You are on page 1of 6

ARTICLE IN PRESS

www.elsevier.de/mambio

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION
Effects of group size on nest attendance in the communally breeding
colonial tuco-tuco
Graciela Izquierdoa, Eileen A. Laceyb,
a
Sección Etologı´a, Instituto de Biologı´a, Facultad de Ciencias. Iguá 4225, Montevideo 11400, Uruguay
b
Museum of Vertebrate Zoology and Department of Integrative Biology, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA

Received 30 May 2007; accepted 27 September 2007

Abstract
Communal nesting is generally assumed to be adaptive, meaning that it confers a fitness advantage on the
individuals that share a nest site. This advantage may accrue directly to adults, or it may affect adult fitness through
gains in offspring survival. In particular, survival of juveniles reared in communal groups may be greater because
adults are present in the nest more often to provide care to young. To test the hypothesis that communal nesting is
associated with increased adult presence in the nest, we used radiotelemetry to examine patterns of adult nest
attendance as a function of group size for free-living colonial tuco-tucos (Ctenomys sociabilis). Burrow systems of this
social, subterranean rodent are inhabited by 1–6 adult females and, in some cases, a single adult male. Data obtained
from residents of 26 burrow systems monitored during 1996–2000 indicated that the percentage of time that the nest
was unattended (no adult present) did not vary predictably with date or time of day during the period between the
birth and weaning of young. The percentage of time that the nest was unattended, however, decreased significantly as
the number of adults per burrow system increased. This difference was most evident when the percentage of time that
the nest was unattended was compared for lone females versus multi-adult groups. We suggest that increased nest
attendance has important implications for the survival of juveniles reared in multi-adult burrow systems but that this
effect may be confounded by the fitness consequences of other costs and benefits associated with communal nesting in
this species.
r 2007 Deutsche Gesellschaft für Säugetierkunde. Published by Elsevier GmbH. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Ctenomys sociabilis; Communal nesting; Group size; Nest attendance; Tuco-tucos

Introduction Macdonald 1981; Pilastro 1992; Ebensperger and


Wallen 2002) or they may act upon the young reared
Communal nesting is expected to occur when the in communal nests (e.g., increased protection from
fitness benefits of sharing a nest exceed those of living infanticide; Brown and Burt 2004; Manning et al. 1995;
and rearing young alone (Gittleman 1985; Lewis and Ebensperger 1998), thereby enhancing the fitness of
Pusey 1997; Hayes 2000). The selective factors favoring parents that nest together. Because different selective
communal nesting may act directly upon the adults factors may have distinct or even competing effects on
that share a nest (e.g., increased predator detection; the actions of individuals, it is important to understand
how the various potential benefits of communal nesting
Corresponding author. interact to shape the behavior of animals that live and
E-mail address: ealacey@berkeley.edu (E.A. Lacey). rear their young together.

1616-5047/$ - see front matter r 2007 Deutsche Gesellschaft für Säugetierkunde. Published by Elsevier GmbH. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2007.09.007 Mamm. biol. ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]]

Please cite this article as: Izquierdo, G., Lacey, E.A., Effects of group size on nest attendance in the communally breeding colonial tuco-tuco.
Mamm. Biol. (2007), doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2007.09.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS
2 G. Izquierdo, E.A. Lacey / Mamm. biol. ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]]

