You are on page 1of 10

Chronobiology Inremarronnl. Vol. 6. No. 1. pp. 93-102, 1989 0742-0528/89 $3.00 t 0.

00
Printed in Great Britain Pergarnon Press plc
1989 International Society of Chronobiology

THE DIM LIGHT MELATONIN ONSET AS A MARKER FOR


CIRCADIAN PHASE POSITION
Alfred J. Lewy and Robert L. Sack
Sleep and Mood Disorders Laboratory, Departments of Psychiatry, Ophthalmology and Pharmacology,
The Oregon Health Sciences University, Portland, OR 97201, U.S.A.
(Received f o r publicarion July 1988)
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of British Columbia on 10/29/14

Abstract-Masking is known to affect a variety of circadian rhythms, making it difficult to use them as reliable
markers of circadian phase position. Melatonin may be unique in that it appears to be masked only by (bright)light.
Sleep and activity do not appear to influence the melatonin rhythm. By measuring the onset ofmelatonin production,
a clearly demarcated event, we can reliably assess circadian phase position, provided blood is sampled under dim
light (the dim light melatonin onset, or DLMO). Ihe DLMO has been useful in assessing the phase-shifting
properties of bright light and in phase typing patients with chronobiologic disorders, such as winter depression.

Key words-Melatonin, circadian rhythms, masking, winter depression, bright light.

Considerable interest has developed in the use of production) and the appearance of melatonin in
human plasma melatonin as a marker for the circulation.
circadian rhythms. Animal and human studies In mammals, production of melatonin occurs
For personal use only.

have shown that circulating levels of melatonin in response to sympathetic stimulation of the
are exclusively derived from the pineal gland (1). pineal with the attendant release of norepine-
Thus, measurement of its plasma levels is an phrine from post-ganglionic neurons and the
accurate assessment of pineal production of action of this neurotransmitter on beta-l-adren-
melatonin in humans. In both diurnal and ergic receptors located on the pinealocytes. The
nocturnal animals, the shape of the melatonin source of this sympathetic stimulation is the
secretory curve has certain similarities: pineal suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) of the hypothal-
production of melatonin occurs mostly at night. amus, which then generates the circadian rhythm
This is unique, since [with the possible exception of melatonin production and perhaps most, if
of CSF vasopressin (2)] the phase relationship of not all, other circadian rhythms as well. The
all other circadian rhythms is conserved with SCN, in turn, is synchronized to the light-dark
respect to the activity-rest cycle and not the cycle through photic input transmitted by the
light-dark cycle. retinohypothalamic tract.
In most species, melatonin levels are low Like other circadian rhythms, the melatonin
during day, during which there is little secretion, rhythm free-runs under conditions of constant
pulsatile or otherwise. Sometime after dusk, darkness with a period slightly different from 24
melatonin levels abruptly rise (the melatonin hr. We have shown that most, if not all,
onset), following which, active secretion is totally blind individuals who have been studied
maintained until before dawn. Thus the shape of free-run with intrinsic periods slightly greater
this curve resembles a square wave more than a than 24 hr (3,4). Unlike other circadian rhythms,
sine wave. What may vary among species-and the melatonin rhythm does not seem to be
perhaps among individuals-is the lag time directly affected by the activity-rest cycle or by
between when the pineal begins its nightly de sleep, activity or stress [reviewed by Lewy (5)].
novo synthesis of mRNA and N-acetyltrans- The shape of the melatonin curve is similar in
ferase (the rate-limiting step in melatonin blind and sighted people. In blind individuals

Correspondence should be addressed to Dr A. J. Lewy, L469, Oregon Health Sciences University, Portland, OR 9720 1, U.S.A.
93
94 Chronobiology International

there are the same active and basal phases of also pointed out that Czeislers demonstration
about 12-hr duration as in sighted individuals; (7) of superior entraining effects of absolute
active secretion stops after about 10 hr, even in dark-light cycles was confounded by the behav-
the complete absence of light perception. There- ioral effects of total darkness. Czeisler (11)
fore, the light-dark cycle is not necessary to agreed with this assessment, at least in so far as
either turn on or turn off melatonin production: the sleep-wake cycle was not held constant in his
the rhythm is self-sustaining and endogenous. earlier study (7): the effect of the light-dark cycle
Like other circadian rhythms, animal studies was not separated from the effect of the
have shown that the melatonin rhythm appears sleep-wake cycle. Czeisler (1 1) has also criticized
to be regulated by the light-dark cycle in a way the Wever study (9) for the same reason. Both the
that can be described by a phase response curve Czeisler and the Wever studies studied the
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of British Columbia on 10/29/14

