You are on page 1of 7

nature publishing group articles

Methods and Techniques

Accurate Measurement of Body Weight


and Food Intake in Environmentally Enriched
Male Wistar Rats
Kylie E.L. Beale1, Kevin G. Murphy1, Eleanor K. Harrison1, Angela J. Kerton2, Mohammad A. Ghatei1,
Stephen R. Bloom1 and Kirsty L. Smith1

Laboratory animals are crucial in the study of energy homeostasis. In particular, rats are used to study alterations in
food intake and body weight. To accurately record food intake or energy expenditure it is necessary to house rats
individually, which can be stressful for social animals. Environmental enrichment may reduce stress and improve
welfare in laboratory rodents. However, the effect of environmental enrichment on food intake and thus experimental
outcome is unknown. We aimed to determine the effect of environmental enrichment on food intake, body weight,
behavior and fecal and plasma stress hormones in male Wistar rats. Singly housed 5–7-week-old male rats were
given either no environmental enrichment, chew sticks, a plastic tube of 67 mm internal diameter, or both chew sticks
and a tube. No differences in body weight or food intake were seen over a 7-day period. Importantly, the refeeding
response following a 24-h fast was unaffected by environmental enrichment. Rearing, a behavior often associated
with stress, was significantly reduced in all enriched groups compared to controls. There was a significant increase in
fecal immunoglobulin A (IgA) in animals housed with both forms of enrichment compared to controls at the termination
of the study, suggesting enrichment reduces hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activity in singly housed rats.
In summary, environmental enrichment does not influence body weight and food intake in singly housed male Wistar
rats and may therefore be used to refine the living conditions of animals used in the study of energy homeostasis
without compromising experimental outcome.

Obesity (2011) 19, 1715–1721. doi:10.1038/oby.2010.331

Introduction to house each animal separately. Feeding is a complex ­behavior


Laboratory animals are vital in the study of energy homeosta- affected by many factors, including stress. Anxiety induced
sis because the physiological mechanisms involved in the con- by housing conditions may therefore be a confounding factor
trol of food intake and energy expenditure are highly complex. when conducting feeding studies (5,6).
Due to the huge economic burden induced by obesity and its As social housing is not possible in experiments involving the
comorbidities it is essential to gain a greater understanding of measurement of food intake and/or energy expenditure, envi-
the pathways controlling food intake to aid the development of ronmental enrichment may be added to cages to overcome the
new treatments for this condition. stressful effects of single housing. Environmental enrichment
Wistar rats are well characterized animal models used to is defined as the use of objects to improve the quality of life
investigate food intake, and rats and humans have comparable of a caged animal and promote species-specific behavior (7).
hypothalamic structures and neuroendocrine circuits which Laboratory rats consistently show a preference for enriched
control food intake and energy expenditure (1). cages over basic cages, for example choosing an environment
Rats are social creatures, and isolation has been shown to containing paper towels, wooden platforms or wood chips
increase anxiety and induce behavioral abnormalities (2,3). over a barren cage (8). When presented with different objects,
European guidelines state that “animals should be socially rats show a preference for objects they can chew, allowing
housed whenever possible”, unless deemed inappropriate by the them to exercise an important species-­typical ­behavior (9).
scientific protocol (4). In order to accurately record the food Among numerous benefits, ­environmental enrichment has
intake or energy expenditure of individual rats, it is necessary been shown to improve cognition and memory, increase

The first two authors contributed equally to this work.


1
Section of Investigative Medicine, Division of Diabetes, Endocrinology and Metabolism, Imperial College London, London, UK; 2CBS Department, Imperial College
London, London, UK. Correspondence: Kevin G. Murphy (k.g.murphy@imperial.ac.uk)
Received 11 August 2010; accepted 6 December 2010; published online 13 January 2011. doi:10.1038/oby.2010.331

