You are on page 1of 9

The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as a

Predictor of Parent-Reported Diagnosis of Autism


Spectrum Disorder and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity
Disorder
Ginny Russell1*, Lauren R. Rodgers2, Tamsin Ford1
1 Institute of Health Services Research, University of Exeter Medical School, Exeter, United Kingdom, 2 NIHR CLAHRC for the South West Peninsula (PenCLAHRC), University
of Exeter Medical School, Exeter, United Kingdom

Abstract
The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) is widely used as an international standardised instrument measuring
child behaviour. The primary aim of our study was to examine whether behavioral symptoms measured by SDQ were
elevated among children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) relative
to the rest of the population, and to examine the predictive value of the SDQ for outcome of parent-reported clinical
diagnosis of ASD/ADHD. A secondary aim was to examine the extent of overlap in symptoms between children diagnosed
with these two disorders, as measured by the SDQ subscales. A cross-sectional secondary analysis of data from the
Millennium Birth Cohort (n = 19,519), was conducted. Data were weighted to be representative of the UK population as a
whole. ADHD or ASD identified by a medical doctor or health professional were reported by parents in 2008 and this was
the case definition of diagnosis; (ADHD n = 173, ASD n = 209, excluding twins and triplets). Study childrens ages ranged
from 6.38.2 years; (mean 7.2 years). Logistic regression was used to examine the association between the parent-reported
clinical diagnosis of ASD/ADHD and teacher and parent-reported SDQ subscales. All SDQ subscales were strongly associated
with both ASD and ADHD. There was substantial co-occurrence of behavioral difficulties between children diagnosed with
ASD and those diagnosed with ADHD. After adjustment for other subscales, the final model for ADHD, contained
hyperactivity/inattention and impact symptoms only and had a sensitivity of 91% and specificity of 90%; (AUC) = 0.94 (95%
CI, 0.900.97). The final model for ASD was composed of all subscales except the peer problems scales, indicating of the
complexity of behavioural difficulties that may accompany ASD. A threshold of 0.03 produced model sensitivity and
specificity of 79% and 93% respectively; AUC = 0.90 (95% CI, 0.860.95). The results support changes to DSM-5 removing
exclusivity clauses.

Citation: Russell G, Rodgers LR, Ford T (2013) The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as a Predictor of Parent-Reported Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum
Disorder and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. PLoS ONE 8(12): e80247. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080247
Editor: Atsushi Senju, Birkbeck, University of London, United Kingdom
Received May 23, 2013; Accepted October 1, 2013; Published December 3, 2013
Copyright: 2013 Russell et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits
unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding: Funding was provided by the ESRC as part of the Secondary Data Analysis Initiative, Grant Number ES/K003356/1. The authors also acknowledge
funding from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care (CLAHRC) for the South West
Peninsula. The views and opinions expressed in this paper are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
* E-mail: g.russell@ex.ac.uk

Introduction The SDQ has been used in in clinical practice as a screening


and/or assessment tool by both school psychologists [3] and
The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) is a brief clinicians [2,4,5]. It is also used extensively in research studies
dimensional measure of psychopathology among children aged throughout Europe [68] the USA [9,10], Asia [5,1113] and
416 that has been widely adopted in both research and in Africa [13]. To date, the SDQ has received over 3,000 research
clinical practice [1]. The instrument is composed of 25 items that citations and this number is growing, particularly as many on-
ask about behavioral attributes of the child and are combined to going longitudinal birth cohorts have used the SDQ for over a
form five subscales (composed of 5 items each). The subscales decade as a repeated measure of child behaviour [7,14,15].
measure emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/ Woerner and colleagues [8] reviewed non-European studies
inattention, peer relationships, and prosocial behavior. There are that psychometrically evaluated the SDQ, applied it to screen for
parallel versions of the SDQ that collect the same data from behaviour disorders, or employed its parent-, teacher- or self-rated
parents, teachers and young people aged 11 or over. A versions as research tools. They found experience gained with the
supplemental impact subscale measures chronicity, distress, SDQ in other continents has supported European evidence of
social impairment, and burden to others, which provides useful good psychometric properties and clinical utility. They note that
additional information for clinicians and researchers [2]. worldwide usage of the SDQ is expected to increase in the future,
although reporting by different participants is context-dependent

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 1 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e80247


The Association of SDQ with Diagnosis of ASD/ADHD

Figure 1. Box plots for parent report of SDQ subscales across three groups: ASD diagnosis, ADHD diagnosis and neither diagnosis.
Diagnosis: dx Increasing score reflects increased impairment in all sub-scales except prosocial scores which measure strengths.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080247.g001