(PRC) (6). Unique to melatonin production, activity-rest and temperature rhythms, which are
light also has an acute suppressant effect: vulnerable to masking influences.
exposure to light during the night immediately In 1983 we reported successful treatment of
and profoundly suppresses melatonin produc- delayed sleep phase syndrome using bright light
tion. Until recently, human chronobiologists scheduled in the morning (12). In 1984, we
[reviewed by Czeisler (711 and pineal physio- published the first data showing phase-shifting
logists [reviewed by Lewy (8)] had come to the effects of light holding the sleep-wake cycle
conclusion that humans were relatively unaffec- constant (13, 14). Four normal volunteer sub-
ted by light. For example, human melatonin jects slept between 2300-0600 for I5 days. For
suppression was thought not to be suppressible the first day of study, the natural (bright) light-
by light, despite the fact that other animal species dark cycle was such that dusk was between
respond in this way to acute light exposure
For personal use only.

1930 and 2100 and dawn was between 0600


during the night. and 0730. Then for 1 week subjects avoided
This thinking changed dramatically following bright light (and sunlight) after 1600; thus, dusk
our report in 1980 that human nighttime was advanced to 1600. For another week, dusk
melatonin production could be suppressed by was held at 1600 and dawn was delayed to 0900,
light, providing it was sufficiently bright (8). i.e. subjects avoided bright light (and sunlight)
Light of 2500 lux almost completely suppressed between 1600-0900. The first day of advanced
melatonin production, whereas ordinary room dusk the onset of melatonin production ad-
light (500 lux) was not very effective. Apparent- vanced 1.5 hr. It remained at this phase position
ly, previous studies used insufficiently intense for 1-2 days and advanced one more hour by the
light [reviewed by Lewy (8)]. This finding end of the week, as did the offset. During the first
suggested that humans had biological rhythms day of delayed dawn there was no change in the
that were cued to the natural (bright) light-dark melatonin rhythm. However, by the end of the
cycle, unperturbed by the use of ordinary- week there was a delay of 1 hr in both the onset
intensity indoor light. This finding further and the offset. These findings are the first data
suggested that bright artificial light might be demonstrating phase shifts of an endogenous
used to experimentally, and perhaps thera- circadian rhythm following shifts of the bright
peutically, manipulate biological rhythms in light-dark cycle (holding the sleep-wake cycle
humans. constant), effects consistent with the common
In 1983, Wever (9) demonstrated that he was features of PRCs described for most species.
able to increase the range of entrainment of We have also interpreted these findings as
human activity-rest and temperature circadian consistent with the clock-gate model that we
rhythms by using 4000 lux light; Eastman (10) have proposed to explain how the suppressant
later confirmed this finding. Wever (9) pointed and entrainment effects of light combine to
out that the previous thinking that light was far produce the shape of the melatonin secretory
inferior to social cues was probably in error, due curve (5). According to this model, in animals
to the use of insufficiently intense light. Wever whose intrinsic periods are > 24 hr, dawn should
Dim Light Melatonin Onset 95

be relatively more important than dusk for the onset and one that is cued primarily to dawn
entrainment, and dusk should be relatively more which controls the offset. While her data are
important than dawn for inelatonin suppression. impressive and her logic is compelling, we
(The opposite should be true for animals whose disagree with her main conclusion, particularly
intrinsic periods are < 24 hr.) For humans, as it might apply to humans.
whose active phase of melatonin production is First of all, melatonin production appears to
never > 10-12 hr in duration, the suppressant be regulated somewhat differently in primates
effect of light should not occur in photoperiods than in rodents [reviewed by Illnerova (16)]. In
shorter than 12 hr in duration. The clock-gate rats, a 1-min pulse of light in the first half of the
model and our data further suggest that bright night suppresses melatonin production for sev-
light can not only suppress active melatonin eral hours. In primates, melatonin production
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of British Columbia on 10/29/14

production once it has begun but also that bright resumes shortly after the light pulse ceases (8,
light can suppress the onset of melatonin 17). In rats, a 1-min light pulse in the second half
production. Consequently, when using mela- of the night stops melatonin production for the
tonin levels to mark circadian phase position, remainder of the night, but this is not the case in
not only should bright light be avoided through- primates (8, 17). Therefore, it would not be
out the night but it should be avoided after 1700 surprising if human melatonin production was
or 1800 so as not to suppress the signal that regulated by one oscillator, even if it were
initiates melatonin production in the evening. established that rodent melatonin production
Melatonin levels can be used in a variety of was regulated by two oscillators.
ways for determining circadian phase position. Indeed, Illnerova first proposed the two-
Because of its square wave pattern, cosinor oscillator model to explain dynamics of suppres-
For personal use only.

