You are on page 1of 10

Behavioural Brain Research 222 (2011) 236245

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behavioural Brain Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr

Review

A bird-brain view of episodic memory


N.C. Rattenborg , D. Martinez-Gonzalez
Max Planck Institute for Ornithology Seewiesen, Sleep and Flight Group, Eberhard-Gwinner-Strasse, 82319 Seewiesen, Germany

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In humans, the hippocampus plays a critical role in the formation of episodic memories. Although non-
Received 28 February 2011 human animals are unable to report whether they also re-experience past events, at least some birds
Accepted 11 March 2011 and mammals exhibit episodic-like memory characterized by an ability to recall what happened where
Available online 21 March 2011
and when. In mammals, the hippocampus interacts closely with virtually the entire neocortex to form
episodic-like memories. The hippocampus receives highly processed information from high-order asso-
Keywords:
ciation areas, and thereby the rest of the neocortex. Distinct neurophysiological hippocampal rhythms
Dorsal ventricular ridge
(theta and sharp-wave ripples) coordinate activity between the hippocampus and high-order associ-
Nidopallium
Mesopallium
ation areas during the encoding and retrieval of information contributing to episodic-like memories.
Prefrontal cortex Although recent studies have demonstrated that food hoarding birds are able to remember what food
Sleep they hid where and when, neuroanatomical and neurophysiological studies suggest that there may be a
fundamental difference between episodic-like memory in birds and mammals. In contrast to the mam-
malian hippocampus, the avian hippocampus only receives visual and olfactory input; most high-order
association areas in the avian brain involved in performing functions similar to those performed by neo-
cortical association areas do not project to the hippocampus or structures providing it with direct input.
Consistent with this neuroanatomical difference, mammalian-like rhythms involved in communicating
between the hippocampus and neocortical high-order association areas have not been found in birds.
Collectively, this suggests that information contributing to episodic-like memory is more limited and
processed in a different manner in birds when compared to mammals.
2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 236
2. Neural substrates of episodic-like memory in mammals and birds . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 237
2.1. Mammals . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 237
2.1.1. Neuroanatomy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 237
2.1.2. Neurophysiology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 237
2.2. Birds . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 239
2.2.1. Neuroanatomy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 239
2.2.2. Neurophysiology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 241
3. Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 242
Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 242
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 242

1. Introduction hoarding birds [2]. During the last 12 years, a growing body of work
has shown that food hoarding birds, such as Western scrub-jays
The human capacity to recall specic personal events from the (Aphelocoma coerulescens), black-billed magpies (Pica pica), and
past is referred to as episodic memory [1]. A recent special issue black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus), are able to remem-
in Behavioural Brain Research highlighted advances in research ber what food they hid where and when [28]. These essential
on episodic memory, including seminal work on memory in food components of episodic memory are stored in an integrated man-
ner, such that retrieval of one component automatically activates
the other components, and thereby possibly the entire memory
Corresponding author. Tel.: +49 8157 932 279; fax: +49 8157 932 344. of a specic event [911]. This integrated form of what, where,
E-mail address: rattenborg@orn.mpg.de (N.C. Rattenborg). when memory is typically referred to as episodic-like memory in

0166-4328/$ see front matter 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.030
N.C. Rattenborg, D. Martinez-Gonzalez / Behavioural Brain Research 222 (2011) 236245 237

non-human animals that cannot verbally report whether they re-


experienced the past event [6,7]. Although it is unknown whether Neocorcal Associaon Areas
PFC
birds experience episodic memories like those experienced by (Dorsal Pallium)
humans [1], the nding that jays plan for future needs [12] suggests
that they have some capacity for mental time travel ([13], but see
[14]), and therefore might use similar cognitive processes to travel
mentally back in time when retrieving hidden food.
The evidence for episodic-like memory in food hoarding birds is Perirhinal Parahippocampal
compelling. However, little is known about how the avian brain Cortex Cortex
processes such memories. In this review, we examine whether
birds and mammals use similar brain areas and processes to gener-
Entorhinal
ate episodic-like memories. Although extensive evidence indicates
Cortex
that the avian hippocampus is involved in processing and storing
spatial information contributing to episodic-like memory [1517],
the potential role of interactions between the hippocampus and
high-order association areas has been largely overlooked. Conse-
Piriform
quently, we focus on this aspect of episodic-like memory. Hippocampus
Cortex
Mammals and birds independently evolved large brains [18,19]
capable of performing complex cognitive processes [2023]. In both Fig. 1. Extensive connections between the mammalian hippocampus and neocorti-
groups, encephalization was due primarily to expansion of the pal- cal association areas. Virtually all association areas in the neocortex (dorsal pallium)
lium, the dorsal part of the neural tube. However, the resulting are reciprocally connected to the hippocampus via the parahippocampal, perirhi-
pallial organization differs markedly between mammals and birds. nal, and entorhinal cortices. The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is thought to inuence the
ow of information to and from the hippocampus via reciprocal connections to
The majority of the mammalian pallium consists of the laminar neo-
each of these regions. The hippocampus communicates with association regions,
cortex, whereas in birds it is organized mainly as nuclear structures and thereby the rest of the neocortex, via the entorhinal, parahippocampal, and
[21,24]. Although recent studies have emphasized the convergent perirhinal cortices. The hippocampus also provides direct input to the PFC.
aspects of cognition, divergence in pallial organization and related
neurophysiology suggest that there may be a fundamental differ-
ence between how pallial structures in mammals and birds process pocampal, perirhinal, and entorhinal cortices [27,35,38]. As a result,
information [25]. Notably, whereas the mammalian hippocampus rather than receiving direct sensory information related to an event,
interacts with virtually the entire neocortex, the available evidence the hippocampus receives highly processed multimodal associa-
suggests that interactions between the avian hippocampus and pal- tive information derived from virtually every neocortical area
lium are more limited, a difference in brain organization that may [27,39,40]. In conjunction with this hierarchical organization, the
have implications for our understanding of episodic-like memory internal circuitry and cellular physiology of the hippocampal sub-
in birds. regions, the dentate gyrus, CA1 and CA3, are thought to instill the
hippocampus with the capacity to rapidly encode [4143] unique
but similar events (pattern separation), and to reconstruct entire
2. Neural substrates of episodic-like memory in mammals episodes in response to activation of a small subset of neurons
and birds involved in the memory (pattern completion), requisites for the
formation and recall of episodic memories [44]. Finally, during
2.1. Mammals recall of an episode, the hippocampus is thought to re-instantiate
the experience via reciprocal connections to the entorhinal cortex,
2.1.1. Neuroanatomy and thereby neocortical areas initially involved in processing the
In humans, the formation of episodic memories for specic experience.
personal events depends on the hippocampus [1]. Patients with
lesions restricted to the hippocampus are unable to form new 2.1.2. Neurophysiology
episodic memories of recently experienced events, or to recall Brain rhythms are fundamental mechanisms through which the
events occurring shortly before the damage [2632]. The hip- brain coordinates information processing between areas [45,46].
pocampus also appears to be necessary for episodic-like memory During memory encoding and recall, the mammalian hippocam-
in non-human mammals [7,33]. The mammalian hippocampus is pus communicates with the neocortex via two mechanistically
connected to the neocortex in a manner that makes it ideally linked reciprocal modes of activity, the theta (612 Hz) rhythm
suited for forming episodic memories derived from information and sharp-wave ripples (SWRs) [47,48]. The hippocampal theta
processed throughout the neocortex [34,35]. Information destined rhythm occurs during translational movement in awake animals
for the hippocampus is conveyed via parallel sensory pathways and and during rapid-eye movement (REM) sleep [49,50]. Projections
then converging associational pathways. Specically, information from theta pacemaker cells in the medial septum to the hippocam-
passes rst through primary and then secondary sensory cortices, pus play an important role in the genesis of the hippocampal theta
association cortices, multimodal association cortices, and then the rhythm [50,51]. In rats, lesions to the medial septum that disrupt
parahippocampal and perirhinal cortices, and nally the entorhi- the hippocampal theta rhythm also disrupt the formation of spa-
nal cortex before entering the hippocampus (Fig. 1) [34,35]. As tial memories [5254]. During encoding, the theta rhythm may
information ows from primary sensory areas to the hippocam- contribute to episodic memories by providing spatial context and
pus it is processed at each step, becoming more associative in temporal order to memories [5557], although the exact mecha-
nature. Notably, rather than simply serving as a relay for infor- nism remains debated (reviewed in [55]). The encoding and retrieval
mation to the hippocampus, the parahippocampal, perirhinal, and of memories as an animal moves through the environment may
entorhinal cortices are also involved in forming high-order asso- occur at different phases of the theta rhythm [58], in conjunction
ciations contributing to episodic-like memories [32,36,37]. The with theta-phase synchronized gamma (30150 Hz) oscillations
prefrontal cortex (PFC) also inuences information entering the [5964]. The theta rhythm may also facilitate recall by synchroniz-
hippocampus via its reciprocal connections with the parahip- ing activity between the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and other
238 N.C. Rattenborg, D. Martinez-Gonzalez / Behavioural Brain Research 222 (2011) 236245

