You are on page 1of 15

Contributions to Conceptual Growth: The Elaboration

of Ellis’s Model for Information-Seeking Behavior

Reijo Savolainen
School of Information Sciences, FIN-33014 University of Tampere, Finland. E-mail: Reijo.Savolainen@uta.fi

Using Ellis’s seminal model of information seeking as an For the modeling of information behavior, the 1980s and
example, this study demonstrates how the elaborations 1990s were a golden age because many of the classic frame-
made to the original framework since the late 1980s have works were developed at that time. Highly influential frame-
contributed to conceptual growth in information-seeking
studies. To this end, nine key studies elaborating Ellis’s works include the model for information-seeking behavior
model were scrutinized by conceptual analysis. The find- proposed by Wilson (1981), the Anomalous State of Knowl-
ings indicate that the elaborations are based on two main edge (ASK) model elaborated by Belkin et al. (1982), the
approaches: adding novel, context-specific components berrypicking model of information searching (Bates, 1989),
in the model and redefining and restructuring the compo- the behavioral model of information seeking (Ellis (1989),
nents. The elaborations have contributed to conceptual
growth in three major ways. First, integrating formerly the Information Search Process (ISP) model (Kuhlthau,
separate parts of knowledge; second, generalizing and 1993), the Comprehensive Model of Information Seeking
explaining lower abstraction-level knowledge through (CMIS) (Johnson, 1996), the model for information-seeking
higher-level constructs; and third, expanding knowledge of professionals (Leckie, Pettigrew, & Sylvain, 1996), and
by identifying new characteristics of the object of study, the general model of information behavior (Wilson, 1997).
that is, information-seeking behavior. Further elaboration
of Ellis’s model toward a theory would require more Since the beginning of the 2000s, the repertoire of models
focused attempts to test hypotheses in work-related envi- has been enriched by new constructs such as the nonlinear
ronments in particular. model of information seeking (Foster, 2004), the integrated
research framework for information seeking and retrieval
(Ingwersen & J€arvelin, 2005), and the information-seeking
and communication model (Robson & Robinson, 2013).
Introduction However, not all of the recent models are entirely novel
There has long been an interest in the development of constructs because they build on the classic frameworks.
conceptual frameworks for information behavior in the field These frameworks are particularly interesting from the view-
of library and information science as well as in other areas point of the present study because they elicit the question
such as health communication (Lambert & Loiselle, 2007; about the potential of elaborating existing models of infor-
Wilson, 2010). These endeavors have resulted in a rich tap- mation behavior instead of adding the number of novel
estry of models that mostly focus on a subset of information frameworks. The elaboration of existing models is rational
behavior, that is, information seeking (Case, 2012, pp. 133– because it serves the ends of cumulating and refining knowl-
198). So far there is a dearth of genuine scientific theories of edge. Unfortunately, as the research tradition of information
information-seeking behavior, if such theories are under- behavior research suggests, this is not always the case
stood as empirically validated constructs capable of explain- because there is a tendency for new models to be published
ing and predicting how people select and access sources of but never developed further (Foster & Urquhart, 2012, p.
information (Case, 2012, pp. 172–174). Overall, research on 785). Although in itself the existence of diverse models is a
information-seeking behavior is still at the pretheoretical or richness enabling a multiperspective view of the phenomena
modeling stage. However, the value of pretheoretical con- of information behavior, the downside is that researchers
structs should not be underestimated by calling them “just have to operate by using multiple maps (Dervin, 2003).
models” because they are of great value in the development The above issues are pertinent from the perspective of
theories (Bates, 2005, p. 3). theoretical growth in information behavior research. The
present study departs from the assumption that theoretical
growth is a long-time process. From this perspective, the
Received September 22, 2015; revised October 12, 2015; accepted elaboration of existing models toward theories may be more
October 13, 2015 productive than the proliferation of new models. According
C 2016 ASIS&T  Published online 24 March 2016 in Wiley Online
V to Vakkari (1998), theoretical growth can be assessed by
Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI: 10.1002/asi.23680 focusing on two major aspects: the conceptual elaboration of

JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY, 68(3):594–608, 2017
substantive (unit) theories and observing the empirical sup- describing actions in a sequential or cyclic fashion (Wilson,
port gained by such theories. The present investigation sug- 2010, p. 2392). Many models of information behavior
gests that the assumptions about theoretical growth can be include textual and graphic representations so that diagrams
expanded to involve scientific models as well. This is are used to express actual or theoretical relationships or ideal
because, similar to substantive theories, models consist of a processes of interaction with information (Wilson, 2010, p.
set of concepts and a set of relations interconnecting those 2393). The components of such models may include, for
concepts in an account of some phenomenon. To strengthen example, the information user and her/his context and the
the focus of the study, the present investigation concentrates information resources available to the user.
on the aspect of conceptual growth. According to Vakkari Scientific models can be evaluated by diverse criteria.
(1998), conceptual growth means that a new theory or model While comparing competing models, attention may be devoted
is conceptually more specific, more general, or more com- to their accuracy, clarity, conceptual coherence, scope, and
prehensive than its earlier variants. Conceptual growth may explanatory power, for example (Pluta, Chinn, & Duncan,
also manifest itself in the introduction of new or alternative 2011, p. 486). Models can also be evaluated by devoting
concepts in the existing frame. attention to their basic aims (Bunge, 1967, p. 383; J€arvelin &
The present study has two overall goals. First, it attempts Wilson, 2003). Because the present investigation is mainly
to identify the ways in which models of information seeking interested in the conceptual growth brought by the elaboration
are elaborated. Second, an attempt is made to find out how of models, the following aims identified by Bunge (1967, p.
the refinement of models contribute to conceptual growth in 383) are particularly relevant to the above aspect:
information-seeking studies. To achieve this, a conceptual
analysis was made by concentrating on the seminal model • Integrating formerly separate parts of knowledge;
developed by Ellis (1987, 1989). His model was chosen for • Generalizing and explaining lower abstraction level knowl-
analysis because, different from most frameworks of edge (or observations, data) through higher level constructs;
information-seeking behavior, it has been elaborated several • Expanding knowledge by deducing new propositions based
times by diverse researchers, thus exemplifying a relatively on selected starting points and collected information;
• Improving the testability of hypotheses through the control
long developmental line and an interest in cumulating
context provided by systems of hypotheses;
knowledge within the research community. • Explaining facts through systems of hypotheses, which entail
The remainder of the article is structured as follows. First,
the facts.
to give background, the nature of scientific models is character-
ized, followed by the specification of the research setting. The
The above list is not exhaustive. Scientific models can
next sections report the findings. The final section discusses
also serve more practical ends such as providing a working
the findings and draws conclusions of their significance.
strategy for a research project (J€arvelin & Wilson, 2003).
This aim refers to the possibilities for further inquiry arising
Scientific Models
from the contributions made through the use of the model in
There is no consensus among researchers about the defini- research and practice. Because the present study concentrates
tion of the concepts of theory and model (Frigg & Hartmann, on the aims dealing with conceptual growth, the practical
2012). However, theory can be generally understood as a set ends of this type are not discussed in greater detail. To
of statements presenting a systematic view of a phenomenon strengthen the focus of the study, it was necessary to outline
with the purpose of explanation or prediction. Models tend to the conceptual field in which the investigation operates. For
focus on more specific problems than do theories, typically this purpose, the framework of human information behavior
making their content more concrete (Jansen & Rieh, 2010 and information systems developed by Jansen and Rieh
p. 1519). Scientific models represent a phenomenon, cover- (2010, p. 1518) was particularly useful. By drawing on Wil-
ing all relatively stable and general features of the world that son (1999), they identified three nested domains of informa-
are interesting from a scientific point of view (Frigg & tion behavior. At the highest level of generality, human
Hartmann, 2012). Models simplify complex reality to make information behavior is the broadest domain, addressing all
it understandable. Nevertheless, models represent a way of aspects of human information interactions with various forms
organizing a body of knowledge to pave the way toward the- of information. At a middle level, a subset is information-
ories. A model can be conceived of as an interim stage in a seeking behavior, which encompasses the range of informa-
research discipline, before a theory can be established, serv- tion seeking employed in discovering and accessing informa-
ing as a “working strategy” for hypothesis testing (J€arvelin & tion resources (humans and systems) in response to goals and
Wilson, 2003). intentions. Finally, at the micro level, information-searching
Models may be textual, mathematical, or graphic con- behavior is a subset of information seeking, referring to the
structs. A model can simply be described in words by pre- actions involved in interacting with an information search
senting a set of theoretical propositions. Mathematical system, including information retrieval (IR).
models can take many forms, for example, differential equa- Given the plethora of frameworks modeling the above
tions. Models can also express the relationships of concepts domains, the present study concentrated on models primarily
in graphic form by means of diagrams or flow charts operating at the middle level, that is, information-seeking

JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017 595
DOI: 10.1002/asi
behavior. Therefore, general models for human information Ellis, Cox, & Hall, 1993; Ellis & Haugan, 1997; Ge, 2010;
behavior (e.g., Wilson, 1997) were excluded from the study. Makri, Blandford, & Cox, 2008a, 2008b; Meho & Tibbo,
Because the boundary between the domains of information 2003; Rhee, 2012; Smith, 1988; Wilson, 1999) were ana-
seeking and information searching has been blurred due to lyzed in detail. The research material was identified by
the increasing popularity of the networked sources, the pres- searching main databases such as EBSCO and LISA. The
ent study looked at models that are hospitable to information keywords used in the search included, for example, Ellis’s
searching. However, models primarily focusing on IR were model, information behavior, human information behavior,
excluded (e.g., Saracevic, 1997). Another major criterion for information seeking, model, and theory. In addition, major
the selection of the model candidates was that they should review articles focusing on information needs, seeking, and
have been elaborated further by the model developer or other use were consulted; these articles were mainly published in
researchers. Many of the candidates, such as Kuhlthau’s the volumes of the Annual Review of Information Science
(1993) ISP model, did not meet this criterion because they and Technology. Moreover, review articles and book chap-
have remained virtually unchanged. Finally, to dig deeper, a ters focusing on this topic were scrutinized (e.g., Case, 2012;
decision was made to concentrate on a single framework Ellis, 2011; Wilson, 1999, 2010). The searches resulted in
meeting best the above criteria, that is, the behavioral model the identification of 124 articles, book chapters, and confer-
proposed by Ellis (1989). The inclusion of other models ence papers reviewing the models of information-seeking
would have provided opportunities for comparison, but this behavior. Of these investigations, 70 items were relevant for
approach would have required a separate article. the analysis of Ellis’s model or its elaborations.
The present study makes an attempt to find out how the The documents chosen for the analysis were scrutinized
elaboration of Ellis’s model exhibits the conceptual growth if by means of conceptual analysis. Following Furner (2004),
it is looked at from the perspective of the basic aims of scien- this method can be defined as an approach that treats the
tific models identified by Bunge (1967, p. 383). However, components of models, for example, users and information
two such aims reviewed above appeared to be unsuitable for sources as classes of objects, events, properties, or relation-
the examination of the elaborations made to Ellis’s model, ships. Conceptual analysis involves defining the meaning of
that is, explaining facts through systems of hypotheses that a given component by identifying and specifying the con-
entail the facts and improving the testability of hypotheses texts in which it is classified under the concept in question.
through the control context provided by systems of hypothe- More specifically, the documents were analyzed by devoting
ses. This is simply a result of the fact that the studies elaborat- attention to how researchers have characterized the compo-
ing Ellis’s framework have not developed hypotheses for nents constitutive of Ellis’s revised or modified model, as
empirical research. Thus, the present study focuses on three well as the relationships between the components.
aims identified by Bunge (1967, p. 383): (a) integrating for- Relevant text portions (paragraphs and sentences) pertain-
merly separate parts of knowledge, (b) generalizing and ing to the research topic were first identified. This material
explaining lower abstraction level knowledge through higher was then read several times to identify individual character-
level constructs, and (c) expanding knowledge by deducing izations of the components and their relationships. More spe-
new propositions based on selected starting points and col- cifically, the texts chosen for analysis were subjected to open
lected information. However, because the models reviewed in coding to identify the categories describing the elaborated
the current study do not deduce “propositions” understood as models. Categories thus identified include, for example,
true statements of the properties of objects, a broader view novel characteristics, redefined characteristics, restructura-
was taken. The expression of “deducing new propositions” tion of components, the way of elaboration, and contribution
was replaced by “identifying new characteristics of the object to conceptual growth. In addition, subcategories were identi-
of study.” Thereby, the above aim was modified as (3) fied, for example, contribution to conceptual growth:
expanding knowledge by identifying new characteristics of expanding knowledge. The contributions to conceptual
the object of study based on selected starting points and col- growth per aims defined by Bunge (1967, p. 383) were iden-
lected information. tified by scrutinizing the diagrams presenting the modified
Drawing on the above specifications, the present study versions of Ellis’s (1981) model as well as their textual
addresses two research questions: explanations. This was achieved by asking (a) what kind of
formerly separate parts of knowledge do the proposed model
• RQ1: In which ways has the model proposed by Ellis been integrate, (b) what kind of higher level constructs are devel-
elaborated since the late 1980s? oped to generalize and explain lower abstraction level
• RQ2: How have the elaborations made to Ellis’s model con- knowledge, and (c) what kind of new characteristics of infor-
tributed to conceptual growth in research on information- mation behavior, seeking, and searching are introduced to
seeking behavior? expand knowledge? However, the study approach was kept
flexible enough to allow for other categories to emerge.
However, no such categories describing aims relevant to
Method
conceptual growth were identified. The conceptual analysis
To answer the above questions, Ellis’s original model and was based on the identification of similarities and differences
its revisions presented in nine key studies (Bronstein, 2007; between various characterizations of the subcategories.

596 JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017
DOI: 10.1002/asi
Finally, the identified subcategories were named according Similar to Wilson, Ellis’s interest in the modeling originated
to the data they contained, for example, “adding novel from the dearth of empirically based models of information-
components.” seeking behavior (Ellis, 2005, p. 138). His research project was
In the analysis, following Torrance (1979), elaboration inspired by the idea that the focus of IR research needs to be
was understood as a process of adding more information to shifted from the IR system to the user and the user’s interaction
existing, relatively simple information to create a more com- with information sources (Ellis, 1984). He intended to derive
plex whole. Thus, elaboration involves developing an idea by an empirically based model depicting information seeking
incorporating details to amplify the original simple idea. It is among academic social scientists. The model could inform the
the process of enhancing ideas and objects by providing design of IR serving this user group but was believed to be of
nuance and more detail. Specifically, elaboration was defined more general interest for information-seeking behavior. This
in the coding as a category that includes activities such as aspiration was realized in his PhD dissertation (Ellis, 1987).
introducing novel components as well as modifying, refining, The empirical data of the PhD study were gathered by inter-
and renaming existing elements of Ellis’s original framework views with 60 academic social scientists who were seeking
or its revised versions. In the coding, a few borderline cases paper-based information in a library setting at the University of
were faced: What kind of modifications can be regarded as Sheffield. The development of the model drew on the assump-
genuine elaborations to Ellis’s original model or its revised tion that underlying the complex patterns of information-
versions? For example, Choo et al. (2000) proposed an inte- seeking behavior there are a relatively small number of
grated model for web searching by cross-tabulating the four different types of activity. Ellis (1989, p. 238) referred to such
modes of environmental scanning identified by Aguilar types by different terms such as characteristics, features, and
(1967), that is, undirected viewing, conditioned viewing, patterns. These factors were understood as generic and higher
informal search, and formal search, with the six characteristics level constructs that are composed of lower-level elements,
of information seeking defined by Ellis (1989). The cross- that is, activities. The characteristics, features, or patterns can
tabulation provides a useful tool for the identification of key interact in various ways in different information-seeking proc-
variables relevant to web searching. However, constructs such esses. Significantly, such constructs do not represent a set of
as these were not regarded as elaborations to Ellis’s model stages or phases that any or all researchers follow when seeking
because the components of his framework were not defined or information. Thus, the model is not meant to indicate sequen-
structured in a new way in the model proposed by Choo et al. ces of events, but instead, the order of actions might vary and
(2000). Other borderline cases were solved in a similar way be iterative as well.
until there were no anomalies. The model was introduced to a wider audience in an arti-
cle published in the Journal of Information Science (Ellis,
Findings 1989). The model identifies six generic characteristics of
The findings are reported in two parts. The ways in which information seeking among social scientists. Although the
Ellis’s model have been elaborated are reviewed first, fol- framework was originally termed “A behavioral model for
lowed by the analysis of the contributions to conceptual information retrieval system,” the characteristics identified
growth. The review mainly proceeds chronologically by Ellis (1989) operate at the levels of information-seeking
because conceptual growth in science is often based on the and information-searching behavior (Jansen & Rieh, 2010,
elaboration of ideas presented in earlier studies. p. 1518). Different from Wilson (1981), Ellis did not depict
his model as a diagram, but described it textually by means
The Point of Departure: Ellis’s Behavioral Model of a series of paragraphs. The six characteristics and their
constitutive components, that is, activities depicted in the
Early models of information seeking date back to the original model (Ellis, 1989) include:
1960s. Paisley (1968) pioneered by proposing a nested con-
ceptual framework depicting factors that affect information • Starting: activities characteristic of the initial search for
seeking among scientists. However, broader interest in the information, for example, consulting a knowledgeable
development of models for information seeking was not colleague;
indicated before the late 1970s. At that time, the information • Chaining: following backward or forward chains of citations
studies field was being drawn toward the social science per- or other form of referential connection between material;
spective that preferred the application of qualitative methods • Browsing: semidirected searching in an area of potential
and exhibited interest in the development of theoretical interest;
models (Ellis, 2011, p. 32). In information behavior • Differentiating: using differences between sources as a filter
research, the novel perspective was exemplified perhaps on the nature and quality of material examined;
• Monitoring: maintaining awareness of developments in a
best by the research project on Information Needs in Social
field through the monitoring of particular sources;
Services (INISS) conducted by Wilson and Streatfield (Wil- • Extracting: systematically working through a particular
son, Streatfield, & Mullings, 1979). The findings of this pro-
source to locate material of interest.
ject were used in Wilson’s (1981) model for information-
seeking behavior, one of pioneering frameworks in this Ellis (2011, p. 30) emphasizes that the above framework
field. exemplifies a concatenated model, the components of which

JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017 597
DOI: 10.1002/asi
could only be put together or articulated in an actual piece cluding the information-seeking process such as building
of information behavior. Thus, the detailed interrelation or final summaries and organizing notes) were included in the
interaction of the features in any individual information pat- extended model (Ellis et al., 1993).
tern depends on the unique circumstances of the information The extended model was tested further among engineers
activities of the person concerned at that particular point in and research scientists in an industrial environment (Ellis &
time. The relationship between the features of the model Haugan, 1997). This study resulted in the identification of
could, therefore, only be indicated in abstract and general two novel features. Distinguishing refers to activities taken
terms unless there is a reference to a particular information when information sources are ranked according to their per-
action. Hence, relationships between features of the model ceived relative importance, whereas filtering characterizes
could only be described hypothetically: for example, starting the use of criteria or mechanisms to make information as rel-
might lead to chaining, and extracting might complement evant and precise as possible. In fact, these features are not
monitoring. However, in terms of any individual information entirely new because they can be interpreted as specialized
pattern or particular sequence of information activities, it forms of differentiating (Makri et al., 2008a, pp. 616–617).
would be possible to state these relationships categorically. Significantly, in both revised versions, the interrelations of
As summarized by Ellis (2011, p. 30), the model presents “a the features were left open and no specific order in which
set of related categories, which taken together, could be the features may appear was indicated. Ellis (1993, p. 483)
used to describe individual information patterns, and help to pointed out that although the features of information seeking
explain details of their topography.” among diverse groups of scientists differ in terminology and
detail, the features can be seen to be representing fundamen-
The Elaboration of Ellis’s Model tally the same activities. Ultimately, such differences are
minor and they stem from subject differences between the
The conceptual analysis indicated that the elaboration of
groups (e.g., English literature researchers vs. chemists).
models for information seeking can be conducted in two
Recently, Ge (2010) made use of Ellis’s (1989) model in
major ways. First, an original model may be complemented
a study examining information-seeking behavior among aca-
by adding novel components such as features of information
demic researchers. The empirical findings confirmed that
seeking. This in an incremental strategy that does not change
the characteristics identified by Ellis continue to play a via-
the fundamental assumptions of the model. Second, and
ble role in research activities. Similar to Ellis and his associ-
more radically, a model can be elaborated by redefining and
ates, Ge (2010, p. 449) adopted an “additive approach” by
restructuring the components of the framework. This
suggesting that the original model can be complemented by
approach can result in a more detailed specification of the
a new feature, that is, preparation and planning. The need
components or change their constellation. However, this
for the novel component was justified by claiming that
strategy does not always manifest itself in a pure form
effective searching requires planning, attention to detail, and
because the redefinition and restructuration of existing com-
successful search strategies particularly because users are
ponents may be complemented by introducing novel compo-
facing an increasing overload of information.
nents. However, for the sake of simplicity, the approach
Overall, adding novel components exemplifies a fairly
based on redefinition and restructuration of components will
straightforward approach to the elaboration of scientific
be discussed as one category that may partially overlap with
models. As suggested by the above studies, this strategy ena-
the strategy of adding novel components.
bles a more detailed and context-sensitive representation of
the target phenomenon. On the other hand, this approach has
Adding Novel Components
its limitations because the addition of still other context-
The model originally presented in Ellis’s (1987) PhD the- specific components may render it more difficult to retain
sis was tested for the first time in Smith’s (1988) study focus- the generic nature of the framework.
ing on information behavior among English literature
researchers (see also Ellis, 1993, p. 483). In addition to the Redefining and Restructuring of Components
starting, chaining, and monitoring identified by Ellis, Smith
Models can also be elaborated by approaching existing
(1988) found three new features: surveying (familiarization
components from a novel perspective or restructuring them
with the literature of the area), selection and sifting (deciding
in a new way. Compared to the “additive approach” dis-
which references to follow up and which to cite), and assem-
cussed above, this strategy has resulted in more sophisticated
bly and dissemination (drawing together material for publica-
elaborations to Ellis’s model.
tion and dissemination). The next elaboration of the original
model took place in an empirical study of information-
From Characteristics to Staged Process (Wilson,
seeking behavior among physicists and chemists (Ellis et al.,
1994, 1999)
1993). This study revealed novel features such as initial
familiarization, chasing, prioritization, maintaining aware- Probably the earliest attempt to approach Ellis’s model
ness, locating, verifying, and ending (Ellis 1993, pp. 482– from an alternative viewpoint was made by Wilson (1994,
483). Of them, however, only two features, that is, verifying pp. 33–34). Inspired by Kuhlthau’s (1993) ISP model, he
(checking the accuracy of the information) and ending (con- suggested that the extended framework proposed by Ellis

598 JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017
DOI: 10.1002/asi
FIG. 1. A stage process version of Ellis’s behavioral model (Wilson, 1999).

