You are on page 1of 9

Optimal Width of Movement Corridors for Root Voles: Not Too Narrow and Not Too Wide

Author(s): Harry P. Andreassen, Stefan Halle and Rolf Anker Ims


Source: Journal of Applied Ecology, Vol. 33, No. 1 (Feb., 1996), pp. 63-70
Published by: British Ecological Society
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2405016 .
Accessed: 09/07/2014 08:43

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

British Ecological Society is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of
Applied Ecology.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 139.133.69.143 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 08:43:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Journalof
AppliedEcology
Optimalwidthofmovement forrootvoles:
corridors
1996,33, nottoo narrowand nottoo wide
63-70
HARRY P. ANDREASSEN, STEFAN HALLE and
ROLF ANKER IMS
DivisionofZoology,Department of Oslo, PO Box 1050 Blindern,
ofBiology,University N-0316Oslo,
Norway

Summary
of male root vole movementsas a functionof corridorwidth
1. The characteristics
weretestedin a 310m longhabitatcorridorconnectingtwohabitatpatches.Detailed
observationsof movementsweremade by meansof radiotelemetry and recordingof
footprints.
in termsoftransference
2. The highestconnectivity, rateofindividualsin thecorridor
system,was observedin theintermediate ofthreecorridorwidthstested(3 m, 1m and
0 4 m).
3. The behaviouralmechanismbehindthe lowerconnectivity of the narrowestcor-
ridorwas a reluctanceofvoles to enterit,whilelinearprogressin thewidestcorridor
was hamperedby a highfrequency of cross-directional
movements.
betweencorridorwidthand movementbehaviourwas unaffected
4. The relationship
by thesimulatedpresenceof competitors and predators.
5. Our resultschallengethe 'the-wider-the-better'principleof movementcorridor
design,and provideelementsforan understanding of the behaviouralmechanisms
underlying themovementecologyof individualsin linearhabitats.
Key-words: corridordesign,Microtus,movementecology.
connectivity,
JournalofAppliedEcology(1996) 33, 63-70

Plummer1993). As veryfewstudieshave been able


Introduction
to obtaindata about animalmovementsin corridors
Habitatfragmentation hasbeenrecognized as a major (Hobbs 1992),theempiricalbasis forcorridordesign
threat
towildlife populations(Diamond1976;Gilpin is poor (Simberloff&Cox 1987;Simberloff etal. 1992;
& Diamond1980;Higgs& Usher1980;Soule 1986; Mann & Plummer1993). For instance,theeffectsof
Lande 1988;Ims & Stenseth1989).As continuous structuralaspects of habitatcorridors,such as cor-
habitatsbecomefragmented, an immediate conse- ridorwidthand continuity,on movementrates are
quenceis thatthemobility of organisms becomes poorly known. Still, specificrecommendationson
restricted(Fahrig& Merriam1985; Stampset al. optimalcorridordesignmaybe foundin theliterature
1987a,b;Fahrig& Paleheimo1988;Burkey1989). (Noss 1987; Harrison 1992; Merriam & Saunders
Thisin turndecreasestheeffective size,and,conse- 1993). The urgentneed forempiricalstudiesaddress-
quently,the viabilityof populations(Soule 1986; ing basic questionsregardingmovementecology of
Boyce1992).It has beenclaimedthatthenegative individuals in linear habitats, has recentlybeen
effectsof habitatfragmentation can be reducedby stressed(see Hobbs 1992; Inglis& Underwood 1992;
connecting isolatedfragments by narrowstripsof Simberloffet al. 1992;Lindenmayer& Nix 1993).
habitat,termed movement corridors(Wilson& Willis We herepresentdata froma studywheremovement
1975;Harris1984;Bennett 1990;Saunders& Hobbs behaviour of the root vole Microtus oeconomus
1991).In thepresent paperwewilladheretothestrict (Pallas) in corridorsofvaryingwidthwas subjectto a
definitionof movement corridorsas linearhabitats detailedanalysis.We used corridorwidthas themain
allowingformovements, butnotforpermanent settle- treatmentvariable,because it is themostbasic struc-
ment. turalcharacteristicoflinearhabitatsuponwhichprac-
The establishment of movement corridors is cur- tical corridordesign inevitablyrequiresa decision.
? 1996British rentlyimplemented as an expensiveconservation Root voles were selectedas the studyorganismsas
Ecological Society strategyworld-wide etal. 1992;Mann&
(Simberloff theyhave proved to be veryusefulmodels forfield