analysis is not appropriate. More appropriate sion of melatonin production in the rat described
ways to determine phase position from the above (16). She originally stated that there need
melatonin pattern would be to use the onset, not be a separate suppressant (as opposed to
peak or offset. Like a train of constant car- entrainment) effect of light, hypothesizing that
length, any point of the curve is suitable for the delay in the resumption of melatonin
determining phase position, assuming that the production in the first half of the night was due to
duration of melatonin is constant (in dim light an immediate phase delay in the onset oscillator
conditions). The onset is preferable for several and that the cessation of melatonin in the second
reasons. Once nighttime melatonin production half of the night was due to an immediate phase
begins, beta-adrenergic receptors in the pineal advance in the offset oscillator to a time at or
become subsensitive (15). Furthermore, during before the light pulse (16).
the course of the night, melatonin precursors can On the other hand, our human data (described
become depleted. Both of these phenomena can above) suggest that there is a suppressant effect
account for decreasing melatonin levels during of light that can be distinguished from an
the night for reasons other than those related to entrainment effect. The immediate advance in
the timing of melatonin production. Of all of the the onset the first night of advanced dusk
portions of the melatonin curve, the onset is suggests removal of a suppressant effect of
theoretically least affected by the development of evening light. (There was no immediate delay in
beta-adrenergic subsensitivity and melatonin the onset or the offset on the first day of delayed
precursor depletion that might develop during dawn to suggest removal of a suppressant effect
the night. of light.) The changes in the onset and offset that
There is some disagreement over whether or occurred with several days of advancing dusk and
not one or two oscillators govern the onset and delaying dawn, were most likely due to the
offset of melatonin production. Illnerova (16) entrainment effect of light, given the expectation
has argued that her rodent light-pulse experi- of transients over the course of phase-resetting.
ments can be best explained by two oscillators, Animal data from naturalistic studies or
one that is cued primarily to dusk which controls experiments in which photoperiod is mani-
96 Chronobiology International

pulated are compatible with either a one- for seasonal changes in the pattern that varies
oscillator or two-oscillator model. Depending on according to the photoperiod, blood should be
ones assumptions, data from all experiments drawn under naturalistic conditions.
performed to date can be explained by either a In humans, there are no melatonin data that
two-oscillator model or a one-oscillator model cannot be explained by a one-oscillator model.
and a suppressant effect of light. Notably, There is another one (18) versus two (19, 20)
Illnerova has modified her one-oscillator model pacemaker controversy, however. This contro-
to include a suppressant effect of light under versy, different from the one described above for
extremely long photoperiods; she has also the melatonin onset and offset, is over whether or
acknowledged that morning light appears to not there are two endogenous pacemakers for
predominate over evening light in entrainment of human circadian rhythms, the activity-rest cycle
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of British Columbia on 10/29/14

both oscillators (17). having a separate endogenous pacemaker from


We originally proposed the clock-gate model the other endogenous circadian rhythms, such as
to explain the seasonal onset-offset phase temperature and melatonin. In our opinion,
paradox (5), which is that the onset and offset there may be untold numbers of oscillators,
manifest phase changes in opposite directions perhaps a hierarchical system of oscillators for
throughout the year: the onset occurs later in the each overt circadian rhythm, which are driven by
summer than in the winter, while the offset slave oscillators entrained to environmental time
occurs earlier in the summer than in the winter. cues by one or more endogenous circadian
This, of course, results in the changing duration pacemakers. Endogenous pacemakers, unlike
of melatonin production during the year which is slave oscillators, must necessarily be responsive
important in the timing of many seasonal to an environmental zeitgeber as described by a
For personal use only.

