You are on page 1of 12

Asian American Journal of Psychology

Parenting Variables Associated With Growth Mindset: An


Examination of Three Chinese-Heritage Samples
Joanna J. Kim, Joey Fung, Qiaobing Wu, Chao Fang, and Anna S. Lau
Online First Publication, February 2, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aap0000064

CITATION
Kim, J. J., Fung, J., Wu, Q., Fang, C., & Lau, A. S. (2017, February 2). Parenting Variables Associated
With Growth Mindset: An Examination of Three Chinese-Heritage Samples. Asian American
Journal of Psychology. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aap0000064
Asian American Journal of Psychology 2017 American Psychological Association
2017, Vol. 8, No. 1, 000 1948-1985/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aap0000064

Parenting Variables Associated With Growth Mindset: An Examination of


Three Chinese-Heritage Samples

Joanna J. Kim Joey Fung


University of California, Los Angeles Fuller Theological Seminary

Qiaobing Wu Chao Fang


Chinese University of Hong Kong University of Science and Technology Beijing
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Anna S. Lau
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

University of California, Los Angeles

An incremental theory of intelligence (TOI), the belief that intelligence is malleable and can be improved
through effort, is associated with childrens academic achievement, mastery goals, and overall psycho-
logical well-being. Although the positive impact of TOI is well established, less is known about
socialization factors such as parenting that foster these orientations. We posited that both autonomy
support (AS) and psychological control (PC) may promote incremental TOI among Chinese heritage
fourth- to fifth-grade children and their mothers where socialization has largely been characterized as low
in AS and high in PC. However, we examined the possibility that these associations may differ across
contexts within the Chinese Diaspora (Beijing, Hong Kong, and Los Angeles) as child rearing evolves
as a function of rapid social change and immigration. A multiple-group structural equation model
revealed a positive association between child-report of maternal PC and child incremental TOI across all
three samples (BJ .37, p .001; HK .17, p .05; LA .26, p .01) and a positive association
between child-report of maternal AS and incremental TOI in Hong Kong only (HK .41, p .001).
Consistent with past studies, childrens perceptions of parenting were more proximal to child beliefs than
mothers reports of their own behaviors. These results are discussed in connection with the literature on
the dynamic role of culture in shaping parenting and associated developmental outcomes in Asian and
Asian American contexts.

What is the public significance of this article?


The present study suggests that psychological control is associated with growth mindset among
Chinese-heritage children in Beijing, Hong Kong, and Los Angeles while autonomy supportive
parenting is linked to growth mindset only in Hong Kong. This is in contrast to past research with
Western samples characterizing psychological control as detrimental and autonomy support as
tantamount to adjustment. Our findings suggest the dynamic role of social context in shaping
parenting and associated developmental outcomes.

Keywords: theories of intelligence, psychological control, autonomy support, Chinese parenting

Implicit theories of intelligence (TOIs), underlying beliefs re- in the face of perceived failure, and avoid challenges that might
garding the malleability or permanence of ones intelligence, are mar their sense of self. In contrast, children with an incremental
thought to influence childrens motivation, goals, achievement, TOI hold a growth mindset marked by the belief that ability is
and psychological well-being (Dweck, 1999). Children with entity malleable. These children tend to prioritize learning, persevere
TOIs believe intellectual abilities are fixed, typically hold perfor- through challenge, and be more invested in the process of gaining
mance goals aiming to showcase their abilities, often act helpless competence than demonstrating ability (Dweck & Leggett, 1988).

Anna S. Lau, Department of Psychology, University of California, Los


Joanna J. Kim, Department of Psychology, University of California, Angeles.
Los Angeles; Joey Fung, School of Psychology, Department of Clinical Chao Fang is now at School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University.
Psychology, Fuller Theological Seminary; Qiaobing Wu, Department of Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Joanna J.
Social Work, Chinese University of Hong Kong; Chao Fang, Depart- Kim, Department of Psychology, University of California, 1193 Franz
ment of Psychology, University of Science and Technology Beijing; Hall, Box 951563, Los Angeles, CA 90095. E-mail: joanna.kim@ucla.edu

