You are on page 1of 11

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/12427985

Approaching avoidance: A step essential to


the understanding of craving

Article in Alcohol research & health: the journal of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism ·
February 1999
Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

106 52

3 authors:

Mary Jo Breiner Werner Stritzke


Florida Department of Children and Famil… University of Western Australia
5 PUBLICATIONS 257 CITATIONS 50 PUBLICATIONS 1,307 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Alan R Lang
Florida State University
55 PUBLICATIONS 2,240 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Understanding suicide risk View project

Substance use and risk taking View project


All content following this page was uploaded by Werner Stritzke on 17 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Approaching Avoidance
A Step Essential to the
Understanding of Craving

Mary Jo Breiner M.S., Werner G. K. Stritzke, Ph.D., and Alan R. Lang, Ph.D.

Craving is only one component of the mental processes that influence drinking behavior.
Alcohol-related cues (ARCs) can set in motion a dynamic competition between inclinations to
approach drinking and inclinations to avoid drinking. Craving can thus be integrated into a
comprehensive model of decisionmaking in which ambivalence or conflict is a key element.
The relative strength of each component of the ARC reaction can fluctuate over time as well
as in response to both subjective states and environmental circumstances. Simultaneously
and independently evaluating these opposing responses puts clinicians in a better position to
influence the relative weight that the patient assigns to the positive and negative outcomes of
alcohol consumption. K EY WORDS : AOD (alcohol and other drug) craving; alcohol cue;
avoidance conditioning; theory of AODU (alcohol and other drug use); causes of AODU;
predictive factors; classical conditioning; AOD sensitivity; expectancy; motivation; causal
model; AOD prevention; intervention; behavior modification; risk assessment; literature review

ost researchers and practition- avoidance and restraint. Accordingly, The concurrent operation of both

M ers in the alcohol field agree


that alcohol “craving,” defined
here as an inclination to approach and
the transformation of alcohol-relevant
cues into signals warning of impending
punishment is a tactic often used by
approach and avoidance inclinations in
people experiencing problems with

consume alcoholic beverages, is a critical practitioners to emphasize the adverse


feature of alcohol use disorders. Such consequences of drinking. MARY JO BREINER, M.S., is a doctoral
craving may be activated by stimuli or The goal of this article is to encourage student working under the supervision
“cues” (see sidebar by Tiffany, p. 216) both scientists and clinicians to appreciate of Dr. Lang in the Department of
that the patient has come to associate the complexity of responses elicited by Psychology at Florida State University,
with reinforcement from drinking. How- alcohol cues, particularly the likelihood Tallahassee, Florida.
ever, an exclusive focus on the forces that these cues can prompt a dynamic
attracting a person toward alcohol con- competition between inclinations to WERNER G. K. STRITZKE, PH.D., com-
sumption is arguably too restrictive and approach drinking and inclinations to pleted his doctoral work with Dr. Lang
contrasts sharply with the thrust of avoid drinking. By simultaneously and and is now an assistant professor of
many traditional treatment strategies, independently evaluating these opposing psychology at the University of Western
which tend to minimize consideration responses, researchers and treatment Australia, Perth, Australia.
of the rewarding aspects of excessive professionals might understand the
drinking. Practitioners typically strive essentially unidimensional construct of ALAN R. LANG, PH.D., is a professor of
to foster abstinence or reduced drinking craving better and integrate it into a psychology at Florida State University,
among problem drinkers by advocating comprehensive motivational model. Tallahassee, Florida.

Vol. 23, No.www.CE-credit.com


3, 1999 - Helping Professionals Help Others 197
alcohol and other addictive substances only why alcoholics return to drinking responses to alcohol-related cues (ARCs)
is not a new idea. Indeed, the signifi- despite resolutions not to do so but also assumes that the relative strength of
cance of these competing motives is why they often succeed, either tem- each component of the reaction can
apparent in current diagnostic criteria porarily or permanently, in refraining fluctuate over time as well as in response
for addiction (i.e., dependence) to from problem drinking. Heather (1998) to both subjective states and environ-
alcohol or other drugs (AODs). These has argued that addictive behavior is mental circumstances. Such a concep-
criteria include using the substance in defined, at least in part, by ambivalence tualization departs from the traditional
larger amounts or over a longer period associated with the decisionmaking pro- view that craving alone drives decisions
than was originally intended, along cess. Intervention strategies consistent about drinking. However, it does
with a desire for the substance despite with this concept attempt to motivate incorporate mechanisms by which
efforts to cut down or control its use recovery by influencing the relative low-intensity, seemingly “irrelevant”
(American Psychiatric Association weight the patient assigns to the positive stimuli, thoughts, and actions can set
1994). Based on these characteristics, and negative outcomes of alcohol con- the stage for later inclinations to approach
AOD-dependent patients are seen as sumption (Prochaska et al. 1997; Miller and consume alcohol (Marlatt and
both drawn toward and repelled from and Rollnick 1991). Gordon 1985). This article strives to
substance use. The theorized role of ambivalence integrate the concept of craving into a
This observation has led a number in alcohol use disorders suggests that comprehensive model that better cap-
of theorists (e.g., Orford 1985) to iden- craving is only one component of a tures the reality of addicts’ struggles
tify ambivalence or conflict as a key multidimensional phenomenon com- along the dual pathways of indulgence
element of excessive appetites of many prised of largely independent inclina- and restraint. A recurrent theme is that
kinds. Thus, an adequate theory of tions to approach and avoid drinking. responses to alcohol-relevant cues are
alcohol use problems must explain not This framework for understanding multifaceted and dynamic.

Historical Factors Expectancies Evaluative Space Decision

• Biochemical reactivity

• Personality
characteristics
Negative
• Sociocultural expectancies
environment
Inclination to not drink

• Past reinforcement Avoidance Ambivalence Not drink

Current Factors
Indifference Approach Drink

• Quantity and
quality of positive Positive
and negative expectancies
Inclination to drink
incentives

• Access to Yes
alternative
valued activities No

A multidimensional model of inclinations to drink or not drink. Dashed lines represent factors that promote alcohol avoidance, whereas
solid lines represent factors that promote the desire to approach alcohol. This table depicts only the most essential connections
with regard to historical factors, expectancies, motivations, and decisions in alcohol use, although other connections may exist.