brains areas (reviewed in [55]). Of particular interest, the hippocam- a particular location [85]. As rats move through their environment,
pal theta rhythm synchronizes activity in the medial PFC (mPFC) place cells re as the animal traverses the place eld of each cell.
[6572], apparently via direct excitatory projections from CA1 Although much work has focused on the spatial properties of place
hippocampal neurons to the mPFC [7375]. Consistent with its pro- cells, they also appear to encode other aspects of an experience
posed executive role in retrieving context-relevant memories from contributing to episodic-like memories [27,8690]. Sequences of
long-term memory in the rest of the neocortex [76], the PFC may place cell ring that occur as an animal moves through its envi-
use information received directly from the hippocampus to mon- ronment are replayed during SWRs occurring during brief pauses
itor and lter information entering and leaving the hippocampus in behavior, quiet wakefulness, and SWS [9195]. Replay during
during exploration via its reciprocal connections to the parahip- wakefulness can occur in a forward or reverse order (reviewed in
pocampal, perirhinal, and entorhinal cortices [35,38,7779]. Lesion [91]), whereas that occurring during SWS-related SWRs occurs only
studies also suggest that the mPFC contributes to retrieving specic in a forward order [96]. Replay during pauses in waking behav-
components of episodic-like memories [33,80]. Finally, the theta ior may be involved in the early stages of processing hippocampal
rhythm occurring during REM sleep has also been implicated in information, or in the retrieval of information to guide future
processing hippocampal memories [81], perhaps by synchronizing behavior [91]. During SWS, replay occurs in a coordinated man-
activity between the hippocampus and mPFC [70] and other areas ner with replay in the neocortex [9799], a phenomenon that may
such as the basolateral amygdala [82]. be involved in integrating new hippocampal memories into pre-
In addition to the theta rhythm, the mammalian hippocampus existing neocortical networks [47,100104]. Several recent studies
generates SWRs, intermittent highly synchronous bursts of activity suggest that the PFC may play a special role in this process [105].
in CA3 neurons that cause high-amplitude (e.g. 2 mV), sharp-wave During SWS, SWR-related reactivation occurs 3060 ms prior to
eld potentials in the dendritic layer and 100200 Hz ripples in reactivation of associated neural activity in the mPFC [65,98,106].
the pyramidal layer of CA1 [50,83,84]. SWRs occur during pauses The hippocampus may drive activity in the PFC via the direct excita-
in translational movements, feeding, grooming, quiet wakefulness, tory pathway from CA1 neurons to the mPFC [7375]. Coordinated
and most frequently during slow-wave sleep (SWS). Functional the- SWS-related reactivation may be involved in a systems level mem-
ories for SWRs stem from their temporal relationship to the activity ory consolidation process that results in the recall of memories
of place cells, hippocampal neurons that re when an animal is in initially encoded in the hippocampus becoming progressively less

Fig. 2. The pallium in representative species of different vertebrates. Schematic diagrams of frontal sections through the telencephalon of representative species of different
vertebrates (a mouse, a pigeon, the lizard Psammodromus, and the frog Xenopus) showing the pallium colored in grey. In the mouse and most mammals, the most representative
pallial structure is the neocortex (a dorsal pallial derivative), whereas in birds and lizards and other non-avian reptiles it is the dorsal ventricular ridge (DVR, a derivative
of the lateral/ventral pallium). Abbreviations: ACo, anterior cortical amygdalar area; Ap, pallial amygdala (frog); BC, basal amygdalar complex; BST, bed nucleus of the stria
terminalis; cc, corpus callosum; Ce, central amygdala; CPu, caudoputamen (dorsal striatum); DC, lizard dorsal cortex; DP, dorsal pallium; DVR, dorsal ventricular ridge; ec,
external capsule; En, endopiriform nucleus; H, avian hyperpallium; HF, hippocampal formation; Hyp, hypothalamus; ic, internal capsule; GP, globus pallidus; LC, lizard lateral
cortex (olfactory cortex); LGE, lateral ganglionic eminence; LP, lateral pallium; LSt, avian lateral striatum; M, avian mesopallium (dorsal part of the DVR); MC, lizard medial
cortex; MGE, medial ganglionic eminence; MP, medial pallium; MSt, avian medial striatum; N, avian nidopallium (ventral part of the DVR); NCx, neocortex; P, pallium; Pa,
pallidum; Pir, piriform (or olfactory) cortex; rf, rhinal ssure; S, subpallium; SAT, lizard striatoamygdaloid transition area; Se, septum; St, striatum; Th, thalamus (dorsal
thalamus); va, vallecula; VP, ventral pallium; VPa, ventral pallidum. Modied with permission from [23].
N.C. Rattenborg, D. Martinez-Gonzalez / Behavioural Brain Research 222 (2011) 236245 239

Fig. 3. (A, B) Histological comparison of the hippocampal formation between the rat and pigeon. (A) Layers of the dentate gyrus (DG) and cornu ammonis (CA) are conspicuous
in the rat. (B) The V-shaped layer is the only readily apparent structure in the pigeon hippocampus (Hp). APH: parahippocampal area. (C, D) Location and extent of the pigeon
hippocampal formation (HF, dark grey) and dorsolateral corticoid area (CDL, light grey). (C) Dorsal view. (D) Transverse section. Scale bars in A, B = 1 mm. Reproduced with
permission from [126].

dependent on this structure and more dependent on the PFC over hippocampal formation was dened as a V-shaped medial area (i.e.,
time [38,105,107116]. the hippocampus) and the parahippocampal area (APH), a poorly
In summary, the formation and recall of mammalian episodic- delineated, thin structure overlying the dorsal portion of the lat-
like memories depends on a hippocampus that receives highly eral ventricle (Fig. 3). In this review, we follow Atoji and Wild [125]
processed associational information originating from virtually the and dene the avian hippocampus as the pallial area medial to the
entire neocortex. The hippocampus uses distinct neurophysiolog- paraventricular sulcus (Fig. 4). This area encompasses the medial
ical rhythms to coordinate activity between the hippocampus and portion of the APH and the hippocampus according to Karten and
neocortical areas during encoding and retrieval of information con-
tributing to episodic-like memories. In particular, several lines of
evidence suggest that direct connections from the hippocampus to
the PFC play an important role in these processes.

2.2. Birds

2.2.1. Neuroanatomy
The pallium enlarged in mammals and birds, however, the
organization of the avian pallium more closely resembles that of
their reptilian relatives than that of their more distant mammalian
relatives (Fig. 2). Comparisons of pallial areas in vertebrates are
most readily discussed within the context of recent developmental
gene expression studies. During development, four pallial sectors
(medial, dorsal, lateral, and ventral) can be identied [23,117121].
The medial sector becomes the hippocampus in mammals and
birds, and the medial cortex in reptiles, structures implicated Fig. 4. Hypothesized homology of hippocampal formation subregions between
in processing and storing spatial information [1517,122,123]. mammals and birds [126]. The medioventral V-shaped layer (shaded light grey) is
Despite this functional similarity, differences in cytoarchitecture, comparable to the mammalian dentate gyrus, the dorsomedial region (DM) to cornu
neurochemistry, and connectivity have complicated attempts to ammonis (CA) and the subiculum, and the dorsolateral region (DL) to the entorhinal
cortex. Alternative interpretations include the V-shaped layer as the CA and portions
dene the boarders of the avian hippocampus and possible homo-
of DM as the dentate gyrus [127]. Other histologically identied regions include the
logues of the mammalian entorhinal cortex, CA1, CA3, and dentate magnocellular region (Ma), the parvocellular region (Pa), and the cell-poor region
gyrus. Historically, according to Karten and Hodos [124], the avian (Po). CDL: dorsolateral corticoid area. Reproduced with permission from [126].
240 N.C. Rattenborg, D. Martinez-Gonzalez / Behavioural Brain Research 222 (2011) 236245