and this limits the reliability of cross-cultural comparisons [7,16]. instrument for ADHD in the clinic because of the large number of
Despite these reservations, the SDQ has been successfully used to false positives identified.
make comparisons of child behaviour across age and culture [17]. According to the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-
Various studies have examined the utility of the SDQ as a 10), for a diagnosis of ASD to be made, children must display
screening device in predicting childhood psychiatric cases [1820] impairments in social interactions and communication, as well as
although few have looked at SDQ as a screen for specific disorders. restricted interests and repetitive behaviour [23]. Iizuka and
In a UK community-based sample, multi-informant ratings colleagues [26] examined the co-occurrence of behavioural
[parents, teachers and older children] identified individuals with symptoms of high-functioning ASD and ADHD using the SDQ
specific psychiatric diagnoses [21]. Sensitivity was over 70% for subscales in Japan. Core symptoms of ASD include social and
identifying conduct and hyperactivity disorders, but the instrument communication impairments and, as expected, the two SDQ
had poor discrimination (,30%) for emotional disorders in this subscales that measure aspects of social interaction- peer problems
general population sample. Varying results are most likely due to and prosocial behaviour- were associated with ASD in particular.
the heterogeneity of symptoms of childhood emotional disorders The study found elevated levels of peer problems and emotional
which have a wide range of symptoms, only some of which are difficulties, and fewer prosocial behaviours among the ASD group
captured by the five questions about emotional difficulties in the compared to those children with ADHD, whilst higher levels of
SDQ. Notably, there are no questions that directly relate to the hyperactivity and more conduct problems were reported for
triad of difficulties that comprise the autism spectrum, although children with ADHD. A large and growing literature has
indirectly, social skills can be inferred from the prosocial and peer demonstrated that ADHD symptoms are relatively common
relationship subscales. In contrast, there are five questions each on among children and adults with ASD and vice-versa [2733].
the more homogenous area of difficulties with attention/ Recently, some researchers have queried whether ASD and
hyperactivity. ADHD should be considered as different manifestations of one
Goodman and colleagues [21] developed an SDQ algorithm overarching disorder [33,34]. Currently, the diagnostic criteria for
that combines teacher, parent and child reports, to predict various childhood disorders laid out in ICD-10 contain an exclusivity
disorders, including Probable Hyperactive Disorder (PHD) in clause that does not allow ADHD to be diagnosed if pervasive
children. The PHD algorithm uses a combination of informants developmental disorder (including ASD) is present, although the
for SDQ scores on the hyperactivity/inattention and impact exclusivity clause has been dropped in the new version of the
subscales [18,22]. Multiple informants are required because Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5),
symptoms must be present across multiple settings if ADHD is in which ASD is listed as a condition which is commonly comorbid
to be diagnosed [23,24]. Ullebo and colleagues [20] tested the with ADHD [24].
PHD algorithm and found that it had an acceptable sensitivity for Given the wide and on-going use of the SDQ in research on
the ADHD combined phenotype. They recommended that developmental disorders, we sought to clarify the predictive power
bespoke cut-offs should be developed according to the purpose of the SDQ subscales in the identification of parent-reported
of its application to research. Brndbo and colleagues [25] clinical diagnosis of two specific disorders: ADHD and ASD. The
cautioned against use of the PHD algorithm as a screening primary aim of our study was to examine whether all behavioral
symptoms measured by SDQ were elevated in children with ASD

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 2 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e80247


The Association of SDQ with Diagnosis of ASD/ADHD

Figure 2. Box plots for teacher report of SDQ subscales across three groups: ASD diagnosis, ADHD diagnosis and neither diagnosis.
Diagnosis: dx Increasing score reflects increased impairment in all sub-scales except prosocial scores which measure strengths.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080247.g002

and ADHD relative to the rest of the population, and the utility of Sample
the SDQ as an indicator of these disorders. A secondary aim was The Millennium Cohort Study (MCS) is a UK-representative
to examine the extent to which symptoms co-occurred in children birth cohort study that used a disproportionate stratified cluster
diagnosed with ASD or ADHD. We hypothesised hyperactivity/ sampling design [35,37]. Children born between 1st September
inattention symptoms would predict clinical diagnosis of ADHD, 2000 and 11th January 2002 and listed on the Child Benefit
and prosocial and peer relationship problems would predict ASD. Records were eligible for the study. Child Benefit was a financial
This study therefore assesses the utility of the SDQ in identifying benefit payable to all parents of UK children at this time, with
these disorders. near universal take up. Data were first collected when children
were 9 months old (1st wave), further data were recorded
Methods concerning the childrens health and development when the
children were 3 years old (2nd wave), 5 years old (3rd wave) and
Ethics Statement 7 years old (4th wave). Within the total MCS cohort of 19, 519
Information was gathered from the sample, the first Millennium children, the current study outcomes, ASD and ADHD status,
Cohort Study (MCS) survey when children were 9 months old, were recorded for 14, 043 children at wave 4 (over 70%). The
and three, five and seven years of age: four sweeps of data MCS provides appropriate standardised weightings to adjust for
collection [35]. Informed written consent was obtained at each the effect of attrition and oversampling, making these results
stage of the study from parents for their participation and the representative of the UK population as a whole. Details of
participation of their child (ren); the MCS ethical review gives sampling design and weighting calculations are documented in
details [36]. Written consent was also obtained for gathering detail elsewhere [37].
information from health, education and economic records and to
contact teachers. The data were analyzed anonymously, with Outcome measures
researchers having no access to participant identities. Identities are
The case definition of the two conditions was based on
protected by the curators of the MCS. Additional ethical approval
responses to an MCS question duplicated from the US National
for the analysis reported here was granted by the Peninsula
Health Interview Survey questionnaire reported in previous
Medical School Ethics committee.
studies [38]. Parents or carers were asked in face-to-face interviews
if a doctor or health professional had identified childhood ADHD
Design or ASD. Consistent with other studies using these data [39],
Our study sought to clarify the predictive power of the SDQ families with twins or triplets where other siblings participated
subscales in the identification of parent-reported diagnosis of ASD were excluded (252 twins, 11 triplets) as both diagnoses have a
and ADHD using logistic regression models. This was compared high heritability. Parent-reported ASD and/or ADHD diagnosis
to the predictive power of the PHD algorithm already in existence was recorded for 14,043 children in 2008/9 with the wording of
[21]. A secondary aim was to examine the extent of overlap in the following questions read out verbatim:
symptoms between children diagnosed with these two disorders, as
measured by the SDQ subscales, in order to inform the debate N Has a doctor or health professional ever told you that (sample child) had
about revisions to diagnostic criteria. attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 3 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e80247


The Association of SDQ with Diagnosis of ASD/ADHD

Table 1. Models showing SDQ sub-scales as predictors with ADHD diagnosis as outcome for children from Milenium Cohort at age
7.

