You are on page 1of 6

COLOR FORUM

Colour Psychology and Colour Therapy:


Caveat Emptor

Zena OConnor*
Environment-Behaviour Studies Research Group, Faculty of Architecture, Design and Planning, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia

Received 17 April 2009; revised 9 September 2009; accepted 11 September 2009

Abstract: Information about colour psychology and THE INTERFACE BETWEEN COLOUR AND HUMAN
colour therapy abounds in popular culture. Articles found RESPONSE: CURRENT RESEARCH
in mass media as well as a plethora of Internet websites
Academic publications include a broad range of studies
are often devoted to colour and a range of psychological,
that discuss the effects of light, as well as the inuence of
biological, and behavioral effects. The information avail-
coloured light-waves in respect to human response. While
able, which varies from scant summaries with catchy titles
human vision is a complex and not yet fully understood
to lengthy and comprehensive discussions, is often pre-
process, the receptor system for detecting light has been
sented in an authoritative manner exhorting the reader to
found to be different from that associated with the circa-
believe a range of claims such as red is physically stimu-
dian cycle.1 Light inuences the human circadian rhythm
lating and arousing and blue is calming, relaxing and
and changes in light-dark exposure can desynchronize the
healing. However, empirical evidence or evidence of any
nature is only rarely cited and, when it is, its often in ref- circadian cycle affecting the ability to sleep and wake, as
erence to ndings that are inappropriately generalized or well as impacting on physiological and metabolic proc-
out-of-date and superseded. This article discusses a range esses. Furthermore, disruptions to the circadian rhythm
of colour psychology and colour therapy claims found in may result in changes in mood and behavior as evidenced
popular culture and notes the similarities between these by studies that focus on seasonal affective disorder
claims and the writings of early theorists such as Birren, (SAD).24 Light has also been found to have an effect on
Goldstein, Gerard and Luscher. While these authors have the human neuroendocrine system and may also suppress
a valid if somewhat dated place in the literature on colour, melatonin and elevate cortisol production, both of which
there are a number of key reasons to be wary of colour may have negative impacts.4,5 Furthermore, a number of
psychology and colour therapy claims made in popular recent studies have indicated that certain wavelengths of
culture. These reasons are discussed in some detail and light may have specic impacts. For example, blue light
provide compelling support for applying the caveat emptor may improve cognitive performance; different coloured
principle to colour psychology and colour therapy claims lenses may assist with reading difculties such as dys-
found in nonacademic sources. 2010 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. lexia; and the human circadian system may be particularly
Col Res Appl, 36, 229 234, 2011; Published online 29 April 2010 in sensitive to short wavelength light.68 Despite many
Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI 10.1002/col.20597 advances in recent research, the precise roles of the rods
and cones of the retina as well as melanopsin in the con-
Key words: psychology; physiology; health; color theory trol of circadian cycles remains to be determined.9
In addition, a plethora of studies exist which suggest
*Correspondence to: Zena OConnor (e-mail: zena.designresearch@ that colour may inuence a range of psychological, physi-
gmail.com). ological, and behavioral responses, and the range and
diversity in terms of the ndings from such studies is
V
C 2010 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. highlighted by an analysis of 30 studies conducted by

