearsal 6
Example: Repeating letter-sound connections until they can be recalled with 100%
accuracy

Sorting information into categories for storage in long-term memory


Organization 10
Example: Grouping spelling words by pattern categories to facilitate studying for a test

Relating new and previously learned information in meaningful ways


Elaboration 15
Example: Relating the term binary to the word bicycle to remember that it means a base 2
numbering system

Creating non-meaningful links between new and previously learned information


Mnemonics 18
Example: Visualizing John Calvin and Martin Luther in referee uniforms to remember that
they were key figures in the Reformation

Directly teaching memory strategies to students, providing them with opportunities for

practice, and giving cues that facilitate strategy transfer from practiced problems to new

ones can solve the production deficiency problem, especially with older children and teens.

However, such instruction does not address the utilization deficiency issue. As a result,

facilitating improvements in WM efficiency in the ways that were mentioned earlier not only

enhances WM but also indirectly influences childrens metamemory functioning. Moreover,

although independent creation of mnemonics does not appear until adulthood in most people,

even very young children have little difficulty learning and using adult-supplied mnemonics

that are linked to specific content, such as using the acronym HOMES to remember the names

of the Great Lakes.

Neo-Piagetian Theories of Cognitive Development

Like IP theorists, advocates of neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development argue that

age differences in the effectiveness and efficiency of the IP system explain childrens

performance on Piagets tasks. However, they integrate a number of Piagets theoretical

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 85

concepts into their explanations of cognitive development. For instance, neo-Piagetian

theories assume that Piagets characterization of schemes and the ways in which experience

changes them are accurate. Likewise, they accept the notion of stages in cognitive

development.

One of the best known advocates of neo-Piagetian theory was the late Robbie Case

(1945-2000) explained cognitive development in terms of operational efficiency, the

maximum number of schemes that a child can hold in WM at one time. He argued that

operational efficiency improves with age as WM capacity and efficiency, which he called

short-term storage space (STSS), increase. Thus, according to Case, the 7-year-old is better

able than the 4-year-old to handle the processing demands of Piagets tasks because the STSS

of the older child possesses superior operational efficiency.

VYGOTSKYS SOCIOCULTURAL THEORY

Russian educational theorist Lev Vygotsky (1907-1934) proposed a sociocultural theory of

cognitive development that emphasizes language, social interaction, and play rather than

cognitive structures. Vygotsky hypothesized that much of cognitive development results from

the childs internalization of information that is acquired socially, primarily through the

medium of language. He argued that nature endows children with basic skills that include

perception, attention, and memory, and that these natural skills enable children to take learn

through language and social interaction.

According to Vygotsky, social environments provide children with scaffolding, a type

of instruction in which an adult adjusts the amount of guidance provided to match a childs

present level of ability. Consequently, through scaffolding, children are able to experience

higher levels of cognitive functioning than they are able to reach on their own. Scaffolding

experiences provide children with models that they internalize as goals. Motivated by these

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 86

internalized goals, children respond with increasing levels of independence as adults

gradually withdraw scaffolding.

Vygotsky argued that scaffolding is likely to be an effective means of facilitating

cognitive development only if it is applied to tasks that are within a childs zone of proximal

development, the developmental boundaries within which a child is capable of functioning

independently. For example, scaffolding will not help a young child learn to do high school

geometry problems. However, it will help a child who knows how to do two-digit addition

problems with regrouping learn to do three-digit problems.

Like Piaget, Vygotsky proposed that development occurs in stages. He proposed four

stages of sociocultural cognitive development:

Primitive stage: The infant possesses mental processes similar to those of animals

Nave psychology stage: The child learns to use language to communicate but does

not understand symbols

Private speech stage: The child uses language as a guide to solving problems

Ingrowth stage: Logical thinking results from internalization of speech acquired

from children and adults in a social world

EVALUATION OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENTAL THEORY

All of the developmental theories you have read about in this chapter have demonstrated

their heuristic value. They have been widely discussed and debated among developmentalists

and learning theories. In addition, with the exception of Vygotskys theory, much research

has been done both with the aim of supporting and refuting the theories. But how well does

each of the theories address the questions below?

