You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Voice

Vol. 11, No. 1, pp. 1-11


01997 Lippincott-Raven Publishers, Philadelphia

Comparison of Vocal Tract Formants in Singing and


Nonperiodic Phonation

Donald G. Miller, *Arend M. Sulter, Harm K. Schutte, and tRienhart F. Wolf

Department of Medical Physiology, Voice Research Laboratory, and *ENT Clinic and "~Department of Radiology,
University Hospital Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands

Summary: The skilled use of nonperiodic phonation techniques in combination


with spectrum analysis has been proposed here as a practical method for lo-
cating formant frequencies in the singing voice. The study addresses the ques-
tion of the degree of similarity between sung phonations and their nonperiodic
imitations, with respect to both frequency of the first two formants as well as
posture of the vocal tract. Using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), linear
predictive coding (LPC), and spectrum analysis, two types of nonperiodic
phonation (ingressive and vocal fry) are compared with singing phonations to
determine the degree of similarity/difference in acoustic and spatial dimensions
of the vocal tract when these phonation types are used to approximate the
postures of singing. In comparing phonation types, the close similarity in
acoustic data in combination with the relative dissimilarity in spatial data in-
dicates that the accurate imitations are not primarily the result of imitating the
singing postures, but have instead an aural basis. Key Words: Vocal tract--
Singing--MRI--Vowel formants--Nonperiodic phonation--Spectrum analy-
sis.

It is common for singers trained in the Western monics, which are integral multiples of the funda-
operatic tradition to conceive of the vocal instru- mental frequency, assures that the formants are
ment as consisting of three basic components: the never very far (in frequency) from a harmonic; how-
respiratory apparatus, the oscillating vocal folds, ever, at the higher frequencies of singing, especially
and the vocal tract. The vocal tract, with its highly that of higher pitched voices, the proximity of for-
variable posture, is directly responsible, not only mants to (widely spaced) harmonics is a prominent
for the text in singing, but also for what is often and even a critical consideration in the singer's
vaguely described as "resonance," an important "resonance."
factor in the strength and quality of a given voice. The same wide spacing of harmonics, which
The effectiveness of this "resonance" presumably makes the locations of the formants along the fre-
depends upon the configuration of the resonances quency spectrum critical, makes it difficult for the
(the formants, especially FI and F2) of the vocal singer (and scientist) precisely to determine formant
tract, such that certain harmonics of the voice frequencies of sung tones. Furthermore, the prob-
source are well amplified. At the relatively low fre- lem is aggravated as F0 (and thus the distance be-
quencies of speech, the close spacing of the har- tween harmonics) increases. An informal method
exists, however, for dealing with this problem: if
Accepted December 6, 1995. the singer is able to maintain, or reproduce, the
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Donald specific posture of the vocal tract used for a given
G. Miller, Department of Medical Physiology, Voice Research
Laboratory, Bloemsingel 10, N L 9712 KZ Groningen, The Neth- combination of vowel and F o, while substituting a
erlands. nonperiodic sound source, the resultant power spec-
2 D.G. MILLER ET AL.

