You are on page 1of 10

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Affective, sensory and empathic sharing of another’s pain: The


Empathy for Pain Scale
M.J. Giummarra1,2, B.M. Fitzgibbon3, N. Georgiou-Karistianis1, M. Beukelman1, A. Verdejo-Garcia1,
Z. Blumberg4, M. Chou4,5, S.J. Gibson2,6,7
1 School of Psychological Science, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
2 Caulfield Pain Management and Research Centre, Caulfield Hospital, Australia
3 Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Centre, Central Clinical School, The Alfred Hospital, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
4 Rehabilitation Services, Caulfield Hospital, Australia
5 Amputee Unit, Caulfield Hospital, Australia
6 National Ageing Research Institute, Parkville, Australia
7 Faculty of Medicine, Dentistry and Health Sciences, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Australia

Correspondence Abstract
Melita Joy Giummarra
E-mail: melita.giummarra@monash.edu Background: Through two studies, we introduce and validate the
Empathy for Pain Scale (EPS), which characterizes the phenomenology of
Funding sources empathy for pain, including the vicarious experience of pain when seeing
This project was supported by a small project
others in pain.
grant from Caulfield Hospital, and bridging
fellowships awarded to MJG and BMF from
Methods: In study 1, 406 individuals completed the EPS and
the Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI). In the EPS, four painful scenarios
Sciences, Monash University. MJG and BMF (witnessing surgery, patient recovering from surgery, assault and
are supported by NHMRC early career accidental injury) were rated for 12 emotional, empathic and sensory
fellowships (APP1036124, APP1070073). responses. In study 2, 59 participants completed the same questionnaires
and then watched and rated videos of sporting injuries.
Conflicts of interest
Results: In study 1, we identified three factors of the EPS with principal
None declared.
component analysis, which were validated with confirmatory factor
Accepted for publication analysis: affective distress; vicarious pain; and empathic concern. The EPS
22 September 2014 demonstrated good psychometric properties, re-test reliability (n = 105)
and concurrent validity. In study 2, we validated the EPS against empathic
doi:10.1002/ejp.607 reactions to the pain of others as displayed in video clips depicting sporting
injuries and showed that the scale has unique utility to characterize
empathic reactions to pain above general trait empathy measures. Both
studies showed that the affective distress and empathic concern subscales
of the EPS correlated with measures of cognitive and affective empathy
from the IRI, whereas the vicarious pain subscale was only correlated with
the personal distress IRI subscale.
Conclusions: The EPS is a psychometrically sound new scale that
characterizes empathy for pain and vicarious pain. The EPS offers valuable
insight to the phenomenological profile of the affective, empathic and
sensory dimensions of empathy for pain.

others involves cognitive appraisal, in which we seek to


1. Introduction
represent the internal mental states of others, also
Empathy is interpersonal social experience that referred to as theory of mind (Blair, 2005), and emo-
involves affective resonance, cognitive appraisal and tional or affective appraisal in which we resonate with
emotion regulation, which together enhance pro- the emotional state of others through empathic
social interactions (Decety, 2010). Empathizing with concern or vicarious emotional responses (Reniers

© 2014 European Pain Federation - EFIC® Eur J Pain •• (2014) ••–•• 1


The Empathy for Pain Scale M.J. Giummarra et al.

What’s already known about this topic? While the neural correlates of empathy for anoth-
• The neural correlates of ‘empathy for pain’ are er’s pain are well characterized (Lamm et al., 2011;
well established. Bernhardt and Singer, 2012), the need to better
• Individual differences are associated with understand the phenomenology of how we empathize
increased (e.g., trait empathy, in-/out-group with others in pain remains. Many studies have failed
biases) and reduced (e.g., autism spectrum disor- to find an association between trait measures of
ders) affective or sensory resonance for others’ empathy and objective neurophysiological assessment
pain. of empathic reactions to others’ pain (Jackson et al.,
2005). This implies that dispositional empathic reac-
tions to others in pain only partially overlap with
What does this study add?
empathic traits in general, and that there is something
• Validation of a new scale that characterizes the
unique to the context of pain that modulates empathic
dispositional profile of empathic, sensory and
responses. In order to better understand the multidi-
affective responses to pain in others.
mensional nature of how we empathize with others in
pain, there is a clear need for tailored psychometric
et al., 2011). Further, sensorimotor dimensions of tools specific to empathy for pain, which are currently
empathy involve evaluating the experiences and lacking. Moreover, considering vicarious pain and dis-
behaviours of others through shared or ‘mirrored’ tress are thought to lead to avoidance rather than
neurophysiological pathways in order to understand empathic pro-social responses (Bernhardt and Singer,
their internal and motivational states (Keysers et al., 2012), being able to characterize these specific dispo-
2010). When witnessing others in pain, most of us sitional traits is of major importance for future
experience embodied empathic reactions to the other research.
person’s distress. Most research in this field has In two studies, we introduce and validate a new
focused on characterizing the neural networks questionnaire: the Empathy for Pain Scale (EPS). We
involved empathizing with others in pain. These aimed to identify the subscales of the EPS and
studies have found that thinking about another in pain examine their validity and reliability. Study 1 admin-
arouses cognitive and affective empathic appraisals, istered the EPS to over 400 individuals to identify the
with increased activation throughout the subscales, test their convergent validity with existing
motivational-affective brain networks (e.g., anterior trait empathy measures and evaluate re-test reliability.
insula, frontal operculum and cingulate cortices; Study 2 examined the convergent validity of the EPS
Singer et al., 2004). Seeing another in pain also brings subscales, compared with alternative measures of trait
about sensorimotor empathic appraisals, with empathy, against ratings of pain intensity and emotion
increased activation in neural regions underpinning in short video clips of sporting injuries.
sensory processing and perspective taking (e.g., soma-
tosensory and inferior parietal cortices; see meta-
analysis: Lamm et al., 2011). Furthermore, while
2. Methods and results
seeing others in pain elicits sensorimotor empathic
2.1 Study 1: EPS subscales identification
resonance, 16–30% of the population also report
and validation
vicarious sensory experiences of pain (Fitzgibbon
et al., 2010; Osborn and Derbyshire, 2010). In this
2.1.1 Participants
context, vicarious (Latin origin: ‘vicãrius’ meaning
‘substituted’) pain is defined as a spontaneous experi- Four hundred and six individuals participated in study 1. A
ence or exacerbation of pain when seeing another in wide cross-section of people were recruited, including 152
pain. Numerous studies have found that seeing others individuals with idiopathic chronic pain conditions (106
in pain increases pain intensity, unpleasantness (Craig with chronic pain, 62% female, age: 21–82 years, M = 50,
and Weiss, 1971), and nociceptive reflexes SD = 15; 46 with chronic-intermittent pain, 41% female,
age: 21.6–91, M = 51, SD = 14), 68 with post-amputation
(Vachon-Presseau et al., 2011), and reduces pain tol-
pain (37% female age: 21–91 years, M = 55, SD = 12) and
erance (de Wied and Verbaten, 2001) and threshold
245 pain-free controls (69% female; age: 18–83, M = 34,
(Morrison et al., 2012). Although vicarious pain SD = 14). Chronic pain was defined in accordance with the
appears to be related to heightened pain-related International Association for the Study of Pain taxonomy
anxiety and attention to pain, it is not known how it (Merskey et al., 2011) as pain that has persisted for more
relates to the key dimensions of empathy (i.e., cogni- than 3 months and/or the expected time to recover from
tive, affective, sensorimotor). injury. Participants were recruited from Monash University

