You are on page 1of 11

Psychological Medicine (2013), 43, 15871596.

f Cambridge University Press 2012 O R I G I N A L AR T I C LE


doi:10.1017/S0033291712002450

The duration and timing of maternal depression as a


moderator of the relationship between dependent
interpersonal stress, contextual risk and early child
dysregulation

E. D. Barker*
Developmental Psychopathology Laboratory, Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, UK

Background. Risk factors that are associated with depression in the mother also negatively aect the child. This
research sought to extend current knowledge by examining the duration and timing of maternal depression as a
moderator of : (1) the impact of dependent interpersonal stress (DIS), such as partner conict or low social support,
and contextual risk (e.g. poverty) on child dysregulation ; and (2) continuity in early child dysregulation.

Method. Motherchild pairs (n=12 152) who participated in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children
(ALSPAC) were examined between pregnancy and age 4 years. Data on maternal depression were collected ve
times between pregnancy and 33 months postpartum ; on DIS and contextual risk three times between pregnancy and
33 months ; and on child dysregulation at age 2 and 4 years.

Results. Longitudinal latent class analysis identied a class of mothers (10 %) who evinced a chronic level of
depression between pregnancy and 33 months. For chronic-depressed versus non-depressed mothers, the results
indicate that : (1) DIS predicted higher child dysregulation if experienced between pregnancy and age 2 ; (2)
contextual risk had a dierential eect on child dysregulation if experienced during pregnancy ; and (3) children had
higher continuity in dysregulation between age 2 and age 4.

Conclusions. Assessing the impact of the timing and duration of maternal depression, and dierent types of
co-occurring risk factors, on child well-being is important. Maternal depression and associated DIS, in comparison to
contextual risk, may be more responsive to intervention.

Received 1 July 2012 ; Revised 3 September 2012 ; Accepted 27 September 2012 ; First published online 6 November 2012

Key words : Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, child dysregulation, contextual risk, dependent
interpersonal stress, maternal depression.

Introduction present research is Goodman & Gotlibs (1999) fourth


mechanism : the stressful contexts of the children.
In their seminal paper, Goodman & Gotlib (1999)
Studies have long shown that children are nega-
posed the question, What is it about having a de-
tively aected not only by their mothers depression
pressed mother that places a child at increased risk for
but also by risk factors associated with their mothers
abnormal development ? (p. 461), and then proposed
depression (Billings & Moos, 1983 ; Cox et al. 1987a ;
a developmental model, with four main mechanisms,
Hammen et al. 1987). Risk factors associated with
to explain the increased risk transmission. The rst
maternal depression are reported to inuence dicult
two mechanisms are biological : the heritability of
childhood temperament (Cicchetti et al. 1998), neuro-
depression (Kendler et al. 2006) and dysfunctional
logical function (Ashman et al. 2008) and externalizing
neuroregulatory mechanism in children (Oberlander
and internalizing disorders (Barker et al. 2012).
et al. 2008). The third is disturbed interpersonal pro-
The Sequenced Treatment Alternatives to Relieve
cesses, including child exposure to negative maternal
Depression (STAR*D) trials (Weissman et al. 2006)
cognitions and maladaptive parenting (Cummings
showed that, although the children of mothers whose
et al. 2005 ; Pawlby et al. 2011). The focus of the
depression remitted also showed signs of improve-
ment, the children who did not improve had mothers
whose depression did not remit and who had experi-
* Address for correspondence : Dr E. D. Barker, Department of
Psychological Science, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street,
enced the most severe risk factors.
London WC1E 7HX, UK. These results suggest that interventions aimed at
(Email : t.barker@bbk.ac.uk) the well-being of children of depressed parents also
1588 E. D. Barker