trum can reveal the formant frequencies with a high in singing a particular vowel and pitch must be
degree of precision. Subsequent comparison of this maintained and/or imitated in the alternative proce-
(continuous) spectrum with the (line) spectrum of dure.
the sung tone can reveal the relationship between
even minute modifications of the vowel and changes METHODS
in the "resonance," interpreted as the total impact
of the various formants on proximate harmonics of Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) has both ad-
the voice source. Although the third, fourth, and vantages and disadvantages when compared with
fifth formants have an important role in forming the other techniques for viewing bodily structures not
"singer's formant," it is generally one or both of available to the unaided eye. Compared with lateral
the two lowest formants that carry the major part of x-ray photography, which historically has been the
the sound pressure of a sung tone. The controlled source of much of our knowledge of the dimensions
adjustments in singing of these two formants, which and positioning of the less accessible portions of the
are the focus of this study, have been described as vocal tract, MRI gives clearer pictures of the soft
"formant tuning" (1-3). tissues, presents no potential danger to the subject,
We have made extensive use of this informal and can locate points in three dimensions. Disad-
method and reported on it in this journal in 1990 (4). vantages of the procedure, apart from its cost, are
While it continues to have an important place in our the relatively long exposure time, high level of
applications to singing pedagogy, an obvious ques- noise, and restriction to the supine position of the
tion remains regarding the degree of similarity be- subject. These disadvantages, however, seemed
tween the vocal tract while singing and the vocal manageable: the apparatus available delivered pic-
tract while attempting to reproduce the singing vo- tures with a good resolution in 30 s, a duration
cal-tract posture. Our practical experience with the within the capability of our singer-subject; phona-
reproducibility of formant frequencies suggested tions for acoustic analysis could be produced se-
that such singing and nonsinging postures might be quentially, rather than simultaneously, with the
highly similar and even, in the best cases, indistin- MRI procedure; informal comparison of singing and
guishable. When an opportunity arose to compare nonperiodic phonation in the supine position re-
them by means of magnetic resonance imaging, it vealed that the formant frequencies did not differ by
became possible to test this hypothesis. more than - 5 % from those produced in the custom-
Three methods were explored of producing a con- ary upright position (5).
tinuous spectrum from a vocal tract in the posture In order to test the degree of spectral and postural
of singing: vocal fry, ingressive phonation, and a similarity between repetitions of sung phonations,
vibrator applied to the exterior of the neck. The first as well as the similarity of phonations using nonpe-
two are techniques of nonperiodic phonation. Ade- riodic techniques (ingressive and vocal fry phona-
quate vocal fry for this purpose can be produced by tion, henceforth termed collectively "imitations"),
most subjects with only a few minutes' practice. It the following procedure was used. The single sub-
needs amplification to achieve comparable intensity ject, a bass-baritone with many years of profes-
with a sung tone. Ingressive phonation, a sort of sional singing and voice training experience (one of
vocal fry in reverse, usually requires more practice, the authors), was placed in the position to be used
but has the advantage of producing a sound with an for the MR image acquisition. He then recorded (at
intensity near that of (loud) speech. The use of the a microphone distance of 20 cm) a sustained, sung
vibrator, intended as a control, has the advantage of /i/, followed by imitations of that phonation, first in
not requiring a type of phonation differing from that ingressive, then vocal fry, with each preceded by
of singing. We had not used it extensively because short repetitions of the sung vowel as an aid to au-
of the weak sound and consequent indistinct spec- ditory recollection. This series was immediately fol-
trum that it generated in some subjects, presumably lowed by the same procedure for the vowels/u/,/a/
because the walls of their necks damped the sound. and/e/. Thus, 12 samples of acoustic data were re-
The obvious disadvantage of all three procedures corded (three phonation types times four vowels).
is that the nonperiodic phonation cannot occur or, Then the subject, positioned in the scanner with the
in the case of the vibrator, be measured (because of vocal tract in the magnetic isocenter, repeated the
its relative weakness) simultaneously with the sing- procedure, sustaining each of the twelve phona-
ing. In each case, the posture of the vocal tract used tions, plus a silent simulation of each vowel, for 30

