You are on page 1of 11

Psychoneuroendocrinology (2009) 34, 486—496

a v a i l a b l e a t w w w. s c i e n c e d i r e c t . c o m

j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / p s y n e u e n

REVIEW

Salivary alpha-amylase as a non-invasive biomarker


for the sympathetic nervous system: Current state
of research
U.M. Nater a,*, N. Rohleder b

a
University of Zurich, Institute of Psychology, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Zurich, Switzerland
b
Brandeis University, Dept. of Psychology, Waltham, MA, USA

Received 13 August 2008; received in revised form 23 January 2009; accepted 27 January 2009

KEYWORDS Summary Development of new biomarkers is a constantly evolving field of research endeavor
Alpha-amylase; in psychoneuroendocrinology. Salivary biomarkers have received special attention since they are
Stress; readily accessible and easily obtained. Salivary alpha-amylase (sAA) has been proposed as a
Biomarker; sensitive biomarker for stress-related changes in the body that reflect the activity of the
Sympathetic nervous sympathetic nervous system (SNS), and a growing body of research is accumulating to support
system the validity and reliability of this parameter. However, questions remain to be answered before
sAA can be accepted as an index of SNS activity. This review describes sAA as an emerging
biomarker for stress and provides an overview of the current literature on stress-related
alterations in sAA. It critically discusses how sAA might reflect changes in the autonomic nervous
system. Finally, current and future fields for the application of sAA measurement are outlined.
# 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 487
2. What is salivary alpha-amylase?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 487
2.1. Anatomy and physiology of the salivary glands . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 487
2.1.1. Saliva . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 487
2.1.2. Secretory pathways . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 488
2.2. Pathways of alpha-amylase secretion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 488
2.2.1. Rat studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 488
2.2.2. Human studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 489
2.2.3. Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 489

* Corresponding author at: University of Zurich, Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, Binzmuehlestr. 14/Box 26, 8050 Zurich, Switzerland.
Tel.: +41 44 635 7382; fax: +41 44 635 7359.
E-mail address: u.nater@psychologie.uzh.ch (U.M. Nater).

0306-4530/$ — see front matter # 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.01.014
Salivary alpha-amylase as a biomarker for the SNS 487

3. Stress-induced salivary alpha-amylase secretion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 489


3.1. Early findings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 489
3.2. Effects of psychological stress on salivary alpha-amylase . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 490
3.3. Effects of exercise on salivary alpha-amylase . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 491
3.4. Is salivary alpha-amylase an indicator for the sympathetic nervous system? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 491
3.5. Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 492
4. Applications of salivary alpha-amylase measurement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 492
4.1. Salivary alpha-amylase as an indicator of autonomic dysregulation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 492
4.2. Measurement of salivary alpha-amylase in treatment studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 492
5. Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 492
Acknowledgement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 493
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 493

1. Introduction which one set is glycosylated and the other contains no


carbohydrate. The molecular weight of the glycosylated form
In recent years, salivary measures have become increasingly is about 57,000; that of the non-glycosylated form is about
important in psychoneuroendocrinological research. In the 54,000. sAA accounts for 40% to 50% of the total salivary
early 1970s, Brown suggested that changes in saliva para- gland-produced protein, most of the enzyme being synthe-
meters could be regarded as an ‘‘index of specific states of sized in the parotid gland (80% of the total) (Zakowski and
psychopathology’’ (Brown, 1970, p. 66). While research is not Bruns, 1985; Makinen, 1989). It is a calcium-containing
yet quite at the point to use salivary measures in the way metalloenzyme that hydrolyzes the a-1,4 linkages of starch
Brown suggested, the scientific understanding of various to glucose and maltose. It is known to be mainly involved in
salivary parameters is progressing rapidly. However, apart the initiation of the digestion of starch in the oral cavity.
from the analysis of hormones such as cortisol and DHEA (see, However, sAA has also been shown to have an important
e.g. Vining and McGinley, 1987; see, e.g. Kirschbaum and bacterial interactive function (Scannapieco et al., 1993).
Hellhammer, 1994), few other salivary components have
been taken into consideration as meaningful physiological 2.1. Anatomy and physiology of the salivary
markers in psychoneuroendocrinological research. A specific glands
focus of interest within this field of research is on stress. In
stress research, subjects might be examined either in the In this section, we review the salivary glands and their major
field or in the laboratory. Whichever setting is chosen, valid product, saliva. A clear understanding of how and where
and reliable measures of changes associated with stress must saliva is produced and how this process is controlled is
be applied. Moreover, the simple handling and easy sampling essential for gaining insight into the relationships between
of a stress measure is of utmost importance. A wide array of salivary parameters and stress.
possible parameters indicating stress-related changes has There are three major salivary glands on each side of the
been proposed over the years. Some have disappeared into face: the parotid, submandibular, and sublingual glands. In
oblivion; others have endured for decades and are still being addition, numerous minor glands contribute to salivary out-
used. Since stress is a multi-faceted phenomenon, it requires flow. The salivary glands are part of the digestive tract.
a multidimensional measurement approach. As a conse- Glands are comprised of different types of cells: acinar cells,
quence, research can gain from additions to the canon of various duct system cells, and myoepithelial cells (Humphrey
psychobiological parameters. One parameter that has been and Williamson, 2001).
suggested to reflect stress-related changes in the body is the
salivary enzyme alpha-amylase (Chatterton et al., 1996; 2.1.1. Saliva
Nater, 2004; Rohleder et al., 2004; Granger et al., 2007). The components of saliva are produced primarily by acinar
Salivary alpha-amylase (sAA) release is known to be elicited cells. Saliva is the main product of the salivary glands. It is a
by activation of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) which clear, slightly acidic mucoserous exocrine fluid comprising a
controls the salivary glands. complex mixture of secretions from major and minor salivary
The aim of this review is to assess whether sAA should be glands and gingival crevicular fluid (Humphrey and Williamson,
incorporated in the canon of psychoneuroendocrinological 2001; Kaufman and Lamster, 2002). This mixture of fluids
parameters measuring stress. The accompanying review by derived from different glands is called ‘‘whole saliva’’,
Rohleder and Nater (2009) provides a detailed account of whereas the fluid secreted by single glands is called ‘‘duct
methodological issues that arise when measuring sAA. saliva’’ (Edgar, 1992). The constant flow of saliva from the
mouth into the gut has a protective function. This flushing
effect transfers, for example, food debris and exogenous and
2. What is salivary alpha-amylase? possibly noxious agents into the gut (Tenovuo, 1998). Saliva is
routinely categorized as resting (unstimulated) or stimulated.
sAA (a-1,4-a-D-glucan 4-glucanohydrolase; EC 3.2.1.1) is one Under unstimulated conditions, 20% of saliva is derived from
of the most important enzymes in saliva. The enzyme was the parotid, 65% from the submandibular, and 7—8% from the
first described in saliva by Leuchs in 1831 (Zakowski and sublingual glands. The minor glands contribute about 10% to
Bruns, 1985). It consists of two families of isoenzymes, of whole saliva. Under stimulation, the contributions of each
488 U.M. Nater, N. Rohleder