FIG. 2. Relating the components of Ellis’s and Kuhlthau’s models (Wilson, 1999).

et al. (1993) could be enhanced by conceptualizing the char- plifies this endeavor by bringing Ellis’s and Kuhlthau’s
acteristics of information seeking as components of a staged (1993) models together.
process. Wilson’s key idea is to approach information seek- Although Ellis’s and Kulthau’s models do not map
ing as a process that proceeds from starting and chaining to directly on to one another, Wilson (1999, p. 256) believed
browsing and differentiating, followed by monitoring, that there are important similarities between the frameworks.
extracing, verifying, and ending. Later on, Wilson (1999, p. Figure 2 suggests that, in effect, Ellis’s stages are an elabora-
254) elaborated the above idea further by providing a dia- tion of the activities described by Kuhlthau. The activities
gram that illustrates a new viewpoint to Ellis’s extended related to recognizing of information sources are closely
model (Figure 1). associated with Ellis’s starting phase, whereas identifying
Figure 1 suggests that starting is a logical point of depar- and formulating the focus of searching are associated with
ture of the information-seeking process and that ending is its Ellis’s chaining, browsing, differentiating, and monitoring.
logical final point. According to Wilson (1999, p. 254), it is Gathering information is closely related to extracting and
equally logical to assume that extracting follows from a spe- verifying, while search closure is equivalent to verifying and
cific search behavior such as browsing. From the temporal ending in Ellis’s extended model. Interestingly, this idea is
perspective, extracting would not be an information behav- contrary to Ellis’s (1989, p. 238) proposition that character-
ior of the same kind as browsing, or chaining or monitoring istics such as browsing, monitoring, and extracting do not
because the latter can be perceived as antecedents of extract- represent a set of stages or phases that any or all researchers
ing behavior. Furthermore, extracting is more closely related follow when seeking information. Thus, Ellis’s model is not
to information use than information searching. In a similar meant to indicate sequences of events, but instead, the order
vein, Wilson (1999, p. 254) suggests that differentiating is a of actions might vary and be iterative as well. However,
different kind of behavior: browsing, chaining, and monitor- Wilson (1999, p. 256) did not regard this as a barrier to the
ing are search procedures, whereas differentiating is a filter- relating of the above models: despite different terminology,
ing process. The remaining behaviors do not necessarily there is a question of an association in that Ellis’s character-
take place in a specific sequence and may be initiated in dif- istics can be depicted similar to Kuhlthau’s stage categories.
ferent sequences at different times in the overall search
process. Sophisticating the Stage Process Version (Meho
To elaborate the above framework further, Wilson (1999, & Tibbo, 2003)
p. 256) returned to Kuhlthau’s (1993) idea about the stages
of information search. Wilson suggested that despite differ- In retrospect, Wilson’s idea of integrating Ellis’s and
ent terminology, diverse models of information seeking Kuhlthau’s models has largely been forgotten in information
share some fundamental elements. For example, Kuhlthau’s behavior research. However, the stage process version
categories exploration and collection can be associated with depicted in Figure 1 has remarkably influenced the elabora-
Ellis’ chaining, browsing, and monitoring, while presenta- tion of Ellis’s model. The idea of the stage process occupies
tion is equivalent to verifying and ending in Ellis’s extended a significant role in the revised model proposed by Meho
model. Wilson believed that the relating of core components and Tibbo (2003). To justify their approach, Meho and
such as these would pave the way toward the development Tibbo (2003, p. 570) drew attention to the fact that Ellis’s
of a general model of information seeking. Figure 2 exem- (1987) PhD study on social scientists was conducted prior to

JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017 599
DOI: 10.1002/asi
FIG. 3. Stages in the information-seeking behavior of academic social scientists (Meho & Tibbo, 2003).

the development of the World Wide Web. Thus, it was cycle of a project. Therefore, this stage has a double mean-
important to take into account new characteristics that are ing: on the one hand, it indicates the final point of research
being brought about by changes in information technology. activity—a main context of information seeking. On the
Based on the findings of an empirical investigation of other hand, it refers to the closure of information seeking.
information-seeking behavior among social scientists study- Interestingly, Meho and Tibbo conceptualized ending as a
ing stateless nations, Meho and Tibbo (2003, p. 584) con- stage, whereas Ellis and associates (1993) termed it a fea-
firmed the validity of the Ellis’s original model. They also ture. Significantly, however, Meho and Tibbo (2003, p.
agreed with Ellis in that the features of information seeking 584) showed that in each of the first three stages, that is,
do not necessarily appear sequentially. They argued, how- searching, accessing, and processing, a number of features
ever, that a fuller description of the information-seeking pro- (or activities) can take place. For example, during the
cess of social scientists should include additional features searching stage, researchers may use starting, chaining,
besides those identified by Ellis. The new features are ana- browsing, monitoring, differentiating, extracting, and net-
lyzing, synthesizing, writing, networking, and information working activities, whereas during the processing stage,
managing. However, except for networking, these categories researchers might use chaining, extracting, differentiating,
are less relevant from the perspective of information seeking verifying, and information-managing activities.
because they primarily deal with information processing and In contrast to Wilson’s (1999) semilinear process stage
use, as well as personal information management. version, Meho and Tibbo’s model is more dynamic and flex-
The most radical change to the original model derives ible because it allows multiple options for the information
from the incorporation of four stages of information seeking: seeker. The flowchart depicted in Figure 3 shows that once
searching, accessing, processing, and ending (Meho & the research cycle starts at the searching stage, a researcher
Tibbo, 2003, p. 583). Different from Wilson’s (1999) semi- may continue to the accessing stage or to the processing
linear framework depicted in Figure 1, Meho and Tibbo stage, or to both, depending on the initial types of informa-
adopted a more flexible approach. In the revised model, the tion used: direct or full-text sources (e.g., an electronic arti-
stages are also named differently, and they are presented as cle immediately accessible on computer screen) and indirect
components of a cycle. The characteristics identified by or non-full-text sources (e.g., a foreign colleague). In the
Ellis’s extended model are reconceptualized as processual case of direct sources, a researcher may skip the accessing
elements characteristic of one or fewer stages, as depicted in stage and go directly to the processing stage. However, if
Figure 3. indirect sources are used, the accessing stage becomes nec-
The revised model can be interpreted by starting from essary because without having access to information sources
the component of searching (Meho & TIbbo, 2003, pp. identified in the searching stage, researchers may not be able
585–585). This stage stands for the period where identify- to proceed to the processing stage. In cases in which both
ing relevant and potentially relevant materials is initiated. direct and indirect sources are used in the searching stage, a
The accessing stage can be defined as the bridge between researcher may proceed to both accessing and processing
the searching stage and the processing stage. The process- stages. Thus, researchers may move back and forth between
ing stage is where the synthesizing and analyzing of the the searching, accessing, and/or the processing stages until
information gathered takes place. Thus, different from the the project is completed.
above stages, it primarily deals with information use. Compared to Wilson’s (1999) process version, Meho and
Finally, the ending stage marks the end of the research Tibbo’s model provides a broader repertoire of components

600 JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017
DOI: 10.1002/asi
FIG. 4. Phase-dependent elements of information-seeking behavior (Bronstein, 2007).

that are contextually sensitive. The inclusion of three new “elements”: starter references and citation tracking. Her
features is an incremental contribution that updated the findings support Meho and Tibbo’s (2003) conclusion about
framework so that it corresponds better to the modes of the strong relationship between the features of information
information seeking from the networked sources. The incor- seeking and the stage of the research or purpose of the
poration of the stages is a more fundamental contribution search. However, in Bronstein’s (2007) model, the role of
that brought the temporal dimension into the model and the research phases is emphasized even more strongly. Her
made it more dynamic. The combination of features and model identifies three time frames or phases of research
stages resulted in a flexible framework that is hospitable to activity:
the cyclic nature of information seeking. As Ellis (1989)
departed from the assumption that the features of informa- 1. initial phase: searching for information on a new subject
tion seeking are not sequential, the idea of cyclic stages or searching for information at the beginning of the
proposed by Meho and Tibbo (2003) exemplifies a compro- research process;
mised view between the extremes of randomness and linear- 2. current awareness phase: searching for information in
ity of information seeking. order to keep up-to-date while doing the research;
3. final phase: actions taken to end the research process.
Elaborating the Temporal Components (Bronstein, 2007)
Different from the diagram developed by Meho and
Bronstein (2007) modified Ellis’s extended model in an
empirical investigation focusing on information-seeking Tibbo, Bronstein’s model consists of two modules. Another
behavior among Jewish studies scholars. Bronstein employed difference is that along with the specification of modules,
different terminology because she preferred the concepts of Bronstein’s framework is less dynamic because there are no
information activity and information strategy over feature, indications of cyclic feedback between the modules. Thereby,
characteristic, or pattern. Even though the above terms are her model bears more resemblance to Wilson’s (1999) stage
used synonymously, they can be interpreted differently process version. The first module identifies the phase-
because Ellis understood activities as constituents of features dependent elements and their relationships (Figure 4).
and did not approach features from the viewpoint of informa- The revised model emphasizes the temporally sensitive
tion strategies. Similar to Meho and Tibbo’s (2003) study, the elements of information-seeking behavior. At the initial
variations in terminology reflect the different views on the phase of the research activity, a starter reference such as an
same phenomenon. On the other hand, such variations may updated article functions as a useful a starting point for cita-
render it more difficult to identify the conceptual growth tion tracking, similar to Ellis’s category of chaining. Brows-
brought by diverse elaborations to the original model. ing and extracting are understood similar to Ellis’s original
The major elaboration made by Bronstein (2007) is based model. As a whole, the characterization of the current
on the positioning of information activities (or Ellis’s fea- awareness phase does not add much to Ellis’s original
tures) within a time frame and relating information activities model with regard to monitoring, or to Meho and Tibbo’s
to a specific type of research activity. This modification was way to define the feature of networking. However, Bronstein
justified by the fact that although all the features identified has refined the picture by specifying the types of informa-
by Ellis were present in the participants’ information behav- tion sources being monitored: electronic materials and
ior, they were not used randomly, as Ellis suggested. Bron- printed materials. At the final phase of research activity,
stein also identified two new information activities or similar to Ellis’s extended model, ending refers to the

JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017 601
DOI: 10.1002/asi
closure of information seeking at the end of a project, for relationships between features (or information activities)
example, during preparation of papers for publication. and stages of research were refined. Seen along a timeline,
The second module depicted in Figure 5 refers to those some information activities, for example, citation tracking,
elements of the information behavior that are not dependent may be used in both the initial phase and the current aware-
on a specific research phase. ness phase, but they are used differently in each phase. Sec-
Again, the three components of information seeking, that ond, Bronstein showed that the nature of the information
is, verification, differentiating, and managing information activities can change according to the purpose of the search
are understood similar to Meho and Tibbo (2003). The main even when talking about the same activity, for example,
contribution lies in the finding that these components are browsing. When researchers browse through back issues of
unrelated to a particular research phase. For example, differ- a journal (extracting), or browse widely looking for a book
entiating can take place at any stage of the research process on a relevant shelf, the aim is to form an opinion on a sub-
when the researcher differentiates between sources of infor- ject with which they are probably not familiar. However,
mation based on the quality of the materials. As described when their goal is to keep up-to-date with the literature pub-
by Bronstein (2007), this information activity is based on lished in their field (monitoring), they will browse only
prior knowledge and past experiences that guide the user through those journals that they know will most benefit their
when choosing one particular information source over others research.
available to them.
In sum, the model proposed by Bronstein (2007) pro- Refining the Stage-Based Approach (Rhee, 2012)
vided two main elaborations to Ellis’s framework. First, the
Recently, Rhee (2012) modeled historians’ information-
seeking behavior. Different from Bronstein (2007), she
selected Meho and Tibbo’s (2003) model as a point of
departure. Rhee believed their model allows the identifica-
tion of unique information-seeking activities of historians by
comparing their behavior with that of social scientists. The
empirical findings provided the basis for the development of
the model depicted in Figure 6.
Following Meho and Tibbo’s (2003) cyclic approach,
Rhee’s (2012) model incorporates four interrelated stages of
information-seeking behavior during a research project. As a
FIG. 5. Phase-independent elements of information-seeking behavior new contribution, Rhee (2012) identified three features charac-
(Bronstein, 2007). teristic of historians’ information-seeking behavior: orienting,

FIG. 6. Stages and features in historians’ information-seeking behavior (Rhee, 2012).

602 JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017
DOI: 10.1002/asi
constructing contextual knowledge, and assessing. Further- their approach comes closer to Ellis’s (1989, 2011, p. 30)
more, different from Meho and Tibbo’s approach, the features idea that the relationship between the features of the model
of analyzing, synthesizing, and writing were considered to be can only be indicated in abstract and general terms unless
information-using activities rather than information-seeking there is a reference to a particular information action. The
activities. model proposed by Makri and associates also comes close to
The main elaboration to Meho and Tibbo’s model is Ellis’s thinking in that the elaborations made to Ellis’s
based on a different conceptualization of the role of individ- model are reported textually; no diagrams are presented.
ual features within diverse stages of the information-seeking The elaborations bear resemblance with a taxonomic
process. Rhee found that during the searching stage— approach because the concepts and their relationships are
different from social scientists—historians may also conduct defined in a detailed way.
orienting, verifying, information managing, constructing The main elaboration of the Ellis (1989) model is framed
contextual knowledge, and assessing activities. Furthermore, around the levels at which Ellis’s behaviors operate (Makri
during the accessing stage, accessing takes a double role et al., 2008a, p. 620). These levels include the resource level
because it is also characterized as a feature. This is because (i.e., the level of the electronic resource itself), the source
historians’ preference for direct sources, particularly original level (i.e., the level of an information source or sources
sources, suggests that the accessing may be more critical and within a particular electronic resource), the document level
difficult for historians than for other researchers. At the proc- (i.e., the level of a document or documents within a particu-
essing stage—different from social scientists—historians my lar information source), and the content level (i.e., the level
conduct constructing contextual knowledge activities and of content within a particular document) and the search
assessing activities. Finally, at the ending stage, analyzing, query/result level (Makri et al., 2008a, pp. 620–621). In addi-
synthesizing, and writing activities are considered to be tion, the authors made a distinction between higher and
information-using activities, rather than information-seeking lower level behaviors. To this end, two higher level behav-
activities because those activities use information that was iors were specified: (a) identifying and locating and
already found and collected (Rhee, 2012). Historians write (b) accessing and selecting and processing, as well as related
up results of their research projects using information they lower level behaviors for each level. For example, at the
collected in their information-seeking process. Hence, Figure level of identifying and locating, there may appear lower-
6 includes synthesizing, analyzing, and writing in the ending level behaviors such as surveying, monitoring, searching,
stage, whereas Meho and Tibbo’s (2003) model includes browsing, chaining, and extracting, whereas lower level
them in the processing stage. Unlike social scientists, histori- behaviors characteristic of accessing and selecting and proc-
ans seem to go through the same process until a project is essing include, for example, filtering, updating, and record-
either completed or abandoned. ing. The study validated and refined Ellis’s model based on
In sum, Rhee’s (2012) model provides an incremental the identification of the higher and lower level behaviors and
contribution to the context-specific description of the fea- specifying the mode of both type behaviors with regard to
tures of information seeking. The most significant elabora- electronic resources. For example, monitoring—a lower
tion is based on the reinterpretation of the nature of level behavior—can appear in two modes: active or passive.
individual features within the four stages. As a whole, how- Furthermore, monitoring can take place at the source level as
ever, these elaborations are minor modifications of the well as at a combined document/content level. As another
framework developed by Meho and Tibbo (2003). This sug- example, recording can be manual or automatic in nature,
gests that a more radical elaboration of Ellis’s original and this lower level behavior can occur at resource, source,
model would require a new “quantum leap” that exceeds the combined document/content, and search query/result levels.
idea of information seeking as a staged cyclic process. Makri et al. (2008a, pp. 619–641; 2000b, pp. 2247–2248)
identified a host of new features characteristic of informa-
Refining the Conceptual Distinctions tion behavior among lawyers. However, all of them are not
directly related to information seeking or searching because
So far, the most systematic conceptual elaboration of the they primarily deal with information use or personal infor-
Ellis (1989) model has been conducted by Makri et al. mation management (PIM). Of PIM-related features, for
(2008a, 2008b). Drawing on the findings of Makri’s (2008) example, recording comes close to the category of informa-
doctoral dissertation, they illustrated the potential of Ellis’s tion managing identified by Meho and Tibbo (2003). Fea-
model by using it as a lens to analyze and make design sug- tures of information use entail analyzing. This behavior was
gestions based on the information-seeking behavior of aca- identified but not elaborated in greater detail by Meho and
demic lawyers. The focus of the study was strengthened by Tibbo (2003); however, it was included in Rhee’s (2012)
concentrating on lawyers’ ways to seek information from model discussed above.
electronic sources. Makri and associates preferred the term From the perspective of the present study, however, novel
Ellis behaviors over characteristics or features. Despite this features of information seeking and searching are more per-
terminological preference, the revised model proposed by tinent. Of them, searching involves formulating a query to
Makri and associates does not draw on the idea of locate information (Makri et al., 2008a, p. 622). Searching
information-seeking behavior as a staged process. Instead, behavior was not discussed by Ellis and his colleagues,

JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017 603
DOI: 10.1002/asi
perhaps due to the paper-based nature of information seek- health sociology. Overall, the contributions to conceptual
ing in these studies. This behavior was, however, character- growth based on integrating formerly separate parts of
ized by Meho and Tibbo (2003) and Rhee (2012), although knowledge have remained quite modest in the context of
they conceptualized it as a stage of information seeking, not Ellis’s model.
a feature or activity. According to Makri et al. (2008a, p. Generalizing and explaining lower abstraction-level
622), searching involves formulating a query to locate infor- knowledge through higher level constructs.
mation within a particular meta-resource such as catalogs. There are a number of elaborations serving this aim. The
Selecting involves carefully choosing resources, sources, or extended versions of the original model (Ellis & Haugan,
documents as being potentially useful for the information 1997; Ellis et al., 1993) identified novel highe level con-
task at hand. As described by Makri et al. (2008b, p. 625), structs such as distinguishing that subsumed lower level con-
selecting shares a conceptual similarity to distinguishing, fil- stituents referred to as activities. According to Ingwersen
tering, and extracting behaviors but is different from distin- and J€arvelin (2005, p. 64), one of the strengths of Ellis’s
guishing because it does not involve ranking resources extended model is that it enables the description of many
based on perceived importance. Selecting is also different information-seeking activities because the characteristics
from filtering because, although various criteria are used identified by Ellis are general enough to fit a large number
when selecting which resource to use, these criteria are not of empirical situations. However, if one is to explain
used as precise, explicit filters to help decide between which information-seeking behavior, for example, in terms of the
document or source to select. Finally, updating is a feature work tasks the subjects are engaged with, the Ellis’s features
ensuring a current understanding of amendments or changes fall short because they are not explicitly related to such
to a particular legal document (Makri et al., 2008a, pp. 628– external possible causative factors. Thus, it is not possible to
629). predict the order in which the categories appear in an
individual-seeking process. J€arvelin and Wilson (2003)
Contributions to Conceptual Growth believed, however, that Ellis’s model may still be of indirect
help in finding explanations for information-seeking behav-
The findings concerning the conceptual growth brought ior. It would mainly be possible to discern differences in any
by the elaborations made to Ellis’s model are discussed with of the features in different situations. For example, some
regard to three basic aims of scientific theories and models persons in some roles may be shown to engage more or less
identified by Bunge (1967, p. 383). Such aims help to evalu- in monitoring than other persons. This may then lead to an
ate the extent to which the elaborations made to scientific examination of the factors that give rise to these differences.
theories and models have advanced knowledge in specific The framework developed by Makri and his associates
fields of research. (2008a, 2008b) serves particularly well the aim of generaliz-
Integrating formerly separate parts of knowledge. ing and explaining lower abstraction level knowledge
From the perspective of this aim, the most significant con- through higher level constructs. Their model identifies
tribution to conceptual growth originates from studies that diverse levels at which the Ellis features operate. Makri and
have brought together or incorporated components captured associates developed a hierarchical setting which makes
from diverse models. Integrating formerly separate parts of lower level constructs such as browsing, chaining and
knowledge has taken two major forms. First, such components searching more understandable by locating them into a
are related or loosely associated within a model, as exempli- higher level category termed identifying and locating. Simi-
fied by Wilson’s (1999) model paralleling Kuhlthaus’s (1993) larly, the models developed by Meho and Tibbo (2003),
categories and Ellis’s characteristics depicted in Figure 2. Sec- Bronstein (2007), and Rhee (2012) contributed to conceptual
ond, integration means that components captured from diverse growth by placing lower level constructs such as features or
frameworks are incorporated as constituents of the refined activities in a meaningful context of stages of information
model. For example, Meho and Tibbo (2003) incorporated the seeking or phases of research activities. By the above crite-
constituent of information managing to the stage of processing rion, elaborations of this type have provided a strong contri-
information, whereas Rhee (2012) embedded the component bution to the conceptual growth.
of constructing contextual knowledge to the searching stage. Expanding knowledge by identifying new characteristics
These contributions have extended the scope of Ellis’s model of the object of study based on selected starting points and
toward a framework of information behavior, thus exceeding collected information.
the limits of modeling information seeking or information Drawing on empirical research, the above studies have
searching alone. served this end by identifying novel components such as fil-
Interestingly, the elaborations to Ellis’s model exhibit tering (Ellis & Haugan, 1997), information managing (Meho
less interest in the utilization of ideas produced outside the & Tibbo, 2003), and preparation and planning (Ge, 2010).
field of information behavior research. For example, the The elaborations have also specified the relationships
development of Wilson’s (1997) general model of informa- between individual components in diverse contexts such as
tion seeking provides a more advanced example because academic research and engineering. These contributions are
Wilson incorporated elements captured from diverse areas based on the additive approach (e.g., Ellis et al., 1993; Ge,
such as consumer studies, communication research, and 2010), as well as redefining and restructuring the components

604 JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017
DOI: 10.1002/asi
TABLE 1. Summary of the research findings.

Main approach used in Contribution to concep-


Author(s) Context of the study the elaboration tual growth Main elaboration to Ellis’s (1981) model

Smith (1988) Academic research Adding novel Expanding knowledge Identifying new characteristics of
(English literature) components information seeking, for example,
surveying and sifting
Ellis, Cox, and Hall Academic research Adding novel Expanding knowledge Identifying new characteristics, for
(1993) (physics, chemistry) components example, initial familiarization,
verifying, and ending
Ellis and Haugan Engineering, science in Adding novel Expanding knowledge Identifying new characteristics:
(1997) industry components distinguishing and filtering
Wilson (1999) Literature review Redefining and Integrating knowledge Relating Ellis’s characteristics and
restructuring Kuhlthaus’s (1993) categories
components
Meho and Tibbo Academic research Redefining and Integrating knowledge Identifying and incorporating novel
(2003) (social science) restructuring Expanding knowledge components such as information
components Generalizing and managing and writing to the revised
explaining model
Approaching features of information
seeking in the context of four stages:
searching, accessing, processing, and
ending
Bronstein (2007) Academic research Redefining and Integrating knowledge Identifying and incorporating novel
(humanities) restructuring Expanding knowledge components such as
components Generalizing and starter references and citation tracking
explaining to the revised model
Approaching features of information
seeking in the context of three phases
of research process
Makri et al. (2008a, Work context Redefining and Integrating knowledge Identifying and incorporating novel
2008b) (academic lawyers) restructuring Expanding knowledge components such as searching,
components Generalizing and recording and updating to the revised
explaining model
Specifying higher and lower level
behaviors and their constituents
Ge (2010) Academic research Adding novel Expanding knowledge Identifying a new characteristic:
(social science) components preparation and planning
Rhee (2012) Academic research Redefining and Integrating knowledge Identifying and incorporating novel
(humanities) restructuring Expanding knowledge components such as constructing
components Generalizing and contextual knowledge to the revised model
explaining Reinterpreting the role of characteristics in
the context of four stages identified by
Meho and Tibbo (2003)

of models (e.g., Bronstein, 2007; Makri et al., 2008a; Meho The first research question dealt with the ways in which
& Tibbo, 2003; Rhee, 2012). However, instead of deriving Ellis’s model has been elaborated since the late 1980s. The
operationalizable propositions for the testing of hypotheses, findings indicate that the elaborations are based in two major
the contributions to conceptual growth are mainly descriptive approaches. First, the original model has been enhanced by
in nature, that is, characterizations of the attributes of novel adding novel components. This approach has resulted in the
components or their relationships. By this criterion, the elabo- identification of contextually sensitive characteristics of
rations provide a useful contribution to the conceptual growth information seeking with regard to diverse groups of users,
and lay a basis for the development of testable hypotheses. for example, chemists, historians, social scientists, and aca-
demic lawyers. Because new contexts or user groups exhibit
novel characteristics of information seeking, the additive
Discussion approach may lead to an endless list of features. The picture
of the context-specific information seeking can be refined,
Focusing on the elaborations made to Ellis’s seminal but due to the increased diversity, the identification of the
model, the present study examined how such refinements core features of this activity will be rendered more difficult.
contribute to conceptual growth in research on information- So far, due to the relatively low number of context-specific
seeking behavior. The research findings are summarized in characteristics of information seeking suggested by the elab-
Table 1. orations, the forest can still be seen for the trees. This issue

JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017 605
DOI: 10.1002/asi
deals with one of the fundamental problems of modeling, research. Naturally, this limits the scope of the refined
that is, the trade-off between simplification and detailed rep- frameworks. Of them, those proposed by Smith (1988),
resentation of a target phenomenon. Bronstein (2007), Makri et al. (2008a, 2008b), Ge (2010),
Second, Ellis’s model has been elaborated by redefining and Rhee (2012) are most strongly dependent on a specific
and restructuring of the components of the model. In this context of information seeking. The versions proposed by
regard, the most radical change originates from the introduc- Ellis and associates operate at a higher level of generality
tion of the idea of stage process approach inspired by and they may be applied more broadly to the study of work-
Kuhlthau’s ISP model (Wilson, 1999). This idea was refined related information seeking. However, as Makri et al.
in diverse ways in empirical studies conducted by Meho and (2008a, p. 620) aptly point out, the elaboration of Ellis’s
Tibbo (2003), Bronstein (2007), and Rhee (2012). The elab- model should not be regarded as a “complete” or “correct”
orations have resulted in the identification of three or four set of behaviors that are intended to generalize to a wide
major stages of information seeking. Diverse features have range of contexts, but as a way of conceptualizing informa-
been incorporated in such components, depending on the tar- tion behaviors in terms of broader parent and lower level
get groups such as social scientists and historians. The elab- behaviors, subtypes of behaviors, and different levels of
orations of this type have also led to the specification of the behaviors. All in all, the elaborations made to Ellis’s (1989)
attributes of the features, as exemplified by the study of model have gradually expanded its scope toward a frame-
Makri et al. (2008a, 2008b). Overall, the main elaborations work of information behavior by erasing the boundary lines
to Ellis’s are based on the redefining and restructuring of between information seeking, searching, and use as well as
components of his model. The findings also suggest that this personal information management.
approach has resulted in the most fundamental changes in Interestingly, the revised versions have not replaced
the original model. Ellis’s original model or the enhanced frameworks devel-
The second research question dealt with the conceptual oped by Ellis and associates. These frameworks are still
growth brought by the elaborations. As a whole, the modifi- widely cited as classic models for information seeking. As
cations and revisions have resulted in the conceptual growth far as I know, Ellis has not commented on the elaborations
in information-seeking behavior research. Table 1 indicates made to his model. For example, while summarizing the
that the “additive approach” to model elaboration has mainly ways in which the behavioral model has been reviewed since
contributed to expanding knowledge by means of identifying the 1990s, Ellis (2005) made no reference to Meho and Tib-
new characteristics of information-seeking behavior. Such bo’s (2003) study, for example. Similarly, in a recent review
characteristics include, for example, distinguishing and fil- of the emergence of conceptual modeling in information
tering (Ellis & Haugan, 1996) and preparation and planning behavior research, the revised versions were not commented
(Ge, 2010). on by Ellis (2011).
The approach based on the redefining and restructuring The evaluation of the conceptual growth brought by the
the components of the model is more sophisticated. On the elaborations to Ellis’s model is rendered difficult due to the
one hand, this approach has contributed to integrating for- fact that other frameworks of information seeking are not
merly separate parts of knowledge. For example, Wilson directly comparable due to their differences in theoretical
(1999) related Ellis’s characteristics and Kuhlthau’s (1993) background and terminology. For example, by certain crite-
categories, whereas Meho and Tibbo (2003) incorporated ria, the models of task-based information seeking are more
components such as information managing and writing to advanced than Ellis’s extended framework, or its revised
the processes of information seeking. On the other hand, the versions. This is particularly evident with regard to the aim
approach based on the redefining and restructuring the com- of explaining facts through systems of hypotheses, which
ponents of the model has contributed to the generalizing and entail the facts (Bunge, 1967, p. 383). By this criterion, the
explaining of lower level knowledge through higher level elaborations to Ellis’s model are wanting when compared
constructs. As a whole, the approach bases of redefining and with the model of task-based information seeking developed
restructuring components has resulted in the most funda- by Bystr€om and J€arvelin (1995), for example. Vakkari
mental elaborations to Ellis’s original model. Meho and (1998) showed that Bystr€om and J€arvelins (1995) model
Tibbo showed that Ellis’s characteristics can be approached meant theory growth in terms of theory precision because
as constituents of a staged and cyclic information-seeking the model explicates dependencies (causality) between task
process, whereas Makri and associates elaborated Ellis’s complexity, information needed, and information sources.
model by specifying the relationships between higher and The strength of their model lies in that it explicitly generates
lower level behaviors and their constituents. For example, hypotheses through systematically categorizing the central
Makri et al. (2008a, 2008b) made individual behaviors such concepts.
as browsing, chaining, and searching more understandable The findings of the present study indicate that the elabo-
by locating them into a higher level category termed identi- rations to Ellis’s model have not progressed to the formulat-
fying and locating. ing and testing of hypotheses. This suggests, more generally,
Table 1 indicates that, except for the studies of Wilson that if a long-time elaboration of a model does not lead to
(1999) and Makri et al. (2008a, 2000b), the elaborations to success in the formulation and testing of hypotheses, the
Ellis’s model have been made in the context of academic refinement of the model may be stopped at a certain level,