198 www.CE-credit.com - Helping Professionals Help Others Alcohol Research & Health
Approaching Avoidance

Pathways to Indulgence genetically based aspects of individual learning processes: (1) the association
and Restraint biochemical reactivity can contribute of previously neutral stimuli (e.g., a
to intrinsically pleasant (e.g., euphoric) pub sign) with alcohol consumption
Many modern motivational theories or unpleasant (e.g., flushing) effects and (2) the subsequent connection of
of alcohol use rest on the premise that precipitated by alcohol consumption these cues with certain reinforcing
problem drinking is mediated by the (Newlin and Thomson 1990). The actions of alcohol that encourage future
same decision processes that govern all strength of certain personality charac- indulgence in drinking. Such models
alcohol use and that people essentially teristics, such as antisociality (e.g., a diverge from one another mainly with
choose between drinking and alternative tendency toward aggressive or criminal respect to the mechanisms by which
actions. According to this view, people behavior) and sensation seeking (e.g., a alcohol cues are presumed to motivate
decide whether to consume alcoholic strong predilection for novel and risky alcohol use. In particular, each model
beverages by comparing the positive experiences), have also been implicated offers different descriptions of the reac-
consequences they expect to experience in the associated risk for alcoholism tions elicited by alcohol cues.
by drinking with those they expect (Lang 1983). External factors, such as Some conditioning models (Wikler
from not drinking. The figure on page sociocultural drinking norms and per- 1948) hypothesize that ARCs stimulate
198 illustrates the parallel nature of the sonal experiences with alcohol-related an aversive state (e.g., elicit subclinical
pathways that promote either indul- consequences, can also support, inhibit, withdrawal symptoms or negative emo-
gence or restraint. This figure adds two or modulate the use of alcohol and influ- tional responses associated with depri-
features to a model proposed originally ence drinking behavior (White 1993). vation) that lies behind craving and the
by Cox and Klinger (1988): (1) it Thus, biochemical reactivity, person- desire to drink. Other conditioning
incorporates a complementary perspec- ality characteristics, the sociocultural models (Stewart et al. 1984) suggest
tive, based on behavioral theories of environment, and personal experience that alcohol cues stimulate an appetitive
choice (Vuchinich and Tucker 1998), of drinking outcomes help determine state by signaling access to desirable
which holds that preference for alcohol a person’s response to ARCs. However, effects (e.g., euphoria) through drinking.
is inversely related to the accessibility the above-mentioned factors are not In the first instance, craving or motiva-
of alternative valued activities, and (2) necessarily internally consistent or tion to drink is described as a desire to
more importantly, it incorporates an static. Consequently, a person’s responses obtain relief from an aversive state (i.e.,
“evaluative space” to represent the to ARCs often represent a fusion of negative reinforcement); in the second
intersection of the opposing pathways attractions and repulsions, both within instance, craving is viewed as a desire to
of approach and avoidance. Such a particular set of circumstances and experience the pleasurable effects of
ambivalent or conflicting inclinations across time. This can, of course, con- alcohol intoxication (i.e., positive rein-
appear to be central both to cognitive- tribute to ambivalence. forcement). A hybrid model (Baker et
processing theories of craving (Tiffany Excessive drinkers tend to develop al. 1987) involves speculation that cues
1990) and to recent shifts in thinking more marked “conditioned” responses can elicit both types of motives or crav-
about the essence of addiction in more to cues (e.g., the smell of alcohol or ing. In any event, these models predict
general terms (Heather 1998) (see the perhaps a certain mood) (see sidebar by a similar outcome: Exposure to ARCs
articles in this issue by Anton, pp. Tiffany, p. 216) that have been repeat- should increase the inclination to
165–173, and by Tiffany, pp. 215–224). edly associated with drinking. In other “approach” alcoholic beverages.
The integration of a variety of compet- words, exposure to such cues can elicit Although these models have provided
ing factors and responses—both posi- physiological, emotional, and cognitive a rich theoretical foundation for the
tive and negative—within a multidi- reactions, including those that consti- initial study of craving and other reactions
mensional evaluative space ultimately tute “craving.” Several influential con- to alcohol cues, they do not address the
determines a person’s choice to drink ditioning models have been advanced potential elicitation of avoidance incli-
or not drink, thereby underscoring the to explain the relationships among ARC nations by the same cues. Moreover, no
potential importance of the evaluative exposure, craving, and subsequent alcohol existing model can adequately account
space and emphasizing the need for an consumption, particularly in alcoholics. for all of the evidence on craving accu-
explicit assessment of the two indepen- These models have been extensively mulated thus far. Recent developments
dent dimensions. reviewed elsewhere in the literature in neuropsychopharmacology appear
(Drummond et al. 1995); a full descrip- to address at least some of these short-
tion of each of these models is beyond comings by providing potentially
The Role of Historical Factors
the scope of this article. important building blocks for the con-
Influences termed “historical factors” However, all conditioning models struction of a more complete picture
contribute to the decisionmaking process posit that ARCs ultimately elicit a craving of the development and operation of
(Cox and Klinger 1988). Such factors response that motivates further alcohol craving. In particular, advances in this
can shape a person’s drinking experiences use. Each such model accounts for arena have yielded valuable informa-
and his or her subsequent inclination approach inclination (i.e., craving) in tion about the specific effects of AODs
to consume alcohol. For example, terms of the action of two interrelated and AOD-relevant stimuli on brain

