You are on page 1of 18

Low-Income Parents' Beliefs about Their Role in Children's Academic Learning

Author(s): Kathryn V. Drummond and Deborah Stipek


Source: The Elementary School Journal, Vol. 104, No. 3 (Jan., 2004), pp. 197-213
Published by: The University of Chicago Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3202949 .
Accessed: 06/03/2014 07:04

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

The University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The
Elementary School Journal.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Low-Income Parents' Abstract

Beliefs about Their In individual telephone interviews, 234 low-


income African-American, Caucasian, and La-
Role in Children's tino parents rated the importance of helping
their second- and third-grade children in read-
Academic Learning ing, math, and homework and of knowing what
their children are learning. Parents reported
whether they had taught their child in math and
reading and read with their child in the past
week. They also answered open-ended questions
about the type of help they deemed appropriate.
Kathryn V. Drummond On questionnaires, teachers rated each student's
University of California,Los Angeles reading and math skills and noted whether they
had given a child's parent suggestions for help-
Deborah Stipek ing with either subject. Findings showed that
parents rated the importance of helping their
StanfordUniversity child with academic work very high. Parents of
second graders tended to rate the importance of
helping higher than did parents of third graders.
Similar to past research, ratings varied system-
atically as a function of parents' perceptions of
children's academic performance and as a func-
tion of whether teachers had offered suggestions;
however, parents perceived helping with read-
ing as more important than helping with math.
Findings implied that teachers who desire more
parent involvement might need to use different
strategies for the two subjects. In addition to spe-
cific approaches for helping with math, reading,
and homework, parents noted other activities
they believed would help their children succeed.

Parent involvement figures centrally in na-


tional goals for improving education (Na-
tional Education Goals Panel, 1994) and in
many current school reform models. Edu-
cators have proposed that increased parent
involvement can improve student achieve-
ment and, subsequently, the importance of
parent support has been emphasized in dis-
cussions about the achievement gap be-
The ElementarySchoolJournal tween economically disadvantaged and
Volume 104, Number 3
o middle-class children (e.g., Epstein, 1996a;
2004 by The University of Chicago. All rights reserved.
0013-5984/2004/10403-0002$05.00 Henderson & Berla, 1994; Hoover-Dempsey

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
198 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

& Sandler, 1997). Studies have shown that nity were less familiar with school curricu-
low-income parents value education as a lum, engaged less in teaching at home, and
route to economic and social mobility were less likely to attend school events. The
(Delgado-Gaitan, 1992; Goldenberg & Gal- lower-income parents explained that they
limore, 1995; Scott-Jones, 1995), but their ac- had less time and flexibility to meet in-
tual involvement often falls short of school volvement expectations. A few of these par-
expectations (Casanova, 1996; Fine, 1993; ents indicated that their responsibilities
Lareau & Shumar, 1996; Olsen & Fuller, were limited to meeting children's basic and
1998). For example, Linek, Rasinski, and social-emotional needs, such as providing
Harkins (1997) found that a majority of clothing, emotional support, and socializ-
teachers were dissatisfied with parent in- ing manners.
volvement, citing a general lack of interest Other work has supported these find-
or concern about school matters. Height- ings. In another ethnographic study (Lar-
ened attention to parent involvement in the eau, 1994), low-income parents reported a
public arena is indisputable (Department of lack of confidence about school matters and
Education, 1998, 2000; Epstein, Williams, & usually deferred to teachers' expertise.
Nesbitt, 2002; Helfand, 2000), yet teachers Based on a series of interviews with 12 low-
and administrators still believe that not all income parents of elementary school stu-
parents are sufficiently involved (e.g., dents, Lareau and Shumar (1996) character-
Baker, 1997; Eccles & Harold, 1996). ized these families as teacher supporters
Because the perception persists that who monitored and encouraged teacher-
there is a mismatch or even a clash between chosen educational activities rather than as
educator and parent values regarding par- teacher collaborators who might intervene
ent involvement, in this study we assessed in or suggest student activities themselves.
how much parents valued helping their These findings indicate a perception by
children learn. A group of diverse, low- some parents that their role should be dif-
income parents rated the importance of ferentiated from the role of teachers. Swap
helping their second- and third-grade chil- (1993) suggested that some parents might
dren in different subjects and answered believe they are most helpful when they
open-ended questions about the type of urge their children to be autonomous in do-
help they deemed appropriate. ing their schoolwork. Overall, this research
suggests variation in parents' beliefs about
Research on Involvement what parents can and should do to help
Low-Income Parents children succeed in school; this was the first,
The study focused on low-income par- broad area that we wanted to explore fur-
ents because their children achieve lower, ther. We assessed five additional factors in
on average, than students from other in- order to provide further guidance for efforts
come levels, and research on parent prac- to increase parent involvement. The ratio-
tices has indicated that low-income parents nale for including each of these factors is
are less involved. A few studies that have summarized below.
explored parents' beliefs have suggested
that low-income parents may resist direct Child's Grade
involvement. In an ethnographic study of Previous studies of parent practices
parent participation, Lareau (1987) com- have shown that parents of younger chil-
pared two first-grade classrooms, one in dren were more likely to be involved in
a middle-class neighborhood and one in educational activities than were parents of
a lower-income neighborhood. Although older children (Griffith, 1998; Stevenson &
teachers at both schools had similar expec- Baker, 1987). This led us to wonder whether
tations, parents in the low-income commu- parents of younger and older children hold

