You are on page 1of 14

Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(4), 525538

Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities

Effectiveness of Video Modeling to teach iPod use to Students


with Moderate Intellectual Disabilities
Diana L. Hammond Abigail D. Whatley
University of Georgia Gwinnett County Public Schools

Kevin M. Ayres and David L. Gast


University of Georgia

Abstract: The primary purpose of this study was to examine the effects of video modeling delivered via computer
on accurate and independent use of an iPod by three participants with moderate intellectual disabilities. In the
context of combined multiple probes across participants and replicated across tasks, three female middle school
students learned to watch a movie, listen to music, and look at photos on an iPod. Video clips were created in
point of view, as if participants were performing the task and presented via video modeling on an IBM
computer. During instruction, participants watched the videos of the entire task they were currently learning.
In addition to data on accuracy of responding, data were also collected on efficiency measures (number of
sessions and number and percentage of errors to criterion), as well as on types of errors (latency, duration, and
topographic). Results indicate that participants acquired the response following video modeling and could
independently use the iPod. Students maintained most tasks on follow-up probe trials; however, on skills that
deteriorated, students were effectively retrained with video booster sessions.

It is well documented that systematic response learning or learning skills by watching others
prompting procedures have successfully been perform those skills. Literature exist that sup-
used to teach participants with a variety of ports participants with intellectual disabilities
diagnoses functional skills (Collins, Gast, Wol- have the ability to learn functional skills
ery, Holcombe, & Leatherby, 1991; Hughes, through observational learning (Clark, Kehle,
Schuster, & Nelson, 1993; Schoen, Lentz, & Jenson, & Beck, 1992; Farmer, Gast, Wolery, &
Suppa, 1988), including how to use technol- Winterling, 1991; Gast, Doyle, Wolery, Ault, &
ogy (Mechling & Cronin, 2006; Mechling, Baklarz, 1991; Griffen, Wolery, & Schuster,
Gast, & Barthold, 2004). However, there is 1992). Most often this learning has occurred
growing extant literature on the use of video during small group instruction with live mod-
instruction for functional skills, trying to inte- els. It can be argued that video instruction,
grate technology and systematic response i.e., the presentation of a model via video,
prompting procedures (Mechling, 2004; provides another opportunity for participants
Mechling & Cronin; Mechling & Gast, 2003; to learn through observation, in contrast to
Norman, Collins, & Schuster, 2001). One of live demonstration.
the theoretical foundations of video instruc-
Mechling (2005) in her review of personally
tion relates to observational learning (Ban-
created instructional video programs outlined
dura, 1969, 1977). Bandura discusses stu-
the benefits of video technology, stating video
dents ability to engage in observational
offers an opportunity for participants to view
the same model as often as necessary (Grice &
Blampied, 1994), can be used by a variety of
Correspondence concerning this article should
instructors (Charlop-Christy, Le, & Freeman,
be addressed to Diana L. Hammond, Department of
Communication Sciences and Special Education,
2002), can be available outside the classroom
University of Georgia, 516 Aderhold Hall, Athens, and is potentially cost-efficient when compared
GA 30602. Phone: 770-401-3598; E-mail: dianah@ to in-vivo training (Branham, Collins, Schuster,
uga.edu & Kleinert, 1999). Ayres and Langone (2005)

Video Modeling / 525


also discuss the time and scheduling advantages of the studies using group arrangements
of video modeling when compared to using live (Cihak et al., 2006; Norman et al., 2001).
models. Additional benefits of video over live This brief review of the literature on video
models include editing ability by customizing instruction of functional skills supports incor-
video footage for participants by emphasizing porating video into instruction for students
salient task features and manipulating video with developmental disabilities. In all studies
length (Coy & Hermansen, 2007). Further, Wis- reviewed, participants viewed video as a total
sick, Gardner, and Langone (1999) argue that task model prior to engaging in the task or as
video can create an authentic training environ- prompts delivered prior to each step in the
ment which students can then relate to real task. Further, the literature provides support
experiences. Video footage can also be down- for the combination of video instruction
loaded onto portable devices and played on de- paired with computer delivered response
mand which can be advantageous on commu- prompt strategies. Considering this literature,
nity based instruction (CBI) or vocational video supported instruction is a valuable tool
training sites, places where live models may not for educators but currently lacks sufficient em-
be available (Mechling, Gast, & Fields, 2008). pirical support for using video in the commu-
Portable devices such as PDAs, iPods, and nity. The current study was designed as a first
iPhones also allow students to use, as needed, step toward moving toward a model where
video prompts to learn new skills and complete video is used by learners in situ. Before learn-
learned skills across novel settings with relative ers use a device (iPod, cell phone, etc.) in
ease. natural environments as a self-prompting tool,
they have to learn to access the device and
Video modeling and prompting involve cre-
independently manipulate the media materi-
ating and presenting video footage of persons
als they would use to prompt themselves.
engaging in targeted tasks. A range of studies
Therefore, this current study was designed to
exist; some presenting video models or
evaluate video modeling as a means to teach
prompts to participants before or during
independent use of an iPod. This study evalu-
video instruction. Definitions within the field
ated the use of video modeling on a computer
of special education exist to differentiate
to teach independent use of an iPod to access
video modeling from video prompting. Video
videos, music, and photos. The research ques-
modeling is the presentation of the entire
tion addressed was: Will video modeling in-
target behavior to participants (Haring,
crease learners with moderate intellectual
Kennedy, Adams, & Pitts-Conway, 1987; Grice disabilities independent use of an iPod to nav-
& Blampied, 1994), whereas video prompting igate to movies, music, and photos?
typically refers to playing a video clip of each
step in the target behavior being completed in
isolation (Cihak, Alberto, Taber-Doughty, & Method
Gama, 2006). Video modeling and prompting
have been successfully used to teach a variety Participants
of functional skills such as withdrawing money Three students, ranging in age from 12 years 8
from an ATM (Alberto, Cihak, & Gama, months to 14 years 11 months, who attended a
2005), purchasing items in a store (Alcantara, suburban middle school in Northeast Georgia
1994; Haring et al.), daily life skills such as and received special education services in a
brushing teeth (Charlop-Christy et al., 2000), self-contained classroom for students with
setting the table (Cannella-Malone et al., moderate intellectual disabilities, participated
2006), cooking (Shipley-Benamou, Lutzker, & in the study. Participants had the following
Tauman, 2002), and vocational skills (Mech- prerequisite skills, the ability to (a) follow
ling & Ortega-Hurndon, 2007). Participants multi-step directions, (b) palmer grasp an
ages ranged from 6 years to 41 years and iPod, (c) visually discriminate video on a 17 in.
included diagnoses of moderate intellectual computer screen according to successfully
disabilities, mild intellectual disabilities and passing a vision test, (d) maintain attention to
autism. Video instructional sessions were most task for 20 min, (e) complete movements as-
often conducted in 1:1 arrangements with two sociated with manipulating buttons on an

