You are on page 1of 9

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27 (2004) 693701

www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev

Review

LSD, 5-HT (serotonin), and the evolution of a behavioral assay


James B. Appela,*, William B. Westa, James Buggyb
a
Behavioral Pharmacology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, 1512 Pendleton Street, Columbia, SC 29208, USA
b
Department of Pharmacology and Physiology, School of Medicine, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC 29208, USA

Abstract
Research in our laboratory, supported by NIDA and facilitated by Roger Brown, has indicated that serotonergic neuronal systems are
involved in the discriminative stimulus effects of LSD. However, the only compounds that fully antagonize the LSD cue act at both serotonin
(5-HT) and dopamine (DA) receptors. In addition, substitution for LSD in standard drug vs. no-drug (DND) discriminations does not
necessarily predict either similar mechanisms of action or hallucinogenic potency because false positives occur when animals are given
drugs such as lisuride (LHM), quipazine, or, possibly, yohimbine. These effects can be greatly reduced by using drug vs. drug (D D), drug
vs. drug vs. no drug (D ND), or drug vs. other drug (saline, cocaine, pentobarbital) training procedures. Additional studies, in which drugs
were administered directly into the cerebral ventricles or specific brain areas, suggest that structures containing terminal fields of serotonergic
neurons might be involved in the stimulus effects of LSD.
q 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: LSD; Serotonin (5-HT); Lisuride (LHM); Drug discrimination; Rats

Contents
1. Antagonism of the LSD cue . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 694
2. False positives . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 695
3. Neuroanatomical substrates of the LSD cue. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 697
4. Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 700
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 700

Not long before Roger Brown became Chief of the ment tests following such training indicated that: (1) the
Neuroscience Research Branch at NIDA, we argued that assay is very sensitive in the sense that doses as low as
drug discrimination is a reliable, robust, sensitive, and 0.0025 mg/kg of LSD can be discriminated from saline; (2)
selective behavioral assay, which has been used with all compounds known to cause hallucinations at relatively
considerable success to analyze the mechanism(s) of action low doses (in humans) and act primarily, if not exclusively,
underlying the subjective effects of hallucinogenic drugs through serotonergic mechanisms appear to have LSD-like
[1]. This argument was based on the results of many effects (in rats) and, (3) both drugs and physiological
experiments conducted in our own and other laboratories interventions that alter the functional activity of serotoner-
involving two-lever, drug vs. no-drug (D ND) or, in one gic neuronal systems can either mimic or, when given in
case, drug vs. drug (D D) discrimination procedures; that combination with LSD, antagonize the LSD cue [1]. (4)
is, situations in which animals were trained to press one Substances that act through other monoaminergic neuronal
lever after receiving a systemic (i.p.) injection of a training systems (e.g. dopaminergic systems) do not alter the
drug such as LSD and a second lever, after a control stimulus properties of LSD either when they are given
injection of saline vehicle (0.9% NaCl) or another alone or in combination with LSD.
psychoactive substance such as the LSD congener, lisuride Since 1983, we continued to use drug discrimination to
hydrogen maleate (LHM). Substitution (generalization), study the mechanisms of action of LSD and several other
combination (antagonism) and various kinds of pre-treat- abused substances including cocaine, MDA, MDMA, PCP,
and marihuana (D9-THC) with the support of NIDA
* Corresponding author. Tel.: 1-803-777-2685; fax: 1-803-777-9558. (USPHS Research Grants R01 DA02543 and R37
E-mail address: appel@sc.edu (J.B. Appel). DA02543) and the active encouragement and assistance of
0149-7634/$ - see front matter q 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.012
694 J.B. Appel et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27 (2004) 693701

Roger Brown. This is largely because of at least two major


problems, the first of which concerns the occurrence of
socalled false positives [2 4] and the second is that
putatively site-selective serotonin (5-HT) antagonists (5-
HT1, 5-HT2, 5-HT3) do not appear to block the LSD cue as
well as substances that act at both 5-HT and dopamine (DA)
receptor sites [5 7]. We will discuss the antagonism
problem first because the methods used to study it are
probably more familiar in that they involved standard, two
lever (D ND) discrimination training [8]. We will then
discuss some more complex procedures we have been using
to analyze the false positive problem and will conclude by
reviewing an experiment in which test drugs were injected
directly into the cerebral ventricles (i.c.v.t.) or specific areas
of the brain (i.c.) thought to contain relatively high
concentrations of serotonergic neurons.

