You are on page 1of 11

JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 7,421-431 (1968)

On the Use and Meaning of Prepositions

HERBERT H. CLARK~
Carnegie-Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania 15213

This study explored the relationship between the use and meaning of 33 prepositions. The
Ss composed sentences for each preposition and then found sensible substitutes for the prep-
positions; other Ss gave free associations to each preposition; and other Ss grouped the
repositions according to their meaning. Patterns of prepositional similarity, derived for the
three procedures, showed that prepositions with overlapping substitutes generally had
overlappingcontexts insentences, hadoverlappingfreeassociations,andfrequentlybelonged
to the same groups. Free associations were most often the substitutes of a preposition and
next most often nominals differing from, but having common properties with, its objects in
sentences. The results implied: (a) that a S treats two prepositions as being semantically re-
lated to the extent that they are interchangeable in sentences; and (b) that a S implicitly uses
hierarchical phrase structure rules and cognitive categories corresponding to prepositional
and noun phrases when he gives free associations and when he makes judgments about
meaning.

Clearly, a word’s subjective meaning in rarely to the day or toward the day. Naturally
isolation must be related to its uses in sen- occurring sentences which contain to, toward,
tences. The present study was designed to and during, therefore, should show the
explore this relationship for 33 common similarity relations among these three words.
English prepositions. To and toward-as compared with to and
Harris (1954) has proposed some general during, or toward and during-should have
relationships between the possible use of a more sensible substitutes in common, more
word within English syntax and its meaning in prepositional objects in common, and more
isolation. He has argued that two words words in common which are modified by their
similar in meaning will often fit into nearly prepositional phrases. In the present study,
identical contexts, but that two dissimilar the necessary sentences were gathered by
words will require quite different contexts. asking Ss to compose sentences for each of the
His notions lead to the prediction that two 33 prepositions and, later, to find substitutes
nearly synonymous prepositions will often be for the prepositions in their sentences.
found, for example, in prepositional phrases Discovering what a word means indepen-
with the same object. Compare to, toward, and dently of any linguistic context is difficult.
during. A speaker might say to the town or Dictionaries typically define a preposition
toward the town, but rarely during the town; in terms of other prepositions which can
similarly, he might say during the day, but substitute for it in various contexts; diction-
aries do not usually indicate what the prep-
1 This study was supported in part by a National
Science Foundation Graduate Fellowship, by the Bell osition “denotes”. This observation points
Telephone Laboratories, Murray Hill, New Jersey, up the need to treat prepositions and their
and by Public Health Service research grants MH- meanings as a system of relations. Inside
06550-04 and MH-07722 from the National Institute of means something mainly in relation to outside,
Mental Health; it is based on a Ph.D. dissertation, The in, into, within, and other prepositions. In the
Johns Hopkins University, 1966. I am grateful to James
Deese and W. S. Torgerson, who advised and en- present experiments, the 33 prepositions were
couraged me generously throughout the study. studied by examining the similarities in
421
422 CLARK

