You are on page 1of 64

CHAPTER 1

Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia


MICHELLE G. CRASKE
DAVID H. BARLOW

The treatment protocol described in this chapter represents one of the success stories in
the development of empirically supported psychological treatments. Results from numer-
ous studies indicate that this approach provides substantial advantages over placebo
medication or alternative psychosocial approaches containing common factors, such as
positive expectancies and helpful therapeutic alliances. In addition, this treatment forms
an important part of every clinical practice guideline in either public health or other
sources from countries around the world, describing effective treatments for panic disor-
der and agoraphobia. Results from numerous studies evaluating this treatment protocol,
both individually and in combination with leading pharmacological approaches, suggest
that this approach is equally effective as the best pharmacological approaches in the short
term and more durable over the long term. But this treatment protocol has not stood still.
For example, we have learned a great deal in the past 5 years about neurobiological
mechanisms of action in fear reduction, and the best psychological methods for effecting
these changes. In this chapter we present the latest version of this protocol, incorporating
these changes and additions as illustrated in a comprehensive account of the treatment of
Julie.D. H. B.

Advances continue in the development of bio- of treatment outcome data, this chapter covers
psychosocial models and cognitive-behavioral recent theoretical and empirical developments
treatments for panic disorder and agoraphobia. in reference to etiological factors, the role
The conceptualization of panic disorder as an of comorbid diagnoses in treatment, ways of
acquired fear of certain bodily sensations, and optimizing learning during exposure therapy,
agoraphobia as a behavioral response to the and the effect of medication on cognitive-
anticipation of such bodily sensations or their behavioral treatments. The chapter concludes
crescendo into a full-blown panic attack, con- with a detailed, session-by-session outline of
tinues to be supported by experimental, clini- cognitive-behavioral treatment for panic disor-
cal, and longitudinal research. Furthermore, der with agoraphobia (PDA). This protocol has
the efficacy of cognitive-behavioral treatments been developed in our clinics; the full proto-
that target fear of bodily sensations and associ- col is detailed in available treatment manuals
ated agoraphobic situations is well established. (Barlow & Craske, 2006; Craske & Barlow,
In addition to presenting an up-to-date review 2006).

1
2 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

NATURE OF PANIC nomic activation reflects anticipatory anxiety


AND AGORAPHOBIA rather than true panic (Barlow et al., 1994), es-
pecially because more severe panics are more
Panic Attacks
consistently associated with accelerated heart
Panic attacks are discrete episodes of intense rate (Margraf et al., 1987). Another example of
dread or fear, accompanied by physical and discordance occurs when perceptions of threat
cognitive symptoms, as listed in the DSM-IV- or danger are refuted despite the report of in-
TR panic attack checklist (American Psychiat- tense fear. This has been termed noncog-
ric Association, 2000). Panic attacks are dis- nitive panic (Rachman, Lopatka, & Levitt,
crete by virtue of their sudden or abrupt onset 1988). Finally, the urgency to escape is some-
and brief duration, as opposed to gradual- times weakened by situational demands for
ly building anxious arousal. Panic attacks in continued approach and endurance, such
panic disorder often have an unexpected qual- as performance expectations or job demands,
ity, meaning that from the patients perspective, thus creating discordance between behavioral
they appear to happen without an obvious trig- responses on the one hand, and verbal or physi-
ger or at unexpected times. Indeed, the diagno- ological responses on the other.
sis of panic disorder is given in the case of re- A subset of individuals with panic disorder
current unexpected panic attacks, followed experience nocturnal panic attacks. Noctur-
by at least 1 month of persistent concern about nal panic refers to waking from sleep in a
their recurrence and their consequences, or by state of panic with symptoms that are very sim-
a significant change in behavior consequent to ilar to panic attacks during wakeful states
the attacks (American Psychiatric Association, (Craske & Barlow, 1989; Uhde, 1994). Noc-
1994). turnal panic does not refer to waking from
As with all basic emotions (Izard, 1992), sleep and panicking after a lapse of waking
panic attacks are associated with strong action time, or nighttime arousals induced by night-
tendencies; Most often, these are urges to es- mares or environmental stimuli (e.g., unex-
cape, and less often, urges to fight. These fight pected noises). Instead, nocturnal panic is an
and flight tendencies usually involve elevated abrupt waking from sleep in a state of panic,
autonomic nervous system arousal needed to without an obvious trigger. Nocturnal panic at-
support such fightflight reactivity. Further- tacks reportedly most often occur between 1
more, perceptions of imminent threat or dan- and 3 hours after sleep onset, and only occa-
ger, such as death, loss of control, or social sionally more than once per night (Craske &
ridicule, often accompany such fightflight re- Barlow, 1989). Surveys of select clinical groups
activity. However, the features of urgency to es- suggest that nocturnal panic is relatively com-
cape, autonomic arousal, and perception of mon among individuals with panic disorder:
threat are not present in every self-reported oc- 4471% report having experienced nocturnal
currence of panic. For example, despite evi- panic at least once, and 3045% report re-
dence for elevated heart rate or other indices of peated nocturnal panics (Craske & Barlow,
sympathetic nervous system activation during 1989; Krystal, Woods, Hill, & Charney, 1991;
panic attacks on average (e.g., Wilkinson et Mellman & Uhde, 1989; Roy-Byrne, Mellman,
al., 1998), Margraf, Taylor, Ehlers, Roth, and & Uhde, 1988; Uhde, 1994). Individuals who
Agras (1987) found that 40% of self-reported suffer frequent nocturnal panic often become
panic attacks were not associated with acceler- fearful of sleep and attempt to delay sleep on-
ated heart rate. Moreover, in general, patients set. Avoidance of sleep may result in chronic
with panic disorder are more likely than non- sleep deprivation, which in turn precipitates
anxious controls to report arrhythmic heart more nocturnal panics (Uhde, 1994).
rate in the absence of actual arrhythmias Nonclinical panic attacks occur occasion-
(Barsky, Clearly, Sarnie, & Ruskin, 1994). ally in approximately 35% of people in the
Heightened anxiety about signs of autonomic general population who do not otherwise meet
arousal may lead patients to perceive cardiac criteria for panic disorder (Norton, Cox, &
events when none exist (Barlow, Brown, & Malan, 1992). Also, panic attacks occur across
Craske, 1994; Craske & Tsao, 1999). We be- a variety of anxiety and mood disorders
lieve that self-reported panic in the absence of (Barlow et al., 1985), and are not limited to
heart rate acceleration or other indices of auto- panic disorder. As stated earlier, the defining
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 3

feature of panic disorder is not the presence of THERAPIST: What worries you most about oth-
panic attacks per se, but involves addition- ers noticing your physical symptoms?
al anxiety about the recurrence of panic or PATIENT: That they will think that I am weird
its consequences, or a significant behavioral or strange.
change because of the panic attacks. It is the
additional anxiety about panic combined with THERAPIST: Would you be anxious in the meet-
catastrophic cognitions in the face of panic that ings if the panic attacks were fully prevent-
differentiate between the person with panic dis- able?
order and the occasional nonclinical panicker PATIENT: I would still be worried about doing
(e.g., Telch, Lucas, & Nelson, 1989) or the per- or saying the wrong thing. It is not just the
son with other anxiety disorders who also hap- panic attacks that worry me.
pens to panic. The following scenario exempli- THERAPIST: Are you worried about panic at-
fies the latter point. tacks in any other situations?
PATIENT: Formal social events and sometimes
PATIENT: Sometimes I lay awake at night think- when I meet someone for the first time.
ing about a million different things. I think
about what is going to happen to my daugh-
ter if I get sick. Who will look after her, or In this case, even though the patient experi-
what would happen if my husband died and ences panic attacks, the real concern is about
we didnt have enough money to give my being judged negatively by others consequent
daughter a good education? Then I think to panic attacks, and the panic attacks do not
about where we would live and how we occur in situations other than social ones.
would cope. Sometimes I can work myself Hence, this presentation is most aptly de-
up so much that my heart starts to race, my scribed as social phobia.
hands get sweaty, and I feel dizzy and scared.
So I have to stop myself from thinking about Agoraphobia
all those things. I usually get out of bed and
turn on the TVanything to get my mind Agoraphobia refers to avoidance or endur-
off the worries. ance with dread of situations from which es-
THERAPIST: Do you worry about the feelings of cape might be difficult or help unavailable in
a racing heart, sweating, and dizziness hap- the event of a panic attack, or in the event of
pening again? developing symptoms that could be incapaci-
tating and embarrassing, such as loss of bowel
PATIENT: No. Theyre unpleasant, but they are control or vomiting. Typical agoraphobic sit-
the least of my concerns. I am more worried uations include shopping malls, waiting in
about my daughter and our future. line, movie theaters, traveling by car or bus,
crowded restaurants, and being alone. Mild
This scenario illustrates the experience of agoraphobia is exemplified by the person who
panic that is not the central focus of the per- hesitates about driving long distances alone but
sons anxiety. More likely, this woman has gen- manages to drive to and from work, prefers to
eralized anxiety disorder, and her uncontrolla- sit on the aisle at movie theaters but still goes to
ble worry leads her to panic on occasion. The movies, and avoids crowded places. Moder-
next example is of someone with social phobia, ate agoraphobia is exemplified by the person
who becomes very concerned about panicking whose driving is limited to a 10-mile radius
in social situations, because the possibility of a from home and only if accompanied, who
panic attack increases her concerns about being shops at off-peak times and avoids large super-
judged negatively by others. markets, and who avoids flying or traveling by
train. Severe agoraphobia refers to very lim-
PATIENT: I am terrified of having a panic attack ited mobility, sometimes even to the point of
in meetings at work. I dread the thought of becoming housebound.
others noticing how anxious I am. They Not all persons who panic develop agora-
must be able to see my hands shaking, the phobia, and the extent of agoraphobia that
sweat on my forehead, and worst of all, my emerges is highly variable (Craske & Barlow,
face turning red. 1988). Various factors have been investigated
4 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

as potential predictors of agoraphobia. Al- miologic Catchment Area (ECA; Myers et al.,
though agoraphobia tends to increase as his- 1984) study.
tory of panic lengthens, a significant pro- Individuals with agoraphobia who seek
portion of individuals panic for many years treatment almost always report that a history
without developing agoraphobic limitations. of panic preceded their development of avoid-
Nor is agoraphobia related to age of onset or ance (Goisman et al., 1994; Wittchen, Reed, &
frequency of panic (Cox, Endler, & Swinson, Kessler, 1998). In contrast, epidemiological
1995; Craske & Barlow, 1988; Kikuchi et al., data indicate that a subset of the population
2005; Rapee & Murrell, 1988). Some studies experiences agoraphobia without a history of
report more intense physical symptoms during panic disorder: 0.8% in the last 12 months
panic attacks when there is more agoraphobia (Kessler, Chiu, et al., 2005) and 1.4% lifetime
(e.g., de Jong & Bouman, 1995; Goisman et prevalence (Kessler, Berglund, et al., 2005).
al., 1994; Noyes, Clancy, Garvey, & Anderson, The discrepancy between clinical and epidemi-
1987; Telch, Brouillard, Telch, Agras, & Tay- ological data has been attributed to misdiag-
lor, 1989). Others fail to find such differences nosis of generalized anxiety, specific and social
(e.g., Cox et al., 1995; Craske, Miller, Ro- phobias, and reasonable cautiousness about
tunda, & Barlow, 1990). On the one hand, certain situations (e.g., walking alone in un-
fears of dying, going crazy, or losing control do safe neighborhoods) as agoraphobia in epide-
not relate to level of agoraphobia (Cox et al., miological samples (Horwath, Lish, Johnson,
1995; Craske, Rapee, & Barlow, 1988). On Hornig, & Weissman, 1993), and to the fact
the other hand, concerns about social conse- that individuals who panic are more likely to
quences of panicking may be stronger when seek help (Boyd, 1986).
there is more agoraphobia (Amering et al., Rarely does the diagnosis of PD/PDA occur
1997; de Jong & Bouman, 1995; Rapee & in isolation. Commonly co-occurring Axis I
Murrell, 1988; Telch, Brouilard, et al., 1989). conditions include specific phobias, social pho-
In addition, in a recent investigation, Kikuchi bia, dysthymia, generalized anxiety disorder,
and colleagues (2005) found that individuals major depressive disorder, and substance abuse
who developed agoraphobia within 6 months (e.g., Brown, Campbell, Lehman, Grishman, &
of the onset of panic disorder had a higher Mancill, 2001; Goisman, Goldenberg, Vasile,
prevalence of generalized anxiety disorder but & Keller, 1995; Kessler, Chiu, et al., 2005).
not major depression. However, whether the Also, 2560% of persons with panic disorder
social evaluation concerns or comorbidity are also meet criteria for a personality disorder,
precursors or are secondary to agoraphobia re- mostly avoidant and dependent personality dis-
mains to be determined. Occupational status orders (e.g., Chambless & Renneberg, 1988).
also predicts agoraphobia, accounting for 18% However, the nature of the relationship be-
of the variance in one study (de Jong & tween PD/PDA and personality disorders re-
Bouman, 1995). Perhaps the strongest predic- mains unclear. For example, comorbidity rates
tor of agoraphobia is sex; the ratio of males to are highly dependent on the method used to es-
females shifts dramatically in the direction of tablish Axis II diagnosis, as well as the co-
female predominance as level of agoraphobia occurrence of depressed mood (Alneas &
worsens (e.g., Thyer, Himle, Curtis, Cameron, Torgersen, 1990; Chambless & Renneberg,
& Nesse, 1985). 1988). Moreover, the fact that abnormal per-
sonality traits improve and some personality
disorders even remit after successful treat-
PRESENTING FEATURES ment of PD/PDA (Black, Monahan, Wesner,
Gabel, & Bowers, 1996; Mavissakalian &
From the latest epidemiological study, the Na- Hamman, 1987; Noyes, Reich, Suelzer, &
tional Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS- Christiansen, 1991) raises questions about the
R; Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, & Walters, validity of Axis II diagnoses. The issue of
2005; Kessler, Chiu, Demler, & Walters, 2005) comorbidity with personality disorders and its
prevalence estimates for panic disorder with or effect on treatment for PD/PDA is described in
without agoraphobia (PD/PDA) are 2.7% (12 more detail in a later section.
month) and 4.7% (lifetime). These rates are The modal age of onset is late teenage years
higher than those reported in the original NCS and early adulthood (Kessler, Berglund, et al.,
(Kessler et al., 1994) and the older Epide- 2005). In fact, a substantial proportion of ado-
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 5

lescents report panic attacks (e.g., Hayward et for stressful life events in general (e.g., Barlow
al., 1992), and panic disorder in children and et al., 1984). Many presumed that pharmaco-
adolescents tends to be chronic and comorbid therapy was necessary for the control of panic.
with other anxiety, mood, and disruptive disor- In contrast, the treatment of agoraphobia was
ders (Biederman, Faraone, Marrs, & Moore, quite specific from the 1970s onward, with pri-
1997). Treatment is usually sought at a much marily exposure-based approaches to target
later age, around 34 years (e.g., Noyes et al., fear and avoidance of specific situations. How-
1986). The overall ratio of females to males is ever, relatively little consideration was given to
approximately 2:1 (Kessler et al., 2006), and, panic attacks in either the conceptualization or
as mentioned already, the ratio shifts dramati- treatment of agoraphobia. The development of
cally in the direction of female predominance specific panic control treatments in the middle
as level of agoraphobia worsens (e.g., Thyer et to late 1980s shifted interest away from agor-
al., 1985). aphobia. Interest in agoraphobia was sub-
Most (approximately 72%) (Craske et al., sequently renewed, specifically in terms of
1990) report identifiable stressors around the whether panic control treatments are sufficient
time of their first panic attack, including inter- for the management of agoraphobia, and
personal stressors and stressors related to phys- whether their combination with treatments
ical well-being, such as negative drug experi- that directly target agoraphobia is superior
ences, disease, or death in the family. However, overall. We address these questions in more de-
the number of stressors does not differ from the tail after describing the conceptualization that
number experienced prior to the onset of other underlies cognitive-behavioral approaches to
types of anxiety disorders (Pollard, Pollard, & the treatment of panic and agoraphobia.
Corn, 1989; Rapee, Litwin, & Barlow, 1990;
Roy-Byrne, Geraci, & Uhde, 1986). Approxi-
mately one-half report having experienced pan- CONCEPTUALIZATION OF
icky feelings at some time before their first ETIOLOGICAL AND MAINTAINING
panic, suggesting that onset may be either in- FACTORS FOR PANIC DISORDER
sidious or acute (Craske et al., 1990). AND AGORAPHOBIA
Finally, PD/PDA tend to be chronic condi-
tions, with severe financial and interpersonal Several independent lines of research (Barlow,
costs; that is, only a minority of untreated indi- 1988; Clark, 1986; Ehlers & Margraf, 1989)
viduals remit without subsequent relapse with- converged in the 1980s on the same basic con-
in a few years (30%), although a similar num- ceptualization of panic disorder as an acquired
ber experience notable improvement, albeit fear of bodily sensations, particularly sensa-
with a waxing and waning course (35%) tions associated with autonomic arousal. Psy-
(Katschnig & Amering, 1998; Roy-Byrne & chological and biological predispositions are
Cowley, 1995). Also, individuals with panic believed to enhance the vulnerability to acquire
disorder overutilize medical resources com- such fear. These interacting vulnerabilities have
pared to the general public and individuals been organized into an etiological conception
with other psychiatric disorders (e.g., Katon of anxiety disorders in general, referred to as
et al., 1990; Roy-Byrne et al., 1999). triple vulnerability theory (Barlow, 1988,
2002; Surez, Bennett, Goldstein, & Barlow, in
press). First, genetic contributions to the devel-
HISTORY OF PSYCHOLOGICAL opment of anxiety and negative affect consti-
TREATMENT FOR PANIC DISORDER tute a generalized (heritable) biological vul-
AND AGORAPHOBIA nerability. Second, evidence also supports a
generalized psychological vulnerability to ex-
It was not until the publication of DSM-III perience anxiety and related negative affective
(American Psychiatric Association, 1980) that states, characterized by a diminished sense
PD/PDA was recognized as a distinct anxiety of control arising from early developmental
problem. Until that time, panic attacks were experiences. Although the unfortunate co-
viewed primarily as a form of free-floating anx- occurrence of generalized biological and psy-
iety. Consequently, psychological treatment ap- chological vulnerabilities may be sufficient to
proaches were relatively nonspecific. They in- produce anxiety and related states, particularly
cluded relaxation and cognitive restructuring generalized anxiety disorder and depression, a
6 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

third vulnerability seems necessary to account tacks in adolescents (Hayward, Killen,


for the development of at least some specific Kraemer, & Taylor, 2000; Schmidt, Lerew, &
anxiety disorders, including panic disorder; Jackson, 1997, 1999), and emotional reactiv-
that is, early learning experiences in some in- ity at age 3 was a significant variable in the
stances seem to focus anxiety on particular ar- classification of panic disorder in 18- to 21-
eas of concern. In panic disorder, the experi- year-old males (Craske, Poulton, Tsao, &
ence of certain somatic sensations becomes Plotkin, 2001). Ongoing studies, such as the
associated with a heightened sense of threat Northwestern/UCLA Youth Emotion Project,
and danger. This specific psychological vulner- are comprehensively evaluating the role of neu-
ability, when coordinated with the generalized roticism in the prediction of subsequent panic
biological and psychological vulnerabilities disorder.
mentioned earlier, seems to contribute to the Numerous multivariate genetic analyses of
development of panic disorder. Fear condition- human twin samples consistently attribute ap-
ing, avoidant responding, and information- proximately 3050% of variance in neuroti-
processing biases are believed to perpetuate cism to additive genetic factors (Eley, 2001;
such fear. It is the perpetuating factors that are Lake, Eaves, Maes, Heath, & Martin, 2000).
targeted in the cognitive-behavioral treatment In addition, anxiety and depression appear to
approach. What follows is a very brief review be variable expressions of the heritable ten-
of some contributory factors with practical rel- dency toward neuroticism (Kendler, Heath,
evance for panic disorder. Martin, & Eaves, 1987). Symptoms of panic
(i.e., breathlessness, heart pounding) may be
additionally explained by a unique source of
Three Vulnerability Factors genetic variance that is differentiated from
Genetics and Temperament symptoms of depression and anxiety (Kendler
et al., 1987) and neuroticism (Martin, Jardine,
The temperament most associated with anxiety Andrews, & Heath, 1988).
disorders, including panic disorder, is neuroti- Analyses of specific genetic markers remain
cism (Eysenck, 1967; Gray, 1982), or prone- preliminary and inconsistent. For example,
ness to experience negative emotions in re- panic disorder has been linked to a locus
sponse to stressors. A closely linked construct, on chromosome 13 (Hamilton et al., 2003;
negative affectivity, is the tendency to experi- Schumacher et al., 2005) and chromosome 9
ence a variety of negative emotions across a va- (Thorgeirsson et al., 2003), but the exact genes
riety of situations, even in the absence of ob- remain unknown. Findings regarding markers
jective stressors (Watson & Clark, 1984). for the cholecystokinin-B receptor gene have
Structural analyses confirm that negative affect been inconsistent (cf. Hamilton et al. [2001]
is a higher-order factor that distinguishes indi- and van Megen, Westenberg, Den Boer, &
viduals with each anxiety disorder (and depres- Kahn [1996]). Also, association and linkage
sion) from controls with no mental disorder: studies implicate the adenosine receptor gene in
Lower-order factors discriminate among anxi- panic disorder (Deckert et al., 1998; Hamilton
ety disorders, with fear of fear being the fac- et al., 2004). But studies of genes involved in
tor that discriminates panic disorder from neurotransmitter systems associated with fear
other anxiety disorders (Brown, Chorpita, & and anxiety have produced inconsistent results
Barlow, 1998; Zinbarg & Barlow, 1996). The (see Roy-Byrne, Craske, & Stein, 2006). Thus,
anxiety disorders load differentially on nega- there is no evidence at this point for a specific
tive affectivity, with more pervasive anxiety link between genetic markers and tempera-
disorders, such as generalized anxiety disorder, ment, on the one hand, and panic disorder on
loading more heavily, panic disorder loading at the other. Rather, neurobiological factors seem
an intermediate level, and social anxiety disor- to comprise a nonspecific biological vulnerabil-
der loading the least (Brown et al., 1998).1 ity.
However, these findings derive from cross-
sectional data sets.
Longitudinal prospective evidence for the
Anxiety Sensitivity
role of neuroticism in predicting the onset of As described earlier, neuroticism is viewed as a
panic disorder is relatively limited. Specifically, higher-order factor characteristic of all anxiety
neuroticism predicted the onset of panic at- disorders, with fear of fear being more
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 7

unique to panic disorder. The construct fear panic, and is weaker than the relationship be-
of fear overlaps with the construct anxiety tween panic and neuroticism. Furthermore,
sensitivity, or the belief that anxiety and its as- these studies have evaluated panic attacks and
sociated symptoms may cause deleterious phys- worry about panic, but not the prediction of
ical, social, and psychological consequences diagnosed panic disorder. Thus, the causal sig-
that extend beyond any immediate physical nificance of anxiety sensitivity for panic disor-
discomfort during an episode of anxiety or der remains to be fully understood.
panic (Reiss, 1980). Anxiety sensitivity is ele-
vated across most anxiety disorders, but it is
History of Medical Illness and Abuse
particularly elevated in panic disorder (e.g.,
Taylor, Koch, & McNally, 1992; Zinbarg & Other studies highlight the role of medical ill-
Barlow, 1996), especially the Physical Con- nesses as contributing to a specific psychologi-
cerns subscale of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index cal vulnerability for panic disorder. For exam-
(Zinbarg & Barlow, 1996; Zinbarg, Barlow, & ple, using the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Study
Brown, 1997). Therefore, beliefs that physical database, we found that experience with per-
symptoms of anxiety are harmful seem to be sonal respiratory disturbance (and parental
particularly relevant to panic disorder and may poor health) as a youth predicted panic disor-
comprise a specific psychological vulnerability. der at age 18 or 21 (Craske et al., 2001). This
Anxiety sensitivity is presumed to confer a finding is consistent with reports of more respi-
risk factor for panic disorder, because it primes ratory disturbance in the history of patients
fear reactivity to bodily sensations. In support, with panic disorder compared to other patients
anxiety sensitivity predicts subjective distress with anxiety disorders (Verburg, Griez, Meijer,
and reported symptomatology in response to & Pols, 1995). Furthermore, in a recent study,
procedures that induce strong physical sensa- first-degree relatives of patients with panic dis-
tions, such as CO2 inhalation (Forsyth, Palav, order had a significantly higher prevalence of
& Duff, 1999), balloon inflation (Messenger & chronic obstructive respiratory disease, and
Shean, 1998), and hyperventilation (Sturges, asthma in particular, than first-degree relatives
Goetsch, Ridley, & Whittal, 1998) in nonclini- of patients with other anxiety disorders (van
cal samples, even after researchers control for Beek, Schruers, & Friez, 2005).
the effects of trait anxiety (Rapee & Medoro, Childhood experiences of sexual and physical
1994). In addition, several longitudinal studies abuse may also prime panic disorder. Retrospec-
indicate that high scores on the Anxiety Sensi- tive reports of such childhood abuse were associ-
tivity Index predict the onset of panic attacks ated with panic disorder onset at ages 1621
over 1- to 4-year intervals in adolescents (Hay- years in a recent longitudinal analysis of New
ward et al., 2000), college students (Maller Zealanders from birth to age 21 (Goodwin,
& Reiss, 1992), and community samples Fergusson, & Horwood, 2005). This finding is
with specific phobias or no anxiety disorders consistent with multiple cross-sectional studies
(Ehlers, 1995). The predictive relationship re- in both clinical and community samples (e.g.,
mains after controlling for prior depression Bandelow et al., 2002; Kendler et al., 2000;
(Hayward et al., 2000). In addition, Anxiety Kessler, Davis, & Kendler, 1997; Moisan &
Sensitivity Index scores predicted spontaneous Engels, 1995; Stein et al., 1996). The association
panic attacks and worry about panic (and anxi- with childhood abuse is stronger for panic disor-
ety more generally), during an acute military der than for other anxiety disorders, such as so-
stressor (i.e., 5 weeks of basic training), even cial phobia (Safren, Gershuny, Marzol, Otto, &
after controlling for history of panic attacks Pollack, 2002; Stein et al., 1996) and obsessive
and trait anxiety (Schmidt et al., 1997, 1999). compulsive disorder (Stein et al., 1996). In addi-
Finally, panic attacks themselves elevate anxi- tion, some studies reported an association be-
ety sensitivity over a 5-week period in adults tween panic disorder and exposure to violence
(Schmidt et al., 1999), and over a 1-year period between other family members, generally
in adolescents, albeit to a lesser extent (Weems, interparental violence (e.g., Bandelow et al.,
Hayward, Killen, & Taylor, 2002). 2002; Moisan & Engels, 1995), whereas the
However, we (Bouton, Mineka, & Barlow, most recent study did not (Goodwin et al.,
2001) have noted that the relationship between 2005). Retrospective reporting of childhood
anxiety sensitivity and panic attacks in these abuse and familial violence in all of these studies,
studies is relatively small, not exclusive to however, limits the findings.
8 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

Interoceptive Awareness antecedent; hence, it represents a false alarm


(Barlow, 1988, 2002). The large majority of
Patients with panic disorder, as well as non- initial panic attacks are recalled as occurring
clinical panickers, appear to have heightened outside of the home, while driving, walking, at
awareness of, or ability to detect, bodily sensa- work, or at school (Craske et al., 1990), gener-
tions of arousal (e.g., Ehlers & Breuer, 1992, ally in public (Lelliott, Marks, McNamee, &
1996; Ehlers, Breuer, Dohn, & Feigenbaum, Tobena, 1989), and on a bus, plane, subway, or
1995; Zoellner & Craske, 1999). Discrepant in social-evaluative situations (Shulman, Cox,
findings (e.g., Antony et al., 1995; Rapee, Swinson, Kuch, & Reichman, 1994). We
1994) exist but have been attributed to meth- (Barlow, 1988; Craske & Rowe, 1997b) be-
odological artifact (Ehlers & Breuer, 1996). lieve situations that set the scene for initial
Ability to perceive heartbeat, in particular, panic attacks are ones in which bodily sensa-
appears to be a relatively stable individual- tions are perceived as posing the most threat,
difference variable given that it does not dif- because of impairment of functioning (e.g.,
fer between untreated and treated patients driving), entrapment (e.g., air travel, elevators),
with panic disorder (Ehlers & Breuer, 1992), negative social evaluation (e.g., job, formal so-
or from before to after successful treatment cial events), or distance from safety (e.g., unfa-
(Antony, Meadows, Brown, & Barlow, 1994; miliar locales). Entrapment concerns may be
Ehlers et al., 1995). Thus, interoceptive accu- particularly salient for subsequent devel-
racy may be a predisposing trait for panic dis- opment of agoraphobia (Faravelli, Pallanti,
order. Ehlers and Breuer (1996) suggested that Biondi, Paterniti, & Scarpato, 1992).
although good interoception is considered
neither a necessary nor a sufficient condition
for panic disorder, it may enhance the probabil- Maintenance Factors
ity of panic by increasing the probability of Acute fear of fear (or, more accurately, anxi-
perceiving sensations that may trigger an at- ety focused on somatic sensations) that devel-
tack if perceived as dangerous (p. 174). ops after initial panic attacks in vulnerable in-
Whether interoceptive awareness is learned, dividuals refers to anxiety about certain bodily
and represents another specific psychological sensations associated with panic attacks (e.g.,
vulnerability, or is more dispositional remains racing heart, dizziness, paresthesias) (Barlow,
to be determined. 1988; Goldstein & Chambless, 1978), and is
Separate from interoception is the issue of attributed to two factors. The first is intero-
propensity for intense autonomic activation. ceptive conditioning, or conditioned fear of in-
As noted earlier, some evidence points to a ternal cues, such as elevated heart rate, because
unique genetic influence on the reported expe- of their association with intense fear, pain, or
rience of breathlessness, heart pounding, and a distress (Razran, 1961). Specifically, intero-
sense of terror (Kendler et al., 1987). Con- ceptive conditioning refers to low-level somatic
ceivably, cardiovascular reactivity presents a sensations of arousal or anxiety becoming con-
unique physiological predisposition for panic ditioned stimuli, so that early somatic compo-
disorder. In support of this, cardiac symptoms nents of the anxiety response come to elicit sig-
and shortness of breath predict later develop- nificant bursts of anxiety or panic (Bouton et
ment of panic attacks and panic disorder (Keyl al., 2001). An extensive body of experimental
& Eaton, 1990). Unfortunately, these data de- literature attests to the robustness of inter-
rive from report of symptoms, which is not oceptive conditioning (e.g., Dworkin &
a good index of actual autonomic state Dworkin, 1999), particularly with regard to
(Pennebaker & Roberts, 1992) and may in- early interoceptive drug-onset cues becoming
stead reflect interoception. conditioned stimuli for larger drug effects (e.g.,
Sokolowska, Siegel, & Kim, 2002). In addi-
Initial Panic Attacks tion, interoceptive conditioned responses are
not dependent on conscious awareness of trig-
From an evolutionary standpoint, fear is a nat- gering cues (Razran, 1961); thus, they have
ural and adaptive response to threatening stim- been observed in patients under anesthesia
uli. However, the fear experienced during the (e.g., Block, Ghoneim, Fowles, Kumar, &
first unexpected panic attack is often unjusti- Pathak, 1987). Within this model, then, slight
fied due to the lack of an identifiable trigger or changes in relevant bodily functions that are
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 9