526 / Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities-December 2010


TABLE 1

Participant Descriptions

Beth Sally Nan

Gender F F F
Special Education Eligibility MOID MOID/SIa MOID/SIa
IQ 45b 52b 44c
ABAS (GAC)d 65 65 49
Age 14 yrs 2 mon 12 yrs 8 mon 14 yrs 11 mon
Grade 8th 6th 8th
Years in Special Education 9 9 4
Ethnicity African-American African-American Hispanic
Free or Reduced Lunch Yes No Yes
Medication No No No

Note: All students were in Grade 8 and were classified as having learning disabilities.
(a) SI: Speech Impairment; (b) Differential Ability Scales (Harcourt Assessment, Inc., 2007); (c) Wechsler
Intelligence Scale for Children IV (Wechsler, 2003); (d) Adaptive Behavior Assessment System (Harrison &
Oakland, 2000), General Adaptive Composite

iPod, (f) imitate steps of tasks analysis, (g) on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Chil-
regularly attend school with one or fewer ab- dren IV (WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2003), which was
sences in the 2 weeks prior to the study, and administered in Spanish, since English was
(h) match to sample the words movies, vid- her second language. During leisure, Nan pre-
eos, photos, all photos music, and ferred to watch videos or play games. Nans
songs. Participants also provided verbal as- strengths included excellent focus on tasks
sent or agreement to participate and parental (i.e. rarely going off-task), assembly tasks,
permisson for participation was obtained for sorting by size and color, and sight word rec-
each participant. Participants who did not pos- ognition. Instructional needs, according to
sess all entry skills were excluded from the her IEP, included number identification, ex-
study. Refer to Table 1 for additional partici- pressive communication, and interpersonal
pant descriptors. behavior. Sally was 12 years 8 months and had
Participants chosen for the study had a a composite score of 52 on the DAS. She was
history of using visual activity schedules to identified as having a speech impairment that
structure daily tasks and transition between primarily impacted her expressive communi-
activities; however, participants required cation. During leisure, Sally would prefer to
prompting to use their schedules effectively. interact with an adult or play on the com-
Participants were unable to transition between puter. Strengths for Sally included matching,
activities and engaged in high rates of aber- filing by number and first letter, locating
rant behaviors instead of chosen leisure activ- items in the community and receptive com-
ities. Beth was 14 years 2 months, and had a munication. None of the participants had pre-
composite score of 45 on the Differential Abil- vious exposure to video modeling for instruc-
ity Scales assessment (DAS; Harcourt Assess- tional purposes. In addition, participants had
ment, Inc., 2007). During leisure, Beth en- never used iPods for instructional or recre-
joyed puzzles, video, playing on the computer, ational purposes; however, this is an age-ap-
and painting her nails. Beths strengths in- propriate leisure activity.
cluded tasks such as filing by first letter, read-
ing basic sight words, and navigating the
Settings and Arrangements
school independently. Weaknesses included
locating items in the community and counting Individual probe trials and intervention ses-
money. The second participant, Nan, was 14 sions took place four times per week in a
years 11 months and had a full scale IQ of 44 self-contained classroom (32 ft 26 ft), dur-

