You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (2007) 877883

www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp

Authoritarianism, social dominance orientation and prejudice:


EVects of various self-categorization conditions
Vincent Dru
Universit de Paris-X-Nanterre, France

Received 29 June 2005; revised 30 August 2006


Available online 27 November 2006
Communicated by Spencer

Abstract

One hundred and seventy nine students Wrst answered RWA and SDO scales were assigned to experimental conditions that primed
diVerent forms of self-categorization, and Wnally responded for prejudice scales for three target ethnic groups. The results showed Wrst,
that RWA and SDO correlate with prejudice in a control condition. Second, RWA and SDO correlated diVerently with prejudice depend-
ing on the way in which membership to social group was primed. When a prime as member of a group oriented to devotion to the in-
group norms and values was used, the correlation of RWA and in-group identiWcation with prejudice was signiWcant, but when a prime as
member of a competitive group was used, only SDO correlated signiWcantly with prejudice. The results were discussed as identifying two
diVerent schemas of social categorization according to which RWA and SDO expressed sets of social beliefs and attitudes relevant for
inter-group relations.
2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Introduction describes various levels of self-categorization, the study


reported here examines the social conditions under which
The origins of prejudice and ethnocentrism (Sumner, attitudinal variables as authoritarianism and social domi-
1906) have been studied using multiple approaches (Duc- nance orientation will correlate with prejudice.
kitt, 1992). Such studies are based on diVerent levels of Right Wing Authoritarianism (RWA), following the
analysis (Doise, 1986) either identifying the cause of preju- Adorno et al.s study (1950) has been one recent theory of
dice as certain personality and attitudinal variables (Duriez individual diVerences in prejudice. Altemeyer (1981) has
& Van Hiel, 2002; Ekehammar, Akrami, Gylje, & Zakris- deWned RWA as the co-variation of three attitudinal clus-
son, 2004) or as salient features of intergroup relationships ters named conventionalism, authoritarian submission and
(LeVine & Campbell, 1972; Tajfel, 1981). Some theorists authoritarian aggression. Most recently, an additional indi-
(Tajfel, 1981) of prejudice have opposed the reliance on vidual-level variable has been proposed as a determinant of
such varied levels of analysis whereas others suggest that prejudice. Social dominance theory (SDT, Pratto, Sidanius,
diVerent analyses are complementary and can be useful in Stallworth, & Malle, 1994; Pratto, Stallworth, & Conway-
studying intergroup relationships (Duckitt, 1992; Mackie & Lanz, 1998) proposes that social attitudes are inXuenced by
Smith, 1998). Based on a self-categorization approach the individuals beliefs about the appropriate structure of
(Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987) which social groups and their relationships in a particular social
system. Social dominance orientation (SDO, Sidanius &

Pratto, 1999) is an individual variable focusing on the gen-
This research was supported by the Laboratory Sport & Culture,
eral tendency to favor hierarchy-enhancing legitimizing
UFR-STAPS, Universit de Paris-X-Nanterre, France. The authors are
grateful to M. Asthon, John Duckitt, Bo Ekehammar, Pierre Vrignaud myths that support group inequalities instead of egalitar-
and to two anonymous reviewers for their helpful suggestions. ian considerations (hierarchy-attenuating legitimizing
E-mail addresses: dru@u-paris10.fr, druvincent@wanadoo.fr myths).

0022-1031/$ - see front matter 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.10.008
878 V. Dru / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (2007) 877883

