You are on page 1of 4

psy 08_11 p586_589 hackley3_Layout 1 18/07/2011 17:07 Page 586

ARTICLE

improved through the use of training


software. 11,430 participants (aged 18-
60) completed the study. Benchmark
assessments conducted before and after
the training programme measured
Coach your cortex reasoning, verbal short-term memory,
spatial working memory and paired-
associated learning. Significantly
Is brain training a sales con or evidence-based exercise? improved performance was seen across
Dan Hackley investigates all the training tasks, but there was no
significant improvement on the untrained
before-and-after assessments. In other
Recent years have seen a surge he all-time best-selling video game words, participants improved at the
in the popularity of brain training
software; games and applications
that purport to stimulate the brain
T in the UK, selling over 17 million
copies worldwide, is called Dr
Kawashimas Brain Training. Developed
specific tasks, but there was no evidence
for generalised improvement in cognitive
function following training, even for tasks
and improve cognitive by Nintendo, it incorporates 11 different which involve similar cognitive functions
performance. Sales are nourished cognitive exercises, including sudoku, to those trained.
by the assumption that cognitive reading aloud, a Stroop test, word memory, This concept is central to most of
abilities can be maintained or speed counting and connecting a maze. the research on cognitive training; the
improved by exercising the brain, The idea is to play regularly and improve distinction between improved performance
by analogy with the way physical your brain score. Nintendo claim that on specifically trained tasks versus a more
fitness is improved by exercising their game was inspired by the work of generalised improvement in cognitive
the body. But is there any evidence Dr Ryuta Kawashima (a Japanese function. As we shall see, this issue of
to substantiate these claims, or is neuroscientist), and use non-scientific generalisability remains a problem for
this a marketing ploy riding on the marketing language to draw analogies researchers intending to demonstrate the
popular wave of self-improvement? to physical exercise, though they have efficacy of various cognitive interventions.
This article aims to examine the avoided claiming that the game has been Although this was a large and
available evidence for the efficacy scientifically validated. substantial study, it has drawn criticism
of software claiming to enhance Similar games have been released on due to high dropout rates, questionable
general cognitive function, and in a variety of hardware platforms, including outcome measures and selection bias
turn allow us to provide the Apple iPhone (Brain Challenge, Brain (Almond, 2010; Zelinski, 2010). It does,
appropriate evidence-based advice Exercise with Dr Kawashima, Brain Toot), however, provide data on a demographic
to patients. PlayStation Portable (Mind Quiz) and PC group that has otherwise received little
(Brain Train Age). There are also a number attention in studies of computerised
of online examples, including cognitive training healthy younger
www.lumosity.com, www.cognifit.com and adults. While there are a number of articles
www.sharpbrains.com. But do they work? looking at cognitive training in younger
Is it possible to improve on basic adults with affective disorders (Naismith
questions

cognitive functions such as attention, et al., 2010; Wolinksy et al., 2009),


concentration, memory, reasoning and Efficacy schizophrenia (Nemoto et al., 2009;
processing speed? This question of efficacy was recently Vinogradov et al., 2009), children with
addressed in a study organised through ADHD (Abikoff, 1991) and preschool
the BBC popular science television children (Thorell et al., 2009), the
programme Bang Goes the Theory. Their evidence base examining these
Brain Test Britain study (Owen et al., interventions in healthy 18- to 60-year-
resources

Owen, A.M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J.A. et 2010) was a six-week randomised olds remains very limited.
al. (2010). Putting brain training to the controlled trial where viewers of the The majority of the existing research
test. Nature, 465(7299), 775778.
www.sharpbrains.com programme were invited to practise in this field focuses on the healthy elderly,
a series of computer-based online tasks in the context of disease-modifying
three times a week in order to ascertain interventions to delay or slow progression
whether cognitive abilities could be to mild cognitive impairment or
references