Unadjusted Unadjusted Adjusted Adjusted OR Adjusted Final OR


Variable Unadjusted n OR & 95% CI p n & 95% CI p1 Final n & 95% CI3 Final p

Emotion 13082 1.50 (1.40,1.60) ,0.001 8133 1.11 (0.96,1.28) 0.17


Parent
Emotion 8511 1.25 (1.16,1.34) ,0.001 8133 0.89 (0.77,1.04) 0.13
Teacher
Conduct 13111 1.85 (1.72,1.99) ,0.001 8133 1.07 (0.93,1.23) 0.37
Parent
Conduct 8514 1.49 (1.41,1.58) ,0.001 8133 0.93 (0.79,1.09) 0.36
Teacher
Hyper 13061 2.17 (1.95,2.42) ,0.001 8133 1.56 (1.34,1.83) ,0.001 8277 1.56 (1.35,1.80) ,0.001
Parent
Hyper 8512 1.60 (1.48,1.72) ,0.001 8133 1.19 (1.06,1.35) 0.003 8277 1.22 (1.11,1.34) ,0.001
Teacher
Prosocial 13116 0.65(0.59,0.70) ,0.001 8133 0.99 (0.87,1.13) 0.87
Parent2
Prosocial 8510 0.68(0.63,0.72) ,0.001 8133 1.05 (0.92,1.19) 0.46
Teacher2
Peer 13094 1.68 (1.56,1.80) ,0.001 8133 0.93 (0.81,1.07) 0.32
Parent
Peer 8511 1.53 (1.42,1.64) ,0.001 8133 1.09 (0.93,1.28) 0.28
Teacher
Impact 12958 2.17 (1.96,2.41) ,0.001 8133 1.56 (1.37,1.78) ,0.001 8277 1.63 (1.45,1.84) ,0.001
Parent
Impact 8404 2.19 (1.98,2.41) ,0.001 8133 1.12 (0.92,1.36) 0.25
Teacher

1 Adjusted models include subscales significant at 10% levels. 2.For prosocial scores the reciprocal of the odds ratios.
Is presented to fit conceptually with the rest of the model, i.e. greater OR = greater association with ADHD. 3. Model constant 0.0002 on odds ratio scale.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080247.t001

N Has a doctor or health professional ever told you that (sample child) had volunteer to comfort. For all the subscales except the prosocial
autism, Aspergers syndrome or autistic spectrum disorder? subscale, high scores indicate difficulties. As the prosocial items ask
about the presence of prosocial behaviour, the subscale measures
Families at wave 4 whose study children were seven years old, the strengths of the child in this area, and increasing scores
who responded with positive or negative answers to the above represent increasingly prosocial behaviour, unlike the other sub-
questions, were included. Families who answered dont know or scales where increasing score represents increasing impairment. In
refused to answer were excluded from the analysis (n = 30 ASD, all cases, answer options for each item are: Not true Somewhat
n = 44 ADHD, of these, n = 17 refused/dont know in both true or Certainly true, and these are scored 0, 1 or 2, giving a
categories). We took this measure to represent a clinical diagnosis total score out of a possible 10 for each subscale. A further impact
of disorder in line with other studies [38,40,41]. In total, from this subscale measures the impact of any difficulties on carers and the
sample, 173 children had reportedly been identified with ADHD children themselves in terms of chronicity, distress, social
and 209 had a parent-reported ASD diagnosis by age 7. Forty-four impairment, and burden to others. This is again scored 010
children had a co-morbid diagnosis of ASD and ADHD, and were with increasing impact producing a higher score. More details
retained in both outcome groups. about the SDQ, the probable hyperactivity disorder (PHD)
algorithm, normative data, background research and how the
Independent variables subscales are scored are available at the SDQ website (www.
The SDQ is composed of 25 items that ask about behavioural sdqinfo.org).
attributes of the child and are combined to form five subscales SDQ scores for each subscale had been taken for the entire
(composed of 5 items each). The emotional symptoms subscale cohort at wave 4 from both parent and teacher informants. Both
contains items that ask about fears, worries, misery, nerves and were added to models, since clinical identification of the disorders
somatic symptoms, the conduct problems subscale inquires about should be documented as causing impairment across settings (for
tantrums, obedience, fighting, lying and stealing, and the example, home and school). Several studies have stressed the need
hyperactivity/inattention subscale covers restlessness, fidgeting, for information from multiple informants when rating symptoms
concentration, distractibility and impulsivity. The peer relation- of a child psychiatric disorder [42].
ships subscale items include questions about popularity, victimi-
zation, isolation, friendship and ability to relate to children as Analysis
compared to adults, and the prosocial subscale covers consider- The ASD, ADHD and general population were compared on
ation of others, ability to share, kindness to younger children, and SDQ subscale scores. Box plots were provided for teacher and
helpfulness when other children are distressed and willingness to parent report of behaviour separately to illustrate how the three

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 4 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e80247