Journal of Voice, Vol. I1, No. 1, 1997


N O N P E R I OD IC P H O N A TION 3

s. Because of the noise generated by MRI, no The accuracy with which the ear discriminates
acoustic recordings were made during image acqui- differences in formant frequencies appears to be
sition. The entire procedure was repeated three greater than that claimed for the measurement tech-
times ("sessions") in the course of 2.5 h, rounded niques used in the Monsen and Engebretson study
off with a fourth and final acoustic session. (6). Flanagan (7) found difference limens of -21 Hz
The choice of vowels--two each of open, close, for the first formant and - 5 0 Hz for F 2. Translating
front, and back--was motivated by the intention to these values into musical terms, it seems plausible
include the basic vowel types. The nominal pitch of to hypothesize that skilled singers could develop a
the sung phonations was always D3 ( - 150 Hz), cho- sensitivity threshold of a semitone difference, at
sen because it is comfortable for the subject to sus- least in ranges where that difference would have
tain (yet sufficiently removed from his habitual noticeable consequences in resonance. For this rea-
speaking pitch to assure a singing model), and be- son we use this frequency difference (+-6%) as a
cause at that relatively high density of harmonics practical criterion of accuracy in locating FI and F 2.
the inclination to " t u n e " the formants is presum- Thus, in the present study, we would consider a
ably not strong. repetition or an imitation (with a different phonation
type) "accurate" if the formant frequencies fall
within the semitone range. (It will be noted that this
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
sets a higher standard for accuracy than the -+60 Hz
The measured data of interest for this study are of of the Monsen and Engebretson study, which is
two basic kinds: spatial data regarding the dimen- greater than a semitone in most of the FI and F 2
sions of the vocal tract, and acoustic data (fre- range.)
quency domain), from the various phonations. Both For a strict quantitative comparison of formant
can be displayed in figures, although comparing the frequencies in repeated, as well as imitated, phona-
two kinds on the basis of formant frequency values tions, it is necessary to assign values to the for-
will require a more strict quantitative treatment. mants in sung phonations. We do this with the aid of
an LPC program (from the ILS package), while not
Acoustic data forgetting the reservations concerning its accuracy
Figure 1 presents spectral displays of the re- mentioned above.
corded phonations, arranged according to vowel. Tables 1 and 2 list the mean formant frequencies
For each vowel, a selected vocal fry and an ingres- for F z and F2, respectively, of the four vowels and
sive phonation is arranged vertically over a sung three phonation types, as measured by the LPC
phonation. A vertical line drawn through each of program. The SDs give an indication of the scatter
the two lowest formants in the ingressive example of data in repeating the measurements over the four
allows a comparison with formant positions in the recording sessions. For the nonperiodic phona-
other two phonation types. As a first approxima- tions, these measurements were checked for gross
tion, it can be seen that the nonperiodic phonations error by locating spectral peaks with a cursor in
give reasonably consistent approximations of the spectral displays illustrated in Fig. I. Using -+l
position of F~ and F 2 in the sung phonations. semitone (6%) as a standard of accuracy, among the
It must be borne in mind, however, that the prob- repeated measurements there are only two cases of
lems in determining the formant frequencies (as (apparent) conspicuous inaccuracy [F I of sung/u/
well as the potential yield in "resonance") of a sung (12%) and/~/(13%)] and two marginally inaccurate
tone grow sharply with increasing F0. In a careful [/u/FI ingressive a n d / u / F 2 vocal fry (each 7%)].
study designed expressly to compare the accuracy Excluding the two exceptionally high values, the
of formant frequency estimation by automated lin- mean percentage deviation in repetitions comes to
ear predictive coding (LPC) with that by skilled 4.1%, well within the narrow standard of accuracy
spectrum analysis, Monsen and Engebretson (6) we adopted and even smaller than the values mea-
found both methods accurate to within -+60 Hz in sured by Flanagan (7).
the F 0 range I00-300, but decreasing greatly in ac- Inspection of the spectra of the four vowels sug-
curacy at 350 Hz and higher. This would imply that gests an explanation of the two anomalous values.
highly accurate formant measurements of sung In b o t h / i / a n d / a / t h e LPC program locates F i al-
tones cannot necessarily be expected even at our most precisely on a dominant harmonic, flanked
chosen F 0 of 150 Hz. symmetrically by weaker harmonics, resulting in

Journal of Voice, Vol. 11, No. 1, 1997


4 D . G . MILLE R E T A L .

vowel lul
vowel Iil F1
F
F2

t I
1 2
I I
3 4
I I
5 I I I I I I
kHz 0 1 2 3 4 5
kHz
vowel / e /
vowel/a/ F.
F1
F2

voo.,,r

I I I I I I
I I I [ I I
0 1 2 3 4 5 0 1 2 3 4 5
kHz kHz
FIG. 1. Spectral display of sung phonations (nominal pitch D~) of four vowels, shown together with their nonperiodic "imitations."
Vertical lines drawn through the first two formants allows comparison with formant locations in phonations of the three different types.
Journal of Voice, Vol. 11, No. 1, 1997
NONPERIODIC PHONATION 5