obesity | VOLUME 19 NUMBER 8 | August 2011 1715


articles
Methods and Techniques

cortical thickness and induce faster ­recovery from brain body weight into one of four groups (n = 6–8 per group). The control
injury (10–12). group received no environmental enrichment in their home cage. The
second group was given two wooden chew sticks measuring 150 mm ×
The response to stress is predominantly mediated by the
20 mm × 1 mm, which were replaced daily. Rats in the third group were
hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. Under stressful given a black plastic tube (200 mm length × 67 mm internal diameter)
conditions, corticotrophin-releasing hormone is produced by and the fourth group received both wooden chew sticks and a tube. The
the hypothalamus and acts at the anterior pituitary gland to tubing was sufficiently large to allow rats weighing up to 450 g to comfort-
stimulate the synthesis and release of adrenocorticotropic hor- ably enter. Cages were changed when enrichment was assigned and once
weekly thereafter. The entire study period was 40 days.
mone (ACTH). ACTH acts at the adrenal cortex to drive the
release of corticosterone, the major glucocorticoid in rodents.
The effect of environmental enrichment on food
Plasma corticosterone is therefore the primary measure of intake and body weight
HPA axis activity in rodents. Fecal immunoglobulin A (IgA) Animals were left to acclimatize to their environmental enrichment
may also be used as a noninvasive marker of long-term well- for 5 days before measurements began. Food intake and body weight
being, as chronic stress induces immunosuppression (13). Rats were then measured daily for 7 days. Food pellets were stored in mesh
hoppers within each cage to prevent spillage and cages were visually
housed singly in enriched cages have lower baseline plasma
inspected daily to check for loose pellets.
corticosterone and ACTH than rats housed in standard cages
and display less abnormal, maladaptive behaviors, indicating The effect of environmental enrichment on behavior
that enrichment may reduce stress in these conditions (7,14). Behavior was continuously monitored for 1 h by two observers in the
Housing conditions have also been shown to affect fecal IgA, early dark phase, as this is when rats are most active, on day 21 of the
although the effect of environmental enrichment on this study using methods adapted from Fray et al. (20,21). Each rat was
parameter has not been well characterized (15). observed for 15 s every 5 min. Each 15-s period was subdivided into
three 5 s periods and the behavior of the rat was noted in each 5 s period.
Despite the benefits of environmental enrichment, its effect Behavior was classified into the following categories: feeding, drinking,
on experimental variability is unclear and concerns have been rearing, locomotion, grooming, burrowing, head down, and sleeping.
raised that by disrupting standardization, enrichment may For animals supplied with tubing, it was noted if an animal was within
reduce the precision of results (16). In particular, the effect of the tubing and the behavior it was performing was recorded to compare
environmental enrichment on the feeding behavior of rats has against the control group.
not been well studied.
The effect of environmental enrichment on
The provision of environmental enrichment has been shown fasting-induced refeeding
to affect numerous processes within the central nervous sys- Animals were fasted from 0900 for 24 h on day 25 of the study. The
tem, for example cerebral development, and neuronal ­plasticity following morning at 0900 a predetermined quantity of food was
(17). Animals housed with enrichment have been shown to returned to the cages. Food intake was measured at 1, 2, 4, 8 and 24 h
adapt more quickly to new environments than those housed after refeeding. Body weight was measured prior to the fast and 24 h
after refeeding.
in barren cages, potentially due to improvements in learning,
memory, and information processing (18,19). As feeding is Fecal IgA analysis
sensitive to environmental conditions, enrichment may there- Fecal IgA extraction was performed as previously described (22). Fresh
fore influence food intake and body weight (5). fecal samples were collected at 0900 on two occasions: one day prior
We hypothesized that environmental enrichment would to enrichment assignment (day 6) and on the final day of the study
improve welfare without compromising experimental out- (day 40). Samples were stored on ice then frozen at −20 °C until extrac-
tion. Samples were dried at 50 °C and homogenized. One milliliter
come in singly housed male Wistar rats. We aimed to deter- 0.01 mol/l phosphate buffered saline was added to 0.1 g of sample and
mine the effect of environmental enrichment on food intake, vortexed for 1 hour. Samples were then centrifuged at 2,500 rpm for
body weight, behavior, and the refeeding response to a 24-h 20 min and the supernatant transferred to a clean test tube. Samples
fast. We also measured fecal IgA before and after enrichment were assayed at a 1 in 2000 dilution using a Rat IgA Enzyme-Linked
and plasma corticosterone and ACTH at the termination of the Immunosorbent Assay Quantitation Kit purchased from Bethyl
Laboratories. (Montgomery, Texas).
study.
Plasma corticosterone and ACTH
Methods and Procedures Rats were killed by decapitation in the early light phase on day 40
Animals and housing of the study. Trunk blood was collected in plastic EDTA tubes for
Animal procedures undertaken were approved by the British Home ACTH analysis and in plastic lithium heparin tubes containing 1,000
Office Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 (Project License Kallikrein Inactivator Units of aprotinin (Nordic Pharma, Reading,
70/6402). UK) for corticosterone analysis. Plasma was separated by centrifuga-
Male Wistar rats (specific pathogen free; Charles River Laboratories, tion, snap frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored at –20 °C until analysis
Margate, UK) of 5–7 weeks of age weighing 150–200g were maintained by radioimmunoassay.
in individual open cages with solid floors and sawdust bedding under Plasma corticosterone was measured using a radioimmunoassay
controlled temperature (21–23 °C) and light (12:12 light-dark cycle; lights kit from MP Biomedicals (Orangeburg, NY) for which the intra- and
on at 0700) conditions with ad libitum access to food (RM1 diet, Special inter-assay coefficients of variation were <10% and 7%, respectively.
Diet Services, Witham, UK) and tap water. Relative humidity was ~55%. Plasma ACTH was measured using an immunoradiometric assay pur-
Cage dimensions were: width 245 mm, length 415 mm, height 185 mm. chased from BioSource Europe S.A. (Nivelles, Belgium) for which the
Following a 7-day acclimatization period during which food intake intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation were 6.4% and 6.2%,
and body weight were monitored, animals were randomly assigned by respectively.