Does it explain the basic facts of learning?

Does it generate testable predictions?

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 87

Does it lead to practical strategies that are relevant to real-world learning and

instruction?

The pattern of age-related change in reason ability that Piaget discovered has helped

developmentalists, learning theorists, teachers, and parents alike better understand how

children perceive the world around them. Moreover, his explanation of the stages and the

factors that influence childrens progression through them provides answers as to why

reasoning skills are difficult to teach and, even when teaching succeeds, children rarely

transfer them from practiced problems to novel tasks. However, Piagets theoretical

construct of the stage itself has been criticized as difficult to test.

Many of Piagets critics argue that the information processing approach provides a

better explanation of his findings than his own theory does. Moreover, IP studies of age-

related change have helped learning theorists gain insight into aspects of cognitive

development that Piaget did not address. These include metacognition and the role of

expertise in cognitive functioning.

Vygotkys sociocultural theory is intuitively appealing and, on first glance, seems to

explain a great deal about cognitive development. However, in contrast to both Piagets

theory and the IP approach, Vygotskys theory has produced few testable hypotheses. Critics

argue that the weakness of the theory lies in its vaguely defined constructs such as

scaffolding and the zone of proximal development.

With regard to instructional strategies, of the three approaches discussed in this

chapter, information processing theory appears to be superior to both Piagets and Vygotskys

theories. Research that employs IP theorys model of working memory has led to a number of

instructional innovations. These studies have revealed the importance of automaticity, for

example, a feature of working memory efficiency that is highly responsive to instruction and

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 88

opportunities for practice. Similarly, IP research has shown that expertise, which can also be

developed through instruction, is also critical to reading comprehension and text learning.

At the same time, research derived from Piagets theory has helped educators

appreciate the issue of developmental timing in curriculum decisions. In addition, it

illustrates the need for translating, in a sense, learning tasks into the language of a

childs developmental stage. For example, when asking children to respond to what if

writing prompts, Piagets findings on the development of hypothetico-deductive thinking

suggest that it would probably best to provide children with alternative answers to the

question from which they can choose one to write about. Adolescents, with their emerging

formal operational schemes, are ready to tackle what if questions in open-ended format.

Thus, one important instructional application of Piagets theory is its inclusion in teacher

training programs. The theory may not give teachers specific strategies to use in the

classroom, but it helps them better understand students thinking and behavior.

Many educators have embraced Vygotskys theory and have developed a number of

teaching strategies that are based on its principles. However, it is critical to note that the

theory does not have a strong empirical base to draw upon when applying it to instruction.

That is, few of its assertions have been thoroughly tested, and some have been refuted. For

example, his theory predicts that students who work in groups will produce better solutions

to problems than students who work alone. Research shows that groups produce the same

solutions that their most advanced individual members produce when they work alone. Thus,

groups perform at the highest level of the most capable individual member.

SUMMARY

In this chapter you have read about three different approaches to cognitive development. All

three assume that developing individuals actively contribute to the learning process.

Moreover, proponents of these theories argue that change results from an interaction of

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 89

nature and nurture, and that both quantitative and qualitative change occur as individuals

develop. They differ, however, in the mechanisms they propose to explain development.