T A B L E 1. Mean frequencies, with SDs, of first formants of four vowels, according to


phonation type, as measured by linear predictive coding
Phonation type Vowel/i/ Vowel/u/ Vowel/a/ Vowel/d
Sung 308 I I Hz 386 48 Hz 591 10 Hz 568 44 Hz
4% 12% 2% 13%
lngressive 366 20 Hz 391 28 Hz 605 22 Hz 538 27 Hz
5% 7% 4% 5%
Vocal fry 358 17 Hz 394 20 Hz 597 21 Hz 530 16 Hz
5% 5% 4% 3%

Mean value SD, percentage deviation.

exceptionally low standard deviations. In /u/ and three semitones. Repeated informal trials showed
/e/, on the other hand, the Ft configuration of har- this to be a predictable systematic error on the part
monics is strongly asymmetrical. It seems more of the subject, an error that would have to be taken
likely that the large error--nearly twice that of any into account in any application of the (nonperiodic
other measurements--is the result of limitations of imitative) method to determine formant frequen-
LPC in measuring harmonic sound (consider the cies. We shall return to this discrepancy in our dis-
---60 Hz error in the Monsen and Engebretson cussion of the acoustic and spatial data taken to-
study), rather than exceptional inconsistency on the gether.
part of the subject. An alternative explanation,
which we find less persuasive, would be that the Spatial data
subject uses the dominant harmonics of Ft in/i/and Figure 2 shows the midsagittal slice taken from a
/a/as a guide for precise repetition. 30-s exposure of the three phonation types in a
The lower level of accuracy in the repetitions of given vowel and session, arranged horizontally.
/u/was not unexpected. This has been a "problem Vertically below these are the corresponding results
vowel" for our subject, who reports never having from one of the remaining sessions. Both were se-
found a consistent, satisfactory pattern for singing lected as those displaying the highest degree of vi-
the vowel. Reduced accuracy of repetition was also sual clarity. Contrary to our hypothesis concerning
evident in the greater scatter of the MRI data for/u/. the similarity of vocal tract postures for similar for-
F 2 o f / u / ( w h e r e vocal fry has a distinctly lower F2 mant frequencies, the differences between phona-
than the other types) is one of only two places in the tion types are readily distinguishable on visual ex-
data where the spread between phonation types ex- amination. However limited the resolution pre-
ceeds the 6% standard of accuracy. sented here, it is apparent that, for a given vowel,
The other instance of excessive spread between similarity is noticeably greater among repetitions of
phonation types presents a more challenging anom- the same phonation type than it is between phona-
aly: the discrepancy between F I in the sung/i/and tion types.
its two nonperiodic imitations, which are in close The calculation of formant frequencies from the
agreement. The difference is on the order of 60 Hz, spatial dimensions of the vocal tract is an elaborate
within the standard of accuracy established in the undertaking. Working with MRI, Baer et al. (8) pub-
Monsen and Engebretson (6) study, but in excess of lished such a study for the "corner vowels"/u/,/a/,

TABLE 2. Mean frequencies, with SDs, of second formants of four vowels, according
to phonation type, as measured by linear predictive coding
Phonation type Vowel/i/ Vowel/u/ Vowel/a/ Vowel//
Sung 1,791 47 Hz 1,009 43 Hz 1,091 33 Hz 1,446 25 Hz
3% 4% 3% 2%
Ingressive 1,732 - 72 Hz 978 -+ 38 Hz 1,034 59 Hz 1,431 -+ 45 Hz
4% 4% 6% 3%
Vocal fry !,758 -+ 91 Hz 894 59 Hz 1,069 30 Hz 1,445 38 Hz
5% 7% 3% 3%

Mean value SD, percentage deviation.