JANUARY 2004

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PARENTS'BELIEFS 199

different beliefs about the importance of in- Other studies have shown that Latino par-
volvement. ents distinguish themselves from parents of
Most studies that have illustrated age- other backgrounds by expressing concern
related differences in parent practices have about showing respect to authority figures
assessed a wide range of ages. For example, (e.g., Holloway, Rambaud, Fuller, & Eggers-
in a longitudinal study of elementary par- Pierola, 1995) and may not become in-
ents, Roeser, Patrick, Yoon, Anderman, and volved in certain ways for fear of showing
Eccles (1994) found that parents tended to disrespect to teachers. Thus, because re-
decrease some types of involvement, such search has suggested the possibility of eth-
as reading and practicing academic activi- nic differences in parents' beliefs about the
ties with their children at home, as their role of parents in children's learning, we ex-
children grew older. They increased other plored this possibility.
activities, such as volunteering at school.
Similarly, in a second- to twelfth-grade sam- Subject Matter
ple, Cooper, Lindsay, and Nye (2000) found Few previous studies have compared
that parents allowed their older children to parents' beliefs about their role in different
be more autonomous with homework. academic domains. In one cross-cultural
Studies that included only a two- or three- study, McGillicudy-DeLisi and Subraman-
grade span have not assessed age differ- ian (1994) asked mothers in the United
ences (e.g., Grolnick, Benjet, Kurowski, & States and Tanzania to rate the importance
Apostoleris, 1997; Okagaki, Frensch, & Gor- of the parent role and the teacher role in
don, 1995). In our study, if differences were teaching children in particular domains.
to be found in a narrow range of grades, we Both sets of mothers ranked the parent role
expected beliefs about being involved to be highest in teaching language, but American
stronger for parents of second graders. mothers placed more importance on teach-
ing language. Neither Tanzanian nor Amer-
Child's Ethnicity ican mothers rated their role in teaching
Some studies have suggested that cul- children math and science highly. The au-
tural norms may play a role in parent beliefs thors reasoned that parents in both coun-
about teaching and learning (reviewed in tries might have felt that the teacher's ex-
Garcia Coll, Meyer, & Brillon, 1995; Hi- pertise was necessary to teach the methods
dalgo, Siu, Bright, Swap, & Epstein, 1995). and technology required in the study of
Much of the empirical work has focused on math and science.
recent immigrant Latino families. For ex- Differences in parents' beliefs with re-
ample, Goldenberg and Gallimore (1995) gard to subject matter seemed important to
found that Latino parents defined educa- explore for three reasons. First, one study,
tion more broadly than many teachers did, which focused on practices, found that par-
extending education to areas of social and ents were more likely to report helping in
moral development. Similarly, Reese, Bal- the subjects of reading and English than in
zano, Gallimore, and Goldenberg (1995) other subjects (Epstein, 1996b). We thought
concluded that Latino parents emphasized that an understanding of parents' beliefs
moral learning, believing that it was the about helping with math versus reading
foundation for all other learning. Compared might provide more insight to this area. Sec-
to Mexican-American and Mexican immi- ond, general feelings of parent efficacy have
grant groups, Anglo-American parents been tied to parent involvement (Hoover-
have been found to rate cognitive traits in Dempsey, Bassler, & Brissie, 1992), and it
children as more important, and motiva- seemed likely that parents would feel more
tion, self-management, and social skills as efficacious in certain domains than others.
less important (Okagaki & Sternberg, 1993). We assumed that parents would feel more

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
200 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

competent in assisting their children with dren's achievement to assess the validity of
reading than with math because they are parents' perceptions.
less likely to use math in their daily lives.
Third, at any given time, certain subjects are Teacher Communication
emphasized more by teachers, schools, and Previous research has suggested that
in the public arena. Because reading is em- parents are more likely to become involved
phasized in schools as a foundational skill, when their child's teacher suggests that
we expected parents in the present study to they do so. In their model of parent involve-
rate parental involvement as more impor- ment, Eccles and Harold (1996) suggest that
tant in reading than in math. To explore teachers and parents reciprocally influence
possible subject-matter differences further, each other's beliefs regarding parents' role
we asked parents several questions about and involvement. Snow, Barnes, Chandler,
the assistance they had recently given their Goodman, and Hemphill (1991) found that
child with reading, math, and homework. teacher-initiated contact with parents was
associated with more parent-initiated com-
Child Achievement munication with school, as well as with im-
Despite broad support for parent in- proved student schoolwork and gains on
volvement among policy makers and edu- reading achievement tests. In an interven-
cators, empirical evidence on its effects on tion study (Ames, 1993), researchers trained
student achievement is inconsistent. Some teachers to communicate with parents fre-
studies have shown a positive association quently and to vary their communication
between parents' involvement at school and strategies throughout the year. Compared
children's achievement (Miedel & Rey- to parents of children in control classrooms,
nolds, 2000; Stevenson & Baker, 1987). Par- parents of children in the intervention class-
ent involvement may cause higher student rooms rated teachers as more effective,
achievement, or perhaps it is easier and rated their own influence on their children's
more pleasant to become involved in chil- learning higher, and reported higher in-
dren's learning when they are performing volvement.
well. Other studies have indicated that par- Accordingly, in the present study we
ent involvement is greater for children who asked teachers if they had given sugges-
are achieving poorly (Delgado-Gaitan, tions to parents in the domains of reading
1992; Grolnick, Ryan, & Deci, 1991; Steven- and math. We predicted that parents who
son, Chen, & Uttal, 1990), suggesting that received advice or suggestions about help-
parents may intervene more when their ing their children would rate the impor-
children are not doing well. tance of parent involvement higher than
In the current study we examined asso- would parents who did not receive such
ciations between parents' perceptions of suggestions.
their children's reading and math achieve-
ment and their ratings of the importance of Study Purpose
their own involvement in their children's The study was conducted to obtain infor-
academic work. Previous studies mostly mation about low-income parents' beliefs
used school indices of children's achieve- about their role that could help guide future
ment and measures of actual involvement efforts to increase or shape their participa-
practices. Using parents' perceptions rather tion. Specifically, our three objectives were
than a teacher rating of achievement allows to learn: (1) how much parents from diverse
a more direct test of the hypothesis that in- backgrounds value involvement in their
volvement is, at least in part, a reaction to children's schooling; (2) what factors are as-
the perception that a child needs help. We sociated with differences among parents
did, however, obtain teacher ratings of chil- in their beliefs about involvement; and