Volume 36, Number 3, June 2011 229


Mikellides.10 For example, it has been suggested that red place . . . The use of color in visualization is most
has a greater capacity for arousal than blue11,12; however, effective, and easiest for the novice to utilise, as color
ndings from a more recent study suggest that there is no has a very strong radiating effect on the whole body.
statistically signicant difference between these two col- Every other form of color therapy is fundamentally
ours in terms of physiological arousal and that it may be symbolic.
hue rather than saturation (intensity) of colour that has an
In a similar vein, Wright17 suggests that eleven
impact.13 In addition, recent studies have found that
basic colours have fundamental psychological properties
responses to colour may vary depending on age, gender,
that are universal, regardless of which particular shade,
culture, and preference.14,15
tone or tint you are using. Each of them has potentially
It is important to note that while many of the recent
positive or negative psychological effects and which of
studies that focus on psychological, physiological, and
these effects is created depends on the relationships
behavioral responses are scientically rigorous, the nd-
within colour combinations. While, Rewell18 provides
ings are often based on an extremely limited range of
a somewhat more detailed discussion about specic
colour samples and/or a small sample group. In addition,
responses to colour in the online WellBeing Magazine
while associations may exist between various colours and
as follows.
a range of different human responses, it does not neces-
sarily follow that such colours can be effectively used in Babies cry more in yellow rooms. Tension increases
therapy or related applications as some sources in popular in people in yellow rooms and people who drive
media suggest. yellow cars are more prone to become aggravating in
heavy traffic . . . Spend time exposed to a lot of
yellow and youll feel like time has sped up . . . A
COLOUR PSYCHOLOGY AND COLOUR THERAPY IN
rejection of yellow indicates a fear of change.
POPULAR CULTURE
Red stimulates the physical and adrenalin. It raises
Sources of information about colour psychology and blood pressure, the heart rate and respiration.
colour therapy in popular culture include mainstream
magazines, online magazines, and Internet websites. Other sources of information about colour psychology
These sources often provide information relating to var- and colour therapy found in popular culture include ar-
ious psychological, biological, and behavioral effects of chitectural and interior design books (for example, see
colour. Internet search engine Google currently provides Kopacz19 and Mahnke20) as well as technical reports,
access to 365,000 websites for information relating to such as those by Hill.21 In addition, a range of short
colour psychology and 284,000 websites for informa- and long term courses and workshops are available
tion relating to colour therapy. For example, www. such as the colour therapy workshops offered by the
about.com and www.colortherapyhealing.com offer Colour Therapy Healing organization in the UK.22 Sim-
numerous pages on colour psychology and colour heal- ilarly, the International School of Colour and Design23
ing, respectively, with the latter providing fairly detailed in Sydney incorporates a colour psychology component
information about the use of colour as a therapeutic within its basic, diploma and distance learning courses;
tool and a range of colour therapy workshops. Simi- and the Nature Care College24 offers a number of
larly, www.colour-affects.co.uk offers detailed informa- colour-related modules as part of its Diploma of Ener-
tion about the psychological properties of colour and getic Healing and Aura Soma 1 colour practitioner
provides a summary of four personality types linked to course.
specic colours. In addition, mass media magazines and
online magazines, such as Conscious Living Magazine DEFINING COLOUR PSYCHOLOGY AND
(www.consciousliving.net.au) and WellBeing Magazine COLOUR THERAPY
(www.wellbeing.com.au) feature articles on colour psy-
The term colour psychology, often used interchangeably
chology and colour therapy.
with the psychological effects of color as suggested by
The information available from popular culture sour-
Van Wagner,25 tends to refer to a range of affective, cog-
ces can vary from broad, generalized articles to detailed
nitive, and behavioral responses and associations linked to
pseudo-scientic discussions. Occasionally, such articles
specic colours. While this denition of colour psychol-
and discussions may be peppered with generalizations,
ogy remains fairly consistent across mainstream media
platitudes, and motherhoods statements such as Colors
and internet websites, some sources extend the denition
are the mother tongue of the subconscious or Color
to include a larger gamut of responses such as colour
heals. A typical example is the following by Camp-
preference, as well as precognitive and biology-based
bell16 which featured in the online magazine Conscious
responses, dened by Kopacz to mean the inuence of
Living.
colour on human metabolism, circulation, and respiratory
Color can repair and heal the body, when the systems (p 92).
frequency of the color aligns with the emotion needed In reference to colour therapy, Kopacz suggests that the
to activate the microparticulars so healing can take biological consequences of colour responses can be a