rhythms in animals. The question arises, how- phase response curve in order to accomplish
ever, that opposite changes in phase cannot phase control as well as entrainment.
easily be accommodated by one pacemaker. We Our human studies of melatonin suggest that
have explained this paradox by proposing the there is only one zeitgeber-light-for which
clock-gate model (5): the apparently later onset evidence for a phase response curve has been
of melatonin production in the summer in demonstrated. In addition to Wevers demon-
animals whose intrinsic periods are > 24 hr is the stration (9) of bright lights ability to increase the
result of the gating effect of a photoperiod range of entrainment of the activity-rest and
> 12 hr. Alternatively, Illnerovas two-oscillator temperature rhythms [followed by Eastman
model can also explain this paradox (16). (lo)], we and others have shown phase-shifting
Perhaps there are two oscillators and a suppres- effects of light holding the sleep-wake cycle
sant effect of light. Resolution of these questions constant (4, 5 , 11, 21). Furthermore, more
will probably not occur until direct observation classical phase response curves are preliminarily
of the underlying oscillators can be accom- being demonstrated by Honma (22). A phase
plished. response curve in humans has not yet been
If there is more than one oscillator for demonstrated for any other zeitgeber. This
melatonin production, we may only be able to suggests that there is only one zeitgeber and only
assume that the melatonin onset is marking the one endogenous pacemaker. Indeed, Czeisler
phase of the evening oscillator. Nonetheless, (one of the main proponents of the two-
these oscillators seem to be tightly coupled. It pacemaker model) has recently (1 1) retracted his
also makes sense to study the melatonin rhythm and Kronauers model (20) in which the
under dim light conditions, if only to eliminate temperature (melatonin) pacemaker was en-
the masking effects of the natural photoperiod. trained to environmental zeitgebers mainly
In other words, to use melatonin levels as a through the activity-rest pacemaker.
marker for circadian phase position, blood We have suggested ( 5 ) that the apparent
should be drawn under dim light conditions after entrainment of the human temperature rhythm
1700-1800. T o use melatonin levels as a marker by sleep is an indirect effect, by which sleep (with
Dim Light Melatonin Onset 97
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of British Columbia on 10/29/14

Figure 1. Sleep and melatonin rhythms in a totally blind subject. Living at home, this
subject had a I-day laboratory determination of his free-running melatonin onset (m)
carried out every few weeks for several months. The data are plotted on a repeating
time scale. Sleep times were more or less entrained to social cues (dark bars). The
melatonin onsets fall reliably ( r = 0.99, P = 0.001) on the fitted regression line n o
matter when they occur in relation to sleep time, suggesting that sleep does not affect
the melatonin circadian rhythm.

the closing of the eyes) imposes its structure upon When using melatonin as a marker for
the (bright) light-dark cycle: the closed loop circadian phase position, we recommend the
For personal use only.

hypothesis. Sleep may be the one circadian melatonin onset. As mentioned above, it is as
rhythm that is directly affected by social cues. It good or better than any other part of the
appears to be more loosely coupled to the melatonin secretory curve for this particular use.
pacemaker than the other circadian rhythms that Furthermore, the melatonin onset is advan-
are not susceptible to social and psychological tageous for several other reasons. Subjects can be
influences. studied in the evening as outpatients. Sleep is not
Further evidence that there is not a separate disturbed. Blood drawing is minimized. High
endogenous pacemaker for the activity-rest resolution at frequent sampling intervals can be
cycle comes from studies of blind people. The conveniently obtained.
free-running melatonin rhythm of totally blind There is only one potential known masking
individuals (Figure 1) does not appear to be effect on melatonin: bright light. However, we do
influenced by social cues or sleep, as it continues not know to what extent dim light might also be
its inexorable free-run, cycle after cycle (Sack capable of causing masking (23, 24). For that
and Lewy, in preparation). This is further matter, we do not know the threshold of intensity
evidence for a single pacemaker that drives all for light to have entraining effects. Conse-
circadian rhythms. quently, we recommend that melatonin samples
As mentioned above, melatonin appears to be be obtained under the dimmest possible light.
less influenced by direct masking effects due to For the onset, dim light conditions should begin
sleep and wakefulness, compared to other no later than 12 hr before dawn (usually we begin
circadian phase markers, such as temperature or dim light conditions between 1700-1800). We
cortisol. We have measured cortisol in some of call this the dim light melatonin onset, or
the same totally blind subjects in whom mela- DLMO.
tonin was measured (Sack and Lewy, in prepar- The DLMO appears to be a useful marker for
ation). It is clear that sleep and wakefulness have circadian phase position. Comparing normals to
a profound affect on cortisol levels (Figure 2) patients has revealed some potential differences
and makes it less desirable than melatonin as a in the PRC that might not have been previously
phase marker. appreciated without this comparison. The bright
98 Chronobiology International

