You are on page 1of 16

Psychological Medicine, 1992, 22, 971-986.

Copyright 1992 Cambridge University Press


Primed in Great Britain

The outcome of childhood conduct disorder:


implications for defining adult personality disorder
and conduct disorder
MARK ZOCCOLILLO,1 ANDREW PICKLES, DAVID QUINTON
AND MICHAEL RUTTER
From the MRC Child Psychiatry Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, London

SYNOPSIS The effect of conduct disorder on adult social functioning in the areas of work,
sexual/love relationships, social relationships and criminality was studied in a sample of young
adults who spent much of their childhoods in group-cottage children's homes and an inner-city
comparison group. Most subjects with conduct disorder had pervasive (but not necessarily severe)
social difficulties compared to peers without conduct disorder. Less than half of this group met
DSM-III adult criteria for antisocial personality disorder and just over half were given a diagnosis
of personality disorder on interviewer clinical ratings. A latent class model that used both the
retrospective and contemporaneous indicators of conduct disorder confirmed the very high
continuity with adult social difficulties. Current diagnoses did not adequately describe this group
and conduct disorder appeared to be an almost necessary condition for multiple social disability
in adults in these samples.

the continuities from conduct disorders to adult


INTRODUCTION social functioning should not confine itself to
Robins (1978) has noted that almost all adults personality disorders, as currently defined. This
with chronic antisocial behaviour had shown is both because there are major deficiencies in
conduct disorder as children, but that only current classifications of personality disorder
about one-third of children with a conduct (Rutter, 1987) and because continuities may
disorder develop antisocial personality disorder occur below the diagnostic threshold.
as adults. Many have interpreted these findings This view is supported by a careful reading of
as indicating that a significant proportion of Robins' classic study (Robins, 1966), in which
children with conduct disorder have a relatively 23 % of the children with conduct disorder were
good outcome. However, part of this apparent malfunctioning in some way that did not meet
discontinuity between behaviours in childhood criteria for antisocial personality disorder or any
and adulthood may be an artefact of using only other standard diagnosis in adult life, and only
antisocial personality disorder as the adult 16% grew up to be completely well-functioning.
measure. By definition, criminal and aggressive It is thus evident that only some of the
acts are a central feature of that disorder maladaptive behaviours in adult life following
(American Psychiatric Association, 1980), but conduct disorder in childhood are well captured
such behaviours may not be the only outcome by current diagnostic criteria. However, it was
from conduct problems: a consideration of a also clear from Robins' data that minor social
wider range of social maladaptation may be maladaptation in adulthood was common; only
appropriate. Although pervasive social mal- 52% of the control group were considered free
adaptation is a feature of all personality dis- of any problems in social adaptation on the
orders (Russell & Hersov, 1983), examination of criteria used. The implication is that conduct
disorder is probably not the usual antecedent of
' Address for correspondence: Dr Mark Zoccolillo, UCHSC,
Suite 120, 1611 S. Federal Boulevard, Denver, CO 80219, USA.
such adult problems, considered singly. The
971
972 M. Zoccolillo and others

extent to which it is the antecedent of more study of the children of parents with some form
pervasive adult difficulties constitutes the main of psychiatric disorder who were at that time
focus of this paper. living in the more deprived areas of one Inner
The data are taken from a study comparing London Borough (Rutter & Quinton, 1981,
the adult social functioning of children who 1984). This control group was taken from the
spent a substantial part of their childhoods in same school class as the children whose parents
large cottage-based children's homes, because of had psychiatric problems, and departed from a
a breakdown in parenting, with a general true random sample only insofar as the children
population comparison group from inner city of patients may have been placed in particular
areas socially comparable to the ones from school classes (although there was no evidence
which the children ' in care' were taken (Quinton that this had occurred) and through the exclusion
et al. 1984; Quinton & Rutter, 1988; Rutter et of the few children who had ever been admitted
al. 1990). Children admitted 'into care' have 'into care' (in order to avoid overlap between
some advantages for the study of conduct the groups). This group was suitable for this
disorder and its adult outcome because they purpose because the subjects were of comparable
show high rates of behavioural difficulties in age and from a geographical area similar to
childhood (Rutter & Wolkind, 1973; Wolkind & those from which the 'in care' sample were
Renton, 1979). taken into the children's homes. The mean age
The data were examined to answer five of both samples at follow-up was 26 years.
questions. Contemporaneous teacher questionnaires
1. If poor adult personality functioning is (Rutter, 1967) and Juvenile Court records were
defined as pervasive social maladaptation, what available for both groups; in addition, parent
percentage of adults with pervasive social mal- questionnaires (Rutter et al. 1970) were com-
adaptation have had conduct disorder in child- pleted on the 'in care' group. Subjects in both
hood? samples were interviewed in their mid-twenties
2. What is the outcome in terms of social using a standardized investigator-based inter-
maladaptation in adult life for children with view and information was obtained on life
conduct disorder (as compared with their peers experiences and histories; behaviour in child-
without conduct disorder)? hood and adolescence; adult social functioning
3. What proportions of subjects with conduct in major role areas; and psychiatric symptoms.
disorder who are not entirely well functioning as Criminal activity was rated from official records.
adults have DSM-III (American Psychiatric The findings are based on 254 subjects: 171
Association, 1980) antisocial personality dis- who had been in care (90 men and 81 women)
order? and 83 in the comparison sample (42 men and 41
4. What is the minimal number of symptoms women). These represent 87% of the women
of conduct disorder to give a high likelihood of and 73 % of the men who had been in care and
pervasive social maladaptation in adult life? 72% of the men and 80% of women in the
5. Is the extent of the continuity between comparison group. For some of the multivariate
childhood conduct disorder and pervasive adult longitudinal analyses, only those subjects with
maladaptation modified by experiences and life- complete data both in childhood and at follow-
changes? up were included; the respective proportions for
these analyses were 83, 67, 71 and 80%.

STUDY DESIGN
MEASURES AND STATISTICAL
As part of a longitudinal study from childhood,
ANALYSES
detailed information was obtained on young
adults who had spent much of their childhoods Conduct disorder
in two group cottage children's homes and on a The interview schedules were reviewed by M.Z.,
quasi-random comparison sample from econ- who had not been involved in the original
omically deprived inner city areas, who had interviewing. The presence of probable conduct
never been in care. The comparison sample disorder (2 symptoms) and definite conduct
included all those from the control group for a disorder (3 or more symptoms) was recorded,
Adult outcome of conduct disorder 973