meaning among them. The total pattern of terns were then compared for their congru-
similarity gives a comprehensive picture of the encies.
semantic relations among all the prepositions.
Two procedures were designed to get Ss to METHOD
reveal their knowledge of words outside of Stimulus Preposiiions. Thirty-three prepositions were
linguistic contexts. In the first, Ss gave free selected from complete lists of English prepositions,
associations to each preposition. Two prep- available in any English handbook. The words chosen
ositions similar in meaning will elicit over- were among the 500 most common English words
lapping sets of associations, so the amount of (Thorndike and Large, 1944) and were prepositions in
their principal usage. The 33 prepositions selected for
overlap is a convenient measure of the pair’s study are listed in Fig. la.
similarity. Deese (1962, 1964, 1966) has used Sentence-Composition Task. The Ss who composed
this method of measuring similarity and has sentences for each preposition were 110 eighth- and
successfully derived interpretable meaning ninth-grade public school students and were, by and
relations within various sets of words. In the large, superior students. They were asked to use each
preposition in “the first simple but good English
second procedure, Ss provided natural group- sentence that came to mind,” but also “to try to write
ings of the 33 prepositions, groupings which sentences about many different things” for different
reflected their meanings. Two prepositions prepositions. (“Simple” here meant simply uncompli-
similar in meaning will often be placed in the cated.) After writing out each sentence, Ss circled the
same group, so a natural measure of their preposition they were to have used in the sentence.
They were reminded that prepositions had objects and
similarity is how often they are grouped were shown a sample written sentence (using behind)
together. with its circled preposition and its object.
It is proposed in the present study that (a) After the Ss had written sentences for all 33 preposi-
people generate sentences which distinguish tions, they were asked to think of a word that could
among the meanings of prepositions and (b) replace the circled preposition in each sentence and to
write it immediately above the circled preposition.
people give free associations and make direct They were told that their sentences, after substitution,
judgments about prepositional meaning using must still make good sense, but need not keep the same
many of the same mechanisms they used in meaning.
generating sentences. Stated differently, the To construct the data forms for sentence composi-
semantic distinctions implicit in free associ- tion, the 33 prepositions were placed in two random
sequences and, for both sequences, were typed along
ations and direct judgments of meaning must the left-hand margins of three successive mimeo-
also be apparent in the surface structure of graphed sheets (11 prepositions per sheet). A given S
language. received the three sheets of either sequence in one of the
From three independent experimental pro- six possible orderings of the three sheets. He wrote his
cedures, five different indices were derived for sentences containing a preposition in the space im-
mediately to the right of the word typed on the sheet.
the similarity of any pair of prepositions. A The Ss worked in groups of 2&30 and were allowed
pair’s similarity was measured by: (a) the 35 min to complete the task. Twenty-five of them were
overlap of the prepositions substitutable for eliminated from analysis because they did not finish
each in sentences; (b) the overlap of their in the time allowed. This left 85 Ss for use in the analy-
objects in sentences; (c) the overlap of the sis.
Three single grammatically defined words were
words modified by their prepositional phrases extracted from each of the 33 sentences written by each
in sentences; (d) the overlap of their free s:
associations; and (e) the frequency that Ss (1) The Preposition Substitutedwas that word each S
placed the pair together in the groups they substituted for the given (circled) preposition in the
formed. A multidimensional scaling technique sentences he wrote. Although the Ss were not specific-
ally asked to do so, they always, with one exception,
was used to construct an overall pattern of substituted prepositions or adverb-preposition clusters
similarity among the prepositions for each (e.g., away from) in their sentences; the few adverb-
criterion separately. The five resulting pat- preposition clusters were treated as single words.
USE AND MEANING OF PREPOSITIONS 423