1716 VOLUME 19 NUMBER 8 | august 2011 | www.obesityjournal.org


articles
Methods and Techniques

Statistics a 160 Control


The study was powered to detect a 10% change in food intake and body Chew sticks
weight with power of 80% at a level of significance of 0.05. All data are 140
Tubing
presented as mean ± s.e.m. other than data from behavioral ­studies, 120
Chew sticks and tubing
which are presented as median (inter-quartile range). Cumulative

Body weight change (g)


food intake and body weight data were analyzed using the general- 100
ized estimating equation (Stata 9; StataCorp LP, College Station, TX).
80
Behavioral studies were analyzed using the Kruskall–Wallis test (Systat,
Evanston, IL) as data were nonparametric. Acute food intake studies 60
and radio­immunoassay data were analyzed using a one-way ANOVA
with Tukey’s post hoc test (GraphPad Prism version 5 for Windows; 40
GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA). A one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s
20
post hoc test was used to compare fecal IgA data between groups at
individual time points, and changes within groups over time were 0
compared using a paired t-test (GraphPad Prism; GraphPad Software). 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
P values <0.05 were taken as significant. Day

b Control
600 Chew sticks
Results
Tubing
The effect of environmental enrichment on food 500 Chew sticks and tubing

Cumulative food intake (g)


intake and body weight
There was no difference in body weight between groups at the 400

beginning of the study. Environmental enrichment had no sta-


300
tistically significant effect on body weight or cumulative body
weight gain during the 5-day acclimatization period or over 200
the 7-day study period (Figure 1a). At the end of the entire
40-day experimental period there was no significant difference 100
in absolute body weight between groups (body weight day 40
0
(g): 394.0 ± 10.3 (control); 414.8 ± 9.9 (chew sticks); 407.3 ± 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
12.1 (tubing); 408.0 ± 8.1 (chew sticks and tubing), n = 6–8 per Day
group).
There was no difference between 24-h food intake the day Figure 1  Effect of environmental enrichment on cumulative body weight
prior to (day 6) and 24-h food intake the day following (day 7) gain and cumulative food intake in male Wistar rats. (a) Cumulative
body weight gain (g) and (b) cumulative food intake (g) in rats housed
assignment of environmental enrichment (average 24 h food with no environmental enrichment, two chew sticks, plastic tubing or
intake (g): 27.8 ± 1.0 (control day 6), 27.6 ± 0.5 (control day 7); chew sticks and tubing from days 1 to 19. Vertical arrow represents
28.3 ± 0.7 (chew sticks day 6), 27.7 ± 0.8 (chew sticks day 7); enrichment assignment on day 7. Animals were subsequently given
28.4 ± 0.5 (tubing day 6), 28.0 ± 0.8 (tubing day 7); 27.8 ± 0.5 5 days to acclimatize. Body weight and food intake were measured daily
(chew sticks and tubing day 6), 28.5 ± 0.7 (chew sticks and from day 13 to day 19. Data is expressed as mean ± s.e.m. for all groups
(n = 6–8 per group).
tubing day 7), n = 6–8 per group). There was no significant dif-
ference in cumulative food intake between groups at any time
point following enrichment (Figure 1b). with tubing alone and 5.6% for those supplied with both chew
sticks and tubing (n = 6–8 per group).
The effect of environmental enrichment on behavior
Control animals displayed significantly more rearing behavior The effect of environmental enrichment on
than all animals with environmental enrichment (P < 0.05, n = fasting-induced refeeding
6–8 per group) (Table 1). Following refeeding after a 24 h fast no significant differences
To examine the general behavioral pattern, behaviors were in food intake were observed between groups at any time point
subsequently grouped into four categories: feeding and drink- studied. The 0–1 h food intake was comparable between all
ing; activity (rearing, burrowing, and locomotion); grooming; groups and decreased at subsequent time points, as expected
and resting (head down, and sleeping) as previously described (24) (Table 2).
(21,23). The control group displayed significantly more “active” There was no significant difference between groups in the
behavior than the two groups with tubing (P < 0.05, n = 6–8 amount of body weight lost during the 24 h fast (body weight
per group) (Table 1). lost during 24 h fast (g): 18.9 ± 3.3 (control); 22.8 ± 1.8 (chew
If an animal was noted to be sitting within the tubing dur- sticks); 21.2 ± 1.6 (tubing); 22.1 ± 1.6 (chew sticks and tubing),
ing behavioral analysis, the behavior the rat was performing at n = 6–8 per group).
that time was recorded to allow comparison with rats housed
without tubing. In all cases the behavior recorded was head Fecal IgA
down. The percentage of time animals spent in their tubing There was no significant difference in fecal IgA between groups
during the 1-h study period was 4.2% for the group supplied prior to the assignment of enrichment (Table  3). However,