Vol. 23, No.www.CE-credit.com


3, 1999 - Helping Professionals Help Others 199
systems and have offered suggestions earlier conditioning models of reactivity hol, this article briefly examines what
about the possible impact of these effects to alcohol cues, can be faulted for its Cox and Klinger (1988) called “current
on subsequent behavior. exclusive focus on the development factors” and the role they might play in
For example, Robinson and Berridge and elicitation of approach inclinations. shaping subsequent decisions to drink
(1993) have highlighted the importance Given the high likelihood that excessive or not drink.
of “neuroadaptation,” or sensitization, drinkers will have histories that include
of certain brain systems due to repeated punishment as well as reinforcement
The Role of Current Factors
substance use. Their theory takes a step from their extensive alcohol use, they
toward the integration of biological and should harbor both negative and posi- Current factors are variables in the imme-
learning processes into a more compre- tive associations with alcohol cues. Not diate situation that influence whether a
hensive model of craving. This analysis only should this dual association lead person is inclined to approach or avoid
holds that psychoactive substance use, to a certain amount of ambivalence in drinking. For example, to the extent
especially by relatively inexperienced their response to alcohol cues, but it that alcohol is available, the context
consumers, can produce a pleasant affec- should also raise questions about what can be seen as conducive to drinking.
tive response by stimulating neural sys- neural substrates might underlie the Similarly, if other people around the
tems associated with reward. This is development and elicitation of avoidance target person encourage drinking, he or
regarded as a simple “liking” for the inclinations or motives to not drink. she is more likely to follow the path of
effects of the substance and can moti- One solution to this dilemma is to approach than the path of avoidance.
vate further use. consider the possibility that in addition Conversely, to the extent that alcohol is
Robinson and Berridge (1993) fur- to mediation by appetitive brain systems unavailable, the immediate context is
ther posit that the reinforcing effects of such as those referred to in connection not appropriate for drinking, and/or
AODs do not maintain long-term sub- with neurobiological models of craving, those around the target person discour-
stance use by addicts. Rather, these two response to alcohol cues and decisions to age drinking, the person is more likely
researchers assert and provide evidence not drink may involve a parallel, aver- to avoid, than approach, alcohol. These
suggesting that the more compulsive sive brain system that governs response immediate circumstances are considered
“wanting” of the substance (i.e., craving) to threats and other negative stimuli. when people assess whether the conse-
may result from repeated substance use Indeed, substantial evidence indicates quences of drinking or not drinking
that provokes a specific neuroadapta- that such a system exists (Gray 1987; P. will likely be positive or negative. In
tion—that is, the progressive and persis- Lang 1995), and certainly its activation addition to situational factors such as
tent hypersensitization of the dopamine by alcohol-related cues is plausible. these, the availability of alternative
pathways (see the article in this issue by Moreover, much recent research behavioral options with predictable
Anton, pp. 165–173) implicated in the indicates that although both the appet- outcomes can mediate the impact of
mediation of wanting or craving the itive and aversive systems are subcorti- the current situation on decisions to
substance. The theory specifically seg- cally based,1 they can interact with more drink or not drink.
regates the mechanisms responsible complex cognitive processes,2 including According to behavioral theories of
for “liking” (based on simple positive attention, perception, imagery, and cer- choice (Vuchinich and Tucker 1998),
reinforcement) from the mechanisms tain types of memory (LeDoux 1995). preference for alcohol consumption
underlying “wanting” (sensitization to Not only do these relatively indepen- varies depending on access to other valued
cues associated with the substance). dent systems provide a neural basis for and enjoyable activities. The availability
This distinction enables the model ambivalence, but their interactions with of alternative rewarding activities pro-
to account for the continued use of the cortex also point to the complemen- motes an indifferent “take it or leave it”
AODs even when the subjective pleasure tary roles of both simple conditioning attitude toward alcohol and predicts
derived from them has disappeared or and higher level cognitive processes in that people with such options will more
diminished greatly, as is often the case reactions to alcohol cues and in inclina- likely follow the path of avoidance than
for addicts. A key element of this model tions to drink or not drink. However, that of approach. However, if alternative
is its proposition that the hypothesized before elaborating on how competing sources of reward are unavailable or
neuroadaptation, or sensitization, is influ- associations and cognitions might influ- limited, or if access to them is delayed
enced by associative learning processes ence the approach or avoidance of alco- or requires more effort, then a person is
in such a way that exposure to cues which more likely to choose to drink rather than
have been reliably paired with substance not drink (Vuchinich and Tucker 1998).
1
use enables the cues themselves to stim- The inner (i.e., subcortical) regions of the brain are In this context, many of the potential
primarily involved in sense perception, motor coor-
ulate sensitization and thereby increase dination, emotion, and the unconscious aspects of
benefits associated with not drinking
and sustain “wanting” for the substance. reinforcement. The brain’s outer layer (i.e., cortex) (e.g., avoiding a hangover) seem partic-
Hence, craving is viewed as involving organizes and interprets the totality of sensory infor- ularly distant when choices are made,
both biochemical and learning processes. mation and plans motor activity. thereby diminishing their impact on
Despite these insights, the theory of 2
Cognition is the process of obtaining, organizing, the decisionmaking process. This fact
neuroadaptation in craving, like the and using intellectual knowledge. enhances the relative impact of the

200 www.CE-credit.com - Helping Professionals Help Others Alcohol Research & Health
Approaching Avoidance