(2) The Object of the Preposition was the head noun Grouping Task. The Ss in the grouping task were 114
or pronoun of the object’s noun phrase; for prepositions Johns Hopkins University male undergraduates, all
with two or more objects only the first one was con- fulfilling a course requirement for the course in intro-
sidered. A sentence using the given word as some- ductory psychology. These 114 Ss included all 82 Ss
thing besides a preposition was deleted from analysis used in the free-association task, which preceded the
altogether. Preposition-words were not considered grouping task.
prepositions when they were contained in verb-plus- The 33 prepositions, each printed in the top middle
particle constructions (such as up in caNed up herfriend) of an IBM data card, were given to each Sin one of eight
or in preposition-with-object-deleted constructions random orders. The Ss were instructed: “You will be
(such as in in let in the dog). In both constructions, the asked to group 33 common English prepositions
particle and “adverb”, respectively, can be exchanged according to their meaning. That is, some prepositions
with their pseudo-object noun phrases (Fraser, 1966). ‘are more related to each other in meaning than are other
(3) The Word Modified by the prepositional phrase prepositions; the prepositions seem to fall into natural
was specified by several rules. In most cases, the Word groups. First, you are to place all 33 prepositions into
Modified was unambiguously the noun or verb immedi- anywhere from two to six groups, the groups that seem
ately preceding the prepositional phrase. A preposi- most natural to you. You can put as many prepositions
tional phrase following a copula modified the head in each group as you want. Then, once you have formed
noun of the subject’s noun phrase. In other cases, the main groups, take each main group and divide it
adverbial prepositional phrases of time, place, or into as many subgroups as you think are necessary.
manner modified the main verb. Where there were two You do not necessarily have to subdivide any of your
or more modified words in parallel, the first was chosen. main groups; and again, the subgroups can be any size
If the word modified was properly a relative, then the you wish to make them. It is stressed that you break the
relative’s antecedent was taken as the Word Modified. prepositions into the groups and subgroups that seem
In all cases, the Word Modified was a single word. most natural to you.”
For the three grammatically defined words, obvious Individual Ss, working at their own pace, spread the
spelling errors were corrected, and plurals and their cards out on their desks, formed the two to six main
singulars were considered identical. The various in- groups, and separated the main groups with colored
flected forms of a verb were also counted as identical. IBM cards. Then they took each main group one at a
For each preposition, then, there were up to 85 single time, formed subgroups, and separated the subgroups
words used as Prepositions Substituted, as Objects of with white IBM cards. The Ss worked on the task in
the Preposition, and as Words Modified. These 33 x 3 groups of five to 30.
sets of words constituted the raw data of the sentence-
composition task. Methods of Analysis
Free-Association Task. The Ss in the free association The goal of part of the data analysis was to construct
task were 82 Johns Hopkins University male under- spatial representations of the 33 prepositions. In order
graduates, all enrolled in the introductory psychology to do so, it was necessary to measure the similarity of
course and all fulfilling a course requirement. each pair of prepositions, since each spatial configura-
Sixty-seven words,chosen from theThomdike-Lorge tion was to reflect the similarities among all possible
(1944) list of the 500 most frequent English words, were pairs of prepositions at once. Graphically, prepositions
added to the list of 33 prepositions to form a gram- with similar meanings will stand near each other, and
matically heterogeneous list. The 67 additional words prepositions withvery different meanings will stand far
included 15 nouns, 15 adjectives, 10 verbs, and 10 ad- apart. The analysis of proximities (Shepard, 1962;
verbs; the remaining 17 words were conjunctions, Kruskal, 1964) is an iterative scaling technique which
pronouns, and articles. The 100 stimulus words were attempts to do this. It yields the best-fitting mono-
each printed alone in the middle of an IBM data card tonically decreasing function between the distances in a
and were given to each S in one of eight different ran- spatial representation of few dimensions and the
dom orders. The Ss printed their free associations to the measured similarities of all pairs of objects in the
right of the printed stimulus word. representation. (For a good description of this tech-
The Ss were told they would be given 100 common nique, with examples, see Shepard, 1963.)
English words; they were to write down the first word The similarity measure used for four of the five
that came to mind for each, working quickly. They similarity criteria was the Intersection Coefficient, a
worked on the free-association task in groups of 10-30. measure of the similarity of two distributions of words
The free-association data, then, consisted of a set of (Deese, 1962). Marshall and Cofer (1963) have called
82 free associations for each preposition. The associa- the same measure the Measure of Relatedness;
tions were corrected for obvious spelling errors, and McNemar (1962) has called it the common-elements
plurals were changed to their singular form. correlation coefficient. For the Intersection Coefficient,
424 CLARK