obesity | VOLUME 19 NUMBER 8 | August 2011 1717


articles
Methods and Techniques

Table 1 Effect of environmental enrichment on behavior and grouped behavior


Behavior Control Chew sticks Tubing Chew sticks and tubing
Effect of environmental enrichment on behavior a

  Feeding 11.0 (8.5–12.5) 11.5 (9.5–13.5) 15.0 (14.3–17.3) 9.5 (5.8–12.3)


  Drinking 0.0 (0.0–1.0) 1.0 (0.3–1.0) 0.0 (0.0–0.0) 1.0 (0.0–2.0)
  Rearing 13.0 (11.0–14.5) 8.5 (8.0–9.0)* 5.0 (3.3–6.0)** 8.0 (4.0–10.3)*
  Locomotion 3.0 (1.5–3.0) 2.5 (2.0–4.5) 2.5 (1.3–3.8) 1.5 (0.8–3.0)
  Grooming 6.0 (5.0–8.0) 7.0 (6.3–8.5) 9.0 (4.8–12.5) 10.5 (8.0–16.0)
  Burrowing 0.0 (0.0–0.5) 0.0 (0.0–1.5) 0.0 (0.0–0.8) 0.0 (0.0–0.0)
  Head down 0.0 (0.0–0.5) 1.0 (0.0–2.0) 0.5 (0.0–1.0) 1.0 (0.0–5.5)
  Sleeping 0.0 (0.0–0.0) 0.0 (0.0–0.0) 0.0 (0.0–0.0) 0.0 (0.0–0.0)
Effect of environmental enrichment on grouped behaviora,b
  Feeding and drinking 13.0 (0.5–14.0) 13.0 (10.5–14.8) 17.0 (14.3–18.0) 17.0 (14.3–18.0)
  Activity 16.0 (14.0–17.0) 15.0 (11.0–15.8) 8.0 (5.5–9.0)** 9.0 (6.5–11.5)*
  Grooming 6.0 (5.0–8.0) 7.0 (6.3–8.5) 9.0 (4.8–12.5) 11.0 (8.0–16.0)
  Nonactivity 0.0 (0.0–0.5) 1.0(0.0–2.0) 5.0 (1.3–7.8) 4.0 (0.0–7.0)
a
Animals were observed for 15 s every 5 min for 1 h in the early dark phase. Each 15-s period was subdivided into three, and the behavior of the rat during each time
period was scored (n = 6–8 per group). Data is presented as median (inter-quartile range). bBehavior was grouped into the following categories: feeding and drinking;
activity (rearing, burrowing, and locomotion); grooming; and resting (head down and sleeping).
*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01 vs. controls.