more immediate rewards associated alcohol expectancies focused on beliefs person may experience a period during
with indulgence. that would attract one to drinking (i.e., which negative expectancies and alco-
foster approach inclinations). In other hol use increase simultaneously (Jones
words, the research highlighted the and McMahon 1998).
The Role of Expectancies
importance of positive expectancies to In their review of studies investigat-
Alcohol outcome expectancies (i.e., understanding alcohol use and its prob- ing the concurrent operation of both
beliefs that people hold regarding the lems. Pertinent evidence suggests that positive and negative alcohol expectan-
behavioral, cognitive, and emotional the more positive expectancies people cies, Jones and McMahon (1998) make
effects of alcohol consumption) repre- hold regarding alcohol, the more likely a compelling case for precisely this point.
sent a third category of variables that They present evidence for the pivotal
influence the likelihood that a person role that negative beliefs about alcohol
will be inclined to either approach or effects and outcomes play in determin-
avoid alcoholic beverages. Such expectan-
cies are shaped by the positive and neg-
Evidence suggests ing decisions about drinking. Their
conclusion is consistent with the pro-
ative consequences that a person has that the more positive posal that much may be gained from
experienced as a result of drinking. considering the avoidance inclinations
However, the fact that beliefs about expectancies people that compete with craving and other
alcohol effects develop in children and hold regarding approach inclinations to influence the
adolescents well before they engage in choice to drink or not drink.
any drinking attests to the strong alcohol, the more Holding particular beliefs about
impact of social and observational
learning on them (Dunn and Goldman
likely they are to alcohol’s effects is not in and of itself a
condition sufficient to cause drinking
1998; Lang and Stritzke 1993). use alcohol. to occur (Leigh 1990). People must also
Regardless of the origin of alcohol value the consequences they expect. In
expectancies, and the recognition that this regard, strong empirical evidence
expectancies can change across time they are to use alcohol and, if they indicates that subjective evaluations of
and context, certain obvious predic- already drink, the more likely they are the desirability of expected outcomes
tions can be made based on aggregated to report higher levels of alcohol con- linked to alcohol use moderate the
expectations. To the extent that alcohol sumption (Lang and Michalec 1990). relationship between expectancies and
effects are believed to be mainly posi- However, in investigations focusing drinking decisions (Jones and McMahon
tive, and potential negative outcomes on negative expectancies, the findings 1998). According to motivational
are viewed as relatively modest, people suggest a more dynamic relationship models of alcohol use, subjective evalu-
should be more likely to follow the path between negative expectancies and alco- ations of expected consequences of
of approach than avoidance. Further- hol consumption (Jones and McMahon indulgence provide motives for drink-
more, this finding should be particularly 1998). For example, negative expectancies ing that are the more proximal deter-
true when anticipated reinforcements in both light and moderate drinkers minants of choices about alcohol use
are immediate and punishments delayed. seem to be associated with less alcohol (Cox and Klinger 1988).
Conversely, to the extent that the effects use, perhaps indicating that negative
of drinking are expected to be predom- expectancies promote avoidance and The Role of Motives
inantly negative, even when some expec- restraint. Because negative expectancies
tation of modest positive effects exists, among lighter drinkers may be relatively The evaluative space of the model
people should ordinarily be motivated mild (e.g., “I would expect my hand- depicted in the figure on page 198 is
to avoid, rather than approach, alcohol. writing to be affected” or “I would useful when considering a patient’s
However, such applications of global expect to feel fuzzy”), positive expectan- motives or reasons for drinking or not
expectancies may overlook the rele- cies may remain dominant. However, drinking alcohol. Research in the area
vance of alcohol beliefs specific to par- people are more likely to experience of motivation to drink has not ordinar-
ticular situations and often fail to fully weightier negative consequences as ily been designed to analyze competing
consider the independent roles of their drinking escalates or persists (e.g., inclinations to either approach or avoid
divergent expectancies. “I would expect to get into a fight” or alcoholic beverages, nor has it consid-
A critical question emerging from “I would expect to lose my job”). This ered changes in motives across time and
alcohol expectancy research as it pertains finding suggests that negative expectan- context. However, research has revealed
to predictions about people’s choice to cies might ultimately challenge initially that different global motives for drink-
drink or not drink concerns the relative dominant positive expectancies and ing are associated with fairly distinct
contribution of positive versus negative subsequently promote ambivalence patterns of alcohol use and abuse (Cooper
expectancies (Jones and McMahon toward drinking. However, until the 1994). For instance, efforts to enhance
1998). As was the case with craving point is reached where negative expectan- pleasure and to cope with negative
research, most of the early studies of cies begin to affect drinking behavior, a emotions have both been identified as

Vol. 23, No.www.CE-credit.com


3, 1999 - Helping Professionals Help Others 201
potentially important motives for alco- perhaps even threatening or enforcing fulfill responsibilities) tend to occur
hol use. Drinking to cope with negative stronger negative sanctions. later in time. Similarly, Heilizer (1964)
emotions, however, is primarily predic- Each of the steps and diverse cate- proposed that the strengthening of
tive of alcohol problems in adolescents gories of variables reviewed thus far approach inclinations could be attributed
(Cooper et al. 1995) and alcohol depen- involves competing forces. These forces, to the increasing salience of prospective
dence in adults (Carpenter and Hasin in turn, must be weighed and combined alcohol reinforcers, relative to punishers,
1998). Unfortunately, distinct biases to determine whether the decisional as one gets closer to the goal. Approach
analogous to those observed in alcohol- balance will tip toward drinking or not stimuli become more prevalent and
expectancy research are also evident in drinking for any one person in any sit- gain intensity, whereas stimuli related
the extant literature on alcohol-related uation. In other words, a full under- to alcohol avoidance (i.e., punishment
motives. Clearly, the focus has been standing of the impact of craving on cues) tend to remain constant. Heilizer
primarily on the reasons why people alcohol use requires consideration of argued that these responses occur because
say they want to drink, rather than on the relative impact of the inclination approach cues are likely to involve
the potentially important reasons why to avoid alcohol use as well. explicit characteristics of the alcohol-
people might want to avoid alcohol drinking context and its immediate
and choose to not drink. consequences. In contrast, avoidance
As a first step toward redefining the The Concept of Ambivalence cues are liable to be more remote
focus of alcohol research, we developed and the Mapping of an and/or abstract (e.g., cognitive repre-
a questionnaire to assess motives for not Evaluative Space sentations of future negative conse-
drinking alcohol. This questionnaire quences) in their associations. Although
was based on the general principles In his treatise on why “excessive appetites” neither of these theorists—Astin nor
of an expanded version of Cox and for alcohol consumption and other Heilizer— actually tested their hypothe-
Klinger’s (1988) motivational model addictive behaviors revolve around ses, their work has obvious relevance to
of alcohol use and applied a measure- conflict or ambivalence as the central, the study of reactivity to alcohol cues.
ment approach similar to that used by defining construct, Orford (1985) cited Moreover, data accumulated in connec-
Cooper and colleagues (1995) to study the work of two independent researchers tion with modern behavioral theories
motives for drinking. who had applied classic conflict theory of choice as applied to drinking
Preliminary results indicate that at (Miller 1944) to the phenomenon of (Vuchinich and Tucker 1998) tend to
least among adolescents, different motives excessive alcohol use. Both Astin (1962) support at least Astin’s assertion regard-
for not drinking are strongly linked to and Heilizer (1964) suggested that for ing the importance of temporal factors.
different aspects of alcohol use. For problem drinkers, alcohol-associated Heather’s (1998) proposal of a three-
example, the frequency of alcohol cues induce an approach-avoidance level conceptual framework for addic-
use and the category of drinker (i.e., conflict. According to both Astin’s and tion also involves both approach and
drinker versus abstainer) are predicted Heilizer’s models, conflict arises in alco- avoidance components. He regards
by constraints associated with religion holics because previous alcohol use has ambivalence, expressed behaviorally as
and family and by motivational indif- been both reinforced and punished. repeated failures to refrain from substance
ference. In contrast, the quantity of Both authors noted that rather than use despite intentions to do so, as the
alcohol consumed on a typical drink- sustaining an ambivalent state, alcoholics basis of the definition of addiction.
ing occasion is predicted only by fear exposed to alcohol cues tend repeatedly Briefly stated, the three components of
of negative consequences. to resolve the conflict in the approach Heather’s framework are as follows: (1)
These findings emphasize the impor- direction (i.e., they choose to drink). In persistent desire to use the substance
tance of studying people’s motives for other words, their desire to drink appears because of its rewarding consequences,
avoiding alcohol and suggest that both to increase as they near the goal (i.e., (2) neuroadaptation to a substance
alcohol education and prevention pro- alcohol), whereas their avoidance incli- resulting from repeated use, and (3) a
grams for teenagers should be tailored nation appears to remain constant or pattern involving the apparent inability
accordingly. If a program’s objective is even decline along the way. of addicts to curtail their indulgence
to prevent or forestall young people’s Astin (1962) proposed that the despite trying to do so. The ambiva-
initiation to drinking, an effective strat- mechanism underlying this observed lence central to Heather’s framework
egy might be to emphasize traditional pattern of resolution in favor of approach represents conflict attributable to inter-
relationships and encourage involvement was related to the timing and nature of play between the neuroadaptations
in rewarding activities that are alterna- the consequences associated with from repeated use and the person’s his-
tives to drinking and thereby increase drinking. Positively reinforcing conse- tory of consequences from use.
indifference toward alcohol. However, if quences of alcohol use (e.g., the eupho- Beyond stressing the importance of
the objective is to reduce alcohol con- ria of intoxication or relief from stress) the role of ambivalence and possible
sumption among drinkers, the most tend to occur soon after consumption, neural substrates involved in it, what is
effective approach might be to focus on whereas adverse consequences (e.g., of particular relevance to the multidi-
the adverse consequences of indulgence, hangover or punishment for failure to mensional framework of reactivity to