Hodos [124], and includes commonly proposed homologues of the the source of excitatory projection neurons, including associational
mammalian CA areas and the dentate gyrus (reviewed in [125,126], neurons in the supragranular layers of the neocortex [142], layers
see also [120,127]). Lateral to the paraventricular sulcus is the dor- not found in the three-layered dorsal cortex of non-avian reptiles
solateral area of the hippocampal formation (DL, part of the former [135,143]. Recent evidence suggests that a SVZ in the dorsal pallium
APH), an area that like the mammalian entorhinal cortex serves of birds also contributes to the expansion of the hyperpallium [144],
as the main source of input to the hippocampus [125]. Laterally, and perhaps the convergent evolution of mammal-like neuronal
DL grades into the dorsolateral corticoid area (CDL), which in turn types found in this structure, but not the dorsal cortex of non-avian
joins the piriform cortex ventrolaterally. Like the DL, the CDL also reptiles [129,143,145]. In addition to the dorsal SVZ, birds have a
appears to transmit information to the hippocampus. Although more prominent SVZ running along the lateral ventricle that con-
the DL and CDL have some connectional properties similar to the tributes neurons to the DVR, the largest pallial structure in birds
entorhinal cortex, recent developmental studies suggest that the [141,144,146,147].
area typically identied as the posterior piriform cortex [124] may The avian DVR is involved in forming high-order associations
be the avian entorhinal cortex [117,120]. Although several ques- similar to those performed by association areas in the neocor-
tions about homology remain unresolved, the conclusions drawn in tex. The mesopallium is involved in various types of learning,
this paper do not hinge on one particular interpretation, but rather including, imprinting [148], song learning [149], avoidance learning
apply to all proposals. [150], tool manufacture and use [151], and innovative and ex-
The dorsal pallium develops into the neocortex in mammals, the ible behaviors in general [152,153]. The nidopallium, the other
hyperpallium in birds, and the dorsal cortex in reptiles (Fig. 2). The major division of the DVR, also appears to be involved in forming
neocortex receives visual and somatosensory input from the lem- high-order associations [154]. Notably, the nidopallium caudolat-
nothalamic nuclei of the dorsal thalamus, and visual and auditory eral (NCL) shares several features in common with the mammalian
input from the collothalamic nuclei [128130], whereas in saurop- PFC [22,155], despite developing from a different pallial sector.
sids (birds and non-avian reptiles) the dorsal pallium receives The NCL is a high-order multimodal association area reciprocally
lemnothalamic visual and somatosensory input, but apparently no connected to secondary sensory areas for processing visual, audi-
direct collothalamic input [131]. The neocortex encompasses the tory, somatosensory and trigeminal information [156159]. The
largest proportion of the pallium in mammals, and, unlike the dor- NCL is also reciprocally connected to other multimodal association
sal pallium of sauropsids, includes large multimodal association areas in the nidopallium and mesopallium [157,159]. In addi-
areas [35,129]. tion to processing multimodal sensory information, the NCL also
The lateral and ventral pallia give rise to the claustrum and appears to be involved in working memory and executive con-
pallial portions of the amygdala in mammals, and the dorsal trol, high-order cognitive functions similar to those performed by
ventricular ridge (DVR) in sauropsids, a largely nuclear struc- the PFC [155,160162]. In both the PFC and NCL, working mem-
ture that bulges into the lateral ventricle (Fig. 2). In birds, the ory is dependent upon the release of dopamine from projections
DVR is particularly large (virtually occluding the lateral ventri- from the midbrain ventral tegmental area and the substantia nigra
cle) and more differentiated than the DVR in non-avian reptiles, [155,163166]. Collectively, the available evidence suggests that
including crocodilians, the closest living relatives to birds [120]. the mammalian PFC and avian NCL perform similar functions.
Although developmental data indicate that the DVR is homolo- Moreover, in general, complex cognition in birds seems to depend
gous as a eld with the mammalian claustrum and pallial amygdala on the DVR, a lateral and ventral pallial derivative, rather than the
[119,132,133], collothalamic visual and auditory input to the DVR dorsal pallium, as in mammals.
is similar to that reaching the extrastriate and auditory neocortex, Despite comprising high-order association areas involved in
respectively [21,131,134,135]. This has led to the suggestion that complex cognitive processes similar to those performed by
certain cell groups in the DVR are homologous to certain layers of association areas in the neocortex, unlike the neocortex, the
the lateral (extrastriate and auditory) neocortex, rather than the DVR shares few connections with the hippocampus (Fig. 5)
claustroamygdalar complex [21,134]. Others have suggested that [125,127,157,158,167,168]. Moreover, although it remains unclear
the DVR is a combination of both the lateral neocortex and claus- which brain area is homologous to the mammalian entorhinal
troamygdalar complex as a developmental eld [136]. Reconciling cortex the main structure through which information reaches
these different lines of evidence remains a challenge for the eld of the mammalian hippocampus candidate entorhinal-like areas
comparative neuroanatomy [137139]. providing input to the avian hippocampus, such as the CDL,
In contrast to the mammalian derivatives of the lateral and APH, DL, or entorhinal cortex, as dened by Puelles et al. [120],
ventral pallia, the DVR encompasses the largest proportion of the also do not appear to receive projections from most of the DVR
pallium in sauropsids. In amphibians, the out-group to amniotes [125,157,158,167169]. Only the frontolateral nidopallium, an area
(mammals and sauropsids), the dorsal and lateral/ventral pallial of the DVR involved in processing high-order visual informa-
sectors are relatively undeveloped when compared to those in tion [170] and the caudoventral nidopallium (NCV), apparently
mammals and sauropsids, respectively (Fig. 2) [140]. Assuming that a part of the limbic system [171], project to the hippocam-
the amphibian condition reects the ancestral state of the amniote pus via the APH and CDL, respectively [125,157,158,167,168].
pallium, the main difference between the brains of mammals and Although not originating in the DVR, the only other sensory modal-
sauropsids rests in the pallial sector that underwent the great- ity that provides input to the hippocampus is olfaction [125].
est development: mammals expanded the dorsal pallium, whereas Olfactory input from the piriform cortex reaches the hippocam-
sauropsids, birds in particular, expanded the lateral and ventral pus directly and indirectly via the CDL, DL, and densocellular
pallia. part of the hyperpallium [125,129,172,173]. Finally, in contrast
Expansion of the respective pallial structures in mammals and to the mammalian PFC, the avian PFC (NCL) does not receive
birds seems to be linked to a subventricular zone (SVZ) of neuronal direct projections from the hippocampus or candidate entorhinal-
proliferation in the developing pallia of each group. A similar SVZ like areas [125,157,158,167,174]. Consequently, in contrast to the
has not been found in the pallium of amphibians and non-avian mammalian hippocampus, which receives input from virtually all
reptiles, including crocodiles, suggesting that this source of neu- high-order association areas, and thereby the rest of the neocor-
ronal proliferation evolved independently in mammals and birds tex, connections between the avian hippocampus and major pallial
[141], and therefore may contribute to encephalization in these association areas in the DVR are limited [34,175]. As expected given
groups. In mammals, a SVZ in the dorsal pallium appears to be their close relationship to birds, few connections have also been
N.C. Rattenborg, D. Martinez-Gonzalez / Behavioural Brain Research 222 (2011) 236245 241

medial septum to the hippocampus, a pathway contributing to


DVR Associaon Areas the mammalian hippocampal theta rhythm (see Section 2.1.2),
(Lateral/Ventral Pallium)
are sparse or absent in birds, including pigeons [125,193195].
Hyperpallium
NCV PFC (NCL) NFL (Dorsal Pallium) In addition to theta, SWRs have not been reported during any