Table 2 Effect of environmental enrichment on fasting-induced refeeding


Control Chew sticks Tubing Chew sticks and tubing
Time period Mean s.e.m. Mean s.e.m. Mean s.e.m. Mean s.e.m.
0–1 h 7.54 0.72 7.35 0.71 8.52 1.04 7.49 0.29
1–2 h 1.50 0.68 3.25 0.86 1.18 0.60 3.35 0.55
2–4 h 1.35 0.88 0.87 0.69 1.27 0.93 0.95 0.48
4–8 h 2.29 0.62 2.77 0.86 1.37 0.60 0.44 0.24
8–24 h 26.70 2.01 28.53 1.14 29.50 1.07 28.99 1.36
0–2 h 9.04 1.21 10.60 1.21 9.70 1.52 10.84 0.39
0–4 h 10.40 0.87 11.47 0.82 11.00 1.04 11.79 0.60
0–8 h 12.69 1.00 14.23 0.86 12.33 1.06 12.19 0.27
0–24 h 39.39 2.42 42.77 0.65 41.83 1.46 41.54 1.08
Animals were fasted from 0900 for 24 h on day 25 of the study (n = 6–8 per group). The following morning at 0900 a predetermined quantity of food was returned to the
cages. Food intake was measured at 1, 2, 4, 8, and 24 h after refeeding. Data is presented as mean and s.e.m. food weight in grams.

Table 3 Effect of environmental enrichment on fecal immunoglobulin A (IgA)


Control Chew sticks Tubing Chew sticks and tubing
Acclimatization period (day 6) 45.47 ± 5.68 39.21 ± 6.27 42.64 ± 6.93 32.43 ± 5.29
Termination of study (day 40) 5.33 ± 1.43** 19.52 ± 6.78 21.48 ± 5.74 48.87 ± 18.37*
Fecal samples were collected prior to the assignment of enrichment (acclimatization period) and at the termination of the study. IgA was extracted and quantified by
enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay. Data is expressed as mean ± s.e.m. (µg/ml) for all groups (n = 6–8 per group).
*P < 0.05 vs. controls,**P < 0.01 vs. controls during acclimatization period.

there was a significant increase in fecal IgA in animals housed (P < 0.01, n = 7) (Table 3). This change was not observed in
with both chew sticks and tubing compared to controls at the any other groups.
end of the study (P < 0.05, n = 6–8 per group) (Table 3).
Within the control group there was a significant reduc- Plasma corticosterone and ACTH
tion in IgA in fecal samples collected on day 40 compared to There was no significant difference in plasma corticosterone
those collected prior to the assignment of enrichment on day 6 (Table 4) or ACTH (data not shown) between groups.

1718 VOLUME 19 NUMBER 8 | august 2011 | www.obesityjournal.org


articles
Methods and Techniques

Table 4 Effect of environmental enrichment on plasma corticosterone


Control Chew sticks Tubing Chew sticks and tubing
Plasma corticosterone (ng/ml) 34.21 ± 9.47 34.11 ± 6.23 27.83 ± 5.42 19.38 ± 4.44
Trunk blood was collected at the termination of the experiment and plasma corticosterone measured by radioimmunoassay. Data is expressed as mean ± s.e.m. for all
groups (n = 6–8 per group).