The Association of SDQ with Diagnosis of ASD/ADHD

groups (ASD, ADHD and general population) differed in SDQ 7 than other participating children without either of these
scores. Children reported as having both diagnoses (n = 44) were diagnoses on every SDQ sub-scale. Hyperactivity scores were
included in both ASD and ADHD groups. particularly high in both diagnosed samples compared to those of
Logistic regression (LR) established the odds of diagnosis of the rest of the population. Impact scores were also higher and
ASD/ADHD using SDQ subscales as independent variables. prosocial skills were lower in both diagnosed groups according to
Parent and teacher ratings of behaviour were treated as separate both informants.
covariates. The odds ratios (OR) from the analyses indicate that Table 1 reports the results of LR for the outcome of ADHD.
the relative increase in odds of being identified with ASD/ADHD These results confirm that all subscales were significantly
corresponded to a one-point increase in the SDQ subscales. All the associated with diagnosis of ADHD, and reflect greater impair-
sub-scales bar the prosocial scale measure impairment, therefore ment across the range of behaviours measured by the SDQ
the reciprocal of the odds ratios for the prosocial scores was used to instrument. After adjustment for the other SDQ subscales, only
fit conceptually with the rest of the model. This means that for all teacher and parent-reported hyperactivity/inattention subscales
SDQ subscales, an odds ratio greater than 1 represents greater and parent-reported impact remained significantly associated with
prediction of diagnosis as childrens difficulties increase. Unad- ADHD diagnosis. A threshold of 0.02 from the model yielded the
justed logistic regression models were fitted in which just one optimal sensitivity and specificity values of 91% and 90%
predictor at a time was included. Multivariable (adjusted) logistic respectively. The positive predictive value (PPV) was low at
regression models were then fitted in which predictors significant 12%, which is to be expected in a population based sample
at the 10% level in the unadjusted analyses were included as screening for rare disorders comprising young children. The Area
covariates. Estimates from LR were weighted to take account of Under the Curve (AUC) = 0.94 (95% CI, 0.900.97) shows the
the disproportionate stratified sample of electoral wards and model is a good fit. The PHD algorithm [21] produces a sensitivity
attrition/non-response by the 4th wave when the study outcomes of just 30%, but a specificity of 98% for the probable hyperactivity
were measured, making the sample representative of the UK disorder category. The positive predictive power was also fairly
population [37]. LR was then used to derive separate models for low at 27%. Examples of scores that exceed the threshold for this
ASD and for ADHD respectively, composed of the SDQ subscales model using optimal values are given in the supporting informa-
that remained significant at 10% levels after adjustment for other tion in Table S1.
subscales. Final models were composed of subscales that remained LR was also used to explore the predictive value of the subscales
significantly associated with outcome at 10% levels after adjust- for ASD (Table 2).
ment for other behaviours. The sensitivity (percentage of children Again all the SDQ subscales were significant in unadjusted
with diagnosis correctly identified as such) and specificity analysis. After adjustment for interdependencies between sub-
(probability that a test result will be negative when the disease is scales, several still remained significantly associated with the
not present or true negative rate, expressed as a percentage) of the outcome of ASD at 10% levels. These were the impact and
final models were examined using Receiver Operating Charac- hyperactivity subscales from both raters, and the prosocial and
teristic (ROC) curves. The area under the curve (AUC) is a emotional symptoms scores rated by parents. The measures with
measure of how well the model can identify children with disorder. the largest effect were the parent-rated subscales of the prosocial
The Youden Index [43] is used to calculate the optimal values for behaviour and impact subscales. Peer problems from either rater
sensitivity and specificity; it determines a threshold that will did not appear in the final model. A threshold of 0.03 produced
maximise the difference between true positive and false positive the optimal values for model sensitivity and specificity of 79% and
rates. For this threshold, the positive predictive value was derived 93% respectively; AUC = 0.90, (95% CI, 0.860.95). The PPV
for each model. In the case of ADHD, the sensitivity and was again low at 18%. Examples of scores that exceed the
specificity were compared to the cut-offs for Probable Hyperac- threshold for this ASD model using optimal values are given in the
tivity Disorder algorithm [21]. supporting information in Table S2. Table 3 shows the threshold,
sensitivity and specificity for higher PPVs for both the ASD and
Results ADHD model, illustrating the varying sensitivity, specificity and
predictive power of the model at various threshold settings.
For 96.7% of families participating, the main respondent on the
outcome measure of ASD or ADHD was the childs mother. At Discussion
the birth of the child, mothers had a mean age of 28 years (range
13 to 48 years), and over 99% were resident at home with the The prevalence of ASD and ADHD was not the focus of this
study child all of the time. The mean child age when outcome paper: we have written about this elsewhere [44]. The low
measures were taken was 7.2 years (SD = 0.2; range, 6.3 to 8.2). prevalence of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis is consistent with
Figures 1 and 2 illustrate the demographic profile of the sample, other UK studies [45] and studies in Scandinavia [46]. The
giving descriptive statistics for parent and teacher-rated SDQ reported prevalence of ASD diagnosis is high compared to
subscales for children with ASD, those with ADHD and those with previous estimates; which may reflect the increasing use of the
neither diagnosis. Clear differences are observed between the ASD label in the UK, a trend that has also been identified in other
children with neither diagnosis (no dx) and children with ADHD/ studies. Results showed elevated behavioral difficulties in multiple
ASD. The figures illustrate differences in the distribution of scores domains for both groups with parent-reported diagnoses, and
between ASD children and those with ADHD but also substantial suggests that many behavioral problems are shared by children
overlap. The inter-rater reliability between parent and teacher diagnosed with ASD and those diagnosed with ADHD.
scores was low to medium, values of the weighted kappa coefficient Despite the exclusivity clause in the current ICD-10 diagnostic
ranged from 0.24 for the emotional symptoms sub-score (95% CI classification systems, there was a high proportion of dual
0.220.27) to 0.47 for hyperactivity/inattention scores (95% CI diagnosis in the two conditions: 23% of children with ADHD
0.4520.47). had a diagnosis of ASD, and 21% with ASD had identified
As Figure 1 and Figure 2 illustrate, children with ADHD and ADHD. Several other recent studies [2933] also suggest that
ASD diagnoses had substantially more impaired behaviour at age children with ASD and ADHD often share symptoms of

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 5 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e80247


The Association of SDQ with Diagnosis of ASD/ADHD

Table 2. Models of SDQ sub-scales as predictors with ASD diagnosis as outcome for children from Milenium Cohort at age 7.