retrospective conduct disorder. Two prospective


Table 1. Operational definition of defining
conduct disorder measures were also used:
criteria for conduct disorder in childhood
official delinquency before the age of 15, and a
Probable conduct disorder: any two of the following behaviours positive rating of conduct, or mixed conduct
occurring before the age of 15. and emotional disorder, on the teacher's ques-
Definite conduct disorder: three or more of the following
behaviours occurring before the age of 15.
tionnaire measure, completed at some point
1. Frequent truancy between the ages of 7-5 and 15 years.
2. Expulsion or suspension from school for misbehaviour It should be noted that the interviews did not
3. Delinquency (arrested or referred to juvenile court because
of misbehaviour) cover all the common conduct disorder (CD)
4. Running away from home overnight at least twice symptoms systematically. Therefore, a positive
5. Persistent lying rating on the retrospective measure should be
6. Repeated sexual intercourse in a casual relationship
7. Repeated drunkenness or substance abuse interpreted as meaning that the subject showed
8. Thefts at least 2 or at least 3 symptoms. Tt is, however,
9. Vandalism unusual for the behaviours on which there was
10. Chronic violations of rules at home or at school (other
than truancy) not systematic information (lying, promiscuity,
11. Initiation of fights initiation of fights, running away) to occur in the
absence of others.
N.B.These criteria deliberately omit that on 'school grades markedly
below expectations' as scholastic underachievement did not
seem part of the general concept of conduct disorder and Adult functioning
because it was not susceptible to operationalization using
interview data. Social functioning since the age of 18 was
rated by the interviewers in the areas of work,
sexual/love relationships, and social relation-
Table 2. Operational criteria for adult social ships. The items used are given in Table 2. With
dysfunction respect to the inclusion of unemployment as one
Work
of the 'work' criteria, it should be noted that the
(One or more of the following) follow-up was undertaken prior to the rapid rise
1. Six or more jobs in past 4 years in unemployment during the 1980s.
2. Unemployed for at least one month or more than twice
since age 18 In the analyses that follow, a positive rating
3. Ever fired since age 18 on any one item in each area was used to define
4. Walked out of three or more jobs since age 18 a subject as having some level of social mal-
5. Persistent friction with workmates
adaptation in that area. Criminality was scored
Social relationships (past three months)
(One or more of the following) positively if the subject had ever been on
1. No confiding relationships probation, in prison, or received a conviction for
2. No shared leisure activities
3. No visiting of or by friends
other than trivial offences, since the age of 18.
4. Persistent friction with friends or neighbours Thus, 4 binary outcome variables: social relation-
Sex/love relationships since age 18 ships, work history, sexual/love relationships,
(One or more of the following) and official criminality were used to model
1. Two or more terminated cohabitations
2. Persistent discord or two or more discordant or
continuities between childhood conduct disorder
disrupted cohabitations and social maladaptation in early adulthood.
3. Violence in two or more relationships In addition, ratings were made of antisocial
4. Two or more deviant (alcohol, criminal, drugs) partners
5. Persistent problems in making or sustaining love personality disorder using DSM-III criteria, but
relationships excluding the requirement that conduct disorder
should be present. These, together with the
interviewers' rating of personality disorder, were
using criterion B for the DSM-III diagnosis of used to compare continuities to social mal-
antisocial personality disorder (American Psy- adaptation using clinical measures of personality
chiatric Association, 1980). These criteria are functioning with continuities that appear when
given in Table 1 and were chosen because they less severe measures, covering a wider range of
were based on the original work by Robins functioning, are used. The interviewers' per-
(1966), have good predictive power, and could sonality disorder ratings were based on assess-
be used to compare her findings with those from ments of pervasively poor functioning from
this study. This measure is referred to as early adolescence onwards, and grouped into
974 M. Zoccolillo and others

the two broad DSM-III clusters: 'dramatic' Their use in the evaluation of psychiatric
disorders (antisocial, borderline, histrionic and diagnostic criteria has been discussed by Young
narcissistic disorders) and 'other' disorders (in (1983), and in the analysis of child smoking
practice these mainly involved elements of the behaviour by Fergusson & Horwood (1989).
avoidant and dependent types). The interviewer Latent class methods can also be used to examine
ratings, however, were not based on DSM-III the switching between latent classes over time,
operationalized criteria. the latent classes being measured by a different
set of indicators on each occasion (Coleman,
Analytic strategy 1964a, b). We describe the model used in two
The analysis is presented in four stages: first, a parts. The first is the measurement model that
descriptive account of the relationship between describes how the observed and latent variables
conduct disorder and clinically defined per- are related. The second is the structural model
sonality disorder; second, analyses of the re- that describes the inter-relationships among
lationship between conduct disorder and the latent variables.
broader measures of social functioning using The data provided both contemporaneous
cross-tabulations and linear logistic modelling and retrospective indicators of childhood CD
(Dunn, 1981;Everitt & Dunn, 1983; McCullagh and contemporaneous indicators of adult
& Nelder, 1983); third, the relationship of functioning in several areas. For each of these
personality disorder to social maladaptation; indicators a measurement equation was defined
and fourth, the fitting of latent class models in that described how the indicator was related to
order better to deal with problems arising from the latent disorder.
classification error. Consider for individual / a fallible binary
In fitting the linear logistic models, the indicatory Y(i), with values 0 and 1, that
significance of an effect was assessed by a measured a latent state of disorder Z(i), also
comparison of the goodness-of-fit, as measured with values 0 and 1. We can define a measure-
by the Scaled Deviance, of pairs of models that ment equation of the linear logistic form that
included or did not include the variable. This relates Y and Z e.g.
difference in Scaled Deviance is a likelihood log[odds of disorder according
ratio test statistic that is asymptotically x2 to measure Y] =
distributed. Significance levels quoted for such \og{Pr[Y(i) = \]/Pr[Y(0 = 0]} = a + bZ(i).
%2s are for two-tailed tests throughout.
Since the main focus of the analyses was upon The parameter ' a ' determines the probability of
the continuity of conduct disorder into adult a positive indicator, given that the individual is,
life, the central analytical problem concerned in fact, not disordered. The parameter '/>'
the transition rates from conduct disorder and determines how much more probable is a positive
non-conduct disorder into adaptive or mal- indicator were the individual actually disordered.
adaptive states in adulthood. The usual simple Similar equations can be defined for other
methods of calculating transition rates assume indicators, but with different parameters ' a ' and
that classification of individuals as to disorder 'b\ In this study the measurement equations
occurs without error at each occasion. Such were in two sets, one for the latent variable of
classification errors usually substantially atten- conduct disorder in childhood and the second
uate, but can sometimes increase, simple for the latent variable of pervasive social
estimates of continuity. dysfunction in early adulthood.
With interval based measures of outcome a The parameters of these equations describe
common solution to this problem has been to the performance of each indicator, the a
base the model on, and to estimate the relation- constants being related to the rate of false
ships between, structural or latent variables that positives, and (a + b) being related to the rate
are not themselves directly observable, but for of false negatives. Two further complications
which multiple indicator measures are available. were allowed for in the application reported
A similar treatment of measurement error is here. First, an indicator need not perform in
possible with measures being used to define the same way for all groups of individuals, for
latent categories, classes or states of disorder. example as appears to be the case with adult
Adult outcome of conduct disorder 975