one set of nr words (tokens) is compared with a second Each configuration is the best possible two-
set of n2 words to find the number of words ni2 in the dimensional representation according to
first set which can be uniquely paired with identical
words in the second set. Then, ZC = n,z/(nrn2)t’*. This Kruskal’s criterion.
measure represents loosely the proportion of identical In addition, each configuration has been
words in the two sets and seems to reflect the similarity supplemented by a clustering solution
between the two sets. (Johnson, 1967), which shows groups of
The ZC was used with the addition of “representa- similar prepositions enclosed in solid or
tional responses” in measuring the similarity of Prep-
dashed lines. The clustering method itself,
ositions Substituted and Free Association (see Deese,
1962); for example, the stimulus preposition itself was Johnson’s “maximum method,” forms sets of
first added to the set of Prepositions Substituted, once successively larger and larger clusters of
for each S still included in the analysis. In a very few similar prepositions, although only two kinds
instances Ss gave no responses at all for the Preposition of clusters have been drawn in each figure.
Substituted, Object of Preposition, Word Modified, or Loose clusters have been enclosed by dashed
Free Association categories. Each blank was treated as
if it were a word different from all other actual and lines, and tight clusters by solid lines. The
blank responses. loose clusters in Fig. la have been con-
For the fifth criterion, the Grouping Task, the simi- structed (a) by taking the 115 largest of the 528
larity measure reflected how often two prepositions possible similarity measures, (b) by forming
were placed in the same groups and subgroups. Any S the best set of mutually exclusive clusters in
sat forth only three levels of judged similarity (related-
ness) in his groupings-high similarity within sub- which the similarity between each pair of prep-
groups, medium similarity within main groups but ositions within a cluster is among these 115
between subgroups, low similarity between main measures, and (c) by enclosing all such clusters
groups. So for a single S, two prepositions were given a in dashed lines. The tight clusters enclosed in
grouping score of 1.O if they were both in the same main solid lines are formed similarly, but only the 35
group and in the same subgroup, a score of .5 if they
were in the same main group but in different subgroups, largest similarities are used; consequently, all
and a score of zero if they were in different main tight clusters are included within loose
groups. The final grouping measure for two preposi- clusters. The loose and tight clusters shown in
tions was the pair’s mean grouping score for all 114 Ss. the five figures are each formed at natural
If many Ss placed two words in the same group or sub- breaking points in the clustering solutions,
group-indicating that the two were judged to be
closely related in meaning-the two would have a high points which are approximately equivalent for
grouping measure and hence a high similarity. the five different figures.
Thus for each of the five criteria, there was a simi- The two-dimensional spatial configuration
larity measure for each of the 528 possible pairings of and the clusters in each figure are meant to
the 33 prepositions; this is represented by a sym- complement each other, since the true
metrical 33 x 33 matrix of similarity measures.
Kruskal’s (1964) multidimensional scaling method was configuration is of many more dimensions.
performed directly on each of these five matrices. The The clusters bring out the relations among
first three criteria-Prepositions Substituted, Objects highly similar prepositions more clearly than
of Prepositions, and Words Modified-came from one the configuration does, while the configur-
population of Ss, and the last two-Free Associations ation shows the gross relations among the
and Grouping Task-came from a different population
clusters of prepositions. The clustering of up
of Ss. Because the similarity indices for the first four
criteria all utilized the Intersection Coefficient and and down in Fig. la, for example, corrects the
about the same number of Ss, they are comparable in impression, because of their distance, that they
sensitivity. The important comparisons do not involve are fairly dissimilar. Small local parts of the
different S populations or varying sensitivity. representation are not always properly
formed; in Fig. la within is nearer to outside
RESULTS than to inside, although their three pair-
Figures la, b, c, d, and e picture the matrices similarity measures show the reverse to be
of similarity measures from the five criteria. true. In general, however, the configurations
USE AND MEANING OF PREPOSITIONS 425

plus the clusters represent the data quite well. between Prepositions Substituted, and Objects
To make comparisons among Figs. la of Prepositions, Free Associations, and the
through e easier, the latter four have been Grouping Task; on the opposite end, the
rotated about their centroids so that they are lowest correlations occurred between Words
each as congruent with Fig. la as possible.2 Modified and all other criteria.
The five pictorial representations, except for A property evident in Figs. la, b, d, and e, is
Words Modified, easily satisfy one’s intuition that the largest differences in meaning among
about prepositional meaning. In the Prep- the prepositions can be attributed to what
ositions Substituted representation (Fig. la), might be called a movement-implied dimen-
the groups formed are of highly related words : sion, roughly corresponding with the vertical
inside, outside, and within form one closely axis. At the top are prepositions implying much
knit group; on, ofs, and upon form another; movement (up, down, above, across, over, off,
between and among form another; and so on. etc.), but at the bottom are prepositions
At a grosser level, each cluster seems to devoid of movement connotations (without,
neighbor on other clusters similar to it: with, during, for, among, etc.). These semantic
up-down is near above-across-over, which in distinctions must be built into a preposition’s
turn is near under-along-off-on-upon; between- context, for the Prepositions Substituted and
among is near at-by-near; and so on. During- Objects of Prepositions reflect the movement
after is quite distant from other prepositions, property quite naturally. The same distinctions
since no others are very similar to the cluster are implicit in Ss’ free associations and in their
at all. The other four representations detail the judgments of the relatedness of two prep-
prepositional relations in much the same way. ositions. The movement-implied dimension
Some of the five configurations appear to be suggests the traditional distinctions among
more related than others. These relations are kinds of prepositional phrases. Moving down
indicated more directly in Table 1, which the graph, one finds generally prepositions
with meanings of direction, of position, of
TABLE 1 manner, and of time (during and after); more
RANK-ORDER CORRELATIONS OF THE 528 SIMILARITY generally, prepositions seem to cluster accord-
MEASURES ACCORDING TO THE FIVE MAIN CRITERIA ing to these distinctions.
In Fig. 1b, the movement-implied dimension
2 3 4 5 can be given an alternative description in
terms of the kinds of Objects each preposition
1. Prepositions Substituted .58 .ll A7 .50 allowed. The Objects to each preposition
2. Objects of Prepositions .12 .24 .38 were first classified as animate, inanimate, or
3. Words Modified -.02 .20 abstract. With Objects classified in this way, it
4. Free Associations .30 was found that the raw percentages of in-
5. Grouping Task
animate Objects correlated .93 with the
projections of all prepositions on a line
shows the Spearman rank-order correlations through the centroid of the figure and 19
among the 528 similarity measures gained degrees clockwise from vertical.3 Up and
from the five criteria. Prepositions Substituted, down, for example, had nothing but inanimate
Objects of Prepositions, Free Associations, Objects; on the opposite end, during had no
and the Grouping Task were all moderately such Objects, and after had only 5%. This
interrelated. The highest correlations occurred
3 The orientation of this line, rotated so that it
2 The rotation was performed using an unpublished correlatedmaximally with thepercentagecriterion, was
computer program written by G. M. Schulman and determined by Schulman and Torgerson’s unpublished
W. S. Torgerson. computer program.
426 CLARK