light suppression of melatonin finding suggested


that humans might have biological rhythms that
respond to the natural (bright) light-dark cycle
unperturbed by the use of ordinary-intensity
room light and that bright artificial light might
10
be used experimentally, and perhaps thera-
10
peutically, to manipulate these rhythms. Our
first test of these implications occurred in the
0
0 winter of 1980, when we successfully treated a
patient (with a recurrent winter depression which
spontaneously remitted each year in the spring)
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of British Columbia on 10/29/14

by exposing him to 2000 lux light between 0600


and 0900 and between 1600 and 1900 (25). After
4 days of this bright light exposure schedule, he
began to switch out of his depression, 2 months
before this was due to happen spontaneously.
Since then, hundreds of other such winter
depressive patients have been similarly studied
[we have reviewed the antidepressant effects of
bright light elsewhere (26)]. Dim light, an
appropriate placebo control treatment, has been
shown to be significantly less antidepressant (27,
For personal use only.

28).
Since then, we have been interested in the
phase-shifting effects of light and whether or not
winter depressive patients might have a circadian
rhythm disorder, rather than a disorder involv-
ing seasonal rhythms. (Of course, seasonal
I rhythms depend to some extent on the circadian
\ timing system.) Winter depressive patients, as
opposed to melancholic major depressives who
often have early morning awakening, have
difficulty getting up in the morning. Conse-
quently, whereas the melanchoIic depressed
patients had been previously hypothesized to be
phase advanced, we thought that winter depres-
sive patients were phase delayed.
We have found that these patients preferen-
tially respond to morning light compared to
evening light (21). In our first study(21), we also
HoLr (milllary time)
found that patients had significantly later
Figure 2. Free-running melatonin and cortisol rhythms in a DLMOs than normal controls, even after a week
totally blind subject. A totally blind subject was studied for of standardized sleep-wake and light-dark
24 hr on three different occasions. The data are double- conditions. Subsequently, we found that the
plotted. The three dates of hormonal determinations were
12/6/86, 12/19/86 and 1/2/87 (top to bottom). A line was decline in depression ratings correlated with the
fitted to the melatonin onsets (time when melatonin levels advance in the melatonin onset. We were also
reached 10 pg/ml). n7e melatonin circadian rhythm (as able to measure the amount of phase shifts after
indicated by the melatonin 0nset)appears to provide a clearer
estimate of circadian phase position compared to the cortisol 2 hr of morning light and after 2 hr of evening
circadian rhythm. light. If we had not studied these phase-delayed
Dim Light Melatonin Onset 99

patients, we might not have demonstrated a differential, which is the advance response in the
statistically significant phase advance after DLMO relative to the baseline DLMO minus the
morning light scheduled between 0600 and 0800. delay response relative to baseline. Theoreti-
This is because controls did not significantly cally, the greater the A-D differential, either the
advance; only the patients did. more delayed is the PRC or the greater is the area
In a subsequent study (Lewy et al., in under the advance portion compared to the delay
preparation), we compared 0.5 and 2 hr of portion, both of which possibilities are consistent
morning light. Two hours of morning light with phase-delayed circadian rhythms due to a
caused more of a phase advance than did 0.5 hr relatively longer intrinsic period.
(PGO.01). In this study, we found a good The melatonin onset seems to be marking the
correlation ( r = 0.95) between the decline in phase of its PRC. The phase delay response to
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of British Columbia on 10/29/14

depression ratings and the advance in the DLMO evening light is related to the baseline phase
(29). In a third study comparing morning versus position of the melatonin onset: the later the
evening light (Sack and Lewy, in preparation), melatonin onset, the less the phase delay effect of
we scheduled evening light 1 hr earlier than in evening light (Figure 3). The phase advance
our first morning versus evening light study and response to morning light is also related to the
found similar results. These studies are all baseline phase position of the melatonin onset:
compatible with a phase response curve showing the later the melatonin onset, the greater the
relatively less phase shifts during the middle of phase advance response to morning light (Figure
the day. 4). Furthermore, the later the melatonin onset,
There was a trend for onsets to be delayed in the greater the A-D differential (Figure 5 ) . These
this study a t baseline, which was significant when data suggest that the melatonin onset is marking
For personal use only.