Unadjusted Unadjusted Unadjusted Adjusted Adjusted OR Adjusted Final ASD OR


Variable n OR & 95% CI p n & 95% CI p1 Final n & 95% CI3 Final p

Emotion 13127 1.52 (1.41,1.64) ,0.001 8162 1.15 (1.00,1.33) 0.04 8180 1.16 (1.01,1.33) 0.04
Parent
Emotion 8536 1.38 (1.29,1.48) ,0.001 8162 1.05 (0.93,1.20) 0.42
Teacher
Conduct 13155 1.63 (1.53,1.74) ,0.001 8162 0.92 (0.80,1.06) 0.26
Parent
Conduct 8539 1.41 (1.33,1.50) ,0.001 8162 0.83 (0.74,0.95) 0.005 8180 0.86 (0.76,0.96) 0.01
Teacher
Hyper 13105 1.75 (1.63,1.88) ,0.001 8162 1.18 (1.04,1.33) 0.009 8180 1.15 (1.03,1.30) 0.02
Parent
Hyper 8537 1.48 (1.38,1.58) ,0.001 8162 1.10 (1.00,1.22) 0.06 8180 1.11 (1.01,1.23) 0.03
Teacher
Prosocial 13159 0.54 (0.49,0.59) ,0.001 8162 1.24 (1.10,1.41) 0.001 8180 1.25 (1.11,1.42) ,0.001
Parent2
Prosocial 8535 0.64 (0.59,0.70) ,0.001 8162 1.10 (0.97,1.24) 0.14
Teacher2
Peer 13136 1.94 (1.80,2.09) ,0.001 8162 1.04 (0.88,1.23) 0.62
Parent
Peer 8536 1.65 (1.53,1.77) ,0.001 8162 1.06 (0.91,1.23) 0.46
Teacher
Impact 13004 2.11 (1.93,2.30) ,0.001 8162 1.51 (1.30,1.75) ,0.001 8180 1.53 (1.35,1.72) ,0.001
Parent
Impact 8430 2.31 (2.10,2.54) ,0.001 8162 1.25 (1.02,1.55) 0.04 8180 1.44 (1.22,1.71) ,0.001
Teacher

1 Adjusted models include subscales significant at 10% levels. 2.For prosocial scores the reciprocal of the odds ratios.
Is presented to fit conceptually with the rest of the model, i.e. greater OR = greater association with ASD. 3. Model constant 0.113 on odds ratio scale.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080247.t002

hyperactivity and other behavioural difficulties. ADHD symptoms biases inherent in obtaining a clinical diagnosis may have clouded
are relatively common in children and adults with autistic-type the relationship. Although the findings suggest that ADHD
symptoms; autism-type symptoms/ behaviours may be less symptoms are also relatively common in children with ASD and
common in children with ADHD [27,28,30,31]. Our findings of vice-versa, in line with findings from other studies [2733], the
elevated behavioral difficulties indicative of both conditions in results do not support the argument that ASD and ADHD should
both diagnosed groups support change to the diagnostic criteria to be considered as different manifestations of one overarching
allow ASD and ADHD to be diagnosed in the same individual. disorder [33,34].
Our findings suggest that this already relatively common in In our study LR models, after statistical adjustment for
practice, so removal of the exclusivity clauses would eliminate interdependencies between different types of behavioral problems,
unnecessary tension between clinical practice and diagnostic rules. a distinctive symptom profile emerged for ADHD based on
After adjustment for other subscales in multivariable models, the hyperactivity and impact sub-scales, but not for ASD. The finding
final model for ADHD was composed of the hyperactivity/ provides evidence to support the assertion of Nicalsen et al. [22]
inattention and impact symptoms only. This finding is highly that the SDQ hyperactivity-inattentive subscale shows good
predictable and as initially hypothesised, although the selection agreement with the diagnostic criteria for attention-deficit

Table 3. Varying sensitivity, specificity and positive predictive values for ASD and ADHD models derived from Millennium Cohort
Data.

PPV ADHD ASD

Threshold Sensitivity Specificity Threshold Sensitivity Specificity

20% 0.05 81% 95% 0.04 76% 94%


30% 0.13 63% 98% 0.08 66% 97%
40% 0.39 34% 99% 0.17 53% 98%
50% 0.74 15% 100% 0.28 47% 99%
60% 0.92 5% 100% 0.57 29% 100%

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080247.t003

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 6 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e80247