Some important investigations have demonstrated that tition) whereas an instrumental function (illustrated by the
RWA and SDO were the most important predictors of RCT) might be involved when meaningful inter-group con-
prejudice. McFarland and Adelson (1996) and Altemeyer ditions are salient such as competitive circumstances (real-
(1998) reported that no other individual variables were istic competition). The identity and the instrumental
found to explain additional variance in prejudice over the functions parallel the conceptual view proposed by Duckitt
contributions of RWA and SDO. When these constructs (1989,2001) in identifying RWA as an orientation to
are entered in a regression equation, they together ingroup norms and values and SDO as an intergroup and
accounted both for more than 50% of variance in prejudice competitive orientation.
as assessed by various measures. Moreover, RWA and Unfortunately, the researches using the RWA and SDO
SDO could not be confounded as possible predictors of scales in studying prejudice within these orientations and
prejudice because they usually do not correlate one to the functions have used mainly correlational data and SEM
other and they usually diVerentiate one to the other in pre- analyses. In a search of a distinction between these scales in
dicting several criteria (Heaven & Connors, 2001). Theoret- an experimental way, but also to assess causality between
ically, Altemeyer (1998) identiWed RWA and SDO as a RWA, SDO and prejudice depending of diVerent social ori-
submissive and a dominant authoritarian personality, entations and contexts, news designs are needed.
respectively, with diVerent consequences. Recent studies In this methodological direction, recent studies have
stipulated that RWA and SDO might not be considered as examined the inXuence of self-categorization conditions on
personality dimensions (Duckitt, 2001; Stone, Lederer, & the correlation between RWA, SDO and prejudice. Self
Christie, 1993, p. 232). They are usually considered as large Categorization Theory (SCT, Turner et al., 1987) distin-
sets of social beliefs and attitudes. guishes personal identity and interpersonal relationships
In the domain of social attitudes and world-views, Duc- from collective or social identity and inter-group relation-
kitt (2001) have suggested some important distinctions ships. SCT postulates that behaviours, cognitions and feel-
between RWA, SDO and two motivational goal-schemas ings will be determined by diVerent levels of cognitive
for prejudice. One motivational schema has been identiWed categorization of the self. During everyday life situations,
as a search for control and security, expressing the social individuals might meet some conditions of personal catego-
and cultural values of conformity, traditionalism and rization or social categorization. Thus, when an individual
respect for in-group norms. Another motivational schema is self-categorized as a person, attributes of the personal
has been identiWed as a search for superiority and domi- identity will be activated and individuals will react to social
nance between groups and for strong interest for winning. stimuli according to their personality and attitudes. At the
The social dominance scale uses to correlate with measures opposite, when an individual is self-categorized as member
representing competitiveness and competitive-jungle of a group, attributes of their social identity, social identiW-
worldviews. These motivational schemas might also corre- cation and in-group stereotypes will be activated and indi-
spond with two diVerent kinds of national identiWcation, viduals will evaluate social stimuli according to their group
named as patriotism and nationalism (Baughn & Yaprak, membership. Following this theory, RWA and SDO, as
1996; Kosterman & Feshbach, 1989). Mummendey, Klink, personal attributes and individual attitudes towards social
and Brown (2001) have shown that patriotism parallels an issues, will be the stronger predictors of prejudice when a
evaluative and aVective attachment to the country (values personal categorization is activated, whereas in-group iden-
an in-group norms) whereas nationalism corresponds to a tiWcation, as the strength of attachment to the social group,
competitive identiWcation to the nation (an us Wrst and in-group stereotypes will be the best predictors of prej-
national orientation). udice when a group categorization is activated.
This dual model of prejudice has similarities with diVer- In this area, it must be noted that the results appeared
ent studies about intergroup relations. In this domain, real- with a lot of inconsistencies (Heaven & Quintin, 2003; Rey-
istic conXict theory (RCT) suggests that prejudice is the nolds, Turner, Haslam, & Ryan, 2001; Verkuyten & Hagen-
result of a realistic competition for scarce resources doorn, 1998). Heaven and Quintin (2003) showed hat RWA
between groups (LeVine & Campbell, 1972) and social and SDO correlated signiWcantly with prejudice in personal
identity theory (SIT, Tajfel, 1981) postulates that a strong and social identities conditions. Verkuyten and Hagendo-
need for a positive identiWcation to in-group norms and orn (1998) found that RWA and SDO correlated signiW-
values is responsible for greater prejudice. More recently, cantly with prejudice only in a personal identity condition.
an important distinction between a social and a realistic Finally, Reynolds et al. (2001) showed that RWA corre-
competitive strategy has been made with the use of the con- lated with prejudice in some particular primes of group
textual model of inter-group diVerentiation elaborated by membership. One possible reason for examining these
Scheepers, Spears, Doojse, and Manstead (2002). These inconsistencies is to look at the diVerent instructions given
authors argued that inter-group diVerentiation and preju- to the participants to prime group membership. Looking to
dice could serve two diVerent functions. The Wrst one is an the diVerent conditions of identity salience manipulated by
identity function (illustrated by the SIT) which appeared to Verkuyten and Hagendoorn (1998) and Heaven and Quin-
be the primary motivation to improve the in-group position tin (2003), it must be observed that some fundamental and
through the use of intergroup diVerentiation (social compe- distinct dimensions of inter-group relationships are
V. Dru / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (2007) 877883 879