need to target associated risk factors. Goodman & maternal depression (from pregnancy to 33 months).
Gotlib (1999) suggested that at least two types of It was expected that up to 25 % of the sample could
information may further inform interventions : (1) dif- show a chronic class of clinical depression (Goodman
ferentiating dependent interpersonal stress (DIS) from & Gotlib, 1999). The second aim was to examine the
contextual risk ; and (2) the timing and duration of degree to which a class of chronic depression might
depression. With regard to stress, Hammen (1991, moderate the impact of DIS and contextual risk
2006) distinguished DIS, such as relationship disputes on child emotional-behavioral psychopathology in
or low social support (Kendler & Gardner, 2010), from the following manner : (1) DIS would have signi-
more independent contextual risks (e.g. poverty, cantly greater impact on child psychopathology for
crime), in that depressed persons can generate and chronic depressed versus non-depressed mothers ; and
increase DIS whereas rates of contextual risks are (2) compared with the DIS, the impact of contextual
less inuenced by depression. Stressors such as DIS, risk on child psychopathology would not show
however, seem to occur at elevated rates in high-risk consistent contrasts. The rationale for the diering
contexts (Brown et al. 2008 ; Kessler et al. 2010), as does hypotheses of contextual risks versus DIS is from pre-
maternal depression (Barker et al. 2012), and each vious research showing that, during infancy, contex-
can impact child well-being. For example, one study tual risk can aect caregiver mental health more than
reported that dicult-to-manage preschool children that of the child (Kohen et al. 2008). The third aim was
had depressed mothers with low social support and to determine whether, as expected, child emotional-
high poverty (Parry, 1986). The distinction between behavioral dysregulation would show higher continuity
contextual risk and DIS is important ; if high DIS for chronic-depressed versus non-depressed mothers.
is at last partially dependent on depression, these
stressors may be more responsive to interventions
than contextual risks. Method
Goodman & Gotlib (1999) also suggested that the Sample
duration and timing of child exposure to depression
could act as a moderator in the link between maternal The Avon Longitudinal Study of Children and Parents
depression and long-term courses of child psycho- (ALSPAC) was established to understand how genetic
pathology. The authors stated that : (1) as many as 25 % and environmental characteristics inuence health
of persons who experience a rst major depression and development in parents and children. All preg-
may go on to experience chronic depression ; and (2) nant women resident in a dened area in the South
long-term eects of chronic maternal depression on West of England, with an expected date of delivery
child psychopathology may be more pronounced between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992, were
early in development. Biological systems relevant to eligible and 13 761 women (contributing 13 867 preg-
regulation of arousal are functionally immature dur- nancies) were recruited. These women have been
ing pregnancy and at birth, but mature gradually followed over the past 1922 years (Fraser et al. 2012).
during the rst years of life (Glover, 2011). A healthy When compared to 1991 National Census Data, the
maternal lifestyle (prenatal and early postnatal) and ALSPAC sample was found to be similar to the UK
the sensitive regulation of the infants emotions and population as a whole (Boyd et al. 2012). Ethical ap-
behaviors are most needed during the rst few years proval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC
of life, to ensure mastery of early social and behavorial Law and Ethics Committee and the Local Research
skills (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999 ; Feldman et al. 2009). Ethics Committees.
However, the presence of chronic depression and as-
sociated risk factors may render a mother incapable of Measures
providing such care to herself and to her child during Mothers completed questionnaires during their preg-
pregnancy and the rst years of a childs life. Research nancy and their childs infancy and childhood. The
has indeed shown that DIS related to maternal de- early risk factors examined here were drawn from the
pression can negatively aect adolescent psycho- family adversity measure (Bowen et al. 2005) com-
pathology (Hammen et al. 2004 ; Garber & Cole, 2010). pleted between birth and approximately 4 years of
The degree to which similar eects are present early child age.
in development, within an integrated developmental
model that incorporates (a) DIS and contextual risk
Maternal depression
and (b) the duration and timing of maternal de-
pression, is yet to be evaluated. Maternal depression was assessed repeatedly (at 32
The focus of the present research was threefold. The weeks prenatal, and 8 weeks, 8 months, 21 months and
rst aim was to evaluate the duration and timing of 33 months postnatal) with the Edinburgh Postnatal
The duration and timing of maternal depression 1589

Depression Scale, a widely used 10-item self-report to in times of trouble (pregnancy : at 12 weeks ; age
questionnaire that has been shown to be valid during 02 : 2 and 8 months)] ; and (5) major family problems
and outside the postnatal period (Cox et al. 1987b ; as examined through social services data [e.g. child in
Murray & Carothers, 1990). A score of 13 predicts a the household taken into extra-familial care, child in
clinical diagnosis of depression (Murray & Carothers, household registered with at-risk register, child no
1990). longer living in household (pregnancy : at 18 weeks ;
age 02 : 21 months ; age 24 : 33 and 47 months)].
Contextual risk factors
Contextual risk factors consisted of mother reports, Child emotional and behavioral dysregulation
during pregnancy, between child age 0 and 2 years, Child emotional and behavioral dysregulation at
and between child age 2 and 4 years. At each age, any ages 2 and 4 was measured by mother reports on the
indication of the risk resulted in a score of 1 (0=none). Carey Infant Temperament Scale (Carey & McDevitt,
There were seven total risks, resulting in a potential 1978) and the Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire
range of 07 risk exposures. The seven risks included : (SDQ ; Goodman, 2001) respectively. Four subscales
(1) inadequate basic living conditions such as not from the Carey Infant Scale were included : activity,
having a working bath/shower, no hot water, no adaptability, intensity and mood. For the SDQ, three
indoor toilet and/or no working kitchen (pregnancy : subscales where included : hyperactivity, conduct
assessed at 8 weeks ; age 02 : assessed at 2, 8 and 21 problems and emotional diculties. These subscales
months ; age 24 : assessed at 33 and 47 months) ; were used as indicators of the latent construct of
(2) inadequate housing as any indication of crowding child dysregulation. In a two-factor conrmatory
(pregnancy : at 8 weeks ; age 02 : 21 months ; age 24 : solution, model t was marginal [Comparative Fit
33 months) and/or homelessness (pregnancy : at Index (CFI)=0.91, root mean square error of approxi-
18 weeks ; age 02 : 2, 8 and 21 months ; age 24 : mation (RMSEA)=0.087, 95 % condence interval (CI)
33 months) ; (3) housing defects as any indication of 0.0810.089] ; the standardized loadings for the latent
mold, roof leaks, and rats, mice or cockroaches dysregulation construct for the Carey Scales (range
(pregnancy : at 18 weeks ; age 02 : 8 and 21 months ; 0.570.83) and the SDQ (range 0.430.73) were accept-
age 24 : 33 months) ; (4) poverty, coded using the able (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001) ; there were no sex
Registrar Generals social class scale (OPCS, 1991) dierences in the loadings (Dx27=7.52, p=0.377) ; and
(pregnancy : at 32 weeks ; age 02 : 8 and 21 months ; the latent coecient of prediction was high (b=0.597,
age 24 : 33 months) ; (5) being a single caregiver [e.g. S.E.=0.013, p<0.0001) and suggestive of homotypic
not cohabiting, not in a relationship (pregnancy : continuity of a construct.
at 32 weeks ; age 02 : 6 and 21 months ; age 24 : 33
and 47 months)] ; (6) early parenthood [f19 years (at Attrition and missing data
18 weeks in pregnancy)] ; and (7) low educational
attainment [e.g. did not nish mandatory schooling A total of 12 151 of the original 13 867 mothers were
(pregnancy : at 32 weeks ; age 02 : 21 months ; age 23 : included in the analysis. Mothers who were excluded
33 months)]. had complete missing data on the ve self-reports of
depression. Of the 12 151 who were included, 1.1 %
DIS had one report of depression, 8.4 % had two reports of
depression, 13.7 % had three reports of depression,
Data on DIS were collected at the same times as that on 25.7 % had four reports of depression and 51 % had
the contextual risks. The ve DIS factors resulted in ve reports of depression. The included mothers had
a possible range of 05 exposures, and included : complete data available for at least one of both the
(1) mother experiencing partner cruelty [e.g. any scale scores of DIS and contextual risk. Child dysre-
indication of emotional and/or physical abuse from gulation was available for 85 % of the 12 151 mothers.
partner (pregnancy : assessed 18 weeks ; age 02 : 2, 6 As listwise deletion of families with partial complete
and 21 months ; age 24 : 33 and 47 months)] ; (2) low data can increase sample bias (e.g. exclude the most
partner aection to mother [e.g. partner does not high risk families; Enders, 2010), missing for child
show aection, does not hug/kiss, low intimate bond dysregulation was handled via Full Information
(pregnancy : at 12 weeks ; age 02 : 8 months ; age 24 : Maximum Likelihood Estimation (Enders, 2010).
33 months)] ; (3) low partner social support [does not
discuss feelings with, feel supported by (pregnancy : at
Analysis
18 weeks ; age 02 : 2 and 8 months)] ; (4) low practical
support [i.e. whether there is anyone who could lend The analysis proceeded in three main steps. In the rst
the mother 100 and/or anyone the mother could turn step we estimated a longitudinal latent class (LLC)
1590 E. D. Barker