Abikoff, H. (1991). Cognitive training in cognitive-decline-via-cognitive- Alzheimers disease. Experimental Fitness effects on the cognitive
ADHD children: Less to it than meets training Aging Research, 31(3), 261289. function of older adults; a meta-
the eye. Journal of Learning Ball, K., Berch, D.B., Helmers, K.F. et al. Clare, L., Woods, R.T., Moniz Cook, E.D., analytic study. Psychological Science,
Disabilities, 24(4), 205209. (2002). Effects of cognitive training et al. (2003). Cognitive rehabilitation 14(2), 125130.
Almond, N. (2010). Needed: funding for interventions with older adults. and cognitive training for early-stage Davis, R.N., Massman, P.J. & Doody, R.S.
innovative research on slowing Journal of the American Medical Alzheimer's disease and vascular (2001). Cognitive intervention in
cognitive decline via cognitive Association, 288(18), 22712281. dementia. Cochrane Database of Alzheimer disease: A randomized
training. Retrieved 22 August 2010 Cherry, K.E. & Simmons-DGerolamo, Systematic Reviews, Issue 4. Art. No.: placebo-controlled study. Alzheimer
from www.sharpbrains.com/blog/ S.S. (2005). Long-term effectiveness CD003260. doi: 10.1002/14651858. Disease & Associated Disorders, 15(1),
2010/08/09/needed-funding-for- of spaced-retrieval memory training CD003260. 19.
innovative-research-on-slowing- for older adults with probable Colcombe, S. & Kramer, A.F. (2003). Ernst, C., Olson, A.K., Pinel, J.P. et al.

586 vol 24 no 8 august 2011


psy 08_11 p586_589 hackley3_Layout 1 18/07/2011 17:07 Page 587

brain training

Alzheimers disease. Two of the largest based training programme (Brain Fitness though the authors acknowledge that the
studies conducted in this population are Program from Posit Science) was used. two larger trials dominate the analysis. In
the American ACTIVE and IMPACT This choice was based on the hypothesis their analysis, similar types of training in
studies. that in older adults, the quality of sensory the various studies were grouped together
The ACTIVE (Advanced Cognitive information processing may be linked with to reflect the categories used in the
Training for Independent and Vital cognitive ability. Forty training sessions ACTIVE study. Small effect sizes were
Elderly) study (Ball et al., 2002) was the were spread over eight weeks. The primary found for improvement in memory,
first multicentre, randomised controlled outcome also measured auditory-related reasoning, processing speed and
trial to examine both the immediate and performance (as part of the Repeatable multimodal training. They conclude that
long-term effects (up to five years post- Battery for the Assessment of training improves immediate performance
intervention) of cognitive training on daily Neuropsychological Status), though other on related tasks though overall found no
functioning and cognitive abilities in older tests were also used to circumvent any evidence for generalisability of training
adults. Participants were included if they potential ceiling effects. Significant and scant evidence for long-term effects
were over 65, living independently and improvement on the trained tasks was or delay of progression to dementia
possessed good found; and, unique (largely due to insufficient follow-up
cognitive status to this study, the periods). The authors do acknowledge
a score on the Mini improvement also that combining effect sizes across differing
Mental State generalised to most training regimens may have dampened
Examination of of the untrained the effect of gains in more specific tasks,
22/30. Three standardised a limitation which may have nullified the
intervention groups measures (effect findings of generalisability of the IMPACT
received 10 group size 0.23). No such study. Overall, more research is needed, in
sessions and four similar effect was particular of long-term benefits.
booster sessions of observed in the
memory, reasoning control group
or speed of who underwent Cognitive impairment
processing training, a learning-based Although not a demographic targeted
with a control training programme by most brain-training games, similar
group receiving no with similar studies have been carried out using
intervention. amounts of training participants with pre-existing diagnoses
Outcome measures time and computer of dementia or cognitive impairment.
included tests use to the Findings of these studies are similar to
closely related to experimental group. those conducted in the healthy elderly.
the training tasks, Although these results A number of studies report significant
general simulations Is exercising the brain analogous to are promising, the degree cognitive improvements, though these
of everyday tasks, the way physical fitness is improved of observed are generally domain-specific and without
and performance by exercising? generalisation would generalisation (Cherry & Simmons-
of activities of daily appear limited; all of the DGerolamo, 2005; Davis et al., 2001;
living and driving. measures were restricted Hoffman et al., 1996). A Cochrane
The authors conclude that performance to auditory memory and attention, the systematic review (Clare et al., 2003)
improved significantly on the measures study focused on memory-related tasks combined nine RCTs that compared
that were specifically trained: again, this as opposed to more general cognitive cognitive training with control conditions
improvement did not generalise to function, and the observed effect size was in early-stage Alzheimers. Despite a wide
measures of everyday function. small. diversity of outcome measures, no
In 2009 the IMPACT (Improvement Both of the above studies were significant positive effects of cognitive
in Memory with Plasticity-based Adaptive included in a recent meta-analysis (Papp et training were observed, though the
Cognitive Training) study (Smith et al., al., 2009) that aimed to review the studies evidence base was acknowledged as
2009) evaluated the effect of a published after 1992 on cognitive training limited. A more recent literature review
computerised cognitive training in the healthy elderly and to assess and (Yu et al., 2009) explored the effects of
programme in 487 adults over the age of compare the efficacy of different cognitive various non-pharmacological
65 with an MMSE score of 26 or greater. interventions. Eight smaller randomised interventions in early-stage dementia.
Unlike most previous studies, an audio- controlled trials were also included, Here, cognitive training was divided into

