You are on page 1of 8

Applied Ergonomics 46 (2015) 76e83

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Applied Ergonomics
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/apergo

Assessment model for perceived visual complexity of automotive


instrument cluster
Sol Hee Yoon a, Jihyoun Lim b, Yong Gu Ji a, *
a
Dept. of Information and Industrial Engineering, Yonsei University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
b
Dept. of Industrial Engineering, Hongik University, Seoul, Republic of Korea

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This research proposes an assessment model for quantifying the perceived visual complexity (PVC) of an
Received 9 July 2013 in-vehicle instrument cluster. An initial study was conducted to investigate the possibility of evaluating
Accepted 7 July 2014 the PVC of an in-vehicle instrument cluster by estimating and analyzing the complexity of its individual
Available online 15 August 2014
components. However, this approach was only partially successful, because it did not take into account
the combination of the different components with random levels of complexity to form one visual
Keywords:
display. Therefore, a second study was conducted focusing on the effect of combining the different
Perceived visual complexity
components. The results from the overall research enabled us to suggest a basis for quantifying the PVC
Quantiable measurement variables
Assessment model
of an in-vehicle instrument cluster based both on the PVCs of its components and on the integration
effect.
2014 Elsevier Ltd and The Ergonomics Society. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction processing (Gregory, 1970; Gibson, 1966; Lim and Liu, 2009).
Therefore, increased complexity in visual perception can negatively
Advancements in automotive information technology have led affect how drivers process visual information (Noy et al., 2004; Van
to the introduction of new interactive devices for drivers, such as der Horst, 2004).
the in-vehicle information system (IVIS; Horrey et al., 2006). An Tsimhoni and Green (2001) investigated the inuence of visual
IVIS has the ability to display more information than a conventional demands on driving performance in an experiment that measured
instrument cluster, including information on the current driving visual demands and mental workloads when participants read an
status, as well as information indirectly related to driving, like electronic map displayed in a vehicle both while driving and while
maps, entertainment information, and multimedia. To address this parked. Their results highlighted the payoff that exists between
opportunity, full LCD displays are replacing conventional analog visual demands and driving performance. High demand for visual
instrument clusters, which consist of a single display that shows attention to a specic visual display decreases drivers' visual
information related to speed, fuel level, navigation, driving assis- attention to the road (Lee et al., 2007; Liang and Lee, 2010; Xia et al.,
tance, and more (Bellotti et al., 2004; Huang, 2007). 2010). This is likely why increasing the amount of information in in-
The addition of information in visual displays is closely linked vehicle displays could disturb driving performance and lead to
with visual perception and complexity. The principles of visual safety concerns. In solving the potential problem of visual distrac-
perception are commonly explained by a top-down interpretation tion arising from an excess of information provided by in-vehicle
of what humans see based on sensory information from the visual displays, the design and amount of information presented
physical world (Gregory, 1970), and bottom-up information driven to drivers must be controlled and optimized.
by human knowledge (Gibson, 1966). In this research we dened The visual complexity of in-vehicle visual displays has long been
PVC as the degree of detail or intricacy that the user perceives in a a topic of concern. Several studies have focused on the possibility of
visual stimulus by a combination of bottom-up and top-down measuring complexity by objective metrics (Lavie et al., 2011;
Huang, 2007; Cummings and Tsonis, 2006). Previous studies
applied an image compression technique whereby an image's de-
gree of visual complexity is calculated based on its compressed size
* Corresponding author. Dept. of Information and Industrial Engineering, Yonsei
(Tuch et al., 2009). Other researchers have proposed an approach
University, 262 Seongsanno, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 120-749, Republic of Korea.
Tel.: 82 10 9058 9810. whereby an image's visual complexity is calculated based on the
E-mail addresses: yongguji@yonsei.ac.kr, yongguji@gmail.com (Y.G. Ji). length of edges within the image (Lim et al., 2010; Schmieder and

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2014.07.005
0003-6870/ 2014 Elsevier Ltd and The Ergonomics Society. All rights reserved.
S.H. Yoon et al. / Applied Ergonomics 46 (2015) 76e83 77

