You are on page 1of 8

J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:24842490

DOI 10.1007/s10803-013-1796-9

BRIEF REPORT

Brief Report: Regression Timing and Associated Features


in MECP2 Duplication Syndrome
S. U. Peters R. J. Hundley A. K. Wilson
C. M. B. Carvalho J. R. Lupski M. B. Ramocki

Published online: 2 March 2013


Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Abstract The aim of this study was to determine the Introduction


frequency, timing, and associated features of develop-
mental regression in MECP2 duplication syndrome. We Mutations in the methyl CpG-binding protein 2 gene
also examined whether duplication size was associated (MECP2) were first identified as a primary cause of Rett
with regression. Comprehensive psychological evaluations syndrome (RTT) in females (Amir 1999). The more
were used to assess 17 boys with MECP2 duplication recently described MECP2 duplication syndrome is the
syndrome. Information about regression was gathered via most common subtelomeric duplication identified in a
parent report. Eight of 17 boys exhibited regression in clinical population surveyed by high resolution human
language skills, while seven of 17 exhibited regression in genome analyses using array Comparative Genomic
other skill areas. Regression in other skill areas coin- Hybridization (aCGH). This duplication syndrome pri-
cided with seizure onset and with a prior autism diagnosis marily affects males, and is an X-linked genomic disorder
in six of seven participants. Regression was not associated (Lupski 2009) characterized by infantile hypotonia, absent
with duplication size. Questions remain as to why some to limited speech, severe intellectual disability, progressive
boys regress, and future work is necessary to understand neurological difficulties (including spasticity and seizures),
the underlying mechanism(s) that causes regression. recurrent respiratory infections, and gastrointestinal diffi-
culties (Friez et al. 2006; Ramocki et al. 2009; Van Esch
Keywords Regression  MECP2  Seizures 2012). Phenotypic features are due to a MECP2 gene
dosage effect as further evidenced by a more severe phe-
notype conveyed by triplication of this locus (Carvalho
et al. 2011). Features of autism spectrum disorder (ASD),
including repetitive behaviors, sensory processing diffi-
S. U. Peters  R. J. Hundley
Departments of Pediatrics and Psychiatry, Vanderbilt University, culties, and difficulties with social affect are present in a
Nashville, TN, USA large proportion of affected individuals (Peters et al. 2013;
Ramocki et al. 2009). Females are less often affected, and
S. U. Peters (&)  R. J. Hundley  A. K. Wilson
the spectrum of phenotypes ranges from mild to the same
Vanderbilt Kennedy Center for Research on Human
Development, PMB 74; 230 Appleton Place, Nashville, severity as affected boys depending on the underlying
TN 37203-5721, USA genetic factors (Bijlsma et al. 2012; Grasshoff et al. 2011).
e-mail: sarika.u.peters@vanderbilt.edu Developmental regression, inclusive of a partial or com-
plete loss of acquired highest spoken language skills
C. M. B. Carvalho  J. R. Lupski
Department of Molecular and Human Genetics, (including babbling), and a loss of motor skills are major
Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA diagnostic criteria for classic Rett Syndrome (RTT) (Neul
et al. 2010). Because RTT is rarely diagnosed prior to the
M. B. Ramocki
regression phase, studies of regression are based primarily
Department of Pediatrics, Section of Pediatric Neurology and
Developmental Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, on retrospective parental report (Neul et al. 2010). The
Houston, TX, USA phenomenon of regression is less clear within MECP2