(2006). Antidepressant effects of 22(3), 612613. Medicine and Rehabilitation, 85, Journal of Psychiatric Research, 30(6),
exercise: Evidence for an adult- Green, C.S. & Bavelier, D. (2008). 19641704. 493501.
neurogenesis hypothesis? Journal of Exercising your brain: A review of Hillman C.H., Erickson, K.I. & Kramer, Kempermann, G., Gast, D. & Gage, F.H.
Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 31, 8492. human brain plasticity and training- A.F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your (2002). Neuroplasticity in old age:
Etnier, J.L., Salazar, W., Landers, D.M. et induced learning. Psychology and heart: Exercise effects on brain and Sustained fivefold induction of
al. (1997). The influence of physical Aging, 23(4), 692701. cognition. Nature Reviews hippocampal neurogenesis by long-
fitness and exercise upon cognitive Heyn, P., Abreu, B.C. & Ottenbacher, J.J. Neuroscience, 9(1), 5865. term environmental enrichment.
functioning. Journal of Sport & (2004). The effects of exercise Hoffman, M., Hock, C., Khler, A. & Annals of Neurology, 52, 135143.
Exercise Psychology, 19, 249277. training on elderly persons with Mller-Spahn, F. (1996). Interactive Mackrous, I. & Proteau, L. (2007).
Gage, F.H. (2002). Neurogenesis in the cognitive impairment and dementia: computer-based cognitive training in Specificity of practice results from
adult brain. Journal of Neuroscience, A meta-analysis. Archives of Physical patients with Alzheimer's disease. differences in movement planning

read discuss contribute at www.thepsychologist.org.uk 587


psy 08_11 p586_589 hackley3_Layout 1 18/07/2011 17:07 Page 588

brain training

cognitive stimulation (active


or passive), memory
rehabilitation, reality
orientation and
neuropsychological
rehabilitation. The emerging
evidence suggested that
cognitive training
interventions improve
cognition, with an average
improvement of 1.5 points
on MMSE. However, the
selection criteria for the
review were broad and poorly
defined, little assimilation of
the data was attempted and
no comment was made on
the generalisability of these
observed improvements.

Generalisability
Generalisability therefore
remains a key outcome in
these kinds of study. It may
be defined as the spread or
transfer of performance gains
from specifically trained tasks to transferable, then they may confer a demand a greater degree of parallel
untrained, more general abilities. beneficial effect on everyday cognition, information-processing in different
It implies a shift from more basic forms function and performance or in financial domains, a feature shared with musical
of learning derived from repetitive terms, healthcare costs. and athletic tasks. Other shared
practice to improvement of more general A review article by Green and Bavelier characteristics that were identified
cognitive functions, such as memory, (2008) addressed this issue of included small incremental increases
attention, reasoning and speed of generalisation by aiming to identify those in difficulty, optimal levels of motivation
information processing. training regimens that led to generalised and arousal, feedback and reward, and
The literature on skill learning does and persistent improvements, and to variability of the task itself. This would
indeed confirm that we are capable of clarify those factors that contributed to a seem to imply that in order to elicit
improving on effectively any task with more general learning outcome. Learning performance gains that are generalisable,
practice, though this improvement tends was noted to be highly specific throughout the training tasks should themselves be of
to be specific with little transfer even to cognitive, motor and perceptual domains, a general or complex rather than a specific
similar tasks (Mackrous & Proteau, 2007; though they identified three training nature.
Seidler, 2004; Sowden et al., 2002). In paradigms where learning has been shown
many studies, generalisability has been to be more generalised action video
assumed to be associated with delayed games, music and athletics. Action video In the brain
cognitive decline, especially if there has games were shown to be particularly Learning, the cognitive process of
been evidence of any long-term benefits. effective in terms of generalisability of acquiring skill or knowledge, has thus
This concept relates to the brain health learning to untrained tasks, though these been shown to be an ability that
model that nourishes the sale of tended to be visuospatial tasks. These continues throughout life and that works
commercially available brain training games were defined as fast-paced and best when the rehearsed material is as
games. The issue of generalisability is thus unpredictable, requiring monitoring of the similar to the intended or measured
a link between study conditions and real- entire screen with rapid decision-making. outcome as possible. Research is ongoing
world function; if these gains are This complexity was considered to into the structural and neurochemical