Weathersby, 1983). However, these methods for quantifying the In this study, we conducted a survey and a controlled experi-
complexity of visual stimulus are difcult to apply in the process of ment to evaluate our hypothesis. We developed a statistical model
designing an IVIS. In-vehicle information has the specic charac- to estimate perceived complexity based on subjective and objective
teristic that different types of information are combined in a single measurements of each of the eight components. We then evaluated
display. Hence, it is important to consider the complexity of the the relationship between the estimated PVCs of the components
display's individual components. Accordingly, in this research we and the subjective perception of the visual complexity of the in-
analyzed the PVC of an in-vehicle instrument cluster based on the vehicle instrument cluster as a whole. To assess the model to
PVC of each of its components. quantify the PVC of the whole display in relation to the PVCs of the
To discuss issues related to the combination of different com- components, a multiple linear regression model was used in
ponents to form a single visual stimulus, we revisit Gestalt theory of addition to a factor quantifying the integration effect.
perception, which states that humans tend to focus on a whole
stimulus rather than on its individual parts (Graham, 2008). Gestalt 2.2. Measurement variables
theory introduced the idea that there are differences between the
perception of the whole and the perception of its components Eleven objective, quantiable measurement variables were
(Rock and Palmer, 1990). The theory states that different percep- selected from previous studies on visual complexity. These mea-
tions of the whole are created when different components interact surement variables were applied to objectively quantify factors of
(Rock and Palmer, 1990). This impacts the concept of organization, the visual stimulus. The objective measurement variables used in
and provides insights into differences between the perception of this study were stimulus size, icon size, component quantity,
individual components and the whole (Rock and Palmer, 1990). In number of divisions, color variety, font variety, icon quantity, blank
the context of an in-vehicle instrument cluster, individual compo- space percentage, text percentage, graphic percentage, and text-to-
nents can affect a system in different ways when they are graphic ratio (Cummings and Tsonis, 2006; Forsythe, 2009; Kemps,
combined. 1999; Olivia et al., 2004; Harper et al., 2009; Michailidou, 2008;
In this study, we investigated individual components' inuences Stickel et al., 2010; Xing, 2007; Rosenholtz et al., 2007). Table 1
on the overall PVC and also evaluate their interrelationships. In this lists the denitions of these variables.
way, this research provides insights into the possibility of evalu- Moreover, for each component we determined which subset of
ating the PVC of an in-vehicle instrument cluster by estimating the objective quantiable measurement variables were relevant to the
complexity of its components. However, as in previous studies, we characteristics of each component, as summarized in Table 1.
determined that the combination of different components impacts
the perception of complexity (Rock and Palmer, 1990). We con- 2.3. Components
ducted a study to propose an assessment model for quantifying the
PVC by including the PVC of the estimated components, and then Components refer to the types of information that are included
added a factor that considered the effect caused by the combination in an in-vehicle instrument cluster and presented to the driver.
of the components; these two approaches were combined to form a Eight components were identied in the instrument cluster based
complete assessment model to quantify the visual complexity on the types of information they presented to drivers. The com-
perceived by drivers. ponents were classied as providing either conventional informa-
tion or additional information. Conventional information includes
2. Research framework the standard information traditionally provided in a vehicle in-
strument cluster, such as speedometer, tachometer, other gauges, and
2.1. Research hypotheses the gear position indicator. The component labeled other gauges
refers to other related components that present information in
In the present study, we suggest an assessment model to gauge format in the instrument cluster, such as fuel level and en-
quantify the PVC of an in-vehicle instrument cluster. We rst pro- gine temperature readings. Finally, the gear position indicator refers
pose the possibility of quantifying the PVC of the cluster by esti- to the display showing the gear rotation information as the driver
mating the perceived complexity of its components. That is to say, selects or changes gears. Additional information refers to infor-
our rst hypothesis was that the estimated PVC of the components mation that has been integrated into vehicle instrument clusters
that form the in-vehicle instrument cluster can be applied to more recently, with technological advancements. In this research,
explain the PVC of the whole instrument cluster. we categorized the additional information into four groups: navi-
However, the estimated PVC of each component did not gation, assistive information, entertainment, and menu. Navigation
signicantly inuence the PVC of the whole cluster. Thus, to eval- refers to information related to vehicle location. Assistive informa-
uate the combination of components, we conducted a second study tion refers to information to help drivers optimize their driving
based on Gestalt theory, which states that the whole is different performance. Entertainment refers to information related to
from the sum of its parts, and thus suggests why we were unsuc- enjoyment while driving that is unrelated to the main task of
cessful in explaining overall PVC in terms of the components. Our driving. Finally, menu refers to a graphic user interface that allows
second research hypothesis was that an integration effect exists drivers to select which information is displayed.
when different components are combined into the one stimulus of
an in-vehicle instrument cluster. Therefore, we added this inte- 3. Study 1: in-vehicle instrument cluster and PVC of the
gration effect as a new factor in the assessment model. Herein the components
integration effect is dened as the effect of combining different
components such as the speedometer, navigation display, and 3.1. Method
menu into a single display; this denition is based on previous
studies of Gestalt theory (Rock and Palmer, 1990; Palmer and Rock, The aim of this rst study was to verify the inuence of the
1994; Stickel et al., 2010; Xiang et al., 2007). Thus, we suggest an estimated PVC of each component upon that of the overall in-
assessment model to quantify the PVC of the whole in-vehicle in- vehicle instrument cluster. The study proposes a model to esti-
strument cluster by considering its components and the effect of mate the visual complexity of a component based on subjective
their integration. responses and objective performance measurements. Subjective
78 S.H. Yoon et al. / Applied Ergonomics 46 (2015) 76e83

