You are on page 1of 6

Journal of Gerontology

1978, Vol. 33, No. 3, 402-407

Adult Age Differences in


Reflection-Impulsivity
Andrew C. Coyne, BA,1 Susan K. Whitbourne, PhD2
and David S. Glenwick, PhD3
Adult age differences in reflection-impulsivity were examined on a modified version of Kagan's adult
Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFF). The sample consisted of 20 elderly adults (mean age = 70.6)
and 20 young adults (mean age = 21.9), each of whom was administered the modified MFF by an adult
examiner. On half of the 24 items, the original (i.e., the standard) was present among the four alter-
natives ("original present" condition), while on the other half, all the alternatives were variants of the
standard ("original not present" condition). The elderly subjects were generally more impulsive (had
shorter latencies and committed more errors) than were the young adults. The results, though, varied
somewhat with the two stimulus conditions employed. An analysis of the mean scores of both age groups
indicated that the elderly took less time to respond only in the "original present" condition. A higher
number of errors was observed for the older group in both conditions, and both age groups had fewer
errors in the "original present" condition. Possible interpretations of these findings, in terms of age
group differences in search strategy, anxiety, and other aspects of decision style are offered, and implica-
tions for further research are provided.

HE term cognitive style refers to a person's to compare young adult and elderly groups of
T characteristic approach to a problem-solv-
ing situation. Reflection and impulsivity, as
men and women on a modification of the adult
version of the MFF to determine whether
originally defined by Kagan (1965a), are two reflection-impulsivity differs over the span
extreme points along one dimension of cogni- of adulthood.
tive style. Reflective persons take longer to Indirect evidence on the elderly may be
produce a response and make fewer errors than found in studies on reaction time, in which a
impulsive individuals, who perform more slowing-down of behavior is generally ob-
quickly and less accurately. served (Birren, 1964; Birren & Botwinick,
The Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFF) 1955; Da vies, 1968; Surwillo, 1968). This may
(Kagan, 1965a) is a perceptual match-to- be due to alterations in central processing
sample task frequently used to measure mechanisms responsible for decision-making
reflection-impulsivity. It yields scores on (Fozard & Thomas, 1975), an increase in
errors and latencies, from which categorization random noise in the nervous system (Welford,
of a subject as reflective or impulsive can be 1977), and/or overall health status (Botwinick
made. Among children, the two measures are & Thompson, 1971). Moreover, motivational
negatively correlated, ranging from -.60 to -.30 factors, amount of practice, and testing condi-
(Eska & Black, 1971; Kagan, 1965a). Kagan tions can also affect the degree of response
and Kogan (1970) have also noted that with slowing exhibited by the elderly adult in a given
increasing age in childhood, regardless of sex, task (Botwinick, 1973; Fozard & Thomas,
there is an increase on the MFF in response 1975). When given sufficient time to respond,
times and a decrease in errors. older persons can maintain accurate
Despite the wealth of data accumulated on performance (e.g., Thompson et al., 1963).
children, there is comparatively little on This raises the question of the degree to which
reflection-impulsivity in young adults (Drake, response slowing is due to greater cautiousness
1970; Wolfe et al., 1972) and even less in the in old age or to diminished sensorimotor
elderly. The purpose of the present study was abilities. According to Welford (1977):
Determining the relative contributions of both has
'Dept. of Psychology, Washington Univ. practical as well as theoretical implications: funda-
2
(Formerly Susan K. Gordon.) Assistant Professor, Education & Psy-
chology, College of Education, Univ. of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14627. mental changes are essentially a physical problem whereas
3
Kent State Univ. changes of strategy may be amenable to training.