2 Eur J Pain •• (2014) ••–•• © 2014 European Pain Federation - EFIC®


M.J. Giummarra et al. The Empathy for Pain Scale

students and staff (N = 165), community advertisements ous painful sensations; see Supporting Information Appen-
(N = 126), amputee support groups (N = 8), and the dix S1 for further details), which will be reported elsewhere.
Amputee Unit (N = 59) and Pain Management & Research The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) (Davis, 1980) is a
Centre (N = 47) at Caulfield Hospital in Australia (see Sup- 28-item measure of trait empathy that comprises four sub-
porting Information Table S1 for further details on recruit- scales: perspective taking (predisposition to assume the psy-
ment and participant demographics). chological outlook of another person; Cronbach’s alpha =
Of the initial 406 participants, 253 were invited to com- 0.54), fantasy scale (tendency to project oneself in the persona
plete the EPS a second time via email in order to determine of a fictional character; Cronbach’s alpha = 0.77), empathic
the test–retest reliability. One hundred and five responded concern (ability to feel sympathy and compassion for another;
(42% response rate; M = 5.70 months later; SD = 3.6 Cronbach’s alpha = 0.69) and personal distress (the extent to
months), including 64 (61%) pain-free individuals, 15 which an individual feels anxiety as a result of witnessing
amputees and 41 individuals with chronic or chronic- another’s emotional distress; Cronbach’s alpha = 0.50). Cron-
intermittent pain. bach’s alphas reported above are for the present sample. The
IRI has previously been reported to have high internal con-
sistency and good test–retest reliability and convergent valid-
2.1.2 Materials and procedure ity (Davis, 1983).

The study was approved by local hospital and university


human research ethics committees, and all participants pro- 2.1.3 Data analysis
vided informed consent. Participants completed a simple
The data were analysed with IBM SPSS version 20.0 and
demographics survey and measures of empathy and
AMOS version 20.0. Analyses included principal component
empathy for pain. The demographics survey included ques-
analysis (PCA), confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and reli-
tions about whether participants were experiencing acute or
ability testing. Prior to PCA, a mean (‘aggregate’) of each
chronic pain and, where relevant, asked for further details
emotion and sensation rating was generated across the four
about the cause, frequency, duration, intensity and impact of
scenarios to reflect average empathic responses to pain in
pain with the brief pain inventory (Cleeland, 1989) and
others. PCA with varimax rotation was performed with the
graded chronic pain scale (Von Korff et al., 1992).
12 aggregated items to identify the probable subscales of the
The EPS was developed for this study (see Supporting
EPS. Component loadings were retained if they were ≥±0.40
Information Appendix S1). The items in the EPS were based
(Hair et al., 1998), and eigenvalues greater than 1.0 were
on examination of the vast empathy for pain literature to
retained as component factors. The fit of the factor structure
identify the key types of phenomena investigated in relation
identified in the PCA was examined for goodness of fit using
to empathy for pain, and focus group discussion with people
CFA, with a maximum likelihood extraction. Modification
who experience vicarious pain, who were identified from
indices were examined to account for cross-loadings and
our previous studies (Giummarra and Bradshaw, 2008;
correlations between items. Model fit was based on exami-
Fitzgibbon et al., 2010). In this scale, empathy for pain was
nation of the root mean square residual (RMR: <0.05),
measured across 4 scenarios using 12 identical items rated on
goodness-of-fit index (GFI: >0.95), comparative fit index
a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree). The
(CFI: >0.95) and root mean square error of approximation
scenarios were: (1) a person undergoing a surgical procedure
(RMSEA: <0.05) (Byrne, 2009). To ensure the applicability of
(e.g., as part of a television hospital drama); (2) a person who
the subscales in both the chronic pain and the pain-free
has recently had a surgical procedure (e.g., they have stitches or
groups, CFA was conducted first in the whole sample, and
bandaged amputation stump); (3) a person being accidentally
then in the chronic pain and pain-free samples. Subscale
injured (e.g., in a car accident); and (4) a person being physi-
scores were the average of the items belonging to each
cally assaulted. The four scenarios were selected so that the
subscale.
EPS could be used to examine both ‘general’ empathy for
Psychometric properties (internal consistency and re-test
pain (average response to others in pain), as well as context-
reliability) for the subscales were examined. Re-test reliabil-
specific scenarios. The 12 response items were distress, dis-
ity of the subscales was tested using within-subjects
comfort, disgust, fear, restlessness, sense of compassion,
repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) and
sense of what it feels like, a need to get help, a desire to look
Pearson correlations. Finally, we examined the concurrent
away, non-painful sensations, painful sensations and visceral
validity of the subscales against the subscales of the IRI using
sensations (e.g., nausea). Prior to the present study, a longer
correlation analysis.
version of the scale was piloted in 24 individuals, including
17 prone to vicarious pain (Giummarra et al., 2012). As the
scale aimed to characterize vicarious pain, three scenarios 2.1.4 Results
that did not elicit these experiences in people who were
normally prone to vicarious pain were removed.
2.1.4.1 Component structure of the EPS
When participants indicated experiences of vicarious pain
they were asked additional questions about the nature of PCA with varimax rotation of the aggregate ratings
that pain (e.g., onset, intensity, location and quality of vicari- explained 72% of the variance. All but one loading was

