You are on page 1of 9

J Physiol 589.

1 (2011) pp 4957 49

TOPICAL REVIEW

The neural basis of visual attention


James W. Bisley
Department of Neurobiology and Jules Stein Eye Institute, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, and Department of Psychology and the Brain
Research Institute, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA

Visual attention is the mechanism the nervous system uses to highlight specific locations, objects
or features within the visual field. This can be accomplished by making an eye movement to bring
the object onto the fovea (overt attention) or by increased processing of visual information in
neurons representing more peripheral regions of the visual field (covert attention). This review
will examine two aspects of visual attention: the changes in neural responses within visual cortices
due to the allocation of covert attention; and the neural activity in higher cortical areas involved
The Journal of Physiology

in guiding the allocation of attention. The first section will highlight processes that occur during
visual spatial attention and feature-based attention in cortical visual areas and several related
models that have recently been proposed to explain this activity. The second section will focus
on the parietofrontal network thought to be involved in targeting eye movements and allocating
covert attention. It will describe evidence that the lateral intraparietal area, frontal eye field and
superior colliculus are involved in the guidance of visual attention, and describe the priority map
model, which is thought to operate in at least several of these areas.

(Received 5 May 2010; accepted after revision 27 August 2010; first published online 31 August 2010)
Corresponding author J. Bisley: Department of Neurobiology, PO Box 951763, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1763, USA.
Email: jbisley@mednet.ucla.edu

Visual attention described as looking out of the corner of your eye.


As the eyes do not move, this is referred to as covert
Although we are not always aware of it, visual attention
attention (Posner, 1980). Both covert and overt attention
is incredibly important for visual perception and for
can be allocated voluntarily (commonly referred to as
all the uses we put perception to, such as learning,
top-down) or involuntarily (bottom-up). Classic examples
memory and our interactions with the visual world. This
of bottom-up attractors of attention are flashing or moving
is highlighted by behavioural protocols such as change
objects, such as the lights on top of emergency vehicles.
blindness, which require subjects to identify changes in a
In contrast, top-down attention can refer to any voluntary
scene, but remove the usual cues of these changes (such as
attention or attention that is driven by factors other than
movement or sudden onset) by having the entire display
external stimuli.
flash off and then on again at the time of the change
(Rensink, 2000). Subjects are remarkably poor at detecting The effects of attention on neurons in visual
these changes when broadly viewing the entire scene, but
cortical areas
improve dramatically when their attention is allocated to
the location of the change. In addition to these types of In the lab, covert attention provides a number of
protocols, the importance of attention is highlighted in behavioural benefits. Subjects attending a particular
patients with parietal lesions who often show signs of
attentional deficits as striking as the inability to notice James Bisley is an Assistant Professor of Neurobiology at UCLA.
anything in a particular region of space (Husain & Nachev, In 1998 he received his PhD from the University of Melbourne,
2007; Adair & Barrett, 2008). Australia. His research uses physiological, psychophysical and
Generally we think of focusing our attention onto computational techniques to study the cognitive processing of visual
information, such as attention, short-term memory and spatial
objects or features in terms of making a rapid eye processing. His work on the neural mechanisms underlying the
movement (a saccade) to bring the object of our attention allocation of visuospatial attention has been recognized by awards
to the centre of gaze. However, our visual system can from the McKnight Foundation, the Sloan Foundation and the
also process information from selected peripheral regions Klingenstein Foundation.
of the retina. When this is done consciously, it is often


C 2010 The Author. Journal compilation 
C 2010 The Physiological Society DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.192666
50 J. W. Bisley J Physiol 589.1