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:24842490 2485

duplication syndrome. Prior studies based on retrospective gradual or sudden, and whether a prior diagnosis of ASD
parental report reveal that regression can occur within predicts a later decline in skills. We also examined whether
MECP2 duplication syndrome (del Gaudio et al. 2006; or not duplication size corresponded to a loss of skills.
Ramocki et al. 2009), but the frequency, timing, and
associated features of regression have not been formally
characterized. Methods
Regression is typically defined as a partial or complete
loss of skills related to language, socialization, self-help Participants
skills, and/or motor skills. In ASD and RTT, the loss of
skills occurs at an early age (usually between 15 and The participants (n = 17) were all male and ranged
30 months), and may or may not be associated with seizure between the ages of 310 years. Females were not included
onset or medical illness (Barger et al. 2012; Neul et al. given that their phenotype is more variable (Grasshoff et al.
2010; Ozonoff et al. 2008). In some situations, a child may 2011). All participants with a confirmed genetic diagnosis
exhibit developmental delays prior to a loss of skills (early were offered enrollment and were ascertained as part of
onset pattern), or no delays may be apparent before the loss their attendance at the first ever worldwide conference on
(Ozonoff et al. 2008). A recent study based on video MECP2 duplication syndrome. All were additionally
analysis found that approximately 38.5 % of children with evaluated by a pediatric clinical neurologist (M.B.R.) prior
ASD exhibited a regressive course (Ozonoff et al. 2011). In to study enrollment. This study was approved by the
ASD, studies suggest that regression occurs within the Institutional Review Boards at Vanderbilt University and
context of intact motor abilities and even advanced motor Baylor College of Medicine, and all families gave
milestones, suggesting a different pattern and course from informed consent to participate.
that which occurs in RTT and MECP2 duplication syn-
drome (Bernabei et al. 2007; Jones and Campbell 2010). In Genetic Testing
MECP2 duplication syndrome, all boys have early onset
developmental delays and there are some reports of sei- All participants had an established clinical diagnosis. To
zures and associated regression, occurring at varying ages independently confirm the diagnosis and to determine the
(i.e. late childhood to early adolescence) and with varying size, genomic extent and gene content for each rearrange-
percentages (Vignoli et al. 2012). A prior study of MECP2 ment, we designed a tiling-path oligonucleotide microarray
duplication syndrome revealed that 3 out of 9 boys spanning 4.6 MB across the MECP2 region on Xq28.
exhibited regression/loss of skills at the time of evaluation The custom 4 9 44 k Agilent Technologies (Santa Clara,
(Ramocki et al. 2009). In one of these cases, the onset of CA, USA) microarray was designed using the Agilent
regression occurred at a younger age (36 months). In the e-array website (http://earray.chem.agilent.com/earray/).
two other cases, however, a loss of skills occurred during We selected 22,000 probes covering Chromosome X:
late childhood and coincided with the onset of seizures. 150,000,000154,600,000 (NCBI build 36), including the
It is unclear whether regression is a core phenotypic MECP2 gene, which represents an average distribution of 1
feature of MECP2 duplication syndrome that occurs sec- probe per 250 bp. Probe labeling and hybridization were
ondary to overexpression of MECP2 directly and with performed as previously described (Carvalho et al. 2011).
variable phenotypic expression, or whether regression only To estimate duplication size, we calculated the distance
occurs in a small proportion of individuals due to factors between breakpoints/coordinates (i.e. where probe signals
indirectly related to MECP2 overexpression. We have not traversed from no gain to gain, signaling increased dosage
been able to reliably predict when or in which children of the genomic interval complementary to the interrogating
regression will occur, and it is unknown as to whether oligonucleotide probe).
duplication size predicts regression. A better understanding
of regression in this patient population is essential in order Instruments
to facilitate prognosis and to help families and physicians
make the best long-term treatment plans and choices for To assess regression, we utilized the Autism Diagnostic
affected individuals. It is also important to inform future InterviewRevised (ADI-R) questions regarding regres-
preclinical and clinical therapeutic trials. In this study, we sion. A portion of the ADI-R contains questions about early
examined a larger cohort of 17 boys with MECP2 dupli- milestones, symptom onset, and regression. Since the ADI-
cation syndrome. Our goal was to determine the average R questions regarding language-regression require the
age of regression, the skills that were lost (language, social, minimum use of at least five different words (other than
self-help, or motor skills), whether regression coincides mama and dada) on a daily basis for at least 3 months, a
with seizures and/or medical illness, whether losses are research-based regression interview was designed as an