230 COLOR research and application


valuable tool in health management for the treatment of energising, therefore, it is helpful for tiredness and leth-
various ailments, and this appears to be a fairly common argy, to stimulate low blood pressure, to boost sluggish
understanding of the term (p 93). While Logan-Clarke circulation . . . Red is energising and excites the emotions,
and Appleby26 suggest that colour therapy, or chromather- and can stimulate the appetite (p 10). Similarly, Rewell
apy, can be used prescriptively as an holistic, noninva- contends Red stimulates the physical and adrenalin. It
sive, and powerful therapy (p 1). raises blood pressure, the heart rate and respiration
Across most popular culture sources, the range of (p32); and Kopacz suggests Red is believed to
responses linked to colour varies enormously by type and sensitise the taste buds and sense of smell, increasing the
extent. That is, symbolic colour associations and colour appetite . . . all this occurs because the heart rate instinc-
meanings are often mentioned in conjunction with a range tively quickens, which causes a release of adrenalin into
of psychological responses to colour including affect, the bloodstream raising blood pressure and stimulating
preference, and cognitive judgements; and these in turn the nerves (p 76); and the sight of the colour blue
are often comingled with biological and behavioral causes the body to release tranquilising hormones when it
responses to colour. An example of the intermingling of a is surveyed, particularly a strong blue sky and many
range of different types of response is provided by Van believe (blue) can lower blood pressure, slow the pulse
Wagner as follows. rate and decrease body temperature (p 79).
While a number of sources note that colour psychology
Red is a bright, warm colour that evokes strong information is often based on anecdotal evidence, many
emotions; Red is also considered an intense, or even imply that various colour-related claims have sound em-
angry, colour that creates feelings of excitement or pirical support as in Hills assertion above. Similarly,
intensity. Kopacz notes the lack of evidential support but offers the
Blue calls to mind feelings of calmness or seren- work of eight writers, psychologists, and designers includ-
ity. It is often described as peaceful, tranquil, ing Birren and Mahnke as well as Morton Walker (author
secure, and orderly. Blue can also create feelings of Bald No More, Foods for Fabulous Sex, Your Guide to
of sadness or aloofness. Blue is often used to deco- Foot Health as well as The Power of Color) and Wright
rate ofces because research has shown that people (colour consultant of www.colour-affects.co.uk) as sup-
are more productive in blue rooms. port for colour psychology and colour healing informa-
Green is restful, soothing, cheerful and health-giv- tion. Similarly, Mahnke notes the inconclusive nature of
ing. Green is thought to relieve stress and help heal. ndings from research that focuses on the interface
Those who have a green work environment experi- between colour and human response, and also refers to
ence fewer stomach aches. Green has long been a the work of Birren and Goldstein among others as support
symbol of fertility (p 57). for the psychophysiological effects of colour.
In regard to colour healing, it is suggested that colour
Similarly, symbolic associations of colour are inter- can be used as a treatment tool in conjunction with the
mingled with psychological, physiological, and behavior seven chakras of the body.19,20,26 The notion of chakras,
responses in the following example from Logan-Clarke considered to be energy centres within the human body,
and Appleby. belong to a belief system originating from the Hindu
Red is the colour for courage, strength, and pio- scriptures known as the Upanishads, dating from the rst
neering spirit . . . It is the colour of anger, violence millennium BCE. Under the colour-chakra theory, a
and brutality. colour is linked to each of the seven chakra and these col-
ours are associated with body functions and dysfunctions
Blue is calming, relaxing, and healing (but) not as
within each chakra area. For example,
sedentary as indigo.
Green is the colour of balance and harmony and Red: Activates the circulation system and benets
can, therefore, be helpful in times of stress. If one the ve senses; used to treat colds, paralysis, anaemia,
has experienced trauma, a green silk wrapped around ailments of the bloodstream and ailments of the lung;
the shoulders can have a very therapeutic effect Blue: Raises metabolism; is used to stabilize the
(p 5,6,9). heart, muscles and bloodstream; used to treat burns,
skin diseases, glaucoma, measles and chicken pox,
Sources of information in popular culture tend to suggest
and throat problems;
that an irrefutable, causal relationship exists between colour
Green: Strengthens bones and muscles, disinfects
and human response as an example from Hill illustrates:
bacteria and virus, and relieves tension; used to treat
Based on numerous studies by Drs. Morton Walker,
malaria, back problems, cancer, nervous disorders,
Gerard, and Faber Birren, the link between colour and
and ulcers, and to manage heart problems and blood
physiological responses has been well documented (p 7).
pressure.19
In addition, most sources imply that the link between
colour and human response is universal irrespective of The allocation of colours to each of the chakras is rem-
individual or cultural differences. For example, Logan- iniscent of the doctrine of the four colour-linked humors
Clarke and Appleby assert: Red . . . is stimulating and of the body from ancient Greek medicine: black bile,