Piaget hypothesized that changes in a cognitive structure, the scheme, account for cognitive

development. Information-processing theorists argue that the feature of the developing IP

system constrain development, while neo-Piagetians combine the approach of Piaget with

that of the IP theorists. In contrast to Piaget, IP theorists, and the neo-Piagetians, Vygotsky

argued that language and social interactions are more critical to cognitive development than

mental structures and processes are.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 90

KEY TERMS, CONCEPTS, AND PEOPLE

cognitive development
neurons
glial cells
growth spurts
pruning
cerebral cortex
myelination
hippocampus
association areas
reticular formation
lateralization
corpus callosum
spatial perception
Jean Piaget
Alfred Binet
sensorimotor stage
object permanence
semiotic function
preoperational stage
concrete operational stage
conservation
formal operational stage
hypothetico-deductive thinking
schemes
organization
adaptation
assimilation
accommodation
equilibration
expertise
metacognition
selective attention
cognitive monitoring
metamemory
production deficiencies
utilization deficiencies
neo-piagetian theories
Robbie Case
operational efficiency
short-term storage space (stss)
Lev Vygotsky
sociocultural theory
scaffolding
zone of proximal development

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 91

SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY

LEARNING THROUGH IMITATION

LEARNING THROUGH MODELING

EVALUATION OF SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY

SUMMARY

KEY TERMS, CONCEPTS, AND PEOPLE

In pursuit of the most comprehensive theory of learning possible, social cognitive theory

(SCT) borrows ideas from most of the theories you have read about in previous chapters. It

focuses largely on learning through observation. In addition, SCT addresses the effects of the

social environment on learners internalized expectations regarding the likely outcomes of

behavioral options. Like information processing and cognitive developmental theorists, SCT

advocates, the best known of whom is Albert Bandura, assume that learners actively

participate in the learning process. However, SCT shares with behaviorist epistemology the

assumption that environmental factors more strongly influence learning than those within the

learner do and that learning primarily involves quantitative change. Nevertheless, Bandura

argues that the outcome of social cognitive learning is an internal set of expectations derived

from interactions with others (the social cognitive element in learning) that motivates

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 92

learners to engage the environment in ways that either facilitate or impede learning.

Consequently, SCT is at least somewhat consistent with constructivist epistemology.

LEARNING THROUGH IMITATION

As we noted above, the best known proponent of social cognitive theory is Albert Bandura.

Early in his career, Banduras views were consistent with those of Skinner, whose operant

conditioning theory you should remember from Chapter 2. However, when Bandura became

interested in studying the acquisition of complex social behaviors, such as aggression, he

observed that a great deal of social behavior is learned through imitation rather than through

reinforcement. Consequently, he began to look beyond the conditioning theories in search of

an explanation of imitative learning.

Early Theories of Imitative Learning

Early explanations of imitative learning arose from general theories of learning. For

example, noting the existence of imitative learning in several animal species, some early

twentieth century biologists argued for the existence of a genetically programmed instinct

for imitative behavior. B. F. Skinner argued against the instinct explanation. According to his

view, individuals learn from models when the models reinforce them for imitative behavior.

In opposition to both the instinct and Skinnerian approaches, Piaget hypothesized that

imitative learning is a form of assimilation in that it provides developing individuals with

opportunities to practice existing schemes. In support of his hypothesis, Piaget cited research

showing that children readily imitate problem-solving strategies for which they have

developed appropriate schemes but do not imitate more advanced strategies.

Perhaps the most plausible explanation of imitative learning that was proposed prior

to the mid-twentieth century was that of Neal Miller and John Dollard (1941). They

hypothesized that imitative learning is a form of instrumental learning, a type of change that

organisms display in order to attain specific outcomes. For example, you have learned to pay

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 93

your bills on time in order to attain a specific outcome, that of avoiding late fees. Thus, you

have acquired this habit through an instrumental learning process.

Miller and Dollard argued that the degree to which an individual is willing to match the

behavior of a model (i.e., imitate the model) is dependent on his expectation of

reinforcement. Therefore, they coined the term matched-dependent behavior to refer to

instrumental learning that is acquired through imitation. Miller and Dollards approach was an

advance over earlier theories, but, like them, it failed to explain imitative learning that

occurred in the absence of direct reinforcement or imitative learning that is performed long

after observation of a model occurs.