1. Antagonism of the LSD cue

When a variety of 5-HT antagonists were given 1-h Fig. 2. Effects of six more recently developed 5-HT receptor antagonists
before the training dose of LSD to rats trained to given i.p. 1-h prior to LSD in rats trained to discriminate LSD (0.08 mg/kg,
discriminate LSD (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from saline, Fig. 1 i.p.) from saline.
shows that methiothepin blocked the stimulus effects of and dose dependently; metergoline, Ly 53857, and pizotifen
LSD cue completely (20% of control), according to criteria (BC 105), blocked the cue partially.
suggested by Appel et al. [1]. The structural congeners of The situation was complicated when, in an effort to
LSD, BOL and methysergide (UML) blocked the LSD cue determine the 5-HT receptor subtype most involved in the
partially (30% of control) as did another centrally acting stimulus effects of LSD, we gave site-selective antagonists
5-HT antagonist, cyproheptadine; the peripherally acting in combination with LSD to animals trained to discriminate
5-HT antagonist xylamidine had no effect at any dose tested. LSD from saline [7]. Fig. 3 shows that the putative 5-HT2
Even more impressive results were obtained with more antagonists SR 46349B and LY 237733 blocked the cue
recently synthesized 5-HT antagonists (Fig. 2), particularly only partially (40 50%) and that neither the 5-HT1
those developed and studied extensively at Janssen antagonist WAY 100635 nor the 5-HT3 antagonist MDL
Pharmaceutica in Belgium [5,6]. Both pirenpirone and
ritanserin blocked the LSD (0.08 mg/kg) cue completely

Fig. 3. Effects of putatively site-selective 5-HT receptor subtype


Fig. 1. Effects of six putative 5-HT antagonists given i.p. 1-h prior to LSD antagonists given i.p. 1-h prior to LSD in rats trained to discriminate
in rats trained to discriminate LSD (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from saline. LSD (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from saline.
J.B. Appel et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27 (2004) 693701 695

Fig. 4. Effects of four DA antagonists given i.p. 1 h prior to LSD in rats


trained to discriminate LSD (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from saline.

72222 had any effect at all. Assuming that sufficient


amounts of these compounds reached the brain following
their systemic (i.p.) administration, the data do not support
Fig. 5. False positives. Three drugs, which are not hallucinogenic (in
the idea that activity at any of these receptor sites is humans) substitute completely (lisuride, quipazine) or partially (yohim-
critically or uniquely involved in the LSD cue; however, bine) for LSD in rats trained to discriminate LSD (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from
they do reinforce the hypothesis that activity at 5-HT2 is saline.
more important than activity at either 5-HT1 or 5-HT3
receptor subtypes [8,9]. them in a two-lever, D D (LSD LHM) situation [11 13].
While it is certainly possible that other monoaminergic However, systematic manipulation of agonists and antag-
systems in addition to 5-HT play a role in the behavioral and onists in both LSD- and LHM-trained animals is time-
subjective effects of LSD, it is clear from Fig. 3 that, in consuming and the two-lever D D procedure has some
contrast to compounds that have affinity for both 5-HT and important disadvantages: (1) it requires a prolonged training
DA receptors (Fig. 2), drugs that act primarily by blocking period during which active substances must be injected
DA receptors do not alter the LSD cue (Fig. 4). almost every day and, (2) it can result in relatively flat
generalization gradients when dissimilar drugs are tested
[14,15]. We therefore trained a single group of animals to
discriminate both LSD (0.08 mg/kg) and LHM (0.04 mg/
2. False positives
kg) from saline in a three-lever, D D ND procedure,
which, because it combines D ND and D D procedures,
The problem of false positives arises when drugs, might have some of the advantages of both [16,17].
which are not known to be hallucinogenic or may not act Fig. 7 shows the results of dose-response tests with the
primarily through serotonergic mechanisms, substitute for two training drugs. When animals were given LSD
LSD (Fig. 5). Such effects are important because they cast (left panel), they responded in a dose-related fashion on
doubt on the hypotheses that substitution for LSD predicts
hallucinogenic potency in humans, similarity in neuronal or
receptor mechanisms, or both [1,4,10]. We therefore
devoted a considerable amount of time and effort to
comparing the stimulus effects of LSD to those of perhaps
the most interesting false positive, the LSD congener
lisuride (LHM; Fig. 6).
In a series of experiments conducted by F. J. White in our
laboratory, it was possible to demonstrate that LSD and
LHM could be differentiated either by extensive treatment
with both 5-HT and DA agonists and antagonists in standard
D ND situations (LSD-saline and LHM-saline discrimi- Fig. 6. Structures of LSD and its non-hallucinogenic congener, lisuride
nations) or by training animals to discriminate between hydrogen maleate (LHM).
696 J.B. Appel et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27 (2004) 693701