combined with the first morning versus evening the phase of its PRC.
light study but was not significant when the 0.5 Although the A-D differential correlates very
versus 2 hr study was included in this pooling of well with the baseline DLMO, the A-D differen-
the data. In the first study and in a replication tial may be a more reliable method of phase
study, we found that patients advanced more in typing because it takes into account inter-
response to morning light and delayed less in individual differences in melatonin physiology,
response to evening light than did normal such as in the biochemical lag time between the
controls. This finding led us to propose the A-D onset of sympathetic stimulation of the pineal
and the appearance of melatonin in the circula-
tion, assuming of course that the intraindividual

r
D i m
3 -
.
.
5 5 2 -
2 2 -
L

0-
3 s -
a d 1-

2-
6 4 2 0 2 4 0

Time beiwccn GLMO and onsel of PM lighl (hours) 3 1 . , . , . , . , , , . . - ,


17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24
Figure 3. Baseline DLMO predicts the delay response to Ba5~111nc
mr?Istoriin o n i e l lime (hour of day)
evening light. Melatonin onsets are determined in both
normal controls (0)and winter depressive patients (m) aftera Figure 4. Baseline DLMO predicts the advance response to
week of baseline conditions and after a week of evening rnornin light. Melatonin onsets are determined in both
normafcontrols (0)and winter depressive patients (m) after a
bright light (1900 to 2100 or 2000 to 2200). The fitted
week of baseline conditions and after a week of morning
regression line showed a statistically significant (P<0.001) bright light (0600to 0800).The fitted regression line showed a
correlation (r = 0.86), indicating that the later the baseline statistically significant (P < 0,001) correlation (r = 0.78),
melatonin onset, the less the phase delay response to evening indicating that the later baseline melatonin onset,,the greater
light. This finding is consistent with the hypothesis that the the phase advance response to morning light. This finding I S
melatonin onset is marking the phase position of its phase consistent with the hypothesis that the melatonin onset is
response curve. marking the phase position of its phase response curve.
100 C h r o n o b i o l o g y International

$ 6 - sponses to light will become a useful marker for


n .
-
1 $ 4 - circadian phase position, for phase typing and
g
5c :
c
a
2:
:
for monitoring phase-shifting effects of light.
1 *1
This may be important, because of the masking
8 p 2- effects of sleep, wakefulness and stress on other
;
m g
? 4: - phase markers, such as temperature and cortisol.
9 6: The melatonin onset may also be useful
0 .
, , , , . , , , , , , , , , because of the controversy that surrounds the
17 18 19 20 21
22
Baseline melatoninonset lime (haw 01 day)
23 24 constant routine. Originally proposed by Mills
and co-workers (31) and then promulgated by
Figure 5. Baseline DLMO predicts the A-D differential. Czeisler and co-workers (32)9 the constant
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of British Columbia on 10/29/14

Melatonin onsets are determined in both normal controls (0)


and winter depressive patients )(. after a week of baseline
routine has been thought - by- some to eliminate
masking effects due to the sleepwake cycle. This
conditions, after a week of morning bright light and after a
week of evening bright light (see Figures 3 and 4). The A-D is not clear, since deprivation has been
differential is the advance response-to morning light minus reported by some of the same investigators to
the delay response to evening light. The fitted regression line phask shifts. ~ ~this procedure
~
showed a statistically significant ( P < 0.001) correlation
( r = 0.86). indicating that the later the baseline melatonin is cumbersome and may be associated with Some
onset, the greater -the A-D differential. This finding is stress. If the constant routine proves not to be
consistent with the hypothesis that the melatonin onset is
marking the phase position of its phase response curve. optimal for eliminating masking effects in the
estimation of circadian phase position, the
attractiveness of the DLMO increases. However,
variation in the lag time is small, which is a accurate and sensitive assays will be needed to
For personal use only.

reasonable assumption. Perhaps this is why use melatonin as a phase marker, particularly the
comparing patients to controls, the A-D differ- DLMO. DLMOs using salivary levels of mela-
ential yields a more significant difference tonin (which are about one-third those of
( P < 0.001) than the baseline DLMO ( P < 0.01). plasma) may have great applicability in the study
Furthermore, our one evening light responder of circadian rhythms at home, when travelling
had the most negative A-D differential of that and in the work-place. However, very sensitive
group of patients, but did not have the most assays, such as thegas chromatography-negative
phase advanced baseline DLMO. chemical ionization mass spectrometric assay
It is possible that a circadian rhythm in light (33), will be needed to determine daytime
sensitivity exists (30), but it is unlikely that a melatonin levels in saliva. Less sensitive assays
1-2 hr phase shift in this rhythm would signifi- (34) may have to use the entire nighttime
cantly increase interindividual variability in secretory curve to determine circadian phase
these phase shift responses. The A-D differential position or perhaps use some other (perhaps less
is also theoretically superior to the advance precise) technique to determine the melatonin
response to morning light alone [which also can onset, such as the half-maximum, which will also
reliably distinguish patients from controls require blood sampling during the middle of the
(PG O.OOl)] or to the delay response to evening night.
light alone, because the A-D differential takes In conclusion, measurement of plasma mela-
into account interindividual differences in light tonin levels appears to provide useful informa-
sensitivity per se. However, should it be shown tion about circadian phase position. The DLMO,
that there is no difference between certain patient which is obtained under dim light conditions,
populations and normals in light sensitivity (as appears to be free of significant masking effects,
assessed, for example, by the nighttime mela- has many advantages as a research tool and
tonin suppression test), then the morning light appears to be clinically useful as well. Our studies
advance response might be used as a simpler way of the baseline DLMO and its phase shift
to distinguish patients from normals. responses to morning arid to evening light have
Hopefully, the melatonin onset or its re- provided important data concerning the human
Dim Light Melatonin Onset 101