location have perceptual benefits when examining a Striking effects of attention are seen when two stimuli
stimulus at that location, such as improved contrast are placed within the neurons receptive field (Fig. 2A,
sensitivity (Cameron et al. 2002) or spatial resolution right panel). The logic underlying these experiments
(Yeshurun & Carrasco, 1999). Furthermore, reaction times is that as receptive fields get bigger in progressively
are improved, such that subjects respond more rapidly to higher visual areas, mechanisms must still be available
an event at an attended location than at another location to focus attention to a discrete region of space. Thus,
(Posner, 1980). In this section, we briefly describe the these experiments generally place both a preferred and
effects of attention on neuronal responses in a number of non-preferred stimulus within the receptive field and have
visual areas (light grey regions, Fig. 1) that may underlie the animals attend one of the two stimuli. In addition,
these behavioural benefits. extra stimuli are often placed outside of the receptive
field, so that the responses to the stimuli within the
Spatial attention. Just as eye movements are aimed at receptive field can be measured under conditions in which
particular regions of space, covert attention can also attention is not focused on either. The result of such an
be focused on discrete locations in the visual scene. To experiment is illustrated in Fig. 2B, which shows the mean
examine this, experiments have been performed in which responses from 70 MT neurons with a single or two stimuli
a single stimulus was placed in the receptive field of within the receptive field (Lee & Maunsell, 2010). The
the neuron being studied and one or more stimuli were effects of attention under these conditions are clear; the
placed outside of the receptive field (Fig. 2A, left panel). response of the population is biased towards its normal
Responses were then recorded when the animal focused its response to the attended stimulus when it is presented
attention towards the stimulus within the receptive field or alone. Thus, if the attended stimulus is the non-preferred
outside of the receptive field. Generally, neurons respond stimulus (continuous grey trace, Fig. 2B), then the activity
significantly more to the stimulus in the receptive field is suppressed compared to the non-attended response
when it is the locus of the animals attention than when to both stimuli (dashed black trace, Fig. 2B), and if the
it is ignored. These results have been found in a host of attended stimulus is the preferred stimulus, then the
visual areas from the lateral geniculate nucleus through response is enhanced (dotted black trace, Fig. 2B). Similar
to the middle temporal area (MT) and V4 (Motter, 1993; effects have been seen in V2, V4 and in the temporal lobe
Vidyasagar, 1998; McAdams & Maunsell, 1999b; Treue & (Moran & Desimone, 1985; Chelazzi et al. 1993, 2001;
Maunsell, 1999; Reynolds et al. 2000; Casagrande et al. Luck et al. 1997; Reynolds et al. 1999; Treue & Maunsell,
2005; McAdams & Reid, 2005; McAlonan et al. 2008). 1999; Martinez-Trujillo & Treue, 2002).
However, the magnitude of this effect is not particularly
strong and in some studies the effect is almost absent
in certain cortical areas (Luck et al. 1997; Seidemann & Feature-based attention. When searching for a particular
Newsome, 1999). item, we are able to effectively ignore objects that do not
share any features with our target. This ability to ignore
completely irrelevant distractors while focusing attention
on categories of objects based on their composition
is termed feature-based attention. Studies examining
this form of attention have shown that neurons that
preferentially respond to a feature have elevated responses
to that feature when it is part of an object within the
neurons receptive field and when that feature is being
attended even if the specific object in the receptive field is
not the focus of attention (Motter, 1994; Treue & Martinez
Trujillo, 1999; McAdams & Maunsell, 2000; Bichot et al.
2005; Buracas & Albright, 2009). For example, if a subject
is looking for a red ball and a red star is in the receptive
field of a neuron that has a preference for red objects, then
the response will be enhanced compared to if the subject
is looking for a blue ball. In this way, the visual system is
Figure 1. Illustration of the primate brain highlighting objects that are more likely to be the goal of
The locations of visual areas containing neurons whose responses the search and, thus, increasing the efficiency of search.
are modulated by visual attention are shown in light grey. Cortical The response modulations under feature-based
areas involved in the allocation of attention and the guidance of eye
attention and spatial attention have been examined
movements are shown in dark grey. The superior colliculus, also
involved in this process, is not visible. MT, the middle temporal area; together in a number of studies (McAdams & Maunsell,
LIP, the lateral intraparietal area; FEF, the frontal eye field. 2000; Busse et al. 2006; Katzner et al. 2009). These studies


C 2010 The Author. Journal compilation 
C 2010 The Physiological Society
J Physiol 589.1 Neural basis of visual attention 51

Figure 2. Studying the effects of attention on MT neurons


A, the two main ways that the effects of attention have been studied in visual cortices. Usually at least 1 preferred
stimulus (a direction of motion or oriented bar) and 1 non-preferred stimulus (the opposite direction of motion or
a bar rotated 90 deg from the preferred orientation) are presented on the screen. Either a single stimulus is placed
in the receptive field (left panel), with the other stimulus in an opposite location, or both stimuli are placed in
the receptive field (right panel). B, the response of a population of 70 MT neurons under 5 attentional conditions
(illustrated on right). The dashed grey oval represents the receptive field (RF), the cross represents the fixation point
(FP), and the black cone illustrates the focus of attention. Adapted from Lee & Maunsell (2010) with permission
from the Society for Neuroscience.