responses were based on questionnaires regarding the PVC of manipulate and design the experiment. SMI Experiment Center
various design components; each questionnaire included eight software was employed to design the search task experiment, and
sections, one for each component. The objective performance SMI BeGaze software was used to analyze the experimental data.
measure was based on a laboratory experiment in which subjects'
performance and ocular motor behavior were objectively measured 3.1.3. Participants
during a visual search task. Multiple linear regression models were Sixty-two males and forty females between the ages of 20 and
used to obtain the model used to estimate the PVC of components. 60 years (mean 36.44 years, SD 12.53) with driving experience
Statistical analysis was then performed to investigate the rela- participated in the questionnaire for Study 1. Demographic data on
tionship between the PVC of the components and that of the whole. the participants were gathered regarding their average time spent
In this way, the study enabled us to propose an estimation model to driving per day and the total distance they had driven (driving
quantify the PVC of individual components, and to investigate the experience). Among the participants, 57.84% drove an average of
possibility of estimating the PVC of the whole in-vehicle instru- less than one hour per day, 30.39% drove between one and two
ment cluster. hours, 6.86% drove between two and three hours, and 1.96% drove
between three and four hours; 30.39% of the participants had
3.1.1. Stimuli driven less than 2000 km, 19.61% had driven between 2000 km and
A total of 101 visual stimuli of individual components were used 20,000 km, and 48.04% had driven more than 20,000 km.
(15 for the speedometer, 10 for the tachometer, 14 for the gauge, 10 Forty-ve participants were recruited for the visual search
for the gear position indicator, 13 for the navigation display, 13 for experiment (23 males and 22 females). Data from ve participants
assistive information, 13 for entertainment, and 13 for the menu). (2 males and 3 females) were excluded due to eye movement
Each stimulus was saved as a video document (.SWF) and a ash tracking ratios below 95% (participant ocular limitation). The
document (.FLA) for the questionnaire, and the size of the image remaining 40 participants (21 males and 19 females) each had a
presented in the questionnaire was kept constant regardless of the driver's license and driving experience. Their ages ranged between
size of the screen used. For the visual search experiments, a small 22 and 56 years (mean 36.98 years, SD 9.89). Demographic data
star-shape gure (4  4 pixels) search target was embedded in each on the participants were gathered regarding their average time
of the 101 visual stimuli. The locations of the target were ran- spent driving per day and driving experience.
domized to make them unpredictable. A yellow star was used to
avoid confusion with the background, and to facilitate target 3.1.4. Questionnaire design
identication, the target was not placed in any regions of the visual The design of the questionnaire was based on previous research
stimulus that were of similar color. Moreover, 25 images were regarding an information questionnaire for an air trafc control
selected to evaluate the PVC of the whole in-vehicle instrument visual display (Xing, 2008) and an information complexity ques-
cluster. They were also saved as video and ash documents, and tionnaire to validate visual interfaces (Ling et al., 2011). Our rede-
their evaluation was included in the questionnaire. signed questionnaire consisted of three questions related to the
perception of each of the visual stimuli; participants scored each
3.1.2. Equipment/apparatus image on a 7-point Likert scale (1: strongly disagree, 7: strongly
A remote eye tracking system (SMI RED-m, SensoMotoric In- agree). All participants were Korean; therefore, the entire ques-
struments; SMI) was used to obtain data on the response time and tionnaire was translated into Korean. The questionnaire was
number of xations; it was attached to a 24-inch monitor where divided into eight sections to cover each of the eight components of
the visual stimuli were presented. An extra laptop was used to the in-vehicle instrument cluster; the components were presented

Table 1
Quantiable measurement variables for components.

Quantiable Denition Conventional information components Additional information components


measurement
Speedometer Tachometer Other Gear Navigation Assistive Entertainment Menu
gauges position

Stimulus size Entire region (area) covered by a visual stimulus, Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y


measured in mm2
Icon size Average size of a symbol that is used to express N N Y Y Y Y Y Y
a well-known meaning.
Component quantity Number of components in the visual stimulus. N N N N Y Y Y N
Number of divisions Number of areas on the visual stimulus that can N N N N Y Y Y Y
be visually distributed by lines or different
background colors.
Color variety Number of different colors perceived. (Gradation Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
of color was counted as two colors, for the initial
color and nal color).
Font variety Number of different fonts used in the visual stimulus. Y Y Y N Y Y Y Y
Icon quantity Number of different symbols used in the visual N N Y N Y Y Y Y
stimulus.
Blank space Percentage of area in the visual stimulus that does Y Y Y Y Y Y Y N
percentage not provide any kind of information.
Text percentage Percentage of area in the visual stimulus that provides Y Y Y N Y Y Y Y
information in text form.
Graphic percentage Percentage of area in the visual stimulus that provides Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
information in graphic form.
Text-to-graphic ratio Ratio between the total area of text information to the Y Y Y N Y Y Y Y
total area of graphic information in the visual stimulus.

Y quantiable; N not quantiable.


S.H. Yoon et al. / Applied Ergonomics 46 (2015) 76e83 79

in random order to prevent ordering effects. The same question- the relationship between the PVC of each component and the PVC
naire was also used for the whole in-vehicle instrument cluster of whole in-vehicle instrument clusters comprising those compo-
stimuli. nents. In the rst stage, we discovered the objective quantiable
The questionnaire was administered online to provide the same measurements that were statistically signicantly correlated with
visual stimulus size to all participants regardless of the monitor the subjective questionnaire data on the PVC of the components
used. In the questionnaire, the demographic questions were asked and the objective visual search task results (i.e., response time and
rst, followed by those related to the eight in-vehicle components. number of xations). We performed a Pearson correlation analysis,
Participants had to answer each question about PVC for all 101 which provided a condence interval of 95%. As a result, objective
visual stimuli. The questionnaire on the 25 whole in-vehicle in- measurement variables were selected for the next stage. Secondly,
strument cluster stimuli was administered one month after the a model to estimate PVC was presented for each of the components.
questionnaire on the components. This estimation model was developed using a stepwise forward
regression model. Finally, in stage 3, we conducted a Pearson cor-
3.1.5. Visual search experiment design relation analysis with a condence interval of 5% to verify the
To observe the relationship between users' behavior and the ability of the estimated PVC of each component to predict the
PVC, an experiment was conducted in which participants per- subjective PVC of the whole in-vehicle instrument cluster. PASW
formed a visual search task while their searching behaviors were Statistics 18 software was employed for all statistical analyses.
encoded and analyzed by an eye tracking system. The experiment
was designed based on research on visual clutter by Rosenholtz 3.2. Results
et al. (2007). The data collected were the response time and
number of xations, and the visual stimuli used were the same 101 3.2.1. Estimation model for conventional information components
components presented in the questionnaire. Demographic data was Among the speedometers' quantiable measurement variables,
gathered before the experiment, and a brief introduction to the stimulus size, color variety, font variety, blank space percentage, text
experiment was given, during which the task was explained. Par- percentage, graphic percentage, and text-to-graphic ratio measure-
ticipants were asked to nd the target hidden within the visual ments were signicantly correlated with the speedometers' PVC
stimulus. Once the target was found, the participants had to click on (Table 2). Stimulus size, blank space percentage, and text percentage
it in order to continue to the next visual stimulus. were signicantly correlated with response time; stimulus size,
The experiment consisted of three sets, with 50 or 51 visual color variety, blank space percentage, text percentage, and graphic
stimuli in each set. The order of stimuli within the sets was ran- percentage were signicantly correlated with the number of xa-
domized to prevent any ordering effect. Between each set, each tions (Table 2). The estimation model for the PVC of the speed-
participant was given a 10-min break. Response time was measured ometer (r 0.624; r2 0.390) was developed using the blank space
as the time it took for participants to click on the hidden target. The percentage (p < 0.001) and text percentage (p < 0.001) measure-
number of xations was counted by analyzing a participant's scan ments (Table 4).
path while searching for each hidden target. Fig. 1 illustrates how Most of the tachometers' objective measurement variables
the data were obtained using the eye tracking equipment. except the text percentage were signicantly correlated with their
PVC; however, the user experiment indicated that the text per-
3.1.6. Statistical analysis centage had the highest correlation with tachometer visual search
Three types of statistical analysis were conducted: 1) analysis of performance and visual search behavior. More specically, stimulus
the relationship between quantiable measurements for each of size, font variety, blank space percentage, and text percentage were
the components to determine which measurement variables were signicantly correlated with response time in the user experiment,
signicantly correlated and thus were suitable for inclusion in the while stimulus size, font variety, blank space percentage, and text
estimation model; 2) regression model analysis to suggest an percentage were signicantly correlated with the number of xa-
estimation model for the PVC of each component; and 3) analysis of tions (Table 2). The estimation model for the PVC of the tachometer