© 2014 European Pain Federation - EFIC® Eur J Pain •• (2014) ••–•• 3


The Empathy for Pain Scale M.J. Giummarra et al.

Table 1 Component loadingsa and communalities (h2) for principal com- all observed variables (see Table 2), except the ‘need to help’
ponent analysis with varimax rotation of aggregate data. variable (β = 0.42). Examination of the goodness-of-fit sta-
Component tistics (RMR = 0.03, GFI = 0.95; CFI = 0.98; RMSEA = 0.05)
showed that the model fit was supported in this sample. In
Construct 1 2 3 h2 both the chronic pain and pain-free samples, the uncon-
Distress 0.78 0.75 strained estimates of the regression weights were all signifi-
Discomfort 0.80 0.76 cant, and the latent constructs explained a moderate to large
Disgust 0.78 0.63 portion of the variance (except ‘need to help’ variable in the
Restless 0.70 0.77 pain-free sample; R2 = 0.28).
Fear 0.77 0.72
Avoidance 0.76 0.69
Visceral sensations 0.73 0.72 2.1.4.4 Reliability and psychometric properties of
State empathy 0.75 0.69
the EPS
Compassion 0.86 0.78
Need to help 0.59 0.52 The psychometric properties of the 12-item EPS were strong,
Vicarious sensations 0.86 0.83 with high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.91)
Vicarious pain 0.86 0.83 and good reliability (split-half reliability of 0.91). Each of the
Rotated eigenvalues 6.43 1.14 1.12 subscales of affective distress (Cronbach’s alpha: 0.93),
% Variance explained 53.55 9.50 9.36 vicarious pain (Cronbach’s alpha: 0.83) and empathic
a
Loadings ≤0.40 are suppressed. concern (Cronbach’s alpha: 0.70) also showed moderate to
high levels of internal consistency. A repeated-measures
ANOVA found that the affective distress (MT1 = 2.88;
≥0.70, and no item loaded on more than one factor, indicat- MT2 = 2.92), vicarious pain (MT1 = 1.97; MT2 = 2.04) and
ing that it is a robust model. A three-factor solution was empathic concern (MT1 = 3.39; MT2 = 3.50) EPS subscale
obtained on the basis of the scree test. See Table 1 for load- scores did not differ significantly between time 1 and time 2;
ings and communalities for the Varimax-rotated factors. The F(3,102) = 1.61, p = 0.19. Correlations between time 1 and 2 for
first component was labelled ‘affective distress’, the second the affective distress (r(105) = 0.79, p < 0.001), vicarious pain
‘empathic concern’ and the third ‘vicarious pain’. (r(105) = 0.65, p < 0.001) and empathic concern (r(105) = 0.62,
p < 0.001) subscales also demonstrated strong re-test
reliability.
2.1.4.2 EPS factor structure validation: CFA
CFA was conducted in the full sample (N = 406) to examine
the validity of the component structure indicated by the PCA. 2.1.4.5 EPS: Concurrent validity
Examination of the goodness-of-fit statistics showed good fit The concurrent validity of the EPS was examined against the
(RMR = 0.04; GFI = 0.91; CFI = 0.94; RMSEA = 0.09). Some IRI subscales. The affective distress EPS subscale was signifi-
cross-loadings were identified and once these were accom- cantly correlated with the perspective taking [r(390) = 0.13,
modated, the model fit improved further (RMR = 0.03; p < 0.05], fantasy scale [r(390) = 0.28, p < 0.001], empathic
GFI = 0.96; CFI = 0.98; RMSEA = 0.05). Over 80% of factor concern [r(390) = 0.20, p < 0.001] and personal distress
loadings were ≥0.70, all unconstrained estimates of the [r(390) = 0.31, p < 0.001] subscales of the IRI. Likewise, the
regression weights were significant, and the latent variables empathic concern subscale of the EPS was significantly cor-
explained a moderate to large portion of the variance in the related with perspective taking (rs = 0.27, p < 0.001), fantasy
respective observed variables. See Table 2 for all regression scale (rs = 0.15, p < 0.01), empathic concern (rs = 0.33,
statistics and coefficients, and cross-loading regression p < 0.001) and personal distress (rs = 0.11, p < 0.05) subscales
weights. of the IRI. Vicarious pain was correlated with personal dis-
tress (rs = 0.12, p < 0.05), but not with any of the other
subscales of the IRI.
2.1.4.3 Validation of the factor structure in chronic
pain and pain-free groups
In the chronic pain sample (N = 152), a CFA of the three- 2.2 Study 2: Validation of the EPS subscales
factor model was supported by standardized factor loadings against actual empathic reactions to
≥0.60 on 83% of the observed variables (see Table 2), except others’ pain
the distress (β = 0.54) and ‘need to help’ (β = 0.52) variables.
Examination of the goodness-of-fit statistics (RMR = 0.05,
2.2.1 Participants
GFI = 0.92; CFI = 0.97; RMSEA = 0.07) shows that the
model has a good fit to measure empathy for pain in people Fifty-nine individuals, aged 18–55 (median = 23), partici-
with chronic pain. In the pain-free sample (N = 245), a CFA pated in response to university and community advertise-
of the three-factor model resulted in factor loadings ≥0.60 on ments. The sample was predominantly female (n = 46; 78%)