have found that single neurons in areas MT and V4 exhibit response shift was seen and when the stimulus was larger
similar modulations due to both feature-based attention than the attended area, a response gain was seen.
and spatial attention, suggesting that the mechanism used A number of recently published models have been
to produce the modulation for the two processes may be attempting to explain all attentional modulation using
the same (Katzner et al. 2009). a response normalization mechanism. These models
explain the response modulations seen with both one
and two stimuli within neurons receptive fields by
Activity modulation. It is intuitive to imagine that either implementing an input gain (Ghose, 2009) or a
attention effectively shrinks the receptive field to normalization of the output signal (Lee & Maunsell, 2009;
encompass the attended stimulus when multiple objects
are in the receptive field (continuous grey or dotted black
traces, Fig. 2B). However, understanding what is occurring
when only a single stimulus is within the RF is less obvious
because the attentional modulation varies with both the
neuronal tuning and stimulus contrast (Martinez-Trujillo
& Treue, 2002; Reynolds & Desimone, 2003). In coming
up with an explanation for the results found when a
single stimulus is presented in the receptive field, two
theories have dominated the literature. The first suggests
that attention shifts the stimulusresponse curve (Fig. 3A;
Reynolds et al. 2000). The second suggests that attention
modulates the gain of the neural response above base-
line (Fig. 3B; McAdams & Maunsell, 1999a,b; Treue & Figure 3. Theoretical effects of attention on the contrast
Martinez Trujillo, 1999). One study set out to differentiate response function
between these two possibilities, but found that the data A, the response shift model predicts that the response to an
were very well fitted by both models; what variance there attended stimulus of a given contrast is the same as the response to
an unattended stimulus of higher contrast. Thus, attention is similar
was in the neural data did not allow the responses to fit
to turning up the brightness of the stimulus. B, the response gain
unambiguously into one category or the other (Williford model predicts that the response to an attended stimulus of a given
& Maunsell, 2006). However, recent modelling work contrast, is a multiplicative increase in response to that contrast.
has suggested that the differences in the changes to the Thus, attention is similar to just turning up the gain of the response.
stimulusresponse curves may be due to the stimulus Note that the response gain model (B) suggests that the peak
response to a bright stimulus can be increased, whereas the
size relative to the area over which the animal is paying
response shift model (A) does not. Continuous lines, unattended
attention (Reynolds & Heeger, 2009). They found that contrast response functions; dashed lines, attended contrast
when the stimulus was smaller than the attended area, a response functions.


C 2010 The Author. Journal compilation 
C 2010 The Physiological Society
52 J. W. Bisley J Physiol 589.1

Reynolds & Heeger, 2009). Although differing in subtle 1990; Tanaka et al. 1990), so they are ideally suited for the
ways, these models show that it is likely that a unifying collection of visual information and the feedback to visual
mechanism is used to modulate activity under most, if not areas in order to guide attention. It is notable that most
all, attentional conditions (Lee & Maunsell, 2010). These of these areas are also involved in the processing of eye
models are a welcome consensus in the field as they meld movements, which are really just overt shifts in attention.
two sets of data that were treated by many as separate Thus, it is likely that the same network guides both overt
problems to be solved. and covert attention (Bisley & Goldberg, 2010).

Other effects of attention. In addition to modulating Neural evidence of attentional allocation by the
firing rate, attention has been found to influence neural parietofrontal network. As it is difficult to know exactly
activity in other ways. Focusing attention increases the where an animals focus of covert attention is, there are
synchronization of local field potentials (LFPs; thought only a limited number of studies that have explicitly related
to represent synaptic input) in V4 (Fries et al. 2001, the activity of neurons to a qualitatively defined locus of
2008) and increases the synchronization between the LFP attention. This has been done explicitly in LIP (Bisley
and the spiking output (Bichot et al. 2005; Fries et al. & Goldberg, 2003; Herrington & Assad, 2010) and SC
2008). Attention also produces more reliable responses in (Ignashchenkova et al. 2004). However, the use of visual
putative interneurons in V4, as indicated by a reduction search tasks, in which subjects must covertly shift their
in the variance (Mitchell et al. 2007). There is also attention around an array of objects to find a target without
recent evidence from V4 that attention reduces inter- moving their eyes, gives an indirect measure of attention
neuronal correlations (Cohen & Maunsell, 2009), which that can be used to identify attention-related activity. Such
would improve the population encoding of information studies have found covert attention-related activity in LIP
by reducing correlated noise that would otherwise remain (Thomas & Pare, 2007; Ipata et al. 2009), SC (McPeek
after pooling. Finally, when attention is allocated to a & Keller, 2002b) and FEF (Juan et al. 2004; Thompson
stimulus close to, but not within, the receptive field, then et al. 2005; Buschman & Miller, 2007). Indeed, one recent
the centre of the receptive field appears to shift towards the study has found evidence showing what appears to be a
attended location (Connor et al. 1996, 1997; Womelsdorf neural correlate of shifting covert attention in the activity
et al. 2006). It is thought that this may be related to of FEF neurons during such search (Buschman & Miller,
the changes in centresurround interactions seen due 2009). In this study, the monkeys appeared to have a
to attention (Sundberg et al. 2009) and, consistent with tendency to covertly scan the array in a clockwise direction
the normalization models, may be due to multiplicative (Fig. 4A). Figure 4B shows the responses of FEF neurons
attentional modulation at earlier stages within the visual sorted based on where their receptive fields were relative
system (Womelsdorf et al. 2008). to the target location. When the target was in the neurons
preferred location, the response was elevated just prior to
the animals signal that he had found the target (indicated
Mechanisms underlying the allocation of attention
by a saccade). When the target was clockwise to the
Thus far, we have talked about the ways in which attention neurons preferred location, the response was elevated
modulates the activity of neurons in visual cortices. Now earlier as if the animal checked the stimulus in the
we will describe the mechanism thought to guide the neurons preferred location and then moved on to the
allocation of attention. Although we will focus on results next stimulus where it found the target. A similar, but
from monkeys, there is a growing literature in humans more exaggerated effect was seen when the target was
based on lesion, TMS and functional imaging (Corbetta opposite the RF as if the animal checked the stimulus in
et al. 2008), which shows that the animal model is the neurons preferred location and then checked the next
providing a good foundation for our understanding of one before finding the target at the third location. This
these processes in the human. result suggests that the activity in FEF and, by extension,
the parietofrontal network, guides covert attention on a
The parietofrontal network. In both humans and moment-by-moment basis.
monkeys, the guidance of attention appears to be focused To test whether this is the case, several studies have
on a network involving parietal and frontal areas, as well as recorded the activity in the parietofrontal network and in
the superior colliculus (SC). In the monkey, it is thought a mid-level visual area. They have found that attention
that the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) and the frontal eye leads to enhanced synchrony in responses across the areas
field (FEF) are the specific regions within the parietal and (Saalmann et al. 2007; Gregoriou et al. 2009). These data
frontal cortices, respectively, which are involved in this suggest that neurons in the parietofrontal network can
process (Fig. 1). Anatomically, these areas are all inter- modulate the activity in visual areas and are consistent
connected with each other and with visual cortical areas with the hypothesis that these areas guide the allocation
(Stanton et al. 1988, 1995; Andersen et al. 1990; Blatt et al. of covert attention.