JANUARY 2004

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PARENTS'BELIEFS 201

(3) what parents say, in their own words, that they should not be involved, they were
they should be doing to help their children probed for the reasons. We assessed the fac-
succeed in school. tors that might relate to parents' overall be-
The study adds to parent involvement liefs about their role-their child's grade in
research by examining parents' beliefs school, family ethnicity, subject matter, chil-
about involvement and differentiating their dren's achievement, and teachers' requests
beliefs about involvement in different do- for parent assistance. Also of interest was
mains, using a rating scale and open-ended whether parents' recent involvement in
questions. The sample for this study was a their children's math, reading, and home-
group of low-income parents; many were work was related to their beliefs.
minorities, and some were new immigrants
and did not speak English. Previous re- Method
search has suggested that many teachers as- Participants
sume that low-income parents do not care All families in this study were involved
or do not want to be involved, an assump- in a longitudinal study of children from
tion we questioned. Most research on par- early childhood through elementary school.1
ents' beliefs about being involved in chil- The subsample for the current study in-
dren's education has been ethnographic cluded all the children who had just fin-
and based on a small number (5-30) of par- ished second (n = 80) or third grade (n =
ents (e.g., Lareau, 1987; Lareau & Shumar, 201) for whom both parent interviews (98%
1996). Many larger studies have examined completion rate) and teacher questionnaires
involvement practices, but parents' role (85% return rate) were complete. This re-
construction (e.g., Hoover-Dempsey & San- sulted in a sample of 234 children in second
dler, 1997) has not been studied as often. In (31%) or third (69%) grade. The children
addition, previous research has commonly were fairly evenly divided between boys
limited parents to rating aspects of involve- (52%) and girls (48%), and they were from
ment in a predetermined list of activities diverse ethnic groups (41% Caucasian, 36%
(e.g., Epstein, 1986; Griffith, 1998; Kohl, African American, 16% Latino, 2% Asian,
Lengua, & McMahon, 2000), or in activities and 1% Native American). For analyses on
that take place at school (e.g., Grolnick, Ben- ethnicity, we considered the first three pre-
jet, Kurowski, & Apostoleris, 1997; Steven- dominant groups.
son & Baker, 1987). Large-scale studies on The children lived in three geographical
parent beliefs have examined such issues areas: a rural community in the northeast, a
as child-rearing, educational aspirations, large urban northeastern city, and a large
achievement orientation, and conceptions urban west coast city. Participants were
of children's intelligence (Marjoribanks, originally recruited through a nationally
1995; Okagaki & Frensch, 1998; Wentzel, funded early-intervention program for low-
1998), but they have not studied parents' income families. At the time of the present
beliefs about their own role in their chil- study, family income was still low, with 70%
dren's education. of the families below an annual income of
In interviews, over 200 low-income, eth- $30,000. Specifically, 33% of the parents had
nically diverse parents of second- and third- incomes less than $15,000, 20% between
grade children were asked to rate how im- $15,000 and $20,000, and 15% between
portant they believed it was for them to $20,000 and $30,000. An additional 17%had
play a role in several domains of their incomes over $30,000, and 15% did not
child's learning. If they indicated that par- know or refused to answer the question.
ents did have a role in a particular domain, Educational levels of the parents were also
they were asked to describe what they be- low: 9% of the sample had finished less than
lieved parents should do; if they indicated ninth grade, 18% finished less than twelfth

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
202 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

grade, 31% completed high school but did study child's parent "special suggestions"
not attend college, and 36% had completed regarding helping with math or reading.
some college, including 2-year degree pro- In the spring the teachers of participat-
grams. ing students received a mailing with a letter
Students' skill levels varied substan- describing the study and requesting their
tially, although by national standards their participation and the questionnaire. Teach-
cognitive skills at school entry were low ers who agreed to participate answered the
(mean of 86.8 on the Peabody Picture Vo- questionnaire and mailed it back in late
cabulary Test at age 5 years). For the current spring. They received a modest stipend for
study, teachers rated their students as fol- each completed questionnaire.
lows for reading: below average (45%), av- Parent interviews. Parent interviews
erage (35%), above average (19%);for math: took place in the late spring and early sum-
below average (37%), average (46%), above mer, allowing parents to reflect on the entire
average (18%). Fifty-one children (22%) school year. The interviews were conducted
were receiving special education services. over the telephone at a time convenient for
Thirty (13%) did not speak English as their the parent. Mostly mothers (90%) were in-
primary language; of these, about 40% were terviewed. Other adult relatives were inter-
rated by their teachers as low or very low viewed only if they had primary care-
in English proficiency. taking responsibility for the child. For a few
The 234 child participants were distrib- parents who did not have a phone or for
uted across 165 classrooms in 103 schools. those who requested it, a face-to-face inter-
The modal number of participating children view was conducted in the home. All inter-
per classroom was one. Schools varied con- viewers were trained graduate students or
siderably in the demographics of their stu- professionals who had experience working
dent population, although they tended to with children and families. Native Spanish
serve more children from low-income fam-
speakers conducted and translated inter-
ilies than would be expected in a nationally views with Spanish-speaking parents.
representative sample. The proportion of Twelve percent of the interviews were con-
children qualifying for a free or reduced- ducted in Spanish; the others were con-
price lunch in the sample schools ranged ducted in English. The full interview took 1
from 0 to 100%,with a mean of 61%.Teacher hour, on average. The interviews were tape-
experience ranged from 1 to 37 years, with a recorded with phone recording devices and
mean of 16. transcribed. Families were paid a small sti-
pend for the interview.
Procedures The parent interview covered a range of
Teacher questionnaires. The question- topics, including demographic information
naire covered a range of topics, a few of and parents' impressions of their child. For
which were analyzed for this study. Teach- the present study, parents rated their child's
ers rated children's current reading and abilities in math and reading on a five-point
math skills relative to classroom peers and scale. To assess parents' beliefs about
predicted fifth-grade reading and math whether they should be involved in their
skills on a five-point scale, from "well be- children's academic learning, parents heard
low children this age" to "well above chil- the following description: "Parents have
dren this age." Ratings in the two domains different beliefs about what is important for
were highly correlated (for reading, r = .82, parents to do to help their children succeed
p < .001; for math, r = .96, p < .001), and in school. Some of the things that some par-
the average was used in analyses. Teachers ents think they should do, other parents
also reported whether they had given each might not think they should do. We aren't