Low (54%)
Medium (36%)
16 Clinical (10%)
Mean level depression: clinical cutoff  13

14

12

10

0
Pregnancy 8 weeks 8 months 21 months 33 months

Fig. 1. Longitudinal latent classes of maternal depression. One-class model : entropy=not applicable (N.A.), Bayesian
Information Criterion (BIC)=320814.32, LoMendellRubin likelihood ratio test (LMR-LRT)=N.A. ; two-class model :
entropy=0.84, BIC=303650.96, LMR-LRT=0.0000 ; three-class model : entropy=0.81, BIC=298662.28, LMR-LRT=0.0000 ;
four-class model : entropy=0.78, BIC=297433.04, LMR-LRT=0.0000 ; ve-class model : entropy=0.78, BIC=296784.33,
LMR-LRT=0.4625.

model of maternal depression, a type of analysis Bollen, 2001). In this modeling approach, each variable
that accounts for time ordering of variables and can in the model is regressed on all of the variables that
function as a piecewise growth mixture model (see precede it in time, which includes autoregressions
Croudace et al. 2003). More generally, an LLC model (child dysregulation predicting child dysregulation)
can easily describe classes of mothers who may follow and cross-lags (DIS predicting child dysregulation).
dierent non-linear patterns of depression (e.g. pre- For moderation, the ARCL coecients can be strati-
natal only, postnatal only, chronic depression) over ed by group (e.g. depressed versus non-depressed
time. A series of models was tted, starting with a mothers) and nested dierences can be tested. Within-
one-class model and moving to a ve-class model. time correlations (DIS and contextual risk during
Previously, recommendations (e.g. Muthen, 2004) on pregnancy) can also be estimated and controlled for in
selecting the best model for the data included refer- the cross-lagged eects.
ence to t indices such as the Bayesian Information All analyses were conducted in Mplus version 6.21
Criterion (BIC ; lower values indicate a more parsi- (Muthen & Muthen, 19982010). To provide robust
monious model), the LoMendellRubin likelihood estimates and to account for missing values, full
ratio test (LMR-LRT : a k 1 LR-based method for information maximum likelihood estimation with
determining the ideal number of trajectories) and robust standard errors (MLR) was used. Model t was
entropy (a measure of classication accuracy with determined through the CFI (acceptable t o0.90)
values closer to 1 indexing greater precision). Clark & (Bentler & Bonett, 1980) and RMSEA (acceptable t
Muthen (unpublished observations) have recently sug- f0.08) (Browne & Cudeck, 1993). SatorraBentler
gested that entropy is crucial, with a recommended scaled x2 dierence tests (Satorra, 2000) were used
value of 0.80. to test nested model comparisons, which adjust for
In the second step, correlations between the vari- non-normality in the distribution of the data.
ables by the dierent latent classes of maternal de-
pression were examined. For example, here it was
assessed the degree to which DIS and contextual risks Results
correlated higher with child psychopathology for
Step 1 : LLC models of maternal depression
depressed versus non-depressed mothers.
In the third step, the patterns in the correlations Figure 1 shows the three-class model of maternal
were tested in a multiple group, autoregressive cross- depression, and the t statistics for the one-class to
lagged (ARCL) model. The ARCL technique is widely ve-class models. As can be seen, although the BIC
used to assess associations in data derived from non- decreased with additional classes, at the four-class
experimental, longitudinal research designs (Curran & model the entropy was less than 0.80 at the ve-class
The duration and timing of maternal depression 1591