a b Rank of largest
dtssimilarity
Rank of largest within cluster
dissimilarity
withln cluster - 29

- 35
_.__ g,g
I yP :’ ,...
---- 115
!W
,:’
,~;ocross~ .&g

Rank of iargtst d
dissimilorlty R@nk of largest
witbin cluster
- 40
*-_- 148

“PO”.

l agoinst
during* 1 __*’
: /
.-about ., ;.at
within. :
.__.:
USE AND MEANING OF PREPOSITIONS 427

inanimate dimension is almost identical to the virtue of this knowledge. Sentence com-
subjectively defined movement-implied dimen- position, free associations, and semantic
sion, which shows up in all but the Words judgments were expected to be closely related,
Modified configuration. The relatipnship and they were found to be so ; the five con-
between the two is semantically interpretable: figurations derived were, in the main, very
movement is experienced with respect to congruent. But how is this congruence to be
objects, not abstractions, and with respect to interpreted in terms of a speaker’s linguistic
inanimate fixed objects more often than to knowledge?
animate ones. Currently, most linguists distinguish the
grammatical from the semantic components
DISCUSSION in a linguistic description of sentences (Katz
English speakers daily compose sentences and Fodor, 1964). The grammatical com-
with prepositions relying on their knowledge ponent describes sentences in terms of
of language. When asked, they can also give grammatical categories, like nouns, adjectives,
free associations and make judgments of prep- and determiners; the semantic component
ositional relatedness, both somehow by describes the selection of words in sentences
once the grammatical component has been
Rank of largest determined. Grammatical variation has been
e dissimilarity excluded in the present study since only
wthin cluster
prepositions have been examined. What
- 27
---- 144 remains is the semantic component. (Strictly
speaking, a few grammatical distinctions can
be made among prepositions, but for the
present these distinctions will be called
semantic.)
In this light, the main finding is that two
prepositions are treated as semantically
related when they are interchangeable in
discourse. On the grammatical level, two
nouns will be judged to be more closely
related than will a noun and an adverb. This
judgment is based on the knowledge that the
,---..____ two nouns, when substituted for each other in
:..‘uith -..%____
y -._ discourse, are more likely to form sensible
.. . .
.. sentences. The present results show the same to
k_
be true on the semantic level. Free Associations
after. *during
‘U
and the Grouping Task-both assumed to
reflect meaning relations-were most closely
related to Prepositions Subsituted, a criterion
FIG. 1. The two-dimensional configuration and reflecting mainly how interchangeable two
clustersfor thefivecriteria. For Fig. la, within the tight
clusters(enclosedby solid lines)the leastsimilar pair of prepositions were in meaningful sentences.
prepositionsranked among the 35 largest similarities; Taken alone, however, this main result glosses
within the loose clusters(enclosedby dashed lines) it over the more detailed linguistic knowledge
ranked among the 115largestsimilarities.The clusters Ss relied on in the present study.
of Figs. lb, c, d, and e, have analogous properties. First, it is clear that the Ss reflected an
la. PrepositionsSubstituted; lb. Objects of Preposi-
tions; lc. Words Modified; Id. Free Associations; implicit knowledge of English syntax in all
le. Grouping Task. three tasks. According to phrase-structure
428 CLARK