Journal of Voice, Vol. I1, No. 1, 1997


6 D . G . M I L L E R E T AL.

vowel/i/
sung ingressive vocal fry

session 1

session 2
FIG. 2. Midsagittal views taken
from 30-s MR1 e x p o s u r e s of
phonating vocal tract. For each
I vowel, phonation types can be
compared with each other, as
vowel/u/ well as with similar phonations
from a different session (contin-
ues on opposite page).
sung ingressive vocal fry

session 2

session 3

/ae/, and /i/ in 1991. By varying the plane of the tic) values of the first two formants exceeded sub-
imaging, they were able to establish values for stantially the standard of precision adopted in the
three-dimensional slices of the whole length of the present study. For the vowels/a/,/ae/, and/i/, the
vocal tract, thus arriving at measured vocal tract mean discrepancy was 10.4% for F~ and 12.5% for
area functions for two subjects, both including and F 2. For/u/(which appeared to be a problem vowel
excluding the piriform sinuses. Adopting a previ- in their study as well as the present one), the dis-
ously developed algorithm, they calculated formant crepancies were 19% and 54% for FI and F2, re-
values and compared them with those analysed spectively.
from acoustic measurements. The discrepancies be- In the present study, where spatial data is limited
tween the calculated (spatial) and measured (acous- to (two-dimensional) midsagittal MR images, there

Journal of Voice, Vol. II, No. I, 1997


NONPERIODIC PHONA TION 7

vowel/a/
sung ingressive vocal fry

session 1

session 2

FIG. 2. (continued) Midsagittal


views taken from 30-s MRI
exposures of phonating vocal
tract. vowel/E/
sung ingressive vocal fry

session 1

session 2

can be no question of accurately calculating for- frequencies reflect similar vocal tract postures
mant frequencies, let alone achieving the 6% stan- among the phonation types, is the dimension vocal
dard of precision adopted for the acoustic data. tract length. In the case of the vowel/~/, which has
Some key dimensions of the vocal tract, however, no evident tongue constriction, the resonant behav-
can be extracted from the midsagittal images for ior of the vocal tract ideally resembles that of a pipe
quantitative treatment. We present two of these be- closed at one end, and the formant frequencies have
low, together with the rationale for considering a nearly linear relationship to the vocal tract length
them key dimensions. (9). Table 3 gives the values of this dimension, mea-
The first of these, which also presents the stron- sured along the midline of the vocal tract, for the
gest support for the hypothesis that similar formant phonation types singing, ingressive, and vocal fry in

Journal of Voice, Vol. 11, No. I, 1997


8 D . G . MILLER ET AL.

TABLE 3. Values for vocal tract length, measured where F is the resonant frequency, c the speed of
from MR images, arranged by phonation type and sound, S the area of constriction, I ' the effective
session, for the vowel/~" length of the constriction, and V the enclosed vol-
Session ume of the resonant cavity.
i 2 3 Because the constriction area has a nearly linear
relationship to the square of the constriction width
Singing 188.3 190.0 191. I
Ingressive 188.8 189.4 196.8 (8), it is apparent from Eq. 1 that a doubling of this
Vocal fry 188.2 195.8 189.5 dimension could be expected to have a pronounced
a The differences fall within a 4% range, and there is no sys-
effect on the resonance frequency. The values for
tematic variation by phonation type. the tongue constriction for all the vowels are dis-
played graphically in Fig. 3. The scatter of values is
quite large. This could be due in part to the limit of
all three sessions. In spite of the differences among resolution in the images, which was estimated at - 1
the phonation types in direction and magnitude of mm (the width of a single pixel), but that factor
air pressure on the larynx, the variation is only cannot account for the major part of the scatter.
-+4%, comparable to the high acoustic standard of Considering the data for the constriction width as
precision. a function of phonation type, it becomes apparent
The other dimension examined here is that of the that there is a systematic basis for differences in this
tongue constriction, i.e., the distance between the dimension: in all four vowels ingressive phonation
tongue and the opposite side of the vocal tract at its results in a consistently greater constriction width.
narrowest point in the midsagittal section. We con- A Tukey post hoc test indicates a significant differ-
sider the cases o f / i / and /a/, vowels where the ence (p < 0.05) between the ingressive phonation
"back cavity" of the vocal tract is presumed to type and both singing and vocal fry.
behave like a so-called Helmholtz resonator (9).
The dimension "constriction area" occurs in the Data considered together
basic formula for the lowest resonance of such a Comparing the acoustic with the spatial data, one
resonator: is faced with a puzzle. On the one hand, the subject
demonstrates the ability to produce accurate acous-
c~S tic imitations, with respect to the first two formant
F = 2-~ ' . V (1)
frequencies, among various phonation types. On

