You are on page 1of 15

Acta Neuropsychiatrica 2008: 20: 236–250 # 2008 The Authors

All rights reserved Journal compilation # 2008 Blackwell Munksgaard


DOI: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2008.00315.x
ACTA NEUROPSYCHIATRICA

Review article

The recognition of facial expressions of


emotion in Alzheimer’s disease: a review
of findings
McLellan T, Johnston L, Dalrymple-Alford J, Porter R. The recognition Tracey McLellan1,2,
of facial expressions of emotion in Alzheimer’s disease: a review of Lucy Johnston1,2,
findings. John Dalrymple-Alford1,2,
Richard Porter3
Objective: To provide a selective review of the literature on the 1
Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury,
recognition of facial expressions of emotion in Alzheimer’s disease (AD),
Christchurch, New Zealand; 2Van der Veer Institute for
to evaluate whether these patients show variation in their ability to Parkinson's and Brain Research, Christchurch, New
recognise different emotions and whether any such impairments are Zealand; and 3Department of Psychological Medicine,
instead because of a general decline in cognition. University of Otago, Christchurch, New Zealand
Methods: A narrative review based on relevant articles identified from
PubMed and PsycInfo searches from 1987 to 2007 using keywords
ÔAlzheimer’s’, Ôfacial expression recognition’, Ôdementia’ and Ôemotion
processing’.
Conclusion: Although the literature is as yet limited, with several
methodological inconsistencies, AD patients show poorer recognition of
facial expressions, with particular difficulty with sad expressions. It is
unclear whether poorer performance reflects the general cognitive decline
Keywords: Alzheimer's disease; dementia; emotion;
and/or verbal or spatial deficits associated with AD or whether the facial expressions
deficits reflect specific neuropathology. This under-represented field of
study may help to extend our understanding of social functioning in AD. Tracey McLellan, Department of Psychology,
University of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800,
Future work requires more detailed analyses of ancillary cognitive Christchurch, New Zealand.
measures, more ecologically valid facial displays of emotion and Tel: 164 3 3642987;
a reference situation that more closely approximates an actual social Fax: 164 3 3642181;
interaction. E-mail: tlm36@student.canterbury.ac.nz

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a progressive neuro- quality of life (3). The degradation of social skills
degenerative disorder that is characterised by de- and social comprehension also affects the manage-
terioration of intellectual functioning and change ment of behavioural problems. The behavioural
in personality. For the diagnosis of AD, the initial and psychiatric symptoms of dementia are reported
dysfunction necessarily requires impaired memory, to occur in approximately 90% of dementia pat-
but varying impairments in visuospatial abilities, ients (4) and are the most important factor for care-
language skills, complex attention and mental givers considering institutionalisation (5). Poor social
speed are common changes that support its clinical functioning may well contribute to the anxiety
delineation and trajectory (1). The majority of and phobia behaviours found to be problematic in
research interest has focused on the cognitive and AD (2) as well as the agitation and aggressive be-
psychiatric profiles associated with AD. Surpris- haviours often associated with the management of
ingly, few studies have examined how AD influen- those with AD (2).
ces social functioning. The interpersonal problems with social function-
Interpersonal and social problems are often, ing in AD are likely to be influenced by impaired
however, a feature of AD. These problems are emotional processing and in particular, deficits in
associated with the increase in caregiver burden (2) the ability to recognise the affective state of another
and are potentially a major factor in decreased person (6). Given that successful communication

236
Facial expression recognition in Alzheimer’s disease

and appropriate interaction requires an ability to tially dissociable neural pathways are involved with
detect and react to the felt state of others, there the processing of specific emotions. For example,
exists a need to establish whether AD diminishes activations in the anterior cingulate and subcallosal
such fundamental skills associated with emotional cingulate have been associated with sadness (28),
processing. while the amygdala is consistently linked with fear
Arguably, the most accessible information spec- (25,28,35) and the insular and basal ganglia with
ifying an individual’s affective state is the informa- disgust (36,37).
tion conveyed through the facial expression. Ekman In normal adult, ageing frontal and striatal
and Friesen (7) provide convincing evidence for regions are affected by ageing earlier and more
the universality of basic expressions of happiness, profoundly than other regions (38–42). Subregions
sadness, fear, anger, disgust and surprise. There are within the frontal cortex show different age-related
clear commonalities when normally functioning changes, specifically, stronger volume loss in
adults from different cultures are asked to pose lateral and orbital frontal grey matter (43). Cell
or recognise the basic expressions (8). Studies of loss and decreased dendritic branching underlies
emotion processing generally examine the recogni- the vulnerability of the prefrontal cortex to normal
tion of basic facial expressions (9–11). Generally, ageing, with distinct rates of decline in dorsal vs.
prototypical expressions are readily identifiable and anterior regions (42,44). Normal ageing also affects
marked confusion is rare among normally function- the medial temporal lobe, particularly the hippo-
ing adults. A growing literature has subsequently campus (44, 45). The impact of normal ageing on
developed concerning when such abilities are im- brain regions implicated in emotion processing
paired and which disorders affect such abilities. might explain the difficulty experienced with the
Recently, several studies have addressed the recognition of certain facial expressions.
impact of normal adult ageing on facial expression Individuals with AD experience far more perva-
recognition. The recognition of sadness (10,12–16), sive structural and morphological changes in the
anger (10,12,15,17,18) and fear (10,12,13,17,18) brain than that shown by normal ageing. Early
has proved problematic for older adults compared pathology is known to preferentially affect the
with younger adults. By contrast, recognition of medial temporal lobe structures; in particular,
disgust (12–16) and happiness (12,14,16,18) are characteristic neurofibrillary tangles are found in
largely spared in the healthy elderly, while there the entorhinal cortex, hippocampus and amygdala
is even some evidence that the expression of disgust (46–49). As stated, several studies highlight the
is more readily identified in advancing years central role of the amygdala in emotion process-
(10). The latter finding is of considerable interest ing (23,25,50,51), and the hippocampus and/or
because disgust is normally recognised at a similar amygdala–hippocampal junction have also been
level to sadness, anger and fear in young adults implicated in the modulation of facial expression
(10,13,16). The disparate negotiation of these ex- perception (52,53). It appears reasonable, there-
pressions in older adults suggests that any aged impair- fore, to hypothesise that people suffering from AD
ment, like that found for sad, anger and fear, is not may well experience a specific impairment in the
likely the result of them being harder expressions to ability to process affective information, and in
recognise. particular, that this will manifest in an impaired
Impaired facial expression recognition may well ability to recognise facial expressions of emotion. It
result from pathology affecting neural substrates may well be, given the preferential impact of ageing
specific to emotion processing (19–22). A large on the recognition of specific expressions, that the
range of structures participate in the recognition of neurodegeneration associated with AD affects
facial expressions (9,23), but the amygdala (24–27), some emotions more than others.
medial prefrontal cortex (28) and fusiform cortex
(27) are thought to provide key components in the
Search strategy
distributed neural systems responsible for general
vigilance of salient affective information. The sup- An electronic search was conducted from
# #
erior temporal sulcus (25,27,29–31), ventral stria- PubMed and PsycInfo using combinations of
tum (32–34), anterior cingulate (9, 28, 31) and the keywords ÔAlzheimer’s’, Ôfacial expression
insula (9, 28, 31) have also been implicated in recognition’, Ôdementia’ and Ôemotion processing’.
emotional information processing. Specific emo- The reference lists of retrieved articles were also
tions have also been associated with certain brain searched for additional relevant studies. Only
regions, and although the evidence to suggest studies in English were included.
which brain areas subserve which emotion is Fifteen studies were identified that investi-
tentative, there is sufficient agreement that par- gated the ability to recognise facial expressions of