4 Eur J Pain •• (2014) ••–•• © 2014 European Pain Federation - EFIC®


M.J. Giummarra et al.

Table 2 Goodness-of-fit statistics, regression statistics (R2), standardized (β) and unstandardized (B) coefficients and scalar estimates (SE) for full model of confirmatory factor analysis, after post hoc
modifications in the full sample, and specific to the chronic pain and pain-free samples.

Full sample Chronic pain sample Pain-free sample

© 2014 European Pain Federation - EFIC®


N = 406 N = 152 N = 245

RMR = 0.03; GFI = 0.96; CFI = 0.98; RMSEA = 0.05 RMR = 0.05, GFI = 0.92; CFI = 0.97; RMSEA = 0.07 RMR = 0.03, GFI = 0.95; CFI = 0.98; RMSEA = 0.05
2 2
Observed variable Latent variable R β B SE R β B SE R2 β B SE

Fear Affective distress 0.64 0.80 1.21 0.09 0.77 0.88 1.32 0.18 0.53 0.73 1.12 0.10
Distress Affective distress 0.45 0.61 1.00 0.35 0.54 1.00 0.58 0.69 1.00
Discomfort Affective distress 0.73 0.79 1.24 0.09 0.70 0.79 1.28 0.18 0.76 0.80 1.21 0.09
Disgust Affective distress 0.48 0.69 0.93 0.08 0.54 0.74 0.98 0.14 0.45 0.67 0.92 0.09
Restless Affective distress 0.76 0.87 1.42 0.10 0.86 0.93 1.45 0.19 0.68 0.83 1.40 0.11
Avoidance Affective distress 0.65 0.84 1.33 0.10 0.75 0.90 1.37 0.18 0.57 0.78 1.28 0.11
Visceral sensations Affective distress 0.71 0.84 1.38 0.10 0.78 0.89 1.43 0.19 0.62 0.79 1.33 0.11
Vicarious sensations Vicarious Pain 0.76 0.87 1.00 0.82 0.91 1.00 0.75 0.86 1.00
Vicarious pain Vicarious Pain 0.69 0.83 0.83 0.06 0.73 0.85 0.88 0.08 0.64 0.80 0.76 0.07
State empathy Empathic concern 0.51 0.72 1.00 0.45 0.67 1.00 0.55 0.74 1.00
A need to help Empathic concern 0.37 0.50 0.57 0.06 0.45 0.55 0.56 0.10 0.28 0.42 0.54 0.09
Compassion Empathic concern 0.58 0.82 0.82 0.07 0.49 0.77 0.71 0.11 0.59 0.82 0.89 0.10
Cross-loadings
Help Fear 0.18 0.16 0.19 0.16 0.17 0.16
Distress A need to help 0.13 0.15 0.10 0.15 0.15 0.15
Discomfort Distress 0.10 0.09 0.10 0.09 0.09 0.09
Discomfort Vicarious pain −0.10 −0.10 −0.10 −0.10 −0.10 −0.10
Discomfort Compassion 0.10 0.13 0.09 0.13 0.11 0.13
Avoid A need to help −0.08 −0.09 −0.08 −0.09 −0.08 −0.09
Compassion Vicarious pain −0.12 −0.09 −0.12 −0.09 −0.12 −0.09

RMR, root mean square residual; GFI, goodness-of-fit index; CFI, comparative fit index; RMSEA, root mean square error. of approximation.