123
2486 J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:24842490

extension of the ADI-R to capture sub-threshold losses of autism diagnosis was related to regression status (language
skills as separated into language (e.g. loss of cooing, bab- and non-language), we used contingency analysis. To
bling, vocal imitation, use of 3 or more words, communi- determine the relationship between duplication size and
cative intent), and non-language skills (hand use, motor regression (separated into language vs. non-language), we
skills, self-help skills, social interest, and curiosity) (Luy- used bivariate correlations.
ster et al. 2005; Parr et al. 2011). This interview utilizes the
same coding and similar questions to the ADI-R, and is
being utilized as part of the (ASD) Simons Simplex Col- Results
lection (SSC). Information regarding the age of the loss,
duration of the loss, as well as any association with phys- Table 1 lists the number of boys who exhibited language
ical illness is captured. There are also questions regarding regression versus no language regression, and then
any recovery of skills after the loss (or whether the skills regression in other skill areas versus no loss in other
loss persists). skill areas. Six boys exhibited losses in both language and
Participants also received a comprehensive develop- non-language areas. Two boys exhibited language losses
mental evaluation. The Vineland Adaptive Behavior alone (without any losses to date in other skill areas). One
ScalesSecond Edition (VABS-II) was completed as a boy exhibited losses in other skill areas, but his parents
semi-structured interview with parents to assess overall noted that he never attained enough language (e.g. even
adaptive behavior. Visual reception/nonverbal cognitive cooing or babbling skills) to exhibit a loss in any language
abilities and fine motor skills (nonverbal cognitive abili- skills. Six of eight boys who exhibited a language regres-
ties), gross motor skills, and expressive/receptive language sion had a prior ASD diagnosis, but five of nine boys
abilities were assessed using the Bayley Scales of Infant without language regression also had a prior ASD diag-
and Toddler Development-Third Edition (BSID-III). This nosis and hence there was no significant association
instrument was selected given the developmental level of (Contingency Coefficient = .20; p = .40). Six of seven
our participants. For our participants who were mobile boys with regression in other skill areas had a prior ASD
(n = 9), we also completed the Autism Diagnostic diagnosis, while five of ten without regression in other skill
Observation Schedule, Module 1 to assess Social Affect areas had a prior ASD diagnosis (Contingency Coeffi-
(inclusive of communication and reciprocal social inter- cient = .34; p = .12). There are no significant differences
action) and Restricted/Repetitive behaviours related to in chronological age between language regressors and non-
ASD. Our assessments were based on the recently revised regressors (p = .37), or between boys who exhibit regres-
ADOS algorithms (Gotham et al. 2007) which include the sion in other skills versus no regression in other skills
domains of Social Affect and Restricted-Repetitive (p = .23).
Behaviors. All participants (regardless of their chronolog- Table 2 characterizes the skills that were lost amongst
ical age) received Module 1, which is appropriate for the boys who did exhibit regression. For language skills, it
individuals who are nonverbal. In order to assess skills was most common for boys to exhibit losses in lower level
such as joint attention, it is important that the participant skills such as babbling, cooing, and the meaning/intent of
have some mobility. Even in cases where we could not communication. Two of the eight boys with language
perform the ADOS due to loss of mobility (n = 8), we regression alone (without losses in motor skills) were able,
conducted detailed interviews (i.e. the ADI-R, clinical with intensive therapy, to recover language skills at the
interview) to determine whether or not there was a prior level that they functioned at prior to the loss. The most
diagnosis of ASD. We detail the results for part of our common losses in other skill areas included loss of
sample (n = 9) on these measures elsewhere (Peters et al. motor skills (related to posture, gait, and coordination),
2013). curiosity, and hand use. One boy with a regression in
other skill areas re-learned to walk (but he still
Statistical Analysis

For the purposes of all analyses, language regression was Table 1 Language regression, regression in other skills, and prior
autism diagnosis
analyzed separately from regression in other skill areas. We
utilized descriptive statistics to examine the average age Number (n) Prior autism diagnosis
(mean, median) and age range of language and non-lan-
No Language regression 9 5/9
guage regression. Because of the small sample size, non-
Language regression 8 6/8
parametric statistics (MannWhitney U test) were used to
No regression in other skills 10 5/10
compare any differences between regressors versus non-
Regression in other skills 7 6/7
regressors for chronological age. To check whether an