Table 1. Correlations and descriptive statistics of the variables included in the study by non-depressed mothers (above the diagonal) and
chronic-depressed mothers (below the diagonal)

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

(1) Child dysregulation : age 2 0.577 0.064 0.069 0.057 0.083 0.125 0.046
(2) Child dysregulation : age 4 0.683 0.119 0.164 0.153 0.099 0.159 0.141
(3) Contextual risk : pregnancy 0.201 0.230 0.632 0.423 0.255 0.216 0.123
(4) Contextual risk : age 02 0.155 0.251 0.625 0.666 0.216 0.207 0.139
(5) Contextual risk : age 24 0.077 0.209 0.390 0.624 0.156 0.168 0.172
(6) DIS : pregnancy 0.243 0.251 0.278 0.274 0.278 0.429 0.188
(7) DIS : age 02 0.143 0.264 0.208 0.267 0.257 0.445 0.159
(8) DIS : age 24 0.031 0.114 0.093 0.133 0.390 0.170 0.391
Non-depressed
Mean (S.D.) 75.53 (13.94) 7.16 (3.76) 0.34 (0.69) 0.67 (0.91) 0.53 (0.81) 0.34 (0.69) 0.61 (0.90) 0.25 (0.55)
Raw skew x0.03 0.65 1.87 1.53 1.75 2.30 1.56 2.29
log skew 1.62 0.88 1.75
Depressed
Mean (S.D.) 82.28 (14.79) 9.41 (4.37) 0.87 (1.00) 1.21 (1.21) 1.01 (1.06) 1.51 (1.23) 1.84 (1.47) 1.06 (0.79)
Raw skew x0.02 0.42 1.12 0.98 1.18 1.07 0.35 1.02
Log skew 0.48 x0.29 0.66

DIS, Dependent interpersonal stress ; S.D., standard deviation ; , not transformed.


All correlations are signicant at p<0.05.

model, and the LMR-LRT was non-signicant, which, for chronic-depressed versus non-depressed mothers.
when combined, suggested that the three-class or Skew was high for the DIS of non-depressed mothers
two-class model was a better t to the data. The three- and was therefore transformed.
class model was chosen. The principal dierence
between the three-class and two-class models was that
Step 3 : Multiple group ARCL model
in the former, a chronic-depressed group of mothers
(i.e. the scores on the EPDS, at each assessment, were To examine dierences in the chronic-depressed versus
>13) was identied whereas the latter had two non- non-depressed mothers we tested a series of nested
depressed classes mothers, who varied in symptom model comparisons, including : (1) the cross-lagged
levels. predictions of the DIS, contextual risks and child dys-
The three classes identied were those mothers who regulation ; (2) the auto-regressions of the DIS, con-
were chronic-depressed (10 %), those who reported textual risks and child dysregulation ; and (3) the
medium symptoms (36 %) and those who reported within-time associations between DIS and contextual
low symptoms (54 %). The latter two classes of risks. The model where all parameters were free to
mothers (i.e. the medium and low) were combined to vary between depressed and non-depressed mothers
allow comparisons of chronic-depressed mothers with showed an acceptable t (x2105=2645.43, p<0.001,
the non-depressed mothers. CFI=0.92, RMSEA=0.063, 90 % CI 0.0610.065). This
model was the comparison model for all nested tests
presented below.
Step 2 : Correlations of study variables for
The cross-lagged predictions varied signicantly
chronic-depressed versus non-depressed mothers
between the chronic-depressed and non-depressed
Table 1 contains the correlations, means, standard mothers (Dx210=41.18, p<0.001). (Note that there were
deviations and skew of the study variables. These 10 degrees of freedom for the overall dierence test as
statistics are separated by the LLC analyses of ma- there were 10 total cross-lagged parameters ; see
ternal depression. With regard to correlations, child Fig. 2.) Nested dierence tests showed that the fol-
dysregulation was associated with both contextual lowing predictions were greater for chronic-depressed
risk and DIS, and these were somewhat larger for versus non-depressed mothers : (1) contextual risks
chronic-depressed than non-depressed mothers. With (pregnancy) predicted child dysregulation (age 2)
regard to mean levels of risk exposures, prevalence (Dx21=5.16, p=0.023) ; (2) DIS (pregnancy) predicted
was low but, on average, at least two times greater child dysregulation (age 2) (Dx21=8.75, p=0.003) ;
in contextual risks and three times greater for DIS, and (3) DIS (age 02) predicted child dysregulation
1592 E. D. Barker

DIS 0.42/0.40 DIS 0.39/0.33 DIS

0.20/0.07 0.11 0.12


0.14/0.07

Child
0.28/0.24 0.14/0.08 Child 0.65/0.56 0.22/0.10
Dysreg
Dysreg

0.15/0.05 0.04 0.11 0.06

C. Risk 0.62 C. Risk 0.64 C. Risk

Pregnancy Age 02 Age 24

Fig. 2. Multiple group path analysis. Path coecients : depressed/non-depressed or averaged across all mothers ; single arrow
paths=predictions ; double arrow paths=correlations ; circles=latent variables (items not shown for the sake of a simple
presentation) ; rectangular=manifest variable. All parameters shown are signicant at p<0.05. The pathways from child
emotional and behavioral dysregulation (Child Dysreg) to dependent interpersonal stress (DIS) and contextual risks (C. Risk)
failed to reach signicance; predictions between DIS and C. Risk are not shown for the simplicity of the presentation (and
because these relationships are not central to the current research questions), but are available on request from the author.