Banduras Approach to Learning through Imitation

One of the shortcomings that Bandura found in all of the theories of imitative learning he

examined was that they failed to take into account Edward Tolmans discover of latent

learning, change that occurs in the absence of reinforcement but is demonstrated when

reinforcement is made available. In his classic studies of maze learning, Tolman showed that

laboratory rats learned to find their way around complex mazes even when they were not

reinforced for doing so. They demonstrated their learning by rapidly moving from the

beginning to the end of the maze as soon as he provided a bit of food at the end of the maze.

Thus, Bandura noted, lack of performance, demonstration of learned behavior, should not be

taken to mean that an organism has not learned a behavior. He argued that the

learning/performance distinction was especially relevant to learning through imitation

because an observer may learn a behavior by mentally imitating it and overtly perform it long

afterward, if at all.

In addition to distinguishing between learning and performance, Bandura argued that a

comprehensive explanation of learning should differentiate between the direct and indirect

effects of the environment on learning. He used the term enactive learning to refer to

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 94

change that is acquired through direct action of the environment on the learner. He adopted

the term vicarious learning to refer to change that is acquired through the observation of the

action of the environment on a model.

Bandura also noted that the major theories of the day tended to take an either-or

approach to internal and external influences on imitative learning. Piaget and the biologists

focused on internal factorsschemes and instincts, respectivelyto the exclusion of external

variables. In contrast, Skinner, as well as Miller and Dollard, emphasized external agents

models as sources of reinforcementat the expense of internal factors. Consequently,

Bandura sought to develop a theory that integrated both internal and external variables.

Bandura used the term reciprocal determinism to refer to his model of change,

because it included internal factors, external factors, and interactions among them (See

Figure 5.1). His model suggests that three sets of interacting factors influence both learning

and performance. Personal factors include internal variables such as temperament, prior

learning, developmental stage, and so on. Situational factors are characteristics of the

context in which learning and performance occur such as whether one is being watched and

the presence of incentives to learn and/or perform behavior. Behavioral factors are the

behaviors that a learner performs.

Figure 5.1
Banduras Reciprocal Determinism

Personal Factors

Situational Factors Behavioral Factors

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 95

LEARNING THROUGH MODELING

Based on the theoretical rationale outlined above, Bandura developed and carried out

research supporting a comprehensive theory of modeling, a collective term that encompasses

the entire process of learning from a model. When Bandura first began to publish his ideas

and research findings, his theory was known as social learning theory (e.g., Bandura &

Walters, 1963). However, as Bandura and others developed the theorys empirical foundation

across a wide range of studies, they discovered that the role of cognition was greater than

they originally assumed. Consequently, the theory ultimately became known as social

cognitive theory in the 1980s.

What and How We Learn from Models

Across numerous studies, Bandura and others found that, in general, modeling involves

four types of learning:

Observational learning: Acquisition of new behavior after observing a model

Facilitation: Performance of a previously learned socially acceptable behavior

after observing a model

Inhibition: Abstention from performance of a previously learned deviant behavior

after observing a model

Disinhibition: Performance of a previously learned deviant behavior after

observing a model

Each of these types of learning is caused by the development of an outcome expectancy, the

belief that one will be rewarded or punished for performing an observed behavior, in the

mind of the observer. When the observer expects to be rewarded for performing an observed

behavior, the chances are good that he will store the behavior and its associated outcome

expectancy in his long-term memory. He may or may not perform the learned behavior

immediately. In fact, he may never perform it at all. It is the outcome expectancy that is

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 96

learned, not the behavior itself. An observer also stores outcome expectancies in long-term

memory when observation of a model leads him to believe that he will be punished for

performing an observed behavior. As a result, he may be less likely to perform the behavior

himself in the future.