(Fig. 8); very little responding occurred on the LSD lever.


Similar tests with quipazine, a 5-HT agonist, engendered
responding on the LSD-, but not the LHM-appropriate lever;
saline-appropriate responding occurred following injections
of pentylene tetrazol (PTZ), a substance that has little effect
at either DA or 5-HT receptors.
When given in combination with LSD, the 5-HT
antagonist pizotifen (BC105) reduced the amount of
responding on the LSD-appropriate lever (Fig. 9). Interest-
ingly, as this antagonism occurred, the percent of alternative
choices was divided between the LHM and the saline levers.
Pizotifen had no effect on the LHM cue (data not shown).
Pirenpirone proved to be a more effective antagonist of LSD
than pizotifen (Fig. 9) in that this substance not only dose-
dependently reduced, but also completely eliminated
responding on the LSD-appropriate lever at the highest dose
tested (1 mg/kg). Pirenpirone also antagonized the LHM cue
(when given in combination with LHM), but did so only
partially and at doses 10 times higher than doses that
Fig. 7. Results of dose-response tests with LSD, lisuride (LHM) and saline completely blocked the LSD cue (Fig. 10). The DA antagonist
in rats trained to discriminate LSD (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from LHM haloperidol blocked the LHM, but had no effect on the LSD
(0.04 mg/kg, i.p.) and saline in a three-lever (D D ND) drug discrimi- cue (Fig. 10).
nation task. (Reprinted from [16, p. 15], with the permission of Springer-
Verlag).
Thus, the results of studies involving both two-lever D
D and three-lever D D ND discrimination procedures
the LSD-appropriate lever; interestingly, most mistakes suggest that, while the effects of LSD and LHM may be
occurred on the saline-appropriate lever, suggesting that similar in some D ND situations, the mechanisms of action
the stimulus effects of LSD are more similar to those of of these compounds are, in fact, quite different; LSD acts
saline (ND) than they are to those of LHM. When the same primarily as an agonist at 5-HT receptors while LHM acts
animals were tested with LHM (right panel), they primarily at DA receptors [18].
responded on the LHM-appropriate lever in a dose-related More recently, we used another two-lever training
manner; once again, mistakes occurred predominantly on situation in an attempt to eliminate or reduce the incidence
the saline-appropriate lever. of false positives; namely, a drug-other (D O) procedure
During substitution tests with the DA agonist apomor- first described by Overton [19]. In this experiment, animals
phine, responding occurred on the LHM-appropriate lever were trained to discriminate LSD from a group of other

Fig. 8. Results of substitution tests with apomorphine, quipazine and pentelene tetrazol (PTZ) in rats trained to discriminate LSD (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from LHM
(0.04 mg/kg, i.p.) and saline in a three-lever (D D ND) drug discrimination task. (Reprinted from [16, p.15], with the permission of Springer-Verlag).
J.B. Appel et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27 (2004) 693701 697