Video Modeling / 527


ing participants independent work time. iPod by the principal investigator and recorded in
instructional sessions occurred in the back of first person point of view onto the video cam-
the classroom at a table adjacent to partici- eras hard drive. The resulting video showed
pants computer. During video modeling in- only the hands of the investigator performing
struction, participants sat at an IBM Lenovo the task. As the investigator performed each
desktop computer in their classroom to watch step in a tasks analysis, the special education
the DVD of how to use the iPod. Participants teacher narrated each step beginning with the
did not have access to the DVDs or iPod during task direction. Video clips were then down-
non-instructional time and when not used, the loaded onto a Dell Inspiron 1420 Laptop and
DVDs and iPod were stored in a locked file each task analysis was burned to separate
cabinet in the classroom. Prior to intervention DVD. The duration of video clips were, 2 min
the iPod was taken out of the file cabinet and 16 s for movies, 3 min 43 s for music, and 1
placed on the teachers desk. During Probe min 21 s for photos.
trials and Video-Modeling condition sessions The iPod was an 8GB light green third gen-
participants held the iPod and when finished, eration iPod Nano, which measured approxi-
handed the iPod to the teacher. mately 3 in. 2 in. and weighed 1 lb 7 oz. The
The classroom was equipped with one front viewing screen measured 134 in. 114 in. Ten
bookcase that housed several leisure activities. photos of participants preferred musical
Student desks were arranged in two clusters of band were downloaded onto the iPod. Photo
four. Each cluster was positioned on either slide show settings were preset for this study;
side of a group of eight study carrels used for time per photo was set to 2 s, and repeat and
independent work in the center of the class- shuffle photos were turned off. For music, a
room. On one side of the room was a rectan- preferred song from The Disney High School
gular table and microwave which were adja- Musical 2 Soundtrack, was downloaded to the
cent to a bathroom and a kidney-shaped table music category. Three movies were down-
used for small group instruction. Behind stu- loaded to the movies section of the iPod; one 1
dent desks and study carrels, at the rear of the min movie clip of a couple dancing, one 1 min
room, was a second rectangular table used for movie clip of a boy riding a skate board and
additional leisure activities, a small sitting area one movie clip showing 1 min of the play The
comprised of a futon and folding chair, and Hobbit. Preferred movies, music, and photos
two teacher desks behind two partitions. Ad- were based on student report of interest. Ma-
jacent to the sitting area and second rectan- terials needed for data collection included
gular table was a computer station utilized by data sheets, four pens, two clipboards, a three
students and teachers, a round table used for ring binder to store datasheets and two Taylor
small group instruction, and a rolling cart digital timers, model number 5827-21.
equipped with age-appropriate books.
Data were collected by participants special
Response Definitions and Recording Procedures
education teacher and reliability data were
collected by an independent special educa- Initial Probe Trials/Video Modeling Sessions/
tion teacher. The special education teacher Maintenance Trials. The primary dependent
sat approximately 5 ft behind participants at measure was the percentage of steps accu-
the student computer and adjacent table, rately and independently completed for each
while a paraprofessional was responsible for target task. Data were collected using trial re-
monitoring nonparticipants whom were in- cording (Ayres & Gast, 2010) following the
volved in various activities outside of the class- task analyses presented in Table 2.
room. A student response was scored as correct if a
participant independently initiated the cor-
rect topography within 15 s and completed
Materials and Equipment
the step in the task analysis within 15 s of the
Video clips were created using a Sony Handy- task direction or completion of the previous
cam DCR-SR42. Three task analyses delin- step. An incorrect response was scored if the
eated in Table 2, for navigating an iPod to student (a) did not initiate within 15 s (no
access movies, music, and photos were written response), (b) did not complete her response

528 / Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities-December 2010


TABLE 2

Tasks Analyses and Response Definitions for Using the iPod

Movie Music Photos

1. Press green center button to 1. Press green center button to 1. Press green center button to
turn the iPod on. turn the iPod on. turn the iPod on.
2. Wait for menu screen to 2. Wait for menu screen to 2. Wait for menu screen to
appear. appear. appear.
3. Put thumb on menu 3. Make sure Music is 3. Put thumb on menu button.
button. highlighted in blue. 4. Scroll clockwise until the word
4. Scroll clockwise until the 4. Press green button to select Photos is highlighted in
word Videos is highlighted Music. blue.
in blue. 5. Scroll clockwise until the word 5. Press green button with thumb
5. Press green button with Songs is highlighted in blue. to open Photos menu.
thumb to open Videos 6. Press green button to open 6. Press green button again with
menu. Songs menu. thumb to open All Photos
6. Press green button again 7. Put thumb on play button. menu.
with thumb to open 8. Press play button to start 7. Put thumb on play button.
Movies menu. music. 8. Press play button to start
7. Put thumb on menu 9. When song is finished, press photo slideshow.
button. and hold play/pause button 9. Watch slideshow and wait until
8. Scroll clockwise until task for 2 s to shut iPod off. photo show is finished.
movie is highlighted in blue. 10. Hand iPod to teacher. 10. When photo show is finished,
9. Press green button with press and hold play/pause
thumb to start movie. button for 2 s to shut iPod off.
10. Watch movie and wait until 11. Hand iPod to teacher.
movie is finished.
11. Press and hold play/pause
button for 2 s to shut iPod
off.
12. Hand iPod to teacher.

within 15 s of initiation, or (c) completed the nance probe trials to assist in completing tar-
step incorrectly or out of sequence (Mechling get behaviors. Adult prompts were monitored
& Ortega-Hurdon, 2007). An incorrect re- using event recording and incorporated into
sponse was also scored according to the type procedural reliability. Each time an adult pro-
of error: (a) latency errors (L), no initiation vided a verbal prompt (telling a participant
within 15 s of either task direction or comple- what to do), a model (showing a participant
tion of prior step in task analysis; (b) duration what to do), or a partial or full physical
errors (D), task analysis step not completed prompt (hand over hand assisting a partici-
within 15 s; and (c) topography errors (T), as pant through task analysis step), a tally mark
incorrectly completing behavioral response or was recorded on the data sheet.
performing steps in the task analysis out of
order from teaching or task sequence. After
General Procedure
task completion, percentage of steps com-
pleted accurately and independently was cal- Individual sessions were conducted four times
culated by dividing the number of steps accu- weekly with each participant. Each session
rately and independently completed by the lasted between 1520 min, including video
total number of steps in the task analysis and modeling during intervention probe trials.
multiplying by 100. Natural reinforcement procedures, access to
No adult verbal, model, partial or full phys- the selected movie, song, or photos, were held
ical prompt was to occur during initial probe constant across all conditions and no addi-
trials, video modeling sessions, or mainte- tional preferred items or activities were pro-