For juveniles, one potential benefit of communal by Lacey and Wieczorek (2003), consisted of a ca. 10-ha area
nesting is the increased number of adults available to of open meadow characterized by seasonal grasses, sedges, and
provide parental care. Among communally nesting several species of woody shrubs. Each year, the site contained
mammals, parental care occurs in a variety of forms, 13–25 burrow systems occupied by C. sociabilis. Typically,
about half of these burrow systems contained a single adult
including provisioning young with food (Gittleman
female, with the remaining burrow systems occupied by a
1985; Baker et al. 1998; Doolan and Macdonald 1999;
mean (71 SD) of 3.0 (71.2) females (range ¼ 2–6; Lacey and
Hayes and Solomon 2004), protecting young from Wieczorek 2004). Approximately 44% of burrow systems also
predators (Pilastro 1992; Hoogland 1995), and helping contained an adult male (Lacey and Wieczorek 2004), yielding
young to thermoregulate (Trune and Slobodchikoff a mean (71 SD) population density of 4.0 (72.7) adults per
1976; Bozinovic et al. 1988; Jacquot and Vessey 1994; hectare.
Cutrera et al. 2003). For at least some forms of care, C. sociabilis is almost exclusively subterranean. Individuals
increasing the number of adults associated with a nest typically emerge only briefly from their burrows to crop
may increase the benefits accrued by young. For surface-growing vegetation and, while at the surface, the
example, if the presence of an adult in the nest is critical animals rarely emerge more than half a body length from their
to thermoregulation by neonates, then the presence of burrows. Members of the study population were captured as
they emerged to forage using hand-held nooses (Lacey et al.
more adults or the presence of at least one adult for a
1997). Capture localities were recorded to the nearest meter
greater total proportion of time may enhance the growth
using a geo-referenced grid (4 m  4 m cell size) established on
and survival of all young. Thus, patterns of nest the study site in 1996. The criteria used to determine when all
attendance by adults may offer critical insights into animals resident in a burrow system had been captured are
fitness benefits to juveniles associated with communal described in Lacey et al. (1997). In brief, by placing a small
nesting. twig across each entrance to a burrow system, we were able to
Studies of the colonial tuco-tuco (Ctenomys sociabilis) determine whether uncaptured animals remained in that
provide an ideal opportunity to explore the adaptive system; if no twigs were displaced during two consecutive
consequences of communal nesting. Field research on foraging periods (morning and evening), we considered all
these subterranean rodents has revealed that nearly 50% animals in that system to have been captured.
of burrow systems are occupied by multiple adults, Upon first capture, members of the study population were
including male–female pairs and multi-female groups individually marked by injecting a magnetically coded bead
(IMI-1000 Implantable Transponders, BioMedic Data Sys-
that may or may not include an adult male (Lacey et al.
tems, Seaford, DE) beneath the skin at the nape of the neck.
1997; Lacey and Wieczorek 2004). During the period Implanted beads were read using a hand-held scanner (DAS
when unweaned young are present, all animals in a 4004 Pocket Scanner, BioMedic Data Systems). Typically,
burrow system share a single, communal nest site (Lacey adults in the study population were captured during early to
et al. 1997). Although typically all females in single- and mid-October, which corresponds to the period when females
multi-female groups give birth to young, the per capita are giving birth to their single litter of young per year. Adults
number of pups weaned each year is significantly less for were recaptured and juveniles were captured for the first time
group-living than for lone females (Lacey 2004), raising between mid-November and mid-December, as soon as young
intriguing questions regarding the adaptive bases for of the year began foraging for themselves on surface-growing
communal nesting in this species. vegetation (age: ca. 4–5 weeks). Used in conjunction with
As a first step toward understanding how communal radiotelemetry (see below), this protocol allowed us to
determine the number and identities of adults resident in
nesting influences juvenile survival and, hence, parental
almost all burrow systems occupied during 1996–2000.
fitness, we explored the effects of group size on patterns Because the extent of a burrow system cannot be accurately
of nest attendance by adults. To test the hypothesis that assessed from surface evidence of activity (e.g., freshly
communal nesting is associated with increased nest excavated dirt, active burrow entrances), capture localities
attendance by adults, we quantified the percentage of alone do not provide a reliable means of determining which
time that nests were unattended (no adult present) as a adults share a burrow system. Consequently, we used radio-
function of the number of adults per burrow system. telemetry to confirm that animals captured in close proximity
These data provide the first quantitative analyses of to one another occupied the same burrow system. Adults
communal nest use by free-living C. sociabilis and yield captured in October were fitted with small (o7 g) radiocollars
critical insights into the potential benefits of sociality in consisting of an acrylic-encased transmitter (SM1-Mouse
this species. transmitters, AVM Instruments, Inc., Colfax, CA) attached
to a plastic cable tie. Following their release, the locations of
radiocollared animals were determined several times per day
using hand-held antennas and receivers (Yagi antennas, CE-12
Materials and methods receivers, AVM Instruments, Inc.). Previous research has
revealed that localities recorded using this procedure are
The population of C. sociabilis studied was located on accurate to within 0.5 m (Lacey et al. 1997). Individuals that
Estancia Rincon Grande, Provincia Neuquén, Argentina exhibited extensive (466%) spatial overlap and that shared
(401560 S, 711030 W). The study site, which is described in detail the same nest site were considered resident in the same burrow