not consciously recognized may elicit condi- Wiedemann, 2005). In addition, they are more
tioned anxiety or fear and panic due to previ- likely to become anxious in procedures that
ous pairings with panic (Barlow, 1988; Bouton elicit bodily sensations similar to the ones expe-
et al., 2001). rienced during panic attacks, including benign
The second factor, offered by Clark (1986) cardiovascular, respiratory, and audioves-
to explain acute fear of panic-related body sen- tibular exercises (Antony, Ledley, Liss, &
sations, is catastrophic misappraisals of bodily Swinson, 2006; Jacob, Furman, Clark, &
sensations (misinterpretation of sensations as Durrant, 1992), as well as more invasive proce-
signs of imminent death, loss of control, etc.). dures, such as CO2 inhalations, compared to
Debate continues as to the significance of cata- patients with other anxiety disorders (e.g.,
strophic misappraisals of bodily sensations ver- Perna, Bertani, Arancio, Ronchi, & Bellodi,
sus conditioned (emotional, non-cognitively- 1995; Rapee, 1986; Rapee, Brown, Antony, &
mediated) fear responding. We have taken issue Barlow, 1992) or healthy controls (e.g.,
with the purely cognitive model of panic disor- Gorman et al., 1994). The findings are not fully
der by stating that it cannot account for panic consistent, however, because patients with
attacks devoid of conscious cognitive appraisal panic disorder did not differ from patients with
without turning to constructs such as auto- social phobia in response to an epinephrine
matic appraisals, which prove to be untest- challenge (Veltman, van Zijderveld, Tilders, &
able (Bouton et al., 2001). Catastrophic mis- van Dyck, 1996). Nonetheless, individuals
appraisals may accompany panic attacks with panic disorder also fear signals that
because they are a natural part of the constella- ostensibly reflect heightened arousal and false
tion of responses that go with panic, or because physiological feedback (Craske & Freed,
they have been encouraged and reinforced 1995; Craske, Lang, et al., 2002;
much like sick role behaviors during child- Ehlers, Margraf, Roth, Taylor, & Birnbaumer,
hood. In addition, such thoughts may become 1988).
conditioned stimuli that trigger anxiety and Distress over bodily sensations is likely to
panic, as demonstrated via panic induction generate ongoing distress for a number of rea-
through presentation of pairs of words in- sons. First, in the immediate sense, autonomic
volving sensations and catastrophic outcomes arousal generated by fear in turn intensifies the
(Clark et al., 1988). In this case, catastrophic feared sensations, thus creating a reciprocating
cognitions may well be sufficient to elicit con- cycle of fear and sensations that is sustained
ditioned panic attacks, but not necessary. until autonomic arousal abates or the individ-
Whether cognitively or noncognitively ual perceives safety. Second, because bodily
based, excessive anxiety over panic-related sensations that trigger panic attacks are not al-
bodily sensations in panic disorder is well sup- ways immediately obvious, they may generate
ported. Persons with panic disorder endorse the perception of unexpected or out of the
strong beliefs that bodily sensations associated blue panic attacks (Barlow, 1988) that causes
with panic attacks cause physical or mental even further distress (Craske, Glover, &
harm (e.g., Chambless, Caputo, Bright, & DeCola, 1995). Third, the perceived uncontrol-
Gallagher, 1984; McNally & Lorenz, 1987). lability, or inability to escape or terminate
They are more likely to interpret bodily sensa- bodily sensations, again, is likely to generate
tions in a catastrophic fashion (Clark et al., heightened anxiety (e.g., Maier, Laudenslager,
1988), and to allocate more attentional re- & Ryan, 1985; Mineka et al., 1984). Unpre-
sources to words that represent physical threat, dictability and uncontrollability, then, are seen
such as disease and fatality (e.g., Ehlers, as enhancing general levels of anxiety about
Margraf, Davies, & Roth, 1988; Hope, Rapee, When is it going to happen again? and
Heimberg, & Dombeck, 1990); catastrophe What do I do when it happens?, thereby con-
words, such as death and insane (e.g., tributing to high levels of chronic anxious ap-
Maidenberg, Chen, Craske, Bohn, & prehension (Barlow, 1988, 2002). In turn, anx-
Bystritsky, 1996; McNally, Riemann, Louro, ious apprehension increases the likelihood of
Lukach, & Kim, 1992); and heartbeat stimuli panic by directly increasing the availability of
(Kroeze & van den Hout, 2000). Also, individ- sensations that have become conditioned cues
uals with panic disorder show enhanced brain for panic and/or attentional vigilance for these
potentials in response to panic-related words bodily cues. Thus, a maintaining cycle of panic
(Pauli, Amrhein, Muhlberger, Dengler, & and anxious apprehension develops. Also, sub-
10 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

tle avoidance behaviors are believed to main- In the second setting, the natural environ-
tain negative beliefs about feared bodily sensa- ment, cognitive restructuring and other anxiety
tions (Clark & Ehlers, 1993). Examples management skills are put into practice, and
include holding onto objects or persons for fear the patient faces feared situations. The latter is
of fainting, sitting and remaining still for fear called in vivo exposure and can be conducted
of a heart attack, and moving slowly or search- with the aid of the therapist or alone.
ing for an escape route because one fears acting Therapist-directed exposure is particularly use-
foolish (Salkovskis, Clark, & Gelder, 1996). ful for patients who lack a social network to
Finally, anxiety may develop over specific con- support in vivo exposure assignments, and
texts in which the occurrence of panic would more valuable than self-directed exposure for
be particularly troubling (i.e., situations associ- patients with more severe agoraphobia
ated with impairment, entrapment, negative (Holden, OBrien, Barlow, Stetson, &
social evaluation, and distance from safety). Infantino, 1983). Therapist-directed exposure
These anxieties may contribute to agora- is essential to guided mastery exposure, in
phobia, which in turn maintains distress by which the therapist gives corrective feedback
preventing disconfirmation of catastrophic about the way the patient faces feared situa-
misappraisals and extinction of conditioned re- tions to minimize unnecessary defensive behav-
sponding. iors. For example, patients are taught to drive
in a relaxed position at the wheel and to walk
across a bridge without holding the rail. On the
TREATMENT VARIABLES one hand, guided mastery exposure has been
shown to be more effective than stimulus ex-
Setting
posure when patients attempt simply to en-
There are several different settings for conduct- dure the situation alone until fear subsides,
ing cognitive-behavioral therapy for panic without the benefit of ongoing therapist feed-
disorder and agoraphobia. The first, the outpa- back (Williams & Zane, 1989). On the other
tient clinicoffice setting, is suited to psych- hand, self-directed exposure is very valuable
oeducation, cognitive restructuring, assign- also, especially to the degree that it encourages
ment and feedback regarding homework independence and generalization of the skills
assignments, and role-play rehearsals. In addi- learned in treatment to conditions in which the
tion, certain exposures can be conducted in the therapist is not present. Thus, the most benefi-
office setting, such as interoceptive exposure to cial approach in the natural environment is to
feared bodily sensations described later. Re- proceed from therapist-directed to self-directed
cently, outpatient settings have extended from exposure.
mental health settings to primary care suites In an interesting variation that combines the
(e.g., Craske, Roy-Byrne, et al., 2002; Roy- office and the natural environment, telephone-
Byrne et al., 2005; Sharp, Power, Simpson, guided treatment, therapists direct patients
Swanson, & Anstee, 1997). This extension is with agoraphobia by phone to conduct in vivo
particularly important because of the higher exposure to feared situations (NcNamee,
prevalence of panic disorder in primary care OSullivan, Lelliot, & Marks, 1989; Swinson,
settings (e.g., Shear & Schulberg, 1995; Fergus, Cox, & Wickwire, 1995) or provide in-
Tiemens, Ormel, & Simon, 1996). However, struction in panic control skills (Cote,
whether a mental health or a primary care of- Gauthier, Laberge, Cormier, & Plamondon,
fice is being used, the built-in safety signals of 1994). In addition, one small study showed
such an office may limit the generalizability of that cognitive-behavioral therapy was as effec-
learning that takes place in that setting. For ex- tive when delivered by videoconference as in
ample, learning to be less afraid in the presence person (Bouchard et al., 2004).
of the therapist, or in an office located near a Self-directed treatments, with minimal direct
medical center, may not necessarily generalize therapist contact, take place in the natural en-
to conditions in which the therapist is not pres- vironment, and are beneficial for highly moti-
ent, or the perceived safety of a medical center vated and educated patients (e.g., Ghosh &
is not close by. For this reason, homework as- Marks, 1987; Gould & Clum, 1995; Gould,
signments to practice cognitive-behavioral Clum, & Shapiro, 1993; Lidren et al., 1994;
skills in a variety of different settings are partic- Schneider, Mataix-Cols, Marks, & Bachofen,
ularly important. 2005). On the other hand, self-directed treat-
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 11

ments are less effective for more severely af- ally formatted treatment suggests that group
fected patients (Holden et al., 1983), or those treatment is as effective as individual therapy.
with more comorbidity (Hecker, Losee, Also, Lidren and colleagues (1994) found that
Roberson-Nay, & Maki, 2004), less motiva- group therapy is as effective as individual
tion, and less education; or for patients who bibliotherapy, although they did not include a
are referred as opposed to recruited through comparison with individualized cognitive-
advertisement (Hecker, Losee, Fritzler, & Fink, behavioral therapy. In direct comparisons, a
1996). Self-directed treatments have expanded slight advantage is shown for individual for-
beyond workbooks and manuals to computer- mats. Specifically, Neron, Lacroix, and Chaput
ized and Internet versions (e.g., Carlbring, (1995) compared 1214 weekly sessions of in-
Ekselius, & Andersson, 2003; Richards, Klein, dividual or group cognitive-behavioral therapy
& Austen, 2006; Richards, Klein, & Carlbring, (N = 20), although the group condition re-
2003). In general, these treatments yield posi- ceived two additional 1-hour individual ses-
tive results, although not quite as positive sions. The two conditions were equally effec-
as fully therapist-delivered treatments. Spe- tive for measures of panic and agoraphobia at
cifically, a four-session computer-assisted posttreatment and 6-month follow-up. How-
cognitive-behavioral therapy for panic disorder ever, the individual format was more successful
was less effective than 12 sessions of therapist- in terms of generalized anxiety and depressive
delivered cognitive-behavioral therapy at post- symptoms by the follow-up point. In addition,
treatment, although the groups did not differ at individual treatments resulted in more clini-
follow-up (Newman, Kenardy, Herman, & cally significant outcomes than group formats
Taylor, 1997). More recently, 12 sessions of in primary care (Sharp, Power, & Swanson,
therapist-delivered cognitive-behavioral ther- 2004). Furthermore, 95% of individuals as-
apy was more effective than six sessions signed to the waiting-list condition in the latter
of either therapist-delivered or computer- study stated a clear preference for individual
augmented therapy (Kenardy et al., 2003). treatment when given the choice at the end of
Also, findings from computerized programs for the waiting list.
emotional disorders in general indicate that Most studies of cognitive-behavioral therapy
such treatments are more acceptable and suc- for panic and agoraphobia involve 1020
cessful when combined with therapist involve- weekly treatment sessions. Several studies
ment (e.g., Carlbring et al., 2003). show that briefer treatments may be effective
The third setting, the inpatient facility, is as well. Evans and colleagues (1991) compared
most appropriate when conducting very inten- a 2-day group cognitive-behavioral treatment
sive cognitive-behavioral therapy (e.g., daily to a waiting-list condition, although without
therapist contact), or treating severely disabled random assignment. The 2-day program com-
persons who can no longer function at home. prised lectures (3 hours); teaching skills, such
In addition, certain medical or drug complica- as breathing, relaxation, and cognitive chal-
tions may warrant inpatient treatment. The lenging (3 hours); in vivo exposure (9 hours);
greatest drawback to the inpatient setting is and group discussion plus a 2-hour support
poor generalization to the home environment. group for significant others. Eighty-five percent
Transition sessions and follow-up booster ses- of treated patients were reported to be either
sions in an outpatient clinicoffice or in the pa- symptom-free or symptomatically improved,
tients own home facilitate generalization. and these results were maintained 1 year later.
In contrast, the waiting-list group did not dem-
onstrate significant changes. A recent pilot
Format
study similarly indicated effectiveness with in-
Cognitive-behavioral therapy for panic disor- tensive cognitive-behavioral therapy over 2
der and agoraphobia may be conducted in indi- days (Deacon & Abramowitz, 2006). Other
vidual or group formats. Several clinical out- studies have evaluated the effectiveness of
come studies have used group treatments (e.g., cognitive-behavioral therapy when delivered
Craske, DeCola, Sachs, & Pontillo, 2003; Ev- over a fewer number of sessions. In a random-
ans, Holt, & Oei, 1991; Feigenbaum, 1988; ized study, patients with PDA who awaited
Hoffart, 1995; Telch et al., 1993). The fact that pharmacotherapy treatment were assigned to
their outcomes are generally consistent with four weekly sessions of either cognitive-
the summary statistics obtained from individu- behavioral therapy or supportive nondirective
12 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

therapy (Craske, Maidenberg, & Bystritsky, My boyfriend really tries hard to help me.
1995). Cognitive-behavioral therapy was more Hes always cautious of my feelings and
effective than supportive therapy, particularly doesnt push me to do things that I cant do.
with less severely affected patients, although He phones me from work to check on me.
the results were not as positive as those typi- He stays with me and holds my hand when I
cally seen with more sessions. Also, we found feel really scared. He never hesitates to leave
that up to six sessions (average of three ses- work and take me home if Im having a bad
sions) of cognitive-behavioral therapy com- time. Only last week we visited some of his
bined with medication recommendations friends, and we had to leave. I feel guilty be-
yielded significantly greater improvements on cause we dont do the things we used to en-
an array of measures, including quality of life, joy doing together. We dont go to the mov-
compared to treatment as usual for individuals ies anymore. We used to love going to ball
with panic disorder in primary care settings games, but now its too much for me. I am so
(Roy-Byrne et al., 2005). Notably, however, the thankful for him. I dont know what I would
treatment effects substantially increased as the do without him.
number of cognitive-behavioral therapy ses-
sions (up to six) and follow-up booster phone Perhaps some forms of agoraphobia repre-
call sessions (up to six) increased (Craske et al., sent a conflict between desire for autonomy
2006). Finally, in a direct comparison, results and dependency in interpersonal relationships
were equally effective whether cognitive- (Fry, 1962; Goldstein & Chambless, 1978). In
behavioral therapy was delivered across the other words, the preagoraphobic is trapped
standard 12 sessions or across approximately 6 in a domineering relationship without the skills
sessions (Clark et al., 1999). needed to activate change. However, the con-
cept of a distinct marital system that predis-
poses toward agoraphobia lacks empirical evi-
Interpersonal Context
dence. That is not to say that marital or
Interpersonal context variables have been re- interpersonal systems are unimportant to ago-
searched in terms of the development, mainte- raphobia. For example, interpersonal discord/
nance, and treatment of agoraphobia. The rea- dissatisfaction may represent one of several
son for this research interest is apparent from possible stressors that precipitate panic attacks.
the following vignettes: Also, interpersonal relations may be negatively
impacted by the development of agoraphobia
My husband really doesnt understand. He (Buglass, Clarke, Henderson, & Presley, 1977),
thinks its all in my head. He gets angry at me and in turn contribute to its maintenance. Not
for not being able to cope. He says Im weak unlike one of the earlier vignettes, consider the
and irresponsible. He resents having to drive woman who has developed agoraphobia and
me around, and doing things for the kids now relies on her husband to do the shopping
that I used to do. We argue a lot, because he and other errands. These new demands upon
comes home tired and frustrated from work the husband lead to resentment and marital
only to be frustrated more by the problems discord. The marital distress adds to back-
Im having. But I cant do anything without ground stress, making progress and recovery
him. Im so afraid that Ill collapse into a even more difficult for the patient.
helpless wreck without him, or that Ill be Aside from whether interpersonal dysregula-
alone for the rest of my life. As cruel as he tion contributes to the onset or maintenance of
can be, I feel safe around him because he al- PD/PDA, some studies suggest that poor mari-
ways has everything under control. He al- tal relations adversely impact exposure-based
ways knows what to do. treatments (Bland & Hallam, 1981; Dewey &
Hunsley, 1989; Milton & Hafner, 1979). How-
This vignette illustrates dependency on the ever, other studies show no relationship be-
significant other for a sense of safety despite a tween marital distress and outcome from
nonsympathetic response that may only serve cognitive-behavioral therapy (Arrindell &
to increase background stress for the patient. Emmelkamp, 1987; Emmelkamp, 1980;
The second vignette illustrates inadvertent rein- Himadi, Cerny, Barlow, Cohen, & OBrien,
forcement of fear and avoidance through atten- 1986). Another line of research suggests that
tion from the significant other. involving significant others in every aspect of
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 13

treatment may override potential negative im- expertness, self-confidence, and directiveness
pacts of poor marital relations on phobic im- relate positively to outcome, although not con-
provement (Barlow, OBrien, & Last, 1984; sistently. In their own study of junior therapists
Cerny, Barlow, Craske, & Himadi, 1987). Fur- who provided cognitive-behavioral treatment
thermore, involvement of significant others for PD/PDA, Keijsers and colleagues (1995)
resulted in better long-term outcomes from found that more empathic statements and
cognitive-behavioral therapy for agoraphobia questioning occurred in Session 1 than in later
(Cerny et al., 1987). Similarly, communications sessions. In Session 3, therapists became more
training with significant others, compared to active and offered more instructions and expla-
relaxation training, after 4 weeks of in vivo ex- nations. In Session 10, therapists employed
posure therapy, resulted in significantly greater more interpretations and confrontations than
reductions on measures of agoraphobia by previously. Most importantly, directive state-
posttreatment (Arnow, Taylor, Agras, & Telch, ments and explanations in Session 1 predicted
1985), an effect that was maintained over an 8- poorer outcome. Empathic listening in Ses-
month follow-up. Together, these studies sug- sion 1 related to better behavioral outcome,
gest the value of including significant others in whereas empathic listening in Session 3 related
the treatment for agoraphobia. to poorer behavioral outcome. Thus, they dem-
Yet another question is the degree to which onstrated the advantages of different interac-
treatment for panic disorder and agorapho- tional styles at different points in therapy.
bia influences marital/interpersonal relations. Most clinicians assume that therapist train-
Some have noted that successful treatment can ing and experience improve the chances of suc-
have deleterious effects (Hafner, 1984; Hand & cessful outcome. Some believe this to be the
Lamontagne, 1976). Others note that it has no case particularly with respect to the cognitive
effect or a positive effect on marital functioning aspects of cognitive-behavioral therapy (e.g.,
(Barlow et al., 1983; Himadi et al., 1986). We Michelson et al., 1990), and some indirect evi-
(Barlow et al., 1983) suggested that when nega- dence for this supposition exists. Specifically,
tive effects do occur, it may be because expo- cognitive-behavioral therapy conducted by
sure therapy is conducted intensively, without novice therapists in a medical setting
the significant others involvement, which (Welkowitz et al., 1991) was somewhat less ef-
causes major role changes that the significant fective in comparison to the same therapy con-
other perceives as being beyond his or her con- ducted by inexperienced but highly trained
trol. This again speaks to the value of involving therapists in a psychological setting (Barlow,
significant others in the treatment process. Craske, Czerny, & Klosko, 1989), or by experi-
enced and highly trained therapists in a com-
munity mental health setting (Wade, Treat, &
Therapist Variables
Stuart, 1998). Huppert and colleagues (2001),
Only a few studies have evaluated therapist who directly evaluated the role of therapist ex-
variables in relation to cognitive-behavioral perience, found that, in general, therapist expe-
treatments for anxiety disorders. Williams and rience positively related to outcome, seemingly
Chambless (1990) found that patients with ag- because these therapists were more flexible in
oraphobia who rated their therapists as caring/ administering the treatment and better able to
involved, and as modeling self-confidence, adapt it to the individual being treated. Obvi-
achieved better outcomes on behavioral ap- ously, there is a need for more evaluation of the
proach tests. However, an important confound role of therapist experience and training in
in this study was that patient ratings of thera- cognitive-behavioral therapy.
pist qualities may have depended on patient Equally, if not more important is the need to
responses to treatment. Keijsers, Schaap, evaluate how much training of either novice or
Hoogduin, and Lammers (1995) reviewed find- experienced therapists is necessary to at-
ings regarding therapist relationship factors tain therapeutic competency in cognitive-
and behavioral outcome. They concluded that behavioral therapy. This is critically important
empathy, warmth, positive regard, and genu- in the current environment of dissemination of
ineness assessed early in treatment predict posi- cognitive-behavioral treatments for anxiety
tive outcome; patients who view their thera- disorders to real-world settings, in which train-
pists as understanding and respectful improve ing procedures must be adequate but not so
the most; and patient perceptions of therapist costly that they are prohibitive and therefore
14 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

not disseminable. Ongoing research in our set- and compliance with cognitive-behavioral ther-
tings is addressing exactly these issues. Others apy homework. Similarly, Murphy, Michelson,
are investigating the benefits of training general Marchione, Marchione, and Testa (1998)
practitioners in cognitive-behavioral therapy found that depressed persons with PD/PDA en-
for panic disorder (Heatley, Ricketts, & For- gaged in as many self-directed exposures as
rest, 2005). nondepressed persons, although the depressed
group reported more subjective anxiety during
exposures.
Patient Variables
A relatively high co-occurrence exists be-
There has been a recent interest in the effect of tween PD/PDA and avoidant, dependent, and
comorbidity upon the outcomes of cognitive- histrionic personality disorders (e.g., Reich et
behavioral therapy for PD/PDA. Brown, An- al., 1994). Questions of diagnostic reliability
tony, and Barlow (1995) found that comorbidi- and validity aside, comorbid personality disor-
ty with other anxiety disorders did not predict ders are sometimes associated with poorer re-
response to cognitive-behavioral therapy over- sponse than usual to cognitive-behavioral ther-
all, although social phobia was unexpectedly apy for PD/PDA (e.g., Hoffart & Hedley, 1997;
associated with superior outcome for PD/PDA. Marchand, Goyer, Dupuis, & Mainguy, 1998).
In contrast, we (Tsao, Lewin, & Craske, 1998) However, closer examination reveals that al-
found a trend for comorbidity that comprised though individuals with comorbid personality
mostly other anxiety disorders to be associated disorders have greater severity of PD/PDA at
with slightly lower rates of overall success. In a pre- and postcognitive-behavioral therapy, the
subsequent study, however, we replicated the rate of decrease in PD/PDA symptoms usually
finding by Brown et al. (1995) of no relation- is not affected by the comorbid personality dis-
ship between baseline comorbidity comprising order. Thus, Dreessen, Arntz, Luttels, and
mostly other anxiety disorders, and either im- Sallaerts (1994) and van den Hout, Brouwers,
mediate or 6-month outcome for PD/PDA and Oomen (2006) found that comorbid per-
(Tsao, Mystkowski, Zucker, & Craske, 2002). sonality disorders did not affect response to
Depressive disorders are highly comorbid cognitive-behavioral therapy for PD/PDA.
with PD/PDA (e.g., Goisman et al., 1994). In Moreover, Hofmann and colleagues (1998)
contrast to expectations and to pharmacology found that scores on questionnaire subscales
trials, the available evidence does not consis- reflecting Axis II personality disorders did not
tently demonstrate detrimental effects of initial predict panic disorder treatment response to ei-
depression upon outcome from cognitive- ther cognitive-behavioral therapy or to medica-
behavioral therapy for PD/PDA. On the one tion. In fact, some personality traits may asso-
hand, several studies found no relationship ciate positively with outcome, as was reported
with outcome, regardless of whether depres- by Rathus, Sanderson, Miller, and Wetzler
sion was the principal diagnosis or secondary (1995) with respect to compulsive personality
to PD/PDA (Brown et al., 1995; Laberge, features.
Gauthier, Cote, Plamondon, & Cormier, 1993; Substance-related disorders also commonly
McLean, Woody, Taylor, & Koch, 1998). On co-occur with PD/PDA. On the one hand, in a
the other hand, Mennin and Heimbergs (2000) series of single cases (N = 3), Lehman, Brown,
review led them to conclude a mixed pattern of and Barlow (1998) demonstrated successful
results given evidence that patients without control of panic attacks in individuals who
major depression showed greater reductions in were abusing alcohol. On the other hand, the
fears of bodily sensations (Laberge et al., addition of anxiety treatment to a relapse pre-
1993), that patients with primary, but not sec- vention program for abstinent individuals with
ondary, depression had worse outcomes than a primary diagnosis of alcohol dependence and
those without depression (Maddock & Blacker, a comorbid diagnosis of PDA or social phobia
1991), and that treatment completers were less decreased anxiety symptoms relative to a re-
likely than noncompleters to have comorbid lapse prevention program alone (Schade et al.,
depression (Wade et al., 1998). Some propose 2005). However, adding the anxiety treatment
that depression impedes engagement in did not affect rates of alcohol relapse in that
cognitive-behavioral therapy homework exer- study.
cises. However, McLean and colleagues (1998) Another source of comorbidity is medical
reported no relationship between depression conditions, such as cardiac arrhythmias or
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 15

asthma, that may slow improvement rates female African American sample that were
given the additional complications involved in judged to be comparable to those of European
discriminating between anxiety and disease Americans (Carter, Sbrocco, Gore, Marin, &
symptomatology, increases in actual medical Lewis, 2003). The influence of ethnic and cul-
risk, and the stress of physical diseases. Al- tural differences on treatment outcome and de-
though the effect of medical comorbidity on livery clearly needs more evaluation.
outcome has not been assessed to date, Finally, patients understanding of the nature
cognitive-behavioral therapy for panic disorder of their problem may be important to the suc-
has been shown to alleviate self-reported physi- cess of cognitive-behavioral treatments. Given
cal health symptoms (Schmidt et al., 2003). the somatic nature of panic disorder, many pa-
Other patient variables include socioeco- tients seek medical help first. Beyond that,
nomic status and general living conditions. We however, differences in the way the problem is
evaluated perceived barriers to receiving men- conceptualized could lead to the perception
tal health treatment in our primary care study that pharmacological or analytical treatment
of panic disorder (Craske, Golinelli, et al., approaches are more credible than cognitive-
2005). Commonly reported barriers included behavioral treatment approaches. For example,
inability to find out where to go for help individuals who strongly believe their condi-
(43%), worry about cost (40%), lack of cover- tion is due to a neurochemical imbalance
age by ones health plan (35%), and inability to may be more likely to seek medication and to
get an appointment soon enough (35%). Also, refute psychological treatments. Similarly, indi-
in our multicenter trial, attrition from viduals who attribute their condition to some-
cognitive-behavioral and/or medication treat- thing about my pastit must be unconscious
ment for panic disorder with minimal agora- influences may resist cognitive-behavioral in-
phobia was predicted by lower education, terpretations. Also, Grilo and colleagues
which in turn was dependent on lower income (1998) found that patients with PD/PDA who
(Grilo et al., 1998). Similarly, level of education attributed their disorder to specific stressors in
and motivation were associated with dropout their lives were more likely to drop out of
rates in another sample, although the effects cognitive-behavioral or medication treatment,
were small (Keijsers, Kampman, & Hoogduin, perhaps because they saw the offered treatment
2001). Low educationincome may reflect less as irrelevant.
discretionary time to engage in activities such
as weekly treatment. Consider the woman who
Concurrent Pharmacological Treatment
is a mother of two, a full-time clerk, whose
husband is on disability due to back injury, or Many more patients receive medications than
the full-time student who works an extra 25 cognitive-behavioral therapy for panic disorder
hours a week to pay his way through school. and agoraphobia, partly because primary care
Under these conditions, treatment assignments physicians are usually the first line of treat-
of daily in vivo exposure exercises are much ment. Thus, one-half or more of patients with
less likely to be completed. Frustration with panic disorder who attend psychology research
lack of treatment progress is likely to result. clinics already are taking anxiolytic medica-
Therapeutic success requires either a change in tions. The obvious questions, therefore, are the
lifestyle that allows the cognitive-behavioral extent to which cognitive-behavioral therapy
treatment to become a priority or termination and medications have a synergistic effect, and
of therapy until a later time, when life circum- how medications impact cognitive-behavioral
stances are less demanding. In fact, these kinds therapy.
of life-circumstance issues may explain the Results from large clinical trials, including
trend for African Americans to show less treat- our own multisite trial (Barlow, Gorman,
ment benefit in terms of mobility, anxiety, and Shear, & Woods, 2000), suggest no advantage
panic attacks, than European Americans during or immediately after the conclusion of
(Friedman & Paradis, 1991; Williams & treatment combining cognitive-behavioral and
Chambless, 1994). Although, in contrast to pharmacological approaches. Specifically, both
these two studies, Friedman, Paradis, and individual cognitive-behavioral and drug treat-
Hatch (1994) found equivalent outcomes ment and a combination treatment were imme-
across the two racial groups, and the results diately effective following treatment. Further-
from another study yielded outcomes from a more, following medication discontinuation,
16 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

the combination of medication and cognitive- versely, individuals who are resistant to phar-
behavioral therapy fared worse than cognitive- macotherapy may respond positively to
behavioral therapy alone, suggesting the cognitive-behavioral therapy, although these
possibility that state- (or context-) dependent findings were part of an open trial without ran-
learning in the presence of medication may domization (Heldt et al., 2006).
have attenuated the new learning that occurs Findings from the combination of fast-acting
during cognitive-behavioral therapy. On the anxiolytics and, specifically, the high-potency
other hand, in the primary care setting, we benzodiazepines with behavioral treatments
found that the addition of even just one com- for agoraphobia are contradictory (e.g., Marks
ponent of cognitive-behavioral therapy to med- et al., 1993; Wardle et al., 1994). Nevertheless,
ications for PD/PDA resulted in statistically several studies have reliably demonstrated the
and clinically significant improvements at detrimental effects of chronic use of high-
posttreatment and 12 months later (Craske, potency benzodiazepines on short-term and
Golinelli, et al., 2005). long-term outcome in cognitive-behavioral
More recently, our multisite collaborative treatments for panic or agoraphobia (e.g.,
team has been investigating long-term strate- Otto, Pollack, & Sabatino, 1996; van Balkom,
gies in the treatment of panic disorder. We ex- de Beurs, Koele, Lange, & van Dyck, 1996;
amined sequential combination strategies to Wardle et al., 1994). Specifically, there is evi-
determine whether this approach was more dence for more attrition, poorer outcome, and
advantageous than simultaneously combining more relapse with chronic use of high-potency
treatments. In this study, currently in prepara- benzodiazepines. In addition, use of benzo-
tion for publication, 256 patients with panic diazepines as needed was associated with
disorder with all levels of agoraphobia com- poorer outcome than regular use or no use in
pleted 3 months of initial treatment with one small naturalistic study (Westra, Stewart,
cognitive-behavioral therapy. Fifty-eight of & Conrad, 2002).
those patients did not reach an optimal level of Finally, the cost-effectiveness of cognitive-
functioning (high end-state functioning) and behavioral and medication treatments alone
entered a trial in which they received either versus in combination requires further evalua-
continued cognitive-behavioral therapy or tion; currently, cognitive-behavioral therapy is
paroxetine. Paroxetine was administered for considered to be more cost-effective (e.g., dis-
up to 1 year, whereas cognitive-behavioral ability costs, work days missed, health care
therapy was delivered twice a month for 3 use) than pharmacotherapy (Heuzenroeder et
months. At the end of the 1-year period, there al., 2004).
was a strong suggestion, represented as a statis- Understanding the ways in which psy-
tical trend, that more of the patients receiving chotropic medications influence cognitive-
paroxetine achieved responder status com- behavioral therapy may prove useful for devel-
pared to those receiving continued cognitive- oping methods that optimize the combination
behavioral treatment. Specifically, 60% of the of these two approaches to treatment. First,
nonresponders receiving paroxetine became re- medications, particularly fast-acting, potent
sponders, compared to 35% receiving contin- medications that cause a noticeable shift in
ued cognitive-behavioral therapy (p .083). state and are used on an as-needed basis (e.g.,
Further evaluation of effect sizes will help us to benzodiazepines, beta-blockers), may contrib-
evaluate the importance of this difference. This ute to relapse, because therapeutic success is at-
study also evaluated long-term strategies for tributed to them rather than to cognitive-
maintaining gains in those patients who re- behavioral therapy. Patients resultant lack of
sponded to cognitive-behavioral therapy, as de- perceived self-control may increase relapse po-
scribed below. tential when medication is withdrawn or con-
In another study with similar results, pa- tribute to maintenance of a medication regimen
tients who did not respond to cognitive- under the assumption that it is necessary to
behavioral therapy also benefited more from functioning. In support, attribution of thera-
the addition of a serotonergic drug (paroxe- peutic gains to alprazolam, and lack of confi-
tine) to continued cognitive-behavioral therapy dence in coping without alprazolam, even
than from the addition of a drug placebo, with when given in conjunction with behavioral
substantially different effect sizes (Kampman, therapy, predicted relapse (Basoglu, Marks,
Keijsers, Hoogduin, & Hendriks, 2002). Con- Kilic, Brewin, & Swinson, 1994). Second, med-
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 17

ications may assume the role of safety signals, the medication. I cant manage without it
or objects to which persons erroneously attrib- right now.
ute their safety from painful, aversive out-
comes. Safety signals contribute to mainte- Continuation of exposure after medication is
nance of fear and avoidance in the long term withdrawn may offset relapse, because it en-
(Hermans, Craske, Mineka, & Lovibond, hances attributions of personal mastery and re-
2006) and may interfere with corrections of duces the safety signal function of medications.
misappraisals of bodily symptoms. Third, med- In addition, opportunities to practice exposure
ications may block the capacity to experience and cognitive and behavioral strategies without
fear, which, at least initially in exposure ther- the aid of medication overcome state depend-
apy, is a positive predictor of overall outcome ency and enhance generalization of therapeutic
(for a review, see Craske & Mystkowski, gains once treatment is over.
2006). Fourth, medications may reduce the
motivation to engage in practices of cognitive-
behavioral skills, especially ones that effec- CASE STUDY
tively reduce panic and anxiety. Finally,
learning that takes place under the influence of Julie, a 33-year-old European American,
medications may not necessarily generalize mother of two, lives with Larry, her husband of
to the time when medications are removed, 8 years. For the past 3 years she has been
thus contributing to relapse (Bouton & chronically anxious and panic stricken. She de-
Swartzentruber, 1991). Some of these points scribes her panic attacks as unbearable and in-
are illustrated in the following vignettes: creasing in frequency. The first time she felt
panicky was just over 3 years ago, when she
was rushing to be by her grandmothers side in
I had been through a program of cognitive-
the last moments before she died. Julie was
behavioral therapy, but it was really the
driving alone on the freeway. She remembers
Paxil that helped. Because I was feeling so
feeling as if everything were moving in slow
much better, I considered tapering off the
motion, as if the cars were standing still, and
medication. At first I was very concerned
things around her seemed unreal. She recalled
about the idea. I had heard horror stories
feeling short of breath and detached. However,
about what people go through when with-
it was so important to reach her destination
drawing. However, I thought it would be OK
that she did not dwell on how she felt until
as long as I tapered slowly. So, I gradually
later. After the day was over, she reflected upon
weaned myself off. It really wasnt that bad.
how lucky she was not to have had an accident.
Well, I had been completely off the medica-
A few weeks later, the same type of feeling hap-
tion for about a month when the problem
pened again when driving on the freeway. This
started all over again. I remember sitting in a
time it occurred without the pressure of getting
restaurant, feeling really good because I was
to her dying grandmother. It scared Julie be-
thinking about how much of a problem res-
cause she was unable to explain the feelings.
taurants used to be for me before, and how
She pulled off to the side of the road and called
easy it seemed now. Then, whammo. I be-
her husband, who came to meet her. She fol-
came very dizzy and I immediately thought,
lowed him home, feeling anxious all the way.
Oh no, here it comes. I had a really bad
Now, Julie has these feelings in many situa-
panic attack. All I could think of was why
tions. She describes her panic attacks as feel-
didnt I stay on the medication.
ings of unreality, detachment, shortness of
breath, a racing heart, and a general fear of the
I started to lower my dose of Xanax. I was unknown. It is the unreality that scares her the
OK for the first couple of days. . . . I felt re- most. Consequently, Julie is sensitive to any-
ally good. Then, when I woke up on Friday thing that produces unreal types of feelings,
morning, I felt strange. My head felt really such as the semiconsciousness that occurs just
tight and I worried about having the same before falling asleep, the period when daylight
old feelings all over again. The last thing I changes to night, bright lights, concentrating
want to do is to go through that again. So I on the same thing for long periods of time, al-
took my usual dose of Xanax and, within a cohol or drugs, and being anxious in general.
few minutes, I felt pretty good again. I need Even though she has a prescription for
18 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