402

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/geronj/article-abstract/33/3/402/592893


by guest
on 01 April 2018
AGE DIFFERENCES IN REFLECTION-IMPULSIVITY 403

If cautiousness were a factor affecting per- half of the trials, the task becomes more diffi-
formance in the present study, then a greater cult for the trials in both test conditions. Since
proportion of elderly persons could be a series of complex discriminations is involved,
expected to fall into the reflective category, this additional element should further highlight
since they could maintain a lower error rate age differences in reaction time and latencies
by increasing their response latency. (Botwinick et al., 1958). Moreover, potential
In contrast, a number of studies lead to the variations in the search strategies employed by
opposite hypothesis: that the elderly are more persons in the two age groups can be inferred
impulsive than their younger counterparts. from differential performance in the two test
From Wapner and colleagues' (1960) regres- conditions.
sion hypothesis one would expect a return to On the basis of the existing literature on adult
childhood levels of impulsivity in old age. age differences in cognitive tempo, the follow-
In addition, personality research suggests that ing hypotheses were tested: (1) adult age
the elderly may respond more to their instinc- groups will differ in errors and latencies on the
tual drives and hence show greater impulsivity MFF and (2) errors and latencies will differ
than younger adults (Davidson & Kruglov, in the two conditions of "original present" and
1952; Neugarten, 1963, 1964; Pollack & "original not present." The direction of these
Kastenbaum, 1964; Rankin & Johnson, 1962; differences and the relationship of age to the
Singer, 1963). On field independence-depen- original present-absent variable were not
dence, another common measure of cognitive specified, due to inconclusive previous
style, the elderly show greater field depen- evidence.
dence (Marcus, 1971; Schwartz & Karp, 1967).
In light of Witkin and colleagues' (1962) METHOD
conclusion that field dependency is related to Subjects
less effective impulse control, an increase in The sample consisted of 20 elderly adults
impulsivity in old age would then be expected. (mean age = 70.6; range = 61 to 87) and 20
The possibility remains that the MFF young adults (mean age = 21.9; range = 18 to
performance of older adults is neither clearly 27), with 10 males and 10 females in each
impulsive nor clearly reflective. Rabbitt age group. The elderly persons who volun-
(1965,1968) has observed that the elderly have teered were solicited through various com-
greater difficulty in discriminating among munity centers. All were in good health with
complex stimuli. To the extent that cautious- no obvious cognitive impairments. The young
ness cannot compensate for decreased neural adults, none of whom was a college graduate,
efficiency in the elderly, then a pattern of slow volunteered through informal community
and inaccurate performance would be ex- organizations. Visual screening was accom-
pected, since the MFF requires complex dis- plished by having each potential subject
crimination and analytic abilities. complete a sample picture set, similar to those
The typical classification of reflection-impul- in the actual test, and read a caption. Although
siveity, using simultaneous median splits on an effort had been made to obtain the elderly
the two dimensions of latency and errors, and young adult samples from similar socio-
ignores those minorities who either (a) respond economic levels, they differed significantly on
slowly but inaccurately or (b) quickly but years of formal education, ?(38)=3.86,
correctly (Block et al., 1974). To gain a more p < .001. The mean education of the elderly
complete picture of cognitive tempo in the was 9.75 (range = 3 to 14 years) and 12.3
elderly, in the present research all four (range = 9 to 14 years) for the young adults.4
quadrants were considered: (a) slow/accurate
(reflective), (b) fast-inaccurate (impulsive), (c) Materials
slow/inaccurate, and (d) fast/accurate. The original adult version of the MFF con-
The standard MFF was modified in the sists of 12 sets of pictures of familiar objects,
present study, yielding two conditions: such as a dog, a plane, a bed, a lion, and the
presence or absence of a duplicate of the
standard among the four choices presented 4
For practical purposes, the difference in educational level may be seen
as minimal. During the period in which the elderly subjects were of a
to the subject. It may be assumed that when comparable age to the young volunteers, the general population had
the original is not present among the choices in fewer years of formal education. For further discussion of this point, see
Botwinick (1977).