237
McLellan et al.

emotion in well-defined AD samples. (The follow- diagnosis (6,54,59,61,63,66). The specific diagnos-
ing studies were omitted because respective sam- tic criteria used to isolate possible or probable AD
ples were potentially not limited to AD patients. were unclear in two studies (65,67).
Zandi T, Cooper M, Garrison L. Facial recogni- Comparable exclusion criteria between studies
tion: a cognitive study of elderly dementia patients have also been reported (e.g. prosopagnosia; pro-
and normal older adults. Int Psychogeriatr found visual or hearing deficits; severe cardio-
1992;4:215–221. Allen R, Brosgole L. Facial and vascular disease), as have reasonably stringent
Auditory Affect Cognition in Senile Geriatrics, strategies with regard to excluding participants
The Normal Elderly and Young Adults. Int J with other forms of dementia or other disorders
Neurosc 1993;68:33–42. Washburn A, Sands P. that are known to impact on both cognitive status
Social cognition in nursing home residents with (i.e. neurological disorders, alcohol abuse or depen-
and without cognitive impairment. J Gerontol dence and psychiatric illness) and emotion pro-
2006;61:174–170.) Many of these studies have also cessing (clinical depression). Control participants
focused on other modes of emotion recognition have been carers of AD patients (64,67), non-
such as prosody and postural cues; however, the dementia patients in institutionalised/hospital
present review only addressed findings related to care (57,58) and community-based healthy elderly
facial expressions. We addressed the following (56,59,62,65,66). While control groups are reported
questions. (a) Do AD patients have an impaired not to have a history of cognitive decline that
ability to recognise facial expressions of emotion? might be indicative of impending dementia, the
(b) Is there evidence that such impairment is level of actual functioning is often not described.
because of specific emotion-processing deficits? Two studies (6,63) did not compare AD partic-
(c) Is there a selective impairment of specific ipants to healthy controls (HC) and were therefore
emotions? Table 1 presents a summary of all excluded from the review.
studies. Studies that appear with an asterisk were Examination of Mini-Mental State Examination
included in the review of findings. Studies without (MMSE) (69) scores across studies highlights
an asterisk were excluded as a result of methodo- a wide range of cognitive impairment in the patient
logical issues that are discussed in the following groups (Table 1). This global mental status mea-
section. sure is difficult to interpret without additional
information regarding cognition or ancillary meas-
ures such as level of education. The mean MMSE
scores range from a low 12.9 (63) to a very mildly
Methodology
impaired 24.8 (66). Seven of the 13 studies that
Standard criteria were used to identify AD and to compare AD patients with HC groups report
exclude participants in 13 of 15 studies. There are a mean MMSE or Dementia Rating Scale (70)
variations, however, with regard to characteristics score indicative of mild AD, five of moderate,
such as cognitive status/dementia severity, age, while the MMSE in the remaining study was
sample size and sex ratio. Variations in methodol- indicative of very mild AD.
ogy are also noted in relation to the experimental The mean age of participants varied across
tasks including the type of stimuli used, expressions studies. The mean age ranged from 70.1 years
investigated, task difficulty and control tasks. (66) to 90.2 years (58) and in two studies HC were
What follows is a review of the most salient points significantly younger than AD participants (56,67),
of methodological similarity and variation with and therefore, as stated, will not be included in
regard to participant characteristics and experi- the review of findings. Sample size also varied
mental tasks. and ranged from 9 AD patients (66) to 31 AD
patients (57). Overall, more than double the num-
bers of female compared with male AD patients
Participant characteristics
have participated in the reviewed studies, which
Diagnoses of possible or probable AD in 10 studies reflect the prevalence of AD between the sexes
were made according to criteria of the National (71–73).
Institute of Neurological and Communicative Dis- Groups were generally matched on level of
orders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and education (54,57,59,62,64,66,67). Handedness, how-
Related Disorders Association (NINCDS/ADRDA) ever, was rarely used as a control strategy with
(55–60, 62,64,66,68) or based on the DSM-III and three studies opting to include only right-handed
DSM-IV guidelines (6,61,63). Computed tomogra- participants, while the remainder do not report the
phy and/or magnetic resonance imaging examina- handedness of participants (56,62,64). Few showed
tions were occasionally also used to confirm the comparable depressive symptom matching (62,65)

238
Table 1. Summary of research on the recognition of facial expressions in AD

Author Participants Stimuli and emotions Tasks Control tasks Results

Allender and Kaszniak (54)* ADyoung ¼ 13; mean Izard photos: interest, joy, surprise, Identification: instructions – name Identity discrimination: indicate ADY ¼ ADO,HC
age ¼ 66.3; mean DRS ¼ 107.6 sadness, disgust, anger, shame emotion expressed; verbal/point whether same or different person
ADolder ¼ 17; mean age ¼ 79.9; and contempt response from printed list of (Benton Facial Recognition Test) Facial and vocal emotion tasks
mean DRS ¼ 111.4 nine options most strongly correlated with
each other
HC ¼ 13; mean age ¼ 68.1; Both naming ability (BNT) and
mean DRS ¼ 137.5 control task relate to emotion task
but do not entirely explain
relationship.
Specific emotion-processing
deficit in AD
Albert et al. (55)* AD ¼ 19; mean age ¼ 89.6; Ekman photos: happy, sad, anger, Identification: instructions – Identity discrimination: indicate Identification, selection and
m/f ¼ 4/15; mean DRS ¼ 101 neutral name emotion the person is whether same or different person discrimination in AD,HC
feeling; verbal response; verbal
list option
HC ¼ 19; mean age ¼ 87.5; Selection: (same/different person): Discrimination accounted for by
m/f ¼ 6/13 instructions – point to the sad control task
face; point response to one of four
alternatives
Discrimination: (same/different Selection accounted for by BNT
person): instructions – do these
people feel the same of different;
verbal response to pairs of same
or different people
Identification accounted for by
verbal memory (not BNT)
No impairment in perception of
affect independent of cognitive
deficits
Cadieux and Greve (56) Low verbal AD ¼ 10; mean FAB: happy; sad; anger; fear; Identification: instructions – name Identity discrimination: indicate All tasks in LV-AD,HC
age ¼ 77.6; m/f ¼ 2/8; mean neutral emotion expressed; verbal whether same or different person
DRS ¼ 121.3 response; verbal list options
Low spatial AD ¼ 8; mean Selection: instructions – select one . All accounted for by control task/
age ¼ 75.9; m/f ¼ 1/7; of five bearing named expression; BNT (not general cognitive decline)
mean DRS ¼ 123.6 point response from five different
faces and expressions
HC (community senior Matching: instructions – select Selection and discrimination in
citizen) ¼ 15; mean age ¼ 69.1; one of five bearing same LS-AD,HC
m/f ¼ 1/14; mean DRS ¼ 141.2 expression as stimulus face;
point response from five
different faces