gland change, with the parotid contributing more than 50% of 1973; Selinger et al., 1973). In a subsequent study, Anderson
total salivary secretions (Sreebny, 2000; Humphrey and Wil- et al. (1984) examined differential contributions of the two
liamson, 2001). branches of the ANS to sAA secretion. They found that
sympathetic stimulation in unconscious rats led to the secre-
2.1.2. Secretory pathways tion of parotid saliva characterized by low salivary flow rate
For a detailed understanding of the secretory pathways of the and high total protein and amylase concentrations. In con-
salivary glands, a distinction must be drawn between neural trast, parasympathetic stimulation induced a rich flow of
and cellular mechanisms of secretion. This section provides a saliva with low protein content, with mean concentrations
detailed account of these pathways. of sAA approximately 1% of those measured in sympatheti-
Acinar cells are innervated by both the sympathetic and cally stimulated saliva. After performing adrenalectomy,
the parasympathetic branches of the ANS (Emmelin, 1987). the authors observed significant reductions of sAA levels
Autonomic nerves are adjacent to both acinar and ductal upon parasympathetic stimulation of the parotid gland,
cells. The afferent pathways for taste are via the facial and suggesting that circulating catecholamines (originating from
glossopharyngeal nerves to a solitary nucleus in the medulla. the adrenals) might play a specific role in sAA secretion.
There is also input from higher centers in response to smell, Asking (1985) set out to compare sympathetic and parasym-
sight, etc. The parasympathetic efferent pathways for the pathetic stimulation of the parotid gland in the rat both
sublingual and submandibular glands are from the facial separately and concomitantly. As in the Anderson et al.
nerve via the submandibular ganglion; for the parotid gland study, sympathetic stimulation caused a low flow of saliva
they are from the glossopharyngeal nerve via the otic gang- containing amylase in very high concentration, whereas
lion. The sympathetic postganglionic pathways are from the parasympathetic stimulation produced saliva with a low
cervical ganglion of the sympathetic chain. Neurotransmit- concentration of amylase. After combined sympathetic
ters are the first messengers in the communication pathway and parasympathetic stimulation, however, sAA secretion
between nerves and secretion. Neurotransmitters exert their was much higher than the sum of the two separate stimula-
activity at the cell membrane; they communicate with intra- tion concentrations. Using the beta-1-antagonist pafenolol,
cellular second messengers that have direct control of secre- Asking (1985) showed that the higher sAA secretion due to
tory processes (Smith, 1996). Released in response to sympathetic stimulation superimposed on parasympathetic
secretory stimuli, they bind to specific receptor proteins background stimulation was elicited via beta-1-adrenocep-
on the basolateral membrane, causing acute elevation of tors. In a subsequent study (Asking and Gjorstrup, 1987),
intracellular Ca. This results in large-scale fluid and electro- these findings were replicated in that sympathetic and
lyte transport, and modest exocytosis of stored protein. parasympathetic stimulation led to equal sAA concentra-
Norepinephrine from sympathetic neurons binds to both tions and the combination of the two drastically increased
alpha- and beta-adrenergic receptors on the acinar cell. sAA concentrations. The authors found that sAA secreted on
Alpha-receptor activation is linked to elevation of intracel- sympathetic activation consisted of pre-formed amylase,
lular Ca, while beta-receptor activation causes elevation of stored in granules, that was not replaced during secretory
intracellular cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP), which activity under these conditions. Ongoing sympathetic sti-
is linked to the secretion of salivary proteins that are stored mulation led to an attenuation of sAA concentration. Asking
in membrane-bound secretory granules (Baum, 1993; Castle and Proctor (1989) studied the effects of prolonged para-
and Castle, 1998). sympathetic nerve stimulation in rat parotid glands on
The secretory process of salivary proteins may be divided alpha-amylase content in saliva and glands. The concentra-
into the three stages: (1) synthesis, (2) packaging and sto- tion and total output of sAA were measured throughout a
rage, and (3) release. Each of these stages is regulated by stimulatory period of 120 min, as were glandular concentra-
phosphorylation of target proteins brought about by a protein tions of sAA. The results suggest that amylase stores are
kinase such as cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase (protein much more rapidly replenished by synthesis during para-
kinase A) (Smith, 1996). sympathetic than during sympathetic activity, whereas sym-
pathetic nerve excitation causes a pronounced loss of
amylase-containing acinar granules. No such loss could be
2.2. Pathways of alpha-amylase secretion detected on parasympathetic stimulation, although the out-
put of amylase was about the same as during sympathetic
In acinar cells, release of salivary components is under stimulation.
control of neuronal stimuli. Classic neurotransmitters and In a further examination of the adrenergic mechanisms
specific bioactive peptides serve as the main stimuli for sAA involved in sAA secretion, Skov Olsen et al. (1988) found that
secretion. This section presents studies in rats and humans stimulation of the beta-adrenergic receptors in rats
that used blocking or stimulating pharmacological agents or increased the concentration of sAA by a factor of 30, while
electrical stimuli to gain a better understanding of sAA stimulation of the alpha-adrenergic receptors increased the
secretion. concentration of sAA by a factor of 10. Experiments with
several substances showed that sAA secretion was mainly
2.2.1. Rat studies contributed by beta-1-adrenergic mechanisms. Following up
The findings of Batzri and co-workers are among the first to on these studies, Busch et al. set out to investigate the
indicate the specific involvement of the ANS in the secretion differences in release of sAA by the parotid and the sub-
process of sAA. In studies on slices of the parotid gland in mandibular gland in rats. They found that submandibular sAA
rats, they found that beta-adrenergic receptors caused did not respond with an increase to the administration of
secretion of sAA (Batzri and Selinger, 1973; Batzri et al., isoproterenol, a beta-adrenergic agonist, whereas parotid
Salivary alpha-amylase as a biomarker for the SNS 489

sAA did. This effect was inhibited by the selective beta-1- 2.2.3. Summary
antagonist atenolol, but not by the beta-2-antagonist butox- The results of studies on animals and humans indicate that
amine (Busch et al., 2002). the ANS plays a powerful role in the secretion of sAA, with
In sum, these findings demonstrate the involvement of the contributions of both the alpha-adrenergic and the beta-
ANS, and the sympathetic nervous system in particular, in the adrenergic mechanisms. These findings suggest that sAA
release of sAA, with beta-adrenergic mechanisms as the main might be regarded as an indirect indicator of autonomic
contributing factor in sAA secretion. However, findings from activation.
animal studies are not readily transferable to humans.
Furthermore, stimulation of the sympathetic nerves in iso- 3. Stress-induced salivary alpha-amylase
lation, as used to elicit autonomic activation in the studies
secretion
described above, cannot be considered a physiological reflec-
tion of secretory processes in vivo. In humans, this approach
is not feasible. Use of pharmacological agents may be a more As outlined in the previous sections of this review, the
appropriate methodological approach for determining the release of sAA is governed by activation of the ANS. Thus,
role of the ANS in the secretion of sAA in humans, as the an increase in sAA may be expected during psychological
following studies show. stress, when autonomic activation is high. The following
section reviews findings on sAA responses due to psycholo-
2.2.2. Human studies gical stress.
Speirs et al. (1974) provoked a sympathetic response either
by immersing subjects up to the waist in cold water (4—5 8C) 3.1. Early findings
or by administering isoprenaline and propanolol (both are
beta-adrenergic blockers). Exposure to cold water and The idea that sAA may serve as indicator of psychological
isoprenaline raised sAA concentrations in the parotid gland, stress emerged in the late 1970s. To our knowledge, the
whereas propranolol led to a reduction of sAA concentra- relationship between sAA and psychological stress was first
tions. These results offered first evidence for the sympa- examined in a study by Gilman et al. (1979a), in which
thetic control of sAA secretion in humans (Speirs et al., subjects were exposed to eight days of hyperbaric pres-
1974). As shown in a number of the animal studies described sure. Findings showed increased concentrations of sAA.
above, stimulation of beta-adrenergic receptors modulates The authors concluded that these increases were not only
the synthesis and the release of sAA. Laurikainen et al. due to the hyperbaric exposure and its effects on the ANS,
(1988) studied the effects of timolol maleate, a widely used but also due to the psychological stress caused by the
beta-blocking agent, on the quantity and quality of saliva procedure itself. In the 1980s, Donald Morse and his group
secretion controlled by beta-adrenoceptors in healthy sub- undertook a series of experiments with the goal of exam-
jects. Alpha-amylase concentrations in parotid saliva ining the impact of stress on various salivary parameters.
decreased significantly after drug intake, thus corroborat- Their findings proved quite counterintuitive for research-
ing similar findings from rat studies. Nederfors and co- ers interested in salivary alpha-amylase: Morse et al. found
workers investigated the effects of therapeutic doses of that relaxation, not stress, leads to increases in sAA (Morse
the selective beta-1-antagonist atenolol and the non-selec- et al., 1983a,b). However, these studies were methodolo-
tive beta-antagonist propranolol on stimulated glandular gically flawed in that there was no control condition or
saliva. Atenolol, but not propranolol, resulted in a decrease random allocation to treatment groups. Furthermore, the
of parotid amylase in the morning, and in a decrease of ‘‘relaxation condition’’ always followed immediately after
submandibular/sublingual amylase both in the morning and the ‘‘stress condition,’’ suggesting that the increases in sAA
at lunch time (Nederfors et al., 1994). The authors were might be attributable to a stress response occurring after
able to replicate their findings in a well-controlled study on the stressor. Since Morse concluded that salivary para-
human hypertensive subjects with the selective beta-1- meters were valid and reliable indicators for psychological
antagonist metoprolol (Nederfors and Dahlof, 1996). Thus, stress and relaxation, attempts were made to re-evaluate
these studies demonstrate the importance of beta-adre- this statement by comparing the usefulness of salivary
nergic mechanisms of sAA secretion in humans, as already indicators with other known psychophysiological para-
shown in rats. Following up on these findings, van Stegeren meters (Borgeat et al., 1984). Borgeat et al. exposed
et al. (2006) conducted a placebo-controlled double-blind subjects to relaxation (Jacobson’s progressive muscle
study using propanolol in a stress—rest protocol. While the relaxation) and to stress (written exercises, mental arith-
placebo group showed a substantial increase in sAA subse- metic task). In addition to salivary parameters such as sAA,
quent to the stress test, the sAA response in the propanolol they measured frontal muscle EMG, skin conductance,
group was attenuated. Finally, Ehlert et al. (2006) proposed heart rate, and skin temperature. The interventions
that sAA increases might reflect the interaction of stress- resulted in clear and distinct reaction patterns in the
dependent sympathetic and parasympathetic stimulation ‘‘classical’’ psychophysiological parameters, but no sali-
via central nervous noradrenergic input. To examine this vary changes were observed. Thus, initial findings on
hypothesis, they assessed the indirect effect of yohimbine stress-related changes in sAA did not correspond with
hydrochloride, an alpha-2-adrenergic receptor antagonist, the physiological literature outlined above. However,
on sAA release in a randomized placebo-controlled study. further studies conducted under more rigorous conditions
The results showed significant increases in sAA concentra- and employing scientifically advanced designs, e.g. includ-
tion in the yohimbine condition relative to the placebo ing control groups etc., showed clear stress-related
condition. increases in sAA, as summarized in the next section.
490 U.M. Nater, N. Rohleder

