You are on page 1of 2

Chunking by intermittent motor control in music

Rolf Inge Gody


Department of Musicology, University of Oslo, Norway
r.i.godoy@imv.uio.no

(Spivey 2008), the main argument for intermittency is that


ABSTRACT control takes time, and that continuous feedback is just not
feasible (Loram et al. 2011, 2014). The various constraints of
1. Background motor control also fit well with the idea of action gestalts as
optimal for our organism (Klapp and Jagacinski 2011).
Various theories in music perception and cognition, from clas- This means that we need a novel theory of chunking based
sical gestalt theory to more recent experimental work and on the idea of intermittency in motor control. Such a theory
data-driven modeling, have contributed to our understanding should also be capable of accommodating the (seemingly pa-
of chunking in musical experience (see Gody 2008 for a radoxical) coexistence of continuity and discontinuity in mu-
summary). But our own research on music-related body mo- sical experience, something we shall argue is possible by rec-
tion has singled out intermittency in motor control, i.e. a basi- ognizing intermittent motor control of chunks, yet recognizing
cally discontinuous and point-by-point control scheme, as an that chunks unfold continuously in time, and furthermore, that
essential factor in chunking. Classical motor control theories the concatenation of several chunks in succession may give
with claims of so-called closed loop continuous feedback have rise to subjective sensations of continuity in musical experi-
in recent years been challenged by models suggesting inter- ence.
mittent control, manifest in so-called open loop and prepro-
grammed motor commands, because continuous feedback 2. Aims
loops are thought to be too slow for many highly demanding
tasks (Loram et al. 2011, 2014). Various findings in human The aim of the presentation is to demonstrate a novel model
motor control, i.e. the so-called psychological refractory of intermittent motor control in musical performance and its
period, principles of posture-based motion control, of action consequences for chunking in music perception, based on
hierarchies, of goal-directed behavior, etc., and our own re- convergent findings in several domains and our own research
search on music-related body motion, seem to converge in on music-related body motion, in particular our findings on
suggesting the existence of intermittent motor control for coarticulation in music-related body motion, i.e. the fusion
chunking at the short-term timescale of very approximately and the contextual smearing of otherwise distinct motion and
0.5 seconds duration, all in all suggesting that there are what sound events into holistically perceived chunks. The main
could be called quantal elements in musical experience claims of the model are as follows:
(Gody 2013). That chunking is based on a confluence of production
Our interest in chunking stems from several years of re- constraints
search and reflections on what Pierre Schaeffer called sonic That anticipatory cognition is manifest in sound-produc-
objects (Schaeffer 1966), i.e. holistically perceived fragments ing (and also sound-accompanying) body motion
of musical sound in the approximately 0.5. to 5 seconds range, That we have fusion by coarticulation in musical perfor-
and which we with Schaeffer regard as one of the most essen- mance
tial elements of musical experience (Gody 2006). However, That we need a more rigorous analysis and differentiation
this also leads to questions of what are the criteria for the con- of timescales involved in the perception and production of
stitution of such sonic objects, and of how they are perceived music, and notably so, of both sound and corresponding
in musical contexts. It seems that various features in the audi- body motion.
tory signal (e.g. shifts between sound and silence, contrasts in This last point means that we in particular need to differenti-
pitch registers, contrasts in timbre, or the occurrence of re- ate the features of the sub-0.5 seconds timescale, what could
peated patterns, etc.) may be fairly robust cues for chunking, be called the delta timescale, and which typically includes the
but in many cases, these cues may be less salient (e.g. be psychological refractory period, as well as coarticulation and
weak, competing, masked, or even temporarily absent), re- thresholds for so-called phase-transitions, meaning the fusion
quiring the use of other perceptual schemas. Adopting a of singular events into superordinate events (or conversely,
so-called motor theory perspective (Galantucci, Fowler, and the fission of superordinate events into singular events), all
Turvey 2006) on music perception in general and chunking in dependent on the rate (the speed or temporal distance) and du-
particular, we believe images of sound-producing body mo- ration of events (Gody 2014).
tion may be effective in chunking continuous streams of Furthermore, the model should establish chunking in music
sound into somehow meaningful units in musical experience as conditioned by goal-directed behavior, similar to goal-di-
(Gody 2001, 2003, 2013, 2014). rected body motion in various everyday contexts, and as fo-
The basic tenet here is that schemas of sound-producing cused on certain salient postures (Rosenbaum et al. 2007). We
body motion are projected onto whatever musical sound it is could say that chunking is focused on what we call key-pos-
that we are hearing. This in turn means recognizing a number tures, meaning salient shapes and positions of the effectors
of constraints of body motion and motor control, something (fingers, hands, vocal tract, etc.), and that there is continuous,
that suggests an unequal distribution of attention and effort in coarticulated motion between these key-postures. Furthermore,