Klonopin (a high-potency benzodiazepine), she ondary to her worry about panicking and are
rarely, if ever, uses it because of her general fear not excessive. She has some difficulty concen-
of being under the influence of a drug, or of trating but is generally able to function at home
feeling an altered state of consciousness. She and at work, because of the familiarity of her
wants to be as alert as possible at all times, but environment and the safety she feels in the
she keeps the Klonopin with her in the event presence of her husband. Julie works part-time
that she has no other way of managing her as the manager of a business that she and her
panic. She does not leave home without the husband own. She sometimes becomes de-
Klonopin. Julie is very sensitive to her body in pressed about her panic and the limitations on
general; she becomes scared of anything that how far she can travel. Occasionally she feels
feels a little different than usual. Even coffee, hopeless about the future, doubting whether
which she used to enjoy, is distressing to her she will ever be able to escape the anxiety. Al-
now because of its agitating and racy effects. though the feelings of hopelessness and the
She was never a big exerciser, but to think of teariness never last than more than a few days,
exerting herself now is also scary. Julie reports Julie has generally had a low-grade depressed
that she is constantly waiting for the next panic mood since her life became restricted by the
attack to occur. She avoids freeways, driving on panic attacks.
familiar surface streets only. She limits herself Julies mother and her uncle both had panic
to a 10-mile radius from home. She avoids attacks when they were younger. Julie is now
crowds and large groups as well, partly because worried that her oldest child is showing signs
of the feeling of too much stimulation and of being overly anxious, because he is hesitant
partly because she is afraid to panic in front of about trying new things or spending time away
others. In general, she prefers to be with her from home.
husband or her mother. However, she can do
most things as long as she is within her
safety region. ASSESSMENT
Julie describes how she differs from the way
she used to be: how weak and scared she is A functional behavioral analysis depends on
now. The only other incident similar to her cur- several different modes of assessment, which
rent panic attacks occurred in her early 20s, we describe next.
when she had a negative reaction to smoking
marijuana. Julie became very scared of the feel-
Interviews
ing of losing control and feared that she would
never return to reality. She has not taken drugs An in-depth interview is the first step in estab-
since then. Otherwise, there is no history of se- lishing diagnostic features and the profile of
rious medical conditions, or any previous psy- symptomatic and behavioral responses. Several
chological treatment. Julie had some separa- semistructured and fully structured interviews
tion anxiety and was shy as a young child and exist. The Anxiety Disorders Interview
throughout her teens. However, her social anx- ScheduleFourth Edition (ADIS-IV; Di Nardo,
iety improved throughout her 20s to the point Brown, & Barlow, 1994) primarily assesses
that until the onset of her panic attacks, she anxiety disorders, as well as mood and
was mostly very comfortable around people. somatoform disorders. Psychotic and drug con-
Since the onset of her panic attacks, Julie has ditions are screened by this instrument also.
become concerned that others will notice that The ADIS-IV facilitates gathering the necessary
she appears anxious. However, her social anxi- information to make a differential diagnosis
ety is limited to panic attacks and does not re- among anxiety disorders and offers a means to
flect a broader social phobia. distinguish between clinical and subclinical
In general, Julies appetite is good, but her presentations of a disorder. Data on the fre-
sleep is restless. At least once a week she wakes quency, intensity, and duration of panic at-
abruptly in the middle of the night, feeling tacks, as well as details on avoidance behavior,
short of breath and scared, and has great diffi- are embedded within the ADIS-IV; this infor-
culty going to sleep when her husband travels. mation is necessary for tailoring treatment to
In addition to worrying about her panic at- each individuals presentation. The value of
tacks, Julie worries about her husband and her structured interviews is in their contribution to
children, although these latter worries are sec- a differential diagnosis and interrater reliabil-
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 19

ity. Interrater agreement ranges from satisfac- The same types of diagnostic questioning are
tory to excellent for the various anxiety disor- useful for distinguishing between PDA and
ders using the ADIS-IV (Brown, Di Nardo, claustrophobia. Other differential diagnostic
Lehman, & Campbell, 2001). issues can arise with respect to somatoform dis-
Similarly, the Schizophrenia and Affective orders, real medical conditions, and avoidant
Disorders ScheduleLifetime Version (modi- or dependent personality disorders.
fied for the study of anxiety) produces reliable
diagnoses for most of the anxiety disorders
Medical Evaluation
(generalized anxiety disorder and simple
phobia are the exceptions) (Manuzza, Fyer, A medical evaluation is generally recom-
Liebowitz, & Klein, 1990), as does the Struc- mended, because several medical conditions
tured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID), should be ruled out before assigning the diag-
which covers all of the mental disorders (First, nosis of PD/PDA. These include thyroid condi-
Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1994). tions, caffeine or amphetamine intoxication,
Differential diagnosis is sometimes difficult drug withdrawal, or pheochromocytoma (a
because, as described earlier, panic is a ubiqui- rare adrenal gland tumor). Furthermore, cer-
tous phenomenon (Barlow, 1988) that occurs tain medical conditions can exacerbate PD/
across a wide variety of emotional disorders. It PDA, although it is likely to continue even
is not uncommon for persons with specific when the symptoms are under medical control.
phobias, social phobia, generalized anxiety dis- Mitral valve prolapse, asthma, allergies, and
order, obsessivecompulsive disorder, and post- hypoglycemia fall into this latter category. Ac-
traumatic stress disorder to report panic at- cording to the model described earlier, these
tacks. For Julie, there was a differential medical conditions exacerbate PD/PDA to the
diagnostic question regarding social phobia extent that they elicit the feared physical sensa-
and PDA. Shown in Figure 1.1 are the ADIS-IV tions. For example, mitral valve prolapse some-
questions that addressed this differentiation times produces the sensation of a heart flutter,
(Julies answers are in italics). asthma produces shortness of breath, and
As demonstrated in Figure 1.1, Julie experi- hypoglycemia produces dizziness and weak-
ences panic attacks in social situations and is ness, all of which overlap with symptoms of
concerned about being negatively evaluated by panic and may therefore become conditioned
others if her anxiety becomes visibly apparent. cues for panic.
However, despite her history of shyness, Julies
current social discomfort is based primarily on
Self-Monitoring
the possibility of panicking. Because of this,
and because she meets the other criteria for Self-monitoring is a very important part of as-
PDA (i.e., uncued/nonsocial panic attacks and sessment and treatment for panic disorder
pervasive apprehension about future panic at- agoraphobia. Retrospective recall of past epi-
tacks), the social distress is best subsumed sodes of panic and anxiety, especially when
under the domain of PDA. If Julie reported that made under anxious conditions, may inflate
she experiences panic attacks in social situa- estimates of panic frequency and intensity
tions only, or that she worries about panic at- (Margraf et al., 1987; Rapee, Craske, &
tacks in social situations only, then a diagnosis Barlow, 1990). Moreover, such inflation may
of social phobia would be more probable. A re- contribute to apprehension about future panic.
port of uncued panic attacks, as well as self- In contrast, ongoing self-monitoring generally
consciousness about things that she might do yields more accurate, less inflated estimates
or say in social situations regardless of the oc- (for a comprehensive review of self-monitoring
currence of panic, would be consistent with a for panic and anxiety, see Craske & Tsao,
dual diagnosis of PDA and social phobia. In 1999). Also, ongoing self-monitoring is be-
general, individuals with PDA may continue to lieved to contribute to an objective self-
feel anxious even when playing a passive role in awareness. Objective self-monitoring replaces
a social setting, whereas a patient with social negative affect-laden self-statements such as I
phobia is more likely to feel relaxed when he or feel horrible. This is the worst its ever been
she is not the center of attention and does not my whole body is out of control with My
anticipate being evaluated or judged (Dattilio anxiety level is 6. My symptoms include tremu-
& Salas-Auvert, 2000). lousness, dizziness, unreal feelings, and short-
20 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

Parts of ADIS-IV Panic Disorder Section

Do you currently have times when you feel a sudden rush of intense fear or discomfort? Yes.

In what kinds of situations do you have those feelings? Driving, especially on freeways . . . alone at
home . . . at parties or in crowds of people.

Did you ever have those feelings come from out of the blue, for no apparent reason, or in
situations where you did not expect them to occur? Yes.

How long does it usually take for the rush of fear/discomfort to reach its peak level? It varies,
sometimes a couple of seconds and at other times it seems to build more slowly.

How long does the fear/discomfort usually last at its peak level? Depends on where I am at the
time. If it happens when Im alone, sometimes it is over within a few minutes or even seconds. If Im in a
crowd, then it seems to last until I leave.

In the last month, how much have you been worried about, or how fearful have you been about
having another panic attack?
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
No worry Rarely worried/mild Occasionally Frequently worried/ Constantly worried/
no fear fear worried/moderate severe fear extreme fear
fear

Parts of ADIS-IV Social Phobia Section

In social situations, where you might be observed or evaluated by others, or when meeting new
people, do you feel fearful, anxious, or nervous? Yes.

Are you overly concerned that you might do and/or say something that might embarrass or
humiliate yourself in front of others, or that others may think badly of you? Yes.

What are you concerned will happen in these situations? That others will notice that I am anxious.
My face turns white and my eyes look strange when I panic. I am worried that Ill flip out in front of
them, and they wont know what to do.

Are you anxious about these situations because you are afraid that you will have an unexpected
panic attack? Yes (either a panic or that Ill feel unreal).

Other than when you are exposed to these situations, have you experienced an unexpected rush
of fear/anxiety? Yes.

FIGURE 1.1. Julies responses to ADIS-IV questions.

ness of breathand this episode lasted 10 min- about panic are monitored with the Daily
utes. Objective self-awareness usually reduces Mood Record shown in Figure 1.3. This record
negative affect. Finally, self-monitoring pro- is completed at the end of each day. Finally, ac-
vides feedback for judging progress and useful tivities may be recorded by logging daily excur-
material for in-session discussions. sions in a diary, or by checking off activities
Panic attacks are recorded in the Panic At- completed from an agoraphobia checklist.
tack Record, a version of which is shown in A common problem with self-monitoring is
Figure 1.2. This record is to be completed as noncompliance. Sometimes noncompliance is
soon as possible after a panic attack occurs; due to misunderstanding or lack of perceived
therefore, it is carried on-person (wallet size). credibility in self-monitoring. Most often, how-
Daily levels of anxiety, depression, and worry ever, noncompliance is due to anticipation of
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 21

Date 2/16/06 Time began 5:20 P.M.

Triggers Home alone and shortness of breath


Expected x Unexpected

Maximum Fear 012345678910


None Mild Moderate Strong Extreme

Check all symptoms present to at least a mild degree:


Chest pain or discomfort Sweating x
Heart racing/palpitations/pounding x Nausea/upset stomach
Short of breath x Dizzy/unsteady/lightheaded/faint
Shaking/trembling x Chills/hot flushes
Numbness/tingling Feelings of unreality x
Feelings of choking Fear of dying
Fear of losing control/going crazy x

Thoughts: I am going crazy, I will lose control


Behaviors: Called my mother

FIGURE 1.2. Julies Panic Attack Record.

more anxiety as a result of monitoring. This is that anxiety about self-monitoring would sub-
particularly true for individuals whose pre- side with perseverance at self-monitoring, and
ferred style of coping is to distract themselves emphasis on objective versus subjective self-
as much as possible, and to avoid quiet monitoring were helpful for Julie. In addition,
times, when thoughts of panic might become cognitive restructuring in the first few sessions
overwhelming: Why should I make myself helped Julie to be less afraid of the feelings of
worse by asking myself how bad I feel? In unreality; therefore, she was less afraid to be
Julies case, the self-monitoring task was partic- reminded of those feelings by self-monitoring.
ularly difficult, because explicit reminders of Finally, therapist attention to the self-monitored
her anxiety elicited strong concerns about los- information and corrective feedback about the
ing touch with reality. Prompting, reassurance method of self-monitoring at the start of each

012345678910
None Mild Moderate Strong Extreme

Average Average Average worry


Date anxiety depression about panic
2/16 7 5 7
2/17 5 4 5
2/18 4 4 5
2/19 4 3 4
2/20 4 4 5
2/21 2 1 1
2/22 2 2 2

FIGURE 1.3. Julies Daily Mood Record.


22 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

treatment session reinforced Julies self- and targeted manner relevant to anxiety and
monitoring. anxiety disorders compared to more general
locus-of-control scales. A revised 15-item ver-
sion yields three factors, Emotion Control,
Standardized Inventories
Threat Control, and Stress Control, with a
Several standardized self-report inventories higher-order dimension of perceived control
provide useful information for treatment plan- (Brown, White, Forsyth, & Barlow, 2004).
ning and are sensitive markers of therapeutic Changes in this scale from pre to posttreatment
change. The Anxiety Sensitivity Index (Reiss, predicted reductions in comorbidity at follow-
Peterson, Gursky, & McNally, 1986) has re- up in one study (Craske et al., 2007). Finally,
ceived wide acceptance as a trait measure of measures of interpersonal context include the
threatening beliefs about bodily sensations. Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1976), and
It has good psychometric properties and the Marital Happiness Scale (Azrin, Naster, &
tends to discriminate between panic disorder Jones, 1973).
agoraphobia and other types of anxiety disor-
ders (e.g., Taylor et al., 1992; Telch, Sherman,
Behavioral Tests
& Lucas, 1989), especially the Physical Con-
cerns subscale (Zinbarg et al., 1997). More The behavioral test is a useful measure of de-
specific information about which particular gree of avoidance of specific interoceptive cues
bodily sensations are feared the most and what and external situations. Behavioral approach
specific misappraisals occur most often may be tests can be standardized or individually tai-
obtained from the Body Sensations and Agora- lored. The standardized behavioral test for
phobia Cognitions Questionnaire (Chambless agoraphobic avoidance usually involves walk-
et al., 1984). The Mobility Inventory (Cham- ing or driving a particular route, such as a 1-
bless, Caputo, Gracely, Jasin, & Williams, mile loop around the clinic setting. Standard-
1985) lists agoraphobic situations rated in ized behavioral tests for anxiety about physical
terms of degree avoidance when alone and sensations involve exercises that induce panic-
when accompanied. This instrument is very like symptoms, such as spinning in a circle,
useful for establishing in vivo exposure hierar- running in place, hyperventilating, and breath-
chies. Measures of trait anxiety include the ing through a straw (Barlow & Craske, 2006).
StateTrait Anxiety Inventory (Speilberger, Anxiety levels are rated at regular intervals
Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983) and throughout the behavioral tests, and actual dis-
the Beck Anxiety Inventory (Beck, Epstein, tance or length of time is measured. The disad-
Brown, & Steer, 1988). vantage of standardized behavioral tests is that
In addition, we have developed two stan- the specific task may not be relevant to all pa-
dardized self-report inventories that are useful tients (e.g., a 1-mile walk or running in place
for panic disorder and agoraphobia. The first, may be only mildly anxiety provoking); hence,
the Albany Panic and Phobia Questionnaire the value of individually tailored tasks. In the
(Rapee, Craske, & Barlow, 1995), is a 32-item case of agoraphobia, this usually entails at-
questionnaire designed to assess fear and tempts at three to five individualized situations
avoidance of activities that produce feared that the patient has identified as ranging from
bodily sensations, as well as more typical ago- Somewhat difficult to Extremely difficult, such
raphobia and social situations. Factor analyses as driving two exits on freeway, waiting in a
confirmed three distinct factors labeled Agora- bank line, or shopping in a local supermarket
phobia, Social Phobia, and Interoceptive Fears. for 15 minutes. For anxiety about physical sen-
The questionnaire has adequate psychometric sations, individually tailored behavioral tests
properties and is useful in profiling agora- entail exercises designed specifically to induce
phobic versus interoceptive avoidance. The sec- the sensations feared most by a given patient,
ond, the Anxiety Control Questionnaire, is a and may include a tongue depressor to induce
30-item scale that assesses perceived lack of sensations of gagging, smells to induce sensa-
control over anxiety-related events and occur- tions of nausea, or nose plugs to induce sensa-
rences, such as internal emotional reactions or tions of difficulty breathing. As with standard-
externally threatening cues (Rapee, Craske, ized tests, ongoing levels of anxiety and degree
Brown, & Barlow, 1996). This scale is designed of approach behavior are measured in relation
to assess locus of control, but in a more specific to individually tailored behavioral tests.
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 23

Individually tailored behavioral tests are or Im sure my blood pressure is so high that I
more informative for clinical practice, although could have a stroke at any minute. Finally,
they confound between-subject comparisons baseline levels of physiological functioning,
for research purposes. On the one hand, stan- which are sometimes dysregulated in anxious
dardized and individually tailored behavioral individuals, may be sensitive measures of treat-
tests are susceptible to demand biases for both ment outcome (e.g., Craske, Golinelli, et al.,
fear and avoidance prior to treatment, and im- 2005).
provement after treatment (Borkovec, Weerts,
& Bernstein, 1977). On the other hand, behav-
Functional Analysis
ioral tests are an important supplement to self-
report of agoraphobic avoidance, because pa- The various methods of assessment provide the
tients tend to underestimate what they can material for a full functional analysis for Julie.
actually achieve (Craske et al., 1988). In addi- Specifically, the topography of her panic attack
tion, behavioral tests often reveal important in- is as follows: most common symptoms include
formation for treatment planning of which the a feeling of unreality, shortness of breath, and
individual is not yet fully aware. For example, racing heart; average frequency is three per
the tendency to remain close to supports, such week; each panic attack on average lasts from a
as railings or walls, may not be apparent until few seconds to 5 minutes, if Julie is not in a
one observes the patient walk through a shop- crowd; in terms of apprehension, Julie worries
ping mall. In Julies case, the importance of about panic 75% of the day; and she has
changes from daylight to night was not appar- mostly expected panic attacks but some unex-
ent until she was asked to drive on a section of pected ones as well. Julie has both situational
road as a behavioral test. Her response was and internal antecedents to her panic attacks.
that it was too late in the day to drive, because The situational antecedents include driving on
dusk made her feel as if things were unreal. freeways; crowds of people; being alone, espe-
Similarly, it was not until Julie completed a cially at night; restaurants; dusk; reading and
behavioral test that we recognized the impor- concentrating for long periods of time; and aer-
tance of air-conditioning when Julie was driv- obic activity. The internal antecedents include
ing. Julie believed that the cool air blowing on heart rate fluctuations, lightheaded feelings,
her face helped her to remain in touch with re- hunger feelings, weakness due to lack of food,
ality. Finally, we noticed that her physical pos- thoughts of the big one happening, thoughts
ture while driving was a factor that contributed of not being able to cope with this for much
to anxiety: Julies shoulders were hunched, she longer, and anger. Her misappraisals about
leaned toward the wheel, and she held the panic attack symptoms include beliefs that she
wheel very tightly. All of these were targeted in will never return to normality, that she will go
the treatment: driving at dusk was included in crazy or lose control, and that others will think
her hierarchy; air-conditioning was regarded as she is weird. Her behavioral reactions to panic
a safety signal from which she should be attacks include escape behaviors such as pull-
weaned; and driving in a more relaxed position ing off to the side of the road, leaving restau-
was part of mastery exposure. rants and other crowded places, calling her
husband or mother, and checking for her
Klonopin. Her behavioral reactions to the an-
Psychophysiology
ticipation of panic attacks include avoidance of
Ongoing physiological measures are not very driving long distances alone, driving on unfa-
practical tools for clinicians, but they can pro- miliar roads and freeways or at dusk, crowded
vide important information. In particular, the areas, exercise, quiet time with nothing to do,
discrepancy described earlier between reports and doing one thing for a long period of time.
of symptoms and actual physiological arousal In addition, she tries not to think about anxiety
(i.e., report of heart rate acceleration in ab- or feelings of unreality. Her safety signals and
sence of actual heart rate acceleration) may safety-seeking behaviors include having her
serve as a therapeutic demonstration of the role Klonopin on hand at all times, always knowing
of attention and cognition in symptom produc- the location of husband, and having the air-
tion. Similarly, actual recordings provide data conditioning on. The consequences of her PDA
to disconfirm misappraisals such as My heart affect her family: Julies husband is concerned
feels like its going so fast that it will explode and supportive, but her mother thinks she
24 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

should pull herself together because its all in Self-Monitoring


her head. In addition, Julie works but has cut
back the number of hours, and she travels and Self-monitoring is considered essential to the
socializes much less. Her general mood in- personal scientist model of cognitive-behavioral
cludes some difficulty concentrating and sleep- therapy. Self-monitoring is introduced as a way
ing, restlessness, headaches, and muscular to enhance objective self-awareness and in-
pains and aches. In addition, she is occasionally crease accuracy in self-observation. As noted
tearful, sad, and hopeless, and generally feels earlier, patients are asked to keep at least two
down. types of records. The first, a Panic Attack Re-
cord, is completed as soon after each panic at-
tack as possible; this record provides a de-
COMPONENTS OF scription of cues, maximal distress, symptoms,
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL THERAPY thoughts, and behaviors. The second, a Daily
Mood Record, is completed at the end of each
The components of the cognitive-behavioral day to record overall or average levels of anxi-
treatment described in this section are integrat- ety, depression, and whatever else is considered
ed into a session-by-session treatment program important to record. Additionally, patients may
in the next section. keep a daily record of activities or situations
completed or avoided.

Education
Breathing Retraining
The treatment begins with education about the
nature of panic disorder, the causes of panic Breathing retraining is a central component
and anxiety, and the ways panic and anxiety early on in the development of panic-control
are perpetuated by feedback loops among treatments, because many panic patients de-
physical, cognitive, and behavioral response scribe symptoms of hyperventilation as being
systems. In addition, specific descriptions of very similar to their panic attack symptoms. It
the psychophysiology of the fightflight re- is noteworthy, however, that hyperventilation
sponse are provided, as well as an explanation symptom report does not always accurately
of the adaptive value of the various physiologi- represent hyperventilation physiology: only
cal changes that occur during panic and anxi- 50% or fewer patients show actual reductions
ety. The purpose of this education is to correct in end-tidal carbon dioxide values during panic
the common myths and misconceptions about attacks (Hibbert & Pilsbury, 1989; Holt & An-
panic symptoms (i.e., beliefs about going crazy, drews, 1989; Hornsveld, Garssen, Fiedelij
dying, or losing control) that contribute to Dop, & van Spiegel, 1990).
panic and anxiety. The survival value of alarm In early conceptualizations, panic attacks
reactions (panic attacks) is emphasized were related to stress-induced respiratory
throughout. changes that either provoke fear because they
Education also distinguishes between the are perceived as threatening or augment fear al-
state of anxiety and the emotion of fear/ ready elicited by other phobic stimuli (Clark,
panic, both conceptually and in terms of its Salkovskis, & Chalkley, 1985). Several studies
three response modes (subjective, physiologi- illustrated a positive effect of breathing retrain-
cal, and behavioral). This distinction is cen- ing. Kraft and Hoogduin (1984) found that six
tral to the model of panic disorder and to the biweekly sessions of breathing retraining and
remainder of the treatment. Anxiety is viewed progressive relaxation reduced panic attacks
as a state of preparation for future threat, from 10 to 4 per week, but were no more effec-
whereas panic is the fightflight emotion elic- tive than either repeated hyperventilation plus
ited by imminent threat. Panic/fear is charac- control of symptoms by breathing into a bag or
terized by (1) perception or awareness of im- identification of life stressors and problem
minent threat, (2) sudden autonomic solving. Other studies were uncontrolled re-
discharge, and (3) fightflight behavior. Anxi- ports that combined breathing retraining
ety is characterized by (1) perception or and cognitive restructuring, sometimes with
awareness of future threat, (2) chronic ten- in vivo exposure (Clark et al., 1985; Rapee,
sion, and (3) cautiousness, avoidance, and 1985; Salkovskis, Warwick, Clark, & Wessels,
disruption of performance. 1986).
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 25

More recently, the value of breathing retrain- countable for therapeutic gains are further
ing has been questioned. For example, it is un- clouded in the case of applied forms of relax-
clear whether breathing retraining alone is ther- ation given the involvement of exposure-based
apeutic for agoraphobia, and several studies procedures as anxiety-provoking situations are
suggest that the addition of breathing retraining faced.
alone does not improve upon in vivo exposure Ost (1988) reported very favorable results
(e.g., de Beurs, van Balkom, Lange, Koele, & van with applied PMR: 100% of an applied PMR
Dyck, 1995). We found breathing retraining to group (N = 8) were panic-free after 14 sessions
be slightly less effective than interoceptive expo- in comparison to 71.7% of a nonapplied PMR
sure when each was added to cognitive restruc- group (N = 8). Furthermore, the results of the
turing and in vivo exposure (Craske, Rowe, first group were maintained at follow-up (ap-
Lewin, & Noriega-Dimitri, 1997), and in an- proximately 19 months after treatment com-
other study, the inclusion of breathing retraining pletion): All members of the applied PMR
resulted in poorer outcomes than cognitive- group were classified as high end state (i.e.,
behavioral therapy without breathing retrain- nonsymptomatic) at follow-up, compared to
ing, although the findings were not robust 25% of the nonapplied PMR group. Michelson
(Schmidt et al., 2000). From their review of effi- and colleagues (1990) combined applied PMR
cacy and mechanisms of action, Garssen, de with breathing retraining and cognitive train-
Ruiter, and van Dyck (1992) concluded that ing for 10 panickers. By treatment completion,
breathing retraining probably effects change not all subjects were free of spontaneous panics,
through breathing per se, but through distrac- all but one were free of panic attacks alto-
tion and/or a sense of control. Given the recent gether, and all met criteria for high end-state
recognition that tolerance of fear and anxiety functioning. However, the specific contribution
may be a more critical learning experience than of applied PMR to these results is not known.
the elimination of fear (see Eifert & Forsyth, Two subsequent studies by Ost (Ost &
2005), breathing retraining has been Westling, 1995; Ost, Westling, & Hellstrom,
deemphasized, because it may become a method 1993) indicate that applied relaxation was as
of avoidance of physical symptoms or a safety effective as in vivo exposure and cognitive ther-
behavior, and thereby be antitherapeutic. When apy. In contrast, we (Barlow et al., 1989) found
it is included in the treatment, it is essential that that applied PMR was relatively ineffective for
patients not rely upon breathing retraining as a panic attacks, although we excluded all forms
method of avoidance or safety seeking. of interoceptive exposure from the hierarchy of
tasks to which PMR was applied, which was
not necessarily the case in the studies by Ost.
Applied Relaxation
Clark and colleagues (1994) found that cogni-
A form of relaxation known as applied relax- tive therapy was superior to applied PMR
ation has shown good results as a treatment for when conducted with equal amounts of in vivo
panic attacks. Applied relaxation entails train- exposure, whereas Beck, Stanley, Baldwin,
ing patients in progressive muscle relaxation Deagle, and Averill (1994) found very few dif-
(PMR) until they are skilled in cue control re- ferences between cognitive therapy and PMR
laxation, at which point relaxation is used as a when each was administered without exposure
coping skill for practicing exposure to items procedures.
from a hierarchy of anxiety-provoking tasks. A
theoretical basis for relaxation as a treatment
Cognitive Restructuring
for panic attacks has not been elaborated be-
yond the provision of a somatic counter- Initially, cognitive therapy for panic disorder
response to the muscular tension that is likely and agoraphobia did not directly target
to occur during anxiety and panic. However, misappraisals of bodily sensations, but instead
evidence does not lend support to this notion fostered coping self-statements in anxiety-
(Rupert, Dobbins, & Mathew, 1981). An alter- provoking situations. Michelson, Mavissa-
native suggestion is that, as with breathing re- kalian, and Marchione (1985) published the
training, fear and anxiety are reduced to the ex- first of their series of investigations comparing
tent that relaxation provides a sense of control different behavioral treatments to various
or mastery (Bandura, 1977; Rice & Blanchard, coping-oriented cognitive treatments for agora-
1982). The procedures and mechanisms ac- phobia. They compared paradoxical intention,
26 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