(age 4) (Dx21=6.21, p=0.013). Chronic-depressed Discussion


and non-depressed mothers did not signicantly
dier in (1) the signicant prediction of contextual This study is the rst to examine the timing and
risks (age 02) to age 4 child dysregulation or in duration of maternal depression as a moderator of the
(2) the cross-lagged eects between DIS and contex- impact of co-occurring DIS and contextual risk on
tual risks. early child dysregulation, and continuity in early child
The auto-regressions also varied signicantly dysregulation. Of the 12152 ALSPAC mothers, 10 %
between the depressed and non-depressed mothers were identied as being chronically depressed be-
(Dx25=21.17, p<0.001). Nested dierence tests showed tween pregnancy and 33 months postpartum, so the
that, compared to non-depressed mothers, chronic- duration was long and the childs exposure was early.
depressed mothers reported higher continuity in : Compared to children of non-depressed mothers,
(1) age 2 to age 4 child dysregulation (Dx21=5.16, children of chronic-depressed mothers were exposed
p=0.023) ; (2) DIS from pregnancy to age 02 to higher levels of DIS and contextual risks. At both
(Dx21=5.33, p=0.021) ; and (3) DIS from age 02 to age pregnancy and age 2, the DIS impact on later child
24 (Dx21=13.03, p<0.001). Beyond these dierences, dysregulation was signicantly higher for children of
in general, the magnitude of the continuity for con- chronic-depressed mothers. At pregnancy, the impact
textual risk was approximately 1.5 times higher than of contextual risk on child dysregulation was also
the magnitude of continuity of the DIS. signicantly higher for children of chronic-depressed
Within-time associations varied signicantly be- mothers. Furthermore, children of chronic-depressed
tween the chronic-depressed and non-depressed mothers showed higher continuity in dysregulation.
mothers (Dx27=63.13, p<0.001). Nested dierence tests To examine the timing and duration of maternal
showed that the associations between DIS and depression, the current study used an LLC approach
contextual risks were signicantly higher for chronic- and identied 10 % of the mothers to be at a clinical
depressed versus non-depressed mothers at pregnancy level of depression between pregnancy and 33 months
(Dx21=21.86, p<0.001), at age 02 (Dx21=9.06, p<0.001) postpartum. Previous studies using a similar analytic
and at age 24 (Dx21=30.51, p<0.001). The within-time approach have similarly identied a group of chronic
covariance of DIS and contextual risks with child and clinically depressed mothers. Of note, when
dysregulation were signicant, but did not signi- centered around the rst years of a childs life,
cantly vary between chronic-depressed and non- the percentage of mothers in the chronic-depressed
depressed mothers. Of interest, DIS was signicantly class (11 %) was very close to that of the current
higher in correlations with child dysregulation than study (Campbell et al. 2007). However, when the
contextual risks with child dysregulation, at age studies also included later developmental periods
02 (Dx21=4.68, p=0.035) and age 24 (Dx21=6.34, (pre-adolescence and adolescence), the percentage
p=0.011). of chronic-depressed mothers (2.54.0 %) was lower
The duration and timing of maternal depression 1593