involved in the social and national identity salience, for of great importance. This kind of primed membership will
example. Here is the cover story usually used to prime correspond to the instrumental function identiWed by
social and national identity: Scheepers et al. (2002). It is expected that SDO will corre-
late with prejudice in this condition, as Duckitt (2001) has
People belong to all kinds of groups, such as sport
shown that this social attitude is connected to a competi-
clubs, political parties, religious groups, and also to a
tive-jungle worldview which orients individuals to see the
nation. These groups diVer from each other and also
world as a permanent battle for resources and a struggle for
can compare themselves with others. One sports club
life.
can compare itself with another, one political party
To sum, the main hypothesis for this research is that
with another, and one national group with another.
RWA and SDO will correlate with prejudice in two diVer-
The next few questions are about your national
ent conditions of self-categorization as group members. In
group.
a condition of self-categorization as a member of an iden-
It can be seen that the national and social identity condi- tity or cultural group, it is predicted that RWA will corre-
tion have made salient a comparative context focusing late with prejudice, but that SDO will not. In a condition of
mainly on some competitive orientations. The reference to self-categorization as a member of a competitive group, it is
a sport club or a political party primed mainly a search for predicted that SDO will correlate with prejudice, but that
scarce resources, such as a victory in a competition. RWA will not.
The research conduced by Reynolds et al. (2001) primed
group memberships that where based on central values and Method
ideologies for authoritarianism; that is those which
described traditional inter-group categorizations like the Participants
diVerent relationships between gender and aged groups.
Some comparative contexts involving diVerent age and sex- One hundred and seventy nine French physical educa-
ual groups are salient questions for authoritarians because tion and psychology students (age mean, 20.91 years,
relationships between males and females and respect for SD D 1.63) from the north of France were involved in the
older groups are part of some conservative attitudes. This study after they voluntarily agreed to participate. The par-
research did not used SDO scale, but it showed that RWA ticipants were randomly assigned to three experimental
scale correlated signiWcantly with prejudice in these speciWc conditions. One control condition (N D 59, 41 males and 18
group memberships. females) did not prime any group membership. Two other
The study reported here was aimed at establishing that conditions primed either a group-based value membership
RWA and SDO might correlate with prejudice in some (N D 60, 41 males and 19 females) or a competitive group
diVerent conditions of self-categorization. As RWA and membership (N D 60, 40 males and 20 females, note here
SDO may be considered as attitudinal constructs close to that the proportions of males and females were quite simi-
two basic dimensions of inter-group relationships, they lar across experimental conditions).
may be expected to correlate diVerently with prejudice in
two corresponding primed self-categorization conditions. Materials and procedure
The Wrst condition of self-categorization will prime an
attachment to in-group norms and values which will be Participants in the three conditions were invited to
connected to social identiWcation and social identity pro- answer similar questionnaires in Section 1 of the experi-
cesses. RWA has been conceptualized as the co-variation of ment. Ten items from the French version of RWA (Dru,
conventionalism, authoritarian submission, and aggression, 2003) were randomly selected from the complete version
and Duckitt (1989) has proposed a new view of authoritari- (30 items). Half of the selected items were reverse-keyed
anism, conceiving it as an unconditional and strong identi- items, to control for possible acquiescence response bias.
Wcation with in-group authorities (conventionalism and The internal consistency reliability was acceptable (Cron-
authoritarian submission) combined with aggressive and bach  D .73). In the same manner, ten items from the
hostile attitudes and behaviors toward out-groups French version of SDO (Duarte, Dambrum, & Guimond,
(authoritarian aggression), when the inter-group context is 2004) were also randomly selected (Cronbach  D .75).
threatening to the in-groups social identity. It is expected Eight items were also selected from the French version
that RWA will correlate with prejudice in this Wrst condi- (Dru, 2004) of Multi-Group Ethnic Identity Measure
tion, a result that would be consistent with the identity (MEIM) from Phinneys (1992) work to assess French in-
function deWned by Scheepers et al. (2002). Such orienta- group identity (Cronbach  D .84).Some examples of items
tions are typical of cultural and religious groups. The sec- are I have a clear sense of my ethnic background and what
ond condition of self-categorization will prime a it means for me and I have spent time trying to Wnd out
competitive orientation in belonging to social groups which more about my ethnic group such as its history, customs,
are involved in the search of resources as material beneWts, and traditions. Participants answered these measures with
territory, and victory. Such orientations are typical of polit- the use of a nine point likert scale (from strong disagree-
ical and sport groups for whom defeating the opponent is ment to strong agreement).
880 V. Dru / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (2007) 877883