crime. Crime is very much rarer for women than Factors Factors
men, suggesting it to be a rather exceptional influencing influencing
prevalence continuity
behaviour for women. Secondly, where the
observed variable was ordinal, a measurement Sex of Care Deviance
equation of the proportional odds form child status of spouse
(McCullagh & Nelder, 1983) was used in which
the single ' a ' parameter was replaced by a set of
X i
Child
J Adult
disorder P 1-p disorder
parameters or thresholds.
It is useful to contrast the construction of a Child non- 1-q Adult non-
disorder
q disorder
latent measurement with more familiar methods
of combining information from several observed
variables. The standard 'rule-based' methods
Childhood indicators Transition Adult indicators
for the construction of a combined measure of matrix
disorder or systematic diagnosis typically assign
FIG. I. Schematic representation of the latent Markov model of
prior weights to the contributing measures (often continuity/discontinuity in childhood conduct disorder.
all equal) and then specify some threshold for
the weighted sum of measures above which
disorder is indicated, for example two positive was again of the logistic form with the prevalence
measurements out of three. The diagnostic status of conduct disorder allowed to vary between
of a sampled subject is then determined by this boys and girls, and between those 'in-care' and
standardized rule without consideration of data those not.
from other members of the sample. Contextual The second part of the structural equation
considerations are thus limited to those that are model consisted of the simple Markov transition
implicit within the original measures. matrix with transition probabilities p(i) and q(i),
Within a latent class model the measures can the transitions being from the latent states of
be considered as being used as screening non-disorder in childhood to disorder in adult-
instruments, and the assignment of disordered hood and disorder in childhood to non-
status to subjects differs in two ways from the disorder in adulthood. Fig. 1 gives an illustration
standard method. First, a screening instrument of the overall model.
is understood only to be assigning a probability Putting the structural and measurement
of disorder. Secondly, the performance of a equations together allows us to write an ex-
screening instrument, in terms of the relative pression for the probability of any observed set
frequency of false positive and negative cases of indicator values. This can be thought of as the
and also the probability of disorder that is sum of two long expressions, each concerned
assigned to any sampled subject with a given set respectively with the probability of the indicators
of observed measures, depends upon the preval- were the child disordered or non-disordered.
ence of disorder. Applied to a group where the Thus, the first expression is the prior probability
disorder is rare, the occasional positive measures of being disordered, times the probability of the
are most likely to be false positives. For groups observed values of the child indicators given
within which the disorder is common rather that the child is disordered, times the sum of two
more of the positive measures will be true further terms. Of these, the first term represents
positives. In the latent class model the prob- the probability of continuing into the disordered
ability of disorder assigned to a subject therefore adult state times the probabilities of the observed
depends not only on that individual's measure- values of the adult indicators given adult
ments, but also upon the estimated prevalence disorder. The second term represents the prob-
of disorder provided by the context of measures ability of switching into non-disorder in adult-
from the other 'similar' subjects. hood times the probabilities of the observed
An equation for estimating this prevalence or indicators given no adult disorder. The second
prior probability of disorder within the sample long expression is entirely analogous to the first
groups of similar subjects constitutes the first of but consists of the prior probability of not being
two components that form the structural model. disordered in childhood, times the probability of
In the case of the data of this study, the equation the childhood observations given no disorder,
976 M. Zoccolillo and others

times the probability of the adult observations DSM-III symptoms) when there is any mal-
for the two paths one involving continuity of no- function in adult life. The extent to which this
disorder and the other switching into disorder. was so in these samples was examined using the
Expression of this form for the probability of DSM-III adult criteria for antisocial personality
the observed set of indicator values from each (ASP). These analyses were confined to those
individual in the sample, when multiplied cases for whom there were sufficient data to
together give what is called the sample likelihood make this rating and for whom all the ratings of
function. The set of parameter values that social functioning were available.
maximize this likelihood function are the maxi- Of the 35 males with conduct disorder in
mum likelihood estimates. The significance of childhood, 14 (40 %) were rated as showing ASP
effects examined was determined using the same in adult life compared with only 4 % (4/92) of
general procedure as for the linear logistic those without conduct disorder. All these men
models (likelihood ratio tests) to give met criterion D for DSM-III ASP, that is they
asymptotically x* distributed test statistics. showed persistent antisocial behaviour after the
It was possible to obtain a simple picture of age of eighteen. All of the men showing ASP
the process as 'viewed' by the latent Markov were also diagnosed by the interviewers as
model by making use of' posterior probabilities'. showing some form of 'dramatic' personality
The latent variable approach, although pre- disorder, but in addition to these that 'matched'
cluding exact assignment of individuals to states with the ASP ratings, a further 7 were rated by
of disorder, can provide probabilities of disorder the interviewers as showing personality disorder
or probabilities of alternative histories of dis- (5 with a definite or probable 'dramatic' type
order in childhood and adulthood. Summing and two with some other variety - both the
these posterior probabilities over sub-groups of latter diagnoses being complicated by low IQ).
individuals in the sample allows 'expected Thus, 60 % of males with conduct disorder were
frequencies' using the latent classification of the rated as showing personality disorder on one or
model to be directly compared with classi- both schemes.
fications based on the observed measures of Of the 26 females with childhood conduct
disorder. disorder, nine (35%) showed ASP compared
In considering the adult indicators, there arose with none of those without conduct disorder,
the possibility that an element of the continuity and a further four met the behavioural criteria
observed might have been indirect, occurring for ASP after the age of 18 but without the
through the assortative mating of those with persistence of the problems required to fulfil all
conduct disorder followed by the independent the DSM-III adult criteria. Of the 9 with ASP, 6
effect of a deviant spouse on measured adult were rated by the interviewers as showing definite
disorder in the subject. For this reason the personality disorder (all of a 'dramatic' type).
sex/love adult indicator, most sensitive to Five women with definite conduct disorder but
deviation in the spouse, was not included in this without ASP had an interview diagnosis of
model. probable or definite 'dramatic' personality dis-
The linear logistic analyses were performed order. Thus, 54% of women were rated as
using GLIM (Baker & Nelder, 1978) and showing personality disorder on one or both
EGRET (1990). The latent Markov analysis schemes.
was performed by maximum likelihood using The discrepancy between operationally
a purpose written FORTRAN program and defined ASP and the interviewer judgements for
library optimization sub-routines (Numerical women, but not for men, was examined by a
Algorithms Group, 1989). more detailed scrutiny of the case histories. For
the 3 cases with an ASP diagnosis without a
personality disorder rating at interview, the
RESULTS interviewer ratings took into account improved
Conduct disorder and personality disorder social functioning in recent years, including the
Traditionally, antisocial personality disorder has cessation of criminal activities for the 2 women
been seen as the expected adult outcome for with a criminal record. The discrepant cases
were still rated as functioning relatively poorly
definite childhood conduct disorder (3 or more
Adult outcome of conduct disorder 977

but were no longer showing aggressive or


Table 3. Conduct disorder and adult social
disruptive behaviour. This pattern also applied
dysfunction
to 4 women not rated by the interviewer as
showing personality disorder but nevertheless Number of areas of adult
showing ASP behaviours after the age of 18 but dysfunction ('%)
Tol'U
without the additional criterion of persistence. 4 >3 ! S* 1 0 N
These apparent changes for the better in early
adult life (it remains to be seen whether or not (a) Prospectively measured conduct disorder
No CD 3 12 27 49 51 178
they will persist) emphasize the difficulty of CD 8 33 62 73 27 63
diagnosing personality disorder in early adult- (b) Retrospectively measured conduct disorder
hood (Hill et al. 1989). Number of symptoms
These initial analyses suggest that for both 0-1 1 6 17 40 60 159
2 5 34 48 77 23 21
males and females ASP, as measured by the > 2 13 43 81 88 12 61
DSM-III adult criteria, is nearly always preceded
by conduct disorder in childhood. When con-
tinuity was measured on these criteria it applied Table 4. Linear logistic model of adult
to one-third to two-fifths of those with conduct dysfunction (N = 241)
disorder (40% males; 3 5 % females). However,
it was apparent from the interviewer ratings that Parameter Standard
ASP was not the only type of personality Main effects model estimate error