The Association of SDQ with Diagnosis of ASD/ADHD

hyperactivity disorder, as it was intended to do. Our adjusted adequately tapped by the SDQ behaviour subscale. ASD was
results suggest that children with ADHD have focused problems of associated with enhanced emotional problems. These results
hyperactivity/inattention. A combined model derived from both concur with many studies that have found ASD to be associated
parent and teacher hyperactivity and impact scores is a good with anxiety and depression [5153]. Taking account of co-
predictor of diagnosis of ADHD, identifying up to 91% of children occurring symptoms is essential for any child with autism as it may
with parent-reported clinical diagnosis of ADHD. The models have practical ramifications in terms of the type(s) of intervention
developed using the MCS data and the Goodman PHD algorithm required.
differ in two ways. First, the MCS model did not find impact on
teachers to be a significant predictor of ADHD. This contradicts Limitations
arguments of many socially orientated researchers who suggest The current study used parent-report of clinical identification of
that ADHD is partially constructed in response to the need for ASD and ADHD by a doctor or another professional. This means
compliance at school [47]. Others have observed that ADHD is that parents are likely to be well aware of symptoms of these
more likely to be identified in tandem with disruption to the conditions and may therefore be more likely to report them than a
classroom [48]. MCS data suggests for teachers, presence of parent of a child with similar difficulties that have not been
inattention and hyperactivity alone is enough to indicate ADHD. clinically highlighted. Furthermore, parents may have been over-
One partial explanation could be that naming the condition: inclusive in their interpretation of the question: inferring a positive
ADHD being diagnosed; minimises teacher ratings of impact. answer in cases where ASD or ADHD was suggested by a health
The second main difference is that cut-offs (e.g. for identifying worker but not confirmed by further assessment. Clinician
91% of children with disorder) were not fixed as are those in the diagnoses themselves can be inaccurate if unguided by structured
PHD model. This is consistent with the findings of Ullebo and assessment [54].
colleagues [20], who conclude, and the ROC curves demonstrate In addition, the sample will contain other children with ASD
that thresholds can be selected by defining a specificity or and ADHD, and other disorders, as yet unrecognised [55], and
sensitivity value to obtain specified model performance. Appro- research suggests that the unrecognised group may be in the
priate cut-off can then be chosen according to purpose of use. The majority [56]. It is beyond the scope of this article to comment on
coefficients for the logistic regression models can be obtained from differentiation from other comorbid groups. As children with other
the odds ratios in Tables 1 and 2. In a clinical setting, the disorders were mixed in with general population, the mean SDQ
probability of an ADHD/ASD diagnosis can be calculated given a scores of the general population are likely to have been elevated
set of SDQ scores. The probability of a diagnosis can then be and would serve to make the mean SDQ scores more similar to
compared to the optimal threshold. those of children with parent- reported ASD and ADHD. Thus,
Goodman and Mullick [12] and Ullebo and colleagues [20] our detection of differences between children with a clinical
cautiously recommend use of the SDQ as a screening tool for diagnosis of ADHD and ASD is likely to be robust.
childhood disorder and specifically ADHD/hyperkinetic disor- Other limitations relate to the ADHD group. First, we did not
der, Brndbo and colleagues [25] caution against it. All these have access to pharmaceutical data, but evidence suggests
studies used well-validated scales measuring symptoms of ADHD. treatment with methylphenidate may have improved symptoms
Our study used an outcome measure of parent-reported clinical of hyperactivity and may have led parents and teachers to under-
diagnosis of disorder: as clinical assessments are highly variable report difficulties in any children with diagnoses who were taking
and subject to local bias [49], our findings have no clinical medication [57]. Second, we did not have information on sub-type
application until replicated against standardised ADHD scales. It of ADHD. Ullebo and colleagues [20] found the SDQ was a good
should be remembered that previous work on the algorithm [21] predictor for the combined ADHD subtype, but less informative
predicted against diagnoses made using a research instrument, for other subtypes. Third, seven years old is still early in life for
while the current study uses parent-report of a clinical diagnosis; clinical identification of ADHD [58], which may partially explain
both studies report from a general population sample. For MCS, why the sensitivity of the PHD algorithm was so low in our study.
the PHD algorithm had low sensitivity at 30%, but a specificity It identified less than a third of the children with ADHD, a much
of 98%. poorer performance than witnessed in some other studies [20,21].
The resulting LR model for autism shows that many types of At this age it is likely many cases are yet to be identified:
difficulties may complicate the picture for a child with ASD. This assessment with a research-based diagnostic measure may have
is to be expected, as there is not a specific autism spectrum revealed different results.
subscale that focuses on the core difficulties as there is with Despite these limitations our results provide further evidence to
ADHD. Prosocial behaviour emerged as the strongest predictor of suggest that the SDQ algorithm is a useful tool as an indicator of
ASD, which again is not surprisingly as social impairments are ADHD symptoms for research purposes. As the SDQ instrument
core deficits. Furthermore, ASD diagnosis has been associated is widely used in research studies already [613], bespoke cut-offs
with the low scores on the prosocial subscale in other UK cohorts could be developed according to purpose of application to
[50]. Our findings suggest that a range of other difficulties such as research. However, we do not currently recommend using the
anxiety and conduct problems are likely to commonly co-occur SDQ as a screening tool for either disorder in clinical practice due
with both ASD and ADHD, which, for those working with to the high number of false positives and limitations of case
children who have these difficulties, echoes clinical experience. It is definition in our study. Should our findings be replicated against
intriguing that ASD is not associated with conduct problems; structured research assessments, they could be used by clinicians to
indeed higher conduct problem ratings lower the odds of an ASD identify children at risk of ADHD who warrant further assessment.
diagnosis. It may be that social difficulties inhibit the overt The study is part of a large and growing literature that
externalising behaviours covered by the SDQ, several of which demonstrates that ADHD symptoms are also relatively common in
require a social orientation towards others. Behaviour that children and adults diagnosed with ASD and vice-versa [2733]. It
challenges others among children with ASD often results from a supports changes to DSM-5 dropping the exclusivity clause to
failure to recognise or conform to social expectations and/or allow dual diagnosis of ASD and ADHD, and suggests ICD
rigidity around routine or preferred activity, which may not be criteria should follow suit: indeed dual diagnosis occurs in practice

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 7 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e80247


The Association of SDQ with Diagnosis of ASD/ADHD

already. These results also suggest that for children with ASD, the Table S2 Examples of scores over the threshold for ASD
presence of other co-occurring impairments in behaviour is likely model. In MCS data there are 671 combinations which produce
to be the rule, not the exception. In this way the work contributes a value over the threshold. They are all unique. Table S2 gives 10
to the debate raised by Hattori et al. [33,34] about whether the combinations which are over the threshold, for illustrative
current diagnostic configuration for ADHD and autism are valid. purposes.
The findings support the removal of exclusivity clauses in current (DOCX)
revisions to DSM-5, and inform on-going debates about revisions
to ICD-11. Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the Millennium Cohort Study families for their
Supporting Information time and cooperation, as well as the Millennium Cohort Study team at the
Institute of Education, London, UK. We would also like to thank Dr.
Table S1 Examples of scores over the threshold for Obioha Ukoumunne for his helpful comments on this article.
ADHD model. There are 1331 combinations of 3 SDQ scales
(010) of which 928 combinations would produce a value over the Author Contributions
threshold. The most frequent combinations in MCS are included
for illustrative purposes. Conceived and designed the experiments: GR LR TF. Performed the
experiments: LR GR. Analyzed the data: GR LR. Wrote the paper: GR
(DOCX) LR TF.