C 2010 The Author. Journal compilation 
C 2010 The Physiological Society
J Physiol 589.1 Neural basis of visual attention 53

Visual search paradigms can also be used to relate neural stimulation of LIP has been shown to bias orienting
activity to saccade generation and, thus, overt attention. to a flashed stimulus (Cutrell & Marrocco, 2002), while
The principle of these studies is that if the neural activity is stimulation of the SC has biased performance in several
used to guide eye movements, then once a target has been attention-demanding tasks (Cavanaugh & Wurtz, 2004;
selected by these neurons a set time should elapse before Muller et al. 2005). The most impressive data from micro-
an eye movement is triggered to the location represented stimulation come from a series of studies in which Moore
by the neurons. We can think of this in the following way: and colleagues have stimulated FEF, using currents that
once the neuron selects the target, it tells the oculomotor are too low to induce eye movements. These studies have
system what to aim for and the remaining time is some shown that microstimulation of FEF improves stimulus
set time it takes for the oculomotor system to generate a detection behaviourally (Moore & Fallah, 2001) and that
saccade. Such a relationship between saccade goal selection microstimulation induces response changes in V4 neurons
and saccadic onset has been seen in LIP (Ipata et al. 2006a; reminiscent of the modulations seen when attention
Thomas & Pare, 2007), FEF (Bichot et al. 2001; Sato et al. is allocated to those neurons (Moore & Armstrong,
2001) and SC (McPeek & Keller, 2002a; Shen & Pare, 2007). 2003; Armstrong et al. 2006). One of the downsides of
Thus, the same areas that appear to be involved in guiding microstimulation experiments is that it is possible that
covert attention are also involved in providing targeting the behavioural or neural effects are primarily due to
data to the oculomotor system for overt attention. remote stimulation of other more relevant areas via anti-
dromic or orthodromic stimulation of neurons connecting
Causal evidence of attentional allocation by the to the stimulated cluster, or even via stimulation of
parietofrontal network. In addition to correlations of axons that pass close to the stimulated area, but are
neural activity with measures of behaviour, a growing not connected to it. Given the robust changes elicited
number of studies have found that inactivation or micro- by Moore and colleagues and the fact that the regions
stimulation of single areas within the parietofrontal within the parietofrontal network appear to work together,
network biases the allocation of attention. As a rough it is generally accepted that their data are very strong
tool, these methods can ask whether artificially increasing evidence for this network playing a role in the allocation
or decreasing the activity within an area has an effect on of attention.
the allocation of attention. But, they also allow the testing The role of an area can also be tested by artificially
of specific hypotheses; one can make a prediction about reducing the activity of neurons within that area. In this
what would happen to the allocation of attention after field, it is most commonly done with muscimol, which
stimulation or inactivation and see if the effects match the inhibits neurons with GABAA receptors for hours at a
predictions. time. This removes the main concern of more classical
Microstimulation involves injecting low current levels lesion studies, which is that any main roles the area may
into clusters of physiologically defined neurons. Micro- play could be masked by retraining or because redundant

Figure 4. Evidence of shifting covert attention in frontal eye field (FEF)


A, in this experiment, monkeys had to fixate on a small point (the fixation point, FP) and covertly find a target
amongst 3 distractors. Based on the behavioural data, the authors found that the monkeys were serially attending
the stimuli in a clockwise direction (illustrated by the dashed line). B, the responses (illustrated as z-scores by the
colour coding) of FEF neurons are aligned by the onset of the choice saccade and are sorted based on where
the target was relative to the preferred location (RF). Asterisks show significance with Bonferroni correction, dots
show uncorrected significant. Adapted from Buschman & Miller (2009) with permission from Elselvier.