202 www.CE-credit.com - Helping Professionals Help Others Alcohol Research & Health
Approaching Avoidance

alcohol cues and decisions about drinking cated in the figure on page 198 depicts approach (i.e., “craving”) and avoid-
is Heather’s speculation about the this framework, which is described by ance (i.e., “aversion”) inclinations. The
cognitive-behavioral mechanisms under- four quadrants. Craving, in its classic participants, who were on methadone
lying the development of conflict. Drawing form as intense and unrestrained “want- maintenance for heroin addiction while
from Ainslie’s (1975) theory of impul- ing” is synonymous with the approach undergoing treatment for cocaine
sive behavior, Heather observed that quadrant, whereas strong inclination to dependence, viewed a videotape depict-
when faced with a choice between not drink, in the absence of any signifi- ing persons using cocaine. The partici-
“early small” and “late large” rewards, cant inclination to indulge, is represented pants also were asked to handle their
addicts repeatedly fail to implement by the avoidance quadrant. If both preferred type of cocaine paraphernalia.
normal cognitive compensation for their response inclinations are balanced but at Before and after exposure to these cues,
“irrational preferences.” In other words, a low level of intensity, a person is char- the participants were asked to use rat-
addicts appear to have difficulty basing acterized by indifference about drinking ing scales to respond to the following
their decisionmaking on anticipation of alcohol, whereas if both inclinations are questions: “How much do you crave
future outcomes, an ability that ordinar- balanced and at a high level of inten- cocaine right now?” and “How much
ily enables humans to adjust their cur- sity, a person struggles with ambivalence does the idea of using cocaine turn you
rent behavior in order to obtain larger about choices to drink or not drink. off right now?” Results indicated that
rewards (or avoid punishment) later. overall craving and aversion ratings
Although the reasons for this failure were negatively correlated at baseline,
are not specified, a basis for speculation Evidence for a but they were not significantly corre-
does exist. For example, impaired deci- Multidimensional, lated after cocaine cue exposure, sug-
sionmaking may occur because the level Ambivalence Model gesting that these inclinations vary
of immediate reinforcement available independently of one another.
provokes the brain to adapt to repeated A small but growing number of empir- Further analysis revealed the presence
exposures to the psychoactive substances, ical findings support the view that an of four subsamples of patients demon-
and the operation of neural systems investigation of a multidimensional or strating differing response patterns to
underlying the inclinations of addicts ambivalence model of choices about the cues. One group showed an increase
ultimately begins to deviate from that drinking may be an important step in craving and a decrease in aversion,
evident in nonaddicts. In this connec- toward better understanding the rela- indicating a clear shift toward the
tion, Heather notes that his perspective tionship between craving and substance approach quadrant of the evaluative
is compatible with the neuroadaptation use. Greeley and colleagues explicitly space. Another group showed an increase
theory of Robinson and Berridge (1993) acknowledged the potential of alcohol- in craving but no decrease in aversion.
discussed earlier. Moreover, an explana- related cues to elicit avoidance inclina- Depending on the initial level of aver-
tion for the development of a “desire to tion (Greeley et al. 1993a; Greeley et sion, their craving may or may not
curtail indulgence,” so central to Heather’s al. 1993b). They used a bidirectional have been balanced by an equivalent
conflict perspective, can be derived “craving scale” to measure alcoholics’ level of aversion, and thus they could
from consideration of associations link- and social drinkers’ subjective reactions be represented in either the approach
ing drug cues to addicts’ discomfort to alcohol cues relative to neutral cues. or the ambivalence quadrant. A third
with their compulsion and other pun- At one extreme of this scale was “defi- group showed no increase in craving,
ishing consequences of repeated use. nitely do not want a drink of alcohol,” but a decrease in aversion, indicating
Regarding these insights, the use of whereas at the other end of the scale a shift away from the avoidance and
restraint when faced with the proximal was “an extreme desire for a drink of toward either approach or indifference,
temptations associated with indulgence alcohol.” Unfortunately, because the depending on the initial level of crav-
requires the processing of information researchers attempted to assess approach ing. Finally, a group of “nonrespon-
about consequences that are often dis- and avoidance by means of a single ders” showed no increase in craving
tal and perhaps more abstract as well. scale, participants were required to col- and no decrease in avoidance.
Thus, the decision to not drink may be lapse the two inclinations and arrive When analyzed together, these results
more cognitively demanding than the at a “sum.” The authors acknowledged indicated that levels of craving and
decision to drink, therefore rendering that this summation process obscured aversion, constructs that can be readily
restraint the relatively more difficult measurement of the true level of mapped onto the dimensions of the
path to follow. ambivalence that participants may have evaluative space, could be altered by
In sum, craving may be best con- experienced. Consequently, the authors exposure to substance-related cues.
ceptualized within a broader, multidi- called for future studies to measure Moreover, evidence suggests that these
mensional perspective that incorporates approach and avoidance inclinations changes not only varied independently,
the relative influence of an inclination independently. but could also be influenced by indi-
to not drink. This involves a frame- Avants and colleagues (1995) sought vidual differences. For example, further
work in which competing motives are to examine the reactivity of addicts to analyses revealed that individual differ-
evaluated. The evaluative space indi- drug cues using separate assessments of ences in two areas distinguished nonre-