1
2 KIM, FUNG, WU, FANG, AND LAU

Furthermore, teaching students incremental TOI promoted positive (Barber, Olsen, & Shagle, 1994; Pettit et al., 2001). Relatedly, it
changes in class motivation and an upward trajectory in grades could be argued that PC behaviors such as invalidating childrens
(Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007) and appears to buffer feelings, criticizing their behavior, or making upward social com-
the effects of stereotype threat, leading to increased academic parisons may lead children to feel ineffective and helpless, thereby
engagement and performance (Aronson, Fried, & Good, 2002). thwarting their motivation to improve themselvesa psychologi-
Interestingly, the belief in incremental TOI is largely consistent cal state associated with entity orientations.
with what has been described as a valued goal of child socializa- Yet, these premises about the implications of PC for child
tion in East Asian cultures. Not only do Confucian teachings developmental outcomes are based largely on SDT, which emerges
emphasize the responsibility of children to learn through effort, but from a Western, independent cultural point of view. East Asian
they also underline the duty of parents to teach children to value parenting has been globally characterized as lower in AS and
hard work (Kim & Wong, 2002; Park, Coello, & Lau, 2014). higher in PC than Western parenting (Lin & Fu, 1990; Wang &
Indeed, Asian American students more often attribute success and Phinney, 1998). For example, compared to European Americans,
failure to effort compared to their European American counterparts Chinese and Chinese American parents exert more control over
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(Mizokawa & Ryckman, 1990). Compared to European Ameri- child conduct, granting less autonomy and expecting more obedi-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cans, individuals of Asian descent are more likely to manifest a ence (Fung & Lau, 2012; Wu, 1996). Based on the reasoning
self-improving orientation (i.e., tendency to focus on ones relative above, one might expect Chinese children to have poorer adjust-
weaknesses in order to improve the self) rather than self-enhancing ment, have lower achievement, and prescribe to an entity TOI. Yet,
orientation (i.e., tendency to focus on ones talents and successes some data suggest that East Asian youth are more likely than
to maintain a positive view of the self; Heine et al., 2001). European American youth to hold incremental theories and persist
in the face of academic challenge (Li, 2003; Stevenson & Stigler,
1992). Given the general characterization of traditional Chinese
Autonomy Support and Psychological Control
parenting as high on PC and low on AS, it may suggest a paradox
Although the impact of TOI on academic achievement and if Chinese youth were also more likely to hold growth mindsets.
motivation, performance, and resilience is well established, less is In particular, such a pattern might suggest that the effects of PC
known about naturalistic socialization factors that foster these on child adjustment may be shaped by the broader cultural context.
orientations. Emerging evidence suggests that TOI can be fostered For instance, Fung and Lau (2012) found that parent reliance on
(Blackwell et al., 2007). For instance, mothers use of person- PC was associated with internalizing and externalizing behavior
centered praise appears to lead to increases in child entity TOI and problems among European American children but not among
avoidance of challenge over time (Pomerantz & Kempner, 2013). Hong Kong children. Although PC can be viewed as invalidating
These findings reflect beliefs described by Asian-heritage parents and detrimental to child well-being in Western contexts where the
that liberal praise may spoil children by inflating esteem in ways development of autonomy is prized, these strategies may carry
that lead to reduced persistence and efforts to improve (Lau, 2012; different meanings and implications in a more interdependent
Lau, Fung, & Yung, 2010). Aside from praise, the role of other cultural context such as Hong Kong. PC may be congruent with
parenting behaviors in shaping TOI remains unclear. Autonomy more interdependent socialization goals of interpersonal attun-
support (AS) involves parents promoting childrens independence ement and sensitivity to relational role demands and social rules in
in ways that may foster a sense of agency and self-efficacy. AS is Asian-origin families.
linked to developmental outcomes such as intrinsic motivation, Although studies have associated specific styles of parenting
achievement, social competence, and well-being (e.g., Ryan & with child academic and psychological adjustment across cultural
Deci, 2000), which, in turn, are associated with incremental TOI in groups (e.g., Chao, 2001; Eamon, 2005; Hill, 2001; Spera, 2005),
children (Grolnick & Ryan, 1989). These indirect links suggest few studies have investigated the associations between perceived
that autonomy supportive parenting may lead to outcomes, includ- parenting and childrens implicit TOI. Work in this domain has
ing growth mindset and perseverance. It stands to reason that focused primarily on the downstream effects of types of praise on
encouraging children to explore independently and validating their childrens attribution of success and failure. Conventional under-
perspectives may engender a sense control and mastery, which standing derived from research on Western parenting styles might
may translate into beliefs that their efforts can foster ability. suggest that parents PC is detrimental and AS beneficial for the
Conversely, parental reliance on psychological control (PC) has development of growth mindset, but this may not necessarily be
been associated with child anxiety (Pettit, Laird, Dodge, Bates, & the case in Asian-origin families. Among Chinese-heritage fami-
Criss, 2001), aggression (Loukas, Paulos, & Robinson, 2005), and lies, PC may in fact prompt children to increase effort toward
other indicators of poor adjustment in children (e.g., Barber, 1996). mastery goals. Likewise, instead of providing opportunities for
PC tactics aim to control childrens behavior by constraining their children to fail and then learn from missteps, AS may allow
emotional expression, invalidating complaints, and inducing guilt children to make choices that avoid challenge and ultimately foster
for transgressions. Within self-determination theory (SDT), PC is an entity TOI.
thought to jeopardize the specific needs of child autonomy, relat-
edness, and competence, which are believed to be crucial to
Different Social Contexts for Three Chinese Samples
motivation and subsequent achievement and well-being (Deci &
Ryan, 2008; Ryan & Deci, 2000). SDT posits that PC acts to Furthermore, it is crucial to keep in mind the vast social changes
thwart children in their development of a sense of personal agency, that have occurred in East Asian in recent decades. The Peoples
mastery, and competence, which may help explain why PC is Republic of China, in particular, has experienced unprecedented
robustly linked to increased child distress and behavior problems economic growth accompanied by massive social change and
PARENTING AND GROWTH MINDSET 3

rapidly shifting values toward individualism and child rearing that The Present Study
emphasizes child-centered autonomy and personal choice (Chen,
Cen, Li, & He, 2005; Park, Coello, & Lau, 2014; Pearson, & Rao, The present study aims to address the question of how maternal
PC and AS may be associated with incremental TOI among
2003; Sun & Ryder, 2016). Some of this change in parenting
Chinese-heritage children. Additionally, we aim to examine if and
orientation may be attributed to Chinas one-child family plan-
how these associations may vary within the Chinese Diaspora.
ning policy (Arnold & Zhaoxiang, 1986). With social and eco-
Given differences in social, political, and economic contexts, there
nomic changes, traditional characterizations of Chinese parenting
is large diversity in language, cultural traditions, and values among
as high in PC and low in AS may no longer hold (Lai, Zhang, &
Chinese-heritage individuals, and it has become increasingly more
Wang, 2000). In urban Mainland China, traditions in child social-
important to examine the impact of such heterogeneity (Fiske,
ization appear to be shifting toward independent developmental
2002; Ho, Peng, Lai, & Chan, 2001; Tardif & Miao, 2000).
competencies favored in a competitive market economy, such as
Although past studies have focused on comparisons between im-
fostering assertiveness and self-esteem through child-centered par-
migrant Chinese and Mainland Chinese samples or between im-
enting (Chen et al., 2005; Way et al., 2013). In contrast, a more
migrant Chinese and Hong Kong Chinese samples, this study is a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

traditional focus on parental authority is observed among cohorts novel study that examines the heterogeneity of Chinese-heritage
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of Chinese parents who migrated prior to economic reform or who parenting across three different social contexts. As mothers per-
have resided in more stable economies, such as Hong Kong (Lai et ceptions of their own parenting style may shape childrens per-
al., 2000; Luo, Tamis-LeMonda, & Song, 2013). Confucian tradi- ceptions of parenting, we examine the associations of both moth-
tions on child socialization may be more stable in places like Hong ers and childrens perceived maternal parenting behaviors with
Kong and Taiwan that have not made a recent, rapid transition to incremental TOI. Past research has often focused on the effects of
market economies. Hong Kong in particular has been host to a parenting on adolescents social cognitions (e.g., Molden &
much more static and stable Chinese society (Yau & Smetana, Dweck, 2006). However, social cognitions have already begun to
2003). Hong Kong families appear to be largely emphasizing develop by as early as kindergarten, and childrens beliefs, goals,
parenting goals such filial piety and family hierarchy, as well as and motivations appear to change developmentally (Kinlaw &
parenting marked by high demandingness and PC (Chan, Bowes, Kurtz-Costes, 2007). Thus, in the present study, fourth- to fifth-
& Wyver, 2009; Fung & Lau, 2012; Pearson & Rao, 2003). grade elementary schoolchildren were targeted so as to capture the
In the discussion of Chinese parenting and differing social preadolescent development of childrens theories of intelligence at
contexts, it is crucial to consider the experiences of immigrant an age where maternal socialization is likely to influence children
Chinese families, specifically that of Chinese American families and their still developing self-theories (Pomerantz & Kempner,
for whom the immigrant experience often influences the parents 2013).
own prioritization of socialization and values. Much prior research We hypothesized that PC would be related to greater incremen-
has illuminated differences between Chinese American parenting tal TOI given elements of PC that may prompt children to strive to
and European American parenting styles with particular attention do better and improve upon themselves but that AS would be
on Chinese-heritage parents use of control and expectations to associated with lower incremental TOI within Hong Kong children
train children (Chao, 2000, 2001). Comparisons of Chinese who live in a context of parenting traditions of training for im-
American immigrant and Chinese families remaining in distinctive provement. However, we predicted that in Beijing, a dynamic
sending countries provide a lens to understand shifts in values and context of Mainland urban China affected by rapid social change,
parenting practices related to social change and migration. For we may see converse relationships such that PC may constrain
instance, Chinese American parents raising bicultural children may incremental TOI while AS may facilitate it. Chinese American
experience an explicit tension in rearing their children to value families are in a unique position with immigrant parents largely
parental authority and family interdependence in a context that raised within traditional Chinese child-rearing values, but with
largely promotes child independence and autonomy (Cheah, immigration introducing new priorities toward child autonomy. As
Leung, & Zhou, 2013; Lau, 2010). The struggle to find balance such, we did not pose directional hypotheses for Chinese American
between heritage and host cultural values in parenting may be families but explored whether relations between parenting and TOI
unique to the immigrant experience (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008) would conform to those predicted within the context of social
and may actually evoke more parental control than in nonimmi- change toward independence promotion (as in Beijing) or tradi-
grant families (Nguyen, Kim, Ngo, Weiss, & Lau, 2017). How- tional orientations toward interdependence (as in Hong Kong). To
ever, while the immigrant context may shape levels of parental our knowledge, these relations have not yet been examined in
control observed in families, it is unclear how the immigrant literature and have not been examined with elementary-age chil-
family context may shape the associations between parental con- dren whose implicit TOIs are likely continuing to form.
trol and child developmental outcomes. Although some studies
have shown associations between facets of PC with poor youth Method
outcomes such as dampened emotional functioning, deviant be-
havior, lower academic competence, and behavioral symptoms
Participants
(e.g., Chao & Aque, 2009; Lamborn, Dornbusch, & Steinberg,
1996), emerging evidence also points to benefits of parental con- Participants were 424 fourth- and fifth-grade children and their
trol among Asian Americans. For instance, perceived maternal mothers (166 dyads from Beijing, 150 dyads from Hong Kong, and
control has been shown to enhance the impact of parental accep- 132 dyads from Los Angeles). Mothers age in the original sample
tance on youth academic achievement (Kim & Rohner, 2002). ranged from 29 65 years. However, only mother-child dyads in
4 KIM, FUNG, WU, FANG, AND LAU