15 15
/i/ [] session I
/u/
0 session2
session 3
E J0 J0
r~ O
o V "02
.o V o
V
0
~ 5
o
0
u
0
u O O
FIG. 3. Values for the "tongue
constriction" (see text), mea-
SING IN VF SING IN VF sured from the three sessions of
15 MR images, arranged by vowel
/a/ /El and phonation type (singing, in-
gressive, and vocal fry). The
systematically higher magnitude
10 ,~, I0
x7 ,..., of this dimension in ingressive
O 0 phonation is evident.
",~
u

O 0
O

SING IN VF SING IN VF

voice type voice type

Journal of Voice, Vol. It, No. l, 1997


NONPERIODIC PHONA TION 9

the other hand, the MR images reveal markedly, the "vowel tuning" o f / i / i s focused o n F 2 and thus
and in some respects systematically, dissimilar vo- tolerates imprecision in FI. It is conceivable that
cal tract articulations among the phonation types. discrepancies in F 2 (especially vocal fry) in the
These facts seem sufficient evidence to falsify the vowel/u/result from a similar, but opposite, focus
hypothesis that the accurate imitations are achieved of the vowel tuning on the first formant. The data on
by maintaining or repeating a specific posture of the /u/, however, are clouded by the problem of repeat-
vocal tract. ability mentioned earlier.
The accurate reproduction of the first two for- The hypothesis offered regarding aural, rather
mant frequencies of the vowels appears to have an than postural, control of "vowel tuning" requires
aural basis. In order to understand the control for confirmation a broader study than the present
mechanism involved, it is helpful to distinguish be- one with only one subject. There is one more point
tween three types of tuning which the singer en- in the present study, however, which supports the
gages in. The first, which we designate "pitch hypothesis of aural control. In the present study,
matching," refers to what is meant in saying that each time MR images of sung, ingressive, and vocal
someone sings in (or out of) tune: his pitch matches fry phonations were made, a third imitative type
that of accompanying instruments or, more funda- was added: a mute holding of the position of the
mentally, stands in a correct relationship to pitches sung phonation. The intention was to use this as the
previously sung. This tuning is achieved by adjust- MRI equivalent of an acoustic recording employing
ing the repeat pattern (Fo) of the voice source. a vibrator applied externally to the neck wall as a
The second is an adjustment of the vocal tract for "voice source." (The strong magnetic field of the
the purpose of obtaining the desired "resonance" apparatus precluded using the vibrator during MR
from a given vowel and harmonic spacing. Because image acquisition.) The 12 images thus acquired of-
the harmonic spacing is determined by the pre- ten showed vocal tract postures far removed, in
scribed F 0, the adjustment is made in the formants various directions, from those made while sustain-
of the vocal tract. Within the constraints presented ing vocal sound. In the absence of aural feedback,
by the requirement that a vowel be recognizable our subject apparently had very little awareness of
(or, say, have a particular "color"), the singer "modi- the posture his vocal tract had assumed.
ties" the vowel to achieve greater loudness, "vowel
equalization" (a singers' term referring to a desired FINAL REMARKS AND CONCLUSIONS
degree of similarity among the various vowels), or
the like. We shall call this adjustment "resonance Using the first two formant frequencies as crite-
tuning," a term that would include the narrower ria, the experiment demonstrated a high degree of
sense of "formant tuning," or adjusting the center accuracy (with the exceptions noted) in one singer's
frequency of a formant to coincide with that of a repetitions and (nonperiodic) imitations of sung
harmonic. tones. The generalizations that we can draw from
The third is also an adjustment of the vocal tract this, however, are limited. Nonperiodic imitations
to (re)produce, in detail, the nuances of a given can never give a definitive determination of formant
vowel (including those adopted in "resonance tun- frequencies during singing, but only a more or less
ing"), but now without respect to harmonic struc- accurate indication of where they might be. The
ture of the voice source. We give this the name present measurements only show that, at least un-
"vowel tuning." In the case where a subject imi- der certain conditions, it is possible for a skilled
tates his own sounds at different Fo's or with non- subject to make accurate imitations. Extrapolation
periodic phonation, we would expect to find Ft and of these results to other situations depends upon
F2 (the "vowel formants") as close as possible, in two basic factors: the skill of the subject and the
the "imitation," to their frequencies in the original difficulty of the given imitation. Elements of the
sound. skill include control of the voice source and aural
If it is indeed the aural control of such "vowel sensitivity in the three types of tuning, areas where
tuning" that accurately reproduced the formant fre- trained singers might be expected to excel. Regard-
quencies in the imitations of the sung tones, then ing the degree of difficulty of the imitation, the vo-
what can account for the apparent lapse of such cal tract may assume extreme positions in some
control in the case of FI of the vowel/i/? A specu- parts of the singer's range, and these are not always
lative, but plausible answer to this question is that easily maintained in combination with nonperiodic