JANUARY 2004

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PARENTS'BELIEFS 203

asking which of these you actually do, but specific categories and then combined
which you think are good to do." closely related categories.
Because the goal was to learn about par- Because negative parent beliefs (defi-
ents' values, questions were posed so par- nitely or probably should not) were uncom-
ents need not actually have engaged in an mon, only four categories were needed to
activity they were asked about. Rather, they describe all parent responses. For affirma-
were asked whether they thought they tive parent beliefs (definitely or probably
should be involved. Questions concerned should), we developed separate coding
four domains of involvement: homework in schemes for giving help in homework, read-
general, reading, math, and being informed ing, and math. Additional categories were
about what their child was learning. Parents created for the ways in which parents said
were also asked a fifth question about they should find out what their child is
whether they thought they should help learning and for other things parents said
their child learn other things related to they should do to help their child do well
school. in school.
To questions about involvement regard- For each parent interview, trained cod-
ing math, reading, homework, and know- ers marked every category in each domain
ing what their children are learning, parents as either present or not present. Parents
responded on a four-point scale ("definitely were therefore given credit for as many
should not," "probably should not," "prob- categories as they mentioned in response to
ably should," and "definitely should"). If any one question. Reliability, calculated as
parents answered negatively (definitely or the number of agreements divided by the
probably should not), they were asked, number of agreements plus number of dis-
"Why do you believe this?" If parents an- agreements, was assessed by having two
swered affirmatively (probably or definitely coders rate 24 interviews. Reliability for dif-
should), they answered the probe, "What ferent questions ranged from .85 to .96 with
do you believe you should do?" If parents a mean of .89.
answered affirmatively to the fifth question
about whether they believed they should Results: Factors Associated with
help their child learn other things related to Parent Involvement Ratings
school, they answered the probe, "What do Generally, parents rated involvement as
you believe you should do?" very important (see Table 1). A relatively
Parents were asked to think about the low alpha (.46) for the four specific areas
previous week and whether they had done (homework and projects, reading, math,
anything to teach their child in math and and knowing what the child is learning)
reading and whether they had read to their suggests that parents did not rate the im-
child or listened to him or her read. They portance of involvement the same in all ar-
were also asked how many times they had eas. Parents' explanations for the fourth
helped their child with homework in the broad, open category (help child learn other
past week. things) are provided in the specific activities
Coding of open-ended questions. Re- section below.
sponses from open-ended questions were
analyzed using grounded analysis, which Grade and Ethnicity
involves organizing categories based on A beliefs (4) x grade (2) MANOVA in-
ideas that emerge from data by intensively dicated a main effect of grade (0 = .96, F(1,
reviewing and sorting responses (Strauss, 223) = 2.50, p < .05). Univariate analysis
1987). First, an inventory of broad catego- revealed differences between grade levels
ries was created to encompass the answers for math (F(1, 227) = 5.50, MSE = 2.20,
parents gave. Next, we created a range of p < .05) and knowing what children are

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
204 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

TABLE1. Means and Standard Deviations of Parent Belief Ratings in Five Domains (N = 234)

Second Grade Third Grade Total Sample

Domain M SD M SD M SD

Help with reading 3.86 .42 3.71C .68 3.77 .61


Help with math 3.81a .43 3.60ac .70 3.67 .64
Help with homework and projects 3.89 .36 3.82 .45 3.84 .42
Know what child is learning 3.93b .26 3.81b .41 3.85 .37
Help child with other things 3.63 .66 3.52 .73 3.55 .71

abSame subscripts denote significant difference between grade.


cSame subscript denotes significant difference between subject.

learning (F(1, 227) = 4.97, MSE = .68, p < correlated with reported helping behaviors
.05). In both domains, parents of second for parents of second graders but not of
graders rated the importance of being in- third graders (see Table 2).
volved higher than parents of third graders
(see means in Table 1). A beliefs (4) x eth- Child Achievement
nicity (3) MANOVA showed that parents' Table 2 shows results of correlations be-
beliefs did not vary significantly by ethnic- tween parents' and teachers' perception of
ity. children's achievement, parents' beliefs
about how involved they should be, and
Subject Matter their reported practices related to reading
Paired t tests were used to compare par- and math. For both subjects, there was a
ents' ratings in math and reading. Because high association between teacher and par-
math ratings were differentiated by grade, ent ratings of child achievement. For read-
we conducted separate analyses for parents ing, correlations revealed that the lower
of second and third graders. Among second children's achievement ratings were, the
graders, ratings for math and reading were higher their parents rated the importance of
not significantly different (t = .94 (71), p = helping them learn to read. For math, cor-
.35). Among third graders, ratings for read- relations were conducted separately for
ing were significantly higher than math (t each grade; there was no strong association
= 2.16 (157), p < .05), as shown in Table 1. between teacher and parent achievement
On the questions about parents' prac- ratings and parents' rating the importance
tices, differences again emerged between of helping children in math. There was little
reading and math. A paired t test revealed relation between achievement and reported
that parents were more likely to report that practices for both reading and math.
they had taught their child about reading
than about math (t = 8.04 (229), p < .001). Teacher Communication
Parents were also more likely to report that To assess the association between
they had read or listened to their children teacher communication and parent beliefs
read than that they had taught them about about involvement, parents were divided
math (t = 2.62 (229), p < .05). Grade level into two groups, depending on whether the
was not associated with reported helping teacher responded "yes" or "no" to the
behaviors for reading or math. For reading, question about making special suggestions
there was a modest correlation between to the parent for helping the child with
parents' beliefs about the importance of math and or reading. Teachers made special
helping with reading and reported helping suggestions about helping for 96 children
the child read (see Table 2). For math, the (41%) in reading and for 83 children (36%)
belief about the importance of helping was in math. Analyses of variance were con-

JANUARY2004

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PARENTS'BELIEFS 205

TABLE2. Correlations among Achievement Ratings, Parent Belief Ratings, and Helping
Practices for Math and Reading

Grade

N 2 3 4 5

Math:
Second graders: 72
Achievement rating:
By teacher (1) .52*** -.02 -.12
By parent (2) -.12 -.03
Parent belief (3) .30*
Help child learn math (4)
Third graders: 162
Achievement rating:
By teacher (1) .58*** -.13 -.08
By parent (2) -.02 -.06
Parent belief (3) .04
Help child learn math (4)
Reading: 234
Achievement rating:
By teacher (1) .56*** -.21** -.07 -.04
By parent (2) -.18** -.01 0
Parent belief (3) .14* .05
Help child learn to read (4) .02
Read with child (5)

*p < .05.
**p < .01.
***p< .001.