63

This content downloaded from 139.133.69.143 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 08:43:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
64 experiments involvinghabitatmanipulations(Ims & movementsacross footprintplates (see below). The
Optimalwidthof Stenseth1989; Ims et al. 1993; Wiens et al. 1993). area betweenthefencesand thecorridorwas cleared
movement Moreover,althoughwe primarily designedthisstudy of vegetativecover by means of herbicides(Fig. 1).
corridors to serveas an empiricalmodel system(sensu Ims & The fenceswere establishedto hinderthe intrusion
Stenseth1989) foraddressingbasic biologicalmech- of othervoles and mammalianpredatorsduringthe
anisms that impingeon corridordesign(e.g. move- experiment.We expectedthatthe absence of female
mentbehaviour),our resultsmay have some direct voles should motivateexploratorymovement(Ims
implicationsfortheconservationofremnantrootvole 1988) or breedingdispersal (Kawata 1989) in the
populationsin Europe (van Apeldoornet al. 1992). experimentalmales. To ensure that the olfactorial
We showthatoptimalwidthofmovementcorridors environment in thecorridorat thestartof theexperi-
in termsof habitatconnectivity for root voles is a mentwas not verydifferent to thesituationlateron,
compromisebetweennot too narrow and not too some adult males were temporarilyreleased in the
wide. Hence, our resultschallengethe commonpre- corridora fewdays beforethefirstexperimental trial
sumptionthat widercorridorsnecessarilyare better was executed.
corridors(Noss 1987; Harrison 1992; Merriam & We testedthreedifferent corridorwidthsin com-
Saunders 1993). binationwithabsence/presence ofpredatorsand com-
petitors(Table 1), whichalso may affectmovements
(Saunders & Hobbs 1991). The studystartedwith
Methods
trialsin thewidestcorridor(3 m). The two narrower
corridors(I m and 0 4 m) weremade subsequentlyby
STUDY PLOT AND MANIPULATIONS
mowingand herbicideuse (see timescheduleforthe
The studytookplace at EvenstadField Station,south- varioustreatments in Table 1).
eastNorwayduringAugust-October1992.The move- Presenceof competitorsand predatorsweresimu-
ment behaviourof individualmale root voles was lated by six 'barriercages' whichwere permanently
studiedin a fencedsystemconsistingof two habitat situatedacross thecorridorat fixedintervals(Fig. 1).
patchesconnectedby a 310m long corridor(Fig. 1). The barriercages were made of wire mesh with a
The lengthof the corridorwas about one order of centralpassage tunnelencompassingthemid-runway
magnitudelonger than the diameterof an average of the corridor.The lengthof the barriercages was
male rootvole homerangein non-linearhabitats(Ims adjustedaccordingto thedecreasingwidthofthecor-
et al. 1993,and unpublished). ridor.For the treatment'competitor'(Table 1), one
The twohabitatpatchesused as releasepoints(size: live adult male was keptin each barriercage. Earlier
5 x 5 m) and thecorridorconsistedof dense,homo- studiesin which wire mesh cages of a similartype
geneous meadow vegetation,knownto be preferred have been used to simulatepresenceof conspecific
habitatby root voles (Ims et al. 1993). An artificial individualsin microtineshave produced significant
runwaysystem(Fig. 1) was establishedas 0 1 m wide responses(Ims 1988, 1990; Nelson 1994). For the
vegetation-freepathsalong and acrossthecorridorto treatment 'predator',the barriercages weresupplied
facilitatemovements in theotherwiseverydensegrass withfreshfoxscatssealed in small,perforatedplastic
carpet: one 310m long mid-runwayand cross-run- boxes. Simulatedpresenceof predatorsby artificial
waysat 10-mintervals.Theserunwaystendedto direct additionof predatorscentshave earlierbeen shown

15m 30m 60m lOOm 60m 30m 15m

I i Iiu
Fence 0 Barriercage
- Vegetation - - - Runwaysystem
* Release point A Trackingplate

Fig.1. Designofthestudy plot.Thetwohabitat andthecorridor


patches consistedofdense,homogeneous meadow vegetation
(shadedarea).Theareaoutsidethecorridor (whiteareainsidethefence)consisted ofbareground.Theenlarged sectionof
? 1996 British thecorridor (inset)showsthedesignoftheartificial
runway releasepointandthepositioning
system, platesinthe
oftracking
Ecological Society, 3 mwidecorridor. Runways as 0 1mvegetation-free
wereestablished pathsalong(mid-runway) andacrossthecorridor
(cross-
JournalofApplied runways at 10-mintervals).Trackingplateswereplacedin bothtypesofrunways forthe0-4m
(exceptin thecross-runways
Ecology,33, 63-70 corridor)as wellas alongtheedgesofthecorridor.