which maternal age was 50 years old or younger were included in tary school and two local weekend Chinese-heritage language
the analyses. In order to try to capture the socialization goals and schools in LA with the permission and administrative help of
strategies of a singular cohort of mothers, we elected to exclude school officials. Students and parents were informed that the study
respondents in the baby boomer generation of mothers born was an international collaboration studying parenting among
between 1946 and 1964. This criterion resulted in removing from Chinese-descent families and child adjustment. Although only one
the analytic sample two participants from the Beijing sample, eight elementary school per city was targeted, in each of the cities, the
from Hong Kong, and seven from Los Angeles, for a total of 17 selected elementary schools were considered typical schools. That
observations. For the Beijing (BJ) sample, maternal age ranged is, none were reputed to be particularly high- or low-achieving and
from 3250 years (M 39.54, SD 3.21) and child age ranged enrolled residents generally representative of their surrounding
from 8 12 years (M 9.87, SD .79). For the Hong Kong (HK) neighborhoods. Children were given study packets, which in-
sample, maternal age ranged from 29 50 years (M 40.41, SD cluded consent and questionnaires to take home to discuss with
5.39) and child age ranged from 8 11 years (M 9.65, SD .68). their parents and complete the questionnaires if they decided to
In the Los Angeles (LA) sample, maternal age ranged from 3250 participate in the study. HK children and parents were provided
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

years (M 42.43; SD 4.16) and child age ranged from 712 questionnaires in traditional Chinese while BJ children and parents
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

years (M 9.61; SD .99). Table 1 presents demographic were provided questionnaires in simplified Chinese. One hundred
information on the three samples. Approximately half of the child percent of invited students in the HK and BJ elementary schools
participants were female (49.66% in BJ, 50.34% in HK, and participated in the study, but the participation rate information is
46.02% in LA). not available for the LA sample as we were unable to estimate the
All participants were of Chinese heritage, and the majority of total number of eligible children approached through the LA
child were born in their area of residence (95.70% in BJ, 95.24% elementary school and Chinese-language schools. LA children
in HK, and 80.56% in LA). In addition to Mainland China (n 16, completed their surveys in English while mothers completed the
14.81%), HK (n 1, 0.93%), and the United States (n 87, surveys in their preferred language (English, simplified Chinese,
80.56%), LA children were also born in Europe (n 2, 1.85%) or traditional Chinese). All children and their mothers completed
and other countries in Asia (n 2, 1.85%). Among those who questionnaires independently from one another. All data for the
mothers who reported their birthplace, 85.71% of BJ mothers were three samples were collected during the 20122013 academic year.
born in Mainland China and 77.45% of HK mothers were born in
HK. Among LA mothers who reported their birthplace, 51 Measures
(40.48%) were born in Mainland China, 13 (10.32%) in HK, 29
Implicit TOIs. Child participants were given the six-item
(23.02%) in Taiwan, 18 (14.29%) in other Asian countries, and 15
Implicit Theories of Intelligence Scale (Dweck, 1999) and indi-
(11.90%) in the United States. Among LA children for whom we
cated level of agreement on a 5-point Likert scale (0 strongly
had both child and mother birthplace, 21 (19.44%) were first-
disagree to 4 strongly agree). Three statements were designed
generation Chinese Americans and 87 (80.56%) were at least
to tap into participants entity TOI, indicating participants belief
second-generation Chinese American. Less than half of LA fam-
that intelligence is fixed and cannot be changed (e.g., You can
ilies lived in historical ethnic enclaves with high Asian- and
learn new things, but you cant really change your basic intelli-
Latino- immigrant family density (n 54, 40.91%), and the
gence.). Cronbach alphas are .80 for BJ, .76 for HK, and .62 for
remaining LA families hailed from other diverse neighborhoods
LA. The remaining three statements in the scale were designed to
around greater Los Angeles that were not particularly known for
measure participants incremental TOI or the belief that intelli-
high Chinese American representation (Tseng, 1994).
gence is malleable and can be changed with effort and time (e.g.,
No matter who you are, you can change your intelligence a lot.).
Procedure
Cronbach alphas are .87 for BJ, .79 for HK, and .82 for LA.
Participants were recruited through one local elementary school Childrens incremental theory orientation was significantly corre-
in BJ, one local elementary school in HK, and one local elemen- lated with childrens entity orientation at r .29 (p .01) for BJ

Table 1
Sample Demographics by City

Characteristic Beijing (n 166), n (%) Hong Kong (n 150), n (%) Los Angeles (n 132), n (%)

Gender (female) 73 (49.66), nmissing 19 74 (50.34), nmissing 3 52 (46.02), nmissing 19