Video Modeling / 529


vided to participants. A task direction for the determine whether participants could navi-
target skill (e.g., Lets watch a movie, Lets gate the iPod. Materials for the task were pre-
listen to some music, or Lets look at some arranged and each participant was provided
photos) was provided to participants, de- with the task direction to initiate the target
pending on the target task. behavior (e.g., Lets watch a movie). Data
were collected on each participants ability to
accurately and independently complete each
Experimental Design
step of the task in sequence. Probe trials were
A combination multiple probe design (Gast & discontinued when a participant either cor-
Ledford, 2010) across three participants, rep- rectly performed the entire task analysis or
licated across three behaviors, was used to made an error (e.g., latency, duration, topo-
evaluate the effects of video modeling to teach graphic). If a participant responded correctly,
participants to use the iPod. Three experimen- she was allowed to continue to use the iPod to
tal conditions were conducted for each partic- listen to or view her selection; however, if a
ipant: (a) Initial Probe trials, (b) Video Mod- participant responded incorrectly, the teacher
eling sessions (Pre-Video Modeling probe trial stated, Ok, we are done, and took the iPod
and Post-Video Modeling probe trial), and (c) from the participant. If the participant at-
Maintenance probe trials. Initial Probe and tempted to exit the area of the classroom
Maintenance trials consisted of one trial and during a probe trial, she was verbally redi-
Video Modeling sessions consisted of two tri- rected (Lets finish) back to the iPod and
als, a Pre-Video Modeling trial and a Post- given 15 s to initiate the next step in the task
Video Modeling trial. analysis. Video modeling instruction was im-
Initial Probe trial data were collected for all plemented after three stable data points
target behaviors; navigating the iPod to access across a minimum of two days.
movies, music, and photos for all three partic-
ipants, for a minimum of 2 days and 3 sessions
Video Modeling Sessions
and until a stable level and trend were estab-
lished. Once Initial Probe trial data were sta- Video Modeling sessions consisted of Pre-
ble, video modeling was introduced to Partic- Video Modeling probe trials, video modeling
ipant 1 on the first target behavior (i.e., instruction, and Post-Video Modeling probe
selecting movies) until criterion was met. trials. Pre-Video Modeling probe trials and
Once criteria were met, probe trial data were Post-Video Modeling probe trials were con-
collected on all three target behaviors for all ducted identical to Initial Probe trials. Pre-
participants. After which video-modeling was Video Modeling probe trials were cold trials;
then introduced to Participant 1 to teach nav- in other words, the student had not yet re-
igating to music and for Participant 2 to teach ceived instruction that day for using the iPod.
navigating to movies. Staggering the introduc- The purpose of the Pre-Video Modeling
tion of instruction on the first target behavior probe trials was to assess participant respond-
(i.e., selecting movies) based on participant ing prior to daily instruction. If participants
skill acquisition allowed for objective replica- did not reach criteria, 100% accurate re-
tion of effects across participants (inter-sub- sponding on the Pre-Video Modeling probe
ject direct replication). Replication of inter- trial, video modeling instruction was imple-
vention effectiveness across two additional mented. Only Pre-Video Modeling probe trial
skills, selecting music and selecting photos, data were graphed and mastery criterion was
provides direct intra-subject replication (i.e., established at 100% accurate and indepen-
repeating the experimental effect with the dent responding during Pre-Video Modeling
same participant). probe trials for two sessions for all three par-
ticipants. Establishing acquisition criterion on
the Pre-Video Modeling trials was more strin-
Initial Probe Trials
gent, requiring participants to maintain target
Single opportunity Initial Probe trials (Coo- behaviors over a minimum of two days.
per, Heron, & Heward, 2007) were conducted Following Pre-Video Modeling probe trials,
for all three tasks with each participant to if the participant failed to respond with 100%

530 / Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities-December 2010


accuracy, video modeling sessions were con- multiplied by 100 (Ayres & Gast, 2010). Dur-
ducted. Instructional sessions were conducted ing Initial Probe trials, Pre-Video Modeling
individually and involved the teacher showing probe trials and Post-Video Modeling probe
participants the entire video clip depicting trials, occurrence/nonoccurrence PF data
navigation of the iPod to access movies, music, were collected on (a) adult prompting, (b)
or photos, depending on target task. The re- delivery of correct task direction, (c) partici-
searcher directed a participant to the com- pant positioning, (d) participant being given
puter and told the participant to sit down 15 s to initiate a response, (e) participant
(Have a seat at the computer). Once the given 15 s to complete step, and (f) no deliv-
participant was seated and the DVD was in- ery of an additional preferred item or activity.
serted into the computer, the teacher di- Similarly, during video modeling occurrence/
rected, Watch this, and the video clip dis- nonoccurrence data were collected on above
playing the target skill was shown on the behaviors plus, whether the (a) correct DVD
computer beginning with the task direction. was handed to participant, (b) correct DVD
Participants were required to sit at the com- was played, and (c) task direction watch this
puter and watch the video clip of the entire provided. PF was converted to percentage by
task analysis. Immediately following comple- dividing the number of emitted researcher
tion of the video clip, a Post-Video Modeling behaviors by the number of planned re-
probe trial was conducted identical to Pre- searcher behaviors and multiplying by 100
Video Modeling probe trial. Post-Video Mod- (Billingsley, White, & Munson, 1980). At least
eling trials were conducted to assess immedi- 90% agreement was required for both IOA
ate recall of target behavior and to provide and PF in each condition or reliability raters
participants practice with the task analysis im- were retrained.
mediately after viewing the video clip. IOA and PF data were collected 25.48%
across all conditions for all participants. Mean
IOA was 100% across all conditions for Beth,
Maintenance Probe Trials
Sally, and Nan respectively. During Initial
Once a participant met criteria, maintenance Probe trials and Maintenance probe trials,
data were periodically collected following mean PF was 100%. Mean PF during Pre-
probe trial procedures. A minimum of one Video Modeling probe trials and Post-Video
maintenance probe trial was conducted per Modeling probe trials was 98.55% (range
task for each participant. Maintenance probe 92.59%100%) for movies, 98.88% (range
trials were conducted during sessions 15, 21, 96.66%100%) for music, and 100% for pho-
26, 31, 46, 56, 60 and 70 for Beth; sessions 31, tos. Procedural reliability errors included pro-
46, 56, 60 and 70 for Sally; and sessions 47, 60, viding a gestural prompt to Beth during one
and 70 for Nan. Pre-Video Modeling probe trial during mov-
ies, providing Beth with an additional verbal
praise when she completed navigating to mov-
Reliability
ies and not providing Sally with 15 s to com-
Procedural fidelity (PF) and interobserver plete a step in the task analysis during navigat-
agreement (IOA) data were collected simulta- ing to music.
neously by an independent observer, who was
a second special education teacher. The reli-
Social Validity
ability observer was provided with a copy of
task analyses, a digital timer, a data sheet on At the conclusion of the study, social validity
which to record data, a clipboard, and a pen. data were collected on the perceived out-
IOA and PF data were collected on a mini- comes of video modeling as an instructional
mum of 20% across sessions and behaviors for tool for teaching participants with moderate
all participants. intellectual disabilities to use an iPod. A survey
IOA was calculated for each step in the task using a Likert rating scale, ranging from 1,
analysis using point-by-point method of com- representing strongly disagree, to 5, repre-
parison by dividing the number of agreements senting strongly agree was completed for each
by number of agreements plus disagreements participant. Surveys were completed by para-