Please cite this article as: Izquierdo, G., Lacey, E.A., Effects of group size on nest attendance in the communally breeding colonial tuco-tuco.
Mamm. Biol. (2007), doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2007.09.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G. Izquierdo, E.A. Lacey / Mamm. biol. ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] 3

system (Lacey et al. 1997). Radiocollars were removed when were in the nest by the associated total number of scans. Time
adults were recaptured during November and December. series analyses were then run using date and time of day as the
Radiotelemetry was also used to document patterns of nest independent variables. t-tests were used to compare correlation
attendance by adult C. sociabilis during the period between the coefficients from all social units against the null hypothesis of
birth and weaning of young. Previous field studies (Lacey et al. no consistent relationship (mean r ¼ 0) between nest atten-
1997) have revealed that during this portion of the reproduc- dance and date or hour of data collection (Zar 1999).
tive cycle, all residents of a burrow system share a single, The effects of social unit size on nest attendance were
communal nest. For each burrow system monitored during examined using ANOVAs and linear regressions. For all other
this study, the location of the nest was identified by analyses, parametric statistical tests were used whenever the
determining the location at which all radiocollared adults associated assumptions of normality and homogeneity of
spent the majority of their time (Lacey et al. 1997). This was variances were met. All statistical analyses were completed
done by recording the locations of all radiocollared animals using Statistica 6.0 (Statsoft, Inc., 2002). Throughout the text,
X6 times per day (minimum of 1 h between successive data means are reported 71 SD.
points) for a period of 3–4 days. In addition, the locations
of radiocollared animals were determined after sunset.
C. sociabilis is strictly diurnal and adults only rarely leave
the nest during non-daylight hours (Lacey et al. 1997). As a
Results
result, telemetry data obtained at night provided a convenient
means of determining nest locations. Nest attendance by adult C. sociabilis was monitored
Once the nest site for a burrow system had been determined, for 61 individuals in 26 social units resident on the study
the presence of adults in the nest was monitored by placing a site during 1996–2000. All adults in 24 (92.3%) of these
Yagi antenna directly over the nest site, with the long axis of social units were monitored via radiotelemetry. Although
the antennae held perpendicular to the soil surface. The one adult in each of the remaining social units was not
identities of all adults present in the nest were determined by captured until after telemetry data had been collected,
quickly scanning through the radiotransmitter frequencies for visual inspection of the data obtained from these groups
adults resident in that burrow system and noting the intensity revealed no apparent differences in nest attendance
of associated radio signals. Intensity was assessed both aurally between these social units and same-sized groups in
and by noting the amplitude of the signal as indicated on the
which all adults were monitored. As a result, analyses of
radio receiver. This procedure provided an unambiguous
nest attendance for the two incomplete social units were
means of determining whether an individual was present in
the nest and produced no detectable effects on adult behavior. based on the total number of adults resident in each
Scans of adult presence were repeated at 5-min intervals for a burrow system. Twelve of the social units monitored
total of 30 min. Typically, up to three 30-min scan sessions contained a single adult female; the remaining social units
were completed daily between 0800 and 2100 h, with a each contained multiple adult females (mean ¼ 2.071.1
minimum of 2 h allowed between scan sessions for the same females per social unit, range ¼ 2–4; Table 1). Ten
burrow system. The times at which scans were completed for a (38.5%) social units contained an adult male; the
given burrow system varied so that all portions of the day were distribution of males between burrow systems occupied
represented in the data collected from each system. by one versus multiple adult females was not significantly
Because the number of adults per burrow system varied different from that expected if males are equally
(Lacey et al. 1997; Lacey and Wieczorek 2004), we use the term
represented in both types of social units (w21 ¼ 1:38,
‘‘social unit’’ to describe the set of animals – adults and
P40.05). For each social unit, a minimum of 10 30-min
juveniles – that were resident in a single burrow system during
a given field season. In terms of benefits to juveniles of nest attendance scans (mean ¼ 24.7710.8 scans per
communal nesting, the variable that seemed most likely to social unit; range ¼ 10–48) was completed over at least
influence offspring growth and survival was the percentage of 5 different days (mean ¼ 23.6710.5 days per social unit,
time that the nest was unattended (no adults in the nest) during range ¼ 5–40) between the birth and weaning of pups
each 30-min scan session. Although tuco-tucos produce reared by that social unit.
relatively precocial young (Lacey and Ebensperger 2007), As expected, within each multi-adult social unit, all
studies of captive animals indicate that juvenile C. sociabilis adults shared a common nest site; in no case did the nest
remain closely associated with the nest for the first few weeks
following birth (M. Soares pers. comm.). As a result, the
Table 1. Composition of the social units monitored during
percentage of time that the nest is unattended may reflect
this study
periods during which parental care is not available to young.
Although each social unit represented an independent data Number of adult females Total
set, data collected from the same social unit during different
30-min scan sessions were not independent and thus time series 1 2 3 4
analyses were used to assess whether nest attendance was
influenced by the date or the time of day when scans were Male present 3 3 2 2 10
conducted. For each social unit, the percentage of time that the No male present 9 1 5 1 16
nest was unattended was calculated per scan session and per Total 12 4 7 3 26
day by dividing the number of scans during which no adults