239
Facial expression recognition in Alzheimer’s disease
Table 1. Continued

240
Author Participants Stimuli and emotions Tasks Control tasks Results

Discrimination: instructions – Impairment independent of


indicate whether expressions language (control/BNT)
are same or different; verbal
McLellan et al.

response
to pairs of different people
No impairment of affect
processing in LV-AD independent
of language dysfunction
Impairment of affect processing in
LS-AD
Roudier et al. (57) AD ¼ 31; mean age ¼ 80.47; m/ Ekman photos: happy, sad, anger, Identification (verbal/point Identity discrimination: (same/ Identification in AD,HC
f ¼ 2/29; mean MMSE ¼ 16.18 indifference condition): instructions – Is this different emotion): indicate
person happy, sad, angry or whether same or different person
neutral/ point to happy face;
verbal/point response from verbal
list/four photos of same person
with different expressions
HC (hospitalised non- Discrimination (same/different Discrimination in AD ¼ HC
neurological) ¼ 14; mean person): instructions – is emotion
age ¼ 81.07; m/f ¼ 1/13; expressed same or different;
MMSE ¼ 26.07 verbal response from pairs of
same and then different people
Control task in AD,HC
Operations of facial discrimination
and emotion discrimination are
distinct
Impairment in identification likely
to be the result of verbal deficit
Koff et al. (58)* AD ¼ 23; mean age ¼ 90.2; m/ Videotaped vignettes: no speech; Identification: instructions – how Visuospatial ability – figure Identification in AD,HC
f ¼ 4/19; mean MMSE ¼ 20.3 actors; scripted; posed; .80% is person feeling – happy, sad, copying (CERAD)
naïve rater agreement angry or neutral; verbal response
from verbal list options
HC ¼ 19; mean age ¼ 88.9; m/ Abstraction ability – Similarities Accounted for by abstract
f ¼ 4/15 (WAIS-R) reasoning
Difficulties are secondary to
cognitive deficits
Lavenu et al. (59) AD ¼ 20; mean age ¼ 70.7; m/ Ekman photos: happy, sad, anger, Identification (trial 1): instructions Identification in AD ¼ HC.FTD
f ¼ 4/16; mean MMSE ¼ 22.9 fear, surprise, disgust, contempt – point to label that best describes
the emotion of person; point to
one of seven options on card
HC ¼ 12; mean age ¼ 65.7; m/ Consistency (trial 2): as above, Detection in AD ¼ HC ¼ FTD
f ¼ 6/6; mean MMSE ¼ 29.5 then rate intensity with score
between 0–8
Table 1. Continued

Author Participants Stimuli and emotions Tasks Control tasks Results

FTD ¼ 18; mean age ¼ 67.6; m/ Detection: instruction – which face Identification of fear and contempt
f ¼ 7/11; mean MMSE ¼ 24.9 expresses emotion; point to in AD ¼ FTD,HC
neutral or expressive photo of
same person
Different neural substrates
underlie recognition of various
emotions
Ogrocki et al. (60) AD ¼ 17; mean age ¼ 73.9; m/ Ekman photos: happy, sad, anger, Identification: instructions – is this Identification in AD ¼ HC
f ¼ 7/10; mean MMSE ¼ 21.8; neutral person feeling - or -? Verbal
CDR n ¼ 1 mild n ¼ 16 moderate response from two verbal options
Reduced visual exploration in AD
Shimokawa et al. (61) AD ¼ 25; mean age ¼ 80.2; m/ Line drawings: happy, sad, anger, Matching: instructions – match Figure identification: Select target Total affect tasks in AD,HC
f ¼ 6/19; mean MMSE ¼ 13 fear, surprise target face with one of four from three other distractors from
alternatives; point/verbal response same category (objects and
to another example of same expressions)
emotion & three other expressions
HC ¼ 12; mean age ¼ 76.5; m/ Selection: instructions – point to Total affect tasks in AD.VD
f ¼ 5/7; MMSE ¼ 28. the sad faces/choose face that
matches situation; point response
to one of four alternatives
VD ¼ 25; mean age ¼ 78.9; m/ General cognition in AD ¼ VD
f ¼ 9/16; mean MMSE ¼ 14.4.
Visuoperception in AD ¼ VD
No affect task correlated with
MMSE in AD
All affect tasks correlated with
MMSE in VD
Relationship between cognitive
and emotion deficits differ
according to dementia type
Shimokawa et al. (6) AD ¼ 38; mean age ¼ 79.5; m/ Line drawings: happy, sad, anger, Same as Shimokawa et al. (61) Same as Shimokawa et al. (61) Impaired emotion recognition
f ¼ 12/26; MMSE ¼ 15.4 surprise correlated with impaired behaviour
Impaired emotion recognition not
correlated with cognition
Emotion recognition deficits, not
cognitive deficits, influence
impaired behaviour
Hargrave et al. (62)* AD ¼ 22; mean age ¼ 74; m/ JACFEE colour photos: posed; Identification: instructions – select Identity discrimination: indicate Identification in AD,HC¼PC
f ¼ 12/10; mean MMSE ¼ 18.5 FACS coded. Happy, sad, anger, emotion label that reference face whether same or different person
fear, surprise, disgust depicts; verbal or point response (Benton Facial Recognition Test)
to seven option printed below

241
Facial expression recognition in Alzheimer’s disease
Table 1. Continued

242
Author Participants Stimuli and emotions Tasks Control tasks Results

HC ¼ 14; mean age ¼ 68; m/ Matching: instructions – match Matching and discrimination in
f ¼ 4/10; mean MMSE ¼ 29.1 reference face with one of six AD ¼ PC,HC
alternatives; verbal or point
McLellan et al.

response to another view of


reference emotion & five different
people distracters
PC ¼ 10; mean age ¼ 70; m/ Discrimination: instructions – state Impairment in facial emotion
f ¼ 9/1; mean MMSE ¼ 28.6 whether depicting same or processing independent of deficit
different emotion; verbal response in facial processing
to pairs of different people
HC ¼ 14; mean age ¼ 68; m/ Selective identification impairment
f ¼ 4/10; mean MMSE ¼ 29.1. for sad
Shimokawa et al. (63) AD ¼ 16; mean age ¼ 81.8; m/ Line drawings and Ekman photos: Same as Shimokawa et al. (61) Same as Shimokawa et al. (61) Total affect tasks in AD.VD
f ¼ 4/12; mean MMSE ¼ 12.9 happy, sad, anger, surprise with two additional but similar
tasks.
VD ¼ 15; mean age 78.3; m/ No affect task correlated with
f ¼ 3/12; mean MMSE ¼ 13.1. MMSE in AD
All affect tasks correlated with
MMSE in VD
Relationship between cognitive
and recognition of facial
expression deficits differ according
to dementia type
Bucks and Radford (64)* AD ¼ 12; mean age ¼ 75.5; m/ FAB: happy, sad, anger, fear, Identification: instructions – name Identity discrimination: indicate Selection in AD,HC
f ¼ 4/8; mean MMSE ¼ 18.8 neutral emotion expressed; verbal whether same or different person
response; verbal list options
HC ¼ 12; mean age ¼ 74.4; m/ Selection: instructions – select Other facial affect tasks in
f ¼ 5/7; mean MMSE ¼ 28 from five alternatives the AD ¼ HC
expression that matched verbal
label; point response from five
different faces and expressions
Matching: instructions – select Ability preserved relative to
one of five bearing same general cognitive ability
expression as stimulus face; point
response from five different faces
Discrimination: instructions – Poor selection performance may
indicate whether expressions are be due to cognitive load
same or different; verbal response associated with this more difficult
to pairs of different people task
Burnham and Hogervorst (65) AD ¼ 13; mean age ¼ 76; m/ Ekman photos: happy, sad, anger Identification: instructions – say or Identification in AD ¼ HC
f ¼ 8/5; mean MMSE ¼ 21 fear, surprise, disgust point to label that best describes
the expression; point/verbal
response to one of six options
on card
Table 1. Continued