Video Modeling / 531


TABLE 3

Social Validity Questionnaire: Mean Score and Range for each Question

Beth Sally Nan


Mean
Questions: Mean Range Mean Range Mean Range Response

1. The identified behavior was 4.2 35 4.4 45 4.8 45 4.7


important to increase for the
participant.
2. Questions regarding the research 4.8 45 4.8 45 4.8 45 4.8
were answered in a prompt manner
and to my satisfaction.
3. The outcomes, increase participants 4.8 45 4.2 45 4.8 45 4.6
ability to navigate/use iPod was
significant to the participant.
4. The participant became more 4.6 45 3.8 34 4.8 45 4.4
independent as a result of instruction.
5. I now believe the participant is able 4.6 45 4.0 35 4.6 45 4.4
to learn from video modeling.
6. I believe video modeling was solely 4.2 35 3.8 35 4.4 45 4.1
responsible for the participants
learning.
7. My opinions about using video 3.4 35 3.4 25 3.4 25 3.4
modeling for instruction purposes
have changed.
Total Respondents 5 5 5

professionals, special education teachers, eling (3.4) were lower with most respondents
therapists, and significant others. Specifically, reporting an agree or no opinion.
social validity data were collected on: (a) the
identified behavior was important to increase
for the participant; (b) questions regarding Results
the research were answered in a prompt man-
ner and to my satisfaction; (c) the outcomes, Effectiveness of Video Modeling
increase participants ability to navigate/use
Movies. Figure 1 presents data on the per-
iPod, were significant to the participant; (d)
centage of steps completed accurately and in-
the participant became more independent as
dependently for Beth, Sally, and Nan. Navigat-
a result of instruction; (e) I now believe the
ing to movies is represented by an open
participant is able to learn from video model-
square (), navigating to music is represented
ing; (f) I believe video modeling was solely
by an open triangle () and navigating to
responsible for the participants learning; and photos is represented by an open circle ().
(g) my opinions about using video modeling Initial Probe trial data were collected on all
for instruction purposes have changed. Data tasks during the first three sessions of the
were summed and averaged and the range/ study for all participants. Introduction of
mean scores were reported for each question video modeling was staggered across partici-
for each participant. Table 3 presents the pants, which allowed additional Initial Probe
mean score for each question, for each partic- trial data to be collected for Sally during ses-
ipant. A total of 5 respondents per participant sion 15 and for Nan during sessions 15 and 31.
provided social validity feedback. Mean rat- All participants demonstrated 0% indepen-
ings related to effectiveness of intervention dent responding during Initial Probe trials for
(4.1) and practical application of video mod- all target behaviors. Video modeling instruc-

532 / Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities-December 2010


Figure 1. Percentage of Steps Completed Independently for Beth, Sally, and Nan. Daily Probe for iPod
Movies, Daily Probe for iPod Music, Daily Probe for iPod Photos.

tion was implemented with Beth during ses- crease in responding to 83%. Similar to Beth,
sion 4, session 16 for Sally, and session 32 for Sally increased independent responding for
Nan. Upon introduction of video modeling to navigating to music (20%) and photos (45%),
the first behavior, selecting movies, partici- indicating similar steps between the three task
pants required 3, 3, and 5 Pre-Video Modeling analyses generalized. Nan maintained 100%
Probe trials before an increase in correct per- responding for navigating to movies and in-
formance was observed. During Post-Video creased responding for navigating to music
Modeling Probe trials, the addition of a verbal (20%) and photos (27%) during her Mainte-
prompt (You can go ahead) only on the first nance Probe trial (session 47). An increase in
target behavior (selecting movies) was neces- navigating to music and photo responding
sary after participants responding remained at prior to video modeling instruction can be
0% for three consecutive days. During Post- attributed to the overlap in steps across the
Video Modeling trials for navigating to music three task analyses. Four steps across tasks
and photos, participants were provided only were identical.
with the task direction and initiated the task Music. Data on the percentage of steps
without needing the verbal prompt. completed independently for navigating to
Criterion was mastered in 11 sessions for music are also presented on Figure 1, denoted
Beth and 15 sessions for Sally and Nan. Mean by an open triangle. All participants demon-
number of sessions to acquisition was 13.6. strated 0% accurate responding for navigating
Beth maintained 100% responding (session to music during Initial Probe trials. Upon in-
15) for navigating to movies and increased troduction of video modeling, 5, 14, and 12
responding for both navigating to music sessions (Mean 10.3 sessions) were required
(40%) and photos (45%). Sally had difficulty for Beth, Sally, and Nan, respectively to ac-
navigating to movies (session 31), with a de- quire the target behavior. Beth maintained