graduated exposure, and progressive deep mus- restructuring combined with breathing retrain-
cle relaxation, although all participants con- ing and/or interoceptive exposure is as effective
ducted self-directed in vivo exposure between as self-directed in vivo exposure (Craske et al.,
sessions. At posttreatment and 3 months later, 2003; de Ruiter, Garssen, Rijken, &
paradoxical intention demonstrated equivalent Kraaimaat, 1989; Rijken, Kraaimaat, de
rates of improvement, but significantly more Ruiter, & Garssen, 1992) for individuals with
participants remained symptomatic compared varying levels of agoraphobia. Other studies
to those treated with graduated exposure and found that cognitive therapy is slightly less ef-
relaxation. Michelson, Mavissakalian, and fective than guided mastery and in vivo expo-
Marchione (1988) replicated this design with sure for agoraphobia (Bouchard et al., 1996;
almost twice as many participants. Contrary to Williams & Falbo, 1996). Furthermore, several
the first study, few significant differences were studies found no added benefit when cognitive
detected between treatments. Lack of differ- therapy that targeted misappraisals of bodily
ences was replicated in a third study sensations was added to in vivo exposure (Ost,
(Michelson et al., 1990). Thus, coping-oriented Thulin, & Ramnero, 2004; van den Hout,
cognitive treatments appeared to be as effective Arntz, & Hoekstra, 1994).
as behaviorally oriented treatments, although Behavioral exposure-based strategies are
the cognitive treatments were all heavily con- usually included in cognitive therapy as vehi-
taminated by behavioral self-directed expo- cles for obtaining data that disconfirm
sure. In a slightly different design, Murphy, misappraisals. The importance of exposure-
Michelson, Marchione, Marchione, and Testa based strategies to the effectiveness of cognitive
(1998) compared cognitive therapy combined therapy is not known, although 2 weeks of fo-
with therapist- and self-directed exposure, re- cused cognitive therapy with antiexposure in-
laxation combined with therapist- and self- structions reduced panic attacks in all but one
directed exposure, and just therapist and self- of a series of seven cases in a single-case, mul-
directed exposure. Again, overall there were tiple baseline design (Salkovskis, Clark, &
few significant differences, although the condi- Hackmann, 1991).
tion that included cognitive therapy yielded the In terms of implementation, cognitive ther-
most potent and stable changes. Without the apy begins to provide a treatment rationale
self-directed exposure component, Emmel- with discussion of the role of thoughts in gener-
kamp and colleagues found that coping- ating emotions. Next, thoughts are recognized
oriented cognitive therapy (rationalemotive as hypotheses rather than fact, and are there-
therapy and self-instruction training) was sig- fore open to questioning and challenge. De-
nificantly less effective than prolonged in vivo tailed self-monitoring of emotions and associ-
exposure for agoraphobia on an array of ated cognitions is instituted to identify specific
behavioral and self-report measures of anxiety beliefs, appraisals, and assumptions. Once rele-
and avoidance (Emmelkamp, Brilman, Kuiper, vant cognitions are identified, they are catego-
& Mersch, 1986; Emmelkamp, Kuipers, & rized into types of typical errors that occur
Eggeraat, 1978; Emmelkamp & Mersch, during heightened emotion, such as over-
1982). estimations of risk of negative events or
Cognitive therapy that targets misappraisals catastrophizing of meaning of events. The pro-
of bodily sensations is clearly effective with cess of categorization, or labeling of thoughts,
samples with mild to moderate levels of agora- is consistent with a personal scientist model
phobia, producing results that are either as ef- and facilitates an objective perspective by
fective as or superior to applied relaxation which the validity of the thoughts can be evalu-
(Arntz & van den Hout, 1996; Beck et al., ated. Thus, in labeling the type of cognitive dis-
1994; Clark et al., 1994; Ost & Westling, tortion, the patient is encouraged to use an em-
1995; Stanley et al., 1996). Results with more pirical approach to examine the validity of his
severe levels of agoraphobia are mixed. One or her thoughts by considering all of the avail-
study indicated that cognitive therapy targeting able evidence. Therapists use Socratic question-
misappraisals of bodily sensations is as effec- ing to help patients make guided discoveries
tive as guided mastery exposure delivered in- and question their anxious thoughts. Next,
tensively over 6 weeks for individuals with more evidence-based alternative hypotheses are
moderate to severe agoraphobia (Hoffart, generated. In addition to surface-level apprais-
1995), and other studies showed that cognitive als (e.g., That person is frowning at me be-
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 27

cause I look foolish), core-level beliefs or up assessment: 76% of the intensive group ver-
schemas (e.g., I am not strong enough to with- sus 35% of the graded group reported
stand further distress or I am unlikable) are themselves to be completely free of symptoms.
questioned in the same way. Importantly, cog- When 104 subjects were added to the intensive
nitive restructuring is not intended as a direct exposure format, the same results were ob-
means of minimizing fear, anxiety, or unpleas- tained. Of 129 subjects, 78% were reportedly
ant symptoms. Instead, cognitive restructuring completely symptom-free 5 years later. This
is intended to correct distorted thinking; even- dramatic set of results suggests that an in-
tually fear and anxiety are expected to subside, tensive approach, which is likely to produce
but their diminution is not the first goal of cog- higher levels of arousal than a graduated ap-
nitive therapy. proach, can be very beneficial (at least when
conducted in a massed format). Unfortunately,
the validity of the outcome measures in this
Exposure
study is somewhat questionable, and replica-
Exposure is a critical phase of treatment and tion by independent investigators has yet to be
once begun, is a major focus of treatment ses- reported.
sions as well as between treatment session Critical to in vivo exposure is the removal of
homework, since limited exposure practice is safety signals and safety behaviors. Examples
of small benefit and may even be detrimental. of safety signals include other people, water,
The exposure is designed to disconfirm mis- money (to call for help), empty or full medica-
appraisals and extinguish conditioned emo- tion bottles, exit signs, and familiar landmarks
tional responses to external situations and con- when traveling. Safety behaviors similarly pro-
texts, through in vivo exposure, as well as to vide a sense of safety, and include seeking reas-
bodily sensations, through interoceptive expo- surance or checking for exits. Reliance on
sure. safety signals and safety behaviors attenuate
distress in the short term but maintain exces-
sive anxiety in the long term. With the thera-
In Vivo Exposure pists guidance, the patient identifies and finds
In vivo exposure refers to repeated and system- ways gradually to eliminate his or her own
atic real-life exposure, in this case, to agora- safety signals and behaviors. In addition, in
phobic situations. As indicated from the studies vivo exposure is eventually combined with
reviewed earlier, a long history of research has interoceptive exposure, by deliberately induc-
established the efficacy of in vivo exposure for ing feared sensations in feared situations.
agoraphobia. The amount of time devoted to in vivo expo-
Most often, in vivo exposure is conducted in sure is very dependent on the patients agora-
a graduated manner, proceeding from the least phobia profile. Obviously, more time is needed
to the most anxiety-provoking situations on an for patients with more severe agoraphobia.
avoidance hierarchy. However, there is some Also, as reviewed earlier, evidence indicates
evidence to suggest that intensive or ungradu- that inclusion of significant others in the treat-
ated exposure may be effective. In a study by ment process can improve treatment outcomes
Feigenbaum (1988), treatment sessions were (e.g., Cerny et al., 1987). The benefit obtained
conducted in a massed format over the course from involving significant others may depend
of 610 consecutive days. One group received on the pervasiveness of agoraphobia and the
ungraded exposure (N = 25), beginning with extent to which family roles and interactions
the most feared items from avoidance hierar- have been affected by or contribute to the
chies. Another group received graded exposure agoraphobic pattern.
(N = 23), beginning with the least feared hier-
archy items. Approximately one-third of this
severely agoraphobic sample was housebound
Interoceptive Exposure
at initial assessment. At posttreatment and 8 In interoceptive exposure, the goal is to deliber-
months later, the conditions proved to be ately induce feared physical sensations a suffi-
equally effective (although, intriguingly, the cient number of times, and long enough each
graded group reported the treatment to be time so that misappraisals about the sensations
more distressing). However, ungraded expo- are disconfirmed and conditioned anxiety re-
sure was clearly superior at the 5-year follow- sponses are extinguished. A series of studies
28 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

have reported on the effects of interoceptive ex- breathing retraining, cognitive therapy, and in
posure independent of other therapeutic strate- vivo exposure for individuals with varying lev-
gies. Early on, Bonn, Harrison, and Rees els of agoraphobia. The condition that in-
(1971) and Haslam (1974) observed successful cluded interoceptive exposure was slightly su-
reduction in reactivity with repeated infusions perior to breathing retraining at posttreatment
of sodium lactate (a drug that produces panic- and 6 months later (Craske et al., 1997). Simi-
type bodily sensations). However, panic was larly, Ito, Noshirvani, Basoglu, and Marks
not monitored in these investigations. Griez (1996) found a trend for those who added
and van den Hout (1986) compared six ses- interoceptive exposure to their self-directed in
sions of graduated CO2 inhalations to a treat- vivo exposure and breathing retraining to be
ment regimen of propranolol (a beta-blocker more likely to achieve at least a 50% improve-
chosen because it suppresses symptoms in- ment in phobic fear and avoidance. Recently,
duced by CO2 inhalations), both conducted an intensive, 8-day treatment with a sensation-
over the course of 2 weeks. CO2 inhalation focused approach was developed for individu-
treatment resulted in a mean reduction from 12 als with moderate to severe agoraphobia, and
to 4 panic attacks, which was superior to the initial results are promising (Morisette, Spiegel,
results from propranolol. In addition, inhala- & Heinrichs, 2005). But breathing education,
tion treatment resulted in significantly greater breathing retraining, and repeated intero-
reductions in reported fear of sensations. A 6- ceptive exposure to hyperventilation did not in-
month follow-up assessment suggested mainte- crease the effectiveness of in vivo exposure for
nance of treatment gains, although panic fre- agoraphobia (de Beurs, Lang, van Dyck, &
quency was not reported. Beck and Shipherd Koele, 1995).
(1997) similarly found positive effects from re- Interoceptive exposure is now a standard
peated CO2 inhalations, although it had little component of cognitive-behavioral therapy for
effect on agoraphobia (Beck, Shipherd, & panic disorder (e.g., Barlow et al., 2000;
Zebb, 1997). Broocks and colleagues (1998) Craske, Lang, et al., 2005), although different
tested the effects of exercise (with once-weekly groups give different emphases to interoceptive
supportive contact from a therapist) in com- exposure, with some emphasizing it as a means
parison to clomipramine or drug placebo over for extinguishing fear responses (Barlow &
10 weeks. The exercise group was trained to Craske, 2006) and others, as a vehicle for
run 4 miles, three times per week. Despite high disconfirming misappraisals (Clark, 1996).
attrition from exercise (31%), exercise was In terms of implementation, a standard list
more effective than the drug placebo condition. of exercises, such as hyperventilating and spin-
However, clomipramine was superior to exer- ning, are used to establish a hierarchy of
cise. interoceptive exposures. With a graduated ap-
In the first comparison to other cognitive proach, exposure begins with the less distress-
and behavioral treatments, we (Barlow et al., ing physical exercises and continues with the
1989) compared applied PMR, interoceptive more distressing exercises. It is essential that
exposure plus breathing retraining and cogni- the patient endure the sensations beyond the
tive restructuring, their combination with ap- point at which they are first noticed, for at least
plied PMR, and a waiting-list control, in a sam- 30 seconds to 1 minute, because early termina-
ple with panic disorder with limited tion of the task may eliminate the opportunity
agoraphobia. The two conditions involving to learn that the sensations are not harmful and
interoceptive exposure, breathing retraining that the anxiety can be tolerated. The coping
and cognitive restructuring, were significantly skills of cognitive restructuring and slow dia-
superior to applied PMR and waiting-list con- phragmatic breathing are used after each exer-
ditions. The results were maintained 24 cise, followed by a discussion of what the pa-
months following treatment completion for the tient learned during the exercise about bodily
group receiving interoceptive exposure, breath- sensations, fear, and avoidance. These intero-
ing retraining, and cognitive restructuring ceptive exercises are practiced daily outside of
without PMR, whereas the combined group the therapy session to consolidate the process
tended to deteriorate over follow-up (Craske, of learning. Interoceptive exposure extends to
Brown, & Barlow, 1991). As already men- naturalistic activities that inherently induce so-
tioned, we compared interoceptive exposure, matic sensations (e.g., caffeine consumption,
cognitive therapy, and in vivo exposure to exercise).
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 29

Optimizing Learning during Exposure in small groups for 11 sessions. In one condi-
tion (N = 16), patients were instructed to
The ways in which learning during exposure monitor bodily sensations and thoughts objec-
therapy is optimized are open to continuing in- tively throughout in vivo exposures, and to use
vestigation. In this section, we highlight the lat- thought stopping and focusing self-statements
est developments in the research. to interrupt distraction. In a second condition
(N = 14), they were taught to use specific dis-
LENGTH OF AN EXPOSURE PRACTICE traction tasks during in vivo exposures (word
rhymes, spelling, etc.), and to use thought stop-
Expectancies regarding the likelihood of aver- ping and distracting self-statements to inter-
sive events are central to human fear condition- rupt the focus of attention upon feared bodily
ing. For example, contingency awareness (i.e., sensations and images. The treatment groups
knowledge that a specific conditional stimulus did not differ at posttreatment or at follow-up
[CS] predicts a specific unconditioned stimulus assessment, but, consistent with previous
[US]), although of debatable necessity for con- findings with obsessivecompulsive disorder
ditioned responding (cf. Lovibond & Shanks (Grayson, Foa, & Steketee, 1982), the focused
[2002] and Ohman & Mineka [2001]) is a exposure group improved significantly from
strong correlate of conditioned responding. posttreatment to follow-up, in contrast to a
Differential autonomic conditioning in particu- slight deterioration in the distracted exposure
lar is strongly associated with verbal measures group. However, the degree to which partici-
of contingency knowledge (e.g., Purkis & Lipp, pants were actually distracted versus focused
2001). Expectancies also are important for ex- was not ascertainable. Also, other results re-
tinction; extinction is posited to follow from a garding the detrimental effects of distraction
mismatch between the expectancy of an aver- during exposure therapy have been contradic-
sive event and the absence of its occurrence tory (e.g., Kamphuis & Telch, 2000; Oliver &
(Rescorla & Wagner, 1972), or from the per- Page, 2003; Rodriguez & Craske, 1995; Rose
ception of a negative change in the rate at & McGlynn, 1997). The equivocal nature of
which aversive events are associated with the the findings may derive from lack of an opera-
CS (Gallistel & Gibbon, 2000); that is, expec- tional definition of distraction, from con-
tancies for the US are violated during extinc- founds with the affective quality of the
tion. Thus, exposure tasks designed to violate distractor, and from the unknown amount of
expectancies for negative outcomes are hypoth- distraction that actually takes place.
esized to be the most effective form of exposure Nonetheless, given the recent advances in re-
(Craske & Mystkowski, 2006). Indirect evi- search, showing that neither physiological ha-
dence derived from several studies of phobic bituation nor the amount of fear reduction
samples indicates that a single, massed expo- within an exposure trial is predictive of overall
sure is more effective than a series of short ex- outcome (see Craske & Mystkowski, 2006),
posures of the same total duration, such as one and given that self-efficacy through perfor-
60-minute duration versus three 20-minute du- mance accomplishment is predictive of overall
rations of exposure (e.g., Chaplin & Levine, phobia reductions (e.g., Williams, 1992), and
1981; Marshall, 1985). Conceivably, the that toleration of fear and anxiety may be a
lengthier (massed) exposure is more effective, more critical learning experience than the elim-
because it provides sufficient time to learn that ination of fear and anxiety (see Eifert &
aversive outcomes do not occur (i.e., to dis- Forsyth, 2005), the focus now is on staying in
confirm negative outcome expectancies) the phobic situation until the specified time,
(Craske & Mystkowski, 2006). However, no when patients learn that what they are most
study to date has directly evaluated outcome worried about never or rarely happens, and/or
expectancies or manipulated exposure dura- that they can cope with the phobic stimulus
tion in relation to outcome expectancies. and tolerate the anxiety. Thus, the length of a
Related, however, is the body of work on the given exposure trial is based not on fear reduc-
role of distraction during exposure, because tion but on the conditions necessary for new
distraction in essence represents disrupted (i.e., learning, in which fear and anxiety eventually
unmassed) exposure. We (Craske, Street, & subside across trials of exposure. Essentially,
Barlow, 1989) administered therapist- and self- the level of fear or fear reduction within a given
directed exposure to patients with agoraphobia trial of exposure is no longer considered an in-
30 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

dex of learning, but a reflection of perfor- respiratory control, distraction techniques, and
mance; learning is best measured by the level of paradoxical intention. However, some subjects
anxiety experienced the next time the patient were unwilling to accept massed exposure, cre-
encounters the phobic situation or at some ating a sample selection bias. In addition,
later time. Therefore, we have moved away Chambless pointed out that her results may
from the model of Stay in the situation until lack generalization, because spaced exposure is
fear has declined to Stay in the situation un- usually interspersed with homework assign-
til you have learned what you need to learn, ments, which may increase outcome efficacy.
and sometimes that means learning that you Nevertheless, she concluded by suggesting that
can tolerate fear. Exposure tasks, therefore, the choice for massed versus spaced exposure is
are to be defined clearly in advance, indepen- the decision of the therapist and patient. Some
dent of level of fear reduction in a given day of of the contradiction arises from inconsistent
practice. For example, patients are encouraged operationalization of massed and spaced
to practice inducing sensations of shortness of scheduling across studies. Studies have com-
breath for a predetermined amount of time, pared arbitrarily chosen fixed durations and
and driving on the freeway for a predetermined schedules of exposure, and sometimes what is
distance to gain experience that disconfirms labeled as massed in one study is labeled as
what they fear most. If patients are most wor- spaced in another.
ried about their fear remaining elevated Nonetheless, given the strength of the exper-
throughout the entire exposure, then the goal imental data on spacing of learning trials for
of exposure is reframed as learning to be able nonemotional learning (Bjork & Bjork, 1992),
to tolerate a sustained level of fear. Neverthe- the evidence for superior outcomes from a
less, there may be occasions when the therapist schedule of progressively increasing durations
judges that the most effective learning comes between exposure trials in circumscribed pho-
from enduring an exposure task until fear has bias (e.g., Rowe & Craske, 1998), and the evi-
declined, such as would be the case for patients dence for substantially improved outcomes
who maintain that their fear will decrease only with monthly follow-up phone calls after
when they exit from the situation. weekly cognitive-behavioral therapy for panic
disorder in primary care settings (Craske et al.,
2006), a schedule of weekly sessions followed
SCHEDULE OF EXPOSURE PRACTICES
by progressively longer intervals between ses-
A second way of potentially optimizing expo- sions may be advisable.
sure is through the scheduling of exposure ses-
sions. Spacing between exposure days (as op-
LEVEL OF AROUSAL WITHIN
posed to the duration of a given exposure
AN EXPOSURE PRACTICE
practice) pertains to consolidation of learning.
Unfortunately, research in human samples has Clinically, on the one hand, there is wide sub-
failed simultaneously to address both massing scription to the theory that corrective learning
within exposure trials and spacing between ex- is maximal when physiological arousal is ini-
posure trials; that is, studies of spacing between tially activated, then allowed to subside within
exposure days have been conducted without and between exposure sessions (i.e., emotional
ensuring necessarily that exposure is suffi- processing theory) (Foa & McNally, 1996).
ciently lengthy within each exposure day to vi- However, recent post hoc analyses indicate that
olate negative expectancies effectively; hence, the degree to which physiological responding
the results have been mixed. Foa, Jameson, declines from the beginning to the end of an ex-
Turner, and Payne (1980) found greater decre- posure trial is not predictive of overall outcome
ments in anxiety and avoidance behavior in (see Craske & Mystkowski, 2006). In addition,
those receiving massed rather than spaced ex- empirical and theoretical developments suggest
posure sessions for agoraphobia, whereas that a certain level of sustained excitation dur-
Ramsay, Barends, Brueker, and Kruseman ing extinction training may yield even more ef-
(1966) found spaced schedules to be superior fective results upon retesting. Specifically, Cain,
to massed schedules for desensitization for spe- Blouin, and Barad (2004) have found that
cific phobias. Chambless (1990) found no dif- anxiogenic drugs such as yohimbine facilitate
ferences between weekly versus daily sessions extinction in mice, and in general suggest that
of graduated in vivo exposure and training in drugs or conditions that enhance adrenergic
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 31

transmission overcome a natural inhibitory sponses during exposure therapy, especially


constraint upon extinction. However, extant given the very direct implications for clinical
data in humans are limited to post hoc observa- practice.
tions of a positive relationship between sus- Such research may be directed at medica-
tained excitation (i.e., heart rate) during expo- tions that can become safety signals, because
sure and overall outcome with circumscribed their availability reassures patients that the
phobias (e.g., Rowe & Craske, 1998). dangers of extreme fear are controllable. Attri-
On the other hand, there is evidence for det- bution of safety to medications impedes correc-
rimental effects of safety signals and safety be- tion of misperceived danger (e.g., It is safe for
haviors, which presumably lower anxiety and me to drive on the freeway even when unmedi-
arousal during exposures. As mentioned ear- cated), and attribution of therapeutic gains to
lier, common safety signals for patients with a medication (alprazolam) in patients with
panic disorder are the presence of another per- panic disorder and agoraphobia predicted sub-
son, therapists, medications, and food or drink sequent withdrawal symptoms and relapse
(Barlow, 1988). Although they alleviate distress (Basoglu et al., 1994). Thus, the greater relapse
in the short term, safety signals are assumed to following exposure combined with anxiolytics
sustain anxiety in the long term (Siddle & (especially high-potency, short-acting drugs)
Bond, 1988). These effects have been explained compared to exposure alone (e.g., Marks et al.,
by associative and attributional mechanisms. 1993) may be attributable to medications func-
The associative model assumes that the nega- tioning as safety signals.
tive associative strength of the inhibitory stim-
ulus cancels out the positive associative
THE EFFECT OF CONTEXT ON RETURN OF FEAR
strength of the excitatory stimulus, so that
there is no change from what is predicted by all A fourth consideration to optimize learning
cues (Lovibond, Davis, & OFlaherty, 2000). during exposure therapy derives from condi-
The attributional model implies that if subjects tioning models in which extinction involves
attribute the absence of an expected outcome learning new, inhibitory CSno US associations
to the inhibitory stimulus, then there is no rea- as opposed to unlearning original CSUS asso-
son to change the causal status of the excit- ciations. Thus, Bouton (1993) proposed that
atory stimulus (Lovibond et al., 2000). the original excitatory meaning of the CS is not
In terms of treatment, Sloan and Telch erased during extinction; rather, an additional
(2002) reported that claustrophobic partici- inhibitory meaning is learned. The resulting
pants who received an exposure treatment in dual meaning of the CS creates an ambiguity
which they were encouraged to use safety sig- that is resolved only by the current context of
nals, reported more fear at posttest and follow- the CS. Bouton uses the analogy of an ambigu-
up than those encouraged to focus on their fear ous word; that is, reaction to the word fire
during exposure. In a subsequent study, depends largely on the context in which it oc-
Powers, Smits, and Telch (2004) found that the curs; fire may elicit a panic reaction in a
perception of safety (i.e., availability of safety crowded theater and elicit very little reaction in
behaviors regardless of whether they were a carnival shooting gallery. Thus, the context
used) rather than use of safety was detrimental determines which meaning is expressed at any
to treatment outcome, because level of fear re- given time. In terms of anxiety treatments,
duction was unaffected by actual use of safety bodily sensations may mean sudden death
behaviors. However, in both studies, the effects when experienced in a context that reminds the
of safety signal encouragement may have been person of intense panic attacks before treat-
attributable to distraction, and the results were ment, whereas the same sensations may mean
limited to circumscribed phobias. In another unpleasant but harmless when experienced
study, Salkovskis (1991) showed that within- in a context that reminds a person of his or her
situation safety behaviours interfered with the success with treatment. The effects of context
benefits of exposure therapy for panic and anx- shifts have been tested in circumscribed pho-
iety, and that teaching anxious patients to re- bias, and indeed, return of fear is greater when
frain from these behaviors leads to greater fear participants are subsequently assessed in a con-
reduction after an exposure session. Clearly text distinctly different rather than the same as
much more direct investigation is needed on that in which they were treated (for reviews,
the effects of safety signals and avoidance re- see Craske & Mystkowski, 2006; Hermans et
32 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

al., 2006). Hence, what is learned in the con- time. Thus, rates of eventual therapeutic suc-
text of exposure therapy may not be retrieved cess may be underestimated when success is
at reencounters with the previously feared pho- defined by continuous panic-free status since
bic object or situation after therapy is over. the end of active treatment.
Conceivably, conducting exposure therapy The effectiveness extends to patients who ex-
in multiple contexts minimizes the context re- perience nocturnal panic attacks (Craske,
newal effect after therapy is over. Unfortu- Lang, Aikins, & Mystkowski, 2005). Also,
nately, extant research with humans is limited cognitive-behavioral therapy is effective even
to one study of circumscribed phobias when there is comorbidity, and some studies in-
(Vansteenwegen et al., 2007). Because it is not dicate that comorbidity does not reduce the ef-
always feasible to conduct exposures in origi- fectiveness of cognitive-behavioral therapy for
nal fear-acquisition or multiple contexts, we panic disorder (e.g., Allen & Barlow, 2006;
(Mystkowski, Craske, Echiverri, & Labus, Brown, Antony, & Barlow, 1995; McLean et
2006) sought to investigate whether a contex- al., 1998). Furthermore, cognitive-behavioral
tually based return of fear could be counter- therapy results in improvements in comorbid
acted via mental rehearsal. Phobic participants anxiety and mood disorders (Brown et al.,
who were instructed to recall the exposure 1995; Tsao et al., 1998; Tsao, Mystkowski,
learning environment just prior to being re- Zucker, & Craske, 2002, 2005), although re-
tested with a spider in a novel context showed sults in one study indicated that the benefits for
less return of fear than those who were in- comorbid conditions may lessen over time,
structed to recall unrelated events. Although when assessed 2 years later (Brown et al.,
these findings were based on circumscribed 1995). Nonetheless, the general finding of im-
phobias, they raise the possibility that simply provement in comorbidity is significant given
reminding patients to recall their treatment ex- that it suggests the value of remaining focused
periences may offset return of fear when they on the treatment for panic disorder even when
reencounter their previously feared situations comorbidity is present, because the comorbidi-
after treatment is over. ty will be benefited as well, at least up to 1 year.
Finally, applications of cognitive-behavioral
therapy have proven very helpful in lowering
OVERALL EFFICACY OF relapse rates upon discontinuation of high-
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL THERAPY potency benzodiazepines (e.g., Otto et al.,
1993; Spiegel, Bruce, Gregg, & Nuzzarello,
Cognitive-behavioral therapy, involving most 1994).
or all of the components just listed, yields Results in samples with moderate to severe
panic-free rates in the range of 7080% and agoraphobia are generally slightly less positive
high end-state rates (i.e., within normative than those in samples with no or mild agora-
ranges of functioning) in the range of 5070%, phobia (e.g., Williams & Falbo, 1996). How-
for panic disorder with minimal agoraphobia ever, data typically show patterns of continuing
(e.g., Barlow et al., 1989; Clark et al., 1994). improvement over time. Furthermore, Fava,
Two meta-analyses reported very large effect Zielezny, Savron, and Grandi (1995) found
sizes of 1.55 and 0.90 for cognitive-behavioral that only 18.5% of their panic-free patients re-
therapy for panic disorder (Mitte, 2005; lapsed over a period of 57 years after
Westen & Morrison, 2001). Also, results gen- exposure-based treatment for agoraphobia. As
erally maintain over follow-up intervals for as mentioned, some research suggests that the
long as 2 years (Craske et al., 1991). One anal- trend for improvement after acute treatment is
ysis of individual profiles over time suggested a facilitated by involvement of significant others
less optimistic picture in that one-third of pa- in every aspect of treatment for agoraphobia
tients who were panic-free 24 months after (e.g., Cerny et al., 1987).
cognitive-behavioral therapy had experienced a As noted earlier, recently, our multicenter
panic attack in the preceding year, and 27% group evaluated strategies for maintaining re-
had received additional treatment for panic sponse in those who are considerably improved
over that same interval of time (Brown & after cognitive-behavioral treatment. Spe-
Barlow, 1995). Nevertheless, this approach to cifically, 157 patients who had responded well
analysis did not take into account the general to initial treatment were randomized to receive
trend toward continuing improvement over either no further cognitive-behavioral treat-
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 33

ment or one maintenance session a month for 9 which the various components of treatment are
months. At that point all treatment was discon- emphasized vary by the functional assessment
tinued for 1 year. At the end of that year, conducted for each patient.
97.3% of the patients receiving the booster ses-
sions continued to maintain their response,
whereas 81.9% maintained their response Overview
without the booster sessions; that is, 18.1% The basic aim of the treatment protocol is to
showed some loss of response compared to influence directly the catastrophic misap-
only 2.7% of those receiving the booster ses- praisals and avoidance of bodily sensations and
sions, a significant difference. In this large agoraphobic situations. This is done first
study, the value of occasional continued through the provision of accurate information
booster sessions was demonstrated. as to the nature of the fightflight response. By
Most of the outcome studies to date are con- provision of such information, patients are
ducted in university or research settings, with taught that they experience sensations and
select samples (although fewer exclusionary not panics, and that these sensations are nor-
criteria are used in more recent studies). Conse- mal and harmless. Second, treatment aims to
quently, of major concern is the degree to teach a set of skills for developing evidence-
which these treatment methods and outcomes based appraisals regarding bodily sensations
are transportable to nonresearch settings, with and agoraphobic situations. At the same time,
more severe or otherwise different populations, specific information concerning the effects of
and with less experienced or trained hyperventilation and its role in panic attacks is
cliniciansa topic that is just now receiving at- provided, with extensive practice of breathing
tention. Wade and colleagues (1998) used a retraining. Then, the crux of the treatment in-
benchmarking strategy to compare their results volves repeated exposure to feared internal
from a community mental health center with cues and agoraphobic situations.
results from research sites. One hundred ten
individuals underwent cognitive-behavioral
therapy for PD/PDA, concomitant with psy- Session 1
chopharmocotherapy where appropriate.
The goals of Session 1 are to describe fear and
Therapists were trained extensively. As in prior
anxiety; to help patients understand the cycli-
studies, treatment completion correlated posi-
cal influences among behavioral, physiological,
tively with years of education. Overall, the per-
and cognitive responses; to understand that
cent of panic-free individuals and the percent
panic attack symptoms are not harmful; and to
achieving normative levels of functioning on a
begin self-monitoring, if it was not already be-
variety of measures were comparable to
gun with the initial assessment. Therapy begins
percents obtained from research sites. As men-
with identifying anxiety patterns and the situa-
tioned, we are now evaluating the degree to
tions in which anxiety and panic attacks are
which these treatment results can be obtained
likely to occur. Many patients have difficulty
in other settings (e.g., primary care) and with
identifying specific antecedents, reporting that
less-well-trained therapists. In our first study of
panic can occur at almost any time. Therapists
panic disorder in primary care, we found that
help patients to identify internal triggers,
offering a treatment combination of cognitive-
specifically, negative verbal cognitions, cata-
behavioral therapy (up to six sessions) and
strophic imagery, and physical sensations. The
pharmacotherapy yielded highly significant
following interchange took place for Julie:
outcomes relative to treatment as usual (TAU)
in primary care settings, with relatively novice
therapists (Roy-Byrne et al., 2005). THERAPIST: In what situations are you most
likely to panic?
JULIE: Crowded restaurants and when Im driv-
TREATMENT DESCRIPTION: ing on the freeway. But sometimes I am driv-
PROTOCOL ing along, feeling OK, when all of a sudden it
hits. And other times I can be sitting at home
What follows is a description of a 12-session feeling quite relaxed and it just hits. Thats
cognitive-behavioral therapy for PDA tailored when I really get scared, because I cant ex-
to Julies presentation. Of course, the degree to plain it.
34 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