Eur J Pain •• (2014) ••–••


5
The Empathy for Pain Scale
The Empathy for Pain Scale M.J. Giummarra et al.

and were mostly students (n = 40; 68%), scientists (n = 7, and vicarious pain subscales of the EPS correlated with the
12%), or administrative assistants or professionals (n = 11; perspective taking and personal distress subscales of the IRI,
19%). whereas the empathic concern subscale of the EPS only
correlated with the IRI empathic concern subscales (see
Table 3).
2.2.2 Materials and procedure The concurrence between the EPS subscales and ratings of
the injury video clips were examined using correlation
The study was approved by the university human research
analyses. The affective distress subscale was correlated with
ethics committee, and all participants provided informed
the participant’s ratings of their own distress (r(53) = 0.29,
consent. Participants completed a simple demographics
p < 0.05), arousal (r(53) = 0.28, p < 0.05) and the intensity of
survey and the EPS and IRI, described in study 1. They were
vicarious pain (r(53) = 0.41, p < 0.01). The vicarious pain sub-
then asked to watch 10 short clips of professional athletes
scale was correlated with the participants’ arousal
playing Australian Rules Football. In each of these clips, a
(r(53) = 0.28, p < 0.05) and the intensity of vicarious pain
player sustained a painful injury (e.g., knee dislocation, head
(rs = 0.52, p < 0.001), and the empathic concern subscale
clash, leg fracture). After each clip, participants were
correlated with evaluations of the players’ emotions
prompted to rate the intensity of pain (R1; 0 = no pain;
((53)r = 0.31, p < 0.05), and the participants’ experience of
10 = worst possible pain) and emotions (R2; 0 = very negative;
discomfort ((53)r = 0.28, p < 0.05) and arousal ((53)r = 0.28,
10 = very positive) in the player, and their own discomfort
p < 0.05) when viewing the injury videos. The IRI subscales,
(R3; 0 = no discomfort; 10 = extreme discomfort), arousal (R4;
on the other hand, showed slightly different associations
0 = calm; 10 = excited) and vicarious pain (R5; 0 = no pain;
with the empathic reactions to others’ pain. The perspective
10 = strong pain) on numerical rating scales (NRSs) with
taking (r(59) = 0.29, p < 0.05) and empathic concern
semantic anchors at each extreme of the rating scale.
(r(59) = 0.31, p < 0.05) subscales were significantly correlated
with ratings of the intensity of the players’ pain and the
personal distress subscale only correlated with the intensity
2.2.3 Data analysis of vicarious pain (rs = 0.32, p < 0.05). The fantasy scale did
We first examined the psychometric properties (internal con- not correlate with any ratings, and no IRI subscales were
sistency) of the subscales of the EPS and IRI in this sample, as associated with ratings of the players’ emotions, or the par-
well as the consistency in ratings of the injury video clips. We ticipants’ own discomfort or arousal.
used correlation analyses to investigate whether we could An independent samples t-test demonstrated significant
replicate the associations between the IRI and EPS subscales differences in the vicarious pain subscale scores between
reported in study 1. Next, correlation analyses examined groups who then reported experiencing vicarious pain when
whether the EPS and IRI subscales corresponded to actual viewing the sporting injuries (M = 3.55, SD = 1.66) com-
empathic reactions to the pain of others (i.e., players’ pain pared with those who did not report vicarious pain
and emotions, and the participants’ own discomfort, arousal (M = 0.15, SD = 0.38); t(57) = −13.11, p < 0.0001. A linear
and vicarious pain). Because the vicarious pain subscale is regression assessed the ability of the EPS subscales (empathic
novel, we undertook two final analyses to examine whether concern, vicarious pain and affective distress) to predict the
it was a sensitive measure of vicarious pain. First, an inde- intensity of vicarious pain. The model explained 38.6% of
pendent samples t-test was conducted to demonstrate the variance, F(3, 49) = 10.25, p < 0.001. Moreover, the
whether the vicarious pain subscales, completed before par- vicarious pain subscale significantly predicted vicarious pain
ticipants observed the video clips, were higher in those who intensity (β = 66, p < 0.001), but the affective distress
then reported vicarious pain to the injury clips compared (β = .−0.16, p = 0.31) and empathic concern (β = 0.12,
with those who did not. A linear regression examined p = 0.34) subscales did not.
whether the intensity of vicarious pain when viewing the
injury video clips was predicted by the vicarious pain
subscale. 3. Discussion
These studies achieved our objective to identify and
2.2.4 Results validate the subscales of the EPS. The subscales were
psychometrically sound, replicable between samples
The ratings of the injury video clips were highly consistent, (i.e., chronic pain and pain free), reliable over time,
whereby they were rated on average as moderately painful
and concurred with empathic appraisals and
(M = 6.14/10; Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83) with negative emo-
responses when witnessing others in pain. The EPS
tions in the players (M = 1.34/10; Cronbach’s alpha = 0.73),
see Table 3. The clips elicited vicarious pain in 20 participants measured three core components of empathy for
(34%), consistent with prevalence rates reported in study 1 pain: affective distress (distress, discomfort, fear,
and previous studies (Osborn and Derbyshire, 2010). The avoidance, restlessness, visceral sensations), vicarious
psychometric properties of the IRI subscales were low to pain (both non-painful and painful vicarious sensa-
moderate, like in study 1. In study 2, the affective distress tions) and empathic concern (feelings of compassion,

6 Eur J Pain •• (2014) ••–•• © 2014 European Pain Federation - EFIC®


M.J. Giummarra et al.

© 2014 European Pain Federation - EFIC®


Table 3 Correlation matrix of stimulus ratings and questionnaire subscales.