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:24842490 2487

maintained a loss of language skills), but all others had not Discussion
recovered skills that they lost.
Table 3 shows the average age of skills loss, the age In this study, we examined in greater detail regression
range of regression onset, and whether or not losses were timing and related features in MECP2 duplication syn-
associated with seizures. In the majority of cases (6/7), drome. We found that 9 of 17 boys exhibited a loss of
regression in other skill areas was associated with sei- language and/or motor skills, with six boys exhibiting a
zure onset. In five of these six cases, the participants had loss across both areas. The majority of boys who lost
been given a formal diagnosis of intractable epilepsy by language skills lost subthreshold skills such as cooing and
a pediatric neurologist (based on EEG findings and clinical babbling. It is notable that on the original ADI-R, most of
evaluation). In the sixth case, the parent described several our participants would not have met the threshold criteria
staring spells, although a formal diagnosis of epilepsy for a loss of language skills (i.e. use of at least five different
has not been given. Confirmed seizure onset in our patients words other than mama or dada for a period of at least
ranged between 36 and 84 months of age; all but one had a 3 months). This extension of the ADI-R may therefore
seizure onset of age 5 (60 months) and later. In five of six have some utility in capturing subthreshold losses in lower
cases, the age of onset for language regression coincided functioning populations (Parr et al. 2011; Richler et al.
with the age of onset for regression in other skill areas. In 2006). When examining the timing and course of regres-
one case, the age of language regression occurred at sion in MECP2 duplication syndrome, it appears that in
24 months, but he did not begin to lose motor skills until comparison to ASD and to RTT, the onset of skills loss is
84 months of age, which coincided with the onset of later. In addition, the regression in MECP2 duplication
intractable seizures (absence, myoclonic, and atonic sei- syndrome may be distinct from that which occurs within
zures). Not surprisingly, boys who exhibited regression in ASD. When within the context of refractory epilepsy in
other skill areas (n = 7) were significantly lower func- particular, the losses of skills in our participants are dra-
tioning on all of our developmental parameters matic (e.g. loss of mobility, head control, self-help skills,
(p range = .003.023) as compared to boys who had not and/or language, and hand use), and, in some cases, with
regressed (See Table 4). This was primarily attributable to little recovery of functioning. The size of the duplications
a loss of mobility (i.e. walking) and hand use, which hin- in our subset of participants with refractory epilepsy was
dered their ability to interact with manipulatives. Most of quite variable and in fact, our participant with the smallest
these participants previously had some level of language duplication size exhibited a significant regression within
use (at least babbling vowel-consonant sounds), were able the context of early onset seizures at 36 months. More
to walk, engage in functional play, and had developed broadly, our results demonstrate that loss of skills in
some self-help skills (e.g. feeding themselves) prior to MECP2 duplication syndrome is not related to duplication
regression and the onset of intractable epilepsy. size/gene content. It is unclear whether regression associ-
The results of genetic analyses reveal that all subjects ated with MECP2 duplication syndrome therefore repre-
duplicate the MECP2 gene; three boys (BAB 2769, sents: (1) consequences of an epileptic encephalopathy
BAB3255, BAB3259) have partial genomic triplications. (interestingly 3/5 of the boys with intractable epilepsy were
Duplications (and partial triplications) ranged in size clinically diagnosed with Lennox-Gastaut syndrome), (2)
between .31 and 4.7 MB (See Fig. 1). There was no whether regression phenotypes and epilepsy simply repre-
relationship between duplication size and language sent common downstream effects of MeCP2 overproduc-
regression (p = .26) or between duplication size and tion on neurons or neural networks in associated brain
regression in other skill areas (p = .41). We also did regions, (3) whether a secondary cause such as genetic
not find any significant relationships between duplication background or CNS infection and/or CNS inflammation
size and refractory epilepsy, and one participant with contributes to both regression and epilepsy, or (4) whether
refractory epilepsy and significant regression actually had the clinical outcome represents a complex combination of
the smallest duplication size of all of our participants these possibilities. The timing of seizure onset and corre-
(BAB2625). lation with behavioral phenotypes has not been studied in