Learning from models is far from an automatic process. It depends on a number of

factors. One is attention, that is, the observer must attend to the models behavior and to

the outcome that the model experiences in order to develop an outcome expectancy. The

observers memory abilities are also important, as he must retain the behavior and its

associated outcome expectancy in long-term memory. Similarly, the observers capacity for

production, the developmental ability to perform the observed behavior, influences the

degree to which he is likely to acquire an outcome expectancy for an observed behavior.

Finally, to perform the observed behavior himself, the observer must have motivation.

Influences on Modeling

There are several characteristics of models that influence whether an observer will

learn a modeled behavior from them. The status and competence of the model is one such

factor. Observers do not acquire outcome expectancies from low-status models whom they

deem to lack competence. Think about how teens decide what to wear. How often do they

follow their parents clothing choices, and how often do they follow those of their peers? In

their eyes, at least when it comes to fashion, peers have higher status and are more

competent.

The consequences that models experience also influence observers learning. For

example, some students highly value being listed on the school honor roll and work hard to

attain this goal. It might seem that their doing so would serve as a model for students who

are low-achievers. The presumed message is: Work hard and youll get on the honor roll.

However, low-achievers may not value the consequence of getting on the honor roll. As a

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 97

result, seeing high-achievers names on the list will not cause them to develop the outcome

expectancy If I work as hard as the kids on the honor roll do, Ill get there, too.

The honor roll example brings to light another issue in modeling. In order for an

observer to believe that he has a chance of attaining the same consequence as a model does,

he must also believe that he is capable of performing the behavior that yielded the

consequence as well as the model did. Banduras term for the belief that one is capable of

attaining desirable goals is self-efficacy. Thus, low self-efficacy for academic tasks is

probably another reason that low-achievers learn little from observing the behavior of high-

achievers. In contrast, the students who fall slightly short of making the honor roll are likely

to learn from the high-achievers example for two reasons. First, they value the consequence,

and, second, they have high levels of self-efficacy for academic work.

EVALUATION OF SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY

Given that social cognitive theory, like the theories you have read about in earlier chapters,

has proven to be of heuristic value, how does it respond to the remaining three questions that

address a theorys usefulness?

Does it explain the basic facts of learning?

Does it generate testable predictions?

Does it lead to practical strategies that are relevant to real-world learning and

instruction?

Although the roots of social cognitive theory are in the behaviorist epistemological family,

Banduras thoughtful addition of cognitive components that have been demonstrated in

empirical research has given explanations of learning drawn from his theory far more

explanatory power than classical and operant conditioning have. Searches of research data

bases using key terms such as self-efficacy and modeling readily demonstrate the

theorys capacity for generating testable hypotheses. Moreover, these searches show that the

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 98

theory has been applied in practical ways across a wide variety of learning situations. For

example, Banduras ideas are frequently found at the heart of studies that compare varying

approaches to changing peoples health-related behaviors such as compliance with medical

instructions. Similarly, social cognitive theory is the basis of hundreds, if not thousands, of

studies of motivation in students from preschool to graduate school. Finally, Banduras

studies of childrens responses to aggressive models have enlightened parents, educators, and

clinicians about the necessity of monitoring the kinds of models to which children are exposed

through entertainment media.

SUMMARY

In this chapter you have been introduced to Banduras social cognitive theory. Despite what

many believe, learning from models does not happen in every case. This is because of

characteristics of both observers and models. Moreover, certain conditions are necessary for

modeling to be an effective means of learning. The observer must attend to, retain, be

capable of producing, and be motivated to perform the modeled behavior.

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.


Introduction to Learning Theories 99

KEY TERMS, CONCEPTS, AND PEOPLE

social cognitive theory (SCT)


Albert Bandura
instrumental learning
Neal Miller
John Dollard
matched-dependent behavior
Edward Tolman
latent learning
performance
enactive learning
vicarious learning
reciprocal determinism
personal factors
situational factors
behavioral factors
modeling
observational learning
facilitation
inhibition
disinhibition
outcome expectancy
self-efficacy

Denise R. Boyd, Ed.D.

You might also like