Video Modeling / 533


100% responding for navigating to movies could not be collected due to the end of the
and music and concurrently increased re- school year.
sponding for navigating to photos (55%). Results show a functional relation between
Sally maintained her ability to navigate to mu- the use of video modeling and an increase in
sic; however, she continued to have difficulty the percentage of steps completed accurately
navigating to movies showing a decrease in and independently for all iPod tasks. Direct
responding to 33%. She maintained some in- intra-subject replication was achieved by rep-
dependent responding for navigating to pho- lication of effects within each participant
tos (36%). Nan remained at 100% responding across three tasks and direct inter-subject rep-
for navigating to movies and music and navi- lication was obtained by replication of effects
gating to photos remained stable at 27%. across three participants.
Photos. Figure 1 presents data on the per- Error Analysis. During Post-Video Model-
centage of steps completed independently for ing Probe trials, once the participant contin-
photos, represented by an open circle. All ued to make the same error, demonstrated by
participants demonstrated 0% accurate and 10 consecutive sessions, a gestural prompt, a
independent responding for navigating to point indicating the next step in the task anal-
photos during Initial Probe trials. Upon intro- ysis was provided. Participants were then given
duction of video modeling, sessions 22, 47, 15 s to initiate and complete step after ges-
and 61 for Beth, Sally, and Nan, respectively tural prompt. If the step was completed cor-
Beth met criteria in 4 sessions while both Sally rectly, the participant could continue; how-
and Nan required 9 sessions (Mean 7.3 ever, if the participant made an error, the trial
was discontinued and the iPod was removed.
sessions). Independent performance was
The gestural prompt was delivered by the par-
maintained by Beth and Nan through session
ticipants teacher. Gestural prompts were im-
70. Sally maintained 100% performance dur-
plemented for the following: Beth: session 13
ing her initial Maintenance Probe trial, but
of movies, step 11; Sally: session 25 of movies,
her performance decreased on subsequent
step 5 and sessions 41 and 42 of music, step 6;
probe trials (Range 27%36%). Navigating
and Nan: session 41 of movies, step 5 and
to photos required the least number of ses-
session 57 of music, step 5. None of the par-
sions to criterion. Compared to navigating to
ticipants required the use of an additional
movies and music, three and six fewer sessions
gestural prompt during the photo sessions.
were required to teach participants to navi-
Table 4 presents data on the number and
gate to photos. percentage of errors during the probe trials
Video Booster. Following session 70, a for each experimental condition for all partic-
video booster was introduced to tasks which ipants and target behaviors. Since probes were
did not maintain at criterion levels. Video single opportunity, meaning as soon as an
booster sessions were review sessions involving error was emitted, the trial concluded, infor-
a re-introduction of the Video Modeling Con- mation regarding total number of errors
dition: a Pre-Video Modeling Probe trial, ex- made during each condition is equal to the
posure to the video clip, followed by a Post- number of sessions minus sessions with crite-
Video Modeling Probe trial. Video booster ria performance. An analysis of the types of
sessions continued until all behaviors re- errors made, latency (L), duration (D), and
turned to 100% accurate and independent topographic (T), though limited was con-
performance. All participants required video ducted. Though duration errors did not oc-
booster sessions for music and Sally required cur, the types of error (latency and topo-
retraining for photos. Sally reached criteria graphic) emitted by participants changed,
for navigating to music on the first video most noticeably between Initial Probe trials
booster session; while, Beth required 3 ses- and video modeling instruction. During the
sions and Nan required 6 sessions to reach first three sessions of Initial Probe trial data,
criteria. Sally also required 3 video booster participants emitted more latency errors when
sessions to reach previous levels of indepen- compared to topographic errors. When prob-
dent performance on navigating to photos. A ing subsequent tasks, Beth and Sally began to
final follow-up probe was scheduled, but data emit topographic errors, after instruction on

534 / Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities-December 2010


TABLE 4

Percentage of Errors for Each Condition

Video Boost
Initial Probe Video Modeling Maintenance Sessions

Total
L D T L D T L D T L D T Errors

Beth
Movie 3 0 0 5 0 4 0 0 0 12
Music 3 0 1 0 0 3 0 0 3 0 0 3 13
Photos 3 0 2 0 0 2 0 0 0 7
Total Errors 9 0 3 5 0 9 0 0 3 0 0 3 32
Sally
Movie 4 0 0 5 0 8 0 0 3 20
Music 4 0 1 1 0 11 0 0 2 0 0 0 19
Photos 4 0 2 0 0 8 0 0 1 0 0 3 18
Total Errors 12 0 3 6 0 27 0 0 6 0 0 3 57
Nan
Movie 5 0 0 6 0 7 0 0 0 18
Music 5 0 1 2 0 8 0 0 1 0 0 5 22
Photos 7 0 0 1 0 6 0 0 0 14
Total Errors 17 0 1 9 0 21 0 0 1 0 0 5 54
Total Errors* 38 0 7 20 0 57 0 0 10 0 0 11 143
Mean Percentage**
Movie 32% 0% 0% 80% 0% 33% 0% 0% 30% 0% 0% 0% 35%
Music 32% 0% 43% 15% 0% 39% 0% 0% 60% 0% 0% 73% 38%
Photos 36% 0% 57% 5% 0% 28% 0% 0% 10% 0% 0% 27% 27%

Key: (L) Latency Errors; (D) Duration Errors; (T) Topographical Errors;
* Represents total number of errors across all participants for each error type and condition; ** Rep-
resents mean percentage of error type across all participants for each condition for each target behavior;
Data not collected because condition was not implemented for participant.