Fig. 1. Example of data obtained from the eye-tracking experiment.


80 S.H. Yoon et al. / Applied Ergonomics 46 (2015) 76e83

Table 2
Quantiable measurement variables for components of conventional type information.

Quantiable Conventional information components


measurement
Speedometer Tachometer Other gauges Gear position indicator

PVC RT Fix. # PVC RT Fix. # PVC RT Fix. # PVC RT Fix. #

Stimulus size 0.226c 0.251c 0.257c 0.123c 0.301c 0.425c 0.205c 0.218c 0.348c 0.731c 0.095a 0.092a
Icon size e e e e e e 0.371c 0.191c 0.258c 0.086a 0.048 0.027
Color variety 0.282c 0.027 0.073a 0.340c 0.007 0.022 0.257c 0.007 0.068 0.016 0.032 0.041
Font variety 0.224c 0.011 0.066 0.186c 0.123b 0.156b 0.426c 0.062 0.146c e e e
Icon quantity e e e e e e 0.404c 0.159c 0.225c e e e
Blank space percentage 0.599c 0.104b 0.103b 0.712c 0.097a 0.095a 0.389c 0.008 0.021 0.424c 0.014 0.008
Text percentage 0.594c 0.163c 0.193c 0.026 0.164c 0.213c 0.619c 0.107b 0.147c e e e
Graphic percentage 0.258c 0.057 0.103b 0.638c 0.014 0.013 0.238c 0.019 0.061 0.389c 0.030 0.014
Text-to-graphic ratio 0.205c 0.057 0.065 0.119c 0.056 0.080 0.500c 0.167c 0.265c e e e

PVC perceived visual complexity; RT response time; Fix. # number of xations.


a
Signicant at p < 0.05.
b
Signicant at p < 0.01.
c
Signicant at p < 0.001.

(r 0.714; r2 0.509) was developed using the stimulus size 3.2.2. Estimation model for additional information components
(p < 0.001) and blank space percentage (p < 0.05) measurements All of the navigation displays' quantiable measurement vari-
(Table 4). ables were signicantly correlated with their PVC, except compo-
For the other gauges component, all of the quantiable mea- nent quantity and number of divisions. However, all quantiable
surements selected from the expert evaluation were signicantly measurement variables except icon size and font variety were
correlated with the subjective results for PVC. However, our user signicantly correlated with response time and the number of
experiment showed some differences. Stimulus size, icon size, icon xations (Table 3). The estimation model for the PVC of the navi-
quantity, text percentage, and text-to-graphic ratio were signicantly gation display (r 0.594; r2 0.352) was developed using the text
correlated with response time, and stimulus size, icon size, font va- information percentage (p < 0.001) and graphic percentage
riety, icon quantity, text percentage, and text-to-graphic ratio were (p < 0.001) measurements (Table 4).
signicantly correlated with the number of xations (Table 2). The All of the quantiable measurement variables of assistive in-
estimation model for the PVC of the other gauges component formation components were signicantly correlated to their PVC,
(r 0.633; r2 0.401) was developed using the stimulus size except stimulus size and component quantity. Contrastingly, the icon
(p < 0.001), icon quantity (p < 0.001), text percentage (p < 0.001), size, component quantity, number of divisions, color variety, text
and text-to-graphic ratio (p < 0.01) measurements (Table 4). percentage, and text-to-graphic ratio were signicantly correlated
Finally, among the quantiable measurements for gear position with response time, and icon size, component quantity, number of
indicators, stimulus size, icon size, blank space percentage, and divisions, color variety, font variety, text percentage, and text-to-
graphic percentage were signicantly correlated with their PVC. In graphic ratio were signicantly correlated with the number of x-
the user experiment, only stimulus size was signicantly correlated ations (Table 3). The estimation model for the PVC of the assistive
to the response time and number of xations (Table 2). The esti- information component (r 0.573; r2 0.328) was developed
mation model for the PVC of the gear position indicator (r 0.731; using the blank space percentage (p < 0.001) measure (Table 4).
r2 0.534) was developed using the stimulus size (p < 0.001) All of the quantiable measurement variables for the enter-
measure (Table 4). tainment components were signicantly correlated with their PVC,

Table 3
Quantiable measurement variables for components of additional type information.

