You are on page 1of 5

ARTICLE IN PRESS

THE JOURNAL OF PEDIATRICS www.jpeds.com ORIGINAL


ARTICLES
Time Perception during Neonatal Resuscitation
Daniele Trevisanuto, MD1, Giuseppe De Bernardo, MD2, Giulia Res, MD1, Desiree Sordino, MD2, Nicoletta Doglioni, MD1,
Gary Weiner, MD3, and Francesco Cavallin, MS4

Objective To assess the accuracy of time perception during a simulated complex neonatal resuscitation.
Study design Participants in 5 neonatal resuscitation program courses were directly involved in a complex simu-
lation scenario. They were asked to assume the role of team leader, assistant 1, or assistant 2. At the end of the
scenario, each participant completed a questionnaire on perceived time intervals for key resuscitation interven-
tions. During the scenario, actual times were documented by an external observer and video recorded for later
review. In addition, participants were asked to evaluate their self-perceived level of stress and preparation.
Results Health care providers (68 physicians and 40 nurses) were involved in 36 scenarios. Perceived time in-
tervals for the initiation of key resuscitation interventions were shorter than the actual time intervals, regardless of
the participants role in the scenario. Self-assessed levels of stress and preparation did not influence time perception.
Conclusions Health care providers underestimate the passage of time, irrespective of their role in a simulated
complex neonatal resuscitation. Participants self-assessed levels of stress and preparation were not related to the
accuracy of their time perception. These findings highlight the importance of assigning a dedicated individual to
document interventions and the passage of time during a neonatal resuscitation. (J Pediatr 2016;:-).

D
uring neonatal resuscitation, specific time intervals for each intervention are recommended in the neonatal resuscita-
tion program (NRP) and European Resuscitation Council algorithms.1,2 In addition, time intervals are used to guide
the duration of resuscitative efforts and eligibility for therapies such as postresuscitation therapeutic hypothermia. However,
the accuracy of time perception by health care providers during neonatal resuscitation is not known. Studies in cognitive psy-
chology demonstrate that the accuracy of time perception varies depending on several factors such as the complexity of the
event, emotional status, stress, personality, previous experience, and evaluation methods.3-6
Although accurate perception of time is important for responders to medical emergencies, previous studies have shown that
clinicians experience time distortion. Emergency medical technicians in the field and responders to simulated in-hospital adult
cardiac arrest events underestimate the passage of time in some situations and overestimate time in others.7-10 As a result, the
initiation of interventions and the accuracy of documentation reported by the health care providers at the end of an acute event
could be affected. Neonatal resuscitation is unique because most often there is a well-defined start time and an electronic timer
clearly visible; however, the time perceived by health care providers in a neonatal resuscitation setting is unknown.
The aim of the present study was to investigate the time perceived by the health care providers during a simulated complex
neonatal resuscitation in relation to the participants role.

Methods
This prospective observational study was conducted at 5 Italian hospitals. The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Com-
mittee of Azienda Ospedaliera, University of Padova, Padova, Italy.
Consent to record the scenario and to use the data was obtained by all participants. A 2-day NRP course was conducted in
5 Italian hospitals by a team of 6 instructors from February to November 2014. The course consisted of didactic sessions fol-
lowed by hands-on skill stations and practice scenarios. Participants included physicians and nurses who were routinely in-
volved in the care of newborns in the delivery room. At the end of the course, participants were involved in a high-fidelity simulation
using a neonatal simulator (SimNewB, Laerdal, Stavanger, Norway). The scenario consisted of an asphyxiated term infant needing
a complex resuscitation including positive pressure ventilation, endotracheal in-
tubation (ETT), chest compressions, and emergency medications. Heart rate, re-
spiratory rate, and breath sounds were controlled remotely and could be assessed
by auscultation of the thorax, observation of chest movements, and From the 1Department of Womens and Childrens Health,
Maternal-Fetal Medicine Unit, University of Padua School
of Medicine, Padua, Italy; 2AOP Santobono Pausilipon,
Napoli, Italy; 3Department of Pediatrics, Neonatal-
Perinatal Medicine, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI;
and 4Independent Statistician, Padova, Italy
A1 Assistant 1
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
A2 Assistant 2
ETT Endotracheal intubation 0022-3476/$ - see front matter. 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights
NRP Neonatal resuscitation program reserved.
http://dx.doi.org10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.07.003