ducted with teacher communication (yes or TABLE3. Means and Standard Deviations of Parent
Belief Rating by Whether or Not Teachers Gave
no) as the independent variable and par- Suggestions (N = 234)
ents' corresponding belief about the impor-
tance of their involvement as the dependent Suggestions
variable. Yes No
For reading, results indicated that par-
ents rated the importance of involvement Domain M SD M SD

higher when a teacher recommended that Reading 3.91a .28 3.63a .76
they help, F(1, 224) = 11.58, p < .01 (see Math:
Table 3 for means). For math, results re- Second grade 3.80 .48 3.82 .39
Third grade 3.81a .44 3.50a .80
vealed no significant main effect for teacher
recommendations for second graders, but aDenotes significant difference within row.
parents of third graders did rate the impor-
tance of involvement higher when a teacher
recommended that they help, F(1, 152) =
6.91, p < .01. Teacher suggestions to parents should not help their children, if that was
about helping were not associated with re- their response.
ported parent helping behaviors. Few parents claimed that they should
not help their child (homework, n = 3;
Beliefs about Specific Activities reading, n = 12; math, n = 13). When they
Recall that parents were asked a series made this claim, the most common expla-
of follow-up questions to find out what ac- nation was that they did not believe that
tivities they thought they should do to help their child needed help because he or she
their children, or why they thought they was already doing well (seven responses for

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
206 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

reading, 11 responses for math). Parents in for them to do to help their children succeed
this category gave responses such as, "She in school. The most common answers were
already receives an A," "He is good at that," again general ones that emphasized being
or "She's doing well." Eight parents re- present and providing support (e.g., be
ferred to their own inadequacy in providing there for child, give the child whatever he
help (one for homework, two for reading, needs, prepare ahead for the child, 19%).
and six for math), stating, for example, "My Teaching social skills (17%) and teaching
child knows more about reading than I do," another skill, such as piano, computers, or
"I do not know how to do his kind of math," time management (15%), were the second
"I did not learn this." Two parents thought and third most common answers. A fourth
that they should not help with homework category (communicate/participate with
on principle (e.g., "My child needs to do it child, 14%) stressed communication and in-
on his own"). One parent said she should teraction with their children. Answers in-
not help her child become a better reader cluded, "Listen to music together" and
because he did not initiate such assistance "Talk about age-related changes." In the
("He never asks for help"). fifth most common category (12%), parents
Parents' responses when they were suggested that the best way to help their
asked what type of help they thought they child would be to change something or
should give their children are outlined in someone in their environment. Suggestions
Tables 4 and 5. Some parents gave more in this category included, "Learn more for
than one answer per question. Parents who myself so I can help child" and "Move to
believed they should help often gave gen- better neighborhood."
eral responses to the follow-up question
about what they thought they should do
(e.g., "help her" or "guide him"), although Discussion
interviewers asked follow-up questions. In this study we investigated whether eco-
These general responses were given by 56% nomically disadvantaged parents from di-
of the parents for homework, 21% for read- verse ethnic backgrounds believe it is their
ing, and 35% for math (see Tables 4 and 5). responsibility to be involved in their chil-
For reading, many parents referred to hav- dren's schooling. We found that most par-
ing someone read to the child or listen to ents strongly value involvement in their
the child read (48%), or to engaging in children's learning. Means for questions re-
reading-related activities, such as word garding reading, math, helping with home-
games or phonics books (15%). For math, work, and knowing what their child was
answers most commonly related to giving learning were all close to the top ("defi-
the child more math practice through drill nitely should") of the four-point scale. This
sheets, problems, or quizzes (24%) or to is significant because, as Hoover-Dempsey
math-related activities and assistance, such and Sandler (1997) asserted, parents' beliefs
as math games or tutors (18%). about their involvement provide the drive
For the question about knowing what for their actual involvement practices. The
the child was learning, a majority of parents diverse contexts in which the parents
(55%) referred to obtaining the information lived-including rural and urban areas in
from teachers or by going to school. An ad- three states and 103 schools-suggest that
ditional 18% of parents suggested finding our findings have broad generalizability.
out directly from the child, and 16% sug- Because other studies have suggested
gested looking through their child's books that some parents' involvement falls short
or homework. of school expectations, there are likely ei-
Parents gave a range of answers for ther additional barriers for parents in help-
"other" things they thought were important ing their children learn or there is miscom-

JANUARY 2004

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PARENTS'BELIEFS 207

TABLE4. Examples, Frequency, and Percentage of Parents' Recommendations for Helping with Homework,
Knowing What Child Is Learning, and Other Ways to Help Child

Belief Category Example f %

Helping with homework:


Help/support Guide; help with unknown word 162 56
Supervise work Make sure work is neat 40 14
Explain technique/check for Show child way to do it; make sure child
understanding understands it 31 11
Check work/give feedback Look for right answers; point out weaknesses 32 11
Teacher involvement/go to school Talk to teacher about it 15 5
Motivate Get child to do her best 6 2
Parent development Learn how to do it so I can help child 3 1
Total 289 100

Knowing what child is learning:


School/teachers Ask teacher questions; go and find out 163 55
Asking child Ask child 54 18
Child Go through papers; keep an eye on homework 49 16
Conferences Attend conferences 17 6
General communication Ask; find out 14 5
Asking other siblings/parents Ask child's sister; ask neighbor 2 1
Total 299 100
Other ways to help:
Give help/support Be there; teach whatever child needs 44 19
Teach social skills Show how to get along; teach child to read people;
point out which adults will help 39 17
Teach specific skills Get piano lessons; involve in computers; help with
hygiene 35 15
Communicate/participate with child Work on relationship; play games together 32 14
Improve environment Get child to better school; move family to better
neighborhood 29 12
Communicate/participate with
school Go to school; attend field trips 19 8
Motivate Find ways for child to enjoy school; encourage to do
best 19 8
Warn about dangers Talk about drugs; warn about bad friends 8 3
Obtain material goods Buy him computer; get her new clothes 8 3
Total 233 99

NOTE.-Frequencies represent all the answers provided by parents who thought they should help children
with homework (N = 228), know what child is learning (N = 230), and do other things to help (N = 209). Some
parents gave more than one answer. The Other Ways to Help category does not add to 100% due to rounding.

munication about participation between Beliefs about Involvement


educators and parents. Although some Ethnicity. There were no significant dif-
schools and programs are dedicated to ex- ferences among groups from different eth-
ploring involvement pathways and com- nic backgrounds. Parents from all three eth-
munication between educators and parents nic groups, including Latino parents who
(e.g., Epstein et al., 2002), there is clearly had limited or no English proficiency (16%
more work to be done. Our results show of the sample), thought involvement in
that low-income parents strongly believe their children's learning was important.
they should facilitate their children's suc- This finding supports work suggesting that
cess in school. Findings discussed below diverse groups of parents value involve-
provide some further clues about parents' ment in their children's academic learning
beliefs and how schools might help parents and that suggestions to the contrary are in-
act on their beliefs. accurate or based on a singular concept of