Section 2 of the experiment was composed by the with a need to get superior outcomes over other groups
diVerent experimental conditions before answering the through a quest of the win. Examples of items from this
prejudice scales. A Wrst experimental condition was scale conWrm this view: To what degree were you experi-
named the group-based values and norms condition. encing feelings of competitiveness between your group and
Salience to group values was primed with the use of the other groups? To what degree were you concerned about
following cover story: your groups success of failure? or To what degree were
you thinking about whether your groups performance will
People belong to diVerent social groups depending of
be superior or inferior to that of other groups? This scale
the importance given to their social lifestyle, their cul-
was used in an aYrmative wording for the research pre-
tural values and norms or to their patriotic view of their
sented here.
country. These persons shared with each other a strong
Finally for this section, the control condition did not
attachment and devotion to their country, and to their
present any group identity prime. Participants answered the
in-group values, norms and customs. For example, a per-
questionnaires as a large attitudinal survey asking them to
son belonging to an ethnic group will need to meet other
report their opinions on various issues.
group members to share some common cultural tradi-
Section 3 was similar for the three experimental condi-
tions. A religious person will need to meet other religious
tions and invited participants to answer the same six
members to share their faith and beliefs. The next few
items (three reverse-keyed) to assess prejudice towards
questions are about this kind of groups to which you
each of three ethnic minority groups: Arab people, Black
would belong.
people and Asian people. These items have been taken
To strengthen the prime of group-based values and from a larger pool of items used in prejudice studies
norms, participants were invited to answer eight items (Duckitt, Callaghan, & Wagner, 2005, p. 645) and deWni-
derived from the Triandis collectivism scale (Triandis, tively presented as a generalized group attitude scale.
1995) and reported by Oseyrman (1993, p. 966), which Their adaptation for French people was done following
assesses how individuals behave in congruence with the the back translation procedure. Examples of items for
behaviours that the collective expects asks and demands. Arab people (worded analogously for the other ethnic
One example of items are A mature person understands groups) are: It really upsets me to hear anyone say
that he must act in accordance with the honors of the something negative about the Arab people (reversed); I
group and Without group loyalty there is no self-actual- can understand people having a negative attitude to the
ization. Arab people. A nine-point likert scale was used for each
A second experimental condition was named the com- prejudice score. Internal consistency reliabilities were
petitive group membership condition. A strong competitive acceptable for the scales involving these three ethnic
orientation for resources and for winning was primed with groups (Arab people,  D .74, Black people,  D .69, Asian
the use of the following cover story: people,  D .73). An overall aggregated prejudice measure
has been also computed ( D .74) because each prejudice
People belong to diVerent social groups like sport
score was expected to correlate signiWcantly one to the
club or political parties. These persons shared with
other. Just in the control condition (without any kind of
each other a strong need to defeat an opponent
primed membership) prejudice towards Blacks people
belonging to another competing group and have
was linked to prejudice towards Arab (r D .62, p < .000,
nationalistic aims for their country to become a supe-
N D 59) and Asian people (r D .44, p < .001, N D 59). Prej-
rior nation. More precisely, a sport team is involved in
udice towards Arab people correlated signiWcantly with
a competition to search the win and to become supe-
prejudice towards Asian people (r D .57, p < .000, N D 59).
rior to the other opponent team. A political party is in
Section 4 contained two control questions to verify that
conXict with other parties to win the elections. The
the two membership experimental conditions have been
next few questions are about this kind of groups to
understood. Participants were invited to indicate Wrst the
which you would belong.
importance for them (respectively scored 1, not important
To strengthen the prime of this competitive group orien- to 9, very important) of their group values and norms when
tation, participants were invited to answer six items of the responding in the section three (To what degree did you
Brown et al. (1992) autonomous-relational scale which consider the importance of your groups social values and
assesses how individuals evaluate the in-group and its out- norms when answering the previous section just?). The
comes with reference to out-groups and their outcomes. second question was about the degree of competitiveness
Despite the fact that the relational orientation described and superiority of their group (To what degree did you
here has been proposed by Brown et al. (1992) to assess the consider the competitiveness and superiority of your group
degree to which individual adopt a comparative orienta- when you answered the previous section?). Responses
tion, this comparative orientation is not completely similar were scored 1 (as weakly competitive) to 9 (as strongly
to a competitive orientation. However, the autonomous- competitive).
relational scale was constructed to assess not only this com- All participants answered the questionnaires anony-
parative orientation, but also a competitive orientation mously in small classes (N D 1030) at the University.
V. Dru / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (2007) 877883 881