Please cite this article as: Izquierdo, G., Lacey, E.A., Effects of group size on nest attendance in the communally breeding colonial tuco-tuco.
Mamm. Biol. (2007), doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2007.09.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS
4 G. Izquierdo, E.A. Lacey / Mamm. biol. ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]]

sites identified for individuals differ among members of


the same social unit. The percentage of time that the nest
was left unattended (no adults present) did not vary
predictably with the date of data collection (mean
r ¼ 0.02470.352, N ¼ 26; t-test: t23 ¼ 0.35, two-
tailed P40.05), providing no evidence that patterns of
adult activity changed predictably as a function of
juvenile age. Although the percentage of time that the
nest was unattended appeared to increase over the
course of a day (mean r ¼ 0.19370.222, N ¼ 26; t-test:
t23 ¼ 4.43, two-tailed Po0.01), a strict interpretation of
this result indicates only that there was a consistently
non-zero relationship between time of day and nest
attendance among the social units monitored. Closer
inspection of these data revealed that, across social
units, both positive and negative relationships occurred Fig. 1. Percentage of time that nests of C. sociabilis were
between these variables. Further, significant correlations unattended (no adult present) as a function of the total number
between time of day and percentage of time that the nest of adults resident in a burrow system. Data are from 26 social
was unattended were detected for only 2 (7.7%) of the units monitored during 1996–2000. Burrow systems containing
26 social units monitored, indicating that, within social an adult male are indicated. The regression line indicates a
significant (Po0.001) negative relationship between number of
units, nest use by adults was not strongly temporally
adults and percentage of time the nest was unattended.
patterned. Given these findings, we did not partition the
data according to date or time of day at which scans
nest was unattended was detected among social units
were completed. Instead, for each social unit, all scans
containing X2 adults (R2 ¼ 0.114, N ¼ 17, F1,15 ¼ 1.94,
were pooled for the following analyses of nest atten-
P40.100).
dance by adults.
Because not all social units in our sample contained
an adult male (Table 1), we examined the effects of the
presence of a male on patterns of nest attendance by Discussion
females. A two-way analysis of variance revealed no
significant effect of a male on the percentage of time that Our data indicate that nest attendance by free-living
the nest was unattended (ANOVA: F1,18 ¼ 1.23, C. sociabilis was influenced by the number of adults that
P ¼ 0.283). Consequently, we did not distinguish shared a burrow system. When all social units were
between the sexes but instead used the total number of considered, the percentage of time that the communal
adults resident in a burrow system for subsequent nest was unattended (no adults present) decreased
analyses of the effects of group size on nest attendance. significantly as the number of adults per burrow system
When the total number adults per burrow system was increased. The presence of an adult male appeared to
considered, there was a significant effect of social unit have no effect on the percentage of time that the nest
size on the percentage of time that no adults were was unattended. When the number of adults per social
present in the nest (one-way ANOVA: F4,21 ¼ 12.57, unit was considered regardless of sex, the only sig-
Po0.001). Visual inspection of the data suggested that nificant contrasts detected were between lone females
the percentage of time that the nests of lone females and social units containing multiple adults. Thus, the
were unattended was considerably greater than that for greatest effect of communal nesting on nest attendance
social units containing multiple adults (Fig. 1); post hoc occurred with the addition of a second adult to social
comparisons revealed significant contrasts between data units containing only a single female.
from lone females and those from all other sizes of social Reducing the time that the nest is unattended may be
unit (Tukey’s HSD tests, all Po0.01), suggesting that beneficial to juveniles in several ways. In general,
the primary determinant of the percentage of time that a because the presence of an adult is essential to many
nest is left unattended is the presence of one versus forms of parental care, increasing the percentage of time
multiple adults in a burrow system. Accordingly, during which at least one adult is present in the nest may
although the percentage of time that the nest was increase the overall availability of such care, potentially
unattended declined significantly as a function of the leading to enhanced juvenile growth and survival (Hayes
number of adults in a burrow system when all social and Solomon 2006). More specifically, the presence of
units were considered (R2 ¼ 0.533, N ¼ 26, F1,25 ¼ one or more adults may have thermoregulatory benefits
27.35, Po0.001; Fig. 1), no significant relationship for young; juvenile ctenomyids do not thermoregulate
between number of adults and percentage of time the effectively until they are more than 15 days old (Cutrera