Children born in area of residencea 89 (95.70), nmissing 73 120 (95.24), nmissing 24 7 (80.56), nmissing 28
Mothers born in area of residencea 116 (85.92), nmissing 31 79 (77.45), nmissing 48 15 (11.90), nmissing 6
Child first-generation Chinese American 21 (19.44), nmissing 24
Residing in high-density Chinese ethnic enclave 54 (40.91), nmissing 0
Maternal education (total years)
Elementary school (15 years) 5 (3.29) 6 (4.51) 5 (4.07)
Middle school (68 years) 3 (1.97) 27 (20.30) 3 (2.44)
High school (912 years) 25 (16.45) 58 (43.61) 19 (15.45)
Some college (1316 years) 69 (45.39) 31 (23.31) 31 (25.20)
Advanced degree (17 years) 50 (32.89) 11 (8.27) 65 (52.85)
Missing 14 17 9
a
Mainland China and United States included as area of residence for Beijing and Los Angeles families, respectively.
PARENTING AND GROWTH MINDSET 5

and at r .24 (p .05) for LA and marginally significantly Support Scale (Wang et al., 2007). Six items tapped into opinion
correlated at r .16 (p .06) for HK. exchange (e.g., She listens to my opinion or perspective when
Although some studies in the past using this measure have Ive got a problem.) and six items tapped into choice making
reversed-scored the entity or incremental items and then calculated (e.g., She allows me to decide things for myself.). For mothers,
a mean from the full six items (e.g., Dweck, 1999), other studies the items were modified such that they answered about their own
have calculated separate incremental and entity scores from their autonomy supportive practices (e.g., I am usually willing to
respective three items often due to co-occurrence of incremental consider things from my childs point of view.). Each participant
and entity orientations (e.g., Brten & Strms, 2005; Dupeyrat & responded with his or her level of agreement for each statement on
Marin, 2005; Hong, Chiu, Dweck, Lin, & Wan, 1999; Pomerantz a 5-point Likert scale (0 not at all true to 4 very true). The
& Kempner, 2013). Additionally, Dweck and colleagues acknowl- overall scale yielded good internal consistency with alphas of .84
edge the potential for individuals to hold varying degrees of and .89 in BJ, .92 and .93 in HK, and .87 and .88 in LA for children
different orientations simultaneously (Dweck, Chiu, & Hong, and mothers, respectively.
1995). To correct for the co-occurrence of entity and incremental
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

TOI in the current sample (the two variables were correlated at r Results
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

.14, p .05 for the full sample; see above for correlations by city),
we calculated separate childrens entity and incremental TOI ori- Preliminary Analyses
entations to calculate our variable of interest: residualized incre-
mental TOI scores. Childrens self-reports of incremental TOI raw Descriptive statistics for the study variables are listed in Table 2.
scores were regressed on their entity TOI raw scores. The residu- One-way analyses of variance revealed significant group mean
alized scores were used to measure the unique variance in child differences on raw incremental TOI, incremental TOI controlling
incremental TOI in the sample, controlling for entity TOI. Unless for entity theory, and all reports of parenting behaviors among the
otherwise indicated, the residualized scores were used as the three groups. LA childrens report of raw incremental theory was
measured dependent variable in all data analyses for this study. significantly greater than that reported in BJ, t 3.57, p .001,
Psychological control. Children and mothers reported on and HK, t 3.17, p .001. Likewise, we found that when
mothers PC by responding to the 18-item Psychological Control controlling for entity theory orientation, children in LA reported
Scale (Wang, Pomerantz, & Chen, 2007). Children were asked to significantly higher incremental TOI than children in BJ,
rate their level of agreement with 10 statements that measured guilt t 3.30, p .001, and HK, t 3.17, p .001. Maternal use
induction (e.g., She tells me that I should feel guilty when I do not of PC was significantly higher in HK compared with BJ
meet her expectations.), 5 statements that measured love with- (tchild 3.69, p .001; tmother 3.38, p .001) and LA
drawal (e.g., She acts cold and unfriendly if I do something she (tchild 4.30, p .001; tmother 5.47, p .001) based on both
doesnt like.), and 3 statements that measured authority assertion child- and mother-report. Children reported significantly lower
(e.g., She answers my arguments by saying things like, Youll autonomy supportive behaviors in LA compared to BJ, t 3.01,
know better when you grow up.). For mothers, each statement p .01, and HK, t 2.66, p .01. However, per mother-report,
was modified to ask about mothers own practices (e.g., I tell my autonomy supportive behaviors are significantly less utilized in
child that he/she should feel ashamed when he/she does not behave HK than both BJ, t 3.01, p .01, and LA, t 1.87, p .05.
as I wish.). Participants indicated level of agreement with each Correlations are presented for all study variables in Table 3.
statement on a 5-point Likert scale (0 not at all true to 4 very Child-report of AS was positively correlated with mother-report of
true). The overall scale yielded good internal consistency with AS for all groups, whereas child-report of PC was only positively
Cronbach alphas of .89 and .92 in BJ, .88 and .94 in HK, and .92 correlated with mother-report of PC for BJ and LA dyads. Mother-
and .90 in LA for children and mothers, respectively. report of PC and AS was negatively correlated in BJ, positively
Autonomy support. Mothers use of autonomy supportive correlated in HK, and not significantly correlated in LA. Incre-
parenting practices was measured using the 12-item Autonomy mental TOI was positively correlated with child-report of mothers

Table 2
Descriptives for Predictor and Outcome Variables by City

Beijing Hong Kong Los Angeles


Variable M SD Range M SD Range M SD Range F

Child entity TOI 1.44 1.12 0 to 4 1.52 1.02 0 to 4 1.75 .86 0 to 4 2.65
Child incremental TOI 1.74a 1.21 0 to 4 1.87a 1.06 0 to 4 2.30b 1.01 0 to 4 7.42
Incremental TOI controlling
for entity orientation .20a 1.17 2.27 to 2.28 .08a 1.05 2.12 to 2.28 .31b 1.05 2.32 to 2.28 6.39
Child-report PC 1.43a .81 0 to 3.56 1.78b .78 0 to 4 1.33c .87 0 to 3.71 10.88
Child-report AS 2.32a .75 0 to 4 2.33a .98 0 to 4 2.01b .83 0 to 3.75 4.99
Mother-report PC 1.42a .59 0 to 3.53 1.68b .67 0 to 4 1.23a .64 0 to 3.5 16.22
Mother-report AS 2.65a .57 1 to 4 2.43b .64 0 to 4 2.57a .59 0 to 4 4.70
Note. Means with different superscripts denote significantly different means. TOI theory of intelligence; PC psychological control; AS autonomy
support.

p .10. p .01. p .001.
6 KIM, FUNG, WU, FANG, AND LAU

Table 3
Pairwise Correlations for Predictors and Outcomes Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5

Beijing sample
1. Childs incremental theory orientation
2. Child-report mothers psychological control .23
3. Child-report mothers autonomy support .15 .08
4. Mother-report mothers psychological control .02 .36 .07
5. Mother-report mothers autonomy support .15 .12 .21 .30
Hong Kong sample
1. Childs incremental theory orientation
2. Child-report mothers psychological control .31
3. Child-report mothers autonomy support .41 .26
4. Mother-report mothers psychological control .02 .04 .05
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

5. Mother-report mothers autonomy support .07 .12 .22 .28


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Los Angeles sample


1. Childs incremental theory orientation
2. Child-report mothers psychological control .25
3. Child-report mothers autonomy support .07 .01
4. Mother-report mothers psychological control .08 .41 .25
5. Mother-report mothers autonomy support .09 .09 .29 .01
Note. In all analyses, incremental theory orientation is correcting for reported entity theory orientation.

p .05. p .01. p .001.