Box 1
Why are bird brains organized differently from mammalian
brains?
Entorhinal-like Areas
Birds are a derived type of reptile. As such, the brain of early
birds probably resembled the general pattern exhibited by
extant non-avian reptiles; the dorsal cortex consisted of three
layers and the DVR was relatively small. Fossil endocasts
Piriform of the cranial cavity have been used to follow evolution-
Hippocampus
Cortex ary changes in the gross volume of different brain areas in
both mammals and birds [18,19]. In terms of encephaliza-
Fig. 5. Limited connections between the avian hippocampus and association areas tion and shape, fossilized endocasts of the cranial cavity from
in the dorsal ventricular ridge (DVR). The hippocampus receives visual input directly dinosaurs suggest that expansion of the pallium occurred
from the hyperpallium and indirectly via proposed entorhinal-like areas; the dorso- after birds diverged from their dinosaur ancestors [18]. In liv-
lateral portion of the hippocampal formation (DL), the parahippocampal area (APH), ing birds, the hyperpallium and DVR ll two distinct areas
and the dorsolateral corticoid area (CDL), or the entorhinal cortex, as dened by of the cranial cavity that expand dorsally along the mid-
Puelles et al. [120]. High-order visual input from the frontolateral nidopallium (NFL) line and laterally, respectively. Although the data is limited,
also reaches the hippocampus via the entorhinal-like areas. Other than the NFL, the fossil endocasts suggest that the DVR enlarged well before
caudoventral nidopallium (NCV) is the only other area in the DVR providing input
the hyperpallium. In Archaeopteryx lithographica, the earliest
to the hippocampus via the entorhinal-like areas. The caudolateral nidopallium
known bird, expansion of the DVR, but not the hyperpallium,
(NCL) the avian analogue of the mammalian prefrontal cortex (PFC) does not
project to the entorhinal-like areas or hippocampus, and does not receive pro- is evident [199]. More recent birds from the Mesozoic show
jections from these structures. Other high-order association regions in the DVR further expansion of the DVR, but the hyperpallium does not
(nidopallium and mesopallium) are not connected to the entorhinal-like areas or become evident in endocasts until the Cenozoic [19]. Thus, it
hippocampus. Olfactory input from the piriform cortex reaches the hippocampus appears that birds progressively increased the size of the DVR
directly and indirectly via the entorhinal-like areas. for an extended period of time before increasing the size of
the hyperpallium.
The proposed evolutionary sequence of events contributing
detected between the DVR and medial cortex (i.e., hippocampus) to encephalization of the avian brain may explain some of
in non-avian reptiles [34,130,176178]. Similar to birds, the medial the differences in pallial interactions with the hippocampus in
cortex only receives visual input from the dorsal cortex and olfac- mammals and birds. Instead of expanding the dorsal pallium,
tory input from the lateral (piriform) cortex [130]. Consequently, with its connections to the hippocampus, as did mammals,
brain structures allocated to forming high-order associations in birds initially elaborated on the DVR, a structure that may
mammals are intimately connected to the hippocampus, whereas have already been dedicated to forming some high-level asso-
comparable association areas in the sauropsidian DVR are not. This ciations in their reptilian ancestors [138,200202]. As birds
fundamental difference in brain organization undoubtedly inu- elaborated further on the DVR, new associative processes
presumably became dependent on this structure, perhaps
ences how the avian brain processes information, including that
including those related to PFC functions [25,202]. During this
involved in episodic-like memory.
time, as in modern non-avian reptiles and birds, the avian
hippocampus presumably continued to receive visual input
2.2.2. Neurophysiology from the dorsal pallium, as well as olfactory input. The dor-
Consistent with the differences in brain organization observed sal pallium expanded later to form the hyperpallium, perhaps
between mammals and birds, the neurophysiological properties of to enhance visual processing [203]. However, unlike the neo-
the hippocampi also differ between these groups. Both modes of cortex, the role of the hyperpallium in forming multimodal
hippocampal activity observed in mammals (i.e., theta and SWRs) associations was limited, perhaps because such functions
were already performed by the DVR. Thus the early dedica-
seem to be missing in birds during wakefulness and sleep (reviewed
tion of the sauropsidian DVR to performing most associational
in [25]), even though birds independently evolved sleep states in
processes may have shaped or constrained the ultimate orga-
most other respects similar to mammalian SWS and REM sleep nization of the avian brain.
[25,179182]. Although less studied than the mammalian hip- But why did mammals elaborate on the dorsal pallium instead
pocampus [15], most studies suggest that the avian hippocampus of the DVR? Aboitiz et al. [175] suggest that this is attributable
does not generate a mammal-like theta rhythm during wakeful- to olfaction being a particularly important sense for early
ness or REM sleep [25,183190]. A single study reported a slow nocturnal mammals. As a result, early mammals may have
theta rhythm in awake pigeons (Columba livia), but unlike mam- elaborated on the olfactoryhippocampaldorsal cortex cir-
mals, this occurred during feeding and immobility, and not always cuitry inherited from stem amniotes to process associational
while walking [15,191]. Moreover, the authors did not report information. As the importance of this associational circuitry
increased, it may have become benecial to recruit and
whether it also occurred during REM sleep, as expected based
integrate other sensory modalities, especially vision when
on mammals, and non-hippocampal sources for theta were not mammals became diurnal. According to Aboitiz et al. [175],
ruled out ([25], see also [192]). Given these points and the absence this evolutionary sequence led to sensory information from
of reports of hippocampal theta in all other studies, including most modalities (lemnothalamic and collothalamic) reaching
those on pigeons, it remains unclear whether the reported theta the dorsal pallium (neocortex), wherein it is integrated to form
rhythm reects the same phenomenon as the mammalian hip- high-order multimodal associations before being conveyed to
pocampal theta rhythm. Moreover, neuroanatomical differences the hippocampus to form episodic memories [175].
between mammals and birds are also consistent with the absence
of a mammalian-like theta rhythm in birds. Projections from the
242 N.C. Rattenborg, D. Martinez-Gonzalez / Behavioural Brain Research 222 (2011) 236245