3.2. Effects of psychological stress on salivary examination (Schoofs et al., 2007), a standardized test bat-
alpha-amylase tery in toddlers (Fortunato et al., 2008), affective picture
viewing (van Stegeren et al., 2008), and in a peer rejection
Whereas other stress-related factors in saliva, such as sali- paradigm in adolescents (Stroud et al., 2009). Interestingly,
vary cortisol (Kirschbaum and Hellhammer, 1989, 1994), only a few studies have found no changes in sAA in response to
received widespread scientific attention in the following stressful stimuli including noise (Morrison et al., 2003), the
decade, interest in sAA as a stress marker was not sparked heel prick test in neonates (Schaffer et al., 2008), or a
again until Chatterton and colleagues published their findings strange situation paradigm (Hill-Soderlund et al., 2008).
of increases in sAA in a variety of stressful conditions (Chat- The scarcity of non-significant changes suggests that sAA is
terton et al., 1996). Beyond this seminal study, a number of indeed a highly sensitive parameter reflecting changes
studies applying psychological stress protocols have shown caused by psychological stressors.
that sAA is highly sensitive to stress-related changes. This A number of studies have used a standardized psychosocial
section briefly reviews these studies. stress test, i.e. the Trier Social Stress Test or TSST (Kirsch-
In a study of subjects exposed to an academic examina- baum et al., 1993), to assess the usefulness of sAA as a
tion, Bosch et al. measured several salivary parameters biomarker for psychologically induced stress. The TSST com-
including sAA (Bosch et al., 1996). Whole unstimulated saliva prises a mock job interview and a mental arithmetic task,
was taken 30 min before the examination, 2 weeks later, and both performed in front of an audience, leading to marked
6 weeks later. Results indicated an increase in concentration increases in both psychological and physiological stress indi-
and output of sAA during the stress condition, while the cators. In a pilot study, Rohleder et al. (2004) found marked
salivary flow rate did not change. In a separate analysis, increases in sAA due to the TSST. These findings were corro-
the authors found that the number and severity of critical life borated in a subsequent study employing the TSST and a rest
events was related to sAA activity, thus suggesting that condition (Nater et al., 2005) (see Fig. 1). The TSST led to
everyday stress also contributes to stress-dependent changes significant increases in sAA, while the rest condition did not
in salivary parameters (Bosch et al., 1998). Chatterton et al. impact sAA levels.
(1997) studied subjects preparing for skydiving and found A third study using the same study design again showed
increased sAA prior to the jump than in control subjects who marked increases in sAA due to the TSST (Nater et al., 2006b).
did not jump. The highest levels were observed immediately Other studies using the TSST and measuring sAA as a stress
after landing. Using a stressful video game to induce labora- marker have reported sAA increases in response to the TSST in
tory stress, Skosnik et al. found a significant increase in sAA healthy children (Granger et al., 2006), healthy adults (Roh-
after the 15-min stressor (Skosnik et al., 2000). In a further leder et al., 2006a,b; Grillon et al., 2007; Het and Wolf, 2007;
study employing a laboratory task to induce acute stress Payne et al., 2007; Schoofs et al., 2008), postpartum mothers
(Bosch et al., 2003), whole unstimulated saliva was measured (Tu et al., 2006), and adolescents (Gordis et al., 2006, 2008).
before, during, and after an active memory task, passive Nierop et al. (2006) found that pregnant women showed
watching of a gruesome video, and a control condition. sAA lower sAA responses than non-pregnant women when
output differed significantly between the three conditions, exposed to the TSST.
with highest levels being measured during the passive video In conclusion, the discrepancies between earlier and more
task. A decrease in sAA output was found in the active recent findings on the effects of stress on sAA may be
memory task, suggesting that the sAA response depends on attributed to the different stressors and stress paradigms
the nature of the stressor and the active or passive coping used in the respective studies. Results have consistently
capabilities of the subjects. Another research group used a shown changes in sAA in response to psychological stress.
stressful and a relaxing video to induce stress and rest
conditions and directly compared the effects of both on
salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase responses in whole unsti-
mulated saliva (Takai et al., 2004, 2007). The stressful video
induced marked increases in both cortisol and sAA, whereas
the relaxing video produced no changes in salivary cortisol,
but significant decreases in sAA concentrations. Noto and
colleagues examined sAA levels during a mental arithmetic
task (Noto et al., 2005) and observed significant increases in
sAA.
sAA increases have also been reported in response to other
psychologically stressful conditions, such as experience of
medical procedures (Yamaguchi et al., 2006a), adverse musi-
cal stimuli in men (Nater et al., 2006a), mothers watching
their children being exposed to a stressful task (Granger
et al., 2006), the cold pressor test (West et al., 2006; van
Stegeren et al., 2008), achievement and interpersonal stress
(Stroud et al., 2006), a driving simulation (Yamaguchi et al.,
2006b), use of a noise burst and infant arm restraint in Figure 1 Salivary alpha-amylase activity in U/ml. Left: a
depressive mothers (Shea et al., 2006), execution of neck/ marked increase is observed in the stress condition. Right: no
face surgery by medical trainees (Yamakage et al., 2007), a significant changes occur in the rest condition (Nater et al.,
mental arithmetic task (Goi et al., 2007), oral academic 2005).
Salivary alpha-amylase as a biomarker for the SNS 491