Author Participants Stimuli and emotions Tasks Control tasks Results

HC ¼ 13; mean age ¼ 73; m/ Matching: instructions – say or Matching fear, sad and happy in
f ¼ 10/3; mean MMSE ¼ 29 point to face showing the same AD,HC
expression; point/verbal response
to another example of same
expression and three other
expressions
Impairment may be due to
visuospatial dysfunction
Fernandez-Duque and Black AD ¼ 9; mean age ¼ 70.1; m/ Ekman photos through touch Identification: instructions – How Sex discrimination: Indicate Identification in AD ¼ HC.FTD
(66)* f ¼ 5/4; mean MMSE ¼ 24.8 screen computer: happy, sad, is s/he feeling? Is s/he .; point whether same or different sex
anger, fear, surprise, disgust response to one of seven labels on (congruent and incongruent sex/
same screen expression)
HC ¼ 10; mean age ¼ 65.1; m/ Discrimination: instructions – Discrimination in AD ¼ HC.FTD
f ¼ 4/6; mean MMSE ¼ 29 indicate whether expressions are
same or different; verbal response
to pairs of different people (half
same/different sex)
Sex discrimination in
AD ¼ FTD,HC
No impairment in AD found overall
or for any specific expression
Kohler et al. (67) AD ¼ 20; mean age ¼ 75.9; m/ Genuine expressions of emotion: Identification Identification in AD,HC
f ¼ 11/9; mean MMSE ¼ 22.7 happy, sad, anger, fear, neutral
HC ¼ 22; mean age ¼ 69.4; m/ Identify intensity Accounted for by general cognition
f ¼ 9/13; mean MMSE ¼ 29.5
Differentiate intensity Impaired at recognising both
happy and sad when an intensity
judgment was added to the task
No information regarding Impaired at sad intensity
procedure differentiation

BNT, Boston naming test; CERAD, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's disease; DRS, Dementia Rating Scale; FAB, Florida Affect Battery; FACS, Facial Action Coding System; HC, healthy control; JACFEE, Japanese and
Caucasian Facial Expressions of Emotion; LS, low spatial patients; LV, low verbal patients; m/f, male/female; VD, vascular dementia; WAIS-R, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised.
*Studies that were included in the review of findings.

243
Facial expression recognition in Alzheimer’s disease
McLellan et al.

and only one study matched for anxiety at the the former option surely equating to a more
group level (62). No studies appear to have adopted difficult task, especially for cognitively vulnerable
a more stringent case matching design with regard to individuals with AD.
these factors; likewise, participant groups were not Variable terminology, such as naming, identifi-
matched on premorbid IQ. cation, detecting, processing, matching, selection,
discrimination and differentiation, has been used
to describe what essentially the three most frequent
Experimental tasks and stimuli
procedures are: emotion identification, emotion
Still photographs from the Ekman and Friesen discrimination and emotion matching. Different
ÔPictures of Facial Affect’ series have been used by procedures engage the participant in different ways
the majority of studies (55, 57, 59, 60, 62, 63, 65, and require different cognitive skills. For instance,
66). Others have used Izard photographs (54) or the participant must access semantically meaning-
The Florida Affect Battery (56,57), with facial ful information about each of the alternative res-
affect subtests consisting of black and white ponse options and apply this to a single exemplar
photographs of female actors depicting emotional in emotion identification tasks. When only verbal
expressions. Line drawings of cartoon-like facial response options are provided, the participant must
expressions have also been used in studies attempt- also remember each option.
ing to assess facial expression recognition (6,63). In contrast, discrimination tasks require the
The only dynamic stimuli were vignettes generated assessment of two stimuli but can be completed
by actors instructed to Ôact out’ specific emotional by visuoperceptual comparisons that may have
scenarios (58). Only one study (67) employed facial little to do with access or understanding of emo-
stimuli that had been generated as a result of tional information and more to do with config-
evoked, that is, felt emotional experience. urations of visual stimuli. Emotion matching tasks,
Initial interpretation across studies is made in further contrast, require the participant to scan
difficult given the stimuli vary according to several photographs and retain defining infor-
ecological validity and likely intensity. The infor- mation about each, so a match to the target can
mation available in line drawings, for instance, is be made. Visuoperceptual information devoid of
very different to that in grey-scale photographs and emotional content can also be used to complete
again in colour photographs. Consequently, stud- this task. Table 1 presents a summary of the spe-
ies that employed line drawings (6,63) will not cific tasks used in each of the initial studies in-
contribute to the final review of findings because of cluding the main findings.
their poor ecological validity. Similarly, the infor- Although there are several methodological
mation available in posed/deliberate expressions inconsistencies across studies, the present review
and genuine/spontaneous expressions is likely to has established exclusion criteria to remove studies
differ with regard to symmetry (74–78), specific in which a significant age difference was found
muscle contractions/combinations (78–80) and temp- between healthy elderly controls and AD partic-
oral characteristics (81,82), all of these factors are ipants. As discussed earlier, age has been shown to
associated with meaningful recognition of facial influence the ability to recognise facial expressions;
expressions. Given the vast majority of studies, accordingly, age matching is necessary to eliminate
employing posed/deliberate facial stimuli within re- age as a possible explanation for any deficits found
cognition assessment in general, let alone within in AD. It is also important to control for face-
the limited present scope, is not practical to address processing deficits, and although most studies
the limitation by applying this as exclusion criteria. report no diagnosed problems with prosopagnosia
Facial expressions of happiness, sadness and per se in their samples, studies that did not include
anger were examined in all studies. Table 1 shows a face-processing control task will not contribute
that seven studies included facial expressions of towards the review of findings.
fear and surprise. Only four studies looked at the Of the 15 studies that met the initial search
recognition of facial expressions of disgust and criteria, two were excluded because they did not
only six studies included neutral expressions in compare AD participants to healthy elderly con-
their design. A self-paced unlimited presentation of trols. Two further studies were removed because
stimuli material was used in testing tasks across HC were significantly younger than AD partic-
studies with the exception of a limited (30 s) ipants were, and another five studies did not meet
exposure identification task during eye tracking our criteria of including a face-processing task to
(60). As can be seen in Table 1, the response control for problems processing facial information
options often differed with the requirements of not specific to emotion. What follows is a summary
a verbal, point or key press forced choice format, of results from the remaining six studies pertaining