disorder that occurred and that there were Constant -1-946


continuities to poor social functioning that did Sex (female) -1-121 (0-363)
Care status 1-289 (0-411)
not meet criteria for personality disorder. Probable CD 1112 (0-517)
Because of this, the analyses that follow Definite CD 2-844 (0-402)
consider the continuities between conduct dis- Scaled deviance 214-85
order and adult social malfunction, regardless of Tests based on alterations on this model
whether malfunction is associated with a clinical LR
df X P
rating of personality disorder or not.
Removing main effects
Linear logistic analyses Sex 1 1006 0002
Care 1 10-81 0001
Table 3a shows the power in this sample of the CD 2 62-90 < 0001
prospective conduct measure, being positive on Adding interactions
either the teacher rating or official delinquency, Sex.CD 2 119 0-6
Care.CD 2 017 0-9
for predicting adult social functioning. The odds Sex .Care 1 1-63 0-2
ratio for one or more areas of dysfunction was Sex.CD + Care.CD +
2-8 (95% interval 1-5 5-4) and that for two or Sex.Care + Sex.Care.CD 7 4-19 0-8
more areas of dysfunction was 4-4 (2-4-8-4). Pooling of CD categories 1 8-96 < 0001
Logistic regression models were estimated with
adult social functioning as the outcome and the
conduct measure, sex and 'care' status as CD (more than 2 symptoms) were 7-6 (3-317-4)
independent variables. Accounting for the effects for 1 area of dysfunction and 17-2 (7-7-38-4) for
of these other variables reduced the estimated 2 areas, even after accounting for the effects of
odds ratios to 1-9 (1-0-3-7) and 31 (1-6-6-0), for sex and care status within a logistic regression.
1 or more and 2 or more areas of dysfunction The corresponding odds ratios for probable CD
respectively. Although some continuity is clear, (2 symptoms) were 3-7 (1-2-11-5) and 30 (1-1-8-6)
discontinuity is not uncommon with, in par- respectively. The effects of probable and definite
ticular, 38 % of those positive on this prospective CD were not significantly different when con-
measure of conduct disorder going on to have sidering 1 or more areas of adult dysfunction
problems in just one area or none at all. but were significantly different for the more
This element of discontinuity is less evident in severe outcome of 2 or more areas.
the data using the retrospective data shown in Table 4 shows the results of one of the linear
Table 3b. The estimated odds ratios for definite logistic analyses, the model identified being the
978 M. Zoccolillo and others

nothing about possible sex differences) and the


Table 5. Distribution of areas of adult
absence of interaction effects involving conduct
dysfunction by sex, care status and childhood
disorder and group or sex designation were
retrospective conduct disorder symptoms
largely due to the low statistical power of the
Number of areas of adult dysfunction tests.
The greater continuity indicated from retro-
0 1 2 3 4 Total spectively rather than prospectively measured
Control boys CD, may have arisen for one or both of two
CD symptoms reasons. The retrospective measure might have
a-i 21 3 4 0 0 28
2 1 2 1 1 0 5 been a better indicator of actual CD than the
S3 2 0 3 2 1 8 contemporaneous teacher questionnaire score
Total 24 5 8 3 1 41 and delinquency, either because it was a better
Care boys measure or because the teacher questionnaire
CD symptoms
a-i 17 11 8 6 1 43 score missed subjects who developed CD after
2 3 3 2 3 1 12 the administration of the scale (between the ages
S3 1 2 10 9 5 27
of 7-5 and 15). Alternatively, the retrospective
Total 21 16 20 18 7 82
Control girls measure may have been contaminated by
CD symptoms elements of current adult functioning such that,
a-i 28 10 1 0 0 39 ceterisparibus, those with a worse adult outcome
2 1 0 0 0 0 1
S3 | 0 0 0 0 1 recalled more childhood symptomatology than
Total 30 10 1 0 0 41 those with a good outcome. Some evidence
Care girls against the recall bias explanation was obtained
CD symptoms from an additional contemporaneous measure,
a-i 29 13 5 2 0 49
2 0 1 0 2 0 3 the Rutter parental scale, available on the 'in
S3 4 2 10 7 2 25 care' sample only. Twenty-seven per cent of the
Total 33 16 15 11 2 77 'in care' sample were positive on this measure.
But among those positive on the retrospective
measure, but negative on both the prospective
same whether forwards addition or backwards teacher scale and delinquency, 40% were posi-
elimination of higher order terms was used (see, tive on the parent scale.
for example, Aitkin et al. 1989). Whether using Visual inspection of Table 3 suggested that
either conduct measure or either outcome CD might be particularly associated with per-
measure, the effect of conduct disorder on the vasive adult dysfunction, the contrast with the
outcome was consistently identified as occurring no-CD distribution being generally greater
solely through a simple main effect and was not where multiple areas of dysfunction were con-
involved in any interaction effects with sex and cerned. Table 6 examines this question further.
care status. This result tempts the conclusion It shows that single isolated areas of dysfunction
that the effect of conduct disorder was common were almost always more common among those
to boys and girls from both care and general without CD than those with CD, the expected
population groups. However, as Table 5 makes higher rates of disorder for those with CD being
clear, there were only 2 girls with conduct seen only where two or more areas were
disorder in the general population sample concerned. The great majority (80%) of those
(control group) and, as it happened, neither with 3 or more areas of dysfunction had shown
showed any social dysfunction in adult life. CD on the retrospective measure, but 9 of them
Although greater in number, there were still only had not. Four of these 9 showed adult onset
13 with conduct disorder among the general psychiatric disorders (one alcohol abuse and
population boys, and 5 of these showed one or crime, one atypical psychotic disorder, and two
fewer areas of adult dysfunction, Thus, the recurrent or chronic depression). Of the re-
pattern of results stem largely from the maining 5, four had been identified as conduct
institution reared sample. Nothing can be said disordered on the prospective measures. The
about the adult outcome of conduct disorder in remaining one appeared to have started de-
girls from the general population (and hence linquent behaviour at about age 15, having been
Adult outcome of conduct disorder 979