Volume 36, Number 3, June 2011 231


yellow bile, green phlegm and red blood. The linking of referred to as psychological primaries) in the colour test:
colour with the humors, the four elements (earth, re, Orange-red, bright yellow, blue-green and dark-blue; as
water and air) as well as the seasons, was a belief system well as four auxiliary colours: violet, brown, black, and
that continued through to the Renaissance as evidenced neutral grey. Luscher, citing anecdotal evidence, assigned
by the colour correspondences depicted in the engraving specic associations and affective characteristics to each
by Nicoletto Rosex.27 colour. For example, red has a stimulating effect on the
Aside from ancient belief systems, much of the infor- nervous system, blood pressure increases, respiration rate
mation currently available in popular culture about colour and heartbeat both speed up; while blue has the reverse
psychology and colour therapy echo, if not directly quote, effect, blood pressure falls, heartbeat and breathing both
the work of a number of key earlier theorists such as slow down (p 12). The Luscher Color Test is essentially
Birren,28,29 Gerard,30 Goldstein,31,32 and Luscher.33 a colour preference test and its use in personality testing
and assessment has been cautioned by Walters et al.36
While the theories of Birren, Gerard, Goldstein, and
BIRREN, GERARD, GOLDSTEIN, AND LUSCHER
Luscher have their place in the literature on colour, it is
Extensively quoted by Kopacz and Mahnke mong others, important to note that their theories have, to a large
Faber Birren published more than 40 books and over 250 extent, been superseded. It is unfortunate that despite the
articles on colour psychology, colour therapy as well as breadth and depth of contemporary empirical studies that
functional colour and colour vision. Tunney34 advises that focus on the interface between humans and colour or col-
Birren was a leading authority on colour in the mid 20th oured light, the ndings of these studies rarely, if ever,
century who was retained as a colour consultant by appear in the information currently available in popular
Dupont, General Electric, Sears & Roebuck and the culture. This and a number of other reasons discussed in
United States Navy. In Color psychology and color ther- detail below provide a compelling case for applying the
apy, Birren28 suggests that an unambiguous and universal principle of caveat emptor in respect to colour psychology
causal link exists between colour and human response, and colour therapy information found in popular culture.
and he cites Goldsteins assertion that It is probably not
a false statement if we say that a specic color stimula-
Caveat emptor
tion is accompanied by a specic response pattern of the
entire (human) organism. (p 144). Latin for buyer beware, caveat emptor implies that in
Kurt Goldstein published The Organism31 in 1939 and the absence of a warranty, the buyer is at risk and the
was considered a highly inuential authority on the psy- onus for carefully assessing goods and services before
chological aspects of colour by Birren, who included sev- purchase remains with the buyer. There are a number of
eral excerpts from Goldstein in his Color psychology and key reasons why the principle of caveat emptor should
color therapy publication such as under the inuence of prevail in regard to colour psychology and colour therapy
red light, time is likely to be overestimated. Conversely, information found in popular culture.
under the inuence of green or blue light, time is likely to First, while ndings from research suggest that colour
be underestimated and under red light, weights will be inuences human response, the existence of an irrefutable
judged as heavier; under green light they will be judged and universal causal link between colour and an unlimited
lighter (p 211). Birren as well Mahnke cite Goldsteins range of psychological, biological, and behavioral
1942 study,32 which discussed the perceived stimulating responses remains an unsupported hypothesis. Wise et al.
effects of red and the opposite effects of green. assert that there are no hard-wired linkages between
Robert Gerard,12,30 whose 1958 doctoral thesis (Differ- environmental colours and particular judgemental or emo-
ential effects of colored lights on psychophysical func- tional states (p 46). In addition, as aforementioned, a
tions) was subsequently presented as a conference paper range of studies exist which support a link between colour
(Color and emotional arousal), is also frequently cited by and human response; however, the ndings are limited
Birren and Mahnke. Gerards key ndings from his study by, and contingent on, the setting and context of the
on the arousal properties of red, blue, and white illumina- study, the size and composition of the sample group, as
tion, as reported by Wise et al.,35 include: statistical dif- well as the limited range of colours used in the stimuli.
ferences between red-blue (illumination) conditions for all Furthermore, the ndings of many such studies are
physiological measures except heart rate-responses to the also limited because of the methodological weakness of
white light varied but most often were similar to those of studying a complex and subjective phenomenon such as
the red condition (p 5). Wise et al. also note that colour in isolation.
Gerard, whose study involved a small sample of 24 male Another reason to be wary of some colour psychology
university students, advised caution in terms of the gener- and colour therapy claims is the notion that the ancient
alizability of his ndings. It is unfortunate that Gerards wisdom embedded within some claims represents eviden-
advice was not reported by subsequent authors such as tial proof. For example, Kopacz cites the link between
Birren or Mahnke. colour and charkas per ancient Hindu scriptures; and Van
Max Luscher,33 who developed and published the Wagner suggests that chromatherapy was practiced by
Luscher Color Test, included four basic colours (which he several ancient cultures, including the Egyptians and