THERAPIST: So, when you are driving on the THERAPIST: What was your very next reaction
freeway, what is the very first thing you no- to that feeling?
tice that tells you youre about to panic? JULIE: I held onto the chair. I thought some-
JULIE: Well, the other cars on the road look as if thing was wrong. I thought it could get
they are moving really slowly. worse and that Id collapse.
THERAPIST: And what is the first thing you no- THERAPIST: So it began with a physical sensa-
tice when youre at home? tion, and then you had some very specific
JULIE: An unreal feeling, like Im floating. thoughts about those sensations. What hap-
THERAPIST: So, it sounds like the panic attacks pened next?
that seem to occur for no reason are actually JULIE: I felt very anxious.
tied in with the sensations of unreality or THERAPIST: And what happened next?
when things look as if they are moving in
slow motion. JULIE: Well, the dizziness seemed to be getting
worse and worse. I became really concerned
JULIE: I guess so. I always thought the physical that it was different from any other experi-
feelings were the panic attack, but maybe ence I had ever had. I was convinced that this
they start the panic attack. was it.
Next, the three-response system model for THERAPIST: So, as you became more anxious,
describing and understanding anxiety and the physical feelings and the thoughts that
panic is introduced. This model contributes to something bad was going to happen intensi-
an objective self-awarenessto becoming a fied. What did you do next?
personal scientistand provides the ground- JULIE: I called my husband and lay on the bed
work for an alternative conceptual framework until he came home. It was horrible.
for explaining panic and anxiety that replaces THERAPIST: Can you see how one thing fed off
the patients own misassumptions. Patients are another, creating a cycle? That it began with
asked to describe cognitive, physiological, and a sensation, then some anxious thoughts,
behavioral aspects to their responding: to iden- then feeling anxious, then more sensations
tify the things that they feel, think, and do and more thoughts, and more fear, and so
when they are anxious and panicky. As de- on?
scribed earlier, differences between the re-
sponse profiles of anxiety and panic are high-
lighted. After grasping the notion of three Reasons why panic attacks first began are
responses that are partially independent, inter- addressed briefly. Patients are informed that it
actions among the response systems are de- is not necessary to understand the reasons why
scribed. The patient is asked to describe the they began to panic to benefit from the treat-
three-response system components in a recent ment, because factors involved in onset are not
panic attack and to identify ways in which they necessarily the same as the factors involved in
interacted to produce heightened distress. For the maintenance of a problem. Nevertheless,
example, the initial panic attack is described as a mani-
festation of anxiety/stress. The stressors sur-
THERAPIST: How would you describe the three rounding the time of the first panic attack are
parts to the panic attack you had at home explored with the patient, particularly in terms
last week? of how stressors may have increased levels of
physical arousal and primed certain danger-
JULIE: Well, physically, my head felt really light,
laden cognitive schemas.
and my hands were clammy. I thought that I
Next, the therapist briefly describes the
would either pass out or that I would some-
physiology underlying anxiety and panic, and
how dissolve into nothingness. My behavior
the myths about what the physical sensations
was to lie down and call my husband, who
might mean. The main concepts covered in this
was at work.
educational phase are (1) the survival value or
THERAPIST: What was the very first thing you protective function of anxiety and panic; (2)
noticed? the physiological basis to the various sensa-
JULIE: When I stood up, my head started to feel tions experienced during panic and anxiety,
really weird, as if it was spinning inside. and the survival function of the underlying
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 35

physiology; and (3) the role of specific learned ing unreal help me deal with a danger situa-
and cognitively mediated fears of certain bodily tion?
sensations. The model of panic we described THERAPIST: OK, remember that its the physio-
earlier in this chapter is explained. In par- logical events that are protectivenot the
ticular, the concepts of misappraisals and sensations. The sensations are just the end
interoceptive conditioning are explained as ac- result of those events. Now, feelings of unre-
counting for panic attacks that seem to occur ality can be caused by changes in your blood
from out of the bluethat are triggered by very flow to your brain (although not danger-
subtle internal cues or physical sensations that ously so), or from overbreathing, or from
may occur at any time. Not only does this in- concentrating too intensely on whats going
formation reduce anxiety by decreasing uncer- on inside you. So the unreality sensation may
tainty about panic attacks but it also enhances not be protective, but the changes in blood
the credibility of the subsequent treatment pro- flow and overbreathing are.
cedures. This information is detailed in a hand-
out given to the patient to read over the next JULIE: I understand how I can create a panic at-
week (for the handout, see Barlow & Craske, tack by being afraid of my physical feelings,
2006). like my heart racing or feeling unreal. But
This information was very important for sometimes it happens so quickly that I dont
Julie, because the inability to explain her panic have time to think.
attacks was a major source of distress. Here are THERAPIST: Yes, these reactions can occur very
some of the questions she asked in her attempt quickly, at times automatically. But remem-
to understand more fully: ber, we are tuned to react instantaneously to
things (including our own bodies) that we
JULIE: So, if I understand you correctly, youre think mean danger. Imagine yourself walk-
saying that my panic attacks are the same as ing through a dark alley, and you have rea-
the fear I experienced the time we found a son to believe that somewhere in the dark-
burglar in our house. It doesnt feel the same ness lurks a killer. Under those conditions,
at all. you would be extremely attentive to any
sign, any sound, or any sight of another per-
THERAPIST: Yes, those two emotional states
son. If you were walking through the same
an unexpected panic attack and fear when
alley and were sure there were no killers, you
confronted with a burglarare essentially
might not hear or detect the same signals you
the same. However, in the case of the
picked up on in the first case. Now lets
burglar, where were you focusing your
translate this to panic; the killer in the dark
attentionon the burglar or on the way you
alley is the panic attack, and the signs,
were feeling?
sounds, and smells are the physical sensa-
JULIE: The burglar, of course, although I did no- tions you think signal the possibility of a
tice my heart was going a mile a minute. panic attack. Given the acute degree of sensi-
THERAPIST: And when you have a panic attack, tivity to physical symptoms that signal a
where are you focusing your attentionon panic attack, it is likely that you are noticing
the people around you or on the way you are normal noises in your body that you
feeling? would otherwise not notice, and on occa-
sion, immediately become fearful because of
JULIE: Well, mostly on the way Im feeling, al-
those noises. In other words, the sensa-
though it depends on where I am at the time.
tions are often noticeable because you attend
THERAPIST: Being most concerned about whats to them.
going on inside can lead to a very different
type of experience than being concerned Next, the method of self-monitoring was de-
about the burglar, even though basically the scribed and demonstrated with in-session prac-
same physiological response is occurring. tice of completing a Panic Attack Record. Julie
For example, remember our description of was concerned that self-monitoring would only
the way fear of sensations can intensify the elevate her distress, by reminding of the very
sensations. thing she was afraid of (panic and unreality).
JULIE: But what about the feelings of unreality? The therapist clarified the difference between
How can they be protective or how can feel- objective and subjective self-monitoring, and
36 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

explained that distress would subside as Julie to treatment. A team approach to treatment
persevered with self-monitoring. planning and progress was agreed upon, so
The homework for this session was to self- that Julie did not feel that she would be forced
monitor panic attacks, daily anxiety, and mood to do things she did not think she could do.
and to read the handout. In fact, we encourage
patients to reread the handout several times,
Session 2
and to actively engage in the material by cir-
cling or marking the most personally relevant The goals of this session are to begin the devel-
sections or areas in need of clarification, be- opment of a hierarchy of agoraphobic situa-
cause effort enhances long-term retention of tions and coping skills of breathing retraining
the material learned. Of course, for some pa- and cognitive restructuring. The individualized
tients, reading the material draws their atten- hierarchy comprises situations that range from
tion to things they fear (just as with self- mild to moderate anxiety, all the way up to ex-
monitoring). In this case, therapists can discuss treme anxiety. These situations become the ba-
the role of avoidance versus that of exposure, sis of graduated in vivo exposure. Although in
and how, with repeated readings, distress levels vivo exposure exercises are not scheduled to
will most likely subside. take place until Session 4, the hierarchy is in-
At the end of the session, Julie suddenly be- troduced now, so that cognitive restructuring
came highly anxious. She felt unable to tolerate skills can be practiced in relation to each situa-
either the treatment procedures or her anticipa- tion on the hierarchy before in vivo exposure
tion of them. She became very agitated in the begins. Moreover, the hierarchy will be refined
office and reported feelings of unreality. She as a result of the cognitive restructuring prac-
opened the office door to find her husband, tice, because the latter highlights specific fea-
who was waiting outside. The therapist helped tures of agoraphobic situations that are most
Julie understand how the cycle of panic had anxiety provoking.
emerged in the current situation: (1) The trig- Julie was asked to develop a hierarchy over
ger was the treatment descriptionhaving to the following week. She expressed some doubt
eventually face feared sensations and situa- that she would ever be able to accomplish any,
tions; (2) this was anxiety producing, because let alone all, of the items on her hierarchy. The
Julie believed that she could not cope with the therapist helped Julie by asking her to think of
treatment demands, that the treatment would any situation in her lifetime that used to be dif-
cause her so much anxiety that she would flip ficult but became easier with practice. Julie re-
out and lose touch with reality permanently, membered how anxious she used to be when
or that she would never improve because she she first started working with customers at her
could not tolerate the treatment; (3) the current husbands officeand how that discomfort
anxiety in the office elicited sensations of unre- subsided over time. This was used to help Julie
ality and a racing heart; (4) Julie began to realize that the same might happen with the sit-
worry that she might panic and lose touch with uations listed on her hierarchy. Julies final hi-
reality permanently within the next few min- erarchy comprised the following situations:
utes; (5) the more anxious Julie felt, and the driving home from work alone; sitting in a
stronger her attempts to escape and find safety, crowded movie theater; spending 2 hours alone
the stronger the physical sensations became; at home during the day; alone at home as day
and (6) she felt some relief upon finding her turned to night; driving on surface streets to
husband, because his presence reassured her her brothers house (10 miles) alone; driving
that she would be safe. Julie was reassured that two exits on freeway 444, with her husband
treatment would progress at a pace with which following in the car behind; driving two exits
she was comfortable, but at the same time she on freeway 444, alone; driving four exits on
was helped to understand that her acute dis- freeway 444; and driving on the freeway to her
tress about the feeling of unreality would be the brothers house alone. Then, Julie was to repeat
precise target of this type of treatment, there- all of these tasks without taking Klonopin, and
fore attesting to the relevance of this treatment without knowing the location of her husband.
for her. She was also calmed by preliminary Breathing retraining also is begun in this ses-
cognitive restructuring of the probability of sion. Patients are asked to hyperventilate vol-
permanently losing touch with reality. After a untarily by standing and breathing fast and
lengthy discussion, Julie became more receptive deep, as if blowing up a balloon, for 1 min-
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 37

utes. With prompting and encouragement from respiratory sensations, because the exercise di-
the therapist, patients can often complete the rects their attention to breathing. It also can be
full 1 minutes, after which time they are difficult for patients who are chronic over-
asked to sit, close their eyes, and breathe very breathers, and patients for whom any interrup-
slowly, pausing at the end of each breath, until tion of habitual breathing patterns initially in-
the symptoms have abated. The experience is creases respiratory symptomatology. In both
then discussed in terms of the degree to which cases, continued practice is advisable, with re-
it produced symptoms similar to those that oc- assurance that sensations such as shortness of
cur naturally during anxiety or panic. Approxi- breath are not harmful. The goal is to use
mately 5060% of patients report close sim- breathing skills training to encourage contin-
ilarity of the symptoms. Often, however, ued approach toward anxiety and anxiety-
similarity of the symptoms is confused with producing situations. On occasion, patients
similarity of the anxiety. Because the exercise is mistakenly view breathing retraining as a way
conducted in a safe environment and the symp- of relieving themselves of terrifying symptoms,
toms have an obvious cause, most patients rate thus falling into the trap of fearing dire conse-
the experience as less anxiety provoking than if quences should they not succeed in correcting
the same symptoms had occurred naturally. their breathing. This is what happened for
This distinction is important to make, because Julie:
it demonstrates the significance of perceived
safety for the degree of anxiety experienced. JULIE: So, all I have to do is to slow down my
Julie rated the hyperventilation exercise as very breathing, then everything will be OK.
anxiety provoking (8 on a 0- to 10-point scale), THERAPIST: Certainly, slowing down your
and rated the symptoms as being quite similar breathing will help to decrease the physical
to her panic symptoms (6 on a 0- to 10-point symptoms that you feel, but I am not sure
scale). She terminated the task after approxi- what you mean when you ask whether ev-
mately 40 seconds, in anticipation of experi- erything will be OK.
encing a full-blown panic attack. The therapist
and Julie discussed this experience in terms of JULIE: That proper breathing will prevent me
the three response systems, and the role of from losing touch with realitythat I wont
misappraisals and interoceptive conditioning disappear.
described during the previous session. THERAPIST: Remember, whether you breathe
Then, Julie was briefly educated about the slowly or quickly, from your chest or from
physiological basis to hyperventilation (see your abdomen, you will not disappear. In
Barlow & Craske, 2006). As before, the goal of other words, it is a misinterpretation to
the didactic presentation was to allay misinter- think that the sense of unreality means that
pretations of the dangers of overbreathing, and you are permanently losing touch with real-
to provide a factual information base on which ity or that you will disappear. Breathing re-
to draw when actively challenging misinterpre- training will help you to feel more relaxed
tations. The educational content is tailored to and, therefore, less likely to feel the sense of
the patients own educational level and covered unreality, but the sense of unreality is not a
only to the degree that it is relevant to the pa- sign of actual loss of touch with reality and
tient. disappearance.
In the next step, the therapist teaches breath-
ing retraining, which begins by teaching pa- The homework is to practice diaphragmatic
tients to rely more on the diaphragm (abdo- breathing for at least 10 minutes, two times a
men) than chest muscles. In addition, patients day in relaxing environments.
are instructed to concentrate on their breath- Therapists introduce in this session cognitive
ing, by counting on their inhalations, and restructuring by explaining that errors in think-
thinking the word relax on exhalations. ing occur for everyone when anxious, thus
(Slow breathing is introduced in Session 3.) helping the patient to expect his or her thinking
Therapists model the suggested breathing pat- to be distorted. Patients are informed that these
terns, then provide corrective feedback to pa- distortions have an adaptive function: Chances
tients while they practice in the office setting. of survival are greater if we perceive danger as
Initial reactions to the breathing exercise probable and worthy of attention than if we
may be negative for patients who are afraid of minimize danger. Therefore, anxiety leads us to
38 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

judge threatening events as being more likely and valence. These two main types of cogni-
and more threatening than they really are. tive errors are described to patients. Risk
However, the cognitive distortions are unneces- translates to overestimation, or jumping to
sary, because there is no real threat in the case conclusions by viewing negative events as being
of panic disorder. probable events, when in fact they are unlikely
Then, patients are taught to treat their to occur. The patient is asked to identify
thoughts as hypotheses or guesses rather than overestimations from the anxiety and panic in-
as facts. The notions of automatic thinking and cidents over the past couple of weeks: Can
discrete predictions are also explained, to em- you think of events that you felt sure were go-
phasize the need of becoming an astute ob- ing to happen when you panicked, only to find
server of ones own habitual self-statements in out in the end that they did not happen at all?
each situation. This leads to a downward ar- Usually, patients can identify such events easily,
row technique to identify specific predictions but with protestations. For example,
made at any given moment, as shown with
Julie. JULIE: Well, several times I thought that I really
was going to lose it this time . . . that I would
THERAPIST: What is it that scared you about flip out and never return to reality. It never
feeling detached in the movie theater last actually happened, but it could still happen.
night? THERAPIST: Why do you think it could still
JULIE: It is just such a horrible feeling. happen?
THERAPIST: What makes it so horrible? JULIE: Part of me feels like Ive always managed
to escape it just in time, by either removing
JULIE: I cant tolerate it.
myself from the situation or by having my
THERAPIST: What makes you think you cannot husband help me, or by holding on long
tolerate it? What is the feeling of detachment enough for the feeling to pass. But what if
going to do to you that makes you think it is next time I cant hold on?
horrible and intolerable?
THERAPIST: Knowing what we know about our
JULIE: It might get to be so intense that it over- thoughts when we are anxious, can you clas-
whelms me. sify any of the ideas you just expressed, of
THERAPIST: And if it overwhelms you, what just holding on or just escaping in time,
would happen? as overestimations?
JULIE: I could become so distressed that I lose JULIE: I suppose youre saying that I can hold
touch with reality. on or I can always escape in time.
THERAPIST: What would it mean if you lost THERAPIST: More that you feel the need to hold
touch with reality? on and the need to escape, because you are
overestimating the likelihood of flipping out
JULIE: That I would be in a different mind state
and never returning to reality.
foreverI would never come back to reality.
That I would be so crazy that I would have JULIE: But it really feels like I will.
to be carted out of the movie theater to a THERAPIST: The confusion between what you
mental hospital and locked away forever. think will happen and what actually happens
is the very problem that we are addressing in
Overly general self-statements, such as I this session.
feel terriblesomething bad could happen,
are insufficient, nontherapeutic, and may serve The reasons why overestimations persist de-
to intensify anxiety by virtue of their global spite repeated disconfirmation are explored.
and nondirective nature. Instead, detail in Typically, patients misattribute the absence of
thought content, such as I am afraid that if I danger to external safety signals or safety be-
get too anxious while driving, then Ill lose con- haviors (e.g., I only made it because I man-
trol of the wheel and drive off the side of the aged to find help in time, If I had not taken
road and die, permits subsequent cognitive re- Xanax last week when I panicked in the store,
structuring. Im sure I would have passed out or I would-
Analysis of anxious thought content yields nt have made it if I hadnt pulled off the road
two broad factors that are labeled as risk in time), or to luck, instead of realizing the
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 39

inaccuracy of the original prediction. Similarly, JULIE: Well, maybe I havent lost complete
patients may assume that the only reason they touch with reality. But what if I do?
are still alive, sane, and safe, is because the big THERAPIST: How many times have you felt de-
one has not happened. In this case, patients tached?
err by assuming that intensity of panic attacks
increases the risk of catastrophic outcomes. JULIE: Hundreds and hundreds of times.
The method for countering overestimation THERAPIST: And how many times have you lost
errors is to question the evidence for probabil- touch with reality permanently?
ity judgments. The general format is to treat JULIE: Never. But what if the feelings dont go
thoughts as hypotheses or guesses rather than away? Maybe Ill lose it then?
as facts and to examine the evidence and gener-
ate alternative, more realistic predictions. This THERAPIST: So what else tells you that this is a
is best done by the therapist using a Socratic possibility?
style, so that patients learn the skill of examin- JULIE: Well, what about my second cousin? He
ing the content of their statements and arrive at lost it when he was about 25, and now hes
alternative statements or predictions after they just a mess. He can hardly function at all,
have considered all of the evidence. Question- and he is constantly in and out of psychiatric
ing of the logic (e.g., How does a racing heart hospitals. They have him on a bunch of
lead to heart attack?), or the bases from heavy-duty drugs. Ill never forget the time I
which judgments are made (e.g., misinforma- saw him totally out of it. He was talking to
tion from others, unusual sensations) is useful himself in jibberish.
in this regard. Continuing with the previous ex- THERAPIST: So, do you make a connection be-
ample from Julie, the questioning took the fol- tween him and yourself?
lowing course:
JULIE: Yes.
THERAPIST: One of the specific thoughts you THERAPIST: What are the similarities between
have identified is that you will flip out and the two of you?
never return to reality. What specifically JULIE: There are none really. Its just that he is
leads you to think that that is likely to hap- what I think I will become.
pen? THERAPIST: Did he ever feel the way you feel
JULIE: Well, I guess it really feels like that. now?
THERAPIST: Describe the feelings? JULIE: I dont know.
JULIE: Well, I feel spacey and unreal, like things THERAPIST: And if another one of your cousins
around me are different and that Im not had severe back problems, would you be
connected. concerned that you would end up with se-
THERAPIST: And why do you think those feel- vere back problems?
ings mean that you have actually lost touch JULIE: No.
with reality?
THERAPIST: Why not?
JULIE: I dont knowit feels as if I have.
JULIE: Because it never crosses my mind. It is
THERAPIST: So, lets examine that assumption. not something that I worry about.
What is your behavior like when you feel un-
THERAPIST: So, it sounds like you think you will
real? For example, do you respond if some-
end up like your cousin because you are
one asks you a question during those epi-
afraid of ending up like him.
sodes?
JULIE: I suppose so.
JULIE: Well, I respond to you even though I feel
that way sometimes in here. THERAPIST: So, lets look at all of the evidence
THERAPIST: OK, and can you walk or write or and consider some alternatives. You have felt
drive when you feel that way? unreal hundreds of times, and youve never
lost touch with reality, because youve con-
JULIE: Yes, but it feels different. tinued to function in the midst of those feel-
THERAPIST: But you do perform those functions ings, and they have never lasted. You are
despite feeling detached. So, what does that afraid of becoming like your cousin, but
tell you? there are no data to show that you and he
40 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

have the same problem. In fact, the data sug- this is a skill, just like learning to ride a bike,
gest otherwise, because you function and he and you cannot expect it to be easy from the
does not. So what is the realistic probability get-go. Second, it sounds like you experi-
that you will lose touch with reality perma- enced some uncomfortable physical symp-
nently? Use a scale of 0 to 100, where 0 = No toms that worried you. You said it felt like
chance at all and 100 = Definitely will hap- you were running out of air. Based on what
pen. we talked about last week, what do you
JULIE: Well, maybe it is lower than I thought. think might have caused that feeling?
Maybe 20%. JULIE: Well, maybe I wasnt getting enough air
THERAPIST: So that would mean that you have into my lungs, because its really hard for me
actually lost touch with reality in a perma- to use my diaphragm muscle. I felt like I was
nent way once every five times you have felt suffocating myself.
unreal. THERAPIST: Possibly its just a matter of learn-
JULIE: When its put like that, I guess not. ing to use the diaphragm muscle, but were
Maybe its a very small possibility. you really suffocating or was it an interpre-
tation that you might be suffocating?
THERAPIST: Yes, so what is an alternative expla-
nation? JULIE: I dont know. Ive had the feeling of suf-
focating before, especially when Im trapped
JULIE: Perhaps the feelings of unreality are in a crowded room.
caused by feeling anxious or overbreathing,
and having those feelings does not mean that THERAPIST: So, how do you know you were
I am actually losing touch with reality, and suffocating?
that I am not like my cousin at all. JULIE: I dont know. It just felt that way.
THERAPIST: So, lets put the evidence together.
For homework, in addition to continuation Youve had the feelings before and never suf-
of self-monitoring and practice of diaphrag- focated. As we discussed last time, anxiety
matic breathing, Julie was asked to identify her can sometimes create a sensation of short-
anxious thoughts in relation to every item on ness of breath even though you are getting
her agoraphobia hierarchy, and to use the in- plenty of air. Can you think of an alternative
session steps of examining the evidence and explanation?
generating alternative evidence based interpre-
JULIE: Well, maybe I wasnt suffocating. Maybe
tations for errors of overestimating the risk.
it just felt like that.
She was to do the same for every panic attack
that occurred over the next week.
Julies complaints represent typical concerns
that should be addressed. The next step is to
Session 3 slow the rate of breathing until the patient can
comfortably span a full inhalation and exhala-
The goals of this session are to develop breath- tion cycle of 6 seconds. Again, the therapist
ing retraining and to continue active cognitive models slowed breathing, then provides correc-
restructuring. The therapist reviews the pa- tive feedback on practice in the session. The pa-
tients week of diaphragmatic breathing prac- tient is instructed to continue to practice slow
tice. Julie was disappointed with her attempts breathing in safe or relaxing environments,
to practice. and is discouraged from applying slow breath-
ing when anxious or panicking, until fully
JULIE: I just didnt seem to be able to do it the skilled in its application.
right way. Sometimes I would start off OK Also, cognitive restructuring is continued by
and then the more I tried, the more it felt like addressing the second cognitive error, which in-
I was running out of air, and Id have to take volves viewing an event as dangerous, in-
a big gulp between breaths. At other times, I sufferable, or catastrophic, when in actual-
felt dizzy and the unreal feelings would start, ity it is not. Typical examples of catastrophic
at which point I would stop and do busy errors are If I faint, people will think that Im
work to keep my mind occupied. weak and that would be unbearable or Panic
THERAPIST: It sounds like quite a few things attacks are the worst thing I can imagine, and
were going on. First of all, remember that The whole evening is ruined if I start to feel
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 41

anxious. Decatastrophizing means to face the police did come when you were yelling
the worst, to realize that the occurrences are and screaming, and what if the police did
not as catastrophic as stated, and to think take you away? As scary as that may sound
about actual ways to cope with negative events to you, lets consider what actually would
rather than how bad they are. A key princi- happen.
ple underlying decatastrophizing is that events JULIE: I have this image of myself not being able
can be endured even though they are uncom- to tell them what is really going onthat I
fortable. Recognition of the time-limited na- am so out of it I dont have the ability to let
ture of discomfort contributes to the develop- them know I am just anxious.
ment of a sense of being able to cope. The
critical distinction here is that although pa- THERAPIST: If you were so distraught that you
tients might prefer that these events not occur, could not clearly communicate, how long
they can tolerate the discomfort, if necessary. would that last?
Thus, for the person who states that negative JULIE: Youre right. I would eventually exhaust
judgments from others are unbearable, it is im- myself and then I could speak more clearly.
portant to discuss what he or she would do to But what if they didnt believe me?
cope should someone else make a direct nega- THERAPIST: What if they did not believe you at
tive judgment. Similarly, for the person who first? How long would it take before they
states that the physical symptoms of panic are would realize that you were not crazy?
intolerably embarrassing, the following type of
JULIE: I guess that after a while they would see
questioning is helpful:
that I was OK, and maybe I could call a
friend or my doctor to explain what was go-
JULIE: I am really worried that I might lose con-
ing on.
trol and do something crazy, like yell and
scream. THERAPIST: Thats right. Now remember, all of
this is about events that are unlikely to hap-
THERAPIST: Aside from the low likelihood of
pen. At the same time, it is helpful to face
that happening (as we discussed before), lets
worst-case scenarios (even though unlikely)
face the worst and find out what is so bad
and realize that they are not as bad as you
about it. What would be so horrible about
first thought.
yelling and screaming?
JULIE: I could never live it down. The homework for this session, in addition
THERAPIST: Well, lets think it through. What to continued self-monitoring, is to practice
are the various things you could do in the sit- slow and diaphragmatic breathing in relaxing
uation? You have just yelled and screamed environments, and to identify errors of catas-
now what? trophizing in relation to each item on the ago-
JULIE: Well, I guess the yelling and screaming raphobia hierarchy, followed by practice of
would eventually stop. decatastrophizing and generation of ways to
cope. In addition, Julie was to use the skill of
THERAPIST: Thats rightat the very least you
decatastrophizing for panic attacks that oc-
would eventually exhaust yourself. What
curred over the following week.
else?
JULIE: Well, maybe I would explain to the peo-
ple around me that I was having a really bad Session 4
day but that I would be OK. In other words, The main goal of this session is to use breathing
reassure them. retraining skills as a coping tool, to review cog-
THERAPIST: Good. What else? nitive restructuring skills, and to begin in vivo
JULIE: Maybe I would just get awayfind exposure to the first item on the agoraphobia
someplace to calm down and reassure myself hierarchy.
that the worst is over. Now that patients have practiced slow and
diaphragmatic breathing sufficiently in relax-
THERAPIST: Good.
ing environments, they are ready to use these
JULIE: But what if the police came and took me methods in distracting environments and in
away, locked me up in a mental ward? anxious situations. Patients are encouraged to
THERAPIST: Again, lets face the worst. What if use breathing skills as a coping technique as
42 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

they face fear, anxiety, and anxiety-provoking methods of questioning the evidence to gener-
situations. Some patients use breathing skills as ate realistic probabilities, facing the worst, and
a safety signal or a safety behavior; in other generating ways of coping with each item on
words, they believe that they will be at risk for the agoraphobia hierarchy and any panic at-
some mental, physical, or social calamity if tacks that occurred over the past week. Par-
they do not breathe correctly. This issue came ticular corrective feedback is given when
up with Julie, as shown below. patients lack specificity in their cognitive re-
structuring (e.g., patients who record that they
JULIE: When I panicked during the week, I tried are most worried about panicking should be
to use the breathing. It didnt work. It made encouraged to detail what it is about panicking
me feel worse. that worries them) or rely on blanket reassur-
THERAPIST: It sounds as if you might have at- ance (e.g., patients who record that Every-
tempted to use the breathing exercise as a thing will be OK as their evidence and/or
desperate attempt to control the feelings you ways of coping should be encouraged to list the
were experiencing. evidence and/or generate actual coping steps).
Next, attention is given to how to practice
JULIE: Yes, thats right.
the first item on the agoraphobia hierarchy. If
THERAPIST: What did you think would have appropriate, reasons why previous attempts at
happened if you had not been able to control in vivo exposure may have failed are reviewed.
the feelings? Typical reasons for patients past failures at in
JULIE: I was really worried that I might not be vivo exposure include attempts that are too
able to handle the feelings. haphazard and/or brief, or spaced too far
THERAPIST: And if you werent able to handle apart, and attempts conducted without a sense
the feelings, what would happen? of mastery, or while maintaining beliefs that ca-
tastrophe is very possible. Julie had tried to
JULIE: It just feels like I will lose it, permanently. face agoraphobic situations in the past, but
THERAPIST: So this is one of those thoughts that each time she had escaped, feeling over-
we were talking about last time. What does whelmed by panic and terrified of losing touch
your evidence tell you about the likelihood with reality permanently. The therapist helped
of losing touch with reality permanently? Julie realize how to approach the agoraphobic
JULIE: So you mean even if I dont control my situations differently to benefit from the expo-
breathing, then I will be OK? sure. Julies typical safety signals were the pres-
ence of her husband, or at least knowing his
THERAPIST: Well, you had not lost touch with
whereabouts, and Klonopin (which she carried
reality permanently before you learned the
but rarely used). The therapist discussed the
breathing exercise, so what does that tell
importance of eventual weaning from those
you?
safety signals.
JULIE: OK, I get it. As mentioned earlier, the goal of exposure
THERAPIST: The breathing exercise is best therapy is not immediate reduction in fear and
thought of as a tool to help you face what- anxiety; rather the goal is for the patient to
ever is provoking anxiety. So, as you face sit- learn something new as a result of exposure.
uations and your anxiety increases, use the Clarification of what patients are most worried
breathing exercise first, then use your cogni- about as they face their feared situations and
tive skills, so that you can continue to face the conditions that best help patients to learn
rather than run away from anxiety. that what they are most worried about never or
rarely happens, and/or that they can cope with
Patients who consistently use the breathing the situation and tolerate anxiety is essential
skills as a safety behavior might be discouraged for effective exposure. If a patient is most wor-
from using the breathing skills, so that they ried that fear and anxiety will remain elevated
learn that what they are most worried about ei- for the entire duration of the practice, then cor-
ther does not happen or it can be managed rective learning involves toleration of sustained
without using the breathing skills. anxiety. For Julie, the first situation on her hier-
In terms of the cognitive restructuring, thera- archy was to drive home from work, alone. She
pists give corrective feedback to patients on the stated that what most worried her in that situa-
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 43

tion was that she would panic and lose touch JULIE: I just dont think I could do it. It would
with reality, therefore losing control of the car be too hard. I might really freak out and lose
and dying in an accident. She also stated that to touch with reality for ever.
drive at dusk was the condition under which THERAPIST: OK, so lets think about that
she was most convinced of these eventualities. thought. What does your experience tell
Thus, the task that the therapist considered you? How many times have you perma-
most effective in teaching Julie that she would nently lost touch with reality, including days
not lose touch with reality and have an acci- when you were worn down?
dent, or that she could cope with the sensations
of unreality and panic, was to drive home from JULIE: Well, never.
work at dusk. THERAPIST: So, what does that tell you?
Delineation of the exposure task as con- JULIE: OK, but it still feels difficult to drive on
cretely as possible, so that patients clearly un- those days.
derstand exactly what the practice entails (e.g.,
THERAPIST: How about you start with Monday
Walk around inside of mall for 10 minutes by
or Tuesday, but quickly move to the other
myself), reduces uncertainty about whether
days of the week when you are feeling worn
the practice was conducted correctly. Without
down, so that you get a really good opportu-
such concrete details, patients might decide
nity to learn whether you permanently lose
that they failed. Importantly, the practice
touch with reality or not?
should not be ended because of anxiety (e.g.,
Continue driving on the freeway until I feel
anxious) because the exposure practice would The homework for this session involves con-
then reinforce avoidance of anxiety. tinued self-monitoring, continued use of cogni-
Julie was reminded to use her coping skills tive restructuring and breathing retraining in
should she panic as she practiced the task; that the event of elevated anxiety or panic, and
is, in moments of fear, patients are encouraged practicing the first item on the agoraphobia hi-
to use their breathing and thinking skills to erarchy at least three times, with at least one of
complete the assigned task; the coping skills are those times being without her husband Larry.
not intended as means to reduce fear and anxi-
ety, but to tolerate it. Session 5
Patients are encouraged to maintain a regu-
lar schedule of repeated in vivo exposure prac- The goals of this session are to review the prac-
tices at least three times per week, and to con- tice of in vivo exposure, to design another ex-
duct these practices regardless of internal (e.g., posure task to be practiced over the next week,
having a bad day, feeling ill) or external (e.g., and to begin interoceptive exposure. Note that
inclement weather, busy schedules) factors that in vivo and interoceptive exposure can be done
may prompt postponement of practices. Julie simultaneously or sequentially. For Julie, in
expressed some concerns about being able to vivo exposure was begun in Session 4, whereas
practice at least three times over the following interoceptive exposure was begun in this ses-
week: sion, but they could easily have been done in
the opposite order.
It is essential to review the weeks practice of
JULIE: I dont know if I can practice three times, in vivo exposure. An objective evaluation of
because more days than not I feel pretty performance is considered necessary to offset
worn down; maybe I can practice on just subjective and damaging self-evaluations. As
Monday and Tuesday, because they are the demonstrated in experimental literature on
days I typically feel better. learning and conditioning, appraisals of aver-
THERAPIST: What is it you are worried about sive events after they have occurred can influ-
happening if you practice on a day when you ence anxiety about future encounters with the
already feel worn down? same types of aversive events. Any practice that
is terminated prematurely is to be reviewed
JULIE: I feel more fragile on those days.
carefully for contributing factors, which can
THERAPIST: And if you feel more fragile, what then be incorporated into subsequent trials of
might happen? in vivo exposure. Recognition of the precipi-
44 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