PC and AS for the HK sample but only correlated with child-report tion modeling (SEM) was used to create a multiple-group model
of PC for BJ and LA cohorts. Incremental TOI was not correlated (Brown, 2012). The Mplus default of Full Information Maximum
with mother-report of either PC or AS. Likelihood was used in which all available data, including obser-
vations with missing data on one or more variables, are included in
Multiple-Group Structural Equation Model analyses of parameter estimates.
The statistical package Mplus 7.0 was used for all analyses A multiple-group SEM was used to test if the hypothesized
(Muthn & Muthn, 1998 2012). Analyses investigated the asso- relationships among the reported parenting behaviors and child
ciation between incremental TOI and multi-informant variables of TOI were equivalent across cities (see Figure 1). Child sex, child
parenting: child and mother-report of PC and AS. Structural equa- age, and mothers education were considered control variables.

Figure 1. Multiple-group structural equation model of parenting variables and associations with incre-
mental theory of intelligence (TOI). Note: Each path and factor loading is labeled with three coefficients
indicating Beijing/Hong Kong/Los Angeles. Standardized coefficients depicted. Dashed line indicates path
where all group coefficients have p .05. PC psychological control; AS autonomy support; C
child-report; M mother-report. p .05. p .01. p .001.
PARENTING AND GROWTH MINDSET 7

However, child sex was not significantly correlated with any study AS was only significantly correlated for HK children (HK .23,
variables and therefore was not included as a covariate in the final p .05).
model. Child age was significantly correlated with child-report of
AS and PC, and mothers education was significantly correlated Discussion
with both informants reports of PC. As such, both were included
in the final model as covariates. We examined perceptions of maternal parenting and their rela-
The multiple-group model displayed good fit with Root Mean tion to child incremental TOI among families in BJ, HK, and LA.
Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) .069, Comparative Contrary to the general notion that AS and PC have opposing
Fit Index (CFI) .924, Tucker Lewis Index (TLI) .913, and influences on child development, we found that childrens reported
Square Root Mean Residual (SRMR) .080. A joint Wald test of experience of maternal PC was positively associated with chil-
structural paths was significant, indicating that it is most appro- drens incremental TOI orientations in BJ, HK, and LA. Childrens
priate to examine data within a multiple-group model, 2(12) perceptions of AS were positively associated with childrens in-
26.85, p .01. Factor loadings on latent factors ranged from cremental TOI in HK and marginally associated in BJ, but there
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.71.94. A joint Wald test of all measurement paths demonstrated was no association between incremental TOI and child-reported
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

metric noninvariance, 2(24) 85.69, p .001. Following the AS in LA. Chinese American children reported that their mothers
Byrne, Shavelson, and Muthn (1989) framework for assessing used less AS compared to their BJ and HK counterparts. Given the
measurement invariance, we tested individual measurement paths relatively low endorsement of child-report of AS among the LA
to find that 3 of the 12 measurement paths in the model were sample, a floor effect may have impeded our ability to detect a
noninvariant across groups but at least one factor was invariant for significant association between child-perceived AS and incremen-
each latent construct, thus meeting criteria for partial invariance. tal TOI.
Given partial invariance, we are able to make inferences about Contrary to the general notion that PC is harmful (e.g., Barber,
group differences within the full model (Byrne et al., 1989; van de 1996) and AS is beneficial (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 2008; Ryan & Deci,
Schoot, Lugtig, & Hox, 2012). 2000) to children, our study found evidence that both maternal PC
Contrary to predictions that PC would be negatively related to and AS were linked to incremental TOI in Chinese-heritage chil-
dren in HK and that PC was associated with incremental TOI in the
incremental TOI in BJ and positively related in HK, child-report of
BJ and LA samples. Although studies have generally documented
maternal PC was significantly positively associated with child
the deleterious effects of PC on child outcomes, we found that
incremental TOI for all three groups (BJ .37, p .001; HK
maternal PC contributed to a theoretically beneficial type of social
.17, p .05; LA .26, p .01). The magnitude of associations
cognition for Chinese-heritage children across all three cities.
between child TOI and child-reported PC was not significantly
Findings echo recent research calling into question the deleterious
different across the three groups, 2(2) 1.85, p .05. Child-
effects of parental PC and child emotional and behavior outcomes
report of maternal AS was significantly associated with child
in Asian interdependent cultural contexts (e.g., Fung & Lau, 2012;
incremental TOI for HK children (HK .41, p .001), margin-
Olsen et al., 2002; Pomerantz & Wang, 2009). PC includes be-
ally associated in BJ (BJ .19, p .10), and not significantly
haviors meant to control child behavior through disapproval as
associated for LA (LA .08, p .05). The magnitude of
well as behaviors that communicate high expectations. In cultural
associations between child-reported AS and child TOI was mar- contexts that are more interdependent, stressing the impact of the
ginally significantly different between the three groups, 2(2) childs actions or efforts on others or placing the childs behavior
4.95, p .10. This was inconsistent with our expectation that AS in comparison to peers may spur a growth mindset. There is also
would be negatively related to incremental TOI specifically for a focus on attuning the child to the threat of bringing shame or
HK children. embarrassment to the mother or the family or alerting the child to
Mother-report of their own PC was significantly associated with how his or her conduct may make the parent feel. Such parenting
child-report of maternal PC for BJ and LA dyads (BJ .38, p practices may be a salient determinant of child effort and persis-
.01; LA .40, p .001) but not for HK (HK .01, p .05). tence in interdependent cultural contexts where relational esteem is
The magnitude of these associations did not differ significantly often valued over personal esteem. Although autonomy granting
between BJ and LA samples, 2(1) .01, p .05. For AS, enhances intrinsic motivation among children from independent
mother-report of parenting was significantly associated with child- cultures, youth from interdependent cultures may benefit more
report for all groups (BJ .28, p .05; HK .25, p .01; from decisions made for them by trusted authority figures as often
LA .31, p .01). The strength of this relationship did not differ occurs in high-PC households (Iyengar & Lepper, 1999). Future
significantly between the three groups, 2(2) .17, p .05. To research with other cultural groups is needed to understand the
examine how PC and AS may be associated with one another, we implications of PC for TOI in contexts that lean toward indepen-
also examined structural paths from mother-report of PC and AS to dent developmental pathways.
child-report of AS and PC, respectively. Mother-report of AS was Despite the significant associations between child-report of par-
not significantly associated with child-report of PC in any of the enting and incremental TOI, mother-reports of their own PC and
three groups. Mother-report of PC was significantly associated AS behaviors were not significantly correlated with incremental
with child-report of AS only in LA (LA .32, p .01). The TOI for any of the three samples. However, this is consistent with
multiple-group SEM also revealed that mother-report of PC and previous findings that child perceptions of socialization and other
AS was significantly negatively correlated for BJ dyads but sig- experiences are most proximally related to child outcomes (e.g.,
nificantly positively correlated for HK families (BJ .36, p Barry, Frick, & Grafeman, 2008; Paulson & Sputa, 1996). Never-
.001; HK .22, p .01). Additionally, child-report of PC and theless, mothers intentions and reports of their own parenting
8 KIM, FUNG, WU, FANG, AND LAU