244
Facial expression recognition in Alzheimer’s disease

to the application of respective identification, requirement as the expression discrimination task


discrimination and matching tasks. and found the significant impairment in the AD
group remained. The poorer emotion discrimina-
tion performance was accounted for by the identity
Review of findings control task in the remaining study (55).
Identification of facial expressions
Tasks defined as identification tasks involved the Matching and selecting facial expressions
presentation of a single photo whereby the partici- Matching and selecting tasks require the participant
pant is required to choose which emotion label best to match a target expression to one of several
accounts for the expression shown. alternative expressions and select a target expression
Each of the six studies investigated the ability of from several alternatives, respectively.
AD patients to identify expressions. No significant Four selection and/or matching tasks were
difference in performance was found between AD performed in three studies (55,62,64). As with the
and HC in two studies (60,64). Of the four studies discrimination task, the poor performance in
that did find that AD patients performed signifi- matching and selecting was accounted for by an
cantly worse than the HC group, only two attribute identity control task in the first study (55). AD
this impaired performance to a specific emotion- participants did not have any difficulty with the
processing deficit (54,62). The remaining two matching task in the second study (64) but did
studies either attribute poor performance to verbal show impairment in the selection task. The authors
deficits because the task required linguistic oper- concluded that the more difficult selection pro-
ations (62) or difficulty with verbal memory cedure of scanning and identifying the emotion
because group differences that were not accounted present in five alternative expressions might have
for by either an identity discrimination control task resulted in the poor performance, particularly as
or naming ability were eliminated when the this was the only task in their study to show deficits
contribution of verbal memory was assessed (55). compared with HC. AD patients in the final study
Unfortunately, in the former study, an effective (62) did not appear to have the same difficulty with
measure of verbal ability was neither obtained nor the matching task as they had with both the
controlled. The conclusion reached in the later identification and discrimination tasks. While a
study is also difficult to evaluate without an poorer performance was found, the group differ-
appropriate control task. Participants did have to ence was only marginally significant once the
recruit verbal memory skills to respond to the scores on the control task were controlled for.
verbal list of response options provided at each
trial, but the simple Ôyes/no’ identity discrimination
task used does not account for the verbal memory Performance with specific emotions
skills required to complete the verbally more All the reviewed studies used expressions depicting
complex identification task. Whether this indicates several of the basic emotions as task stimuli in their
that poor performance was secondary to verbal respective emotion-processing tasks. Only three
memory skill is unclear. It may well be that both studies, however, considered the performance of
verbal memory and expression recognition are groups on specific emotions separately (62,64,66).
effected by AD pathology. Of these three, only one established the relative
performance score between emotions and found
Discrimination of facial expressions a deficit with regard to sad expressions relative to
the other misidentified expressions of surprise and
A discrimination task requires the participant to disgust (62). A potential deficit found with happy
look at pairs of photographs and indicate whether expressions (64) is likely because of a ceiling effect
the expressions shown are the same or different. as HC performed happy discriminations without
Emotion processing was assessed by discrimina- error. No deficit was found overall or for any
tion tasks in five studies; three of which found no particular expression in the third study (66).
impaired performance of AD compared with HC
(57,64,66). Two found that AD participants per-
formed significantly worse than controls (55,62)
Summary of past findings
and only one (62) concluded this deficit was
because of a specific emotion-processing impair- In summary, two studies identified consistent
ment. This study covaried an identity discrimina- problems and found AD participants were im-
tion score on the basis that it had a similar verbal paired relative to controls in all three tasks (55,62),

245
McLellan et al.

although only the later study concluded that this What conclusions, if any, can we make with
was because of a specific emotion-processing regard to our three questions? Firstly, do AD
deficit. The same conclusion was reached by patients have an impaired ability to recognise facial
another study (54), although they only assessed expressions of emotion? AD patients do demon-
skills using an identification task. A fourth study strate poorer performance in facial expression
consistently found that AD participants were not recognition, with 61% of recognition tasks per-
impaired as evidenced by performances similar to formed poorly compared with healthy aged-
HC in all tasks (66). The remaining two studies matched controls (Table 2). Secondly, whether this
produced inconsistent findings across tasks, one performance reflects a specific emotion-processing
demonstrated that individuals with AD had impairment is less clear, although 45% of tasks
difficulty with facial expression identification but continued to show group differences independent
not with the discrimination of expressions (57) and of face processing or specific cognitive abilities.
the other found the ability to process facial Lastly, information relating to deficits in specific
expressions was preserved except when assessed emotions could only be garnered from three studies
using a selection task (64). and no consistent impairment was found, although
A comparison across these six studies is difficult it is noteworthy that sad expressions were proble-
because of a couple of methodological inconsis- matic for AD participants relative to the difficulty
tencies, specifically, the response formats chosen they had even with other problematic express-
by respective studies contained either a list of ions (62).
verbally presented response options or a list of The limited number of studies from which to
printed response options, the former option draw conclusions or rather the high number of
equating to a higher level of task difficulty. For studies that were removed from consideration pro-
instance, of the four studies that found impaired mpted the identification of two particular limita-
identification performance in AD, the two that tions in the existing literature. The following section
minimised the requirements of verbal memory by discusses these two limitations as possible avenues
providing printed response options concluded that to extend the current literature and, therefore,
deficits exist in emotion processing independent of establish a better position within which to address
the decline in cognitive skills. In contrast, the two our three questions in the future.
studies that relied on verbal response options both
concluded that observed difficulties were because
Control tasks and task difficulty
of verbal deficits associated with AD, although as
stated above, this was not assessed adequately. It is The most frequent control task used to account for
also noteworthy that the AD participants in the general perception of faces was an identity dis-
two studies that did not find any impaired per- crimination task, which simply required partici-
formance across any of the tasks (64,66) had the pants to indicate whether the photographs of
mildest AD sample (according to MMSE scores) as people were the same or different. While useful in
well as the smallest sample size (n ¼ 12 and 9, terms of expression discrimination analyses, the
respectively), both factors potentially impacted on task is quite dissimilar to identification tasks.
the ability to find meaningful group differences. Several studies showed differences in performance
The studies reviewed for the present paper have between identification and discrimination tasks
largely set about to determine if people with AD and suggest that different abilities underlie the
have an impairment in emotion processing, that is, successful completion of each. It seems that
does AD affect the ability to perceive the affective researchers may need to administer additional
state of another person. To do this, the ability to control tasks. This is particularly relevant to
recognise facial expressions of emotion has been clinical samples such as AD groups as the cognitive
investigated in individuals with AD. Given the skills required to complete identity discrimination
overlap between the neural structures affected by
AD pathology and the areas thought to be Table 2. Percent of studies showing a deficit in recognition of emotion tasks
implicated in emotional processing, it would have
appeared reasonable to hypothesise that such %AD,HC after
Task %AD ¼ HC %AD,HC control*
specific deficits would be shown. While the studies
often employed very similar identification, discrim- Identification (n ¼ 6) 33 67 50
ination and matching tasks, several methodological Matching/selection (n ¼ 4) 25 75 50
Discrimination (n ¼ 5) 60 40 40
limitations and inconsistencies have resulted in
Total 39 61 45
only a limited number of studies from which to
draw conclusions. *Facial identity task or cognitive decline