strategies. Experimental Brain study using the NEAR approach. hippocampus: A neural network Seidler, R.D. (2004). Multiple motor
Research, 183(2), 181193. Journal of Affective Disorders, 121, approach to causality. Nature Reviews learning experiences enhance motor
Mazoyer, B., House, O., Joliot, M. et al. 258262. Neuroscience, 9(1), 6575. adaptability. Journal of Cognitive
(2009). Regional cerebral blood flow Nemoto, T., Yamazawa, R., Kobayashi, H. Owen, A.M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J.A. Neuroscience, 16(1), 6573.
increases during wakeful rest et al. (2009). Cognitive training for et al. (2010). Putting brain training to Smith, G.E., Housen, P., Yaffe, K. et al.
following cognitive training. Brain divergent thinking in schizophrenia: the test. Nature, 465(7299), 775778. (2009). A cognitive training program
Research Bulletin, 80(3), 133138. A pilot study. Progress in Neuro- Papp, K.V., Walsh, S.J. & Snyder, P.J. based on principles of brain
Naismith, S.L., Redoblado-Hodge, M.A., Psychopharmacology & Biological (2009). Immediate and delayed plasticity. Journal of the American
Lewis, S.J. et al. (2010). Cognitive Psychiatry, 33(8), 15331536. effects of cognitive interventions in Geriatric Society, 57(4), 594603.
training in affective disorders Neves, G., Cooke, S.F. & Bliss, T.V. (2008). healthy elderly. Alzheimers & Sowden, P.T., Rose, D. & Davies, I.R.
improves memory: A preliminary Synaptic plasticity, memory and the Dementia, 5(1), 5060. (2002). Perceptual learning of