Quantiable Additional information components


measurement
Navigation display Assistive information Entertainment Menu

PVC RT Fix. # PVC RT Fix. # PVC RT Fix. # PVC RT Fix. #


b b c c c c
Stimulus size 0.080 0.105 0.213 0.003 0.045 0.033 0.134 0.021 0.139 0.045 0.256 0.316c
Icon size 0.322c 0.026 0.034 0.087b 0.043 0.055 0.039 0.029 0.016 0.290c 0.146c 0.161c
Component quantity 0.018 0.085a 0.143c 0.007 0.196c 0.212c 0.053 0.033 0.150c e e e
Number of divisions 0.005 0.096a 0.132b 0.063a 0.090a 0.045 0.178c 0.143c 0.261c 0.216c 0.256c 0.329c
Color variety 0.322c 0.100b 0.112b 0.116c 0.200c 0.189c 0.098b 0.132b 0.174c 0.246c 0.127b 0.207c
Font variety 0.188c 0.024 0.043 0.102b 0.146c 0.180c 0.256c 0.077a 0.040 0.045 0.122b 0.148c
Icon quantity 0.185c 0.340c 0.375c 0.105b 0.068 0.060 0.658c 0.126b 0.445c 0.041 0.063 0.099b
Blank space 0.081b 0.317c 0.304c 0.573c 0.146c 0.145c 0.204c 0.036 0.044 e e e
percentage
Text percentage 0.583c 0.195c 0.188c 0.141c 0.085a 0.153c 0.097b 0.055 0.194c 0.602c 0.095a 0.158c
Graphic percentage 0.151c 0.321c 0.308c 0.548c 0.090a 0.063 0.187c 0.089a 0.114b 0.172c 0.108b 0.093a
Text-to-graphic ratio 0.330c 0.185c 0.173c 0.234c 0.009 0.067 0.067a 0.033 0.069 0.291c 0.234c 0.317c

PVC perceived visual complexity; RT response time; Fix. # number of xations.


a
Signicant at p < 0.05.
b
Signicant at p < 0.01.
c
Signicant at p < 0.001.
S.H. Yoon et al. / Applied Ergonomics 46 (2015) 76e83 81

except icon size and quantity of items. The number of divisions, color Table 5
variety, font variety, icon quantity, and graphic percentage were Correlation analysis between the perceived visual complexity of components and
the in-vehicle instrument cluster.
signicantly correlated with response time, and stimulus size,
component quantity, number of division, color variety, icon quantity, Category Component Correlation of component PVC
text percentage, and graphic percentage were signicantly correlated with whole-cluster PVC

with the number of xations (Table 3). The estimation model for R Signicance level
the PVC of the entertainment component (r 0.658; r2 0.433) Conventional Speedometer 0.055b 0.005
was developed using the icon quantity (p < 0.001) measure type of Tachometer 0.011 0.658
(Table 4). information Other gauges 0.071a 0.015
Gear position indicator 0.053a 0.012
Finally, all of the quantiable measurement variables for the
menus were signicantly correlated with their PVC, except font Additional Navigation display 0.041 0.227
variety and icon quantity. All the quantiable measurement vari- information Assistive information 0.028 0.496
Entertainment 0.054 0.097
ables except icon quantity were signicantly correlated with
Menu 0.019 0.533
response time, and all were signicantly correlated with the
a
Signicant at p < 0.05.
number of xations (Table 3). The estimation model for the PVC of b
Signicant at p < 0.01.
the menu (r 0.607; r2 0.368) was developed using the text
percentage (p < 0.001) and text-to-graphic ratio (p < 0.001) mea-
surements (Table 4).
suggest an assessment model for quantifying the PVC of an in-
3.2.3. Relationship between components and the overall in-vehicle vehicle instrument cluster based on the PVCs of its components.
instrument cluster PVC First, we selected the quantiable measurements that were most
Once we obtained a quantiable model for the PVC of each suitable for determining the integration effect, choosing stimulus
component (Table 4), we applied the model to estimate the PVC of size, component quantity, number of divisions, color variety, font
each of the components' visual stimuli. variety, icon quantity, blank space percentage, text percentage,
We then conducted a correlation analysis between the PVC of graphic percentage, clutter, and text-to-graphic ratio. Component
each component and the PVC of the in-vehicle visual display (the quantity refers to the number of components in the instrument
whole visual stimulus). Our results show that only the speedometer cluster (Olivia et al., 2004; Xing, 2007). That is, if the visual stimulus
(p < 0.01), other gauges (p < 0.05), and gear position indicator of an instrument cluster is composed of a speedometer, tachometer,
(p < 0.05) were signicantly correlated with the PVC of the overall and gear position indicator, its component quantity is three. Clutter
in-vehicle instrument cluster (Table 5). refers to a state in which there are an excessive number of items
(based on representation, method, or organization) and a resulting
4. Study 2: integration effect and PVC degradation in performance (Rosenholtz et al., 2007; Forsythe,
2009). Thus, we measured clutter by determining the edge prob-
Results from Study 1 showed that not all components' PVCs ability of the visual stimulus.
were signicantly correlated with the PVC of the in-vehicle in- In Study 2, we applied the same methodology that was used for
strument cluster: the speedometer, other gauges, and gear position Study 1: we analyzed questionnaire results for 25 visual stimuli of
indicator were the only components that showed signicant cor- whole in-vehicle instrument clusters, and the results of user ex-
relations. These results agree with the Gestalt law of perception. periments on visual search performance and behavior. Then, cor-
Accordingly, it is impossible to suggest a model that quanties the relation analysis was conducted to select signicant variables
PVC of an in-vehicle instrument cluster by knowing only the PVC of which were later used for estimating the PVC of integration effect.
each component. Therefore, we developed a quantitative model for An additional correlation analysis was then conducted between
the PVC of an instrument cluster, applying the ndings from Study the estimated integration effect and the subjective PVC of the
1 and conducting a second study to develop a factor to account for whole in-vehicle instrument cluster. The integration effect was
Gestalt effects. considered to be an additional component, accounting for the
perception of the combination of different components. Thus, once
4.1. Method the results were obtained, we suggested a nal model to enable
the quantication of the PVC for an in-vehicle instrument cluster
In Study 2, we investigated a factor capable of explaining the based on an estimation of the PVC of the components and the
effects of combining different components, thus enabling us to integration effect.