FLA 5.4.0 DTD YMPD8513_proof August 4, 2016


THE JOURNAL OF PEDIATRICS www.jpeds.com Volume

palpation of the umbilical cord. The vital signs that are typi- Participants were informed only at the end of the scenario
cally available in the delivery room (heart rate and hemoglo- that they were required to estimate the time (retrospective
bin oxygen saturation via pulse oximetry [SpO2]) were displayed timing).
on the bedside monitor about 40 seconds after the position- The primary outcome of the study was the difference
ing of the oximeter probe on the right hand of the manikin. between the actual time and perceived time according to the
The SpO2 was not shown on the monitor when the heart rate participants role in the scenario (team leader, A1, A2). In ad-
was less than 60 beats per minute. The external observer pro- dition, we assessed the relationships between self-perceived stress
vided verbal feedbacks during the scenario only if specifi- and preparation with time perception.
cally required by the team and not provided by the manikin Statistical analysis was performed using R 2.12 software (R
(ie, the presence of secretions). A bedside Apgar timer ringing Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).11 Given
at 1, 5, and 10 minutes was available for the team. the lack of information about the time perceived by health care
Participants were divided into groups of 3 and were asked providers during neonatal resuscitation, the sample size could
to assume the roles of team leader responsible for coordinat- not be estimated using mathematical methods. Therefore, all
ing the team and managing the airway, assistant 1 (A1) re- the participants in the NRP courses were included in the study
sponsible for chest compressions, and assistant 2 (A2) sample. Time data were expressed as median and IQR and rep-
responsible for umbilical catheter insertion and medication ad- resented the difference (seconds) between perceived time and
ministration. All the other tasks, such as time recording during actual time measured by the observer. These time differences
the scenario, were left to the decision of the team. During each were compared using the Wilcoxon signed rank test. Self-
simulation, an external observer documented the actual time perceived stress level and preparation were expressed as a
of each intervention and the duration of the entire scenario median score (IQR) and were compared among the 3 roles
for calculation of accurate time intervals. All scenarios were using the Kruskal-Wallis test. The correlation between stress
video-recorded, stored, and reviewed by the same observer to level and preparation was evaluated using the Spearman rank
confirm the documented intervention times. We chose this ap- correlation coefficient.
proach to have a double evaluation of time intervals; however, Two multiple regression models were performed to iden-
video tape was used as the gold standard method of ascer- tify the effects of role, stress level, and preparation on the per-
taining timing. ception of time from birth to the first spontaneous breath and
At the end of the scenario, each participant completed a the duration of the entire scenario. A P value of less than .05
13-item questionnaire. Participants were asked to estimate the was considered statistically significant.
time elapsed from birth to the following events: beginning
positive pressure ventilation, ETT, beginning chest compres- Results
sions, administration of the first dose of adrenaline, first spon-
taneous breath, and duration of the entire scenario. The Health care providers (68 physicians and 40 nurses) attended
questionnaire did not include the assignment of the Apgar the courses and were involved in a total of 36 complex sce-
score. Finally, each participant was asked to describe how stress- narios. The role of team leader was most frequently assumed
ful he/she found the scenario by using the following Likert by physicians (97.2%), and the roles of A1 and A2 were dis-
scale: 0 = not stressful; 1 = mildly stressful; 2 = moderately stress- tributed between physicians (A1: 55.6%; A2: 36.1%) and nurses
ful; and 3 = very stressful. A similar scale was used to measure (A1: 44.4%; A2: 63.9%) (P < .001).
each participants assessment of their preparation for the re- Study participants perceived that the time interval between
suscitation scenario: 0 = very well prepared; 1 = moderately birth and the initiation of key resuscitation interventions was
well prepared; 2 = unprepared; and 3 = very unprepared. significantly shorter than the actual time interval (Table