The prevalence rates of the four indicators


Table 6. Distributions of areas of adult
among the general population men and women
dysfunction by sex and childhood retrospective
varied substantially, being 15 and 0% for crime,
conduct disorder
12 and 2 % for sex/love relationships, 20 and
Men Women 15% for social and 32 and 12% for work. This
reinforced the view that these indicators could
Adult dysfunclion No CD % CD % No CD % CD % not be assumed equivalent - with, for example,
None 54 13 65 20 severity or degree of pervasiveness of dysfunc-
One tion being accurately represented by the simple
Crime 7 2 2 0 count of areas affected. In addition, conflict
Marriage 1 2 3 3
Social 3 2 11 0
among the childhood conduct disorder measures
Work g g 9 7 had already been noted. The presence of these
Two measurement problems might have resulted in
CxM 1 2 0 3 the true pattern of continuity being obscured.
CxS 1 4 0 3
CxW 6 17 1 0 We attempted to recover that true pattern
MxS 1 4 6 3 through the use of the latent class model.
MxW 1 2 0 13
SxW 6 2 0 10
The latent class model fitted used the 2-
Three level categorical (binary) prospective conduct
CxM xS 3 2 0 0 measures of the teacher rating, official delin-
CxM xW 1 12 0 10 quency and house-parent rating (in care sample
CxSxW 1 g 0 7
MxSxW 3 g 2 13
only) and the 3-level ordinal retrospective
Four interview measure. These four observed
CxM xSxW 1 13 0 7 variables were each related to a latent binary
Sample si/e (N) 71 52 g8 30 variable of conduct disorder by means of logistic
measurement equations of the form of equation
1 (see Appendix). The three observed binary
a persistent truant before that. In adult life he adult measures of adult crime and dysfunction
had a severe recidivist crime record with multiple in social and work areas were similarly related to
social problems. a latent binary variable of pervasive adult
dysfunction. The sex/love indicator was dropped
Personality disorder and social maladaptation at this stage because of a concern to focus as
It is clear that the measures of social mal- much as possible on direct continuity. The
adaptation show more continuity between con- process of assortative mating to a deviant
duct disorder and adult dysfunction than per- partner, followed by that chosen partner's own
sonality disorder diagnoses. Eight-six per cent of contribution to dysfunction in this area, con-
the 35 men with three or more symptoms of CD stitute a possible indirect route for continuity to
showed social maladaptation in two or more occur, one that we wished to exclude for this
areas, whereas only 40% were given a DSM-III analysis.
diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder and The specification of these measurement equa-
a further 20% an interviewer diagnosis of tions required decisions on what were to be free
personality disorder of some other type. The parameters. This required detailed consideration
comparable figures for the 26 women with a of the prevalences of each of the indicators and
similar level of conduct disorder were 73 % with their sensitivity and specificity to the underlying
pervasive social maladaptation, 35 % with DSM- latent condition. As part of this process multi-
III antisocial personality disorder and 19% with variate logistic regressions were undertaken
another type of personality disorder. using the random effects logistic regression
program of EGRET (1990). As a result of these
Latent class analysis analyses common parameters were assumed for
Table 6 raised the question as to whether each of several of the indicators, allowing some im-
the adult indicators was equally associated with provement in statistical efficiency. In addition,
this pervasive dysfunction that appeared to the performance of the crime indicator was
constitute the adult outcome for childhood CD. identified as being different for men and women.
980 M. Zoccolillo and others

Table 7. Measurement characteristics, prevalence Table 8. Shifts in probability of conduct


estimates and transition rates from the latent disorder with information on indicators
class model
Probability of CD (%)
Childhood disorder No information
Prior probability 10
False positive False negative or prevalence
Prospective indicators
Delinquency + 58
Teacher or house-parent Delinquency 8
questionnaire score 4 53
(CD or mixed) B score (or A score) + 56
Delinquency by age 15 2
B score (or A score) 6
75
Retrospective interview Both B-score and delinquency + 97
2 or more symptoms 10 29 Both B-score and delinquency 4
3 or more symptoms 5 45 Retrospective indicator
Definite + 55
Poor adult social functioning Probable + 44
Definite - 3
False positive False negative

Adult crime many positives owing to high rates of false


Boys 10 negatives (probability of a child with CD being
33
Girls 3 66
Social 16
negative on an indicator measure). The retro-
56
Work 13 spective measure using 2 or more symptoms as
25
the cut-off identified many more of the positives
at the expense of a higher rate of false positives.
Prevalence levels of Tightening the cut-off to 3 or more symptoms
childhood CD
Care boys 64
had the expected result of identifying fewer of
Control boys 23 the conduct disordered children but reducing
Care girls 41 the number falsely identified as conduct dis-
Control girls 3
Transition probabilities ordered. Among the adult measures, that for
From childhood crime possessed a low rate of false positives but
CD to good adult a high rate for false negatives. This is consistent
social functioning
From childhood non-CD to poor adult with our expectation, especially for women, that
social functioning crime identified a more unusual, possibly more
extreme, group. The estimated prevalence rate
of the latent or underlying CD showed greater
The final specification of the seven measurement range across the 4 sex and care status categories
equations (see Appendix) involved 11 free than any of the 4 observed indicators, consistent
parameters. with the attenuation effects of misclassifications.
As explained, the structural model consisted Table 8 shows an alternative way in which the
of specifying free parameters for the subgroups measurement error results can be presented, that
of children that differed with respect to the is in terms of the impact on the estimated
prevalence of underlying or latent conduct probability of conduct disorder of knowledge of
disorder and similarly specifying free parameters the values of the indicators. Thus, official
for the transition rates between latent conduct delinquency alone shifts the (arbitrarily chosen)
disorder and latent adult dysfunction. The prior probability from 10 to 58% but with a
results shown in Table 7 are for a structural positive teacher scale score as well the assignment
model that allows 4 prevalence levels, 1 for each probability leaps up to 97%, emphasizing the
sex and group designation, and 2 simple tran- value of multiple indicators.
sition r a t e s - 6 free parameters in all. Returning to Table 7, the fitted model
The contemporaneous measures of CD are estimated the transition rate from no-disorder in
seen to have had very low false positive rates childhood to the disordered state in adulthood
(probability of a child without CD being positive as being very low. It should be emphasized here
on the indicator measure) but to have missed that adult disorder here refers to one that at an
Adult outcome of conduct disorder 981

Table 9. The effect of spouse: assortative mating, indirect through reduction of disorder or direct
upon adult indicators
Model allowing for

Indirect Direct
effect effect
Assortative through on Scaled LR
mating disorder indicators deviance (X2) df P

A No No No 1638-36
B Yes No No 1622-53 (A B) 15-83 2 00004
C Yes Yes No 1616-42 (B-C) 611 2 005
D Yes Yes Yes 1607-75 (C D) 8-67 2 001
E No Yes Yes 1621-34 (E-D) 13-59 2 0001
F Yes No Yes 1608-76 (F-D) 101 2 0-6

Table 10. Patterns of continuity of disorder as the gaining of a non-deviant partner (Quinton &
shown by frequencies on observed and latent Rutter, 1988). This was examined by allowing
measures (N = 227) various aspects of the model to depend upon the
presence of a non-deviant partner. Allowing the
Adult Social Adaptation prevalence of childhood CD to vary with partner
Boys
characteristics took account of assortative
Girls
mating (Table 9). Allowing the transition
Childhood CD Good Poor Good Poor probabilities to vary accounted for any role in
Using combination of observed measures of child
actual switching between states of latent dis-
and adult disorders* order. Separate parameters for each of these
No 59 15 83 5 effects were estimated for men and women.
Yes 10 30 12 13
Comparisons of model A with model B
Using retrospective measure alone
No 51 15 81 3 indicated that the assortative mating effect was
Probable 11 5 2 2 significant, and model B with model C indicated
Definite 7 25 12 13
a significant effect for a non-deviant spouse on
ig
Estimated using latent or underlying
child and adult disorders
discontinuity between childhood disorder and
No 49 5 80 0 adult functioning. Further investigation showed
Yes 8 52 3 30 substantial sex-differences in these effects, with
* Conduct disordered if definite on retrospective or two out of
assortative mating appearing only to occur for
three childhood indicators positive. Poor adult social adaptation if 2 conduct disordered girls, who were rarely ending
or more out of 3 indicators positive. up with non-deviant men, while there was little
difference between conduct disordered and non-
disordered boys. As a result, even though the
underlying level is pervasive (though not necess- estimated transition rate out of conduct disorder
arily severe). Particular circumstances may still was raised to 4 8 % for women with a non-
generate poor (or good) functioning in a single deviant partner, as compared to less than 4 %
social domain independently of any pervasive for those without, the rarity of such women gave
dysfunction. Greater evidence was found of this little statistical significance. The effect for
discontinuity out of childhood CD though this men was somewhat less substantial, 26 versus
rate was estimated with considerable impre- 0%, but more men found non-deviant partners.
cision. As a result, the rate of 8% estimated Allowing for a third possible route for the
possessed a wide confidence interval, being not effect of a non-deviant partner, such that the
significantly different from zero (likelihood ratio adult functioning of all individuals was im-
f 201, P = 0-2) and having an upper 9 5 % proved, whether conduct disordered or not,
likelihood interval estimate of 19%. confused the picture. A comparison of model D
Previous research had suggested that for the with C suggested this effect to be significant and,
'in care' girls such transitions out of childhood in its presence, assortative mating (E-D) but not
disorder had been particularly associated with the switching effect (F-D) remained significant.
982 M. Zoccolillo and others