Results Table 1
Correlations between prejudice scales and RWA, SDO and in-group iden-
tiWcation in three experimental conditions
Manipulation checks
Control Group Competitive
N D 59 Values N D 60 membership
Results from the Wrst control question showed that N D 60
participants assigned to the condition of competitive group
Attitudes to Arab people
membership considered their in-group values as less In-group identiWcation .17 .35 .08
important (M D 4.41, SD D 1.75, N D 60) than the partici- RWA .43 .39 .21
pants assigned to the condition of group-based values SDO .47 .18 .30
and norms (M D 6.45, SD D 2.32, N D 120, F(1,178) D 52.38, Mean prejudice score (and SD) 4.54 (.63) 4.17 (.92) 4.25 (1.09)
p < .000). It must be noted here that participants from the Attitudes to Black people
competitive group condition reported a moderate level of In-group identiWcation .00 .35 .15
importance to their in-group values (in the middle of the RWA .30 .37 .15
19 scale) showing that a competitive inter-group context SDO .46 .08 .42
Mean prejudice score (and SD) 3.86 (.78) 4.24 (1.02) 4.06 (.89)
of membership might inXuence some favourable in-group
evaluation (This is one hypothesis proposed by RCT; Dru, Attitudes to Asians
In-group identiWcation .18 .47 .00
2002; LeVine & Campbell, 1972, see also Dru, 2006).
RWA .27 .34 .14
Results from the second control condition have shown that SDO .44 .12 .26
participants assigned to the condition of group based Mean prejudice score (and SD) 3.64 (.65) 3.98 (.97) 3.75 (.71)
values and norms have considered their group as less Aggregated prejudice measure
competitive (M D 2.88, SD D 1.28, N D 60) than the In-group identiWcation .11 .40 .10
participants assigned to the condition of the competitive RWA .37 .38 .16
group condition (M D 6.08, SD D 2.68, N D 120, F(1,118) SDO .45 .14 .39
D 71.81, p < .000). Mean prejudice score (and SD) 4.01 (.68) 4.13 (.97) 4.02 (.90)

p < .05.

Correlational and multiple regression analyses p < .01.

p < .001.