Table 4
Estimation model for perceived visual complexity of components.

Component Estimation model r r2

Speedometer 0.465 0.032 * (blank space percentage)c  0.033 * (text percentage)c 0.624 0.390
Tachometer 10.583  0.087 * (stimulus size)c  0.0003 * (blank space percentage)a 0.714 0.509
Other gauges 2.471  0.142 * (stimulus size)c 0.133 * (icon quantity)c  0.004 * (text percentage)c 0.633 0.401
0.482 * (text-to-graphic ratio)b
Gear position indicator 2.365 0.080 * (stimulus size)c 0.731 0.534
Navigation display 2.126 0.090 * (text percentage)c 0.002 * (graphic percentage)c 0.594 0.352
Assistive information 5.357  0.036 * (blank space percentage)c 0.573 0.328
Entertainment 2.306 0.231 * (icon quantity)c 0.658 0.433
Menu 2.426 0.041 * (text percentage)c 0.044 * (text-to-graphic ratio)b 0.607 0.368
a
Signicant at p < 0.05.
b
Signicant at p < 0.01.
c
Signicant at p < 0.001.
82 S.H. Yoon et al. / Applied Ergonomics 46 (2015) 76e83

4.2. Statistical model analysis following estimation model was used for the integration effect
(r 0.453; r2 0.205):
The statistical analysis was divided into four stages: 1) statistical
analysis of the relationship between the quantiable measurement Integration effect 5:388 0:133*Component quantity
variables and the integration effect for an in-vehicle instrument  1:542E  7*Clutter
cluster; 2) stepwise forward regression model analysis to suggest
an estimation model for the integration effect; 3) statistical analysis
 0:040*Blank space percentage
of the inuence of the integration effect on the PVC of an in-vehicle  0:040*Graphic percentage
instrument cluster; and 4) stepwise forward regression model 0:001*Stimulus size
analysis to suggest an estimation model for the PVC of an in-vehicle
instrument cluster based on both the estimated PVC of the com- Once we obtained the estimation model for the integration ef-
ponents and the integration effect. fect, we measured the estimated integration effect for each of the
In the rst stage, we analyzed the effect of computational visual stimuli on the in-vehicle instrument cluster. The Pearson
objective measurement variables on the integration effect, visual correlation coefcient for the correlation between the integration
search performance, and visual search behavior. A Pearson corre- effect and the overall PVC of the display was r 0.575 (p < 0.001).
lation analysis was performed with a condence interval of 5%, and Therefore, the integration effect was signicantly correlated with
the measurement variables showing a signicant correlation for all the PVC of the whole in-vehicle instrument cluster, thus solving the
the three cases were selected as relevant quantiable measurement problem arose in the results of Study 1.
variables for the estimation model. In the second stage, a model A stepwise forward regression model was conducted using the
was developed to estimate the integration effect; this estimation PVCs of the speedometer, other gauges, and gear position indicator,
model was based on a stepwise forward regression model that as well as the integration effect. Table 7 lists the results of this
utilized the quantiable measurement variables selected from statistical analysis and presents the estimation model for the PVC of
stage 1. A stepwise forward regression model starts with no vari- the overall in-vehicle instrument cluster.
ables, and then uses predened selection criteria to sequentially Thus, we developed an estimation model for the PVC of an in-
add variables that improve the outcome; accordingly, only the vehicle instrument cluster. The components used in this model
quantiable measurement variables that showed a signicant were the integration effect (p < 0.001) and the estimated PVC of the
correlation (p < 0.05) were considered in the model. In the speedometer (p < 0.001). The model is given as follows, and rep-
following stage, Pearson correlation analysis with a condence resents a method for quantitatively assessing the PVC of the in-
interval of 5% was performed to test the correlation between the vehicle instrument cluster by considering its components
integration effect and the PVC of the whole in-vehicle instrument (r 0.580; r2 0.337):
cluster. Finally, in the last stage, the signicantly correlated com-
ponents obtained from Study 1 and the estimated integration effect PVC of the in-vehicle instrument cluster
were applied in a regression model analysis with respect to the 0:289 1:162*Integration effect
subjective PVC for the whole in-vehicle instrument cluster. Thus,
 0:083*Estimated PVC of speedometer
we proposed a model to assess the PVC of the instrument cluster.
PASW Statistics 18 software was employed for all statistical
analyses. 5. Discussion and conclusions