This content downloaded from 139.133.69.143 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 08:43:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
65 Table 1. Experimentalprotocol:Competitor,otheradultmalesin thebarriercages (Fig. 1); Predator,foxscatsin thebarrier
H. P. Andreassen, cages; Control,emptybarriercages. Ten different
male rootvoles wereused in each of the9 treatmenttypes
S. Halle & Numberof samplerecords
R. A. Ims
Treatment Period* Radiotrackingt Footprintst

Width,3 m
Control 1-9 Aug 10 5
Predator 10-16 Aug 10 2
Competitor 18-26 Aug 9 5
Width,1m
Competitor 26 Aug-i Sep 10 5
Control 2-7 Sep 10 5
Predator 8-15 Sep 10 4
Width,0-4m
Predator 19-23 Sep 10 5
Competitor 24-8 Sep 9 4
Control 29 Sept-3 Oct 10 4

* The variablelengthsof the experimentalperiods(e.g. periods > 5 days) werecaused by instancesin whichindividual
malesin a trialwerenotcaughtbefore08.00h thenextday.
t Reductionin thesamplesize (from10 to 9) of radiotracking
recordsfortwoof thetreatments werecaused bydysfunction
of radiotransmitters.
t The reductionsin thenumberoffootprint recordsfroma maximumoffive(= numberofexperimental dayspertreatment)
forfourtreatment typeswerecaused by heavyrainfallsmakingfootprint platesunreadable.

to affectspace use in microtines(Jedrzejewskiet al. (1977). Devices for recordingfootprints(tracking


1993). For the 'control'treatment,the barriercages plates) were situatedalong the corridoredges and
were leftempty.Beforethe onset of each treatment, in the mid-runway and the cross-runways insidethe
the barriercages were carefullywashed to remove corridor(Fig. 1). The use of corridoredges and the
remainingscent fromthe previoustreatment.Fur- two runwaytypesby voles werescoredaccordingto
thermore,the cages were always placed on plastic thenumberofplateswithfootprints at theend ofeach
sheetsto preventthesoil beneaththecages becoming trial.Footprintcountswererecordedperexperimental
contaminatedby scent. day ratherthan per individualsince footprintsof a
pair of individualsreleasedsimultaneously could not
be distinguished whenevertheyhad passed each other
TREATMENT AND MONITORING OF THE
inthecorridor.Radiotrackingshowedthatsuchcross-
STUDY ANIMALS
ing occurredin at least 29% of the trials.Due to
On each day,tworadiocollared(Biotrack,SS-2 trans- the generallow sample size of footprintrecordsand
mitters),adult male voles werereleasedat 08.00h in missingdata, leadingto unbalanceddesigns(Table 1),
the two habitatpatchesin the opposite ends of the statisticaltestsofcompetitor/predator treatmentwere
corridor(one male per patch, cf. Fig 1). Each trial not appliedto thefootprint data.
was terminated at 18.00h. Thereafter,
themaleswere The males used in thisstudywerelaboratoryborn
caughtbymeansoflivetrapsactivatedinthecorridor. F2-F4 generationdescendantsof animalscaughtin
Trials weregenerallyconductedon consecutivedays, Pasvik, north-eastNorway. We chose to use lab-
provided that males from the previous trial were oratory-raised animalsto standardizepriorexposure
caughtbefore08.00h thenextmorning. to factorswhich mighthave influencedmovement
Longitudinalmovements inthecorridorweremoni- behaviour.Males werethechosensexas theyaremore
toredby locatingthe males (to the nearestmetrein suitable'models' thanfemalesforthistypeof study;
the longitudinaldirectionof the corridor)by means theyappear to be more motivatedto disperse(Ims
of a probe antenna(Andreassenet al. 1993) at every 1989;Kawata 1989),and theyare lesssubjectto short-
fullhour from09.00h to 17.00h. The resultingnine termbehaviouralchanges due to reproductivehor-
radio-trackingpositionsobtainedpermale wereused mone cyclesthatwould have introducedextraneous
to estimatethe distancemoved,the speed of move- variancein thedata.
mentand thetimespentin thecorridorforeach male Beforethetrialsin thecorridor,maleswerehoused
(forfurtherdescriptionoflongitudinalmovement par- separatelyin laboratorycages exposed to constant
? 1996British
Ecological Society, ameters,see footnotesto Table 2). light,to mimicthe natural lightregimeduringthe
JournalofApplied Cross-directionaluse of thecorridorwas registered summerin north-east Norway,as wellas to avoid any
Ecology,33, 63-70 bythefootprint recordingtechniqueofKing & Edgar effectsof changingphotoperiodin the course of the

This content downloaded from 139.133.69.143 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 08:43:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
66 Table 2. The effectof corridorwidthon movementand corridoruse parameters.Data are lumpedforthe treatment
factor
Optimalwidthof absence/presenceofcompetitor/predator.Values are presentedas mean +-standard errorof themean
movement Corridorwidth
corridors Parameters 3m Im 0 4m

Longitudinalmovements*
Maximumdistancereached(m) 75 + 16 205 + 22 35 + 15
Cumulativedistancetraversed(m) 163 + 33 440 + 57 51 + 21
Movementspeed/efficiencyt
Time spentin corridor(h) 79+ 0't 59 + 04 3-9 + 11
Maximumspeed (m/h) 61 + 11 160 + 19 93 + 24
Averagespeed (m/h) 21 + 4 76 + 11 51 + 13
Progressrate(m/h) 10 + 2 40 + 10 35 + 10
Cross-directional
movementst(footprint
counts)
Edge counts 1 42 + 0 51 0 50 + 0 20 0 21 + 0 11
Edge/mid-runway ratio 0 23 + 0-09 0 03 + 0-01 0 09 + 0-07
ratio
Cross-runway/mid-runway 1 56 + 0 31 0-36+ 0 05