Table 10 provides a simple comparison of the social dysfunction only rarely occurred in the
extent of childhood to adulthood continuity as absence of childhood conduct disturbance except
shown by classifications based directly upon the when it was secondary to some form of adult-
observed data and by that using the expected onset psychiatric disorder such as alcoholism,
frequencies or posterior probabilities from the psychosis or affective disorder. This was ap-
latent class model allowing for non-deviant parent from both the linear logistic and latent
partner effects. The latter model suggests that class analyses. The finding confirms the tradi-
the pervasive adult dysfunction is not only very tional psychiatric assumption that personality
sensitive to prior childhood CD, but also a disorder almost always has its onset in childhood
highly specific sequela. or adolescence (although it may not be recog-
nized as such until adulthood).
Thirdly, conduct disorder was not a good
DISCUSSION predictor of isolated areas of social dysfunction;
indeed, most individuals with just one domain of
Even after the effects in being in-care, and of sex, social dysfunction were in the general population
were controlled, conduct disorder in the popu- comparison group. Fourthly, although it was
lation studied had a powerful effect on later decidedly uncommon for there to be a transition
social functioning. Looking forward from child- from normal conduct in childhood to personality
hood to adulthood, about two-fifths of children disorder in adult life even when social cir-
with CD (40 % of males and 35 % of females) cumstances were poor, the reverse did not apply.
went on to show a DSM-III antisocial per- A minority of individuals with conduct disorder
sonality disorder in early adult life. Many of the in childhood went on to show satisfactory social
remainder had persistent social difficulties that functioning in adult life: 13% of males and 9%
were pervasive across several domains of their of females on the latent class analysis which
life. The precise proportion, of course, depends infers pervasive (but not necessarily severe)
on the criteria used. However, just over two- social malfunction after correcting for measure-
thirds (86 % of males and 73 % of female) in our ment error, and 23 and 48 % respectively using
samples showed dysfunction in at least two social the combination of observed childhood
domains (see Table 5). The latent class analysis measures and the direct measure of adult social
(see Table 10) gave a broadly similar picture in dysfunction. This transition was strongly
that it estimated pervasive social dysfunction in associated with the presence of a supportive
88% of adults with CD. We may conclude, non-deviant spouse.
therefore, that probably about three-quarters of Fifthly, even holding conduct disorder in
children with a clinically significant conduct childhood constant, the fact of being reared in
disorder will go on to exhibit pervasive and an institution (a variable that indexed a range of
persistent social malfunction (although often adversities) increased the risk of pervasive social
well below the threshold for diagnoses of dysfunction in adult life. Although conduct
personality disorder). Our sample of control disorder as such is the major risk factor, rearing
girls with CD was much too small for any circumstances (and all that they reflect) added to
conclusions, however, and further research is the risk. Sixthly, only a few symptoms of conduct
needed to determine whether these conclusions disorder (not necessarily including violent or
apply to girls in the general population. extreme behaviour) were needed for there to be
Six features of these findings require particular pervasive social malfunction in adult life. This
emphasis. First, as already noted, antisocial suggests that the threshold to identify persistent
personality disorder accounted for only about clinically significant conduct disorder set in
half the cases of pervasive persistent social DSM-III-R may be too high. Nevertheless, a
dysfunction in early adult life. This was evident poor outcome in adult life was significantly
in Robins' (1966) classical follow-up of a child more frequent for young people with definite
guidance clinic sample and it was also apparent CD than with probable CD; also it was more
in her more recent analysis of the Epi- likely when CD was evident on several in-
demiological Catchment Area data (Robins, dependent measures. Clearly, further research is
1986). Secondly, pervasive, persistent, adult required to determine the most appropriate
Adult outcome of conduct disorder 983

threshold for diagnosis; as well as to delineate Thirdly, very few of our sample experienced
the particular pattern of CD most strongly major therapeutic interventions or environ-
associated with personality disorder in adult mental manipulations. Accordingly, we cannot
life. Other research suggests that it may know the extent to which the adult outcome is
involve associated hyperactivity/inattention modifiable. The better outcome associated with
(Farrington et al. 1990). harmonious marriage to a non-deviant spouse
These findings derive from a study of a (in the small minority where this occurred)
particular sample and it is necessary to consider suggests the potential for change. The challenge
four key respects in which sample characteristics to develop effective modes of treatment is there,
may impose constraints on generalizations from but it has to be admitted that the successes
our findings. First, we have defined conduct recorded to date are distinctly modest (Rutter &
disorder in terms of the mostly quite severe Giller, 1983; Robins, 1991).
behaviours specified in DSM-III. That definition Fourthly, our follow-up was undertaken when
would not include many cases of conduct the subjects were still in their mid-20s and we do
disorder as specified in ICD-9 (World Health not know whether or not improvements in
Organization, 1978), the definition of which functioning will take place later (we are currently
extends into what would now be considered undertaking a further follow-up to examine this
oppositional-defiant disorder. While it is clear possibility). It should be noted that Robins'
that some instances of the latter in early (1966) follow-up took place when the subjects
childhood go on to become DSM-III style were a decade or so older than ours.
conduct disorders in later childhood or ado- With these features (and caveats) in mind we
lescence (Loeber, 1992), systematic data on adult need to consider the implications for concepts of
outcome are lacking. childhood conduct disorder and for adult per-
Secondly, the main sample was comprised of sonality disorder. The traditional emphasis on
children from high-risk (presumably genetic and the continuity between conduct disorder and
environmental) backgrounds reared in institu- antisocial personality disorder, although truly
tions. There was some suggestion that the reflecting one crucial aspect of the empirical
outcome may have been somewhat better findings, has had several unfortunate conse-
(although still poor) in the comparison group; a quences. To begin with, it has led to under-
larger sample is needed to test this possibility. It estimates of the relatively poor social outcome
is also relevant that the comparison group was in early adult life for children with clinically
itself socially disadvantaged and it is necessary significant conduct disturbance. Our findings
to query whether the results would be similar in suggest that some three quarters showed per-
young people from a middle-class background. sistent social dysfunction that extended over
Robins (1966) found a non-significantly slightly several domains of their life (including love
lower risk of sociopathy in middle class youths; relationships, work, and friendships) as well as
her analysis of retrospective data from a general often, but by no means always, including
population sample (Robins, 1986) showed criminality. The diagnosis of antisocial per-
findings broadly similar to those reported here; sonality disorder does not provide an adequate
and Zoccolillo & Rogers' (1991) short-term coverage of the range of disabilities in adult life
follow-up of girls admitted to a psychiatric that are the sequelae of conduct disorder in
hospital with conduct disorder also showed a childhood. As already noted, this was also
poor outcome. Further research, studying chil- apparent in Robins' (1966, 1986) studies. In
dren from varying backgrounds, is necessary to addition, it was evident in Zeitlin's (1986) follow-
identify the features that carry a particularly up of child psychiatric patients who received
high risk of a poor adult outcome. However, the psychiatric treatment in adult life and in Quinton
degree of congruence of findings across studies et a/.'s (1990) follow-up of children reared by
suggests that there is likely to be substantial mentally ill parents. The data are rather sparse
generalizability of the broad pattern of our so far and it would be premature to draw
findings, even though the precise level of risk for conclusions on the range of patterns of adult
poor adult functioning may vary with as-yet-to- dysfunction following conduct disorder in child-
be-identified risk factors. hood.
984 M. Zoccolillo and others