Vol. 23, No.www.CE-credit.com


3, 1999 - Helping Professionals Help Others 203
sponders from those who showed the approach and avoidance reactions to alcohol-specific responses for both types
most pronounced shift toward the cigarette stimuli. Results indicated that of outcomes than did light social drinkers.
approach quadrant. Relative to nonre- nonsmokers were characterized by a This finding further supports the
sponders, those who reported an increase combination of low approach and high notion that approach or craving should
in craving and a decrease in aversion avoidance inclinations, representing be integrated with avoidance to reflect
also perceived cocaine to be more rein- the avoidance quadrant in the model. the multidimensional nature of responses
forcing and less punishing, and they In contrast, regular smokers not trying that seem especially likely to accrue as
saw themselves as less able to avoid to quit reported high approach and low repeated alcohol use strengthens associ-
using cocaine in certain high-risk situa- ations in memory that link alcohol
tions. Thus, the work of Avants and cognitions to both positive and nega-
colleagues (1995) provided general tive consequences of drinking.
support for a multidimensional, ambiva- Strong memory associations to
lence model of reactivity to drug cues Researchers need to alcohol-related cues and behaviors are
and helped establish the model’s clinical
relevance by noting that observed shifts
understand how also central to Tiffany’s (1990) influen-
tial cognitive processing model of crav-
appeared to be associated with variables problem drinking does ing and substance use. According to
such as perceptions of the net benefit this model, if practiced regularly, drug
of drug taking and of self-efficacy in not develop and what use becomes automatized and, like
coping with high-risk situations.
More direct evidence in support of
produces protection other highly practiced skills, relies on
strong memory associations for rapid
this model stems from research con- against it. and effortless execution.
ducted in our own laboratory (Breiner Within the framework of the evalu-
et al. 1997). Using a large sample of ative space, this response would be
undergraduate students, we measured characteristic of the craving associated
separate approach and avoidance reac- avoidance inclinations, placing them with the approach quadrant. However,
tions to photographic stimuli depicting in the approach quadrant in the model. Tiffany maintains that craving does not
several kinds of consumable substances, Most interesting was the finding that involve automatic processing. In his
including alcohol and cigarettes. smokers who were trying to quit reported model, craving refers to a constellation
Respondents with various patterns of high approach and high avoidance incli- of responses supported by nonauto-
routine usage of these substances were nations characteristic of the ambiva- matic, effortful cognitive processes acti-
asked to view slides, responding after lence quadrant. Thus, it was not the vated only if the habitual sequence of
each one to the following questions: level of craving, but rather avoidance, drug use behaviors is blocked by limited
“How much do you want to consume that identified smokers who were ready access to the substance or by an inten-
the item pictured in the slide?” and to change. tional effort to curtail use. Implicit in
“How much do you want to avoid con- In addition to evidence from research at least the latter of these scenarios is
suming the item pictured in the slide?” on reactions to drug cues, data from the experience of ambivalence, opera-
Whereas ratings for approach and studies investigating memory processes tionalized in the model as the simulta-
avoidance were significantly negatively are consistent with a multidimensional neous activation of opposing response
correlated in abstainers from both cate- or ambivalence perspective. For instance, inclinations. To what extent can the
gories (nonsmokers and nondrinkers), Leigh and Stacy (1998) reported that two views be reconciled?
results for light and moderate alcohol memory associations related to both One perspective on cognitive con-
drinkers and for occasional and daily reinforcing and punishing consequences flict or ambivalence provides for the
smokers indicated no significant corre- of alcohol use can be activated by explicit definition and measurement of
lations between approach and avoid- the same type of cognitive task. They component dimensions, whereas con-
ance. This independence of variation examined associative memory and alcohol flict in Tiffany’s cognitive processing
argues for the need to separate these use and demonstrated that participants’ model is inferred from the increased
two dimensions and for the potential histories of alcohol use (i.e., quantity cognitive effort thought to be associ-
of the competing inclinations to coexist. and frequency of drinking) predicted ated with the dual processing required
Data from a college student sample their associative memory responses to when competing response inclinations
also yielded some particularly interesting both positive and negative outcomes of are present (Cepeda-Benito and Tiffany
information on reactions to cigarette- drinking. When given a list of positive 1996; Sayette et al. 1994). However,
relevant stimuli as a function of desire and negative outcomes not specific to the two models are complementary in
to change behavior. Participants were alcohol (e.g., “feeling good”; “forgetting that Tiffany’s distinction between the
divided into three groups: nonsmokers, problems”; and “being more social” automatic processing associated with
regular smokers not trying to quit, and versus “feeling sick,” “being depressed,” direct, unimpeded approach and the
regular smokers currently trying to quit. and “losing control”), heavy social effortful processing associated with
We then compared the participants’ drinkers generated significantly more craving is also a distinction that differ-