C 2010 The Author. Journal compilation 
C 2010 The Physiological Society
54 J. W. Bisley J Physiol 589.1

circuits kick in during the recovery phase. Performance in covert attention and together with the neural correlations
visual search requiring shifts in covert attention has been and microstimulation studies described above, strongly
affected by inactivation of LIP (Wardak et al. 2004; Balan imply that the frontoparietal network, in which we have
& Gottlieb, 2009; Liu et al. 2010), FEF (Wardak et al. 2006; included SC, guides visual attention.
Monosov & Thompson, 2009) and SC (McPeek, 2008).
In addition, Lovejoy & Krauzlis (2010) have shown that The priority map hypothesis. Many results from
cued covert attention is disrupted by inactivation of SC recording, inactivation and microstimulation studies
(Fig. 5). In this study, monkeys were cued by a red ring to are consistent with the idea that the frontoparietal
attend a particular location in space and, when a pulse of network may operate as a priority map. A priority
motion occurred in that location, they had to indicate the map is a representation of the visual world in which
direction of motion. Further, the monkeys had to ignore a items, objects or locations are represented by activity
distractor pulse of motion that was given in the opposite that is proportional to their attentional priority. The
location. When the cue was in the intact quadrant (right hypothesis is based on the saliency map model (Itti
panel), the animals performance was good and consistent & Koch, 2001); however, we use the term priority to
both before and during inactivation. On the opposite side, remove any implication that the responses are due to
performance was good before inactivation (left panel), bottom-up inputs alone (Serences & Yantis, 2006). The
but when the cue was in the inactivated quadrant during attentional priority is some combination of bottom-up
inactivation, the animal often indicated the direction of input and top-down influences, which include task goals,
the motion pulse in the distractor location (yellow points) evaluation of importance and personal biases (Fig. 6).
rather than the cued location (red points). This means The hypothesis is that eye movements are made to the
that during inactivation the animals often attended the peak of the map at the time they were programmed and,
incorrect location. As the monkeys had to indicate the on a moment-to-moment basis, covert attention is also
direction of motion by a saccade or by a button press allocated to the peak of the map. This latter statement,
(in different experiments), the authors concluded that supported by some neurophysiological data (Bisley &
the attentional deficit was not related to a deficit in eye Goldberg, 2003, 2006), is somewhat contentious because
movement generation. Thus, these data strongly suggest subjects can generally split their attention over long
that the SC is involved in allocating or maintaining guided periods of time. However, it is not clear whether this
splitting is due to a single spotlight shuttling between
locations or due to an actual splitting of attentional
resources.
It is likely that a priority map operates in FEF and SC,
or within the parietofrontal network as a whole; however,
the majority of research studying this hypothesis has been
performed on LIP. These studies have shown that LIP

Figure 5. Inactivation of the superior colliculus (SC) creates a


deficit in the allocation of attention
In this experiment, monkeys were cued by a red circle to pay
attention to a stimulus in a region of visual space represented by
inactivated SC neurons (left panel) or in the opposite location (right Figure 6. Theoretical priority map response during visual
panel). The animals had to indicate the direction of a pulse of search
motion in the cued location (red) and ignore a pulse of motion in the A, stimulus arrangement and eye movements (white dashed lines) in
opposite location (yellow). These graphs show the proportion of a hypothetical visual search task. B, theoretical responses on a
choices made when the monkey correctly indicated the direction of priority map to the search performed in A. Red stimuli are
the cued motion pulse (red points), indicated the direction of the represented by low activity, blue stimuli are represented by higher
motion pulse in the distractor location (yellow points) and did not activity. Bars oriented the same way as the target are represented by
indicate either of these directions (grey) before and after an injection greater activity than bars that are not. The bright yellow pop-out
of muscimol. Adapted from Lovejoy & Krauzlis (2010) with stimulus is represented by elevated activity due to its inherent
permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature Neuroscience, salience. The middle blue bar that had just been fixated is
C 2010. represented by reduced activity.


C 2010 The Author. Journal compilation 
C 2010 The Physiological Society
J Physiol 589.1 Neural basis of visual attention 55