References
1. Goodman R (1997) The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research 20. Ullebo AK, Posserud M-B, Heiervang E, Gillberg C, Obel C (2011) Screening
note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38: 5816. for the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder phenotype using the Strength and
2. Goodman R (1999) The extended version of the Strengths and Difficulties Difficulties Questionnaire. Eur Child Adolesc Psych 20(9): 4518.
Questionnaire as a guide to child psychiatric caseness and consequent burden. 21. Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, Gatward R, Meltzer H (2000) Using the
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 40: 7919. Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric
3. Levitt JM, Saka N, Romanelli LH, Hoagwood K (2007) Early identification of disorders in a community sample. BJP. 177(6): 5349.
mental health problems in schools: The status of instrumentation. J Sch Psychol 22. Niclasen J, Teasdale TW, Andersen A-MN, Skovgaard AM, Elberling H, et al.
45: 16391. (2012) Psychometric Properties of the Danish Strength and Difficulties
4. Borg A-M, Kaukonen P, Salmelin R, Joukamaa M, Tamminen T (2012) Questionnaire: the SDQ assessed for more than 70,000 raters in four different
Reliability of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire among Finnish 49- cohorts. PLoS One 7(2). Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/
year-old children. Nord J Psychiatry 66: 40313. PMC3288071 Accessed 2013 Jan 25.
5. Yao S, Zhang C, Zhu X, Jing X, McWhinnie CM, et al. (2009) Measuring 23. World Health Organization. International classification of diseases and related
adolescent psychopathology: psychometric properties of the self-report Strengths health problems (ICD-10). [Internet]. Geneva, WHO; 1992. Available: www.
and Difficulties Questionnaire in a sample of Chinese adolescents. J. Adolesc who.int/classifications/icd/en/greenbook.pdf p.179 Accessed 2013 Jan 25.
Health 56: 5562. 24. American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force (2013) Diagnostic and
6. Muris P, Meesters C, van den Berg F (2012) The Strengths and Difficulties statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Arlington, Va.: American
Questionnaire (SDQ). Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 12: 18. Psychiatric Association.
7. Obel C, Heiervang E, Rodriguez A, Heyerdahl S, Smedje H, et al. (2004) The 25. Brndbo PH, Mathiassen B, Martinussen M, Heiervang E, Eriksen M, et al.
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in the Nordic countries. Eur Child (2011) The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as a screening instrument
Adolesc Psychiatry 13 Suppl 2: II3239. for Norwegian child and adolescent mental health services, application of UK
8. Woerner W, Fleitlich-Bilyk B, Martinussen R, Fletcher J, Cucchiaro G, et al. scoring algorithms. Child Adolesc Psychiatry 5(1): 32.
(2004) The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire overseas: evaluations and 26. Iizuka C, Yamashita Y, Nagamitsu S, Yamashita T, Araki Y, et al. (2010)
applications of the SDQ beyond Europe. Eur. Child Adolesc Psychiatry 13: 47 Comparison of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) scores
54. between children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (HFASD) and
9. Dickey WC, Blumberg SJ (2001) Revisiting the factor structure of the strengths attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD). Brain Dev 32: 60912.
and difficulties questionnaire: United States. J Am Acad Child Adolesc 27. Hofvander B, Delorme R, Chaste P, Nyden A, Wentz E, et al. (2009) Psychiatric
Psychiatry 43:115967. and psychosocial problems in adults with normal-intelligence autism spectrum
10. Mason WA, Chmelka MB, Thompson RW (2012) Responsiveness of the disorders. BMC Psychiatry 9: 35.
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) in a sample of high-risk youth in 28. Reiersen AM, Constantino JN, Grimmer M, Martin NG, Todd RD (2008)
residential treatment. Child Youth Care Forum. 41: 47992. Evidence for shared genetic influences on self-reported ADHD and autistic
11. Leung PWL, Wong MMT (2003) Measures of child and adolescent symptoms in young adult Australian twins. Twin Res Hum Genet 11(6): 57985.
psychopathology in Asia. Psychol Assess 15: 26879. 29. Reiersen AM, Todd RD (2008) Co-occurrence of ADHD and autism spectrum
12. Mullick MSI, Goodman R (2001) Questionnaire screening for mental health disorders: phenomenology and treatment. Expert Rev Neurother 8(4): 65769.
problems in Bangladeshi children: a preliminary study. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr 30. Santosh PJ, Mijovic A (2004) Social impairment in Hyperkinetic Disorder
Epidemiol 36: 9499. relationship to psychopathology and environmental stressors. Eur Child Adolesc
13. Kashala E, Elgen I, Sommerfelt K, Tylleskar T (2005) Teacher ratings of mental Psychiatry. 3: 14150.
health among school children in Kinshasa, Democratic Republic of Congo. Eur 31. Simonoff E, Pickles A, Charman T, Chandler S, Loucas T, et al. (2008)
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 14: 20815. Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence,
14. Chittleborough CR, Lawlor DA, Lynch JW. (2011) Young maternal age and comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. J Am Acad
poor child development: predictive validity from a birth cohort. Pediatrics. 127: Child Adolesc Psychiatry 8: 9219.
E1436E1444. 32. St Pourcain B, Mandy WP, Heron J, Golding J, Davey Smith G, et al. (2011)
15. Griffiths LJ, Dezateux C, Hill A (2011) Is obesity associated with emotional and Links between co-occurring social-communication and hyperactive-inattentive
behavioural problems in children? Findings from the Millennium Cohort Study. trait trajectories. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 50(9): 892902.e5.
Int. J. Pediatr Obes 22: E423E432. 33. Van der Meer JMJ, Oerlemans AM, van Steijn DJ, Lappenschaar MGA, de
16. Stone LL, Otten R, Engels RCME, Vermulst AA, Janssens JMAM (2010) Sonneville LMJ, et al. (2012) Are autism spectrum disorder and attention-
Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the Strengths and deficit/hyperactivity disorder different manifestations of one overarching
Difficulties Questionnaire for 4-to 12-year-olds: a review. Clin Child Fam disorder? Cognitive and symptom evidence from a clinical and population-
Psychol Rev 3: 25474. based sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51: 11601172.e3.
17. Collishaw S, Maughan B, Goodman R, Pickles A (2004) Time trends in 34. Hattori J, Ogino T, Abiru K, Nakano K, Oka M, et al. (2006) Are pervasive
adolescent mental health. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45: 135062. developmental disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder distinct
18. Goodman R, Renfrew D, Mullick M (2000) Predicting type of psychiatric disorders? Brain Dev 28(6): 3714.
disorder from Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) scores in child 35. University of London. Institute of Education. Centre for Longitudinal Studies
mental health clinics in London and Dhaka. Eur. Child Adolesc Psychiatry 9: (2008) Millennium Cohort Study: Fourth Survey, [computer file]. 4th Edition.
12934. Colchester, Essex: UK Data Archive [distributor], SN: 6411, http://dx.doi.org/
19. Mathai J, Anderson P, Bourne A (2004) Comparing psychiatric diagnoses 10.5255/UKDA-SN-6411-3.
generated by the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire with diagnoses made 36. Shepherd P (2012). Millennium Cohort Study: Ethical review and consent.
by clinicians. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 38: 63943. Centre for Longitudinal Studies, London: UK.