* Longitudinalmovementparametersare based on the completedata set of radiotrackingrecords(Table 1). Maximum


distancereached= maximumdistancereachedfromreleasepatch.Cumulativedistancetraversed= cumulativemovedduring
thetrialas revealedby hourlyradiotracking.
t Movementspeed/efficiency parametersincludeonlytrialsin whichmales enteredthecorridor(N3.. = 26, NJ...= 30 and
No 4, = 8). Time spentin corridor= totaltimeperioda male was foundin thecorridor.Maximumspeed = longestdistance
movedduringone hour.Averagespeed = totaldistance/time spentin corridor.Progressrate = maximumdistance/time spent
in corridor.
t Cross-directional
use parametersare based on footprint countrecords(Table 1) fromthefootprint platessituatedalong
thecorridoredges (edge counts),in themid-runway and thecross-runways (Fig. 1). No estimateof theratiovariable(cross-
runway/mid-runway), is givenforthe0 4 m corridor,sincefootprints
werenot recordedin thecross-runways forthiscorridor
width.

experiment.All males used were sexually mature, all parameterstestedwithrespectto thistreatment (all
adult animals(> 50 g and scrotaltestes),and thusof partialr2< 0 04, P > 0 05).
adequate size forthe2 5 g radiocollars. Voles generallymoved furthest and fastestin the
Twenty-six differentindividualswereavailable for intermediate width (1 m) corridor (Table 2).
thisstudy.Most ofthesehad thusto be usedin several Maximum distancesreached by voles in the inter-
trials(X = 2 6 + 0 1 [SE] trialsper male), but never mediatewidthcorridorwere on average threeand
more than once per treatment combination.Gener- six timeslongerthan in the widerand the narrower
ally,10 differentmalesweretestedpertreatment com- corridors,respectively. In the intermediatecorridor
bination(Table 1). Males used in morethanone treat- morethanone-thirdofthevolesreachedtheopposite
ment were released in alternatingpatches with the end, while this rarelyhappened in the wideror the
longestpossible inter-trial intervalto avoid habitu- narrowercorridor(Fig. 2).
ation to thestudyregime.Numberof trialsper male, The low successof the narrowestcorridorin con-
when included as a covariate in ANCOVA-models, nectingthe habitatpatcheswas caused by reluctance
neverexplainedmorethan4% ofvariance(P > 0 50) of voles to enterit (Fig. 2); the narrowestcorridor
of any of themovementindicesestimated(Table 2). (04 m) was enteredonly in 27% of the trials,while
Thisresultindicatesthatneitherhabituationnorother the two widercorridorswereenteredin themajority
possibleconfoundingtimeeffects, e.g. scentaccumu- of thetrials(100% and 83% entranceratein the 1m
lation (see also Discussion) were importantin this and the 3m corridor, respectively)(X2= 40.3,
study.Males partneredin trialsinfrequently crossed d.f. = 2, P < 0 001). This apparentaversionby voles
each otherin the corridor(29% of all trials) so it to thenarrowestcorridoris corroboratedbytheshort
is unlikelythatdirectinterference betweenthe free- time spent in this corridorby voles that actually
rangingmalesinfluenced theresults.Hence,we findit enteredit; on average,males enteringthewidestcor-
justifiableto treateach trialpermale as thestatistical ridorspenttwiceas muchtimeinthecorridoras males
unit(cf.Table 1) in theanalyses. enteringthenarrowestcorridor(Table 2).
The speed of linear movementwas considerably
lowerformales in the 3 m wide corridorsystemthan
Results
in the two narrowersystems.For instance,the pro-
Corridorwidthhad a substantialeffecton all move- gressrate(expressedas maximumdistancereachedin
? 1996British
EcologicalSociety, mentparameters(Table 2) (two-wayANOVAS, all par- thecorridordividedby timespentin corridor)in the
JournalofApplied tialr2> 0 25, P < 0 001). Presence/absence
of pred- 1m wide corridorwas on averagefourtimeshigher
Ecology,33, 63-70 atorsand competitors wererelatively
unimportant for thanin the3 m widecorridor,whereasthisparameter