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
00 C)
"llr-- r-I U')
C
4-j

(N O (N c,

4-j CIO
CI,O ;..o C~V, C
>11
c
-d -O N

C)75C)cA
>< 4-j m
-u-o -r

a) -I >
-U PU
lzt 10 00 0

-4-

u 4-j(
Pt ~ ~O~c,~
-Uc 4-j0 ~ ~ (

-d 4 0
4-j-

k C 3
-Z U d 4-jd
CIO 40j 0

5c ;- o e -d
(d~C
I -d -d-4
V ~
j
U- 0 ., ?cr

E~~~~
" 0~`~
S 4-j

cc -

E; cn~ca a )~

(d CIO4-q
O~s4-j4-jS
CIO
.,.
tco -
O CI

;I , ."=Z a) bc
4-j CIO a)b uL , O ~
(9 a) m 4-omm
-j -4-
a) 0 H P
,r~c, k-t C 4-j~'0
- d e-
U?U
(d
c, 4-j
0~";u b(9-(9" C- aI
?r

a)
0- S
CIO ;-4

4-j

u u 0 CIO

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PARENTS'BELIEFS 209

involvement (i.e., visiting the school, vol- cantly higher than the importance of help-
unteering in the classroom) (e.g., DeMoss & ing in math. Perhaps parents believe that
Vaughn, 2000; Moles, 1993). Research sug- reading is a more critical skill to develop in
gests there may be variation in how beliefs the early grades. Parents' emphasis on read-
are enacted among parents from different ing may also be related to the considerable
cultural backgrounds. Differences in meth- public attention currently paid to literacy.
odology among studies may be important. From local efforts, such as the Los Angeles
Past work on this topic has either been eth- Times's "Read by 9" campaign, to the na-
nographic or has compared different facets tional Reading First grants, public cam-
of parents' own ratings (help in academic paigns encourage parents to read to chil-
domains vs. help in social domains, or par- dren and to teach children to read. Math has
ents' ratings of their responsibility vs. teach- not received the same public attention.
ers' responsibilities). Also, the different nature of reading and
Grade and subject matter. Greater ef- math may be responsible. Much math work
forts on the part of educators may be re- leaves less allowance for spontaneous help
quired as children get older. Parents of third and requires more understanding of con-
graders in this study rate the importance of tent.
being involved lower than parents of sec- Achievement and teacher communica-
ond graders, at least with regard to being tion. Differentiation between math and
informed of what their child is learning and reading persists regarding achievement rat-
to helping with math. Studies of children ings and suggestions by teachers. Parents
with wider age ranges have shown declines who perceive their children's achievement
in parent involvement as children advance to be lower rate the importance of helping
in school (Eccles & Harold, 1996; Stevenson them higher for reading but not for math.
& Baker, 1987; Stevenson & Lee, 1990). Our Parents who feel their children are strug-
findings suggest that even a difference of gling with math may think they are not
one grade can affect parent perceptions qualified to meet their children's needs. Par-
about involvement and that teachers may ents may feel less qualified to help with
need to be more encouraging and explicit third- than with second-grade math but
about involving parents as children ad- equally qualified to help with reading in
vance through the grades. both grades. This seems plausible, given the
Why might parents believe they should low education level of many of the parents
be more involved when their children are in this study, and that math, by nature, re-
young? Perhaps they believe they need to quires knowledge of operations or equa-
monitor their children's schoolwork more tions in order to derive correct answers.
closely in the early grades, when children There are a few hints of feelings of in-
are still adjusting to school and beginning adequacy in some parents' responses to the
to develop study habits. Growing auton- open-ended questions. Six of the 11 parents
omy-promoted by teachers or desired by who claimed that they should not help their
children-may also contribute to the de- child in math said they feel restricted by
cline. Parents of second graders may feel their own inadequate knowledge; no parent
more welcome to give help, either by teach- said this about reading. For reading, many
ers or by their own children, than parents parents (48%) feel they are helping by sim-
of third graders. ply reading to their child or encouraging
The study also suggests that more ag- the child to read. Most math-related help
gressive efforts to involve parents of older involves working with the child, giving
children may be more important for math problems, explaining techniques, doing
than reading. Parents of third graders rate math activities, and the like. Finally, for
the importance of helping in reading signifi- math, 11 (4%) of the suggestions were to

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
210 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

contact the teacher or school to find out how with reading. Likewise, for parents of sec-
to help; only one parent referred to contact- ond graders, rating the importance of help-
ing the teacher with regard to helping with ing children with math higher was associ-
reading. Parents' feelings of competency in ated with reporting more helping behaviors
math versus reading warrant further con- in math. Overall, though, the relation was
sideration because previous research shows weak. Our questions about parent practices
that parents' sense of efficacy for helping were minimal, covered only a 1-week pe-
their children can affect involvement (Eccles riod, and perhaps were too general to show
& Harold, 1996; Hoover-Dempsey & San- a strong association. It could also be that
dler, 1997) and that parents who feel more these parents have difficulty turning their
efficacious may use more efficient helping beliefs into specific behaviors; in response
strategies (Hoover-Dempsey et al., 1992). to the questions about what they should be
In terms of teacher communication, doing, many parents' responses were vague
when teachers gave suggestions to parents despite interviewers' probes.
about helping children in reading, parents Parents reported a wide range of an-
rated the importance of giving help in read- swers when asked about "other things" that
ing higher. For math, this pattern held true they should do for their children, even
only for parents of third graders. This find- though the question was worded "related
ing suggests that parents of older children to school." As a whole, the answers form a
(whose beliefs about the importance of picture of parents who see school success in
helping in math are waning) may be par- a larger context of community and family
ticularly responsive to teacher suggestions. support and their child's general well-
Although we did not ask parents about the being. Parents refer to the importance of
nature of teachers' suggestions, it seems maintaining good relationships with their
practical for teachers to give parents specific children and making sure their children
and easy-to-follow strategies. In a parent have good relationships with others. Par-
survey, Epstein (1986) found that fewer than ents want to keep their children safe and
30% of parents reported that teachers ad- teach them new skills. Parents also express a
vised them on how to help their children in need to improve their living conditions and
reading and math; over 80% of parents said family life, suggesting, for example, that
they would do more if shown effective they should move to a better neighborhood
learning activities to conduct at home. For or spend more time with their children.
children who are achieving poorly, particu-
larly in math, parents may need more in- What Teachers Can Do
formation about how to help children and Our finding that over half of the parents
teachers and schools may need to be more claim that they should go to the school or
explicit and consistent. ask their child's teacher to find out what
their child is learning in school suggests
Parents' Reports of Involvement that most parents view the school as a via-
Practices ble source of information. Schools and
Additional support for the importance teachers need to remain committed to com-
of specific teacher suggestions or modeling municating with parents about children's
for math comes from parents' reports of re- learning. Teachers should ask parents what
cent practices and responses about things they are doing at home to help their chil-
they should be doing. In general, reports of dren academically and reinforce parents' in-
giving help were higher for reading than terest in helping their child. Newsletters
math. Rating the importance of helping sent home to communicate about topics be-
children with reading higher was associ- ing covered in class can include information
ated with reports of having helped the child about how parents can help children master