activity has a bottom-up component (Roitman & Shadlen, Bisley JW & Goldberg ME (2006). Neural correlates of
2002; Balan & Gottlieb, 2006) and top-down influences. attention and distractibility in the lateral intraparietal area. J
The top-down inputs can enhance activity due to task Neurophysiol 95, 16961717.
demands (Gottlieb et al. 1998; Ipata et al. 2009), reward Bisley JW & Goldberg ME (2010). Attention, intention, and
expectation (Platt & Glimcher, 1999; Dorris & Glimcher, priority in the parietal lobe. Annu Rev Neurosci 33, 121.
Blatt GJ, Andersen RA & Stoner GR (1990). Visual receptive
2004; Sugrue et al. 2004) and motor plans (Gnadt &
field organization and cortico-cortical connections of the
Andersen, 1988), and can suppress responses to salient, lateral intraparietal area (area LIP) in the macaque. J Comp
but behaviourally distracting, stimuli (Ipata et al. 2006b) Neurol 299, 421445.
and to stimuli that have been judged as unimportant Buracas GT & Albright TD (2009). Modulation of neuronal
after being foveally examined (Mirpour et al. 2009). All responses during covert search for visual feature
these inputs and influences appear to be summed almost conjunctions. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106, 1685316858.
linearly (Ipata et al. 2009) and the output predicts saccadic Buschman TJ & Miller EK (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up
latency (Ipata et al. 2006a; Thomas & Pare, 2007), the control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal
shifting time of covert attention (Bisley & Goldberg, cortices. Science 315, 18601862.
2003, 2006; Herrington & Assad, 2010) and monkeys Buschman TJ & Miller EK (2009). Serial, covert shifts of
decisions on two-alternative forced choice tasks (Roitman attention during visual search are reflected by the frontal eye
fields and correlated with population oscillations. Neuron 63,
& Shadlen, 2002). Thus, the concept of a priority map
386396.
for visual attention is particularly appealing because it Busse L, Katzner S & Treue S (2006). Spatial and feature-based
explains the varied roles that had been assigned to LIP effects of exogenous cueing on visual motion processing.
(Bisley & Goldberg, 2010) while incorporating a strong Vision Res 46, 20192027.
and plausible model of attentional allocation (Itti & Koch, Cameron EL, Tai JC & Carrasco M (2002). Covert attention
2001). Indeed, the model is part of the foundation for affects the psychometric function of contrast sensitivity.
an exciting series of experiments which appear to have Vision Res 42, 949967.
successfully aided patients with spatial impairments due Casagrande VA, Sary G, Royal D & Ruiz O (2005). On the
to parietal lesions (Bays et al. 2010). impact of attention and motor planning on the lateral
geniculate nucleus. Prog Brain Res 149, 1129.
Cavanaugh J & Wurtz RH (2004). Subcortical modulation of
References attention counters change blindness. J Neurosci 24,
1123611243.
Adair JC & Barrett AM (2008). Spatial neglect: clinical and Chelazzi L, Miller EK, Duncan J & Desimone R (1993). A
neuroscience review: a wealth of information on the poverty neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex.
of spatial attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1142, 2143. Nature 363, 345347.
Andersen RA, Asanuma C, Essick G & Siegel RM (1990). Chelazzi L, Miller EK, Duncan J & Desimone R (2001).
Corticocortical connections of anatomically and Responses of neurons in macaque area V4 during
physiologically defined subdivisions within the inferior memory-guided visual search. Cereb Cortex 11, 761772.
parietal lobule. J Comp Neurol 296, 65113. Cohen MR & Maunsell JH (2009). Attention improves
Armstrong KM, Fitzgerald JK & Moore T (2006). Changes in performance primarily by reducing interneuronal
visual receptive fields with microstimulation of frontal correlations. Nat Neurosci 12, 15941600.
cortex. Neuron 50, 791798. Connor CE, Gallant JL, Preddie DC & Van Essen DC (1996).
Balan PF & Gottlieb J (2006). Integration of exogenous input Responses in area V4 depend on the spatial relationship
into a dynamic salience map revealed by perturbing between stimulus and attention. J Neurophysiol 75,
attention. J Neurosci 26, 92399249. 13061308.
Balan PF & Gottlieb J (2009). Functional significance of Connor CE, Preddie DC, Gallant JL & Van Essen DC (1997).
nonspatial information in monkey lateral intraparietal area. J Spatial attention effects in macaque area V4. J Neurosci 17,
Neurosci 29, 81668176. 32013214.
Bays PM, Singh-Curry V, Gorgoraptis N, Driver J & Husain M Corbetta M, Patel G & Shulman GL (2008). The reorienting
(2010). Integration of goal- and stimulus-related visual system of the human brain: from environment to theory of
signals revealed by damage to human parietal cortex. J mind. Neuron 58, 306324.
Neurosci 30, 59685978. Cutrell EB & Marrocco RT (2002). Electrical microstimulation
Bichot NP, Rossi AF & Desimone R (2005). Parallel and serial of primate posterior parietal cortex initiates orienting and
neural mechanisms for visual search in macaque area V4. alerting components of covert attention. Exp Brain Res 144,
Science 308, 529534. 103113.
Bichot NP, Thompson KG, Chenchal Rao S & Schall JD (2001). Dorris MC & Glimcher PW (2004). Activity in posterior
Reliability of macaque frontal eye field neurons signaling parietal cortex is correlated with the relative subjective
saccade targets during visual search. J Neurosci 21, 713725. desirability of action. Neuron 44, 365378.
Bisley JW & Goldberg ME (2003). Neuronal activity in the Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE & Desimone R (2001).
lateral intraparietal area and spatial attention. Science 299, Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by
8186. selective visual attention. Science 291, 15601563.