204 www.CE-credit.com - Helping Professionals Help Others Alcohol Research & Health
Approaching Avoidance

entiates the quadrants in the multidi- than factors promoting approach incli- sonal goals and the harm stemming
mensional, ambivalence model. nations. Clearly, the full picture involves from their strong inclination to drink
Preliminary laboratory evidence is more than the stimulation of craving (Miller 1998). By encouraging an
pertinent here (Stritzke et al. 1997). by cues that have been associated with assessment and weighing of the pros
Participants who simultaneously reported drinking. and cons of drinking versus not drink-
high levels of both approach and avoid- Reactions associated with alcohol ing, the aim is to strengthen a client’s
ance inclinations in connection with cues are multifaceted and capable of inclination to avoid alcohol relative to
the viewing of appetizing food slides interacting with a wide range of other the inclination to approach it. In terms
showed a significant increase in heart factors to touch off parallel and poten- of the concept of an evaluative space,
rate during slide viewing. Such an ele- tially contradictory response chains “discrepancy” has been achieved when
vation of heart rate is consistent with that must be resolved. Recognition of a client moves from the approach quad-
the engagement of greater cognitive these complexities and a shift toward rant into the ambivalence quadrant.
effort during the viewing of food slides simultaneous consideration of compet- When working with clients who expe-
(Hare 1972). In contrast, participants ing approach and avoidance inclina- rience addictive behavior problems, the
who rated their reactions to the same tions are essential to a better under- application of an ambivalence model,
slides as high in approach but low in standing of craving and choices about with its two-dimensional evaluative
avoidance showed a decrease in heart drinking. A multidimensional, ambiva- space, has significant advantages over
rate. This decrease in heart rate reflects lence model provides a framework for unidimensional assessments of craving.
simple orienting but no elaborative future investigations in this area as well Clients who know that the strength of
processing (Graham and Clifton 1966). as suggests avenues for prevention of their inclination to not drink will also
Using similar picture-viewing protocols, drinking by young people and for the be measured seem more able to
comparable contrasts in heart rate treatment of anyone who suffers from acknowledge their inclination to drink.
responses were also found for restrained problem drinking. This works to improve the validity of
(i.e., ambivalent) eaters versus nonre- Most prevention programs emphasize self-reports because it diminishes the
strained eaters (Stritzke et al. 1997) in a the adverse consequences of drinking demand to deny craving that is often
combined sample. These data support and promote abstinence from all drink- so intense in clinical settings.
the applicability of a multidimensional, ing, leaving children to wonder why Furthermore, ambivalence can be
ambivalence model to a wide range of anyone would drink or how they could a normal and important step toward
addictive and habitual behavior problems. drink moderately and responsibly increasing readiness and maintaining
under appropriate circumstances as efforts to change. In fact, to the extent
adults (Lang and Stritzke 1993). How- that clients struggle with strong incli-
Summary and Implications ever, societal ambivalence about alcohol nations to drink alcohol, ambivalence
for Prevention and is liable to be reflected in the ambiva- may be the only buffer between the
Intervention lence many young people experience resolve not to drink and relapse during
as they face the challenge of making the initial stages of treatment. In this
This article began by noting that the responsible decisions about drinking. connection, we wish to emphasize that
traditional focus of alcohol research on Evidence suggests that developmental ambivalence is associated with inaction,
craving as the force driving individuals shifts occur in the way children evalu- whereas drinking is a state of action.
down the path toward alcohol con- ate alcohol’s positive and negative As long as ambivalence is maintained,
sumption fails to adequately account effects (Dunn and Goldman 1998). lapses into drinking should be mini-
for the role of competing inclinations For prevention strategies to be optimally mized. It also follows that fluctuations
to avoid alcohol and not drink. A simi- effective, researchers need to under- in the strength of avoidance inclina-
lar bias also exists in research on “his- stand how problem drinking does not tions may be better predictors of treat-
torical” and dispositional risk factors as develop and what produces protection ment outcome than the strength of
well as on situational or “current” fac- against it (Zucker and Gomberg 1986). approach inclinations, which often
tors relevant to choices about drinking. Utilizing a framework that accounts for remain high and fairly constant, at least
A brief review of recent research devel- the balance between approach and through the early phase of interven-
opments, especially in the areas of alco- avoidance inclinations is an important tion. Tiffany (1990) has pointed out
hol expectancies and drinking motives, step in that direction. that craving alone is not necessary for
has further revealed that avoidance When considering interventions for substance use. However, according to
inclinations associated with the pathway people with drinking problems, every a more integrative and comprehensive
of restraint are potentially important clinician knows that motivation is a analysis, when craving occurs, it must
determinants of the choices to drink or vital element. Consequently, an initial be counterbalanced by avoidance incli-
not drink. Within this context, evi- and fundamental goal of contemporary nations that often demand intense cog-
dence indicates that factors promoting motivational interviewing techniques is nitive effort to produce ambivalence
avoidance inclinations appear to be to provoke clients to recognize a “dis- if substance use is ultimately to be
better predictors of treatment outcome crepancy” between their important per- restrained. ■