(Ashman et al. 2008 ; Campbell et al. 2009). These nd- chronic-depressed mothers might support the critical
ings corroborate the previous reports that rates of period hypothesis, where, for example, high stress and
depression in females peak during pregnancy and in depression during pregnancy can lead to an intra-
the early postnatal years (Kessler, 2003), and that uterine environment that is not conducive to healthy
latent class approaches can capture this peak but only fetal development (Weinstock, 2008), and can then
if focused within early development (e.g. Nagin, 2005). increase risk for behavioral and emotional maladjust-
However, whatever the duration of the develop- ment in the child (Glover, 2011). However, the exact
mental period, Campbell et al. (2007, 2009) reported mechanisms of this pre- and post-natal dierence
that youth whose mothers were in a latent class of could not be tested because of the non-biological
chronic-depression had the highest internalizing and nature of the data in this study (Barker et al. 2011).
externalizing diculties (e.g. at both at school entry Continuity in child deregulation (age 24) was
and at age 15), in addition to the highest exposure to found to be higher for children of chronic-depressed
associated risk factors. The use of latent classes as mothers. It is important to note that in Goodman &
predictors of outcomes, however, can somewhat limit Gotlibs (1999) theoretical model, this age range was
the assessment of dynamic co-occurring developmen- dened as a period of increased vulnerability for long-
tal processes, such as risk exposures, that might also term maladjustment (in adolescence and adulthood).
contribute to child dysregulation (see, for example, Hence, the current research should be considered a
Barker et al. 2010). There is good reason to assess test of early vulnerability in children. That said,
dynamic processes related to maternal depression, as the current results do show a transition from age 2
previous research has indicated that associated risk dysregulated temperament (Carey & McDevitt, 1978)
factor exposure can explain half (or more) of the risk to a measure (at age 4) that has been validated in
transmission of maternal depression (Cicchetti et al. children and adolescents, and that has good predict-
1998 ; Barker et al. 2012). Moreover, risk exposure (e.g. ability of adolescent conduct disorder and depression
poverty, poor inter-relationships with others, poor (Goodman, 2001).
housing conditions) is greatest while the mother is Of note, however, the high continuity in dysregu-
clinically depressed (Cox et al. 1987 a ; Hammen et al. lation for children of chronic-depressed mothers was
1987), and when maternal depression remits, children above and beyond the impact of DIS and contextual
also tend to improve (Garber et al. 2011). risks. Hence, in accordance with Goodman & Gotlib
The present study is the rst the use latent classes (1999), additional factors, such as the heritability of
of maternal depression to assess the dynamic co- depression, neuroregulatory mechanisms and mal-
occurrence of dierent types of risk factors and also adaptive parenting, need to be examined to explain
continuity in child dysregulation. Children of chronic- the increased continuity in deregulation. A related
depressed mothers were more exposed to and aected point is that the DIS measure used here is broad and
by DIS at pregnancy and age 2. Contextual risk, how- includes only one item that assesses the most re-
ever, had more eect on children of chronic-depressed searched, and perhaps inuential (Rutter & Quinton,
mothers at pregnancy only. The distinction between 1984), interpersonal stressor : the eect of marital dis-
DIS and contextual risk may be important. The stress cord on children (Cummings et al. 2004). Therefore, the
generation hypothesis (Hammen, 1991) that underlies current results may have underestimated the eect of
DIS has emphasized that depressed persons can create marital discord. Moreover, the marital discord item
interpersonal stress (e.g. conict with partner, low used here came from mothers self-reporting partner
social support, low partner aection). Therefore, the cruelty. Although this was conceptualized as partially
generation of DIS by a depressed caregiver can serve dependent on mothers depression, this may not be
to maintain a risk environment that is proximal to the the case for all women. Certain of these women may
social milieu during pregnancy and the rst years of have been victimized by their partners, which could
life, when the child is particularly dependent on the also result in higher depression and DIS, along with
quality of the environment provided by the caregiver increased potential of co-occurring child maltreatment
(Goodman & Gotlib, 1999). (Holden, 2003).
Of note, after pregnancy, the contextual risks tran- The present results should be interpreted in the
sitioned to function as a general risk for dysregulation context of six main limitations. First, much of the
in children of chronic-depressed and non-depressed research on dependent stressors follows contextual
mothers alike, which underscores the ubiquitous threat methods established by Brown & Harris (1978)
nature of poverty on the well-being of children and Hammen et al. (1989) that include interviews that
and adults (e.g. Miller et al. 2009 ; Shonko et al. objectify the circumstances and impact of the stress,
2009). However, the fact that contextual risk during which are meant to minimize potential biases as-
pregnancy impacted later child dysregulation for sociated with self-reports. Second, the current study
1594 E. D. Barker

was limited by the use of symptom scales to assess them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes
depression in mothers and dysregulation in children interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians,
rather than more thorough diagnostic interviews clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers,
and/or observations. Third, most measures were based managers, receptionists and nurses; and to colleagues
on maternal reports, raising the possibility of shared (R. T. Salekin, E. Viding, B. Maughan, T. Kretschmer)
method variance. Future studies should incorporate for providing the feedback on a previous version of
multiple informants. Fourth, although the mothers and the manuscript. The UK Medical Research Council
children of ALSPAC represent a broad spectrum of (grant ref. 74883), the Wellcome Trust (grant ref.
socio-economic status (SES) backgrounds, the sample 0754567) and the University of Bristol provide core
includes relatively low rates of ethnic minorities. The support for ALSPAC. E. D. Barker had full access to all
present results will need replication with more ethni- of the data in this study and takes responsibility for
cally diverse samples. Fifth, the present paper focused the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data
on maternal depression and associated risk factors. As analysis. The analysis and writing of the paper was
highlighted in the introduction and discussion sections supported by a grant from the National Institute of
of this paper, however, the quality of parenting a child Child and Human Development to E. D. Barker
receives is an important potential mediator of the (NIH-1R01 HD068437-01A1).
association measures of maternal psychopathology and
child maladjustment. Sixth, the analytic routine chosen
Declaration of Interest
for this study was specic to the overall research
question : comparing the relative impacts of DIS and None.
contextual risks on child dysregulation of chronic-
depressed versus non-depressed mothers. Testing the
References
specics of the stress generation hypothesis would
require a dierent analytic model. Ashman SB, Dawson G, Panagiotides H (2008). Trajectories
The current study is useful if the results can be of maternal depression over 7 years : relations with child
translated into practical knowledge on how risk psychophysiology and behavior and role of contextual
factors associated with depression in the mother may risks. Development and Psychopathology 20, 5577.
Barker ED, Copeland W, Maughan B, Jaee SR, Uher R
aect childrens adjustment. Our results underscore
(2012). Relative impact of maternal depression and
that early timing and extensive duration of maternal
associated risk factors on ospring psychopathology.
depression co-occur with early timing and long
British Journal of Psychiatry 200, 124129.
duration of child exposure to DIS and contextual risks. Barker ED, Jaee SR, Uher R, Maughan B (2011). The
Because the children of the chronic-depressed mothers contribution of prenatal and postnatal maternal anxiety
were impacted by the depression, DIS and contextual and depression to child maladjustment. Depression and
risk, beginning in pregnancy and in the rst years Anxiety 28, 696702.
of life, preventive interventions that target at-risk Barker ED, Oliver BR, Maughan B (2010). Co-occurring
families as early as possible are warranted. For ex- problems of early onset persistent, childhood limited, and
ample, identifying at-risk women during pregnancy adolescent onset conduct problem youth. Journal of Child
with the intention to provide intensive postpartum Psychology and Psychiatry 51, 12171226.
Bentler PM, Bonett DG (1980). Signicance tests and
support may reduce postnatal depression for these
goodness of t in the analysis of covariance structures.
women (Dennis, 2005). Hence, the children of at-risk
Psychological Bulletin 88, 588606.
mothers who receive such support may be less Billings AG, Moos RH (1983). Comparisons of children
exposed to DIS than at-risk mother who do not. As of depressed and nondepressed parents : a social-
suggested by Garber et al. (2011), practical and cost- environmental perspective. Journal of Abnomal Child
ecient support, at the community level, should Psychology 11, 463485.
also be considered a valuable resource. For example, Bowen E, Heron J, Waylen A, Wolke D (2005). Risk for
obstetric clinics and community agencies (e.g. schools, domestic violence during and after pregnancy : ndings
churches, health-care settings) may be used to provide from a British longitudinal study. British Journal of
at-risk parents with information about how parental Obstetrics and Gynaecology 112, 10831089.
Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, Lawlor DA, Fraser A,
depression can work to generate stressful inter-
Henderson J, Molloy L, Ness A, Ring S, Davey Smith G
personal life events that can also aect their children.
(2012). Cohort Prole : The Children of the 90s the index
ospring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and
Acknowledgments Children. International Journal of Epidemiology. Published
online : 16 April 2012. doi :10.1093/ije/dys064.
I am very grateful to all the families who took part in Brown GW, Craig TKJ, Harris TO (2008). Parental
this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting maltreatment and proximal risk factors using the
The duration and timing of maternal depression 1595