246
Facial expression recognition in Alzheimer’s disease

tasks may not account for subtle deficits in perceiver to respond in an appropriate or advan-
cognition that may hinder the successful comple- tageous manner. It follows, therefore, that such
tion of emotion identification tasks. a subsequent behaviour would be of interest to
A more suitable control for expression identi- flesh out more fully the ability to accurately
fication tasks would comprise the individual pre- perceive facial emotion.
sentation of non-emotional facial information
and require the participant to respond to non-
Facial displays of emotion
emotional response options that closely mirror the
emotional counterpart (i.e. choose from a list of six It may be that two separate questions are potentially
alternative labels). That is, processing faces to being asked when investigating the recognition of
identify, through a labelling response, some other facial expressions of emotion. (a) Can participants
aspect of person not related to emotion. An age- recognise facial expressions? (b) Can participants
band labelling/identification task may suffice as recognise facial emotion? If the aim of an investi-
a suitable control task. gation is to answer a question relating to whether
The majority of research investigating recognition participants have or preserve the ability to detect the
of facial expressions of emotion has employed the affective state of another person through their facial
three main types of experimental tasks. Often these expression then the expressions from which they are
tasks require skills that place a cognitively vulner- asked to make judgments should contain informa-
able participant under a substantial working load. tion relevant to how the target feels. Acted or posed
The identification tasks, for instance, may require expressions are Ôrepresentative of’ not Ôevidence of’
the participant to remember the verbal options emotion and as such provide different information
provided, while matching and selection tasks require to the social perceiver. Several studies have shown
that the participant scan several photographs and that perceivers are sensitive to the differences
make several identifications and discriminations. In between posed and genuine expressions (82,83)
addition, while these procedures require a variety of and that this sensitivity then has an impact on
cognitive skills that arguably overshadow or even subsequent behaviour (84). Indeed, individuals with
interfere with emotion recognition, they also suffer paranoid schizophrenia have shown deficits in the
from poor face validity in respect to the reference recognition of posed expressions that are not
situation of an actual social interaction. Researchers evident in the recognition of genuine expressions
may well benefit from designing laboratory experi- (85). The neural structures or pathways innervated
ments that more closely capture the everyday during the fundamental detection of felt emotion
exposure to facial emotion. might differ from the neural recruitment necessary
The association between expression recognition for the perception of configurations that represent
and general cognitive ability (as measured by emotion.
MMSE) is also unclear. A significant relationship Only one of the initial studies (67) addressed
was found between cognitive ability and expression affective state adequately by using genuine expres-
identification (57,58) but not with expression dis- sions of felt emotion. The other studies reviewed
crimination (57). Although present with regard to have used facial expressions potentially unrelated
vascular dementia patients (63), three particular to emotional experience. The terms Ôemotion’ and
studies showed no relationship between cognition Ôexpression’ are often used as if synonymous, not
and emotion recognition in AD patients (6,61,67). only in general discussion but also in various pro-
The former found everyday behaviour however cedures. In three studies, for instance (58,60,66),
was associated with emotion recognition. The the participant was asked how the person was
variation in the relationship between MMSE as feeling and given acted displays where the relevant
a measure of general cognition and facial expres- information was simply not present. The partici-
sion recognition scores suggests that this is not pant can answer what this person was showing or
a straightforward relationship. It appears other what emotion is being represented, but they are
factors may be pertinent that research has not unable to answer how the person is feeling when
accounted for. the facial expression provided has been decoupled
The relationship found between behaviour and from the affective state. In addition to imposing
emotion recognition (6) together with the dissoci- potential confusion in the judgement process,
ation found with cognition further serves to remind such a reliance on posed displays may also dilute
researchers of the importance of investigating the area of investigation into a means to examine
potential impact on subsequent behaviour. As the recognition of facial expressions rather than
previously noted, the ability to recognise the an attempt to examine the perception of affec-
affective state of another person allows the tive state.

247
McLellan et al.

Conclusion have difficulty in accurately perceiving and pro-


cessing the facial expressions of emotion.
Despite the existing research, questions remain as
to whether AD patients do have impairments in
their ability to accurately decode facial expressions
of emotion. References
It is also somewhat unclear whether any such
1. HODGES JR. Alzheimer’s centennial legacy: origins, land-
impairment reflects the general cognitive decline marks and the current status of knowledge concerning
and/or verbal or spatial deficits associated with cognitive aspects. Brain 2006;129:2811–2822.
AD or whether it is indicative of pathology aff- 2. CHIU MJ, CHEN TF, YIP PK, HUA MS, TANG LY.
ecting specific emotion-processing mechanisms. Behavioral and psychologic symptoms in different types
Research looking at the differences in affect of dementia. J Formos Med Assoc 2006;105:556–562.
3. BUHR GT, WHITE HK. Difficult behaviors in long-term
perception between AD and fronto-temporal care patients with dementia. J Am Med Dir Assoc
dementia (FTD), for instance, cite comparable 2006;7:180–192.
levels of cognitive decline yet show more impair- 4. TARIOT PN, MACK JL, PATTERSON MB et al. The Behavior
ment in expression recognition by participants with Rating Scale for Dementia of the Consortium to establish
FTD (68). Likewise, within AD, comparable a registry for Alzheimer’s disease. The American Journal
of Psychiatry 1995;152:1349
cognitive status but incomparable emotion recog- 5. STEELE C, ROVNER B, CHASE GA, FOLSTEIN M. Psychiatric
nition was found and cognitively comparable symptoms and nursing home placement of patients with
control and affect tasks were negotiated with Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 1990;147:1049
disparate success. Certainly, no clear relationship 6. SHIMOKAWA A, YATOMI N, ANAMIZU S et al. Influence of
between traditionally measured cognitive status deteriorating ability of emotional comprehension on
interpersonal behavior in Alzheimer-type dementia. Brain
and traditionally measured affect recognition skill Cogn 2001;47:423–33.
can be concluded. 7. EKMAN P, FRIESEN WV. Constants across cultures in the
Similarly, there is no clear evidence for a specific face and emotion. J Pers Soc Psychol 1971;17:124–129.
emotion-processing deficit, independent of per- 8. EKMAN P. Universal facial expressions in emotion. Studia
ceptual skills non-exclusive to facial affect recog- Psychologica 1973;15:140–147.
9. PHILLIPS ML, DREVETS WC, RAUCH SL, LANE R.
nition. Individuals with AD did not appear to have Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis
difficulty with any particular expression compared of normal emotion perception. Biol Psychiatry 2003;
with healthy age-matched controls, as no particu- 54:504–514.
lar impairment was replicated within the remaining 10. CALDER AJ, KEANE J, MANLY T et al. Facial expression
reviewed studies. The difficulty posed by sad recognition across the adult life span. Neuropsychologia
2003;41:195–202.
expressions, however, was found to result in poorer 11. SURGULADZE SA, YOUNG AW, SENIOR C, BREBION G,
performance relative to even the other expressions TRAVIS MJ, PHILLIPS ML. Recognition accuracy and
that were not recognised well. As discussed earlier, response bias to happy and sad facial expressions in
sad facial expressions are often problematic for patients with major depression. Neuropsychology
healthy elderly, therefore, a potential deficit over 2004;18:212–218.
12. SULLIVAN S, RUFFMAN T. Emotion recognition deficits in
and above a vulnerable baseline is worthy of par- the elderly. Int J Neurosci 2004;114:403–432.
ticular attention. 13. KEIGHTLEY ML, WINOCUR G, BURIANOVA H, HONGWANISHKUL
To investigate AD and the perception of facial D, GRADY CL. Age effects on social cognition: faces tell
expressions and consequently the detection of a different story. Psychol Aging 2006;21:558–572.
affective state in another, future research needs to 14. SUZUKI A, HOSHINO T, SHIGEMASU K, KAWAMURA M.
Decline or improvement? Age-related differences in facial
adopt a more systematic approach. Firstly, the expression recognition. Biol Psychol 2007;74:75–84.
distinction between facial expressions and facial 15. PHILLIPS LH, MACLEAN RDJ, ALLEN R. Age and the
expressions of actual emotion needs to be acknowl- understanding of emotions: neuropsychological and socio-
edged and secondly, tasks that more closely cognitive perspectives. J Gerontol 2002;57B:P526.
approximate even basic social interaction should 16. MACPHERSON SE, PHILLIPS LH, DELLA SALA S. Age,
executive function, and social decision making: a dorsolat-
be employed, with an eye to whether and how this eral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychol Aging
might affect subsequent behaviour and the social 2002;17:598–609.
functioning of people with AD. Due diligence to 17. ISAACOWITZ DM, LOCKENHOFF CE, LANE RD et al. Age
such methodological considerations may help shed differences in recognition of emotion in lexical stimuli and
light on the cause or causes of emotion-processing facial expressions. Psychol Aging 2007;22:147–159.
18. SULLIVAN S, RUFFMAN T, HUTTON SB. Age differences in
deficits, whether they be cognitive or specifically emotion recognition skills and the visual scanning of
emotional in nature, as well as promote the need to emotion faces. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2007;
emphasise the consequences to individuals who 62:P53–P60.