This content downloaded from 139.133.69.143 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 08:43:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
25 Corridorwidth:3m
67 questions about corridordesign,for which all the
20
H. P. Andreassen, requiredelementsof properexperimental designs(i.e.
15
S. Halle &
R.A.Ims
10 + randomization
independent
oftreatment
measurements)
combinationsas to ensure
are satisfied,
has recently
been discussedby Inglis& Underwood(1992).
Our studylacks some statisticalrigourof a proper
25 Corridorwidth:1m experiment in thesensethatwe werenot able to have
201. more than one corridorsystem.To obtain proper
15.,
randomizationof treatmentcombinationswith the
10 + same sample size would have required45 corridor
5 F 171 systems.The present study should thereforebe
regardedas a thoroughlycontrolled,manipulative,
251+ observationalstudyratherthana properexperiment.
Corridorwidth:0-4m
20O. However,we are not able to imagineconfounding
15fl variables that could have produced the very pro-
nounced patternof our results.With respectto the
iof
101 most obvious confoundingfactorof the study,i.e.
time,our resultsare not compatiblewitha consistent
timebias, e.g. broughtabout by an accumulationof
0 60 120 180 240 300
scentthroughtime.Such a bias shouldhavegenerated
Fig.2. Frequencydistribution of maximumdistances
reached(20-mintervals)fromthereleasepoint(see Fig.1) a generaldecreaseor increasein movementestimates
forthethreetested widths.
corridor Blackbarsattheendof bothwithinand betweenthetreatments.
thecorridor givethenumber oftimesmalesdid notleave Our resultscorroboratethreeearlierstudiesshow-
theirreleasepatchor reachedtheoppositepatch,respec- ing that structuralaspects of corridorsaffectmove-
Arrows
tively. denotethemediansofthethreedistributions. mentratesin smallmammals(Lorenz& Barrett1990;
Merriam& Lanoue 1990; La Polla & Barrett1993).
However,our data do not lend supportto thenotion
did not differ betweenthe 1m and 0 4 m
significantly thatwidercorridorsnecessarilyconnecthabitatpat-
corridor(Table 2). chesmoreefficiently thannarrowcorridors(e.g. Har-
The footprint recordsindicatedan increaseduse of rison1992).Of threecorridorwidthstested,theinter-
corridoredges(edge counts;Table 2) withincreasing mediate width stimulatedthe highestfrequencyof
corridorwidth.The two ratios'edge counts/mid-run- long-distance movements. Similar results have
way counts' and 'cross-runwaycounts/mid-runway recentlybeen obtained for meadow voles Microtus
counts', whichboth may serveas indicesfor cross- pennsylvanicus (La Polla & Barrett1993)undermore
directionaluse of the corridor,had highestmean natural,butlesscontrolledconditions.In themeadow
values in the 3 m wide corridor(Table 2). This indi- vole study,therewereless movementsin 5 m thanin
cates that therewere more cross-directional move- 1m wide corridorsconnectinghabitatpatches with
mentsin thewidestcorridorthanin thetwonarrower free-ranging subpopulationsof voles. Whereas the
corridors. meadowvole corridorswereonly10m long(La Polla
& Barrett 1993) and probably supported mainly
within-homerange movements,the long-distance
Discussion
movementsin our 310m long corridorare probably
Many observationalstudieshave indicatedthatlinear morerepresentative forexploratorymovementsdur-
structuresin fragmentedlandscapes may play an ing dispersal(sensuStenseth& Lidicker1992). Typi-
importantrole as movementcorridorsand hence, cally exploratorymovements,e.g. duringdispersal
influencethe abundance and spatial structuring of events,take place on groundwhichis novel to the
fragmentedpopulations (e.g. Wegner & Merriam movinganimal(thisstudy),whilewithin-home-range
1979; Middleton & Merriam 1983; Hansson 1987; movements(the studyof La Polla & Barrett1993)
Szacki 1987;Verboom& van Apeldoorn1990;Zhang are normallybased on spatial memory(Ims 1994).
& Usher 1991; Fitzgibbon1993). However,veryfew However,therelationship betweencorridorwidthand
studieshave assessed movementratesof individuals movementrate was the same for the two Microtus
as functionsofqualitativeand quantitativeaspectsof species, despite the fact that theywere studied in
such corridors,whichin turncould serveas empiri- different settings,presumablyperformingdifferent
cally based guidelinesfor design of movementcor- movementtypes at different spatial scales (Wiens
ridorsforconservationpurposes.Preferentially, such 1989; Johnsonet al. 1992). This bears promisefor
guidelinesshould stemfromexperimental studiesin generalizations.The potential robustness of our
? 1996British
Ecological Society, which the causal relationship between corridor resultsis further emphasizedby thefactthatthegen-
JournalofApplied characteristics
and connectivitycould be established. eral relationshipbetweencorridorwidthand move-
Ecology,33, 63-70 The greatdifficulties
of conductingstudiesaddressing mentbehaviourin M. ceconomuswas notchangedby