state using recording methods that readily detect them in mam- [5] Zinkivskay A, Nazir F, Smulders TV. What-where-when memory in magpies
mals [25,181,184,185,187,189191,196]. Of particular relevance (Pica pica). Anim Cogn 2009;12:11925.
[6] Clayton NS, Bussey TJ, Dickinson A. Can animals recall the past and plan for
for this discussion of episodic-like memory, SWRs and theta were the future? Nat Rev Neurosci 2003;4:68591.
not reported in hippocampal local eld potential recordings dur- [7] Crystal JD. Episodic-like memory in animals. Behav Brain Res
ing wakefulness, SWS, or REM sleep in black-billed magpies (P. 2010;215:23543.
[8] Feenders G, Smulders TV. Episodic-like memory in food-hoarding birds. In:
pica [189]), a food hoarding species which exhibits episodic-like Dere E, Easton A, Nadel L, Huston JP, editors. Handbook of behavioral neu-
memory [5]. The apparent absence of neurophysiological rhythms roscience, Vol 18, handbook of episodic memory. The Netherlands: Elsevier;
thought to mediate interactions between the hippocampus and 2008. p. 197608.
[9] Clayton NS, Grifths DP, Emery NJ, Dickinson A. Elements of episodic-like
associative areas of the neocortex is consistent with the absence memory in animals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2001;356:148391.
of connections between the avian hippocampus and comparable [10] Dally JM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS. Food-caching western scrub-jays keep track
associative areas in the DVR. of who was watching when. Science 2006;312:16625.
[11] Emery NJ, Clayton NS. Effects of experience and social context on prospective
caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature 2001;414:4436.
3. Discussion [12] Raby CR, Alexis DM, Dickinson A, Clayton NS. Planning for the future by west-
ern scrub-jays. Nature 2007;445:91921.
[13] Clayton NS, Russell J, Dickinson A. Are animals stuck in time or are they
Collectively, the neuroanatomical and neurophysiological dif- chronesthetic creatures? Top Cogn Sci 2009;1:5971.
ferences between mammals and birds suggest that there may be a [14] Suddendorf T, Corballis MC. The evolution of foresight: what is mental time
fundamental difference between how episodic-like memories are travel, and is it unique to humans? Behav Brain Sci 2007;30:299313.
[15] Bingman VP, Gagliardo A, Hough GE, Ioale P, Kahn MC, Siegel JJ. The avian
processed in each group. In contrast to the mammalian hippocam-
hippocampus, homing in pigeons and the memory representation of large-
pus, the avian hippocampus does not seem to be privy to much scale space. Integr Comp Biol 2005;45:55564.
of the high-order multimodal associational information processed [16] Colombo M, Broadbent N. Is the avian hippocampus a functional homologue
of the mammalian hippocampus? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2000;24:46584.
in the DVR. Consequently, the visual and olfactory information
[17] Sherry DF. Neuroecology. Ann Rev Psychol 2006;57:16797.
that reaches the avian hippocampus may be sufcient for what, [18] Jerison HJ. Fossils, brains and behavior. In: Watanabe S, Hofman MA, edi-
where, and when memory, at least in food hoarding birds, where this tors. Integration of comparative neuroanatomy and cognition. Tokyo: Keio
has been established. However, components of daily experiences University Press; 2007. p. 1331.
[19] Milner AC, Walsh SA. Avian brain evolution: new data from Palaeogene birds
processed in the DVR may not contribute to such episodic-like (Lower Eocene) from England. Zool J Linn Soc 2009;155:198219.
memories. Although it is conceivable that information processed [20] Emery NJ, Clayton NS. Evolution of the avian brain and intelligence. Curr Biol
in the DVR reaches the hippocampus via an unknown route, the 2005;15:R94650.
[21] Jarvis ED, Gntrkn O, Bruce L, Csillag A, Karten H, Kuenzel W, et al. Avian
apparent anatomical differences are consistent with the absence brain nomenclature consortium. Avian brains and a new understanding of
of neurophysiological rhythms implicated in coordinating the pro- vertebrate brain evolution. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005;6:1519.
cessing of episodic memories between areas of the mammalian [22] Kirsch JA, Gntrkn O, Rose J. Insight without cortex: lessons from the avian
brain. Consci Cogn 2008;17:47583.
brain. Specically, isolated from much of the DVR, including the [23] Medina L, Abelln A. Development and evolution of the pallium. Sem Cell Dev
avian PFC, the avian hippocampus may not require the rhythms Biol 2009;20:698711.
used by the mammalian hippocampus to communicate with com- [24] Wang Y, Brzozowska-Prechtl A, Karten HJ. Laminar and columnar auditory
cortex in avian brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010;107:1267681.
parable structures in the neocortex.
[25] Rattenborg NC, Martinez-Gonzalez D, Roth TC, Pravosudov VV. Hippocampal
The divergence in brain organization and activity exhibited memory consolidation during sleep: a comparison of mammals and birds.
by mammals and birds raises several challenging questions. Does Biol Rev Camb Philo Soc, in press, doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00165.x.
information processed by the DVR contribute to episodic-like [26] Bayley PJ, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. The fate of old memories after medial tem-
poral lobe damage. J Neurosci 2006;26:133117.
memories? Is this information integrated during encoding and [27] Eichenbaum H. A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nat
retrieval with hippocampal information? Are episodic-like memo- Rev Neurosci 2000;1:4150.
ries in birds less associative than those in mammals ([197], see also [28] Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal
lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1957;20:1121.
[34,198])? Why did mammals and birds evolve such different brains
[29] Smith CN, Squire LR. Medial temporal lobe activity during retrieval of seman-
(Box 1)? Are there trade-offs associated with the different types of tic memory is related to the age of the memory. J Neurosci 2009;29:9308.
brain organization? Are there differences in cognition attributable [30] Teng E, Squire LR. Memory for places learned long ago is intact after hip-
pocampal damage. Nature 1999;400:6757.
to these differences in brain organization? Finally, despite the dif-
[31] Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Watkins KE, Connelly A, Van Paesschen W,
ferences between mammalian and avian brains, are there shared Mishkin M. Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic
traits that provide insight into fundamental processes contributing and semantic memory. Science 1997;277:37680.
to complex cognition? Attempts to answer these challenging ques- [32] Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR, Amaral DG. Human amnesia and the medial tempo-
ral region: enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited
tions may enhance our general understanding of how the brain to eld CA1 of the hippocampus. J Neurosci 1986;6:295067.
generates memories. [33] DeVito LM, Eichenbaum H. Distinct contributions of the hippocampus and
medial prefrontal cortex to the what-where-when components of episodic-
like memory in mice. Behav Brain Res 2010;215:31825.
Acknowledgements [34] Manns JR, Eichenbaum H, Evolution of the hippocampus In: Kaas JH, Krubitzer
LA, editors. Solution of the nervous system: a comprehensive reference, 3.
London: Academic Press; 2007. p. 46589.
We thank John A. Lesku for valuable comments on an earlier
[35] Simons JS, Spiers HJ. Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-
draft of the manuscript. This work was supported by the Max Planck term memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003;4:63748.
Society. [36] Fyhn M, Molden S, Witter MP, Moser EI, Moser MB. Spatial representation in
the entorhinal cortex. Science 2004;305:125864.
[37] van Strien NM, Cappaert NL, Witter MP. The anatomy of memory: an inter-
References active overview of the parahippocampal-hippocampal network. Nat Rev
Neurosci 2009;10:27282.
[1] Tulving E, Markowitsch HJ. Episodic and declarative memory: role of the [38] Frankland PW, Bontempi B, Talton LE, Kaczmarek L, Silva AJ. The involvement
hippocampus. Hippocampus 1998;8:198204. of the anterior cingulate cortex in remote contextual fear memory. Science
[2] Salwiczek LH, Watanabe A, Clayton NS. Ten years of research into avian 2004;304:8813.
models of episodic-like memory and its implications for developmental and [39] Burwell RD, Amaral DG. Cortical afferents of the perirhinal, postrhinal, and
comparative cognition. Behav Brain Res 2010;215:22134. entorhinal cortices of the rat. J Comp Neurol 1998;398:179205.
[3] Clayton NS, Dickinson A. Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by [40] Suzuki WA, Amaral DG. Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the
scrub jays. Nature 1998;395:2724. macaque monkey: cortical afferents. J Comp Neurol 1994;350:497533.
[4] Feeney MC, Roberts WA, Sherry DF. Black-capped chickadees (Poecile atri- [41] Morris RG, Frey U. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity: role in spatial learning
capillus) anticipate future outcomes of foraging choices. J Exp Psychol Anim or the automatic recording of attended experience? Philos Trans R Soc Lond
Behav Process 2011;37:3040. B Biol Sci 1997;352:1489503.
N.C. Rattenborg, D. Martinez-Gonzalez / Behavioural Brain Research 222 (2011) 236245 243