Journal of Voice, Vol. 11, No. 1, 1997


10 D. G. M I L L E R E T AL.

phonation. Nonextreme postures of the vocal tract more, the ability to perceive and to imagine desir-
were used in the present study because of the 30-s able nuances of sound. This constitutes a major part
duration requirement of MRI and in order to pro- of both the "talent" of the singer--as aural/vocal
vide sufficient density of harmonics for a clear first imagination--and the task of the teacher, as anti-
approximation of the formants in the sung phona- cipating and guiding a long-term development to-
tion. ward a sound which suits the individual organ (see,
With all the caveats, however, nonperiodic pho- for example, Franziska Martienssen-Lohman [10]).
nation in conjunction with real time spectrum anal- The acquisition and refined application of this aural
ysis remains a potentially powerful practical tool in imagination occupy the singer's efforts to an extent
exploring the resonances of the vocal tract, partic- that would hardly occur to the average layman.
ularly as these relate to varying fundamental fre- Vowel "color" and voice "placement" are con-
quency. In other words, it may prove to be a sig- cepts from singers' terminology that are frequently
nificant control for what we have called resonance used in reference to the sounds singers produce.
tuning. Its advantages include the simplicity and Skilled singers become adept at reproducing spe-
quickness of immediately comparing a sung tone cific combinations of pitch, vowel and intensity
with its nonperiodic imitation. It is self-correcting, with what they consider a proper placement and
in that the sung spectrum can always be used to desired color. We have referred to this process
check the plausibility of the imitation; thus, skill in above as "resonance tuning." Although the con-
its use may develop rapidly. cept of placement can be regarded as an illusion that
A more thorough assessment of the value of non- serves as the singer's control--the voice cannot lit-
periodic phonation in exploring the vocal tract res- erally be placed--to the extent that such "place-
onances during singing will have to come from a ment" can be heard, it is not illusory, but must
broader study than this. Here we shall restrict our- necessarily be present in the acoustic signal. Locat-
selves to two conclusions: (a) that it is possible for ing the formants is the first step in an objective
a skilled practitioner to match, with nonperiodic description of resonance tuning, which, as defined
phonation, the first two formants of his own sung here, includes the singers' "placement."
phonation with a high degree of accuracy; and (b) Locating the formants by imitating sung sounds
that the imitative process in such matching of for- with a nonperiodic voice source is not part of the
mants does not appear to be based primarily on normal practice of singers. In order to accomplish
proprioception of the posture of the vocal tract. it, singers must increase their aural skills by adding
And we present an hypothesis: that the control "vowel tuning" to the "resonance tuning" which is
mechanism for adjustment of the vocal tract in this already part of their practice. Acquiring accuracy
process is primarily aural. and consistency in this method need not be a hap-
hazard process, because it can be guided by spec-
A PEDAGOGICAL NOTE ON LOCATING tral comparison of the sung sound and its nonperi-
FORMANT FREQUENCIES odic imitation. Implausible results are rejected, and
problematical ones are explored further, preferably
The initial hypothesis--that the similarity of for- in the company of others who have already gained
mant configurations between sung tones and their experience. In this respect it has much in common
imitations with a nonperiodic source implies a com- with established practices of voice training.
parable similarity in the postures of the vocal As the use of feedback from spectrum analysis
tract--was falsified by the experiment. This falsifi- becomes more commonplace, it is to be expected
cation, however, makes the high level of precision that singers and their teachers will increasingly sup-
in matching the frequencies of the first two for- plement their basic aural information on resonance
mants seem all the more remarkable. The authors tuning with objective measurements of the acoustic
submit that the information obtained in this practice signal. When there is a more widespread realization
of locating formants can be of considerable impor- of the critical importance of formant frequencies in,
tance in singing pedagogy. say, the transitions between what singers recognize
In the writings of leading teachers of singing one as registers (l 1-13), it seems likely that this power-
can find a strong emphasis on the role of the sing- ful tool will get more attention, whether through the
er's " e a r . " What is meant, of course, is not only the practice of locating formants by nonperiodic pho-
ability to sing in tune ("pitch matching"), but far nation or by some other practical application.