This content downloaded from 139.133.69.143 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 08:43:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
68 circumstances suchas simulatedpresenceofpredators mortality rateand corridorwidth.Movementsin nar-
Optimalwidthof or competitorsin the corridor.The apparentunim- rowcorridorsarebelievedto be moreriskythanmove-
movement portanceof thesetwo potentiallymodifyingfactors mentsin wide corridorsdue to a higherinfluenceof
corridors mighthave been due to a generalfailureto simulate variousedge effects,such as predation(Simberloff&
predators and competitorsadequately. However, Cox 1987; Noss 1987; Andren & Angelstam1988;
similarmethodsused in earlierstudies(althoughnot Henein & Merriam 1990; Saunders & Hobbs 1991;
conductedincorridors)producedclearresponses(Ims Harrison 1992; Merriam & Saunders 1993; Paton
1988,1990;Jedrzejewski et al. 1993;Nelson 1994). 1994). The voles' reluctanceto enterthe narrowest
The detailedobservationsmade duringthepresent bya low entrancerate;
corridorin our study(reflected
studysuggestseveral behaviouralmechanismsthat 27%), may reflectrisk-aversionbehaviour (McNa-
may account for the variationin connectivity with mara & Houston 1987). However, our resultsalso
corridorwidth.The low efficiency of the narrowest pointto thepossibilityof a higheraccumulatedmor-
corridorin transferringvolesbetweenhabitatpatches talityrisk in wide corridorssince voles spentmore
seemedto be caused by a behaviouralaversionbythe timein the widestcorridor,and since theyused the
voles to enterthecorridor(cf. the low entrancerate corridoredges more intensively (cf. numberof edge
in the 04 m corridor;Fig. 2). Moreover,voles that designedto mea-
counts;Table 2). Studiesspecifically
enteredthe corridorspent less time in the narrow surehow mortality rateschangewithcorridorwidth,
and theintermediate corridorcomparedto thewidest will be an importantnextstep towarda knowledge
corridor(Table 2). For the intermediate widthcorri- about whichcharacteristics may determinewhether
dor, however,the short residencytime probably corridorsfunctionas links or sinks in a landscape
reflectsthehighlinearmovementrateratherthanany context.
aversionbehaviour(cf. 100% entranceratein the 1m
wide corridor;Fig. 2). The voles frequently reached
theoppositeend quicklywhentestedin the 1m wide
corridor.The low linearmovementspeedin thewidest Conclusion
corridor,despitea relativelyhighentrancerate(83%), We have shownthatthe efficiency of linearhabitats
may have been caused by a highfrequencyof cross- as corridorsconnectingisolated habitat patches is
directional(zig-zag) movements.This was indicated dependenton theirwidth,but in a differentmanner
by the highproportionateuse of cross-runways and thanoftensupposed.Furthermore, we have identified
edgesin the3 m widecorridor(cf. theratio-variables what may be importantbehavioural mechanisms
based on footprint counts;Table 2). Zig-zaggingmay underlyingmovementpatternsin linearhabitats.
be a movementmode which is commonlyused by
animals in the absence of physicalorientationcues
(Bell 1991; Ims 1994). In movementcorridors,the
edgesare likelyto functionas themaincue facilitating Acknowledgments
directionalmovements. Whileanimalsmovingin nar- OttarBj0rnstad,JorunFauske, Barbara Halle, Edda
rowcorridorsmay be able to perceivetheedgeseven Johannesen,Piotr Pawlak, Gytis Racius and Anna-
fromtheinteriorof thecorridor,zig-zagmovements BarbaraUtelliparticipated duringthefieldwork.Bar-
may be necessaryin widerlinearstripsof habitatto bara Halle computerizedthe data. Ottar Bj0rnstad,
obtainfrequent visualcontactswiththeedges.In fact, Gary Fry,LennartHansson,Karine Hertzberg,Har-
animalswillprobablynotperceivea linearpatchas a ald Steen and Nils Chr. Stensethprovidedvaluable
movementcorridorabove a givenwidth,but as an commentsto draftsof the manuscript.This studyis
elongatedhabitat(see also La Polla & Barrett1993). part of the project 'The effectof habitatcorridors
It has recently
beenemphasizedthatgeneralprinciples on biodiversityin agriculturallandscapes' which is
in landscape ecology will most likelyemergefrom supportedfinancially bytheResearchCouncilofNor-
detailed studies unravellingthe behaviouralmech- way (NFR).
anismsinvolvedin the movementecologyof species
(Johnsonet al. 1992;Wienset al. 1993;Ims 1994). In
particular,we believe that this argumentapplies to
the generationof principlesof movementcorridor References
functionand design.
Andreassen.,Ims, R.A, Stenseth,N.C. & Yoccoz, N.G.
Mortalityratesof animalsin movementcorridors (1993) Investigatingspace use by meansof radiotelemetry
have neverbeenestimated,althoughitis an extremely and othermethods:a methodologicalguide. The Biology
importantfactordetermining whethercorridorsfunc- of Lemmings(eds N. C. Stenseth& R. A. Ims), pp. 589-
tion as links or sinks (sensu Pulliam 1988) in the 618. AcademicPress,London.
dynamicsof fragmented populations(Fahrig& Mer- Andren,H. & Angelstam,P. (1988) Elevatedpredationrates
? 1996British as an edgeeffectin habitatislands:experimentalevidence.
EcologicalSociety, riam1985;Henein& Merriam1990).Still,onepremise Ecology,69,344-547.
JournalofApplied underlying the presumptionthatwidercorridorsare Bell, W.J. (1991) SearchingBehaviour.Chapman & Hall,
Ecology,33, 63-70 bettercorridorsis an inverserelationshipbetween London.