Injury stimuli ratings IRI EPS

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5
Player’s pain Player’s emotion Own discomfort Own arousal Vicarious paina PT FS PD EC AD VP EC

Injury stimuli R1: Player’s pain 1


ratings R2: Player’s emotion −0.57*** 1
R3: Own discomfort 0.42*** −0.41*** 1
R4: Own arousal 0.15 (ns) −0.06 (ns) 0.76*** 1
R5: Vicarious paina 0.09 (ns) −0.09 (ns) 0.33* 0.27* 1.000
IRI PT 0.29* −0.01 (ns) 0.19 (ns) 0.13 (ns) 0.23 (ns) 1
FS −0.11 (ns) 0.05 (ns) 0.05 (ns) 0.07 (ns) −0.06 (ns) 0.19 (ns) 1
PD 0.21 (ns) −0.02 (ns) 0.22 (ns) 0.26 (ns) 0.32* 0.36** 0.31* 1
EC 0.31* −0.17 (ns) 0.21 (ns) 0.14 (ns) −0.09 (ns) 0.58*** 0.23 (ns) 0.14 (ns) 1
EPS AD 0.14 (ns) −0.13 (ns) 0.29* 0.28* 0.41** 0.45*** −0.06 (ns) 0.34* 0.23 (ns) 1
VP −0.08 (ns) 0.14 (ns) 0.19 (ns) 0.28* 0.52*** 0.29* −0.05 (ns) 0.39** 0.05 (ns) 0.62*** 1
EC 0.22 (ns) −0.29* 0.28* 0.28* 0.25 (ns) 0.26 (ns) −0.03 (ns) 0.18 (ns) 0.37** 0.45*** 0.35* 1

R1: players pain intensity (0 = no pain; 10 = worst possible pain); R2: players emotions (0 = very negative; 10 = very positive); R3: Participants own discomfort (0 = no discomfort; 10 = extreme discomfort);
R4: Player’s own arousal (0 = calm; 10 = excited); R5: player’s vicarious pain (0 = no pain; 10 = strong pain).
*p < 0.05.
**p < 0.01.
***p < 0.001.
a
Non-parametric correlations with these ratings.

Eur J Pain •• (2014) ••–••


7
The Empathy for Pain Scale
The Empathy for Pain Scale M.J. Giummarra et al.

desire to help, state empathy). The affective distress


3.1 Why are empathy and empathy for
subscale comprised many more items than the latter
pain different?
two subscales; however, we suggest that this indicates
that affective reactions to others’ pain are a complex Trait empathy is a stable personality characteristic that
and multifaceted sub-dimension of empathy for pain. enables us to share and understand the emotional and
While the affective distress and empathic concern sensory experiences of others. However, pain is a
subscales concurred with cognitive and affective trait unique sensory and emotional experience that can
empathy, they also demonstrated divergent validity elicit different types of empathic or compassionate
and showed better associations with actual appraisal responses compared with other social situations.
of others in pain compared with measures of trait Whether we experience pain directly, or others are in
empathy. The EPS offers a novel way to evaluate pain, we are evolutionarily programmed to allocate
vicarious pain, which was only correlated with the attention to it, appraise this threat and respond adap-
personal distress dimension of empathy in the IRI. tively (Van Damme et al., 2010). Cognitive-affective
Ultimately, the EPS measures unique, although inter- appraisals of pain mobilize and regulate homeostatic
related, aspects of empathy for pain and makes a sig- and physiological states in readiness for a behavioural
nificant contribution to how we measure and response (Bufalari and Ionta, 2013). Evaluations of
understand this multidimensional interpersonal pain therefore correspond to the emotional and
process. sensory qualities of pain in that moment as well as
In study 1, we found that the affective distress and factors such as the future implications of the pain or
empathic concern subscales of the EPS were correlated injury. Moreover, the way that we respond empathi-
with both cognitive (i.e., perspective taking and cally to another’s pain appears to be influenced by
empathic concern) and affective (i.e., fantasy scale and prior experience (Preis and Kroener-Herwig, 2012).
personal distress) dimensions of empathy measured by The unique characteristics of pain have key impli-
the IRI. However, in study 2, we found that the IRI cations for how its empathic appraisal should be
subscales were not associated with the intensity of studied should be studied. Some studies report that
discomfort or arousal when witnessing others in pain, the tendency to project oneself into the persona of a
while the EPS subscales were. The ‘need to help’ item fictional character (IRI fantasy scale) is related to
loaded poorly on the empathic concern subscale of the increased motivational-affective appraisal of another’s
EPS, implying that there is a distinction between pain (Costantini et al., 2008), and we found it to be
empathic feelings and compassionate motivations in correlated with increased empathic concern and affec-
the context of pain. Taken together, empathy is a mul- tive distress (EPS subscales; study 1). However, it was
tidimensional construct comprising affective, not related to any empathic appraisals of actual
empathic and sensory dimensions, which are well dynamic stimuli depicting athletes in pain in our study
characterized by the EPS subscales. 2. Moreover, none of the general trait empathy mea-
The vicarious pain subscale measured proneness for sures were associated with appraisals of the injured
vicarious painful and non-painful sensations, and was person’s emotional state, or the individual’s own dis-
correlated with the tendency to experience personal comfort or arousal when actually viewing individuals
distress. This subscale, which participants completed in pain, yet the EPS empathic concern subscale was
before observing any stimuli depicting pain or injury associated with each of these appraisals. The present
in study 2, significantly predicted the intensity of findings therefore concur with previous studies, such
vicarious pain, highlighting that self-report for these that general measures of trait empathy may not be
experiences concurs with actual experiences of vicari- sufficiently sensitive to all empathic reactions to
ous pain. Being able to characterize this phenomenon others’ pain (Avenanti et al., 2005; Jackson et al.,
is of major social importance considering it may 2005).
impact on behaviour when one is confronted with
emergency situations (e.g., horrific accidents) or when
3.2 Future directions
a patient or loved one is in severe pain (Robins et al.,
2009), particularly considering the tendency to expe- There appears to be a lack of applied research address-
rience vicarious distress may motivate withdrawal to ing the clinical and social issues pertinent to empathy
avoid personal distress (Bernhardt and Singer, 2012). for pain. The majority of research on empathy for pain
In sum, personal distress and vicarious pain are to date has predominantly relied on assessments of
uniquely associated, and may not be beneficial for the trait empathy in conjunction with neurophysiological
observer or the observed. or neuroimaging protocols. Large representative