[42] Lee I, Kesner RP. Differential contribution of NMDA receptors in hippocampal [77] Paz R, Bauer EP, Par D. Learning-related facilitation of rhinal interactions by
subregions to spatial working memory. Nat Neurosci 2002;5:1628. medial prefrontal inputs. J Neurosci 2007;27:654251.
[43] Nakazawa K, Sun LD, Quirk MC, Rondi-Reig L, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S. [78] Paz R, Bauer EP, Par D. Measuring correlations and interactions among
Hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors are crucial for memory acquisition of four simultaneously recorded brain regions during learning. J Neurophys
one-time experience. Neuron 2003;38:30515. 2009;101:250715.
[44] Rolls ET. A computational theory of episodic memory formation in the hip- [79] Pelletier JG, Apergis J, Par D. Low-probability transmission of neocorti-
pocampus. Behav Brain Res 2010;215:18096. cal and entorhinal impulses through the perirhinal cortex. J Neurophysiol
[45] Buzski G. Rhythms of the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2006. 2004;91:207989.
[46] Fell J, Axmacher N. The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. [80] Li JS, Hsiao KY, Chen WM. Effects of medial prefrontal cortex lesions in rats
Nat Rev Neurosci 2011;12:10518. on the what-where-when memory of a fear conditioning event. Behav Brain
[47] Buzski G. Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for noisy Res 2011;218:948.
brain states. Neuroscience 1989;31:55170. [81] Poe GR, Nitz DA, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA. Experience-dependent
[48] Dragoi G, Carpi D, Recce M, Csicsvari J, Buzski G. Interactions between hip- phase-reversal of hippocampal neuron ring during REM sleep. Brain Res
pocampus and medial septum during sharp waves and theta oscillation in the 2000;855:17680.
behaving rat. J Neurosci 1999;19:61919. [82] Popa D, Duvarci S, Popescu AT, Lna C, Par D. Coherent amygdalocortical
[49] Buzski G. Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron 2002;33:32540. theta promotes fear memory consolidation during paradoxical sleep. Proc
[50] Sirota A, Buzski G. Interaction between neocortical and hippocampal net- Natl Acad Sci USA 2010;107:65169.
works via slow oscillations. Thal Rel Syst 2005;3:24559. [83] Buzski G. Hippocampal sharp waves: their origin and signicance. Brain Res
[51] Hangya B, Borhegyi Z, Szilgyi N, Freund TF, Varga V. GABAergic neurons of 1986;398:24252.
the medial septum lead the hippocampal network during theta activity. J [84] Ulanovsky N, Moss CF. Hippocampal cellular and network activity in freely
Neurosci 2009;29:8094102. moving echolocating bats. Nat Neurosci 2007;10:22433.
[52] Givens BS, Olton DS. Cholinergic and GABAergic modulation of medial septal [85] OKeefe J, Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary
area: effect on working memory. Behav Neurosci 1990;104:84955. evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res 1971;34:
[53] McNaughton N, Ruan M, Woodnorth MA. Restoring theta-like rhythmic- 1715.
ity in rats restores initial learning in the Morris water maze. Hippocampus [86] Anderson MI, Jeffery KJ. Heterogeneous modulation of place cell ring by
2006;16:110210. changes in context. J Neurosci 2003;23:882735.
[54] Winson J. Loss of hippocampal theta rhythm results in spatial memory decit [87] Clearwater JM, Bilkey DK. Place, space, and taste: combining context and spa-
in the rat. Science 1978;201:1603. tial information in a hippocampal navigation system. Hippocampus, in press,
[55] Buzski G. Theta rhythm of navigation: link between path integration doi:10.1002/hipo.20911.
and landmark navigation, episodic and semantic memory. Hippocampus [88] Ginther MR, Walsh DF, Ramus SJ. Hippocampal neurons encode dif-
2005;15:82740. ferent episodes in an overlapping sequence of odors task. J Neurosci
[56] Fortin NJ, Agster KL, Eichenbaum HB. Critical role of the hippocampus in 2011;31:270611.
memory for sequences of events. Nat Neurosci 2002;5:45862. [89] Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Treves A, Meyer R, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL,
[57] Kesner RP, Gilbert PE, Barua LA. The role of the hippocampus in memory for et al. Progressive transformation of hippocampal neuronal representations
the temporal order of a sequence of odors. Behav Neurosci 2002;116:28690. in morphed environments. Neuron 2005;48:34558.
[58] Manns JR, Zilli EA, Ong KC, Hasselmo ME, Eichenbaum H. Hippocampal CA1 [90] Smith DM, Mizumori SJ. Hippocampal place cells, context, and episodic mem-
spiking during encoding and retrieval: relation to theta phase. Neurobiol ory. Hippocampus 2006;16(9):71629.
Learn Mem 2007;87:920. [91] Carr MF, Jadhav SP, Frank LM. Hippocampal replay in the awake state: a
[59] Colgin LL, Denninger T, Fyhn M, Hafting T, Bonnevie T, Jensen O, et al. Fre- potential substrate for memory consolidation and retrieval. Nat Neurosci
quency of gamma oscillations routes ow of information in the hippocampus. 2011;14:14753.
Nature 2009;462:3537. [92] Cheng S, Frank LM. New experiences enhance coordinated neural activity in
[60] Hasselmo ME, Bodelon C, Wyble BP. A proposed function for hippocampal the hippocampus. Neuron 2008;57:30313.
theta rhythm: separate phases of encoding and retrieval enhance reversal of [93] Kudrimoti HS, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal cell
prior learning. Neural Comput 2002;14:793817. assemblies: effects of behavioral state, experience, and EEG dynamics. J Neu-
[61] Huerta PT, Lisman JE. Bidirectional synaptic plasticity induced by a sin- rosci 1999;19:4090101.
gle burst during cholinergic theta oscillation in CA1 in vitro. Neuron [94] ONeill J, Senior T, Csicsvari J. Place-selective ring of CA1 pyramidal cells
1995;15:105363. during sharp wave/ripple network patterns in exploratory behavior. Neuron
[62] Jensen O, Colgin LL. Cross-frequency coupling between neuronal oscillations. 2006;49:14355.
Trends Cogn Sci 2007;11:2679. [95] Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memo-
[63] Jutras MJ, Fries P, Buffalo EA. Gamma-band synchronization in the macaque ries during sleep. Science 1994;265:6769.
hippocampus and memory formation. J Neurosci 2009;29:1252131. [96] Lee AK, Wilson MA. Memory of sequential experience in the hippocampus
[64] Tort AB, Komorowski RW, Manns JR, Kopell NJ, Eichenbaum H. Theta-gamma during slow wave sleep. Neuron 2002;36:118394.
coupling increases during the learning of item-context associations. Proc Natl [97] Ji D, Wilson MA. Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hip-
Acad Sci USA 2009;106:209427. pocampus during sleep. Nat Neurosci 2007;10:1007.
[65] Benchenane K, Peyrache A, Khamassi M, Tierney PL, Gioanni Y, Battaglia FP, [98] Peyrache A, Khamassi M, Benchenane K, Wiener SI, Battaglia FP. Replay of
et al. Coherent theta oscillations and reorganization of spike timing in the rule-learning related neural patterns in the prefrontal cortex during sleep.
hippocampal-prefrontal network upon learning. Neuron 2010;66:92136. Nat Neurosci 2009;12:91926.
[66] Jones MW, Wilson MA. Phase precession of medial prefrontal cortical activity [99] Qin YL, McNaughton BL, Skaggs WE, Barnes CA. Memory reprocessing in cor-
relative to the hippocampal theta rhythm. Hippocampus 2005;15:86773. ticocortical and hippocampocortical neuronal ensembles. Philos Trans R Soc
[67] Jones MW, Wilson MA. Theta rhythms coordinate hippocampal prefrontal Lond B Biol Sci 1997;352:152533.
interactions in a spatial memory task. PLoS Biol 2005;3:e402. [100] Lesburgures E, Gobbo OL, Alaux-Cantin S, Hambucken A, Trilieff P, Bon-
[68] Siapas AG, Lubenov EV, Wilson MA. Prefrontal phase locking to hippocampal tempi B. Early tagging of cortical networks is required for the formation of
theta oscillations. Neuron 2005;46:14151. enduring associative memory. Science 2011;331:9248.
[69] Sigurdsson T, Stark KL, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Gordon JA. Impaired [101] Marr D. Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B
hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony in a genetic mouse model of schizophre- Biol Sci 1971;262:2381.
nia. Nature 2010;464:7637. [102] McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, OReilly RC. Why there are complementary
[70] Sirota A, Montgomery S, Fujisawa S, Isomura Y, Zugaro M, Buzski G. Entrain- learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the suc-
ment of neocortical neurons and gamma oscillations by the hippocampal cesses and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol
theta rhythm. Neuron 2008;60:68397. Rev 1995;102:41957.
[71] Hyman JM, Zilli EA, Paley AM, Hasselmo ME. Medial prefrontal cortex cells [103] Rauchs G, Feyers D, Landeau B, Bastin C, Luxen A, Maquet P, et al.
show dynamic modulation with the hippocampal theta rhythm dependent Sleep contributes to the strengthening of some memories over oth-
on behavior. Hippocampus 2005;15:73949. ers, depending on hippocampal activity at learning. J Neurosci 2011;31:
[72] Wang GW, Cai JX. Disconnection of the hippocampal-prefrontal cortical cir- 25638.
cuits impairs spatial working memory performance in rats. Behav Brain Res [104] Squire LR, Alvarez P. Retrograde amnesia andmemory consolidation: a neu-
2006;175:32936. robiological perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1995;5:16977.
[73] Jay TM, Witter MP. Distribution of hippocampal CA1 and subicular efferents [105] Frankland PW, Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories.
in the prefrontal cortex of the rat studied bymeans of anterograde transport Nat Rev Neurosci 2005;6:11930.
of Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin. J Comp Neurol 1991;313:57486. [106] Wierzynski CM, Lubenov EV, Gu M, Siapas AG. State-dependent spike-timing
[74] Swanson LW. A direct projection from Ammons horn to prefrontal cortex in relationships between hippocampal and prefrontal circuits during sleep. Neu-
the rat. Brain Res 1981;217:1504. ron 2009;61:58796.
[75] Thierry AM, Gioanni Y, Dgntais E, Glowinski J. Hippocampo-prefrontal [107] Akirav I, Maroun M. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex is obligatory for con-
cortex pathway: anatomical and electrophysiological characteristics. Hip- solidation and reconsolidation of object recognition memory. Cereb Cortex
pocampus 2000;10:4119. 2006;16:175965.
[76] Tomita H, Ohbayashi M, Nakahara K, Hasegawa I, Miyashita Y. Top-down sig- [108] Bontempi B, Laurent-Demir C, Destrade C, Jaffard R. Time-dependent reor-
nal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval. Nature ganization of brain circuitry underlying long-term memory storage. Nature
1999;401:699703. 1999;400:6715.
244 N.C. Rattenborg, D. Martinez-Gonzalez / Behavioural Brain Research 222 (2011) 236245