232 COLOR research and application


Chinese. The implication being that not only is the wis- individuals personality, cultural experiences and affective
dom of the ancients above question but it provides evi- state. For example, an individuals stimulus screening
dential proof of the veracity of some colour-related ability is rarely mentioned in colour psychology and
claims. It is important to note that ancient belief systems colour healing information found in popular culture. Meh-
are not always a guarantee of veracity as evidenced by rabian42 found that individual differences exist in terms of
the superseded beliefs that the world is at, and the sun stimulus screening ability, and high screeners are able to
and moon orbit the earth. While not intending to dispar- automatically screen out less important components of
age the wisdom of the ancients, a fair proportion of this environmental stimuli such as colour and sound as
wisdom has been superseded by later scientic discovery opposed to low screeners. Therefore, it is likely that an
and the existence of a link with ancient wisdom should individuals stimulus screening ability may negate claims
not of itself be used as evidential proof of any claim. may about responses to colour in popular culture.
A third reason to be cautious about colour psychology Finally, a seventh reason to question some colour psy-
and colour therapy information is the practice of occa- chology and colour therapy information is the inherent
sionally presenting factoids as facts. Coined by author bias that is often evident. Referred to as subjective valida-
Norman Mailer, factoids are suppositions or inventions tion, this occurs when two unrelated events are perceived
presented as fact as illustrated by the Ferrell and McKay37 or judged to be related because of an expectation of such,
line in Anchorman: Its anchorman not anchor-lady, and or that an existing hypothesis demands a relationship. For
thats a scientic fact! Colour psychology and colour example, to apply the notion that green relieves stress
therapy information available in popular culture occasion- to the following claims: green silk wrapped around the
ally appear to be factoids presented as facts, such shoulders can have a very therapeutic effect and Those
Rewells18 assertions that: We are hard-wired to yellow who have a green work environment experience fewer
as a stimulus . . . If your environment is boring and time stomach aches indicates subjective validation and ren-
passes slowly, surround yourself with small amounts of ders the internal validity of such claims awed.
yellow and People who dislike yellow often favour To conclude, colour psychology and colour therapy
blue to calm themselves and feel secure. If you drink cof- claims information found in popular culture suggest that
fee for a pick-me-up, try drinking it from a yellow cup. colour prompts a range of different human responses: psy-
Similarly, Van Wagner25 claims: Yellow can also create chological, biological, and behavioral. Many of these
feelings of frustration and anger. While it is considered a claims lack substantiation in terms of empirical support,
cheerful colour, people are more likely to lose their tem- exhibit fundamental aws (such as causal oversimplica-