This content downloaded from 139.133.69.143 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 08:43:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
69 Bennett,A.F. (1990) Habitatcorridorsand theconservation backed vole Clethrionomys rufocanusbedfordiae.Oikos,
H. P. Andreassen, of small mammalsin a fragmented forestenvironment. 54, 220-226.
LandscapeEcology,4, 109-122. King, C.M. & Edgar, R.I. (1977) Techniquesfortrapping
S. Halle & Boyce, M.S. (1992) Population viabilityanalysis. Annual and trackingstoats (Mustela erminea);a review,and a
R.A.Ims Reviewsin Ecologyand Systematics,23, 481-506. new system.New Zealand Journalof Zoology, 4, 193-
Burkey,T. (1989) Extinctionin naturereserves.The effect of 212.
fragmentation and the importanceof migrationbetween La Polla, V.N. & Barrett,G.W. (1993) Effectsof corridor
reservefragments. Oikos,55, 75-81. widthand presenceon the populationdynamicsof the
Diamond, J.M. (1976) Island biogeography and con- meadow vole (Microtuspennsylvanicus). LandscapeEcol-
servation:Strategyand limitations.Science, 193, 1027- ogy,8, 25-37.
1029. Lande, R. (1988) Genetics and demographyin biological
Fahrig,L. & Merriam,G. (1985) Habitatpatchconnectivity conservation.Science,241, 1455-1460.
and populationsurvival.Ecology,66, 1762-1768. Lindenmayer, D.B. & Nix, H.A. (1993) Ecologicalprinciples
Fahrig,L. & Paleheimo,J. (1988) Effectof spatial arrange- forthedesignof corridors.Conservation Biology,7, 627-
mentofhabitatpatcheson local populationsize. Ecology, 630.
69, 468-475. Lorenz,G.C. & Barrett,G.W. (1990) Influenceof simulated
Fitzgibbon,C.D. (1993) The distributionof greysquirrel landscapecorridorson house mouse (Mus musculus)dis-
dreysin farmwoodland: the influenceof wood area, iso- persal.AmericanMidlandNaturalist,123,348-356.
lation and management.Journalof AppliedEcology,30, Mann, C.C. & Plummer,M. (1993) The highcosts of biodi-
736-742. versity.Science,160, 1868-1871.
Gilpin,M.E. & Diamond, J. (1980) Subdivisionof nature McNamara, J.M. & Houston, A.I. (1987) Starvationand
reservesand themaintenanceof speciesdiversity. Nature, predationas a factorslimitingpopulationsize. Ecology,
285, 567-568. 68, 1515-1519.
Hansson,L. (1987) Dispersalroutesofsmallmammalsat an Merriam,G. & Lanoue, A. (1990) Corridoruse by small
abandonedfieldin centralSweden.HolarcticEcology,10, mammals:fieldmeasurements forthreeexperimental types
154-159. of Peromyscusleucopus.LandscapeEcology,4, 123-131.
Harris, L.D. (1984) The FragmentedForest: Island Bio- Merriam,G. & Saunders,D. (1993) Corridorsin restoration
graphicTheoryAnd The Preservation Of BioticDiversity. of fragmented landscapes.NatureConservation 3: Recon-
University of Chicago Press,Chicago. struction Of Fragmented Ecosystems(eds D. Saunders,R.
Harrison,R.L. (1992) Towards a theoryof inter-refuge cor- J.Hobbs & P. Erlich).SurreyBeatty& Sons, Australia.
ridordesign.Conservation Biology,6, 293-295. Middleton,J. & Merriam,G. (1983) Distributionof wood-
Henein,K.M. & Merriam,G. (1990) The elementof con- land speciesin farmlandwoods. Journalof AppliedEcol-
nectivitywhere corridorquality is variable. Landscape ogy,20, 625-644.
Ecology,4, 157-170. Nelson,J.(1994) Spacingbehaviour, matingsystem, and indi-
Higgs,A.J.& Usher,M.B. (1980) Should naturereservesbe vidualreproductive successin malefieldvoles.PhD thesis,
largeor small?Nature,285, 568-569. University of Lund, Sweden.
Hobbs, R.J. (1992) The role of corridorsin conservation: Noss, R. (1987) Corridorsin real landscapes: a replyto
Solutionor bandwagon?TrendsinEcologyand Evolution, Simberloff and Cox. Conservation Biology,1, 159-164.
7, 389-392. Paton,P.W. (1994) The effect ofedgeon aviannestsuccesses:
Ims, R.A. (1988) Spatial clumpingof sexually receptive How strongis theevidence?Conservation Biology,8, 17-
femalesinducesspace sharingamongmale voles. Nature, 26.
335, 541-543. Pulliam,H.R. (1988) Sources,sinks and population regu-
Ims,R.A. (1989) Kinshipand origineffects on dispersaland lation.AmericanNaturalist,132,652-661.
space sharingin Clethrionomys rufocanus.Ecology,70, Saunders, D.A. & Hobbs, R.J. (eds)(1991) Nature Con-
607-616. servation2: The Role Of Corridors.SurreyBeattyand
Ims, R.A. (1990) Mate detection success of male Cle- Sons, Australia.
thrionomys rufocanusin relationto thespatialdistribution Simberloff, D., Farr,J.A.,Cox, J.& Mehlman,D.W. (1992)
of sexuallyreceptivefemales.Evolutionary Ecology,4, 57- Movement corridors: conservationbargains or poor
61. investment. Conservation Biology,6, 493-504.
Ims, R.A. (1995) Movementpatternsin relationto spatial Simberloff, D. & Cox, J. (1987) Consequencesand costs of
structures.Mosaic Landscapes and Ecological Processes conservationcorridors.Conservation Biology,1, 63-71.
(eds L. Hansson, L. Fahrig& G. Merriam).Chapman & Soule, M. (1986) ConservationBiology. Sinauer, Mas-
Hall, London. sachusetts.
Ims,R.A. & Stenseth,N.C. (1989) Divided thefruitflies fall. Stamps,J.A, Buechner,M & Krishnan,V.V. (1987a) The
Nature,342, 21-22. effectsof edge permeability and habitatgeometry on emi-
Ims, R.A., Rolstad,J. & Wegge,P. (1993) Predictingspace grationfrompatchesofhabitat.AmericanNaturalist,129,
use responsesto habitatfragmentation: can volesMicrotus 533-552.
oeconomusserveas an experimental model system(EMS) Stamps,J.A, Buechner,M & Krishnan,V.V. (1987b) The
forcapercailliegrouse Tetraourogallusin boreal forest.? effectof habitat geometryon territorialdefencecosts,
BiologicalConservation, 63, 261-268. intruderpressure in bounded populations. American
Inglis,G. & Underwood,A.J. (1992) Commentson some Zoologist,27, 307-325.
designsproposedforexperiments on thebiologicalimport- Stenseth,N.C. & Lidicker,W.Z. (1992) AnimalDispersal.
ance ofcorridors.Conservation Biology,6, 581-586. Small Mammalsas a Model. Chapman& Hall, London.
Jedrzejewski, W., Rychlic,L. & Jedrzelewska,B. (1993) Szacki,J.(1987) Ecologicalcorridorsas a factordetermining
Responses of bank voles to odours of seven species of thestructure and organisationof a bank vole population.
predators,experimental data and theirrelevanceto natural Acta theriologica,32, 31-44.
predator-volerelationships. Qikos,68, 251-257. van Apeldoorn,R., Hollander,H., Nieuwenhuizen,W. &
? 1996British Johnson,A.R., Milne, B.T. & Wiens,J.A. (1992) Diffusion Vliet,F. van der(1992) De Noordsewoelmuisin hetDelta-
Ecological Society, in fractallandscapes:simulationsand experimental studies gebied.Is er een relatietussenhabitatfragmentatie en con-
JournalofApplied of tenebrionid beetlemovements.Ecology,73, 1968-1983. currentieop de schaal van het landschap?Landschap,9,
Ecology,33, 63-70 Kawata, M. (1989) Growthand dispersaltimingin malered- 189-202.