[109] Gais S, Albouy G, Boly M, Dang-Vu TT, Darsaud A, Desseilles M, et al. Sleep [139] Nomura T, Takahashi M, Hara Y, Osumi N. Patterns of neurogenesis and ampli-
transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci tude of Reelin expression are essential for making a mammalian-type cortex.
USA 2007;104:1877883. PLoS One 2008;3:e1454.
[110] Jung MW, Baeg EH, Kim MJ, Kim YB, Kim JJ. Plasticity and memory in the [140] Dicke U, Roth G. Evolution of the amphibian nervous system. In: Kaas JH, Krub-
prefrontal cortex. Rev Neurosci 2008;19:2946. itzer LA, editors. Evolution of the nervous system: a comprehensive reference,
[111] Maviel T, Durkin TP, Menzaghi F, Bontempi B. Sites of neocortical reorganiza- 3. London: Academic Press; 2007. p. 61124.
tion critical for remote spatial memory. Science 2004;305:969. [141] Charvet CJ, Owerkowicz T, Striedter GF. Phylogeny of the telencephalic sub-
[112] Takashima A, Petersson KM, Rutters F, Tendolkar I, Jensen O, Zwarts MJ, et al. ventricular zone in sauropsids: evidence for the sequential evolution of pallial
Declarative memory consolidation in humans: a prospective functional mag- and subpallial subventricular zones. Brain Behav Evol 2009;73:28594.
netic resonance imaging study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006;103:75661. [142] Cheung AF, Kondo S, Abdel-Mannan O, Chodroff RA, Sirey TM, Bluy LE, et al.
[113] Takehara K, Kawahara S, Kirino Y. Time-dependent reorganization of the brain The subventricular zone is the developmental milestone of a 6-layered neo-
components underlying memory retention in trace eyeblink conditioning. J cortex: comparisons in metatherian and eutherian mammals. Cereb Cortex
Neurosci 2003;23:9897905. 2010;20:107181.
[114] Takehara-Nishiuchi K, McNaughton BL. Spontaneous changes of neocortical [143] Medina L, Reiner A. Do birds possess homologues of mammalian primary
code for associative memory during consolidation. Science 2008;322:9603. visual, somatosensory and motor cortices? Trend Neurosci 2000;23:112.
[115] Takehara-Nishiuchi K, Nakao K, Kawahara S, Matsuki N, Kirino Y. Systems con- [144] Charvet CJ, Striedter GF. Developmental origins of mosaic brain evolution:
solidation requires postlearning activation of NMDA receptors in the medial morphometric analysis of the developing zebra nch brain. J Comp Neurol
prefrontal cortex in trace eyeblink conditioning. J Neurosci 2006;26:504958. 2009;514:20313.
[116] Wiltgen BJ, Brown RA, Talton LE, Silva AJ. New circuits for old memories: the [145] Surez J, Dvila JC, Real MA, Guirado S, Medina L. Calciumbinding proteins,
role of the neocortex in consolidation. Neuron 2004;44:1018. neuronal nitric oxide synthase, and GABA help to distinguish different pal-
[117] Abelln A, Legaz I, Vernier B, Rtaux S, Medina L. Olfactory and amygdalar lial areas in the developing and adult chicken. I. Hippocampal formation and
structures of the chicken ventral pallium based on the combinatorial expres- hyperpallium. J Comp Neurol 2006;497:75171.
sion patterns of LIM and other developmental regulatory genes. J Comp Neurol [146] Cheung AF, Pollen AA, Tavare A, DeProto J, Molnr Z. Comparative aspects of
2009;516:16686. cortical neurogenesis in vertebrates. J Anat 2007;211:16476.
[118] Puelles L. Thoughts on the development, structure and evolution of the mam- [147] Charvet CJ. A reduced progenitor pool population accounts for the rudimen-
malian and avian telencephalic pallium. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci tary appearance of the septum, medial pallium and dorsal pallium in birds.
2001;356:158398. Brain Behav Evol 2010;76:289300.
[119] Puelles L, Kuwana E, Puelles E, Bulfone A, Shimamura K, Keleher J, et al. Pallial [148] Horn G. Pathways of the past; the imprint of memory. Nat Rev Neurosci
and subpallial derivatives in the embryonic chick and mouse telencephalon, 2004;5:10820.
traced by the expression of the genes Dlx-2, Emx-1, Nkx-2. 1, Pax-6, and Tbr-1. [149] Bolhuis JJ, Okanoya K, Scharff C. Twitter evolution: converging mechanisms
J Comp Neurol 2000;424:40938. in birdsong and human speech. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010;11:74759.
[120] Puelles L, Martinez-de-la-torre M, Paxinos G, Watson C, Martnez S. The chick [150] Rose SP. Gods organism? The chick as a model system for memory studies.
brain in stereotaxic coordinates: an atlas featuring neuromeric subdivisions Learn Mem 2000;7:117.
and mammalian homologies. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2007. [151] Mehlhorn J, Hunt GR, Gray RD, Rehkamper G, Gunturkun O. Tool-making
[121] Smith-Fernandez AS, Pieau C, Reprant J, Boncinelli E, Wassef M. Expression new caledonian crows have large associative brain areas. Brain Behav Evol
of the Emx-1 andDlx-1 homeobox genes dene threemolecularly distinct 2010;75:6370.
domains in the telencephalon of mouse, chick, turtle and frog embryos: [152] Lefebvre L, Reader SM, Sol D. Brains, innovations and evolution in birds and
implications for the evolution of telencephalic subdivisions in amniotes. primates. Brain Behav Evol 2004;63:23346.
Development 1998;125:2099111. [153] Timmermans S, Lefebvre L, Boire D, Basu P. Relative size of the hyperstriatum
[122] Rodrguez F, Lpez JC, Vargas JP, Gmez Y, Broglio C, Salas C. Conservation ventrale is the best predictor of feeding innovation rate in birds. Brain Behav
of spatial memory function in the pallial forebrain of reptiles and ray-nned Evol 2000;56:196203.
shes. J Neurosci 2002;22:2894903. [154] Lefebvre L, Nicolakakis N, Boire D. Tools and brains in birds. Behaviour
[123] Roth TC, Pravosudov VV. Hippocampal volumes and neuron numbers increase 2002;139:93973.
along a gradient of environmental harshness: a large-scale comparison. Proc [155] Gntrkn O. The avian prefrontal cortex and cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol
R Soc Biol Sci 2009;276:4015. 2005;15:68693.
[124] Karten HJ, Hodos W. A stereotaxic atlas of the brain of the pigeon (Columba [156] Braun K, Bock J, Metzger M, Jiang S, Schnabel R. The dorsocaudal neostriatum
livia). Balimore: The Johns Hopkins Press; 1967. of the domestic chick: a structure serving higher associative functions. Behav
[125] Atoji Y, Wild JM. Anatomy of the avian hippocampal formation. Rev Neurosci Brain Res 1999;98:2118.
2006;17:315. [157] Krner S, Gntrkn O. Afferent and efferent connections of the caudolateral
[126] Atoji Y, Wild JM. Limbic system in birds: morphological basis. In: Watan- neostriatum in the pigeon (Columba livia): a retro- and anterograde pathway
abe S, Hofman MA, editors. Integration of comparative neuroanatomy and tracing study. J Comp Neurol 1999;407:22860.
cognition. Tokyo: Keio University Press; 2007. p. 97123. [158] Leutgeb S, Husband S, Riters LV, Shimizu T, Bingman VP. Telencephalic
[127] Szkely AD, Krebs JR. Efferent connectivity of the hippocampal formation of afferents to the caudolateral neostriatum of the pigeon. Brain Res
the zebra nch (Taenopygia guttata): an anterograde pathway tracing study 1996;730:17381.
using Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. J Comp Neurol 1996;368:198214. [159] Metzger M, Jiang S, Braun K. Organization of the dorsocaudal neostriatal
[128] Manger PR, Elston GN, Pettigrew JD. Multiple maps and activity-dependent complex: a retrograde and anterograde tracing study in the domestic chick
representational plasticity in the anterior Wulst of the adult barn owl (Tyto with special emphasis on pathways relevant to imprinting. J Comp Neurol
alba). Eur J Neurosci 2002;16:74350. 1998;395:380404.
[129] Medina L. Do birds and reptiles posses homologues of the mammalian visual, [160] Browning R, Bruce Overmier J, Colombo M. Delay activity in avian prefrontal
somatosensory, and motor cortices? In: Kaas JH, Krubitzer LA, editors. Evolu- cortex sample code or reward code? Eur J Neurosci 2011;33:72635.
tion of the nervous system: a comprehensive reference, 3. London: Academic [161] Kalenscher T, Gntrkn O, Calabrese P, Gehlen W, Kalt T, Diekamp B. Neural
Press; 2007. p. 16394. correlates of a default response in a delayed go/no-go task. J Exp Anal Behav
[130] Ulinski PS. The cerebral cortex of reptiles. In: Jones EG, Peters A, editors. 2005;84:52135.
Cerebral cortex, 8A. New York: Plenum; 1990. p. 21765. [162] Lissek S, Diekamp B, Gntrkn O. Impaired learning of a color reversal task
[131] Butler AB. The evolution of the dorsal pallium in the telencephalon of after NMDA receptor blockade in the pigeon (Columba livia) associative fore-
amniotes: cladistic analysis and a new hypothesis. Brain Res Brain Res Rev brain (neostriatum caudolaterale). Behav Neurosci 2002;116:5239.
1994;19:66101. [163] Divac I, Mogensen J, Bjrklund A. The prefrontal cortex in the pigeon. Bio-
[132] Martnez-Garca F, Martnez-Marcos A, Lanuza E. The pallial amygdala of chemical evidence. Brain Res 1985;332:3658.
amniote vertebrates: evolution of the concept, evolution of the structure. [164] Gntrkn O, Durstewitz D. Multimodal areas of the avian
Brain Res Bull 2002;57:4639. forebrainblueprints for cognition? In: Roth G, Wullimann M, editors.
[133] Striedter GF. The telencephalon of tetrapods in evolution. Brain Behav Evol Brain, evolution and cognition. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag;
1997;49:179213. 2001. p. 43150.
[134] Karten HJ. Evolutionary developmental biology meets the brain: the origins [165] Metzger M, Jiang S, Wang J, Braun K. Organization of the dopaminergic
of mammalian cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997;94:28004. innervation of forebrain areas relevant to learning: a combined immunohis-
[135] Reiner A. Neurotransmitter organization and connections of turtle cortex: tochemical/retrograde tracing study in the domestic chick. J Comp Neurol
implications for the evolution of mammalian isocortex. Comp Biochem Phys- 1996;376:127.
iol A Comp Physiol 1993;104:73548. [166] Wynne B, Gntrkn O. Dopaminergic innervation of the telencephalon of the
[136] Molnr Z, Butler AB. Neuronal changes during forebrain evolution in pigeon (Columba livia): a study with antibodies against tyrosine hydroxylase
amniotes: an evolutionary developmental perspective. Prog Brain Res and dopamine. J Comp Neurol 1995;357:44664.
2002;136:2138. [167] Atoji Y, Wild JM, Yamamoto Y, Suzuki Y. Intratelencephalic connections of the
[137] Aboitiz F. Genetic and developmental homology in amniote brains. Toward hippocampus in pigeons (Columba livia). J Comp Neurol 2002;447:17799.
conciliating radical views of brain evolution. Brain Res Bull 2011;84:12536. [168] Csillag A, Szkely AD, Davies DC. Termination pattern of medial hyperstriatum
[138] Luzzati F, Bonfanti L, Fasolo A, Peretto P. DCX and PSA-NCAM expression ventrale efferents in the archistriatum of the domestic chick. J Comp Neurol
identies a population of neurons preferentially distributed in associative 1994;348:394402.
areas of different pallial derivatives and vertebrate species. Cereb Cortex [169] Atoji Y, Wild JM. Afferent and efferent connections of the dorsolateral
2009;19:102841. corticoid area and a comparison with connections of the temporo-parieto-
N.C. Rattenborg, D. Martinez-Gonzalez / Behavioural Brain Research 222 (2011) 236245 245