Childhood Experience of Care & Abuse (CECA) Fraser A, Macdonald-Wallis C, Tilling K, Boyd A,
instrument : a life-course study of adult chronic Golding J, Davey Smith G, Henderson J, Macleod J,
depression 5. Journal of Aective Disorders 110, 222233. Molloy L, Ness A, Ring S, Nelson SM, Lawlor DA (2012).
Brown GW, Harris T (1978). Social Origins of Depression : Cohort prole : the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents
A Study of Psychiatric Disorder in Women. Tavistock : and Children : ALSPAC mothers cohort. International
London. Journal of Epidemiology. Published online : 16 April 2012.
Browne MW, Cudeck R (1993). Alternative ways of assessing doi :10.1093/ije/dys066.
model t. In Testing Structural Equation Models (ed. K. A. Garber J, Ciesla JA, McCauley E, Diamond G, Schloredt KA
Bollen and J. S. Long), pp. 230258. Sage : Newbury Park, CA. (2011). Remission of depression in parents : links to
Campbell SB, Matestic P, von Stauenberg C, Mohan R, healthy functioning in their children. Child Development 82,
Kirchner T (2007). Trajectories of maternal depressive 226243.
symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and childrens functioning Garber J, Cole DA (2010). Intergenerational transmission
at school entry. Developmental Psychology 43, 12021215. of depression : a launch and grow model of change
Campbell SB, Morgan-Lopez AA, Cox MJ, McLoyd VC across adolescence. Development and Psychopathology 22,
(2009). A latent class analysis of maternal depressive 819830.
symptoms over 12 years and ospring adjustment in Glover V (2011). Annual Research Review : Prenatal stress
adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 118, 479493. and the origins of psychopathology : an evolutionary
Carey WB, McDevitt SC (1978). Revision of the Infant perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52,
Temperament Questionnaire. Pediatrics 61, 735739. 356367.
Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Toth SL (1998). Maternal Goodman R (2001). Psychometric properties of the
depressive disorder and contextual risk : contributions to Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire (SDQ). Journal of
the development of attachment insecurity and behavior the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 40,
problems in toddlerhood. Development and Psychopathology 13371345.
10, 283300. Goodman SH, Gotlib IH (1999). Risk for psychopathology
Cox AD, Puckering C, Pound A, Mills M (1987a). The in the children of depressed parents : a developmental
impact of maternal depression in young children. Journal approach to the understanding of mechanisms.
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 28, 917928. Psychological Review 106, 458490.
Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R (1987b). Detection of Hammen C (1991). Generation of stress in the course of
postnatal depression : the development of the Edinburgh unipolar depression. Journal of Abnomal Psychology 100,
10-item Postnatal Depression Scale. British Journal of 555561.
Psychiatry 150, 782786. Hammen C (2006). Stress generation in depression :
Croudace TJ, Jarvelin MR, Wadsworth MEJ, Jones PB reections on origins, research, and future directions.
(2003). Developmental typology of trajectories to nighttime Journal of Clinical Psychology 62, 10651082.
bladder control : epidemiologic application of longitudinal Hammen C, Ellicott A, Gitlin M, Jamison KR (1989).
latent class analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology 157, Sociotropy/autonomy and vulnerability to specic life
834842. events in patients with unipolar depression and bipolar
Cummings EM, Goeke-Morey MC, Papp LM (2004). disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 98, 154160.
Everyday marital conict and child aggression. Journal of Hammen C, Gordon D, Burge D, Adrian C, Jaenicke C,
Abnormal Child Psychology 32, 191202. Hiroto D (1987). Maternal aective disorders, illness,
Cummings EM, Keller MB, Davies DR (2005). Towards a and stress : risk for childrens psychopathology. American
family process model of maternal and paternal depressive Journal of Psychiatry 144, 736741.
symptoms : exploring multiple relations with child and Hammen C, Shih JH, Brennan PA (2004). Intergenerational
family functioning. Journal of Child Psychology and transmission of depression : test of an interpersonal stress
Psychiatry 46, 479489. model in a community sample. Journal of Consulting and
Curran PJ, Bollen KA (2001). The best of both worlds : Clinical Psychology 72, 511522.
combining autoregressive and latent curve models. In Holden GW (2003). Children exposed to domestic violence
New Methods for the Analysis of Change (ed. L. M. Collins and child abuse : terminology and taxonomy. Clinical Child
and A. G. Sayer), pp. 105135. American Psychological and Family Psychology Review 6, 151160.
Association : Washington, DC. Kendler KS, Gardner CO (2010). Dependent stressful life
Dennis C-L (2005). Psychosocial and psychological events and prior depressive episodes in the prediction of
interventions for prevention of postnatal depression : major depression : the problem of causal inference in
systematic review. British Medical Journal 331, 15. psychiatric epidemiology. Archives of General Psychiatry 67,
Enders CK (2010). Applied Missing Data Analysis. 11201127.
Guilford Press : New York. Kendler KS, Gatz M, Gardner CO, Pedersen NL (2006).
Feldman R, Granat A, Pariente C, Kanety H, Kuint J, A Swedish national twin study of lifetime major
Gilboa-Schechtman E (2009). Maternal depression and depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 109114.
anxiety across the postpartum year and infant social Kessler RC (2003). Epidemiology of women and depression.
engagement, fear regulation, and stress reactivity. Journal of Journal of Aective Disorders 74, 513.
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 48, Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ,
919927. Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Aguilar-Gaxiola S,
1596 E. D. Barker