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/geronj/article-abstract/33/3/402/592893


by guest
on 01 April 2018
404 COYNE, WHITBOURNE, AND GLENWICK

like. Each set is composed of one picture (i.e., latency) on a manually-operated stop-
(the standard) on a page horizontally facing a watch. Following completion of the MFF, each
second page on which there are eight pictures subject was asked to indicate his or her age and
of the same object. Of the eight pictures, only number of years of education.
one is an exact duplicate of the standard, while
the other seven are slight variations. Each RESULTS
picture is approximately 2.5 in by 2.5 in and is Preliminary analyses were carried out to
reproduced in black ink on a white back- determine whether the typically found negative
ground. correlation between errors and latencies would
In the present study, the number of choices be observed in the present sample. With age
for each set in the MFF was reduced to four. partialled out, the correlation in the original
There were 24 sets of pictures: (a) one set of 12 present condition was -.43, (p < .01), and in
standards with three variants and one duplicate the original not present it was -.45 (p < .01).
serving as the choices ("original present") and These correlations fall within the range of those
(b) a second set of the same 12 standards with reported in the reflection-impulsivity literature
four variants and no duplicate serving as the on children. The data from the 12 trials of the
choices ("original not present"). The variants "original present" condition were then
were those used in the standard MFF. Each of analyzed using the fourfold categorization of
the 24 standards appeared on a page horizon- cognitive tempo by simultaneous median splits
tally facing the four choices on a second page. on number of errors and latency. The median
For each trial, the subject performed one latency for the total sample was 36.05 sec, and
matching task. The 24 trials were arranged the median number of errors was 4.17 errors.
according to two different random orders. The mean latencies for response times in the
Each half of the sample received one of the fast and slow groups were 25.09 and 54.29 sec,
random orders. The choices were also dis- respectively. In the accurate group, the mean
tributed randomly within each of the two number of errors was 2.1, while in the inaccu-
conditions. rate group the mean was 6.6. No scores fell
exactly on the median. A breakdown of the
Procedure subjects, based on these median splits, is
Each subject was seated at a table in a presented in Table 1.
well-lit room with the modified MFF (de- A chi-square test performed on these data to
scribed above) on a stand in front of him or her. detect differences in distribution between the
The following instructions were read: two age groups over the categories was signifi-
cant, x2(3) = 18.16, p < .001, C = .56. Further
I am going to show you a number of sets of pictures examination indicated that the greatest incre-
of familiar objects. Each set will consist of one pic-
ture on the top page and four similar pictures of the ments to the total chi-square value were
same object on the bottom page. I would like you to contributed by the disproportionately high
tell me whether or not you find an exact duplicate of number of elderly in the impulsive category,
the picture on the top page among the four on the and the relatively greater number of younger
bottom page. If you do not find a duplicate, please
indicate this to me. If you do find a duplicate, please
persons in the fast/accurate grouping.
indicate which of the four pictures it is. Age differences in errors and latencies were
then analyzed for the two testing conditions,
After being shown a sample set of pictures collapsed over trials. Two 2 (Age Group) x 2
and being reminded that each set may or may
not contain a duplicate, each subject was Table 1. Classification of Subjects by MFF Errors
individually administered the MFF by a male and Latency.
young adult examiner. Contrary to standard
instructions, each subject was allowed only Age Stow/ Fast/
one response per item. This procedure was Group Inaccurate Impulsive Reflective Accurate
designed to eliminate possible interference Old 5
caused by negative feedback and to reduce the
total testing time. The subject's response to Young 10
each set of pictures was recorded, along with
the number of seconds for each response Note: N for each group is 20.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/geronj/article-abstract/33/3/402/592893


by guest
on 01 April 2018
AGE DIFFERENCES IN REFLECTION-IMPULSIVITY 405

(Conditions) analyses of variance were con- adults on a modified version of the adult MFF,
ducted, with repeated measures on the second the older subjects were found to be generally
factor. Although there had been age dif- more impulsive than the young group who, in
ferences in years of education, these did not turn, were more accurate at both latencies.
alter the results on the two dependent mea- This result, when contrasted to data obtained
sures of the study.5 in research on children, suggests a curvilinear
The means and standard deviations of error relationship between age and cognitive tempo.
scores by age group and condition are shown in This conclusion, however, needs to be
Table 2. The main effects of age, F(l,38) = qualified by consideration of performance
38.18, p < .001, and condition, F(l,38) = differences under the two stimulus conditions.
130,00, p < .001, were found to be significant.
Thus, both age groups committed fewer errors Table 3. Means and Standard Deviations of
when the original was included in the matching Latencies (Sec) on Matching Familiar Figures
choices, and the elderly had a higher error rate Task by Age and Condition.
than the younger group in both test conditions.
Original Original
Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations of Present Not Present
Errors on Matching Familiar Figures Task by Age Group X SD X SD
Age and Condition. Elderly 35.82 15.03 33.56 16.15
(N = 20)
Original Original
Present Not Present Young Adult 43.56 19.29 30.28 10.12
Age Group ~X SD ~X SD (N = 20)