circadian pacemaker, chronobiologic disorders Simonton for their help. Supported by PHS grants
and bright light therapy. CRR00334, MH43016 (R.L.S.), MH00703 (A.J.L.) and
MH40161 (A.J.L.).
Acknowledgements-The authors thank G. Clark, M. Mc
Reynolds, M. Blood, D. Jennings, J. Peterson and C.

References
1. Neuwelt E. A. and Lewy A. J. Disappearance of plasma melatonin after removal of a neoplastic pineal gland. New Engl
J M e d 308, 1132-1135, 1983.
2. Reppert S. M., Artman H. G., Swaminathan S. and Fisher D. A. Vasopressin exhibits a rhythmic daily pattern in
cerebrospinal fluid but not in blood. Science 213, 1256-1257, 1981.
3. Lewy A. J. and Newsome D. A. Different types of melatonin circadian secretory rhythms in some blind subjects. J Clin
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of British Columbia on 10/29/14

Endocr Metab 56, 1103-1 107, 1983.


4. Sack R. L., Lewy A. J. a n d Hoban T . M. Free-running melatonin rhythm in blind people: phase shifts with melatonin
and triazolam administration. In: Rensing L., an der Heiden U. and Mackey M. C., eds. TemporalDisorder in Human
Oscillatory Systems. Springer, Heidelberg, pp. 219-224, 1987.
5. Lewy A. J. Biochemistry and regulation of mammalian melatonin production. In: Relkin R. M., ed. The Pineal Gland.
Elsevier/North-Holland, New York, pp. 77-128, 1983.
6. Binkley S., Muller G. and Hernandez T. Circadian rhythm in pineal N-acetyltransferase activity: phase-shifting by light
pulses (1). J Neurochem 73, 798-800, 1981.
7. Czeisler C. A,, Richardson G. S., Zimmerman J. C . , Moore-Ede M. C. and Weitzman E. D. Entrainment of human
circadian rhythms by light-dark cycles: a reassessment. Photochem Photobiol34, 239-247, 1981.
8. Lewy A. J., Wehr T. A., Goodwin F. K., Newsome D. A. and Markey S. P. Light suppresses melatonin secretion in
humans. Science 210, 1267-1269, 1980.
9. Wever R. A,, Polasek J. and Wildgruber C. M. Bright light affects human circadian rhythms. Pflugers Arch 396,85-87,
1983.
10. Eastman C. I. Bright light improves the entrainment of the circadian rhythmsof body temperature to a 26-hr sleep-wake
For personal use only.

schedule in humans. Sleep Res 15, 271, 1986.