C 2010 The Author. Journal compilation 
C 2010 The Physiological Society
56 J. W. Bisley J Physiol 589.1

Fries P, Womelsdorf T, Oostenveld R & Desimone R (2008). McAdams CJ & Maunsell JH (1999a). Effects of attention on
The effects of visual stimulation and selective visual the reliability of individual neurons in monkey visual cortex.
attention on rhythmic neuronal synchronization in macaque Neuron 23, 765773.
area V4. J Neurosci 28, 48234835. McAdams CJ & Maunsell JHR (1999b). Effects of attention on
Ghose GM (2009). Attentional modulation of visual responses orientation-tuning functions of single neurons in macaque
by flexible input gain. J Neurophysiol 101, 20892106. cortical area V4. J Neurosci 19, 431441.
Gnadt JW & Andersen RA (1988). Memory related motor McAdams CJ & Maunsell JH (2000). Attention to both space
planning activity in posterior parietal cortex of macaque. and feature modulates neuronal responses in macaque area
Exp Brain Res 70, 216220. V4. J Neurophysiol 83, 17511755.
Gottlieb JP, Kusunoki M & Goldberg ME (1998). The McAdams CJ & Reid RC (2005). Attention modulates the
representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex. responses of simple cells in monkey primary visual cortex. J
Nature 391, 481484. Neurosci 25, 1102311033.
Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H & Desimone R (2009). McAlonan K, Cavanaugh J & Wurtz RH (2008). Guarding the
High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal gateway to cortex with attention in visual thalamus. Nature
and visual cortex during attention. Science 324, 12071210. 456, 391394.
Herrington TM & Assad JA (2010). Temporal sequence of McPeek RM (2008). Reversal of a distractor effect on saccade
attentional modulation in the lateral intraparietal area and target selection after superior colliculus inactivation. J
middle temporal area during rapid covert shifts of attention. Neurophysiol 99, 26942702.
J Neurosci 30, 32873296. McPeek RM & Keller EL (2002a). Saccade target selection in
Husain M & Nachev P (2007). Space and the parietal cortex. the superior colliculus during a visual search task. J
Trends Cogn Sci 11, 3036. Neurophysiol 88, 20192034.
Ignashchenkova A, Dicke PW, Haarmeier T & Thier P (2004). McPeek RM & Keller EL (2002b). Superior colliculus activity
Neuron-specific contribution of the superior colliculus to related to concurrent processing of saccade goals in a visual
overt and covert shifts of attention. Nat Neurosci 7, 5664. search task. J Neurophysiol 87, 18051815.
Ipata AE, Gee AL, Bisley JW & Goldberg ME (2009). Neurons Martinez-Trujillo J & Treue S (2002). Attentional modulation
in the lateral intraparietal area create a priority map by the strength in cortical area MT depends on stimulus contrast.
combination of disparate signals. Exp Brain Res 192, Neuron 35, 365370.
479488. Mirpour K, Arcizet F, Ong WS & Bisley JW (2009). Been there,
Ipata AE, Gee AL, Goldberg ME & Bisley JW (2006a). Activity seen that: a neural mechanism for performing efficient visual
in the lateral intraparietal area predicts the goal and latency search. J Neurophysiol 102, 34813491.
of saccades in a free-viewing visual search task. J Neurosci 26, Mitchell JF, Sundberg KA & Reynolds JH (2007). Differential
36563661. attention-dependent response modulation across cell classes
Ipata AE, Gee AL, Gottlieb J, Bisley JW & Goldberg ME in macaque visual area V4. Neuron 55, 131141.
(2006b). LIP responses to a popout stimulus are reduced if it Monosov IE & Thompson KG (2009). Frontal eye field activity
is overtly ignored. Nat Neurosci 9, 10711076. enhances object identification during covert visual search. J
Itti L & Koch C (2001). Computational modelling of visual Neurophysiol 102, 36563672.
attention. Nat Rev Neurosci 2, 194203. Moore T & Armstrong KM (2003). Selective gating of visual
Juan CH, Shorter-Jacobi SM & Schall JD (2004). Dissociation signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex. Nature 421,
of spatial attention and saccade preparation. Proc Natl Acad 370373.
Sci U S A 101, 1554115544. Moore T & Fallah M (2001). Control of eye movements and
Katzner S, Busse L & Treue S (2009). Attention to the color of a spatial attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98, 12731276.
moving stimulus modulates motion-signal processing in Moran J & Desimone R (1985). Selective attention gates visual
macaque area MT: evidence for a unified attentional system. processing in the extrastriate cortex. Science 229, 782784.
Front Syst Neurosci 3, 12. Motter BC (1993). Focal attention produces spatially selective
Lee J & Maunsell JH (2009). A normalization model of processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the
attentional modulation of single unit responses. PLoS ONE presence of competing stimuli. J Neurophysiol 70, 909919.
4, e4651. Motter BC (1994). Neural correlates of feature selective
Lee J & Maunsell JH (2010). Attentional modulation of MT memory and pop-out in extrastriate area V4. J Neurosci 14,
neurons with single or multiple stimuli in their receptive 21902199.
fields. J Neurosci 30, 30583066. Muller JR, Philiastides MG & Newsome WT (2005).
Liu Y, Yttri EA & Snyder LH (2010). Intention and attention: Microstimulation of the superior colliculus focuses attention
different functional roles for LIPd and LIPv. Nat Neurosci 13, without moving the eyes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102,
495500. 524529.
Lovejoy LP & Krauzlis RJ (2010). Inactivation of primate Platt ML & Glimcher PW (1999). Neural correlates of decision
superior colliculus impairs covert selection of signals for variables in parietal cortex. Nature 400, 233238.
perceptual judgments. Nat Neurosci 13, 261266. Posner MI (1980). Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32,
Luck SJ, Chelazzi L, Hillyard SA & Desimone R (1997). Neural 325.
mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, Rensink RA (2000). Seeing, sensing, and scrutinizing. Vision
and V4 of macaque visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 77, 2442. Res 40, 14691487.