Please cite this article as: Izquierdo, G., Lacey, E.A., Effects of group size on nest attendance in the communally breeding colonial tuco-tuco.
Mamm. Biol. (2007), doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2007.09.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G. Izquierdo, E.A. Lacey / Mamm. biol. ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] 5

et al. 2003; Zenuto et al. 2002) and, hence, regular nest Museum of Vertebrate Zoology and Department of
attendance may reduce the chances of hypothermia of Integrative Biology at the University of California,
young. At the same time, the presence of an adult may Berkeley. This work was conducted in compliance with
increase the tendency for pups to remain inside the nest all relevant institutional, national, and international
(M. Soares, pers. comm.), thereby decreasing the risk of regulations.
predation as well as hypothermia. If, as these arguments
suggest, the regular presence in the nest of X1 adult
increases juvenile survival, then decreasing the time that Zusammenfassung
the nest is unattended may be an adaptive consequence
of group living in C. sociabilis. Einfluss der Gruppengröße auf die Nestanwesenheit ko-
If increased nest attendance is an adaptive benefit of lonialer Tucotucos (Ctenomys sociabilis)
group living, why do individuals in multi-female social
units rear fewer young to weaning than lone females? On Gemeinschaftliche Jungenaufzucht wird als adaptiv
an annual basis, the per capita direct fitness of female angenommen, wenn es den Individuen, die dasselbe Nest
C. sociabilis declines significantly as a function of group benutzen, einen Fitnessvorteil verleiht. Dieser Vorteil
size (Lacey 2004), indicating that social units in which kann direkt für die adulten Tiere nützlich sein oder deren
nest attendance by adults is greater produce fewer Fitness durch längere Überlebensrate ihrer Nachkommen
surviving pups per female. Communal nesting is a beeinflussen. Insbesondere Jungtiere, die in gemeinschaf-
complex social phenomenon that likely reflects the tlichen Gruppen aufgezogen wurden, dürften eine längere
combined effects of multiple selective factors that confer Überlebensrate durch die häufigere Anwesenheit meh-
both benefits and costs on animals that live and rear rerer pflegeversorgenden Adulten haben.
their young together (Alexander 1974; Hayes 2000). As a Es wurden radiotelemetrische Analysen durchgeführt,
result, while increasing nest attendance may improve um zu prüfen, wie gemeinschaftliche Vermehrung mit
juvenile survival, other aspects of communal nesting erhöhter Anwesenheit adulter Tiere in Verbindung steht,
(e.g., reproductive skew, infanticide, and other manifes- um damit die Hypothese zu testen, dass das Anwesen-
tations of increased competition among burrow-mates: heitsmuster adulter Tiere im Nest abhängig von der
Alexander 1974; Clutton-Brock 1998; Ebensperger 1998; Gruppengröße freilebender kolonialer Tucotucos (Cte-