8 Eur J Pain •• (2014) ••–•• © 2014 European Pain Federation - EFIC®


M.J. Giummarra et al. The Empathy for Pain Scale

studies are lacking (Bufalari and Ionta, 2013). The Finally, we conducted our CFA in the same sample as
present study is a first step towards filling this gap by the PCA, which is generally not recommended. Our
validating the EPS, which provides a means of char- purpose was to first explore which items may belong to
acterizing the affective, sensory and empathic trait the subscales, and then apply rigorous structural equa-
profile unique to the context of pain. Moreover, while tion modelling to confirm the variance that each latent
laboratory experiments investigating the neural basis variable explained in the respective subscales. Only
of how we empathize with others in pain have pro- with this method did we identify that the ‘need to help’
vided important insight to the neurophysiological item, with moderate component loadings in the PCA,
mechanisms involved, the ecological validity of these actually loaded poorly on the empathic concern
studies may be questionable. In particular, these subscale.
studies provide limited insight to how cognitive-
affective appraisals of another’s pain are influenced by
both personal trait characteristics unique to pain, and 3.4 Conclusions
with respect to broader socio-economic factors. We demonstrate that empathy for pain is a multidi-
Studies are now required to bring this research out of mensional phenomenon, and that the EPS character-
the laboratory, and to focus on the social context in izes the empathic, affective and sensory responses to
which it may be most important. For example, it is pain in others, which are partially unique from
pertinent to examine how certain traits, specific to general trait empathy. The empathic concern and
pain appraisal, relate to the ability to empathize with affective distress subscales demonstrated both concur-
loved ones in either acute or chronic pain, and how rent and divergent validity with trait empathy. The
these characteristics impact on their role in helping a vicarious pain subscale correlated with personal dis-
loved one to cope with their pain (Issner et al., 2012). tress, making an important and novel contribution to
Similarly, although clinicians must regulate their characterizing of this phenomenon. Although the EPS
empathic resonance to protect themselves from subscales characterized the tendency to experience
burnout or secondary trauma (Cheng et al., 2007; affective distress, empathic concern or vicarious pain,
Robins et al., 2009), patients must nonetheless expe- and not the intensity of these experiences, the vicari-
rience that these interactions are empathic for ous pain subscale was nonetheless a significant predic-
maximum benefit (Shaw, 2004). Patients who per- tor of the intensity of vicarious pain. Future research
ceive a lack of empathy have greater psychological should investigate whether these characteristics are
distress and poorer recovery (Murgatroyd et al., predictive of approach or avoidance tendencies, espe-
2011), whereas perceiving affective and cognitive cially pro-social and altruistic responses towards
reassurance is associated with improved clinical out- others who are suffering.
comes (Pincus et al., 2013). Evidently, how we empa-
thize with others in pain, and how they perceive that
empathy, have major implications when applied to Author contributions
these contexts. Finally, it appears that empathizing
Each co-author contributed significantly to the work
with others in pain has unique variance that is only
described, discussed the results and commented on the
partly explained by trait empathy, but that is measured manuscript. M.J.G. planned and executed studies 1 and 2,
by the EPS. developed the questionnaire, recruited and gained consent
from participants, collected and analysed data, and wrote the
paper with guidance and feedback from the co-authors.
3.3 Limitations
B.M.F. contributed to questionnaire development and
Several limitations of this study must be addressed. design, interpretation of analyses, and writing and editing
First, limited socio-economic participant characteristics the manuscript. M.B. assisted in the design, stimulus editing,
were collected in study 1. However, the majority (63%) data collection and analysis for study 2. N.G-K. contributed
were recruited from outside the university community to research design, interpretation of analyses and editing the
manuscript. A.V.G. contributed to interpretation of analyses,
ensuring that the findings reflect both the general
and writing and editing the manuscript. Z.B. contributed to
population and a highly educated cohort. Second,
research design, data collection, interpretation of the find-
while the IRI is commonly used to measure empathy in ings and editing the manuscript. M.C. contributed to
many clinical and community populations, the internal research design, data collection, interpretation of the find-
consistency for some of the subscales was low in our ings and editing the manuscript. S.J.G. contributed to
studies. This may explain why the IRI correlated poorly research design, data analysis, interpretation of the findings
with the EPS subscales and ratings of pain in study 2. and editing the manuscript.

© 2014 European Pain Federation - EFIC® Eur J Pain •• (2014) ••–•• 9


The Empathy for Pain Scale M.J. Giummarra et al.