behavior are important in that they are systematically related to al., 2003). Kagitibasi (2005) argues that in the context of devel-
child perceptions of parenting experiences and thus generate sig- opment and modernization as independence orientations rise, we
nificant indirect effects on child TOI. These findings emphasize may not observe a corresponding decrease in relatedness. The HK
the need for multiple informants when examining the effects of sample suggests this autonomy-relatedness complex of socializa-
parenting on child development (Bgels & van Melick, 2004; tion. In contrast, within the LA Chinese American sample, there
Bgels, van Oosten, Muris, & Smulders, 2001). was no association between maternal reliance on AS and PC,
Beyond the main effects of parenting behaviors on child incre- suggesting an orthogonal relationship between PC and AS, where
mental TOI, we observed a variety of differences across cities in reliance on one strategy may not be systematically associated with
the variables of interest. Notably, child-report of PC was lower in use of the other.
LA than in BJ, which was lower than PC in HK. Similarly, The findings of the study should be considered in light of its
mother-report of PC was lower in LA and BJ compared to HK. In limitations. First, the cross-sectional design precludes us draw-
terms of AS, LA childrens reported AS levels were lower than ing conclusions about directionality of influence between par-
that of BJ and HK children while HK mothers endorsed signifi- enting and child growth mindset. Although parenting behaviors
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cantly less AS than their BJ and LA counterparts. Our results are may foster incremental or entity TOI in children, it is equally
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

consistent with studies that found HK mothers to be more author- possible that children with incremental orientations may prompt
itarian and controlling than BJ mothers (e.g., Lai et al., 2000) and parents to engage in more PC and AS. Indeed, it is plausible that
conceptually in line with data indicating that HK Chinese are there may be either child perception differences shaped by TOI
significantly less individualistic than European Americans (Oyser- or actual child evocative effects at play. For example, children
man, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002). The observed higher levels of who perceive themselves as more agentic and/or have a stronger
reported PC in HK compared to BJ are consistent with our pre- growth orientation may be more likely to perceive their parents
diction that interdependent socialization via maternal use of PC as being more psychologically controlling (perhaps than is
has been more stably maintained outside the mainland than in the warranted). Alternatively, highly agentic children with growth
context of rapid social change in urban China. Recall that prior to mindset may exhibit signs of drive that prompt parents to push
the sweeping social and economic shifts in Mainland China, tra- them toward achievement harder. Longitudinal investigation will
ditional Chinese parenting was characterized by an emphasis on
allow for illumination on the directionality of the socialization-growth
parental respect, unquestioning obedience, and training children
mindset relationship. Future prospective research would allow us to
about the impact of their interpersonal interactions (Chao, 1995; Ho,
draw conclusions to the development of family-based interventions to
1989). Although seminal studies comparing Chinese and Western
facilitate the development of childrens growth mindset.
parenting have shown Chinese parenting to be more authoritarian
Another limitation is the relatively low endorsement of in-
and emphasizing one-way parent-to-child communication over
cremental and entity TOI among children in our sample. Mean
that of Chinese American and Anglo American families (Chao,
absolute ratings of incremental TOI were 1.74 for BJ, 1.87 for
1994; Chiu, 1987), emerging evidence points us to shifts away
HK, and 2.30 for LA (range: 0 4). Compared to previous
from PC within Mainland China (Way et al., 2013). Another
studies, this represents relatively low endorsement of incremen-
reason that may account for the observed group differences in
tal TOI orientations in the case of BJ and HK. In a study of
child perceptions of parenting may involve the bicultural con-
text of the LA sample. Socialization experiences of Chinese student teachers and business students, mean incremental TOI
American children in school and society may shape frame-of- was reported as 2.44 and 2.15, respectively (means adjusted to
reference effects in reports of mothering. For example, chil- match the scale of the current study; Brten & Strms, 2005).
drens perceptions of their mothers use of control may be Although Chinese children are traditionally thought to hold
impacted by their observations of school culture, peer family more incremental theories of intelligence (Stevenson & Stigler,
dynamics, and media depictions of family process. Finally, LA 1992), our data suggest that there may be a developmental trend
mothers endorsing the lowest levels of PC may suggest either with implicit theory orientations starting relatively low in
acculturative effects as mothers adopt U.S. parenting values Grades 4 5 and increasing with age through increased educa-
while relinquishing more traditional Chinese child-rearing tion and socialization.
styles of control or selection effects of more individualistic Notwithstanding these concerns, our findings reveal preliminary
Chinese mothers having chosen to immigrate west. evidence that childrens perceptions of maternal parenting were
In terms of covariation among parenting constructs, our data robustly associated with growth mindset in Chinese-heritage chil-
showed that mother-report of AS and PC was negatively correlated dren across three distinct social contexts. Our findings provide
in BJ but positively correlated in HK and not correlated in LA. evidence to support the association of PC with one positive child
This pattern of an inverse relationship between PC and AS found characteristic within an interdependent social context. Despite the
only in our BJ sample is generally observed in Western indepen- commonly accepted convention, our study provides one data point
dent cultural settings (Silk, Morris, Kanaya, & Steinberg, 2003). suggesting that PC may have a redeeming benefit. However, this
The positive correlation observed in BJ is thus consistent with finding has to be considered alongside multiple studies showing
theories on the impact of social change on shifts toward indepen- untoward effects of PC on childrens emotional and behavioral
dent cultural orientations in Mainland China (Greenfield, 2009; development even within Chinese-heritage samples. Additionally,
Sun & Ryder, 2016). In contrast, within the HK sample, the two while AS is touted as paramount for optimal child adjustment, our
parenting dimensions may coexist in a dialectical fashion wherein findings provide more limited support for associations with this
maternal socialization practices can be simultaneously fostering of aspect of social cognition. Given the limitations of this initial
child autonomy and opinion formation with facets of PC (Silk et study, replication and prospective inquiry are needed to defini-
PARENTING AND GROWTH MINDSET 9

tively inform family-based interventions to foster growth mindset Chao, R. K. (2001). Extending research on the consequences of parenting
in elementary-age children. style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Child Develop-
ment, 72, 18321843. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00381
Chao, R. K., & Aque, C. (2009). Interpretations of parental control by
References Asian immigrant and European American youth. Journal of Family
Psychology, 23, 342354. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015828
Arnold, F., & Zhaoxiang, L. (1986). Sex preference fertility and family Cheah, C. S., Leung, C. Y., & Zhou, N. (2013). Understanding tiger
planning in China. Population and Development Review, 12, 221246. parenting through the perceptions of Chinese immigrant mothers: Can
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1973109 Chinese and U.S. parenting coexist? Asian American Journal of Psy-
Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of chology, 4, 30 40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031217
stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping Chen, X., Cen, G., Li, D., & He, Y. (2005). Social functioning and
theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, adjustment in Chinese children: The imprint of historical time. Child
113125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jesp.2001.1491 Development, 76, 182195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005
Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a ne- .00838.x
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

glected construct. Child Development, 67, 3296 3319. http://dx.doi.org/ Chiu, L. H. (1987). Child-rearing attitudes of Chinese, Chinese-American,
10.2307/1131780
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and Anglo-American mothers. International Journal of Psychology, 22,