In this study, we examined the inuence of the components of


4.3. Results an in-vehicle instrument cluster on the PVC of the whole cluster. An
estimation model was proposed to quantify the PVC of each of the
The results from the correlation analysis showed that stimulus components as a function of their quantiable measurements, such
size (p < 0.01), icon size (p < 0.05), component quantity (p < 0.001), as size, quantities, and ratios of design elements (font, icon, and
blank space percentage (p < 0.05), graphic percentage (p < 0.01), and text). The results of Study 1 showed that not all components had
clutter (p < 0.001) were signicantly correlated with the results of any measures studied that were signicantly correlated with the
the questionnaire and the user experiment. Therefore, we selected PVC of the entire instrument cluster. The usability of each mea-
the following quantiable measurement variables to propose an surement as an estimator of PVC was tested by correlation analysis
estimation model for the integration effect. Table 6 shows results between each measurement and subjective and objective measures
from the forward regression model. of PVC. Stimulus size, icon size, component quantity, blank space
For the estimation model, the component quantity, clutter, blank percentage, graphic percentage, and clutter were identied as
space percentage, graphic percentage, and stimulus size were selected factors that correlate with PVC. This reduced set of attributes was
as signicant quantiable measurements (p < 0.001). Thus, the utilized to develop an estimation model for PVC of the whole in-

Table 6 Table 7
Results of regression model analysis for the integration effect. Results of regression model analysis for the perceived visual complexity of the in-
vehicle instrument cluster.
Beta Standard t-Value Signicance
error level Beta Standard error t-Value Signicance
level
Intercept 5.388 0.457 11.788 0.000a
Component quantity 0.133 0.008 17.039 0.000a Intercept 0.289 0.108 2.666 0.008a
Clutter 1.542E7 0.000 9.190 0.000a Integration effect 1.162 0.032 36.150 0.000b
Blank space percentage 0.040 0.005 8.671 0.000a Estimated PVC of the 0.083 0.018 4.648 0.000b
Graphic percentage 0.040 0.005 7.772 0.000a speedometer
Stimulus size 0.001 0.000 4.851 0.000a a
Signicant at p < 0.01.
a b
Signicant at p < 0.001. Signicant at p < 0.001.
S.H. Yoon et al. / Applied Ergonomics 46 (2015) 76e83 83