Elderly 6.10 2.13 10.80 J.96


(N = 20)
More specifically, thefindingscan lead to the
Young Adult 2.60 2.16 7.10 2.65 interpretation that in the' 'original not present''
(N = 20) condition, the two age groups took approxi-
mately the same amount of time to answer,
The means and standard deviations on but the elderly made relatively fewer correct
latencies are presented in Table 3. There was decisions. In the "original present" condition,
a significant main effect of condition, F(l,38) both age groups increased their latencies and
= 23.45, p < .001, but not age, F(l,38) < 1. decreased the number of errors (compared
The interaction of age x condition was to their performance in the "original not
significant, F( 1,38) = 11.79,/? < .001. Post-hoc present" condition). However, the young
comparisons showed that the interaction was adults increased their latencies more than did
accounted for by the relatively greater dif- the elderly.
ference in latencies between the two conditions An explanation for the abovefindingscannot
for the young adults,F( 1,38) = 34.27,/? <.001, be determined from the present data. Clearly,
than the elderly, F(l,38), p < 1. The older there were combined effects of age differences
persons, in contrast, demonstrated similar in neurological functioning and cognitive
response times in both the original present strategy reflected in the differential response-
and the original not present conditions. time performance in the two conditions.
Among the most plausible interpretations are
DISCUSSION the following:
(1) The two age groups could have employed
Cognitive tempo is considered to be a
different search strategies; the young adults
process that undergoes developmental change
using a dissimilarity strategy, while the elderly
in childhood (Kagan, 1965a). In the present
might have used a similarity strategy, attempt-
research, this process was investigated with
ing to locate the one alternative that matched
respect to age differences in adulthood.
the standard by seeking similarities among the
Through comparisons of elderly and young
choices and the standard.
(2) Alternatively, the two age groups could
'Separate 2 x 2 analyses of covariance, with years of education as the
covariate, yielded similar main effects and interactions as the analyses
have employed the same search strategy, with
of variance reported here. the young adults being more thorough in their

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/geronj/article-abstract/33/3/402/592893