occipital area in the pigeon (Columba livia). J Comp Neurol 2005;485: [186] Ookawa T. The electroencephalogram and sleep in the domestic chicken.
16582. Avian Poult Biol Rev 2004;15:18.
[170] Husband SA, Shimizu T. Efferent projections of the ectostriatum in the pigeon [187] Sugihara K, Gotoh J. Depth-electroencephalograms of chickens in wakefulness
(Columba livia). J Comp Neurol 1999;406:32945. and sleep. Jpn J Physiol 1973;23:3719.
[171] Atoji Y, Saito S, Wild JM. Fiber connections of the compact division of the [188] Susic VT, Kovacevic RM. Sleep patterns in the owl Strix aluco. Physiol Behav
posterior pallial amygdala and lateral part of the bed nucleus of the stria 1973;11:3137.
terminalis in the pigeon (Columba livia). J Comp Neurol 2006;499:16182. [189] Szymczak JT. Distribution of sleep and wakefulness in 24-h light-dark cycles
[172] Bingman VP, Casini G, Nocjar C, Jones TJ. Connections of the piriform cortex in in the juvenile and adult magpie, Pica pica. Chronobiologia 1987;14:27787.
homing pigeons (Columba livia) studied with fast blue and WGA-HRP. Brain [190] Van Twyver H, Allison T. A polygraphic and behavioral study of sleep in the
Behav Evol 1994;43:20618. pigeon (Columba livia). Exp Neurol 1972;35:13853.
[173] Reiner A, Karten HJ. Comparison of olfactory bulb projections in pigeons and [191] Siegel JJ, Nitz D, Bingman VP. Hippocampal theta rhythm in awake, freely
turtles. Brain Behav Evol 1985;27:1127. moving homing pigeons. Hippocampus 2000;10:62731.
[174] Casini G, Bingman VP, Bagnoli P. Connections of the pigeon dorsome- [192] Boiko VP, Bures J. Electrical phenomena in the telencephalon of the pigeon
dial forebrain studied with WGA-HRP and 3H-proline. J Comp Neurol during pecking. Neurosci Behav Physiol 1985;15:26570.
1986;245:45470. [193] Atoji Y, Wild JM. Fiber connections of the hippocampal formation and septum
[175] Aboitiz F, Morales D, Montiel J. The evolutionary origin of the mammalian iso- and subdivisions of the hippocampal formation in the pigeon as revealed by
cortex: towards an integrated developmental and functional approach. Behav tract tracing and kainic acid lesions. J Comp Neurol 2004;475:42661.
Brain Sci 2003;26:53552. [194] Krayniak PF, Siegel A. Efferent connections of the septal area in the pigeon.
[176] Hoogland PV, Vermeulen-VanderZee E. Medial cortex of the lizard Gekko Brain Behav Evol 1978;15:389404.
gecko: a hodological study with emphasis on regional specialization. J Comp [195] Montagnese CM, Szkely AD, Adm A, Csillag A. Efferent connections of
Neurol 1993;331:32638. septal nuclei of the domestic chick (Gallus domesticus): an anterograde path-
[177] Ten Donkelaar HJ. Reptiles. In: Nieuwenhuys R, ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C, way tracing study with a bearing on functional circuits. J Comp Neurol
editors. The central nervous system of vertebrates, 2. Berlin: SpringerVerlag; 2004;469:43756.
1998. p. 131525. [196] Ookawa T, Gotoh J. Electroencephalogram of the chicken recorded from the
[178] Ulinski PS. Dorsal ventricular ridge: a treatise on forebrain organization in skull under various conditions. J Comp Neurol 1965;124:114.
reptiles and birds. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1983. [197] Aboitiz F, Montiel J. Origin and evolution of the vertebrate telencephalon,
[179] Lesku JA, Vyssotski AL, Martinez-Gonzalez D, Wilzeck C, Rattenborg NC. Local with special reference to the mammalian neocortex. Adv Anat Embryol Cell
sleep homeostasis in the avian brain: convergence of sleep function in mam- Biol 2007;194:1116.
mals and birds? Proc R Soc B, in press, doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.2316. [198] Strasser R, Ehrlinger JM, Bingman VP. Transitive behavior in hippocampal-
[180] Low PS, Shank SS, Sejnowski TJ, Margoliash D. Mammalian-like features of lesioned pigeons. Brain Behav Evol 2004;63:1818.
sleep structure in zebra nches. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008;105:90816. [199] Alonso PD, Milner AC, Ketcham RA, Cookson MJ, Rowe TB. The avian nature
[181] Martinez-Gonzalez D, Lesku JA, Rattenborg NC. Increased EEG spectral of the brain and inner ear of Archaeopteryx. Nature 2004;430:6669.
power density during sleep following short-term sleep deprivation in [200] Andreu MJ, Dvila JC, Real MA, Guirado S. Intrinsic connections in the anterior
pigeons (Columba livia): evidence for avian sleep homeostasis. J Sleep Res dorsal ventricular ridge of the lizard Psammodromus algirus. J Comp Neurol
2008;17:14053. 1996;372:4958.
[182] Rattenborg NC, Martinez-Gonzalez D, Lesku JA. Avian sleep homeostasis: [201] Lanuza E, Belekhova M, Martinez-Marcos A, Font C, Martinez-Garcia F. Iden-
convergent evolution of complex brains, cognition and sleep functions in tication of the reptilian basolateral amygdala: an anatomical investigation
mammals and birds. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2009;33:25370. of the afferents to the posterior dorsal ventricular ridge of the lizard Podarcis
[183] Berger RJ, Walker JM. Sleep in the burrowing owl (Speotyto cunicularia hypu- hispanica. Eur J Neurosci 1998;10:351734.
gaea). Behav Biol 1972;7:18394. [202] Martnez-Garcia F, Novejarque A, Lanuza E. Evolution of the amygdala
[184] Dos Santos MM, Hoeller AA, dos Santos TS, Felisbino MB, Herdt MA, da Silva ES, in vertebrates. In: Kaas JH, Krubitzer LA, editors. Evolution of the ner-
et al. Behavioural and electroencephalographic effects of systemic injections vous system: a comprehensive reference, 3. London: Academic Press; 2007.
of 8-OHDPAT in the pigeon (Columba livia). Behav Brain Res 2009;201:24456. p. 255334.
[185] Fuchs T, Siegel JJ, Burgdorf J, Bingman VP. A selective serotonin reup- [203] Iwaniuk AN, Heesy CP, Hall MI, Wylie DR. Relative Wulst volume is correlated
take inhibitor reduces REM sleep in the homing pigeon. Physiol Behav with orbit orientation and binocular visual eld in birds. J Comp Physiol A
2006;87:57581. Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2008;194:26782.

You might also like