This content downloaded from 139.133.69.143 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 08:43:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
70 Verboom,B. & van Apeldoorn,R. (1990) Effectof habitat Wilson,E.O. & Willis,E.O. (1975) Applied biogeogral
Optimalwidthof fragmentation on the red squirrel,Sciurus vulgarisL. Ecologyand theEvolutionofCommunities (eds M. L. C
LandscapeEcology,4, 171-176. & J.M. Diamond),pp. 522-536.HarvardUniversity PI
movement Wegner,J.F. & Merriam,G. (1979) Movementof birdsand Massachusetts.
corridors small mammalsbetweenwood and adjoining farmland Zhang,Z. & Usher,M.B. (1991) Dispersalofwood mice,
habitats.JournalofAppliedEcology,16, 349-358. bank volesin an agricultural
landscape.Acta theriolog
Wiens,J.A.(1989) Spatialscalinginlandscapeecology.Func- 36, 239-245.
tionalEcology,3, 386-397.
Wiens, J.A., Stenseth,N.C., Horne, B. Van & Ims, R.A.
(1993) Ecological mechanismsand landscape ecology.
Oikos,66, 369-380. Received27 April1994; revisionreceived21 October199<

? 1996British
Ecological Society,
JournalofApplied
Ecology,33, 63-70

This content downloaded from 139.133.69.143 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 08:43:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like