Thus, sAA can be regarded as an excellent indicator of stress- after exercise (Kivlighan and Granger, 2006). Interestingly,
related body changes. Whereas the studies outlined above sAA measures were positively associated with performance in
measured changes in sAA in response to acute stress stimuli, this study. Finally, in a study of exercise-induced sexual
only a handful of studies have reported associations of arousal in women, exercise produced significant sAA
chronic stress and sAA. In one study, chronic stress was found increases relative to a non-exercise condition (Hamilton
to be associated with diurnal profiles of sAA (Nater et al., et al., 2008).
2007). Subjects reporting relatively high chronic stress had Taken together, a marked increase in sAA can be observed
higher momentary sAA activity than subjects reporting low during exercise. Exercise is known to increase sympathetic
chronic stress levels. Rohleder et al. recently showed that activity, and the high protein level in saliva following exercise
daily sAA secretion is associated with self-reported shame may be due to increased adrenergic activity in the salivary
and depression in young women (Rohleder et al., 2008). glands.
Another study reported higher basal values of sAA activity
in women exposed to Hurricane Katrina than in non-exposed 3.4. Is salivary alpha-amylase an indicator for
controls (Vigil et al., in press). Finally, Wolf and colleagues the sympathetic nervous system?
found lower diurnal sAA in children with asthma than in
healthy controls (Wolf et al., 2008). The latter two studies The evidence summarized above supports the view that sAA
highlight the potential of measuring sAA as an index of may be a useful indicator for activity of the sympathetic
chronic stress in selected high-risk populations. nervous system. Some studies have tested this hypothesis by
directly testing associations of sAA with various established
3.3. Effects of exercise on salivary alpha- measures of sympathetic activity.
amylase A series of studies with this specific aim was undertaken by
Chatterton and colleagues. In these studies, subjects were
Another strong activator of the sympathetic nervous system exposed to physical (running, exercise, exposure to heat and
is physical activity. A variety of studies have examined the cold) and psychological (examination) stressors (Chatterton
effects of exercise on sAA. Gilman and co-workers observed a et al., 1996). Increases observed in both sAA and plasma
significantly higher concentration of sAA during exercise than catecholamines (norepinephrine and epinephrine) in
in a control period. The authors discussed sAA as a valid response to physical and (in some cases) psychological stres-
measure of sympathetic activity via adrenergic receptors sors led Chatterton to conclude the two parameters might be
(Gilman et al., 1979b). Nexo and co-workers examined the measured as substitutes for each other. Indeed, the authors
impact of a 2-h long cross-country race, collecting stimulated found significant correlations between sAA and both plasma
whole saliva before and after the race. A marked increase in norepinephrine and epinephrine (r = 0.64 and r = 0.49,
sAA was found after the race; the median level increased respectively) in the exercise conditions of their study. The
almost sevenfold compared to the beginning of the race authors therefore suggested that sAA might serve as an
(Nexo et al., 1988). In two studies reported by Chatterton indicator for plasma catecholamines (specifically, norepi-
et al. (1996), sAA was determined in subjects participating in nephrine). A close inspection of this study, however, shows
a running and a bicycle exercise task. Both protocols resulted that only a small and non-significant correlation (r = 0.17)
in increases of sAA. Another study investigated salivary was observed in the psychological stress condition (examina-
components before, during and 1 h after a 2-h marathon. tion). The results therefore indicate that similar mechanisms
sAA activity increased significantly due to the strenuous underlie increases in both catecholamines and sAA during
activity, and levels remained high 1 h after the marathon physiological stress experience. A pilot study using a stan-
(Ljungberg et al., 1997). In another study, sAA was used as an dardized stress protocol reported correlations between
indicator for the anaerobic threshold in subjects performing increases in sAA and plasma norepinephrine (r = 0.54) (Roh-
a treadmill exercise test. Since the correlation between sAA leder et al., 2004). In contrast, a study using the same stress
and the anaerobic threshold was almost 1 (r = 0.93), the protocol in a bigger sample found no associations when the
authors concluded that sAA was a good and valid measure plasma catecholamines norepinephrine and epinephrine
of the anaerobic threshold (Calvo et al., 1997). In a study of were measured concomitantly with sAA levels (Nater
triathletes, several salivary parameters in unstimulated et al., 2006b). Another study found increases in both plasma
whole saliva were measured before and after a race consist- norepinephrine and sAA after carbon dioxide inhalation, but
ing of swimming, cycling, and running. Mean sAA activity these two parameters did not correlate significantly (Wether-
increased significantly during the triathlon (Steerenberg ell et al., 2006). Thus, it seems doubtful that there is a 1:1
et al., 1997). Another study examined eight well-trained association between plasma catecholamines and sAA in the
athletes during a high-performance 60-min cycle exercise body’s response to stressful conditions.
task. Unstimulated whole saliva was collected before exer- Further attempts have been made to relate sAA to other
cise, immediately after the task, and 1, 2.5, 5 and 24 h after indicators of sympathetic or parasympathetic activity. In a
exercise. A significant increase in sAA activity was found after study of subjects exposed to a stressful video showing a
exercise (Walsh et al., 1999). Similar results were obtained in surgical procedure or to a memory task, Bosch et al.
a more recent study (Bishop et al., 2006) examining the (2003) measured both sAA and various cardiovascular para-
influence of caffeine intake on sAA measures in an exercise meters. The authors found negative correlations between
paradigm. The authors found that caffeine intake resulted in left ventricular ejection time and sAA (in the video condition)
an additional increase in sAA that was not observed in a and between the square root of the mean squared differences
placebo condition. A study examining the impact of exercise of normal-to-normal intervals (RMSSD, as an index of para-
on a rowing ergometer on sAA levels showed increased levels sympathetic tone) and sAA during the memory task. These
492 U.M. Nater, N. Rohleder