For all the participants (N D 179, taking from the three


experimental conditions), RWA and SDO correlated sig- the one calculated between SDO and prejudice in the group
niWcantly (r D .39, p < .000). In addition RWA and in-group values and norms condition (z D 1.38, p < .08). In the same
identiWcation correlated signiWcantly (r D .25, p < .000). way, correlation of SDO with prejudice was also marginally
SDO and in-group identiWcation also correlated signiW- higher that the one calculated between RWA and prejudice
cantly (r D .21, p < .005). Before computing correlations in the competitive membership condition (z D 1.34, p < .09).
between the diVerent measures, Table 1 reported that no Correlation of RWA with prejudice was marginally higher
diVerences in prejudice (for each group and for the aggre- in the group values and norms condition that the one calcu-
gated prejudice score) between the experimental conditions lated for the competitive condition (z D 1.27, p < .10). And
appeared (Fs < 1). the correlation of SDO with prejudice was also marginally
Table 1 presents the correlation between the prejudice higher in the competitive condition that the one calculated
measures, RWA, SDO and in-group identiWcation in the for the group values and norms condition (z D 1.45, p < .07).
three experimental conditions. Finally, multiple regression analyses were conducted to
First, RWA and SDO correlated signiWcantly with the examine which variables were the best predictors of the
prejudice scales in the control condition whereas in-group prejudice scales in each experimental condition. Multiple
identiWcation failed to correlate with these same measures regression analyses allowed examination of the relation of
in that condition. Second, RWA, SDO and in-group identi- each predictor variables (RWA, SDO and in-group identiW-
Wcation correlated diVerently with prejudice depending on cation) with the prejudice measures while controlling for
the group membership conditions (values vs. competitive). the eVect of the other predictors. Table 2 displays these
In-group identiWcation and RWA correlated signiWcantly results.
with prejudice scales when a membership to groups sharing The results obtained by simple correlations (Table 1)
cultural and social values was salient whereas SDO corre- were partially replicated whenever all the betas value were
lated with the same measures only in the competitive mem- in the same direction, even if not always signiWcant. First, in
bership condition. Third, the three main results reported the control condition SDO was always the strongest predic-
above were replicated with the three diVerent ethnic groups. tor of prejudice for the all three ethnic groups, whereas
These correlations between the RWA, SDO and prejudice RWA was a signiWcant predictor of prejudice towards Arab
scores diVered signiWcantly from zero, whereas they mar- people, and in-group identiWcation was never a signiWcant
ginally diVer in the predicted direction from one condition predictor of prejudice towards the three ethnic groups
to the other. In considering the aggregated measure, corre- (despite a marginally signiWcant beta for Arab people).
lation of RWA with prejudice was marginally higher that Second, in-group identiWcation and RWA were signiWcant
882 V. Dru / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (2007) 877883

Table 2 sets of social attitudes and beliefs, would be diVerently


Betas values from multiple regression analyses in three experimental con- operative in predicting prejudice in self-categorization con-
ditions
ditions involving two diVerent primes of membership.
Control Group Competitive Inspection of correlations of Table 1 and of beta values
N D 59 values N D 60 membership
N D 60
(Table 2) showed that RWA and in-group identiWcation
were signiWcant predictors of prejudice towards the ethnic
Attitudes to Arab people
groups when group values and in-group norms were
In-group identiWcation .26() .46 .03
RWA .36 .32 .07 primed whereas SDO failed to predict these same measures.
SDO .35 .24 .26() At the opposite, SDO correlated with, and predicted preju-
Attitudes to Black people
dice scores when a competitive membership was salient
In-group identiWcation .05 .49 .22 whereas RWA failed to predict prejudice in a competitive
RWA .18 .37 .04 context.
SDO .40 .01 .47 These results are consistent with the identiWcation of two
Attitudes to Asians diVerent kinds of social categorization schemas which have
In-group identiWcation .24 .40 .06 been identiWed by Duckitt (2001). The Wrst one is connected
RWA .20 .36 .01 to a motivation to avoid threatening social situations for
SDO .39 .10 .29
values and in-group norms. It will underlie a schema to
Aggregated prejudice measure consider out-groups as deviant, bad, and as threatening to
In-group identiWcation .19 .43 .13 conventional rules. RWA, as a speciWc set of social beliefs,
RWA .31 .34 .06
SDO .38 .08 .37
might identify this process. This kind of social categoriza-
()
tion is also close to the identity function of inter-group
p < .10.