Table. Difference between perceived time and actual time in relation to the participants role (team leader, A1, A2) in
the scenario
Difference Difference Difference
Team leader-EO A1-EO A2-EO Actual time
Beginning PPV 13 (40 to 5)* 6 (44 to 5) 25 (43 to 0)* 57 (43-70)
Insertion of endotracheal tube 81 (226 to 9.5) 239 (420 to 90) 136 (340 to 54) 246 (174-451)
Beginning chest compressions 65 (119 to 15) 45 (87 to 0) 50 (90 to 0) 128 (100-211)
Administration first dose of adrenaline 93 (160 to 30) 109 (215 to 55) 96 (199 to 41) 304 (243-395)
Duration of chest compressions 25 (86 to 70) 15 (76 to 48) 30 (88 to 100) 148 (108-215)
First spontaneous breath 60 (135 to 0) 60 (170 to 0) 60 (300 to 0) 600 (360-600)
Duration of the entire scenario 60 (120 to 5) 60 (174 to 20) 38 (120 to 0) 600 (447-630)

EO, external observer; PPV, positive pressure ventilation.


Data (seconds) expressed as median (IQR).
*P < .05.
P < .01.
P < .001.

2 Trevisanuto et al

FLA 5.4.0 DTD YMPD8513_proof August 4, 2016


2016 ORIGINAL ARTICLES

Figure. Difference between A, perceived time and B, actual time in relation to the participants role (TL, A1, A2) in the sce-
nario. Data are expressed as medians (IQR). *P < .05; P < .01. PPV, positive pressure ventilation; TL, team leader.

and Figure, A) regardless of the participants role in the sce-


Discussion
nario. Participants also underestimated the time of the first
spontaneous breath and the duration of the whole scenario. In this study, we assessed time perception by the health care
There was no significant difference between the perceived and providers during a simulated complex neonatal resuscitation
actual duration of chest compressions (Table and Figure, B). in relation to the participants role. Our results showed that
Participants needed a median of 2 (IQR 1-3) attempts for suc- participants consistently underestimated the time interval from
cessful intubation. The median duration of ETT was 22 (IQR birth until key resuscitation interventions were performed, and
15-40) seconds. their time perception was not influenced by their role in the
The self-assessed stress level was significantly greater scenario.
(P < .001) among the team leaders (team leader median 3; IQR During medical emergencies, time plays a crucial role. Pub-
2-3) compared with either assistant (A1 median 2; IQR 1.5- lished guidelines for cardiopulmonary resuscitation recom-
3, A2 median 2; IQR 2-2); however, the level of self-assessed mend specific time intervals for interventions, duration of
preparation was not significantly different (P = .80) (team leader intubation attempts, and total duration of resuscitative efforts.1,2
median 2; IQR 2-2, A1 median 2; IQR 2-2, A2 median 2; IQR An altered perception of time might, therefore, affect the
2-2). There was no statistically significant association between efficacy of interventions, the patients outcome, and the ac-
self-perceived levels of stress and self-perceived preparation curacy of clinical documentation. Furthermore, inaccurate
among either team leaders or assistants (team leader rho -0.23; documentation caused by distorted time perception may alter
P = .18, A1 rho 0.17; P = .31, A2 rho 0.01; P = .99). medical decisions concerning postresuscitation care, such as
Using multivariable analyses, perception of time elapsed from eligibility for therapeutic hypothermia, and have medical-
birth to the first spontaneous breath was not associated with legal consequences. For example, in infants with an Apgar score
role (P = .72), self-perceived stress level (P = .34), or self- of 0 after 10 minutes of resuscitation, if the heart rate remains
perceived preparation (P = .82). Similarly, perception of the du- undetectable, it may be reasonable to stop assisted ventilation.2
ration of the whole scenario was not associated with role However, it remains to be established if these timing errors
(P = .77), self-perceived stress level (P = .71), or self-perceived could have serious consequences to the infant or if they are
preparation (P = .14). merely recall biases. Studies using aids, such as voice prompts
Time Perception during Neonatal Resuscitation 3