findings suggest a predominant role of the sympathetic ner- examined in healthy participants during a mental arithmetic
vous system in the secretion process of sAA, together with task (Noto et al., 2005). The task induced significant
vagal withdrawal, under psychological stress. They were increases in both state anxiety and sAA, with a correlation
complemented by a study using a standardized psychosocial of r = 0.589 between the two variables. These examples
stress protocol (Nater et al., 2006b), which reported a demonstrate the potential of sAA measurement in a psychia-
positive correlation (r = 0.39) between sAA and the low tric context. To date, however, few studies with psychiatric
frequency/high frequency ratio thought to reflect sympa- patients have employed sAA as an indicator for autonomic
thetic tone. In another study, a positive association between changes. A notable exception is a recent study by Labudda
sAA and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) was observed et al. (2007), who measured sAA in pathological gamblers and
(r = 0.36), indicating an association of increased sAA with found significant increases only in a subgroup of patients who
vagal suppression (Granger et al., 2006). Finally, a recent showed less disadvantageous decision-making patterns.
study revealed a moderate positive association (r = 0.26) However, sAA might be also measured in the context of
between sAA and basal skin conductance level (SCL), which somatic disorders in which autonomic dysregulation might be
is regarded as a sympathetic activity marker (El-Sheikh et al., present. Exaggerated autonomic responses to different sti-
2008). muli can be observed in hypertensive patients (Fredrickson
et al., 1991; al’Absi and Wittmers, 2003) and patients with
3.5. Summary HIV (Cole et al., 2001). It might be of interest to measure sAA
concentrations in these groups of patients. ANS dysregulation
Taken together, it seems that sAA response patterns to both can also be found in atopic diseases. In a study of patients
physical and psychological stressors correspond to the with dermatitis, Crespi and co-workers measured parotid
response patterns of the sympathetic nervous system. secretory response to intra-oral citric acid. Protein concen-
Whereas no correspondence of plasma norepinephrine and tration as well as sAA activity was significantly decreased in
sAA could be established, associations between other mar- patients with atopic dermatitis. The authors interpreted
kers of the ANS, such as cardiovascular parameters, have these findings as a reflection of impaired beta-adrenergic
been found. It should be noted, however, that the correla- mediated responses in atopic dermatitis (Crespi et al., 1982).
tions between sAA and sympathetic markers are relatively In another study, Wolf et al. (2008) examined diurnal sAA in
small. Based on the pharmacological and electrophysiologi- patients with asthma and in controls. The found lower daily
cal literature reviewed above, the pathways that lead to sAA output in patients with asthma than in the control group.
secretion of sAA are clearly sympathetic/parasympathetic in
nature. 4.2. Measurement of salivary alpha-amylase in
treatment studies
4. Applications of salivary alpha-amylase
measurement Given these findings of increased or attenuated sAA in clinical
populations, use of sAA to measure the effects of psychother-
apy, e.g. stress management training (Gaab et al., 2003) seems
The findings on the association of sAA and the sympathetic
warranted. A recent study has used sAA to assess stress levels in
nervous system summarized above indicate that sAA can
healthy women administered Relora (a medication including
function as a useful biomarker in acute and chronic stress
extracts of Magnolia officinalis and Phellodendron amurense)
studies. An overview of previous and potential applications in
(Kalman et al., 2008). Another study defined sAA as the end-
stress research is given below.
point of a treatment study using reflexology in nursing home
residents with dementia (Hodgson and Andersen, 2008).
4.1. Salivary alpha-amylase as an indicator of Finally, a third study found decreased sAA levels in patients
autonomic dysregulation undergoing surgery while exposed to natural sounds than in
patients who were not exposed to any sounds (Arai et al.,
Based on the knowledge that has been accumulated about 2008). These examples show that sAA measurement is a pro-
the role of stress and its underlying physiological mechanisms mising approach for studies of treatment effects.
in the secretion of sAA, it seems reasonable to conclude that Taken together, sAA has already been used as a marker in a
sAA concentration can serve as an index for pathological variety of conditions. More recent developments suggest that
dysregulation of the ANS in specific clinical and subclinical it may also be a useful marker in the context of pain (Shirasaki
conditions. Anxiety-related conditions are accompanied by et al., 2007) or sleep (Seugnet et al., 2006) research. It may
autonomic changes (Friedman et al., 1993; Shalev and Rogel- be particularly useful in ambulatory settings, such as field
Fuchs, 1993; Southwick et al., 1993; Lang et al., 2000; studies, where assessment of sAA through saliva as an indi-
Laederach-Hofmann et al., 2002; Coupland et al., 2003). cator of autonomic functioning might present an easy, non-
sAA measurements might provide additional information on invasive, and efficient sampling method.
the autonomic changes occurring in anxiety patients. Some
preliminary findings indicate that sAA measurement may be
useful not only as an indicator of autonomic changes but also 5. Conclusion
as a marker for anxiety reports. Takai et al. (2004) used a
stressful video to induce stress and found sAA peak levels and The aim of this review was to evaluate the role of sAA activity
state anxiety measures to correlate highly (r = 0.535). A as a potential indicator for sympathetic activation in
similarly high correlation between state anxiety and response to stress. Although first findings on the role of
sAA was found in another study, in which sAA levels were sAA in stress processes were published almost three decades
Salivary alpha-amylase as a biomarker for the SNS 493

ago, no attempts were made to scientifically elucidate this sAA may well become an important parameter in stress
relationship until the mid-1990s. Numerous studies have research.
since shown that changes in sAA are indeed dependent on
stressful stimuli, either physiological or psychological in Role of the funding source
nature. The biological meaning of this phenomenon remains
to be clarified, however. In this context, it may be worthwhile
NR was supported by personal (salary) fellowships by the
reconsidering the main property of alpha-amylase, namely its
German Research Association (DFG; Ro 2353/4-1) and the
digestive action. Alpha-amylase hydrolyzes starch to glucose
Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research. UMN has no
and maltose, initiating the digestion of starch in the oral
funding source to report.
cavity; it has also been shown to have a bacterial interactive
function (Scannapieco et al., 1993). More studies are needed
to examine the cellular processes occurring after initiation of Conflicts of interest
stress-related responses in the oral environment. It is diffi-
cult to deduct the function of short-term increases in alpha- The authors have no conflicts of interest and declare no
amylase while the biological meaning of transient rises in the financial interests.
anti-bacterial action of the enzyme remains unclear. How-
ever, such short-term increases may be useful to the body in
that energy is made available by increased digestive action in Acknowledgments
response to stress. Physiological stress reactions comprise
orchestrated actions throughout the body, putting the organ- The authors would like to thank Ulrike Ehlert and Clemens
ism in a state of overall preparedness to engage in fight or Kirschbaum for their support and theoretical input. Further-
flight. Thus, increases in amylase activity may be one of many more, NR acknowledges the support of the German Research
actions involved in activating the body’s resources to cope Foundation (DFG; grant no. Ro 2353/4-1) and the Michael
with stressful events or threats to homeostasis. However, this Smith Foundation for Health Research.
explanation applies only to reactions to short-term, acute
stressors. Further studies are needed to examine long-term References
changes in sAA concentrations. If clinical or subclinical con-
ditions associated with an increase of activity in the ANS al’Absi, M., Wittmers, L.E., 2003. Enhanced adrenocortical responses
result in chronically elevated concentrations of sAA, the to stress in hypertension-prone men and women. Ann. Behav.
sustained higher sAA activity and therefore digestive action Med. 25, 25—33.
may prove detrimental to oral health. Anderson, L.C., Garrett, J.R., Johnson, D.A., Kauffman, D.L., Keller,
The studies summarized in this review clearly show that sAA P.J., Thulin, A., 1984. Influence of circulating catecholamines on
is increased in states of stress, i.e. when autonomic activation protein secretion into rat parotid saliva during parasympathetic
is increased. We can therefore conclude that increases in sAA stimulation. J. Physiol. 352, 163—171.
may reflect changes in the ANS. Further studies are needed to Arai, Y.C., Sakakibara, S., Ito, A., Ohshima, K., Sakakibara, T., Nishi,
T., Hibino, S., Niwa, S., Kuniyoshi, K., 2008. Intra-operative
conclusively determine whether sAA can be included in the
natural sound decreases salivary amylase activity of patients
canon of biological stress markers. In particular, the mechan-
undergoing inguinal hernia repair under epidural anesthesia. Acta
isms underlying elevations in sAA in response to stress warrant Anaesthesiol. Scand..
examination. Although a variety of studies have examined the Asking, B., 1985. Sympathetic stimulation of amylase secretion
physiological mechanisms of sAA production and secretion in during a parasympathetic background activity in the rat parotid
animals, studies in humans are scarce. Use of pharmacological gland. Acta Physiol. Scand. 124, 535—542.
agents that inhibit or activate the ANS may prove particularly Asking, B., Gjorstrup, P., 1987. Synthesis and secretion of amylase in
useful here, providing more detailed insights into the branches the rat parotid gland following autonomic nerve stimulation in
of the ANS responsible for increases in sAA. Clearly, more vivo. Acta Physiol. Scand. 130, 439—445.
invasive techniques are available in animal studies. Experi- Asking, B., Proctor, G.B., 1989. Parasympathetic activation of amy-
lase secretion in the intact and sympathetically denervated rat
mental damaging of nerve fibers or direct stimulation of nerve
parotid gland. Q. J. Exp. Physiol. 74, 45—52.
cells is not feasible in a basic research context. It may be
Batzri, S., Selinger, Z., 1973. Enzyme secretion mediated by the
interesting, however, to use electrical stimulation techniques epinephrine-receptor in rat parotid slices. Factors governing
in awake or anaesthesized humans, e.g. in the clinical context efficiency of the process. J. Biol. Chem. 248, 356—360.
of a hospital. Measurement of direct sympathetic nerve activ- Batzri, S., Selinger, Z., Schramm, M., Robinovitch, M.R., 1973.
ity via microneurography is considered to be the most accurate Potassium release mediated by the epinephrine-receptor in rat
technique for assessing sympathetic activation (Grassi and parotid slices. Properties and relation to enzyme secretion. J.
Esler, 1999). Beyond peripheral measurements, the relation- Biol. Chem. 248, 361—368.
ship between central parameters and changes in sAA might Baum, B.J., 1993. Principles of salivary secretion. Ann. N. Y. Acad.
prove very interesting. Since cerebrospinal fluid concentra- Sci. 694, 17—23.
Bishop, N.C., Walker, G.J., Scanlon, G.A., Richards, S., Rogers, E.,
tions of norepinephrine reflect other mechanisms than do
2006. Salivary IgA responses to prolonged intensive exercise
peripheral catecholamines, it would be worthwhile comparing
following caffeine ingestion. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 38, 513—519.
sAA and central norepinephrine concentrations (Goldstein, Borgeat, F., Chagon, G., Legault, Y., 1984. Comparison of the salivary
1998). changes associated with a relaxing and with a stressful procedure.
In summary, assessment of sAA as a non-invasive biomar- Psychophysiology 21, 690—698.
ker for the sympathetic nervous system offers a multitude of Bosch, J.A., Brand, H.S., Ligtenberg, A.J., Bermond, B., Hoogstra-
possibilities in different research areas. If further evaluated, ten, J., Nieuw Amerongen, A.V., 1998. The response of salivary
494 U.M. Nater, N. Rohleder