pers in yellow rooms and babies tend to cry more in yel- tion and subjective validation), and may include factoids
low rooms. Without substantiation or empirical evi- presented as facts. In addition, such claims often refer to
dence, such claims remain factoids not facts. outdated research without referring to current research
An additional reason to be wary of colour psychology ndings. It is therefore advisable to apply the principle of
and colour therapy information found in popular culture caveat emptor when evaluating magazine articles and
has to do with the level of external and ecological validity Internet websites that focus on colour psychology and
embedded within such information, whether or not such colour therapy, plus check with a medical practitioner or
information is supported by empirical evidence. To psychologist before accepting or acting upon such
explain, while various studies exist that focus on the inter- information.
face between human response and colour, Arygrous,38
Campbell and Stanley,39 and Coolican40 consider it
empirically and methodologically unsound to draw gener- 1. Brainard GC, Hanin JP, Greeson JM, Byrne B, Glickman G, Gerner
E, Rollag MD. Action spectrum for melatonin regulation in humans,
alised conclusions from such studies and transfer them to
evidence for a novel circadian photoreceptor. J Neurosci 2001;21:
different settings or contexts. 64056412.
The fallacy of the single cause, which provides a fth 2. Kasper S, Wehr TA, Bartko JJ, Gaist PA, Rosenthal NE. Epidemio-
reason to be cautious about colour psychology and colour logical ndings of seasonal changes in mood and behavior. Arch
therapy claims, suggests that one single cause for an out- Gen Psychiatry 1989;46:823833.
come represents causal oversimplication. Under the post- 3. Harmatz MG, Well AD, Overtree CE, Kawamura KY, Rosal M,
Ockene IS. Seasonal variation of depression and other moods: A lon-
positivist paradigm, recent theorists such as Hard and
gitudinal approach. J Biol Rhythms 2000;15:344350.
Sivik41 consider the interface between colour and human 4. Stevens RG, Blask DE, Brainard GC, Hansen J, Lockley SW,
response to be highly complex and open to the inuence Provencio I, Rea MS, Reinlib L. Meeting report: The role of
of a wide range of factors and mediating variables. While environmental lighting and circadian disruption in cancer and other
correlation may indicate the existence of an association diseases. Environ Health Persepect 2007;115:13571362.
between one variable (such as colour) and human 5. Skene DJ, Lockley SW, Thapan K, Arendt J. Effects of light on
human circadian rhythms. Reprod Nutr Dev 1999;39:295304.
response, proving a causal link without allowing for medi-
6. Irlen H. Reading problems and Irlen colored lenses. Dyslexia review
ating variables and other inuences is now considered 1997;8:47.
empirically and methodologically unsound.38,40 7. Lehrl S, Gerstmeyer K, Jacob JH, Frieling H, Henkel AW, Meyrer
Furthermore, key mediating variables in terms of the R, Wiltfang J, Kornhuber J, Bleich S. Blue light improves cognitive
interface between colour and human response include an performance. J Neural Transm 2007;114:457460.