Nevertheless, it does seem that, at least in differentiating feature. Our measures of the
adult life and especially in women, antisocial pattern of behaviour in childhood were not
behaviour may not constitute the key denning sufficiently discriminating for any useful sub-
characteristic of the personality disorder that division of conduct disorders. Other research
shows continuity with conduct disturbance. Our has suggested that early onset, the presence of
data raise the possibility that it is pervasive and hyperactivity/inattention, and poor peer
persistent social dysfunction that provides the relations constitute important predictors of a
coherence, and not criminal activities. Such poor adult outcome (Henn et al. 1980; Parker &
dysfunction in males usually includes crimin- Asher, 1987; Magnusson, 1988; Loeber, 1988,
ality, but in women that is much less often the 1990; Farrington et al. 1990). However, it
case. Of course, the DSM-III criteria emphasize remains quite uncertain whether these define
that the concept includes inconsistency in work, different varieties of conduct disorder or rather
impaired parenting, a lack of stable love/sex whether they constitute measures of severity. It
relationships, failure to honour financial may also be queried whether conduct disorder
obligations, impulsivity and recklessness - as should be regarded as a diagnostic category or a
well as lying, unlawful activities and aggress- behavioural dimension. Robins & McEvoy
iveness. Clearly, there is a need for diagnostic (1990) found that it functioned as a dimension in
assessments to provide systematic coverage of a its associations with substance abuse. However,
person's functioning in a range of social domains they relied on retrospective data and the possi-
(Hill et al. 1989). The breadth of social mal- bility of bias in recall must be considered.
function inevitably raises queries on whether Prospective data are required to answer the
the DSM-III subcategorizations of different question.
varieties of personality disorder have validity. Obviously, not every child who commits a
Certainly, other research has shown the high delinquent act or who truants or who initiates
frequency with which there is overlap between fights can be considered at a serious risk for
supposedly different varieties of personality adult personality disorder. That is because the
disorder (see e.g. Pope et al. 1983) and between prevalence of these behaviours in childhood is
personality disorder, affective disturbance, sub- much too high (Rutter & Giller, 1983) for that
stance abuse, somatization disorder, and suicidal to be the case. Many essentially normal children
attempts (Robins, 1966; Bohman et al. 1984; engage in occasional disruptive or defiant or
Goodwin & Guze, 1984; Zoccolillo & Cloninger, illegal behaviours at some time. Nevertheless,
1986). Moreover, conduct symptoms have been our findings indicate that the prognosis for
reported as antecedents of borderline personality clinically significant conduct disturbance is more
disorder as well as antisocial personality disorder serious than often appreciated. The key question
(Soloff & Millward, 1983; Andrulonis & Vogel, is at what level or persistence or pervasiveness of
1984). We conclude that there is a great need conduct disorder is there an appreciable risk of
for research examining communalities and adult social dysfunction? Is the risk the same
differences between supposedly different per- regardless of the age of the child at which the
sonality disorders. Research is also needed to conduct disorder occurs? Further research is
determine whether the childhood antecedents of required to answer these questions.
other varieties of personality disorder (such as A further question concerns the reasons for
'avoidant') are similar to or different from those the continuity between conduct disorder in
found for ASP and other 'dramatic' personality childhood and personality disorder in adult life
disorders. The subdivisions of personality dis- and the modifiability, or otherwise, of the
order in DSM-III-R, problematical as they are, process. Our findings show that substantial
do reflect interesting differences in qualities and change is possible even in adult life, given
style of social functioning. The question to be particularly good social circumstances. Of
tackled now is whether these differences in course, such change occurs in only a minority of
pattern have different antecedents or conse- individuals. However, it is necessary to ask
quences. whether that is because conduct disordered
A further issue concerns the homogeneity or individuals act in ways that make it unlikely that
otherwise of conduct disturbance in childhood. they will experience favourable social conditions
Adult outcome might constitute a useful or because patterns of behaviour in such
Adult outcome of conduct disorder 985

individuals are intrinsically resistant to change? Table Al. Measurement equation: free
Also, we have to enquire whether the continuities parameters
derive from persistently adverse environmental
Males Females
circumstances or from intrinsic qualities in the
conduct/social disorder as such? Would a lasting a b a b
remission of conduct disorder be more common Childhood measures
if more children experienced markedly improved Houseparent B-score al bl al bl
family circumstances? Alternatively, do the Teacher B-score al bl al bl
Delinquency a2 bl a2 bl
persistent conduct disorders differ funda- Retrospective CD
mentally from the transient ones in having a Probable a3 bl a3 bl
greater genetic component? It is striking that the Definite a4 bl a4 bl
few available data suggest that the heritability of Adult measures
Adult crime a5 b2 a6 b2
juvenile delinquency is very low whereas that of Adult work a7 b2 a7 b2
adult crime is much greater (Rutter et al. 1990). Adult social a8 b3 a8 b3
Our data are not of a kind that can provide
answers to these questions. However, what they
do show is that childhood conduct disorder is an REFERENCES
important disorder with relatively serious implic- Aitken, M., Anderson, D, Fraser, B. & Hinde, J. (1989). Statistical
ations for adult social dysfunction. One aspect Modelling in GLIM. Oxford University Press: Oxford.
of the continuity lies in the links with antisocial American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders {3rd edn) - DSM-III. American
personality disorder, but the connections are Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.
also shown with other varieties of personality Andrulonis, P. A. & Vogel, N. G. (1984). Comparison of borderline
disorder. That finding raises queries as to personality subcategories to schizophrenic and affective disorders.
British Journal of Psychiatry 144, 358-363.
whether personality disorder should be con- Baker, R. S. & Nelder, S. A. (1978). The GLIM System, Release 3:
ceptualized in terms of pervasive persistent social Generalized Linear Interactive Modelling. Royal Statistical Society:
London.
dysfunction that has been present from Bohman, M., Cloninger, C. R., von Knorring, A. L. & Sigvardson, S.
childhood/adolescence onwards rather than in (1984). An adoption study of somatoform disorders. III. Cross-
terms of its current multiple subdivisions. fostering analysis and genetic relationship to alcoholism and
criminality. Archives of General Psychiatry 41, 872- 878.
Coleman, J. S. (1964a). Models of Change and Response Uncertainty.
Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Coleman, J. S. (I964A). Introduction to Mathematical Sociology. Free
This work was supported by the Medical Research Press of Glencoe: New York.
Council and by grant 1-F32 MH09307-01 from the Dunn, G. (1981). The role of linear models in psychiatric epi-
National Institute of Mental Health (Dr M. S. demiology. Psychological Medicine 11, 179-184.
Zoccolillo). EGRET (1990). EGRET Reference Manual 1st draft. Statistical and
Epidemiological Research Corporation: Seattle, WA.
Everilt, B. S. & Dunn, G. (1983). Advanced Methods of Data
Exploration and Modelling. Heinemann Educational Books:
London.
APPENDIX. THE SPECIFICATION OF Farrington, D. P., Loeber, R. & Van Kammen, W. B. (1990). Long-
term criminal outcomes of hyperactivity-impulsivity attention
THE MEASUREMENT EQUATIONS deficit and conduct problems in childhood. In Straight and Devious
Each measurement equation involved two parameters, Pathways from Childhood to Adulthood (ed. L. N. Robins and M.
Rutter), pp. 62-81. Cambridge University Press: New York.
a and h, and was of the form: Fergusson, D. M. & Horwood, L. J. (1989). A latent class model of
smoking experimentation in children. Journal of Child Psychology
log[odds of disorder in individual i according to and Psychiatry 30, 761-774.
measure Y\ Goodwin, D. G. & Guze, S. B. (1984). Psychiatric Diagnosis. Oxford
= log{Pr[y(i)=l]/Pr[y(i) = 0]} University Press: New York.
= a + bZ(i) (1) Henn, F. A., Bardwell, R. & Jenkins, R. L. (1980). Juvenile
delinquents revisited. Archives of General Psychiatry 37,1160 1163.
where Y(i) and Z{i) were the observed and latent Hill, J., Harrington, R., Fudge, H., Rutter, M. & Pickles, A. (1989).
The Adult Personality Functioning Assessment: development and
classification of the individual. Initial explo- reliability. British Journal of Psychiatry 155, 24-35.
ration of the measures using a random effects Loeber, R. (1988). Behavioural precursors and accelerators of
logistic regression model suggested that the delinquency. In Explaining Crime (ed. W. Buikhuisen and S. A.
number of the parameters could be substantially Mendick), pp. 51 67. Brill: Leiden.
Loeber, R. (1990). Development and risk factors of juvenile antisocial
reduced by making them equal across several behaviour and delinquency. Clinical Psychology Review 10, 1 4 1 .
measures. Table A1 shows the final set of 11 free Loeber, R. & Hay, D. (1993). Developmental approaches to
parameters estimated. aggression and conduct problems. In Developmental Principles and
986 M. Zoccolillo and others