tant to escape is very important, because the change). The way in which the agoraphobic
urge to escape is usually based on the predic- problem has disrupted daily routines and dis-
tion that continued endurance would result in tribution of home responsibilities is explored
some kind of danger. For example, patients and discussed also. Examples might include so-
may predict that the sensations will become in- cial activities, leisure activities, and household
tense and lead to an out-of-control reaction. chores. The therapist explains that family ac-
This prediction can be discussed in terms of tivities may be structured around the agora-
jumping to conclusions and blowing things out phobic fear and avoidance to help the patient
of proportion. At the same time, escape itself function without intense anxiety. At the same
need not be viewed catastrophically (i.e., as time, reassignment of the patients tasks to the
embarrassing, or as a sign of failure). In addi- significant other may actually reinforce the
tion, therapists reinforce the use of breathing agoraphobic pattern of behavior. Conse-
and cognitive skills to help patients remain in quently, the importance of complying with in
the situation until the specified duration or task vivo exposure homework instructions, even
has been completed, despite uncomfortable though the patient may experience some dis-
sensations. tress initially, is emphasized.
Again, it is important for patients to recog- The significant other is encouraged to be-
nize that the goal is to repeatedly face situa- come an active participant by providing his or
tions despite anxiety, not to achieve a total ab- her perception of the patients behavior and
sence of anxiety. Toleration of fear rather than fearfulness, and the impact on the home envi-
immediate fear reduction is the goal for each ronment. Sometimes significant others have
exposure practice; this approach leads to an provided information of which the patient was
eventual fear reduction. Anxiety that does not not fully aware, or did not report, particularly
decline over repeated days of in vivo exposure in relation to how the patients behavior affects
may result from too much emphasis on imme- the significant others own daily functioning.
diate fear and anxiety reduction; that is, trying Larry, for example, described how he felt re-
too hard or wishing too much for anxiety to stricted at home in the evenings; whereas, be-
decline typically maintains anxiety. fore, he occasionally played basketball with his
Julie had success with her first in vivo expo- friends at the local gym, he now stays at home,
sure practice; she managed to drive home from because he feels guilty if he leaves Julie alone.
work at dusk, alone, four different times. She The next step is to describe the role of the
noted that the first time was easier than she had significant other regarding in vivo exposure
expected; the second was harder, and the one tasks. The significant other is viewed as a
time she pulled off to the side of the road. The coach, and the couple is encouraged to ap-
therapist helped Julie identify the thoughts and proach the tasks as a problem-solving team.
sensations that led her to escape from the sit- This includes deciding exactly where and when
uation: the sensations of unreality and fears of to practice in vivo exposure. In preparation for
losing touch with reality. Julie had waited for a practices, the patient identifies his or her
few minutes, then continued driving homean misappraisals about the task and generates
action that was highly reinforced by the thera- cognitive alternatives. The significant other is
pist. The third and fourth times were easier. encouraged to help the patient question his or
Julies husband Larry attended Session 5, so her own anxious thoughts. Role plays of this
that he could learn how to help Julie overcome type of questioning of the patient by the signifi-
her PDA. He was supportive and eager to help cant other may be conducted in the session, so
in any way possible, expressing frustration at that the therapist can provide corrective feed-
having had no idea how to help in the past. back to each partner. Throughout in vivo expo-
The general principles for involvement of sure, the significant other reminds the patient
significant others in treatment are as follows. to apply cognitive challenges and/or breathing
First, a treatment conceptualization is provided skills. Because the significant other is usually a
to the significant other to reduce his or her safety signal, tasks are less anxiety provoking.
frustration and/or negative attributions about However, the patient must be weaned from the
the patients emotional functioning (e.g., Oh, safety signal eventually. Therefore, initial at-
shes just making it up. Theres nothing really tempts at facing agoraphobic situations are
wrong with her or He has been like this since conducted with the significant other, and later
before we were married, and hell never trials are conducted alone. Weaning from the
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 45

significant other may be graduated, as in the moving away from the aisle, toward the middle
case of (1) Julie driving first with Larry in the of the row, because that was the condition in
car, (2) with him in a car behind, (3) meeting which she was most concerned that she would
the significant other at a destination point, and lose control and draw attention to herself. Julie
(4) driving alone. and Larry rehearsed their approach to the in
Very important to the success of this collabo- vivo exposure task in session, while the thera-
ration is style of communication. On the one pist provided corrective feedback using the
hand, significant others are discouraged from principles of communication and coping de-
magnifying the experience of panic and are en- scribed earlier. They were instructed to practice
couraged to help the patient apply coping state- this task at least three times over the next week.
ments when anxious. On the other hand, sig- On at least one occasion, Julie was to practice
nificant others are encouraged to be patient the task alone.
given the fact that progress for the patient may Next, interoceptive exposure was intro-
be erratic. The patient and significant other are duced. As with in vivo exposure, through re-
instructed to use a 0- to 10-point rating scale to peated exposures to feared sensations, patients
communicate with each other about the pa- learn that they are not harmed by the sensa-
tients current level of anxiety or distress, as a tions, and they achieve increased confidence in
way of diminishing the awkwardness associ- their ability to tolerate symptoms of anxiety.
ated with discussion of anxiety, especially in The procedure begins with assessment of the
public situations. The patient is warned about patients response to a series of standardized
the potential motivation to avoid discussing his exercises. The therapist models each exercise
or her feelings with the significant other, due to first. Then, after the patient has completed the
embarrassment or an attempt to avoid the anx- exercise, the therapist records the sensations,
iety for fear that such discussion and concen- anxiety level (0 to 10), sensation intensity (0 to
tration on anxiety may intensify his or her dis- 10), and similarity to naturally occurring panic
tress level. Avoidance of feelings is discouraged, sensations (0 to 10). The exercises include
because distraction is viewed as less beneficial shaking the head from side to side for 30 sec-
in the long term than is objectively facing what- onds; placing the head between the legs for 30
ever is distressing and learning that predicted seconds and lifting the head to an upright posi-
catastrophes do not occur. The patient is reas- tion quickly; running in place or using steps for
sured that the initial discomfort and embar- l minute; holding ones breath for as long as
rassment will most likely diminish as the cou- possible; complete body muscle tension for 1
ple becomes more familiar with discussing minute or holding a push-up position for as
anxiety levels and their management. Further- long as possible; spinning in a swivel chair for 1
more, the patients concerns about the signifi- minute; hyperventilating for 1 minute; breath-
cant other being insensitive or too pushy are ing through a narrow straw (with closed nasal
addressed. For example, a significant other passages) or breathing as slowly as possible for
may presume to know the patients level of 2 minutes; and staring at a spot on the wall or
anxiety and anxious thoughts without confir- at ones mirror image for 90 seconds. If none of
mation from the patient, or the significant these exercises produce sensations at least
other may become angry toward the patient for moderately similar to those that occur natu-
avoiding or escaping from situations, or being rally, other, individually tailored exercises are
fearful. All of these issues are described as rela- generated. For example, tightness around the
tively common and understandable patterns of chest may be induced by a deep breath before
communication that are nevertheless in need of hyperventilating; heat may be induced by wear-
correction. In-session role-playing of more ing heavy clothing in a heated room; choking
adaptive communication styles during episodes sensations may be induced by a tongue depres-
of heightened anxiety is a useful learning tech- sor, high-collared sweater, or a necktie; and
nique. On occasion, more specific communica- startle may be induced by an abrupt, loud noise
tions training may be beneficial, especially if in the midst of relaxation. For Julie, the sensa-
the partners frequently argue in their attempts tions produces by hyperventilating, spinning,
to generate items or methods for conducting in and staring at a spot on the wall were most
vivo exposure. anxiety provoking.
The next in vivo exposure task for Julie was Patients who report little or no fear because
to sit in a crowded movie theater, gradually they feel safe in the presence of the therapist are
46 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

asked to attempt each exercise alone, either (e.g., How is this different from when you feel
with the therapist out of the office or at home. dizzy at work?). In other words, cognitive re-
At the same time, discussing the influence of structuring extends the cognitive reprocessing
perceived safety as a moderating factor in the already taking place implicitly as a result of re-
amount of fear experienced reinforces the value peated interoceptive exposure.
of cognitive restructuring. For a minority of pa- Specific, previously unrecognized cognitions
tients, the known cause and course of the sen- sometimes become apparent during repeated
sations override the fear response; that is, be- exposure. For example, when Julie began to
cause the sensations are predictably related to a conduct repeated exposures to hyperventila-
clear cause (the interoceptive exercise), and be- tion and spinning, she became more aware of
cause the sensations can be relatively easily her implicit assumption that sensations of
controlled by simply terminating the intero- spaciness or lightheadedness would lead her to
ceptive exercise, fear is minimal. Under these lose control of her limbs. This related to her
conditions, discussion can productively center concern about causing an accident when driv-
on the misassumptions that render naturally ing. During repeated hyperventilation exer-
occurring sensations more frightening than the cises, and with prompting of what ifs from
ones produced by the interoceptive exercises. the therapist, Julie discovered her fear of not
Typically, these misassumptions are that natu- being able to move her arms or legs. The thera-
rally occurring sensations are unpredictable, pist then behaviorally challenged this assump-
that unpredictable sensations are more harm- tion by having Julie overbreathe for longer
ful, and that if naturally occurring sensations periods of time, followed immediately by walk-
are not controlled, then they pose a potential ing, picking up objects, and so on.
threat. The majority of patients fear at least Homework practice is very important, be-
several of the interoceptive exercises despite cause safety signals present in the clinic setting
knowing the cause of the sensations and their or that derive from the therapist per se may,
controllability. again, prevent generalizability to the natural
Interoceptive exercises rated as producing at setting. Patients are instructed to practice the
least somewhat similar sensations to naturally interoceptive items conducted in session on a
occurring panic (at least 3 on the 0- to 10-point daily basis, three times each day. Julie was to
scale) are selected for repeated exposure. A practice hyperventilation over the following
graduated approach is used for interoceptive week. She expressed some concern at doing the
exposure, beginning with the lowest item on exercises alone, so the therapist helped Julie to
the hierarchy established in Session 4. For each use her cognitive restructuring skills in relation
trial of exposure, the patient is asked to begin to being alone. In addition, more graduation of
the induction, to indicate when the sensations homework was suggested, so that Julie would
are first experienced (e.g., by raising a hand), practice hyperventilating when her husband
and to continue the induction for at least 30 was at home the first couple of days, then when
seconds longer to permit corrective learning. he was not at home the rest of the time.
After terminating the induction, anxiety is
rated, and the patient is given time to apply
Sessions 6 and 7
cognitive and breathing coping skills. Finally,
the therapist reviews the induction experience The primary goal of these sessions is to review
and the application of management strategies the past week of in vivo exposure practices, de-
with the patient. During this review, the thera- sign new exposures, review between-session
pist emphasizes the importance of experiencing practices of interoceptive exposure, conduct re-
the sensations fully during the induction, of peated interoceptive exposure in session, and
concentrating objectively on the sensations ver- assign those as homework for the next week.
sus distracting from them, and the importance The in vivo exposure is reviewed, as in the
of identifying specific cognitions and challeng- previous session. In this case, Julie and Larry
ing them by considering all of the evidence. In had done well with the movie theater practice.
addition, the therapist asks key questions to Julie even practiced going to the movies on her
help the patient realize his or her safety (e.g., own. On that occasion she reported higher
What would have happened if you had con- anxiety than when she was with Larry for fear
tinued spinning for another 60 seconds?), and of having to get up and leave the theater and
to generalize to naturally occurring experiences worries about bothering others in the audience.
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 47

The therapist helped Julie to identify what JULIE: Well, nothing. Id just feel terrible.
worry led her to think about leaving in the first THERAPIST: The word terrible carries a lot of
place; in other words, what did she think might meaning. Lets see if we can pin down your
happen if she could not leave? Julie indicated anxious thoughts that make the feelings so
that she had thoughts of losing control and terrible.
causing a scene, to which she was then
prompted to apply her cognitive restructuring JULIE: I just cant tolerate the feeling.
skills of evidence-based analyses and decatas- THERAPIST: What tells you that you cannot tol-
trophizing. She was ready to move to the next erate it? How do you know you cant toler-
items on her hierarchy: to spend 2 hours alone ate it?
at home during the day and to stay alone at
home as day turned to night. As with every in And the discussion continued, so that Julie
vivo exposure task, Julie identified what she realized what was most important for her to
most feared happening in those situations, and learn by the repeated hyperventilation: She
the best conditions under which to practice to could tolerate the sensations and anxiety.
learn that either those eventualities would not However, after the subsequent week of re-
happen and/or that she could cope with the peated practice, Julie remained cautious for
worst. fear that the exercises would cause her to revert
The past week of interoceptive exposure to her state of several weeks earlier; that is, she
practice is reviewed in session with a mind to- was concerned that the inductions would leave
ward avoidance: either overt failure to practice, her in a persistent symptomatic state. Further-
or covert avoidance by minimizing the intensity more, she was particularly reluctant to practice
or duration of the sensations induced, or limit- interoceptive exposure at the end of the day,
ing practice to the presence of a safety signal when she was more likely to feel unreal, or on a
(e.g., a significant other) or times when back- day when an important social event was sched-
ground anxiety is minimal. Reasons for avoid- uled. Again, these avoidance patterns were re-
ance may include continued misinterpretation lated to fears that the symptoms would become
of the dangers of bodily sensations (i.e., too intense or result in some type of mental or
I dont want to hyperventilate, because Im social catastrophe. These types of avoidance
afraid that I wont be able to stop over- patterns are addressed in the following vi-
breathing and no one will be there to help me) gnette:
or the belief that anxiety will not reduce with
repetition of the task. THERAPIST: When did you practice deliberately
For the first week, Julie practiced intero- spinning and hyperventilating?
ceptive exposure exercises about half of the
days between sessions. The therapist used a JULIE: Usually in the mornings. One day I left it
downward arrow method to explore Julies until the end of the day, and that turned out
reasons for not practicing every day. to be a bad idea. I felt terrible.
THERAPIST: Lets think about that a bit more.
JULIE: I tried hyperventilating on my own. What made it terrible when you practiced at
However, I wasnt very successful, because I the end of the day?
felt too scared and I stopped it as soon as I JULIE: Well, I was already feeling pretty
noticed the strange feelings. unrealI usually do around that time of the
THERAPIST: What did you think would happen day. So I was much more anxious about the
if the sensations became more intense? symptoms.
JULIE: I thought the feelings would get worse THERAPIST: Being more anxious implies that
and worse and worse, and just overwhelm you thought the symptoms were more harm-
me. I didnt want to have that feeling of ful. Is that what happened on the day that
panic again. you practiced interoceptive exposure when
you were already feeling unreal?
THERAPIST: If you did become overwhelmed,
then what would happen to you? JULIE: Yes, I felt that because I was already feel-
ing unreal, I was on the edge, and that I
JULIE: Then Id feel really terrible. might push myself over the edge if I tried to
THERAPIST: And if you felt really terrible? increase the feelings of unreality.
48 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

THERAPIST: What do you mean by push my- THERAPIST: From our previous discussions, lets
self over the edge? think of potential reasons why you might
JULIE: That I would make the feelings so intense feel dizzy or unreal at a particular time?
that I really would lose itgo crazy. JULIE: I know. I have to keep remembering that
THERAPIST: So there is one of those hypotheses: it could be my breathing, or just feeling anx-
to feel more intense unreality means to be ious, or tired, or a bunch of different things.
closer to going crazy. Lets examine the evi- THERAPIST: OK. And why is it so important to
dence. Is it necessarily the case that more in- know when those feelings will occur?
tense unreality means you are closer to crazi- JULIE: Because I dont want them to be there at
ness? all.
THERAPIST: And why not . . . what are you
In sessions, the therapist continued practice
afraid of?
of interoceptive exposure with the next item on
Julies hierarchy, which was to stare at a spot JULIE: I guess its the same old thing . . . that Ill
on the wall and to spin around. lose it somehow?
The homework from this session is to con- THERAPIST: So lets go back to the cognitive re-
tinue self-monitoring, in vivo exposure to an structuring that you have been doing. What
item from the agoraphobia hierarchy at least specifically are you afraid of? How likely is it
three times, and daily practice of interoceptive to happen? What are the alternatives?
exposure.
JULIE: I understand.
THERAPIST: So, now you see that whether the
Sessions 8 and 9 sensations of dizziness or unreality are pro-
duced by anxiety, overbreathing, diet, or the
The primary goals of these sessions are to con-
exercises we do here, theyre all the same
tinue in vivo exposure, as described in the prior
they are just uncomfortable physical sensa-
sessions, and to extend interoceptive exposure
tions. The only reason they perturb you
to natural activities. Julie had practiced staying
more when you are driving or at home is be-
at home for 2 hours alone during the day and
cause of the meaning you still give to them in
as daylight turned to dusk, with good results.
those situations.
In particular, she experienced a couple of panic
attacks during these in vivo exposure practices
but continued with the assigned practice re- Naturalistic interoceptive exposure refers
gardless. This was critical for Julie, as it al- to exposure to daily tasks or activities that have
lowed her to learn that she could survive the been avoided or endured with dread because of
feeling of panic; it was the first time she had re- the associated sensations. Typical examples in-
mained in a situation despite panicking. clude aerobic exercise or vigorous physical ac-
In reviewing the weeks practice of intero- tivity, running up flights of stairs, eating foods
ceptive exposure, it became apparent that Julie that create a sensation of fullness or are associ-
was separating the practices from real-life ex- ated with sensations of choking, saunas or
periences of bodily sensations in a way that steamy showers, driving with the windows
would limit generalization. This was addressed rolled up and the heater on, caffeine consump-
as follows: tion, and so on. (Of course, these exercises may
be modified in the event of actual medical com-
JULIE: After spinning and hyperventilating sev- plications, such as asthma or high blood pres-
eral times, I really do feel much less anxious. sure.) From a list of typically feared activities
I was terrified at the start, but now I am only and generation of items specific to the individ-
mildly anxious, if at all. But this is different uals own experience, a hierarchy is established.
than what happens to me when Im on the Each item is ranked in terms of anxiety ratings
freeway or at home. (010). Julies hierarchy was as follows: look-
ing out through venetian blinds (anxiety = 3);
THERAPIST: How is it different? watching One Flew over the Cuckoos Nest
JULIE: I dont know when the feelings of dizzi- (anxiety = 4); playing tennis (anxiety = 4); scan-
ness and unreality are going to hit. ning labels on a supermarket shelf (anxiety =
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 49

5); concentrating on needlework for an hour cises over the past week, and to combine ex
(anxiety = 6); driving with windows closed and posure to feared and avoided agoraphobic situ-
heater on (anxiety = 7); a nightclub with strobe ations with deliberate induction of feared sen-
lights (anxiety = 8); and rides at Disneyland sations into those situations. As with earlier
(anxiety = 10). interoceptive exposure homework assign-
Like the symptom exercises, the activity exer- ments, it is important to evaluate and correct
cises are designed to be systematically graduated tendencies to avoid naturalistic interoceptive
and repetitive. Patients may apply the breathing exposure tasks, mainly by considering the un-
and cognitive skills while the activity is ongoing. derlying misassumptions that are leading to
This is in contrast to the symptom induction ex- avoidance. Remember also that a form of
ercises, in which coping skills are used only after avoidance is to rely on safety signals or safety
completion of the symptom exercise, because behaviors, so careful questioning of the way in
the activities often are considerably longer than which the naturalistic exposure was conducted,
the symptom induction exercises. Nevertheless, and under what conditions, may help to iden-
patients are encouraged to focus on the sensa- tify inadvertent reliance on these unnecessary
tions and experience them fully throughout the precautions. Julie reported that she was suc-
activity, and not use the coping skills to prevent cessful in looking at the venetian blinds, even
or remove the sensations. though she experienced sensations of unreality.
Patients are instructed to identify maladap- She had more difficulty watching One Flew
tive cognitions and rehearse cognitive restruc- over the Cuckoos Nest, because it tapped di-
turing before beginning each activity. In-session rectly into her worst fears of losing touch with
rehearsal of the cognitive preparation allows reality permanently; she tried but terminated
therapists to provide corrective feedback. Julie the film early. The second time she watched it
did this with her therapist for her first two nat- with Larry, who prompted Julie to remember
uralistic activities, which were to look at her cognitive and breathing skills, and she was
venetian blinds and to watch One Flew over able to watch the entire film. She watched the
the Cuckoos Nest. Julie realized that she was film one more time on her own. Two new natu-
most worried about sensations of unreality and ralistic exposure items were selected for the
fears of going crazy, although, as a result of her coming week, with special attention to wean-
various exposure exercises up to this point, she ing or removing safety signals and safety be-
quickly was able to recognize that such sensa- haviors, and rehearsal of cognitive restructur-
tions were harmless and that she could tolerate ing in session. For Julie, these were playing
them, and that such fears were unrealistic tennis (something she had avoided for years)
based on the evidence. and scanning items on supermarket shelves.
As with all exposures, it is important to iden- The notion of deliberately inducing feared
tify and remove (gradually, if necessary) safety bodily symptoms within the context of feared
signals or protective behaviors, such as porta- agoraphobic situations derives from the evi-
ble phones, lucky charms, walking slowly, dence that compound relationships between
standing slowly, and staying in close proximity external and internal cues can be the most po-
to medical facilities. These safety signals and tent anxiogenic agent; that is, it is neither just
behaviors reinforce catastrophic misappraisals the situation nor just the bodily sensation that
about bodily sensations. Julies safety behaviors triggers distress, but the combination of the
were identified as checking the time on the bodily sensation and the situation that is most
clock (as a reassurance that she was in touch distressing. Thus, effective exposure targets
with reality) and pinching herself (again, to feel both types of cues. Otherwise, patients run the
reality). She was asked to practice the two nat- risk of later return of fear. For example, re-
uralistic interoceptive exposures at least three peated practice walking through a shopping
times each before the next treatment session, mall without feeling dizzy does not adequately
without the safety behaviors. prepare patients for occasions on which they
feel dizzy walking through a shopping mall,
and without such preparation, patients may be
Sessions 10 and 11
likely to panic and escape should they feel dizzy
The primary goals of these sessions are to re- in this or similar situations in the future.
view the in vivo and naturalistic exposure exer- Wearing heavy clothing in a restaurant helps
50 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

patients to learn to be less afraid of not only the Session 12


restaurant but also of feeling hot in a restau-
rant. Other examples include drinking coffee The last treatment session reviews the princi-
before any of the agoraphobic tasks, turning ples and skills learned and provides the patient
off the air-conditioning or turning on the with a template of coping techniques for poten-
heater while driving, breathing very slowly in a tial, high-risk situations in the future. Julie fin-
crowded area, and so on. ished the program after 12 sessions, by which
Patients choose an item from their hierarchy time she had not panicked in 8 weeks, rarely
of agoraphobia situations, either one already experienced dizziness or feelings of unreality,
completed or a new item, and also choose and was driving further distances. There were
which symptom to induce and ways of induc- some situations still in need of exposure prac-
ing that symptom in that situation. Julies task tices (e.g., driving very long distances away
was to drink coffee as she went to a movie. She from home and on the freeway at dusk). How-
expressed the following concerns: ever, Julie and Larry agreed to continue in vivo
exposure practices over the next few months to
JULIE: Do you really think I am ready to drink consolidate her learning and to continue her
coffee and go to the movie? improvement.
THERAPIST: What worries you about the combi-
nation of coffee and the movie theater?
CONCLUSION
JULIE: Well, Ive practiced in the movie theaters
a lot, so that feels pretty good, but the coffee
As noted earlier in this chapter, cognitive-
is going to make me feel very anxious.
behavioral treatments for panic disorder and
THERAPIST: And if you feel very anxious in the agoraphobia are highly effective and represent
movie theater, then what? one of the success stories of psychotherapy. Be-
JULIE: Then, I dont know what. Maybe I will tween 80 and 100% of patients undergoing
get those old feelings again, like I have to get these treatments will be panic free at the end of
out. treatment and maintain these gains for up to 2
THERAPIST: Based on everything you have years. These results reflect substantially more
learned, how can you manage those feelings? durability than medication treatments. Fur-
thermore, between 50 and 80% of these pa-
JULIE: Well, I guess my number one rule is never tients reach a point of high end state, mean-
to leave a situation because I am feeling anx- ing within normative realms of symptoms and
ious. I will stick it out, no matter what. functioning, and many of the remainder have
THERAPIST: That sounds great. It means you are only residual symptomatology. Nevertheless,
accepting the anxiety and taking the oppor- major difficulties remain.
tunity to learn that you can tolerate it. What First, these treatments are not foolproof. As
else? many as 50% of patients retain substantial
JULIE: I can ask myself what is the worst that symptomatology despite improvement from
can happen. I know I am not going to die or baseline, and this is particularly likely for those
go crazy. I will probably feel my heart rate with more severe agoraphobia. Further re-
going pretty fast because of the coffee. search must determine how treatments can be
improved or better individualized to alleviate
THERAPIST: And if your heart rate goes fast,
continued suffering. For example, one of us
what does that mean?
(D. H. B.) saw a patient several years ago who
JULIE: I guess it just means that my heart rate had completed an initial course of treatment
will go fast. but required continued periodic visits for over
THERAPIST: This will be a really good way for 4 years. This patient was essentially improved
you to learn that you can tolerate the anxiety for approximately 9 months but found himself
and the symptoms of a racing heart. relapsing during a particularly stressful time at
work. A few booster sessions restored his func-
The homework for this session is to continue tioning, but he was back in the office 6 months
self-monitoring, to practice in vivo exposure later with reemerging symptomatology. This
combined with interoceptive exposure, and to pattern essentially continued for 4 years and
continue naturalistic interoceptive exposure. was characterized by symptom-free periods fol-
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 51

lowed by (seemingly) stress-related relapses. could be further from the truth. The clinical art
Furthermore, the reemerging panic disorder involved in this, and in all treatments described
would sometimes last from 3 to 6 months be- in this book, requires a careful adaptation to
fore disappearing again, perhaps with the help these treatment strategies to the individual
of a booster session. case. Many of Julies symptoms revolved
Although this case was somewhat unusual in around feelings of unreality (derealization and
our experience, there was no easy explanation depersonalization). Emphasizing rational ex-
for this pattern of relapses and remissions. The planations for the production of such feelings,
patient, who has a graduate degree, understood as well as adapting cognitive and exposure ex-
and accepted the treatment model and fully im- ercises to maximize these sensations, is an im-
plemented the treatment program. There was portant part of this treatment program. Al-
also no question that he fully comprehended though standard interoceptive provocation
the nature of anxiety and panic, and the intri- exercises seemed sufficient to produce relevant
cacies of the therapeutic strategies. While in the symptomatology in Julies case, we have had to
office, he could recite chapter and verse on the develop new procedures to deal with people
nature of these emotional states, as well as the with more idiosyncratic symptoms and fears,
detailed process of his own reaction while in particularly those involving feelings of unreal-
these states. Nevertheless, away from the of- ity or dissociation. Other innovations in both
fice, the patient found himself repeatedly hop- cognitive and behavioral procedures will be re-
ing that he would not go over the brink dur- quired by individual therapists as they apply
ing a panic, despite verbalizing very clearly the these procedures.
irrationality of this concept while in the office. Although these new treatments seem highly
In addition, he continued to attempt to reduce successful when applied by trained therapists,
minor physiological symptoms associated with treatment is not readily available to individuals
anxiety and panic, despite a full rational under- with these disorders. In fact, these treatments,
standing of the nature of these symptoms (in- although brief and structured, are far more dif-
cluding the fact that they are the same symp- ficult to deliver than, for example, pharmaco-
toms that he experienced during a state of logical treatments (which are also often misap-
excitement, which he enjoyed). His limited tol- plied). Furthermore, few people are currently
erance of these physical sensations was also skilled in the application of these treatments.
puzzling in view of his tremendous capacity to What seems to be needed for these and other
endure pain. successful psychosocial treatments is a new
Any number of factors might account for method of disseminating them, so that they
what seemed to be overvalued ideation or reach the maximum number of patients. Modi-
very strongly held irrational ideas during peri- fication of these treatment protocols into more
ods of anxiety, including the fact that the pa- user-friendly formats, as well as brief periods of
tient has several relatives who have repeatedly training for qualified therapists to a point of
been hospitalized for emotional disorders certification, would be important steps in suc-
(seemingly mood disorders or schizoaffective cessfully delivering these treatments. This may
disorder). Nevertheless, the fact remains that be difficult to accomplish.
we do not know why this patient did not re-
spond as quickly as most people. Eventually he
made a full recovery, received several promo- NOTE
tions at work, and considered treatment to be
the turning point in his life. But it took 5 years. 1. Specific phobias were not assessed, but by being
Other patients, as noted earlier, seem unin- most circumscribed, they would be hypothesized to
load the least on negative affectivity.
terested in engaging in treatment, preferring to
conceptualize their problems as chemical im-
balances. Still others have difficulty grasping
REFERENCES
some of the cognitive strategies, and further at-
tempts are necessary to make these treatments Allen, L. B., & Barlow, D. H. (2006). The treatment of
more user-friendly. panic disorder: Outcomes and basic processes. In B.
It also may seem that this structured, O. Rothbaum (Ed.), Pathological anxiety: Emotional
protocol-driven treatment is applied in a very processing in etiology and treatment (pp. 166180).
standard fashion across individuals. Nothing New York: Guilford Press.
52 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