588 vol 24 no 8 august 2011


psy 08_11 p586_589 hackley3_Layout 1 18/07/2011 17:07 Page 589

brain training

correlates of this process. fitness, positive behaviour and cognitive their software or services to the general
Cognitive training has been function were noted. A more recent review public. This growth has been potentiated
shown to increase metabolic of the subject (Hillman et al., 2008) through the use of advertising to cultivate
demands of various brain confirms this beneficial effect of exercise a public perception of efficacy of these
regions, with other regions throughout the lifespan, with effect size products, sometimes bolstered with small
becoming more active in the varying depending on the aspect of self-sponsored studies (which are seldom
resting state following training cognitive function measured (0.68 for peer-reviewed). Published research has
(Mazoyer et al., 2009). This may executive tasks, 0.48 for controlled tasks, been carried out on cognitive training in
imply synaptic potentiation and 0.44 for spatial tasks and 0.3 for speed a wide variety of populations, including
neurogenesis, which is known to tasks). The magnitude of these effects has healthy adults, those at risk of cognitive
continue throughout life with been found to be related to the length of decline, and those with cognitive
neural stem cells remaining the fitness intervention, the type of impairment or dementia. In all of these
active in various regions (Gage, intervention and the duration of training groups, the research data generally shows
2002). Neurogenesis in the sessions (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003). similar patterns; increased cognitive
hippocampus has been linked These effect sizes are significantly larger performance is usually seen for the
to learning and memory than most of those quoted for any specific tasks being trained, though this
(Kempermann et al., 2002; cognitive-training interventions. improvement does not tend to generalise
Neves et al., 2008). Overall, the relevant underlying to the untrained or benchmark tests.
Valenzuela & Sachdev (2009) neurobiological mechanisms behind To enable generalisation of learning, the
further examine the link between any observed improvements in cognitive training tasks themselves may have to be
neurobiological mechanisms and performance following training remain of a more general and multidimensional
cognitive function and place this unclear. As mentioned at the beginning nature. The research base is, however,
in the context of the of this article, companies targeting the very limited, with a wide variety of
environmental enrichment commercial or entertainment-based training methods and outcomes used,
paradigm. This paradigm is based cognitive training market often use making it difficult to combine studies to
on evidence suggesting that pseudoscientific or unsubstantiated claims address these hypotheses. Given the ease
individuals with greater education or to enhance product of access to these
cognitively demanding occupations are at sales. Indeed, it is clear programmes and the
lower risk of developing dementia. In their from the advertising potential gains, more
article they found mental stimulation to market that the appeal of a scientific research would be
be a robust trigger for both brain-derived neuroscientific explanation often clouds warranted.
neurotrophic factor and nerve growth information tends to individual reasoning Overall, though, there
factor (proteins involved in neural cell bolster consumers is little evidence at present
proliferation) and a prompt for increased interest in a product be to recommend this type of
synaptic plasticity and synaptogenesis, it a shampoo, anti-ageing cream or brain- intervention to those looking to improve
particularly in the hippocampus. training programme. However, as Weisberg their cognitive function. One intervention
These effects are not unique to et al. (2008) succinctly demonstrate in that has repeatedly been shown to improve
cognitive training, however. their article The seductive allure of cognitive function is physical exercise
Antidepressants and physical exercise neuroscience explanations, the appeal when discussing these issues with patients,
have both been shown to exert similar of a scientific explanation often clouds the decision to advise them to engage in
neurogenerative effects (Ernst et al., 2006). individual reasoning or judgement to the such an intervention with an already
Indeed, a robust evidence base has extent where explanations may be firmly established evidence base would
developed for the direct beneficial effects uncritically accepted, even where the seem a no-brainer.
of physical exercise on cognitive function information presented is irrelevant to the
in a variety of populations. Meta-analyses explanation.
have quoted effect sizes varying from 0.25 Dan Hackley
when examining acute and long-term ST6 in General Adult
effects (Etnier et al., 1997) to 0.48 in Conclusions Psychiatry, Gartnavel Royal
sedentary older adults (Colcombe & The commercial brain training industry Hospital, Glasgow
Kramer, 2003), to 0.57 in those aged over is a substantial, global and growing field, danhackley@gmail.com
65 with cognitive impairment (Heyn et al., with increasing numbers of companies
2004), where beneficial effects on physical jumping on the bandwagon to promote

luminance contrast detection: of dementia? American Journal of Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, (2009). Cognitive training for early-
Specific for spatial frequency and Geriatric Psychiatry, 17(3), 179187. 20(3), 470477. stage Alzheimers disease and
retinal location but not orientation. Vinogradov S., Fisher, M., Warm, H. et al. Wolinsky, F.D., Mahncke, H.W., Weg. M.W. dementia. Journal of Gerontological
Vision Research, 42(10), 12491258. (2009). The cognitive cost of et al. (2009). The ACTIVE cognitive Nursing, 35(3), 2329.
Thorell, L.B., Lindqvist, S., Nutley, S.B. et anticholinergic burden: Decreased training interventions and the onset Zelinski, E. (2010). Scientific critique of
al. (2009). Training and transfer response to cognitive training in of and recovery from suspected BBC/Nature brain training
effects of executive functions in schizophrenia. American Journal of clinical depression. Journals of experiment. Retrieved 22 August
preschool children. Developmental Psychiatry, 166(9), 10551062. Gerontology Series B Psychological 2010 from www.sharpbrains.com/
Science, 12(1), 106113. Weisberg, D.S., Keil, F.C., Goodstein, E.R. Sciences & Social Sciences, 64, blog/2010/05/10/scientific-critique-
Valenzuela, M. & Sachdev, P. (2009). Can & Gray, J.R. (2008). The seductive 577585. of-bbc-nature-brain-training-
cognitive exercise prevent the onset allure of neuroscience explanations. Yu, F., Rose, K.M., Burgener, S.C. et al. experiment

read discuss contribute at www.thepsychologist.org.uk 589

You might also like