Fig. 9. Results of combination tests with Pizotifen (BC 105) and


pirenpirone given i.p. 1-h prior to LSD, in rats trained to discriminate
LSD (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from LHM (0.04 mg/kg, i.p.) and saline in a three-
lever (DD ND) drug discrimination task. (Reprinted from [16, p.16],
with the permission of Springer-Verlag).

substances, which consisted of cocaine (10 mg/kg), pento-


barbital (5 mg/kg) or saline [2]. Dose-response or substi- Fig. 11. (Top) Acquisition of discrimination between LSD (0.08 mg/kg,
tution tests with various hallucinogens or putative false i.p.) and saline (0.9% NaCl) using a standard drug-no drug (D ND)
positives were then given to both D O and D ND (control) training procedure. Criterion (at least 80% of the responses occurring
before the first FR was completed for seven consecutive sessions) was
trained animals.
attained in 26 days. (Bottom) Acquisition of discrimination between LSD
Unlike the three-lever procedure, which required an (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) and a set of other compounds (D-Other) consisting of
average of 72 days to learn to criterion, Fig. 11 shows that saline, cocaine (10 mg/kg, i.p.) and pentobarbital (5 mg/kg). Criterion
the D O discrimination was acquired rapidly (34 days) (above) was attained in 31 days. (Reprinted from [2, p. 354], with the
compared with controls (31 days). permission of Elsevier Scientific Publishers).

As we expected, LSD and the indole-amine hallucino-


gens DOM and DMT substituted for the training drug
(0.08 mg/kg of LSD) in a dose-dependent manner in both
D O and D ND trained animals (Fig. 12). However, the
non-hallucinogenic false positives LHM, quipazine and
yohimbine, which mimicked LSD at least partially when
tested following standard DND training, engendered
saline-appropriate responding following D O training
(Fig. 13).
Thus, the two-lever D O procedure seems to be as
selective as, and more efficient than the D D ND, three-
lever procedure.

3. Neuroanatomical substrates of the LSD cue

The final variation on the drug discrimination procedure


I will discuss involved the administration of drugs into the
cerebral ventricles or various regions of the brain that have
Fig. 10. Results of combination tests with haloperidol (Haldol) and been postulated to play a role in the behavioral effects of
pirenpirone given i.p. 1-h prior to LSD, in rats trained to discriminate LSD
(0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from LHM (0.04 mg/kg, i.p.) and saline in a three-lever
LSD. In these experiments, which were conducted in
(D D ND) drug discrimination task. (Reprinted from [16, p. 17], with the collaboration with Dr James Buggy (University of South
permission of Springer-Verlag). Carolina School of Medicine), W. B. West, and others, rats
698 J.B. Appel et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27 (2004) 693701

Fig. 12. Results of substitution tests with three indole-amine hallucinogens (LSD, DOM and DMT) in rats trained to discriminate LSD (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from
either saline (left panels) or a group of other drugs (saline, cocaine and pentobarbital; right panels). (Reprinted from [2, p. 355], with the permission of Elsevier
Scientific Publishers).

trained to discriminate LSD from saline were implanted instance LHM, delivered over a 1 h period through these
with chronic, indwelling cannulae in either the third or cannulae, substituted for LSD (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) was then
lateral ventricles, or, bilaterally in the dorsal raphe, nucleus determined.
accumbens (NAc), or fimbria striatum. The extent to which Fig. 14 shows that injections of LSD directly into the
a series of intracerebroventricular (icvt) or intracebral (i.c.) third ventricle substituted completely (left panel), while
injections of small amounts of LSD (mg) or, in one injections into the lateral ventricle substituted partially for
systemic (i.p.) LSD.