Clinical Issues in Psychology and Psychiatry (ed. M. Rutter and D. completion by teachers: preliminary findings. Journal of Child
Hay). Blackwell Scientific: Oxford. (In the press.) Psychology and Psychiatry 8, I I I .
McCullagh, P. (1977). Regression models for ordered data. Journal Rutter, M. (1987). Temperament, personality and personality
of the Royal Statistical Society B 42, 109-142. disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry ISO, 443 -458.
McCullagh, P. & Nelder, J. A. (1983). Generalized Linear Models. Rutter, M. & Giller, H. (eds) (1983). juvenile Delinquency: Trends
Chapman and Hall: London. and Perspectives. Penguin: Harmondsworth, Middlesex.
Magnusson, D. (ed). (1988). Paths Through Life: A Longitudinal Rutter, M. & Quinton, D. (1981). Longitudinal studies of institutional
Research Program. Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ. children and children of mentally ill parents (United Kingdom). In
Numerical Algorithms Group (1989). NAG Fortran Subroutine Prospective Longitudinal Research: An Empirical Basis for the
Library, Mark 16. Numerical Algorithms Group: Oxford. Primary Prevention of Psychosocial Disorders (ed. S. A. Mednick
Parker, J. G. & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal and A. E. Baert), pp. 297-305. Oxford University Press: Oxford.
adjustment: are low-accepted children at risk? Psychological Rutter, M. & Quinton, D. (1984). Parental psychiatric disorder:
Bulletin 102, 357 -389. effects on children. Psychological Medicine 14, 853 880.
Pope, H. G., Jones, J. M., Hudson, J. I., Cohen, B. M. & Gunderson, Rutter, M. & Wolkind S. (1973). Children who have been in care.
J . G . (1983). The validity of DSM-1II borderline personality Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 14, 97 105.
disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 40, 23-30. Rutter, M., Tizard, J. & Whitmore, K. (eds) (1970). Education Health
Quinton, D. & Rutter, M. (1988). Parenting Breakdown: The Making and Behaviour. Longmans: London.
and Breaking of Inter-Generational Links. Avebury: Aldershot. Rutter, M., MacDonald, H., Le Couteur, A., Harrington, R.,
Quinton, D., Rutter, M. & Liddle, C. (1984). Institutional rearing, Bolton, P. & Bailey, A. (1990a). Genetic factors inchild psychiatric
parenting difficulties and marital support. Psychological Medicine disorders II Empirical findings. Journal of Child Psychology and
14, 107-124. Psychiatry 31, 39-83.
Quinton, D., Gulliver, L. & Rutter, M. (1990). Continuities in Rutter, M., Quinton, D. & Hill, J. (I990A) Adult outcome of
psychiatric disorders from childhood to adulthood in the children institution-reared children: Males and females compared. In
of psychiatric patients. In Straight and Devious Pathways from Straight and Devious Pathways from Childhood to Adulthood (ed. L.
Childhood to Adulthood (ed. L. Robins and M. Rulter), Robins and M. Rutter), pp, 135 157. Cambridge University
pp. 259-278. Cambridge University Press: New York. Press: New York.
Robins, L. N. (1966). Deviant Children Grown Up. Williams and Soloff, P. H. & Millward, J. W. (1983). Developmental histories of
Wilkins: Baltimore. borderline patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry 24, 574 588.
Robins, L. N. (1978). Sturdy childhood predictors of adult antisocial Wolkind, S. & Renton, G. (1979). Psychiatric disorders in long-term
behaviour: replications from longitudinal studies. Psychological residential care: a follow-up study. British Journal of Psychiatry
MedicineS, 611-622. 135, 129-135.
Robins, L. N. (1986). The consequences of conduct disorder in girls. World Health Organization (1978). International Classification of
In Development of Antisocial and Prosocial Behaviour: Research, Diseases (9th edn.). WHO: Geneva.
Theories and Issues (ed. D. Olweus, J. Block and M. Radke- Young, M. A. (1983). Evaluating diagnostic criteria: a latent class
Yarrow), pp. 385414. Academic Press: New York. paradigm. Journal of Behavioural Research 17, 285 296.
Robins, L. N. (1991). Conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology Zeitlin, H. (1986). The Natural History of Disorder in Childhood.
and Psychiatry Annual Review 32, 193-212. Institute of Psychiatry/Maudsley Monograph No. 29. Oxford
Robins, L. N. & McEvoy, L. (1990). Conduct problems as predictors University Press: Oxford.
of substance abuse. In Straight and Devious Pathways from Zoccolillo, M. & Rogers, K. (1991). Characteristics and outcome of
Childhood to Adulthood (ed. L. Robins and M. Rutter), hospitalized adolescent girls with conduct disorder. Journal of the
pp. 182-204. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 30,973 981.
Russell, G. F. M. & Hersov, L. (eds) (1983). Handbook of Psychiatry Zoccolillo, M. S. & Cloninger, C. R. (1986). Somatization disorder:
4: The Neuroses and Personality Disorders. Cambridge University psychologic symptoms, social disability, and diagnosis. Com-
Press: Cambridge. prehensive Psychiatry 27, 65 73.
Rutter, M. (1967). A children's behaviour questionnaire for

You might also like