Table 2 Number of participants showing a loss of skills by domain


Cooing Babbling Vocal Use of 3 or Communicative Hand Motor skills (posture, Self-help Social Curiosity
imitation more words intent use gait, coordination) skills engagement

5/8 7/8 4/8 2/8 6/8 4/7 7/7 4/7 3/7 5/7

123
2488 J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:24842490

Table 3 Average age of regression


Average age of loss (mean, SD) Range of ages for losses (months) Loss associated with seizures

Language regression 39.12 months; 13.58 months 2460 4/8


Regression in other skills 54.57 months; 20.82 months 3084 6/7

Table 4 Comparison of developmental scores between regression and non-regression groups


Cognitive skills Receptive language Expressive language Fine motor Gross motor

Regression (n = 7) 8.00 4.50 7.00 10.00 11.00


Non-regression (n = 10) 15.00 10.00 10.00 15.00 18.00
Data are depicted in age equivalents (months) and are shown as the Median for each group
GABRA3

SLC6A8
ABCD1
L1CAM

MECP2
IRAK1

G6PD
FLNA

F8
Cen Tel

153.0
149.0

151.0

154.0
152.0

Fig. 1 Genomic region harboring duplications in our cohort of 17 Genomic coordinates are given relative to NCBI Build 36 for the X
patients analyzed by oligonucleotide array CGH. Solid red bars chromosome. *Duplication is larger than the resolution of our custom
represent duplications and blue bars represent triplicated regions. aCGH ([4.7 MB) (Color figure online)

mouse models, but may offer further clues as to etiology/ minimum, our results suggest the need for year-round
mechanisms and treatment. therapeutic services, constant repetition of skills and con-
While over 50 % of our sample exhibited a loss of skills cepts, and within the context of regression, retraining of
at some point during development (for a period of at least lost skills. In cases where there has been loss of hand-use
6 months), it is notable that two boys with language and mobility, there may be utility in using alternative
regression alone were able to recover functioning with the methods of communication, such as eye-gaze technology
assistance of intensive speech/language therapy. Further as is being utilized in RTT (Djukic et al. 2012). In com-
studies will be required in order to examine whether the parison to previous studies (Ramocki et al. 2009), our
majority of boys with MECP2 duplication syndrome will/ results reflect a higher percentage of boys with MECP2
will not recover any function post-regression. At a duplication syndrome exhibiting a loss of skills. This may