Vol. 23, No.www.CE-credit.com


3, 1999 - Helping Professionals Help Others 205
GRAHAM, F.K., AND CLIFTON, R.C. Heart-rate MILLER, N.E. Experimental studies of conflict. In:
References change as a component of the orienting response. Hunt, J.M., ed. Personality and the Behavior Disorders.
Psychological Bulletin 65:305–320, 1966. New York: Ronald Press, 1944.
AINSLIE, G. Specious reward: A behavioural theory
of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological GRAY, J.A. The Psychology of Fear and Stress. 2d ed. MILLER, W.R. Enhancing motivation for change.
Bulletin 82:463–496, 1975. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, In: Miller, W.R., and Heather, N., eds. Treating
1987. Addictive Behaviors. 2d ed. New York: Plenum
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Press, 1998. pp. 121–132.
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth GREELEY, J.D.; SWIFT, W.; AND HEATHER, N. To
Edition. Washington, DC: the Association, 1994. drink or not to drink? Assessing conflicting desires MILLER, W.R., AND ROLLNICK, S. Motivational
in dependent drinkers in treatment. Drug and Alcohol Interviewing. New York: Guilford Press, 1991.
ASTIN, A. “Bad habits” and social deviation: A pro- Dependence 32:169–179, 1993a.
posed revision in conflict theory. Journal of Clinical NEWLIN, D.B., AND THOMSON, J.B. Alcohol chal-
Psychology 18:227–231, 1962. GREELEY, J.D.; SWIFT, W.; PRESCOTT, J.; AND lenge with sons of alcoholics: A critical review and
HEATHER, N. Reactivity to alcohol-related cues in analysis. Psychological Bulletin 108:383–402, 1990.
AVANTS, K.S.; MARGOLIN, A.; KOSTEN, T.R.; AND heavy and light drinkers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol
COONEY, N.L. Differences between responders and 54:359–368, 1993b. ORFORD, J. Excessive Appetites: A Psychological View
nonresponders to cocaine cues in the laboratory. of Addictions. New York: Wiley & Sons, 1985.
Addictive Behaviors 20:215–224, 1995. HARE, R.D. Response requirements and directional
fractionation of autonomic response. Psychophysiology PROCHASKA, J.O.; DICLEMENTE, C.C.; AND
BAKER, T.B.; MORSE, E.; AND SHERMAN, J.E. The 9:419–427, 1972. NORCROSS, J.C. In search of how people change:
motivation to use drugs: A psychobiological analy- Applications to addictive behaviors. In: Marlatt,
sis of urges. In: Rivers, C., ed. The Nebraska HEATHER, N. A conceptual framework for explain- G.A., and VandenBos, G.R., eds. Addictive Behaviors:
Symposium on Motivation: Alcohol Use and Abuse. ing drug addiction. Journal of Psychopharmacology Reading on Etiology, Prevention, and Treatment.
Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press, 1987. 12(1):3–7, 1998. Washington, DC: American Psychological
pp. 257–323. Association, 1997.
HEILIZER, F. Conflict models, alcohol, and drinking
BREINER, M.J.; STRITZKE, W.; CURTIN, J.J.; PATRICK patterns. Journal of Psychology 57:457–473, 1964. ROBINSON, T.E., AND BERRIDGE, K.C. The neural
C.J.; AND LANG, A.R. “Individuals Respond to basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization
Appetitive Cues Based on Learning Histories with JONES, B.T., AND MCMAHON, J. Alcohol motiva- theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews 18:
Substances.” Poster presented at the American tions as outcome expectancies. In: Miller, W.R., 247–291, 1993.
Psychological Society Meeting in Washington, D.C., and Heather, N., eds. Treating Addictive Behaviors.
2d ed. New York: Plenum Press, 1998. pp. 75–91. SAYETTE, M.A.; MONTI, P.M.; ROHSENOW, D.J.;
in May 1997. AND GULLIVER, S.B. The effects of cue exposure on
LANG, A.R. Addictive personality: A viable construct? reaction time in male alcoholics. Journal of Studies
CARPENTER, K.M., AND HASIN, D. A prospective
In: Levison, P.K.; Gerstein, D.R.; and Maloff, on Alcohol 55:629–633, 1994.
evaluation of the relationship between reasons for
D.R., eds. Commonalities in Substance Abuse and
drinking and DSM-IV alcohol-use disorders. STEWART, J.; DEWIT, H.; AND EIKELBOOM, R.
Habitual Behavior. Lexington, MA: Lexington
Addictive Behaviors 23:41–46, 1998. Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects
Books, 1983. pp. 157–236.
CEPEDA-BENITO, A., AND TIFFANY, S.T. The use of in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants.
LANG, A.R., AND MICHALEC, E.M. Expectancy Psychological Review 91:251–268, 1984.
a dual-task procedure for the assessment of cognitive
effects in reinforcement from alcohol. In: Cox,
effort associated with cigarette craving. Psycho- STRITZKE, W.G.K.; DROBES, D.J.; LANG, A.R.;
E.M., ed. Why People Drink. New York: Gardner
pharmacology 127:155–163, 1996. PATRICK, C.J.; AND LANG, P.J. Cardiac reactivity
Press, 1990. pp. 193–232.
COOPER, M.L. Motivations for alcohol use among during appetitive picture processing: Dietary restraint
LANG, A.R., AND STRITZKE, W.G.K. Children and and ambivalence. Psychophysiology 34:S87, 1997.
adolescents: Development and validation of a four-
alcohol. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments
factor model. Psychological Assessment 6:117–128, TIFFANY, S.T. A cognitive model of drug urges and
in Alcoholism: Volume 11. Ten Years of Progress.
1994. drug use behavior: Role of automatic and non-
New York: Plenum Press, 1993. pp. 73–85.
COOPER, M.L.; FRONE, M.R.; RUSSELL, M.; AND automatic processes. Psychological Review 97:
LANG, P. The emotion probe: Studies of motiva- 147–168, 1990.
MUDAR, P. Drinking to regulate positive and nega-
tion and attention. American Psychologist 50:
tive emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. VUCHINICH, R.E., AND TUCKER, J.A. Choice,
372–385, 1995.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69: behavioral economics, and addictive behavior pat-
990–1005, 1995. LEDOUX, J.E. Emotion: Clues from the brain. terns. In: Miller, W.R., and Heather, N., eds.
Annual Review of Psychology 46:209–235, 1995. Treating Addictive Behaviors. 2d ed. New York:
COX, W.M., AND KLINGER, E. A motivational model
Plenum, 1998. pp. 93–104.
of alcohol use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology LEIGH, B.C. Alcohol expectancies and reasons for
97:168–180, 1988. drinking: Comments from a study of sexuality. WHITE, H.R. Sociology. In: Galanter, M., ed.
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 4:91–96, 1990. Recent Developments in Alcoholism: Volume 11. Ten
DRUMMOND, D.C.; TIFFANY, S.T.; GLAUTIER, S.;
Years of Progress. New York: Plenum, 1993. pp.
AND REMINGTON, B., EDS. Addictive Behaviour: Cue LEIGH, B.C., AND STACY, A.W. Individual differ- 7–27.
Exposure Theory and Practice. Chichester, England: ences in memory associations involving the positive
Wiley, 1995. and negative outcomes of alcohol use. Psychology of WIKLER, A. Recent progress in research on the neu-
Addictive Behaviors 12:39–46, 1998. rophysiologic basis of morphine addiction.
DUNN, M.E., AND GOLDMAN, M.S. Age and American Journal of Psychiatry 105:329–338, 1948.
drinking-related differences in the memory organi- MARLATT, G.A., AND GORDON, J.R. Relapse
zation of alcohol expectancies in 3rd-, 6th-, 9th-, Prevention: Maintenance Strategies in the Treatment ZUCKER, R.A., AND GOMBERG, E.S.L. Etiology of
and 12th-grade children. Journal of Consulting and of Addictive Behaviors. New York: Guilford Press, alcoholism reconsidered. American Psychologist
Clinical Psychology 66:579–585, 1998. 1985. 41:783–793, 1986.

206 www.CE-credit.com - Helping Professionals Help Others Alcohol Research & Health
View publication stats

You might also like