Jackson, P.L., Meltzoff, A.N., Decety, J. (2005). How do we perceive the


Acknowledgement pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in
empathy. Neuroimage 24, 771–779.
The authors acknowledge Katrina Simpson for assistance Keysers, C., Kaas, J.H., Gazzola, V. (2010). Somatosensation in social
and advice on statistical analysis. perception. Nat Rev Neurosci 11, 417–428.
Lamm, C., Decety, J., Singer, T. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence
for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly
References experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54, 2492–
2502.
Avenanti, A., Bueti, D., Galati, G., Aglioti, S.M. (2005). Transcranial Merskey, H., Lindblom, U., Mumford, J.M., Nathan, P.W., Sunderland, S.
magnetic stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for (2011). Pain terms: A current list with definitions and notes on usage.
pain. Nat Neurosci 8, 955–960. International Association for the Study of Pain.
Bernhardt, B.C., Singer, T. (2012). The neural basis of empathy. Annu Rev Morrison, I., Tipper, S.P., Fenton-Adams, W.L., Bach, P. (2012). ‘Feeling’
Neurosci 35, 1–23. others’ painful actions: The sensorimotor integration of pain and action
Blair, R.J.R. (2005). Responding to the emotions of others: Dissociating information. Hum Brain Mapp 34, 1982–1998.
forms of empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric popula- Murgatroyd, D.F., Cameron, I.D., Harris, I.A. (2011). Understanding the
tions. Conscious Cogn 14, 698–718. effect of compensation on recovery from severe motor vehicle crash
Bufalari, I., Ionta, S. (2013). The social and personality neuroscience of injuries: A qualitative study. Inj Prev 17, 222–227.
empathy for pain and touch. Front Hum Neurosci 7, 393. Osborn, J., Derbyshire, S.W.G. (2010). Pain sensation evoked by observ-
Byrne, B.M. (2009). Structural Equation Modeling with AMOS: Basic Concepts, ing injury in others. Pain 148, 268–274.
Applications and Programming (New York: Routledge). Pincus, T., Holt, N., Vogel, S., Underwood, M., Savage, R., Walsh, D.A.,
Cheng, Y., Lin, C.-P., Liu, H.-L., Hsu, Y.-Y., Lim, K.-E., Hung, D., Decety, Taylor, S.J.C. (2013). Cognitive and affective reassurance and patient
J. (2007). Expertise modulates the perception of pain in others. Curr Biol outcomes in primary care: A systematic review. Pain 154, 2407–
17, 1708–1713. 2416.
Cleeland, C.S. (1989). Measurement of pain by subjective report. In Preis, M.A., Kroener-Herwig, B. (2012). Empathy for pain: The effects of
Advances in Pain Research and Therapy, Vol. 12, Issues in Pain Measure- prior experience and sex. Eur J Pain 16, 1311–1319.
ment, C.R. Chapman, J.D. Loeser, eds. (New York: Raven Press) pp. Reniers, R., Corcoran, R., Drake, R., Shryane, N.M., Vollm, B.A. (2011).
391–403. The QCAE: A questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy. J Pers
Costantini, M., Galati, G., Romani, G.L., Aglioti, S.M. (2008). Empathic Assess 93, 84–95.
neural reactivity to noxious stimuli delivered to body parts and non- Robins, P.M., Meltzer, L., Zelikovsky, N. (2009). The experience of sec-
corporeal objects. Eur J Neurosci 28, 1222–1230. ondary traumatic stress upon care providers working within a chil-
Craig, K.D., Weiss, S.M. (1971). Vicarious influences on pain-threshold dren’s hospital. J Pediatr Nurs 24, 270–279.
determinations. J Pers Soc Psychol 19, 53–59. Shaw, R. (2004). The embodied psychotherapist: An exploration of the
de Wied, M., Verbaten, M.N. (2001). Affective pictures processing, atten- therapists’ somatic phenomena within the therapeutic encounter. Psy-
tion, and pain tolerance. Pain 90, 163–172. chother Res 14, 271–288.
Davis, M.H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differ- Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R.J., Frith, C.
ences in empathy. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology 10, 85. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory com-
Davis, M.H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evi- ponents of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162.
dence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 44, 113–126. Vachon-Presseau, E., Martel, M.O., Roy, M., Caron, E., Jackson, P.L.,
Decety, J. (2010). The neurodevelopment of empathy in humans. Dev Rainville, P. (2011). The multilevel organization of vicarious pain
Neurosci 32, 257–267. responses: Effects of pain cues and empathy traits on spinal nociception
Fitzgibbon, B.M., Enticott, P.G., Rich, A., Giummarra, M.J., and acute pain. Pain 152, 1525–1531.
Georgiou-Karistianis, N., Tsao, J.W., Weeks, S.R., Bradshaw, J.L. Van Damme, S., Legrain, V., Vogt, J., Crombez, G. (2010). Keeping pain in
(2010). High incidence of ‘synaesthesia for pain’ in amputees. Neuro- mind: A motivational account of attention to pain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
psychologia 48, 3675–3678. 34, 204–213.
Giummarra, M.J., Blumberg, Z., Fitzgibbon, B.M., Gibson, S.J., Chou, M., Von Korff, M., Ormel, J., Keefe, F.J., Dworkin, S.F. (1992). Grading the
Georgiou-Karistianis, N. (2012). A new scale to measure emotion, severity of chronic pain. Pain 50, 133–149.
empathy and pain experience in response to pain in others: A pilot
study 14th World Congress on Pain, Milan.
Giummarra, M.J., Bradshaw, J.L. (2008). Synaesthesia for pain: Feeling
pain with another. In The Role of Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition,
Supporting Information
J.A. Pineda, ed. (San Diego, CA: Humana Press Inc) pp. 287–307.
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the
Hair, J.F., Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.L., Black, W.C. (1998). Multivariate
Data Analysis (Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall). online version of this article at the publisher’s web-site:
Issner, J.B., Cano, A., Leonard, M.T., Williams, A.M. (2012). How do I
empathize with you? Let me count the ways: Relations between facets Table S1. Participant demographics and recruitment.
of pain-related empathy. J Pain 13, 167–175. Appendix S1. Empathy for Pain Scale.

10 Eur J Pain •• (2014) ••–•• © 2014 European Pain Federation - EFIC®

You might also like