vehicle instrument cluster. The results suggested that drivers References


respond to specic properties of the display design; therefore,
these properties should be prioritized during the design process. Bellotti, F., De Gloria, A., Poggi, A., Andreone, L., Damiani, S., Knoll, P., 2004.
Designing congurable automotive dashboards on liquid crystal displays. Cogn.
Regarding conventional information displays, only the esti- Technol. Work 6, 247e265.
mated PVCs of the speedometer, the other gauges, and the gear Cummings, M.L., Tsonis, C.G., 2006. Partitioning complexity in air trafc manage-
position indicator showed signicant correlations with the sub- ment tasks. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol. 16 (3), 277e295.
Forsythe, A., 2009. Visual complexity: is that all there is?. Engineering psychology
jective PVC of the complete in-vehicle instrument cluster; the and cognitive ergonomics. In: HCII 2009. LNAI, vol. 5639, pp. 158e166.
estimated PVC of the tachometer was not signicantly correlated Gibson, J.J., 1966. The problem of temporal order in stimulation and perception.
with the overall display complexity. This suggests that the J. Psychol. Interdiscip. Appl. 62 (2), 141e149.
Graham, L., 2008. Gestalt theory in interactive media design. J. Hum. Soc. Sci. 2 (1),
tachometer is less important to drivers than the speedometer, other 1e12.
gauges, and gear position indicator. Gregory, R.L., 1970. On how little information controls so much behaviour. Ergo-
The PVCs of the additional information displays were not sta- nomics 13 (1), 25e35.
Harper, S., Michailidou, E., Stevens, R., 2009. Toward a denition of visual
tistically signicantly correlated with the PVC of the whole in-
complexity as an implicit measure of cognitive load. ACM Trans. Appl. Percept. 6
strument cluster; this might be due to participants' unfamiliarity (2), 1e18.
with the additional information given in the in-vehicle instrument Horrey, W.J., Wickens, C.D., Consalus, K.P., 2006. Modeling driver's visual attention
allocation while interacting with in-vehicle technologies. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl.
cluster. Accordingly, additional factors, such as user knowledge and
12 (2), 67e78.
preference, may play a role in the perception of visual complexity. Huang, K.C., 2007. Effects of computer icons and gure/background area ratios and
Thus, further research is needed to investigate factors that inu- color combinations on visual search performance on an LCD monitor. Displays
ence perceptions of complexity. 29, 237e242.
Kemps, E., 1999. Effects of complexity on visuo-spatial working memory. Eur. J.
The results of this study agreed with the Gestalt law. However, Cogn. Psychol. 11 (3), 335e356.
the research objective was to propose a model to quantify the PVC Lavie, T., Oron-Gilad, T., Meyer, J., 2011. Aesthetics and usability of in-vehicle nav-
of whole in-vehicle instrument clusters. Thus, we followed up with igation displays. Int. J. Hum.eComp. Stud. 69 (1e2), 80e99.
Lee, Y.C., Lee, J.D., Boyle, N.L., 2007. Visual attention in driving: the effects of
a study to investigate the possibility of identifying a new quanti- cognitive load and visual disruption. Hum. Factors 49 (4), 721e733.
able component capable of representing the effect of integrating Liang, X., Lee, J.D., 2010. Combining cognitive and visual distraction: less than the
the eight components of the in-vehicle instrument cluster, since sum of its parts. Assess. Saf. Driv. Simul. 42 (3), 881e890.
Lim, J.H., Liu, Y., 2009. Modeling the inuence of cyclic top-down and bottom-up
these components had only been considered separately in the processes for reinforcement learning in eye movements. IEEE Trans. Syst.
initial study. Man Cybernet. Part A: Syst. Hum. 39 (4), 706e714.
In the case of an in-vehicle instrument cluster, there may be a Lim, J.H., Tsimhoni, O., Liu, Y., 2010. Investigation of driver performance with night
vision and pedestrian detection systems e part I: empirical study on visual
perceptual grouping based on the Gestalt law. The cluster consists
clutter ad glace behavior. IEEE Trans. Intell. Transp. Syst. 11 (3), 670e677.
of several components, and the PVC of its whole visual stimulus Ling, C., Lopez, M., Shehab, R., 2011. Complexity questionnaires of visual displays: a
depends on how the components are organized. Hence, grouping validation study of two information complexity questionnaires of visual dis-
plays. Hum. Factors Ergon. Manuf. Serv. Ind. 23 (5), 1e21.
and organizing the components depending on their perceptual
Michailidou, E., 2008. Determining Users' Perception of Web Page Visual
factors, such as size, common region, location, and color (Palmer, Complexity and Aesthetic Characteristics (HCW Technical Report). University of
1992), might provide different results. Manchester, School of Computer Science, Manchester, United Kingdom.
The model developed to estimate the integration effect was Noy, Y.I., Lemoine, T.L., Klachan, C., Burns, P.C., 2004. Task interruptability and
duration as measure of visual distraction. Appl. Ergon. 35, 207e213.
evaluated by analyzing the relationship between the integration Olivia, A., Mack, M.L., Shrestha, M., Peeper, A., 2004. Identifying the perceptual
effect, as calculated by using the estimation model, and the PVC of dimensions of visual complexity of scenes. In: Paper Presented at the 26th
the overall in-vehicle visual display. As expected, there was a sig- Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Chicago, United States.
Palmer, S., 1992. Common region: a new principle of perceptual grouping. Cogn.
nicant relationship between them. The results of this evaluation Psychol. 24, 436e447.
emphasized the strong relationship between the integration effect Palmer, S., Rock, I., 1994. Rethinking perceptual organization: the role of uniform
and the PVC of the overall in-vehicle display. The Pearson correla- connectedness. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 1 (1), 29e55.
Rock, I., Palmer, S., 1990. The legacy of gestalt psychology. Sci. Am. 263 (6), 84e90.
tion coefcient for the integration effect was 0.575, which was the Rosenholtz, R., Li, Y., Nakano, L., 2007. Measuring visual clutter. J. Vision 7 (2), 1e22.
largest of all the components. This demonstrates the importance of Schmieder, D.E., Weathersby, Marshall R., 1983. Detection performance in clutter
harmoniously and meticulously integrating each component when with variable resolution. IEEE Trans. Aerosp. Electron. Syst. 19 (4), 622e630.
Stickel, C., Ebner, M., Holzinger, A., 2010. The XAOS metric e understanding visual
attempting to decrease the perception of complexity in a visual
complexity as measure of usability. In: Leitner, G., Hitz, M., Holzinger, A. (Eds.),
display. The results of this study suggest that the integration effect HCI in Work and Learning, Life and Leisure, vol. 6389. Springer, Berlin, Hei-
can be used as a component to complement the estimation model delberg, pp. 278e290.
Tsimhoni, O., Green, P., 2001. Visual demand of driving and the execution of display-
for the PVC of an in-vehicle instrument cluster.
intensive in-vehicle tasks. In: Proceeding of the Human Factors and Ergonomics
This research aimed to develop an assessment model to quantify Society 45th Annual Meetings, pp. 1586e1590.
drivers' perceptions of the visual complexity of in-vehicle instru- Tuch, A.N., Bargas-Avila, J.A., Opwis, K., Wilhelm, F.H., 2009. Visual complexity of
ment clusters. The results revealed the differences in human websites: effects on user's experience, physiology, performance, and memory.
Int. J. Hum.eComp. Stud. 67, 703e715.
perception when visual stimuli are seen together or separately. We Van der Horst, R., 2004. Occlusion as a measure for visual workload: an overview of
focused herein on in-vehicle instrument clusters, but we expect TNO occlusion research in car driving. Appl. Ergon. 35, 189e196.
that our ndings apply more broadly to other visual displays with Xia, R.T., Fan, J., Wang, Z., 2010. The spatial allocation of drivers' visual attention
during driving in trafc environment. In: Paper Presented in 2010 International
similar characteristics, namely, displays composed of different and Conference of Intelligent Computation Technology and Automation, Chansha,
independent components. This research was conducted using static Hunan, China.
visual stimuli; further studies are required to examine the Xiang, P., Yang, X., Yuanchun, S., 2007. Web page segmentation based on Gestalt
theory. In: IEEE International Conference on Multimedia and Expo, July, Beijing,
perceived complexity of dynamic visual displays. pp. 2253e2256.
Xing, J., 2007. Information complexity in air trafc control displays. In: Human-
Acknowledgment eComputer Interaction, Part IV, HCII 2007. LNCS, vol. 4553, pp. 797e806.
Xing, J., 2008. Designing Questionnaires for Controlling and Managing Information
Complexity in Visual Displays (Tech. Rep. to Federal Aviation Administration No.
This work was supported by Mid-career Researcher Program DOT/FAA/AM-08/18). Civil Aerospace Medical Institute, Ofce of Aerospace
through NRF grant funded by the MSIP (Ministry of Science, ICT and Medicine, Oklahoma, United States.
Future Planning) (Grant-# NRF-2013R1A2A2A03014150).

You might also like