FLA 5.4.0 DTD YMPD8513_proof August 4, 2016


THE JOURNAL OF PEDIATRICS www.jpeds.com Volume

and monitor alarms, suggest that these will improve timeli- suscitation. Overall, we found an underestimation of time
ness and intervention in emergency situations.12 A recent study elapsed from birth to the initiation of all resuscitative inter-
showed that health care professionals using a decision support ventions. In addition, our data show that time perception was
tool exhibit significantly fewer deviations from the NRP al- comparable between participants assuming the role of team
gorithm compared with those working from memory alone leader or assistant, suggesting that the level of responsibility
during simulated neonatal resuscitation,13 but implications of plays a limited role in time perception during neonatal resus-
such recall on the real patient remain to be evaluated. citation. Importantly, distorted time perception may be a factor
Our results suggest that, in addition to the 3 persons re- explaining the frequent inaccuracies found in the medical
quired for a complex resuscitation, such as described in this records documenting neonatal resuscitations.17 Based on pre-
study, a further member of the team is required for accurate vious experimental psychology studies showing the influ-
timing. For those units that are less well-resourced, it could ence of stress and experience on the perception of the time,3,5,16
be a problem because there may not be sufficient personnel we evaluated the participants self- perceived levels of stress
to perform the recording role in addition to the resuscitation and preparation using a Likert scale. We found that the team
interventions. We found that times recorded by the external leader reported a higher level of stress than either of the as-
observer and those captured by the video were nearly identi- sistants, even though the self-perceived preparation was similar
cal, suggesting that a member of the team exclusively dedi- between the 2 roles. Neither stress level, nor self-perceived
cated to the time documentation is very accurate. On the other preparation, were associated with the perception of time elapsed
hand, our results suggest that video recording should be a part from birth to the first spontaneous breath or the time of the
of training simulations during NRP to support the review and entire scenario.
discussion of the team performance during the debriefing phase. Participants were not informed before the experiment that
We believe that the use of video during actual resuscitations they would have to estimate time after the scenario. This choice
is also important for evaluation of neonatal resuscitation per- could have influenced the results because prospective and ret-
formance of health care providers.14,15 rospective time estimation involves different mechanisms. In
Emotional state and stress have been shown to cause situations in which participants are asked to judge time ex-
distorted time perception. Cognitive psychology studies in- plicitly, cognitive psychologists make a distinction between 2
volving adult volunteers have shown that participants tend paradigms: one in which participants are informed before they
to overestimate the perceived time duration when exposed perform the task that they are required to make a time-
to high-arousal conditions and negative emotional states.16 related judgment (prospective timing), and the other in which
These situations seem to fit medical emergencies, supporting they receive no prior warning (retrospective timing). Retro-
the hypothesis that medical emergencies will seem to last spective timing is mainly associated with memory processes,
longer than they actually do.2,3,16 Small studies evaluating but the structure of events is a critical determinant of remem-
health care providers time perception during adult emergen- bered duration.6
cies, however, have shown conflicting results.8-10 In a simulation The strength of the present study consists of the measure-
study, Eisen et al10 showed that internal medicine residents ment of times perceived by a large group of health care
overestimated the total duration of an adult cardiac arrest providers involved in a scenario of advanced neonatal resus-
scenario by an average of 42.6 seconds; however, the sub- citation. Some limitations should be considered when
group of first year residents tended to underestimate the interpreting the results. The study was undertaken immedi-
duration. This variation in the experience of time suggests ately after participation in an NRP course, which could have
that previous experience, preparation, stress, or the level of influenced the time perception of participants. The subjects
responsibility during the event may effect time perception. were studied during simulated neonatal resuscitation sce-
Hosbond et al8 showed an overall underestimation of the narios, and these findings should be replicated during actual
elapsed time (median 22.5 seconds shorter) during a simu- resuscitations. In addition, the sample size could not be
lated cardiac arrest when the participants were queried during estimated using mathematical methods because of the lack
the event but an overestimation of the total elapsed time of information about the topic. For this reason we used a
(median 36.5 seconds longer) when participants were queried convenience sample.
after the event was completed. In another clinical study, an Our results show that providers underestimate the time in-
independent observer was placed with paramedic teams to terval of key interventions irrespective of their role in the re-
determine the paramedics perception of elapsed field time. suscitation scenario. The participants level of stress and
Team-member perception of elapsed time for the entire run preparation did not affect their time perception. Considered
varied from the actual time by an average absolute value of together, studies assessing the perception of time during medical
20%, with the greatest errors occurring in their estimates of emergencies show conflicting results and suggest that more re-
on scene time. Paramedics tended to overestimate elapsed search is needed to identify factors associated with time per-
time during short runs and underestimate elapsed time ception and quantify the role of time distortion. Furthermore,
during long runs.9 these findings highlight the importance of assigning a team
As the accuracy of time perceived by health care providers member to document interventions and the passage of time
involved in the context of neonatal resuscitation is unknown, during a neonatal resuscitation to ensure accurate documen-
we aimed to measure it in a scenario of advanced neonatal re- tation and decision making.
4 Trevisanuto et al