Alneas, R., & Torgersen, S. (1990). DSM-III personality ture and treatment of anxiety and panic (2nd ed.).
disorders among patients with major depression, New York: Guilford Press.
anxiety disorders, and mixed conditions. Journal of Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Craske, M. G. (1994).
Nervous and Mental Disease, 178, 693698. Definitions of panic attacks and panic disorder in the
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic DSM-IV: Implications for research. Journal of Ab-
and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). normal Psychology, 103, 553564.
Washington, DC: Author. Barlow, D. H., Cohen, A., Waddell, M., Vermilyea, J.,
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic Klosko, J., Blanchard, E., et al. (1984). Panic and
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). generalized anxiety disorders: Nature and treatment.
Washington, DC: Author. Behavior Therapy, 15, 431449.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic Barlow, D. H., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mastery of
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., your anxiety and panic: Patient workbook (4th ed.).
text rev.). Washington, DC: Author. New York: Oxford University Press.
Amering, M., Katschnig, H., Berger, P., Windhaber, J., Barlow, D. H., Craske, M. G., Cerny, J. A., & Klosko, J.
Baischer, W., & Dantendorfer, K. (1997). Embarrass- S. (1989). Behavioral treatment of panic disorder.
ment about the first panic attack predicts agorapho- Behavior Therapy, 20, 261282.
bia in disorder patients. Behaviour Research and Barlow, D. H., Gorman, J. M., Shear, M. K., & Woods,
Therapy, 35, 517521. S. W. (2000). Cognitive-behavioral therapy, imipra-
Antony, M. M., Brown, T. A., Craske, M. G., Barlow, D. mine, or their combination for panic disorder: A ran-
H., Mitchell, W. B., & Meadows, E. A. (1995). Accu- domized controlled trial. Journal of the American
racy of heartbeat perception in panic disorder, social Medical Association, 283(19), 25292536.
phobia, and nonanxious subjects. Journal of Anxiety Barlow, D. H., OBrien, G. T., & Last, C. G. (1984).
Disorders, 9, 355371. Couples treatment of agoraphobia. Behavior Ther-
Antony, M. M., Ledley, D. B., Liss, A., & Swinson, R. P. apy, 15(1), 4158.
(2006). Responses to symptom induction exercises in Barlow, D. H., OBrien, G. T., Last, C. G., & Holden, A.
panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, E. (1983). Couples treatment of agoraphobia. In K.
8598. D. Craig & R. J. McMahon (Eds.), Advances in clini-
Antony, M. M., Meadows, E. A., Brown, T. A., & cal behavior therapy (pp. 99127). New York: Brun-
Barlow, D. H. (1994). Cardiac awareness before and ner/Mazel.
after cognitive-behavioral treatment for panic disor- Barlow, D. H., Vermilyea, J., Blanchard, E., Vermilyea,
der. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 8, 341350. B., Di Nardo, P., & Cerny, J. (1985). Phenomenon of
Arnow, B. A., Taylor, C. B., Agras, W. S., & Telch, M. J. panic. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 320
(1985). Enhancing agoraphobia treatment outcome 328.
by changing couple communication patterns. Behav- Barsky, A. J., Cleary, P. D., Sarnie, M. K., & Ruskin, J.
ior Therapy, 16, 452467. N. (1994). Panic disorder, palpitations, and the
Arntz, A., & van den Hout, M. (1996). Psychological awareness of cardiac activity. Journal of Nervous and
treatments of panic disorder without agoraphobia: Mental Disease, 182, 6371.
Cognitive therapy versus applied relaxation. Behav- Basoglu, M., Marks, I. M., Kilic, C., Brewin, C. R., &
iour Research and Therapy, 34, 113121. Swinson, R. P. (1994). Alprazolam and exposure for
Arrindell, W., & Emmelkamp, P. (1987). Psychological panic disorder with agoraphobia: Attribution of im-
states and traits in female agoraphobics: A controlled provement to medication predicts subsequent re-
study. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral lapse. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 652659.
Assessment, 9, 237253. Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A.
Azrin, N., Naster, B., & Jones, R. (1973). Reciprocity (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety:
counselling: A rapid learning-based procedure for Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and
marital counselling. Behaviour Research and Ther- Clinical Psychology, 56, 893897.
apy, 11, 365382. Beck, J. G., & Shipherd, J. C. (1997). Repeated expo-
Bandelow, B., Spath, C., Tichaner, G. A., Brooks, A., sure to interoceptive cues: Does habituation of fear
Hajak, G., & Ruther, E. (2002). Early traumatic life occur in panic disorder patients?: A preliminary re-
events, parental attitudes, family history, and birth port. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 551557.
risk factors in patients with panic disorder. Compre- Beck, J. G., Shipherd, J. C., & Zebb, B. J. (1997). How
hensive Psychiatry, 43, 269278. does interoceptive exposure for panic disorder
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying work?: An uncontrolled case study. Journal of Anxi-
theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, ety Disorders, 11, 541556.
84, 191215. Beck, J. G., Stanley, M. A., Baldwin, L. E., Deagle, E. A.,
Barlow, D. H. (1988). Anxiety and its disorders: The na- & Averill, P. M. (1994). Comparison of cognitive
ture and treatment of anxiety and panic. New York: therapy and relaxation training for panic disorder.
Guilford Press. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62,
Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: The na- 818826.
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 53

Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Marrs, A., & Moore, P. treatment outcome and course of comorbid diagno-
(1997). Panic disorder and agoraphobia in consecu- ses following treatment. Journal of Consulting and
tively referred children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63, 408418.
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy- Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1995). Long-term out-
chiatry, 36(12), 214223. come in cognitive-behavioral treatment of panic dis-
Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1992). A new theory of dis- order: Clinical predictors and alternative strategies
use and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In A. for assessment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Healy, S. Kosslyn, & R. Shiffrin (Eds.), From learning Psychology, 63, 754765.
processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of Brown, T. A., Campbell, L. A., Lehman, C. L., Grisham,
William K. Estes (Vol. 2, pp. 3567). Hillsdale, NJ: J. R., & Mancill, R. B. (2001). Current and lifetime
Erlbaum. comorbidity of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disor-
Black, D. W., Monahan, P., Wesner, R., Gabel, J., & ders in a large clinical sample. Journal of Abnormal
Bowers, W. (1996). The effect of fluvoxamine, cogni- Psychology, 110(4), 585599.
tive therapy, and placebo on abnormal personality Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (1998).
traits in 44 patients with panic disorder. Journal of Structural relationships among dimensions of the
Personality Disorders, 10, 185194. DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders and dimensions
Bland, K., & Hallam, R. (1981). Relationship between of negative affect, positive affect, and autonomic
response to graded exposure and marital satisfaction arousal. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(2),
in agoraphobics. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 179192.
19, 335338. Brown, T. A., Di Nardo, P. A., Lehman, C. L., & Camp-
Bonn, J. A., Harrison, J., & Rees, W. (1971). Lactate- bell, L. A. (2001). Reliability of DSM-IV anxiety and
induced anxiety: Therapeutic application. British mood disorders: Implications for the classification of
Journal of Psychiatry, 119, 468470. emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
Borkovec, T., Weerts, T., & Bernstein, D. (1977). As- ogy, 110(1), 4958.
sessment of anxiety. In A. Ciminero, K. Calhoun, & Brown, T. A., White, K. S., Forsyth, J. P., & Barlow, D.
H. Adams (Eds.), Handbook of behavioral assess- H. (2004). The structure of perceived emotional con-
ment (pp. 367428). New York: Wiley. trol: Psychometric properties of a revised Anxiety
Bouchard, S., Gauthier, J., Laberge, B., French, D., Control Questionnaire. Behavior Therapy, 35(1),
Pelletier, M., & Godbout, D. (1996). Exposure ver- 7599.
sus cognitive restructuring in the treatment of panic Buglass, P., Clarke, J., Henderson, A., & Presley, A.
disorder with agoraphobia. Behaviour Research and (1977). A study of agoraphobic housewives. Psycho-
Therapy, 34, 213224. logical Medicine, 7, 7386.
Bouchard, S., Paquin, B., Payeur, R., Allard, M., Rivard, Cain, C. K., Blouin, A. M., & Barad, M. (2004).
V., Gournier, T., et al. (2004). Delivering cognitive- Adrenergic transmission facilitates extinction of con-
behavior therapy for panic disorder with agorapho- ditional fear in mice. Learning and Memory, 11(2),
bia in videoconference [Special issue: Telemedicine in 179187.
Canada]. Telemedicine Journal and E-Health, 10(1), Carlbring, P., Ekselius, L., & Andersson, G. (2003).
1324. Treatment of panic disorder via the Internet: A ran-
Bouton, M. E. (1993). Context, time and memory re- domized trial of CBT vs. applied relaxation. Journal
trieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 9099. 34, 129140.
Bouton, M. E., Mineka, S., & Barlow, D. H. (2001). A Carter, M. M., Sbrocco, T., Gore, K. L., Marin, N. W.,
modern learning-theory perspective on the etiology & Lewis, E. L. (2003). Cognitive-behavioral group
of panic disorder. Psychological Review, 108(1), 4 therapy versus a wait-list control in the treatment of
32. African American women with panic disorder. Cog-
Bouton, M. E., & Swartzentruber, D. (1991). Sources of nitive Therapy and Research, 27(5), 505518.
relapse after extinction in Pavlovian conditioning Cerny, J. A., Barlow, D. H., Craske, M. G., & Himadi,
and instrumental conditioning. Behavioral Neurosci- W. G. (1987). Couples treatment of agoraphobia: A
ence, 104, 4455. two-year follow-up. Behavior Therapy, 18, 401415.
Boyd, J. H. (1986). Use of mental health services for the Chambless, D. L. (1990). Spacing of exposure sessions
treatment of panic disorder. American Journal of in treatment of agoraphobia and simple phobia.
Psychiatry, 143, 15691574. Behavior Therapy, 21, 217229.
Broocks, A., Bandelow, B., Pekrun, G., George, A., Chambless, D. L., Caputo, G., Bright, P., & Gallagher,
Meyer, T., Bartmann, U., et al. (1998). Comparison R. (1984). Assessment of fear in agoraphobics: The
of aerobic exercise, clomipramine, and placebo in the Body Sensations Questionnaire and the Agoraphobic
treatment of panic disorder. American Journal of Cognitions Questionnaire. Journal of Consulting and
Psychiatry, 155, 603609. Clinical Psychology, 52, 10901097.
Brown, T. A., Antony, M. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1995). Chambless, D. L., Caputo, G., Gracely, S., Jasin, E., &
Diagnostic comorbidity in panic disorder: Effect on Williams, C. (1985). The Mobility Inventory for Ag-
54 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

oraphobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, D. C. (2003). Panic control treatment of agorapho-
3544. bia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17(3), 321333.
Chambless, D. L., & Renneberg, B. (1988, September). Craske, M. G., Farchione, T., Allen, L., Barrios, V.,
Personality disorders of agoraphobics. Paper pre- Stoyanova, M., & Rose, D. (2007). Cognitive behav-
sented at World Congress of Behavior Therapy, Edin- ioral therapy for panic disorder and comorbidity:
burgh, Scotland. More of the same or less of more. Behaviour Re-
Chaplin, E. W., & Levine, B. A. (1981). The effects of search and Therapy, 45(6), 10951109.
total exposure duration and interrupted versus con- Craske, M. G., & Freed, S. (1995). Expectations about
tinuous exposure in flooding therapy. Behavior Ther- arousal and nocturnal panic. Journal of Abnormal
apy, 12(3), 360368. Psychology, 104, 567575.
Clark D. A. (1996). Panic disorder: From theory to ther- Craske, M. G., Glover, D., & DeCola, J. (1995). Pre-
apy. In P. M. Salkovskis (Ed.), From frontiers of cog- dicted versus unpredicted panic attacks: Acute versus
nitive therapy: The state of the art and beyond (pp. general distress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
318344). New York: Guilford Press. 104, 214223.
Clark, D. M. (1986). A cognitive approach to panic. Be- Craske, M. G., Golinelli, D., Stein, M. B., Roy-Byrne, P.,
haviour Research and Therapy, 24, 461470. Bystritsky, A., & Sherbourne, C. (2005). Does the ad-
Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (1993). An overview of the dition of cognitive behavioral therapy improve panic
cognitive theory and treatment of panic disorder. Ap- disorder treatment outcome relative to medication
plied and Preventive Psychology, 2, 131139. alone in the primary-care setting? Psychological
Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P., & Chalkley, A. (1985). Re- Medicine, 35(11), 16451654.
spiratory control as a treatment for panic attacks. Craske, M. G., Lang, A. J., Aikins, D., & Mystkowski,
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psy- J. L. (2005). Cognitive behavioral therapy for noctur-
chiatry, 16, 2330. nal panic. Behavior Therapy, 36, 4354.
Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P., Gelder, M., Koehler, C., Craske, M. G., Lang, A. J., Rowe, M., DeCola, J. P.,
Martin, M., Anastasiades, P., et al. (1988). Tests of a Simmons, J., Mann, C., et al. (2002). Presleep attri-
cognitive theory of panic. In I. Hand & H. Wittchen butions about arousal during sleep: Nocturnal panic.
(Eds.), Panic and phobias II (pp. 7190). Berlin: Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 5362.
Springer-Verlag. Craske, M. G., Maidenberg, E., & Bystritsky, A. (1995).
Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P. M., Hackmann, A., Middle- Brief cognitive-behavioral versus non directive ther-
ton, H., Anastasiades, P., & Gelder, M. (1994). A apy for panic disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy
comparison of cognitive therapy, applied relaxation and Experimental Psychiatry, 26, 113120.
and imipramine in the treatment of panic disorder. Craske, M. G., Miller, P. P., Rotunda, R., & Barlow, D.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 759769. H. (1990). A descriptive report of features of initial
Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P. M., Hackmann, A., Wells, unexpected panic attacks in minimal and extensive
A., Ludgate, J., & Gelder, M. (1999). Brief cognitive avoiders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 395
therapy for panic disorder: A randomized controlled 400.
trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Craske, M. G., & Mystkowski, J. L. (2006). Exposure
67, 583589. therapy and extinction: Clinical studies. In M. G.
Cote, G., Gauthier, J. G., Laberge, B., Cormier, H. J., & Craske, D. Hermans, & D. Vansteenwegen (Eds.),
Plamondon, J. (1994). Reduced therapist contact in Fear and learning: From basic processes to clinical
the cognitive behavioral treatment of panic disorder. implications (pp. 217233). Washington, DC: Amer-
Behavior Therapy, 25, 123145. ican Psychological Association.
Cox, B. J., Endler, N. S., & Swinson, R. P. (1995). An Craske, M. G., Poulton, R., Tsao, J. C. I., & Plotkin, D.
examination of levels of agoraphobic severity in (2001). Paths to panicagoraphobia: An exploratory
panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, analysis from age 3 to 21 in an unselected birth co-
5762. hort. American Journal of Child and Adolescent Psy-
Craske, M. G., & Barlow, D. H. (1988). A review of the chiatry, 40, 556563.
relationship between panic and avoidance. Clinical Craske, M. G., Rapee, R. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1988).
Psychology Review, 8, 667685. The significance of panicexpectancy for individual
Craske, M. G., & Barlow, D. H. (1989). Nocturnal patterns of avoidance. Behavior Therapy, 19, 577
panic. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 592.
177(3), 160167. Craske, M. G., & Rowe, M. K. (1997a). A comparison
Craske, M. G., & Barlow, D. H. (2006). Mastery of of behavioral and cognitive treatments of phobias. In
your anxiety and panic: Therapist guide (3rd ed.). G. C. L. Davey (Ed.), Phobiasa handbook of the-
New York: Oxford University Press. ory, research and treatment (pp. 247280). West Sus-
Craske, M. G., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1991). sex, UK: Wiley.
Behavioral treatment of panic disorder: A two-year Craske, M. G., & Rowe, M. K. (1997b). Nocturnal
follow-up. Behavior Therapy, 22, 289304. panic. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 4,
Craske, M. G., DeCola, J. P., Sachs, A. D., & Pontillo, 153174.
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 55

Craske, M. G., Rowe, M., Lewin, M., & Noriega- al treatment of agoraphobia: A meta-analytic review.
Dimitri, R. (1997). Interoceptive exposure versus Anxiety Research, 2(2), 6983.
breathing retraining within cognitive-behavioural Di Nardo, P., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1994).
therapy for panic disorder with agoraphobia. British Anxiety Disorders Interview ScheduleFourth Edi-
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 8599. tion (ADIS-IV). New York: Oxford University Press.
Craske, M. G., Roy-Byrne, P., Stein, M. B., Donald- Dreessen, L., Arntz, A., Luttels, C., & Sallaerts, S.
Sherbourne, C., Bystritsky, A., Katon, W., et al. (1994). Personality disorders do not influence the re-
(2002). Treating panic disorder in primary care: A sults of cognitive behavior therapies for anxiety dis-
collaborative care intervention. General Hospital orders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 35(4), 265274.
Psychiatry, 24(3), 148155. Dworkin, B. R., & Dworkin, S. (1999). Heterotopic and
Craske, M. G., Roy-Byrne, P., Stein, M. B., Sullivan, G., homotopic classical conditioning of the baroreflex.
Hazlett-Stevens, H., Bystritsky, A., et al. (2006). CBT Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science,
intensity and outcome for panic disorder in a primary 34(3), 158176.
care setting. Behavior Therapy, 37, 112119. Ehlers, A. (1995). A 1-year prospective study of panic
Craske, M. G., Street, L., & Barlow, D. H. (1989). In- attacks: Clinical course and factors associated with
structions to focus upon or distract from internal maintenance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104,
cues during exposure treatment for agoraphobic 164172.
avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, Ehlers, A., & Breuer, P. (1992). Increased cardiac aware-
663672. ness in panic disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
Craske, M. G., & Tsao, J. I. C. (1999). Self-monitoring ogy, 101, 371382.
with panic and anxiety disorders. Psychological As- Ehlers, A., & Breuer, P. (1996). How good are patients
sessment, 11, 466479. with panic disorder at perceiving their heartbeats?
Dattilio, F. M., & Salas-Auvert, J. A. (2000). Panic dis- Biological Psychology, 42, 165182.
order: Assessment and treatment through a wide-an- Ehlers, A., Breuer, P., Dohn, D., & Fiegenbaum, W.
gle lens. Phoenix, AZ: Zeig, Tucker. (1995). Heartbeat perception and panic disorder:
Deacon, B., & Abramowitz, J. (2006). A pilot study of Possible explanations for discrepant findings. Behav-
two-day cognitive-behavioral therapy for panic dis- iour Research and Therapy, 33, 6976.
order. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 807 Ehlers, A., & Margraf, J. (1989). The psychophysio-
817. logical model of panic attacks. In P. M. G.
de Beurs, E., Lange, A., van Dyck, R., & Koele, P. Emmelkamp (Ed.), Anxiety disorders: Annual series
(1995). Respiratory training prior to exposure in of European research in behavior therapy (Vol. 4,
vivo in the treatment of panic disorder with agora- pp. 129). Amsterdam: Swets.
phobia: Efficacy and predictors of outcome. Austra- Ehlers, A., Margraf, J., Davies, S., & Roth, W. T.
lian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 29, (1988). Selective processing of threat cues in subjects
104113. with panic attacks. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 201
de Beurs, E., van Balkom, A. J., Lange, A., Koele, P., 219.
& van Dyck, R. (1995). Treatment of panic dis- Ehlers, A., Margraf, J., Roth, W. T., Taylor, C. B., &
order with agoraphobia: Comparison of fluvoxa- Birbaumer, N. (1988). Anxiety induced by false heart
mine, placebo, and psychological panic management rate feedback in patients with panic disorder. Behav-
combined with exposure and of exposure in vivo iour Research and Therapy, 26(1), 111.
alone. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 683 Eifert, G. H., & Forsyth, J. P. (2005). Acceptance and
691. commitment therapy for anxiety disorders: A practi-
Deckert, J., Nothen, M. M., Franke, P., Delmo, C., tioners treatment guide to using mindfulness, accep-
Fritze, J., Knapp, M., et al. (1998). Systematic muta- tance, and value-based behavior change strategies.
tion screening and association study of the A1 and Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
A2a adenosine receptor genes in panic disorder sug- Eley, T. C. (2001). Contributions of behavioral genetics
gest a contribution of the A2a gene to the develop- research: Quantifying genetic, shared environmental
ment of disease. Molecular Psychiatry, 3, 8185. and nonshared environmental influences. In M. W.
de Jong, M. G., & Bouman, T. K. (1995). Panic disor- Vasey & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental
der: A baseline period: Predictability of agoraphobic psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 4559). New York:
avoidance behavior. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 9, Oxford University Press.
185199. Emmelkamp, P. (1980). Agoraphobics interpersonal
De Ruiter, C., Garssen, B., Rijken, H., & Kraaimaat, problems. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, 1303
F. (1989). The hyperventilation syndrome in panic 1306.
disorder, agoraphobia, and generalized anxiety dis- Emmelkamp, P. M., Brilman, E., Kuiper, H., & Mersch,
order. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27(4), P. (1986). The treatment of agoraphobia: A compari-
447452. son of self-instructional training, rational emotive
Dewey, D., & Hunsley, J. (1990). The effects of marital therapy, and exposure in vivo. Behavior Modifica-
adjustment and spouse involvement on the behavior- tion, 10, 3753.
56 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

Emmelkamp, P. M., Kuipers, A. C., & Eggeraat, J. B. Ghosh, A., & Marks, I. M. (1987). Self-treatment of ag-
(1978). Cognitive modification versus prolonged ex- oraphobia by exposure. Behavior Therapy, 18, 316.
posure in vivo: A comparison with agoraphobics as Goisman, R. M., Goldenberg, I., Vasile, R. G., & Keller,
subjects. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 16, 33 M. B. (1995). Comorbidity of anxiety disorders in a
41. multicenter anxiety study. Comprehensive Psychia-
Emmelkamp, P. M., & Mersch, P. P. (1982). Cognition try, 36, 303311.
and exposure in vivo in the treatment of agorapho- Goisman, R. M., Warshaw, M. G., Peterson, L. G., Rog-
bia: Short-term and delayed effects. Cognitive Ther- ers, M. P., Cuneo, P., Hunt, M. F., et al. (1994). Panic,
apy and Research, 6, 7790. agoraphobia, and panic disorder with agoraphobia:
Evans, L., Holt, C., & Oei, T. P. S. (1991). Long term Data from a multicenter anxiety disorders study.
follow-up of agoraphobics treated by brief intensive Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 182, 7279.
group cognitive behaviour therapy. Australian and Goldstein, A. J., & Chambless, D. L. (1978). A re-
New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 25, 343349. analysis of agoraphobia. Behavior Therapy, 9, 47
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personal- 59.
ity. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Goodwin, R. D., Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J.
Faravelli, C., Pallanti, S., Biondi, F., Paterniti, S., & (2005). Childhood abuse and familial violence and
Scarpato, M. A. (1992). Onset of panic disorder. the risk of panic attacks and panic disorder in young
American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 827828. adulthood. Psychological Medicine, 35, 881890.
Fava, G. A., Zielezny, M., Savron, G., & Grandi, S. Gorman, J. M., Papp, L. A., Coplan, J. D., Martinez, J.
(1995). Long-term effects of behavioural treatment M., Lennon, S., Goetz, R. R., et al. (1994).
for panic disorder with agoraphobia. British Journal Anxiogenic effects of CO2 and hyperventilation in
of Psychiatry, 166, 8792. patients with panic disorder. American Journal of
Feigenbaum, W. (1988). Long-term efficacy of ungraded Psychiatry, 151(4), 547553.
versus graded massed exposure in agoraphobics. In I.
Gould, R. A., & Clum, G. A. (1995). Self-help plus min-
Hand & H. Wittchen (Eds.), Panic and phobias:
imal therapist contact in the treatment of panic disor-
Treatments and variables affecting course and out-
der: A replication and extension. Behavior Therapy,
come. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
26, 533546.
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J.
Gould, R. A., Clum, G. A., & Shapiro, D. (1993). The
B. W. (1994). Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I
use of bibliotherapy in the treatment of panic: A pre-
DSM-IV Disorders. New York: Biometric Research
liminary investigation. Behavior Therapy, 24, 241
Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute.
252.
Foa, E. B., Jameson, J. S., Turner, R. M., & Payne, L. L.
Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An
(1980). Massed vs. spaced exposure sessions in the
enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal
treatment of agoraphobia. Behaviour Research and
system. New York: Oxford University Press.
Therapy, 18, 333338.
Foa, E. B., & McNally, R. J. (1996). Mechanisms of Grayson, J. B., Foa, E. B., & Steketee, G. (1982). Habit-
change in exposure therapy. In R. M. Rapee (Ed.), uation during exposure treatment: Distraction versus
Current controversies in the anxiety disorders attention-focusing. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
(pp. 329343). New York: Guilford Press. 20, 323328.
Forsyth, J. P., Palav, A., & Duff, K. (1999). The absence Griez, E., & van den Hout, M. A. (1986). CO2 inhala-
of relation between anxiety sensitivity and fear con- tion in the treatment of panic attacks. Behaviour Re-
ditioning using 20% versus 13% CO2-enriched air as search and Therapy, 24, 145150.
unconditioned stimuli. Behaviour Research and Grilo, C. M., Money, R., Barlow, D. H., Goddard, A.
Therapy, 37(2), 143153. W., Gorman, J. M., Hofmann, S. G., et al. (1998).
Friedman, S., & Paradis, C. (1991). African-American Pretreatment patient factors predicting attrition from
patients with panic disorder and agoraphobia. Jour- a multicenter randomized controlled treatment study
nal of Anxiety Disorders, 5, 3541. for panic disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 39,
Friedman, S., Paradis, C. M., & Hatch, M. (1994). 323332.
Characteristics of African-American and white pa- Hafner, R. J. (1984). Predicting the effects on husbands
tients with panic disorder and agoraphobia. Hospital of behavior therapy for agoraphobia. Behaviour Re-
ande Community Psychiatry, 45, 798803. search and Therapy, 22, 217226.
Fry, W. (1962). The marital context of an anxiety syn- Hamilton, S. P., Fyer, A. J., Durner, M., Heiman, G. A.,
drome. Family Process, 1, 245252. Baisre de Leon, A., Hodge, S. E., et al. (2003). Fur-
Gallistel, C. R., & Gibbon, J. (2000). Time, rate, and ther genetic evidence for a panic disorder syndrome
conditioning. Psychological Review, 107(2), 289 mapping to chromosome 13q. Proceedings of Na-
344. tional Academy of Science USA, 100, 25502555.
Garssen, B., de Ruiter, C., & van Dyck, R. (1992). Hamilton, S. P., Slager, S. L., De Leon, A. B., Heiman, G.
Breathing retraining: A rational placebo? Clinical A., Klein, D. F., Hodge, S. E., et al. (2004). Evidence
Psychology Review, 12, 141153. for genetic linkage between a polymorphism in the
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 57

adenosine 2A receptor and panic disorder. Hoffart, A. (1995). A comparison of cognitive and
Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 55865. guided mastery therapy of agoraphobia. Behaviour
Hamilton, S. P., Slager, S. L., Helleby, L., Heiman, G. A., Research and Therapy, 33, 423434.
Klein, D. F., Hodge, S. E., et al. (2001). No associa- Hoffart, A., & Hedley, L. M. (1997). Personality traits
tion or linkage between polymorphisms in the genes among panic disorder with agoraphobia patients be-
encoding cholecystokinin and the cholecystokinin B fore and after symptom-focused treatment. Journal
receptor and panic disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 6, of Anxiety Disorders, 11, 7787.
5965. Hofmann, S. G., Shear, M. K., Barlow, D. H., Gorman,
Hand, I., & Lamontagne, Y. (1976). The exacerbation J. M., Hershberger, D., Patterson, M., et al. (1988).
of interpersonal problems after rapid phobia re- Effects of panic disorder treatments on personality
moval. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Prac- disorder characteristics. Depression and Anxiety,
tice, 13, 405411. 8(1), 1420.
Haslam, M. T. (1974). The relationship between the ef- Holden, A. E. O., OBrien, G. T., Barlow, D. H., Stetson,
fect of lactate infusion on anxiety states and their D., & Infantino, A. (1983). Self-help manual for ago-
amelioration by carbon dioxide inhalation. British raphobia: A preliminary report of effectiveness.
Journal of Psychiatry, 125, 8890. Behavior Therapy, 14, 545556.
Hayward, C., Killen, J. D., Hammer, L. D., Litt, I. F., Holt, P., & Andrews, G. (1989). Hyperventilation and
Wilson, D. M., Simmonds, B., et al. (1992). Pubertal anxiety in panic disorder, agoraphobia, and general-
stage and panic attack history in sixth- and seventh- ized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Ther-
grade girls. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, apy, 27, 453460.
12391243. Hope, D. A., Rapee, R. M., Heimberg, R. G., &
Hayward, C., Killen, J. D., Kraemer, H. C., & Taylor, C. Dombeck, M. J. (1990). Representations of the self in
B. (2000). Predictors of panic attacks in adolescents. social phobia: Vulnerability to social threat. Cogni-
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Ado- tive Therapy and Research, 14, 177189.
lescent Psychiatry, 39(2), 18. Hornsveld, H., Garssen, B., Fiedeldij Dop, M., & van
Heatley, C., Ricketts, T., & Forrest, J. (2005). Training Spiegel, P. (1990). Symptom reporting during volun-
general practitioners in cognitive behavioural ther- tary hyperventilation and mental load: Implications
apy for panic disorder: Randomized-controlled trial. for diagnosing hyperventilation syndrome. Journal of
Journal of Mental Health, 14(1), 7382. Psychosomatic Research, 34, 687697.
Hecker, J. E., Losee, M. C., Fritzler, B. K., & Fink, C. M. Horwath, E., Lish, J. D., Johnson, J., Hornig, C. D., &
(1996). Self-directed versus therapist-directed cogni- Weissman, M. M. (1993). Agoraphobia without
tive behavioral treatment for panic disorder. Journal panic: Clinical reappraisal of an epidemiologic find-
of Anxiety Disorders, 10, 253265. ing. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1496
Hecker, J. E., Losee, M. C., Roberson-Nay, R., & Maki, 1501.
K. (2004). Mastery of your anxiety and panic and Huppert, J. D., Bufka, L. F., Barlow, D. H., Gorman, J.
brief therapist contact in the treatment of panic dis- M., Shear, M. K., & Woods, S. W. (2001). Therapist,
order. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18(2), 111126. therapist variables, and cognitive-behavioral therapy
Heldt, E., Manfro, G. G., Kipper, L., Blaya, C., Isolan, outcome in a multicenter trial for panic disorder.
L., & Otto, M. W. (2006). One-year follow-up of Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
pharmacotherapy-resistant patients with panic disor- 69(5), 747755.
der treated with cognitive-behavior therapy: Out- Ito, L. M., Noshirvani, H., Basoglu, M., & Marks, I. M.
come and predictors of remission. Behaviour Re- (1996). Does exposure to internal cues enhance ex-
search and Therapy, 44(5), 657665. posure to external exposure to external cues in ago-
Hermans, D., Craske, M. G., Mineka, S., & Lovibond, raphobia with panic. Psychotherapy and Psycho-
P. F. (2006). Extinction in human fear conditioning. somatics, 65, 2428.
Biological Psychiatry, 60, 361368. Izard, C. E. (1992). Basic emotions, relations among
Heuzenroeder, L., Donnelly, M., Haby, M. M., emotions, and emotion cognition relations. Psycho-
Mihalopoulos, C., Rossell, R., Carter, R., et al. logical Review, 99, 561565.
(2004). Cost-effectiveness of psychological and phar- Jacob, R. G., Furman, J. M., Clark, D. B., & Durrant, J.
macological interventions for generalized anxiety dis- D. (1992). Vestibular symptoms, panic, and phobia:
order and panic disorder. Australian and New Zea- Overlap and possible relationships. Annals of Clini-
land Journal of Psychiatry, 38(8), 602612. cal Psychiatry, 4(3), 163174.
Hibbert, G., & Pilsbury, D. (1989). Hyperventilation: Is Kamphuis, J. H., & Telch, M. J. (2000). Effects of dis-
it a cause of panic attacks? British Journal of Psychi- traction and guided threat reappraisal on fear reduc-
atry, 155, 805809. tion during exposure-based treatments for specific
Himadi, W., Cerny, J., Barlow, D., Cohen, S., & fears. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(12),
OBrien, G. (1986). The relationship of marital ad- 11631181.
justment to agoraphobia treatment outcome. Behav- Kampman, M., Keijsers, G. P. J., Hoogduin, C. A. L., &
iour Research and Therapy, 24, 107115. Hendriks, G.-J. (2002). A randomized, double-blind,
58 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

placebo-controlled study of the effects of adjunctive disorders in the United States: Results from the Na-
paroxetine in panic disorder patients unsuccessfully tional Comorbidity Study. Archives of General
treated with cognitive-behavioral therapy alone. Psychiatry, 51, 819.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 63(9), 772777. Keyl, P. M., & Eaton, W. W. (1990). Risk factors for the
Katon, W., Von Korff, M., Lin, E., Lipscomb, P., onset of panic disorder and other panic attacks in a
Russo, J., Wagner, E., et al. (1990). Distressed high prospective, population-based study. American Jour-
utilizers of medical care: DSM-III-R diagnoses and nal of Epidemiology, 131, 301311.
treatment needs. General Hospital Psychiatry, Kikuchi, M., Komuro, R., Hiroshi, O., Kidani, T.,
12(6), 355362. Hanaoka, A., & Koshino, Y. (2005). Panic disorder
Katschnig, H., & Amering, M. (1998). The long-term with and without agoraphobia: Comorbidity within
course of panic disorder and its predictors. Journal of a half-year of the onset of panic disorder. Psychiatry
Clinical Psychopharmacology, 18(6, Suppl. 2), 6S and Clinical Neurosciences, 58, 639643.
11S. Kraft, A. R., & Hoogduin, C. A. (1984). The hyper-
Keijsers, G. P., Kampman, M., & Hoogduin, C. A. ventiliation syndrome: A pilot study of the effective-
(2001). Dropout prediction in cognitive behavior ness of treatment. British Journal of Psychiatry, 145,
therapy for panic disorder. Behavior Therapy, 32(4), 538542.
739749. Kroeze, S., & van den Hout, M. A. (2000). Selective at-
Keijsers, G. P., Schaap, C. P., Hoogduin, C. A., & tention for cardiac information in panic patients. Be-
Lammers, M. W. (1995). Patienttherapist interac- haviour Research and Therapy, 38, 6372.
tion in the behavioral treatment of panic disorder Krystal, J. H., Woods, S. W., Hill, C. L., & Charney, D.
with agoraphobia. Behavior Modification, 19, 491 S. (1991). Characteristics of panic attack subtypes:
517. Assessment of spontaneous panic, situational panic,
Kenardy, J. A., Dow, M. G., Johnston, D. W., Newman, sleep panic, and limited symptom attacks. Compre-
M. G., Thomson, A., & Taylor, C. B. (2003). A com- hensive Psychiatry, 32(6), 474480.
parison of delivery methods of cognitive-behavioral Laberge, B., Gauthier, J. G., Cote, G., Plamondon, J., &
therapy for panic disorder: An international Cormier, H. J. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral therapy
multicenter trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical of panic disorder with secondary major depression: A
Psychology, 71(6), 10681075. preliminary investigation. Journal of Consulting and
Kendler, K. S., Bulik, C. M., Silberg, J., Hettema, J. M., Clinical Psychology, 61, 10281037.
Myers, J., & Prescott, C. A. (2000). Childhood sex- Lake, R. I., Eaves, L. J., Maes, H. H., Heath, A. C., &
ual abuse and adult psychiatric and substance use Martin, N. G. (2000). Further evidence against the
disorders in women: An epidemiological and co-twin environmental transmission of individual differences
analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 953 in neuroticism from a collaborative study of 45,850
959. twins and relatives of two continents. Behavior Ge-
Kendler, K. S., Heath, A. C., Martin, N. G., & Eaves, L. netics, 30(3), 223233.
J. (1987). Symptoms of anxiety and symptoms of de- Lehman, C. L., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1998).
pression: Same genes, different environments? Ar- Effects of cognitive-behavioral treatment for panic
chives of General Psychiatry, 44, 451457. disorder with agoraphobia on concurrent alcohol
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., & abuse. Behavior Therapy, 29, 423433.
Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of- Lelliott, P., Marks, I., McNamee, G., & Tobena, A.
onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the Na- (1989). Onset of panic disorder with agoraphobia:
tional Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of Toward an integrated model. Archives of General
General Psychiatry, 62, 593602. Psychiatry, 46, 10001004.
Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., & Walters, E. E. Lidren, D. M., Watkins, P., Gould, R. A., Clum, G. A.,
(2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distri- Asterino, M., & Tulloch, H. L. (1994). A comparison
butions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comor- of bibliotherapy and group therapy in the treatment
bidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psy- of panic disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
chiatry, 62(6), 593602. Psychology, 62, 865869.
Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Jin, R., Ruscio, A. M., Shear, Lovibond, P. F., Davis, N. R., & OFlaherty, A. S.
K., & Walters, E. E. (2006). The epidemiology of (2000). Protection from extinction in human fear
panic attacks, panic disorder, and agoraphobia in the conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38,
National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives 967983.
of General Psychiatry, 63, 415424. Lovibond, P. F., & Shanks, D. R. (2002). The role of
Kessler, R. C., Davis, C. G., & Kendler, K. S. (1997). awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: Empirical evi-
Childhood adversity and adult psychiatric disorder in dence and theoretical implications. Journal of Exper-
the U.S. National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological imental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 28,
Medicine, 27, 11011119. 326.
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. Maddock, R. J., & Blacker, K. H. (1991). Response to
B., Hughes, M., Eshkeman, S., et al. (1994). Lifetime treatment in panic disorder with associated depres-
and 12 month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric sion. Psychopathology, 24(1), 16.
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 59