Discussion the cage environment have previously been shown to reduce


The aim of this work was to determine the effect of environ- baseline food intake and thus mask the anorectic effect of
mental enrichment on body weight, food intake, behavior, intra-peritoneal administration of the gut hormones peptide
and fecal and plasma stress hormones in male Wistar rats. YY3-36 and oxyntomodulin in male Wistar rats (5). We found
Although the welfare benefits of environmental enrichment no difference in 24-h food intake the day prior to and the day
have been well characterized, the effects on experimental vari- following the addition of enrichment, suggesting that these
ability are not clear. Enrichment provides a significant refine- forms of enrichment do not acutely alter food intake in rats.
ment to animal husbandry because single housing, a necessity However, further work is required to confirm that enrichment
when studying energy homeostasis, is considered stressful for does not have more subtle effects, for example, influencing
social animals such as rats. the magnitude of the anorectic or orexigenic effects of other
Chew sticks were chosen as a form of environmental enrich- compounds.
ment because rats show a preference for chewable objects Behavior was studied in the early dark phase as this is
which allow them to express an important species-specific when rats are most active. A significant reduction in rear-
behavior (9). Tubing was chosen as the second form of enrich- ing was observed in the three groups with enrichment com-
ment as rats are prey species and show a preference for cages pared to controls. Additionally, when behaviors were grouped
containing dark, enclosed spaces to hide in (25). Although into categories, control rats demonstrated significantly more
these forms of enrichment are not as complex as some used in “active” behavior (rearing, locomotion, and burrowing) than
other studies, they are cheap, simple to reproduce and easy to the two groups with tubing. This is in agreement with work in
implement. It should be noted that although bedding can be which rats singly housed in barren cages showed significantly
considered a form of enrichment, all animals in our study were increased locomotion, rearing, and exploratory behavior com-
supplied with sawdust bedding in accordance with European pared to animals housed with environmental enrichment (18).
guidelines (4). Care should be taken when choosing bedding These behaviors are hypothesized to be stress-related, as intrac-
material for studies measuring food intake or other metabolic erebroventricular administration of corticotrophin-releasing
parameters. If an edible bedding material such as corn cob hormone significantly increases locomotion and rearing (29).
bedding is provided, rats may eat the bedding material, par- However, rearing and exploratory behavior can also be inter-
ticularly during a fast. This has been reported to confound end preted as reflecting a reduced state of fearfulness (30), while
point measurements including plasma glucose and triglycer- grooming can be considered a stress-related behavior in rats
ides (26). However, the sawdust bedding provided in this study (31). Although we did not observe any differences in grooming
was inedible. behavior between groups, it is possible that the differences in
Environmental enrichment had no effect on body weight rearing observed reflect altered fearfulness. There were no dif-
gain or food intake over the 5-day acclimatization period or ferences in body weight gain between groups at the end of the
the 7-day study period. Although the growth curves began to study, suggesting that the differences in behavior observed were
diverge by day 7, at the end of the entire 40-day study there was insufficient to significantly alter long-term energy expenditure
no significant difference in body weight between groups. These and hence body weight.
findings are in agreement with previous studies in mice which The percentage of time animals housed with tubing spent
showed that the addition of plastic refuges or pipes to the cages within the tubing was also recorded and found to be relatively
of adult animals had no effect on food intake and weight gain low. This may be because the behavioral analysis was carried
over a 12-day period (27,28). out at night when the animals are relatively active; through-
The effect of environmental enrichment on the refeeding out the course of the study, animals were observed to use their
response to a 24 h fast was investigated because fasting animals ­tubing to sleep in during the day.
is a standard procedure often used prior to administration of The reduction in stress-related behavior often observed
an anorectic agent (6). Importantly, there was no significant when animals are housed with environmental enrichment may
difference in the refeeding response to a 24 h fast between be due to effects on neuronal plasticity and cerebral develop-
groups. This suggests that environmental enrichment does not ment. By improving learning and memory, enrichment may
affect variability in this experimental paradigm and that estab- allow animals to adapt to new environments more quickly and
lished acute food intake protocols can be used as normal in hence reduce stress (17).
enriched animals. IgA is an antibody found in saliva, tears, and respiratory and
We found no chronic effects of environmental enrichment intestinal secretions. It is well known that chronic HPA axis
on food intake and body weight. However, acute changes in activation induces immunosuppression (13). In rats, fecal IgA