musical experience. Although the underlying neurocognitive we think these key-postures are found at salient moments in
processes of our organism may very well be continuous time, typically at downbeats or other accented points in the
music, at what we call goal-points. In music, we think of these Develop experimental methods for more systematic
key-postures at goal-points as surrounded by prefixes (tra- studies of intermittency in practice
jectories to the key-postures) and suffixes (trajectories from EMG measurements to indicate temporal distribution of
the key-postures), with overlap of suffixes and prefixes in se- effort in sound-producing body motion
quences of chunks producing the effect of continuous motion. Computational models (i.e. so-called reverse engineering)
The basic tenet here is that all motion takes time, i.e. that
of intermittent control in sound-producing body motion
instantaneous displacement of the effectors is impossible, and
hence, that there always will be contextual smearing of mo- 5. Keywords
tion leading to coarticulation within any chunk (Gody 2013,
2014, in press). Intermittency, motor control, action gestalts, coarticulation,
Also, we think we can detect these goal-points and key- contextual cohesion
postures in motion capture data of musicians' sound-produc- REFERENCES
ing body motion, as points associated with maximal accele-
ration and velocity reversals (Gody 2013, 2014), points in Galantucci, B., Fowler, C. A., and Turvey, M. T. (2006). The motor
time that we in turn understand as the basis for intermittent theory of speech perception reviewed. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 13 (3), 361-377.
control.
Gody, R. I. (2001). Imagined Action, Excitation, and Resonance. In
3. Main Contribution R. I. Godoy and H. Jorgensen (Eds.), Musical Imagery (pp.
239-252). Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger.
The main contribution of the presentation is a novel theory of Gody, R. I. (2003). Motor-mimetic Music Cognition. Leonardo
chunking in music based on constraints of human motor cog- 36(4), 317-319.
nition, in particular on the need for anticipatory motor control, Gody, R. I. (2006) Gestural-Sonorous Objects: embodied exten-
its manifestation in intermittent control, and the associated sions of Schaeffers conceptual apparatus. Organised Sound,
coarticulation and contextual smearing of motion and sound 11(2), 149-157.
within any chunk. This will all come together in a conceptual Gody, R. I. (2008). Reflections on Chunking in Music. In A.
model that will include: Schneider (Ed.), Systematic and comparative musicology:
1. Intermittent motor control, focused on key-postures at concepts, methods, findings (pp 117-132). Frankfurt: Peter
Lang.
goal-points
Gody, R. I. (2013). Quantal Elements in Musical Experience. In. R
2. Continuity, coherence, fusion, integration, etc. within any Bader (Ed.), Sound Perception Performance. Current Re-
chunk because of continuous motion trajectories with search in Systematic Musicology, Vol. 1 (pp. 113-128). Berlin,
coarticulation Heidelberg: Springer.
Gody, R. I. (2014). Understanding Coarticulation in Musical
One attractive feature of this model is that the duality of dis- Experience. In M. Aramaki, M. Derrien, R. Kronland-Martinet &
continuity and continuity is built into the model: the within S. Ystad (Eds.), Sound, Music, and Motion. Lecture Notes in
chunk coherence is due to a single discontinuous impulse, i.e. Computer Science Volume 8905 (pp. 535-547). Berlin: Springer.
to the intermittent, so-called serial ballistic (Loram et al. Gody, R. I. (in press). Key-postures, trajectories and sonic shapes.
2014), control impulse, focused on one goal-point. The evi- In D. Leech-Wilkinson and H. Prior (Eds.), Music & Shape. Ox-
dence for this model so far is in various other cognitive sci- ford: Oxford University Press.
ence research on human motor control, as well as the inter- Klapp, S. T., and Jagacinski, R. J. (2011). Gestalt Principles in the
pretation of our own empirical findings: Control of Motor Action. Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 137, No. 3,
Motion capture data on sound-producing motion tra- pp. 443462.
jectory shapes and their corresponding velocity and Loram, I. D., Gollee, H., Lakie, M., and Gawthrop, P. J. (2011).
acceleration features suggest the existence of goal-points Human control of an inverted pendulum: Is continuous control
necessary? Is intermittent control effective? Is intermittent
Motion capture data clearly indicate the coarticulatory control physiological? The Journal of Physiology, 589, 2, pp.
shaping of sound-producing motion trajectories. 307-324.
4. Implications Loram, I. D., van De Kamp, C., Lakie, M., Gollee, H., and Gawthrop,
P. J. (2014). Does the motor system need intermittent control?
One consequence of recognizing intermittent motor control is Exercise and Sport Science Review, Vol. 42, No. 3, pp. 117-125.
to modify our basic understanding of chunking in music so Rosenbaum, D., Cohen, R. G., Jax, S. A., Weiss, D. J., and van der
that we see chunking as just as much dependent on intermit- Wel, R. (2007). The problem of serial order in behavior:
tent motor control schemas as on purely acoustic features. Lashley's legacy. Human Movement Science, 26(4), 525-554.
Specifically, such an understanding of chunking may: Schaeffer, P. (1966). Trait des objets musicaux. Paris: ditions du
Seuil.
Account for chunking difficult to induce by purely sig- Spivey, M. (2008). The Continuity of Mind. New York: Oxford
nal-driven schemes University Press.
In focusing on goal-points, shift our attention towards
chunk centroids from chunk boundaries, i.e. boundaries
are seen as secondary to centroids, and this model may
also accommodate/tolerate fuzzy boundaries.
Account for the internal coherence and cohesion within
the chunk as the result of one singular and intermittent
control impulse
But clearly, we also need to:

You might also like