Volume 36, Number 3, June 2011 233


8. Warman VL, Dijk DJ, Warman GR, Arendt J, Skene DJ. Phase 25. Van Wagner K. Color psychology: How colors impact moods, feel-
advancing human circadian rhythms with short wavelength light. ings and behaviours. Psychology. Volume 2009: Available at:
Neurosci Lett 2003;342:3740. About.com; Accessed on 2009.
9. Webb A. Considerations for lighting in the built environment: Non- 26. Logan-Clarke V, Appleby J. What is color therapy? Color Therapy
visual effects of light. Energ Build 2006;38:721727. Healing Workshops. Bognor Regis, UK; 2009.
10. Mikellides B. Emotional and behavioural reactions to color. In: Sivik 27. Gage J. Color and Culture. London: Thames & Hudson; 1995.
L, editor. Color and Psychology: AIC Interim Meeting. Gothenburg, 28. Birren F. Color Psychology and Color Therapy. Secaucus, NJ: Cita-
Sweden: Scandinavian Color Institute AB; 1996. del Press; 1961.
11. Ali MR. Pattern of EEG recovery under photic stimulation by light 29. Birren F. Color & Human Response. New York: Van Nostrand Rein-
of different colors. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1972;33: hold; 1978.
332335. 30. Gerard RM. Color and emotional arousal (Abstract from the program
12. Gerard RM. Differential effects of colored lights on psychophysio- of the sixty-sixth annual convention of the American Psychological
logical functions, PhD Thesis. Los Angeles: University of California; Association). Am Psychol 1958;13:340.
1958. 31. Goldstein K. The organism: A holistic approach to biology derived
13. Mikellides B. Color and psychological arousal. J Architec Planning from pathological data in man. New York: Zone; 1939/1995.
Res 1990;7:1320.
32. Goldstein K. Some experimental observations concerning the inu-
14. Manav B. Color-emotion associations and color preferences: A case
ence of colors on the function of the organism. Occup Ther Rehabil
study for residences. Color Res Appl 2007;32:144150.
1942;21:147151.
15. Ou LC, Luo MR, Woodcock A, Wright A. A study of colour emo-
33. Luscher M. The Luscher color test. New York: Random House; 1969.
tion and colour preference. Part III: Color preference modelling.
34. Tunney S. Guide to the Faber Birren Papers. New Haven, Connecti-
Color Res Appl 2004;29:381389.
cut: Yale University Press; 2006.
16. Campbell L. Mind body healingHow it works. Conscious Living
35. Wise BK, Wise JA, Beach LR. The human factors of color in envi-
Magazine. Available at: http://www.consciousliving.net.au; Accessed
ronmental design: A critical review. NASA Grant No. NCC 2404.
on 2009.
Moffett Field, CA: NASA Ames Research Centre; 1988.
17. Wright A. Psychological properties of colours. Color Affects. Avail-
able at: http://www.colour-affects.co.uk); Accessed on 2007. 36. Walters J, Apter MJ, Svebak S. Color preference, arousal, and the
18. Rewell C. Hue are you?: WellBeing Magazine. Available at: http:// theory of psychological reversals. Motiv Emot 1982;6:193215.
www.wellbeing.com.au; Accessed on 2007. 37. Ferrell W, McKay A. Anchorman: The legend of Ron Burgundy.
19. Kopacz J. Color in Three-Dimensional Design. New York: McGraw- USA: Dreamworks; 2004.
Hill; 2003. 38. Argyrous G. Statistics for Social and Health Research. London:
20. Mahnke F. Color, Environment and Human Response. New York: Sage; 2001.
Wiley; 1996. 39. Campbell DT, Stanley JC. Experimental and Quasi-experimental
21. Hill TR. Using Color to Create Healing Environments. Atlanta, GA: Designs for Research. Boston: Houghton Mifin; 1963.
Little Fish Think Tank/Dupont; 2008. 40. Coolican H. Research Methods and Statistics in Psychology. London:
22. CTH. Color Therapy Healing workshop. Color Therapy Healing. Avail- Hodder & Stoughton; 2004.
able at: http://www.colortherapyhealing.com/; Accessed on 2009. 41. Hard A, Sivik L. A theory of colors in combinationA descriptive
23. ISCD. International School of Colour and Design. Sydney: Interna- model related to the NCS color-order system. Color Res Appl 2001;26:
tional School of Colour and Design; 2009. Available at: http:// 428.
www.iscd.edu.au/default2.asp. 42. Mehrabian A. Individual differences in stimulus screening and arous-
24. NCC. Nature Care College. 2009. Available at: http://naturecare.com.au/. ability. J Pers 1977;45:237250.

234 COLOR research and application

You might also like