protein levels and S-IgA to an academic examination are asso- tion of chronic hyperbaric exposure. Undersea Biomed. Res. 6,
ciated with daily stress. J. Psychophysiol. 12, 384—391. 303—307.
Bosch, J.A., Brand, H.S., Ligtenberg, T.J., Bermond, B., Hoogstraten, Gilman, S.C., Thornton, R., Miller, D., Biersner, R., 1979b. Effects of
J., Nieuw Amerongen, A.V., 1996. Psychological stress as a exercise stress on parotid gland secretion. Horm. Metab. Res. 11,
determinant of protein levels and salivary-induced aggregation 454.
of Streptococcus gordonii in human whole saliva. Psychosom. Goi, N., Hirai, Y., Harada, H., Ikari, A., Ono, T., Kinae, N., Hiramatsu,
Med. 58, 374—382. M., Nakamura, K., Takagi, K., 2007. Comparison of peroxidase
Bosch, J.A., De Geus, E.J., Veerman, E.C., Hoogstraten, J., Nieuw response to mental arithmetic stress in saliva of smokers and non-
Amerongen, A.V., 2003. Innate secretory immunity in response to smokers. J. Toxicol. Sci. 32, 121—127.
laboratory stressors that evoke distinct patterns of cardiac auto- Goldstein, D.S., 1998. Catecholamines in the periphery. Overview.
nomic activity. Psychosom. Med. 65, 245—258. Adv. Pharmacol. 42, 529—539.
Brown, C.C., 1970. The parotid puzzle: a review of the literature on Gordis, E.B., Granger, D.A., Susman, E.J., Trickett, P.K., 2006.
human salivation and its applications to psychophysiology. Psy- Asymmetry between salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase reactiv-
chophysiology 7, 65—85. ity to stress: relation to aggressive behavior in adolescents.
Busch, L., Sterin-Borda, L., Borda, E., 2002. Differences in the Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 976—987.
regulatory mechanism of amylase release by rat parotid and Gordis, E.B., Granger, D.A., Susman, E.J., Trickett, P.K., 2008.
submandibular glands. Arch. Oral Biol. 47, 717—722. Salivary alpha amylase-cortisol asymmetry in maltreated youth.
Calvo, F., Chicharro, J.L., Bandres, F., Lucia, A., Perez, M., Alvarez, Horm. Behav. 53, 96—103.
J., Mojares, L.L., Vaquero, A.F., Legido, J.C., 1997. Anaerobic Granger, D.A., Kivlighan, K.T., Blair, C., El-Sheikh, M., Mize, J.,
threshold determination with analysis of salivary amylase. Can. J. Lisonbee, J.A., Buckhalt, J.A., Stroud, L.R., Handwerger, K.,
Appl. Physiol. 22, 553—561. Schwartz, E.B., 2006. Integrating the measurement of salivary
Castle, D., Castle, A., 1998. Intracellular transport and secretion of alpha-amylase into studies of child health, development, and
salivary proteins. Crit. Rev. Oral Biol. Med. 9, 4—22. social relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Relat. 23, 267—290.
Chatterton Jr., R.T., Vogelsong, K.M., Lu, Y.C., Ellman, A.B., Hud- Granger, D.A., Kivlighan, K.T., el-Sheikh, M., Gordis, E.B., Stroud,
gens, G.A., 1996. Salivary alpha-amylase as a measure of endo- L.R., 2007. Salivary alpha-amylase in biobehavioral research:
genous adrenergic activity. Clin. Physiol. 16, 433—448. recent developments and applications. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.
Chatterton Jr., R.T., Vogelsong, K.M., Lu, Y.C., Hudgens, G.A., 1997. 1098, 122—144.
Hormonal responses to psychological stress in men preparing for Grassi, G., Esler, M., 1999. How to assess sympathetic activity in
skydiving. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 82, 2503—2509. humans. J. Hypertens. 17, 719—734.
Cole, S.W., Naliboff, B.D., Kemeny, M.E., Griswold, M.P., Fahey, J.L., Grillon, C., Duncko, R., Covington, M.F., Kopperman, L., Kling, M.A.,
Zack, J.A., 2001. Impaired response to HAART in HIV-infected 2007. Acute stress potentiates anxiety in humans. Biol. Psychiatry
individuals with high autonomic nervous system activity. Proc. 62, 1183—1186.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 12695—12700. Hamilton, L.D., Fogle, E.A., Meston, C.M., 2008. The roles of tes-
Coupland, N.J., Wilson, S.J., Potokar, J.P., Bell, C., Nutt, D.J., 2003. tosterone and alpha-amylase in exercise-induced sexual arousal
Increased sympathetic response to standing in panic disorder. in women. J. Sex. Med. 5, 845—853.
Psychiatry Res. 118, 69—79. Het, S., Wolf, O.T., 2007. Mood changes in response to psychosocial
Crespi, H., Armando, I., Tumilasci, O., Levin, G., Massimo, J., stress in healthy young women: effects of pretreatment with
Barontini, M., Perec, C., 1982. Catecholamines levels and parotid cortisol. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 11—20.
secretion in children with chronic atopic dermatitis. J. Invest. Hill-Soderlund, A.L., Mills-Koonce, W.R., Propper, C., Calkins, S.D.,
Dermatol. 78, 493—497. Granger, D.A., Moore, G.A., Gariepy, J.L., Cox, M.J., 2008.
Edgar, W.M., 1992. Saliva: its secretion, composition and functions. Parasympathetic and sympathetic responses to the strange situa-
Br. Dent. J. 172, 305—312. tion in infants and mothers from avoidant and securely attached
Ehlert, U., Erni, K., Hebisch, G., Nater, U., 2006. Salivary alpha- dyads. Dev. Psychobiol. 50, 361—376.
amylase levels after yohimbine challenge in healthy men. J. Clin. Hodgson, N.A., Andersen, S., 2008. The clinical efficacy of reflexol-
Endocrinol. Metab. 91, 5130—5133. ogy in nursing home residents with dementia. J. Altern. Comple-
El-Sheikh, M., Erath, S.A., Buckhalt, J.A., Granger, D.A., Mize, J., ment. Med. 14, 269—275.
2008. Cortisol and children’s adjustment: the moderating role of Humphrey, S.P., Williamson, R.T., 2001. A review of saliva: normal
sympathetic nervous system activity. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. composition, flow, and function. J. Prosthet. Dent. 85, 162—
36, 601—611. 169.
Emmelin, N., 1987. Nerve interactions in salivary glands. J. Dent. Kalman, D.S., Feldman, S., Feldman, R., Schwartz, H.I., Krieger,
Res. 66, 509—517. D.R., Garrison, R., 2008. Effect of a proprietary Magnolia and
Fortunato, C.K., Dribin, A.E., Granger, D.A., Buss, K.A., 2008. Sali- Phellodendron extract on stress levels in healthy women: a pilot,
vary alpha-amylase and cortisol in toddlers: differential relations double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Nutr. J. 7, 11.
to affective behaviors. Dev. Psychobiol.. Kaufman, E., Lamster, I.B., 2002. The diagnostic applications of
Fredrickson, M., Tuomisto, M., Bergman-Losman, B., 1991. Neuroen- saliva–—a review. Crit. Rev. Oral. Biol. Med. 13, 197—212.
docrine and cardiovascular stress reactivity in middle-aged nor- Kirschbaum, C., Hellhammer, D.H., 1989. Salivary cortisol in psy-
motensive adults with parental history of cardiovascular disease. chobiological research: an overview. Neuropsychobiology 22,
Psychophysiology 28, 656—664. 150—169.
Friedman, B.H., Thayer, J.F., Borkovec, T.D., Tyrrell, R.A., Johnson, Kirschbaum, C., Hellhammer, D.H., 1994. Salivary cortisol in psy-
B.H., Columbo, R., 1993. Autonomic characteristics of nonclinical choneuroendocrine research: recent developments and applica-
panic and blood phobia. Biol. Psychiatry 34, 298—310. tions. Psychoneuroendocrinology 19, 313—333.
Gaab, J., Blattler, N., Menzi, T., Pabst, B., Stoyer, S., Ehlert, U., Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.M., Hellhammer, D.H., 1993. The ‘Trier
2003. Randomized controlled evaluation of the effects of cogni- Social Stress Test’–—a tool for investigating psychobiological
tive-behavioral stress management on cortisol responses to acute stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology
stress in healthy subjects. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28, 767— 28, 76—81.
779. Kivlighan, K.T., Granger, D.A., 2006. Salivary alpha-amylase response
Gilman, S.C., Fischer, G.J., Biersner, R.J., Thornton, R.D., Miller, to competition: relation to gender, previous experience, and
D.A., 1979a. Human parotid alpha-amylase secretion as a func- attitudes. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 703—714.
Salivary alpha-amylase as a biomarker for the SNS 495