navigating to movies. Perhaps participants exists supporting the effectiveness of video


had become familiar with the nature of the modeling to teach use of portable technology.
task and were attempting the target behavior. Results indicate participants learned how to
Initial Probe trials for Nan showed no consis- navigate the iPod, though some tasks did not
tent pattern of errors. During instruction, maintain. All participants had difficulty main-
though participants continued to emit latency taining how to navigate to music. Unless the
errors, a majority of errors were topographic. iPod settings are altered, a variety of ways exist
After instruction for each task, all subsequent for participants to access music on an iPod.
errors were topographical. After video modeling instruction, both Beth
and Nan maintained their ability to navigate
to music; however, it was not according to the
Discussion
task analysis. Both participants made errors on
This study evaluated the effectiveness of video step 5 of the task analysis and selected play-
modeling on participants percentage of accu- list instead of songs. The iPod settings can
rate and independent responding to navigate be altered to narrow the menu options avail-
to movies, music, and photos on the iPod. able to participants, a recommendation for
While video modeling has been demonstrated future research.
to be effective in teaching community skills Probe data for Sally on session 60 resulted
and other functional skills, limited literature in low levels of maintenance for all tasks (mov-

Video Modeling / 535


ies 25%; music 40%; photos 36%). The higher if a multiple opportunity probe strat-
researcher learned that at the time probe data egy were used.
were collected, Sally was in an argument with The use of the iPod was reinforcing and
a fellow classmate, which could have adversely age-appropriate for middle school students
affected her performance during that probe with moderate intellectual disabilities. Each
trial, particularly on navigating to movies, participant often would ask to use the iPod
since independent performance during the during independent leisure time. At the com-
next daily probe trial was 100%. Sally still had pletion of the study all participants expressed
difficulty navigating to music and photos, but a desire to obtain iPods of their own, express-
quickly returned to criterion with video ing pride in their learning and excitement at
booster sessions. the prospect of having and using a piece of
All participants required a verbal prompt technology their same-age peers in regular
(You can go ahead) on the first target be- education often used.
havior. It was expected that since the task
direction was provided on the video clip prior
to task initiation that the task direction would Limitation
cue participants to initiate the target re-
Though results support effectiveness, a limita-
sponse. This did not occur, therefore, partic-
tion of this study was conducting single oppor-
ipants were provided the task direction and a
tunity probe trials. Due to the single opportu-
teacher verbal prompt to begin the task. The
nity probe, it was difficult to discern the
addition of the verbal prompt was required
information participants were learning from
only on the first task and not during navigat-
the video when the iPod was taken away after
ing to music or photo instruction. Future re-
the first error. It is possible single opportunity
searchers may want to evaluate the effective-
probe trials underestimated participants ac-
ness of audio recording the verbal prompt on
tual performance levels because trials were
the video during video modeling instruction
terminated as soon as participants made an
to determine if the teachers presence during
error. Future research should incorporate a
instruction is necessary or if the video can
multiple opportunity probe strategy, provid-
provide all necessary task directions and
ing participants maximum opportunity to re-
prompts. Audio recordings in this study were
spond and complete all of the steps in the task
completed by the participants teacher to pro-
analysis. Data should be collected to deter-
vide them with a familiar voice delivering in-
mine if using a multiple opportunity probe
struction. Future researchers may evaluate au-
strategy would eliminate the need to incorpo-
dio recording non-familiar adults/peers
rate a gesture prompt.
providing task directions to students to deter-
mine if it is necessary that video modeling
instruction incorporate participants teachers Future Research
voice or if a novel person can provide audio
for the video clips. Throughout the course of the study, there was
A trend in the types of errors participants an upgrade in technology and the fourth gen-
emitted emerged. Data indicate participants eration iPod Nano was introduced to the mar-
did not initiate responses during the first ket. Due to lack of availability and time con-
three sessions of the probe condition, most straints, the current study was unable to
likely because participants did not know how conduct a generalization probe across novel
to respond to the task direction and were technology. However, since technology
willing to wait for teacher directed instruction. changes rapidly, future research may want to
Data also show that after instruction on the assess participants ability to generalize
initial task, participants became familiar with trained behaviors to novel technology up-
task expectations and initiated navigating to grades. Important questions to answer could
music and photos. An analysis of error data include: (a) Will participants generalize pre-
shows that video modeling instruction lead to viously trained skills across novel technology
a high rate of errors, particularly topographic upgrades?, and (b) If generalization does not
errors. The number of errors may have been occur, how effective and efficient would