606 JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017
DOI: 10.1002/asi
preventing the developers of the model from taking the next References
step, which would turn the model into an empirically vali- Aguilar, F.J. (1967). Scanning the business environment. New York:
dated theory. This task is even more demanding if there are Macmillan.
diverse versions of the same model, as exemplified by sev- Bates, M.J (1989). The design of browsing and berrypicking techniques
eral elaborations made to Ellis’s framework. An opposite for the online search interface. Online Review, 13(5), 407–424.
Bates, M.J. (2005). An introduction to metatheories, theories and mod-
example is provided by the systematic attempts to test
els. In K.E. Fisher, S. Erdelez, & L. McKechnie (Eds.), Theories of
Kuhlthau’s (1993) ISP model. Beheshti et al. (2015) demon- information behavior (pp. 1–24). Medford, NJ: Information Today.
strate in this context that on the way from a model to a Beheshti, J., Cole, C., Abuhimed, D., & Lamourex, I. (2015). Tracking
theory it is vitally important to strengthen the empirical middle school students information behavior via Kuhlthau’s ISP model:
validity of the existing framework by carefully testing it in Temporality. Journal of the Association for Information Science and
Technology, 66(5), 943–960.
diverse contexts, instead of just incorporating novel compo- Belkin, N.J., Oddy, R.N., & Brooks, H.M. (1982). ASK for information
nents or changing their constellation. Similarly, it is obvious retrieval. Part I: Background and theory. Journal of Documentation, 38(2),
that the further elaboration of Ellis’s model would require a 61–71.
more focused approach with the intent of operationalizing Bronstein, J. (2007). The role of the research phase in information seek-
ing behavior of Jewish studies scholars: A modification of Ellis’s
key concepts to develop and test specific hypotheses. The
behavioral characteristics. Information Research, 12(3). Retrieved
“feature-based” approach preferred by Makri et al. (2008a, from http://www.informationr.net/ir/12-3/paper318.html.
2008b) holds good promise in this regard because the key Bunge, M.A. (1967). Scientific research. Vol. 1. The search for system.
concepts are defined in detail and they can be operational- Berlin: Springer.
ized for the needs of empirical research. Similarly, the Bystr€om, K., & J€arvelin, K. (1995). Task complexity affects information
seeking and use. Information Processing & Management, 31(2), 191–
“stage-based” approach developed by Meho and Tibbo 213.
(2003) and Rhee (2012) is hospitable to the development Case, D.O. (2012). Looking for information. A survey of research on infor-
and testing of hypotheses. mation seeking, needs and behavior (3rd ed.). Bingley, UK: Emerald.
Choo, C.W., Detlor, B., & Turnbull, D. (2000). Information seeking on
the Web: An integrated model of browsing and searching. First Mon-
day, 5(2). Retrieved from http://www.firstmonday.dk/issues/issue5_2/
Conclusion choo/index.html#author.
Dervin, B. (2003). Human studies and user studies: A call for methodo-
Ellis’s (1989) seminal model provided a fertile ground logical inter-disciplinarity. Information Research, 9(1). Retrieved from
for the study because it has been a subject to numerous mod- http://InformationR.net/ir/9-1/paper166.html.
ifications. The findings suggest more generally that the elab- Ellis, D. (1984). Theory and explanation in information retrieval
oration of existing models enables gradual cumulation of research. Journal of Information Science, 8(1), 25–38.
Ellis, D. (1987). The derivation of a behavioural model for information
knowledge, thus helping researchers to fight the problems retrieval systems design (Unpublished PhD thesis). University of
caused by the proliferation of frameworks. The present study Sheffield, Sheffield, UK
provided two major contributions. First, the findings shed Ellis, D. (1989). A behavioural model for information retrieval system.
light on the ways in which scientific models are elaborated. Journal of Information Science, 15(4–5), 237–247.
Ellis, D. (1993). Modeling the information-seeking patterns of academic
The simplest way is to supplement existing models by add-
researchers: A grounded theory approach. Library Quarterly, 63(4),
ing novel components. A more sophisticated way is to refine 469–486.
and restructure the components of the framework. Second, Ellis, D. (2005). Ellis’s model of information behavior. In K.E. Fisher,
the study showed how the elaborations to scientific models S. Erdelez, & L. McKechnie (Eds.), Theories of information behavior
contribute to conceptual growth. It may be based on integrat- (pp. 138–142). Medford, NJ: Information Today.
Ellis, D. (2011). The emergence of conceptual modelling in information
ing formerly separate parts of knowledge, generalizing and behavior research. In A. Spink & J. Heinstr€ om (Eds.), New directions
explaining observations of the object of inquiry, and expand- in information behavior (pp. 17–35). Bingley, UK: Emerald.
ing knowledge by identifying new characteristics of the Ellis, D., Cox, D., & Hall, K. (1993). A comparison of the information
object of study. seeking patterns of researchers in the physical and social sciences.
As an in-depth analysis of the elaborations made to a Journal of Documentation, 49(4), 356–369.
Ellis, D., & Haugan, M. (1997). Modeling the information seeking pat-
seminal model, the findings complement the general level terns of engineers and research scientists in an industrial environment.
reviews of models for information-seeking behavior (e.g., Journal of Documentation, 53(4), 384–403.
Case, 2012, pp. 133–162; Wilson, 2010). The findings also Foster, A. (2004). A nonlinear model of information-seeking behavior.
suggest that the basic aims of scientific theories identified Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Tech-
nology, 55(3), 228–237.
by Bunge (1967, p. 383) provide a useful point of departure
Foster, A., & Urquhart, C. (2012). Modelling nonlinear information
for the analysis of conceptual growth of models. The main behavior: Transferability and progression. Journal of Documentation,
limitation of the present study is that it examines one model 68(6), 784–805.
only. Thus, the findings cannot be generalized to apply to all Frigg, R., & Hartmann, S. (2012). Models in science. In Stanford Ency-
models of information behavior. However, it is believed that clopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/
entries/models-science/.
the research results provide relevant material for compara- Furner, J. (2004). Conceptual analysis: A method for understanding
tive studies, thus serving the ends of “digging deeper” in information as evidence, and evidence as information. Archival Sci-
information-behavior research. ence, 4(3–4), 233–265.

JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017 607
DOI: 10.1002/asi
Ge, X. (2010). Information-seeking behavior in the digital age: A multi- Paisley, W.J. (1968). Information needs and uses. In C.A. Cuadra (Ed.),
disciplinary study of academic researchers. College & Research Libra- Annual review of information science and technology (pp. 1–30). Chi-
ries, 71(5), 435–445. cago: Encyclopedia Britannica.
Ingwersen, P., & J€arvelin, K. (2005). The turn: Integration of information Pluta, W., Chinn, C.A., & Duncan, R.G. (2011). Learners epistemic criteria for
seeking and retrieval in context. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer. good scientific models. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48(5), 486–
Jansen, B.J., & Rieh, S.Y. (2010). The seventeen theoretical constructs 511.
of information searching and information retrieval. Journal of the Rhee, H.L. (2012). Modelling historians’ information-seeking behavior with
American Society for Information Science and Technology, 61(8), an interdisciplinary and comparative approach. Information Research,
1517–1534. 17(4). Retrieved from http://InformationR.net/ir/17-4/paper544.html.
J€arvelin, K., & Wilson, T.D. (2003). On conceptual models for informa- Robson, A., & Robinson, L. (2013). Building of models of information
tion seeking and retrieval research. Information Research, 9(1). behavior: Linking information seeking and communication. Journal of
Retrieved from http://InformationR.net/ir/9-1/paper163.html. Documentation, 69(2), 169–193.
Johnson, J.D. (1996). Information seeking: An organizational dilemma. Saracevic, T. (1997). The stratified model of information retrieval inter-
Westport, CT: Quorum Books. action: Extension and applications. In Proceedings of the American
Kuhlthau, C.C. (1993). A principle of uncertainty for information seek- Society for Information Science (pp. 313–327). Medford, NJ: Infor-
ing. Journal of Documentation, 49(4), 339–355. mation Today.
Smith, K. (1988). An investigation of the information-seeking behaviour
Lambert, S.D., & Loiselle, C.G. (2007). Health information-seeking
of academics active in the field of English Literature. (Unpublished
behavior. Qualitative Health Research, 17(8), 1006–1019.
M.A. thesis). University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK.
Leckie, G.J., Pettigrew, K.E., & Sylvain, C. (1996). Modelling the infor-
Torrance, E.P. (1979). The search for satori and creativity. Buffalo, NY:
mation seeking of professionals: A general model derived from
Bearly Limited.
research on engineers, health care professionals and lawyers. Library
Vakkari, P. (1998). Growth of theories on information seeking: An anal-
Quarterly, 66(2), 161–193.
ysis of growth of a theoretical research program on the relation
Makri, S. (2008). A study of lawyers’ information behaviour leading to
between task complexity and information seeking. Information Proc-
the development of two methods for evaluating electronic resources
essing & Management, 34(2), 361–382.
(PhD thesis). University College London, London. Retrieved from Wilson, T.D. (1981). On user studies and information needs. Journal of
http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/14729/1/14729.pdf. Documentation, 37(1), 3–15.
Makri, S., Blandford, A., & Cox, A.L. (2008a). Investigating the Wilson, T.D. (1994). Information needs and uses: Fifty years of pro-
information-seeking behaviour of academic lawyers: From Ellis’s gress. In B.C. Vickery (Ed.), Fifty years of information progress (pp.
model to design. Information Processing & Management, 44(2), 613– 15–51). London: Aslib.
634. Wilson, T.D. (1997). Information behavior: An interdisciplinary perspec-
Makri, S., Blandford, A., & Cox, A.L. (2008b). Using information tive. Information Processing & Management, 33(4), 551–572.
behaviors to evaluate the functionality and usability of electronic Wilson, T.D. (1999). Models in information behavior research. Journal
resources: From Ellis’s model to evaluation. Journal of the American of Documentation, 55(3), 249–270.
Society for Information Science and Technology, 59(14), 2244– Wilson, T.D. (2010). Information behavior models. In M.J. Bates & M.
2267. Niles Maack (Eds.), Encyclopedia of library and information sciences
Meho, L.I., & Tibbo, H.R. (2003). Modelling the information-seeking (3rd. ed.) (pp. 2392–2400). London: Taylor & Francis.
behavior of social scientists: Ellis’s study revisited. Journal of the Wilson, T.D., Streatfield, D., & Mullings, C. (1979). Information needs
American Society for Information Science and Technology, 54(6), in local authority service departments: A second report on INISS.
570–597. Journal of Documentation, 35(2), 120–136.

608 JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY—March 2017
DOI: 10.1002/asi

You might also like