Woodroffe and Macdonald 2000) may counter this nomys sociabilis) ist. Die Bausysteme dieses sozialen,
potential fitness gain. Hence, to understand the net unterirdischen Nagetieres werden von 1–6 adulten
fitness consequences of communal nesting, it is neces- Weibchen und in einigen Fällen von einem einzigen
sary to consider the full range of selective pressures adulten Männchen bewohnt. Die Daten, die wir von
leading to this behavior. Future studies of communal 1996 bis 2000 von 26 überwachten Bausystemen
nesting in C. sociabilis will address this issue by erhielten, deuteten an, dass die Zeit, in der das Nest
exploring other mechanisms (e.g., communal nursing) ohne adulte Tiere zwischen Geburt und Absetzen war,
by which young may benefit from increased parental nicht von Datum oder Tageszeit abhing. Die Zeit, in der
presence, as well as by exploring the fitness conse- das Nest unbeaufsichtigt war, verminderte sich mit der
quences to adults of sharing a nest with conspecifics. Zunahme an erwachsenen Tieren für jedes Bausystem
erheblich. Als die Zeit, in der das Nest unbegleitet war,
für solitäre Weibchen mit der von Gruppen mehrerer
Acknowledgements Weibchen verglichen wurde, war der Unterschied
offensichtlich. Es wird vorgeschlagen, dass zunehmende
For permission to work on Estancia Rincon Grande, Nestanwesenheit eine wichtige Rolle für das Überleben
we thank Alain Thouyaret and the Delegación Tecnica von Jungtieren, die in ‘‘Multi-Erwachsenen’’-Bausyste-
de Parques Nacionales Argentinas, in particular Claudio men aufgezogen werden, spielt. Dieses könnte jedoch
Chehebar and Eduardo Ramilo. For logistic assistance, mit den Fitnessfolgen anderer Kosten und Vorteile, die
we thank Miguel Christie, Gustavo Iglesias, and the mit der gemeinsamen Jungenaufzucht in dieser Art
members of the Sociedad Naturalista Andino Patago- verbunden sind, verwechselt werden.
nica in Bariloche, Argentina. Field assistance was
provided by A. Chiesa, I. Tomasco, and, in particular,
M. Soares and J. Wieczorek. Previous versions of this
References
manuscript benefited from comments by G. Francescoli,
M. Hauber, M. Soares, and J. Wieczorek. Melitta Alexander, R.D., 1974. The evolution of social behavior. Ann.
Meneghel kindly translated the abstract to German. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 5, 325–388.
Financial support was provided by the Comision Baker, P.J., Robertson, C.P.J., Funk, S.M., Harris, S., 1998.
Sectorial de Investigacion Cientifica and Programa de Potential fitness benefits of group living in the red fox,
Desarrollo de las Ciencias Básicas (Uruguay), and the Vulpes vulpes. Anim. Behav. 56, 1411–1424.

Please cite this article as: Izquierdo, G., Lacey, E.A., Effects of group size on nest attendance in the communally breeding colonial tuco-tuco.
Mamm. Biol. (2007), doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2007.09.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS
6 G. Izquierdo, E.A. Lacey / Mamm. biol. ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]]