536 / Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities-December 2010


video booster sessions be in re-training tar- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30,
get behaviors? 537552.
Future research should also focus on repli- Cihak, D., Alberto, P. A., Taber-Doughty, T., &
cation of procedures across age groups and Gama, R. (2006). A comparison of static picture
prompting and video prompting simulation strat-
disability categories. Recommendations for
egies using group instructional procedures. Focus
procedure changes include (a) modifying the
on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 21,
iPod menu settings to limit the selection op- 89 99.
tions to participants, (b) including the verbal Clark, E., Kehle, T. J., Jenson, W. R., & Beck, D. E.
prompt on the video, (c) using a multiple (1992). Evaluation of the parameters of self-mod-
opportunity probe strategy, (d) using a novel eling interventions. School Psychology Review, 21,
person to record the audio onto the videos, 246 254.
and (e) comparing video modeling and video Collins, B. C., Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., Holcombe, A.,
prompting on efficiency measures including & Leatherby, J. G. (1991). Using constant time
topographical errors. delay to teach self-feeding to young students with
severe/profound handicaps: Evidence of limited
effectiveness. Journal of Developmental and Physical
Disabilities, 3, 157178.
References
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L.
Alberto, P. A., Cihak, D. F., & Gama, R. L. (2005). (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper
Use of static picture prompts versus video model- Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall.
ing during simulation instruction. Research in De- Farmer, J. A., Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., & Winterling,
velopmental Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, V. (1991). Small group instruction for students
26, 327339. with severe handicaps: A study of observational
Alcantara, P. R. (1994). Effects of videotape instruc- learning. Education and Training in Mental Retar-
tional package on purchasing skills of children dation, 26, 190 201.
with autism. Exceptional Children, 61, 40 55. Gast, D. L., Doyle, P. M., Wolery, M., Ault, M. J., &
Ayres, K. M., & Gast, D. L. (2010). Dependent mea- Baklarz, J. L. (1991). Acquisition of incidental
sures and measurement procedures. In D. L. Gast information during small group instruction. Edu-
(Ed.), Single subject research methodology in behavioral cation and Treatment of Children, 14, 118.
sciences. New York: Routledge Publishers. Gast, D. L. & Ledford, J. (2010). Variations of mul-
Ayres, K. M. & Langone, J. (2005). Intervention and tiple baseline designs and combination designs.
instruction with video for students with autism: A In D. L. Gast (Ed.), Single subject research methodol-
review of the literature. Education and Training in ogy in behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge Pub-
Developmental Disabilities, 40, 183196. lishers.
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. Griffen, A. K., Wolery, M., & Schuster, J. W. (1992).
New York: Holt, Rinchart & Winston. Triadic instruction of chained food preparation
Bandura, A., (1977). Social learning theory. Upper responses: Acquisition and observational learn-
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. ing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 193
Billingsley, F. F., White, O. R., & Munson, A. R. 204.
(1980). Procedural reliability: A rationale and an Harcourt Assessment, Inc. (2007). Differential Ability
example. Behavioral Assessment, 2, 229 241. Scales Assessment. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.
Branham, R. S., Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W., & Harrison and Oakland. (2007). Adaptive Behavior
Kleinert, H. (1999). Teaching community skills to Assessment System. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psy-
students with moderate disabilities: Comparing chological Services.
combined techniques of classroom stimulation, Haring, T. G., Kennedy, C. H., Adams, M. J., &
videotape modeling and community-based in- Pitts-Conway, V. (1987). Teaching generalization
struction. Education and Training in Mental Retar- of purchasing skills across community settings to
dation and Developmental Disabilities, 34, 170 181. autistic youth using videotape modeling. Journal
Cannella-Malone, H., Sigafoos, J., OReilly, M., De la of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 89 96.
Cruz, B., Edrisinha, C., & Lancioni, G. E. (2006). Hughes, M., W., Schuster, J. W., & Nelson, N.
Comparing video prompting to video modeling (1993). The acquisition of independent dressing
for teaching daily living skills to six adults with skills by students with multiple disabilities. Journal
developmental delay. Education and Training in of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 5, 233
Developmental Disabilities, 41, 344 356. 251.
Charlop-Christy, M. H., Le, L., & Freeman, K. A. Le Grice, B., & Blampied, N. M. (1994). Training
(2000). A comparison of video modeling with in pupils with intellectual disability to operate edu-
vivo modeling for teaching children with autism. cation technology using video prompting. Educa-

Video Modeling / 537


tion and Training in Mental Retardation and Devel- adults with moderate intellectual disabilities. The
opmental Disabilities, 29, 321330. Journal of Special Education, 42, 179 190.
McCoy, K., & Hermansen, E. (2007). Video model- Mechling, L. C. & Ortega-Hurndon, F., (2007).
ing for individuals with autism: A review of model Computer-based video instruction to teach young
types and effects. Education and Treatment of Chil- adults with moderate intellectual disabilities to
dren, 30, 183213. perform multiple step job tasks in a generalized
Mechling, L. C. (2005). The effect of instructor- setting. Education and Training in Developmental
created video programs to teach students with Disabilities, 42, 24 37.
disabilities: A literature review. Journal of Special Norman, J. M., Collins, B. C., & Schuster, J. W.
Education Technology, 20, 2536. (2001). Using an instructional package including
Mechling, L. C. (2004). Effects of multi-media, com- video technology to teach self-help skills to ele-
puter-based instruction on grocery shopping flu- mentary students with mental disabilities. Journal
ency. Journal of Special Education Technology, 19, of Special Education Technology, 16, 518.
2334. Schoen, S. F., Lentz, F. E., & Suppa, R. J. (1998). An
Mechling, L. C., & Cronin, B. A. (2006). Computer- examination of two prompt fading procedures
based video instruction to teach augmentative and opportunities to observe in teaching handi-
communication device use for making purchases capped preschoolers self-help skills. Journal of the
in fast food restaurants. Journal of Special Educa- Division for Early Childhood, 12, 349 358.
tion, 39, 234 245. Shipley-Benamou, R., Lutzker, J. R., & Taubman, M.
Mechling, L. C. & Gast, D. (2003). Multimedia in- (2002). Teaching daily living skills to children
struction to teach grocery store location: A study with autism through instructional video model-
of generalization. Education and Training in Devel- ing. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4,
opmental Disabilities, 38, 6276. 165175.
Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. & Barthold, S. (2004). Weschler, D. (2003). Weschsler intelligence scale for
Multimedia instruction to teach students with children (4th Ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psycho-
moderate intellectual disabilities to use a debit logical Corporation.
card to make purchases. Exceptionality, 1, 239 Wissick, C. A., Gardner, J. E., & Langone, J. (1999).
254. Video-based simulations: Consideration for teach-
Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. L., & Fields, E. (2008). ing students with developmental disabilities. Ca-
Evaluation of a portable DVD player as a self- reer Development for Exceptional Children, 22, 233
prompting device to teach cooking tasks to young 249.

538 / Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities-December 2010

You might also like