Journal of Voice, Vol. II, No. 1, 1997


NONPERIODIC PHONA TION 11

Acknowledgment: This work was supported in part by a resonance characteristics of the vocal tract: a study with
grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific MRI. Magn Reson lmag 1992;10:365-73.
Research (NWO). We appreciate very much the assis- 6. Monsen RB, Engebretson AM. The accuracy of formant fre-
tance of Meindert Goslinga for his work on the figures. quency measurements: a comparison of spectrographic anal-
This article was first presented at the 22nd Annual Sym- ysis and linear prediction. J Speech Hear Res 1983 ;26: 89-97.
7. Flanagan JL. A difference limen for vowel formant fre-
posium on the Care of the Professional Voice, Philadel-
quency. J Acoust Soc Am 1955;27:613-7.
phia, Pennsylvania, June 1993. 8. Baer T, Gore JC, Gracco LC, Nye PW. Analysis of vocal
tract shape and dimensions using magnetic resonance imag-
ing: vowels. J Acoust Soc Am 1991 ;90:799--828.
REFERENCES 9. Fant CGM. Acoustic theory o f speech production. Paris:
Mouton, 1970.
1. Sundberg J. The acoustics of the singing voice. Sci Am 1977; 10. Martienssen-Lohman F. Der wissende Siinger. Gesangslexi-
236:82-92. kon in Skizzen. Z/irich: Atlantis Musikbuch-Verlag, 1981:
2. Miller DG. Formant tuning in a professional baritone. J 175-8.
Voice 1990;4:231-7. II. Schutte HK, Miller DG. Belting and pop, nonclassical ap-
3. Schutte HK, Miller DG. The effect of fo/f~ coincidence in proaches to the female middle voice: some preliminary con-
soprano high notes on pressure at the glottis. J Phonetics siderations. J Voice 1993;7:142-50.
1986; 14:385-92. 12. Miller DG, Schutte HK. Toward a definition of male " h e a d "
4. Miller DG, Schutte HK. Feedback from spectrum analysis register, "passaggio" and " c o v e r " in Western operatic
applied to the singing voice. J Voice 1990;4:329-34. singing. Fo! Phon Logop 1994;46:157-70.
5. Sulter AM, Miller DG, Wolf RF, Schutte HK, Wit HP, 13. Miller DG, Schutte HK. Physical definition of the "flageolet
Mooyaart EL. On the relation between the dimensions and register." J Voice 1993;7:206-12.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 11, No, I, 1997

You might also like