Fig. 13. Results of substitution tests with three false positives (lisuride,
quipazine and yohimbine) in rats trained to discriminate LSD (0.08 mg/kg,
i.p.) from either saline (left panels) or a group of other drugs (saline, Fig. 14. Results of substitution tests with LSD injected through cannulae
cocaine and pentobarbital; right panels). (Reprinted from [2, p. 357], with implanted into the cerebral ventricles in rats trained to discriminate LSD
the permission of Elsevier Scientific Publishers). (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from saline.
J.B. Appel et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27 (2004) 693701 699

however, since neither substance substituted for systemi-


cally administered LSD completely, these results should be
interpreted with caution.
Since the doses of LSD or LHM administered i.c.v.t. or
i.c. were 100 1000 times less than those usually given
systemically, it is unlikely that that their effects were caused
by diffusion into the circulation or other tissue. However, it
is certainly possible that i.c.v.t. injections mimicked the
LSD cue most readily because substances administered in
this manner do tend to diffuse within the brain and therefore
stimulate a variety of structures in close proximity to actual
injection sites. Thus, injections of LSD into the third
ventricle might have affected the habenular nuclei, hippo-
campus, and para-ventricular nuclei, all of which receive
input from serotonergic terminals of neurons originating in
the dorsal raphe [20]. It is also interesting that the highest
concentration of 5-HT2C receptors, for which LSD has a
high affinity, is found in the non-neuronal choroid plexus of
the third ventricle [9]. Since structures adjacent to the lateral
ventricle, such as the NAc and caudate-putamen, are more
dopaminergic than serotonergic [21], it is not surprising that
Fig. 15. Results of substitution tests with LSD injected through cannulae
implanted into the dorsal raphe (left panels) or fimbria striatum (right LSD injected into this region substituted less than when
panels) in rats trained to discriminate LSD (0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from saline. injected into the third ventricle.
The dorsal raphe has been implicated as a possible site of
Direct administration of LSD into the dorsal raphe action of LSD for many years [22]. Both direct adminis-
substituted partially for i.p. LSD while injections into the tration of LSD and electrical stimulation of this region have
fimbria striatum induced responding primarily on the saline- been found to substitute for i.p. injections of LSD [23,24].
appropriate lever (Fig. 15). However, in the present experiment, LSD substituted only
The results of tests in which drugs were injected directly partially when injected into the raphe. In addition, it should
into the NAc were interesting in that LHM produced more be mentioned that i.p. LSD did not induce FOS expression
responding on the LSD lever than did LSD itself (Fig. 16); in this area but rather, caused intense FOS expression in
regions such as the cortex, hippocampus, and habenula,
which, as mentioned previously, contain projections from
neurons that originate in the dorsal raphe. Thus, the present
results support the hypothesis that 5-HT terminal fields are
more likely responsible for the physiological effects of LSD
than are 5-HT cell bodies [25].
Injections of LSD into the NAc, an area involved in the
reinforcing stimulus effects of many, if not most abused
drugs [26], did not substitute for i.p. LSD. While this may
appear to contradict previously reported findings [27], the
dose used in the earlier experiment (1.0 mg) was almost two
times higher than that used herein. Perhaps more impor-
tantly, the sites within the NAc differed in the two studies:
Nielsen & Scheel Kruger [27] used the NAc core and the
present study used the NAc shell, an area with significant
DA innervation [21]. Perhaps for this reason, i.c. LHM
produced more responding on the LSD-appropriate lever
responding than did i.c. LSD. It should be noted, however,
that LHM is about twice as potent as LSD, at least when
given systemically (i.p.).
The fimbria striatum is one of the few regions of the brain
that is reported to have higher concentrations of 5-HT2C
Fig. 16. Results of substitution tests with LSD or lisuride (LHM) injected
through cannulae implanted into the nucleus accumbens (NAc; left panels) than 5-HT2A receptors [20]. Since direct injections of LSD
or given systemically (i.p.; right panels) in rats trained to discriminate LSD into this area substituted only partially for i.p. LSD, it might
(0.08 mg/kg, i.p.) from saline. be argued that 5-HT2A is the primary receptor involved in
700 J.B. Appel et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27 (2004) 693701

the LSD cue and that 5-HT2C serves as a modulator [28]. assistance and hard work, none of the research reported
However, this hypothesis should be made with caution herein would have been conducted.
because 5-HT2C receptors in other areas of the brain could
play a critical role in the action of these drugs [9].
References