C 2010 The Author. Journal compilation 
C 2010 The Physiological Society
J Physiol 589.1 Neural basis of visual attention 57

Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L & Desimone R (1999). Competitive Thomas NW & Pare M (2007). Temporal processing of saccade
mechanisms subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4. targets in parietal cortex area LIP during visual search. J
J Neurosci 19, 17361753. Neurophysiol 97, 942947.
Reynolds JH & Desimone R (2003). Interacting roles of Thompson KG, Biscoe KL & Sato TR (2005). Neuronal basis of
attention and visual salience in v4. Neuron 37, 853863. covert spatial attention in the frontal eye field. J Neurosci 25,
Reynolds JH & Heeger DJ (2009). The normalization model of 94799487.
attention. Neuron 61, 168185. Treue S & Martinez Trujillo JC (1999). Feature-based attention
Reynolds JH, Pasternak T & Desimone R (2000). Attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex.
increases sensitivity of V4 neurons. Neuron 26, 703714. Nature 399, 575579.
Roitman JD & Shadlen MN (2002). Response of neurons in the Treue S & Maunsell JH (1999). Effects of attention on the
lateral intraparietal area during a combined visual processing of motion in macaque middle temporal and
discrimination reaction time task. J Neurosci 22, medial superior temporal visual cortical areas. J Neurosci 19,
94759489. 75917602.
Saalmann YB, Pigarev IN & Vidyasagar TR (2007). Neural Vidyasagar TR (1998). Gating of neuronal responses in
mechanisms of visual attention: how top-down feedback macaque primary visual cortex by an attentional spotlight.
highlights relevant locations. Science 316, 16121615. Neuroreport 9, 19471952.
Sato T, Murthy A, Thompson KG & Schall JD (2001). Search Wardak C, Ibos G, Duhamel JR & Olivier E (2006).
efficiency but not response interference affects visual Contribution of the monkey frontal eye field to covert visual
selection in frontal eye field. Neuron 30, 583591. attention. J Neurosci 26, 42284235.
Seidemann E & Newsome WT (1999). Effect of spatial Wardak C, Olivier E & Duhamel JR (2004). A deficit in covert
attention on the responses of area MT neurons. J attention after parietal cortex inactivation in the monkey.
Neurophysiol 81, 17831794. Neuron 42, 501508.
Serences JT & Yantis S (2006). Selective visual attention and Williford T & Maunsell JH (2006). Effects of spatial attention
perceptual coherence. Trends Cogn Sci 10, 3845. on contrast response functions in macaque area V4. J
Shen K & Pare M (2007). Neuronal activity in superior Neurophysiol 96, 4054.
colliculus signals both stimulus identity and saccade goals Womelsdorf T, Anton-Erxleben K, Pieper F & Treue S (2006).
during visual conjunction search. J Vis 7, 15 113. Dynamic shifts of visual receptive fields in cortical area MT
Stanton GB, Bruce CJ & Goldberg ME (1995). Topography of by spatial attention. Nat Neurosci 9, 11561160.
projections to posterior cortical areas from the macaque Womelsdorf T, Anton-Erxleben K & Treue S (2008). Receptive
frontal eye fields. J Comp Neurol 353, 291305. field shift and shrinkage in macaque middle temporal area
Stanton GB, Goldberg ME & Bruce CJ (1988). Frontal eye field through attentional gain modulation. J Neurosci 28,
efferents in the macaque monkey: II. Topography of terminal 89348944.
fields in midbrain and pons. J Comp Neurol 271, 493506. Yeshurun Y & Carrasco M (1999). Spatial attention improves
Sugrue LP, Corrado GS & Newsome WT (2004). Matching performance in spatial resolution tasks. Vision Res 39,
behavior and the representation of value in the parietal 293306.
cortex. Science 304, 17821787.
Sundberg KA, Mitchell JF & Reynolds JH (2009). Spatial Acknowledgements
attention modulates center-surround interactions in
macaque visual area v4. Neuron 61, 952963. J.W.B. is supported by a Klingenstein Fellowship Award in
Tanaka M, Lindsley E, Lausmann S & Creutzfeldt OD (1990). the Neurosciences, an Alfred P. Sloan Foundation Research
Afferent connections of the prelunate visual association Fellowship, a McKnight Scholar Award, and the National Eye
cortex (areas V4 and DP). Anat Embryol (Berl) 181, 1930. Institute (R01 EY019273).


C 2010 The Author. Journal compilation 
C 2010 The Physiological Society

You might also like