Labudda, K., Wolf, O.T., Markowitsch, H.J., Brand, M., 2007. Deci- Rohleder, N., Chen, E., Wolf, J.M., Miller, G.E., 2008. The psycho-
sion-making and neuroendocrine responses in pathological gam- biology of trait shame in young women: extending the social self
blers. Psychiatry Res. 153, 233—243. preservation theory. Health Psychol. 27, 523—532.
Laederach-Hofmann, K., Mussgay, L., Buchel, B., Widler, P., Ruddel, Rohleder, N., Nater, U.M., 2009. Determinants of salivary alpha-
H., 2002. Patients with erythrophobia (fear of blushing) show amylase in humans and methodological considerations. Psycho-
abnormal autonomic regulation in mental stress conditions. Psy- neuroendocrinology this issue.
chosom. Med. 64, 358—365. Rohleder, N., Nater, U.M., Wolf, J.M., Ehlert, U., Kirschbaum, C.,
Lang, P.J., Davis, M., Ohman, A., 2000. Fear and anxiety: animal 2004. Psychosocial stress-induced activation of salivary alpha-
models and human cognitive psychophysiology. J. Affect. Disord. amylase: an indicator of sympathetic activity? Ann. N. Y. Acad.
61, 137—159. Sci. 1032, 258—263.
Laurikainen, K., Laurikainen, E., Tenovuo, J., Kaila, T., Vilja, P., Rohleder, N., Wolf, J.M., Herpfer, I., Fiebich, B.L., Kirschbaum, C.,
1988. Effects of a beta-blocking agent, timolol maleate, on saliva Lieb, K., 2006a. No response of plasma substance P, but delayed
in healthy volunteers. Scand. J. Dent. Res. 96, 121—127. increase of interleukin-1 receptor antagonist to acute psychoso-
Ljungberg, G., Ericson, T., Ekblom, B., Birkhed, D., 1997. Saliva and cial stress. Life Sci. 78, 3082—3089.
marathon running. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 7, 214—219. Rohleder, N., Wolf, J.M., Maldonado, E.F., Kirschbaum, C., 2006b.
Makinen, K.K., 1989. Salivary enzymes. In: Tenovuo, J.O. (Ed.), The psychosocial stress-induced increase in salivary alpha-amy-
Human Saliva: Clinical Chemistry and Microbiology, vol. 2. CRC lase is independent of saliva flow rate. Psychophysiology 43, 645—
Press, Boca Raton, pp. 93—119. 652.
Morrison, W.E., Haas, E.C., Shaffner, D.H., Garrett, E.S., Fackler, Scannapieco, F.A., Torres, G., Levine, M.J., 1993. Salivary alpha-
J.C., 2003. Noise, stress, and annoyance in a pediatric intensive amylase: role in dental plaque and caries formation. Crit. Rev.
care unit. Crit. Care Med. 31, 113—119. Oral Biol. Med. 4, 301—307.
Morse, D.R., Schacterle, G.R., Esposito, J.V., Chod, S.D., Furst, M.L., Schaffer, L., Burkhardt, T., Muller-Vizentini, D., Rauh, M., Tomaske,
DiPonziano, J., Zaydenberg, M., 1983a. Stress, meditation and M., Mieth, R.A., Bauersfeld, U., Beinder, E., 2008. Cardiac auto-
saliva: a study of separate salivary gland secretions in endodontic nomic balance in small-for-gestational-age neonates. Am. J.
patients. J. Oral Med. 38, 150—160. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 294, H884—890.
Morse, D.R., Schacterle, G.R., Zaydenberg, M., Furst, M.L., Pollack, Schoofs, D., Hartmann, R., Wolf, O.T., 2007. Neuroendocrine stress
R.L., Moskow, A., Esposito, J.V., 1983b. Salivary volume and responses to an oral academic examination: no strong influence of
amylase activity. I. Relaxation versus relaxed chewing. J. Am. sex, repeated participation and personality traits. Stress 1.
Soc. Psychosom. Dent. Med. 30, 85—96. Schoofs, D., Preuss, D., Wolf, O.T., 2008. Psychosocial stress induces
Nater, U.M., 2004. The Role of Salivary Alpha-Amylase in Stress working memory impairments in an n-back paradigm. Psycho-
Research. Cuvillier, Göttingen. neuroendocrinology 33, 643—653.
Nater, U.M., Abbruzzese, E., Krebs, M., Ehlert, U., 2006a. Sex Selinger, Z., Batzri, S., Eimerl, S., Schramm, M., 1973. Calcium and
differences in emotional and psychophysiological responses to energy requirements for K+ release mediated by the epinephrine-
musical stimuli. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 62, 300—308. receptor in rat parotid slices. J. Biol. Chem. 248, 369—372.
Nater, U.M., La Marca, R., Florin, L., Moses, A., Langhans, W., Koller, Seugnet, L., Boero, J., Gottschalk, L., Duntley, S.P., Shaw, P.J., 2006.
M.M., Ehlert, U., 2006b. Stress-induced changes in human salivary Identification of a biomarker for sleep drive in flies and humans.
alpha-amylase activity–—associations with adrenergic activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 19913—19918.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 49—58. Shalev, A.Y., Rogel-Fuchs, Y., 1993. Psychophysiology of the post-
Nater, U.M., Rohleder, N., Gaab, J., Berger, S., Jud, A., Kirschbaum, traumatic stress disorder: from sulfur fumes to behavioral genet-
C., Ehlert, U., 2005. Human salivary alpha-amylase reactivity in a ics. Psychosom. Med. 55, 413—423.
psychosocial stress paradigm. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 55, 333—342. Shea, A.K., Steiner, M., Brennan, P., Walker, E., Newport, D.J.,
Nater, U.M., Rohleder, N., Schlotz, W., Ehlert, U., Kirschbaum, C., Stowe, Z.N., 2006. Maternal depression and salivary alpha-amy-
2007. Determinants of the diurnal course of salivary alpha-amy- lase response to stress in their infants–—abstract. Psychosom.
lase. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 392—401. Med. 68, A5.
Nederfors, T., Dahlof, C., 1996. Effects on salivary flow rate and Shirasaki, S., Fujii, H., Takahashi, M., Sato, T., Ebina, M., Noto, Y.,
composition of withdrawal of and re-exposure to the beta 1- Hirota, K., 2007. Correlation between salivary alpha-amylase
selective antagonist metoprolol in a hypertensive patient popu- activity and pain scale in patients with chronic pain. Reg. Anesth.
lation. Eur. J. Oral Sci. 104, 262—268. Pain Med. 32, 120—123.
Nederfors, T., Ericsson, T., Twetman, S., Dahlof, C., 1994. Effects of Skosnik, P.D., Chatterton Jr., R.T., Swisher, T., Park, S., 2000.
the beta-adrenoceptor antagonists atenolol and propranolol on Modulation of attentional inhibition by norepinephrine and
human parotid and submandibular—sublingual salivary secretion. cortisol after psychological stress. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 36,
J. Dent. Res. 73, 5—10. 59—68.
Nexo, E., Hansen, M.R., Konradsen, L., 1988. Human salivary epi- Skov Olsen, P., Kirkegaard, P., Rasmussen, T., Magid, E., Poulsen,
dermal growth factor, haptocorrin and amylase before and after S.S., Nexo, E., 1988. Adrenergic effects on secretion of amylase
prolonged exercise. Scand. J. Clin. Lab. Invest. 48, 269—273. from the rat salivary glands. Digestion 41, 34—38.
Nierop, A., Bratsikas, A., Klinkenberg, A., Nater, U.M., Zimmermann, Smith, P.M., 1996. Mechanisms of secretion by salivary glands. In:
R., Ehlert, U., 2006. Prolonged salivary cortisol recovery in Edgar, W.M., O’Mullane, D.M. (Eds.), Saliva and Oral Health.
second-trimester pregnant women and attenuated salivary British Dental Association, London, pp. 9—25.
{alpha}-amylase responses to psychosocial stress in human preg- Southwick, S.M., Krystal, J.H., Morgan, C.A., Johnson, D., Nagy,
nancy. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 91, 1329—1335. L.M., Nicolaou, A., Heninger, G.R., Charney, D.S., 1993. Abnormal
Noto, Y., Sato, T., Kudo, M., Kurata, K., Hirota, K., 2005. The noradrenergic function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch.
relationship between salivary biomarkers and state-trait anxiety Gen. Psychiatry 50, 266—274.
inventory score under mental arithmetic stress: a pilot study. Speirs, R.L., Herring, J., Cooper, W.D., Hardy, C.C., Hind, C.R., 1974.
Anesth. Analg. 101, 1873—1876. The influence of sympathetic activity and isoprenaline on the
Payne, J.D., Jackson, E.D., Hoscheidt, S., Ryan, L., Jacobs, W.J., secretion of amylase from the human parotid gland. Arch. Oral
Nadel, L., 2007. Stress administered prior to encoding impairs Biol. 19, 747—752.
neutral but enhances emotional long-term episodic memories. Sreebny, L.M., 2000. Saliva in health and disease: an appraisal and
Learn. Mem. 14, 861—868. update. Int. Dent. J. 50, 140—161.
496 U.M. Nater, N. Rohleder