4. Conclusions [1] Appel JB, White FJ, Holohean AM. Analyzing mechanism(s) of
hallucinogenic drug action with drug discrimination procedures.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1982;6:52936.
Indole- and phenylethyl-amine hallucinogens substitute [2] Appel JB, West WB, Rolandi WG, Alici T, Pechersky K. Increasing
for LSD in two-lever DND procedures. Under similar the selectivity of drug discrimination procedures. Pharmacol Biochem
conditions, other false positives such as lisuride, quipazine Behav 1999;64:3538.
and yohimbine that are not known to be hallucinogenic (in [3] Colpaert FC, Janssen PAJ. Factors regulating drug cue sensitivity: the
humans) and, in some instances, may not act primarily at effects of dose ratio and absolute dose level in the case of fentanyl
dose-dose discrimination. Arch Internat Pharmacodyn Ther 1982;258:
5-HT receptor sites, also substitute for LSD. 283 99.
Substances such as ritanserin and pirenpirone that [4] Fiorella D, Rabin RA, Winter JC. Role of 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C
antagonize the stimulus effects of LSD most consistently receptors in the stimulus effects of hallucinogenic drugs II:
act at 5-HT, but also at other receptor sites. Among reassessment of LSD false positives. Psychopharmacol 1995;121:
putatively more site-selective drugs, 5-HT2 antagonists 357 63.
[5] Colpaert FC, Niemegeers CJE, Janssen PAJ. A drug discrimination
block the LSD cue to a greater extent than either 5-HT1 or analysis of lysergic acid diethylamide LSD: in vivo agonist and
5-HT3 antagonists but do not block it completely. Thus, it is antagonist effects of purported serotonin antagonists and of pirenper-
likely that neuronal or receptor systems in addition to 5-HT2 one, an LSD-antagonist. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1982;221:20614.
may be involved in the complex, in vivo effects of LSD. [6] Colpaert FC, Meert TF, Niemegeers CJE, Janssen PAJ. Behavioral
Since D D, D D ND, and D O procedures appear to and 5-HT antagonist effects of ritanserin: a pure and selective
antagonist of LSD discrimination in rat. Psychopharmacol 1985;86:
reduce or eliminate false positives such as lisuride, it
45 54.
might be profitable to use them in future studies of the [7] West WB, Millus DM, Buggy J, Appel JB. Effects of 5-HT2 and DA
neuronal and receptor mechanisms underlying the stimulus antagonism on the stimulus properties of LSD. Soc Neurosci Abstr
and other subjective effects of drugs such as LSD. 1995;21:1130.
The results of studies involving the administration of [8] Cunningham KA, Appel JB. Neuropharmacological reassessment of
the discriminative stimulus properties of dlysergic acid diethylamide
drugs directly into the brain must be considered preliminary
LSD. Psychopharmacol 1987;91:6773.
because so few areas have been explored and even fewer [9] Burris KD, Bredding M, Sanders-Bush E. ( ) Lysergic Acid
drugs have been studied; however, they do at least suggest Diethylamide, but not its non-hallucinogenic congeners, is a potent
that it might be profitable to examine structures that contain serotonin 5-HT1C receptor agonist. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1991;258:
terminal fields of serotonergic neurons (cortex, habenular 8916.
[10] Appel JB, Baker LE, Barrett RL, Broadbent J, Michael EM, Riddle
nuclei, hippocampus, and para-ventricular nuclei) in the
EE, Van Groll BJ. Use of drug discrimination in drug abuse research.
search for the neuronal substrates of in vivo effects of LSD. In: Glennon RA, Jarbe TUC, Frankenheim J, editors. Drug
Finally, it appears that at least one lifetime of research discrimination: applications to drug abuse research. NIDA Res
with LSD and 5-HT gives us little reason to question either Monogr, 16.; 1991. p. 369 98.
of the following conclusions that were made at the end of [11] White FJ, Appel JB. Training dose as a factor in LSD-saline
discrimination. Psychopharmacol 1982;76:205.
the last century: Serotonin gave birth to the field of
[12] White FJ, Appel JB. Lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) and lisuride:
neuroscience [29]; in spite of, or perhaps, because of the differentiation of their neuropharmacological actions. Science 1982;
fact that this substance is an enigma; it is at once 216:53537.
implicated in virtually everything but responsible for [13] White FJ, Appel JB. The role of dopamine and serotonin in the
nothing [30]. discriminative stimulus effects of lisuride. J Pharmacol Exp Ther
1982;221:4217.
[14] Jarbe TUC, Swedberg MDB. In: Colpaert FC, Slangen JL, editors. A
conceptualization of drug discrimination learning. Drug discrimi-
Acknowledgements nation: applications in CNS pharmacology, Amsterdam: Elsevier;
1982. p. 327 41.
[15] Swedberg MDB, Jarbe TUC. Drug discrimination procedures: roles of
Supported by USPHS Research Grants DA R01 02543 relative stimulus control in two-drug cases. Psychopharmacol 1985;
and R37 DA 02543, from the National Institute on Drug 86:44451.
Abuse. We thank the many graduate and undergraduate [16] Callahan PM, Appel JB. Differentiation between the stimulus effects
students who participated in the activities of the Behavioral of LSD and lisuride using a three choice drug discrimination
procedure. Psychopharmacology 1990;100:131 8.
Pharmacology Laboratory during its 30 years of existence:
[17] Swedberg MDB, Jarbe TUC. Drug discrimination procedures:
most especially, Donald M. Kuhn, Francis J. White, Kathryn differential characteristics of the drug A vs. drug B and the drug
A. Cunningham, Patrick M. Callahan, Barbara B. Simon, A vs. drug B vs. no drug cases. Psychopharmacol 1986;90:
Walter G. Rolandi, and Tevfic Alici. Without their 341 6.
J.B. Appel et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27 (2004) 693701 701