by guest
on 01 April 2018
406 COYNE, WHITBOURNE, AND GLENWICK

use of it. Thus, over trials in the "original to regulate nonverbal behavior. Employing
present" condition, the young adults had such a procedure, Labouvie-Vief and Gonda
longer latencies than did the elderly because (1976) produced gains on measures of inductive
they were more thorough and repetitious in reasoning. Other training procedures aimed at
checking out all dimensions of the pictorial increasing the elderly's cognitive abilities have
stimuli before offering a response. involved modeling of appropriate questions
This interpretation receives support from (Denney & Denney, 1974) and increasing
visual scanning research, which has been therapy group members' awareness of coping
extensively conducted with children (e.g., strategies useful in handling problematic
Ault et al., 1972; Siegelman, 1969). Further- situations (Kahana, Ref. Note 1). These and
more, Drake (1970) found that both impulsive similar approaches appear to warrant addi-
and reflective undergraduates appeared to tional exploration and application for poten-
employ the same general task strategy of tially improving cognitive performance among
looking for differences between the variants the aged.
and the standards; however, the reflective
undergraduates applied this approach more SUMMARY
precisely. Adult age differences in reflection-impul-
(3) Research with children has suggested sivity were examined on a modified version
(Messer, 1976) that impulsives are anxious of Kagan's adult Matching Familiar Figures
over a basic inability to perform competently Test. Twenty elderly and 20 young adult males
on a wide variety of tasks, while reflectives and females were presented with two condi-
are anxious due to uncertainty over making tions of the test, each consisting of 12 standards
mistakes on intellectual tasks. The elderly may and four choices per standard. In the "original
have been more anxious about having their present" condition, the standard was included
cognitive abilities tested (Whitbourne, 1976). among the picture stimuli presented, while in
This may have resulted, in the present study, the "original not present" condition, all stimuli
in the elderly's answering more quickly (on the presented were variants of the standard. The
trials in the "original present" condition) and standard analysis, in which subjects' per-
more inaccurately than the young adults. formance are categorized on the basis of
Further research might investigate the median error scores and median latencies, was
practical significance for the elderly of an initially employed. From the results of
impulsive or reflective cognitive style. Such categorization into cognitive tempos, it was
studies exist on the personality, social, and suggested that the older subjects were more
intellectual correlates of the MFF in children. impulsive than their younger counterparts.
For example, reflective pupils are superior to Further examination of the data, based on
impulsives in reading (Kagan, 1965b) and role- analyses of variance for performance by age
taking ability (Glenwick & Burka, 1975) and group and condition, suggested that perhaps
are rated more positively by peers and teachers the older persons were not simply more impul-
(Block et al., 1974; Glenwick et al., 1976). It sive than the young. Interpretations for these
would be interesting to explore whether findings included the possibility of the two age
impulsive elderly persons are at a similar groups employing different search strategies,
disadvantage as their younger counterparts. the differential use of the same strategy,
Finally, the present results provide further and the effects of anxiety on each group's per-
support and impetus for the development of formance. Implications of the present study for
cognitive training strategies for the elderly. research concerning the significance for the
Such strategies are based on the assumption elderly of an impulsive or reflective cognitive
that the elderly's reduced performance on style and the development of cognitive training
tasks such as the MFF is due primarily to strategies for the elderly were also discussed.
environmental and experiential factors rather
than to physiological decrement. Thus,
Meichenbaum (1974) has advocated self-
instructional training programs to provide the REFERENCE NOTE

elderly with covert self-statements to improve 1. Kahana, B. Training the aged for "competent
task comprehension, production of verbal coping" - a psychotherapeutic strategy. Unpub-
lished manuscript, 1976, Available from author,
mediators, and utilization of such mediators Oakland University.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/geronj/article-abstract/33/3/402/592893