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:24842490 2489

still reflect an underestimate of true level of regression perhaps partially improve other neurological phenotypes
since our sample only included boys between the ages of and overall quality of life? Further studies in mouse models
310. More specifically, future studies should include older and in human patients are needed to dissect the underlying
individuals as there are reports of refractory epilepsy, pathogenesis of disease in affected patients and to develop
neurological declines, and sudden death in boys who are in treatments that decrease both morbidity and mortality.
later adolescence-early adulthood (Van Esch 2012). Therapeutics that specifically decrease gene expression or
Regardless of age, given our findings and those of another levels of the MeCP2 protein should theoretically improve
recent study (Vignoli et al. 2012), we do suggest that if a or potentially reverse some clinical phenotypes.
patient without epilepsy presents with regression, then a
careful history to assess for subtle seizures and an EEG Acknowledgments Funding for this project has been provided by a
Vanderbilt Kennedy Center Hobbs Discovery Grant (to SUP), by
study is indicated. 5P30HD015052-30 (to Elisabeth Dykens PI), and by NINDS grant
A limitation of our study is that we relied on parental 5K08NS062711 (to M.B.R.). We also wish to thank the individuals
reports of regression as opposed to retrospective or even and families who so graciously participated in this study as part of the
prospective video studies. As has been demonstrated in first conference on MECP2 duplication syndrome.
studies of individuals with ASD, there may be some benefit
to using video studies, since retrospective parent reports do
not always correspond to what is noted on video analysis References
(particularly as related to social communication skills)
Amir, R. E., Van den Veyver, I. B., Wan, M., Tran, C. Q., Francke,
(Ozonoff et al. 2011). More specifically, this study of ASD U., & Zoghbi, H. Y. (1999). Rett syndrome is caused by
participants demonstrated that less than half of the partic- mutations in X-linked MECP2, encoding methyl-CpG-binding
ipants whose home videos displayed clear evidence of a protein 2. Nature Genetics, 23(2), 185188. doi:10.1038/13810.
decline in social-communication were reported to have had Barger, B. D., Campbell, J. M., & McDonough, J. D. (2012).
Prevalence and onset of regression within autism spectrum
a regression by parents. Similarly, less than half of the disorders: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Autism and
participants were reported by their parents as having early Developmental Disorders,. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1621-x.
onset delays in spite of clear evidence of social commu- Bernabei, P., Cerquiglini, A., Cortesi, F., & DArdia, C. (2007).
nication delays from video analysis. As applied to MECP2 Regression versus no regression in the autistic disorder: Devel-
opmental trajectories. Journal of Autism and Developmental
duplication syndrome, future studies should incorporate Disorders, 37(3), 580588. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0201-3.
retrospective video analysis in cases where a genetic Bijlsma, E. K., Collins, A., Papa, F. T., Tejada, M. I., Wheeler, P.,
diagnosis occurs after a regression. In cases where genetic Peeters, E. A., et al. (2012). Xq28 duplications including MECP2
diagnosis occurs prior to any loss of skills, we propose in five females: Expanding the phenotype to severe mental
retardation. European Journal of Medical Genetics, 55(67),
longitudinal, prospective developmental assessments that 404413. doi:10.1016/j.ejmg.2012.02.009.
incorporate objective video analysis across settings (clinic Carvalho, C. M., Ramocki, M. B., Pehlivan, D., Franco, L. M.,
and home) to document the earliest detection of skill loss Gonzaga-Jauregui, C., Fang, P., et al. (2011). Inverted genomic
and timing and may offer further clues for intervention and segments and complex triplication rearrangements are mediated
by inverted repeats in the human genome. Nature Genetics,
treatment. 43(11), 10741081. doi:10.1038/ng.944.
A recent study demonstrated that duplication of the del Gaudio, D., Fang, P., Scaglia, F., Ward, P. A., Craigen, W. J.,
MECP2 gene causes immune dysfunction due in part to the Glaze, D. G., et al. (2006). Increased MECP2 gene copy number
suppression of IFN-c production from T helper cells in as the result of genomic duplication in neurodevelopmentally
delayed males. Genetic Medicine, 8(12), 784792. doi:10.1097/
both mice and humans (Yang et al. 2012). Although not 01.gim.0000250502.28516.3c.
examined within this study, future studies should examine Djukic, A., Valicenti McDermott, M., Mavrommatis, K., & Martins,
whether insidious infection or neuro-inflammation con- C. L. (2012). Rett syndrome: Basic features of visual processing-
tribute to regression and epilepsy in affected patients. The a pilot study of eye-tracking. Pediatric Neurology, 47(1), 2529.
doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2012.04.009.
seizures in the subset of boys reported here are excep- Friez, M. J., Jones, J. R., Clarkson, K., Lubs, H., Abuelo, D., Bier,
tionally refractory to medical treatment, and in fact 2/5 J. A., et al. (2006). Recurrent infections, hypotonia, and mental
boys have also failed the ketogenic diet, and 2/5 have failed retardation caused by duplication of MECP2 and adjacent region
vagus nerve stimulation. These data suggest that the in Xq28. Pediatrics, 118(6), e1687e1695. doi:10.1542/
peds.2006-0395.
mechanism of epileptogenesis in this syndrome may be Gotham, K., Risi, S., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2007). The autism
distinct from the pathways currently targeted by available diagnostic observation schedule: Revised algorithms for
epilepsy medications; and the question remains whether or improved diagnostic validity. Journal of Autism and Develop-
not excellent seizure control would prevent or reverse mental Disorders, 37(4), 613627. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-
0280-1.
regression in affected patients. Furthermore, if underlying Grasshoff, U., Bonin, M., Goehring, I., Ekici, A., Dufke, A., Cremer,
infection or inflammation has a role, then could targeted K., et al. (2011). De novo MECP2 duplication in two females
treatment potentially prevent epilepsy, regression, and with random X-inactivation and moderate mental retardation.