Steerenberg, P.A., van Asperen, I.A., Van Nieuw Amerongen, A., Vigil, J.M., Flinn, M.V., Granger, D.A., Geary, D.C., in press. Sex
Biewenga, J., Mol, D., Medema, G., 1997. Salivary levels differences in stress responses following Hurricane Katrina. Child
of immunoglobulin A in triathletes. Eur. J. Oral Sci. 105, Dev.
305—309. Vining, R.F., McGinley, R.A., 1987. The measurement of hormones in
Stroud, L.R., Foster, E., Papandonatos, G.D., Handwerger, K., Gran- saliva: possibilities and pitfalls. J. Steroid. Biochem. 27, 81—94.
ger, D.A., Kivlighan, K.T., Niaura, R., 2009. Stress response and Walsh, N.P., Blannin, A.K., Clark, A.M., Cook, L., Robson, P.J.,
the adolescent transition: performance versus peer rejection Gleeson, M., 1999. The effects of high-intensity intermittent
stressors. Dev. Psychopathol. 21 (1), 47—68. exercise on saliva IgA, total protein and alpha-amylase. J. Sports
Stroud, L.R., Handwerger, K., Granger, D.A., Kivlighan, K.T., 2006. Sci. 17, 129—134.
Salivary alpha-amylase stress reactivity in children and adoles- West, S.G., Granger, D.A., Kivlighan, K.T., Hurston, K.L., 2006.
cents: validity, associations with cortisol, and links to behavior–— Salivary alpha-amylase response to the cold pressor is corralled
abstract. Psychosom. Med. 68, A5. with cardiac markers of sympathetic activation–—abstract. Psy-
Takai, N., Yamaguchi, M., Aragaki, T., Eto, K., Uchihashi, K., Nishi- chosom. Med. 68, A4—A5.
kawa, Y., 2004. Effect of psychological stress on the salivary Wetherell, M.A., Crown, A.L., Lightman, S.L., Miles, J.N., Kaye, J.,
cortisol and amylase levels in healthy young adults. Arch. Oral Vedhara, K., 2006. The four-dimensional stress test: psychologi-
Biol. 49, 963—968. cal, sympathetic—adrenal—medullary, parasympathetic and
Takai, N., Yamaguchi, M., Aragaki, T., Eto, K., Uchihashi, K., Nishi- hypothalamic—pituitary—adrenal responses following inhalation
kawa, Y., 2007. Gender-specific differences in salivary biomarker of 35% CO2. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 736—747.
responses to acute psychological stress. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. Wolf, J.M., Nicholls, E., Chen, E., 2008. Chronic stress, salivary
1098, 510—515. cortisol, and alpha-amylase in children with asthma and healthy
Tenovuo, J., 1998. Antimicrobial function of human saliva–—how children. Biol. Psychol. 78, 20—28.
important is it for oral health? Acta Odontol. Scand. 56, 250— Yamaguchi, M., Takeda, K., Onishi, M., Deguchi, M., Higashi, T.,
256. 2006a. Non-verbal communication method based on a biochem-
Tu, M.T., Lupien, S.J., Walker, C.D., 2006. Multiparity reveals the ical marker for people with severe motor and intellectual dis-
blunting effect of breastfeeding on physiological reactivity to abilities. J. Int. Med. Res. 34, 30—41.
psychological stress. J. Neuroendocrinol. 18, 494—503. Yamaguchi, M., Wakasugi, J., Sakakima, J., 2006b. Evaluation of
van Stegeren, A., Rohleder, N., Everaerd, W., Wolf, O.T., 2006. driver stress using biomarker in motor-vehicle driving simulator.
Salivary alpha amylase as marker for adrenergic activity during Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 1, 1834—1837.
stress: effect of betablockade. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, Yamakage, M., Hayase, T., Satoh, J.I., Namiki, A., 2007. Work stress
137—141. in medical anaesthesiology trainees. Eur. J. Anaesthesiol. 24,
van Stegeren, A.H., Wolf, O.T., Kindt, M., 2008. Salivary alpha 809—811.
amylase and cortisol responses to different stress tasks: impact Zakowski, J.J., Bruns, D.E., 1985. Biochemistry of human alpha
of sex. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 69, 33—40. amylase isoenzymes. Crit. Rev. Clin. Lab. Sci. 21, 283—322.

You might also like