by guest
on 01 April 2018
AGE DIFFERENCES IN REFLECTION-IMPULSIVITY 407

REFERENCES processes. In P. Mussen (Ed:), Carmichael's manual


Ault, R. L., Crawford, D. E., & Jeffrey, W. E. Visual of child psychology. John Wiley, New York, 1970.
scanning strategies of reflective, impulsive, fast- Labouvie-Vief, G., & Gonda, J. N. Cognitive strategy
accurate, and slow-inaccurate children on the training and intellectual performance in the elderly.
Matching Familiar Figures Test. Child Development, Journal of Gerontology, 1976,5/, 327-332.
1972,45, 1412-1417. Markus, E. J. Perceptual field dependence among aged
Birren, J. E. The psychology of aging. Prentice-Hall, persons. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 1971,55, 175-178.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 1964. Meichenbaum, D. Self-instructional strategy training: A
Birren, J. E., & Botwinick, J. Speed of response as a cognitive prothesis for the aged. Human Development,
function of perceptual difficulty and age. Journal of 1974, 17, 273-280.
Gerontology, 1955, 10, 433-436. Messer, S. B. Reflection-impulsivity: A review. Psycho-
Block, J., Block, J. H., & Harrington, D. M. Some logical Bulletin, 1976.55, 1036-1052.
misgivings about the Matching Familiar Figures Test as Neugarten, B. L. Personality and the aging process. In
a measure of reflection-impulsivity. Developmental R. H. Williams, C. Tibbitts, & W. Donahue (Eds.),
Psychology, 1974, 10, 611-632. Processes of aging, Vol. 1. Atherton Press, New York,
Botwinick, J. Aging and behavior: A comprehensive 1963.
integration of research findings. Springer, New York, Neugarten, B. L. Personality in middle and late life.
1973. Atherton Press, New York, 1964.
Botwinick, J. Intellectual abilities. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Pollack, K., & Kastenbaum, R. Delay of gratification in
Schaie (Eds.). Handbook of the psychology of aging. later life: An experimental analog. In R. Kastenbaum
Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York, 1977. (Ed.), New thoughts on old age. Springer, New York,
Botwinick, J., Brinley, J. F., & Robbin, J. S. The inter- 1964.
action effects of perceptual difficulty and stimulus ex- Rabbitt, P. An age-decrement in the ability to ignore
posure time on age differences in speed and irrelevant information. Journal of Gerontology, 1965,
accuracy of response. Gerontologia, 1958,2, 1-10. 20, 233-238.
Botwinick, J., & Thompson, L. W. Cardiac functioning Rabbitt, P. Age and transmitted information. In G. A.
and reaction time in relation to age. Journal of Genetic Talland (Ed.), Human aging and behavior. Academic
Psychology, 1971, 119, 127-132. Press, New York, 1968.
Davidson, H., & Kruglov, L. Personality characteristics of Rankin, R., & Johnson, J. Influence of age and sex on the
the institutionalized aged. Journal of Consulting IES test. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 1962, 15,
Psychology, 1952, 16, 5-12. 775-778.
Davies, D. R. Age differences in paced inspection tasks. Schwartz, D., & Karp, S. Field dependence in a geriatric
In G. A. Talland (Ed.), Human aging and behavior. population. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 1967, 24,
Academic Press, New York, 1968. 495-504.
Denney, N. W., & Denney, D. R. Modeling effects on the Siegelman, E. Reflective and impulsive observing be-
questioning strategies of the elderly. Developmental havior. Child Development, 1969,40, 1213-1222.
Psychology, 1974, 10, 400-404. Singer, M. Personality measurements in the aged. In J. E.
Drake, D. M. Perceptual correlates of impulsive and Birren (Ed.), Human aging. USDHEW, Bethesda,
reflective behavior. Developmental Psychology, 1963.
1970,2, 202-214. Surwillo, W. W. Timing of behavior in senescence and the
Eska, B., & Black, K. N. Conceptual tempo in young role of the central nervous system. In G. A. Talland
grade-school children. Child Development, 1971, 42, (Ed.), Human aging and behavior. Academic Press,
505-516. New York, 1968.
Fozard, J. L., Thomas, J. C. Psychology of aging: Basic Thompson, L. W., Opton, E., & Cohen, L. D. Effects of
findings and some psychiatric application. In J. G. age, presentation speed, and sensory modality on
Howells (Ed.), Modern perspectives in. the performance of a 'vigilance' task. Journal of Geron-
psychiatry of old age. Bruner/Mazel, New York, 1975. tology, 1963, 18, 366-369.
Glenwick, D. S., Barocas, R., & Burka, A. A. Some Wapner, S., Werner, H., & Comalli, P. E. Perception of
interpersonal correlates of cognitive impulsivity in part-whole relationships in middle and old age.
fourth-graders. Journal of School Psychology, 1976, Journal of Gerontology, 1960,75, 412-416.
14, 212-221. Welford, A. T. Motor performance. In J. E. Birren, & K.
Glenwick, D. S., & Burka, A. A. Cognitive impulsivity W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of
and role-taking skills in elementary-school children. aging. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York, 1977.
Perceptual & Motor Skills, 1975, 41, 547-552. Whitbourne, S. K. Test anxiety in elderly and young
Kagan, J. Impulsive and reflective children: Significance of adults. International Journal of Aging & Human
conceptual tempo. In J. D. Krumboltz (Ed.), Learning Development, 1976, 7, 201-209.
and the educational process. Rand McNally, Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R. B., Faterson, H. F., Goodenough,
Chicago, 1965. (a) D. R., & Karp, S. A. Psychological differentiation.
Kagan, J. Reflection-impulsivity and reading ability in John Wiley, New York, 1962.
primary grade children. Child Development, 1965, 36, Wolfe, R., Egleston, R., & Powers, J. Conceptual
609-628. (b) structure and conceptual tempo. Perceptual & Motor
Kagan, J., & Kogan, N. Individual variation in cognitive Skills, 1972,55, 331-337.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/geronj/article-abstract/33/3/402/592893


by guest
on 01 April 2018

You might also like