Barber, B. K., Olsen, J. E., & Shagle, S. C. (1994). Associations between 409 419. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207598708246782
parental psychological and behavioral control and youth internalized and Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Self-determination theory: A mac-
externalized behaviors. Child Development, 65, 1120 1136. http://dx rotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Canadian Psy-
.doi.org/10.2307/1131309 chology, 49, 182185. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012801
Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J., & Grafeman, S. J. (2008). Child versus parent Dupeyrat, C., & Marin, C. (2005). Implicit theories of intelligence, goal
reports of parenting practices: Implications for the conceptualization of orientation, cognitive engagement, and achievement: A test of Dwecks
child behavioral and emotional problems. Assessment, 15, 294 303. model with returning to school adults. Contemporary Educational Psy-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191107312212 chology, 30, 4359. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2004.01.007
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality,
theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent tran- and development. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
sition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78, Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C. Y., & Hong, Y. Y. (1995). Implicit theories:
246 263. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00995.x Elaboration and extension of the model. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 322
Bgels, S. M., & van Melick, M. (2004). The relationship between child- 333. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0604_12
report, parent self-report, and partner report of perceived parental rearing Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to
behaviors and anxiety in children and parents. Personality and Individ- motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256 273. http://
ual Differences, 37, 15831596. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004 dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256
.02.014 Eamon, M. K. (2005). Social-demographic, school, neighborhood, and
Bgels, S. M., van Oosten, A., Muris, P., & Smulders, D. (2001). Familial parenting influences on the academic achievement of Latino young
correlates of social anxiety in children and adolescents. Behaviour adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34, 163174. http://dx
Research and Therapy, 39, 273287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005- .doi.org/10.1007/s10964-005-3214-x
7967(00)00005-X Fiske, A. P. (2002). Using individualism and collectivism to compare
Brten, I., & Strms, H. I. (2005). The relationship between epistemo- cultures-A critique of the validity and measurement of the constructs:
logical beliefs, implicit theories of intelligence, and self-regulated learn- Comment on Oyserman et al. (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128,
ing among Norwegian postsecondary students. British Journal of Edu- 78 88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.128.1.78
cational Psychology, 75, 539 565. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/ Fung, J., & Lau, A. S. (2012). Tough love or hostile domination? Psycho-
000709905X25067 logical control and relational induction in cultural context. Journal of
Brown, T. A. (2012). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. Family Psychology, 26, 966 975. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030457
New York, NY: Guilford. Greenfield, P. M. (2009). Linking social change and developmental
Byrne, B. M., Shavelson, R. J., & Muthn, B. (1989). Testing for the change: Shifting pathways of human development. Developmental Psy-
equivalence of factor covariance and mean structures: The issue of chology, 45, 401 418. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014726
partial measurement invariance. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 456 466. Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles associated with
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.105.3.456 childrens self-regulation and competence in school. Journal of Educa-
Chan, S. M., Bowes, J., & Wyver, S. (2009). Chinese parenting in Hong tional Psychology, 81, 143154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.81
Kong: Links among goals, beliefs and styles. Early Child Development .2.143
and Care, 179, 849 862. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0300443070 Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, T., Ide, E., Leung, C.,
1536525 & Matsumoto, H. (2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure
Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting in Japan and North America: An investigation of self-improving moti-
style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of vations and malleable selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
training. Child Development, 65, 11111119. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/ chology, 81, 599 615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.4.599
1131308 Hill, N. E. (2001). Parenting and academic socialization as they relate to
Chao, R. K. (1995). Chinese and European American cultural models of school readiness: The roles of ethnicity and family income. Journal of
the self reflected in mothers childrearing beliefs. Ethos, 23, 328 354. Educational Psychology, 93, 686 697. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/eth.1995.23.3.02a00030 0663.93.4.686
Chao, R. K. (2000). The parenting of immigrant Chinese and European Ho, D. Y. (1989). Continuity and variation in Chinese patterns of social-
American mothers: Relations between parenting styles, socialization ization. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51, 149 163. http://dx.doi
goals, and parental practices. Journal of Applied Developmental Psy- .org/10.2307/352376
chology, 21, 233248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(99) Ho, D. Y., Peng, S. Q., Lai, A., & Chan, S. F. F. (2001). Indigenization and
00037-4 beyond: Methodological relationalism in the study of personality across
10 KIM, FUNG, WU, FANG, AND LAU

cultural traditions. Journal of Personality, 69, 925953. http://dx.doi development. American Psychologist, 61, 192203. http://dx.doi.org/10
.org/10.1111/1467-6494.696170 .1037/0003-066x.61.3.192
Hong, Y. Y., Chiu, C. Y., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M. S., & Wan, W. (1999). Muthn, L. K., & Muthn, B. O. (1998 2012). Mplus users guide (7th
Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 588 599. http://dx Nguyen, J. D., Kim, J. J., Ngo, V., Weiss, B., & Lau, A. S. (2017).
.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.588 Relations between parent-adolescent acculturation conflict and adoles-
Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of choice: A cent mental health symptoms among Vietnamese American families.
cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Manuscript submitted for publication.
Social Psychology, 76, 349 366. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514 Olsen, S. F., Yang, C., Hart, C. H., Robinson, C. C., Wu, P., Nelson, D. A.,
.76.3.349 . . . Wo, J. (2002). Maternal psychological control and preschool chil-
Kagitibasi, C. (2005). Autonomy and relatedness in cultural context drens behavioral outcomes in China, Russia, and the United States. In
implications for self and family. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, B. K. Barber (Ed.), Intrusive parenting: How psychological control
36, 403 422. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022105275959 affects children and adolescents (pp. 235262). Washington, DC: Amer-
Kim, K., & Rohner, R. P. (2002). Parental warmth, control, and involve- ican Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10422-000
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ment in schooling predicting academic achievement among Korean Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