obesity | VOLUME 19 NUMBER 8 | August 2011 1719


articles
Methods and Techniques

decreases in response to social stress, surgery, and housing in In conclusion, this work has shown that the provision of
metabolic cages and it is therefore a useful marker for assess- environmental enrichment to singly housed male Wistar rats
ing long-term well-being (15,32). Fecal sampling also confers does not influence food intake or body weight over a short
advantages over other methods, for example plasma and saliva period of time. These data suggest that environmental enrich-
analysis, because samples can be collected easily without dis- ment should be routinely included in food intake and body
turbing the animal. weight assessment protocols in rats.
The effect of environmental enrichment on fecal IgA has
Acknowledgments
not previously been well characterized. There was no differ- This work was funded by two NC3Rs/LASA Small Awards. The animals
ence in fecal IgA between groups during the acclimatization were kindly donated by Charles River Laboratories. K.E.L.B. is supported
period. However, at the end of the study there was a significant by a Jean Shanks Foundation Studentship. Both K.G.M. and K.L.S. hold
increase in IgA in animals housed with both types of enrich- Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council New Investigator
Awards. The Department is funded by the National Institute for Health
ment, suggesting these animals had lower HPA axis activity Research Biomedical Research Centre Funding Scheme. Funded under
compared to controls. Interestingly, within the control group, FP7-HEALTH-2009-241592 (European Union) EurOCHIP.
fecal IgA was significantly lower on day 40 compared to dur-
ing the acclimatization period. This effect was not seen in any Disclosure
The authors declared no conflict of interest.
other group. This suggests that single housing causes a chronic
stress reaction in male Wistar rats, which is attenuated by envi- © 2011 The Obesity Society
ronmental enrichment.
REFERENCES
Corticosterone is released from the adrenal cortex of rodents 1. Thibault L, Woods SC, Westerterp-Plantenga MS. The utility of animal
in response to stress. It is secreted in a diurnal rhythm; in rats, models of human energy homeostasis. Br J Nutr 2004;92 Suppl 1:
plasma corticosterone levels peak in the early dark phase and S41–S45.
2. Hatch AM, Wiberg GS, Zawidzka Z et al. Isolation syndrome in the rat.
reach a nadir in the early light phase. We collected plasma cor- Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 1965;7:737–745.
ticosterone in the early light phase to allow small differences 3. Sharp JL, Zammit TG, Azar TA, Lawson DM. Stress-like responses to
between groups to be detected (33). Rats housed with both common procedures in male rats housed alone or with other rats. Contemp
Top Lab Anim Sci 2002;41:8–14.
tubing and chew sticks had the lowest plasma corticosterone, 4. Council of Europe. Appendix A of the European Convention For The
which suggests that environmental enrichment (particularly Protection Of Vertebrate Animals Used For Experimental and Other
tubing) may reduce stress. However, there was no difference in Scientific Purposes (ETS No 123). 15 June 2006, Strasbourg, pp.
1–109.
plasma ACTH between groups. This mismatch between corti- 5. Abbott CR, Small CJ, Sajedi A et al. The importance of acclimatisation
costerone and ACTH may be due to the ultradian rhythm of and habituation to experimental conditions when investigating the
the HPA axis. In addition to the daily circadian rhythm, bursts anorectic effects of gastrointestinal hormones in the rat. Int J Obes (Lond)
of corticosterone secretion occur approximately once an hour 2006;30:288–292.
6. Ellacott KL, Morton GJ, Woods SC, Tso P, Schwartz MW. Assessment of
in the rat and hence the matching of plasma corticosterone and feeding behavior in laboratory mice. Cell Metab 2010;12:10–17.
ACTH pulses is often poor (34). 7. Belz EE, Kennell JS, Czambel RK, Rubin RT, Rhodes ME.
It has previously been shown that male and female rats Environmental enrichment lowers stress-responsive hormones in singly
housed male and female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2003;76:
housed singly with environmental enrichment have sig- 481–486.
nificantly lower baseline plasma ACTH and corticosterone 8. Bradshaw AL, Poling A. Choice by rats for enriched versus standard home
compared to those housed without enrichment (7). We were cages: Plastic pipes, wood platforms, wood chips, and paper towels as
enrichment items. J Exp Anal Behav 1991;55:245–250.
not able to replicate this finding, perhaps because we only 9. Chmiel DJ Jr, Noonan M. Preference of laboratory rats for potentially
measured plasma corticosterone at one time point, rather enriching stimulus objects. Lab Anim 1996;30:97–101.
than continuously over several days. Additionally, our 10. Harati H, Majchrzak M, Cosquer B et al. Attention and memory in aged rats:
Impact of lifelong environmental enrichment. Neurobiol Aging 2009; e-pub
group sizes may not have been sufficiently large to detect
ahead of print 22 April 2009.
this change, as our study was powered to detect differences 11. Passineau MJ, Green EJ, Dietrich WD. Therapeutic effects of environmental
in body weight and food intake rather than changes in the enrichment on cognitive function and tissue integrity following severe
HPA axis. However, taken together, the changes detected in traumatic brain injury in rats. Exp Neurol 2001;168:373–384.
12. Diamond MC. Response of the brain to enrichment. An Acad Bras Cienc
behavior and fecal IgA suggest a reduction in HPA axis activ- 2001;73:211–220.
ity in animals housed with environmental enrichment and 13. Herbert TB, Cohen S. Stress and immunity in humans: a meta-analytic
confirm previously reported benefits of enrichment to animal review. Psychosom Med 1993;55:364–379.
14. Powell SB, Newman HA, McDonald TA, Bugenhagen P, Lewis MH.
welfare. Development of spontaneous stereotyped behavior in deer mice: effects of
Young adult male Wistar rats are commonly used to study early and late exposure to a more complex environment. Dev Psychobiol
energy homeostasis. However, further work is required to 2000;37:100–108.
15. Eriksson E, Royo F, Lyberg K, Carlsson HE, Hau J. Effect of metabolic
investigate the effect of environmental enrichment on food cage housing on immunoglobulin A and corticosterone excretion in
intake and body weight in different strains of rats, using a faeces and urine of young male rats. Exp Physiol 2004;89:427–433.
range of diets. In particular, the effect of environmental enrich- 16. Wurbel H, Garner JP. Refinement of rodent research through
ment in Sprague Dawley rats supplied on a high fat diet, which environmental enrichment and systematic randomization. 1–9
(1 Jan 2007).
are known to show a wide variation in body weight gain (35), 17. van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Neural consequences of
would be of interest. environmental enrichment. Nat Rev Neurosci 2000;1:191–198.