FLA 5.4.0 DTD YMPD8513_proof August 4, 2016


2016 ORIGINAL ARTICLES

8. Hosbond S, Folkestad L, Brabrand M. Does cardio-pulmonary resusci-


Submitted for publication Jan 7, 2016; last revision received Jun 3, 2016; tation influence perception of time? Scand J Trauma Resusc Emerg Med
accepted Jul 5, 2016
2009;17(Suppl 2):P8.
Reprint requests: Daniele Trevisanuto, MD, Department of Womens and 9. Jurkovich GJ, Campbell D, Padrta J, Luterman A. Paramedic perception
Childrens Health Medical School, University of Padua, Azienda Ospedaliera of elapsed field time. J Trauma 1987;27:892-7.
Padova Via Giustiniani, 3, 35128 Padua, Italy. E-mail: trevo@pediatria.unipd.it
10. Eisen L, Pellechia C, Mayo P. Assessment of residents time perception
of in-hospital cardiopulmonary resuscitation (IHCPR) scenarios using
a computerized patient simulator (CPS). Chest 2007;132:446S.
11. R Development Core Team. R: A language and environment for statis-
References tical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Comput-
1. American Heart Association. American Academy of Pediatrics. In: Neo- ing; 2011 http://www.R-project.org/. Accessed July 28, 2016.
natal resuscitation textbook. 6th ed. Dallas (TX): American Academy of 12. Field LC, McEvoy MD, Smalley JC, Clark CA, McEvoy MB, Rieke H, et al.
Pediatrics and American Heart Association; 2011. Use of an electronic decision support tool improves management of simu-
2. Wyllie J, Bruinenberg J, Roehr CC, Rdiger M, Trevisanuto D, Urlesberger lated in- hospital cardiac arrest. Resuscitation 2014;85:138-42.
B. European resuscitation council guidelines for resuscitation 2015: section 13. Fuerch JH, Yamada NK, Coelho PR, Lee HC, Halamek LP. Impact of a
7. Resuscitation and support of transition of babies at birth. Resuscita- novel decision support tool on adherence to Neonatal Resuscitation
tion 2015;95:249-63. Program algorithm. Resuscitation 2015;88:52-6.
3. Angrilli A, Cherubini P, Pavese A, Mantredini S. The influence of affec- 14. ODonnell CP, Kamlin CO, Davis PG, Morley CJ. Ethical and legal aspects
tive factors on time perception. Percept Psychophys 1997;59:972- of video recording neonatal resuscitation. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neona-
82. tal Ed 2008;93:F82-4.
4. Loftus EF, Schooler JW, Boone SM, Kline D. Time went by so slowly: over- 15. Trevisanuto D, Bertuola F, Lanzoni P, Cavallin F, Matediana E, Manzungu
estimation of event duration by males and females. Appl Cogn Psychol OW, et al. Effect of a neonatal resuscitation course on healthcare pro-
1987;1:3-13. viders performances assessed by video recording in a low-resource setting.
5. Droit-Volet S. Time perception, emotions and mood disorders. J Physiol PLoS ONE 2015;10:e0144443.
(Paris) 2013;107:255-64. 16. Droit-Volet S, Meck WH. How emotions colour our perception of time.
6. Grondin S. Timing and time perception: a review of recent behavioral Trends Cogn Sci 2007;11:504-13.
and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Atten Percept 17. Schilleman K, Witlox RS, van Vonderen JJ, Roegholt E, Walther FJ, te Pas
Psychophys 2010;72:561-82. AB. Auditing documentation on delivery room management using video
7. Eisen LA. Time perception is distorted during responses to medical emer- and physiological recordings. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed
gencies. Med Hypotheses 2009;72:626-8. 2014;99:F485-90.

Time Perception during Neonatal Resuscitation 5

FLA 5.4.0 DTD YMPD8513_proof August 4, 2016

You might also like