Maidenberg, E., Chen, E., Craske, M., Bohn, P., & tacks: New clinical findings and theoretical implica-
Bystritsky, A. (1996). Specificity of attentional bias in tions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 1204
panic disorder and social phobia. Journal of Anxiety 1207.
Disorders, 10, 529541. Mennin, D. S., & Heimberg, R. G. (2000). The impact
Maier, S. F., Laudenslager, M. L., & Ryan, S. M. (1985). of comorbid mood and personality disorders in the
Stressor controllability, immune function and endog- cognitive-behavioral treatment of panic disorder.
enous opiates. In F. R. Brush & J. B. Overmeier Clinical Psychology Review, 20(3), 339357.
(Eds.), Affect, conditioning and cognition: Essays on Messenger, C., & Shean, G. (1998). The effects of anxi-
the determinants of behavior (pp. 183201). ety sensitivity and history of panic on reactions to
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. stressors in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Behav-
Maller, R. G., & Reiss, S. (1992). Anxiety sensitivity in ior Therapy, 29, 279288.
1984 and panic attacks in 1987. Journal of Anxiety Michelson, L., Mavissakalian, M., & Marchione, K.
Disorders, 6(3), 241247. (1985). Cognitive and behavioral treatments of ago-
Mannuzza, S., Fyer, A. J., Liebowitz, M. R., & Klein, raphobia: Clinical, behavioral, and psychophysio-
D. F. (1990). Delineating the boundaries of social logical outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
phobia: Its relationship to panic disorder and ago- Psychology, 53, 913925.
raphobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 4(1), 41 Michelson, L., Mavissakalian, M., & Marchione, K.
59. (1988). Cognitive, behavioral, and psychophysio-
Marchand, A., Goyer, L. R., Dupuis, G., & Mainguy, N. logical treatments of agoraphobia: A comparative
(1998). Personality disorders and the outcome of outcome investigation. Behavior Therapy, 19, 97
cognitive-behavioural treatment of panic disorder 120.
with agoraphobia. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Michelson, L., Mavissakalian, M., Marchione, K.,
Science, 30(1), 1423. Ulrich, R., Marchione, N., & Testa, S. (1990).
Margraf, J., Taylor, C. B., Ehlers, A., Roth, W. T., & Psychophysiological outcome of cognitive, behavior-
Agras, W. S. (1987). Panic attacks in the natural envi- al, and psychophysiologically-based treatments of
ronment. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, agoraphobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28,
175, 558565. 127139.
Marks, I. M., Swinson, R. P., Basoglu, M., Kuck, K., Milton, F., & Hafner, J. (1979). The outcome of behav-
Noshirvani, H., OSullivan, G., et al. (1993). ior therapy for agoraphobia in relation to marital ad-
Alprazolam and exposure alone and combined in justment. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36, 807
panic disorder with agoraphobia: A controlled study 811.
in London and Toronto. British Journal of Psychia- Mineka, S., Cook, M., & Miller, S. (1984). Fear condi-
try, 162, 776787. tioned with escapable and inescapable shock: The ef-
Marshall, W. L. (1985). The effects of variable exposure fects of a feedback stimulus. Journal of Experimental
in flooding therapy. Behavior Therapy, 16, 117135. Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 307
Martin, N. G., Jardine, R., Andrews, G., & Heath, A. 323.
C. (1988). Anxiety disorders and neuroticism: Are Mitte, K. A. (2005). Meta-analysis of the efficacy of
there genetic factors specific to panic? Acta psycho- and pharmacotherapy in panic disorder with
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 77, 698706. and without agoraphobia. Journal of Affective Dis-
Mavissakalian, M., & Hamman, M. (1987). DSM-III orders, 88, 2745.
personality disorder in agoraphobia: II. Changes Moisan, D., & Engels, M. L. (1995). Childhood trauma
with treatment. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 28, 356 and personality disorder in 43 women with panic dis-
361. order. Psychological Reports, 76, 11331134.
McLean, P. D., Woody, S., Taylor, S., & Koch, W. J. Morisette, S. B., Spiegel, D. A., & Heinrichs, N. (2005).
(1998). Comorbid panic disorder and major depres- Sensation-focused intensive treatment for panic dis-
sion: Implications for cognitive-behavioral therapy. order with moderate to severe agoraphobia. Cogni-
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, tive and Behavioral Practice, 12(1), 1729.
240247. Murphy, M. T., Michelson, L. K., Marchione, K.,
McNally, R. J., & Lorenz, M. (1987). Anxiety sensitiv- Marchione, N., & Testa, S. (1998). The role of self-
ity in agoraphobics. Journal of Behavior Therapy directed in vivo exposure in combination with cogni-
and Experimental Psychiatry, 18(1), 311. tive therapy, relaxation training, or therapist-assisted
McNally, R. J., Riemann, B. C., Louro, C. E., Lukach, exposure in the treatment of panic disorder with ago-
B. M., & Kim, E. (1992). Cognitive processing of raphobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 12,
emotional information in panic disorder. Behaviour 117138.
Research and Therapy, 30, 143149. Myers, J., Weissman, M., Tischler, C., Holzer, C.,
McNamee, G., OSullivan, G., Lelliott, P., & Marks, I. Orvaschel, H., Anthony, J., et al. (1984). Six-month
M. (1989). Telephone-guided treatment for house- prevalence of psychiatric disorders in three commu-
bound agoraphobics with panic disorder: Exposure nities. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 959967.
vs. relaxation. Behavior Therapy, 20, 491497. Mystkowski, J. L., Craske, M. G., Echiverri, A. M., &
Mellman, T. A., & Uhde, T. W. (1989). Sleep panic at- Labus, J. S. (2006). Mental reinstatement of context
60 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

and return of fear in spider-fearful participants. continuation of benzodiazepine treatment: Efficacy


Behavior Therapy, 37(1), 4960. of cognitive-behavioral therapy for patients with
Neron, S., Lacroix, D., & Chaput, Y. (1995). Group vs panic disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry,
individual cognitive behaviour therapy in panic dis- 150(10), 14851490.
order: An open clinical trial with a six month follow- Pauli, P., Amrhein, C., Muhlberger, A., Dengler, W., &
up. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 27, Wiedemann, G. (2005). Electrocortical evidence for
379392. an early abnormal processing of panic-related words
Newman, M. G., Kenardy, J., Herman, S., & Taylor, C. in panic disorder patients. International Journal of
B. (1997). Comparison of palmtop-computer- Psychophysiology, 57, 3341.
assisted brief cognitive-behavioral treatment to Pennebaker, J. W., & Roberts, T. (1992). Toward a his
cognitive-behavioral treatment for panic disorder. and hers theory of emotion: Gender differences in
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, visceral perception. Journal of Social and Clinical
178183. Psychology, 11(30), 199212.
Norton, G. R., Cox, B. J., & Malan, J. (1992). Nonclin- Perna, G., Bertani, A., Arancio, C., Ronchi, P., &
ical panickers: A critical review. Clinical Psychology Bellodi, L. (1995). Laboratory response of patients
Review, 12, 121139. with panic and obsessivecompulsive disorders to
Noyes, R., Clancy, J., Garvey, M. J., & Anderson, D. J. 35% CO2 challenges. American Journal of Psychia-
(1987). Is agoraphobia a variant of panic disorder or try, 152, 8589.
a separate illness? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 1, Pollard, C. A., Pollard, H. J., & Corn, K. J. (1989).
313. Panic onset and major events in the lives of agora-
Noyes, R., Crowe, R. R., Harris, E. L., Hamra, B. J., phobics: A test of contiguity. Journal of Abnormal
McChesney, C.M., & Chaudhry, D. R. (1986). Rela- Psychology, 98, 318321.
tionship between panic disorder and agoraphobia: A Powers, M. B., Smits, J. A. J., & Telch, M. J. (2004).
family study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, Disentangling the effects of safety behavior utiliza-
227232. tion and safety-behavior availability during expo-
Noyes, R., Reich, J., Suelzer, M., & Christiansen, J. sure based treatments: A placebo- controlled trial.
(1991). Personality traits associated with panic disor- Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72,
der: Change associated with treatment. Comprehen- 448454.
sive Psychiatry, 32, 282294.
Purkis, H. M., & Lipp, O. V. (2001). Does affective
Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and
learning exist in the absence of contingency aware-
preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and
ness? Learning and Motivation, 32, 840899.
fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483522.
Rachman, S., Lopatka, C., & Levitt, K. (1988). Experi-
Oliver, N. S., & Page, A. C. (2003). Fear reduction dur-
mental analyses of panic: II. Panic patients. Behav-
ing in vivo exposure to bloodinjection stimuli: Dis-
iour Research and Therapy, 26, 3340.
traction vs. attentional focus. British Journal of Clin-
Ramsay, R. W., Barends, J., Breuker, J., & Kruseman, A.
ical Psychology, 42(1), 1325.
(1966). Massed versus spaced desensitization of fear.
Ost, L.-G. (1988). Applied relaxation vs. progressive re-
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 4(3), 205207.
laxation in the treatment of panic disorder. Behav-
iour Research and Therapy, 26, 1322. Rapee, R. (1986). Differential response to hyperventila-
Ost, L.-G., Thulin, U., & Ramnero, J. (2004). Cognitive tion in panic disorder and generalized anxiety disor-
behavior therapy vs exposure in vivo in the treatment der. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 2428.
of panic disorder with agoraphobia. Behaviour Re- Rapee, R. M. (1985). A case of panic disorder treated
search and Therapy, 42(1), 11051127. with breathing retraining. Behavior Therapy and Ex-
Ost, L. G., & Westling, B. E. (1995). Applied relaxation perimental Psychiatry, 16, 6365.
vs cognitive behavior therapy in the treatment of Rapee, R. M. (1994). Detection of somatic sensations in
panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32,
145158. 825831.
Ost, L. G., Westling, B. E., & Hellstrom, K. (1993). Ap- Rapee, R. M., Brown, T. A., Antony, M. M., & Barlow,
plied relaxation, exposure in vivo, and cognitive D. H. (1992). Response to hyperventilation and inha-
methods in the treatment of panic disorder with ago- lation of 5.5% carbon dioxide-enriched air across the
raphobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, DSM-III-R anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal
383394. Psychology, 101, 538552.
Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., & Sabatino, S. A. (1996). Rapee, R. M., Craske, M. G., & Barlow, D. H. (1990).
Maintenance of remission following cognitive behav- Subject described features of panic attacks using a
ior therapy for panic disorder: Possible deleterious ef- new self-monitoring form. Journal of Anxiety Disor-
fects of concurrent medication treatment. Behavior ders, 4, 171181.
Therapy, 27, 473482. Rapee, R. M., Craske, M. G., & Barlow, D. H. (1995).
Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., Sachs, G. S., Reiter, S. R., Assessment instrument for panic disorder that in-
Meltzer-Brody, S., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (1993). Dis- cludes fear of sensation-producing activities: The Al-
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 61

bany Panic and Phobia Questionnaire. Anxiety, 1, (1992). A follow-up study on short-term treatment of
114122. agoraphobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30,
Rapee, R. M., Craske, M. G., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, 6366.
D. H. (1996). Measurement of perceived control over Rodriguez, B. I., & Craske, M. G. (1995). Does distrac-
anxiety-related events. Behavior Therapy, 27(2), tion interfere with fear reduction during exposure? A
279293. test among animal-fearful subjects [Special issue: Ex-
Rapee, R. M., Litwin, E. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1990). perimental pain as a model for the study of clinical
Impact of life events on subjects with panic disorder pain]. Behavior Therapy, 26(2), 337349.
and on comparison subjects. American Journal of Rose, M. P., & McGlynn, F. D. (1997). Toward a stan-
Psychiatry, 147, 640644. dard experiment for studying post-treatment return
Rapee, R. M., & Medoro, L. (1994). Fear of physical of fear. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 11(3), 263
sensations and trait anxiety as mediators of the re- 277.
sponse to hyperventilation in nonclinical sub- Rowe, M. K., & Craske, M. G. (1998). Effects of an
jects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(4), expanding-spaced vs massed exposure schedule on
693699. fear reduction and return of fear. Behaviour Research
Rapee, R. M., & Murrell, E. (1988). Predictors of and Therapy, 36, 701717.
agoraphobic avoidance. Journal of Anxiety Disor- Roy-Byrne, P., Craske, M. G., Stein, M. B., Sullivan, G.,
ders, 2, 203217. Bystritsky, A., Katon, W., et al. (2005). A randomized
Rathus, J. H., Sanderson, W. C., Miller, A. L., & effectiveness trial of cognitive-behavioral therapy
Wetzler, S. (1995). Impact of personality functioning and medication for primary care panic disorder. Ar-
on cognitive behavioral treatment of panic disorder: chives of General Psychiatry, 62, 290298.
A preliminary report. Journal of Personality Disor- Roy-Byrne, P. P., & Cowley, D. S. (1995). Course and
ders, 9, 160168. outcome in panic disorder: A review of recent follow-
Razran, G. (1961). The observable unconscious and the up studies. Anxiety, 1, 151160.
inferable conscious in current soviet psychophysi- Roy-Byrne, P. P., Craske, M. G., & Stein, M. B. (2006).
ology: Interoceptive conditioning, semantic condi- Panic disorder. Lancet, 368, 10231032.
tioning, and the orienting reflex. Psychological Re- Roy-Byrne, P. P., Geraci, M., & Uhde, T. W. (1986). Life
view, 68, 81147. events and the onset of panic disorder. American
Reich, J., Perry, J. C., Shera, D., Dyck, I., Vasile, R., Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 14241427.
Goisman, R. M., et al. (1994). Comparison of per- Roy-Byrne, P. P., Mellman, T. A., & Uhde, T. W. (1988).
sonality disorders in different anxiety disorder diag- Biologic findings in panic disorder: Neuroendocrine
noses: Panic, agoraphobia, generalized anxiety, and and sleep-related abnormalities [Special issue: Per-
social phobia. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 6(2), spectives on panic-related disorders]. Journal of Anx-
125134. iety Disorders, 2, 1729.
Reiss, S. (1980). Pavlovian conditioning and human Roy-Byrne, P. P., Stein, M. B., Russo, J., Mercier, E.,
fear: An expectancy model. Behavior Therapy, 11, Thomas, R., McQuaid, J., et al. (1999). Panic disor-
380396. der in the primary care setting: Comorbidity, disabil-
Reiss, S., Peterson, R., Gursky, D., & McNally, R. ity, service utilization, and treatment. Journal of Clin-
(1986). Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency, and ical Psychiatry, 60(7), 492499.
the prediction of fearfulness. Behaviour Research Rupert, P. A., Dobbins, K., & Mathew, R. J. (1981).
and Therapy, 24, 18. EMG biofeedback and relaxation instructions in the
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of treatment of chronic anxiety. American Journal of
Pavolvian conditioning: Variations in the effective- Clinical Biofeedback, 4, 5261.
ness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. Safren, S. A., Gershuny, B. S., Marzol, P., Otto, M. W.,
H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical condition- & Pollack, M. H. (2002). History of childhood abuse
ing II: Current research and theory (pp. 6499). New in panic disorder, social phobia, and generalized anx-
York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. iety disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Dis-
Rice, K. M., & Blanchard, E. B. (1982). Biofeedback in ease, 190(7), 453456.
the treatment of anxiety disorders. Clinical Psychol- Salkovskis, P., Clark, D., & Hackmann, A. (1991).
ogy Review, 2, 557577. Treatment of panic attacks using cognitive therapy
Richards, J., Klein, B., & Carlbring, P. (2003). Internet- without exposure or breathing retraining. Behaviour
based treatment for panic disorder. Cognitive Behav- Research and Therapy, 29, 161166.
iour Therapy, 32, 125135. Salkovskis, P., Warwick, H., Clark, D., & Wessels, D.
Richards, J. C., Klein, B., & Austin, D. W. (2006). (1986). A demonstration of acute hyperventilation
Internet cognitive behavioural therapy for panic dis- during naturally occurring panic attacks. Behaviour
order: Does the inclusion of stress management infor- Research and Therapy, 24, 9194.
mation improve end-state functioning? Clinical Psy- Salkovskis, P. M. (1991). The importance of behaviour
chologist, 10(1), 215. in the maintenance of anxiety and panic: A cognitive
Rijken, H., Kraaimaat, F., de Ruiter, C., & Garssen, B. account [Special issue: The changing face of behav-
62 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

ioural psychotherapy]. Behavioural Psychotherapy, panic attacks. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32,
19(1), 619. 1720.
Salkovskis, P. M., Clark, D. M., & Gelder, M. G. Siddle, D. A., & Bond, N. W. (1988). Avoidance learn-
(1996). Cognitionbehaviour links in the persistence ing, Pavlovian conditioning, and the development of
of panic. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 453 phobias. Biological Psychology, 27, 167183.
458. Sloan, T., & Telch, M. J. (2002). The effects of safety-
Schade, A., Marquenie, L. A., van Balkom, A. J., Koeter, seeking behavior and guided threat reappraisal on
M. W., de Beurs, E., van den Brink, W., et al. (2005). fear reduction during exposure: An experimental in-
The effectiveness of anxiety treatment on alcohol- vestigation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(3),
dependent patients with a comorbid phobic disorder: 235251.
A randomized controlled trial. Alcoholism: Clinical Sokolowska, M., Siegel, S., & Kim, J. A. (2002).
and Experimental Research, 29(5), 794800. Intraadministration associations: Conditional
Schmidt, N. B., Lerew, D. R., & Jackson, R. J. (1997). hyperalgesia elicited by morphine onset cues. Journal
The role of anxiety sensitivity in the pathogenesis of of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Pro-
panic: Prospective evaluation of spontaneous panic cesses, 28(3), 309320.
attacks during acute stress. Journal of Abnormal Psy- Spanier, G. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New
chology, 106, 355364. scales for assessing the quality of marriage and simi-
Schmidt, N. B., Lerew, D. R., & Jackson, R. J. (1999). lar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38,
Prospective evaluation of anxiety sensitivity in the 1538.
pathogenesis of panic: Replication and extension. Spiegel, D. A., Bruce, T. J., Gregg, S. F., & Nuzzarello,
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 532537. A. (1994). Does cognitive behavior therapy assist
Schmidt, N. B., McCreary, B. T., Trakowski, J. J., Santi- slow-taper alprazolam discontinuation in panic dis-
ago, H. T., Woolaway-Bickel, K., & Ialong, N. order? American Journal of Psychiatry, 151(6), 876
(2003). Effects of cognitive behavioral treatment on 881.
physical health status in patients with panic disorder.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P.
Behavior Therapy, 34(1), 4963.
R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State
Schmidt, N. B., Woolaway-Bickel, K., Trakowski, J.,
Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI, Form Y). Palo Alto,
Santiago, H., Storey, J., Koselka, M., et al. (2000).
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Dismantling cognitive-behavioral treatment for panic
Stanley, M. A., Beck, J. G., Averill, P. M., Baldwin, L. E.,
disorder: Questioning the utility of breathing retrain-
Deagle, E. A., & Stadler, J. G. (1996). Patterns of
ing. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
change during cognitive behavioral treatment for
68(3), 417424.
panic disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Dis-
Schneider, A. J., Mataix-Cois, D., Marks, I. M., &
ease, 184, 567572.
Bachofen, M. (2005). Internet-guided self-help with
or without exposure therapy for phobic and panic Stein, M. B., Walker, J. R., Anderson, G., Hazen, A. L.,
disorders. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 74(3), Ross, C. A., Eldridge, G., et al. (1996). Childhood
154164. physical and sexual abuse in patients with anxiety
Schumacher, J., Jamra, R. A., Becker, T., Klopp, N., disorders and a community sample. American Jour-
Franke, P., Jacob, C., et al. (2005). Investigation of nal of Psychiatry, 153, 275277.
the DAOA/G30 locus in panic disorder. Molecular Sturges, L. V., Goetsch, V. L., Ridley, J., & Whittal, M.
Psychiatry, 10, 428429. (1998). Anxiety sensitivity and response to hyperven-
Sharp, D. M., Power, K. G., Simpson, R. J., Swanson, V., tilation challenge: Physiologic arousal, interoceptive
& Anstee, J. A. (1997). Global measures of outcome acuity, and subjective distress. Journal of Anxiety
in a controlled comparison of pharmacological and Disorders, 12(2), 103115.
psychological treatment of panic disorder and agora- Surez, L., Bennett, S., Goldstein, C., & Barlow, D. H.
phobia in primary care. British Journal of General (in press). Understanding anxiety disorders from a
Practice, 47, 150155. triple vulnerabilities framework. In M. M. An-
Sharp, D. M., Power, K. G., & Swanson, V. (2004). A thony & M. B. Stein (Eds.). Oxford handbook of
comparison of the efficacy and acceptability of group anxiety and related disorders. New York: Oxford
versus individual cognitive behaviour therapy in the University Press.
treatment of panic disorder and agoraphobia in pri- Swinson, R. P., Fergus, K. D., Cox, B. J., & Wickwire,
mary care. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, K. (1995). Efficacy of telephone-administered behav-
11(2), 7382. ioral therapy for panic disorder with agoraphobia.
Shear, M. K., & Schulberg, H. C. (1995). Anxiety disor- Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 465469.
ders in primary care. Bulletin of the Menninger Taylor, S., Koch, W. J., & McNally, R. J. (1992). How
Clinic, 59(2, Suppl. A), A73A85. does anxiety sensitivity vary across the anxiety disor-
Shulman, I. D., Cox, B. J., Swinson, R. P., Kuch, K., & ders? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 6, 249259.
Reichman, J. T. (1994). Precipitating events, loca- Telch, M. J., Brouillard, M., Telch, C. F., Agras, W. S., &
tions and reactions associated with initial unexpected Taylor, C. B. (1989). Role of cognitive appraisal in
Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia 63

panic-related avoidance. Behaviour Research and van den Hout, M., Brouwers, C., & Oomen, J. (2006).
Therapy, 27, 373383. Clinically diagnosed Axis II co-morbidity and the
Telch, M. J., Lucas, J. A., & Nelson, P. (1989). Nonclin- short term outcome of CBT for Axis I disorders.
ical panic in college students: An investigation of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 13(1), 56
prevalence and symptomatology. Journal of Abnor- 63.
mal Psychology, 98, 300306. van Megen, H. J., Westenberg, H. G., Den Boer, J. A., &
Telch, M. J., Lucas, J. A., Schmidt, N. B., Hanna, H. H., Kahn, R. S. (1996). The panic-inducing properties of
LaNae, Jaimez, T., et al. (1993). Group cognitive- the cholecystokinin tetrapeptide CCK4 in patients
behavioral treatment of panic disorder. Behaviour with panic disorder. European Neuropsychophar-
Research and Therapy, 31, 279287. macology, 6, 18794.
Telch, M. J., Sherman, M., & Lucas, J. (1989). Anxiety Vansteenwegen, D., Vervliet, B., Iberico, C., Baeyens, F.,
sensitivity: Unitary personality trait or domain spe- van den Bergh, O., & Hermans, D. (2007). The re-
cific appraisals? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 3, 25 peated confrontation with videotapes of spiders in
32. multiple contexts attenuates renewal of fear in
Thorgeirsson, T. E., Oskarsson, H., Desnica, N., Kostic, spider-anxious students. Behavior Research and
J. P., Stefansson, J. G., Kolbeinsson, H., et al. (2003). Therapy, 45(6), 11691179.
Anxiety with panic disorder linked to chromosome Veltman, D. J., van Zijderveld, G., Tilders, F. J., & van
9q in Iceland. American Journal of Human Genetics, Dyck, R. (1996). Epinephrine and fear of bodily sen-
72, 12211230. sations in panic disorder and social phobia. Journal
Thyer, B. A., Himle, J., Curtis, G. C., Cameron, O. G., of Psychopharmacology, 10(4), 259265.
& Nesse, R. M. (1985). A comparison of panic disor- Verburg, K., Griez, E., Meijer, J., & Pols, H. (1995). Re-
der and agoraphobia with panic attacks. Compre- spiratory disorders as a possible predisposing factor
hensive Psychiatry, 26, 208214. for panic disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 33,
Tiemens, B. G., Ormel, J., & Simon, G. E. (1996). Oc- 129134.
currence, recognition, and outcome of psychological Wade, W. A., Treat, T. A., & Stuart, G. L. (1998). Trans-
disorders in primary care. American Journal of Psy- porting an empirically supported treatment for panic
chiatry, 153, 636644. disorder to a service clinic setting: A benchmarking
Tsao, J. C. I., Lewin, M. R., & Craske, M. G. (1998). strategy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
The effects of cognitive-behavior therapy for panic ogy, 66, 231239.
disorder on comorbid conditions. Journal of Anxiety Wardle, J., Hayward, P., Higgitt, A., Stabl, M., Blizard,
Disorders, 12, 357371. R., & Gray, J. (1994). Effects of concurrent diazepam
Tsao, J. C. I., Mystkowski, J. L., Zucker, B. G., & treatment on the outcome of exposure therapy in ag-
Craske, M. G. (2002). Effects of cognitive-behavioral oraphobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32,
therapy for panic disorder on comorbid conditions: 203215.
replication and extension. Behavior Therapy, 33, Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity:
493509. The disposition to experience aversive emotional
Tsao, J. C. I., Mystkowski, J. L., Zucker, B. G., & states. Psychological Bulletin, 96(3), 465490.
Craske, M. G. (2005). Impact of cognitive- Weems, C. F., Hayward, C., Killen, J., & Taylor, C. B.
behavioral therapy for panic disorder on comorbidi- (2002). A longitudinal investigation of anxiety sensi-
ty: A controlled investigation. Behaviour Research tivity in adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
and Therapy, 43, 959970. ogy, 111(3), 471477.
Uhde, T. W. (1994). The anxiety disorders: Phenomenol- Welkowitz, L., Papp, L., Cloitre, M., Liebowitz, M.,
ogy and treatment of core symptoms and associated Martin, L., & Gorman, J. (1991). Cognitive-
sleep disturbance. In M. Kryger, T. Roth, & W. De- behavior therapy for panic disorder delivered by psy-
ment (Eds.), Principles and practice of sleep medicine chopharmacologically oriented clinicians. Journal of
(pp. 871898). Philadelphia: Saunders. Nervous and Mental Disease, 179, 473477.
van Balkom, A. J., de Beurs, E., Koele, P., Lange, A., & Westen, D., & Morrison, K. (2001). A multidimensional
van Dyck, R. (1996). Long-term benzodiazepine use meta-analysis of treatments for depression, panic,
is associated with smaller treatment gain in panic dis- and generalized anxiety disorder: An empirical exam-
order with agoraphobia. Journal of Nervous and ination of the status of empirically supported thera-
Mental Disease, 184, 133135. pies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
van Beek, N., Schruers, K. R. J., & Friez, E. J. L. (2005). 69(6), 875899.
Prevalence of respiratory disorders in first-degree rel- Westra, H. A., Stewart, S. H., & Conrad, B. E. (2002).
atives of panic disorder patients. Journal of Affective Naturalistic manner of benzodiazepine use and cog-
Disorders, 87, 337340. nitive behavioral therapy outcome in panic disorder
van den Hout, M., Arntz, A., & Hoekstra, R. (1994). and agoraphobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,
Exposure reduced agoraphobia but not panic, and 16(3), 223246.
cognitive therapy reduced panic but not agorapho- Wilkinson, D. J., Thompson, J. M., Lambert, G. W.,
bia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 447451. Jennings, G. L., Schwarz, R. G., Jefferys, D., et al.
64 CLINICAL HANDBOOK OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS

(1998). Sympathetic activity in patients with panic Williams, S. L., & Zane, G. (1989). Guided mastery and
disorder at rest, under laboratory mental stress, and stimulus exposure treatments for severe performance
during panic attacks. Archives of General Psychiatry, anxiety in agoraphobics. Behaviour Research and
55(6), 511520. Therapy, 27, 237245.
Williams, K. E., & Chambless, D. (1990). The relation- Wittchen, H.-U., Reed, V., & Kessler, R. C. (1998).
ship between therapist characteristics and outcome The relationship of agoraphobia and panic in a
of in vivo exposure treatment for agoraphobia. community sample of adolescents and young
Behavior Therapy, 21, 111116. adults. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(11),
Williams, K. E., & Chambless, D. L. (1994). The results 10171024.
of exposure-based treatment in agoraphobia. In S. Zinbarg, R. E., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Structure of
Friedman (Ed.), Anxiety disorders in African Ameri- anxiety and the anxiety disorders: A hierarchical
cans (pp. 149165). New York: Springer. model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105(2),
Williams, S. L. (1992). Perceived self-efficacy and pho- 184193.
bic disability. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-efficacy: Zinbarg, R. E., Barlow, D. H., & Brown, T. A.
Thought control of action (pp. 149176). Washing- (1997). Hierarchical structure and general factor
ton, DC: Hemisphere. saturation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index: Evi-
Williams, S. L., & Falbo, J. (1996). Cognitive and dence and implication. Psychological Assessment,
performance-based treatments for panic attacks in 9, 277284.
people with varying degrees of agoraphobic dis- Zoellner, L. A., & Craske, M. G. (1999). Interoceptive
ability. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 253 accuracy and panic. Behaviour Research and Ther-
264. apy, 37, 11411158.

You might also like