11. Czeisler C. A,, Allan J . A,, Strogatz S. H., Ronda J. M., Sanchez R., Rios C. D., Freitag W. O., Richardson G. S. and
Kronauer R. E. Bright light resets the human circadian pacemaker independent of the timing of the steep-wake cycle.
Science 233, 667-671, 1986.
12. Lewy A. J., Sack R. L., Fredrickson R. H., Reaves M., Denney D. D. and Zielske D. R. The use of bright light in the
treatment of chronobiologic sleep and mood disorders: the phase-response curve. Psychopharmacol Bull 19,523-525,
1983.
13. Lewy A. J., Sack R. L. and Singer C. M. Assessment and treatment of chronobiologic disorders using plasma melatonin
levels and bright light exposure: the clock-gate model and the phase response curve. Psychopharmacol Bull 20,
561-565, 1984.
14. Lewy A. J., Sack R. L. and Singer C. M. Immediate and delayed effects of bright light o n human melatonin production:
shifting dawn and dusk shifts the dim light melatonin onset (DLMO). Ann NY Acad Sci 453, 253-259, 1985.
15. Romero J. A,, Zatz M., Kebabian J. W. and Axelrod J. Circadian cycles in binding of 3H-alprenolol to beta-adrenergic
receptor sites in rat pineal. Nature 258, 435-436, 1975.
16. Illnerova H. and Vanecek J. Two-oscillator structure of the pacemaker controlling the circadian rhythm of
N-acetyltransferase in the rat pineal gland. J Comp Physiol 145, 539-548, 1982.
17. Illnerova H. Circadian Rhythms in the Mammalian Pineal Gland. Academia Nakladatelstvi Akademie Ved, Praha, p. 70,
1986.
18. Daan s., Beersma D. G. M. and Borberly A. Timing of human sleep: recovery process gated by a circadian pacemaker.
Amer J Physiol246, R161-Rl78, 1984.
19. Wever R. A. The Circadian System of Man. Springer, New York, 1979.
20. Kronauer R. E., Czeisler C. A., Pilato S. F., Moore-Ede M. C. and Weitzman E. D. Mathematical model of the human
circadian system with two interacting oscillators. Amer J Physiol 242, R3-RI7, 1982.
21. Lewy A. J., Sack R. L., Miller L. S. and Hoban T. M. Antidepressant andcircadian phase-shiftingeffectsof light. Science
235, 352-354, 1987.
22. Honma K . , Honma S. and Wada T. Phasedependent shift of free-running human circadian rhythms in response to a
single bright light pulse. Experientia 43, 1205-1207, 1987.
23. Lewv A. J.. Numberger J. I . , Wehr T. A,, Pack D., Becker L. E., Powell R-L., Simmons-Alling S., Goodwin F. K. and
Gershon E. S. Supersensitivity to light: possible trait marker for manic-depressive illness. Amer J Psychrat 142,
725-727, 1985.
24. Bojkowski C. J., Aldhous M. E., English J., Franey C., Poulton A. L., Skene D. J. and Arendt J. Suppression of
nocturnal plasma melatonin and 6-sulphatoxymelatonin by bright and dim light in man. Horm Metabol Res 19,
437-440, 1987.
25. Lewy A. J . , Kern H. E., Rosenthal N. E. and Wehr T. A. Bright artificial light treatment of a manicdepressive patient
with a seasonal mood cycle. Amer J Psychiat 139, 1496-1498, 1982.
26. Lewy A. J. and Sack R. L. Minireview: light therapy and psychiatry. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 183, 11-18, 1986.
102 Chronobiology International

27. Rosenthal N. E., Sack D. A., Gillin J. C., Lewy A. J., Goodwin F. K., Davenport Y., Mueller P. S., Newsome D. A. and
Wehr T. A. Seasonal affective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiai 41, 72-80, 1984.
28. Rosenthal N. E., Sack D. A., Carpenter C. J., Parry B. L., Mendelson W. B. and Wehr T. A. Antidepressant effects of
light in seasonal affective disorder. Amer J Psychiar 142, 163-170, 1985.
29. Lewy A. J., Sack R. L., Singer C. M., White D. M. and Hoban T. M. Winter depression and the phase shift hypothesis
for bright lights therapeutic effects: history, theory and experimental evidence. J Biol Rhythms 3, 121-134, 1988.
30. Terman M. and Terman J. A circadian pacemaker for visual sensitivity? Ann N Y Acad Sci 453, 147-161, 1985.
31. Mills J. N., Minors D. S. and Waterhouse J. M. Adaptation to abrupt time shifts of the oscillator(s) controlling human
circadian rhythms. J Physio/ 285, 455-470, 1978.
32. Czeisler C. A., Brown E. M., Ronda J. M., Kronauer R. E., Richardson G. S. and Freitag W. 0. A clinical method to
assess the endogenous circadian phase (ECP) of the deep circadian oscillator in man. Sleep Res 15, 295, 1985.
33. Lewy A. J. and Markey S. P. Analysis of melatonin in human plasma by gas chromatography negative chemical
ionization mass spectrometry. Science 201, 741-743, 1978.
34. Nowak R., McMillen I. C., Redman J. and Short R. V. The correlation between serum and salivary melatonin
concentrations and urinary 6-hydroxymelatonin sulphate excretion rates: two non-invasive techniques for monitoring
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of British Columbia on 10/29/14

human circadian rhythmicity. Clin Endocrinol27, 445-452, 1987.


For personal use only.

You might also like