American adolescents. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 127 individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions
140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022102033002001 and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 372. http://dx.doi.org/
Kim, S. Y., & Wong, V. Y. (2002). Assessing Asian and Asian American 10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3
parenting: A review of the literature. In K. S. Kurasaki, S. Okazaki, & Park, H., Coello, J. A., & Lau, A. S. (2014). Child socialization goals in
S. Sue (Eds.), Asian American Mental Health (pp. 185201). New York, East Asian versus Western nations from 1989 to 2010: Evidence for
NY: Springer. social change in parenting. Parenting, 14, 69 91. http://dx.doi.org/10
Kinlaw, C. R., & Kurtz-Costes, B. (2007). Childrens theories of intelli- .1080/15295192.2014.914345
gence: Beliefs, goals, and motivation in the elementary years. The Paulson, S. E., & Sputa, C. L. (1996). Patterns of parenting during ado-
Journal of General Psychology, 134, 295311. http://dx.doi.org/10 lescence: Perceptions of adolescents and parents. Adolescence, 31, 369
.3200/genp.134.3.295-312
381. Retrieved from https://search.proquest.com/docview/1295
Lai, A. C., Zhang, Z.-X., & Wang, W. Z. (2000). Maternal child-rearing
899088?accountid14512
practices in Hong Kong and Beijing Chinese families: A comparative
Pearson, E., & Rao, N. (2003). Socialization goals, parenting practices, and
study. International Journal of Psychology, 35, 60 66. http://dx.doi.org/
peer competence in Chinese and English preschoolers. Early Child
10.1080/002075900399529
Development and Care, 173, 131146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Lamborn, S. D., Dornbusch, S. M., & Steinberg, L. (1996). Ethnicity and
0300443022000022486
community context as moderators of the relations between family deci-
Pettit, G. S., Laird, R. D., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Criss, M. M.
sion making and adolescent adjustment. Child Development, 67, 283
(2001). Antecedents and behavior-problem outcomes of parental moni-
301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01734.x
toring and psychological control in early adolescence. Child Develop-
Lau, A. S. (2010). Physical discipline in Chinese American immigrant
ment, 72, 583598. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00298
families: An adaptive culture perspective. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Pomerantz, E. M., & Kempner, S. G. (2013). Mothers daily person and
Minority Psychology, 16, 313322. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018667
process praise: Implications for childrens theory of intelligence and
Lau, A. S. (2012). Reflections on adapting parent training for Chinese
motivation. Developmental Psychology, 49, 2040 2046. http://dx.doi
immigrants: Blind alleys, thoroughfares, and test drives. In G. E. Bernal
.org/10.1037/a0031840
& M. M. Domenech Rodrguez (Eds.), Cultural adaptations: Tools for
evidence-based practice with diverse populations (pp. 133156). Wash- Pomerantz, E. M., & Wang, Q. (2009). The role of parental control in
ington, DC: American Psychological Association. childrens development in Western and East Asian countries. Current
Lau, A. S., Fung, J. J., & Yung, V. (2010). Group parent training with Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 285289. http://dx.doi.org/10
immigrant Chinese families: Enhancing engagement and augmenting .1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01653.x
skills training. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 66, 880 894. http://dx Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the
.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20711 facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being.
Li, J. (2003). US and Chinese cultural beliefs about learning. Journal of American Psychologist, 55, 68 78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-
Educational Psychology, 95, 258 267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022- 066X.55.1.68
0663.95.2.258 Silk, J. S., Morris, A. S., Kanaya, T., & Steinberg, L. (2003). Psychological
Lin, C. Y. C., & Fu, V. R. (1990). A comparison of child-rearing practices control and autonomy granting: Opposite ends of a continuum or distinct
among Chinese, immigrant Chinese, and Caucasian-American parents. constructs? Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 113128. http://dx
Child Development, 61, 429 433. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131104 .doi.org/10.1111/1532-7795.1301004
Loukas, A., Paulos, S. K., & Robinson, S. (2005). Early adolescent social Spera, C. (2005). A review of the relationship among parenting practices,
and overt aggression: Examining the roles of social anxiety and maternal parenting styles, and adolescent school achievement. Educational Psy-
psychological control. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34, 335345. chology Review, 17, 125146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-005-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-005-5757-2 3950-1
Luo, R., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Song, L. (2013). Chinese parents goals Stevenson, H., & Stigler, J. W. (1992). The learning gap: Why our schools
and practices in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, are failing and what we can learn from Japanese and Chinese education.
28, 843 857. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.08.001 New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.
Mizokawa, D. T., & Ryckman, D. B. (1990). Attributions of academic Sun, J., & Ryder, A. G. (2016). The Chinese experience of rapid modern-
success and failure: A comparison of six Asian-American ethnic groups. ization: Sociocultural changes, psychological consequences? Frontiers
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 21, 434 451. http://dx.doi.org/ in Psychology, 7, 477. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00477
10.1177/0022022190214003 Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Way, N., Hughes, D., Yoshikawa, H., Kalman,
Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding meaning in psychology: R. K., & Niwa, E. Y. (2008). Parents goals for children: The dynamic
A lay theories approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social coexistence of individualism and collectivism in cultures and individu-
PARENTING AND GROWTH MINDSET 11

als. Social Development, 17, 183209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- investigation in the United States and China. Child Development, 78,
9507.2007.00419.x 15921610. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01085.x
Tardif, T., & Miao, X. (2000). Developmental psychology in China. Way, N., Okazaki, S., Zhao, J., Kim, J. J., Chen, X., Yoshikawa, H., . . .
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 68 72. http://dx Deng, H. (2013). Social and emotional parenting: Mothering in a chang-
.doi.org/10.1080/016502500383485 ing Chinese society. Asian American Journal of Psychology, 4, 6170.
Tseng, Y.-F. (1994). Chinese ethnic economy: San Gabriel Valley, Los http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031204
Angeles County. Journal of Urban Affairs, 16, 169 189. http://dx.doi Wu, D. Y. H. (1996). Chinese childhood socialization. In M. H. Bond
.org/10.1111/j.1467-9906.1994.tb00324.x (Ed.), The handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 143154). Hong Kong,
van de Schoot, R., Lugtig, P., & Hox, J. (2012). A checklist for testing China: Oxford University Press.
measurement invariance. European Journal of Developmental Psychol- Yau, J., & Smetana, J. (2003). Adolescent-parent conflict in Hong Kong
ogy, 9, 486 492. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.686740 and Shenzhen: A comparison of youth in two cultural contexts. Inter-
Wang, C. C., & Phinney, J. S. (1998). Differences in child rearing attitudes national Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 201211. http://dx.doi
between immigrant Chinese mothers and Anglo-American mothers. .org/10.1080/01650250244000209
Early Development & Parenting, 7, 181189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(SICI)1099-0917(199812)7:4181::AID-EDP1693.0.CO;2-Y Received April 26, 2016


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Wang, Q., Pomerantz, E. M., & Chen, H. (2007). The role of parents Revision received November 29, 2016
control in early adolescents psychological functioning: A longitudinal Accepted November 30, 2016

You might also like