1720 VOLUME 19 NUMBER 8 | august 2011 | www.obesityjournal.org


articles
Methods and Techniques

18. Varty GB, Paulus MP, Braff DL, Geyer MA. Environmental enrichment 27. Robertson KL, Rowland NE. Effect of two types of environmental enrichment
and isolation rearing in the rat: effects on locomotor behavior and startle for singly housed mice on food intake and weight gain. Lab Anim (NY)
response plasticity. Biol Psychiatry 2000;47:864–873. 2005;34:29–32.
19. Park GA, Pappas BA, Murtha SM, Ally A. Enriched environment primes 28. Moons CP, Van Wiele P, Odberg FO. To enrich or not to enrich: providing
forebrain choline acetyltransferase activity to respond to learning experience. shelter does not complicate handling of laboratory mice. Contemp Top Lab
Neurosci Lett 1992;143:259–262. Anim Sci 2004;43:18–21.
20. Fray PJ, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW, Koob GF, Iversen SD. An 29. Song C, Earley B, Leonard BE. Behavioral, neurochemical, and
observational method for quantifying the behavioural effects of dopamine immunological responses to CRF administration. Is CRF a mediator of
agonists: contrasting effects of d-amphetamine and apomorphine. stress? Ann N Y Acad Sci 1995;771:55–72.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1980;69:253–259. 30. Townsend P. Use of in-cage shelters by laboratory rats. Anim Welf
21. Scott V, Kimura N, Stark JA, Luckman SM. Intravenous peptide YY3‑36 1997;6:95–103.
and Y2 receptor antagonism in the rat: effects on feeding behaviour. 31. Katz RJ, Roth KA. Stress induced grooming in the rat–an endorphin
J Neuroendocrinol 2005;17:452–457. mediated syndrome. Neurosci Lett 1979;13:209–212.
22. Pihl L, Hau J. Faecal corticosterone and immunoglobulin A in young adult 32. Royo F, Björk N, Carlsson HE, Mayo S, Hau J. Impact of chronic
rats. Lab Anim 2003;37:166–171. catheterization and automated blood sampling (Accusampler) on serum
23. Cooke JH, Patterson M, Patel SR et al. Peripheral and central administration corticosterone and fecal immunoreactive corticosterone metabolites and
of xenin and neurotensin suppress food intake in rodents. Obesity immunoglobulin A in male rats. J Endocrinol 2004;180:145–153.
(Silver Spring) 2009;17:1135–1143. 33. Kasanen IH, Inhilä KJ, Vainio OM et al. The diet board: welfare impacts
24. Gardiner JV, Bataveljic A, Patel NA et al. Prokineticin 2 is a hypothalamic of a novel method of dietary restriction in laboratory rats. Lab Anim
neuropeptide that potently inhibits food intake. Diabetes 2010;59:397–406. 2009;43:215–223.
25. Manser CE, Broom DM, Overend P, Morris TH. Investigations into the 34. Bornstein SR, Engeland WC, Ehrhart-Bornstein M, Herman JP.
preferences of laboratory rats for nest-boxes and nesting materials. Lab Dissociation of ACTH and glucocorticoids. Trends Endocrinol Metab
Anim 1998;32:23–35. 2008;19:175–180.
26. Zahorsky-Reeves J, Castellani LW. Housing Mice on Corncob Bedding 35. Levin BE, Dunn-Meynell AA, Balkan B, Keesey RE. Selective breeding for
versus Hardwood Chip May Confound Research Models. Am Assoc Lab diet-induced obesity and resistance in Sprague-Dawley rats. Am J Physiol
Anim Sci (10 October 2010). 1997;273:R725–R730.

obesity | VOLUME 19 NUMBER 8 | August 2011 1721

You might also like