123
2490 J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:24842490

European Journal of Human Genetics, 19(5), 507512. multiplex sample. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
doi:10.1038/ejhg.2010.226. ders, 41(3), 332340. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1055-2.
Jones, L. A., & Campbell, J. M. (2010). Clinical characteristics Peters, S. U., Hundley, R. J., Wilson, A. K., Warren, Z., Vehorn, A.,
associated with language regression for children with autism Carvalho, C. M. B., et al. (2013). The behavioral phenotype in
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental MECP2 duplication syndrome: A comparison with idiopathic
Disorders, 40(1), 5462. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0823-3. autism. Autism Research, 6, 4250.
Lupski, J. R. (2009). Genomic disorders ten years on. Genetic Ramocki, M. B., Peters, S. U., Tavyev, Y. J., Zhang, F., Carvalho, C.
Medicine, 1(4), 42. doi:10.1186/gm42. M., Schaaf, C. P., et al. (2009). Autism and other neuropsychi-
Luyster, R., Richler, J., Risi, S., Hsu, W. L., Dawson, G., Bernier, R., atric symptoms are prevalent in individuals with MeCP2
et al. (2005). Early regression in social communication in autism duplication syndrome. Annals Neurology, 66(6), 771782.
spectrum disorders: A CPEA Study. Development Neuropsy- doi:10.1002/ana.21715.
chology, 27(3), 311336. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2703_2. Richler, J., Luyster, R., Risi, S., Hsu, W. L., Dawson, G., Bernier, R.,
Neul, J. L., Kaufmann, W. E., Glaze, D. G., Christodoulou, J., Clarke, et al. (2006). Is there a regressive phenotype of Autism
A. J., Bahi-Buisson, N., et al. (2010). Rett syndrome: Revised Spectrum Disorder associated with the measles-mumps-rubella
diagnostic criteria and nomenclature. Annals Neurology, 68(6), vaccine? A CPEA study. Journal of Autism and Developmental
944950. doi:10.1002/ana.22124. Disorders, 36(3), 299316. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0070-1.
Ozonoff, S., Heung, K., Byrd, R., Hansen, R., & Hertz-Picciotto, I. Van Esch, H. (2012). MECP2 duplication syndrome. Molecular
(2008). The onset of autism: Patterns of symptom emergence in Syndromology, 2(35), 128136. doi:10.1159/000329580.
the first years of life. Autism Research, 1(6), 320328. Vignoli, A., Borgatti, R., Peron, A., Zucca, C., Ballarati, L., Bonaglia,
doi:10.1002/aur.53. C., et al. (2012). Electroclinical pattern in MECP2 duplication
Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A. M., Young, G. S., Hepburn, S., Thompson, M., syndrome: Eight new reported cases and review of literature.
Colombi, C. et al. (2011). Onset patterns in autism: correspon- Epilepsia, 53(7), 11461155. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.
dence between home video and parent report. Journal of 03501.x.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(8), Yang, T., Ramocki, M. B., Neul, J. L., Lu, W., Roberts, L., Knight, J.,
796806 e791. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2011.03.012. et al. (2012). Overexpression of methyl-CpG binding protein 2
Parr, J. R., Le Couteur, A., Baird, G., Rutter, M., Pickles, A., impairs TH1 responses. Science Translational Medicine, 4(163),
Fombonne, E., et al. (2011). Early developmental regression in 163ra158. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.3004430.
autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from an international

123
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without
permission.

You might also like