[18] White FJ. Comparative effects of LSD and lisuride, clues to specific [24] Minnema D, Krynock G, Young R, Glennon R, Rosecrans J. LSD as a
hallucinogenic drug actions. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1986;24: discriminative stimulus: role of dorsal raphe nucleus. Subst Alcohol
36579. Actions Misuse 1980;1:2934.
[19] Overton DA. Multiple drug training as a method for increasing the [25] Waeber C, Palacios JM. Binding sites for 5-hydroxytryptamine-2
specificity of the drug discrimination procedure. J Pharmacol Exp receptor agonists are predominantly located in striosomes in the
Ther 1982;221:166 72. human basal ganglia. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 1994;24:199 209.
[20] Palacios JM, Waeber C, Hoyer D, Mengod G. Distribution of [26] Wise RA, Bozarth MA. A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction.
serotonin receptors. Ann NY Acad Science 1990;600:36 52. Psychol Rev 1987;94:46972.
[21] Deutch AY, Cameron DS. Pharmacological characterization of [27] Nielsen EB, Scheel-Kruger J. Cueing effects of amphetamine and
dopamine systems in the nucleus accumbens core and shell. Neurosci LSD: elicitation by direct microinjection of the drugs into the nucleus
1992;46:4956. accumbens. Eur J Pharmacol 1986;125:8592.
[22] Haigler HJ, Aghajanian GK, Mescaline SD:. direct and indirect effects [28] Glennon RA, Titeler M, McKenney JD. Evidence for 5-HT2
on serotonin-containing neurons in brain. Eur J Pharmacol 1973;21: involvement in the mechanism of action of hallucinogenic agents.
5360. Life Sci 1984;35:250511.
[23] Hirschhorn ID, Hayes RL, Rosecrans JA. Discriminative control of [29] Whitaker-Azmitia PM. The discovery of serotonin and its role in
behavior by electrical stimulation of the dorsal raphe nucleus: neuroscience. Neuropsychopharmacol 1999;21:2S 8S.
generalization to lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD). Brain Res 1975; [30] Jacobs BL, Fornal CA. Activity of serotonergic neurons in behaving
86:1348. animals. Neuropsychopharmacol 1999;21:9S15S.

You might also like