analyses (Gleason, 1961), a sentence such as associations, 29 %; and the other associations,
The boy went to the store contains the prep- 26 %. Fillenbaum and Jones (1965) classified
ositional phrase to the store as a constituent the associations to 15 prepositions by gram-
at one level of analysis, and the word modified matical class and found, similarly, that prep-
went plus the prepositional phrase as a ositions made up 42% of the associations,
constituent at another level, (went (to the nouns and pronouns together, 24 %, and other
store)). By this analysis, a preposition and its words, 34%. The present percentages again
object are more closely related than either reflect the relative importance to the meaning
element is to the word modified. The corre- of a preposition, of other prepositions, its
lations among the similarity measures of the objects, and the words modified (which fall
three sentence-composition criteria (Table 1) into the other-association category).
bear out phrase-structure analysis: within the According to further analysis, however,
prepositional phrase boundaries, the corre- preposition- and object-associations appar-
lation was .58; across its boundaries, the ently contain different kinds of information:
correlations were .I 1 and .12. one relates mainly to Prepositions Substituted,
Although the phrase structure itself does and the other to Objects of Prepositions. Two
not contain meaning, it does indicate where matrices of similarity measures-Intersection
meaning relations should be located. Harris Coefficients-were computed separately for
(1954) conjectured that similarity in meaning the preposition- and object-associations,
should be correlated with the distributional exactly as the measures had been computed
properties of words within sentences, just for all Free Associations. The similarity
those properties formally described by phrase measures from the preposition-associations
structure. He would predict, then, that if the correlated .51 with those of Prepositions
Free Association and Grouping Tasks were Substituted and .19 with those of Objects of
really measuring similarity in meaning- Prepositions; the respective correlations for
as it was assumed-their similarity measures the object-associations were .24 and .28.
should agree very closely with those of the Prepositions Substituted and Objects were
Prepositions Substituted, somewhat less with also more closely related to preposition- and
those of the Objects, and even less with those object-associations, respectively, than to the
of the Words Modified. The correlations of full set of Free Associations, This last finding is
Table 1 confirm these predictions: the simi- surprising, since the preposition- and object-
larity measures from Free Associations corre- association similarity measures, each based on
lated .47, .24, and -.02, respectively, with fewer responses, might be expected to be less
those from the sentence-composition task; reliable than the Free Associations measures.
the similarity measures from the Grouping These comparisons, along with direct support
Task correlated .50, .38, and .20, respectively. from the associations, indicate that: (a) the
A more detailed look at the Ss’ Free paradigmatic preposition-associations reflect
Associations showed that they reflected a how prepositions are substitutable for each
phrase-structure analysis of the prepositional other; and (b) the syntagmatic object-associ-
phrase quite directly. The Free Associations to ations reflect the properties of the Objects of
each preposition were classified into three Prepositions used in sentences.
categories: (a) words able to be used as The Objects of a Preposition and its object-
prepositions; (b) nouns or pronouns judged associations differed in one important respect.
able to serve as objects of the preposition; and The Objects were rarely pronouns (4x), but
(c) other associations. The first category, the object-associations were often pronouns
preposition-associations, made up 45 % of the (48 %). Fillenbaum and Jones (1965) similarly
associations; the second category, the object- found that 50 % of the nominal associations to
USE AND MEANING OF PREPOSITIONS 429