Alhamzawi AO, Alonso J, Angermeyer M, Benjet C, glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1) and infant cortisol
Bromet E, Chatterji S, de Girolamo G, Demyttenaere K, stress responses. Epigenetics 3, 97106.
Fayyad J, Florescu S, Gal G, Gureje O, Haro JM, Hu C-Y, OPCS (1991). Standard Occupational Classication. Volume 3.
Karam EG, Kawakami N, Lee S, Lepine J-P, Ormel J, Oce of Population Census and Surveys. HMSO : London.
Posada-Villa J, Sagar R, Tsang A, Ustun TB, Vassilev S, Parry G (1986). Paid employment, life events, social support,
Viana MC, Williams DR (2010). Childhood adversities and mental health in working-class mothers. Journal of
and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health and Social Behavior 27, 193208.
Health Surveys. British Journal of Psychiatry 197, 378385. Pawlby S, Hay D, Sharp D, Waters CS, Pariante CM (2011).
Kohen DE, Leventhal T, Dahinten SV, McIntosh CN (2008). Antenatal depression and ospring psychopathology :
Neighborhood disadvantage : pathways of eects for the inuence of childhood maltreatment. British Journal of
young children. Child Development 79, 156169. Psychiatry 199, 106112.
Miller GE, Chen E, Fok AK, Walker H, Lim A, Nicholls EF, Rutter M, Quinton D (1984). Parental psychiatric disorder :
Cole S, Kobor MS (2009). Low early-life social class leaves eects on children. Psychological Medicine 14, 853880.
a biological residue manifested by decreased Satorra A (2000). Scaled and adjusted restricted tests in
glucocorticoid and increased proinammatory signaling. multi-sample analysis of moment structures. In Innovations
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106, in Multivariate Statistical Analysis (ed. R. D. H. Heijmans,
1471614721. D. S. G. Pollock and A. Satorra), pp. 233247. Kluwer
Murray L, Carothers A (1990). The validation of the Academic Publishers : London.
Edinburgh Post-natal Depression Scale on a community Shonko JP, Boyce WT, McEwen BS (2009). Neuroscience,
sample. British Journal of Psychiatry 157, 288290. molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health
Muthen B (2004). Latent variable analysis : growth mixture disparities : building a new framework for health
modeling and related techniques for longitudinal data. In promotion and disease prevention. Journal of the American
Handbook of Quantitative Methodology for the Social Sciences Medical Association 301, 22522259.
(ed. D. Kaplan), pp. 345368. Sage Publications : Newbury Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS (2001). Using Multivariate
Park, CA. Statistics. Allyn & Bacon : Boston, MA.
Muthen LK, Muthen BO (19982010). Mplus. Statistical Weinstock M (2008). The long-term behavioural
Analyses with Latent Variables. Users Guide. Muthen & consequences of prenatal stress. Neuroscience and
Muthen : Los Angeles, CA. Biobehavioral Reviews 32, 10731086.
Nagin DS (2005). Group-Based Modeling of Development Weissman MM, Pilowsky DJ, Wickramaratne PJ, Talati A,
Over the Life Course. Harvard University Press : Wisniewski SR, Fava M, Hughes CW, Garber J, Malloy E,
Cambridge, MA. King CA, Cerda G, Sood AB, Alpert JE, Trivedi MH,
Oberlander TF, Weinberg J, Papsdorf M, Grunau R, Rush AJ ; STAR*D-Child Team (2006). Remissions in
Misri S, Devlin AM (2008). Prenatal exposure to maternal depression and child psychopathology. Journal of
maternal depression, neonatal methylation of human the American Medical Association 295, 13891398.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without
permission.

You might also like