JANUARY 2004

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PARENTS'BELIEFS 211

material, practice skills, or relate new views. Because of our research method, we
knowledge to multiple contexts. Teachers could not trace whether parents' beliefs
can also give parents directions on how to about involvement changed over time
model strategies for children. Suggestions (from second to third grade). In addition,
for how to help with homework could be this work only begins to differentiate par-
useful, for example, in teaching parents the ents' beliefs about involvement across sub-
difference between assisting their child in ject matter and grade level. The study did
completing work and telling their child the not involve groups from different income
answers. Teachers can send materials home, levels, so it is unknown whether variables
such as math games, so that parents can found to be significant with low-income
play with their children. parents, such as subject matter or grade
Back-to-school nights or home visits can level, would be significant in middle- or
involve subject matter, and teachers can do
high-income samples. Finally, the study
more than simply report on what children
mainly addressed parents' beliefs, so reports
are doing in school. Parents can be given an from parents on practices were minimal.
opportunity to engage in the kind of work Future research should strike a balance
their children are doing to better under- between sample scope and size and depth
stand the skills children are developing and in exploring parents' involvement. Re-
the nature of school tasks. More individu- searchers need to examine parents' beliefs
alized instructions for helping students can in greater depth and look further at how
be given at parent-teacher conferences.
parents can better translate their beliefs to
Teachers can suggest enrichment activities
for high achievers and ways parents can practices. The findings from this study, that
very low-income parents believe they
help address problems if their children are should be involved in their children's learn-
having difficulties. Book clubs and math ing, suggest that parents will be receptive to
clubs that involve children and parents can
efforts to help them translate their beliefs
enable school personnel to encourage and
into practice.
model practices that parents can use in the
home and at club meetings. Teachers can in-
form parents about neighborhood re-
sources-for example, grandparents who Notes
provide after-school care or community
centers that run homework clubs. Correspondenceregardingthis articleshould
We also suggest asking parents what be directed to KathrynDrummond,4114 Davis
they need to know to be involved more ac- P1. NW #108, Washington, DC 20007. E-mail:
kscudder@ucla.edu
tively in their children's learning. Although 1. The principal investigators of the larger
more research on the barriers to involve- School Transition study include Deborah Stipek,
ment would be useful, there is likely local Penny Hauser-Cram, Walter Secada, Heather
variation in the significance of barriers. Weiss, and Jennifer Greene. Data collection for
Transportation to the school and neighbor- the present study was funded by a grant from
hood safety might be important factors in the MacArthur Foundation.
one community, whereas language and cul-
tural differences may play a more important
role in another. Schools need to know what References
inhibits parent involvement in their own lo-
cal context.
Ames, C. (1993). How school-to-home commu-
nications influence parent beliefs and per-
Study Limitations and Future Research
ceptions. Equity and Choice,9, 44-49.
The study focused on parent beliefs and Baker, A. (1997). Improving parent involvement
was based primarily on telephone inter- programs and practices: A qualitative study

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
212 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

of teacher perceptions. School Community Bornstein (Ed.), Handbookof parenting (Vol. 2,