prepositions were pronouns. Despite this preposition-, object-, and other associations.
difference, prepositions demanding mostly A S’s semantic knowledge might also be
animate objects in the composed sentences characterized within this grammatical frame-
also elicited mostly animate object-associ- work. Under, it was said, could suggest the
ations; the rank correlation between the category Preposition and elicit as free associ-
percentage of animate Objects and of animate ations other members of that category.
object-associations was .73 (p < .OOl). It is Under, however, would not elicit all prep-
important to note that it was not the identity of ositions, but only a restricted subset of
the specific Objects and object-associations them-those prepositions which could sen-
that determined the correlation between the sibly replace under in sentences (like under
two. It was their underlying properties that itself, over, and below). This restriction would
were similar-properties such as animateness, account for the preposition-associations being
concreteness, and so forth. Stated differently, almost identical to the Prepositions Sub-
the object-associations are not necessarily the stituted of the sentence composition task.
objects Ss would use in sentences, but they Likewise, under would not elicit all members
have important properties in common with of the Prepositional Phrase category, but only
those objects. phrases which could sensibly replace under-
From this evidence, one can attempt to phrases in sentences (including the under-
characterize the knowledge-both gram- phrase itself). This restriction would account
matical and semantic-that a S brings to bear for the object-associations to under having the
in free association. An important property of same underlying properties as the Objects of
phrase structure rules is their hierarchical under in the sentence composition task. Simi-
character (Chomsky, 1965). By such rules, for larly, under would not elicit all members of the
example, under directly belongs to the category Verb Phrase category, but only substitutable
named Preposition; the Preposition, along members of the category. This restriction
with a Noun Phrase, in turn belongs to a Prep- would help account for the other associations
ositional Phrase; the Prepositional Phrase Ss gave to under. These linguistic categories,
might in turn belong to a Verb Phrase and restricted by semantic considerations, will be
follow a Verb. These hierarchical rules can be called cognitive units, since they have no
incorporated into a model of free association. linguistic definitions.
This model assumes that the stimulus under Such cognitive units are needed to explain
suggests each of the higher categories in turn the differences between Objects and object-
with decreasing probabilities. Under most often associations. Earlier accounts of free associ-
suggests Preposition, next Prepositional ation, such as Saporta’s (1955), assumed that
Phrase, and least often Verb Phrase. If under in generating object associations Ss simply
suggests the category Preposition, Ss must gave nouns or pronouns which often follow
give other members of the category-other under in sentences. With this assumption, the
prepositions-as free associations. If under object-associations and the Objects for under
suggests Prepositional Phrase, Ss must choose should be the same distribution of words. But
whether to give a Preposition or Noun they are not. To explain the difference, under
Phrase and, if they choose Noun Phrase, must be assumed to elicit a cognitive unit
whether to give a noun or pronoun. If corresponding to a prepositional phrase,
under suggests Verb Phrase, Ss must choose from which a noun phrase is derived. This
whether to give a Verb (the word modified in procedure does not specify whether the noun
the present study) or a part of the Prep- phrase is to be a noun or pronoun. It does
ositional Phrase. Thus the present model specify that the noun phrase contain the
accounts for the relative frequency of semantic properties of the higher cognitive
430 CLARK