Bozinovic, F.M., Rosenmann, M., Veloso, C., 1988. Termor- Lacey, E.A., 2004. Sociality reduces individual direct fitness in
regulación conductual en Phillotis darwini (Rodentia: a communally breeding rodent, the colonial tuco-tuco
Cricetidae): efecto de la temperatura ambiente, uso de (Ctenomys sociabilis). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 56, 449–457.
nidos y agrupamiento social sobre el gasto de energı́a. Rev. Lacey, E.A., Ebensperger, L.A., 2007. Social structure in
Chil. Hist. Nat. 61, 81–86. octodontid and ctenomyid rodents. In: Wolff, J.O., Sher-
Brown, J.S., Burt, P., 2004. Hazardous duty pay and the man, P.W. (Eds.), Rodent Societies: An Ecological and
foraging cost of predation. Ecol. Lett. 7, 999–1014. Evolutionary Perspective. University of Chicago Press,
Clutton-Brock, T.H., 1998. Reproductive skew, concessions, Chicago, IL, USA, pp. 403–415.
and limited control. Trends Ecol. Evol. 13, 288–292. Lacey, E.A., Wieczorek, J.R., 2003. The ecology of sociality in
Cutrera, A.P., Antinuchi, C.D., Busch, C., 2003. Thermo- rodents: a ctenomyid perspective. J. Mammal. 84, 1198–1211.
regulatory development in pups of the subterranean rodent Lacey, E.A., Wieczorek, J.R., 2004. Kinship in colonial tuco-
Ctenomys talarum. Physiol. Behav. 79, 321–330. tucos: evidence from group composition and population
Doolan, S.P., Macdonald, D.W., 1999. Cooperative rearing by structure. Behav. Ecol. 15, 988–996.
slender tailed meerkats (Suricata suricatta) in the southern Lacey, E.A., Braude, S.H., Wieczorek, J.R., 1997. Burrow sharing
Kalahari. Ethology 105, 851–866. by colonial tuco-tucos (Ctenomys sociabilis). J. Mammal. 78,
Ebensperger, L.A., 1998. Strategies and counterstrategies to 556–562.
infanticide in mammals. Biol. Rev. Cambridge Philos. Soc. Lewis, S.E., Pusey, A.E., 1997. Factors influencing the
73, 321–346. occurrence of communal care in plural breeding mammals.
Ebensperger, L.A., Wallen, P.K., 2002. Grouping increases the In: Solomon, N.G., French, J.A. (Eds.), Cooperative
ability of the social rodent, Octodon degus, to detect Breeding in Mammals. Cambridge University Press, Cam-
predators when using exposed microhabitats. Oikos 98, bridge, pp. 335–363.
491–497. Macdonald, D.W., 1981. Dwindling resources and the social
Gittleman, J.L., 1985. Functions of communal care in mammals. behaviour of capybaras (Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris)
In: Greenwood, P.J., Harvey, P.H., Slatkin, M. (Eds.), (Mammalia). J. Zool. (London) 194, 371–391.
Evolution: Essays in Honour of John Maynard Smith. Manning, C.J., Dewsbury, D.A., Wakeland, E.K., Potts, W.K.,
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 187–205. 1995. Communal nesting and nursing in house mice, Mus
Hayes, L.D., 2000. To nest communally or not to nest musculus domesticus. Anim. Behav. 50, 741–751.
communally: a review of rodent communal nesting and Pilastro, A., 1992. Communal nesting between breeding
nursing. Anim. Behav. 59, 677–688. females in a free-living population of fat dormouse (Glis
Hayes, L.D., Solomon, N.G., 2004. Costs and benefits of glis L). Boll. Zool. 59, 63–68.
communal rearing to female prairie voles (Microtus Trune, D.R., Slobodchikoff, C.N., 1976. Social effects of
ochrogaster). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 56, 585–593. roosting on the metabolism of the pallid bat (Antrozous
Hayes, L.D., Solomon, N.G., 2006. Mechanisms of maternal pallidus). J. Mammal. 57, 656–663.
investment by communal prairie voles, Microtus ochroga- Woodroffe, R., Macdonald, D.W., 2000. Helpers provide no
ster. Anim. Behav. 72, 1069–1080. detectable benefits in the European badger (Meles meles).
Hoogland, J.L., 1995. The Black-Tailed Prairie Dog: The J. Zool. (London) 250, 113–119.
Social Life of a Burrowing Mammal. University of Chicago Zar, J.H., 1999. Biostatistical Analysis. Prentice-Hall, New
Press, Chicago. Jersey.
Jacquot, J.J., Vessey, S.H., 1994. Non-offspring nursing in the Zenuto, R.R., Antinuchi, C.D., Busch, C., 2002. Bioenergetics of
white-footed mouse, Peromyscus leucopus. Anim. Behav. reproduction and pup development in a subterranean rodent
48, 1238–1240. (Ctenomys talarum). Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 75, 469–478.

Please cite this article as: Izquierdo, G., Lacey, E.A., Effects of group size on nest attendance in the communally breeding colonial tuco-tuco.
Mamm. Biol. (2007), doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2007.09.007

You might also like