248
Facial expression recognition in Alzheimer’s disease

19. ADOLPHS R, TRANEL D, DAMASIO AR. Dissociable neural 40. PAUL R, GRIEVE SM, CHAUDARY B et al. Relative
systems for recognizing emotions. Brain Cogn 2003; contributions of the cerebellar vermis and prefrontal lobe
52:61–69. volumes on cognitive function across the adult lifespan.
20. PHILLIPS ML, DREVETS WC, RAUCH SL, LANE R. Neurobiol Aging 2007 (Epub ahead of print). doi: 10.1016/
Neurobiology of emotion perception II: implications for j.neurobiolaging.2007.07.017
major psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2003;54:515–528. 41. TISSERAND DJ, JOLLES J. On the involvement of
21. BENUZZI F, MELETTI S, ZAMBONI G et al. Impaired fear prefrontal networks in cognitive ageing. Cortex
processing in right mesial temporal sclerosis: a fMRI 2003;39:1107–1128.
study. Brain Research Bulletin 2004;63:269–281. 42. RAJAH MN, D’ESPOSITO M. Region-specific changes in
22. BISEUL I, SAULEAU P, HAEGELEN C et al. Fear recognition is prefrontal function with age: a review of PET and fMRI
impaired by subthalamic nucleus stimulation in Parkin- studies on working and episodic memory. Brain
son’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2005;43:1054–1059. 2005;128:1964–1983.
23. ADOLPHS R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr 43. TISSERAND DJ, PRUESSNER JC, SANZ ARIGITA EJ et al.
Opin Neurobiol 2002;12:169–177. Regional frontal cortical volumes decrease differentially in
24. HABEL U, WINDISCHBERGER C, DERNTL B et al. Amygdala aging: an MRI study to compare volumetric approaches and
activation and facial expressions: explicit emotion discrim- voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 2002;17:657–669.
ination versus implicit emotion processing. Neuropsycho- 44. BURKE SN, BARNES CA. Neural plasticity in the ageing
logia 2007;45:2369–2377. brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2006;7:30–40.
25. VUILLEUMIER P, POURTOIS G. Distributed and interactive 45. GUTCHESS AH, WELSH RC, HEDDEN T et al. Aging and the
brain mechanisms during emotion face perception: evi- neural correlates of successful picture encoding: frontal
dence from functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia activations compensate for decreased medial-temporal
2007;45:174–194. activity. J Cogn Neurosci 2005;17:84–96.
26. HAXBY JV, HOFFMAN EA, GOBBINI MI. Human neural 46. POULIN P, ZAKZANIS KK. In vivo neuroanatomy of
systems for face recognition and social communication. Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from structural and func-
Biol Psychiatry 2002;51:59–67. tional brain imaging. Brain Cogn 2002;49:220–225.
27. WINSTON JS, O’DOHERTY J, DOLAN RJ. Common and 47. WENK GL. Neuropathologic changes in Alzheimer’s
distinct neural responses during direct and incidental disease. J Clin Psychiatry 2003;64(Suppl. 9):7–10.
processing of multiple facial emotions. Neuroimage 48. BOCCARDI M, LAAKSO MP, BRESCIANI L et al. The MRI
2003;20:84–97. pattern of frontal and temporal brain atrophy in fronto-
28. PHAN KL, WAGER T, TAYLOR SF, LIBERZON I. Functional temporal dementia. Neurobiol Aging 2003;24:95–103.
neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion 49. ROSEN HJ, WILSON MR, SCHAUER GF et al. Neuroana-
activation studies in PET and fMRI*1. Neuroimage tomical correlates of impaired recognition of emotion in
2002;16:331–348. dementia. Brain 2005;28:2612–2625.
29. NORRIS CJ, CHEN EE, ZHU DC, SMALL SL, CACIOPPO JT. 50. ADOLPHS R, SPEZIO M. Role of the amygdala in processing
The interaction of social and emotional processes in the visual social stimuli. Prog Brain Res 2006;156:363–378.
brain. J Cogn Neurosci 2004;16:1818–1829. 51. BLAIR RJ, MORRIS JS, FRITH CD, PERRETT DI, DOLAN RJ.
30. SIMON D, CRAIG KD, MILTNER WHR, RAINVILLE P. Brain Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of
responses to dynamic facial expressions of pain. Pain sadness and anger. Brain 1999;122(Pt 5):883–893.
2006;126:309–318. 52. HOLT DJ, WEISS AP, RAUCH SL et al. Sustained activation
31. BRITTON JC, TAYLOR SF, SUDHEIMER KD, LIBERZON I. of the hippocampus in response to fearful faces in
Facial expressions and complex IAPS pictures: common schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:1011–1019.
and differential networks. Neuroimage 2006;31:906–919. 53. REINDERS AA, GLASCHER J, DE JONG JR, WILLEMSEN AT,
32. SPRENGELMEYER R, JENTZSCH I. Event related potentials den BOER JA, BUCHEL C. Detecting fearful and neutral
and the perception of intensity in facial expressions. faces: BOLD latency differences in amygdala-hippocampal
Neuropsychologia 2006;44:2899–2906. junction. Neuroimage 2006;33:805–814.
33. BURGDORF J, PANKSEPP J. The neurobiology of positive 54. ALLENDER J, KASZNIAK AW. Processing of emotional cues
emotions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006;30:173–187. in patients with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Int J
34. LEPPANEN JM. Emotional information processing in mood Neurosci 1989;46:147–155.
disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging 55. ALBERT MS, COHEN C, KOFF E. Perception of affect in
findings. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2006;19:34–39. patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol
35. WHALEN PJ, RAUCH SL, ETCOFF NL, MCINERNEY SC, 1991;48:791–795.
LEE MB, JENIKE MA. Masked presentations of emotional 56. CADIEUX NL, GREVE KW. Emotion processing in Alz-
facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without heimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1997;3:411–419.
explicit knowledge. J Neurosc 1998;18:411–418. 57. ROUDIER M, MARCIE P, GRANCHER AS, TZORTZIS C,
36. PHILLIPS ML, WILLIAMS LM, HEINING M et al. Different- STARKSTEIN S, BOLLER F. Discrimination of facial identity
ial neural responses to overt and covert presentations of and of emotions in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Sci
facial expressions of fear and disgust. Neuroimage 2004;21: 1998;154:151–158.
1484–1496. 58. KOFF E, ZAITCHIK D, MONTEPARE J, ALBERT MS. Emotion
37. SPRENGELMEYER R, SCHROEDER U, YOUNG AW, EPPLEN JT. processing in the visual and auditory domains by patients
Disgust in pre-clinical Huntington’s disease: a longitudinal with Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1999;
study. Neuropsychologia 2006;44:518–533. 5:32–40.
38. ALLEN G, BARNARD H, MCCOLL R et al. Reduced 59. LAVENU I, PASQUIER F, LEBERT F, PETIT H, VAN DER
hippocampal functional connectivity in Alzheimer disease. LINDEN M. Perception of emotion in frontotemporal
Arch Neurol 2007;64:1482–1487. dementia and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc
39. GRIEVE SM, CLARK CR, WILLIAMS LM, PEDUTO AJ, Disord 1999;13:96–101.
GORDON E. Preservation of limbic and paralimbic struc- 60. OGROCKI PK, HILLS AC, STRAUSS ME. Visual exploration
tures in aging. Hum Brain Mapp 2005;25:391–401. of facial emotion by healthy older adults and patients with