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 8 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e80247


The Association of SDQ with Diagnosis of ASD/ADHD

37. Plewis I (2007) The Millennium Cohort study: technical report on sampling. 47. Bailey S, Thomson P (2009) Routine (dis)order in an infant school. Ethnogr
Centre for Longitudinal Studies, London; UK. Available: http://www.esds.ac. Educ. 4: 21127.
uk/doc/5350/mrdoc/pdf/mcs_technical_report_on_sampling_4th_edition.pdf 48. Abikoff HB, Jensen PS, Arnold LLE, Hoza B, Hechtman L, et al. (2002)
Accessed 2013 Jan 25. Observed classroom behavior of children with ADHD: relationship to gender
38. Pastor PN, Rueben CA (2008) Diagnosed attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and comorbidity. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1;30: 34959.
and learning disability: United States, 20042006. US Department of Health 49. Reid R, Hakendorf P, Prosser B (2002) Use of psychostimulant medication for
and Human Services, Centres for disease control and prevention, Office of ADHD in South Australia. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 41: 90613.
Analysis and Epidemiology. Vital and health statistics series 10, no. 237. 50. Russell G, Steer C, Golding J (2011) Social and demographic factors that
39. Totsika V, Hastings R, Emerson E, Berridge D, Lancaster G (2011) Behavior influence the diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr
problems at 5 years of age and maternal mental health in autism and intellectual Epidemiol 46: 128393.
disability. J Ab Child Psychol 39: 113747. 51. Hallett V, Ronald A, Happe F (2009) Investigating the association between
40. Boyle CA, Boulet S, Schieve LA, Cohen RA, Blumberg SJ, et al. (2011) Trends autistic-like and internalizing traits in a community-based twin sample. J Am
in the prevalence of developmental disabilities in US children, 19972008. Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr 48: 61827.
Pediatrics. 127(6): 103442. 52. Smalley SL, McCracken J, Tanguay P (1995) Autism, affective disorders, and
41. Kogan MD, Blumberg SJ, Schieve LA, Boyle CA, Perrin JM, et al. (2007) social phobia. Am J Med Gen 60: 1926.
Prevalence of parent-reported diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder among 53. Tantam D (2000) Psychological disorder in adolescents and adults with Asperger
children in the US Pediatrics 124: 1395403. syndrome. Autism 4(1): 4762.
42. Goodman R, Ford T, Corbin T, Meltzer H (2004) Using the Strengths and 54. Bruchmuller K, Margraf J, Schneider S (2012) Is ADHD diagnosed in accord
Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) multi-informant algorithm to screen looked- with diagnostic criteria? Overdiagnosis and influence of client gender on
after children for psychiatric disorders. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry Suppl 2: diagnosis. J Consult Clin Psychol 80: 12838.
II2531. 55. Meltzer H, Gatward R, Goodman R, Ford T (2003) Mental health of children
43. Fluss R, Faraggi D, Reiser B (2005) Estimation of the Youden Index and its and adolescents in Great Britain. Int Rev Psychiatry 15: 1857.
associated cutoff point. Biom J. Aug;47: 45872. 56. Ford T, Hamilton H, Meltzer H, Goodman R (2007) Child mental health is
44. Russell G, Rodgers LR, Ukoumunne OC, Ford T (2013) Prevalence of parent- everybodys business: the prevalence of contact with public sector services by
reported ASD and ADHD in the UK: findings from the Millennium Cohort type of disorder among British school children in a three-year period. Child
Study. J Autism Dev Disord. (e pub ahead of print). Adolesc Ment Health 12(1): 13.
45. Ford T, Goodman R, Meltzer H (2003) The British Child and Adolescent 57. Barbaresi WJ, Katusic SK, Colligan RC, Weaver AL, Leibson CL, et al. (2006)
Mental Health Survey 1999: the prevalence of DSM-IV disorders. J Am Acad Long-term stimulant medication treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Oct;42: 120311. disorder: results from a population-based study. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 27: 110.
46. Kvist AP, Nielsen HS, Simonsen M (2013) The importance of childrens ADHD 58. Kieling C, Kieling RR, Rohde LA, Frick PJ, Moffitt T, et al. (2010) The age at
for parents relationship stability and labor supply. Soc Sci Med. 88: 3038. onset of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 167: 146.

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 9 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e80247

You might also like