unit. As a consequence, the procedure allows erent ways (compare Figs. Id and e with Fig.
Objects and object-associations to differ in la).
how often they will be pronouns, yet it requires There is also reason to believe that 5’sin free
them to be similar in all other underlying association did not always react to the 33
properties. stimulus words as prepositions. For example,
The semantic relations of antonymy and near’s most frequent association was far, an
synonymy were clearly implicit in the Ss’ adjective or adverb. Fraser (1966), however,
treatment of prepositions. In Figs. la, b, d, and has pointed out,that many other adverbial uses
e, opposites formed some of the tightest of preposition-words can be derived (in a
groups. Consider seven pairs of opposites fires- generative grammar) from prepositional
ent : up-down, inside-outside, on-off, over-under, phrases by deleting the object. The command
to-from, with-without, for-against. All their wait inside is related to wait inside the house in
similarity measures were above the median for which the house has been left unspecified; in
Prepositions Substituted, for Free Associ- this example, inside’s most sensible replace-
ations, and for the Grouping Task (p < .008, ment is outside, the same as if the object
for each set, by a sign test); all but one ranked were present. In many cases, then, it does not
above the median for Objects of Prepositions matter whether Ss treated the stimuli as
(p < .062). The reasons for this tight clustering prepositions or as abverbs, because their
are seen in the data. With few exceptions, a associations would reflect the same under-
preposition’s most frequent substitute in lying process.
sentence composition was its opposite, if it In summary, an English speaker uses speci-
had one; its most frequent association was fiable linguistic knowledge in composing
also its opposite. In both tasks, antonyms sentences, giving free associations, and judging
usually took precedence over synonyms; the relatedness of prepositions. In the most
for example, inside’s most frequent substi- general terms, he treats two prepositions as
tute and association was outside, not in or semantically related if they are mutually inter-
within. changeable in meaningful sentences. He will
Similarity, as measured by the five criteria, is often give one interchangeable word as an
not synonymous with synonymy, since association to the other and will judge the two
opposites (like up and down), as well as near to be related. Opposites, for him, are among
synonyms (like on and upon), were very the most closely related pairs of words. In
similar on each of the indices. This observ- associating and judging, he tacitly acknow-
ation contradicts Harris (1954), who has ledges phrase-structure rules. He treats prep-
implied that similarity in use should correlate ositions as if their meaning were correlated
with synonymy. The present results suggest, most highly with the distributional properties
on the contrary, that antonymy might be a of the prepositions themselves, next with the
more central semantic relation for prep- linguistically proximal objects of prepositions,
ositions than synonymy. and least with the more distant words modified.
Some of what Ss do in free association seems His free associations to prepositions reflect
to have little relation to the linguistic know- the hierarchical character of phrase-structure
ledge they need for composing sentences. rules. He first emits other members of the
Within often elicited without in free association, Preposition category, then other parts of the
probably because of the strong in-out associ- Prepositional-Phrase category, and then other
ation; and in the Grouping Task without parts of still higher-order categories. This
was grouped as often with within as it was process implies that free associations originate
with with. The Ss composing sentences, in underlying cognitive units and not directly
however, used the two words in quite diff- in sentences that he might produce.
USE AND MEANING OF PREPOSITIONS 431

REFERENCES KATZ, J. J., AND FODOR, J. A. The structure of a


semantic theory. Language, 1963,39,170-210.
CHOMSKY, N. Review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior,
KRUSKAL, J. B. Multidimensional scaling by optimizing
Language, 1959,35,26-58.
goodness of fit to a nonmetric hypothesis. Psycho-
CHOMSKY, N. Aspects of a theory of syntax, Cambridge,
metrika, 1964, 29, l-27.
Mass.: M.I.T. Press, 1965.
MARSHALL, G. R., AND C~FER, C. N. Associative
DEESE, J. On the structure of associative meaning.
indices as measures of word relatedness: a sum-
Psychol. Rev., 1962,69,161-175.
mary and comparison of ten methods. J. verb.
DEESE, J. The associative structure of some common
Learn. verb. Behav., 1963,1,408-421.
English adjectives. J. verb. Learn. verb. Behav.,
MCNEMAR, Q. Psychological statistics. New York:
1964,3,347-357.
Wiley, 1962.
DEESE, J. The structure of associations in language
SAPORTA, S. Linguistic structure as a factor and as a
and thought. Baltimore: Hopkins Univ. Press,
measure in word association. Paper presented at
1966.
Minnesota Conference on Associative Processes in
FILLENBAUM, S., AND JONES,L. V. Grammatical con-
Verbal Behavior, University of Minnesota, 1955.
tingencies in word association. J. verb. Learn. verb.
SHEPARD, R. N. The analysis of proximities: Multi-
Behav., 1965,4,248-255.
dimensional scaling with an unknown distance
FRASER, J. B. Some remarks on the verb-particle con-
function, 1:and II. Psychometrika, 1962, 27, 125-
struction in English. Monograph Series on 139,219-246.
Languages and Linguistics, 1966,19,45-62.
SHEPARD,R. N. Analysis of proximities as a technique
GARNER, W. R. Uncertainty and structure as psycho-
for the study of informational processing in man.
logicalconcepts. New York: Wiley, 1962. Human Factors, 1963,5,3348.
GLEASON,H. A. An introduction to descriptivelinguistics.
THORNDIKE, E. L., AND LARGE, I. The teacher’s word
New York: Holt, 1961.
book of 30,000 words. New York: Bureau of
HARRIS, Z. Distributional structure. Word, 1954, 10,
Publications, Teachers College, Columbia Uni-
146-162. versity, 1944.
JOHNSON, S. G. Hierarchical clustering schemes.
Psychometrika, 1967,32,241-254. (Received January 3 1,1967)

You might also like