249
McLellan et al.

Alzheimer disease. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav population: the Sydney Older Persons Study. Int J Geriatr
Neurol 2000;13:271–278. Psychiatry 2001;16:680–689.
61. SHIMOKAWA A, YATOMI N, ANAMIZU S et al. Comprehen- 74. SKINNER M, MULLEN B. Facial asymmetry in emotional
sion of emotions: comparison between Alzheimer type and expression: a meta-analysis of research. Br J Soc Psychol
vascular type dementias. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 1991;30:113–124.
2000;11:268–274. 75. RINN WE. The neuropsychology of facial expression: a
62. HARGRAVE R, MADDOCK RJ, STONE V. Impaired recogni- review of the neurological and psychological mechanisms for
tion of facial expressions of emotion in Alzheimer’s producing facial expressions. Psychol Bull 1984;95:52–77.
disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002;14:64–71. 76. GOSSELIN P, PERRON M, LEGAULT M, CAMPANELLA P.
63. SHIMOKAWA A, YATOMI N, ANAMIZU S, TORII S, ISONO H, Children’s and adults’ knowledge of the distinction
SUGAI Y. Recognition of facial expressions and emotional between enjoyment and nonenjoyment smiles. J Nonverbal
situations in patients with dementia of the Alzheimer and Behav 2002;26:83–108.
vascular types. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2003;15: 77. BROWN WM, MOORE C. Smile asymmetries and reputation
163–168. as reliable indicators of likelihood to cooperate: An
64. BUCKS RS, RADFORD SA. Emotion processing in Alz- evolutionary analysis. In: SHOHOV SP ed. Advances in
heimer’s disease. Aging Ment Health 2004;8:222–232. psychology research, Vol. 11. Hauppauge: Nova Science
65. BURNHAM H, HOGERVORST E. Recognition of facial Publishers, Inc, 2002;19–36.
expressions of emotion by patients with dementia of 78. EKMAN P, DAVIDSON RJ, FRIESEN WV. The Duchenne
the Alzheimer type. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2004; smile: emotional expression and brain physiology: II. J
18:75–79. Pers Soc Psychol 1990;58:342–353.
66. FERNANDEZ-DUQUE D, BLACK SE. Impaired recognition of 79. EKMAN P, FRIESEN WV, O’SULLIVAN M. Smiles when lying.
negative facial emotions in patients with frontotemporal In: EKMAN P, ROSENBERG EL, eds. What the face reveals:
dementia. Neuropsychologia 2005;43:1673–1687. basic and applied studies of spontaneous expression using
67. KOHLER CG, ANSELMO-GALLAGHER G, BILKER W, the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). Series in
KARLAWISH J, GUR RE, CLARK CM. Emotion-discrimination affective science. New York: Oxford University Press,
deficits in mild Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 1997:201–216.
2005;13:926–933. 80. KOHLER CG, TURNER T, STOLAR NM et al. Differences in
68. LAVENU I, PASQUIER F. Perception of emotion on faces facial expressions of four universal emotions. Psychiatry
in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a Res 2004;128:235–244.
longitudinal study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2005;19: 81. HESS U, KLECK RE. Differentiating emotion elicited and
37–41. deliberate emotional facial expressions. Eur J Soc Psychol
69. FOLSTEIN MF, FOLSTEIN SE, MCHUGH PR. ‘‘Mini-mental 1990;20:369–385.
state’’. A practical guide for grading the cognitive state of 82. FRANK MG, EKMAN P, FRIESEN WV. Behavioral markers
patients for the clinicians. J Psychiatr Res and recognizability of the smile of enjoyment. J Pers Soc
1975;12:189–198. Psychol 1993;64:83–93.
70. MOLLOY DW, STANDISH TI. A guide to the standardized 83. MILES L, JOHNSTON L. Not all smiles are created equal:
Mini-Mental State Examination. Int Psychogeriatr perceiver sensitivity to the differences between posed and
1997;9(Suppl 1):87–94; discussion 143–150. genuine smiles. In: FLINN-FLETCHER CM, HABERMAN GM,
71. AZAD NA, AL BUGAMI M, LOY-ENGLISH I. Gender differ- eds. Cognition and language: perspectives from New Zealand.
ences in dementia risk factors. Gend Med 2007;4:120–129. Queensland: Australian Academic Press, 2006:51–64.
72. HEBERT LE, SCHERR PA, MCCANN JJ, BECKETT LA, EVANS 84. PEACE V, MILES L, JOHNSTON L. It doesn’t matter what you
DA. Is the risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease greater wear: the impact of posed and genuine expressions of
for women than for men? Am J Epidemiol 2001;153: happiness on product evaluation. Soc Cogn 24:137–168.
132–136. 85. DAVIS PJ, GIBSON MG. Recognition of posed and genuine
73. WAITE LM, BROE GA, GRAYSON DA, CREASEY H. The facial expressions of emotion in paranoid and nonparanoid
incidence of dementia in an Australian community schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 2000;109:445–450.

250

You might also like