Journal,7, 27-55. pp. 189-209). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Casanova, U. (1996). Parent involvement: A call Goldenberg, C., & Gallimore, R. (1995). Immi-
for prudence. EducationalResearcher,25, 30- grant Latino parents' values and beliefs
32. about their children's education: Continui-
Cooper, H., Lindsay, J. J., & Nye, B. (2000). ties and discontinuities across cultures and
Homework in the home: How student, fam- generations. In M. Maehr & P. Pintrich (Eds.),
ily, and parenting-style differences relate to Advances in motivationand achievement(Vol. 9,
the homework process. ContemporaryEduca- pp. 183-228). Greenwich, CT: JAI.
tional Psychology,25, 464-487. Griffith, J. (1998). The relation of school structure
Delgado-Gaitan, C. (1992). School matters in the and social environment to parent involve-
Mexican-American home: Socializing chil- ment in elementary schools. Elementary
dren to education. American EducationalRe- School Journal,99, 53-89.
searchJournal,29, 495-513. Grolnick, W., Benjet, C., Kurowski, C., & Apos-
DeMoss, S., & Vaughn, C. (2000). Reflections on toleris, N. (1997). Predictors of parent in-
theory and practice in parent involvement volvement in children's schooling. Journalof
from a phenomenographical perspective. EducationalPsychology, 89, 538-548.
School CommunityJournal,10, 45-59. Grolnick, W., Ryan, R., & Deci, E. (1991). Inner
Department of Education: Partnership for Fam- resources for school achievement: Motiva-
ily Involvement in Education. (1998). Part- tional mediators of children's perceptions of
ners for learning: Preparing teachersto involve their parents. Journal of EducationalPsychol-
families. Teacherand administratorpublication ogy, 83, 508-517.
kit [CD-ROM]. Washington, DC: U.S. Gov- Helfand, D. (2000, May 28). Parent teachers. Los
ernment Printing Office. Angeles Times,pp. B1, B5.
Department of Education: Partnership for Fam- Henderson, A., & Berla, N. (1994). A new gener-
ation of evidence:Thefamily is critical to student
ily Involvement in Education. (2000). Part-
ners for family involvement in education [CD- achievement. Columbia, MD: National Com-
ROM]. Washington, DC: U.S. Government mittee for Citizens in Education.
Printing Office. Hidalgo, N., Siu, S., Bright, J., Swap, S., & Ep-
Eccles, J., & Harold, R. D. (1996). Family in- stein, J. (1995). Research on families, schools,
volvement in children's and adolescents' and communities: A multicultural perspec-
tive. In J. Banks & C. Banks (Eds.), Handbook
schooling. In A. Booth & J. F. Dunn (Eds.),
Family-school links (pp. 3-34). Mahwah, NJ: of researchon multiculturaleducation(pp. 428-
Erlbaum. 524). New York: Macmillan.
Epstein, J. (1986). Parents' reactions to teacher Holloway, S., Rambaud, M., Fuller, B., & Eggers-
Pierola, C. (1995). What is "appropriate prac-
practices of parent involvement. Elementary tice" at home and in child care? Low-income
SchoolJournal,86, 277-294.
mothers' views on preparing their children
Epstein, J. (1996a). Advances in family, commu-
for school. Early ChildhoodResearchQuarterly,
nity, and school partnerships. New Schools, 10, 451-473.
New Communities,12, 5-13.
Hoover-Dempsey, K., Bassler, O., & Brissie, J.
Epstein, J. (1996b). Perspectives and previews on (1992). Exploration in parent-school rela-
research and policy for school, family, and tions. Journalof EducationalResearch,85, 287-
community partnerships. In A. Booth & J. 294.
Dunn (Eds.), Family-school links (pp. 209-
Hoover-Dempsey, K., & Sandler, H. (1997). Why
246). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. do parents become involved in their chil-
Epstein, J. L., Williams, K. J., & Nesbitt, M. G. dren's education? Review of Educational Re-
(2002, April). Five-year-study: State and dis- search,67, 1-42.
trict leadership in developing programs of Kohl, G., Lengua, L., & McMahon, R. (2000). Par-
partnership. In O. Moles (Chair), Five-year- ent involvement in school: Conceptualizing
study: School, family, and community part- multiple dimensions and their relations with
nerships in states, districts, and schools. family and demographic risk factors. Journal
Symposium conducted at the annual meet- of School Psychology,38, 501-523.
ing of the American Educational Research Lareau, A. (1987). Social class differences in
Association, New Orleans. family-school relationships: The importance
Fine, M. (1993). [Ap]parent involvement. Equity of cultural capital. Sociology of Education,60,
and Choice,9, 4-8. 73-85.
Garcia Coll, C., Meyer, E., & Brillon, L. (1995). Lareau, A. (1994). Parent involvement in school-
Ethnic and minority parenting. In M. H. ing: A dissenting view. In C. Fagnano & B.

JANUARY2004

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PARENTS'BELIEFS 213

Z. Werber (Eds.), School, family, and commu- families. In M. L. Fuller & G. Olsen (Eds.),
nity interaction:A view from thefiring lines (pp. Home-school relations: Working successfully
61-73). Boulder, CO: Westview. with parents and families (pp. 1-10). Boston:
Lareau, A., & Shumar, W. (1996). The problem of Allyn & Bacon.
individualism in family-school policies. So- Reese, L., Balzano, S., Gallimore, R., & Golden-
ciology of Education [Special issue], 24-39. berg, C. (1995). The concept of educacion:La-
Linek, W., Rasinski, T., & Harkins, D. (1997). tino family values and American schooling.
Teacher perceptions of parent involvement in International Journal of Educational Research,
literacy education. Reading Horizons, 38, 90- 23, 57-81.
107. Roeser, R., Patrick, H., Yoon, K., Anderman, E.,
Marjoribanks, K. (1995). Parents' involvement in & Eccles, J. (1994, April). A longitudinalstudy
learning as an opportunity structure: A of patternsof parentinvolvementin schoolacross
model for evaluation. Studies in Educational the elementaryyears: Teacherand parent reports.
Evaluation,21, 73-83. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Educational Research Association,
McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A., & Subramanian, S.
(1994). Tanzanian and United States mothers' San Francisco.
beliefs about parents' and teachers' roles in Scott-Jones, D. (1995). Parent-child interactions
children's knowledge acquisition. Interna- and school achievement. In B. Ryan & L.
tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 17, Hampton (Eds.), Thefamily-schoolconnection:
209-237. Theory, research, and practice (pp. 75-107).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Miedel, W., & Reynolds, A. (2000). Parent in-
volvement in early intervention for disad- Snow, C., Barnes, W., Chandler, J., Goodman, I.,
& Hemphill, L. (1991). Unfulfilledexpectations:
vantaged children: Does it matter? Journalof Home and school influences on literacy. Cam-
School Psychology, 37, 379-402.
Moles, O. C. (1993). Collaboration between bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
schools and disadvantaged parents: Obsta- Stevenson, D., & Baker, D. (1987). The family-
school relation and the child's school perfor-
cles and openings. In N. R. Chavkin (Ed.),
mance. Child Development,58, 1348-1357.
Families and schools in a pluralistic society (pp. Stevenson H., Chen, C., & Uttal, D. (1990). Beliefs
21-49). Albany, NY: SUNY Press. and achievement: A study of Black, White,
National Educational Goals Panel. (1994). Na- and Hispanic children. Child Development,62,
tional education goals report:Building a nation 508-523.
of learners. Washington, DC: U.S. Govern- Stevenson, H., & Lee, S. (1990). Contexts of
ment Printing Office. achievement. Monographsof the Societyfor Re-
Okagaki, L., & Frensch, P. (1998). Parenting and searchin ChildDevelopment,55(1-2, Serial No.
children's school achievement: A multiethnic 221).
perspective. American Educational Research Strauss, A. L. (1987). Qualitativeanalysis for social
Journal,35, 123-144. scientists. New York: Cambridge University
Okagaki, L., Frensch, P., & Gordon, E. (1995). En- Press.
couraging school achievement in Mexican- Swap, S. (1993). Developing home-schoolpartner-
American children. Hispanic Journalof Behav- ships: From concepts to practice. New York:
ioral Sciences, 17, 160-179. Teachers College Press.
Okagaki, L., & Sternberg, R. (1993). Parental be- Wentzel, K. (1998). Parents' aspirations for chil-
liefs and children's school performance. dren's educational attainments: Relations to
Child Development,64, 36-56. parental beliefs and social address variables.
Olsen, G., & Fuller, M. (1998). An introduction to Merrill-PalmerQuarterly,44, 20-37.

This content downloaded from 14.1.193.93 on Thu, 6 Mar 2014 07:04:18 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like