p < .05.
diVerentiation elaborated by Scheepers et al. (2002) and

p < .01. would correspond to the group values and norms primed

p < .001. condition. The second kind of social categorization is


linked to a motivation for competition and social domi-
predictors of prejudice scores towards the three ethnic nance. It will underlie a schema to consider out-groups
groups in the group values primed condition whereas SDO along an inferior-superior dimension. SDO, as another set
did not correlate with these scores. Finally, SDO was the of social beliefs about inequality, might identify this pro-
unique predictor of prejudice in the condition of competi- cess. This kind of social categorization is also close to the
tive membership. For attitudes toward Arab people, the instrumental function of inter-group diVerentiation elabo-
beta value for SDO was only marginally signiWcant. How- rated by Scheepers et al. (2002) and would correspond to
ever, with the use of the aggregated measure, in-group iden- the competitive membership condition (See also Esses,
tiWcation and RWA were signiWcant predictors of prejudice Jackson, & Armstrong, 1998 for their instrumental model
in the group values condition whereas SDO failed to do so. of group conXict).
This last measure was a signiWcant predictor of prejudice in The research presented in this article involved two diVer-
the competitive condition whereas RWA failed to do so. ent social attitudes (RWA and SDO) whereas the in-group
Finally,t-tests were computed to compare the diVerent beta identiWcation measure used in this study was only of one
values between conditions. The regression coeYcient for kind. The in-group identiWcation scale was taken from
RWA in the group values condition was marginally diVer- Phinneys (1992) work and assessed a cultural identiWcation
ent from the one calculated for the competitive condition to the in-group. This might be partially responsible for the
(t(118) D 1.89, p D .06). The regression coeYcient for SDO signiWcant correlation and beta value obtained for the links
in the group values condition was signiWcantly diVerent between in-group identiWcation and prejudice in the group
from the one calculated for the competitive condition values and in-group norms condition. Another interesting
(t(118) D 2.15, p D .033). The regression coeYcient for RWA avenue will not focus only on diVerent social attitudes,
in the group values condition was marginally diVerent from diVerent kinds of social categorization, but also on various
the one calculated for the SDO scale in the same condition forms of social identiWcation. It would be interesting to
(t(118) D 1.81, p D .072). And the regression coeYcient for study the eVect of two diVerent kinds of national identiWca-
RWA in the competitive condition was signiWcantly diVer- tion, named as patriotism and nationalism (Baughn & Yap-
ent from the one calculated for the SDO scale in the same rak, 1996; Kosterman & Feshbach, 1989; Mummendey
condition (t(118) D 1.99, p D .048). et al., 2001), which have been considered respectively to
parallel an evaluative and aVective attachment to the coun-
Discussion try (values an in-group norms) and a competitive identiWca-
tion to the nation (an us Wrst national orientation).
The research presented in this article was aimed at study- In a similar vein, a competitive group orientation might
ing the eVects of various self-categorization conditions on also prime some diVerences in ethnic status and correla-
prejudice. The results are consistent with the main hypothe- tions between SDO and prejudice in this context might be
sis. It was proposed that RWA and SDO, as two diVerent higher for low status target groups than for high status
V. Dru / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (2007) 877883 883

(consistent with a group position model, Bobo, 1999). Duckitt, J., Callaghan, J., & Wagner, C. (2005). Group identiWcation and
DiVerential status and the search for a higher status might outgroup attitudes in four South African ethnic groups. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 633646.
be connected to competitiveness (realistic competition). An Duriez, B., & Van Hiel, A. (2002). The march of modern fascism: A com-
in-group norms orientation might prime some diVerences in parison of social dominance orientation and authoritarianism. Person-
beliefs and values and correlations between RWA and prej- ality and Individual DiVerences, 32, 11991213.
udice in this context would be higher for groups who Ekehammar, B., Akrami, N., Gylje, M., & Zakrisson, I. (2004). matters
appear diVerent than for groups that are similar (consistent more for prejudice: Big Wve personality, social dominance or right-
wing authoritarianism? European Journal of Personality, 18, 463482.
with the similarity-attraction hypothesis, Smith & Kalin, Esses, V., Jackson, L., & Armstrong, T. (1998). Intergroup competition and
2006). Similarity and the search for some social diVerentia- attitudes toward immigrants and immigration: An instrumental model
tion between groups in values and norms might be con- of group conXict. Journal of Social Issues, 54, 699724.
nected to a positive social identity (social competition). Heaven, P. C. L., & Connors, J. R. (2001). A note on values correlates of
Status and similarity would be interesting moderators of social dominance and right wing authoritarianism. Personality and
Individual DiVerences, 31, 925930.
the link between SDO, RWA and prejudice respectively in Heaven, P. C. L., & Quintin, D. (2003). Personality factors predict preju-
primed contexts of competition and norms and conformity. dice. Personality and Individual DiVerences, 34, 625634.
Future research would be needed to address these diVerent Kosterman, R., & Feshbach, S. (1989). Toward a measure of patriotic and
issues to extend in an experimental direction the potential nationalistic attitudes. Political Psychology, 10, 257274.
eVects of RWA and SDO on prejudice and their connection LeVine, R., & Campbell, D. (1972). Ethnocentrism, theories of conXict, eth-
nic attitudes, and group behavior. New York: Wiley.
to fundamental inter-group processes. Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (1998). Intergroup relations: Insights from a
theoretically integrative approach. Psychological Review, 105(3), 499
References 529.
McFarland, S., & Adelson, S. (1996). An omnibus study of personality, val-
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswick, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. ues, and prejudice. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Inter-
(1950). The authoritarian personality. New York: Harper & Row. national Society of Political Psychology, Vancouver, Canada.
Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. Winnipeg: University of Mummendey, A., Klink, A., & Brown, R. (2001). and patriotism: National
Manitoba Press. identiWcation and out-group rejection. British Journal of Social Psy-
Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other authoritarian personality. In M. P. chology, 40, 159172.
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 30, pp. Oseyrman, D. (1993). The lens in personhood: viewing the self and others
4792). San Diego: Academic Press. in a multicultural society. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Bobo, L. (1999). Prejudice as group position: Microfoundations of a socio- 65, 9931009.
logical approach to racism and race relations. Journal of Social Issues, Phinney, J. S. (1992). The multigroup ethnic identity measure. A new scale
55, 445472. for use with diverse groups. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7, 156176.
Brown, R. J., Hinkle, S., Ely, P. G., Fox-Cardonne, D. L., Maras, P., & Tay- Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L., & Malle, B. (1994). Social domi-
lor, L. A. (1992). Recognizing group diversity: Individualist-collectivist nance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political
and autonomous-relational social orientations and their implications attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 741763.
for intergroup processes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 327 Pratto, F., Stallworth, L. M., & Conway-Lanz, S. (1998). Social dominance
342. theory and the legitimization of policy. Journal of Applied Social Psy-
Baughn, C., & Yaprak, A. (1996). Economic nationalism: Conceptual and chology, 20, 127150.
empirical development. Political Psychology, 17, 759778. Reynolds, K. J., Turner, J. C., Haslam, S. A., & Ryan, M. K. (2001). The
Doise, W. (1986). Levels of explanation in social psychology. Cambridge, role of personality and group factors in explaining prejudice. Journal of
UK/Paris: Cambridge University Press/Maison des Sciences de Experimental and Social Psychology, 37, 427434.
lHomme. Scheepers, D., Spears, R., Doojse, B., & Manstead (2002). Integrating iden-
Dru, V. (2002). Dogmatism and competitive relationships between soccer tity and instrumental approaches to intergroup diVerentiation : DiVer-
teams. International Journal of Group tensions, 31, 267284. ent contexts, diVerent motives. Personality and Social Psychology
Dru, V. (2003). Relationships between an ego orientation scale and a Bulletin, 28, 14551467.
hypercompetitive scale: Their correlates with dogmatism and authori- Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: an intergroup theory of
tarianism factors. Personality and Individual DiVerences, 35, 15091524. social hierarchy and oppression. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dru, V. (2004). Adaptation Franaise dune chelle didentiWcation cultu- Smith, S. M., & Kalin, R. (2006). Right-wing authoritarianism as a moder-
relle. [French adaptation of an ethnic identiWcation scale]. International ator of the sililarity-attraction eVect. Canadian Journal of Behavioural
Review of Social Psychology, 17, 103118. Science, 38, 6371.
Dru, V. (2006). Behavioral responses to a threatened ethnic identity in a Stone, W., Lederer, G., & Christie, R. (1993). The status of authoritarian-
sports setting. ConXict Management and Peace Science, 23, 2336. ism. In W. Stone, G. Lederer, & R. Christie (Eds.), Strength and weak-
Duarte, S., Dambrum, M., & Guimond, S. (2004). La domination sociale ness: The authoritarian personality today (pp. 229245). New York:
et les mythes lgitimateurs : validation dune version franaise de Springer.
lchelle dorientation la domination sociale. [Social dominance Sumner, W. (1906). Folkways. New York: Ginn.
and legitimizing myths: Validation of a French form of the social Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge, MA:
dominance scale]. International Review of Social Psychology, 17, 97 Cambridge University Press.
126. Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: West-
Duckitt, J. (1989). Authoritarianism and group identiWcation: A new view view Press.
of an old construct. Political Psychology, 10, 6384. Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S.
Duckitt, J. (1992). Psychology and prejudice: A historical analysis and (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory.
integrative framework. American Psychologist, 47(10), 11821193. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual process cognitive-motivational theory of ideol- Verkuyten, M., & Hagendoorn, L. (1998). Prejudice and self-categoriza-
ogy and prejudice. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental tion: the variable role of authoritarianism and in-group stereotypes.
social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 41113). San Diego: Academic Press. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 99110.

You might also like